26.03.2013 Views

Girl Friend, Boy Friend, Girlfriend, Boyfriend: Broadening Our ...

Girl Friend, Boy Friend, Girlfriend, Boyfriend: Broadening Our ...

Girl Friend, Boy Friend, Girlfriend, Boyfriend: Broadening Our ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Transform your PDFs into Flipbooks and boost your revenue!

Leverage SEO-optimized Flipbooks, powerful backlinks, and multimedia content to professionally showcase your products and significantly increase your reach.

Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology<br />

2007, Vol. 36, No. 4, 491–502<br />

<strong>Girl</strong> <strong>Friend</strong>, <strong>Boy</strong> <strong>Friend</strong>, <strong>Girl</strong>friend, <strong>Boy</strong>friend: <strong>Broadening</strong> <strong>Our</strong><br />

Understanding of Heterosocial Competence<br />

Rachel L. Grover<br />

Loyola College in Maryland<br />

Douglas W. Nangle and Agnieszka Serwik<br />

University of Maine<br />

Karen R. Zeff<br />

Brown University<br />

Heterosocial interactions, or social interactions with other-sex peers, are<br />

theorized to serve a number of unique developmental functions for adolescents<br />

(e.g., companionship, intimacy, experimentation with sex-role behaviors and<br />

sexual activity). Yet despite the importance of heterosocial competence in adolescent<br />

social development, there exists little research on the construct. Early<br />

research, although informative, reflects a narrow focus on heterosocial anxiety<br />

associated with date initiation situations. More recent research has broadened<br />

its purview to include investigations of the relation of heterosocial competence<br />

to the normative development of romantic relationships, as well as depression,<br />

anxiety, and violence in adolescent relationships. To facilitate further research,<br />

an expanded, well-defined description of the construct of adolescent heterosocial<br />

competence is needed that includes other-sex interactions in casual<br />

relationships, friendships, and romantic relationships. In this article we detail<br />

the historical and current definitions of the construct and encourage a broader<br />

understanding in the context of the developmental psychopathology framework.<br />

We follow with a review of our decade-long efforts to better operationally define<br />

and assess the construct by summarizing the development and validation of<br />

two measures: The Measure of Adolescent Heterosocial Competence and the<br />

Measure of Adolescent Heterosocial Competence–Young Adult Version.<br />

Finally, we suggest directions for future research.<br />

Interactions with other-sex peers, also referred to<br />

as heterosocial interactions, serve unique developmental<br />

functions for adolescents. In fact, major<br />

developmental theorists view this dimension of<br />

social behavior as critical to long-term adjustment<br />

(e.g., Erikson, 1963; Sullivan, 1953). The emergence<br />

of mixed-sex peer groups is theorized to aid in<br />

aspects of both social and emotional development.<br />

Portions of this paper were presented as part of the symposium<br />

<strong>Girl</strong> friend, <strong>Boy</strong> friend, girlfriend, boyfriend: Other-sex<br />

relationships and adolescent adjustment (R.L. Grover & D.W.<br />

Nangle, Chairs) in March 2004 at the meeting of the Society<br />

for Research on Adolescence, Baltimore, MD.<br />

Correspondence should be addressed to Rachel L. Grover,<br />

PhD, Department of Psychology, Loyola College in Maryland,<br />

4501 N. Charles St. Baltimore, MD, 21210. E-mail: rlgrover@<br />

loyola.edu<br />

491<br />

Copyright # 2007 by<br />

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.<br />

Participation in mixed-sex peer groups extends<br />

and diversifies the social support available to<br />

adolescents (Bukowski, Sippola, & Hoza, 1999;<br />

Kuttler, La Greca, & Prinstein, 1999). Moreover,<br />

heterosocial interactions set the stage for adolescents<br />

to learn and practice adultlike behaviors<br />

and roles (Furman & Shaffer, 2003; Sippola,<br />

1999). Heterosocial interactions are also assumed<br />

to serve unique functions including opportunities<br />

for companionship, intimacy, and experimentation<br />

with sex-role behaviors and sexual activity<br />

(Hansen, Christopher, & Nangle, 1992).<br />

Heterosocial competence is hereby broadly<br />

defined as the ability to effectively negotiate social<br />

situations that involve the other sex including<br />

acquaintanceships, friendships, romantic, and sexual<br />

relationships. The requisite social skills are


therefore referred to as heterosocial skills (Hansen<br />

et al., 1992). Although largely undefined, initial<br />

research suggests that heterosocial skills are both<br />

highly related to yet distinct from the broader class<br />

of general social skills (Grover, Nangle, & Zeff,<br />

2005). The overlap is not surprising as both sameand<br />

other-sex relationships require the ability to<br />

initiate and maintain conversation, negotiate conflict,<br />

and act in socially appropriate ways (Kelly,<br />

1982). On the other hand, heterosocial relationships<br />

also necessitate the ability to negotiate the<br />

anxiety that is unique to other-sex situations,<br />

establish potentially deeper levels of intimacy,<br />

and integrate emerging sexuality and passion<br />

(Connolly, Furman, & Konarski, 2000).<br />

Over the past few decades, the social transition<br />

in adolescence from same-sex peer groups to<br />

mixed-sex friendships and romantic relationships<br />

has garnered increased attention from both developmental<br />

and clinical researchers (e.g., Connolly<br />

et al., 2000; Glickman & La Greca, 2004). With<br />

this attention has come an increased understanding<br />

of the importance of heterosocial competence.<br />

Although early clinical research in this area<br />

reflected a narrow focus on heterosocial anxiety<br />

associated with date initiation situations, more<br />

recent clinical research has broadened its purview<br />

to include investigations of how heterosocial competence<br />

may be related to depression, anxiety,<br />

academic adjustment, and violence in teen relationships<br />

(e.g., Davila, Steinberg, Kachadourian,<br />

Cobb, & Fincham, 2004; Glickman & La Greca,<br />

2004). To facilitate further research, an expanded<br />

understanding of the construct of adolescent heterosocial<br />

competence is needed. Next we detail<br />

the past and present trends in research and theory<br />

pertaining to the construct and encourage a<br />

broader understanding in the context of the developmental<br />

psychopathology framework. Then we<br />

summarize our decade-long efforts to better operationally<br />

define and assess the construct. Finally,<br />

we suggest some directions for future research<br />

including an increased focus on diversity and<br />

developmental issues.<br />

Historical and Current Considerations<br />

of the Construct<br />

Despite the pivotal role of heterosocial competence<br />

in adolescent social development, for decades<br />

there was a paucity of relevant research on<br />

this construct. The lack of focus on adolescent heterosocial<br />

competence in the developmental literature<br />

appeared to be part of a broader absence of<br />

interest in sex differences in peer relations research<br />

(Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker, 1998). The indices of<br />

492<br />

GROVER, NANGLE, SERWICK, ZEFF<br />

social competence upon which this literature was<br />

based reflect and maintain such neglect. For<br />

instance, both social status and peer acceptance<br />

are typically determined by collecting either<br />

same-sex peer nominations=ratings or by aggregating<br />

peer ratings without regard to sex. Similarly,<br />

even self-report measures based on broad conceptualizations<br />

of adolescent social competence<br />

devote few, if any, items to other-sex interactions<br />

(e.g., Cavell & Kelley, 1992; Inderbitzen & Foster,<br />

1992).<br />

In contrast, the heterosocial competence construct<br />

has long been of interest in the clinical literature;<br />

however, until recently, such research was<br />

narrowly focused on functioning in date initiation<br />

situations. In the late 1970 s, clinical interest in heterosocial<br />

interactions peaked, as researchers came<br />

to view them as an ideal analog for anxiety<br />

research because of the negligible demand effects<br />

and the strong and not easily habituated physiological<br />

reactions associated with such encounters<br />

(Nangle & Hansen, 1998). Anxiety engendered<br />

by dating and heterosocial interactions is common<br />

and, in its more extreme forms, is linked to a number<br />

of negative outcomes. For instance, studies<br />

using large college samples indicated that as many<br />

as 37% of men are very anxious about dating (e.g.,<br />

Dodge, Heimberg, Nyman, & O’Brien, 1987). One<br />

study revealed that highly socially anxious individuals,<br />

compared to low-anxious controls, participated<br />

in fewer other-sex interactions, exhibited<br />

poorer social performance, and reported lower satisfaction<br />

in such interactions (Dodge et al., 1987).<br />

As expected, these detrimental effects appear to<br />

extend to same-sex interactions as well. For<br />

instance, Himadi and colleagues found that highly<br />

socially anxious male participants evidenced more<br />

difficulties in same-sex friendship interactions and<br />

general adjustment problems than their less<br />

anxious peers (see Himadi, Arkowitz, Hinton, &<br />

Perl, 1980).<br />

The majority of studies published during this<br />

era compared socially anxious and nonanxious<br />

male participants on a variety of self-report and<br />

behavioral measures. In one of the first studies of<br />

this nature, and one of the few to include both<br />

male and female participants, Glasgow and<br />

Arkowitz (1975) evaluated the behavioral differences<br />

among high- and low-frequency daters. To<br />

identify aspects of behavior that differentiated<br />

the groups, the researchers administered selfreport<br />

measures and assessed specific behaviors<br />

(e.g., gazing, talk balance ratio) in a verbal interaction<br />

with an other-sex confederate. In addition to<br />

the assessment phase, some researchers then implemented<br />

an intervention aimed at improving the<br />

heterosocial interactions of the minimal daters.


In one such study, Twentyman and McFall (1975)<br />

required college men self-identified as ‘‘unable to<br />

interact with women’’ to record daily interactions<br />

and participate in a battery of behavioral tests.<br />

Following the assessment, half of the shy group<br />

was randomly assigned to an intervention phase<br />

consisting of behavioral rehearsal, modeling, and<br />

coaching that targeted telephone and face-to-face<br />

conversational skills. At posttest, the treatment<br />

group improved both in the analog role-play tasks<br />

and self-monitoring reports of frequency and duration<br />

of interactions with women.<br />

Naturally, attempts to identify the more molecular<br />

skills composing heterosocial competence<br />

were limited by the indexes then employed to<br />

define competence. As just described, most existing<br />

heterosocial research has relied on date<br />

initiation as the sole index of competence.<br />

Attempts to identify the social skills that differentiate<br />

low- and high-frequency daters have resulted<br />

in a range of behaviors including eye contact,<br />

smiles, voice quality, facial expressions, appropriate<br />

gestures, duration of speech, self-disclosing<br />

statements, conversational questions, compliments,<br />

and date requests (Conger & Conger,<br />

1982; Hansen et al., 1992; Kelly, 1982). Extended<br />

silences and response delays, speech dysfluencies,<br />

and negative statements have been linked to less<br />

successful heterosocial interaction (Kelly, 1982).<br />

The few studies that included female participants<br />

found similar results with occasional gender<br />

differences such as topic initiation, touching<br />

while laughing, and attractiveness (Muehlenhard,<br />

Koralewski, Andrews, & Burdick, 1986). Despite<br />

the wide range of identified molecular skills associated<br />

with successful daters, the component skills<br />

are similar to those that comprise general conversational<br />

competence (Hansen et al., 1992).<br />

Although problems associated with adolescent<br />

heterosocial interactions, such as HIV=AIDS and<br />

unwanted pregnancy, have increasingly demanded<br />

national attention, clinical interest waned in the<br />

1990 s (Nangle & Hansen, 1998). In their review<br />

of the publication archives of three major clinical<br />

journals (i.e., Journal of Consulting and Clinical<br />

Psychology, Behavior Therapy, Behavior Modification),<br />

Nangle and Hansen reported a clear<br />

downward trend in related articles from 1975 to<br />

1994. Over 3 decades, the majority of reviewed studies<br />

used primarily male participants, investigated<br />

heterosocial anxiety, relied almost exclusively on<br />

role-play assessment methodology, and investigated<br />

assessment rather than treatment. This sampling<br />

of studies clearly reflects the singular focus<br />

of the examination of heterosocial competence in<br />

the clinical literature up to that time. The nearexclusive<br />

interest in heterosocial anxiety may have<br />

UNDERSTANDING OF HETEROSOCIAL COMPETENCE<br />

prevented further investigation into the construct<br />

including performance in other than dating interactions,<br />

gender differences, and developmental<br />

issues.<br />

Long-standing definitions of heterosocial competence<br />

suggest that the use of a single index of<br />

competence, such as date initiation, is inadequate.<br />

Barlow and colleagues defined heterosocial skills<br />

as those learned behaviors necessary for initiating,<br />

maintaining, and terminating social and=or sexual<br />

relationships with the other sex (Barlow, Abel,<br />

Blanchard, Bristow, & Young, 1977). This comparatively<br />

broad definition suggests that an<br />

adequate determination of what skills compose<br />

heterosocial competence will be a particularly<br />

challenging task. Not only would the component<br />

skills most likely change in various stages of a<br />

friendship or relationship, the situations in need<br />

of survey are likely to be varied. In a recent study,<br />

high school students described numerous situations<br />

involving the other sex as problematic. Using<br />

a focus group methodology, Grover and Nangle<br />

(2003) identified nine distinct themes within participating<br />

adolescents’ responses encompassing<br />

dyadic interactions within the bounds of casual<br />

relationships, friendships, romantic relationships,<br />

working relationships, and abusive relationships.<br />

For these reasons, a global definition or description<br />

of specific heterosocial skills may not be<br />

meaningful. To improve upon previous conceptualizations<br />

of the heterosocial competence construct,<br />

we propose a new and expanded view that bridges<br />

the extant clinical and developmental literature.<br />

Heterosocial Competence: Assumptions,<br />

Functions, and Deficits<br />

We view heterosocial competence as falling<br />

under the umbrella of the more general social competence<br />

construct, requiring overlapping yet somewhat<br />

unique skill sets. Consistent with existing<br />

developmental models, heterosocial competencies<br />

are presumed to develop as an outgrowth of<br />

same-sex social competencies (Connolly et al.,<br />

2000; Grover et al., 2005; Nangle & Hansen,<br />

1998). Even with such overlap, heterosocial competence<br />

is assumed to remain distinct in form<br />

and function. Moreover, deficits in heterosocial<br />

competence likely result from deviations from<br />

normative pathways.<br />

Overlap with Same Sex Social Competence<br />

Although considered to be substantial, the<br />

exact nature of the overlap between the skills comprising<br />

competence in same- and other-sex social<br />

493


interactions is unknown (Kelly, 1982; Nangle &<br />

Hansen, 1998). The two types of interactions have<br />

previously been viewed as being distinct, yet they<br />

may differ only in terms of contextual variants,<br />

such as the sex of the interaction partner (Conger<br />

& Conger, 1982). Both require the ability to converse,<br />

match social behaviors, manage conflict,<br />

and generally act within social norms (Kelly,<br />

1982). Overlap in the molecular component skills<br />

(e.g. eye contact, smiles, and nervous gestures)<br />

would also be expected (Chee & Conger, 1989).<br />

Interactions of both types are also presumed to<br />

serve similar functions for adolescents, including<br />

affection, companionship, intimacy, and a sense<br />

of inclusion (Connolly et al., 2000; Kuttler et al.,<br />

1999). Both same- and other-sex friendships provide<br />

social support, and research indicates that<br />

teens consider both sexes when establishing friendships<br />

(Bukowski et al., 1999; Kuttler et al., 1999).<br />

There is strong evidence that same-sex friendships<br />

offer a buffer against loneliness and depression<br />

(Erdley, Nangle, Newman, & Carpenter, 2001),<br />

and other-sex friends may serve a similar protective<br />

function. Both same- and other-sex friendships and<br />

romantic relationships may influence academic<br />

achievement and career aspirations as well as<br />

enhance identity development in terms of facilitating<br />

participation in hobbies and refinement of interests<br />

and opinions (Furman & Shaffer, 2003).<br />

Underlying this overlap in form and function is<br />

the presumption that heterosocial competence<br />

develops from the springboard of same-sex peer<br />

interactions and competence (Connolly et al.,<br />

2000; Dunphy, 1963; Nangle & Hansen, 1998).<br />

Dunphy observed that teens move from small<br />

same-sex cliques to mixed-sex crowds over the<br />

course of adolescence and theorized that mixedsex<br />

groups provide the opportunity to meet and<br />

become romantically involved with the other sex.<br />

In support of Dunphy’s initial observations,<br />

Connolly and colleagues (2000) documented that<br />

the size of the same-sex peer group predicts the size<br />

of the other-sex peer network for high school<br />

teens. Similarly, Bukowski and colleagues (1999)<br />

found that more popular adolescents had more<br />

other-sex friends than those at the midrange of<br />

popularity. Further, a positive relationship exists<br />

between same-sex social cognitive skills, popularity,<br />

and perception of competence and the same<br />

variables examined in other-sex relationships<br />

(Miller, 1990). As a possible explanation for the<br />

overlap, Nangle and Hansen (1998) theorized that<br />

socially competent adolescents seek out similarly<br />

competent peers and are affiliated with larger<br />

social networks that provide additional opportunities<br />

to learn heterosocial skills and facilitate the<br />

transition to mixed-sex peer groups.<br />

494<br />

GROVER, NANGLE, SERWICK, ZEFF<br />

Distinct Features of Heterosocial Competence<br />

Context plays a central role in determining<br />

competence, and other-sex social interactions have<br />

a number of unique contextual features. Adolescents<br />

behave differently with other-sex peers<br />

than with their same-sex friends in terms of tone<br />

and conflict strategies (Bukowski, Sippola, &<br />

Newcomb, 2000; Tezer & Demir, 2001). Moreover,<br />

teens associate both other-sex friendships and<br />

romantic relationships with opportunities for intimate<br />

involvement (Connolly, Craig, Goldberg, &<br />

Pepler, 1999). At the same time, heterosocial interactions<br />

typically engender increased anxiety due<br />

to their ambiguity and potential for romantic<br />

involvement (Darling, Dowdy, Van Horn, &<br />

Caldwell, 1999; Glickman & LaGreca, 2004).<br />

Romantic dissolution can also trigger significant<br />

distress and has been linked to the first occurrence<br />

of a major depressive episode (Monroe, Rohde,<br />

Seeley, & Lewinsohn, 1999). Emerging sexuality<br />

is yet another distinct contextual variant (Nangle<br />

& Hansen, 1998).<br />

Interactions with other-sex peers may require<br />

different behaviors, and similar behaviors may be<br />

judged differently as a function of the gender of<br />

the interaction partner. For example, in an examination<br />

of conflict behaviors, Tezer and Demir<br />

(2001) found that late adolescent boys were more<br />

competitive in situations involving same-sex rather<br />

than other-sex peers and chose avoidance as a<br />

strategy during conflict situations with girls but<br />

not with other boys. In addition, the gender of<br />

the interaction partner may lead to different interpretations<br />

of similar behaviors. For example,<br />

although girls become increasingly attracted to<br />

aggressive boys in middle school, they are less<br />

attracted to aggressive girls (Bukowski et al.,<br />

2000). Further, during a videotaped interaction<br />

with best friends of both the same and other sex,<br />

teens exhibited a differential pattern of behavior<br />

depending on the sex of the partner (McBride &<br />

Field, 1997). Specifically, girls were more playful<br />

with same-sex friends and elicited playful behavior<br />

from their male partners. However, boys did not<br />

display the same degree of playfulness with samesex<br />

friends.<br />

Romantic relationships, when compared to<br />

other-sex friendships, present unique challenges<br />

and place new demands on other-sex social competencies.<br />

In a study examining age-related differences<br />

in the conceptualization of friendships and<br />

romantic relationships, Connolly and colleagues<br />

(1999) found that adolescents are able to distinguish<br />

between friendships and romances based<br />

on self-reported qualities, such as passion, commitment,<br />

affiliation, and increased intimacy. In


addition, older adolescents replaced reports of<br />

intense feelings of passion, including physical<br />

attraction and sexual activity, with descriptions<br />

of intimacy, indicating a possible shift from more<br />

superficial attraction to more adultlike romantic<br />

relationships. In making the transition from<br />

other-sex friendships to romantic relationships,<br />

adolescents need to learn to cope with complex<br />

emotions and handle many novel situations. In<br />

fact, young adults perceive interpersonal situations<br />

with romantic partners as more difficult than those<br />

with close same-sex friends (Buhrmester, Furman,<br />

Wittenberg, & Reis, 1988). Emerging body image<br />

concerns, jealousy, blurred lines between friendship<br />

and romance, unrequited attraction, and<br />

sexual activity are just a few of the potential<br />

challenges (Grover & Nangle, 2003).<br />

Underscoring the unique developmental functions,<br />

major developmental theorists view heterosocial<br />

competence as critical to long-term<br />

adjustment, especially in the area of adult romantic<br />

relationships. According to Sullivan’s (1953)<br />

developmental stage theory of interpersonal relationships,<br />

different types of social relationships<br />

fulfill specific interpersonal needs that become<br />

more complex with age. Application of Sullivan’s<br />

theory would suggest that socially competent adolescents<br />

whose needs were successfully met by<br />

same-sex peers in early adolescence would be more<br />

likely to successfully negotiate other-sex intimacy<br />

needs in middle and late adolescence. Further,<br />

Sullivan suggests that relations with other-sex peers<br />

build on the earlier development of intimacy and<br />

fulfill the need for sexual contact. The combination<br />

of intimacy and sex prepares teens for adult romantic<br />

relationships that contain both experiences.<br />

Erikson (1963) contended that adolescent social<br />

relationships have a direct impact on the young<br />

adult’s ability to achieve intimacy. Upon the successful<br />

resolution of the intimacy versus isolation<br />

crisis, a key developmental challenge of early<br />

adulthood, the individual can form a loving and<br />

deep union with a romantic partner. Further,<br />

Erikson maintained that adolescents must first form<br />

a sense of identity, without which romantic relationships<br />

only have ‘‘pseudo-intimacy’’ and thus must<br />

resolve the two complimentary challenges of identity<br />

formation and development of intimacy through<br />

their interactions with the other sex. Thus, both<br />

Erikson and Sullivan viewed adolescent heterosocial<br />

skills and interactions are integral for establishing<br />

and developing later mature adult relationships.<br />

Skills Deficits and Clinical Implications<br />

Competence within a mixed-sex peer group<br />

contributes to the emotional well-being of the<br />

UNDERSTANDING OF HETEROSOCIAL COMPETENCE<br />

adolescent. Participation constitutes a leisure<br />

activity by increasing the opportunity for pleasurable<br />

activities, hobbies, and sexual stimulation,<br />

with comfort in mixed-sex situations predicting<br />

better self-esteem (Darling et al., 1999). These<br />

positive psychological benefits seem especially evident<br />

for adolescent boys as girls provide boys with<br />

validation of their accomplishments, a phenomenon<br />

absent in male same-sex interactions (Kuttler<br />

et al., 1999). Moreover, heterosocially competent<br />

teens experience an overall sense of group<br />

inclusion by participating in new mixed-sex peer<br />

groups with their previous same-sex friends<br />

(Connolly et al., 2000). Further, relationships with<br />

the other sex, especially those that are romantic in<br />

nature, enhance social status within the peer group<br />

(Hansen et al., 1992).<br />

In addition to heterosocial skills being associated<br />

with general well-being, deficits in these skills<br />

have been linked to several clinical difficulties.<br />

The developmental psychopathology approach<br />

has emerged as a major organizing framework<br />

for examining the development and persistence of<br />

pathology through the life span (Cicchetti &<br />

Cohen, 1995). Pathology is viewed as a deviation<br />

from normative development over time (Rutter &<br />

Sroufe, 2000). Consistent with this framework,<br />

social deficits in adolescence and difficulties in<br />

other-sex interactions are seen as departures from<br />

a normative pathway. Although directionality<br />

remains unclear, there is evidence that suggests<br />

that certain developmental pathways are associated<br />

with heterosocial skills deficits. Researchers<br />

have suggested two such deviant pathways: one<br />

involving socially withdrawn and anxious youth,<br />

and another for aggressive youth (Connolly &<br />

Goldberg, 1999).<br />

Regarding the first proposed deviant pathway,<br />

social withdrawal and anxiety tend to interrupt<br />

the normal development of peer relationships<br />

across adolescence (Connolly & Goldberg, 1999).<br />

Studies indicate that the effects of social anxiety<br />

are cumulative and increasingly detrimental. In<br />

one study, social isolation at age 7 predicted low<br />

levels of social competence, self-worth, loneliness,<br />

and peer-group insecurity at age 14 (Rubin, Chen,<br />

McDougall, Bowker, & McKinnon, 1995). Furthermore,<br />

La Greca and Lopez (1998) determined<br />

that adolescents in the 10th through 12th grades<br />

with higher levels of social anxiety reported lower<br />

peer acceptance, fewer friendships, and less intimacy<br />

and support in close friendships. With the<br />

demonstrated close links between same- and<br />

other-sex relationships, it seems likely that disturbances<br />

in the development of other-sex friendships<br />

and romantic relationships would follow. Indeed,<br />

anxiety has been associated with fewer date<br />

495


initiation behaviors and interactions with the<br />

opposite sex that in turn may delay entry into<br />

romantic relationships (Connolly & Goldberg,<br />

1999; Hansen et al., 1992). In an analysis of retrospective<br />

reports, individuals with a history of<br />

social anxiety reported acute feelings of selfconsciousness<br />

during social situations in junior<br />

high school and had fewer dating partners than a<br />

comparison group between the ages of 12 and 21<br />

(Albano, Marten, Holt, Heimberg, & Barlow,<br />

1995). Further reflecting developmental continuity,<br />

those teens that rarely date in adolescence<br />

tend to exhibit decreased social skills, social<br />

withdrawal, and romantic dysfunction later in<br />

adulthood (Collins & Sroufe, 1999; Nangle &<br />

Hansen, 1998).<br />

In contrast to withdrawn youth, aggressive<br />

youth tend to encounter a different set of social<br />

difficulties not characterized by social isolation.<br />

Although they are generally accepted, aggressive<br />

children tend to affiliate with their similarly<br />

aggressive peers, thus enhancing the chances that<br />

such behavior is reinforced (e.g., Dishion,<br />

Spracklen, Andrews, & Patterson, 1996). As these<br />

children enter adolescence, they are more likely to<br />

extend their aggressive behavior to other-sex peers<br />

and romantic relationships (Connolly & Goldberg,<br />

1999). For example, youth identified as bullies are<br />

more likely to harass other youth and more likely<br />

to report using physical aggression in a romantic<br />

relationship than those not identified as bullies<br />

(Connolly et al., 1999). Researchers have recently<br />

begun to examine longitudinal predictors of adolescent<br />

dating violence. One such study found that<br />

if their peers were involved in aggressive dating<br />

relationships, adolescents were more likely to<br />

engage in violent dating behaviors themselves<br />

(Foshee, Linder, MacDougall, & Bangdiwala,<br />

2001). In other studies, a history of aggressive<br />

behavior, particularly for males, predicted<br />

aggression in dating relationships (O’Keefe, 1997).<br />

In addition to the heterosocial problems associated<br />

with anxious and aggressive teens just cited,<br />

heterosocial skills deficits are hypothesized to be<br />

associated with several other problems, including<br />

unwanted sexual contact and inconsistent and=or<br />

ineffective contraceptive use. Approximately 10%<br />

of male and 20% of female adolescents have<br />

experienced unwanted sexual activity, ranging<br />

from unwanted touches to forced intercourse (Jezl,<br />

Molidor, & Wright, 1996). Although research on<br />

unwanted sexual activity has focused on incidence<br />

rates rather than causes, there is a small body of<br />

literature to suggest that some instances of<br />

unwanted sexual activity are linked to heterosocial<br />

skills deficits. Specifically, sex offenders frequently<br />

exhibit faulty perspective taking by concluding<br />

496<br />

GROVER, NANGLE, SERWICK, ZEFF<br />

that their victims enjoyed the experience despite<br />

evidence suggesting otherwise (Abel, Becker, &<br />

Cunningham-Rathner, 1984; Nangle, Hecker,<br />

Grover, & Smith, 2003). Similarly distressing,<br />

contraceptive use in the teen years is frequently<br />

inadequate, inconsistent, or incorrect. For<br />

example, Warzak, Grow, Poler, and Walburn<br />

(1995) reported that roughly 78% of adolescents<br />

reported not using any form of contraception<br />

during their last sexual intercourse experience.<br />

Interventions designed to increase contraceptive<br />

use usually target communication, assertion, and<br />

problem-solving skills (DiClemente, 1993). However,<br />

only those social skill measures assessing<br />

social skill in contraceptive situations, as opposed<br />

to more traitlike measures, appear to be directly<br />

related to reports of condom and contraceptive<br />

use (Nangle & Grover, 2001).<br />

Efforts to Assess Adolescent Heterosocial<br />

Competence<br />

Despite the importance of developing competence<br />

in other-sex social situations, our current<br />

understanding of the heterosocial competence construct<br />

is not well formulated, and progress in fully<br />

understanding the construct is hampered by the<br />

lack of established measures. In our review of<br />

more recent studies, we uncovered measures that<br />

assess general social competence in adolescents<br />

and several measures that assess what appear to<br />

be correlates of heterosocial competence rather<br />

than direct indexes. The few broad-based measures<br />

of adolescent social competence devote few if any<br />

items to other-sex. For instance, the Measure of<br />

Adolescent Social Performance (MASP; Cavell &<br />

Kelley, 1992) contains 1 item pertaining to othersex<br />

interactions out of 50 social problem-solving<br />

vignettes. Similarly, the Teenage Inventory of<br />

Social Skills (TISS; Inderbitzen & Foster, 1992)<br />

includes only 1 heterosocial situation in 40 items.<br />

Other relevant measures found included self-report<br />

measures of heterosexual-social anxiety (e.g.,<br />

Survey of Heterosexual Interactions; Twentyman<br />

& McFall, 1975), other-sex relationship quality<br />

(e.g., Network of Relationships Inventory;<br />

Furman & Buhrmester, 1985), and dating conflict<br />

(Conflict in Adolescent Dating Relationships<br />

Inventory; Wolfe et al., 2001). Many of these<br />

measures focus solely on dating relationships<br />

despite recent research that stresses the importance<br />

of including skills used in a broader range of<br />

situations.<br />

In response to the need for a broad assessment<br />

of heterosocial competence, we set out to develop<br />

a measure of adolescent heterosocial competence.


In a series of systematic investigations spanning 10<br />

years and involving more than 1,500 participants,<br />

we developed two versions of a self-report measure<br />

to assess competence (i.e., Measure of Adolescent<br />

Heterosocial Competence [MAHC]; MAHC-<br />

Young Adult Version [MAHC-YAV]; copies of<br />

both measures can be obtained by contacting first<br />

author Rachel Grover). Guiding this process was a<br />

situational-based model of social competence<br />

(Bem & Allen, 1974; Goldfried & D’Zurilla,<br />

1969). More specifically, we followed the behavioral-analytic<br />

model put forth by Goldfried and<br />

D’Zurilla, one of the most widely used approaches<br />

for assessing competence over a variety of relevant<br />

situations. The Goldfried and D’Zurilla approach<br />

advocates including problematic situations and<br />

response choices generated by the target population,<br />

thus ensuring external validity. Moreover,<br />

by including all relevant situations from one<br />

domain of competence, a summary score of the<br />

resulting measure represents the average competence<br />

within the domain. This versatile approach<br />

has been applied to an extensive list of subject matter<br />

including unwanted sexual contact (Murnen,<br />

Perot, & Byrne, 1989), interpersonal decisionmaking<br />

skill in college-age women (Goddard &<br />

McFall, 1992), and adolescent general social<br />

competence (Cavell & Kelley, 1992).<br />

In developing the MAHC, we completed a systematic<br />

series of five studies that included nearly<br />

700 participating teens and 12 cooperating professionals<br />

consistent with the Goldfried and D’Zurilla<br />

(1969) approach (see Grover et al., 2005). For the<br />

initial step, an extensive list of heterosocial situations<br />

was collected through focus groups and an<br />

open-ended survey study involving approximately<br />

200 adolescents (see Grover & Nangle, 2003).<br />

Next, a new sample of nearly 200 adolescents rated<br />

the collected situations on dimensions of commonness<br />

and difficulty to identify the ‘‘critical’’ situations.<br />

To collect responses to the situations, a new<br />

sample of more than 150 adolescents were asked<br />

to imagine themselves in vignettes describing the<br />

critical heterosocial situations and to write down<br />

what they would actually do and=or say in response.<br />

In the next step, 12 expert judges (e.g., high school<br />

teachers, adolescent health care providers, academic<br />

experts in the study of adolescence) rated the adolescent-generated<br />

responses for competency.<br />

The final step involved both the creation and<br />

psychometric evaluation of the resulting measure.<br />

The resulting measure is a 40-item multiple-choice<br />

social problem solving questionnaire. Respondents<br />

are asked to read brief vignettes of heterosocial<br />

situations and select their response from four<br />

choices of varying competence. Responses are<br />

scored with values from 1 to 4, with higher values<br />

UNDERSTANDING OF HETEROSOCIAL COMPETENCE<br />

assigned to more competent choices. To evaluate<br />

the psychometric properties of the MAHC, more<br />

than 200 adolescents completed the measure along<br />

with other measures of theoretically related and<br />

unrelated constructs. The reliability of the MAHC<br />

was acceptable (a ¼ .73). Investigation of convergent<br />

and discriminant validity yielded initial support<br />

for construct validity. As expected,<br />

adolescents with higher MAHC scores also tended<br />

to score higher on a measure of more general<br />

social competence (i.e., MASP) and report lower<br />

levels of anxiety in heterosexual-social situations<br />

[i.e., Survey of Heterosexual Interactions (SHI)].<br />

Contrary to expectations, the MAHC was not correlated<br />

with peer ratings of social acceptance. The<br />

overall pattern of interrelationships (i.e., MAHC<br />

correlated with MASP and SHI; MASP not correlated<br />

with SHI) suggests that our findings are not<br />

just artifacts of common method variance; however,<br />

multimethod validation is clearly required<br />

in future research with the MAHC. In support of<br />

discriminant validity, the MAHC was not associated<br />

with a measure of socioeconomic status.<br />

Developed in a similar manner, we have<br />

recently completed a construct validation of the<br />

MAHC-YAV (Serwik, Zeff, Grover, & Nangle,<br />

2005; see Table 2 for sample questions from both<br />

measures). More than 400 college students completed<br />

a battery of measures assessing a full range<br />

of social behavior dimensions and relationship<br />

quality variables hypothesized to be associated<br />

with heterosocial competence. Overall, strong<br />

evidence of convergent validity was found. Specifically,<br />

higher MAHC-YAV scores were associated<br />

with increased overall social competence,<br />

decreased overall social anxiety and social sensitivity,<br />

and decreased anxiety in heterosexual-social<br />

situations. Likewise, the MAHC-YAV proved to<br />

be closely related to a full range of relationship<br />

quality variables. More heterosocially competent<br />

individuals tended to report lower levels of negative<br />

interaction with other-sex friends and dating<br />

partners. In response to conflict situations, higher<br />

competence was predictive of increased negotiation,<br />

decreased psychological aggression, and<br />

decreased sexual coercion. Finally, as with the<br />

original version, MAHC-YAV scores were not<br />

related to socioeconomic status and adequate<br />

internal consistency (a ¼ .77) was obtained.<br />

More recently, Zeff (2005) used the MAHC in a<br />

study designed to test some of the hypotheses put<br />

forth in the deviant pathway for socially anxious<br />

youth proposed by Connolly and her colleagues<br />

(Connolly & Goldberg, 1999). Recall that this<br />

pathway suggests that social anxiety would first<br />

impact same-sex peer affiliations and later exert a<br />

cumulative negative effect on the formation of<br />

497


498<br />

Table 1. Themes and Exemplars of Problematic Heterosocial Situations on the MAHC and MAHC-YAV<br />

Theme MAHC MAHC-YAV<br />

Disagreeing with something that someone said. Having an argument with a romantic partner.<br />

Introducing yourself to a member of the other-sex. Calling someone you are attracted to for the<br />

first time.<br />

Becoming romantically interested in a friend. Jealously of romantic partners of other-sex friends.<br />

Asking someone on a date. Breaking up.<br />

Discussing contraception with your partner. Communicating sexual desires with your partner.<br />

Refusing drugs from a romantic partner. Sexual contact while intoxicated.<br />

Sexually offensive jokes.<br />

Continued sexual comments despite being asked<br />

to stop.<br />

Giving a speech in a class in front of the other sex.<br />

Joining a club that has many members of the<br />

other sex.<br />

Being asked out by a coworker.<br />

General communication<br />

Situations surrounding conversations.<br />

Initiation<br />

Situations at the start of a friendship<br />

or relationship.<br />

<strong>Friend</strong>ships<br />

Situations that involve other-sex friends.<br />

Dating<br />

Situations that involve dating.<br />

Sexual situations<br />

Situations that involve sexual contact.<br />

Drugs and alcohol<br />

Situations that include alcohol=drugs.<br />

Harassment=abusive situations<br />

Situations that could be harassment<br />

or abusive.<br />

School situations<br />

Situations at school involving the<br />

other sex.<br />

Work situations<br />

Situations that include working<br />

with the other sex.<br />

Note: MAHC ¼ Measure of Adolescent Heterosocial Competence; MAHC-YAV ¼ MAHC-Young Adult Version.


Table 2. Sample Questions from the MAHC and MAHC-YAV<br />

Version Item<br />

MAHC You are at a school dance. You notice a girl across the room that you would like to talk to.<br />

You know her name, but you have never talked to her before. What would you do?<br />

( ) Ask her to dance and then make conversation while dancing.<br />

( ) Go up to her and introduce myself.<br />

( ) I would be too shy to go up and talk to her.<br />

( ) Get a friend to walk over with me and then start talking to her.<br />

MAHC You are at a party with a bunch of friends. A boy friend comes over to you and offers you a beer.<br />

When you say no, he says, ‘‘Oh come on, I brought this over just for you. You have to drink it!’’<br />

What would you do?<br />

( ) Drink it.<br />

( ) Tell him why I am not drinking.<br />

( ) Say, ‘‘No thanks,’’ and walk away.<br />

( ) Say, ‘‘Maybe later,’’ and don’t do it later.<br />

MAHC-YAV You and a female friend have been spending a lot of time together. Her boyfriend begins to get<br />

jealous of the amount of time the two of you spend together. What would you do and say?<br />

( ) Talk to her boyfriend, let him know that you are just friends, and that he is welcome to hang<br />

out too.<br />

( ) Tell the guy to chill out and explain that you’ve been around a lot longer than he has and<br />

you’ll be around long after he leaves.<br />

( ) Leave it to the friend to handle, but argue why it is just jealousy on his part.<br />

( ) Stop seeing her so much and explain that her boyfriend is more important.<br />

MAHC-YAV You have a male friend that you like very much. In fact, you want to tell him that you’d like to<br />

be more than friends. What would you do and say?<br />

( ) Don’t tell him and just remain his friend.<br />

( ) Find out how he feels about you through friends and then give him gifts.<br />

( ) Tell him that you really like him now that you’ve gotten to know him and that you’d like to<br />

be more than friends.<br />

( ) Look for clues to determine if he is interested in being more than friends and then show him<br />

your feelings more through actions than through words.<br />

romantic relationships. In the Zeff investigation,<br />

more than 450 adolescents, divided into two age<br />

cohorts (i.e., 9th–10th graders; 11th–12th graders),<br />

were assessed for social anxiety, peer acceptance,<br />

heterosocial competence, gender composition of<br />

peer networks, dating history, and relationship<br />

quality. Consistent with expectations, social anxiety<br />

predicted impairments in the same-sex and<br />

mixed-sex peer groups, as well as romantic relationships.<br />

Moreover, significant grade effects were<br />

found, with social anxiety predicting more marked<br />

impairment for the older adolescents. Of most<br />

interest in our review, the MAHC proved to be a<br />

robust measure of social behavior. Teens higher<br />

in competence tended to be less socially anxious,<br />

have more other-sex friends, garner increased<br />

social support in other-sex relationships, and<br />

experience less negative interactions with samesex<br />

friends.<br />

The results of these studies provide support for<br />

the MAHC and the MAHC-YAV in terms of validity<br />

as well as utility in the process of defining<br />

the dimension of competence possibly underlying<br />

UNDERSTANDING OF HETEROSOCIAL COMPETENCE<br />

Note: MAHC ¼ Measure of Adolescent Heterosocial Competence; MAHC-YAV ¼ MAHC-Young Adult Version.<br />

performance across the heterosocial situations.<br />

From these studies, we gathered further evidence<br />

that the construct of adolescent heterosocial competence<br />

overlaps with general social competence.<br />

At the same time, results suggest that heterosocial<br />

competence during the teen years appears to be<br />

uniquely related to social anxiety. Further<br />

research investigating the extent of the overlap<br />

with anxiety is needed to determine whether anxiety<br />

in heterosocial situations is the single determinant<br />

of competence. Worth noting, in our<br />

validation of the MAHC-YAV, scores were predictive<br />

of romantic relationship quality even after<br />

controlling for more general social competence<br />

and social anxiety suggesting that anxiety may<br />

contribute to, but not fully explain, competence<br />

(Strout et al., 2004). Moreover, as the MAHC<br />

and the MAHC-YAV both require participants<br />

to respond to vignettes of problematic situations,<br />

it is possible that the measures may be best at predicting<br />

competence in particularly difficult social<br />

situations rather than initiation or approach<br />

situations.<br />

499


Directions for Future Research: Diversity<br />

and Developmental Considerations<br />

Adding to the breadth of our expanded understanding<br />

of adolescent heterosocial competence is<br />

the potential variation in the construct among cultural<br />

groups and with sexual minority teens.<br />

Unfortunately, the majority of existing research<br />

has focused on primarily White, presumably heterosexual<br />

teens (e.g., Connolly et al., 2000; Grover<br />

et al., 2005). The small body of existing research<br />

on minority groups is focused almost exclusively<br />

on dating and sexual norms rather than the broad<br />

construct of heterosocial competence. The actual<br />

skills and competencies composing adolescent heterosocial<br />

competence are likely shaped by the<br />

accepted social goals and may differ depending<br />

on the culture. For example, existing research on<br />

Latino culture suggests that the family, rather than<br />

the peer group, may play a prominent role in sexual<br />

socialization. For teens in traditional Latino<br />

families, dating may be considered forbidden<br />

behavior and casual mixing between the sexes during<br />

adolescence may be rare (Raffaelli & Ontai,<br />

2001). Additional research suggests a different pattern<br />

of sexual development for female Latino teens<br />

characterized by a later onset of sexual activity and<br />

an earlier age at marriage and birth of a child than<br />

compared to their White non-Latino peers<br />

(Upchurch, Aneshensel, Mudgal, & McNeely,<br />

2001). Of interest, the difference between the<br />

developmental timing appears to be moderated<br />

by level of acculturation with more ‘‘Americanized’’<br />

Latino teens displaying trajectories similar<br />

to those of White non-Latino Americans<br />

(Upchurch et al., 2001). Culture may affect the<br />

types of heterosocial situations experienced by<br />

teens, the accepted social solutions to the heterosocial<br />

situations experienced, and the accepted goals<br />

for social development. In contrast, regardless of<br />

culture, there may exist certain crucial developmental<br />

heterosocial tasks that the adolescent needs<br />

to master to facilitate the transition to the adult<br />

world. Further research in this area would help<br />

determine whether the current definition of<br />

heterosocial competence needs to be modified to<br />

accommodate cultural variation.<br />

Similarly, our current understanding of adolescent<br />

heterosocial competence may need modification<br />

to include the experiences of sexual<br />

minority youth. Although research on adolescents<br />

with sexual minority identification has greatly<br />

increased over the past 2 decades, there is still<br />

much to be learned regarding the impact of sexual<br />

orientation on heterosocial competence. Whether<br />

a separate construct definition or measures are<br />

needed for sexual minority youth is questionable,<br />

500<br />

GROVER, NANGLE, SERWICK, ZEFF<br />

however, as researchers have recently pointed out<br />

the drawbacks of assuming that social or sexual<br />

development is inherently different for heterosexual<br />

and sexual minority youth (see Diamond,<br />

2003). In fact, much overlap would be expected<br />

as sexual-minority youth have many other-sex<br />

social interactions in common with heterosexual<br />

teens. Research using representative samples<br />

reveals that more teens report attractions to both<br />

sexes rather than attraction only to the same sex<br />

(Garofalo, Wolf, Wissow, Woods, & Goodman,<br />

1999). About 10% of adolescents will eventually<br />

identify as sexual minorities (D’Augelli, 1988).<br />

For many, the realization of same-sex attraction<br />

will occur during adolescence and some have suggested<br />

that these youth may use heterosexual interactions<br />

and dating to experiment and help solidify<br />

their sexual orientation (Diamond, Savin-Williams,<br />

& Dube, 1999). Still, studies of heterosocial competence<br />

that include both heterosexual and sexual<br />

minority youth could reveal information regarding<br />

the form and developmental function of same-sex<br />

versus other-sex friendships and romantic relationships<br />

for both groups.<br />

In addition to research on diverse populations,<br />

research with different ages, especially preadolescents<br />

and young adults, would help establish<br />

the developmental trajectory of the construct.<br />

Including younger populations may enable the<br />

researcher to document the initial development<br />

of heterosocial skills. Initial studies utilizing the<br />

MAHC revealed no age differences among high<br />

school participants. It is possible that a preadolescent<br />

sample may be needed to examine the beginning<br />

of this pathway. For example, in their<br />

investigation of romantic partners as emotional<br />

support figures, Furman and Buhrmester (1992)<br />

examined 4th, 7th, 10th, and 13th grades to<br />

fully capture the developmental trajectory.<br />

Similarly, inclusion of an older population would<br />

allow the researcher to investigate the link between<br />

adolescent and adult functioning in other-sex<br />

relationships.<br />

Finally, the developmental functions of adolescent<br />

heterosocial competence should be investigated.<br />

Right now, there are several gaps in the<br />

developmental literature regarding the role of heterosocial<br />

skills. For instance, does participation in<br />

a mixed-sex peer group facilitate the development<br />

of heterosocial competence? What role does heterosocial<br />

competence play in the onset and quality<br />

of dating relationships? Future inclusion of measures<br />

of heterosocial competence in developmental<br />

research can help move the developmental literature<br />

from descriptive investigations and theories<br />

regarding cross-sex relationships to a greater depth<br />

of inquiry.


References<br />

Abel, G. G., Becker, J. V., & Cunningham-Rathner, J. (1984).<br />

Complications, consent, and cognition in sex between<br />

children and adults. International Journal of Law and<br />

Psychiatry, 7, 89–103.<br />

Albano, A. M., Marten, P. A., Holt, C. S., Heimberg, R. G.,<br />

& Barlow, D. H. (1995). Cognitive-behavioral group<br />

treatment for social phobia in adolescents: A preliminary<br />

study. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183,<br />

649–656.<br />

Barlow, D. H., Abel, G. G., Blanchard, E. B., Bristow, A. R., &<br />

Young, L. D. (1977). A heterosocial skills behavior checklist<br />

for males. Behavior Therapy, 2, 229–239.<br />

Bem, D. J. & Allen, A. (1974). On predicting some of the people<br />

some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies<br />

in behavior. Psychological Review, 81, 506–520.<br />

Buhrmester, D., Furman, W., Wittenberg, M. T., & Reis, H. T.<br />

(1988). Five domains of interpersonal competence in peer<br />

relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,<br />

55, 991–1008.<br />

Bukowski, W. M., Sippola, L., & Hoza, B. (1999). Same and<br />

other: Interdependency between participation in sameand<br />

other-sex friendships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,<br />

28, 439–459.<br />

Bukowski, W. M., Sippola, L. K., & Newcomb, A. F. (2000).<br />

Variations in patterns of attraction of same- and othersex<br />

peers during early adolescence. Developmental Psychology,<br />

36, 147–154.<br />

Cavell, T. A. & Kelley, M. L. (1992). The measure of adolescent<br />

social performance: Development and initial validation.<br />

Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 107–114.<br />

Chee, M. & Conger, J. C. (1989). The relationship between heterosocial<br />

and homosocial competence. Journal of Clinical<br />

Psychology, 45, 214–222.<br />

Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (1995). Perspectives on developmental<br />

psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen<br />

(Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol.1: Theory and<br />

methods (pp. 3–20). Oxford, UK: Wiley.<br />

Collins, A. W. & Sroufe, A. L. (1999). Capacity for intimate<br />

relationships: A developmental construction. In W.<br />

Furman & B. B. Brown (Eds.), The development of romantic<br />

relationships in adolescence. Cambridge studies in social<br />

and emotional development (pp. 125–147). New York:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Conger, J. C. & Conger, A. J. (1982). Components of heterosocial<br />

competence. In J. P. Curran & P. M. Monti (Eds.),<br />

Social skills training: A practical handbook for assessment<br />

and treatment (pp. 313–347). New York: Guilford.<br />

Connolly, J., Craig, W., Goldberg, A., & Pepler, D. (1999).<br />

Conceptions of cross-sex friendships and romantic<br />

relationships in early adolescence. Journal of Youth and<br />

Adolescence, 28, 481–494.<br />

Connolly, J., Furman, W., & Konarski, R. (2000). The role of<br />

peers in the emergence of heterosexual romantic relationships<br />

in adolescence. Child Development, 71, 1395–1408.<br />

Connolly, J. & Goldberg, A. (1999). Romantic relationships<br />

in adolescence: The role of friends and peers in their<br />

emergence and development. In W. Furman, B. B. Brown,<br />

& C. Feiring (Eds.), The development of romantic relationships<br />

in adolescence (pp. 266–290). New York: Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Darling, N., Dowdy, B. B., Van Horn, M. L., & Caldwell, L. L.<br />

(1999). Mixed-sex settings and the perception of competence.<br />

Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 461–480.<br />

D’Augelli, A. R. (1988). The adolescent closet: Promoting the<br />

development of the lesbian or gay male teenager. The<br />

School Psychologist, 42, 2–3.<br />

UNDERSTANDING OF HETEROSOCIAL COMPETENCE<br />

Davila, J., Steinberg, S. J., Kachadourian, L., Cobb, R., &<br />

Fincham, F. (2004). Romantic involvement and depressive<br />

symptoms in early and late adolescence: The role of a<br />

preoccupied relational style. Personal Relationships, 11,<br />

161–178.<br />

Diamond, L. M. (2003). New paradigms for research on heterosexual<br />

and sexual-minority development. Journal of<br />

Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32, 490–498.<br />

Diamond, L. M., Savin-Williams, R. C., & Dube, E. M. (1999).<br />

Sex, dating, passionate friendships and romance: Intimate<br />

peer relations among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adolescents.<br />

In W. Furman, B. B. Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.),<br />

The development of romantic relationships in adolescence<br />

(pp. 175–210). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University<br />

Press.<br />

DiClemente, R. J. (1993). Confronting the challenge of AIDS<br />

among adolescents: Directions for future research. Journal<br />

of Adolescent Research, 8, 156–166.<br />

Dishion, T. J., Spracklen, K. M., Andrews, D. W., & Patterson,<br />

G. R. (1996). Deviancy training inmale adolescents friendships.<br />

Behavior Therapy, 27, 373–390.<br />

Dodge, C. S., Heimberg, R. G., Nyman, D., & O’Brien, G. T.<br />

(1987). Daily heterosocial interactions of high and low<br />

socially anxious college students: A diary study. Behavior<br />

Therapy, 18, 90–96.<br />

Dunphy, D. (1963). The social structure of urban adolescent<br />

peer groups. Sociometry, 26, 230–246.<br />

Erdley, C. A., Nangle, D. W., Newman, J. E., & Carpenter,<br />

E. M. (2001). Children’s friendship experiences and psychological<br />

adjustment: Theory and research. In D. W. Nangle &<br />

C. A. Erdley (Eds.), The role of friendship in psychological<br />

adjustment (pp. 5–24). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York:<br />

Norton.<br />

Foshee, V. A., Linder, F., MacDougall, J. E., & Bangdiwala, S.<br />

(2001). Gender differences in the longitudinal predictors<br />

of adolescent dating violence. Preventive Medicine: An<br />

International Journal Devoted to Practice & Theory, 32,<br />

128–141.<br />

Furman, W. & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s perceptions<br />

of the personal relationships in their social networks.<br />

Developmental Psychology, 21, 1016–1024.<br />

Furman, W. & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences<br />

in perceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child<br />

Development, 63, 103–115.<br />

Furman, W. & Shaffer, L. (2003). The role of romantic relationships<br />

in adolescent development. In P. Florsheim (Ed.),<br />

Adolescent romantic relations and sexual behavior: Theory,<br />

research, and practical implications (pp. 3–22). Mahwah,<br />

NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.<br />

Garofalo, R., Wolf, R. C., Wissow, L. S., Woods, E. R., &<br />

Goodman, E. (1999). Sexual orientation and risk of suicide<br />

attempts among a representative sample of youth.<br />

Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 153,<br />

487–493.<br />

Glasgow, R. E. & Arkowitz, H. (1975). The behavioral assessment<br />

of male and female social competence in dyadic<br />

heterosexual interactions. Behavior Therapy, 6, 488–498.<br />

Glickman, A. R. & La Greca, A. M. (2004). The Dating Anxiety<br />

Scale for Adolescents: Scale development and associations<br />

with adolescent functioning. Journal of Clinical Child<br />

& Adolescent Psychology, 33, 566–578.<br />

Goddard, P. & McFall, R. M. (1992). Decision-making skills<br />

and heterosocial competence in college women: An<br />

information-processing analysis. Journal of Social and<br />

Clinical Psychology, 11, 401–425.<br />

Goldfried, M. R. & D’Zurilla, T. J. (1969). A behavioralanalytic<br />

model for assessing competence. In C. D. Spielberger<br />

501


(Ed.), Current topics in clinical and community psychology<br />

(pp. 151–196). New York: Academic.<br />

Grover, R. L. & Nangle, D. W. (2003). Adolescent perceptions<br />

of problematic heterosocial situations: A focus group<br />

study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 129–139.<br />

Grover, R. L., Nangle, D. W., & Zeff, K. R. (2005). The<br />

Measure of Adolescent Heterosocial Competence: Development<br />

and initial construct validation. Journal of Clinical<br />

Child and Adolescent Psychology, 282–291.<br />

Hansen, D. J., Christopher, J. S., & Nangle, D. W. (1992).<br />

Adolescent heterosocial interactions and dating. In V. B.<br />

VanHasselt & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of social development:<br />

A lifespan perspective (pp. 371–394). New York:<br />

Plenum.<br />

Himadi, W. G., Arkowitz, H., Hinton, R., & Perl, J. (1980).<br />

Minimal dating and its relationship to other social problems<br />

and general adjustment. Behavior Therapy, 11,<br />

345–352.<br />

Inderbitzen, H. M. & Foster, S. L. (1992). The Teenage Inventory<br />

of Social Skills: Development, reliability, and validity.<br />

Psychological Assessment, 4, 451–459.<br />

Jezl, D. R., Molidor, C. E., & Wright, T. L. (1996). Physical,<br />

sexual, and psychological abuse in high school dating relationships:<br />

Prevalence rates and self-esteem issues. Child and<br />

Adolescent Social Work Journal, 13, 69–87.<br />

Kelly, J. A. (1982). Social skills training: A practical guide for<br />

interventions. New York: Springer.<br />

Kuttler, A. F., La Greca, A. M., & Prinstein, M. J. (1999).<br />

<strong>Friend</strong>ship qualities and social-emotional functioning of<br />

adolescents with close, cross-sex friendships. Journal of<br />

Research on Adolescence, 9, 339–366.<br />

La Greca, A. M. & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among<br />

adolescents: Linkages with peer relations and friendships.<br />

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 83–94.<br />

McBride, C. K. & Field, T. (1997). Adolescent same-sex and<br />

opposite-sex best friend interactions. Adolescence, 32,<br />

515–522.<br />

Miller, K. E. (1990). Adolescents’ same-sex and opposite-sex<br />

peer relations: Sex differences in popularity, perceived<br />

social competence, and social cognitive skills. Journal of<br />

Adolescent Research, 5, 222–241.<br />

Monroe, S. M., Rohde, P., Seeley, J. R., & Lewinsohn, P. M.<br />

(1999). Life events and depression in adolescence:<br />

Relationship loss as a prospective risk factor for first onset<br />

of major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,<br />

108, 606–614.<br />

Muehlenhard, C. L., Koralewski, M. A., Andrews, S. L., &<br />

Burdick, C. A. (1986). Verbal and nonverbal cues that convey<br />

interest in dating: Two studies. Behavior Therapy, 17,<br />

404–419.<br />

Murnen, S. K., Perot, A., & Byrne, D. (1989). Coping with<br />

unwanted sexual activity: Normative responses, situational<br />

determinants, and individual differences. Journal of Sex<br />

Research, 26, 85–106.<br />

Nangle, D. W. & Grover, R. L. (2001). Social behavior and<br />

condom use among males: A multidimensional assessment.<br />

Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 26, 90–99.<br />

Nangle, D. W. & Hansen, D. J. (1998). Adolescent heterosocial<br />

competence revisited: The implications of an extended<br />

conceptualization for the prevention of high-risk sexual<br />

interactions. Education and Treatment of Children, 21,<br />

431–446.<br />

Nangle, D. W., Hecker, J. E., Grover, R. L., & Smith, M. G.<br />

(2003). Perspective taking and adolescent sex offenders:<br />

502<br />

GROVER, NANGLE, SERWICK, ZEFF<br />

From developmental theory to clinical practice. Cognitive<br />

& Behavioral Practice, 10, 73–84.<br />

O’Keefe, M. (1997). Predictors of dating violence among high<br />

school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 12,<br />

546–568.<br />

Raffaelli, M. & Ontai, L. (2001). ‘‘She’s 16 years old and there’s<br />

boys calling over to the house’’: An exploratory study of<br />

sexual socialization in Latino families. Culture, Health &<br />

Sexuality, 3, 295–310.<br />

Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (1998). Peer interactions,<br />

relationships and groups. In W. Damon (Series<br />

Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology:<br />

Vol 3. Social, emotional and personality development<br />

(pp. 619–700). New York: Wiley.<br />

Rubin, K. H., Chen, X., McDougall, P., Bowker, A., &<br />

McKinnon (1995). The Waterloo Longitudinal Project:<br />

Predicting internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescence.<br />

Development and Psychopathology, 7, 751–764.<br />

Rutter, M. & Sroufe, A. L. (2000). Developmental psychopathology.<br />

Development and Psychopathology, 12, 265–296.<br />

Serwik, A., Zeff, K. R., Grover, R. L., & Nangle, D. W. (2005,<br />

April). Initial construct validation of the measure of adolescent<br />

heterosocial competence-college student version.<br />

Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for<br />

Research in Child Development, Atlanta, GA.<br />

Sippola, L. K. (1999). Getting to know the ‘‘other’’: The characteristics<br />

and developmental significance of other-sex<br />

relationships in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,<br />

28, 407–418.<br />

Strout, J., Makson, A., Munster, J., Serwik, A., Zeff, K. R., &<br />

Nangle, D. W. (2004, May). <strong>Girl</strong> friend, boy friend, girlfriend,<br />

boyfriend: Social competence and anxiety as predictors<br />

of relationship quality. Paper presented at the 25th<br />

annual Maine Psychological Association Research Symposium,<br />

Waterville, ME.<br />

Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry.<br />

New York: Norton.<br />

Tezer, E. & Demir, A. (2001). Conflict behaviors toward samesex<br />

and opposite-sex peers among male and female late<br />

adolescents. Adolescence, 36, 525–533.<br />

Twentyman, C. T. & McFall, R. M. (1975). Behavioral training<br />

of social skills in shy males. Journal of Consulting and Clinical<br />

Psychology, 43, 384–395.<br />

Upchurch, D. M., Aneshensel, C. S., Mudgal, J., & McNeely,<br />

C. S. (2001). Sociocultural contexts of time to first sex<br />

among Hispanic adolescents. Journal of Marriage and the<br />

Family, 63, 1158–1169.<br />

Warzak, W. J., Grow, C. R., Poler, M. M., & Walburn, J. N.<br />

(1995). Enhancing refusal skills: Identifying contexts that<br />

place adolescents at risk for unwanted sexual activity.<br />

Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 16, 98–100.<br />

Wolfe, D. A., Reitzel-Jaffe, D., Wekerle, C., Grasley, C.,<br />

Straatman, A., & Scott, K. (2001). Development and validation<br />

of the Conflict in Adolescent Dating Relationships<br />

Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 13, 277–293.<br />

Zeff, K. R. (2005). The effect of social anxiety on the development<br />

of romantic relationships in adolescence. Unpublished<br />

doctoral dissertation, University of Maine.<br />

Received April 18, 2005<br />

Accepted December 16, 2005

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!