12.12.2012 Views

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

16 The Praeger H<strong>and</strong>book of Education <strong>and</strong> Psychology<br />

complexes, contexts, <strong>and</strong> relationships that shape the lives of diverse individuals. Knowledge in<br />

a critical interpretivist epistemology no longer simply resides in textbooks <strong>and</strong> students’ brains.<br />

Instead, critical interpretivist knowledge is always being constructed, always being produced in<br />

the interaction of perspectives generated in diverse contexts. As learners examine these diverse<br />

knowledge constructions <strong>and</strong> their relationships to one another, they begin to aspire to a higher<br />

domain of cognitive thought. The process of moving to these higher levels of thinking is a<br />

powerful <strong>and</strong> exciting activity. Its promise of new insights about self <strong>and</strong> world motivate me to<br />

engage in this work on educational psychology.<br />

MOVING TO A NEW EPISTEMOLOGICAL TERRAIN<br />

Many scholars have argued over the last three or four decades that a correspondence epistemology<br />

promotes a misleading portrait of the process of recognition. Recognition does not<br />

consist of simply comparing two pictures with one another. The process is much more complex,<br />

as illustrated in human beings’ recognition of emotional feelings, justice, <strong>and</strong> genius. One does<br />

not hold a picture of genius up to what he or she is observing in the lived world—other types<br />

of thinking are operating in this context. The individual here is producing situated <strong>and</strong> implicit<br />

knowledges that help him or her interpret the nature <strong>and</strong> meaning of the phenomenon he or she<br />

is encountering. Thus, a simple correspondence-based test cannot be used in such situations to<br />

determine if the observer has accurately represented reality.<br />

Jeanette Bopry in her chapter on Francisco Varela extends this epistemological point. This<br />

correspondence dynamic, she asserts, does not help us underst<strong>and</strong> the way dogs perceive the<br />

world. Dogs’ ways of constructing the world is very different from humans but is not “wrong.”<br />

Such a reality implies that there are numerous ways of making sense of the world that work<br />

for the individual or animal that constructs them. Perceptions emerge when cognitive systems<br />

interact with the environmental context surrounding them. Bopry adeptly articulates this point:<br />

“My description of a sunset is not a description of an external phenomenon as much as it is a<br />

description of my own visual field.” Thus, knower <strong>and</strong> known are eternally joined together, as no<br />

constructions of reality can be made without the presence of both mind <strong>and</strong> environment.<br />

In this context we can clearly underst<strong>and</strong> the epistemological foundations on which interpretivism<br />

rests. The interaction/connection between the individual <strong>and</strong> culture <strong>and</strong> the knower <strong>and</strong><br />

the known is central to an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the learning process. Indeed, the cultural system of<br />

which one is a part profoundly shapes the ways one thinks, the ways one constructs the world<br />

around oneself. Because of the diversity of such contexts <strong>and</strong> the infinite ways they shape cognitive<br />

behavior, mechanistic efforts to generate universal general laws are futile. Guided by a<br />

constructivist epistemology, interpretivists view cognition as a contextually specific, interactive,<br />

ever-evolving process in which the person both constructs <strong>and</strong> is constructed by the various<br />

contexts enveloping him or her.<br />

Operating on this new epistemological terrain, interpretivists underst<strong>and</strong> they must be better<br />

scholars than those who preceded them in educational psychology. They must gain an interdisciplinary<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the cognitive process. (See Lara Lee’s chapter, “Reconnecting the<br />

Disconnect in Teacher–Student Communication in Education,” on the role of communications in<br />

an interdisciplinary educational psychology.) In this context they enter the bricolage, making use<br />

of diverse disciplinary tools <strong>and</strong> perspectives to gain a deeper <strong>and</strong> thicker view of these complex<br />

social, cultural, economic, political, philosophical, <strong>and</strong> psychological dynamics. Such insights<br />

dramatically reorient our pedagogical underst<strong>and</strong>ings, as we are empowered as scholar-teachers<br />

to discern the ways particular students in specific circumstances construct their own meanings<br />

of academic experiences (see Alison Cook-Sather’s chapter “Recognizing Students among <strong>Educational</strong><br />

Authorities”). Contrary to the pronouncements of many, such epistemological/cognitive

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!