04.04.2013 Views

Download the report - The Healing Foundation

Download the report - The Healing Foundation

Download the report - The Healing Foundation

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Identifying <strong>the</strong><br />

psychosocial<br />

factors and<br />

processes<br />

contributing<br />

to successful<br />

adjustment<br />

to disfiguring<br />

conditions<br />

by<br />

THE APPEARANCE RESEARCH<br />

COLLABORATION<br />

Professor Nichola Rumsey,<br />

Dr James Byron-Daniel,<br />

Dr Rodger Charlton,<br />

Dr Alex Clarke,<br />

Dr Sally-Ann Clarke,<br />

Dr Diana Harcourt,<br />

Ms Hayley James,<br />

Mrs Elizabeth Jenkinson,<br />

Dr Antje Lindenmeyer,<br />

Dr Tim Moss,<br />

Professor Rob Newell,<br />

Professor Stanton Newman,<br />

Mrs Krysia Saul,<br />

Dr Andrew Thompson,<br />

Ms Eleanor Walsh,<br />

Dr Paul White,<br />

Miss Emma Williams<br />

Final Report submitted to:<br />

Project Manager:<br />

Professor Nichola Rumsey,<br />

Centre for Appearance Research,<br />

Department of Psychology<br />

University of <strong>the</strong> West of England, Bristol


Advisory Panel<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

Brendan Eley, Chief Executive of <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong><br />

Olivia Giles, <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> Ambassador<br />

John Kenealy, Consultant Plastic Surgeon, North Bristol NHS Trust<br />

Terry Paterson, GP- Bristol<br />

Pam Warren, <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> Ambassador<br />

Luke Wisemen, <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> Ambassador<br />

Bristol Site (Principal Investigator: Professor Nichola Rumsey)<br />

Laser centre, Frenchay Hospital:<br />

Mr John Kenealy and Ms Kay Thomas, Consultants in Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery,<br />

Sue Prior and Susan Large, administrative support<br />

Skin cancer clinic, Frenchay Hospital:<br />

Mr John Kenealy (as above)<br />

Joanne Watson, Clinical Nurse Specialist<br />

Emma Mills, administrative support<br />

General plastics dressing clinic, Frenchay Hospital:<br />

Mr Timothy Burge and Ms Catalina Estela, Consultants in Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery<br />

Eileen Watkins, administrative support<br />

Ophthalmology clinic, Bristol Eye Hospital:<br />

Mr Richard Harrad, Clinical Director and Consultant Ophthalmologist<br />

General Practitioner Surgeries:<br />

Dr Terry Paterson and Dr Fiona Ormerod, Pilning Surgery, Bristol<br />

Keith Minty and Dr Ulrich Freudenstein, <strong>The</strong> Family Practice, Cotham, Bristol


Follow-on Studies:<br />

Emma Halliwell, Senior Lecturer, UWE<br />

Katie Egan, MSc Health Psychology Student, UWE<br />

Habib Naqvi, Health Psychologist in Training, UWE<br />

Jennifer Hughes, Health Psychologist in Training, UWE<br />

Heidi Williamson, Health Psychologist in Training, UWE<br />

James Kennedy, Research Assistant, UWE<br />

UCL Site ( Lead: Professor Stanton Newman)<br />

Rheumatology Department, University College London Hospital:<br />

Dr Michael Shipley, Consultant Rheumatologist<br />

Abigail Olaleye, Specialist Rheumatology Nurse<br />

Samantha Moore, Specialist Rheumatology Nurse<br />

Moorfields Eye Hospital, London:<br />

Dr Daniel Ezra, NIHR Clinical Lecturer<br />

Mr Geoff Rose, Consultant Surgeon<br />

Mr Nigel Sapp, Chief Ocularist<br />

Dept of Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, Royal Free and University College Hospitals:<br />

Professor Peter Butler, Consultant in Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery<br />

Mr Patrick Mallucci, Consultant in Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery<br />

Mr Hugo Henderson, Consultant in Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery<br />

Mr Simon Wi<strong>the</strong>y, Consultant in Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery<br />

Ms Veronica Brough, Assistant Psychologist<br />

Ms Fran Smith, Honarary Assistant Psychologist<br />

Sheffield/Bradford Site (Leads: Professor Rob Newell & Dr. Andrew Thompson)<br />

Bradford Teaching Hospitals NHS <strong>Foundation</strong> Trust, St. Luke’s Hospital:<br />

Dr. Wright, Consultant Dermatologist<br />

Dr. Worrell, Consultant Ear Nose & Throat Surgeon<br />

Dr. Bem, Consultant Ear Nose & Throat Surgeon


Dr. McCall, Consultant Ear Nose & Throat Surgeon<br />

<strong>The</strong> Mid Yorkshire NHS Hospital Trust, Pinderfields General Hospital:<br />

Dr. Ford, Consultant Clinical Psychologist Burn Care<br />

Sheffield Teaching Hospitals NHS <strong>Foundation</strong> Trust, <strong>The</strong> Royal Hallamshire Hospital, <strong>The</strong><br />

Nor<strong>the</strong>rn General Hospital, <strong>The</strong> Charles Clifford Dental Hospital<br />

Mr. Westin, Consultant Ear Nose & Throat Surgeon<br />

Mr. Smith, Consultant Oral Maxillofacial Surgeon<br />

Mr. Bro<strong>the</strong>rston, Consultant in Plastic & Reconstructive Surgery<br />

Mr. Freedlander, Consultant in Plastic & Reconstructive Surgery<br />

Mr. Ralston, Consultant in Plastic & Reconstructive Surgery<br />

Mr. Fitzgerald, Consultant in Plastic & Reconstructive Surgery<br />

Dr. Datta, Consultant Pros<strong>the</strong>tist & Surgeon<br />

Miss. Currie, Consultant Ophthalmologist<br />

Miss. Tan, Consultant Ophthalmologist<br />

Professor Rennie, Consultant Ophthalmologist<br />

Mr. Rundell, Consultant Ophthalmologist<br />

Professor Gawkrodger, Consultant Dermatologist<br />

Barnsley NHS <strong>Foundation</strong> Trust<br />

Dr. Sabroe, Consultant Dermatologist<br />

Nottingham University Hospitals NHS <strong>Foundation</strong> Trust, Cleft Lip & Palate Service<br />

Ms. Martin, Clinical Director of Cleft Lip & Palate Service<br />

Follow-on Studies:<br />

Ms. Vines, Chief Executive, <strong>The</strong> Vitiligo Society<br />

Professor Gawkrodger, Consultant Dermatologist, <strong>The</strong> Royal Hallamshire Hospital<br />

Dr. Datta, Consultant Pros<strong>the</strong>tist & Surgeon, <strong>The</strong> Nor<strong>the</strong>rn General Hospital


Warwick Site (Lead: Dr. Rodger Charlton)<br />

General Practitioner Surgeries:<br />

Gabby Harris, Practice Manager, Castle Medical Centre, Kenilworth<br />

Stephen Gallagher, Practice Manager, Clarendon Lodge, Leamington Spa<br />

Lizzie Higginson, Head Receptionist, Clarendon Lodge, Leamington Spa<br />

Jenny Creighton, Practice Manager, Croft Medical Centre, Leamington Spa<br />

Janet Hastings, Practice Manager, Engleton House Surgery, Coventry<br />

Karen Shirley, Practice Manager, Green Lane Medical Centre, Coventry<br />

Chris Mullett, Practice Manager, Park Leys Medical Practice, Coventry<br />

Janet Bench, Administrator, Longford Primary Care Centre, Coventry<br />

Glyn Rawlings, Practice Manager, Red Roofs Surgery, Nuneaton<br />

Tom Ganner, Practice Manager, Ro<strong>the</strong>r House Medical Centre, Stratford upon Avon<br />

Mrs Gina Moore, Practice Manager, Broad Street Surgery, Coventry<br />

Dr Jag Sihota, General Practitioner, Broad Street Surgery, Coventry<br />

Follow-on Studies:<br />

Professor Mark Johnson, De Montford University<br />

Daya Singh, Community Facilitator<br />

Gurnam Singh, Community Facilitator<br />

Rukhsana Bibi, Community Facilitator<br />

Akm Kamruzzaman, Community Facilitator<br />

Fatima Marzia, Community Facilitator<br />

Mahomed Saleh, Community Advisor<br />

Rita Saujani, Community Advisor<br />

This research was supported by <strong>the</strong> generous funding and management by<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> and support by <strong>the</strong> Worshipful Company of Tin<br />

Plate Workers


SECTION 1<br />

CONTENTS<br />

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 1<br />

LAY SUMMARY OF RESEARCH PROGRAMME 7<br />

BACKGROUND & RATIONALE FOR THE PROGRAMME 11<br />

MAIN STUDY<br />

Factors and processes involved in adjustment to disfigurement: a<br />

cross-sectional study (Study 1)<br />

Qualitative responses to free text question in <strong>the</strong> cross sectional study<br />

(Study 2)<br />

Longitudinal quantitative study of change and stability in<br />

psychological adjustment to appearance (Study 3)<br />

A qualitative investigation of change and stability in psychological<br />

adjustment to appearance (Study 4)<br />

SECTION 2<br />

FOLLOW –ON STUDIES<br />

Page<br />

Rationale 115<br />

A qualitative study of <strong>the</strong> experiences of people who identify <strong>the</strong>mselves<br />

as having adjusted positively to a visible difference (Study 5)<br />

117<br />

Focus group studies of BME community views of facial disfigurement<br />

and visible differences (Study 6)<br />

125<br />

A qualitative study exploring <strong>the</strong> experiences of UK South Asians living<br />

with vitiligo (Study 7)<br />

137<br />

An investigation of appearance-related social comparisons amongst<br />

individuals with visible differences (Study 8)<br />

151<br />

Appearance concern, hostility & social situations (Study 9) 155<br />

Developing a scale to measure <strong>the</strong> impact of appearance concerns on<br />

intimate behaviours (Study 10)<br />

163<br />

Women with limb pros<strong>the</strong>ses: experiences and adjustment (Study 11) 171<br />

Does appearance matter in <strong>the</strong> context of Rheumatoid Arthritis? (Study<br />

12)<br />

183<br />

Working with patients with visible differences: general practitioners’<br />

beliefs, decision making processes and training needs (Study 13)<br />

191<br />

23<br />

71<br />

85<br />

99


SECTION 3<br />

Page<br />

OVERALL SYNTHESIS OF RESULTS 197<br />

THE INTERVENTIONS STRAND 203<br />

CONCLUSIONS AND THE WAY FORWARD 211<br />

REFERENCES 215<br />

SECTION 4<br />

APPENDICES 229<br />

1. Publication Strategy<br />

2. Dissemination Strategy<br />

3. Questionnaire booklet (study 1)<br />

4. Ethics approvals<br />

5. Information sheet (study 1)<br />

6. Consent form<br />

7. Operating Manual<br />

8. Percentage <strong>report</strong>ing sensitive area<br />

9. Questionnaire booklet (study 3)<br />

10. Semi-structured interview schedule (study 4)<br />

11. Interventions manual


LIST OF TABLES<br />

Study 1<br />

1.1 Sample means and standard deviations for outcome measures according<br />

to method of recruitment<br />

35<br />

1.2 Sample means and standard deviations for outcome measures according<br />

to geographical area of recruitment<br />

35<br />

4.1 Pearson’s correlation matrix for outcome measures 37<br />

4.2 Correlation of quantitative variables with outcome variables 38<br />

5.1(a) Age and gender frequency for whole sample 38<br />

5.1(b) Mean values for outcome variables by gender 38<br />

5.1(c) Frequency of number of areas <strong>report</strong>ed as sensitive according to<br />

gender.<br />

40<br />

5.1(d) Mean values for outcome measures by visibility to o<strong>the</strong>rs 40<br />

5.2(a) Correlation of Outcome Variables with Age 40<br />

6.1(a) Frequency and percentage <strong>report</strong>ing area of concern 42<br />

6.2(a) Means and standard deviations for visible and non visible areas on<br />

outcome measures<br />

42<br />

6.3(a) Frequency of number of areas <strong>report</strong>ed as sensitive according to<br />

gender.<br />

43<br />

6.3(b) Mean and standard deviations for number of areas of concern by<br />

outcome variable<br />

43<br />

7.1 Correlation of optimism with outcome variables 44<br />

8.1 Mean and standard deviations for all socio-cognitive factors 44<br />

8.2 Correlation of socio-cognitive factors with outcome variables 44<br />

9.1 Mean and standard deviations for all appearance cognitions 44<br />

9.2 Correlation of <strong>the</strong> appearance cognitions and <strong>the</strong> outcome variables 45<br />

9.3 Mean and standard deviations for each outcome variable according to<br />

level of disguisability.<br />

45<br />

9.4 Mean Outcome variables according to <strong>report</strong>ed level of visibility when<br />

clo<strong>the</strong>d<br />

46<br />

10.1 Regression Model for DAS24<br />

49<br />

10.2 Regression Model for HADS Anxiety<br />

50<br />

10.3 Regression Model for HADS Depression 51<br />

10.4 Regression Model for Aggression 53<br />

10.5 Regression Model for Positive Affect 54<br />

10.6 Regression Model for Negative Affect 55<br />

11.1 Mean and standard deviations for each outcome variable by clinic 56<br />

11.3 Percentage of participants with low (0-7), mild (8-11), and moderate/high<br />

(12- 21) levels of anxiety and depression<br />

58<br />

11.4 Percentage of participants with low (0-7), mild (8-10), and moderate/high<br />

(11- 21) levels of anxiety and depression by clinic category<br />

58<br />

Page


Study 3<br />

3.1 Response rates for responders and non-responders at follow-up 87<br />

3.2 T-test comparisons between responders and non-responders at follow-up 87<br />

3.3 Descriptive and Inferential Statistics for responders and non-responders<br />

at follow-up<br />

88<br />

3.4 Regression Model for DAS24 89<br />

3.5 Regression Model for Anxiety 90<br />

3.6 Regression Model for Depression 91<br />

3.7 Regression Model for Aggression 92<br />

3.8 Regression Model for Positive Affect 93<br />

3.9 Regression Model for Negative Affect 94<br />

Study 6<br />

6.1: Interventions suggested by Bengali Older Females 136<br />

Study 12<br />

12.1 Demographic data by group 186<br />

12.2: Means and standard deviations for all variables by group 187<br />

LIST OF FIGURES<br />

1 Working framework of adjustment to disfiguring conditions 15<br />

2 Representation of <strong>the</strong> Research Programme 18<br />

3 Operationalising <strong>the</strong> Framework – Choosing appropriate measures 28<br />

4.1(a) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for DAS24 by geographical location 35<br />

4.1(b) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for HADS Anxiety by geographical<br />

location<br />

36<br />

4.1(c) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for HADS Depression by<br />

geographical location<br />

36<br />

5.1 DAS24, HADS Anxiety, HADS Depression and Aggression by gender 39<br />

5.2 DAS 24 against Age 41<br />

6.195% confidence interval for mean DAS according to number of sensitive<br />

areas <strong>report</strong>ed.<br />

43<br />

7.1(a) Scores on <strong>the</strong> DAS 24 and responses to <strong>the</strong> question “How difficult is it<br />

to disguise <strong>the</strong> aspect of your appearance you are concerned about?”<br />

46<br />

8.1 Fur<strong>the</strong>r development of <strong>the</strong> model = hierarchical stepwise multiple<br />

regression<br />

47<br />

9.1(a) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for DAS 24 scores by condition 56<br />

9.1(b) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for HADS Anxiety scores by<br />

condition<br />

57<br />

Page


Page<br />

9.1(c) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for HADS depression scores by 57<br />

condition<br />

10 Fur<strong>the</strong>r Development of Model of Adjustment 59<br />

11 Model of Processing Influence of Appearance Self-Schema (Rosser, 2008) 68<br />

12 Resulting proposed framework for understanding adjustment to<br />

202<br />

appearance based on research programme<br />

13 Stepped care framework for interventions to promote psychosocial 209<br />

adjustment in appearance concern<br />

14 Discussion framework for <strong>the</strong> routine availability of screening, assessment 212<br />

and intervention


Introduction<br />

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY<br />

This <strong>report</strong> summarises <strong>the</strong> results of a three year research programme designed to investigate<br />

<strong>the</strong> psychosocial factors and processes contributing to successful adjustment to disfiguring<br />

conditions. <strong>The</strong> project was generously funded and managed by <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> and<br />

supported by <strong>the</strong> Worshipful Company of Tin Plate Workers.<br />

<strong>The</strong> research team was led by Professor Nichola Rumsey and researchers from <strong>The</strong> Centre for<br />

Appearance Research, UWE Bristol, and carried out by a team of researchers from <strong>the</strong><br />

Universities of Bradford, Sheffield, Warwick and UWE Bristol, University College London and<br />

<strong>the</strong> Royal Free Hospital, London.<br />

An advisory panel of people with personal experience of disfigurement and clinicians involved in<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir care contributed to <strong>the</strong> project throughout, including <strong>the</strong> design, <strong>report</strong>ing & dissemination<br />

stages.<br />

Background<br />

Visible disfigurement can have a profound psychological impact. In excess of 1million people in<br />

<strong>the</strong> UK have a significant disfigurement to <strong>the</strong> face or body, as <strong>the</strong> result of a congenital or birth<br />

condition, from trauma, disease, or from medical or surgical interventions to treat o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

conditions. However, not all people are equally affected and a proportion adapt positively to <strong>the</strong><br />

demands upon <strong>the</strong>m, in some cases perceiving <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference as a positive advantage.<br />

Never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong> amount of research examining adaptation has been small, and no adequate<br />

account of <strong>the</strong> process of adjustment has been developed. This programme of research<br />

presents and explores a model for adjustment.<br />

Aims<br />

• To clarify <strong>the</strong> psychosocial factors and processes which contribute to variation in<br />

adjustment in people with visible disfigurement<br />

• To use <strong>the</strong> results to inform <strong>the</strong> development of packages of support and intervention<br />

Methods of investigation<br />

Participants: People with visible difference recruited from hospital clinics and <strong>the</strong> community<br />

(some substudies involved GPs, and members of <strong>the</strong> general public).<br />

1


Procedures: This programme of research used 3 approaches:<br />

1. A large scale questionnaire-based cross-sectional study involving participants with a<br />

range of disfiguring conditions (n=1265), using a battery of validated measures relevant<br />

to <strong>the</strong> cognitions, emotions and behaviours of people with visible difference (Study 1),<br />

and including free text questions which were qualitatively analysed (Study 2).<br />

2. Two longitudinal studies: repeat of <strong>the</strong> questionnaire measures after a 9 month interval<br />

(n=360) (Study 3); a qualitative interview study of stability and change in adjustment with<br />

a subsection (n= 26) of <strong>the</strong> participants from study 3 (Study 4).<br />

3. Nine follow-on studies employing a range of methodologies to focus in more depth on<br />

specific topics: positive adjustment (Study 5); black & minority ethnic community views<br />

(Study 6); South Asians with vitiligo (Study 7); social comparison processes (Study 8);<br />

appearance concern, hostility and social situations (Study 9); scale development for<br />

intimate behaviours (Study 10); women with limb pros<strong>the</strong>ses (Study 11); appearance<br />

and rheumatoid arthritis (Study 12); general practitioners’ beliefs, experiences and<br />

training needs (Study 13).<br />

Results<br />

In <strong>the</strong> body of <strong>the</strong> <strong>report</strong>, study results are presented sequentially. To avoid repetition, in this<br />

summary, results are presented according to main outcome <strong>the</strong>mes, based on <strong>the</strong> syn<strong>the</strong>sis of<br />

results in <strong>the</strong> <strong>report</strong>.<br />

Profiles of Adjustment and Distress: Participants demonstrated very variable profiles of<br />

adjustment and distress and revealed characteristics of both more and less well adjusted<br />

people. Difficulties and distress were often considerable, both for those currently seeking and<br />

not seeking intervention, indicating unmet need (Studies 1, 4, 5).<br />

Adjustment is multi-factorial (all Studies) and is contributed to by dispositional style<br />

(optimism/pessimism) (Studies 1, 4) and cognitive processes such as levels of satisfaction with<br />

social support (Studies 1, 3, 4, 5, 11), fear of negative evaluation (Studies 1, 4) perceived social<br />

acceptance (Studies 1, 4) and interpretation of <strong>the</strong> responses of o<strong>the</strong>rs (Studies 2, 11),<br />

appearance specific cognitions, including salience, valence (value) and appearance-related self<br />

discrepancies (Studies 1, 3, 4). <strong>The</strong> role of some factors in adjustment to disfigurement, hi<strong>the</strong>rto<br />

neglected in research, have also been highlighted, including aggression in response to<br />

appearance-related threats (Studies 1, 9), and <strong>the</strong> role of functional disability and <strong>the</strong> desire to<br />

function independently (Studies 11, 12). <strong>The</strong>se appearance-specific issues may cause distress<br />

in <strong>the</strong> absence of o<strong>the</strong>r more general signs of psychological need (for example, anxiety or<br />

depression) (Studies 1, 4, 9, 10), but never<strong>the</strong>less impact negatively on many aspects of <strong>the</strong><br />

lives of those affected.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r factors and processes produced contradictory findings within this programme and are in<br />

need of fur<strong>the</strong>r research, including <strong>the</strong> issue of <strong>the</strong> visibility of a disfigurement to o<strong>the</strong>rs (Studies<br />

1, 3),and social comparison processes (Studies 1, 8).<br />

Positive Adjustment and Resilience: People who self <strong>report</strong>ed as positive adjusters described<br />

<strong>the</strong> experience of disfigurement as one of personal growth (Studies 5, 11) and <strong>report</strong>ed a<br />

number of strategies that appeared to work well, including pragmatism and ‘getting on with it’ in<br />

<strong>the</strong> face of challenges (Studies 5, 11), acceptance and determination (Study 2), and engaging<br />

with problems and difficulties ra<strong>the</strong>r than avoiding <strong>the</strong>m (Studies 3, 5, 7).<br />

2


<strong>The</strong> Impact of Appearance-Related Distress: Appearance related distress impacted on a wide<br />

range of daily activities and cognitions about <strong>the</strong> self, social functioning and intimate<br />

relationships (Studies 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, 7, 10).<br />

<strong>The</strong> Multiple Nature of Appearance Concerns: Distress relating to appearance can relate to<br />

many different aspects of <strong>the</strong> body (Studies 1, 2) and can include an exclusive focus on<br />

disfigurement, but also may result from concerns about weight, shape, and <strong>the</strong> effects of ageing<br />

on appearance. In some cases <strong>the</strong>se latter concerns caused more distress than <strong>the</strong><br />

disfigurement. Interestingly, <strong>the</strong> consequences for <strong>the</strong> individual of concerns about aspects of<br />

appearance which are not normally considered to be disfigurements (such as body shape and<br />

size and <strong>the</strong> effects of ageing) appeared to be strikingly similar to <strong>the</strong> distress caused by<br />

disfigurement (Studies 1, 2).<br />

<strong>The</strong> Dynamic Nature of Adjustment: Fluctuations in <strong>the</strong> salience and impact of appearance<br />

concerns are triggered by a variety of events and changes, including life events, developmental<br />

milestones, signs of ageing, and <strong>the</strong> cumulative impact of daily stressors such as <strong>the</strong> reactions<br />

of o<strong>the</strong>rs (Studies 2, 4). Even when adjustment is positive over long periods of time, coping with<br />

a visible difference can be an ongoing strain on resources (Studies 2, 11).<br />

<strong>The</strong> Impact of Age, Gender and Ethnicity: <strong>The</strong> focus on this research programme has been on<br />

those psychological factors and processes which are amenable to change, yet <strong>the</strong>se issues are<br />

also contextualised by characteristics which cannot be changed.<br />

<strong>The</strong> span of ages of <strong>the</strong> participants was large (18-91 years) (Studies 1, 2, 3, 4). <strong>The</strong>re were<br />

overall decreases in levels of distress with age, but individual variation was considerable, and<br />

large numbers of older people were distressed about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance.<br />

Appearance related distress was higher in women, but differences between men and women<br />

were small, and many men had significant and, in some cases, debilitating concerns about <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance (Studies 1,2,3,4).<br />

Within <strong>the</strong> ethnic groupings included in Studies 6 and 7, <strong>the</strong>re was evidence of <strong>the</strong> strong<br />

influence on <strong>the</strong> beliefs, attitudes and personal experience of people with disfigurement<br />

resulting from cultural and religious beliefs.<br />

Conclusion<br />

This has been <strong>the</strong> largest programme of research to date into psychosocial aspects of<br />

disfigurement. It has made a significant contribution to knowledge concerning adjustment to a<br />

visible difference by providing evidence to support a model of adjustment that will inform both<br />

fur<strong>the</strong>r research and provision of care.<br />

Scores on all measures were not indicative of high levels of distress. This is an important finding<br />

which recognises many people with visible difference adjust well. However, <strong>the</strong> variation in <strong>the</strong><br />

sample was considerable, and substantial numbers of participants did <strong>report</strong> high levels of<br />

distress, highlighting unmet need, even though o<strong>the</strong>rs were well adjusted. Contrary to<br />

expectations, levels of adjustment and distress in <strong>the</strong> cross sectional study (Study 1) were<br />

similar in <strong>the</strong> samples recruited from hospital clinics and from <strong>the</strong> community. In <strong>the</strong> clinic<br />

sample, levels of distress were not explained by <strong>the</strong> type of clinic attended.<br />

3


Adjustment and distress are multi-factorial. A syn<strong>the</strong>sis of findings from all <strong>the</strong> studies indicates<br />

that <strong>the</strong> psychological characteristics of those who were positively adjusted included higher<br />

levels of optimism, greater feelings of social acceptance and satisfaction with social support, a<br />

lack of concern about negative evaluations by o<strong>the</strong>rs, and a self-system with lower levels of<br />

salience and valence afforded to appearance related information. Levels of social anxiety and<br />

social avoidance were low, as were levels of negative affect, general anxiety and depression.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se people felt <strong>the</strong>ir disfigurement was reasonably easy to disguise, and tended to be older<br />

than people with higher levels of distress.<br />

By contrast, those experiencing high levels of distress were characterised by a more pessimistic<br />

outlook on life, higher levels of fear about negative evaluations by o<strong>the</strong>rs and perceptions and<br />

lower levels of satisfaction with levels of social support. <strong>The</strong> discrepancy between how <strong>the</strong>y<br />

perceived <strong>the</strong>ir appearance to be, and how <strong>the</strong>y thought it should be was considerable. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

believed <strong>the</strong>ir disfigurement was visible to o<strong>the</strong>rs, and hard to disguise. Appearance concerns<br />

occupied a more central position in <strong>the</strong>ir cognitive architecture and in <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>y process<br />

information from <strong>the</strong>ir social environment.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se findings have consequences for <strong>the</strong> wellbeing of people with visible difference, as many<br />

of <strong>the</strong> characteristics of those with higher levels of distress are amenable to intervention for<br />

which <strong>the</strong>re is a growing evidence base. Accordingly, <strong>the</strong> results of this programme of research<br />

have been used to inform <strong>the</strong> development of an intervention manual which is included in this<br />

<strong>report</strong>. This manual will form <strong>the</strong> basis of a training package to be piloted in November 2009.<br />

Summarised recommendations<br />

Intervention<br />

Packages of support and intervention tailored to individual need should be developed, trialled<br />

and refined.<br />

Support and intervention should address <strong>the</strong> socio-cognitive and appearance specific cognitions<br />

identified in this and earlier studies.<br />

Support and intervention should be offered in a timely way according to a tiered model from<br />

generalist to specialist interventions.<br />

Screening and referral pathways to facilitate this approach should be developed.<br />

Interventions should have regard for <strong>the</strong> cultural context of different groups of people with visible<br />

difference.<br />

Training and education<br />

Training packages for health professionals should be developed emphasising screening, tiered<br />

treatment and referral.<br />

Public education initiatives should be developed to reduce <strong>the</strong> pressure from <strong>the</strong> media and<br />

society on those vulnerable to appearance concerns.<br />

4


Research<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>r investigation should take place of key issues raised and explored by this programme<br />

(e.g. visibility and disguisability, functional disability, gender, appearance concerns in those with<br />

and without disfigurements).<br />

Culturally sensitive measures of social and appearance concerns in those with visible<br />

differences should be developed and validated.<br />

Resources<br />

Funding is needed for all aspects of intervention, training and education, and <strong>the</strong> research<br />

associated with <strong>the</strong>se.<br />

5


IDENTIFYING THE PSYCHOSOCIAL FACTORS AND PROCESSES CONTRIBUTING TO<br />

SUCCESSFUL ADJUSTMENT TO DISFIGURING CONDITIONS<br />

A project funded by <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> conducted by:<br />

<strong>The</strong> Appearance Research Collaboration 1<br />

LAY SUMMARY OF THE RESARCH PROGRAMME<br />

Visible disfigurement can have a profound psychological impact. In excess of 1 million people in<br />

<strong>the</strong> UK have a significant disfigurement to <strong>the</strong> face or body. <strong>The</strong> difficulties experienced can be<br />

pervasive and debilitating, and include negative experiences related to social encounters with<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs and adverse effects on self-esteem and quality of life.<br />

Not all people are equally affected and a proportion adapt positively to <strong>the</strong> demands upon <strong>the</strong>m,<br />

in some cases perceiving <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference as a positive advantage.<br />

<strong>The</strong> amount of research examining adaptation has been small, and no adequate account of <strong>the</strong><br />

process of adjustment has been developed. This project explores and describes <strong>the</strong> process of<br />

adjustment.<br />

<strong>The</strong> project<br />

A three year research programme designed to identify and investigate <strong>the</strong> psychosocial factors<br />

and processes contributing to successful adjustment to disfiguring conditions, with particular<br />

emphasis on those aspects which are amenable to change. <strong>The</strong> project would have been<br />

impossible without <strong>the</strong> generous funding and management of <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> and<br />

support of <strong>the</strong> Worshipful Company of Tin Plate Workers.<br />

An advisory panel of people with personal experience of disfigurement and clinicians involved in<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir care contributed to <strong>the</strong> project throughout, including <strong>the</strong> design, <strong>report</strong>ing & dissemination<br />

stages.<br />

Aims<br />

• To clarify <strong>the</strong> psychosocial factors and processes which contribute to variation in<br />

adjustment in people with visible disfigurement<br />

• To use <strong>the</strong> results to inform <strong>the</strong> development of packages of support and intervention<br />

<strong>The</strong> studies<br />

• A large survey of 1,265 participants with visible differences from a variety of causes has<br />

been carried out.<br />

• A fur<strong>the</strong>r survey following up 360 participants over 9 months.<br />

• An interview study following up 26 participants in depth after 9 months<br />

• 9 smaller studies looking at emerging issues in greater depth 2<br />

1 <strong>The</strong> research team was led by Professor Nichola Rumsey and researchers from <strong>The</strong> Centre for Appearance<br />

Research, UWE Bristol, and carried out by a team of researchers from <strong>the</strong> Universities of Bradford, Sheffield,<br />

Warwick and UWE Bristol, University College London and <strong>the</strong> Royal Free Hospital, London.<br />

2 Studies examined positive adjustment ; black & minority ethnic community views; South Asians with vitiligo; social<br />

comparison processes; appearance concern, hostility and social situations; scale development for intimate<br />

behaviours; women with limb pros<strong>the</strong>ses; appearance and rheumatoid arthritis; general practitioners’ beliefs,<br />

experiences and training needs.<br />

7


Participants were recruited from hospital out-patient clinics and from a variety of community<br />

settings in Bristol, London and <strong>The</strong> Midlands. <strong>The</strong>y ranged in age from 18-91 years.<br />

Main findings<br />

<strong>The</strong> results of all studies emphasise <strong>the</strong> importance of understanding <strong>the</strong> inner worlds of those<br />

affected by appearance concerns – how <strong>the</strong> person engages with and interprets <strong>the</strong> world.<br />

Adjustment and distress: People in <strong>the</strong> studies showed very variable profiles of adjustment and<br />

distress and revealed characteristics of both more and less well adjusted people. Difficulties and<br />

distress were often considerable. Previous studies have suggested women and people with<br />

disfigurement of normally visible parts of <strong>the</strong> body are more distressed. However, in this<br />

programme of research <strong>the</strong>se differences were modest, and both men and people with normally<br />

hidden disfigurements experienced significant distress.<br />

Thought processes and behaviours: <strong>The</strong> importance of appearance related cognitions is<br />

highlighted: <strong>the</strong> value attributed to appearance and <strong>the</strong> salience of it in processing information,<br />

<strong>the</strong> way people process appearance-related information, and <strong>the</strong> gap between how <strong>the</strong>y view<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir appearance and how <strong>the</strong>y would ideally like it to be. In people troubled by <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance, <strong>the</strong>se thoughts take centre stage, and lead to a self-reinforcing cycle in which<br />

(often negative) appearance information is frequently attended to and used in relating to <strong>the</strong><br />

world.<br />

Features of positive adjustment: People who self <strong>report</strong>ed as positively adjusted often described<br />

<strong>the</strong> experience of disfigurement as one of personal growth, and <strong>report</strong>ed a number of strategies<br />

that appeared to work well: pragmatism, acceptance, determination, engaging with problems<br />

and difficulties ra<strong>the</strong>r than avoiding <strong>the</strong>m.<br />

Ethnicity and cultural beliefs: Studies of beliefs and attitudes of those from minority ethnic<br />

groups (with and without disfigurements) highlighted <strong>the</strong> influence of cultural and religious<br />

beliefs on attitudes concerning <strong>the</strong> causes of disfiguring conditions (myths), <strong>the</strong> impact on <strong>the</strong><br />

family, and levels of social exposure.<br />

Key messages from <strong>the</strong> research programme<br />

• Adjustment to disfigurement is very variable, and, whilst many are well adjusted, <strong>the</strong>re<br />

are also high levels of distress.<br />

• Relationships between adjustment, age, gender and o<strong>the</strong>r demographic features are<br />

equivocal.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> role of thought processes and interactions in mediating adjustment is clear.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> media, society and culture contribute to pressure on those vulnerable to appearance<br />

concerns.<br />

• Support and intervention should be available at a range of levels of intensity to meet<br />

various levels of need.<br />

8


• Findings of <strong>the</strong> research programme support <strong>the</strong> need for interventions which seek to<br />

address unhelpful appearance related thoughts and social behaviour. 3<br />

• Continuing campaigning and publicity efforts are required to modify attitudes within<br />

society and offset <strong>the</strong> effects of <strong>the</strong> media and culture.<br />

• Important research remains to be done into key areas raised by <strong>the</strong> programme,<br />

particularly measurement and intervention.<br />

3 Alongside <strong>the</strong> research element of this programme, <strong>the</strong> research team have reviewed existing interventions based<br />

on <strong>the</strong> research findings and devised an evidence-based intervention manual which is to be piloted in November<br />

2009.<br />

9


BACKGROUND & RATIONALE FOR THE RESEARCH PROGRAMME<br />

Whe<strong>the</strong>r present at birth or acquired later in life, a visible disfigurement can have a profound<br />

psychological impact on those affected. <strong>The</strong> charity Changing Faces estimates that in excess of<br />

1 million people in <strong>the</strong> UK have a significant disfigurement to <strong>the</strong> face or body. <strong>The</strong> annual<br />

incidence is 415,550 people who acquire ei<strong>the</strong>r a significant transitory or permanent facial<br />

disfigurement as <strong>the</strong> result of a congenital or birth condition, from trauma, disease (for example,<br />

skin conditions, stroke, arthritis), or from medical or surgical interventions to treat o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

conditions (for example, cancer). Research to date has focused predominantly on <strong>the</strong><br />

difficulties and distress resulting from visible difference. However, not all are equally affected<br />

and a proportion adapt positively to <strong>the</strong> demands upon <strong>the</strong>m, ei<strong>the</strong>r restricting <strong>the</strong> impact of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

visible difference to a relatively minor role in <strong>the</strong>ir lives, or, in some cases, perceiving it as a<br />

positive advantage.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is a consensus amongst researchers and practitioners in <strong>the</strong> field that individual<br />

adjustment to disfigurement is affected by a complex interplay of physical, cultural and<br />

psychosocial factors (Moss, 1997; Clarke 1999; Endriga & Kapp-Simon, 1999; Thompson &<br />

Kent, 2001; Rumsey & Harcourt, 2004). A comprehensive review of current knowledge is<br />

beyond <strong>the</strong> scope of this <strong>report</strong>, and readers are referred instead to articles by members of this<br />

collaboration and o<strong>the</strong>rs, including: Moss (1997); Clarke, (1999); Newell, (2000); Thompson and<br />

Kent (2001); Kent and Thompson (2002); Rumsey and Harcourt (2004; 2005); Thompson<br />

(2009). Research has highlighted <strong>the</strong> probability that some factors contribute to <strong>the</strong> likelihood of<br />

distress, whilst o<strong>the</strong>r factors may ‘buffer’ a person against <strong>the</strong> stresses and strains of living with<br />

a visible difference. However, this research has been characterised by methodological<br />

difficulties including small sample sizes, unrepresentative and predominantly white participants.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results are <strong>the</strong>refore suggestive ra<strong>the</strong>r than definitive.<br />

Factors identified in <strong>the</strong> literature to date<br />

Physical and treatment-related factors<br />

<strong>The</strong>se include <strong>the</strong> aetiology, extent, type and severity of <strong>the</strong> disfiguring condition, and <strong>the</strong><br />

treatment history of each individual. Contrary to <strong>the</strong> expectations of <strong>the</strong> lay public and many<br />

health care providers, <strong>the</strong> bulk of research, clinical experience and personal accounts written by<br />

those affected, demonstrate that <strong>the</strong> extent, type and severity of a disfigurement are not<br />

consistent predictors of adjustment, although <strong>the</strong> visibility of <strong>the</strong> condition can exacerbate<br />

distress (Thompson & Kent, 2001; Rumsey & Harcourt, 2004).<br />

11


Socio-cultural factors<br />

<strong>The</strong>se factors provide <strong>the</strong> context for adjustment, and include age and developmental stage,<br />

gender, race, social class & cultural milieu. Also included in this category are parental and peer<br />

group influences. In addition, many writers have pointed to <strong>the</strong> role played by <strong>the</strong> media in<br />

creating and exacerbating <strong>the</strong> pressures on those distressed by <strong>the</strong>ir appearance – although <strong>the</strong><br />

impact of media and o<strong>the</strong>r socio-cultural factors has been shown to vary between individuals<br />

(Tiggemann, 2004).<br />

Psychological factors and processes<br />

Factors in this category include <strong>the</strong> structure of a person’s self esteem and self-image (e.g. <strong>the</strong><br />

weight given to <strong>the</strong> opinions of o<strong>the</strong>rs and to broader societal standards), a person’s<br />

personality/disposition, coping repertoire, perceptions of social support, levels of psychological<br />

well-being (e.g. anxiety, depression) and social anxiety, feelings of shame, and <strong>the</strong> perceived<br />

noticeability of <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference to o<strong>the</strong>rs. <strong>The</strong>se factors can be broadly categorised as<br />

affective (relating to feelings), cognitive (relating to thoughts) or related to behaviour, and are on<br />

<strong>the</strong> whole more amenable to change than physical, treatment-related or socio-cultural factors<br />

(Thompson & Kent, 2001; Rumsey & Harcourt, 2005). Fagervik-Morton’s (in preparation)<br />

systematic review of <strong>the</strong> literature relating to adults with visible differences (which initially<br />

examined 1191 potentially relevant articles, and a final inclusion of 12 studies) concluded that<br />

<strong>the</strong> evidence was not sufficiently strong to draw firm conclusions about which factors exacerbate<br />

distress/promote adjustment and whe<strong>the</strong>r variation is related to <strong>the</strong> structure and organisation of<br />

appearance-related information. This review confirmed <strong>the</strong> existing conviction of this research<br />

collaboration that an elucidation of <strong>the</strong>se factors and processes should be a priority in this<br />

programme.<br />

Previous research has focused on <strong>the</strong> problems and difficulties associated with visible<br />

difference. In addition, <strong>the</strong> large majority of participants in previous research have been<br />

recruited from a population seeking treatment for <strong>the</strong>ir condition. In order to understand <strong>the</strong><br />

breadth of adjustment, it is also crucial to ga<strong>the</strong>r information from those who are not seeking<br />

treatment and/or perceive treatment to be irrelevant in addressing <strong>the</strong>ir needs. Accordingly, this<br />

research has also focused considerable recruitment effort on a sample drawn from <strong>the</strong><br />

community.<br />

For <strong>the</strong> researchers and clinicians in this collaboration, <strong>the</strong> key priorities were, first, to clarify<br />

those factors and processes that exacerbate distress and/or promote successful psychological<br />

adjustment in people with visible differences, and second, by focusing on those factors which<br />

are amenable to change, to use <strong>the</strong> resulting knowledge to inform <strong>the</strong> development and<br />

refinement of a comprehensive range of methods of support and intervention designed to<br />

promote positive adjustment.<br />

<strong>The</strong>oretical underpinning<br />

12


<strong>The</strong> challenge of developing comprehensive models and <strong>the</strong>ories to guide and underpin<br />

research relating to appearance have been discussed by Thompson and Kent (2001) and<br />

Rumsey and Harcourt (2005). Early cognition models of body image disturbance focused on<br />

<strong>the</strong> discrepancy between an individual’s perceived actual and ideal selves, and described <strong>the</strong><br />

process by which an individual’s thinking can become more and more dominated by negative<br />

thoughts about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance (Thompson et al 1999). Cash’s (1997) cognitive-behavioural<br />

model recognizes that past, cultural and interpersonal experiences, physical characteristics and<br />

personality attributes influence <strong>the</strong> development of body image attitudes and schema.<br />

In relation specifically to disfigurement, perspectives on stigma (Goffman 1963), shame (Kent &<br />

Thompson, 2002) and social exclusion (Leary 1990) have all informed <strong>the</strong> development of a<br />

number of models. Some researchers (including members of this collaboration) have<br />

developed models of <strong>the</strong> processes involved. For example, Kent (2002) proposed a model<br />

centred around appearance anxiety and stigma. Newell (2000a) developed a testable<br />

fear/avoidance model of social anxiety among people with a visible difference, proposing a<br />

continuum of confrontational and avoidant responses (as opposed to discrete categories of<br />

good or bad adjustment). More recently, Moss & Carr (2004) explored <strong>the</strong> use of <strong>the</strong> multifaceted<br />

self-concept as a means of exploring adjustment to disfigurement. O<strong>the</strong>r models have<br />

attempted to explain appearance issues within specific condition or treatment groups (see, for<br />

example, Sarwer et al (1997) in relation to <strong>the</strong> motivation to seek cosmetic surgery, White<br />

(2002) in relation to cancer, Thompson (2009) in relation to skin conditions) rigorously<br />

highlighting <strong>the</strong> influence of developmental, social-cultural and perceptual factors on body<br />

image and self-esteem. Whilst models may help to organise collective thinking, <strong>the</strong>y cannot, at<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir current level of sophistication, provide a comprehensive representation of <strong>the</strong> complexities<br />

of factors, <strong>the</strong> relationships between <strong>the</strong>se factors and <strong>the</strong> dynamic nature of adjustment. To<br />

date, no single model stands out as offering a comprehensive framework to guide research and<br />

clinical intervention. <strong>The</strong> available models are ei<strong>the</strong>r limited in scope, falling short of providing a<br />

comprehensive understanding of individual experiences of appearance, or incorporate<br />

constructs that cannot easily be measured/assessed. None of <strong>the</strong> existing models have been<br />

tested on a large and diverse population of people with visible differences. However,<br />

exploratory research has provided such a plethora of variables implicated in experiences and<br />

adjustment, that it was considered timely to marshal <strong>the</strong>m into a manageable format for <strong>the</strong><br />

purpose of guiding research.<br />

Mindful of <strong>the</strong> need to share a common approach to this research and of <strong>the</strong> imperative to<br />

inform interventions, <strong>the</strong> members of this collaboration adopted a pragmatic approach in<br />

developing a framework to guide <strong>the</strong> current research programme. This approach integrated<br />

<strong>the</strong> main findings and trends in this area of research, encompassing aspects of previous models<br />

developed by members of <strong>the</strong> team and o<strong>the</strong>r leading commentators in <strong>the</strong> field. <strong>The</strong><br />

framework was designed to facilitate <strong>the</strong> identification and clarification of those factors which<br />

have <strong>the</strong> potential to be amenable to change through psychosocial support and intervention,<br />

ei<strong>the</strong>r as an adjunct, or where appropriate, as an alternative to surgical and medical<br />

intervention. In addition, and in contrast to previous research in this area, which has focused in<br />

<strong>the</strong> main on <strong>the</strong> problems and difficulties associated with disfigurement, this programme aimed<br />

13


to identify and clarify factors contributing to ‘resilience’ and positive adjustment. <strong>The</strong> result was<br />

a descriptive, working framework of inputs, mediating processes and outputs designed to be<br />

broad enough to capture <strong>the</strong> range of experiences of those affected, but specific enough to<br />

allow for predictions about <strong>the</strong> susceptibility to distress or positive adjustment of those affected<br />

by a visible difference. <strong>The</strong> framework was used to consider <strong>the</strong> emerging findings of <strong>the</strong><br />

project, and was fur<strong>the</strong>r refined during <strong>the</strong> analysis process (Figure 1).<br />

14


STAGE ONE<br />

(Inputs/Predisposing<br />

Factors)<br />

Demographics<br />

Parental influence<br />

Peer influence<br />

Societal and Media<br />

Influence<br />

Cause of<br />

Disfigurement<br />

STAGE TWO INTERVENING<br />

COGNITIVE PROCESSES<br />

Appearance Schema<br />

Salience<br />

Valence<br />

Perceived<br />

Noticeability<br />

Ideals<br />

Self<br />

Cultural<br />

Social Comparison Processes<br />

Dispositional Style<br />

Coping Strategies<br />

Perceptions of Social Support<br />

Figure 1: Working framework of adjustment to disfiguring conditions<br />

STAGE THREE<br />

(Outcomes)<br />

Social Anxiety<br />

Social avoidance /<br />

isolation<br />

Psychological<br />

wellbeing<br />

Within this framework, <strong>the</strong> process of adjustment to visible differences is conceptualised as<br />

having three facets. <strong>The</strong> first is <strong>the</strong> social and psychological context comprised of predisposing<br />

factors such as demographic characteristics, socio-cultural setting, and family environment.<br />

While all of <strong>the</strong>se factors play a part in adjustment and an understanding of <strong>the</strong>ir influence is<br />

important, <strong>the</strong>ir relative impermeability may make <strong>the</strong>m less amenable to intervention. <strong>The</strong><br />

current programme of research <strong>the</strong>refore acknowledges <strong>the</strong> presence and influence of <strong>the</strong>se<br />

factors, but maintains a predominant focus on cognitive and behavioural processes, which are<br />

more amenable to change through interventions.<br />

<strong>The</strong> second facet of <strong>the</strong> model comprises intervening cognitive processes, which are believed to<br />

contribute to <strong>the</strong> differentiation between good and poor adjusters. We believe that <strong>the</strong>se are<br />

best understood from a social cognition perspective, with particular reference to <strong>the</strong> functioning<br />

of <strong>the</strong> self-concept. From this viewpoint, individuals can be conceptualised as having a<br />

cognitive representation of <strong>the</strong>mselves, which includes a representation of appearance. <strong>The</strong><br />

appearance aspect of <strong>the</strong> self-representation can be more or less salient at any time, and thus<br />

play a greater or lesser part in <strong>the</strong> working self-concept (Higgins & Brendl, 1995). As a result of<br />

this, <strong>the</strong>re will be variation in <strong>the</strong> extent to which a person’s perception of <strong>the</strong>ir own appearance<br />

is involved in attending to and appraising activity in <strong>the</strong> social environment and in subsequent<br />

memories of past experiences (Moss & Carr, 2004 Bargh, Lombardi & Higgins, 1988). In<br />

addition to <strong>the</strong> salience of appearance within <strong>the</strong> self-concept, <strong>the</strong>re can also be a variation in<br />

<strong>the</strong> evaluative valence of <strong>the</strong> appearance (i.e. <strong>the</strong> way a person looks can be seen by that<br />

person in a positive or negative light (Sarwer & Crerand, 2004)). Appearance can also be<br />

judged by individuals as being closer to, or fur<strong>the</strong>r from <strong>the</strong>ir internalised cultural ideals (Altabe<br />

& Thompson, 1996). When appearance is generally more salient, negatively valenced, and<br />

fur<strong>the</strong>r from ideals, adjustment will be poorer. Some of <strong>the</strong> important ways in which appearance<br />

can be made more salient and prone to negative evaluation include social comparison<br />

processes (Green & Sedikides, 2001) and positive or negative experiences of social<br />

15


encounters, as well as subjective perceptions of <strong>the</strong> perceived severity and <strong>the</strong> perceived<br />

noticeabilty of <strong>the</strong> disfiguration to o<strong>the</strong>rs (Rumsey et al, 2004). O<strong>the</strong>r more stable variables,<br />

such as attributional style (Crocker et al, 1991), some coping processes (Fauerbach et al,<br />

2002), and perceptions of social support (Robinson, 1997) may also serve to exacerbate or<br />

ameliorate distress (see also Moss, 1997). As members of this collaboration recognised <strong>the</strong><br />

need to provide knowledge that would inform effective interventions, particular attention was<br />

paid to those cognitive processes which are amenable to change, including salience, valence,<br />

perceived noticeability and severity, and <strong>the</strong> relation of <strong>the</strong> appearance evaluation to subjective<br />

individual and perceived cultural ideals and norms, and <strong>the</strong> way in which social comparison<br />

relates to <strong>the</strong>se. <strong>The</strong> research programme also provided <strong>the</strong> opportunity to assess <strong>the</strong> value<br />

and utility of conceptualising adjustment in this way.<br />

<strong>The</strong> third facet of <strong>the</strong> model is <strong>the</strong> observable and experienced effects of appearance concerns.<br />

Existing work has already demonstrated <strong>the</strong> importance of social anxiety and avoidance (for<br />

reviews, see Thompson & Kent, 2001; Rumsey & Harcourt, 2004), and to a lesser extent shame<br />

and hostility (Kent & Thompson, 2002). Although conceptualised here as outcomes, it is<br />

acknowledged that <strong>the</strong>se constructs also serve a function in setting <strong>the</strong> psychological context for<br />

<strong>the</strong> cognitive processes described above. For <strong>the</strong> purposes of modelling <strong>the</strong>y are represented<br />

here as outcomes, but <strong>the</strong>re is an ongoing debate amongst researchers in <strong>the</strong> area (including<br />

those in this collaboration) and most are of <strong>the</strong> view that <strong>the</strong>y are not simply <strong>the</strong> last links in a<br />

chain, but part of a dynamic process in which experience informs cognitions.<br />

Design considerations in <strong>the</strong> research process<br />

<strong>The</strong> use of mixed methodologies<br />

This research collaboration has adopted a mixed methods approach. Debate concerning <strong>the</strong><br />

relative merits of quantitative and qualitative methods has ranged for some time, and has been<br />

termed by some “<strong>the</strong> paradigm wars” (Dures, 2009). In <strong>the</strong> recent past, quantitative and<br />

qualitative approaches were viewed by many as incompatible as <strong>the</strong> two approaches are<br />

underpinned by different sets of assumptions about what knowledge can be acquired and <strong>the</strong><br />

methods of acquiring it. However, <strong>the</strong>re is now widespread recognition that adherence solely to<br />

one or o<strong>the</strong>r set of assumptions can impose an unnecessary rigidity on research. Research in<br />

‘real world’ settings, particularly research which is orientated to client needs ra<strong>the</strong>r than driven<br />

primarily by academic concerns, requires a flexible approach that can cope with complex, multifactorial<br />

issues and takes practicalities into account (Dures, 2009). <strong>The</strong> current consensus is<br />

that both methodological approaches have merits, particularly in research in which pragmatism<br />

and critical realism have a place (Denscombe, 2008). Proponents of mixed methods argue that<br />

understanding can be enhanced to a greater extent through <strong>the</strong> use of both approaches ra<strong>the</strong>r<br />

than by one in isolation, but also believe that <strong>the</strong>ir use should be justified in relation to <strong>the</strong><br />

research question and that <strong>the</strong> rationale should be clearly specified. Johnson, Onwuegbuzie<br />

and Turner (2007) have defined mixed methods as ‘a third methodological or research<br />

paradigm…that will often provide <strong>the</strong> most informative, complete, balanced and useful research<br />

results’ (p129). <strong>The</strong> different studies should talk to each o<strong>the</strong>r, much like a conversation<br />

(Bryman 2007).<br />

16


In order to address <strong>the</strong> first aim of this research programme (to identify psychosocial factors and<br />

processes contributing to adjustment), and in view of <strong>the</strong> small sample sizes used in previous<br />

research and <strong>the</strong> tentative nature of previous conclusions, a strong driver for <strong>the</strong> research<br />

collaboration was to generate a large sample size. In addition, we wished to generate sufficient<br />

participant numbers to assess <strong>the</strong> utility of our conceptual framework. Having made <strong>the</strong> decision<br />

to undertake a large scale, quantitative study with only a limited qualitative element, a series of<br />

follow-on studies were designed using a variety of predominantly qualitative methodologies.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se were intended to generate a more in-depth understanding of particular topics and to<br />

illuminate <strong>the</strong> detail of individual experience. <strong>The</strong> studies were developed in order to expand<br />

understanding of elements of <strong>the</strong> framework, including a qualitative study of those identifying<br />

<strong>the</strong>mselves as positive adjusters (Study 4), <strong>the</strong> particular issues relating to ethnicity (Studies 5 &<br />

6), a study of social comparison processes (Study 7) and an examination of appearance<br />

concerns and aggression (Study 8). In response to <strong>the</strong> progamme advisory panel, work was<br />

initiated to develop a measure of <strong>the</strong> role of appearance concerns in intimate relationships<br />

(Study 9), <strong>the</strong> appearance concerns of people with pros<strong>the</strong>tic limbs (Study 13) and to better<br />

understand <strong>the</strong> beliefs, decision making processes and training needs of general practitioners<br />

(Study 12). In addition, <strong>the</strong> role of appearance concerns in adjustment for those with<br />

rheumatoid arthritis was examined as a particular example of <strong>the</strong> potential interplay of functional<br />

and appearance concerns (Study 11).<br />

Amongst o<strong>the</strong>rs, Thompson and Kent (2001) and Rumsey and Harcourt (2005) have noted <strong>the</strong><br />

pressing need for longitudinal research to explore <strong>the</strong> dynamic, fluctuating nature of adjustment<br />

in relation to disfigurement. Accordingly it was also a priority for <strong>the</strong> collaboration to include<br />

such an element in <strong>the</strong> programme of research. Study 3 explores <strong>the</strong> issue of stability and<br />

change in adjustment over time.<br />

For a diagrammatic representation of <strong>the</strong> studies included in <strong>the</strong> research programme, see<br />

Figure 2.<br />

17


Study 3 Study 4 Study 5 Study 6 Study 7 Study 8 Study 9 Study 10 Study 11 Study 12 Study 13<br />

Longitudinal<br />

(n = 365)<br />

Cross<br />

Sectional<br />

survey<br />

completed<br />

again at 9<br />

months<br />

Stability &<br />

change<br />

(n = 26)<br />

In depth<br />

interviews<br />

& template<br />

analysis<br />

Positive<br />

adjusters<br />

(n = 12)<br />

Focus<br />

groups<br />

with self<br />

identified<br />

as<br />

positive<br />

adjusters<br />

Ethnicity<br />

(n = 63)<br />

Focus groups<br />

with BME<br />

groups re:<br />

disfigurement<br />

Figure 2: Representation of <strong>the</strong> research programme<br />

Studies 1 (Quantitative) and 2 (Qualitative)<br />

Multi site cross sectional investigation of psychosocial factors and processes contributing to adjustment (n = 1265)<br />

UK South<br />

Asians<br />

with<br />

vitiligo<br />

(n = 7)<br />

In depth<br />

interviews<br />

and<br />

template<br />

analysis<br />

Social<br />

comparison<br />

(n = 13)<br />

Online<br />

survey<br />

Appearance<br />

concerns &<br />

aggression<br />

(n = 38)<br />

Online<br />

survey<br />

INTERVENTIONS<br />

Intimacy<br />

(n = 145<br />

students, n =<br />

42 patients)<br />

Development<br />

of a scale<br />

Women<br />

with<br />

pros<strong>the</strong>ses<br />

(n = 6)<br />

In depth<br />

interviews<br />

& <strong>the</strong>matic<br />

analysis<br />

Rheumatoid<br />

Arthritis<br />

(n = 49)<br />

Survey<br />

Training<br />

needs of<br />

GPs<br />

(n = 30)<br />

Survey<br />

with case<br />

scenarios<br />

18


Role of <strong>the</strong> advisory panel in <strong>the</strong> research process<br />

<strong>The</strong> Advisory Panel included three representatives with experiences of living with different types<br />

of disfigurement. Additional members included a collaborating GP and <strong>the</strong> Chief Executive of<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong>. Two plastic surgeons showed initial interest in joining <strong>the</strong> Advisory<br />

Panel, however, despite repeated invitations to engage with <strong>the</strong> project in <strong>the</strong> first 12 months,<br />

were unable to contribute. Accordingly, a third Consultant Plastic Surgeon who was a member<br />

of <strong>the</strong> Steering Committee for <strong>The</strong> Centre for Appearance Research (CAR) was kept abreast of<br />

developments in <strong>the</strong> project, and offered comments as appropriate. <strong>The</strong> panel met for <strong>the</strong> first<br />

time in Spring 2006. <strong>The</strong> project team presented <strong>the</strong>ir plans for Study 1, including <strong>the</strong> draft of<br />

<strong>the</strong> questionnaire pack, and <strong>the</strong> letter, information and consent sheets to be given to potential<br />

participants. Panel members offered constructive comments on <strong>the</strong> letters and questionnaires,<br />

and where feasible, appropriate changes were incorporated into <strong>the</strong> final versions. It was also<br />

suggested that <strong>the</strong> community sample be expanded to include more user groups, as <strong>the</strong><br />

experience of panel members was that <strong>the</strong> possibility of participating in research was perceived<br />

as a welcome opportunity to increase understanding of <strong>the</strong> experiences of people with<br />

disfiguring conditions. Following <strong>the</strong> meeting, a preference for individual ra<strong>the</strong>r than collective<br />

meetings was expressed by members of <strong>the</strong> advisory panel. Accordingly, <strong>the</strong> Project Lead met<br />

with panel members individually during February/March 2007 to summarise <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong><br />

preliminary analyses from <strong>the</strong> cross-sectional study (Study 1), and to discuss plans for <strong>the</strong><br />

follow-up studies and for <strong>the</strong> intervention strand of <strong>the</strong> project. At this point, two members<br />

suggested <strong>the</strong> inclusion of a study on <strong>the</strong> effects of appearance concerns on intimate<br />

relationships, and one was especially supportive of <strong>the</strong> idea that a study of people using<br />

pros<strong>the</strong>tic devices would be useful. <strong>The</strong> lack of knowledge and understanding amongst GPs<br />

about <strong>the</strong> impact of visible disfigurements was also highlighted. <strong>The</strong>se ideas were all<br />

incorporated into follow-on studies within <strong>the</strong> programme. Panel members were consulted on<br />

an individual basis once more in June/July 2009, at which time <strong>the</strong> results, plans for outputs and<br />

dissemination, and plans for fur<strong>the</strong>r research were discussed and finalised.<br />

In summary, <strong>the</strong> following actions were taken in response to advice from <strong>the</strong> Advisory Panel:<br />

• Changes to recruitment letter to <strong>the</strong> community sample<br />

• An enlarged base for <strong>the</strong> recruitment of <strong>the</strong> community sample to include more user<br />

groups<br />

• <strong>The</strong> initiation of a study on <strong>the</strong> role of appearance and intimacy<br />

• An exploration of <strong>the</strong> knowledge of General Practitioners in relation to disfigurement<br />

• Inclusion of a study of experiences of people with pros<strong>the</strong>tic devices<br />

• Inclusion of pros<strong>the</strong>tists, psychiatrists and primary care workers in plans for<br />

dissemination<br />

19


Challenges encountered during <strong>the</strong> research process<br />

It is inevitable in a large scale multi-centre programme of research such as this that in addition<br />

to <strong>the</strong> added value of <strong>the</strong> pooled expertise of <strong>the</strong> collaborators, and <strong>the</strong> increased possibilities<br />

for sampling, a number of challenges will be encountered. <strong>The</strong> most significant challenges and<br />

<strong>the</strong> solutions adopted are outlined briefly below. Those associated with <strong>the</strong> design and analysis<br />

stages are described in <strong>the</strong> relevant sections later in this <strong>report</strong>.<br />

Initial delays in <strong>the</strong> contracts & appointments process<br />

Unexpected delays were encountered in relation to <strong>the</strong> subcontracts set up between UWE (<strong>the</strong><br />

lead centre and central budget holder) and <strong>the</strong> Universities of Warwick, UCL, Sheffield &<br />

Bradford. <strong>The</strong>se delays meant that <strong>the</strong> appointments of <strong>the</strong> researchers at all sites were also<br />

delayed. Dr. Antje Lindenmeyer began work in Warwick in July 2006, Dr. Sally Clarke<br />

(Sheffield/Bradford) and Hayley James (UCL/Royal Free) took up <strong>the</strong>ir posts at <strong>the</strong> beginning of<br />

October 2006.<br />

This delay in <strong>the</strong> contracts process (and consequently in recruitment start-dates) meant that <strong>the</strong><br />

longitudinal element of Study 1 had to be shortened in order to fit <strong>the</strong> funding life of <strong>the</strong> project.<br />

Data was collected at 9 month follow-up, instead of 12 months as originally planned.<br />

Ethical and research governance approvals<br />

As <strong>the</strong> Project Team realised that <strong>the</strong> ethical approvals would be complex and time consuming<br />

in view of <strong>the</strong> large number of participating centres, <strong>the</strong> process of gaining approval was started<br />

soon after confirmation of <strong>the</strong> award of <strong>the</strong> grant and prior to <strong>the</strong> appointment of Research<br />

Associates and Research Assistants. Dr. Diana Harcourt (UWE, Bristol) led <strong>the</strong> process of<br />

achieving <strong>the</strong> necessary ethical and research governance approvals for <strong>the</strong> project, with <strong>the</strong><br />

regional Principle Investigators completing site specific applications for ethics and governance<br />

approvals. <strong>The</strong> approvals were achieved with only minor changes to protocols. Subsequent<br />

delays were those inherent in <strong>the</strong> approvals process at that time. COREC approval for <strong>the</strong> first<br />

two studies was achieved in May 2005, with subsequent LREC and local research governance<br />

approvals achieved between November 2005 and October 2006. <strong>The</strong> vast majority of approvals<br />

and of <strong>the</strong> honorary contracts, criminal records bureau checks necessary for all <strong>the</strong> researchers<br />

were also achieved by this time.<br />

Staffing<br />

Dr. James Byron-Daniel (<strong>the</strong> Lead Researcher) applied for a lecturing post at UWE nine months<br />

after <strong>the</strong> beginning of <strong>the</strong> project, and was appointed in September 2006. James subsequently<br />

joined <strong>the</strong> UWE staff team involved in <strong>the</strong> project. Liz Payne (now Jenkinson), who was<br />

originally <strong>the</strong> project administrator acted-up in <strong>the</strong> role of Lead Researcher, and following a<br />

formal selection process, was appointed to <strong>the</strong> post in 2007. She has been supported by Emma<br />

Williams and Eleanor Walsh in an administrative capacity. <strong>The</strong> handover in responsibilities was<br />

smooth, and <strong>the</strong> team was fortunate that Liz was over qualified for her initial role. Liz<br />

established excellent relationships with <strong>the</strong> researchers appointed to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r centres, and her<br />

performance in this role has been key in <strong>the</strong> success of <strong>the</strong> project. <strong>The</strong> collaboration was also<br />

fortunate in relation to its o<strong>the</strong>r research associates and research administrators, and great<br />

20


credit is due to Anjte Lindenmeyer, Sally-Ann Clarke, Hayley James, Krysia Saul, Emma<br />

Williams and Eleanor Walsh for <strong>the</strong>ir tenacity and hard work for <strong>the</strong> duration of this project.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r major tasks associated with <strong>the</strong> research<br />

• Establishing and maintaining effective working relationships (research assistants; project<br />

partners). This was addressed by:<br />

o Facilitating communication between team members (regular phone calls, emails<br />

and workshops<br />

o Setting up of a project blog:<br />

http://carhealingfoundationproject.blogspot.com/<br />

managed by <strong>the</strong> Bristol team. This blog provided a forum all <strong>the</strong> project team<br />

members for updates, news and on-going discussions.<br />

• Training of research assistants in data base management; recruitment techniques;<br />

interviewing skills and qualitative analysis<br />

• Agreeing recruitment strategies and establishing relationships with general practice<br />

partnerships (Bristol & Warwick), key clinicians (Bristol, Sheffield, Bradford, UCL, Royal<br />

Free London)<br />

• Constructing and managing a database large enough to cope with over 300,000 data<br />

entry points<br />

o Access data base training (data base management) was undertaken by <strong>the</strong><br />

Bristol based team researchers<br />

• Agreeing <strong>the</strong> collaborative approach to data analysis<br />

• Agreeing publications and dissemination strategies (see Appendices 1 and 2)<br />

• Co-ordinating <strong>the</strong> project timescales and agreeing a common <strong>report</strong>ing format for followon<br />

studies<br />

21


STUDY 1<br />

FACTORS AND PROCESSES INVOLVED IN ADJUSTMENT TO<br />

DISFIGUREMENT:<br />

A CROSS-SECTIONAL STUDY<br />

23


INTRODUCTION<br />

<strong>The</strong> largest part of <strong>the</strong> research programme in terms of time and resources comprised a crosssectional<br />

multi-centre study. As discussed in <strong>the</strong> Background section, previous work (Carr,<br />

Harris & James, 2000;, Thompson & Kent, 2001; Rumsey et al, 2004; Carr, Moss & Harris 2005;<br />

Fagervik-Morton, in preparation) has provided data on <strong>the</strong> prevalence and consequences of<br />

appearance concerns in people with a range of disfiguring conditions. However, this research<br />

has also raised many questions about <strong>the</strong> psychological factors and processes underpinning <strong>the</strong><br />

considerable variation in adjustment, which could only be answered through a large data set. In<br />

addition, a large sample size was needed to allow an investigation of condition specific effects<br />

and a more detailed examination than hi<strong>the</strong>rto of <strong>the</strong> impact of non-clinical and nonpsychological<br />

variables (for example, age, gender and social economic status) on adjustment.<br />

Key research questions<br />

• What is <strong>the</strong> prevalence of adjustment and distress in participants recruited from <strong>the</strong><br />

community and from hospital out-patient settings?<br />

• What psychosocial factors and processes are associated with positive adjustment and<br />

distress in people with visibly disfiguring conditions?<br />

<strong>The</strong> large scale nature of <strong>the</strong> study dictated a predominantly quantitative approach, however,<br />

mindful of <strong>the</strong> need to capture <strong>the</strong> large range of individual differences in experience and <strong>the</strong><br />

potential richness of qualitative data, we also included an open ended response box to offer an<br />

opportunity for a qualitative element in <strong>the</strong> data collection (see Study 2).<br />

METHODS<br />

Sample and recruitment<br />

Visible differences result from a wide variety of congenital anomalies (e.g. a cleft of <strong>the</strong> lip;<br />

neurofibromatosis), illnesses (e.g. arthritis), chronic conditions (e.g. vitiligo, acne), injuries (e.g.<br />

burns), or surgical interventions (e.g., following surgery to remove malignant tumours). In order<br />

to sample as widely as possible, <strong>the</strong> project was organised around four geographical regions:<br />

Bristol (lead site), London (site 2), Bradford/Sheffield (site 3), and Warwick (site 4), with each<br />

site working to promote a broad spread of respondents at various stages of <strong>the</strong> treatment<br />

journey.<br />

As <strong>the</strong> opportunities for recruitment through clinics and <strong>the</strong> community varied between hospitals<br />

and geographical regions, and as <strong>the</strong> organisational and logistical constraints affecting<br />

recruitment were different in every site, <strong>the</strong> regional principal investigators negotiated <strong>the</strong><br />

practical details of sampling and recruitment on a local basis, within <strong>the</strong> framework agreed for<br />

<strong>the</strong> whole study (i.e. target numbers for each condition, inclusion and exclusion criteria and<br />

ethical guidelines for <strong>the</strong> research).<br />

24


Inclusion criteria<br />

Participants were men and women aged 18 years or over, with fluency in written and spoken<br />

English, who perceived <strong>the</strong>mselves as having a visible difference. (<strong>The</strong> resources necessary to<br />

study children and adolescents are considerable, and data collection is complicated by <strong>the</strong> need<br />

for specialist interviewers and age appropriate assessments. It is anticipated that subsequent<br />

studies focusing specifically on adjustment in children and adolescents will be informed by this<br />

research programme).<br />

Exclusion criteria<br />

Participants with identifiable psychosis or dementia were not included in this research. In this<br />

study, participants unable to read and write in English were excluded, as <strong>the</strong>re were no<br />

measures (with <strong>the</strong> exception of HADS) available which were equivalent to those selected for<br />

<strong>the</strong> study which had been validated in non-English languages. Adequate representation of<br />

members of ethnic minority communities was promoted by actively seeking those who were<br />

able to read and speak English, as <strong>the</strong> research collaboration recognised that an understanding<br />

of cultural differences is an important gap in <strong>the</strong> existing body of knowledge about adjustment to<br />

problems of appearance. Fur<strong>the</strong>r efforts were made to address this in follow-on studies 5 & 6.<br />

Sampling through outpatients clinics<br />

As in previous studies, out-patient clinics were approached as a cost effective means of<br />

accessing patients seeking medical intervention. A wide range of clinics was targeted as<br />

potential sites for recruitment and each site organised recruitment in local clinics, overseen by<br />

<strong>the</strong> regional principal investigator. Out-patients clinics taking part were as follows:<br />

• Sheffield/Bradford: head and neck cancer, pros<strong>the</strong>tics, dermatology, ophthalmology and<br />

general plastics (plastics & burns), cleft lip & palate clinics [trauma/ENT].<br />

• London: general plastics, ocular plastics, ocular pros<strong>the</strong>tics and rheumatology clinics.<br />

• Bristol: laser, general plastics, skin cancer and ophthalmology clinics<br />

In total, 650 participants were recruited via out-patient clinics, from London, Sheffield/Bradford<br />

and Bristol sites. Respondents were 393 women (60.4%) and 234 men (35.9%), ranging in age<br />

from 18-91 years (M= 49.7 years, SD= 16.9 years). Fur<strong>the</strong>r demographic details are included in<br />

<strong>the</strong> results section of this <strong>report</strong>.<br />

Sampling through non-medical sources (<strong>the</strong> ‘community’ sample)<br />

In previous research, <strong>the</strong> emphasis has been on those attending for treatment (particularly<br />

hospital out-patient clinic attendees). Despite <strong>the</strong> associated challenges, it was considered a<br />

priority to learn more about <strong>the</strong> broader constituency of people with visible differences, and in<br />

view of <strong>the</strong> commitment of <strong>the</strong> collaboration to focus on positive adjustment, to also sample<br />

those who were not actively seeking treatment or intervention at <strong>the</strong> time of participation. This<br />

type of recruitment is more challenging, and several strategies were considered by <strong>the</strong><br />

collaboration, including <strong>the</strong> distribution of study materials on a house to house basis,<br />

recruitment via relevant self-help groups, through advertisements on <strong>the</strong> internet, and through<br />

local media. In <strong>the</strong> end, participants were recruited via city centre and rural GP practices in <strong>the</strong><br />

25


South West (via <strong>the</strong> Bristol site) and through practices in <strong>the</strong> Midlands (via <strong>the</strong> Warwick site).<br />

As recruitment (particularly in <strong>the</strong> Midlands) was slow to pick up, additional recruitment was<br />

achieved via advertisements on <strong>the</strong> websites of self help groups and through press releases in<br />

local press.<br />

In total, 615 participants were recruited via <strong>the</strong> community, from <strong>the</strong> Warwick and Bristol sites.<br />

Two large GP surgeries (one rural and one urban) were involved from <strong>the</strong> Bristol region and 10<br />

surgeries across <strong>the</strong> Midlands.<br />

Four hundred and seventy four respondents were women (77.2%) and 120 were men, ranging<br />

in age from 18-89 years (M=44.9 years, SD=16.2 years). Fur<strong>the</strong>r demographic details are<br />

included in <strong>the</strong> results section of this <strong>report</strong>.<br />

Sample size<br />

Estimates of <strong>the</strong> number of people affected by various forms of visible difference vary, as do<br />

methods of categorising <strong>the</strong>m on <strong>the</strong> basis of <strong>the</strong> type of disfigurement (for example,<br />

categorisation on <strong>the</strong> basis of a specific condition such as vitiligo, or according to a broader<br />

categorisation such as ‘skin conditions’). As a result, notions about <strong>the</strong> most appropriate<br />

sample size needed to adequately represent <strong>the</strong> population of those affected and of <strong>the</strong><br />

numbers needed to demonstrate similarities and differences between types of disfigurement are<br />

fluid. As <strong>the</strong>re is now a broad consensus in <strong>the</strong> literature that condition specific effects play a<br />

much more minor role in adjustment and distress than psychological and socio-cultural factors,<br />

<strong>the</strong> benefits (compared to <strong>the</strong> costs) of exhaustive sampling by types and subtypes of condition<br />

are questionable. It was <strong>the</strong>refore agreed that a spread of conditions would be sampled, with<br />

differing out-patients clinics targeted by each site.<br />

Sample size calculations relied on <strong>the</strong> previous experience of <strong>the</strong> group, and on <strong>the</strong> assumption<br />

that within <strong>the</strong> largest study (Study 1), for <strong>the</strong> clinic sample, a minimum of 12 ‘conditions’ would<br />

be sampled. An a-priori power analysis for multiple regression (assuming standard levels of<br />

significance and a power of 0.95) indicated that in <strong>the</strong> presence of small effects (for example, Rsquare<br />

-.04) a minimum sample size of 851 cases would be needed to assess <strong>the</strong> significance<br />

of up to 25 variables in a global hypo<strong>the</strong>sis test. This allowed a range of superficially different<br />

types of disfigurement to be examined and meaningful comparisons of subgroups to be made,<br />

toge<strong>the</strong>r with a purposive sampling of subgroups of participants for <strong>the</strong> qualitative and<br />

longitudinal studies. A process of quota sampling for rarer conditions was adopted in order to<br />

facilitate statistical analysis, with a minimum target of 30.<br />

Materials<br />

Written information<br />

A standardised pack of written information and questionnaires was distributed to all participants.<br />

This included an information sheet clearly stating principles relating to identity protection, <strong>the</strong><br />

right to withdraw from studies at any time, and procedures for <strong>the</strong> storage and destruction of<br />

records. <strong>The</strong>re were minor changes in this information between sites (i.e. university contact<br />

26


information, names of consultants collaborating, etc). A consent form and a form to consent to<br />

be contacted about fur<strong>the</strong>r studies were also included.<br />

Measures<br />

In order to adapt <strong>the</strong> conceptual framework (Figure 1) adopted by <strong>the</strong> research team for data<br />

collection and analysis purposes, it was necessary to consider how best to operationalise <strong>the</strong><br />

variables (see Figure 3).<br />

<strong>The</strong> decisions relating to <strong>the</strong> choice of measure and <strong>the</strong> designation of variables as ‘process’ or<br />

‘outcome’ generated considerable debate within <strong>the</strong> collaboration and among <strong>the</strong> Advisory<br />

Panel. Guiding principles included a primary focus on variables which were considered<br />

modifiable through intervention and on questionnaires which had face validity for participants<br />

and which were straightforward to complete. ‘Generic’ questionnaires (e.g. <strong>the</strong> HADS) enabling<br />

comparisons with previous research and with <strong>report</strong>s of adjustment to a range of o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

conditions were also included. <strong>The</strong> inclusion of measures which included <strong>the</strong> possibility of<br />

capturing profiles indicative of positive adjustment was also a priority. This was particularly<br />

challenging as many of <strong>the</strong> questionnaires used in previous research have an exclusively<br />

negative focus. In <strong>the</strong> case of variables for which suitable measures did not exist (for example,<br />

<strong>the</strong> issue of social acceptance of a disfiguring aspect of appearance), questions based on <strong>the</strong><br />

previous literature and on <strong>the</strong> research and clinical experience of <strong>the</strong> group were constructed to<br />

capture participants’ responses. O<strong>the</strong>r considerations included <strong>the</strong> overall number of<br />

questionnaires that should be included in <strong>the</strong> participant packs, and <strong>the</strong> burden on participants.<br />

For full questionnaire booklet see appendix 3.<br />

27


INPUTS/<br />

PREDISPOSING<br />

FACTORS<br />

Age<br />

Gender<br />

Ethnicity<br />

Visibility to o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

Living Status<br />

Cause<br />

Treatment<br />

Figure 3: Operationalising <strong>the</strong> Framework – Choosing appropriate measures.<br />

PREDISPOSING FACTORS:<br />

INTERVENING COGNITIVE<br />

PROCESSES<br />

Dispositional Style (LOT-R)<br />

Socio-cognitive Factors<br />

Perceptions of Social Support<br />

Perceptions of Social Acceptance<br />

Fear of Negative Evaluation (FNE)<br />

Appearance schema:<br />

Appearance – Related<br />

Processing<br />

Salience (CARSAL)<br />

Valence (CARVAL)<br />

Disguisability<br />

Self / Ideal Discrepancy (PADQ)<br />

Social Comparison (INCOMM)<br />

OUTCOMES<br />

Social Anxiety / Social<br />

Avoidance (DAS)<br />

Aggression/ Hostility (RAQ)<br />

Wellbeing (HADS)<br />

Mood (PANAS)<br />

Demographics<br />

Participants were asked to indicate <strong>the</strong>ir postcode, age, gender, family status/living<br />

arrangements, nationality and ethnicity.<br />

Participants were also presented with two open ended questions at <strong>the</strong> close of <strong>the</strong><br />

questionnaire. <strong>The</strong> first asked for feedback about <strong>the</strong> questionnaire pack and <strong>the</strong> second asked<br />

if <strong>the</strong> respondent had any general comments about <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>ir appearance does/does not<br />

affect <strong>the</strong>ir life positively or negatively (see Study 2).<br />

Cause<br />

Participants were asked to select a cause of <strong>the</strong> main difference in appearance from a list of 11<br />

options (including ‘o<strong>the</strong>r’ with room to specify).<br />

Treatment information<br />

Participants were asked to state whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y had received treatment to change <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance and if so, <strong>the</strong> earliest and latest dates <strong>the</strong>se treatments were received. <strong>The</strong>y were<br />

also asked to state if <strong>the</strong>y were awaiting any treatments.<br />

Area of appearance concern<br />

Participants indicated <strong>the</strong>ir area of concern by ticking on a checklist <strong>the</strong> parts of <strong>the</strong>ir body about<br />

which <strong>the</strong>y were most sensitive. <strong>The</strong>se were later classified into <strong>the</strong> broad headings of visible to<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs/difficult to disguise (face, hands, neck, head) and non-visible (easier to disguise).<br />

28


INTERVENING COGNITIVE PROCESSES:<br />

Dispositional style<br />

Optimism<br />

To assess levels of optimism a shortened four item version of <strong>the</strong> Life Orientation Test-Revised<br />

(LOT-R; Scheier, Carver & Bidges, 1994) was utilized. Responses were on a five point Likert<br />

scale, ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strong disagree). Total scale score range from 4-20<br />

with higher scores indicating a more optimistic outlook. <strong>The</strong> scale is designed to be viewed as a<br />

measuring a continuum, with no "cut-offs" for optimism or pessimism. Scheier, Carver and<br />

Bridges (1994) demonstrate adequate internal consistency (alpha=0.78) and test-retest<br />

reliability that indicates stability over time (r=0.68 at 4 months, r=0.60 at 12 months, r=0.56 at 24<br />

months, r=0.79 at 28 months).<br />

Socio-cognitive factors<br />

Social networks<br />

<strong>The</strong> scale utilized is a shortened four item version of <strong>the</strong> Short Form Social Support<br />

Questionnaire (Sarason, Levine, Basham & Sarason, 1983). <strong>The</strong> quantity responses were<br />

considered primers and were not included in <strong>the</strong> analysis. Quality ratings ranged from 1 (very<br />

satisfied) to 6 (very dissatisfied), scores ranged from 4-24, with higher scores representing<br />

greater satisfaction with one’s social network. <strong>The</strong> authors cite that <strong>the</strong> scale showed good test<br />

retest reliability, with a 4-week retest yielding 0.83 coefficient and good internal reliability (alpha<br />

coefficient=0.97).<br />

Feelings of acceptance<br />

To assess feelings of social acceptance two items with a seven point Likert scale ranging from 1<br />

(not at all) to 7 (completely), assessed <strong>the</strong> extent to which <strong>the</strong> respondent felt accepted by <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

social group and society in general. Total scores range from 2-14, with higher scores indicating<br />

higher levels of acceptance.<br />

Fears of negative evaluation<br />

<strong>The</strong> Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation (FNE) scale (Leary 1983) examines whe<strong>the</strong>r concern<br />

about o<strong>the</strong>r people’s opinions regarding oneself is a characteristic of <strong>the</strong> individual. 12 items are<br />

presented and participants asked to score each statement from 1 (Not at all characteristic of<br />

me) to 5 (Extremely characteristic of me). Scores range from 12-60 with high scores indicating a<br />

greater fear of negative evaluation. <strong>The</strong> authors cite adequate psychometric properties, with<br />

high levels of internal consistency (alpha=0.90), a 4-week test retest reliability with a student<br />

population yielding a 0.75 reliability coefficient and acceptable levels of construct validity.<br />

29


Appearance-related processing<br />

Valence of appearance<br />

<strong>The</strong> CARVAL (Moss, Hobbs & Rosser, in preparation) is a six item valence questionnaire that<br />

measures how positively or negatively a participant evaluates <strong>the</strong>ir own appearance.<br />

Responses range from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree) with higher total scores<br />

indicating a more positive self evaluation of <strong>the</strong>ir own appearance. Potential scale scores range<br />

from 6-36. <strong>The</strong> scale has recently demonstrated good internal consistency (alpha=0.89) and<br />

very good test retest reliability with a student population at three months (r= .95)<br />

Salience of appearance<br />

<strong>The</strong> CARSAL (Moss, Hobbs & Rosser, in preparation) measures <strong>the</strong> extent to which<br />

appearance is part of a person’s working self concept or how important it is to a person<br />

(salience). Response ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree) with higher total<br />

scores indicating appearance forming a greater part of <strong>the</strong>ir self concept. Potential scale scores<br />

range from 6-36. <strong>The</strong> scale has recently demonstrated good internal consistency (alpha=0.86)<br />

and good test retest reliability with a student population at three months (r= .89).<br />

Disguisability<br />

By rating on a seven point Likert scale ranging from 1 (extremely easy) to 7 (impossible)<br />

participants were asked how difficult <strong>the</strong>y felt it was to hide or disguise <strong>the</strong> aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance about which <strong>the</strong>y were most concerned.<br />

Physical appearance discrepancy<br />

<strong>The</strong> PADQ (Altabe, 1996; Altabe & Thompson 1996) distinguishes <strong>the</strong> discrepancy between<br />

how a person perceives <strong>the</strong>y look and how <strong>the</strong>y or <strong>the</strong>ir significant o<strong>the</strong>rs would ideally like <strong>the</strong>m<br />

to look (‘ideal’ discrepancy) or how <strong>the</strong>y ought to or should look in relation to duty, responsibility,<br />

or obligation (‘should’ discrepancy). A high ‘ideal’ discrepancy is associated with feelings of<br />

disappointment, dissatisfaction, shame and embarrassment due to unfulfilled desires and a<br />

belief <strong>the</strong>y have lost esteem in <strong>the</strong> opinion of o<strong>the</strong>rs. A high ‘should’ discrepancy is associated<br />

with fear, feeling threatened, resentment and guilt due to <strong>the</strong> belief that <strong>the</strong>y have transgressed<br />

<strong>the</strong> moral standard of ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>mselves or significant o<strong>the</strong>rs. <strong>The</strong> two subscales of ‘ideal’ and<br />

‘should’ discrepancy consist of 4 items each, with responses ranging from not at all different to<br />

extremely different yielding a total score for each subscale ranging from 4-28, with higher score<br />

indicating greater discrepancy.<br />

Social comparison<br />

<strong>The</strong> social comparison scale is a brief version of <strong>the</strong> Gibbons and Buunk (1999) Iowa-<br />

Ne<strong>the</strong>rlands Social Comparison measure (INCOMM). <strong>The</strong> scale measures frequency of social<br />

comparisons. Participants were asked to complete <strong>the</strong> scale in reference to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance<br />

and responses range from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) with higher total scores<br />

indicating a higher frequency of engagement in social comparison. Potential scale scores range<br />

from 11-55. <strong>The</strong> scale has good internal consistency (alpha=0.83) and concurrent validity<br />

30


(r=0.88). <strong>The</strong> authors also cite adequate test retest reliability for up to four weeks and one year<br />

as yielding correlation coefficients of 0.71 and 0.60 respectively.<br />

OUTCOMES<br />

Social anxiety and social avoidance<br />

Derriford Appearance Scale Short Form (DAS24; Moss, Harris & Carr, 2004).<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAS24 is a 24 item version of <strong>the</strong> DAS59 (Carr, Harris & James, 2000) and is a measure of<br />

social anxiety and social avoidance in relation to appearance. It has been widely used in<br />

research related to disfigurement in <strong>the</strong> recent past. Norms derived from UK populations exist.<br />

Total scores range from 11-96 with lower scores representing low levels of social anxiety and<br />

social avoidance. It has adequate internal consistency (alpha=0.92), test retest reliability<br />

(r=0.82), concurrent validity with <strong>the</strong> DAS 59 (r=0.88) and convergent validity with measures of<br />

anxiety, depression, social avoidance, social distress, fear of negative evaluation, negative<br />

affect and shame (r11 indicates ‘caseness’, a high<br />

likelihood that a person would be diagnosed to be suffering from clinical anxiety or clinical<br />

depression.<br />

Literature reviews of <strong>the</strong> HADS (e.g Bjelland, Dahl, Haug and Neckelman 2002) have found <strong>the</strong><br />

HADS to consistently show adequate internal consistency over a range of studies, including<br />

those translated into languages o<strong>the</strong>r than English and good concurrent validity when compared<br />

to a range of o<strong>the</strong>r anxiety and depression scales (r=0.60 to 0.80). <strong>The</strong> HADS has also been<br />

used to good effect in previous studies with people with facial disfigurements (Newell, 2000b,<br />

Newell and Clarke 2000), and validated for use with people with visible difference (Martin and<br />

Newell 2004).<br />

31


Anger/hostility<br />

<strong>The</strong> Refined Aggression Questionnaire (RAQ) (Bryant and Smith 2001) is a brief version of<br />

Buss and Perry’s (1992) Aggression Questionnaire and has been found to have similar<br />

psychometric properties (Bryant and Smith 2001). This aggression scale uses 12 items to<br />

assess whe<strong>the</strong>r aggressive behaviour is characteristic of <strong>the</strong> respondent, with responses<br />

ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Divided into 4 factors, physical aggression,<br />

hostility, verbal aggression and anger each factor has an individual score with lower scores<br />

representing lower levels of that particular type of aggression. <strong>The</strong> scale score ranged from 3-15<br />

after appropriate item score reversals.<br />

PROCEDURE<br />

Research governance<br />

Ethical approval for <strong>the</strong> study was given by NRES (one original application for all centres was<br />

granted by COREC in addition to site specific approvals from each trust and data collection site)<br />

and each corresponding university ethics committee for <strong>the</strong> research sites (five applications).<br />

Criminal Records Bureau checks and honorary contracts with each NHS trust for each<br />

researcher recruiting in clinics were also secured.<br />

As <strong>the</strong> research programme developed, <strong>the</strong> lead site (UWE) was also required to apply for<br />

substantial and minor amendments to NRES to undertake <strong>the</strong> follow-on studies (six<br />

amendments in total). All additions and minor changes to <strong>the</strong> original protocol (for example, <strong>the</strong><br />

reduction of <strong>the</strong> longitudinal data point for <strong>the</strong> cross sectional questionnaire to nine months from<br />

12 months) were agreed by NRES and local university ethics committees. For copies of <strong>the</strong><br />

main ethical approvals and amendments see appendix 4.<br />

Protocols relating to recruitment<br />

Outpatient clinics<br />

Patients attending <strong>the</strong> participating out-patient clinics were sent invitations to take part along<br />

with an information sheet (see appendix 5) two weeks prior to <strong>the</strong>ir appointment. <strong>The</strong> researcher<br />

approached potential participants on <strong>the</strong> day of <strong>the</strong>ir appointment explaining <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong><br />

study and answering any potential questions. Once informed consent (see appendix 6 for<br />

consent form) was obtained, participants were given a questionnaire booklet ei<strong>the</strong>r to complete<br />

in clinic or to be taken away and sent back via freepost envelope. All participants took part on a<br />

voluntary basis and were not remunerated for <strong>the</strong>ir participation.<br />

Once received, data was stored securely (see below), receipt was logged and thank you letters<br />

were sent to each participant.<br />

32


Community recruitment<br />

In <strong>the</strong> South West (coordinated by <strong>the</strong> Bristol site), READ codes (used by some GPs to classify<br />

patients by condition) were used to identify possible participants with conditions that were<br />

potentially disfiguring. In <strong>the</strong> Midlands (coordinated by <strong>the</strong> Warwick site), participants were<br />

recruited through face to face approaches in practice waiting rooms and via leaflets and posters<br />

displayed in surgeries and community centres. Some participants also responded to adverts<br />

placed in <strong>the</strong> press, online or in local areas (such as community notice boards).<br />

In <strong>the</strong> South West, all patients that were identified via READ codes were sent <strong>the</strong> standardised<br />

pack including invitation letter, information sheet, consent form, fur<strong>the</strong>r studies form and<br />

freepost envelope. Participants were asked in <strong>the</strong> letter to consider taking part, and if <strong>the</strong>y were<br />

willing, to complete all forms and measures and return in <strong>the</strong> freepost envelope.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> Midlands, researchers were present in GPs surgeries and all attendees during a given<br />

time period were given an information sheet and asked to consider taking part. If <strong>the</strong>y were<br />

interested and eligible, <strong>the</strong> researcher explained <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong> study and answered any<br />

questions. Once informed consent was obtained, participants were given a questionnaire<br />

booklet ei<strong>the</strong>r to complete in <strong>the</strong> surgery or to be taken away and sent back via freepost<br />

envelope.<br />

Those participants who volunteered as a result of advertising were sent packs via post (using<br />

<strong>the</strong> same method as in <strong>the</strong> South West GP recruitment) and were given <strong>the</strong> opportunity to<br />

speak to a researcher via email or telephone. Those recruited from adverts or websites, and<br />

who had with no named health professional contact received <strong>the</strong> same pack as all o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

participants, with <strong>the</strong> addition of a section on <strong>the</strong> consent form to provide us with a GP contact.<br />

This contact was informed of <strong>the</strong>se participants engagement in <strong>the</strong> project.<br />

In all cases, participants took part on a voluntary basis and were not remunerated for <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

participation. Once received, data were stored securely (see below), receipts were logged and<br />

thank you letters were sent to each participant.<br />

(For full details of protocols, see Operating Manual in appendix 7)<br />

Data handling, entry and storage:<br />

<strong>The</strong> data were entered by <strong>the</strong> four researchers at each of <strong>the</strong> research sites using a common<br />

database architecture designed by <strong>the</strong> project statistician and researchers from <strong>the</strong> Bristol team.<br />

<strong>The</strong> data were subsequently merged and held by <strong>the</strong> Bristol team. All data were entered and<br />

stored in a way in which participants' identities were protected, and in full compliance with <strong>the</strong><br />

Data Protection Act. No identifiable information was used o<strong>the</strong>r than a participant code. A key<br />

list of participant names and identification codes was kept separately and securely at <strong>the</strong> Bristol<br />

site. It was considered necessary to retain this information to enable sub-samples of participants<br />

to be invited to participate in <strong>the</strong> follow-on studies. This key and any o<strong>the</strong>r identifiable data from<br />

<strong>the</strong> follow-on studies will be destroyed at <strong>the</strong> completion of <strong>the</strong> analysis process.<br />

33


RESULTS<br />

Due to <strong>the</strong> size of <strong>the</strong> data set and <strong>the</strong> number of variables, it would be unwieldy to include all<br />

analyses in this <strong>report</strong>. We have <strong>the</strong>refore chosen to present <strong>the</strong> ‘highlights’ in this section. We<br />

have also been selective in our choice of illustrative tables and figures, frequently including most<br />

relevant to <strong>the</strong> DAS 24 as this is <strong>the</strong> only standardised measure developed specifically to tap<br />

distress associated with disfigurement. To facilitate <strong>the</strong> reader, <strong>the</strong> results and analyses are<br />

presented in <strong>the</strong> following order:<br />

1. Participants<br />

2. Missing Data<br />

3. Internal Consistency<br />

4. <strong>The</strong> Relationship Between Variables<br />

5. Demographic Factors<br />

6. Visibility and Areas of Concern<br />

7. Dispositional Factors<br />

8. Socio-Cognitive Factors<br />

9. Appearance-specific Cognitive Factors<br />

10. Regressions by Outcome Variable<br />

11. Clinic specific data<br />

1. Participants<br />

A total of 1265 participants took part in <strong>the</strong> study, 615 of which were from <strong>the</strong> community and<br />

650 from out-patient clinics.<br />

867 of <strong>the</strong> whole sample were female (68.5%), 354 male (28.0%). 474 of those in <strong>the</strong> community<br />

sample were female (77.2%), 120 were male (19.5%). Similarly, 393 of those in <strong>the</strong> clinic sample<br />

were female (60.4%), 234 were male (35.9%).<br />

<strong>The</strong> mean age of <strong>the</strong> whole sample was 47.3 years (range 18-91, SD 16.7 years) with <strong>the</strong> mean<br />

age in <strong>the</strong> community sample 44.9 years (range 18 – 91; SD 16.2 years), marginally lower than<br />

in <strong>the</strong> clinic mean age 49.7 years ( range 18 – 89; SD 16.9 years).<br />

783 (61.9%) of <strong>the</strong> whole sample <strong>report</strong>ed being married or living with partner, 183 (14.6%)<br />

living with friends or relatives and 287 (22.9%) living alone.<br />

81% of <strong>the</strong> whole sample were white, with <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r 12% ei<strong>the</strong>r Pakistani, Indian, Black<br />

Caribbean, Black African or o<strong>the</strong>r, 7% did not state <strong>the</strong>ir ethnicity. <strong>The</strong> percentages are similar in<br />

both <strong>the</strong> clinic and community sample.<br />

34


Table 1.1 Sample means and standard deviations for outcome measures according to method of<br />

recruitment<br />

Clinic<br />

Community<br />

M(SD)<br />

M(SD)<br />

DAS24 41.26(16.38) 43.44(15.97)<br />

Anxiety 7.96(4.74) 8.46(4.50)<br />

Depression 5.00(3.76) 5.21(3.77)<br />

Aggression 25.15(9.35) 25.59(9.37)<br />

Negative Affect 31.88(8.99) 31.27(8.70)<br />

Positive Affect 19.71(8.61) 20.55(8.60)<br />

Table 1.2 Sample means and standard deviations for outcome measures according to geographical area<br />

of recruitment<br />

N DAS Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Negative<br />

M(S.D) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) Affect Affect<br />

M(SD) M(SD)<br />

London (UCL) 228 44.25(17.09) 8.41(4.63) 5.19(3.86) 25.36(8.96) 31.76(8.81) 20.99(8.64)<br />

Warwick 357 43.93(16.35) 8.93(4.61) 5.64(3.83) 26.38(9.43) 30.53(8.76) 21.52(9.01)<br />

Bristol 396 41.27(15.71) 7.91(4.48) 4.58(3.53) 24.07(9.06) 32.29(8.72) 19.08(8.09)<br />

Sheffield/<br />

Bradford<br />

280 40.16(15.68) 7.52(4.75) 5.08(3.86) 25.89(9.85) 31.79(9.08) 19.05(8.46)<br />

Figure 4.1(a) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for DAS24 by geographical location.<br />

35


Figure 4.1(b) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for HADS Anxiety by geographical location<br />

Figure 4.1(c) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for HADS Depression by geographical location<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were no statistically significant main effects for method of recruitment or geographical<br />

location on any of <strong>the</strong> outcome measures (p > .001). As a consequence <strong>the</strong> decision was taken<br />

to consider <strong>the</strong> data set to be a single group. Data will <strong>the</strong>refore be presented for each outcome<br />

variable separately.<br />

36


2. Missing Data<br />

Any participant for whom more than 50% of <strong>the</strong> items were missing in any scale, or those who<br />

had completed less than 50% of <strong>the</strong> total scale package were excluded. In practice this meant<br />

excluding 9 from <strong>the</strong> community sample. Missing data ranged from 0% for ethnicity to 14.1% for<br />

number of areas of concern. This, <strong>the</strong> missing data is only a small fractional part of <strong>the</strong><br />

database and missing values may be considered to be missing completely at random.<br />

3. Internal Consistency<br />

<strong>The</strong> values for Cronbach’s alpha for all outcome variables across <strong>the</strong> sample and subsets of <strong>the</strong><br />

sample according to gender and recruitment are all high, demonstrating good internal<br />

consistency on all measures ranging from 0.778-0.935.<br />

4. <strong>The</strong> Relationship between Variables<br />

In <strong>the</strong> following section, <strong>the</strong> relationships between outcome variables and between outcome and<br />

process variables in <strong>the</strong> model are presented.<br />

Outcome variables are mutually correlated (Table 4.1) and all correlations based on <strong>the</strong> entire<br />

sample are statistically significant correlations (p < 0.001, in all cases).<br />

Table 4.1 Pearson’s correlation matrix for outcome measures<br />

Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Affect Negative Affect<br />

DAS 24 .637 571 .382 -.398 .610<br />

Anxiety .637 .437 -.394 .700<br />

Depression .380 -.560 .554<br />

Aggression -.267 459<br />

Positive Affect -.408<br />

Table 4.2 summarises <strong>the</strong> degree of correlation between quantitative process and outcome<br />

variables for all strata of <strong>the</strong> population defined by gender. In all cases <strong>the</strong> correlations <strong>report</strong>ed<br />

are statistically significant correlations (p < .001), unless marked † to denote p > 0.05.<br />

37


Table 4.2 Correlation of quantitative variables with outcome variables<br />

DAS24 Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Negative N<br />

Affect Affect<br />

Optimism Female -.537 -.588 -.578 -.497 .473 -.531 832<br />

Male -.543 -.597 -.569 -.417 .471 -.504 331<br />

FNE Female .535 .521 .338 .380 -.340 .541 838<br />

Male .587 .581 .448 .309 -.351 .611 346<br />

Social acceptance Female -.613 -.460 -.510 -.280 .359 -.461 859<br />

Male -.543 -.452 -.512 -.232 .342 -.453 350<br />

Satisfaction with Social<br />

support<br />

Female -.432 -.369 -.455 -.324 .342 -.389 853<br />

Male -.472 -.407 -.484 -.212 .271 -.361 349<br />

Salience Female .473 .386 .207 .264 -.142 .336 857<br />

Male .444 .287 .227 .217 -.141 .275 333<br />

Valence Female .661 .422 .461 .290 -.402 .408 861<br />

Male .580 .416 .472 .205 -.350 .426 335<br />

Social comparison Female .305 .331 .160 .286 .151 .347 859<br />

Male .268 .255 .074† .184 .007† .236 350<br />

Appearance discrepancy Female .584 .334 .391 .203 -.265 .338 856<br />

Male .488 .336 .373 .122 -.241 .315 352<br />

5. Demographic Factors<br />

5.1 Gender<br />

Table 5.1(a) summarises <strong>the</strong> age and gender frequencies for <strong>the</strong> whole study sample, with table<br />

5.1(b) illustrating <strong>the</strong> means and standard deviations for <strong>the</strong> outcome variable by gender.<br />

Table 5.1(a) – Age and gender frequency for whole sample a<br />

Age Group<br />

Male Female Total<br />

N(%)<br />

18-25 35 97 137(11)<br />

26-35 42 169 216(18)<br />

36-45 60 170 235(19)<br />

46-55 61 158 226(18)<br />

56-65 79 138 225(18)<br />

> 65 71 125 199(16)<br />

Total N(%) 354(28) 867(68.5) 1221<br />

a 44 cases (3.5%). Missing data<br />

Table 5.1(b) Mean values for outcome variables by gender<br />

Variable Female Male N<br />

DAS24 44.95(16.18) 35.87(14.56)<br />

Anxiety 8.67(4.64) 6.97(4.37)<br />

Depression 5.16(3.78) 4.97(3.75)<br />

Aggression 25.07(9.42) 26.07(9.06)<br />

Positive Affect 31.02(8.92) 33.10(8.55)<br />

Negative Affect 20.70(8.82) 18.35(7.72)<br />

38


A graphical display of <strong>the</strong> distribution of DAS24, HADS Anxiety, HADS Depression and<br />

Aggression by gender is given in Figure 5.1.<br />

Figure 5.1: DAS24, HADS Anxiety, HADS Depression and Aggression by gender<br />

ANOVAs reveal that <strong>the</strong> scores for women are significantly higher (indicating more distress) on<br />

<strong>the</strong> DAS24 (F(1, 1213) = 71.51, MSE = 247.64, p < .001, ), higher on anxiety (F(1,<br />

1212) = 26.44, MSE = 20.79, p < .001, ) and negative affect (F(1, 1171) = 13.52,<br />

MSE = 72.53, p = .165, ). Conversely, males scored higher than females on positive<br />

affect (F(1, 1172) = 10.63, MSE = 77.80, p = .001, ). However, <strong>the</strong> effect sizes for all<br />

significant differences are judged to be small, ranging from 0.011-0.056 (Table 5.1(a)).<br />

39


Table 5.1(c) Frequency of number of areas <strong>report</strong>ed as sensitive according to gender.<br />

None<br />

Marked<br />

One Two Three Four Five 6 or more Total<br />

Female N 95 201 153 134 90 73 121 867<br />

% 11.0 23.2 17.6 15.5 10.4 8.4 14.0 100<br />

Male N 77 113 58 33 24 11 38 354<br />

% 21.8 31.9 16.4 9.3 6.8 3.1 10.7 100<br />

In relation to <strong>the</strong> number of areas <strong>report</strong>ed as sensitive, <strong>the</strong> application of <strong>the</strong> chi-square test of<br />

association indicates that <strong>the</strong>re are statistically significant differences in <strong>the</strong> profiles between<br />

male and female ( = 51.21, df = 6, p < .001) with females <strong>report</strong>ing considerably more areas<br />

of concern and with a statistically significant linear component ( = 33.99, df = 1, p < .001).<br />

See Table 5.1(c) for frequencies.<br />

Table 5.1(d) Mean values for outcome measures by visibility to o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

Visible/non N DAS24 Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Negative<br />

visible<br />

Affect Affect<br />

Female Yes 404 46.67(15.45) 9.26(4.70) 5.55(3.82) 26.03(9.42) 30.52(8.82) 21.73(9.05)<br />

No 460 43.44(16.67) 8.15(4.52) 4.81(3.73) 24.21(9.35) 31.46(8.99) 19.80(8.52)<br />

Male Yes 165 38.32(15.03) 7.62(4.35) 5.43(3.93) 26.52(9.32) 31.92(9.06) 19.66(8.02)<br />

No 188 33.72(13.82) 6.40(4.33) 4.56(3.54) 25.68(8.82) 34.16(7.93) 17.18(7.26)<br />

* 48 = missing data<br />

Two-way ANOVAs show that females <strong>report</strong> significantly poorer mean values than males on<br />

DAS24, HADS Anxiety, Positive Affect and Negative Affect (p < .001, in all cases) and those<br />

with a visible concern <strong>report</strong> significantly poorer mean values on DAS24, HADS Anxiety, HADS<br />

Depression, Positive Affect and Negative Affect (p < .001, all cases) and on Aggression (p =<br />

.024)<br />

5.2 Age<br />

Table 5.2(a) summarises <strong>the</strong> extent of <strong>the</strong> correlation between age and each outcome measure<br />

according to gender. In all cases <strong>the</strong> correlations <strong>report</strong>ed are statistically significant correlations<br />

(p < .001).<br />

Table 5.2(a) Correlation of outcome variables with age<br />

DAS24 Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Affect Negative Affect N<br />

Female -.325 -.209 -.068 -.321 .121 -.272 865<br />

Male -.393 -.210 -.124 -.277 .000 -.292 351<br />

40


Figure 5.2 DAS 24 against age<br />

Significant negative correlations were found between age and DAS24, anxiety, depression,<br />

aggression and negative affect for <strong>the</strong> whole sample.<br />

6. Visibility and Areas of Concern<br />

6.1 Area of concern<br />

Participants were asked to indicate all areas of <strong>the</strong> body about which <strong>the</strong>y had concern (see<br />

appendix 8). Some participants <strong>report</strong>ed only one area and o<strong>the</strong>rs recorded multiple areas of<br />

concern. <strong>The</strong> frequency with which participants <strong>report</strong>ed each area and percentages of<br />

participants <strong>report</strong>ing each area are presented in Table 6.1(a). Sixty five percent had concerns<br />

about an area normally visible to o<strong>the</strong>rs (including forehead, ears, eyes, nose, mouth, hands or<br />

cheeks). Some participants indicated more than one area of <strong>the</strong> body and accordingly <strong>the</strong><br />

frequency does not equal <strong>the</strong> overall sample size.<br />

41


Table 6.1(a) Frequency and percentage <strong>report</strong>ing area of concern<br />

Visible Area N % Non visible Area N %<br />

Eyes 193 15.3 Abdomen 341 27.0<br />

Nose 172 13.6 Thighs 281 22.2<br />

Cheeks 138 10.9 Breasts 247 19.5<br />

Mouth 133 10.5 Lower Legs 236 18.7<br />

Hands 156 12.3 Upper Arms 213 16.8<br />

Forehead 105 8.3 Buttocks 176 13.9<br />

Ears 81 6.4 Feet 175 13.8<br />

Neck 143 11.3<br />

Knees 139 11.0<br />

Hips 125 9.9<br />

Scalp 115 9.1<br />

Back 114 9.0<br />

Forearms 112 8.9<br />

Chest 110 8.7<br />

Shoulder 72 5.7<br />

Genitalia 60 4.7<br />

6.2 Visibility of main area of concern<br />

Using <strong>the</strong> one area participants indicated as being <strong>the</strong>ir main concern only, 590 of <strong>the</strong><br />

participants were classed for subsequent analyses as having an area of concern normally<br />

visible to o<strong>the</strong>rs and <strong>the</strong> remaining 671 classed as having concerns less often visible to o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

<strong>The</strong> means and standard deviations for scores on <strong>the</strong> outcome measures are presented for<br />

those with ‘visible’ and ‘non visible’ areas of concern in Table 6.2(a).<br />

Table 6.2(a) Means and standard deviations for visible and non visible areas on outcome measures<br />

Visible N DAS24 Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Negative<br />

Affect Affect<br />

Yes 590 44.24(15.66) 8.79(4.66) 5.48(3.83) 26.20(9.42) 30.91(8.91) 21.14(8.78)<br />

No 671 40.63(16.51) 7.68(4.55) 4.76(3.68) 24.62(9.25) 32.17(8.75) 19.22(8.36)<br />

Mean levels on DAS 24 are significantly higher on average amongst those with a visible<br />

concern (F(1, 1209) = 20.598, p < .001), but <strong>the</strong> effect is small (partial eta squared = 0.017).<br />

Mean HADS Anxiety is significantly higher amongst those with a visible concern than those<br />

without (p < .001), as is mean depression (p < .001), mean aggression (p = .014), and mean<br />

negative affect (p < .001). Mean positive affect is significantly lower amongst those with a<br />

visible concern (p = 0.013). (Table 6.2(a)).<br />

6.3 Numbers of area of concern<br />

Table 6.3(a) <strong>report</strong>s <strong>the</strong> percentage of participants <strong>report</strong>ing one or multiple areas of concern.<br />

Means and standard deviations by outcome variable are shown in table 6.3(b), and <strong>the</strong> means<br />

and 95% confidence intervals for <strong>the</strong> DAS are illustrated in Figure 6.1.<br />

42


Table 6.3(a) Frequency of number of areas <strong>report</strong>ed as sensitive according to gender.<br />

None marked One Two Three Four Five 6 or more Total<br />

N 172 314 211 167 114 84 159 1221<br />

% 14.1 25.7 17.3 13.7 9.3 6.9 13.0 100<br />

Table 6.3(b) Mean and standard deviations for number of areas of concern by outcome variable<br />

DAS Total<br />

Total Anxiety<br />

Score<br />

Total Depression<br />

Score Aggression Total Positive affect Negative affect<br />

None 32.71 (14.44) 6.42 (4.58) 4.09 (3.60) 23.08 (8.84) 33.64 (8.58) 16.88 (7.10)<br />

One 37.24 (13.94) 7.24 (4.60) 4.29 (3.38) 23.38 (8.65) 32.84 (8.00) 18.59 (7.96)<br />

Two 39.71 (14.32) 7.80 (4.36) 4.89 (3.42) 25.32 (9.31) 31.53 (9.39) 19.66 (8.13)<br />

Three 43.64 (14.19) 8.71 (4.24) 5.42 (3.83) 26.25 (9.22) 31.74 (8.53) 20.10 (8.49)<br />

Four 47.62 (15.48) 8.84 (4.30) 5.21 (3.51) 26.93 (9.99) 31.34 (9.21) 20.12 (8.16)<br />

Five 51.95 (14.77) 9.76 (4.06) 6.44 (4.24) 26.86 (9.57) 29.75 (9.55) 23.48 (8.95)<br />

Six or more 56.21 (14.95) 10.82 (4.55) 7.01 (4.08) 28.99 (9.47) 27.95 (8.52) 25.43 (9.38)<br />

Total 42.32 (16.21) 8.20 (4.63) 5.10 (3.77) 25.36 (9.36) 31.58 (8.85) 20.12 (8.61)<br />

Figure 6.1 95% confidence interval for mean DAS according to number of sensitive areas <strong>report</strong>ed.<br />

In all cases, a one-way analysis of variance for trends shows that <strong>the</strong>re is a statistically<br />

significant trend with increasing levels of poor adjustment occurring on average with increasing<br />

number of sensitive areas endorsed (Table 6.3(b)).<br />

7. Dispositional Factors<br />

Higher scores on <strong>the</strong> LOT-R indicate greater levels of optimism. <strong>The</strong>re is an overall sample<br />

mean of 13.69 and standard deviation of 3.37 (n=1250).<br />

43


Table 7.1 summarises <strong>the</strong> degree of correlation between optimism and each outcome variable.<br />

In all cases <strong>the</strong> correlations <strong>report</strong>ed are statistically significant (p < .001).<br />

Table 7.1 Correlation of optimism with outcome variables<br />

DAS24 Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Affect Negative Affect N<br />

Optimism -.535 -.592 -.568 -.459 .475 -.529 1205<br />

8. Socio-Cognitive Factors<br />

Table 8.1 <strong>report</strong>s <strong>the</strong> means and standard deviations on all socio-cognitive measures<br />

Table 8.1 Mean and standard deviations for all socio-cognitive factors<br />

M(SD) N<br />

FNE 36.63(10.59) 1199<br />

Social acceptance 11.35(2.83) 1254<br />

Satisfaction with Social support 19.75(4.61) 1251<br />

Table 8.2 summarises <strong>the</strong> degree of correlation between <strong>the</strong> socio-cognitive factors and each<br />

outcome variable. In all cases <strong>the</strong> correlations <strong>report</strong>ed are statistically significant (p < .001).<br />

Table 8.2 Correlation of socio-cognitive factors with outcome variables<br />

DAS24 Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Negative N<br />

Affect Affect<br />

FNE .576 .545 .351 .337 -.351 .563 1241<br />

Social acceptance -.585 -.453 -.500 -.257 .353 -.461 1251<br />

Satisfaction with Social<br />

Support<br />

-.442 -.386 -.460 -.291 .330 -.390 1251<br />

9. Appearance-specific Cognitive Factors<br />

Table 9.1 <strong>report</strong>s means and standard deviations on all appearance-related cognitive measures<br />

Table 9.1 Mean and standard deviations for all appearance cognitions<br />

M(SD) N<br />

Salience 31.44(8.08) 1252<br />

Valance 21.45(8.06) 1261<br />

Social comparison 35.73(7.27) 1255<br />

Appearance discrepancy 29.96(11.41) 1254<br />

Table 9.2 summarises <strong>the</strong> degree of correlation between <strong>the</strong> appearance cognitions and <strong>the</strong><br />

outcome variables. In all cases, <strong>the</strong> correlation <strong>report</strong>ed are statistically significant (p


Table 9.2 Correlation of <strong>the</strong> appearance cognitions and <strong>the</strong> outcome variables<br />

DAS24 Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Negative N<br />

Affect Affect<br />

Salience .495 .370 .197 .233 -.158 .331 1251<br />

Valence .648 .428 .448 .245 -.394 .422 1258<br />

Social comparison .322 .322 .130 .249 .122 .330 1253<br />

Appearance discrepancy .580 .349 .372 .166 -.268 .351 1251<br />

Subjective perceptions of disguisability and visibility when clo<strong>the</strong>d have been treated as<br />

categorical variables as <strong>the</strong>y are single item measures. As expected <strong>the</strong> degree of disguisability<br />

differs on average between those that have a visible concern and those that do not ( =<br />

103.64, df = 6, p


Figure 7.1(a): Scores on <strong>the</strong> DAS 24 and responses to <strong>the</strong> question “How difficult is it to disguise <strong>the</strong><br />

aspect of your appearance you are concerned about?”<br />

Mean levels of each outcome according to <strong>report</strong>ed level of visibility when clo<strong>the</strong>d are given in<br />

Table 9.4. Analysis, using an analysis of variance for a one-way between subjects design<br />

indicates that <strong>the</strong>re are statistically significant differences on all outcome measures for <strong>report</strong>ed<br />

levels of visible concern (p < 0.01, in all cases).<br />

Table 9.4 Mean outcome variables according to <strong>report</strong>ed level of visibility when clo<strong>the</strong>d<br />

DAS24 Anxiety Depression Aggression Positive Negative N<br />

Affect Affect<br />

1 Not at all<br />

visible<br />

36.81(14.24) 7.52(4.71) 4.36(3.58) 24.84(9.25) 33.58(8.03) 18.56(8.01) 270<br />

2 38.48(13.11) 7.22(3.95) 4.35(3.48) 24.02(9.13) 32.55(9.13) 18.93(7.26) 122<br />

3 43.22(16.40) 8.28(4.60) 4.93(3.75) 24.41(8.60) 31.46(8.60) 19.45(8.49) 69<br />

4<br />

44.11(16.64) 8.71(4.30) 5.22(3.53) 25.28(9.39) 30.64(8.33) 19.64(8.14) 196<br />

Moderately<br />

5 48.61(14.83) 9.23(4.44) 6.01(3.74) 28.62(9.76) 30.10(9.26) 22.13(9.12) 97<br />

6 45.90(16.41) 8.59(4.32) 5.82(3.66) 25.96(9.00) 29.69(8.40) 21.11(8.59) 109<br />

Impossible 45.54(16.50) 8.62(4.94) 5.65(4.03) 25.67(9.51) 31.27(9.42) 21.57(9.31) 353<br />

46


10. Regressions by Outcome Variable<br />

Hierarchical stepwise multiple regression was used to assess <strong>the</strong> ability of <strong>the</strong> measured<br />

demographic, visibility, dispositional, socio-cognitive and appearance-related socio-cognitive<br />

factors to predict levels of social anxiety (DAS), psychological wellbeing (PANAS & HADS) and<br />

aggression (RAQ). In <strong>the</strong> following each outcome variable is modeled using hierarchical linear<br />

regression. <strong>The</strong> predictors in block 1 are <strong>the</strong> biographic and demographic variables of age,<br />

gender, recruitment method and living arrangements. Objective visibility forms block 2. <strong>The</strong><br />

variable optimism forms block 3. <strong>The</strong> socio-cognitive factors fear of negative evaluation, social<br />

acceptance and satisfaction with social support form block 4. <strong>The</strong> appearance cognitions of<br />

salience, valence, social comparison, appearance discrepancy and subjective Visibility<br />

(disguisability and visibility when clo<strong>the</strong>d) form block 5. All variables are included in <strong>the</strong><br />

regression as <strong>the</strong> operational model has several outcomes. (See figure 8.1.1).<br />

BLOCK<br />

BLOCK<br />

1<br />

2<br />

Demographics Objective<br />

Visibility<br />

Age<br />

Gender<br />

Family Status<br />

Method of<br />

recruitment<br />

(clinic /<br />

community)<br />

Visible<br />

v<br />

Non<br />

visible<br />

BLOCK<br />

3<br />

Dispositional<br />

Style<br />

Optimism /<br />

pessimism<br />

BLOCK<br />

4<br />

Sociocognitive<br />

factors<br />

Satisfaction<br />

with social<br />

support<br />

Fear of<br />

negative<br />

evaluation<br />

Social<br />

acceptance<br />

BLOCK<br />

5<br />

Appearanc<br />

e<br />

cognitions<br />

Social<br />

comparison<br />

Salience<br />

Valence<br />

Appearance<br />

discrepancy<br />

Subjective<br />

(self <strong>report</strong><br />

disguisability<br />

and visibility<br />

when<br />

clo<strong>the</strong>d)<br />

Figure 8.1 Fur<strong>the</strong>r development of <strong>the</strong> model = hierarchical stepwise multiple regression<br />

Outcomes<br />

Social<br />

acceptance<br />

and<br />

avoidance<br />

Depression<br />

Anxiety<br />

Aggression<br />

Positive<br />

affect<br />

Negative<br />

affect<br />

In <strong>the</strong> model, gender, method of recruitment, living arrangements (with “Living Alone” and<br />

“Living with Friends” collapsed toge<strong>the</strong>r and “Living with Partner” as ano<strong>the</strong>r), objective visibility,<br />

disguisability (coded 0 if easy to disguise and 1 if an expression of difficulty has been made)<br />

and visibility when clo<strong>the</strong>d (coded 0 if not visible or mild; and 1 if moderate through to extreme)<br />

are entered as ‘dummy variables’ (a standard treatment of dichotomous variables in regression<br />

approaches).<br />

47


10.1 Appearance-related Social Anxiety and Avoidance<br />

When entering <strong>the</strong> variables as described previously, 66.2% of <strong>the</strong> observed sample variation in<br />

<strong>the</strong> DAS24 is accounted for by <strong>the</strong> regression model given in Table 9.1 and this effect is<br />

statistically significant, = .662, F(16, 1038) = 127.191, MSE = 85.91, p < .001.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> fitted model for DAS24 <strong>the</strong>re are no significant effects attributable to living arrangements,<br />

recruitment, or social comparison. <strong>The</strong>re are relatively small but statistically significant gender<br />

effects with females scoring 2.2 points higher on DAS 24 than males (p = .002).<br />

In <strong>the</strong> regression model <strong>the</strong>re are relatively strong and statistically significant negative<br />

relationships between age (beta = -0.144, p < .001), optimism (beta = -0.199, p < .001), and<br />

social acceptance (beta = -0.233, p < .001) with DAS24. Likewise <strong>the</strong>re are relatively strong<br />

and statistically significant positive relationships between FNE (beta = 0.131, p < .001), salience<br />

(beta = 0.123, p < .001), valence (beta = 0.240, p < .001), appearance discrepancy (beta =<br />

0.116, p < .001) and disguisability (beta = 0.123, p < .001).<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are smaller but statistically significant effects attributable to visibility of main area of<br />

concern and for visibility when clo<strong>the</strong>d; <strong>the</strong> direction of <strong>the</strong> effects is in <strong>the</strong> opposite direction to<br />

that indicated by <strong>the</strong> bivariate analyses. After allowing for all <strong>the</strong> above mentioned effects, those<br />

with a visible area of concern score lower on average on DAS24 than those without a visible<br />

concern by 2.7 points (beta = -0.053, p = .007) and those <strong>report</strong>ing high levels of visibility when<br />

clo<strong>the</strong>d score lower than those with relatively lower levels of self-<strong>report</strong>ed visibility (beta = -<br />

0.085, p < .001).<br />

48


Table 10.1 Regression Model for DAS24<br />

Variable Coef Std Beta t p VIF Incremental<br />

Error<br />

R 2<br />

P<br />

Constant 43.25 3.471 12.46


HADS Anxiety, and <strong>the</strong> relative magnitude of beta identifies <strong>the</strong>se four variables as being <strong>the</strong><br />

dominant effects.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are relatively small but statistically significant effects in <strong>the</strong> HADS Anxiety model for<br />

satisfaction with social support (beta = -0.061, p = .025), social comparison (beta = 0.063, p =<br />

.021) and visibility when clo<strong>the</strong>d (beta = -0.069, p = .011), with <strong>the</strong> effect in <strong>the</strong> direction contrary<br />

to <strong>the</strong> direction indicated by <strong>the</strong> bivariate analyses.<br />

Table 10.2 Regression Model for HADS Anxiety<br />

Variable Coef Std Beta t p VIF Incremental<br />

Error<br />

R 2<br />

P<br />

Constant 10.900 1.255 8.69


In this model, <strong>the</strong> variables method of recruitment, living arrangements, visibility of main area of<br />

concern, salience, appearance discrepancy, social comparison, disguisability and visibility when<br />

clo<strong>the</strong>d are not statistically significant predictors of HADS Depression. <strong>The</strong> variables optimism<br />

(beta = -0.345, p < .001), social acceptance (beta = -0.245, p < .001) and satisfaction with social<br />

support (beta = -0.183, p < .001) are all negatively associated with HADS Depression and<br />

valence (beta = 0.104, p = .003) positively associated with HADS Depression.<br />

To a lesser extent, <strong>the</strong>re is a statistically significant positive relationship between age and HADS<br />

Depression in <strong>the</strong> sample (beta = 0.060, p = .025) and FNE and HADS Depression (beta =<br />

0.066, p = .038). <strong>The</strong>re is a small gender effect (beta = -0.067, p = .006) with males scoring<br />

0.57 points higher on average than females on HADS Depression after adjusting for all o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

variables in <strong>the</strong> model.<br />

Table 10.3 Regression Model for HADS Depression<br />

Variable Coef Std Beta t p VIF Incremental P<br />

Error<br />

R2<br />

Constant 14.773 1.044 14.15


10.4 Aggression<br />

When entering <strong>the</strong> variables as described previously, 30.7% of <strong>the</strong> observed sample variation in<br />

aggression scores is accounted for by <strong>the</strong> regression model given in Table 10.4 and this effect<br />

is statistically significant, = .307, F(16, 1038) = 28.691, MSE = 61.794, p < .001.<br />

Table 10.4 gives <strong>the</strong> fitted model for aggression. In this model, <strong>the</strong> variables method of<br />

recruitment, living arrangements, visibility of main area of concern, FNE, social acceptance,<br />

salience, valence, appearance discrepancy, disguisability and visibility when clo<strong>the</strong>d are not<br />

statistically significant predictors of aggression. <strong>The</strong> regression model captures strong and<br />

statistically significant negative effects between aggression and optimism (beta = -0.358, p <<br />

.001) and age (beta = -0.218, p < .001). <strong>The</strong>re is an appreciable gender effect with males, on<br />

average, scoring 3.3 points higher than females on <strong>the</strong> aggression scale and this effect is<br />

statistically significant (beta = -0.158, p < .001).<br />

To a lesser extent, <strong>the</strong>re is a statistically significant negative relationship between satisfaction<br />

with social support and aggression (beta = -0.073, p = .018) and between social comparison and<br />

aggression (beta = 0.064, p = .038).<br />

52


Table 10.4 Regression Model for Aggression<br />

Variable Coef Std Beta t p VIF Incremental P<br />

Error<br />

R2<br />

Constant 43.656 2.946 14.82


elatively small but statistically significant positive association between positive affect and<br />

salience (beta = 0.080, p = .014) and between positive affect and social comparison (beta =<br />

0.076, p = .015).<br />

Table 10.5 Regression Model for Positive Affect<br />

Variable Coef Std Beta t p VIF Incremental P<br />

Error<br />

R2<br />

Constant 19.778 2.826 7.00


Table 10.6 gives <strong>the</strong> fitted model for negative affect. In this model, <strong>the</strong> effects for gender,<br />

method of recruitment, living arrangements, visibility of main area of concern, salience, valence,<br />

appearance discrepancy, disguisability and visibility when clo<strong>the</strong>d are not statistically significant<br />

effects. <strong>The</strong> regression model captures a strong and statistically significant positive association<br />

between positive affect and FNE (beta = 0.297, p < .001) and strong and statistically significant<br />

negative associations between negative affect and optimism (beta = -0.222, p < .001) and social<br />

acceptance (beta = -0.138, p < .001). <strong>The</strong>re are weaker, but statistically significant, negative<br />

associations between negative affect and age (beta = -0.068, p = .013) and satisfaction with<br />

social support (beta = -0.078, p = .005) and a positive association between negative affect and<br />

social comparison (beta = 0.067, p =.016).<br />

Table 10.6 Regression Model for Negative Affect<br />

Variable Coef Std Beta t p VIF Incremental P<br />

Error<br />

R2<br />

Constant 23.921 2.444 9.79


11. Clinic Specific Data<br />

In order to explore <strong>the</strong> potential impact of <strong>the</strong> aetiology of <strong>the</strong> disfiguring condition on<br />

adjustment, <strong>the</strong> data derived from out-patient clinics were examined. A summary of <strong>the</strong> outcome<br />

variables by clinic is given in Table 11.1. Box and whisker plots show <strong>the</strong> extent of individual<br />

variation within each out-patient clinic.<br />

Table 11.1 Mean and standard deviations for each outcome variable by clinic<br />

N DAS24<br />

M(SD)<br />

Anxiety<br />

M(SD)<br />

Depression<br />

M(SD)<br />

Aggression<br />

M(SD)<br />

Positive<br />

Affect<br />

M(SD)<br />

Negative<br />

Affect<br />

M(SD)<br />

Plastics 225 46.03(17.23) 8.88(4.65) 5.28(3.77) 26.59(8.88) 31.77(8.50) 21.26(8.84)<br />

Dermatology 89 43.80(14.31) 8.55(4.29) 5.64(4.16) 26.59(10.61) 30.52(8.91) 20.08(9.10)<br />

Laser 38 43.03(16.39) 8.39(4.82) 3.74(3.03) 24.63(9.48) 34.26(7.98) 20.76(8.36)<br />

Cleft Lip and<br />

Palate<br />

11 42.64(14.03) 10.36(6.05) 3.64(2.34) 32.09(11.32) 31.73(6.77) 25.09(8.69)<br />

Burns 18 42.22(17.66) 7.39(4.39) 5.39(4.73) 27.83(10.38) 31.06(8.79) 20.24(7.21)<br />

Ophthalmology 39 39.23(13.33) 7.89(4.89) 4.98(3.24) 22.52(8.55) 32.40(9.67) 19.26(9.11)<br />

Head and<br />

Neck cancer<br />

34 38.09(14.96) 7.24(4.95) 5.76(3.91) 23.97(9.55) 30.18(10.11) 18.64(9.45)<br />

Pros<strong>the</strong>tics 54 36.28(17.00) 6.19(5.08) 4.74(4.01) 25.70(10.00) 34.56(9.94) 17.37(7.70)<br />

Ocular<br />

Pros<strong>the</strong>tics<br />

30 35.00(12.53) 5.97(4.13) 3.37(3.51) 21.80(7.25) 34.69(8.81) 18.10(6.75)<br />

Rheumatoid<br />

Arthritis<br />

36 34.31(12.65) 6.94(3.77) 4.81(3.63) 22.94(8.31) 30.00(8.88) 17.88(6.94)<br />

ENT 17 33.06(17.08) 5.35(3.76) 5.88(3.52) 20.47(6.60) 31.56(8.88) 14.63(6.17)<br />

Melanoma 36 32.33(16.15) 7.25(5.22) 4.08(3.41) 21.64(7.99) 30.00(9.55) 16.81(7.80)<br />

Figure 9.1(a) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for DAS 24 scores by condition<br />

56


Figure 9.1(b) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for HADS Anxiety scores by condition<br />

Figure 9.1(c) Comparative box-and-whiskers plot for HADS depression scores by condition<br />

Table 11.3 shows <strong>the</strong> percentages of participants scoring low, mild, or moderate to high on<br />

HADS for anxiety and depression in <strong>the</strong> overall sample (n=1259).<br />

57


Table 11.3: Percentage of participants with low (0-7), mild (8-10), and moderate/high (11- 21) levels of<br />

anxiety and depression<br />

Anxiety Score Depression Score<br />

Category N 0-7 8-10 11-21 0-7 8-10 11-21<br />

All Participants 1259 48.2 22.2 29.5 76.4 13.4 10.2<br />

Table 11.4 shows <strong>the</strong> percentages of participants scoring low, mild, or moderate to high on<br />

HADS for anxiety and depression split by clinic (n=1259)<br />

Table 11.4 Percentage of participants with low (0-7), mild (8-10), and moderate/high (11- 21) levels of<br />

anxiety and depression by clinic category.<br />

Anxiety Score Depression Score<br />

Clinic Category N 0-7 8-10 11-21 0-7 8-10 11-21<br />

Plastics 226 42.0 25.7 32.3 75.2 13.7 11.1<br />

Dermatology 88 50.0 18.2 31.8 71.6 11.4 17.0<br />

Laser 38 36.8 31.6 31.6 92.1 2.6 5.3<br />

Cleft lip and palate 11 36.4 0.0 63.6 90.9 9.1 0.0<br />

Burns 18 50.0 33.3 16.7 72.2 11.1 16.7<br />

Ophthalmology 61 54.1 16.4 29.5 90.9 9.1 0.0<br />

Head and Neck Cancer 34 50.0 26.5 23.5 67.6 14.7 17.6<br />

Pros<strong>the</strong>tics 54 64.8 16.7 18.5 79.6 9.3 11.1<br />

Ocular Pros<strong>the</strong>tics 30 80.0 6.7 13.3 86.7 10.0 3.3<br />

Rheumatoid Arthritis 36 63.9 22.2 13.9 75.0 13.9 11.1<br />

ENT 17 82.4 5.9 11.8 70.6 23.5 5.9<br />

Melanoma 36 63.9 8.3 27.8 83.3 11.1 5.6<br />

58


DISCUSSION<br />

This <strong>report</strong> contains <strong>the</strong> first wave of analysis of <strong>the</strong> data from <strong>the</strong> research programme. Due to<br />

<strong>the</strong> size of <strong>the</strong> data set (in excess of 300,000 data points) for Study 1, and <strong>the</strong> number of followon<br />

studies, <strong>the</strong> analysis is necessarily a ‘work in progress’. <strong>The</strong> next phase of analysis will<br />

continue over <strong>the</strong> next six months, and will be included in later publications. In <strong>the</strong> discussion<br />

which follows, <strong>the</strong> commentary is organised in relation to <strong>the</strong> model of adjustment presented in<br />

Figure 10 below. This model illustrates <strong>the</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>r developments made to <strong>the</strong> frameworks<br />

presented in <strong>the</strong> introduction (Figures 1 & 3) in <strong>the</strong> light of <strong>the</strong> results and analyses outlined in<br />

<strong>the</strong> previous section.<br />

Demographics Visibility Dispositional<br />

Style<br />

Age<br />

Gender<br />

Family status<br />

Group (clinic/<br />

community)<br />

Visible<br />

v<br />

Non Visible<br />

Optimism /<br />

Pessimism<br />

Figure 10: Fur<strong>the</strong>r Development of Model of Adjustment<br />

Sociocognitive<br />

factors<br />

Satisfaction<br />

with social<br />

support<br />

Fear of<br />

Negative<br />

Evaluation<br />

Social<br />

Acceptance<br />

Appearance<br />

cognitions<br />

Social<br />

comparison<br />

Salience<br />

Valence<br />

Appearance<br />

discrepancy<br />

Subjective<br />

visibility (self<br />

<strong>report</strong><br />

disguise,<br />

noticeability)<br />

Outcomes<br />

Social Anxiety<br />

& Avoidance<br />

Depression<br />

Anxiety<br />

Aggression<br />

Mood<br />

One of <strong>the</strong> most notable achievements of this first study in <strong>the</strong> research programme has been to<br />

achieve and exceed our target recruitment figure of 1000, delivering a substantial body of data<br />

relating to adjustment to appearance concerns from a sample of 1265 participants. To our<br />

knowledge, this is <strong>the</strong> largest scale study in <strong>the</strong> field to date. A particularly noteworthy addition<br />

to previous understanding is <strong>the</strong> data derived from <strong>the</strong> community sample. Levels of distress in<br />

this population are considerable, and comparable to participants recruited from hospital settings.<br />

<strong>The</strong> homogeneity of <strong>the</strong> two data sets resulted in <strong>the</strong> majority of <strong>the</strong>se initial analyses being<br />

performed on <strong>the</strong> complete data set, in contrast to our assumption before embarking on this<br />

research, that we would be comparing and contrasting <strong>the</strong> data from <strong>the</strong> community and<br />

hospital based samples on a routine basis. Clearly previous research, which has focused on <strong>the</strong><br />

problems and difficulties of those actively seeking treatment has revealed only <strong>the</strong> tip of <strong>the</strong><br />

iceberg of appearance-related distress amongst those with disfiguring conditions.<br />

Mean levels of depression were low in <strong>the</strong> sample, echoing previous research (Moss, Harris &<br />

Carr, 2004; Newell, 2000b; Rumsey & Harcourt, 2005; Rumsey et al, 2004). <strong>The</strong> mean level for<br />

59


anxiety was in <strong>the</strong> low to mild range, <strong>the</strong> level for positive affect was quite high, and negative<br />

affect quite low compared with normative values. <strong>The</strong> mean level for aggression fell in <strong>the</strong> low to<br />

moderate range. However, <strong>the</strong> variance in <strong>the</strong> sample was high, and substantial numbers of<br />

participants showed significant levels of distress.<br />

<strong>The</strong> data from clinic attendees was examined for any notable patterns of adjustment or distress.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are many difficulties inherent in classifying <strong>the</strong> various conditions which result in<br />

disfigurement (for example, patients attending a dermatology or plastics clinic will have<br />

disfigurements resulting from a large number of different aetiologies). Accordingly our analysis<br />

was limited to differences in <strong>the</strong> type of clinic attended. <strong>The</strong> overall impression resulting from<br />

this analysis was <strong>the</strong> similarity in profiles from <strong>the</strong> various clinics – once again adding to <strong>the</strong><br />

impression that individual psychological factors and processes were a bigger determinant of<br />

adjustment. Taken on its own however, <strong>the</strong> lack of differences between clinics in this data set<br />

should be treated with caution. Despite minimum recruitment targets for every clinic, <strong>the</strong><br />

challenges of recruitment and <strong>the</strong> resulting sample sizes varied considerably. <strong>The</strong> profile for<br />

participants with a cleft of <strong>the</strong> lip and/or palate is one of greater distress for negative affect,<br />

aggression and anxiety than for participants from o<strong>the</strong>r clinics. However, <strong>the</strong> sample is also <strong>the</strong><br />

smallest (n=11), reflecting <strong>the</strong> low preponderance of patients of 18 years and over attending this<br />

particular out-patient clinic. In addition, adjustment profiles may vary as a result of <strong>the</strong> age<br />

profiles of attendees (for example, <strong>the</strong> preponderance of older adults attending melanoma and<br />

Rheumatoid Arthritis clinics).<br />

Demographic variables<br />

<strong>The</strong> mean age of participants was 47 years, with a range of 18-91 years. <strong>The</strong> respondents in<br />

this study represent a sample with an older profile than in <strong>the</strong> majority of previous research,<br />

offering a useful insight into <strong>the</strong> appearance concerns in middle and older adulthood. <strong>The</strong>re<br />

were negative correlations between increasing age and scores for social anxiety and social<br />

avoidance, aggression, negative affect, depression and anxiety. So, bearing in mind <strong>the</strong><br />

limitations of a cross-sectional sample, <strong>the</strong> picture in this study is one of positive adjustment<br />

increasing with age. However <strong>the</strong> relationships are not dramatic and many older participants<br />

were concerned and distressed about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance. <strong>The</strong>se results are consistent with<br />

previous research, and should not be taken as indicating that appearance concerns are <strong>the</strong><br />

preserve only of <strong>the</strong> young.<br />

<strong>The</strong> importance of appearance in late adolescence and early adulthood has been highlighted by<br />

many (Harris & Carr 2001; Liossi 2003; Lovegrove & Rumsey, 2005), and Cash et al’s (1986)<br />

survey of 15-74 years found that body image concerns diminished with age. However, Rosser<br />

(2008) has also pointed out that <strong>the</strong> role of age in appearance adjustment is not straightforward,<br />

and Lansdown, Lloyd and Hunter (1991) pointed to <strong>the</strong> crucial role of appearance concerns<br />

throughout <strong>the</strong> lifespan. Rumsey and Harcourt (2005) <strong>report</strong>ed data from older people (over 60<br />

years) with skin conditions which indicated that although fewer people in this age group<br />

expressed significant concerns, a sizable proportion were distressed about aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance. Ra<strong>the</strong>r than a reduction in levels of concern compared with younger people, <strong>the</strong><br />

60


focus and nature of <strong>the</strong> appearance concerns appeared to be different. <strong>The</strong> stereotypes of<br />

appearance considered desirable earlier in life were less important, but aspects of self-<br />

presentation, such as appearing smart and fit, remained important.<br />

Several commentators have noted <strong>the</strong> bias in previous research towards female participants<br />

and have highlighted <strong>the</strong> need to explore <strong>the</strong> concerns of men in more detail (Rumsey &<br />

Harcourt, 2005). Although 68.5% of <strong>the</strong> participants in this study were female, we still managed<br />

to recruit a sizeable sample of men (n=354). A higher proportion of men were recruited from<br />

outpatient clinics than from community settings. It may be that women are more helpful, more<br />

interested in sharing <strong>the</strong>ir experiences, or perhaps that <strong>the</strong> GP practices used as a basis for<br />

large segments of <strong>the</strong> community recruitment had more women attending surgeries, and/or who<br />

have attended in <strong>the</strong> past (leading to <strong>the</strong>ir inclusion in <strong>the</strong> READ code classifications used as<br />

<strong>the</strong> basis for mailshots).<br />

Whilst noting <strong>the</strong> lack of research with males, previous studies (including people with and<br />

without visible disfigurement) have in <strong>the</strong> main suggested that women are more prone to having<br />

significant levels of appearance concerns and body dissatisfaction than men (e.g. Cash et al,<br />

2004; Harris & Carr, 2001). This has been attributed to <strong>the</strong> impact of societal norms which infer<br />

that <strong>the</strong> importance of appearance is more specific to women than to men (Rosser 2008), and<br />

greater familial concern with <strong>the</strong> appearance of daughters than sons (Schwartz et al, 1999).<br />

However, in relation to body image concerns in <strong>the</strong> general population, Cash & Pruzinsky<br />

(2002) have commented that both genders experience distress and White (2008) <strong>report</strong>ed that<br />

teenage boys are more distressed about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance than girls. In <strong>the</strong> current study, levels<br />

of social anxiety and social avoidance, anxiety and negative affect were higher for women, but<br />

perhaps <strong>the</strong> most notable finding is that <strong>the</strong> effect sizes were small. Women were also more<br />

likely to have multiple areas of concern than men. <strong>The</strong>re were no significant differences in<br />

relation to depression, however men had a higher mean score on aggression. Levels of positive<br />

affect were higher for women. Newell (2000a) concluded that research findings examining <strong>the</strong><br />

role of gender in adjustment to visible differences are contradictory, and also found varying<br />

results in two different samples. Women in a sample showing less distress <strong>report</strong>ed greater<br />

difficulty than men (Newell, 2000b), but <strong>the</strong>re were no differences between men and women in a<br />

more distressed sample (Newell, 1998), suggesting that differences according to gender may be<br />

confined to less distressed individuals. Our findings endorse <strong>the</strong> suggestion that both males and<br />

females should be included in fur<strong>the</strong>r research on appearance-related adjustment and distress,<br />

and in efforts to meet <strong>the</strong> needs of those adversely affected.<br />

Eighty one percent of <strong>the</strong> sample classified <strong>the</strong>mselves as white, with similar profiles from both<br />

<strong>the</strong> clinic and community recruitment settings. Although <strong>the</strong> sample includes participants from a<br />

greater mix of ethnic backgrounds than much of <strong>the</strong> research hi<strong>the</strong>rto, numbers are<br />

proportionately low, and <strong>the</strong> collaboration felt it appropriate to study <strong>the</strong> effects of cultural<br />

background in more detail in two of <strong>the</strong> follow-on studies (Studies 5 & 6).<br />

61


Visibility and areas of concern<br />

<strong>The</strong> findings relating to <strong>the</strong> visibility or o<strong>the</strong>rwise of a disfigurement to o<strong>the</strong>rs are mixed.<br />

Approximately 65% indicated that <strong>the</strong>ir main concern related to <strong>the</strong> appearance of <strong>the</strong>ir face or<br />

head, and 12.3% indicated <strong>the</strong>ir hands, mirroring earlier research about <strong>the</strong> greater impact of<br />

visible disfigurements, compared with those which are more easily camouflaged or covered up<br />

(see for example, Kent & Keohane, 2001). In addition, increasing levels of distress were<br />

associated with perceptions of increasing difficulty in being able to disguise <strong>the</strong> aspect of<br />

appearance in question. However, in <strong>the</strong> regression analyses, a lack of visibility to o<strong>the</strong>rs in<br />

everyday situations was associated with greater distress (higher levels of depression, anxiety,<br />

aggression and negative affect, lower positive affect and higher levels of fear of negative<br />

evaluation by o<strong>the</strong>rs). It should be noted, however, that <strong>the</strong> effect sizes were small, and o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

appearance-related cognitions had a much greater impact on adjustment. This mixed picture of<br />

<strong>the</strong> effects of <strong>the</strong> visibility or o<strong>the</strong>rwise to o<strong>the</strong>rs may have resulted from shortcomings in our<br />

measures. However, Moss (2005) and Rosser (2008) have also highlighted <strong>the</strong> complex nature<br />

of <strong>the</strong> relationship between visibility and adjustment. Rosser (2008) noted <strong>the</strong> likelihood of an<br />

effect of ease of concealment and also that <strong>the</strong> personal salience and valence attached to a<br />

particular body part may fur<strong>the</strong>r complicate matters. Major & Granzow (1999) and Smart &<br />

Wegner (1999), raised <strong>the</strong> possibility of <strong>the</strong> ‘Preoccupational Model of Secrecy’ which discusses<br />

<strong>the</strong> possible negative impact of concealment associated with <strong>the</strong> threat of <strong>the</strong> stigma being<br />

discovered and/or fear of revealing <strong>the</strong> stigma. Those with a difference which is routinely visible<br />

to o<strong>the</strong>rs may be forced to develop strategies to deal with <strong>the</strong> reactions of o<strong>the</strong>rs (for example,<br />

using camouflage, or developing ‘stock’ answers to common questions), and may perceive this<br />

to be effective. Avoidance of <strong>the</strong> need to develop <strong>the</strong>se strategies may increase fears of<br />

negative evaluation by o<strong>the</strong>rs and may feed insecurity in more intimate situations when <strong>the</strong><br />

disfigurement will become visible. This interpretation is also supported by recent data derived<br />

from patients seeking plastic surgery (Clarke, Hansen, White and Butler, in preparation). Fur<strong>the</strong>r<br />

research is warranted in this area.<br />

Previous research has consistently shown a lack of linear relationship between severity and<br />

concern/distress. In <strong>the</strong> light of <strong>the</strong>se findings, and as <strong>the</strong> focus of this research related to<br />

subjective psychological factors and processes, <strong>the</strong>re was no objective assessment of <strong>the</strong><br />

severity of <strong>the</strong> participants’ disfigurement in this study. Various explanations for <strong>the</strong> lack of<br />

relationship between severity and adjustment/distress have been put forward. Lansdown et al<br />

(1997) suggested that those with ‘minor’ differences have to face a variability in responses that<br />

inhibits <strong>the</strong> ability to predict with any certainty, and this can act as a deterrent to developing self<br />

efficacy in responding effectively. For those with more severe (and visible to o<strong>the</strong>rs)<br />

disfigurements, <strong>the</strong>re is a more consistent reaction (and most react), thus prediction is relatively<br />

easy. Moss (2004) illustrated <strong>the</strong> complexities of <strong>the</strong> severity and <strong>the</strong> disguisability of<br />

disfigurement and adjustment in <strong>report</strong>ing a U shaped parabola in a sample of 400 people with<br />

a variety of disfigurements for visible disfigurements, with no such relationship for those able to<br />

routinely conceal <strong>the</strong>ir difference. Moss refined Lansdown’s explanation in speculating that both<br />

minor and severe disfigurements produce consistent and more easily predictable responses<br />

from o<strong>the</strong>rs, with those in <strong>the</strong> mid range provoking more variety.<br />

62


Several studies, however, have highlighted <strong>the</strong> importance of subjective assessments of<br />

severity. Moss (2004) and Rumsey et al (2004) found a linear relationship between subjectively<br />

assessed severity of appearance and adjustment, highlighting <strong>the</strong> importance of personal<br />

evaluation of a problem. Our results showed a striking relationship between increasing<br />

numbers of areas of concern and growing levels of distress, particularly in relation to social<br />

anxiety and social avoidance around appearance (DAS24). Participants in <strong>the</strong> community were<br />

more likely to <strong>report</strong> multiple areas of concern than those recruited in hospital settings, and<br />

women had greater numbers of areas of concern than men.<br />

Only 25.7% of <strong>the</strong> sample <strong>report</strong>ed being concerned about only one area. It is important to note<br />

that people with a disfiguring condition affecting one body part may also have significant<br />

appearance concerns about o<strong>the</strong>r aspects of appearance. Twenty seven percent <strong>report</strong>ed<br />

concern about <strong>the</strong>ir abdomens, 22% in relation to <strong>the</strong> appearance of <strong>the</strong>ir thighs & 14% re<br />

buttocks. <strong>The</strong>se results may reflect concerns about weight, and it is possible that <strong>the</strong>se worries<br />

may over-ride issues about a disfiguring condition. Weight related concerns were greater in <strong>the</strong><br />

community sample. <strong>The</strong> prevalence of weight concerns in <strong>the</strong> general population comes as no<br />

surprise in <strong>the</strong> current context of rising levels of obesity. However, more research is needed to<br />

unpick <strong>the</strong> similarities and differences in <strong>the</strong> different types of concern.<br />

<strong>The</strong> multivariate nature of adjustment<br />

<strong>The</strong> measures chosen as <strong>the</strong> outcomes in our conceptual framework were significantly<br />

correlated, supporting <strong>the</strong> view that adjustment is multi-factorial (see for example, Rumsey et al,<br />

2004; Rumsey and Harcourt, 2005). <strong>The</strong> psychological characteristics of those who were<br />

positively adjusted included higher levels of optimism, greater feelings of social acceptance and<br />

satisfaction with social support, a lack of concern about negative evaluations by o<strong>the</strong>rs, and a<br />

self-system with lower levels of salience and valence afforded to appearance-related<br />

information. Levels of social anxiety and social avoidance were low, as were levels of negative<br />

affect, general anxiety and depression. <strong>The</strong>se people felt <strong>the</strong>ir disfigurement was reasonably<br />

easy to disguise, and tended to be older than people with higher levels of distress.<br />

An identical regression model was used to interrogate <strong>the</strong> data for each outcome variable. In<br />

relation to social anxiety and social avoidance, an impressive 66.2% of <strong>the</strong> variance was<br />

explained by <strong>the</strong> model. This was anticipated, as <strong>the</strong> measure in question (<strong>the</strong> DAS24) is<br />

designed specifically to assess levels of distress in people with disfigurements. In this<br />

regression model, <strong>the</strong>re were no effects of living status, method of recruitment, or of<br />

appearance-related social comparisons. <strong>The</strong>re was a small gender effect, with women more<br />

distressed than men, and strong negative effects of age (older people were less distressed),<br />

optimism and social acceptance. As levels of fear of negative evaluation, salience and valence<br />

increased, so did distress.<br />

Similar pictures emerged when applying <strong>the</strong> regression model to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r outcome measures.<br />

<strong>The</strong> model predicted approximately 30% of <strong>the</strong> variance for positive affect, 45% for negative<br />

63


affect and 46% for both anxiety and depression. Ra<strong>the</strong>r than comment on <strong>the</strong> results of each<br />

regression in detail (which would be an enormous task), in <strong>the</strong> discussion which follows, we<br />

have chosen to focus most closely on <strong>the</strong> regression for social anxiety and social avoidance, as<br />

<strong>the</strong> DAS24 is <strong>the</strong> outcome measure most specifically relevant to disfigurement, and as o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

research has identified predictors of <strong>the</strong> more general measures of anxiety and depression.<br />

<strong>The</strong> factors involved in <strong>the</strong> regression model are clustered to coincide with <strong>the</strong> diagrammatic<br />

representation in Figure 1.<br />

Dispositional style<br />

In this study, dispositional style, specifically optimism/pessimism, had a strong effect on many of<br />

<strong>the</strong> outcome variables, including depression, anxiety, aggression, negative effect and to a lesser<br />

extent appearance-related social anxiety & avoidance, and increased <strong>the</strong> goodness of fit of<br />

several of <strong>the</strong> regression models to an appreciable extent. In <strong>the</strong> health psychology literature,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re has been a consensus for some time that optimism is adaptive in <strong>the</strong> face of adversity.<br />

Research specifically examining optimism in <strong>the</strong> context of appearance has been lacking, and<br />

this study would suggest that this may be a key factor in adjustment. This is particularly<br />

relevant in <strong>the</strong> context of intervention, as some researchers believe optimism can be taught –<br />

Seligman (1998) for example, developed programmes to help adults and children change from a<br />

pessimistic to an optimistic view of life.<br />

Socio-cognitive processes<br />

Levels of fear of negative evaluation from o<strong>the</strong>rs, social acceptance and satisfaction with social<br />

support correlated significantly with all <strong>the</strong> outcome measures in this study. <strong>The</strong>se results are<br />

consistent with <strong>the</strong> previous literature in this area. Confidence in social situations has been<br />

highlighted as key to adjustment by many commentators in this field (see for example, Partridge<br />

1990). Fear of negative evaluations by o<strong>the</strong>rs and avoidance of social situations have been<br />

thought to play a central role in social difficulties relating to threats to body image, regardless of<br />

<strong>the</strong> actual behaviour of o<strong>the</strong>rs (Newell 2000).<br />

<strong>The</strong> benefits of social support as a positive resource in a variety of situations are widely<br />

accepted, and good quality social support is commonly regarded as an asset which can be<br />

mobilised to act as a buffer to stress in people with disfigurements (Blakeney et al, 1990).<br />

Liossi (2003) found that people with appearance concerns who <strong>report</strong>ed higher levels of social<br />

support had lower levels of social anxiety and social avoidance (also measured by <strong>the</strong> DAS24).<br />

Her data suggested that social support may protect people from distress even when <strong>the</strong>y have<br />

dysfunctional appearance schemata. She proposed that good social support may promote<br />

effective coping strategies, for example by facilitating exposure to feared situations (see also<br />

Baker 1992). Personal accounts testify to <strong>the</strong> benefits of feeling at ease with familiar o<strong>the</strong>rs,<br />

who are perceived as seeing beyond superficial appearances to <strong>the</strong> ‘real person beneath’<br />

(Rumsey & Harcourt, 2005).<br />

<strong>The</strong> role of appearance-related cognitions<br />

In addition to confirming <strong>the</strong> role of <strong>the</strong>se socio-cognitive processes in adjustment, this study<br />

has highlighted <strong>the</strong> considerable contribution of cognitive processes relating more specifically to<br />

64


appearance-related issues. <strong>The</strong> salience of appearance concerns, <strong>the</strong> value attached to<br />

appearance, appearance-related self-ideal discrepancies and appearance-related social<br />

comparison processes correlated significantly with all outcome measures. <strong>The</strong>ir effects were<br />

particularly apparent in relation to scores relating to social anxiety and social avoidance<br />

(DAS24) but <strong>the</strong>ir role also impacted on levels of anxiety, depression, aggression, positive and<br />

negative affect.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results indicate that appearance concerns are complex and can be debilitating. However,<br />

an important positive aspect is that <strong>the</strong>y involve factors which are amenable to intervention,<br />

offering a strong endorsement of <strong>the</strong> potential of cognitive behavioural approaches to address<br />

<strong>the</strong> needs of those affected. <strong>The</strong> results of this study support <strong>the</strong> tentative conclusion drawn<br />

from previous research that <strong>the</strong> appearance concerns of people both with and without<br />

disfigurements contribute to a range of processes including beliefs about <strong>the</strong> impressions<br />

formed by o<strong>the</strong>rs on <strong>the</strong> basis of appearance, and perceptions of <strong>the</strong> self (Thompson & Kent,<br />

2001; Moss & Carr, 2004; Rumsey & Harcourt, 2005). <strong>The</strong> current results fur<strong>the</strong>r indicate that<br />

<strong>the</strong> way people process appearance-related information contributes to <strong>the</strong>ir level of adjustment.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se mechanisms are not new in <strong>the</strong> psychological literature, but until now have not been<br />

demonstrated so clearly in people with disfiguring conditions. Associations between processing<br />

styles and psychological distress including anxiety (Bar-Haim et al, 2007) and depression (Mogg<br />

& Bradley, 2005; Strunk & Adler 2009) have been demonstrated in general population samples,<br />

and <strong>the</strong>re has been some evidence of appearance-related cognitive biases resulting in<br />

increased recall of appearance-related information (Altabe & Thompson, 1996) in people with<br />

high levels of body image concern. An increased likelihood of interpreting ambiguous situations<br />

as appearance-related and negative has been shown in people who are obese (Jansen et al,<br />

2007) and in those with eating disorders (Cooper 1997) compared to those without. Rosser,<br />

Moss & Rumsey (in submission) found in a sample of people without disfigurements that<br />

appearance concerns appear to include a propensity to interpret ambiguous stimuli as<br />

appearance-related and negative (without any priming), and that those with high levels of<br />

concern have a tendency to attend preferentially to stimuli <strong>the</strong>y believe to be appearancerelated<br />

when given a choice.<br />

Taken with <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> current study, a picture is emerging of people (with or without<br />

disfigurements) in whom higher levels of appearance concern result in attentional biases.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se are people who perceive <strong>the</strong> world in a way that reinforces and exacerbates <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

distress. <strong>The</strong>ir processing biases (for example, preferential attention to and interpretation of<br />

ambiguous information as negative) may lead to a perception of <strong>the</strong> world that is dominated by<br />

<strong>the</strong> appearance issue(s) of concern, perhaps resulting in hyper-vigilance towards threatening<br />

stimuli and self-reinforcing perceptions regardless of <strong>the</strong> actual environment (Mat<strong>the</strong>ws, 1990;<br />

Rosser, 2008).<br />

Social comparison processes help us contextualise and understand ourselves in relation to<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs. Rosser (2008), for example, highlighted that people placing a higher value on<br />

appearance in <strong>the</strong>ir self definition are more vulnerable to making negative (upward)<br />

comparisons with o<strong>the</strong>rs. <strong>The</strong> more frequently people engage in social comparisons <strong>the</strong> more<br />

65


likely <strong>the</strong>y are to have difficulties relating to body image, eating behaviours and self esteem<br />

(Thompson, Heinberg & Tantleff, 1991). This is likely to be <strong>the</strong> result of <strong>the</strong> tendency to use<br />

unrealistic standards of comparison (e.g. from <strong>the</strong> media) resulting in unhelpful upward<br />

comparisons associated with more negative affect (Patrick et al 2004). Despite <strong>the</strong> enthusiasm<br />

of several researchers for <strong>the</strong> potential of social comparison processes to extrapolate from <strong>the</strong><br />

body image literature and expect a significant contribution from <strong>the</strong>se processes to adjustment<br />

in <strong>the</strong> context of disfigurement, <strong>the</strong>se effects are not evident in <strong>the</strong> quantitative data of this<br />

study. This may be an artefact of <strong>the</strong> measure used. In <strong>the</strong> light of o<strong>the</strong>r socio-cognitive<br />

processes being specific to appearance-related information (see below), it may be that <strong>the</strong><br />

chosen scale was too general in its orientation. Halliwell (2004) has highlighted <strong>the</strong> complexity<br />

of social comparison processes, and <strong>the</strong> challenges of capturing <strong>the</strong>se in standardised scales.<br />

In addition, as <strong>the</strong>re is evidence of social comparison processes at work in <strong>the</strong> qualitative data<br />

derived from <strong>the</strong> follow-on studies (<strong>report</strong>ed in subsequent sections), we feel this variable<br />

should remain under consideration in future research.<br />

<strong>The</strong> cognitive processes of valence (<strong>the</strong> value attributed to appearance) and <strong>the</strong> salience of<br />

appearance issues in processing information, and <strong>the</strong> perception of <strong>the</strong> discrepancy between<br />

<strong>the</strong> self and social norms were also shown to play significant parts in <strong>the</strong> regression models.<br />

<strong>The</strong> concept of valence relates to <strong>the</strong> positive or negative emotional charge associated with<br />

aspects of <strong>the</strong> self system. <strong>The</strong> valence of a self attribute such as appearance mediates our<br />

feelings about ourselves in relation to that attribute. <strong>The</strong> emotional content of self attributes also<br />

has implications for one’s global self esteem as it plays a part in <strong>the</strong> way self relevant<br />

information is evaluated, and effects <strong>the</strong> positioning of an attribute within a self system. <strong>The</strong><br />

analysis showed that valence contributed to a significant extent to levels of positive affect,<br />

depression and appearance-related distress.<br />

Increased salience in cognitive processing is associated with increased accessibility of<br />

information and processing strategies (Markus & Nurius, 1986). As <strong>the</strong> result of increased<br />

accessibility, more salient information is not only considered more important to <strong>the</strong> individual,<br />

but is also more likely to be frequently employed in <strong>the</strong> perception and experience of <strong>the</strong> world.<br />

Highly salient self information and processing strategies may become chronically accessible.<br />

As salient information is more frequently accessed, it becomes yet fur<strong>the</strong>r defined and<br />

reinforced, more established and has a greater resistance to modification. New information is<br />

more likely to be manipulated to fit prior knowledge ra<strong>the</strong>r than a process of modifying previous<br />

knowledge to incorporate <strong>the</strong> new. In <strong>the</strong> analyses of data from this study, <strong>the</strong> salience of<br />

appearance-related information was shown to contribute to levels of social anxiety and social<br />

avoidance and general anxiety.<br />

Rosser (2008) and o<strong>the</strong>rs have explained how <strong>the</strong> self concept can be conceptualised as being<br />

organised hierarchically into mental representations or self schemata which direct <strong>the</strong><br />

processing of self relevant information. Poorer adjustment is likely when appearance is a more<br />

central aspect of <strong>the</strong> self concept, and when this is dominated by appearance-related (ra<strong>the</strong>r<br />

than non-appearance-related) issues. He has offered a useful overview of how appearancerelated<br />

schemata may influence levels of adjustment and distress in people without<br />

66


disfigurements which may be helpful in interpreting <strong>the</strong> results of this study. Rosser cites<br />

Baldwin’s (1992) description of how <strong>the</strong> self-system is important in constructing templates<br />

(schemata) which influence our interactions with o<strong>the</strong>r people. If new information fits within a<br />

schema’s parameters, <strong>the</strong>n specific assumptions, expectations and predictions will be<br />

implemented by <strong>the</strong> schema to make sense of information and of how best to process it. For<br />

more established schemata, <strong>the</strong> information <strong>the</strong>y contain is more central, accessible and<br />

frequently involved in cognitive processing and interpretation than non-schematic information.<br />

In this way schemata aid processing, and also bias it towards what is expected. Once activated<br />

<strong>the</strong> schema allocates how attention is distributed and how information is processed. <strong>The</strong><br />

potential for bias is considerable as <strong>the</strong> result of preferential attention to schematic information.<br />

Established schemata are more heavily defined and less susceptible to integrating novel<br />

information. <strong>The</strong>y affect how we attend to, store, retrieve and ultimately perceive our<br />

environment. Problems arise when biases cause dysfunctional preferential attention and<br />

misevaluation of information, and as emotional element also associated, this can have<br />

significant emotional implications (Greenberg & Safran, 1987). Self schemata are <strong>the</strong>orised by<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir proponents as central, most salient, highly elaborated and strongly defined within <strong>the</strong> self<br />

system, and are referred to as ‘affect laden’ due to <strong>the</strong>ir emotional as well as informational<br />

content (Altabe & Thompson, 1996). Should a schema with a prevalent negative content gain<br />

dominance in <strong>the</strong> processing system, information processing on all levels could be influenced<br />

and manipulated to reinforce negative expectations. Building on <strong>the</strong> work of Moss and Carr<br />

(2004), Rosser (2008) has described that for people with significant appearance-related<br />

concerns, a maladjusted self schema would become highly central with negative generic<br />

appearance information and processing biases chronically accessible – leading to a self<br />

reinforcing cycle where appearance information is more frequently acknowledged, processed<br />

and employed in processing and interpreting <strong>the</strong> world (see Figure 11).<br />

67


Information Attention Encoding/Storage Retrieval<br />

Processing<br />

Unconscious<br />

Unconscious<br />

Unconscious preferential<br />

Schematic preferential<br />

Influence guiding of<br />

attention<br />

association with<br />

schematic<br />

networked<br />

information<br />

selection of more accessible<br />

information<br />

Schematic<br />

Outcome<br />

Maladaptive<br />

Appearance<br />

Self-Schema<br />

Schematic<br />

information<br />

preferentially<br />

attended<br />

Appearance<br />

related information<br />

chronically<br />

selected for<br />

attention<br />

Figure 11: Model of Processing Influence of Appearance Self-Schema (Rosser, 2008)<br />

Limitations of this study<br />

Information<br />

associated with<br />

schema associated<br />

with stronger<br />

information networks<br />

– stronger encoding<br />

New appearance<br />

related-information<br />

associated with<br />

strong established<br />

negative stored<br />

information about<br />

appearance<br />

Schematic information more<br />

accessible and more readily<br />

available for interpretation of<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r stimuli<br />

New information interpreted<br />

utilising chronically<br />

accessible negative<br />

appearance self-related<br />

information, resulting in<br />

reinforcement of negative<br />

valence of schema and selfinterpretation<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are a number of limitations to this study which should be considered when interpreting <strong>the</strong><br />

results. Prior to <strong>the</strong> commencement of <strong>the</strong> study, <strong>the</strong> assumption was that those actively<br />

seeking treatment for <strong>the</strong>ir condition would have greater levels of appearance-related distress,<br />

and perhaps that previous research which has focused on populations such as <strong>the</strong>se, was<br />

inherently biased in this respect. Accordingly, considerable efforts were made to recruit from<br />

community sources. In <strong>the</strong> event, differences between <strong>the</strong> adjustment profiles for <strong>the</strong> two<br />

groups were minimal. <strong>The</strong> extent to which clinic and community samples are truly different<br />

populations has been a source of debate amongst <strong>the</strong> collaboration during <strong>the</strong> research<br />

process. Although <strong>the</strong> large majority of <strong>the</strong> community sample (those recruited through general<br />

practices) were not accessing treatment at <strong>the</strong> time of <strong>the</strong> study, many will have experienced<br />

treatment in <strong>the</strong> past. Never<strong>the</strong>less, an important message from <strong>the</strong>se results is that substantial<br />

numbers of people who are not seeking treatment will be experiencing appearance-related<br />

distress, which could be amenable to intervention.<br />

Although all participants believed <strong>the</strong>mselves to have a disfigurement of some sort, this was<br />

impossible to verify in relation to <strong>the</strong> community sample (a substantial proportion of whom were<br />

not seen face to face by a member of <strong>the</strong> research team). However, <strong>the</strong> research team<br />

attempted to address this matter by using READ codes as a basis for recruitment in all<br />

participating practices which used <strong>the</strong>m. This enabled targeted recruitment to those classified<br />

by <strong>the</strong>ir GP as having a condition which was potentially disfiguring.<br />

68


<strong>The</strong> response rates to approaches to participate in <strong>the</strong> study varied from clinic to clinic, method<br />

of approach (e.g in person, by poster, by post or online), and within this, from week to week. A<br />

meaningful overall figure is impossible to calculate, thus we are not able to assess <strong>the</strong><br />

representativeness of our sample through conventional means. All <strong>the</strong> researchers involved in<br />

<strong>the</strong> recruitment process were aware of <strong>the</strong> need to sample broadly, and all made considerable<br />

efforts to ensure this was achieved. Never<strong>the</strong>less, as acknowledged above, <strong>the</strong>re is an underrepresentation<br />

of participants from ethnic minority groups in <strong>the</strong> current study, and more women<br />

agreed to take part than men.<br />

It could be also be argued that <strong>the</strong> lack of an objective rating of disfigurement is a limitation of<br />

<strong>the</strong> study, as is <strong>the</strong> broad (type of clinic attended), ra<strong>the</strong>r than a very specific classification of <strong>the</strong><br />

condition of each participant. In addition to <strong>the</strong> lack of a relationship between severity and<br />

distress in <strong>the</strong> previous literature (outlined above), an accurate ‘objective’ rating for every<br />

participant would have been logistically very difficult to achieve. Considerable uncertainty exists<br />

in <strong>the</strong> literature in relation to how to classify <strong>the</strong> severity of various forms of disfigurement, and<br />

individual differences in raters have been shown to be considerable (Roberts-Harry 1997). Our<br />

results offer support to <strong>the</strong> value of assessing subjective assessments of severity, as <strong>the</strong>se are<br />

likely to be more revealing as indicators of adjustment for clinicians, <strong>the</strong>rapists and researchers.<br />

Appealing as <strong>the</strong> explanations of cognitive processes offered above are in attempting to<br />

interpret a large and complex data set, it should be noted that cognitive architecture can only be<br />

inferred as it cannot be directly observed. Concepts such as ‘schemata’ are to some extent<br />

notional, and <strong>the</strong> various processes and biases in attentional processes such as salience,<br />

valence, self discrepancy and social comparison are inevitably somewhat contrived. In reality<br />

<strong>the</strong>y are interconnected, mutually dependent and overlapping. With <strong>the</strong>se limitations in mind,<br />

however, <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> study have led us to revisit and recast <strong>the</strong> conceptual framework of<br />

adjustment presented in <strong>the</strong> introductory section above. In this revised model, <strong>the</strong> important<br />

role of appearance-specific cognitions is acknowledged and highlighted. Although <strong>the</strong> model is<br />

presented in a linear format, <strong>the</strong> dynamic nature of <strong>the</strong> self system is also very evident in this<br />

and subsequent studies.<br />

Variables o<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> ones measured in this study are also certain to play some part in<br />

adjustment. Three main factors are usually presented as promoting resilience or vulnerability in<br />

<strong>the</strong> psychological literature (Luthar et al 2002). <strong>The</strong>se comprise <strong>the</strong> characteristics of <strong>the</strong><br />

individual (cognitive functioning, personality variables, self efficacy and self perception),<br />

characteristics of <strong>the</strong> individual’s family, and <strong>the</strong> individual’s social context. Resilience is a<br />

product of a mutual influence between <strong>the</strong>se three factors, which may function as potential<br />

mediators or moderators of <strong>the</strong> relationship between stressors, risk and outcomes. <strong>The</strong><br />

collaboration chose to focus on those factors which are most amenable to intervention, and thus<br />

variables relating to family functioning and <strong>the</strong> influence of social context are not prominent in<br />

this work. In addition, in order to keep <strong>the</strong> burden of assessment at an acceptable level, and to<br />

include measures which could offer a profile of positive as well as negative adjustment, difficult<br />

decisions were made and additional individual factors which have featured in previous research,<br />

69


such as shame, were omitted. Where <strong>the</strong>se factors are apparent in <strong>the</strong> later follow-on studies,<br />

this is highlighted in later discussion.<br />

Despite <strong>the</strong>se omissions, <strong>the</strong> percentages of <strong>the</strong> variance explained by those variables which<br />

were included in <strong>the</strong> study are impressive, especially when viewed in relation to o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

comparable research. Connor and Sparks (2005) for example <strong>report</strong> that on average 40-50% of<br />

variance in intention and 21-36% of variance in behaviour in studies is accounted for by Social<br />

Cognition Models (e.g. Protection Motivation <strong>The</strong>ory, Health Belief Model, <strong>The</strong>ory of Reasoned<br />

Action / <strong>The</strong>ory of Planned Behaviour).<br />

Implications of <strong>the</strong> results<br />

Advances in knowledge about <strong>the</strong> process and <strong>the</strong> consequences of cognitive mechanisms are<br />

of relevance to understanding <strong>the</strong> development and maintenance of appearance concerns in<br />

people with disfiguring conditions, and are particularly useful in informing <strong>the</strong> development of<br />

interventions designed to meet <strong>the</strong> needs of those adversely affected. <strong>The</strong> results of this study<br />

also emphasise <strong>the</strong> importance of understanding <strong>the</strong> subjective worlds of people with<br />

appearance concerns and <strong>the</strong> place of <strong>the</strong>ir subjective perceptions in <strong>the</strong>ir cognitive<br />

architecture. Taken with findings from previous research discussed above, it is clear that <strong>the</strong>se<br />

are likely to be better indicators of <strong>the</strong> need for intervention than <strong>the</strong> aetiology and physical<br />

characteristics of <strong>the</strong> disfigurement.<br />

As Rosser has recently noted (2008) in relation to <strong>the</strong> appearance concerns of people without<br />

disfigurements, more work is needed to examine in detail <strong>the</strong> influence and role of self referent<br />

appearance-related information within <strong>the</strong> self system, and <strong>the</strong> influence of appearance-related<br />

information processing at all stages from attention to retrieval. However, <strong>the</strong> results of this study<br />

leave no doubt that appearance-specific cognitions play a significant role in adjustment and<br />

distress in people with disfiguring conditions.<br />

<strong>The</strong> implications of <strong>the</strong> results for <strong>the</strong> development of effective methods of meeting <strong>the</strong> needs of<br />

those affected are considerable. <strong>The</strong> findings suggest that interventions designed to tackle<br />

unhelpful cognitions and beliefs about appearance will have an impact on how people adjust to<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir difference. Padesky (1994) for example, has outlined a number of cognitive behavioural<br />

methods to tackle maladaptive schemata which are likely to be relevant for people with<br />

concerns resulting from disfigurement. Her examples include self-schemas (“I am ugly”), o<strong>the</strong>rschemas<br />

(“people prefer o<strong>the</strong>rs who are attractive”) and world-related schemas (“<strong>the</strong> world<br />

values appearance above most attributes”). Interventions challenge <strong>the</strong> maladaptive selfschema<br />

and eventually provide an alternative more adaptive replacement. Alternatively, <strong>the</strong><br />

importance of <strong>the</strong> self-schema can be diminished and made less accessible with <strong>the</strong><br />

development of more effective ones encouraged (Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2002). Veale and<br />

Clarke (2009) have recently published a self-help manual based on cognitive-behavioural<br />

principles for people with body image difficulties.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se approaches are explored fur<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> Interventions manual later in this <strong>report</strong>.<br />

70


STUDY 2<br />

QUALITATIVE ANALYSIS OF FREE TEXT RESPONSES TO<br />

THE QUESTION “DO YOU HAVE ANY GENERAL COMMENTS<br />

ABOUT THE WAY IN WHICH YOUR APPEARANCE AFFECTS<br />

YOUR LIFE, WHETHER POSITIVE OR NEGATIVELY?”<br />

Study lead: Antje Lindenmeyer with additional input from James Kennedy<br />

71


RATIONALE<br />

In an attempt to offer participants in Study 1 <strong>the</strong> opportunity to expand on <strong>the</strong>ir individual<br />

experiences, and in order to supplement responses to standardised measures with qualitative<br />

data, an open ended response question was included in participant packs.<br />

<strong>The</strong>matic analysis was employed to analyse <strong>the</strong>se open ended responses included in <strong>the</strong><br />

baseline questionnaires. <strong>The</strong> major <strong>the</strong>mes are outlined below and are elaborated in <strong>the</strong><br />

section which follows.<br />

I. Falling Short of <strong>the</strong> Ideal<br />

• Oversized (weight issues, large nose, chin, feet)<br />

• Undersized (height, too small breasts, short legs)<br />

• Misshapen (body parts ‘misshapen’ or ‘odd’, or marked by scars, birth marks)<br />

• Feeling abnormal (skin conditions, very visible, disfiguring conditions)<br />

• Tone (dissatisfaction with body that is not taut, e.g. ‘saggy’ ‘droopy’)<br />

• Proportions (body out of proportion, e.g. upper/ lower body, breasts)<br />

• Smoothness (skin that is rough or ‘lumpy’, skin conditions, cellulite)<br />

• Outline (features that mar outline through bulge, ‘sticking out’)<br />

• Gender issues (feeling not manly or not womanly e.g. body hair)<br />

• Criticism of <strong>the</strong> ideal (pressure of matching up to ‘ideals’)<br />

II. Personal Meanings of Appearance<br />

1.) Self and Appearance<br />

Disrupted selves<br />

• Hate and shame (strong negative emotions towards <strong>the</strong> body)<br />

• Troubled (unsure where strong emotions come from)<br />

• Different selves (feeling ones ‘real self’ is hidden)<br />

• Mourning for former selves<br />

• Complex responses<br />

Disrupted biographies (feeling that true purpose in life was thwarted)<br />

• Work (feeling that appearance prevented working at true level)<br />

• Relationships (less attractive; unlikely to find partner)<br />

• Pessimism (appearance prevents from reaching life goals overall)<br />

• Positive outcome (past difficulties resolved)<br />

Confidence<br />

Ageing and feeling old<br />

• Aged by appearance<br />

• Feeling one’s age<br />

72


2.) O<strong>the</strong>r People’s Responses<br />

Direct responses<br />

• Comments and staring<br />

• Bullying<br />

Responses from partners, family and friends<br />

• Positive responses<br />

• Dissonance (between respondent’s and family’s view)<br />

• Hurtful responses<br />

Misjudgements<br />

• ‘People judge’<br />

• Unhealthy (looking ill or contagious)<br />

• Misunderstood (seen as something <strong>the</strong>y were not)<br />

Internalised responses (concerned about possible responses from o<strong>the</strong>rs)<br />

• Visibility (general concerns about visible conditions)<br />

• Self-conscious<br />

• Anticipated response (worry or acting in anticipation of responses)<br />

3.) Coping with Appearance<br />

• Not important<br />

• Pragmatism (one has to accept; no use feeling sorry for oneself)<br />

• Acceptance (positive acceptance, ‘this is who I am’<br />

• Hiding (strategies to hide features, seen positively)<br />

• Not coping (feeling negative; avoiding social situations)<br />

4.) Practical Issues<br />

• Clo<strong>the</strong>s/ Shoes/ Makeup<br />

• Pain and disability<br />

• Time spent on appearance<br />

5.) Time Structures<br />

• Duration of Condition<br />

• Changes (fluctuating conditions, changes in life overall)<br />

• Milestones (significant time spans (e.g. schooldays) and events)<br />

<strong>The</strong> responses from <strong>the</strong> majority of respondents fell into several subcategories. <strong>The</strong>y also<br />

mention more than one body part, emphasising that appearance-related dissatisfaction should<br />

not be assumed to relate to one feature or body part in isolation. Comments about weight issues<br />

were included in <strong>the</strong> analysis, insofar as participants ei<strong>the</strong>r had at least one o<strong>the</strong>r appearance<br />

issue or attributed <strong>the</strong>ir weight to a medical condition (e.g. Polycystic Ovary Syndrome,<br />

medication side effects).<br />

73


FINDINGS<br />

<strong>The</strong>me I. Descriptive Accounts of Appearance Features: Falling Short of Ideals<br />

In <strong>the</strong> answers to <strong>the</strong> ‘feature’ questions, most participants describe <strong>the</strong>mselves as explicitly or<br />

implicitly ‘falling short’ of an ideal appearance defined by size, ‘toned’, smooth, surface, and<br />

ideal proportion. Some go beyond this, naming <strong>the</strong>mselves as ‘ugly’ or <strong>the</strong>ir feature as<br />

‘misshapen’, using strong metaphors (e.g. ‘witch’) or compile long lists of appearance features<br />

<strong>the</strong>y hate.<br />

Sub-categories: Different reasons for ‘falling short’:<br />

Oversized: <strong>The</strong>re are many instances of body parts described as too large/ wide/ big/ fat. This<br />

is often about weight related issues (abdomen/ hips/ breasts/ upper arms) but can also relate to<br />

an implicitly ‘delicate’ body ideal, against which feet, teeth or nose are seen as too large. <strong>The</strong><br />

body part most nominated in this category is <strong>the</strong> abdomen, followed by thighs, nose, upper<br />

arms, bottom, teeth, chin, breasts and feet. This group was comprised of mostly women; a<br />

couple of men thought <strong>the</strong>ir nose was too big, and a few were unhappy with <strong>the</strong> size of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

abdomen (one also with his large feet). While women referred to this part of <strong>the</strong>ir bodies as <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

‘stomach’, ‘tummy’ or ‘abdomen’, <strong>the</strong> men referred to ‘excess weight on my torso’ ‘pot belly’<br />

‘beer belly’ ‘too big belly’ or <strong>the</strong> feminising ‘wide hips’.<br />

Undersized: In contrast to <strong>the</strong> above, bodies were also described as ‘too small’. <strong>The</strong><br />

predominant issue in this category was having too small breasts. For several women, having<br />

breasts that were ‘small’ or even ‘shrunk’ from ageing was a major issue. Several women were<br />

unhappy about being short and especially having short legs. Some participants of both gender<br />

felt <strong>the</strong>y were too thin, and a few people were unhappy because <strong>the</strong>ir height was reduced by a<br />

curved spine or round back. One man complained about a general ‘lack of build’.<br />

Misshapen: Participants felt that parts of <strong>the</strong>ir body did not look ‘right’: <strong>the</strong>y were ‘misshapen’; ‘a<br />

funny shape’; marked by scars or skin conditions, or plain ‘ugly’. Several participants described<br />

scars on <strong>the</strong>ir stomachs, faces or hands (‘stomach misshapen by large appendix scar’; ‘keloid<br />

scar on jaw line, ugly and difficult to hide). <strong>The</strong> size and shape of <strong>the</strong>ir nose (‘bump on <strong>the</strong><br />

bridge; long and crooked) was an issue for some, including one woman who had suffered from<br />

bullying and being called ‘witch’ at school. Several people had ano<strong>the</strong>r facial ‘feature’ (e.g.<br />

vitiligo, birth marks, bad teeth), and some felt that <strong>the</strong> lines in <strong>the</strong>ir face made <strong>the</strong>m look older or<br />

‘miserable’. O<strong>the</strong>r respondents disliked <strong>the</strong>ir legs, as <strong>the</strong>y were marked with broken veins,<br />

varicose veins, scars ‘worry about my scar in terms of meeting someone new’ or just ‘horrid’ or<br />

‘dreadful’. Respondents also said <strong>the</strong>ir hands or feet looked ‘deformed’ or ugly, and that <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

skin was ‘unsightly’ ‘ugly’ or ‘caused me to stay in <strong>the</strong> shadows’ through skin conditions, e.g.<br />

psoriasis, or stretch marks. Some female respondents felt <strong>the</strong>ir breasts were ‘poorly shaped’.<br />

74


Feeling abnormal: Some participants went fur<strong>the</strong>r than that in saying <strong>the</strong>y felt ‘abnormal’<br />

‘freakish’ ‘spoilt’ ‘substandard’ or ‘ruined’. Several men with psoriasis said that <strong>the</strong>y looked ‘odd’;<br />

felt ‘abnormal’ ‘rejected’ or ‘different’. Women said that <strong>the</strong>y ‘felt abnormal’ or ‘would prefer to<br />

look normal’. O<strong>the</strong>rs who felt abnormal named a variety of o<strong>the</strong>r causes: ‘freaky looking’ head,<br />

‘different to o<strong>the</strong>rs’ eyes and forehead, looking ‘like a skeleton’ because of baldness, a blind<br />

right eye that ‘drifts <strong>the</strong> wrong way’, ‘disfigured looking’ hands or hips due to arthritis or odd<br />

shaped toes. Interestingly, in this small sample, feeling abnormal was <strong>the</strong> issue that exercised<br />

<strong>the</strong> greatest number of men.<br />

Tone: Many female participants wrote about falling short of <strong>the</strong> ideal of <strong>the</strong> ‘taut, toned body’<br />

(Bordo 1993). A lot of <strong>the</strong>se women described <strong>the</strong>ir abdomen as ‘flabby’, ‘saggy’ or ‘wobbly’.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r complaints included ‘saggy’ or ‘droopy’ breasts, ‘wobbly’ or ‘undefined’ thighs, upper arms<br />

not being toned enough, and complaints about having a ‘sagging’ or ‘crêpey’ neck. O<strong>the</strong>rs do<br />

not like <strong>the</strong> baggy skin under <strong>the</strong>ir eyes; or <strong>the</strong>y feel generally untoned, e.g. after mastectomy.<br />

What is striking in this category is <strong>the</strong> emotionally charged language in which many women<br />

describe <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>ir body parts hang and move: ‘flabby’, ‘sagging’, ‘droopy boobs’ ‘fat and<br />

wobbly’.<br />

Proportions: Many women described <strong>the</strong>ir body as not balanced, or out of proportion. For some<br />

this was because of a body part (waist, thighs, breasts) that was disproportionally too large in<br />

comparison for <strong>the</strong> rest of <strong>the</strong> body, o<strong>the</strong>rs felt that <strong>the</strong>ir legs were relatively too short, and for<br />

several women, having differently sized breasts was an issue. Having a too short upper body,<br />

too small breasts in relation to <strong>the</strong> overall body, being too broad and having too thin lips were<br />

also issues. One woman described an overall sense of her body being out of proportion, with<br />

eyes, mouth, nose too close toge<strong>the</strong>r, over-sized breasts, short legs, big arms and small hands.<br />

Smoothness: Skin conditions caused many respondents to fall short of <strong>the</strong> smooth-skinned<br />

ideal, especially those with psoriasis who described <strong>the</strong>ir skin as rough, ‘scabby’ and ‘scaly’.<br />

Several women were unhappy about cellulite, ‘lumpy’ or ‘dimpled’ thighs while several<br />

participants felt <strong>the</strong>y had a generally ‘bad complexion’. One woman with acne scars described<br />

herself as feeling ‘like a knobbly-faced witch’. A few complained about unattractive, hard skin on<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir feet.<br />

Outline: Some participants felt that a body part spoiled <strong>the</strong> outline of <strong>the</strong> body. <strong>The</strong>se could be<br />

ears that ‘stick out’, an eye that ‘protrudes’ or an abdomen that ‘bulges’. One respondent felt<br />

unhappy about her ‘protruding lower jaw’. Conditions that resulted in changes in posture (‘round<br />

back’; ‘stooped shoulders’; ‘knocked knees’; ‘walk with a limp’) were also an issue.<br />

Gender issues: For some participants appearance issues were strongly gendered. One male<br />

respondent felt that he was small of build and not masculine enough, while women worried<br />

about ‘masculine’ hair on <strong>the</strong> face or body (‘makes me feel freakish as women are not supposed<br />

to be hairy’; ‘facial hair … not socially/ culturally acceptable on a woman’; ‘I am a woman and<br />

don’t want hairs on my chin, chest and nipples;’ ‘<strong>The</strong> ‘Masculinity’ of <strong>the</strong> hair on my face’).<br />

Ano<strong>the</strong>r woman complained about having masculine hands; One woman also felt that small<br />

75


easts made her ‘flat chested’ and ‘not womanly’. A few women referred to an entrenched<br />

stereotype of <strong>the</strong> ugly witch, describing <strong>the</strong>mselves as ‘like a knobbly-faced witch’ ‘my head tilts,<br />

which … gives me a fat witch like appearance’; ‘a nose which is long and crooked, like a witch’.<br />

One male-to-female transsexual described ‘my appearance is <strong>the</strong> defining key to how o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

react / respond to me…my transition is still incomplete for now, causing me stress in society.’<br />

Criticism of <strong>the</strong> ideal: Participants explicitly criticised appearance ideals and <strong>the</strong> expectation of<br />

perfection. People also expressed <strong>the</strong>ir dislike of <strong>the</strong> media obsession with youth and beauty in<br />

more general terms. One woman stated that she became bulimic ‘striving for vogue<br />

appearance’. <strong>The</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs related ‘society’ to <strong>the</strong>ir personal appearance issues:<br />

‘those of us who are ‘different’ cannot meet <strong>the</strong>ir unrealistic expectations’<br />

‘in society’s eyes I am obese’<br />

‘I know that o<strong>the</strong>rs will judge me by my appearance, and I resent this’<br />

‘society likes people with perfect bodies, so you have to try twice as hard’<br />

‘' <strong>The</strong> Ideal' image <strong>the</strong> media has is probably a young film star, not something most of us<br />

can aspire to! Especially if one is over 25’<br />

<strong>The</strong>me II. Personal Meanings of Appearance<br />

1.) Self and Appearance<br />

In <strong>the</strong> ‘free text’ space, respondents explored in more detail what <strong>the</strong>ir appearance meant to<br />

<strong>the</strong>m in <strong>the</strong> context of <strong>the</strong>ir lives. Appearance was connected strongly to <strong>the</strong>ir sense of self. This<br />

could take <strong>the</strong> form of a disruption to <strong>the</strong>ir sense of self (not accepting or hating <strong>the</strong> present self,<br />

wishing to go back to a former self) or a disruption of biography (not fulfilling one’s potential in<br />

life or not following a ‘normal’ life course). Appearance issues could also take a great toll on<br />

confidence and self-esteem. For some participants, appearance features were also strongly<br />

connected to ageing and feeling old.<br />

Disrupted selves: For some participants appearance issues caused disruptions in <strong>the</strong>ir sense of<br />

identity.<br />

Hate and Shame: Such accounts were characterised by hate and shame participants felt<br />

towards <strong>the</strong>ir bodies, or parts of <strong>the</strong>ir bodies. A man with vitiligo felt ‘ashamed and disgusted by<br />

<strong>the</strong> way I look’; for two women, <strong>the</strong>se strong negative emotions were caused by <strong>the</strong><br />

inescapability of <strong>the</strong> conditions: ‘I hate <strong>the</strong> way my skin [psoriasis] rules my life, but <strong>the</strong>re is no<br />

escape’ ‘It [weight] rules my life—I hate <strong>the</strong> way I look. I hate myself’ ‘I am ashamed of my<br />

appearance and it holds me back’.<br />

Troubled: Several women wrote about having troubled relationships with <strong>the</strong>ir bodies and selves<br />

(‘I cannot identify reasons for my negativity towards my self [weight; Dwayne’s syndrome]’; I<br />

wish I was more positive about my life [mastectomy]’; ‘My skin colour has always troubled me’)<br />

<strong>The</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs thought that <strong>the</strong>y were maybe more negative about <strong>the</strong>mselves than <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance issue warranted.<br />

76


Different selves: A few women felt that <strong>the</strong>ir real selves were ‘hidden’ behind <strong>the</strong>ir appearance<br />

feature. In one case, covering <strong>the</strong> feature could recover <strong>the</strong> ‘real self’ (‘I am sometimes like two<br />

different people—one with my make-up on, but ano<strong>the</strong>r when I have no make-up on [acne]).<br />

Ano<strong>the</strong>r woman felt that her heavy glasses ‘hides <strong>the</strong> real me and I cannot do much about it’,<br />

while ano<strong>the</strong>r thought that her appearance did not reflect her true personality.<br />

Mourning for former selves: A couple of participants saw <strong>the</strong>ir current state as temporary and<br />

wanted <strong>the</strong>ir unblemished former selves back (‘My weight and if I ever will be slim again’; ‘when<br />

will I go back to <strong>the</strong> way I used to look [male, vitiligo])<br />

Complexity: O<strong>the</strong>r women described complex relations between appearance and emotions. One<br />

described how her weight bo<strong>the</strong>red her if she felt low generally, and ano<strong>the</strong>r related bulimia to<br />

psoriasis via <strong>the</strong> stress produced by having an eating disorder.<br />

Disrupted biographies: Many participants wrote about difficulties with achieving <strong>the</strong>ir full<br />

potential in life due to appearance issues.<br />

Work: Some women felt that <strong>the</strong>ir appearance reduced <strong>the</strong>ir confidence and prevented <strong>the</strong>m<br />

working <strong>the</strong>ir level of capability: ‘only ever worked at clerical level’ [issues with nose/<br />

complexion]; ‘ruined my counselling career’ [nervous squint/ eczema]; ‘prevented me getting or<br />

going for jobs’ [facial rash]; ‘I'm sure my boss would be pleased if my eyes are straightened’.<br />

Relationships: Several women thought <strong>the</strong>ir appearance made <strong>the</strong>m less attractive and<br />

<strong>the</strong>refore unlikely to find a (new) partner. Especially one young woman connected her ‘plain<br />

appearance’ to her inability to find a partner: ‘I would like to get married but I suspect I shall<br />

never have <strong>the</strong> chance’ ‘my appearance has meant I have never had a sexual relationship or a<br />

boyfriend’.<br />

Pessimism: <strong>The</strong>re were female participants who related <strong>the</strong> distress in <strong>the</strong>ir lives to <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance; feeling that <strong>the</strong>y could never have <strong>the</strong> life <strong>the</strong>y wanted: ‘I dream all <strong>the</strong> time about<br />

what I want but I know it will never happen because of <strong>the</strong> way I look’ [issues with weight, nose,<br />

hair, face, body, legs, breasts]; ‘if I looked different my life would have turned out better’ [oddshaped<br />

legs]; ‘if I wasn’t feeling so bad about my feature I could do anything I wanted’ [round,<br />

‘babyish’ face] ‘don’t feel I have reached my full potential in life’ [‘abnormal looking’ breasts].<br />

One woman with a birthmark felt that people tended to take pity on those who were ‘different’,<br />

not letting <strong>the</strong>m have a fair chance at proving <strong>the</strong>mselves.<br />

Positive Outcome: Participants spoke of <strong>the</strong>ir past difficulties with adjusting to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance<br />

or dealing with o<strong>the</strong>r people’s responses, which had been resolved positively: ‘I do not feel I<br />

have let it [thin upper body, fat lower body] stop me from living a full life’; ‘I thought that no one<br />

would ever marry me with my skin [psoriasis]’ ‘it has turned out OK with me getting a good job’<br />

[male, cannot straighten knee] ‘My problem is so much better and has improved my life as<br />

much. In <strong>the</strong> past I felt so bad about my appearance.’<br />

Confidence: In addition to those who felt <strong>the</strong>ir potential in life was reduced due to <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance sapping <strong>the</strong>ir confidence, some said that <strong>the</strong>ir appearance reduced <strong>the</strong>ir confidence<br />

or self-esteem in general. This could be related to internal feelings; ‘<strong>the</strong> way I feel in myself<br />

[eczema]’; ‘acne … makes me feel bad about myself’. O<strong>the</strong>r participants related <strong>the</strong>ir loss of<br />

77


confidence to o<strong>the</strong>r people’s reaction (‘it takes one event’; ‘[confidence] alters <strong>the</strong> way people<br />

portray you’). <strong>The</strong>re was a sense of a vicious circle, where it was <strong>the</strong> lack of confidence that<br />

induced people to behave in undermining ways.<br />

Ageing and feeling old: Women related <strong>the</strong>ir appearance to ageing, or felt that <strong>the</strong>y were ‘aged’<br />

by <strong>the</strong>ir appearance. One woman commented that <strong>the</strong>re was a ‘disrespect towards <strong>the</strong> elderly’<br />

which made getting old more difficult.<br />

Aged by appearance: Several women said that <strong>the</strong>ir feature (e.g. ‘turkey neck’; ‘old looking<br />

hands’; ‘bags under my eyes’) made <strong>the</strong>m look older.<br />

Feeling one’s age: Participants of both genders related <strong>the</strong>ir changes in appearance to getting<br />

older. This could be seen with fatalism ‘starting to look my age’; ‘age is catching up with me’ or<br />

<strong>the</strong>re could be a struggle to keep former good looks.<br />

2.) O<strong>the</strong>r People’s Responses<br />

Many participants were strongly attuned to o<strong>the</strong>r people’s responses to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance<br />

‘feature’, whe<strong>the</strong>r from acquaintances and ‘o<strong>the</strong>r people’ in general, or <strong>the</strong>ir own partners, family<br />

and friends. However, some respondents were feeling self-conscious or avoided exposure<br />

because of <strong>the</strong> responses <strong>the</strong>y anticipated from o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

Direct responses from o<strong>the</strong>r people: Many respondents felt that comments or o<strong>the</strong>r reactions<br />

(staring) from o<strong>the</strong>r people were an issue. This could range from casual comments to sustained<br />

bullying.<br />

Comments and staring: Participants noted that comments from o<strong>the</strong>r people about <strong>the</strong><br />

appearance feature (e.g. eye problem/ psoriasis/ scar) were an issue. Some said <strong>the</strong>y were<br />

stared at because of <strong>the</strong>ir appearance feature: ‘People stare specifically at <strong>the</strong>m (eyes)’. A<br />

couple of <strong>the</strong>se especially noted that people looked but did not engage: (‘children are <strong>the</strong> only<br />

people to ask questions’; ‘people will ‘judge’ but not say on appearance’). While comments and<br />

staring were mostly just mentioned as an issue, some noted <strong>the</strong>ir emotional responses. One<br />

woman with Dwayne’s syndrome felt ‘embarrassed and self conscious’ by o<strong>the</strong>rs’ comments<br />

and one man with vitiligo felt ‘ashamed and disgusted’ by <strong>the</strong> way o<strong>the</strong>r people looked. One<br />

woman said she found people staring at her legs amusing, and ano<strong>the</strong>r felt angry when<br />

strangers commented on her scar.<br />

Bullying: A few participants described being bullied: One woman was called ‘witch’ through<br />

childhood because of her crooked nose, <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r because of her psoriasis: ‘I have spent my life<br />

being laughed at, pointed at, and called names’.<br />

Responses from partners, family and friends: Participants often wrote about responses from<br />

people close to <strong>the</strong>m. While <strong>the</strong> majority was very positive, even casual comments by partners<br />

could be very hurtful.<br />

Positive responses: Some participants with appearance issues (e.g. body hair, scar, psoriasis)<br />

felt that <strong>the</strong>ir partner, family and friends gave <strong>the</strong>m support and reassurance, which improved<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir own confidence<br />

78


Dissonance: <strong>The</strong>re were respondents who noted a dissonance between <strong>the</strong>mselves and <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

family’s view of <strong>the</strong>ir appearance ‘feature’. One woman with acne felt it was not taken seriously<br />

by her family, whereas three o<strong>the</strong>rs felt <strong>the</strong>ir family thought <strong>the</strong>y were ‘fine’ or ‘perfect <strong>the</strong> way I<br />

am’. In contrast to this, one respondent said ‘my feature [untoned stomach] is mainly noticeable<br />

to me’ while o<strong>the</strong>rs said <strong>the</strong>ir appearance issues of a general nature (round, ‘babyish’ face;<br />

feeling drab and ‘frumpy’) led to low self esteem.<br />

Hurtful responses: 2 women wrote how hurtful comments made by <strong>the</strong>ir partners affected <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

relationship. One woman said her husband had made a negative comment about her differentsized<br />

breasts ‘when drunk’, which led her to avoid undressing in front of him ‘no matter how<br />

many positive comments he has made before or after’. Ano<strong>the</strong>r woman with pregnancy stretch<br />

marks said that ‘slight comments from [husband] really hurt even if he says he’s joking.’<br />

Misjudgements: Many participants felt that <strong>the</strong>y were being ‘misjudged’ by o<strong>the</strong>rs because of<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir appearance. This was stated in general comments about society and in descriptions of<br />

being ‘misjudged’ because of <strong>the</strong>ir appearance, ei<strong>the</strong>r as unhealthy or contagious, or seen as<br />

something <strong>the</strong>y were not.<br />

‘People judge’: One man with arthritis, one woman with fat imbalance and ano<strong>the</strong>r woman with<br />

very large feet and ‘no interest in fashion’ said <strong>the</strong>y were aware that <strong>the</strong>ir appearance made<br />

people judge <strong>the</strong>m.<br />

Unhealthy: Several people with psoriasis said <strong>the</strong>y were falsely seen as contagious (‘catching’;<br />

‘contagious’ ‘like measles’ ‘people sometimes shun me… afraid that <strong>the</strong>y may catch it, which is<br />

hurtful). One man with scoliosis stated that ‘it looks unhealthy’ and a woman with morphea<br />

sclerosis said it made her ‘look dirty’. One very thin woman said that ‘people think I’m anorexic’,<br />

while one woman who had gained weight was concerned because ‘people think I’m pregnant’.<br />

Misunderstood: 6 participants were seen as something <strong>the</strong>y were not because of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance. Two women said <strong>the</strong>y looked too young ‘childish still’; ‘like a baby’. A woman who<br />

had residual effects from shingles said she was seen as ‘half awake’, whereas two women<br />

complained <strong>the</strong>ir downturned mouth made <strong>the</strong>m look ‘miserable’ or ‘worried’ when <strong>the</strong>y were<br />

not. One woman was unhappy with herself for letting people treat her as a ‘dumb blonde’.<br />

Internalised responses: Many participants described <strong>the</strong>ir own anticipation of o<strong>the</strong>r people’s<br />

responses. Some were worried about <strong>the</strong> visibility of <strong>the</strong>ir condition or felt generally selfconscious<br />

about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance, while o<strong>the</strong>rs were sure that ‘people would notice’ and<br />

described <strong>the</strong>ir anticipatory nervousness.<br />

Visibility: Participants often noted ‘visibility’ of <strong>the</strong>ir feature (e.g. a facial scar, wearing a wig,<br />

psoriasis, thread veins) as <strong>the</strong> reason why it was a concern for <strong>the</strong>m<br />

Self-conscious: Respondents said <strong>the</strong>y were self-conscious because of a visible appearance<br />

feature (e.g. psoriasis, eczema, pregnancy stretch marks).<br />

Anticipated response: O<strong>the</strong>rs described a more detailed anticipation of o<strong>the</strong>r people’s<br />

responses: e.g. ‘I’m worried people will notice [body hair]; I feel people stare at me sometimes<br />

[acne]; I am embarrassed at my eczema…I feel people are looking.’ ‘I feel people are always<br />

looking at <strong>the</strong> worst parts of my body’. Some of <strong>the</strong>se reacted to o<strong>the</strong>r people in anticipation of<br />

79


<strong>the</strong>m noticing: ‘I try to hide my face with hair/ make-up’ ‘nervous, jittery around o<strong>the</strong>rs’, ‘avoid<br />

photographs’. O<strong>the</strong>rs were embarrassed in social situations, e.g. when handing over money<br />

[woman with hand eczema] or when talking to unknown people [woman with facial rash].<br />

3.) Coping with Appearance<br />

This is a very important <strong>the</strong>me given <strong>the</strong> aim of <strong>the</strong> overall study. Many of <strong>the</strong> respondents<br />

wrote about how <strong>the</strong>y coped with <strong>the</strong>ir appearance issues. <strong>The</strong> majority said <strong>the</strong>ir appearance<br />

was not important to <strong>the</strong>m or did not bo<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>m; o<strong>the</strong>r things in life were more important.<br />

Some also developed a pragmatic stance: one should be grateful not to be worse; life goes on;<br />

one should come to terms with what one can not change. O<strong>the</strong>rs said <strong>the</strong>y accepted <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance as a part of <strong>the</strong>m or had stopped trying to hide <strong>the</strong>ir feature. In contrast, o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

coped by concealing <strong>the</strong>ir appearance feature.<br />

Not important: Some respondents said <strong>the</strong>ir appearance issue (e.g. alopecia, mastectomy,<br />

facial scars) did not bo<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>m or were not important to <strong>the</strong>m. Some said that o<strong>the</strong>r things in<br />

life or <strong>the</strong> ‘inner person’ were more important. Never<strong>the</strong>less, people said that <strong>the</strong>y would like<br />

nice clo<strong>the</strong>s/ made an effort for <strong>the</strong>ir friends.<br />

Pragmatism: <strong>The</strong>re were respondents who had developed a pragmatic approach. Some<br />

described appearance as a fact of life which one had to accept; ‘life is too short to worry too<br />

much’ [woman with psoriasis]. For o<strong>the</strong>rs, it was important to be grateful ‘it’s not worse’ [woman<br />

with body hair]; ‘I’m healthy, I value that’ [sensitive about breasts]. <strong>The</strong>re were also those who<br />

saw it as counter productive to dwell on appearance: ‘I try just to get on with it as <strong>the</strong>re is<br />

enough suffering in <strong>the</strong> world without me feeling sorry for myself’ [woman with psoriasis and<br />

general ageing]; ‘I just carry on <strong>the</strong> best I can’ [woman describing herself as ‘pigeon chested<br />

and short legged]; ‘life goes on. I can’t afford to be upset by it.’ [woman with protruding lower<br />

jaw]; ‘I think it is important to be positive and remember that what matters is not how you look<br />

but what sort of person you are. If some people judge by appearances <strong>the</strong>n I consider that to be<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir problem, not mine!’ [woman with collapsed nose bridge].<br />

Acceptance: Some respondents said that <strong>the</strong>y accepted <strong>the</strong>mselves as <strong>the</strong>y were. ‘This is how I<br />

am and I am happy with that’ [man with acne]; ‘It’s who I am and I will not change for anyone’<br />

[woman with large nose, weight issues]; ‘I have a facial birthmark & I’m OK with it’ [female]. For<br />

some, this also meant not trying to hide <strong>the</strong>ir appearance feature: ‘I’d ra<strong>the</strong>r be comfortable than<br />

covered up’ [man with psoriasis]. Of <strong>the</strong>se, some respondents also saw <strong>the</strong>ir issue as an<br />

opportunity for personal growth: ‘I have tried to use my appearance in a positive way to help<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs’ [woman with birthmark]; ‘dealing with o<strong>the</strong>rs’ reaction to my scar has taught me to use a<br />

range of coping strategies’ [female].<br />

Hiding: O<strong>the</strong>r participants, mostly women, described having developed strategies to hide <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

features [body hair, ears that stick out, psoriasis, scarring, thread veins, overhanging abdomen]<br />

through clothing, make-up or hairstyle.<br />

Not coping: Some respondents described not coping to varying degrees. This could be by<br />

‘hiding’ <strong>the</strong>ir feature not as a positive achievement but as a result of how bad <strong>the</strong>y felt about<br />

<strong>the</strong>mselves ‘I try to hide my face with hair/ make-up as I feel people stare at me’ [woman with<br />

80


acne]; ‘Feel very negative about <strong>the</strong> way I look, often try to cover up as much as possible’<br />

[woman with weight issues and bad teeth]. O<strong>the</strong>rs said <strong>the</strong>ir appearance prevented <strong>the</strong>m from<br />

going out and socialising, with a few women saying <strong>the</strong>ir feature [weight issues, psoriasis,<br />

pregnancy stretch marks] prevented <strong>the</strong>m from wearing a swimming costume or swimming.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were also women who said <strong>the</strong>y sometimes could not face going out and socialising with<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs, whilst o<strong>the</strong>r respondents described being completely isolated: ‘I keep myself to myself …<br />

now prefer solitary activities’ [man with psoriasis]; ‘Basically lived under hat for 30 years. No<br />

social network’ [man, scalp transplant gone wrong]; ‘[appearance] has an overall negative effect<br />

on my interactions with people in general’ [woman with issues around weight, knocked knees,<br />

being short].<br />

4.) Practical Issues<br />

Several respondents noted practical difficulties related to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance problem. This could<br />

be a difficulty to find clo<strong>the</strong>s that fitted, or <strong>the</strong>y found that <strong>the</strong>y now could not wear ‘nice clo<strong>the</strong>s’<br />

or stylish shoes. <strong>The</strong> pain and disability of <strong>the</strong> underlying condition was also an issue,<br />

sometimes more than appearance. Many people resented <strong>the</strong> time, effort or expense needed to<br />

treat or conceal <strong>the</strong> condition.<br />

Clo<strong>the</strong>s/ Shoes/ Makeup: Many respondents felt <strong>the</strong>ir appearance problems caused practical<br />

difficulties in finding clothing (or <strong>the</strong>y could not wear <strong>the</strong> clo<strong>the</strong>s <strong>the</strong>y usually wore); o<strong>the</strong>rs could<br />

not wear stylish shoes or could not/ had to wear makeup. <strong>The</strong> main problem in this category<br />

was clo<strong>the</strong>s, especially for women with mastectomies, who found that <strong>the</strong>y had to dress<br />

differently: ‘I can’t wear nice clo<strong>the</strong>s because I bulge’; ‘I have to choose clo<strong>the</strong>s that cover up’;<br />

‘Asymmetry and having to wear expensive bras’. O<strong>the</strong>r women found it hard to buy clo<strong>the</strong>s<br />

because of <strong>the</strong>ir large size, while those with large or asymmetrical breasts found it hard or<br />

expensive to buy bras. Women with large or deformed feet said <strong>the</strong>y were not able to buy<br />

attractive shoes, and a man with psoriasis resented <strong>the</strong> ‘constraints on dress’ this caused. One<br />

woman said she could not put on makeup to cover her acne, and ano<strong>the</strong>r said she had to wear<br />

makeup all <strong>the</strong> time to cover a birthmark.<br />

Pain and disability: For o<strong>the</strong>rs <strong>the</strong> pain, disability and discomfort caused by <strong>the</strong> underlying<br />

condition was as important, if not more so, than <strong>the</strong> appearance issues. This was especially true<br />

for those suffering pain and disability from arthritis. People with psoriasis said that <strong>the</strong>ir skin was<br />

‘itching’, ‘sore’ or ‘irritable’, one man was concerned about <strong>the</strong> skin on his hands ‘because I<br />

work with my hands’. Several women with weight issues felt ‘uncomfy’ ‘debilitated’, ‘slow and<br />

inactive’ or ‘a problem as regards movement/ exercise’. One man with a stoma bag was<br />

concerned with <strong>the</strong> practicalities and smell of changing bags, and ano<strong>the</strong>r man with fibromyalgia<br />

(FMS) was concerned with <strong>the</strong> amount of pain his condition caused.<br />

Time spent on appearance: Some respondents begrudged <strong>the</strong> time, effort and money spent on<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir appearance. Some of those with psoriasis said that treating <strong>the</strong>ir condition was timeconsuming<br />

(applying treatments and taking showers to reduce irritation, intensive hospital<br />

treatment), and several women found <strong>the</strong>ir excess body hair costly and time consuming to<br />

remove.<br />

81


5.) Time Structures<br />

What was striking about many of <strong>the</strong> responses was that people gave it a ‘whole life’ context,<br />

stating how long <strong>the</strong>y had had <strong>the</strong>ir condition and what had changed in <strong>the</strong>ir lives.<br />

Duration: Respondents outlined since when, or for how long, <strong>the</strong>y had <strong>the</strong>ir condition. This<br />

illustrated <strong>the</strong> magnitude of <strong>the</strong>ir distress ‘spent half my life fighting this problem’ [man with RA];<br />

it ‘[acne] still saps all my confidence, 40+ years later [female]. It could also stress that people<br />

were ‘old hands’ at managing <strong>the</strong>ir condition and had learnt how to cope: ‘I am used to [psoriais]<br />

and can hide affected areas’ [female].<br />

Changes: This was relevant for many respondents, and was important for those with<br />

conditions, especially skin conditions that could ‘flare up’ or change according to <strong>the</strong> seasons, or<br />

<strong>the</strong> hormonal cycle. Some respondents wrote how <strong>the</strong>ir appearance, or coping strategies,<br />

improved or worsened over time. O<strong>the</strong>r respondents wrote about changes in coping with <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance, or life changes (divorce, changing jobs, children leaving home) had affected how<br />

<strong>the</strong>y coped with <strong>the</strong>ir appearance.<br />

Milestones: Respondents mentioned especially relevant ‘milestone’ events, such as diagnosis<br />

or beginning of conditions, years at school or childbirth.<br />

COMMENTARY<br />

Respondents wrote of a mix of appearance-related concerns, including those relating to<br />

disfigurement, weight/shape and also <strong>the</strong> effects of ageing. This is an important point. Those<br />

working in <strong>the</strong> area of disfigurement should note that <strong>the</strong> concerns of those affected are not<br />

limited only to <strong>the</strong> consequences of <strong>the</strong>ir disfiguring condition and <strong>the</strong>se should not be treated in<br />

isolation.<br />

<strong>The</strong> pressure exerted by socio-cultural standards and <strong>the</strong> resulting self-ought discrepancy are<br />

widely evident in <strong>the</strong> responses. <strong>The</strong>se are particularly salient in <strong>the</strong> responses to <strong>The</strong>me I,<br />

focusing on <strong>the</strong> sense of falling short of societal ideals. <strong>The</strong> second <strong>the</strong>me illustrates <strong>the</strong><br />

influence of appearance concerns on attributions for many aspects of <strong>the</strong> life course including<br />

disruption to employment and relationships. Change was seen to exacerbate distress, including<br />

<strong>the</strong> impact of life events, such as divorce and children leaving home, changes to social<br />

groupings, and also changes in <strong>the</strong> status of <strong>the</strong>ir condition, for example, fluctuations in chronic<br />

conditions. For some, <strong>the</strong>re was a sense that significant proportions of <strong>the</strong>ir lives had been<br />

dominated or given over to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance issue.<br />

<strong>The</strong> impact of <strong>the</strong> responses of o<strong>the</strong>r people’s reactions and judgements is also highlighted, and<br />

several respondents mentioned <strong>the</strong>ir concern about possible as well as actual responses.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is also a reminder within this <strong>the</strong>me that partners, families and friends can offer both<br />

positive support and be unhelpful.<br />

A wide range of coping strategies are mentioned. Effective techniques include acceptance,<br />

relegating <strong>the</strong> appearance concern to a low level of importance in <strong>the</strong> overall life experience,<br />

and a sense of pragmatism. O<strong>the</strong>rs talked of coping less well, and of hiding <strong>the</strong> object of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance concern to <strong>the</strong> best of <strong>the</strong>ir ability.<br />

82


Practical issues, such as <strong>the</strong> need for particular shoes or clo<strong>the</strong>s, or <strong>the</strong> need to use makeup<br />

are mentioned, and many resented <strong>the</strong> time, effort and money required to deal with, or disguise<br />

aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir appearance. Associated functional issues and pain relating to <strong>the</strong> underlying<br />

condition were also apparent, particularly in those with arthritis, and for those with skin<br />

conditions.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se responses illuminate <strong>the</strong> quantitative data obtained in Study 1. <strong>The</strong>y are also a reminder<br />

of <strong>the</strong> multi-factorial nature both of appearance dissatisfaction – which is often an amalgam of<br />

several issues, and to factors determining levels of adjustment to <strong>the</strong>se issues. <strong>The</strong> responses<br />

are also an expression of <strong>the</strong> dynamic nature of adjustment.<br />

83


STUDY 3<br />

LONGITUDINAL QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF CHANGE AND<br />

STABILITY IN PSYCHOLOGICAL ADJUSTMENT TO<br />

APPEARANCE<br />

Study leads: Nichola Rumsey, Rob Newell & Andrew Thompson<br />

85


RATIONALE<br />

Thompson and Kent (2001), Rumsey and Harcourt (2005), Bessell and Moss (2008), Jenkinson<br />

et al (in preparation) and o<strong>the</strong>r commentators have noted <strong>the</strong> pressing need for longitudinal<br />

research to explore <strong>the</strong> extent of stability and change in adjustment to disfigurement over time.<br />

Study 3 <strong>report</strong>s a longitudinal study in which responses to standardised measures were<br />

repeated over a 9 month period. Study 4 (included in <strong>the</strong> follow-on studies) <strong>report</strong>s qualitative<br />

data from in-depth interviews of a sub-section of respondents who identified <strong>the</strong>mselves as<br />

stable in <strong>the</strong>ir adjustment, and those who perceived <strong>the</strong>ir adjustment levels to have changed.<br />

Due to <strong>the</strong> delay in <strong>the</strong> contracts process, <strong>the</strong> initial data collection point was later than originally<br />

anticipated. In order to complete <strong>the</strong> longitudinal study within <strong>the</strong> life of <strong>the</strong> grant funding, <strong>the</strong><br />

follow-up period was shortened in comparison with <strong>the</strong> plan in <strong>the</strong> original protocol, and <strong>the</strong><br />

design simplified to comprise one additional data collection point, at 9 month follow-up. This<br />

change required a modification to <strong>the</strong> ethical approval via an amendment.<br />

METHOD<br />

All participants in study 1 who opted in for fur<strong>the</strong>r studies (n = 1221) were sent a follow up<br />

questionnaire pack (see appendix 9) at 9 months (see methods section of Study 1). 349<br />

completed questionnaires were returned.<br />

RESULTS<br />

Participants<br />

Of <strong>the</strong> 1265 participants at baseline, 349 participants completed questionnaires again at <strong>the</strong> 9<br />

month follow-up.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was no statistically significant difference between those who responded at time 2<br />

compared to those who did not according to gender ( = 2.707, df = 1, p = 0.100), method of<br />

recruitment ( = 0.691, df = 1, p = 0.406), or objective visibility ( = 0.347, df = 1, p =<br />

0.556) (Table 3.1).<br />

86


Table 3.1 Response rates for responders and non-responders at follow-up<br />

Non-Responders Responders<br />

Gender Female 611 (69.7) 256 (74.4)<br />

Male 266 (30.3) 88 (25.6)<br />

Total 877 (100%) 344 (100%)<br />

Recruitment Community 438 (47.8) 176 (50.4)<br />

Clinic 478 (52.2) 173 (49.6)<br />

916 (100%) 349 (100%)<br />

Concern Visible 424 (46.3) 168 (48.1)<br />

Not Visible 492 (53.7) 181 (51.9)<br />

916 (100%) 349 (100%)<br />

Independent samples t-test revealed that those who responded at follow-up had a significantly<br />

higher mean level of appearance discrepancy (p < 0.001) and valence (p = 0.002) than those<br />

who did not respond, although <strong>the</strong> effects sizes were small (d = 0.24) (Table 3.2).<br />

Table 3.2 T-test comparisons between responders and non-responders at follow-up<br />

Measure Non-Responders 1 Responders 2<br />

Mean SD Mean SD p<br />

Discrepancy 29.21 11.36 31.90 11.33


Table 3.3 Descriptive and Inferential Statistics for responders and non-responders at follow-up (p-value<br />

for paired samples t-test)<br />

Correlation Differences<br />

r ICC1 Mean SD p<br />

Discrepancy .688 .816 -0.36 9.00 .469<br />

Optimism .776 .873 0.11 2.31 .374<br />

Support Satisfaction .596 .747 0.25 4.69 .323<br />

Social Comparison .675 .806 0.06 5.88 .861<br />

Valence .738 .849 -0.33 5.71 .281<br />

Salience .719 .835 0.18 6.00 .579<br />

Fear of Negative Evaluation .757 .861 0.60 7.54 .143<br />

DAS .841 .913 1.18 9.21 .017<br />

HADS Anxiety .768 .869 0.20 3.16 .245<br />

HADS Depression .746 .853 0.17 2.82 .256<br />

Aggression .798 .888 -0.06 5.98 .858<br />

Positive Affect .633 .775 0.64 7.48 .113<br />

Negative Affect .729 .843 0.30 6.73 .416<br />

Predictors of Adjustment at Follow-up<br />

Social anxiety and social avoidance (DAS 24)<br />

70.7% of <strong>the</strong> variance in DAS24 for <strong>the</strong> longitudinal sample is accounted for by <strong>the</strong> regression<br />

model given in Table 3.4 and this effect is statistically significant, = .707, F(16, 262) = 39.599,<br />

MSE = 80.24, p < .001.<br />

In comparison to <strong>the</strong> model for <strong>the</strong> baseline data, statistically significant effects remained for<br />

gender, optimism, FNE, social acceptance, valence and appearance discrepancy, with <strong>the</strong><br />

direction of <strong>the</strong> effects remaining <strong>the</strong> same. For <strong>the</strong> baseline data, age, objective visibility,<br />

satisfaction with social support and salience were all statistically significant predictors of DAS24.<br />

However <strong>the</strong>se effects were no longer significant for <strong>the</strong> longitudinal data. No additional<br />

variables were significant longitudinally compared with <strong>the</strong> baseline model.<br />

88


Table 3.4 Regression Model for DAS24<br />

Variable Coef Std<br />

Error<br />

Beta t p VIF<br />

Constant 41.04 7.421 5.53


Table 3.5 Regression Model for Anxiety<br />

Variable Coef Std<br />

Error<br />

Beta t p VIF<br />

Constant 7.887 2.633 3.00 .003<br />

Age 0.033 0.016 .108 2.12 .035 1.501<br />

Gender<br />

Male --- ---- ---- ---- ----<br />

Female 0.209 0.492 .019 0.42 .672 1.156<br />

Recruitment<br />

Community --- --- --- --- ---<br />

Clinic -.335 0.402 -.037 -0.83 .406 1.121<br />

Living Status<br />

Alone --- --- --- --- ---<br />

With Friends 0.685 0.724 .049 0.95 .345 1.548<br />

With Partner 0.750 0.492 .078 1.53 .128 1.503 .067 .002<br />

Objective Visibility<br />

No --- --- ---- ---- ---<br />

Yes -0.138 0.421 -.015 -0.33 .744 1.224 .002 .489<br />

Optimism -0.470 0.078 -.367 -6.05


Table 3.6 Regression Model for Depression<br />

Variable Coef Std<br />

Error<br />

Beta t p VIF<br />

P<br />

Constant 13.828 2.371 5.83


Table 3.7 Regression Model for Aggression<br />

Variable Coef Std<br />

Error<br />

Beta t p VIF<br />

P<br />

Constant 38.79 6.729 5.76


Table 3.8 Regression Model for Positive Affect<br />

Variable Coef Std<br />

Error<br />

Beta t p VIF<br />

Constant 18.348 5.753 3.19 .002<br />

Age -0.059 0.034 -.104 -1.73 .086 1.495<br />

Gender<br />

Male --- ---- ---- ---- ----<br />

Female -1.443 1.069 -.072 -1.35 .178 1.158<br />

Recruitment<br />

Community --- --- --- --- ---<br />

Clinic -0.652 0.881 -.039 -0.74 .460 1.130<br />

Living Status<br />

Alone --- --- --- --- ---<br />

With Friends -3.015 1.570 -.118 -1.92 .056 1.547<br />

With Partner -2.278 1.067 -0.129 -2.13 .034 1.496 .095


Table 3.9 Regression Model for Negative Affect<br />

Variable Coef Std<br />

Error<br />

Beta t p VIF<br />

Constant 23.481 5.282 4.44


0.016), and optimism (r = -0.183, n = 344, p = 0.001). <strong>The</strong>y were also positively correlated with<br />

appearance discrepancy (r = 0.129, n = 345, p = 0.016), valence (r = 0.211, n = 349, p < 0.001),<br />

salience (r = 0.153, n = 346, p = 0.004) and FNE (r = 0.136, n = 346, p = 0.012).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results suggest that younger, pessimistic people, who do not feel socially accepted, who<br />

self-<strong>report</strong> a large discrepancy from <strong>the</strong> norm, worry about what o<strong>the</strong>rs think and place a high<br />

value on appearance are more likely to change on <strong>the</strong> DAS24. However in a multiple<br />

regression analysis only social acceptance of belonging is a significant predictor of absolute<br />

changes in DAS 24 (i.e. after accounting for <strong>the</strong> effect of acceptance no o<strong>the</strong>r variable provides<br />

an additional contribution to <strong>the</strong> absolute changes in DAS 24).<br />

Anxiety<br />

Changes from baseline to follow-up in anxiety were found to be negatively correlated with age (r<br />

= -.179, n = 345, p = 0.001, two-sided), optimism (r = -0.117, n = 347, p = 0.001) and positively<br />

associated with social comparison (r = 0.134, n = 349, p = 0.012, two-sided).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results suggest that younger, pessimistic people, who compare <strong>the</strong>mselves to o<strong>the</strong>rs are<br />

more likely to experience change in anxiety. However, age is <strong>the</strong> dominant factor and is <strong>the</strong> only<br />

variable predictive of absolute changes in anxiety in a multiple regression analysis.<br />

Depression<br />

Changes from baseline to follow-up in depression were found to be negatively correlated with<br />

age (r = -.174, n = 345, p = 0.001, two-sided), social acceptance (r = -0.143, n = 348, p =<br />

0.007), optimism (r = -0.203, n = 347, p < 0.001), satisfaction with social support (r = -0.115, n =<br />

346, p = 0.033) and positively associated with appearance discrepancy (r = 0.150, n = 348, p =<br />

0.005), valence (r = 0.186, n = 352, p < 0.001) and FNE (r = 0.113, n = 349, p = 0.035, twosided).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results suggest that younger, pessimistic people, who do not feel socially accepted, who<br />

are less satisfied with <strong>the</strong>ir social support, who self-<strong>report</strong> a large discrepancy from <strong>the</strong> norm,<br />

evaluate <strong>the</strong>ir appearance negatively and fear negative evaluations from o<strong>the</strong>rs are more likely<br />

to experience a change in depression. Optimism is <strong>the</strong> best predictor. In a multiple regression<br />

model no variable makes a significant additional contribution into <strong>the</strong> prediction of <strong>the</strong> absolute<br />

change in HADS depression after <strong>the</strong> inclusion of Optimism.<br />

Aggression<br />

Changes from baseline to follow-up in aggression were found to be negatively associated with<br />

optimism (r = -0.232, n = 344, p < 0.001) and positively associated with social comparison (r =<br />

0.187, n = 346, p < 0.001, two-sided) and salience (r = 0.134, n = 347, p = 0.013, two-sided).<br />

95


<strong>The</strong>se results suggest that more pessimistic people, who compare <strong>the</strong>mselves to o<strong>the</strong>rs and<br />

place a high value on appearance, experience a change in aggression. Optimism is <strong>the</strong> best<br />

predictor and in a multiple regression model both optimism and social comparison are<br />

statistically significant predictors of absolute changes in aggression ( = 0.266, F(2, 316) =<br />

12.042, MSE = 16.801, p < 0.001).<br />

Positive Affect<br />

None of <strong>the</strong> variables are significantly correlated with absolute changes in Positive Affect.<br />

Negative Affect<br />

Changes from baseline to follow-up in negative affect were found to be negatively associated<br />

with age (r = -0.169, n = 334, p = 0.002), social acceptance (r = -0.213, n = 336, p < 0.001),<br />

optimism (r = -0.257, n = 336, p < 0.001), satisfaction with social support (r = -0.123, n = 335, p<br />

= 0.025, two-sided) and positively associated with appearance discrepancy (r = 0.227, n = 336,<br />

p < 0.001, two-sided), valence (r = 0.212, n = 340, p < 0.001), social comparison (r = 0.163, n =<br />

338, p = 0.003), salience (r = 0.182, n = 337, p = 0.001, two-sided) and FNE (r = 0.261, n = 340,<br />

p < 0.001).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results suggest that younger, pessimistic people, who do not feel socially accepted, who<br />

are less satisfied with <strong>the</strong>ir social support and self-<strong>report</strong> a large appearance discrepancy from<br />

<strong>the</strong> norm, negatively evaluate <strong>the</strong>ir own appearance, place a high value on appearance and fear<br />

negative evaluations from o<strong>the</strong>rs are more likely to experience change in negative affect.<br />

Optimism and appearance discrepancy are <strong>the</strong> best two predictors and both are jointly<br />

predictive of absolute change in negative affect in a multiple regression ( = 0.098, F(2, 313)<br />

= 16.935, MSE = 20.779, p < 0.001).<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> findings of this study provide fur<strong>the</strong>r, strong support for <strong>the</strong> framework operationalised by<br />

<strong>the</strong> research programme. Indeed, <strong>the</strong> regression model accounted for a very impressive 70.7%<br />

of variation in <strong>the</strong> DAS24 at 9 month follow up – a figure that is greater than <strong>the</strong> (already very<br />

sizeable) variance accounted for at baseline. Similarly, <strong>the</strong> regression model predicted greater<br />

variance in <strong>the</strong> measures of HADS anxiety, HADS depression, and positive and negative affect<br />

at 9 month follow up than at baseline. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong> majority of <strong>the</strong> components that<br />

predicted appearance-related anxiety at baseline continued to do so at follow-up and <strong>the</strong>re was<br />

also a high degree of consistency in <strong>the</strong> predictors of HADS anxiety and depression,<br />

aggression, positive affect and negative affect at both baseline and follow-up.<br />

Overall, <strong>the</strong> results of this study indicate a picture of stability ra<strong>the</strong>r than change over <strong>the</strong> 9<br />

month follow-up period: mean scores on most outcome measures changed relatively little over<br />

time (although <strong>the</strong>re was a small, significant increase in mean DAS scores between baseline<br />

96


and follow up). However, focusing on relatively consistent mean scores could detract from <strong>the</strong><br />

fact that many participants did <strong>report</strong> a change in self-<strong>report</strong> outcomes over time. Participants<br />

who <strong>report</strong>ed a change in outcomes tended to be younger, have a more pessimistic outlook and<br />

be more affected by appearance cognitions (salience and discrepancy) and <strong>the</strong>ir views of o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

(feeling socially excluded and more bo<strong>the</strong>red by o<strong>the</strong>rs’ judgements of <strong>the</strong>m). This follow up<br />

data <strong>the</strong>reby supports <strong>the</strong> findings of study 1 by once again identifying changeable, sociocognitive<br />

factors to inform appropriate, supportive interventions. However, as with <strong>the</strong> crosssectional<br />

study, it would be an error to assume from <strong>the</strong>se findings that appearance-related<br />

distress and concerns are not an issue for older people.<br />

Given <strong>the</strong> challenges of recruitment into longitudinal research (see Rumsey & Harcourt 2005),<br />

<strong>the</strong> response rate achieved in <strong>the</strong> current study (27.5%) is very good and suggests that <strong>the</strong><br />

research questions and study design were acceptable and of importance to <strong>the</strong> study group.<br />

On <strong>the</strong> whole, <strong>the</strong> sample in this study can be considered representative of <strong>the</strong> cross-section<br />

sample (study 1). However, compared with non-respondents, those who participated in <strong>the</strong><br />

follow-up study had, at baseline, <strong>report</strong>ed placing slightly more importance on appearance and<br />

greater discrepancy between self and ideal appearance – supporting <strong>the</strong> suggestion (Rumsey &<br />

Harcourt 2005) that people experiencing difficulties in relation to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance may be more<br />

engaged with research in this area than those who are resilient to appearance-related distress.<br />

Until now, <strong>the</strong>re has been a paucity of longitudinal research into psychosocial adjustment to<br />

visible difference. With a sample of 349, this data set constitutes, to <strong>the</strong> best of our knowledge,<br />

<strong>the</strong> largest follow-up study in this field. However, <strong>the</strong>re is still a need for fur<strong>the</strong>r longitudinal<br />

research with a longer follow-up than was possible in <strong>the</strong> current study. <strong>The</strong>re is also a need to<br />

explore individuals’ subjective experiences of change over time and, for this reason, <strong>the</strong><br />

qualitative study (study 4) was carried out with a subset of participants from this longitudinal<br />

study.<br />

97


STUDY 4<br />

A QUALITATIVE INVESTIGATION OF CHANGE AND<br />

STABILITY IN PSYCHOLOGICAL ADJUSTMENT TO<br />

APPEARANCE<br />

Study lead: Andrew Thompson, Rob Newell, Sally-Ann Clarke, Elizabeth Jenkinson & Hayley<br />

James<br />

99


SUMMARY<br />

• Twenty-six participants were purposively selected from a group who had previously<br />

completed <strong>the</strong> ARC battery of psychometric measures at baseline and follow-up at 9<br />

months.<br />

• Participants identified <strong>the</strong>mselves into one of four groups of adjustment (‘well adjusted<br />

stable’; ‘distressed stable’; ‘improvement in adjustment’; or ‘deterioration in adjustment’).<br />

• Individual questionnaire data were analysed to ascertain levels of adjustment according<br />

to <strong>the</strong> quantitative measures and semi-structured interviews were conducted.<br />

• Template analysis was used to explore and refine <strong>the</strong>mes derived from <strong>the</strong> aims of <strong>the</strong><br />

research programme and <strong>the</strong> conceptual model used in Study 1: predisposing factors to<br />

different levels of adjustment; intervening cognitive processes associated with different<br />

levels of adjustment; and outcomes associated with different levels of adjustment<br />

• <strong>The</strong>re were discrepancies between <strong>the</strong> participants’ self <strong>report</strong>ed perceptions of<br />

adjustment and <strong>the</strong>ir scores on psychometric measures, particularly for <strong>the</strong> stable well<br />

adjusted and improvement groups.<br />

• Predisposing factors including experience of negative reactions from o<strong>the</strong>rs and daily life<br />

stressors were common across all groups.<br />

• Those who identified <strong>the</strong>mselves as well adjusted <strong>report</strong>ed greater cognitive flexibility in<br />

relation to <strong>the</strong>ir core views of <strong>the</strong>mselves and of <strong>the</strong>ir appearance. <strong>The</strong>y also <strong>report</strong>ed<br />

making external attributions (i.e. linking cause to o<strong>the</strong>rs) and to have developed coping<br />

skills that boosted self-efficacy.<br />

• Those who identified <strong>the</strong>mselves as well adjusted <strong>report</strong>ed a high level of engagement in<br />

social behaviour and not engaging in avoidance strategies, whereas <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r groups<br />

<strong>report</strong>ed low levels of engagement and use of avoidance in coping. Maintaining stability<br />

of positive adjustment was described as exerting an ongoing demand on resources.<br />

• Improvement was associated with good aes<strong>the</strong>tic outcomes following treatment, greater<br />

awareness of cognitive coping strategies, and social and professional support.<br />

• Deterioration was associated with <strong>the</strong> occurrence of negative life events, and increase in<br />

appearance salience due to a range of factors (i.e. o<strong>the</strong>rs reactions, functional disability,<br />

need for fur<strong>the</strong>r treatment).<br />

100


• <strong>The</strong> study reinforces and exemplifies findings in <strong>the</strong> related quantitative cross sectional<br />

and longitudinal surveys and has implications for our understanding of adaptation in<br />

visible difference and our ability to respond appropriately.<br />

RATIONALE<br />

Appearance altering conditions can be associated with a range of psychosocial difficulties, yet<br />

many people adapt positively and succeed in minimising <strong>the</strong> impact of visible<br />

difference/disfigurement on <strong>the</strong>ir lives.<br />

Previous research suggests that individual adjustment is affected by a complex interplay of<br />

physical, cultural and psychosocial factors (Thompson & Kent, 2001; Rumsey & Harcourt,<br />

2004). Small scale longitudinal qualitative studies have followed up people with visible<br />

difference over time. Most notably <strong>the</strong> early work of MacGregor (Macgregor et al, 1953;<br />

Macgregor, 1979) involved interviews over a sixteen year period. However, this work has not<br />

focussed on adaptation over <strong>the</strong> course of time and its relationship to events in <strong>the</strong> person’s life.<br />

Little is known about <strong>the</strong> course of psychological adjustment to appearance over time and<br />

factors which facilitate positive coping. Accordingly, our study sought to follow-up people with<br />

visible difference over nine months, with a specific focus on factors associated with better or<br />

worse adaptation to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance and its psychological concomitants.<br />

METHOD<br />

Aim<br />

To explore in depth individual explanations of adjustment to appearance over a nine-month<br />

period amongst those who defined <strong>the</strong>mselves as:<br />

1. Well adjusted to visible difference without variation during <strong>the</strong> study period (well adjusted<br />

stable).<br />

2. Poorly adjusted to visible difference without variation during <strong>the</strong> study period (distressed<br />

stable).<br />

3. Having experienced an improvement in adjustment during <strong>the</strong> study period<br />

(improvement in adjustment’).<br />

4. Having experienced a deterioration in adjustment during <strong>the</strong> study period (‘deterioration<br />

in adjustment’).<br />

Participants and Sampling<br />

Participants were attendees at NHS clinics for a variety of appearance-related issues who had<br />

completed measures at baseline and 9 month follow-up in our associated quantitative study.<br />

Participants answered a questionnaire item in this study to self-select into one of <strong>the</strong> four<br />

categories above. Twenty-six participants were recruited to <strong>the</strong> qualitative longitudinal study.<br />

101


<strong>The</strong>re were ten well adjusted stable’; four ‘distressed stable’; seven ‘improvement in<br />

adjustment’; and five self selecting as ‘deterioration in adjustment’.<br />

Measures<br />

A semi-structured interview schedule (see appendix 10) was constructed by <strong>the</strong> project team<br />

based on a priori <strong>the</strong>mes emerging from previous studies of visible difference. Responses to a<br />

range of quantitative measures used in <strong>the</strong> main study (DAS; FNE; & HADS) were also<br />

examined, using <strong>the</strong>se participants’ responses both at baseline and to <strong>the</strong> longitudinal survey.<br />

Procedure<br />

Participants were interviewed individually at a venue of <strong>the</strong>ir choice by one of three of <strong>the</strong><br />

Research Associates. All interviewers received training in interviewing prior to <strong>the</strong><br />

commencement of <strong>the</strong> day. Interviews were audio recorded and transcribed verbatim.<br />

Data analysis<br />

Template analysis (King 1998) was used to collate and analyse <strong>the</strong> qualitative data in line with<br />

<strong>the</strong> a priori <strong>the</strong>mes. This process involves <strong>the</strong> development and application of templates related<br />

to <strong>the</strong> a priori <strong>the</strong>mes and also <strong>the</strong> modification of <strong>the</strong>se <strong>the</strong>mes through a process of iterative<br />

re-examination and analysis of <strong>the</strong> data.<br />

Questionnaire data (HADS, DAS, and FNE) were compared using <strong>the</strong> Wilcoxon Signed Ranks<br />

test for mean scores and Jacobson’s Reliable Change Index (RCI) for individual scores. Chi<br />

Square was used to compare <strong>the</strong> four self-selected groups (1 stable coping well, 2 stable<br />

distressed, 3 changed-improved, 4 changed-deteriorated) with RCI scores categorised into four<br />

groups (1 no significant change and score within <strong>the</strong> non-clinical range, 2 no significant change<br />

and score within <strong>the</strong> clinical range, 3 significant change-improved, 4 significant changedeteriorated).<br />

RESULTS<br />

Template Analysis<br />

Participants offered a considerable range of responses, which were exhaustively captured<br />

under <strong>the</strong> template headings. Where a <strong>the</strong>me is obvious from its title, no fur<strong>the</strong>r expansion of its<br />

meaning is given. O<strong>the</strong>rwise, a sentence or brief paragraph demonstrates <strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>the</strong><br />

responses, which have given rise to <strong>the</strong> identification of this <strong>the</strong>me. All <strong>the</strong>mes arose directly<br />

from participant responses, but due to space considerations only a subset of <strong>the</strong> supporting<br />

quote material is included here.<br />

<strong>The</strong> data are summarised according to 3 stages: predisposing factors; intervening processes;<br />

outcomes. <strong>The</strong>se are <strong>report</strong>ed here by proceeding from generalised responses to those which<br />

are more specific to <strong>the</strong> subgroups of <strong>the</strong> sample.<br />

102


Stage 1 – predisposing factors – all groups<br />

Life stressors, stigmatisation and appearance concern<br />

Participants in all groups <strong>report</strong>ed a range of life stressors, both general and focused more<br />

specifically on health. <strong>The</strong>se included financial stressors (unemployment, time away from work<br />

due to poor health) social difficulties (relationship / family difficulties); health concerns<br />

(symptoms, pain, disability).<br />

Participants in all groups <strong>report</strong>ed appearance focused stressors. <strong>The</strong>se included perceptions of<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs’ negative reactions towards <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference, and fear of negative evaluation. In <strong>the</strong><br />

stable distressed group, <strong>the</strong>se were a source of significant distress, and appeared to be <strong>the</strong><br />

largest barrier to successful adjustment.<br />

Although <strong>the</strong> design of this study does not permit quantitative comparison between <strong>the</strong> groups,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re was no direct evidence of consistent differences in <strong>the</strong> frequency of such negative<br />

reactions across <strong>the</strong> groups.<br />

Stage 1 - predisposing factors – changed improved group<br />

1. External triggers for positive change<br />

Recent medical intervention leading to improved appearance, health, pain or function<br />

“Yes, but I’ve got a pellet right on <strong>the</strong> radial nerve, on <strong>the</strong> side of my head and I<br />

think that’s what’s causing constant discomfort. To a certain extent I thought<br />

“right, once you have your eye out you’ll feel better” (01/clin/0034/eye)<br />

“It did make a big difference to me having that stomach operation, a very big<br />

difference, I’m in proportion now, I might be fat but I’m in proportion<br />

(04/clin/0051/pla)<br />

2. Social support integral to adjustment / positive change<br />

“You’ve described quite a few changes, how did <strong>the</strong>y come about, can you<br />

just tell me when you first noticed things changing or starting to improve?<br />

Well when I got this support worker. This is through J again, she got him, South<br />

Yorkshire Housing Association <strong>the</strong>y call him M. He's a lovely bloke and he got<br />

me all sorts sorted out...” (03/clin/1047/prosth)<br />

“Do you think it's anything you’ve done to help yourself make that sort of<br />

change or do you think it is just a case of time? I think if you’ve got a good<br />

support system around you like family and friends, if I'd been on my own I don’t<br />

think I'd have coped like I have, so yeah I've had a lot of<br />

support”(05/clin/1002/burns)<br />

103


Stage 1 – predisposing factors – changed deteriorated group<br />

1. External triggers for change: Negative life events<br />

Negative life events appear to contribute to greater appearance salience, greater perceived<br />

differences in appearance compared with <strong>the</strong> norm, disappointment following medical<br />

intervention or realisation that hopes for surgery and a normal appearance were unrealistic and<br />

unachievable, and a sense of identity loss, and disruption to self-concept.<br />

Health condition resulting in visible difference and functional difficulties<br />

Participants described a range of physical health issues related to visible difference and<br />

functional ability that had led, in <strong>the</strong>ir view, to deterioration in <strong>the</strong>ir ability to adapt to visible<br />

difference. <strong>The</strong>se included: physical health issues, side effects, deterioration in physical health /<br />

function and likelihood of future health deterioration.<br />

“<strong>The</strong> pain, unable to walk, everything’s on that, if I could only I could walk better<br />

I'd be fantastic” (03/clin/1030/prosth)<br />

Discrimination at work<br />

“…..<strong>the</strong>y found a reason to make me redundant and I knew perfectly well I would<br />

not get ano<strong>the</strong>r job because every time I go for a job it's always good<br />

communication skills, and when <strong>the</strong>y see me I don’t even get a call back”<br />

(05/clin/1059/derm)<br />

.<br />

Trauma at time of acquiring a visible difference<br />

“<strong>the</strong>n I was taken down <strong>the</strong> week after to have all <strong>the</strong> packing taken out of my<br />

face, obviously I was nil by mouth and when <strong>the</strong>y brought me back on to <strong>the</strong> ward<br />

<strong>the</strong>y’d left <strong>the</strong> top half of my lip just dangling like a horses bit and <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> doctor<br />

handed me a mirror and I'd got my lip actually dangling in my mouth. So<br />

obviously I totally freaked out and it was just, I just felt as though I was being<br />

treated really cruel” (03/clin/1018/H&N)<br />

Stage 2 - Intervening cognitive processes – stable coping, stable distressed groups<br />

<strong>The</strong>se processes were divided into <strong>the</strong> areas of 1) schema/core beliefs, 2) cognitive appraisals<br />

and attributions and 3) self-appraisals and self-efficacy<br />

104


1. Schema / core beliefs<br />

Differences in self identified coping status were reflected in contrasting schemata and beliefs<br />

about <strong>the</strong> self and appearance.<br />

Core schema - representation of self / self-concept<br />

Stable coping group<br />

This group described stable, positive beliefs about self:<br />

“day to day you don’t think about it a lot, Its only like I said those photographs I<br />

didn’t particularly like.... but you don’t really realise things are different really”<br />

03/clin.1003/plast-fitz.<br />

“I was disfigured for life in a very visible part of my face so I mean <strong>the</strong> adjustment<br />

to that has been enormous and I am still going through <strong>the</strong> adjustment six years<br />

on I’m still going through <strong>the</strong> adjustment but its not a case where I’ve ever been<br />

afraid to show myself in public or anything like that I don’t sort of hate myself or<br />

hate <strong>the</strong> way I look” (01/clin/0073/las)<br />

<strong>The</strong> group’s <strong>report</strong>s also suggested low appearance valence (i.e. <strong>the</strong> visible difference was<br />

periphery to self-concept) and a self-concept comprising multiple dimensions, ra<strong>the</strong>r than<br />

focussing on appearance.<br />

“I’m an artistic fairly temperamental person, um, I enjoy socializing and,<br />

goodness! I can define myself by <strong>the</strong> things I enjoy doing I suppose, which is<br />

socializing and cook and doing art, listening to music” (01/clin/0017/eye)<br />

Stable distressed group<br />

By contrast, this group described stable negative beliefs about <strong>the</strong> self, and high appearance<br />

valence (<strong>the</strong> visible difference appeared more central to <strong>the</strong> self-concept) and concern with<br />

visible difference as a defining feature of self.<br />

.<br />

.<br />

“You know I'm just thinking that <strong>the</strong>y're really really lucky <strong>the</strong>y can just walk into<br />

sommat and <strong>the</strong>y’ll just be completely at ease where as I'd just be falling over<br />

myself all <strong>the</strong> time. Sort of trying to impress people and <strong>the</strong> more I try to do that<br />

bigger idiot I feel and look probably” (03/clin/1016/derm-barns)<br />

“it matters to me what people’s impression of me are and because I've got <strong>the</strong>se<br />

symptoms with tummy and eyes and leg that you know <strong>the</strong>y'd think I was<br />

something lesser of a person“ (03/clin/1037/prosth)<br />

105


Appearance schema - beliefs about overall appearance and visible difference<br />

Stable coping group<br />

Responses from this group suggested <strong>the</strong> ability to maintain positive beliefs about overall<br />

appearance, which accommodated visible differences. This occurred in a context of<br />

appearance schemata with multiple dimensions and within which visible difference was<br />

peripheral.<br />

.<br />

“I just don’t consider it to be a disfigurement” (05/clin/1047/ent-sw)<br />

“<strong>the</strong>re’s no option but to accept it and move on..and I think I can’t remove it. It’s<br />

me now. I can’t change it so live with it” (05/clin/1052/ent-sw)<br />

Stable distressed group<br />

This group were concerned about visible difference, which was more central to <strong>the</strong>ir view of<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir appearance, and held negative views about <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>y look. <strong>The</strong>y also held negative<br />

views about visible difference.<br />

“What, how it makes me feel? A lot of <strong>the</strong> times I wish my sister hadn’t saved me,<br />

a lot of <strong>the</strong> times I think to myself I wish <strong>the</strong>y'd taken it off. I mean like if I'm<br />

getting dressed to go out I'll stand in front of <strong>the</strong> mirror and I'll look at myself and<br />

think what would I look like if I was normal, why has it happened to me....”<br />

03/clin/1017/plast-rals<br />

“I feel ugly, I feel really really ugly. Just o<strong>the</strong>r people looking at me, how I feel in<br />

myself, it sort of like a self conscious thing” (03/clin/1016/derm-barns)<br />

.<br />

2. Cognitive appraisals & attributions<br />

Appraising events – o<strong>the</strong>rs reactions to self<br />

Stable coping group<br />

This group described external attributions for o<strong>the</strong>rs’ reactions to visible difference:<br />

“well it varies…you look different, also <strong>the</strong>y think your burns are disgusting as<br />

well, <strong>the</strong>y think somehow <strong>the</strong>y are contagious… and <strong>the</strong>y are just generally bigots<br />

mate but some are a bit slow, <strong>the</strong>y live in a boring little town” (01/clin/0048/pla)<br />

Stable distressed group<br />

106


This group tended to advance internal attributions for o<strong>the</strong>rs’ reactions to visible difference, and<br />

to personalise <strong>the</strong>se responses.<br />

“It's as though I'm making o<strong>the</strong>r people uncomfortable as well as me being<br />

uncomfortable ...” (03/clin/1016/derm-barns)<br />

Appraising self – in relation to o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

Stable coping group<br />

This group tended to make downward social comparisions with o<strong>the</strong>rs – both to o<strong>the</strong>r people<br />

with disfigurement and to o<strong>the</strong>r people more generally, emphasising that <strong>the</strong>y were often in a<br />

better position than o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

“<strong>the</strong>re’s always someone worse off than yourself, you see it all <strong>the</strong> time and you<br />

thank your lucky stars you’re not <strong>the</strong>re...you’ve got to do this” (05/clin/1047/entsw)<br />

Stable distressed group<br />

This group showed an idealised view of o<strong>the</strong>rs and those with no visible difference.<br />

“I think I've let it get on top of me and I think I'm weak for letting that happen<br />

because when you see o<strong>the</strong>r people on, you know if you look at o<strong>the</strong>r people on<br />

<strong>the</strong> telly or things like that, if you see o<strong>the</strong>r people on <strong>the</strong> telly or you hear stories<br />

about what o<strong>the</strong>r people have gone through you're thinking, I sometimes get<br />

angry with myself thinking you're an idiot, not pa<strong>the</strong>tic, you're just silly for letting it<br />

get on top of you, apart from my skin and my asthma, I've got o<strong>the</strong>r things, o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

health problems I've got, <strong>the</strong>y're not limiting, I think I've been limiting myself, you<br />

know” 03/clin/1016/derm-barns<br />

3. Appraising self – perceived efficacy to cope with appearance-related difficulties<br />

Stable coping group<br />

This group recognised <strong>the</strong>ir difficulties as ongoing and demanding but within <strong>the</strong>ir control. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

sought to overcome challenges, and recognised that positive adjustment required personal<br />

resources and taking an active role in coping, employing a range of behavioural and cognitive<br />

strategies. <strong>The</strong> concept of self-efficacy distinguishes well between this group and <strong>the</strong> stable<br />

distressed group. <strong>The</strong> latter group <strong>report</strong>ed less sense of <strong>the</strong>ir ability to successfully adapt to<br />

issues affecting <strong>the</strong>ir lives.<br />

107


“I’m in charge ...I think that’s <strong>the</strong> way I’ve lived my life. Get on with it and do <strong>the</strong><br />

best you can...sod em and get on with It you’ve got to be confident and feel<br />

invincible (in past job) so that has helped me cope with this. I’ve been on plenty<br />

of bad jobs so you have to make <strong>the</strong> most of it‘’ (05/clin/1047/ent-sw)<br />

“I can stand up and again I can get excited and enthusiastic about getting my<br />

point across, but it’s something that has helped me. So I think with work you<br />

actually build that confidence up, you have opportunities and you take those<br />

opportunities.... So through life you have to take opportunities you have to learn<br />

from experiences and move forward, um, but I don’t think you’re conscious of<br />

it....” (01/clin/0080/pla)<br />

“<strong>The</strong> important thing is that you come to terms with it as quickly as possible<br />

because <strong>the</strong> longer it goes on <strong>the</strong> worse it becomes...you can’t hide from it, it’s<br />

<strong>the</strong>re” (05/clin/1052/ent-sw)<br />

Stable distressed group<br />

This group did not <strong>report</strong> perceiving <strong>the</strong>mselves as able to change <strong>the</strong>ir situation, thoughts and<br />

feelings, but ra<strong>the</strong>r viewed <strong>the</strong>ir difficulties as pervasive, uncontrollable, and unmanageable.<br />

Interestingly, one participant <strong>report</strong>ed that constantly changing treatment and o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

circumstances were instrumental in preventing adjustment to <strong>the</strong>ir condition.<br />

“I think because it's been bad, my skin, it's been bad for so long, I think I've just<br />

come to accept that it's bad, I've been told that, well I've been told three things,<br />

one that I've been very unlucky, and <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r one, what was it, that I'll always<br />

have it and I've known since I was little that I'll always have it, one on my<br />

neighbours says oh you’ll grow out of it and I'm saying I'm still waiting to grow out<br />

of it and I think I've just come to accept that it's always going to be bad and that’s<br />

why I've let it get on top of me so I've accepted it in a silly way, you know, and I<br />

think that’s why I let it get on top of me and I let it affect me, you know social life<br />

and o<strong>the</strong>r things because I've accepted that it's bad so because it's bad I<br />

shouldn’t really, I shouldn’t go out, I shouldn’t mix much, not just go out”<br />

(03/clin/1016/derm-barns)<br />

Stage 2 - Intervening cognitive processes – changed improved group<br />

Where participants identified <strong>the</strong>mselves as having experienced an improvement in <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

adaptation, <strong>the</strong>y displayed similar characteristics to <strong>the</strong> stable coping group, in terms of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

core schema and appearance schema focus. <strong>The</strong> group <strong>report</strong>ed making downward social<br />

comparisons and offering external attributions to <strong>the</strong> reactions of o<strong>the</strong>rs to visible difference.<br />

Once again, a sense of self-efficacy was important to this group.<br />

108


Additionally, <strong>the</strong>y showed awareness of <strong>the</strong> role of psychological factors in mediating <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

visible difference issues. Specifically, <strong>the</strong>y <strong>report</strong>ed a need to understand oneself and how one’s<br />

thinking affects one’s behaviour and feelings.<br />

“Hm. I should have given it some more thought. I suppose it’s to do with, <strong>the</strong> only<br />

thing I can think of is, to do with how you value your identity, do you feel that<br />

you’re just a surface or do you feel that you have value as a person”<br />

(01/clin/0034/eye)<br />

Stage 2 – Intervening cognitive processes – changed deteriorated group<br />

This group shared features with <strong>the</strong> stable distressed group. Changes in <strong>the</strong>ir lives seemed to<br />

have provided triggers for disruption to <strong>the</strong> self-concept, sense of independence and sense of<br />

self-efficacy. Life changes also triggered an increase in salience of appearance, both in<br />

response to <strong>the</strong> reactions of o<strong>the</strong>rs and to an understanding that hopes of a ‘normal’<br />

appearance in <strong>the</strong> future might be unrealistic:<br />

“I was desperate, I went back to work pretty much having <strong>the</strong> operation and I<br />

said to <strong>the</strong> girls I'm desperate for a face, all I want is a bloody face and when I<br />

finally got it I was just so disappointed you know this doesn’t look like….”<br />

03/clin/1018/H&N<br />

“Because <strong>the</strong>y're grown up and haven’t seen, I grew up with kids who had polio<br />

and odd things wrong with <strong>the</strong>m but now <strong>the</strong>y won’t <strong>the</strong>y grow up in this super<br />

perfect world and anybody is imperfect is ei<strong>the</strong>r, you know your ei<strong>the</strong>r frightened<br />

of it or you just avoid just to get away. I had <strong>the</strong> extreme of when I went back to<br />

work, someone who wouldn’t even sit near me because she said I made her feel<br />

physically sick. That was <strong>the</strong> extreme” (05/clin/1059/derm)<br />

Stage 3 – Outcomes – stable coping well group, stable distressed group<br />

1. Behavioural outcomes<br />

Social interaction was crucial in all elements of outcome across <strong>the</strong> groups. Within this, <strong>the</strong><br />

ability to interact with o<strong>the</strong>rs, confront issues of visible difference and enlist support where<br />

appropriate distinguished between those who self-identified as being in <strong>the</strong> coping well or<br />

distressed groups.<br />

Stable well<br />

Participants in this group <strong>report</strong>ed pro-social behaviours such as explaining <strong>the</strong>ir visible<br />

difference and talking openly with o<strong>the</strong>rs about this and <strong>the</strong> associated issues:<br />

109


“Only when people don’t understand what I’m saying I mean its not <strong>the</strong>ir fault,<br />

unless I explain that I haven’t got a tongue anymore, most people are fine with<br />

me” (05/clin/1052/ent-sw)<br />

“...and somebody came up and said “what have you done” and I’m thinking you<br />

know me and <strong>the</strong> only thing that has changed is <strong>the</strong> scar <strong>the</strong>re...I said I’ve had<br />

<strong>the</strong> birthmark removed and “oh, right” like that. It’s like playing rugby and having a<br />

bit of a scar after a rugby match and coming to work and “what’s happened to<br />

you?” it doesn’t come across any more than I’ve banged myself, or when you’ve<br />

explained what it is... oh right...never noticed it” (01/clin/0080/pla)<br />

Part of this openness involved not hiding <strong>the</strong> visible difference from o<strong>the</strong>rs or avoiding situations<br />

where it might be seen:<br />

“I always go about like that , I don’t try to cover it up” (05/clin/1047/ent-sw)<br />

“I just started to wear <strong>the</strong>m, oh what <strong>the</strong> hell. <strong>The</strong> benefits it wearing a short<br />

sleeved shirt outside in <strong>the</strong> summer out-weighs any o<strong>the</strong>r sort of thought...”<br />

(05/clin/1052/ent-sw)<br />

“Yeah (laughs) that’s right, especially if you’re interested and you want to see her<br />

again and again and I was thinking it was stupid because you can’t hide an<br />

aspect from someone, <strong>the</strong>y have to see <strong>the</strong> whole picture haven’t <strong>the</strong>y, and I was<br />

trying to hide it from her” (01/clin/0073/las)<br />

.<br />

Stable distressed<br />

By contrast, this group were socially impaired and particularly characterized by avoidance<br />

tactics, both cognitively and behaviourally, in that <strong>the</strong>y would hide <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference from<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs, use distraction and avoidant thinking and deny or conceal <strong>the</strong>ir feelings.<br />

.<br />

.<br />

“I don’t seem to have <strong>the</strong> confidence to let o<strong>the</strong>r people see it if you know what I<br />

mean so consequently I just ei<strong>the</strong>r cover it up or you know don’t do it basically”<br />

(03/clin/1037/prosth)<br />

“As you can see I’m wearing short sleeves at <strong>the</strong> moment which I didn’t do<br />

before. I’m always covered up because I’m self-conscious and I hate people<br />

looking at me when my skins bad. So I just.. Actually before I went into hospital I<br />

didn’t leave <strong>the</strong> house for three weeks. I wouldn’t even go and visit friends I just<br />

felt so miserable. I didn’t want to do anything” (05/clin/1056/derm-brad)<br />

“Well I tend to keep myself as busy as I possibly can, I think it takes your mind off<br />

it. If I'd got, if I was to sit, and I know that’s going to get like that when I get older,<br />

110


.<br />

more infirm as <strong>the</strong>y say, <strong>the</strong>n I shall sit and probably think about it more but I do<br />

try to keep my mind on o<strong>the</strong>r things to take me off it” (03/clin/1037/prosth)<br />

“Well usually when I'm with friends and that or relatives you know you put a brave<br />

face on kind of thing and <strong>the</strong>n I can laugh about it, depending on how I feel in<br />

myself you know and <strong>the</strong>n o<strong>the</strong>r times you know it just gets to you”<br />

(03/clin/1037/prosth)<br />

Stage 3 – outcomes – changed improved group<br />

In this group, planning for change and acceptance of change were <strong>report</strong>ed.<br />

“We are...<strong>the</strong> house is on <strong>the</strong> market. I don’t know when whe<strong>the</strong>r it’s gonna sell<br />

but we are hoping to move up to North Wales to be close to Nigel’s family, and<br />

buy something small and that’s a very significant part of my rehabilitation, can I<br />

call it. This is just; this house is too big for us” (01/clin/0034/eye)<br />

.<br />

Visible difference was also seem as a catalyst for health related behavioural change:<br />

“Stopping smoking [after leg amputation]. I only ever stopped once before in my<br />

life for 8 months because I was a very heavy smoker which caused <strong>the</strong> loss of my<br />

leg actually”(03/clin/1047/prosth)<br />

Stage 3 – Outcomes – changed deteriorated group<br />

It was not possible to identify common <strong>the</strong>mes in behaviour change for this group. This is<br />

probably because this group was itself very heterogeneous, with changes occurring for a broad<br />

range of reasons.<br />

Comparisons between self-selected adjustment groups using data derived from <strong>the</strong><br />

questionnaires<br />

Stable coping well group<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were no significant differences between mean scores at time one and time two on <strong>the</strong><br />

HADS for this subset of participants (anxiety subscale Z = -1.37, p = 0.16, depression subscale<br />

Z = -1.55, p = 0.12), DAS (Z = -0.77, p = 0.44) or FNE (Z = -0.15, p = 0.87). RCI scores however<br />

showed significant improvement on <strong>the</strong> HADS (anxiety subscale n=2, depression subscale n=1)<br />

and FNE (n=2), as well as significant deterioration on <strong>the</strong> FNE (n=1).<br />

Despite having self selected as coping well, three participants scored within <strong>the</strong> clinical range<br />

for <strong>the</strong> HADS anxiety or depression subscale at time one and two. <strong>The</strong>re were also a wide<br />

range of scores for both DAS 24 (22-54) and FNE (21-59) measures suggesting variability in<br />

111


appearance anxiety and coping with some participants clearly not coping well as defined by<br />

questionnaire response.<br />

Stable distressed group<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were no significant differences between mean scores at time one and time two on <strong>the</strong><br />

HADS (anxiety subscale Z = -1.51, p = 0.13, depression subscale Z = -0.37, p = 0.70) DAS (Z =<br />

-0.36, p = 0.71) or FNE (Z = -0.44, p = 0.65), although RCI scores showed that two participants<br />

had experienced significant deterioration in appearance anxiety and coping (FNE n=1, HADS<br />

depression subscale n=1).<br />

As might be expected for those self-selected as distressed, three participants were within <strong>the</strong><br />

clinical range for <strong>the</strong> HADS anxiety or depression subscale at time one and two. DAS (range 60-<br />

75) scores were also higher than those <strong>report</strong>ed by <strong>the</strong> stable coping well group, although FNE<br />

scores (range 28-56) were similar.<br />

Changed improved<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were no significant differences between mean scores on <strong>the</strong> HADS (anxiety subscale Z =<br />

-1.12, p = 0.26, depression subscale Z = -1.45, p = 0.14), DAS (Z = -0.57, p = 0.56), or FNE (Z<br />

= -0.52, p = 0.59), but RCI scores showed a large significant improvement on <strong>the</strong> FNE for two<br />

participants. However, RCI scores also showed that more participants (n=3) had experienced a<br />

significant deterioration (FNE n=1, HADS depression subscale n=2) despite having self-selected<br />

as change improved. Three participants scored within <strong>the</strong> HADS (anxiety subscale n=1,<br />

depression subscale n=2) clinical range at time one and remained within <strong>the</strong> clinical range at<br />

time two.<br />

Changed deteriorated group<br />

Mean scores were higher on all measures at time two compared with time one, but <strong>the</strong>re were<br />

no significant differences on <strong>the</strong> HADS (anxiety subscale Z = -1.35, p = 0.17, depression<br />

subscale Z = -0.55, p = 0.58), DAS (Z = -0.44, p = 0.65), or FNE (Z = -1.82, p = 0.06). RCI<br />

scores however showed significant deterioration on <strong>the</strong> HADS for three participants (anxiety<br />

n=2 and depression subscale n= 3) all of whom had moved into <strong>the</strong> clinical range.<br />

Two participants scored within <strong>the</strong> clinical range for <strong>the</strong> HADS anxiety or depression subscale at<br />

time one and two and RCI scores suggested <strong>the</strong>re had been no significant change.<br />

Finally, Chi-Square analysis suggested no significant relationship between participants selfselected<br />

coping categories and <strong>the</strong>ir individual RCI scores for any measure.<br />

112


DISCUSSION<br />

Stage 1 – predisposing factors<br />

Life events had a considerable effect on <strong>the</strong> ability to adapt to visible difference in all groups.<br />

<strong>The</strong> reactions of o<strong>the</strong>rs to those with visible difference have been <strong>report</strong>ed in many studies, and<br />

this issue is repeated here, with particularly importance for those who coped less well.<br />

Stage 2 - Intervening cognitive processes<br />

Participants’ responses suggest important differences in cognitive processes between <strong>the</strong><br />

coping well and distressed groups, which are similar across stable and changed groups.<br />

Participants in <strong>the</strong> coping groups gave responses consistent with more flexible core and body<br />

appearance schemas and less emphasis on physical appearance and visible difference. <strong>The</strong>ir<br />

responses suggested greater external attribution in <strong>the</strong>ir responses to o<strong>the</strong>rs and greater sense<br />

of self-efficacy.<br />

Stage 3 – Outcomes<br />

Participants in <strong>the</strong> two groups were distinguishable by differing behaviours, with <strong>the</strong> coping<br />

group being more likely to be engaged socially and to <strong>report</strong> behaviours and attitudes<br />

inconsistent with cognitive and behavioural avoidance of issues concerning visible difference.<br />

Discrepancy between self-selected category and questionnaire results<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were striking variations in <strong>the</strong> pictures of adjustment over time offered by participants’<br />

scores on <strong>the</strong> standardised measures and <strong>the</strong>ir self rated adjustment status over time. More<br />

research is needed to interpret <strong>the</strong> reasons for this discrepancy.<br />

However, <strong>the</strong> results highlight <strong>the</strong> need to take account of subjective perceptions of adjustment.<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, this study offers fur<strong>the</strong>r vindication of <strong>the</strong> need for mixed methods studies.<br />

CONCLUSION<br />

<strong>The</strong>se findings, taken in <strong>the</strong> context of <strong>the</strong> previous studies, have clear implications for our<br />

understanding of adaptation to visible difference, and, by extension, for potential strategies to<br />

help those whose adaptation is absent or incomplete. Such intervention could usefully build on<br />

<strong>the</strong> strategies exemplified by people in <strong>the</strong> coping well group here, through emphasis on<br />

addressing issues with regard to core beliefs around <strong>the</strong> role of appearance in its contribution to<br />

<strong>the</strong> self system, <strong>the</strong> importance of behavioural experimentation in mediating and changing such<br />

beliefs, and <strong>the</strong> role of social perception and interaction in mediating successful adaptation. <strong>The</strong><br />

influence of life changes in mediating <strong>the</strong> course of adaptation requires fur<strong>the</strong>r investigation.<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, any intervention should be evaluated with a mixed methods approach.<br />

113


114


FOLLOW-ON STUDIES<br />

STUDIES 5-13<br />

As outlined in <strong>the</strong> section on <strong>the</strong> design considerations for this research programme, this<br />

collaboration adopted a mixed methods approach in addressing <strong>the</strong> question of which<br />

psychosocial factors contribute to adjustment to disfiguring conditions, with <strong>the</strong> aim of delivering<br />

informative, balanced and useful research results. To complement <strong>the</strong> quantitative data from<br />

Studies I and 3, a series of follow-on studies were designed using a range of predominantly<br />

qualitative methodologies.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se studies were developed in order to expand understanding of <strong>the</strong> overall research<br />

framework, (Study 5: A Qualitative Study of <strong>the</strong> Experiences of People who Identify<br />

<strong>The</strong>mselves as Having Adjusted Positively to a Visible Difference), or particular elements<br />

of <strong>the</strong> framework that have been under-researched in <strong>the</strong> past, such as <strong>the</strong> impact of ethnic<br />

group membership (Study 6: Focus Groups of BME Community Views to Facial<br />

Disfigurement and Visible Differences; Study 7: A Qualitative Study Exploring he<br />

Experiences of UK South Asians Living with vitiligo), <strong>the</strong> role of social comparison<br />

processes (Study 8: Social Comparison Processes), and appearance concerns and<br />

aggression (Study 9: Appearance Concern, Hostility & Social Situations).<br />

Condition specific population groups about whom little research exists were also targeted as<br />

being worthy of exploratory study. <strong>The</strong> Advisory Group encouraged <strong>the</strong> collaboration to<br />

consider <strong>the</strong> impact of appearance concerns on intimate situations (Study 10: Developing a<br />

Scale to Measure <strong>the</strong> Impact of Appearance Concerns on Intimate Behaviours), and also<br />

highlighted <strong>the</strong> lack of knowledge and understanding about <strong>the</strong> psychosocial consequences of<br />

disfigurement and appearance concerns amongst general practitioners – <strong>the</strong> primary<br />

gatekeepers to services (Study 13: Working with Patients with Visible Differences: General<br />

Practitioners’ Beliefs, Decision Making Processes and Training Needs). Patients seeking<br />

treatment for arthritis have commented to several of <strong>the</strong> authors that <strong>the</strong>ir appearance concerns<br />

are rarely addressed in <strong>the</strong> health care system (Study 12: In <strong>the</strong> Context of Rheumatoid<br />

Arthritis, Does Appearance Matter?). <strong>The</strong> role of <strong>the</strong> combination of functional needs and<br />

appearance concerns in people with pros<strong>the</strong>sis has been alluded to by researchers in <strong>the</strong> past,<br />

yet remains under-researched (Study 11: Women with Limb Pros<strong>the</strong>ses: Experiences and<br />

Adjustment). <strong>The</strong> follow-on studies also reflected <strong>the</strong> personal research interests of <strong>the</strong><br />

members of <strong>the</strong> collaboration.<br />

115


116


STUDY 5<br />

A QUALITATIVE STUDY OF THE EXPERIENCES OF PEOPLE<br />

WHO IDENTIFY THEMSELVES AS<br />

HAVING ADJUSTED POSITIVELY TO A VISIBLE DIFFERENCE<br />

Study leads: Diana Harcourt & Nichola Rumsey. <strong>The</strong> focus groups were conducted and<br />

analysed by Katie Egan<br />

117


SUMMARY<br />

• This study aimed to explore <strong>the</strong> experiences of people <strong>report</strong>ing positive consequences<br />

of having a visible difference and identifying <strong>the</strong>mselves as having positively adjusted to<br />

living with an appearance that differs from <strong>the</strong> norm.<br />

• Focus groups and interviews were conducted with 12 participants with a variety of visible<br />

differences.<br />

• Data were analysed using inductive <strong>the</strong>matic analysis.<br />

• Four main <strong>the</strong>mes emerged from <strong>the</strong> data: appearance, personal growth, relationships<br />

with o<strong>the</strong>rs, and coping strategies.<br />

• Resilience, determination, positivity, downward comparisons, social support, spirituality,<br />

humour, taking things day-by-day, taking responsibility, distraction, and helping o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

were seen as paramount to positive adjustment.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> findings provide insight into <strong>the</strong> behaviours and personal outlooks that may<br />

contribute to adaptive coping and have significant implications for future research and<br />

intervention strategies aimed to help those who exhibit poor adjustment to visible<br />

difference.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> use of focus groups in this study suggests <strong>the</strong>se can be a fruitful, yet logistically<br />

challenging, approach for fur<strong>the</strong>r research.<br />

118


BACKGROUND<br />

To date, <strong>the</strong> majority of research in <strong>the</strong> field of visible difference has focused on <strong>the</strong> experiences<br />

of people who <strong>report</strong> having problems such as difficulties in social situations, and on negative<br />

outcomes, such as anxiety and depression. However, many people living with a visible<br />

difference do not <strong>report</strong> such problems. For example, whilst 30-50% of participants in a study of<br />

650 people with a visible difference <strong>report</strong>ed significant levels of distress (Rumsey et al 2004),<br />

<strong>the</strong> majority (50-70%) did not. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, more than 70% of respondents to a survey by <strong>the</strong><br />

Cleft Lip and Palate Association <strong>report</strong>ed positive consequences of having <strong>the</strong>ir cleft. Moreover,<br />

Thompson et al. (2002) <strong>report</strong>ed that some people with vitiligo spontaneously developed coping<br />

strategies not unlike those used within treatment. More, recently, Saradijian et al. (2008)<br />

<strong>report</strong>ed that some people using a pros<strong>the</strong>sis were using a variety of strategies to minimise <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

distress. Faulkner and Thompson (2006) purposively selected people whom identified<br />

<strong>the</strong>mselves as coping well with a disfigurement and found a range of naturalistic coping<br />

strategies being used.<br />

Clearly many people do adapt positively to <strong>the</strong> challenges <strong>the</strong>y face, and succeed in minimising<br />

<strong>the</strong> impact of <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference (Rumsey, 2002). It has been suggested (Eisermann, 2001)<br />

that it is unethical to focus on pathology and not to consider positive aspects and outcomes of<br />

visible difference. Whilst some research has examined positive aspects of chronic illness<br />

including benefit finding and adversarial growth (e.g. Petrie et al, 1999; Sodergren & Hyland,<br />

2000; Sodergren, Hyland, Crawford & Partridge, 2004), <strong>the</strong>re is currently very limited research<br />

focussing on visible difference. That which has been conducted has shown that humour, a good<br />

sense of self, family support and faith all contributed to resilience and success amongst<br />

individuals with Moebius syndrome (Meyerson, 2001) and inner strength and a valued social<br />

circle were identified in those with a craniofacial difference (Eiserman, 2001). <strong>The</strong>se studies,<br />

and o<strong>the</strong>rs (e.g. Fortune et al, 2005) have focussed on specific conditions as opposed to visible<br />

difference more generally.<br />

<strong>The</strong> current study was conducted with people with a variety of visible differences. It aimed:<br />

• To investigate positive consequences of living with a visible difference. Specifically, what<br />

are <strong>the</strong>y? Why are <strong>the</strong>y considered positive and what facilitates <strong>the</strong>m?<br />

• To inform <strong>the</strong> development of psychosocial interventions intended for those individuals<br />

who <strong>report</strong> problems associated with having a visible difference.<br />

METHOD<br />

A review of available quantitative measures (e.g. <strong>The</strong> Silver Linings Questionnaire; Satisfaction<br />

with Illness Scale) failed to identify any standardised scales that would give a sufficiently indepth<br />

understanding of <strong>the</strong> issues relevant to people with a visible difference. Instead, a<br />

qualitative methodology was felt more appropriate to capture rich, personal accounts of<br />

participants’ positive experiences and perceptions of living with a visible difference.<br />

119


A focus group methodology was chosen since <strong>the</strong>se groups aim to encourage participants to<br />

evoke memories and attitudes from each o<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>reby providing a richness of data not<br />

possible from one-to-one interviews (Frith 2000). Data collection was approached<br />

pragmatically. Originally it was intended that data collection would be via focus groups alone;<br />

however, due to logistical difficulties, some individual interviews were conducted so as not to<br />

exclude any willing participants who were unable to attend focus group meetings.<br />

Participant Characteristics & Recruitment<br />

All participants had a self-<strong>report</strong>ed visible difference of some kind, had taken part in <strong>the</strong> large<br />

cross-sectional study (study 1), and had indicated that <strong>the</strong>y felt that <strong>the</strong>y had adjusted well to<br />

living with a visible difference and would be interested in taking part in fur<strong>the</strong>r research.<br />

Thirty-eight potential participants were sent details of <strong>the</strong> research. <strong>The</strong> final sample consisted<br />

of 12 participants (9 females) aged between 31 and 80 years (mean age 56.7 years). Reported<br />

visible differences were: psoriasis, port wine stains, scarring, amputations, burn injuries, impact<br />

of thyroid eye disease, mastectomy, alopecia and altered hand or nose.<br />

Procedure<br />

Three focus groups and 4 interviews were conducted by <strong>the</strong> researcher (KE), using an interview<br />

schedule based on previous literature. Each lasted approximately one hour and was audiorecorded<br />

with permission. <strong>The</strong> audio recordings were transcribed verbatim.<br />

Analysis<br />

Transcripts were analysed using an inductive <strong>the</strong>matic analysis (Braun & Clarke 2006). Due to<br />

differences in <strong>the</strong> data collection methodology, focus groups and interviews were initially<br />

analysed separately and <strong>the</strong>n collectively. <strong>The</strong>mes were verified by ano<strong>the</strong>r researcher to check<br />

<strong>the</strong> credibility of <strong>the</strong> analysis and a summary of findings was sent to participants who were<br />

invited to feed back on <strong>the</strong> results.<br />

RESULTS<br />

Four main <strong>the</strong>mes emerged from <strong>the</strong> analysis: importance of appearance, personal growth,<br />

relationships with o<strong>the</strong>rs, and coping (which consisted of 7 sub-<strong>the</strong>mes).<br />

1. Importance of appearance<br />

Participants described how <strong>the</strong>y place little significance on appearance and value o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

characteristics and qualities more highly. Many participants made reference to <strong>the</strong> overimportance<br />

attached to appearance in society and made specific reference to <strong>the</strong> media and<br />

cosmetic surgery. <strong>The</strong>y also discussed trying to overcome <strong>the</strong> stigma associated with being<br />

visibly different by talking about it openly and not covering up.<br />

120


“if you are just yourself with it, you are not kind of hiding away or being<br />

embarrassed by it, if you can just be confident in yourself… <strong>the</strong>n maybe it’s<br />

showing something.” Jane<br />

Some participants did not perceive o<strong>the</strong>rs’ reactions to <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference to be consistently<br />

negative and this interpretation of o<strong>the</strong>r people’s behaviour helped <strong>the</strong>m to be more confident<br />

with <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference.<br />

"people don’t necessarily mock you… people do rate you for what you are and<br />

not necessarily for what you look like " Edna<br />

2. Personal growth<br />

Participants felt <strong>the</strong>y had changed in numerous positive ways as a result of living with a visible<br />

difference, including becoming more resourceful, more resilient, having a calmer approach to<br />

daily life, and taking up new interests and opportunities (e.g. choosing career paths which<br />

centred around helping o<strong>the</strong>rs, meeting inspiring people who <strong>the</strong>y would not have met<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rwise, and finding new religious beliefs). Personal growth extended to improved<br />

relationships with, and understanding of, o<strong>the</strong>r people. Some participants also spoke of how<br />

<strong>the</strong>y hoped <strong>the</strong>y had helped o<strong>the</strong>r people with a visible difference by sharing <strong>the</strong>ir experiences<br />

of how <strong>the</strong>y had coped well.<br />

"it has made me more resilient" Jackie<br />

"I’ve been able to say [to people with similar problems] ‘well yeh I know exactly<br />

what you are going through but it does alter.’ And I think it helps for people to<br />

know” Anne<br />

3. Relationships with o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

Many participants talked about how it was helpful that friends and families treated <strong>the</strong>m ‘no<br />

differently’ because of <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference. O<strong>the</strong>rs appreciated <strong>the</strong> consideration and support<br />

<strong>the</strong>y had received, especially in difficult times. Some discussed how a lack of family support at<br />

a young age had proven to be a positive experience since it had increased <strong>the</strong>ir sense of<br />

independence:<br />

4. Coping<br />

“I learnt to kind of deal with stuff myself. I didn’t really tell my family when I was<br />

growing up…I was quite independent” Jane<br />

When asked ‘How did you deal with difficult times?’, participants explained how <strong>the</strong>y had<br />

chosen to confront and embrace <strong>the</strong> difficulties presented to <strong>the</strong>m in life and to make <strong>the</strong> best of<br />

121


any situation. Strategies involved acceptance, positivity, a sense of humour, determination, and<br />

dealing with things on a daily basis.<br />

“I’ve just learned to accept that you know I can’t change it, <strong>the</strong>re’s no surgery to<br />

get rid of it...don’t hide it, just get out <strong>the</strong>re and enjoy your life and basically be<br />

positive” Mike<br />

“If I am having a bad day…all I need to is keep reminding myself that...its just a<br />

bad day and know that it is going to change” Jane<br />

Participants also described putting <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference in context by placing greater<br />

significance on o<strong>the</strong>r aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir lives and not seeing it as <strong>the</strong> cause of o<strong>the</strong>r difficulties in<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir lives:<br />

“It would be easy to hang every bad day on your disfigurement… Whereas<br />

actually it could be a multitude of o<strong>the</strong>r things that are causing it” Eleanor<br />

Participants frequently <strong>report</strong>ed making downward social comparisons as a means of increasing<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir sense of well-being and bolstering <strong>the</strong>ir self esteem,<br />

“I’m actually quite lucky because I’ve got something that can be quite awful in<br />

some people, so I actually feel quite fortunate” Nick<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r techniques included distraction, practical solutions, giving <strong>the</strong>mselves treats and focusing<br />

on aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir appearance (o<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference) that <strong>the</strong>y felt <strong>the</strong>y did have<br />

some control over:<br />

“it’s encouraged me to put a bit of a higher priority on trying to take care of<br />

whatever physical assets I’ve got…, I try and keep fit and everything, I run a lot. I<br />

think I probably…going back to my hair, having kept all my hair…probably err try<br />

and look after <strong>the</strong> things that are all right, you know.” Nick<br />

Some explained that <strong>the</strong>ir ability to cope with <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference had enabled <strong>the</strong>m to<br />

develop effective coping strategies <strong>the</strong>y could apply to o<strong>the</strong>r situations and difficulties<br />

encountered in <strong>the</strong>ir lives.<br />

"Could I have coped with it [chronic pain] if I hadn’t had this [burns] already, I<br />

don’t know? You’re able to deal with problems better" Mike<br />

Some participants identified how having, or finding, a sense of spirituality and faith had been<br />

important. Several referred to not questioning why <strong>the</strong>y had <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference.<br />

“To my recollection I’ve never said ‘why me?’ Never, you know. ‘Why not me’<br />

more <strong>the</strong> case” Eleanor<br />

122


Finally, participants spoke very positively about <strong>the</strong> benefits of support, in particular being able<br />

to talk to someone who had shared similar experiences. Having found this beneficial had<br />

prompted <strong>the</strong>m to share <strong>the</strong>ir own positive experiences with o<strong>the</strong>rs (see above).<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

<strong>The</strong>se qualitative data offer insight into <strong>the</strong> experiences of adults who perceive <strong>the</strong>ir visible<br />

difference to have led to positive consequences, including becoming resourceful, more resilient,<br />

being able to cope with o<strong>the</strong>r challenges in <strong>the</strong>ir lives and having a different outlook on life.<br />

<strong>The</strong> findings support previous research within <strong>the</strong> visible difference and wider health psychology<br />

literature. Specifically, that positive consequences including resilience, strength and an<br />

increased sense of well-being stemmed from: placing less importance on appearance, positive<br />

perceptions of social support (Rumsey et al., 2004); humour and determination (Meyerson<br />

2001); benefits of positive emotions (Tugade and Fredrickson 2004); downward social<br />

comparisons (Festinger 1954); searching for meaning (Taylor 1984); being open to faith,<br />

spirituality and religion (Sodergren and Hyland 2000; Meyerson 2001); and helping o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

(Roberts et al 1999). <strong>The</strong> emphasis on cognitive processing is striking and is consistent with <strong>the</strong><br />

findings of Study 1.<br />

Many participants displayed attitudes of acceptance, choosing not to question why <strong>the</strong>y had<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir visible difference. <strong>The</strong>y conveyed a sense that everyone is susceptible to adversity and<br />

<strong>the</strong>y did not feel <strong>the</strong>y should be any different.<br />

Whilst all participants in this study felt that <strong>the</strong>y now accepted <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference, this had<br />

not always been <strong>the</strong> case. <strong>The</strong>ir accounts demonstrate how experiences of a visible difference<br />

can change over time and have illustrated a variety of cognitions and behaviours that <strong>the</strong>y<br />

perceive to have played a part in <strong>the</strong>ir adjustment over time.<br />

Implications for provision of care<br />

<strong>The</strong> findings of this study can contribute to informing <strong>the</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>r development of interventions,<br />

for example, <strong>the</strong> need to focus on cognitive processing, including strategies to encourage<br />

individuals to identify positive consequences and not to attribute o<strong>the</strong>r problems to <strong>the</strong>ir visible<br />

difference.<br />

Implications for research<br />

Focus groups produced rich data which benefited from participants’ interaction with one ano<strong>the</strong>r<br />

but it is important to acknowledge <strong>the</strong> logistical difficulties encountered, which necessitated<br />

some individual interviews being carried out and resulted in a relatively small sample size.<br />

123


CONCLUSIONS<br />

Whereas previous research has examined positive aspects of disfigurement in particular<br />

conditions, <strong>the</strong> current study included individuals with a variety of visible differences and<br />

highlighted <strong>the</strong> similarity in positive consequences and effective coping and adjustment across<br />

visible differences. It illustrated that focus groups may be a fruitful methodology for research in<br />

this area and also highlighted <strong>the</strong> utility of taking a positive as opposed to problem-focussed<br />

approach. <strong>The</strong> current study provided insight into a broad range of cognitive mechanisms<br />

which people utilise or develop in order to foster positive adjustment to <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se findings could usefully inform interventions and fur<strong>the</strong>r research aiming to help<br />

individuals who experience difficulty adjusting to a visible difference.<br />

124


STUDY 6<br />

FOCUS GROUP STUDIES OF BME COMMUNITY VIEWS OF<br />

FACIAL DISFIGUREMENT AND VISIBLE DIFFERENCES<br />

Study leads: Rodger Charlton, Krysia Saul, Mark Johnson & Nichola Rumsey<br />

Authors of this brief <strong>report</strong> and data analysts: Heidi Williamson, Jenifer Hughes, Habib Naqvi &<br />

Emma Williams<br />

We would like to acknowledge help in planning and implementing from Daya Singh,<br />

Gurnam Singh, Rukhsana Bibi, Akm Kamruzzaman, Fatima Marzia, Mahomed Saleh &<br />

Rita Saujani.<br />

125


SUMMARY<br />

• Nine focus group interviews were conducted with sixty-three participants representing<br />

four South Asian communities on <strong>the</strong> topic of facial disfigurement and visible difference.<br />

• A number of <strong>the</strong>mes were raised including definitions of disfigurement, cause of visible<br />

differences, <strong>the</strong> role of <strong>the</strong> family, reactions from o<strong>the</strong>rs, social exposure, cultural<br />

differences and medical interventions.<br />

• Religious beliefs and cultural practices cut across <strong>the</strong> majority of <strong>the</strong>mes that emerged.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y were associated with views about <strong>the</strong> cause and origin of <strong>the</strong> visible difference as<br />

well as <strong>the</strong> consequences of that difference.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> focus group interviews highlighted <strong>the</strong> need to implement interventions to reduce<br />

<strong>the</strong> negative aspects of visible difference and disfigurement. <strong>The</strong>se included raising<br />

knowledge and understanding of <strong>the</strong> difference amongst <strong>the</strong> general (minority ethnic)<br />

populations as well as improving <strong>the</strong> lived experience of those living with <strong>the</strong> difference<br />

and <strong>the</strong>ir family.<br />

126


INTRODUCTION<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is a paucity of research exploring <strong>the</strong> views of different cultural and ethnic groups<br />

concerning individuals with visible differences. As few standardised appearance measures have<br />

been translated, most appearance-related research is <strong>the</strong>refore conducted in western cultures<br />

(USA and UK) with individuals who have a good command of <strong>the</strong> English language.<br />

People of South Asian descent, i.e. those ethnic groups originating from <strong>the</strong> Indian<br />

subcontinent, constitute one of <strong>the</strong> largest ethnic minority groups in <strong>the</strong> UK, encompassing 4 per<br />

cent of <strong>the</strong> total population. It is important to be aware that this group is diverse, as it includes<br />

people who speak different languages, possess varying literacy rates, have different religious<br />

beliefs and who are from different cultural heritages, <strong>the</strong>refore <strong>the</strong> heterogeneity of this group<br />

cannot be emphasised strongly enough.<br />

Exploring <strong>the</strong> views of <strong>the</strong> South Asian populations regarding visible differences is a priority,<br />

because issues of disfigurement, stigma and shame may be particularly bound to membership<br />

of specific cultural and ethnic groupings (Papadopoulos et al., 1999).<br />

Accordingly, this study was designed to examine <strong>the</strong> views of members of UK South Asian<br />

communities towards individuals with visible differences<br />

METHOD<br />

Sixty three participants from <strong>the</strong> West Midlands were recruited to 9 focus groups representing<br />

four South Asian communities: Indian (Gujarati) Hindu, Indian (Punjabi) Sikh, Bangladeshi<br />

Muslim and Pakistani Muslim. Groups were formed with attention to age, gender and ethnicity.<br />

In order that questions and topics were culturally relevant, facilitators were chosen on <strong>the</strong> basis<br />

of: relevant language skills; gender; identification with, and knowledge of <strong>the</strong> communities<br />

(Culley, Hudson and Rapport, 2007).<br />

Discussions were held in <strong>the</strong> language of <strong>the</strong> group and translated into English for data<br />

analysis. Transcripts were analysed by a team of four analysts using template analysis (Kent,<br />

2000).<br />

127


RESULTS<br />

Eight key <strong>the</strong>mes were identified; <strong>the</strong>se incorporated various inter-related sub-<strong>the</strong>mes.<br />

1. Definitions<br />

A number of sub <strong>the</strong>mes were associated with how disfigurement and beauty are described.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was also evidence of a misinformed association of visible difference with mental and / or<br />

physical disability.<br />

“you always make <strong>the</strong> assumption that <strong>the</strong>y are not as capable as normal people”<br />

(Bengali young female)<br />

1a. Disfigurement<br />

Definitions ranged from ‘ugly’ to any abnormality or unevenness in <strong>the</strong> colour, shape or features<br />

of <strong>the</strong> face and included scarring and amputation. <strong>The</strong>re was some acknowledgement that<br />

<strong>the</strong>se differences could be ei<strong>the</strong>r congenital or acquired.<br />

1b. Beauty Ideals<br />

Fair, clear skin, symmetrical and proportioned features, a tall and slim physique and long hair<br />

were associated with female beauty. Male participants valued facial hair. Although <strong>the</strong> emphasis<br />

was on physical facial aspects, <strong>the</strong>re was some acknowledgement that clo<strong>the</strong>s can be<br />

important, and that beauty can come from within and is a subjective assessment.<br />

“In our culture beauty is depends on how beautiful someone’s face is” (Bengali<br />

older male)<br />

1c. First Impressions<br />

Physical appearance (focusing on <strong>the</strong> face, body posture and function) and dress are <strong>the</strong> first<br />

things noticed when meeting strangers.<br />

1d. Disfigurement associated with intellectual and physical disability or a ‘poor’<br />

character<br />

Participants frequently felt that disfigurement or visible differences were associated with or<br />

indicative of mental and/or physical disability, or indicated ‘poor’ character: <strong>the</strong> latter particularly<br />

so if <strong>the</strong> visible differences were acquired during a fight or accident.<br />

“if <strong>the</strong>ir appearance is not normal you can surmise that <strong>the</strong>y are not well”’ (Sikh<br />

older male)<br />

128


2. Causation<br />

“you always make <strong>the</strong> assumption that <strong>the</strong>y are not as capable as normal<br />

people”(Bengali young female)<br />

“if some have disfigured face, this person is not normal mentally” (Bengali older<br />

male)<br />

“(if) due to an accident or fight people may blame him for bad character” (Bengali<br />

young male)<br />

A number of explanations were offered to elucidate <strong>the</strong> origins of visible differences in people. In<br />

most cases <strong>the</strong>se explanations had <strong>the</strong>ir foundations within religious belief or cultural myth.<br />

2a. Religion<br />

A general fatalistic acceptance that disfigurement is <strong>the</strong> will of <strong>the</strong> creator and sustainer of <strong>the</strong><br />

world and should be accepted as such was <strong>the</strong> predominant causal explanation of visible<br />

difference that cut across <strong>the</strong> different religious beliefs.<br />

“This was God’s way of punishing you” (Bengali young female)<br />

“This is Allah’s wish” (Bengali young male)<br />

2b. Individual Culpability<br />

<strong>The</strong> individual’s, or <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s past or current actions, were held to be responsible for<br />

disfigurement. This includes Karma (action/attitude in previous life), visible differences due to an<br />

accident or fighting, and blame attributed to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r.<br />

“this was God’s way of punishing you” (Bengali young female)<br />

“that child is paying for <strong>the</strong>ir karma” (Sikh older male)<br />

“<strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>rs along with <strong>the</strong> in-laws blame <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r” (Bengali older female)<br />

2d. Myths/Superstition<br />

<strong>The</strong> origins of disfigurement were in some cases also attributed to deeply held beliefs that can<br />

be classified as cultural myths. For example, visible difference was attributed to engaging in<br />

sexual activity in pregnancy, getting pregnant during a lunar eclipse, or making fun of someone<br />

disfigured. <strong>The</strong> superstitious avoidance of people with a visible difference just in case <strong>the</strong><br />

‘condition’ was infectious was also highlighted.<br />

129


2e. Medical Interpretation<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was an understanding that some visible differences are related to genetic inheritance.<br />

3. Family<br />

This overarching <strong>the</strong>me encompasses a number of sub <strong>the</strong>mes linked to <strong>the</strong> family’s attitude<br />

towards, and management of, an individual (normally a child) with a disfigurement. <strong>The</strong>re was<br />

an overall sense of burden and shame, and evidence that visible differences can impact on<br />

marriageability.<br />

3a. Shame<br />

“parents would be worried about <strong>the</strong>ir child’s marriage” (Pakistani young female)<br />

“we feel conscious about taking <strong>the</strong>m into <strong>the</strong> public and being ridiculed, so for<br />

that reason we would not take <strong>the</strong>m out much” (Sikh older male)<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was evidence of <strong>the</strong> family’s desire to limit <strong>the</strong> amount of social exposure of <strong>the</strong> individual<br />

and evidence that having a family member with a visible difference could be a source of<br />

embarrassment. However - <strong>the</strong>re is no shame if <strong>the</strong> disfigurement was acquired through noble<br />

causes.<br />

3b. Burden<br />

“<strong>the</strong>y tend to hide <strong>the</strong>m away because <strong>the</strong>y see it as kind of a shameful and<br />

embarrassing thing” (Bengali young female)<br />

Many in <strong>the</strong> focus groups expressed a sense of responsibility (sometimes a feeling of burden) to<br />

care for an individual with a disfigurement.<br />

3c. Marriage<br />

“you could get frustrated at times and think it’s a burden…you have to come back<br />

early to take care of your bro<strong>the</strong>r or sister whose disfigured”’ (Sikh young mixed<br />

gender)<br />

“If a boy is disfigured but physically able to earn <strong>the</strong>n he is more accepted in <strong>the</strong><br />

family” (Bengali older female)<br />

Disfigurement was thought to have a negative impact on marriage prospects (especially those<br />

of females) and attempts may be made to match people who have disfigurements. Some<br />

expressed a community concern that disfigurements can be inherited.<br />

130


“it’s not your partner you are choosing but also <strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>r or mo<strong>the</strong>r of your child”<br />

(Bengali young female)<br />

“I think parents would be worried about <strong>the</strong>ir child’s marriage if <strong>the</strong>ir child was<br />

disfigured” (Pakistani young female)<br />

4. Reactions to visible differences<br />

Groups described <strong>the</strong>ir reactions (pity, fear, shock and sorrow) to individuals with visible<br />

differences and expressed a variety of terms <strong>the</strong>y may use in <strong>the</strong>ir own language to describe<br />

<strong>the</strong>m.<br />

“Some times we feel sorry for <strong>the</strong>se people” (Bengali older male)<br />

“By looking at a disfigured person, we get shocked.” (Bengali older female)<br />

4a. Encouragement to correct disfigurement<br />

Individuals may be pressed to seek medical attention and this pressure should be accepted as<br />

well-meaning.<br />

“I would advise <strong>the</strong>m to get medical treatment, it is our duty to do so” (Sikh older<br />

male)<br />

4b. Effects on <strong>the</strong> individual<br />

It was felt by <strong>the</strong> groups that an individual may feel marginalised, lonely and have low self<br />

esteem.<br />

“<strong>The</strong>y will lack confidence. <strong>The</strong>y might be isolated by o<strong>the</strong>rs but <strong>the</strong>y will also<br />

isolate <strong>the</strong>mselves” (Pakistani young female)<br />

4c. Use of Stigmatising Language<br />

One group used specific names for individuals with visible differences.<br />

‘Teerah: cross-eyed. Dabh-kraba: scar or uneven’ (Sikh older male)<br />

4d. Acts of Overcompensation<br />

Some groups stated that individuals should be protected from harm and that <strong>the</strong>y pitied <strong>the</strong>m.<br />

Sewa (an Asian concept of social duty and worthy action) was also mentioned.<br />

131


“I think I am nicer to a person who has got a disability because in a way you<br />

sympathise with <strong>the</strong>m” (Bengali young female)<br />

“<strong>the</strong> only reason that <strong>the</strong>y want to marry her is because <strong>the</strong>y want to do her<br />

sewa” (Sikh young mixed gender)<br />

5. Social Exposure<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are several ways in which o<strong>the</strong>rs (<strong>the</strong> family) or <strong>the</strong> individual with a disfigurement may<br />

limit <strong>the</strong>ir public exposure. Religious events and employment opportunities were two key areas<br />

discussed.<br />

5a. Family limiting<br />

Family may limit <strong>the</strong> public exposure of an individual with a visible difference as a form of<br />

protection, but also due to shame<br />

“we would not take <strong>the</strong>m out much because people would stare” (Sikh older<br />

male)<br />

5b. Self-Limiting<br />

An individual with a visible difference may feel pressured by society to keep <strong>the</strong>mselves out of<br />

<strong>the</strong> public eye.<br />

“I think o<strong>the</strong>r people would make it difficult for <strong>the</strong>m (to socialise). You’ll never<br />

get someone with a disfigurement on a stage” (Pakistani young female)<br />

5c. Religious events<br />

It was viewed as acceptable to expose a disfigurement if attending religious ceremonies.<br />

“We tend to be soft hearted in religious events. So people are more<br />

broadminded.” (Bengali older female)<br />

“Religious events will not affect <strong>the</strong>m as much, in places like that people would<br />

be more sympa<strong>the</strong>tic” (Bengali young female)<br />

5d. Employment<br />

Participants believed <strong>the</strong>re are restricted opportunities for individuals with a visible difference.<br />

“It will be difficult for <strong>the</strong>m to get a job.” (Bengali young male)<br />

132


5e. Concealment & camouflage<br />

Traditional clothing and make-up may be used to conceal a disfigurement.<br />

“Some times he covers his face and head to <strong>the</strong> public places” (Bengali older<br />

male)<br />

6. Cultural differences<br />

A number of differences between <strong>the</strong> South Asian and <strong>the</strong> Western ‘white’ culture were<br />

identified and <strong>the</strong>se differences were also believed to be influenced by generation.<br />

6a. Western values<br />

<strong>The</strong> belief that western media dictates what is beautiful and does not show images of those<br />

with visible differences unless <strong>the</strong> context is medical or to sell products was expressed.<br />

“<strong>The</strong> media puts too much pressure on people to look a certain way. You never<br />

see a disfigured person on TV unless <strong>the</strong>re is an advert specifically about that”<br />

(Pakistani young female)<br />

6b. White community treats people with disfigurements better<br />

A belief that white western families are better educated and more tolerant than Asian families<br />

was evident in <strong>the</strong> discussions.<br />

“<strong>The</strong>ir community [white] is more aware, exposed and educated about<br />

differences.” (Pakistani young female)<br />

“<strong>The</strong>y are more accepting to <strong>the</strong> fact.” (Bengali older female)<br />

6c. Back home<br />

Attitudes are perceived to be different in <strong>the</strong> homeland but <strong>the</strong> influence of Bollywood was<br />

thought to be growing.<br />

“I think <strong>the</strong>y would be more tolerant. <strong>The</strong>re’s too much poverty (back home) to<br />

care about <strong>the</strong>se things” (Pakistani young female)<br />

“With <strong>the</strong> influence of Bollywood, girls want to look more glamorous and I don’t<br />

think it matters anymore if <strong>the</strong>y got a good personality or what sort of person you<br />

are “ (Pakistani older female)<br />

133


6d. Generational differences<br />

Elders were considered by participants from <strong>the</strong> younger generation to be more narrow-minded<br />

and less understanding. <strong>The</strong>re was evidence that skin colour was of particular significance to<br />

elders.<br />

“we (young) understand how things work now, how society is, whereas <strong>the</strong>y<br />

(older) are stuck in <strong>the</strong>ir own times” (Pakistani young male)<br />

7. Medical Interventions<br />

7a. Attitudes to medical intervention<br />

Medical intervention is considered acceptable for clear medical reasons or if one’s doctor<br />

advises intervention. However non-essential cosmetic surgery is considered to be going against<br />

God/Allah’s will<br />

“medical is necessary and cosmetic that is a luxury” (Bengali young female)<br />

“It is right to do surgical reconstruction but wrong to do cosmetic surgery. It is<br />

great sin to try to change <strong>the</strong> way of looks. Allah will punish <strong>the</strong>m” (Bengali older<br />

female)<br />

8. Social Support<br />

<strong>The</strong> immediate family is considered to be <strong>the</strong> main source of support, but shame and<br />

embarrassment may prevent families seeking support from external sources (including <strong>the</strong><br />

extended family, <strong>the</strong>ir community and external support services). <strong>The</strong>re was a general lack of<br />

awareness of <strong>the</strong> support services available to those with a disfigurement /visible difference. For<br />

example, none of <strong>the</strong> participants had heard of <strong>the</strong> charity Changing Faces.<br />

“some people don’t want to go to <strong>the</strong> doctor because <strong>the</strong>y are ashamed.”<br />

(Bengali young female)<br />

“<strong>The</strong> extended family stay uninvolved ei<strong>the</strong>r due to lack of awareness, shame, or<br />

‘just can’t be bo<strong>the</strong>red’ attitude” (Pakistani young male)<br />

“parents are supportive <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong>y’ll do anything <strong>the</strong> can so <strong>the</strong>ir child doesn’t feel<br />

singled out” ( Pakistani young male)<br />

“<strong>the</strong>y would definitely not go to <strong>the</strong> community” (Bengali young female)<br />

134


DISCUSSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> focus group interviews offered a unique insight into views regarding visible differences held<br />

by South Asian communities in <strong>the</strong> UK. A number of <strong>the</strong>mes were identified, some of which<br />

have been highlighted in previous literature, in particular <strong>the</strong> impact of disfigurement upon social<br />

acceptability and <strong>the</strong> prospect of marriage (Rozario, 2007).<br />

Religious beliefs and cultural views and practices appear to be deep-rooted within <strong>the</strong> South<br />

Asian population, so it is no surprise to find <strong>the</strong>se entities cutting across <strong>the</strong> majority of <strong>the</strong>mes<br />

that emerged. <strong>The</strong>y are applied to views about <strong>the</strong> cause and origin of <strong>the</strong> visible difference as<br />

well as to <strong>the</strong> consequences of that difference.<br />

A number of areas were identified that could inform interventions to eradicate <strong>the</strong> negative<br />

consequences of visible difference. <strong>The</strong>se included education to dispel myths, raising<br />

awareness of sources of support, and <strong>the</strong> provision of medical explanations for visible<br />

differences. Members of <strong>the</strong> Bengali older female focus group were particularly keen to express<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir suggestions for interventions to improve <strong>the</strong> experiences of those with visible differences<br />

and those that have children with visible differences (See Table 6.1).<br />

It is also important to emphasise that focus group participants used <strong>the</strong> terms disfigurement and<br />

disability interchangeably suggesting that physical impairments may be regarded as highly<br />

salient visible differences - this may have implications for future interventions with <strong>the</strong>se groups.<br />

Whilst <strong>the</strong> methodological approach utilised for this study enabled pertinent issues regarding<br />

visible difference to be highlighted without imposing ‘external’ ideas, especially <strong>the</strong> use of a<br />

semi-structured ‘topic guide’, it was not always possible to explore issues in depth or allow<br />

participants <strong>the</strong> opportunity to elaborate on points. Resources did not allow us to conduct fur<strong>the</strong>r<br />

interviews with communities of similar backgrounds in o<strong>the</strong>r locations, or to cover <strong>the</strong> full range<br />

of cultural groups represented in <strong>the</strong> UK South Asian population.<br />

135


• <strong>The</strong> myths about <strong>the</strong> foetal disorders should be dispelled.<br />

• Posters can help.<br />

• Bengali health sessions should be arranged more to raise awareness.<br />

• City council can arrange different ‘talks’ and trainings to encourage people to bring light<br />

to <strong>the</strong>se matters.<br />

• Social workers play a very important role. Children in schools can be made more aware<br />

of <strong>the</strong>se issues. Social circles can be arranged by <strong>the</strong> city council.<br />

• Women, both daughter in laws and mo<strong>the</strong>r in laws should attend <strong>the</strong>se types of social<br />

groups.<br />

• ‘Mohila Shomity’ type of women’s groups can be very helpful.<br />

• Bengali people should have more volunteering mentality to help raise awareness<br />

Table 6.1: Interventions suggested by Bengali Older Females<br />

136


STUDY 7<br />

QUALITATIVE STUDY EXPLORING THE EXPERIENCES OF<br />

UK SOUTH ASIANS LIVING WITH VITILIGO<br />

Study leads: Andrew Thompson, Sally Clarke & Robert Newell<br />

Acknowledgements: <strong>The</strong> authors would like to thank Dr. David Gawkrodger & Dr. Andrew<br />

Wright and Jennifer Vines of <strong>The</strong> Vitiligo Society for assistance with recruitment and Nicholas<br />

Rosewell for assistance with analysis.<br />

137


SUMMARY<br />

• Vitiligo is a condition that results in <strong>the</strong> visible loss of pigment or colour from <strong>the</strong> skin.<br />

• Seven in-depth interviews were conducted with British people of South Asian descent or<br />

family of origin living with <strong>the</strong> skin condition vitiligo.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> qualitative method of Template Analysis was used.<br />

• Participants described feeling visibly different and all had experienced stigmatization to<br />

some extent.<br />

• For many, cultural specific issues associated with status and practices such as arranged<br />

marriage were intimately related to both stigmatization and social support and<br />

consequently this influenced <strong>the</strong> individual impact of <strong>the</strong> condition and <strong>the</strong> way that this<br />

impact was managed.<br />

• <strong>The</strong>re was a range of distress described within <strong>the</strong> participant group and many of <strong>the</strong><br />

coping strategies used were not dissimilar to <strong>the</strong> strategies described by o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

populations. For example, concealment and avoidance were commonly described.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> findings of this study broadly support <strong>the</strong> model developed by ARC but highlight <strong>the</strong><br />

need to assess cultural associations of disfigurement. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>y support <strong>the</strong><br />

conclusions of study 6 in suggesting that in addition to individual <strong>the</strong>rapeutic<br />

interventions <strong>the</strong>re may be a need for community interventions aimed at dispelling<br />

myths, and raising awareness of sources of support and treatment.<br />

138


INTRODUCTION<br />

Vitiligo is a visible condition affecting <strong>the</strong> appearance of <strong>the</strong> skin through <strong>the</strong> loss of<br />

pigmentation and is consequently more noticeable in darker skinned individuals. It has been<br />

associated with a range of psychosocial difficulties such as depression, impaired quality of life,<br />

low self–esteem and negative thoughts about appearance, difficulties in sexual relationships,<br />

and poor body image (Ongenae, Beelaert, van Geel, & Naeyaert, 2006).<br />

Beliefs about <strong>the</strong> cause and nature of illness are linked to adjustment and <strong>the</strong>re is some<br />

evidence that such beliefs may vary according to culture and ethnicity (Firooz et al., 2004).<br />

However, <strong>the</strong> majority of psychosocial research in psychodermatology and in disfigurement has<br />

been with white Western populations (Thompson & Kent, 2001). <strong>The</strong>re has been some<br />

research within <strong>the</strong> Black American population with vitiligo that has suggested that <strong>the</strong> loss of<br />

pigment has been associated with stigmatizing causal beliefs associated with being punished<br />

for engaging in sexual activity with White Americans (Porter & Beuf, 1991). Research with<br />

Nigerians with <strong>the</strong> condition has revealed high levels of stigmatization associated with confusion<br />

with leprosy (Onunu & Kubeyinje, 2003). Previous research investigating <strong>the</strong> psychosocial<br />

impact of vitiligo in Asian populations suggests significant impairments in quality of life,<br />

particularly in <strong>the</strong> areas of personal and social relationships and been <strong>the</strong> condition has been<br />

associated with comparatively higher levels of psychological morbidity (Mattoo et al, 2002).<br />

A detailed understanding of <strong>the</strong> role played by ethnicity and culture is limited by a lack of<br />

empirical investigation and a heavy reliance on quantitative assessment, using generic<br />

measures that lack sensitivity to cultural issues. <strong>The</strong> present study is guided by a previous<br />

qualitative investigation of vitiligo in a non-Asian population (Thompson, Kent & Smith, 2002).<br />

<strong>The</strong> aims here is to explore <strong>the</strong> ways in which Asian individuals manage and adjust to vitiligo,<br />

and to explore <strong>the</strong> role of ethnicity and culture.<br />

METHOD<br />

<strong>The</strong> present study used <strong>the</strong> qualitative method of Template Analysis (King, 1998). This involves<br />

<strong>the</strong> iterative development of a coding "template", which summarises <strong>the</strong>mes identified by <strong>the</strong><br />

researcher(s) as important and organises <strong>the</strong>m in a meaningful manner.<br />

Participants<br />

Ethical approval was sought from NHS by way of a substantial amendment to <strong>the</strong> substantive<br />

study protocol. Additionally, fur<strong>the</strong>r ethical approval was sought through <strong>the</strong> University of<br />

Sheffield, Department of Psychology Ethics Board, to extend recruitment to a community<br />

sample via <strong>the</strong> website of <strong>The</strong> Vitiligo Society, who consented to assist with recruitment. A<br />

group of 15 respondents was initially sought. <strong>The</strong> inclusion criteria were that participants<br />

139


identified <strong>the</strong>mselves as South Asian, were over <strong>the</strong> age of eighteen, and had had vitiligo<br />

formally diagnosed.<br />

86 South Asian people with vitiligo were invited via letter, from <strong>the</strong> participating NHS clinics<br />

within <strong>the</strong> Appearance Research Collaboration (ARC) survey in Bradford and Sheffield. Three<br />

people agreed to participate. An advertisement was also placed on <strong>the</strong> website of <strong>The</strong> Vitiligo<br />

Society. Nine people made contact and four people met <strong>the</strong> inclusion criteria and were<br />

interviewed. <strong>The</strong> resulting seven participants were all female and ranged in age from 19 to 52<br />

years. All had had vitiligo for over five years and had it on visible parts of <strong>the</strong>ir bodies. <strong>The</strong><br />

participants described <strong>the</strong>mselves as being ei<strong>the</strong>r of British Pakistani origin or of Indian or<br />

Pakistani origin.<br />

Data collection<br />

Interviews were conducted using a face-to -face semi-structured interview schedule with six of<br />

<strong>the</strong> participants and via email with <strong>the</strong> seventh participant. <strong>The</strong> semi-structured interview<br />

schedule was devised by <strong>the</strong> investigators and broadly followed <strong>the</strong> schedule used by<br />

Thompson et al (2002). Face-to-face interviews took place in an appropriate setting ei<strong>the</strong>r in<br />

<strong>the</strong> University or at <strong>the</strong> participants’ homes depending on preference. All face-to-face interviews<br />

were audio recorded and transcribed verbatim.<br />

Data analysis and interpretation<br />

<strong>The</strong> transcribed interviews were analysed following a number of steps defined by King (1998).<br />

Each transcript was read several times and an initial template was gradually developed which<br />

was <strong>the</strong>n fur<strong>the</strong>r developed through an iterative process of comparison between and within<br />

transcripts. Several strategies were used to confirm <strong>the</strong> emerging <strong>the</strong>mes, including conducting<br />

an audit of <strong>the</strong> findings, and sending <strong>the</strong> findings for comment to two of <strong>the</strong> participants and an<br />

independent British South Asian person living with vitiligo.<br />

RESULTS<br />

In common with <strong>the</strong> majority of people who have a visible disfigurement, all of <strong>the</strong> participants<br />

described experiencing intrusive reactions (and in some cases discrimination), and this is a<br />

<strong>the</strong>me that runs throughout <strong>the</strong> key <strong>the</strong>mes described below. Five key <strong>the</strong>mes were identified,<br />

which incorporated various inter-related sub-<strong>the</strong>mes.<br />

1. Cultural specific impact of vitiligo<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was a range of direct references to <strong>the</strong> impact and role of ethnicity and culture in <strong>the</strong><br />

participants’ accounts. <strong>The</strong> majority of accounts were laden with description of culturally specific<br />

material, whilst for two <strong>the</strong>re was a notable absence of such material.<br />

140


1a. Status, stigma and cultural practices<br />

For some of <strong>the</strong> participants, culturally specific <strong>the</strong>mes emerged that were associated with <strong>the</strong><br />

value of appearance in relation to both individual and family status. One British born young<br />

woman of Pakistani descent vividly described how vitiligo was particularly stigmatizing.<br />

“…I think <strong>the</strong>re is a [Punjabi] slang term for it…Asian society is a lot based on<br />

looks and status and to have vitiligo obviously puts you down…it’s a stigma…if<br />

you don’t fit in <strong>the</strong>n you kind of like, no matter if you’re really smart or like, or you<br />

work hard it’s still, you have to try that little bit harder”. (AA)<br />

Ano<strong>the</strong>r participant commented:<br />

“…Asian communities are like that, <strong>the</strong>y always look at you first, what you’re like,<br />

before <strong>the</strong>y look at anything else…” (BB)<br />

Some also spoke of <strong>the</strong> family shame and of literally being hidden away. This was more evident<br />

when visiting relatives in Pakistan and in contact with <strong>the</strong> older generation. AA described being<br />

excluded from aspects of family life to <strong>the</strong> extent that it has affected her ability to learn to speak<br />

Punjabi:<br />

“…mum's friends come over <strong>the</strong>y're, <strong>the</strong>y won't really try and make conversation<br />

with me… that's affected me in <strong>the</strong> way that I can't, my sisters can speak, Urdu<br />

quite fluently whereas my Urdu's quite bad…” (AA)<br />

“…my grandma [in Pakistan] was like, oh you can’t take her with you [to visit<br />

friends & family] because of her skin, what will o<strong>the</strong>r people say”. (AA)<br />

Understandably, direct family rejection was felt more keenly:<br />

““…<strong>the</strong> older generations still see it as like quite a stigma. Some of <strong>the</strong> more like<br />

traditional people that I've met, <strong>the</strong>y seem to have a problem with it and more<br />

than anyone else……some people would say stuff but I think...that didn't bo<strong>the</strong>r<br />

me like, it was more when like, if my parents said something about it that I'd get<br />

upset ra<strong>the</strong>r than if a stranger said something … <strong>the</strong>y're still quite, traditional…”<br />

(AA)<br />

<strong>The</strong> status of appearance was very evident in discussion of <strong>the</strong> practice of arranged<br />

marriage:<br />

“…once we had this huge argument…when my mum just said, well who’s going to<br />

marry you with your skin like that, because a lot of arranged marriages are pretty<br />

much based on looks and like status”. (AA)<br />

141


“…my parents would have been devastated I'm sure for, you know, devastated<br />

that, you know, <strong>the</strong>ir child's got something like that but even more so thinking,<br />

how are we going to marry her off…” (BB)<br />

Related to stigmatization were cultural beliefs about cause, although this was mentioned<br />

infrequently and not by all, and was again commented upon as being related to generation. <strong>The</strong><br />

lack of awareness was seen as problematic:<br />

“… older generation aren't educated enough to understand…that it's no fault of<br />

<strong>the</strong> individual, why it happens” (BB)<br />

“Lack of awareness is a major issue”. (EE)<br />

1b. Loss of ethnic identity<br />

Most participants commented upon <strong>the</strong> increased visibility of <strong>the</strong> condition for darker skinned<br />

individuals, and heightened visibility per se was seen as problematic:<br />

“It, it turns your skin white, white and obviously it affects <strong>the</strong> darker skin more…”<br />

(BB)<br />

“…definitely <strong>the</strong>y feel embarrassed to go out, particularly if <strong>the</strong>y are dark, <strong>the</strong>n<br />

white patches <strong>the</strong>y look worse for dark skin...” (FF)<br />

As well as pigment loss being part of <strong>the</strong> condition, depigmentation is also used as a treatment<br />

in order to give <strong>the</strong> skin a unified colour. <strong>The</strong> prospect of total loss of pigment through ei<strong>the</strong>r<br />

disease or treatment was perceived in a range of ways. It was seen by some as positive as it<br />

resulted in <strong>the</strong> vitiligo becoming less visible:<br />

“Even though my dad has this it’s a different situation for him as he’s all white so<br />

he looks normal, where as I am patchy”. (CC)<br />

“…I am quite happy I had <strong>the</strong> [depigmentation treatment]…since I became all<br />

white it didn’t affect me…” (FF)<br />

Such participants continued to largely describe <strong>the</strong>mselves as accepted by <strong>the</strong>ir peers or<br />

community:<br />

“…this young man he said if you don’t mind are you Italian, I said no I'm Indian<br />

and <strong>the</strong>n so many places when first time <strong>the</strong>y see me <strong>the</strong>y always think I am<br />

142


European…<strong>The</strong>n funnily enough people don’t understand that in India lots of<br />

people are very fair…so <strong>the</strong>se are <strong>the</strong> thing <strong>the</strong>y say, it doesn’t bo<strong>the</strong>r me really,<br />

why should it bo<strong>the</strong>r me.” (FF)<br />

Some people may say oh does it have some sort of impact on you <strong>the</strong> fact that<br />

people don’t necessarily realise that I'm Indian or Italian? (Interviewer)<br />

“No, I think [I’ve] enough education to take all <strong>the</strong>se things.” (FF)<br />

However, iatrogenic depigmentation was controversial. For some <strong>the</strong> total loss of pigment was<br />

perceived as an additional threat, a risk of loss of ethnic identity, with an associated sense of no<br />

longer being recognized as belonging to <strong>the</strong> community and family:<br />

“I think that's one treatment I would never have because I would just feel like I'd lost<br />

my identity completely…I wouldn't be recognised as Pakistani, I'd be recognised as<br />

White, not that <strong>the</strong>re's anything wrong with that, it's just that that's not me and, if I<br />

went out with my family I wouldn't be recognised as part of <strong>the</strong> family…my heritage<br />

has gone…” (AA)<br />

One participant who described such a loss of recognition almost posited this as a potential<br />

source of envy, commenting on how Asian culture values lighter skin:<br />

“…Asian people, <strong>the</strong>y want fairer skin…” (GG)<br />

However, she appeared to have defensively aligned herself with being “like a British person”,<br />

possibly as a way of minimizing <strong>the</strong> impact of feeling rejected. Yet it was evident that she<br />

continued to feel isolated:<br />

“Yeah, I think more like a British person… when I go to functions and things like<br />

that now I'm like more stared at than anybody else but now it's like a little bit<br />

more flattering that it was before…because I'm <strong>the</strong> only white person <strong>the</strong>re…<br />

(GG)<br />

“She said it to my face, she goes, we were talking about going on holiday or<br />

something and she said oh yeah you could do with a tan <strong>the</strong>n you’d fit in more<br />

with us, but I thought I don’t want to be like you.” (GG)<br />

2. Individual impact of vitiligo<br />

<strong>The</strong> participants that appeared to be coping least well with <strong>the</strong>ir vitiligo were those who seemed<br />

to have internalised <strong>the</strong> importance of appearance, or who placed high value on it, and saw<br />

<strong>the</strong>mselves as spoiled or damaged as a result of having <strong>the</strong> condition. As stated above, <strong>the</strong>re<br />

was often a cultural element to <strong>the</strong> origin of this value.<br />

143


2a. Appearance shame and disgust<br />

Many of <strong>the</strong> participants described feeling ‘disgusted’ and clearly had low self-esteem and were<br />

shame-prone:<br />

“I think it's horrible and disgusting, and I suppose I think that's what o<strong>the</strong>r people<br />

think, see and think when I, I don't have make-up on, and I don't feel like it's<br />

me…” (AA)<br />

“…it's horrible, you know, it's still horrible…” (BB)<br />

“…I know it's my vanity, I don’t know, I'm sure people at work don’t sit and look<br />

at me and see if whe<strong>the</strong>r I've got any blotches, it is me and how I deal with it and<br />

my paranoia.” (EE)<br />

For those that described being less distressed by vitiligo, o<strong>the</strong>r factors or difficulties were ei<strong>the</strong>r<br />

more salient to <strong>the</strong>m, or <strong>the</strong>y valued appearance less in comparison to o<strong>the</strong>r factors such as<br />

education:<br />

“…you never try to cover, you have such a strong personality, I said look so<br />

many problems I have in my life and how I look my face or something, it really<br />

doesn’t bo<strong>the</strong>r me…” (FF)<br />

<strong>The</strong>se beliefs were often attributed to <strong>the</strong>ir background.<br />

2b. Intimacy and being with o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

For some this fear of rejection, of being exposed, tangibly affected <strong>the</strong>ir intimate<br />

relationships:<br />

“…I don't think I could ever let someone get close or be close to someone<br />

enough to do that [get married], I don't want to really have kids, like I would like<br />

to, but <strong>the</strong>n I don't want to because I don't want <strong>the</strong>m to have vitiligo…” (AA)<br />

“…Around boys I am very self conscious in case I’m questioned about my vitiligo,<br />

so I don’t really know any boys”. (CC)<br />

“…even not letting my husband see me when I was naked and that’s a factor in it<br />

really, although my husband has never been bo<strong>the</strong>red by it, I have. It's just me<br />

really, how I perceive it, it's probably not as bad as what I think it is but.” (EE)<br />

144


“…even not letting my husband see it” (GG)<br />

Some participants made negative downward social comparisons in relation to <strong>the</strong>ir own<br />

condition:<br />

“…people have said, well, you know, it could have been worse, it could have<br />

been cancer, you know, and, but <strong>the</strong>n I think to myself, at least <strong>the</strong>y'd still have<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir looks…” (BB)<br />

3. Living with vitiligo: coping strategies<br />

A range of coping strategies were used, although all participants described using concealment<br />

at least some of <strong>the</strong> time.<br />

3a. Avoidance and concealment<br />

“…some of <strong>the</strong> boys just looking at it, at <strong>the</strong> patch, a patch on my leg and just like<br />

whispering and I, after that I just went home and I felt, I just cried for ages, and I<br />

didn't, I don't think I went swimming…” (AA)<br />

“…I didn’t actually build up <strong>the</strong> confidence to go swimming until a week ago...”<br />

(GG)<br />

“…with past relationships I'd come to a point where I just wouldn't want to go any<br />

fur<strong>the</strong>r but, I don't like, like physical, like too close to see, so you can see like<br />

where my make-up is, my make-up comes off as well, I just start getting panicky<br />

and I always like have to have <strong>the</strong> lights off…” (AA)<br />

“…Not going for a smear, that’s ano<strong>the</strong>r example.” (EE)<br />

Most participants <strong>report</strong>ed using concealment, particularly cosmetic camouflage, to some<br />

extent, and described <strong>the</strong> pressures of reliance upon this:<br />

“…I can't even like answer my door without any make-up on because I just, I<br />

would just have a panic attack… I find it quite pa<strong>the</strong>tic that I can't just go to <strong>the</strong><br />

shop, to <strong>the</strong> door without putting on make-up…part of me wishes I'd never<br />

started wearing make-up because maybe <strong>the</strong>n I wouldn't have to rely on it so<br />

145


much…sometimes I just want to because it’s so much effort putting on my make<br />

up…” (AA)<br />

“I would end up feeling really self-conscious and, and nervous and scared and<br />

vulnerable and, it's all because I haven't got make-up on”. (AA)<br />

3b. Confronting and explaining<br />

Some of <strong>the</strong> participants had developed positive methods of dealing with intrusive reactions, but<br />

such coping mechanisms were not widely evident in <strong>the</strong> group:<br />

“I would get <strong>the</strong> occasional like name calls… <strong>the</strong>y're just saying it because<br />

<strong>the</strong>y're scared of stuff that <strong>the</strong>y don't know, because if you just explain it to <strong>the</strong>m<br />

<strong>the</strong>y've got nothing to say…” (AA)<br />

“Control new interaction by informing <strong>the</strong>m beforehand.” (DD)<br />

3c. Overcompensation<br />

For some participants <strong>the</strong>re was a sense of overcompensating or using placation as a means<br />

of mitigating against anticipated rejection:<br />

“…it's really important to me what people think about me and because of that<br />

wanting to prove myself all <strong>the</strong> time I do go out of my way for o<strong>the</strong>r people, even<br />

at work if somebody says can you do this for me I won’t ever say no. It's like<br />

you’ve always got this want in you for people to like you because people have<br />

hated you most or your life or looked down <strong>the</strong>ir nose at you…” (GG)<br />

3d. Finding <strong>the</strong>ir own way<br />

Some participants spoke of rejecting cultural practices and finding <strong>the</strong>ir own way. This was<br />

particularly evident in relation to arranged marriage:<br />

“…in my mind that made me think well what am I going to do and <strong>the</strong>n I decided I<br />

wasn’t going to do it and I was going to go to university instead…because me<br />

and my husband had a love marriage…” (GG)<br />

3e. Denial, wishful thinking and minimizing<br />

Some of <strong>the</strong> more distressed participants described using a range of emotional coping<br />

strategies likely to exacerbate <strong>the</strong>ir adjustment in <strong>the</strong> long run:<br />

146


Denial and wishful thinking<br />

“…<strong>the</strong>re'll be a magic cure and it will be over…” (BB)<br />

“…I feel in denial of it, I just think, I just want to fight it psychologically and not<br />

think about it really…” (BB)<br />

Minimizing emotion<br />

Not disclosing <strong>the</strong> condition or <strong>the</strong> individual impact of it to wider family was <strong>report</strong>ed by<br />

several participants as a way of coping, ei<strong>the</strong>r from <strong>the</strong> fear of causing worry within <strong>the</strong> family<br />

or <strong>the</strong> fear of being stigmatized;<br />

“…I just don't want to worry <strong>the</strong>m because <strong>the</strong>y'll just worry for me and that is <strong>the</strong><br />

reason why I haven't told <strong>the</strong>m…” (BB)<br />

4. Social support<br />

Support came from family, friends, <strong>The</strong> Vitiligo Society, <strong>the</strong> Internet, and from dermatologists.<br />

4a. Seeing things differently and acceptance<br />

For some, <strong>the</strong>re was acceptance and assistance in generating positive illness<br />

representations:<br />

“…my husband has been brilliant and he just said, it wasn't, you know, it wasn't<br />

your own fault, it's just happened…” (BB)<br />

“…she's [a friend] so understanding, she said, you'll be you no matter what, so<br />

don't think about that because people accept you for what you are and if <strong>the</strong>y<br />

don't accept you for what you are <strong>the</strong>n, are <strong>the</strong>y a true friend and don't worry<br />

about it.” (BB)<br />

“I've got this friend at work and she's amazing…she goes if that were me, if I was<br />

going to meet somebody I'd say you ei<strong>the</strong>r take me as you are or you leave me.<br />

This is me accept it or lump it, but I can't do that.” (GG)<br />

147


For respondents like AA (described earlier), where <strong>the</strong>re had been a lack of social support<br />

and acceptance particularly at a young age, <strong>the</strong>re was a sense of being different. This was<br />

also evident in some o<strong>the</strong>r accounts:<br />

“I think it's going right back to my parents never talking about it and being picked<br />

on made to feel <strong>the</strong> odd one out and even now despite all <strong>the</strong> colour change and<br />

everything, even despite that I still feel like <strong>the</strong> odd one out. I haven’t built up<br />

that much confidence in myself that I can actually feel that I belong <strong>the</strong>re and I<br />

don’t, I still don’t now.” (GG)<br />

4b. Medical support<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was a range of experience of medical treatment, particularly in relation to <strong>the</strong><br />

availability and access to different treatments:<br />

“…I don’t know but I feel even being a doctor it was difficult for me to get all this<br />

information, nobody gave me this information…My GP is very caring but he<br />

never gave me any information because he didn’t know.” (FF)<br />

5. Living with vitiligo: ongoing burden<br />

In common with previous participants, <strong>the</strong>re was a <strong>the</strong>me here of <strong>the</strong> ongoing burden vitiligo<br />

placed on participants in relation to <strong>the</strong> uncertainty about progression, <strong>the</strong> reliance on<br />

camouflage and <strong>the</strong> demand on psychological resources to cope:<br />

“Oh, it, it's horrible, how do I sum it up? It's a burden on me because it's <strong>the</strong>re all<br />

<strong>the</strong> time…I know <strong>the</strong>re's no cure…although I put creams on every day it's a kind<br />

of chore, it's awful”. (BB)<br />

Feedback from participants and independent member of <strong>The</strong> Vitiligo Society<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>the</strong>mes were shared with two participants who had requested to see <strong>the</strong> results and an<br />

independent member of <strong>The</strong> Vitiligo Society. One of <strong>the</strong> aims of doing this was to ensure that<br />

<strong>the</strong> study had meaning for participants and appeared warrantable to <strong>the</strong>m. Additionally,<br />

feedback was sought on <strong>the</strong> cultural acceptability of <strong>the</strong> results. Consent was provided to use<br />

<strong>the</strong> following quotes sent by email.<br />

Only one of <strong>the</strong> participants responded and commented positively that:<br />

148


“I don't think that you have made any misinterpretations whatsoever. It is<br />

paramount I feel to show this as you have done in order to get any future<br />

funding for studies as you show how people really feel about <strong>the</strong>mselves. I<br />

also feel that psychological intervention would definitely be a great benefit to<br />

individuals (single girls especially of Asian origin whose parents 'disown' <strong>the</strong>m<br />

as <strong>the</strong>y literally appear to be outcasted from o<strong>the</strong>rs!) I feel for <strong>the</strong>m as I feel<br />

had my vitiligo been so wide spread at that time (before coming of age) my<br />

parents who loved me so much would still have worried in taking me out in<br />

public especially if I was to have had an arranged marriage! Some of <strong>the</strong> girls<br />

in your study would definitely benefit from help in order to make <strong>the</strong>m feel<br />

beautiful and wanted”.<br />

<strong>The</strong> independent member of <strong>The</strong> Vitiligo Society commented:<br />

“Being Indian myself, I could really relate to <strong>the</strong> points mentioned. I have two<br />

additional things to add which were not mentioned. One is <strong>the</strong> myth that people<br />

have vitiligo because <strong>the</strong>y have eaten fish and drunk milk at <strong>the</strong> same time. I<br />

hear this time and time again and find it very frustrating - I only hear it from<br />

Indians!! My o<strong>the</strong>r thought is that many Indians lack confidence because of <strong>the</strong><br />

condition and <strong>the</strong> stigma attached to it”.<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

This study offers a unique insight into <strong>the</strong> experiences of South Asian women living with vitiligo<br />

in <strong>the</strong> UK. A number of <strong>the</strong>mes were identified, several of which have been highlighted in<br />

previous literature, relating to White women living in <strong>the</strong> UK, in particular <strong>the</strong> reality of<br />

experiencing intrusive reactions and <strong>the</strong> high reliance on concealment as a method of coping<br />

(Thompson et al., 2002). Visibility and <strong>the</strong> importance of appearance were also seen to be key<br />

factors, although <strong>the</strong>re were interesting relationships between <strong>the</strong>se and cultural practices.<br />

Much research has focussed on <strong>the</strong> psychological impact of disfigurement but has failed to<br />

consider <strong>the</strong> socio-cultural meaning and context of having a marked appearance, and clearly<br />

here vitiligo is seen as an impediment to social acceptability and <strong>the</strong> prospect of marriage<br />

(Rozario, 2007).<br />

Goffman (1963) describes <strong>the</strong> most serious reaction to stigma as <strong>the</strong> development of <strong>the</strong><br />

‘spoiled identity’ in relation to <strong>the</strong> internalisation of <strong>the</strong> experience of stigmatisation. Thompson<br />

and Kent (2001) and Thompson (2005) have commented <strong>the</strong>oretically upon <strong>the</strong> similarity<br />

between this process and that of becoming shame-prone. For some participants here <strong>the</strong>re was<br />

a deep sense of having a spoiled identity in terms of not only <strong>the</strong>ir personal identity but in<br />

relation to <strong>the</strong>ir ethnicity.<br />

<strong>The</strong> findings of this study broadly support <strong>the</strong> model developed by ARC but suggest <strong>the</strong> need to<br />

assess cultural associations of disfigurement. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>y suggest as do <strong>the</strong> findings from<br />

149


study 6, that in addition to individual <strong>the</strong>rapeutic interventions, <strong>the</strong>re may be a need for<br />

community interventions aimed at dispelling myths, and raising awareness of sources of support<br />

and treatment.<br />

However, caution is needed in interpreting and acting on <strong>the</strong>se findings, as <strong>the</strong> response rate to<br />

this study was very low (3% from <strong>the</strong> clinic population approached), and <strong>the</strong> eventual number of<br />

women interviewed was small. Whilst in-depth interviews with small samples can provide<br />

elaborate data of <strong>the</strong>oretical interest, <strong>the</strong>re can be certainly no claims made about <strong>the</strong><br />

generalizability of <strong>the</strong> findings <strong>report</strong>ed here, and fur<strong>the</strong>r research is needed. None<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong><br />

present study is valuable in revealing some specific cultural factors that might influence<br />

adjustment in <strong>the</strong> British South Asian vitiligo population and additionally in demonstrating <strong>the</strong><br />

complex relationship that exists between <strong>the</strong>se cultural issues and o<strong>the</strong>r social and<br />

psychological factors, most notably visibility and social support.<br />

150


STUDY 8<br />

AN INVESTIGATION OF APPEARANCE-RELATED SOCIAL<br />

COMPARISONS AMONGST INDIVIDUALS WITH VISIBLE<br />

DIFFERENCES.<br />

Study lead: Emma Halliwell and Timothy Moss<br />

151


AIMS<br />

<strong>The</strong> aims of this study were to investigate <strong>the</strong> impact of different types of social comparisons<br />

made by individuals living with visible differences. Social comparisons are related to adjustment,<br />

so that those with good adjustment <strong>report</strong> fewer appearance-related comparisons than those<br />

with poor adjustment (Halliwell, 2002). This study aimed to explore <strong>the</strong> types of comparisons<br />

made by poor and good adjusters in more detail. We planned to examine <strong>the</strong> outcome of<br />

experimentally induced social comparisons between individuals identified as good or poor<br />

adjusters.<br />

BACKGROUND<br />

It has been argued that social comparisons are a universal and necessary tendency (Festinger,<br />

1954). In view of previous research, it was surprising that <strong>the</strong> results of study 1 indicated only a<br />

small effect of social comparison processes. <strong>The</strong>se processes did however feature in <strong>the</strong><br />

qualitative data derived from interviews with participants in study 3. In view of <strong>the</strong> emphasis on<br />

appearance specific cognitive processing evident in study 1, it may be that <strong>the</strong> lack of impact of<br />

social comparison processes is an artefact of <strong>the</strong> generalised measure used. In view of this<br />

possibility, and in <strong>the</strong> light of an acknowledgement of <strong>the</strong> complexities of social comparison<br />

processes by specialist researchers in this area (Halliwell, 2002), this study was designed to<br />

explore appearance specific social comparison processes in more detail. <strong>The</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis was<br />

that good adjusters are less likely to make appearance-related social comparisons than poor<br />

adjusters. A fuller understanding of <strong>the</strong> differences between comparison tendencies between<br />

good and poor adjusters will facilitate an enhanced model of intervention consistent with and<br />

building on current best practice.<br />

METHOD<br />

This study was an experimental study conducted via <strong>the</strong> internet. <strong>The</strong> study was conducted as<br />

an investigation of perceived social skills. Participants viewed an image of an attractive woman<br />

and listened to an audio-tape of her in conversation. <strong>The</strong>y were <strong>the</strong>n asked to rate <strong>the</strong> woman’s<br />

social skills. In <strong>the</strong> first condition <strong>the</strong>y were given no instructions about <strong>the</strong> specific comparisons<br />

to be made between <strong>the</strong>mselves and <strong>the</strong> woman. In <strong>the</strong> second condition <strong>the</strong>y were asked to<br />

rate <strong>the</strong> woman and <strong>the</strong>mselves in relation to a number of specific domains including<br />

“appearance”, so <strong>the</strong>y were forced to make an appearance-related social comparison.<br />

Participants <strong>the</strong>n completed a measure of state mood and body image. We intended to compare<br />

<strong>the</strong> content of <strong>the</strong> comparisons made by “poor” and “good” adjusters (as defined by scores on<br />

<strong>the</strong> DAS24 in <strong>the</strong> cross sectional study) in each condition.<br />

Hypo<strong>the</strong>ses<br />

1) In <strong>the</strong> absence of any instruction about social comparison (condition one), poor adjusters<br />

will <strong>report</strong> less body satisfaction and more negative mood than good adjusters because<br />

<strong>the</strong>y will have engaged in an appearance-related social comparison with <strong>the</strong> target.<br />

152


2) In <strong>the</strong> second condition, when an appearance comparison is forced, <strong>the</strong>re will be no<br />

difference between body image and mood <strong>report</strong>ed by poor and good adjusters. All of<br />

<strong>the</strong> participants will have been forced into upward appearance-related social<br />

comparisons.<br />

Participants<br />

We aimed to recruit a sample of 60 women who had taken part in <strong>the</strong> initial cross-sectional<br />

study. However, from <strong>the</strong> women who indicated that <strong>the</strong>y would be happy to be contacted for<br />

follow up, we were only able to invite 40 women to participate. Of <strong>the</strong>se women, 13 completed<br />

<strong>the</strong> study, 4 in <strong>the</strong> forced comparison condition and 9 in <strong>the</strong> no-comparison instruction condition.<br />

RESULTS<br />

Unfortunately, this small sample meant that we did not have enough statistical power to test <strong>the</strong><br />

hypo<strong>the</strong>ses about poor versus good adjusters. We did examine whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> social comparison<br />

instruction condition had an impact overall.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was no statistically significant difference between body satisfaction amongst women who<br />

had been forced to make an appearance-related social comparison (mean = 4.45, SD= 0.99)<br />

and those who had not (mean = 2.98, SD = 1.23), t (11) = .53 (NS). Nei<strong>the</strong>r was <strong>the</strong>re a<br />

significant difference between mood <strong>report</strong>ed by those in <strong>the</strong> forced comparison (mean= 1.23,<br />

SD= 0.50) and those in <strong>the</strong> no comparison instruction condition (mean = 1.33, SD = 0.50), t(11)<br />

= .56 (NS).<br />

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> sample recruited to <strong>the</strong> current study was too small to be able to test our hypo<strong>the</strong>ses. In<br />

order to achieve an appropriate sample we would have needed a 100% response rate, clearly<br />

this was not realistic. Unfortunately, we could not examine our research questions in <strong>the</strong> current<br />

study. However, as <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical links between social comparisons, adjustment to appearance<br />

and body dissatisfaction apply to <strong>the</strong> population in general we are currently running this study<br />

with undergraduate students. This should be completed by November 2009 and will provide<br />

some evidence relevant to <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses investigated here.<br />

153


154


STUDY 9<br />

APPEARANCE CONCERN, HOSTILITY AND SOCIAL<br />

SITUATIONS<br />

Study leads: Timothy Moss, James Byron-Daniel & Eleanor Walsh<br />

155


SUMMARY<br />

• Participants (n=38) took part in an online survey comprising 6 social situation vignettes<br />

involving judgements about <strong>the</strong> consequences of anti-social behavior<br />

• Participants who were less well adjusted to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance (DAS24) were more<br />

sensitive to <strong>the</strong> behaviour of o<strong>the</strong>rs and likely to be more hostile<br />

• <strong>The</strong> relationship between appearance-related adjustment and hostility was found to be<br />

mediated by social ranking and narcissism<br />

• <strong>The</strong> results indicate that interventions should focus on personally sensitive social<br />

situations (appearance-specific threats) ra<strong>the</strong>r than generic social skills when seeking to<br />

reduce hostility in people with appearance concerns.<br />

156


RATIONALE<br />

Previous work in <strong>the</strong> field of visible differences has suggested <strong>the</strong> importance of hostility as an<br />

aspect of psycho-social adjustment (Moss, 1997), and <strong>the</strong> inclusion of <strong>the</strong> hostility measure in<br />

<strong>the</strong> pack of tests in <strong>the</strong> main study was based on this. Initial analysis of data from <strong>the</strong> crosssectional<br />

study confirmed potentially important relationships between adjustment to visible<br />

difference/disfigurement and feelings of hostility. <strong>The</strong> scientific literature on <strong>the</strong> relationship<br />

between feelings of hostility and people with difference in appearance is under-researched. Due<br />

to our initial findings and <strong>the</strong> lack of research in this area it was considered appropriate to<br />

attempt to investigate this relationship fur<strong>the</strong>r.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

An examination of <strong>the</strong> recent psychological literature investigating appearance concern and<br />

hostility has presented a number of relevant psychological constructs that may be related to<br />

aggression/hostility in people with difference in <strong>the</strong>ir appearance.<br />

In summary, aggression is a likely outcome in people high in narcissism who are faced with<br />

threatening or challenging feedback in social situations (Kernis, Grannemann and Barclay,<br />

1989). As narcissism is often related to, but not identical to, high self esteem alone, it is not<br />

sufficient for hostility to be demonstrated; it is also necessary to have a poor or low “o<strong>the</strong>r”<br />

esteem. According to this formulation, a feeling or sense of superiority could be a precursor to<br />

aggression or hostile behaviour (Baumeister, 1996). It is not yet clear <strong>the</strong> extent to which this is<br />

stable across all situations or whe<strong>the</strong>r it is only specific situations which might elicit hostile<br />

responses in people with a visibly different appearance (e.g. situations in which appearance is<br />

highlighted). Perceived social ranking (Gilbert et al, 2007) may also be important in this<br />

relationship. From this perspective, aggression and hostility may serve to protect a person’s<br />

perceived position in a social ranking system when this is under threat.<br />

It may be <strong>the</strong> case that narcissism and superiority are defence mechanisms functioning to<br />

protect a more hidden, core view of <strong>the</strong> self as weak, vulnerable, or damaged. Aggression and<br />

hostility are <strong>the</strong> manifestation of <strong>the</strong>se defences in <strong>the</strong> face of threat to <strong>the</strong> self through<br />

challenges present in <strong>the</strong> environment (Moss, 1997). It is not difficult to see how this might<br />

occur in people with appearance concerns when in a situation in which appearance becomes<br />

highly salient. In order to investigate <strong>the</strong>se possibilities <strong>the</strong> following study investigated reactions<br />

of people with differences in appearance to hypo<strong>the</strong>tical situations measuring concepts such as<br />

self-esteem, adjustment to disfigurement and hostility.<br />

METHOD<br />

Participants<br />

Thirty-eight out of 146 potential participants completed <strong>the</strong> survey (a 26% response rate).<br />

Twenty-five email invitations were rejected, because email addresses were not recognised,<br />

157


leaving 82 invitations not responded to or partially completed surveys (one person officially<br />

opted out of taking part). Thirty eight participants took part (female n = 30, male n = 8). All<br />

participants were over 18 years, with a median age within <strong>the</strong> age band 41-45 years. Most were<br />

married or living with <strong>the</strong>ir partner (60.5%) and 39.5 were living alone or with friends and<br />

relatives.<br />

Detailed description of measures<br />

Derriford Appearance Scale-24 (DAS-24)<br />

Developed by Carr, Moss and Harris (2005) <strong>the</strong> DAS-24 is a 24-item scale used to assess<br />

appearance concern in clinical and research settings (Moss, 2005). Items relate to participants’<br />

behaviours and concerns surrounding an area of <strong>the</strong>ir body <strong>the</strong>y are self-conscious about. Items<br />

are scored from ‘not at all’/’never’/’almost never’ (with a non-applicable option) to<br />

‘extremely’/’almost always’. High scores indicate high levels of distress.<br />

Rosenberg Self-esteem scale (RSE)<br />

This is a 10-item scale that measures respondents’ general feeling towards <strong>the</strong>mselves<br />

(Rosenberg, 1965). <strong>The</strong> items are scored from 4 (‘strongly agree’) to 1 (‘strongly disagree’),<br />

where higher scores denote higher levels of self-esteem.<br />

Adapted O<strong>the</strong>r esteem scale<br />

This is an adapted version by Burkes and Stets (1999). O<strong>the</strong>r esteem relates to how an<br />

individual believes that o<strong>the</strong>r people perceive <strong>the</strong>m. Items for <strong>the</strong> RSE have been adapted to<br />

incorporate this concept. For instance, “I feel I do not have much to be proud of” is rephrased as<br />

“I feel that o<strong>the</strong>r people feel I do not have much to be proud of”. Similarly, items on this scale<br />

are scored from 4 (‘strongly agree’) to 1 (‘strongly disagree’), with higher scores indicating<br />

higher esteem perceived from o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

Social Ranking Scale (SRS)<br />

This social comparison scale includes 11 items focusing of respondent judgements of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

social rank, relative attractiveness and group fit (Allan & Gilbert, 1995). Items are arranged on a<br />

scale with bipolar opposites at ei<strong>the</strong>r end (e.g. Inferior-Superior; Left Out-Accepted). Responses<br />

range from most negative to most positive judgments of <strong>the</strong> self along <strong>the</strong> scale (1-10,<br />

respectively). Higher scores represent more favourable comparisons.<br />

Refined Aggression scale<br />

This shortened version of <strong>the</strong> Aggression questionnaire (Buss & Perry, 1992) comprises four<br />

subscales (physical aggression, verbal aggression, anger and hostility). Items are scored from 1<br />

(‘extremely uncharacteristic of me’) to 5 (‘extremely characteristic of me’), where higher scores<br />

indicate greater aggression.<br />

158


Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI)<br />

<strong>The</strong> NPI (Raskin & Terry, 1988) consists of 40 items relating to psychometric properties of<br />

narcissism behaviour and perceptions. <strong>The</strong> items are scored using a true or false format. Higher<br />

scores indicate more narcissistic characteristics.<br />

Procedure<br />

Participants from <strong>the</strong> large cross-sectional study, who had given <strong>the</strong>ir consent to be contacted<br />

about fur<strong>the</strong>r research, were sent an email invitation to complete a survey online. This email<br />

provided a link to <strong>the</strong> survey website, where an information sheet was viewed and informed<br />

consent could be given. Only those participants who gave this consent were able to progress<br />

through <strong>the</strong> main body of <strong>the</strong> survey. An online survey website was used to administer <strong>the</strong><br />

questionnaire. <strong>The</strong> online survey was hosted on a secure site, all data was encrypted when<br />

downloaded, and participants each had a unique participant code.<br />

Participants were presented with a series of six social situation vignettes (one at a time),<br />

describing several different social situations where <strong>the</strong> character (i.e. <strong>the</strong> participant) was<br />

subject to a form of anti-social behaviour. Participants are required to read each situation and<br />

respond using two rating scales. <strong>The</strong> first scale required participants to <strong>report</strong> how much<br />

punishment <strong>the</strong>y would hypo<strong>the</strong>tically administer to <strong>the</strong> perpetrator(s). <strong>The</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r scale required<br />

participants to rate <strong>the</strong> extent to which this situation made <strong>the</strong>m feel <strong>the</strong>y had “had a raw deal<br />

out of life”. For example:<br />

You have finished most of what you set out to do, and are about to go home. As<br />

you start heading off, you see a group of four or five men and women in <strong>the</strong>ir late<br />

teens. <strong>The</strong>y stare at you as you approach, and <strong>the</strong>n as you pass, make insulting<br />

comments about <strong>the</strong> way you look.<br />

Two police officers get out of an unmarked car, <strong>the</strong>y have witnessed <strong>the</strong> whole<br />

thing. One of <strong>the</strong>m moves <strong>the</strong> youths into a police van, and <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r lets you know<br />

that <strong>the</strong>y are trying a new scheme of youth justice, where people on <strong>the</strong> receiving<br />

end of anti-social behaviour can set an instant fine on those who <strong>the</strong> police catch in<br />

<strong>the</strong> act.<br />

What fine would you like to give each of <strong>the</strong> youths who commented on your looks?<br />

Please choose from <strong>the</strong> following responses:<br />

No fine £1 -12 £13-24 £25-36 £37-48 £49+<br />

To what extent would this situation make you feel like you have had a raw deal out<br />

of life?<br />

Not at all Strongly agree Disagree Agree Strongly agree Definitely<br />

159


<strong>The</strong> vignettes alternated between describing anti-social behaviour towards <strong>the</strong> character<br />

because of <strong>the</strong>ir appearance and anti-social behaviour that was not due to any personal<br />

characteristic of <strong>the</strong> individual. <strong>The</strong> order of which <strong>the</strong>se appeared to <strong>the</strong> participants was<br />

counter balanced, and <strong>the</strong> participants were assigned to one version of <strong>the</strong> survey using<br />

random selection.<br />

After completing <strong>the</strong> vignettes, participants <strong>the</strong>n completed a number of validated<br />

questionnaires. <strong>The</strong>se measure participants feelings towards <strong>the</strong>ir appearance using <strong>the</strong><br />

Derriford Appearance scale-24 (DAS-24; Carr, Moss & Harris 2005), general self-esteem<br />

(Rosenberg, 1965), o<strong>the</strong>r self-esteem (Burks et al, 1999), Social Ranking Scale (Allan & Gilbert,<br />

1995), Refined Aggression scale (Bryant & Smith, 2001) and <strong>the</strong> Narcissistic Personality<br />

Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry, 1988).<br />

Once participants had completed all six scenarios <strong>the</strong>y were thanked for taking part and<br />

debriefed.<br />

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

From a preliminary analysis of <strong>the</strong> results it was found that <strong>the</strong>re was a significant relationship<br />

between scores on <strong>the</strong> DAS (assessing appearance concern) and hostile feelings after reading<br />

<strong>the</strong> appearance content specific vignettes but not <strong>the</strong> non-appearance vignettes. This suggests<br />

those participants more poorly adjusted to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance were more sensitive to verbal /<br />

behavioural attacks that were associated with appearance compared to those who were better<br />

adjusted. Those who were poorly adjusted appeared to be much more sensitive, and likely to<br />

be hostile, when appearance becomes part of a given situation. It also appears from <strong>the</strong> data<br />

that this relationship between adjustment and hostility is mediated by social ranking and<br />

narcissism. In line with early hypo<strong>the</strong>ses about <strong>the</strong> importance of <strong>the</strong>se constructs in this<br />

context, it appears that those who are negatively adjusted are more likely to react with hostility<br />

to an appearance-related social threat if <strong>the</strong>y rate <strong>the</strong>mselves poorly next to o<strong>the</strong>r members of<br />

society and if <strong>the</strong>y have low narcissism.<br />

CONCLUSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> results highlight <strong>the</strong> potential need to focus on personally sensitive issues and information<br />

in interventions to help people adjust to a difference in appearance, ra<strong>the</strong>r than social skills<br />

training in general, as <strong>the</strong> latter is oriented to more generic social threats. This study highlights<br />

that those who are less well adjusted to <strong>the</strong>ir difference in appearance are more sensitive to<br />

appearance-specific threats and that <strong>the</strong>y are more likely to respond with hostility and anger to<br />

<strong>the</strong>se situations. Results also suggest that levels of narcissism, and how people rank<br />

<strong>the</strong>mselves within <strong>the</strong> social environment <strong>the</strong>y live in are important in mediating this relationship.<br />

Accordingly fur<strong>the</strong>r research, and particularly fur<strong>the</strong>r work focussing on interventions to help<br />

people adjust to differences in appearance, could benefit from focusing on issues around social<br />

ranking and narcissistic beliefs about <strong>the</strong> self. Within <strong>the</strong> context of <strong>the</strong> proposed model, this<br />

160


study supports <strong>the</strong> insertion of aggression as a potential outcome of adjustment to a difference<br />

in appearance.<br />

161


162


STUDY 10<br />

DEVELOPING A SCALE TO MEASURE THE IMPACT OF<br />

APPEARANCE CONCERNS ON INTIMATE BEHAVIOURS<br />

Study leads: Elizabeth Jenkinson, Alex Clarke, Tim Moss and Eleanor Walsh<br />

163


SUMMARY<br />

• <strong>The</strong> study involved <strong>the</strong> construction of a scale to measure intimate behaviours and<br />

validation to establish construct validity.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> scale was administered to 145 students to validate and piloted on sample of 41 outpatient<br />

clinic attendees along with o<strong>the</strong>r standardized scales.<br />

• Scoring profiles were very different for <strong>the</strong> two populations. 4 items were normally<br />

distributed for <strong>the</strong> student population and 2 for <strong>the</strong> clinic sample. None were normally<br />

distributed across <strong>the</strong> two groups.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> clinic sample also <strong>report</strong>ed that <strong>the</strong>ir intimate behaviour was greatly affected by <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance. In contrast, <strong>the</strong> student sample showed little effect.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> results showed that <strong>the</strong> role of appearance concerns in intimacy is a significant<br />

issue for many people with visible differences.<br />

• Good construct and face validity of <strong>the</strong> scale was established but floor and ceiling effects<br />

in <strong>the</strong> data suggests fur<strong>the</strong>r development of this scale is required.<br />

164


AIMS<br />

To develop and validate a scale to measure <strong>the</strong> impact of appearance on engagement in<br />

intimate behaviours for use in clinical settings with patients with visible differences.<br />

BACKGROUND<br />

Anecdotal <strong>report</strong>s from clinicians and from those affected suggest that a substantial number of<br />

people with visible differences in appearance experience problems in <strong>the</strong> initial stages of<br />

physical closeness such as touch, hugging or kissing. Surgical intervention and ongoing<br />

treatment can also mean that patients’ personal lives and relationships with partners are<br />

negatively impacted, and research into <strong>the</strong> psychosocial impact of visible differences highlights<br />

<strong>the</strong> high levels of social anxiety often experienced (Rumsey and Harcourt 2005).<br />

During <strong>the</strong> course of this research programme, members of <strong>the</strong> advisory panel queried why<br />

questions concerning intimate behaviours and sexual relationships were not included in <strong>the</strong><br />

questionnaire pack, as <strong>the</strong>y all agreed that this was an important factor in relation to adjustment<br />

to visible difference. <strong>The</strong> little research that has previously been conducted in this area does<br />

indicate that sexual relationships and intimacy can be affected by appearance concerns. Gamba<br />

and colleagues (1992) found that in patients with head and neck cancer, 74% <strong>report</strong>ed<br />

difficulties with <strong>the</strong>ir sexual relationship after surgery. Porter and colleagues (1990) also found<br />

that in patients with vitiligo, <strong>the</strong> majority <strong>report</strong>ed problems with sexual relationships but also<br />

with behaviours that might lead to intimacy, such as meeting strangers or showing <strong>the</strong>ir body to<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs. Ramsey and O’Reagan (1988) found patients with psoriasis <strong>report</strong>ed similar anxieties.<br />

Following a review of scales, none were deemed suitable for inclusion in <strong>the</strong> cross sectional<br />

questionnaire. Although scales for <strong>the</strong> measurement of sexual satisfaction and sexual behaviour<br />

exist (for overview see Popovic 2005), <strong>the</strong>y tended to neglect <strong>the</strong> types of more subtle social<br />

behaviours that may initiate sex (for example touching behaviours), which are often <strong>report</strong>ed as<br />

difficult by those with appearance concerns. <strong>The</strong> research team decided to address this problem<br />

by developing a scale during <strong>the</strong> course of <strong>the</strong> programme, which could be used in future<br />

research with this population.<br />

In addition to undertaking <strong>the</strong> construction of <strong>the</strong> scale, we sought to assess construct validity,<br />

and also to assess <strong>the</strong> usefulness of <strong>the</strong> tool when used as a research measure with <strong>the</strong><br />

general population. Previous measures developed primarily for use with clinical populations<br />

have proved valuable and responsive when administered to <strong>the</strong> general population (e.g.<br />

Derriford Appearance Scales; see Harris & Carr, 2001).<br />

In order to establish <strong>the</strong> construct validity of <strong>the</strong> scale (i.e. to establish whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> scale is<br />

measuring <strong>the</strong> concept it purports to measure) <strong>the</strong> scale was administered alongside a choice of<br />

psychometrically sound assessments which, based on a literature review, were expected to be<br />

<strong>the</strong>oretically related to <strong>the</strong> construct measured by <strong>the</strong> new intimacy scale.<br />

165


METHOD<br />

Participants<br />

Student sample<br />

One-hundred and forty-five students, from UWE and <strong>the</strong> surrounding area, aged between 18<br />

and 57 years old, completed <strong>the</strong> questionnaire packs (107 female, 38 male, mean age = 22<br />

years old). Ninety-eight percent identified <strong>the</strong>mselves as being heterosexual and <strong>the</strong> majority<br />

described <strong>the</strong>mselves as being ‘white British’ (89%). <strong>The</strong> majority of <strong>the</strong> sample (60%)<br />

described <strong>the</strong>mselves as being in an “exclusive relationship but not living toge<strong>the</strong>r”, reflecting<br />

<strong>the</strong> age and lifestyle of <strong>the</strong> majority of <strong>the</strong> respondents.<br />

Clinic sample<br />

Forty-one patients of <strong>the</strong> Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery department of <strong>the</strong> Royal Free<br />

Hospital, London, completed <strong>the</strong> above measures. <strong>The</strong>re was an even split between men and<br />

women (20:21 female-male ratio) aged between 19 and 51 years old (mean = 35 years old).<br />

Eighty percent described <strong>the</strong>mselves as ‘white British’ and <strong>the</strong> majority identified <strong>the</strong>mselves as<br />

being heterosexual (93%) and “single” (70%). <strong>The</strong> mean length of time since <strong>the</strong>ir last<br />

relationship was 4 months.<br />

Materials<br />

<strong>The</strong> questions in <strong>the</strong> scale were drafted by <strong>the</strong> research team on <strong>the</strong> basis of research, relevant<br />

scales and data from clinical interventions and fur<strong>the</strong>r developed through discussions with<br />

members of <strong>the</strong> advisory panel. <strong>The</strong> scale aimed to measure a range of intimate behaviours and<br />

was designed to be used in clinical practice or research to gauge <strong>the</strong> impact of appearance on<br />

engagement in <strong>the</strong>se behaviours.<br />

Example question:<br />

I feel uncomfortable hugging or being physically close to o<strong>the</strong>r people because of my<br />

appearance.<br />

Several measures were filled in by participants alongside <strong>the</strong> new 12 item Intimacy scale (Royal<br />

Free & Centre for Appearance Research Intimacy Scale (RoFCAR)) for validation on a student<br />

sample.<br />

Scales included in pack for <strong>the</strong> student sample:<br />

• <strong>The</strong> full Personal Assessment of Intimacy in Relationships scale (PAIR; Olson &<br />

Schaefer, 2000) which assesses emotional, social, sexual, intellectual and<br />

recreational intimacy.<br />

• Emotional/Social loneliness and isolation was measured using <strong>the</strong> Emotional-<br />

Social Loneliness Inventory (ESLI; Vincenze & Grabosky, 1987).<br />

166


• An abbreviated version of <strong>the</strong> Multidimensional Sexuality Questionnaire (MSQ;<br />

Snell, Fisher & Walters, 1993) assessed 8 out of <strong>the</strong> 12 original aspects of<br />

human sexuality. <strong>The</strong>se included sexual esteem, anxiety, satisfaction and<br />

assertiveness.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> ‘Appearance’ subscale of <strong>the</strong> Body Esteem questionnaire (Mendelson &<br />

White, 1997) which assesses general feelings towards body and appearance.<br />

<strong>The</strong> clinic sample completed <strong>the</strong> following scales as part of <strong>the</strong>ir routine care, alongside <strong>the</strong><br />

intimacy scale:<br />

RESULTS<br />

Students<br />

• Derriford Appearance Scale (DAS-24; Carr, Moss & Harris 2004)<br />

• Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS, Zigmond & Snaith 1983)<br />

• Fear of Negative Evaluation scale (FNE; Leary, 1983)<br />

<strong>The</strong> mean score for <strong>the</strong> ‘perceived’ subscale of <strong>the</strong> PAIR was 388.96 (SD = 69.9) out of a<br />

potential 576, which indicates a relatively high perceived intimacy score compared with ranges<br />

indicated by Schaeffer and Olson (2000). This mirrored <strong>the</strong> ‘expected’ subscale as <strong>the</strong> mean<br />

score was 344.80 (SD = 22.5) out of a potential 480.<br />

<strong>The</strong> students’ responses on <strong>the</strong> ESLI indicated little or no emotional isolation (M= 4.05),<br />

emotional loneliness (M=5.21), social isolation (M = 4.06) or social loneliness (M= 5.19).<br />

<strong>The</strong> MSQ indicated that <strong>the</strong> students scored ‘average’ values on sexual esteem (M=11.64,<br />

SD=4.4), fear of sexual relationships (M=11.53, SD=64.9), and sexual assertiveness (M=12.59,<br />

SD=4.33). <strong>The</strong> students scored low values on sexual depression (M=8.10, SD=64.55) and<br />

anxiety (M=3.72, SD=4.18) and external sexual control (M=3.29, SD=3.34). <strong>The</strong>y scored highly<br />

on sexual satisfaction (M=14.09, SD=4.93) and internal sexual control (M=12.59, SD=4.33).<br />

Scores on <strong>the</strong> Body Esteem ‘Appearance’ subscale were lower than <strong>the</strong> average (M=18.72,<br />

SD=7.53) indicating relatively high appearance esteem.<br />

<strong>The</strong> students scored low on <strong>the</strong> ROFCAR Intimacy Scale (M=16.09, mean SD=8.57), out of a<br />

potential score of 40, which indicates that <strong>the</strong>ir appearance had relatively little impact on<br />

intimacy and sexual behaviour.<br />

167


Clinic attendees<br />

On <strong>the</strong> anxiety subscale of HADS <strong>the</strong> clinic sample was judged to have mild anxiety issues, with<br />

a mean score of just over <strong>the</strong> clinical cut-off for anxiety (M=8.73, SD=5.20). On <strong>the</strong> depression<br />

subscale of this measure <strong>the</strong> majority of <strong>the</strong> clinic sample scored below <strong>the</strong> clinical cut off for<br />

depression (M=6.70, SD=4.86).<br />

However, <strong>the</strong> DAS-24 scores indicated that <strong>the</strong> clinic sample were more concerned about <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance (M=54.96, SD=5.16) than <strong>the</strong> norms indicated by Carr, Moss & Harris (2005).<br />

<strong>The</strong> clinic sample also scored highly on <strong>the</strong> Fear of Negative Evaluation scale (M=32.52,<br />

SD=8.73) indicating that this sample have high levels of social anxiety.<br />

<strong>The</strong> clinic sample scored highly on <strong>the</strong> RoFCAR Scale (M=34.92, SD=12.41), more than double<br />

<strong>the</strong> mean of <strong>the</strong> student sample.<br />

Validation Results<br />

Four items from <strong>the</strong> RoFCAR Intimacy Scale were normally distributed in <strong>the</strong> student sample:<br />

7. I prefer sex with <strong>the</strong> light off because it means that my body cannot be seen.<br />

8. I feel that I could face <strong>the</strong> prospect of someone new seeing what I look like naked.<br />

9. During sex, I use/would use concealing clothing and/or choose positions to hide aspects of<br />

my appearance.<br />

12. I feel comfortable with my appearance in intimate situations.<br />

Two items from <strong>the</strong> Intimacy Scale were normally distributed in <strong>the</strong> clinic sample:<br />

1. I feel physically attractive to my partner/potential partners<br />

3. I am reluctant to kiss my partner/potential partner because I feel physically unattractive to<br />

<strong>the</strong>m<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore, <strong>the</strong> analysis shows that none of <strong>the</strong> items are normally distributed on both of <strong>the</strong><br />

samples and that <strong>the</strong> validation study highlights <strong>the</strong> differences between <strong>the</strong>se groups. <strong>The</strong><br />

clinic sample self <strong>report</strong>ed that <strong>the</strong>ir intimate behaviour was greatly affected by <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance, showing skewed mean scores at <strong>the</strong> extreme end of <strong>the</strong> scale. <strong>The</strong> student sample<br />

showed <strong>the</strong> mean scores skewed towards <strong>the</strong> bottom end of <strong>the</strong> scale and <strong>report</strong>ed little impact<br />

of <strong>the</strong>ir appearance on <strong>the</strong>ir intimate behaviours.<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> results show that <strong>the</strong> pilot version of <strong>the</strong> RoFCAR intimacy scale has good construct and<br />

face validity. <strong>The</strong> scale covers issues that are, in <strong>the</strong> main, not a problem to young student non-<br />

168


patient populations, but in contrast pose significant difficulties for those presenting to surgical<br />

clinics.<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>r, this shows that people with visible differences have pronounced and sometimes<br />

debilitating concerns about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance which impact on <strong>the</strong>ir engagement in intimate<br />

behaviours. Socialising with potential partners and hugging were self-<strong>report</strong>ed as problematic<br />

for a large proportion of this sample of patients seeking surgery. This supports <strong>the</strong> limited<br />

previous research with condition specific groups (e.g. Porter et al 1990) and is in line with <strong>the</strong><br />

high levels of social anxiety experienced by this group. Future research considering <strong>the</strong> impact<br />

of appearance concerns on intimacy with people with visible differences is required in order to<br />

develop <strong>the</strong> scale fur<strong>the</strong>r. <strong>The</strong> ceiling effects on responses to <strong>the</strong> questions by <strong>the</strong> clinic sample<br />

indicate that future versions of <strong>the</strong> scale need to include more non-sexual intimate behaviours<br />

(e.g. touch, dating, talking to potential partners, exposure of body parts) and <strong>the</strong>ir anticipatory<br />

cognitions about performing <strong>the</strong>se behaviours (worry about dating, looking attractive to o<strong>the</strong>rs).<br />

CONCLUSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> role of appearance concerns in intimacy and sexual relationships is an under-researched<br />

but important issue to consider when seeking to understand <strong>the</strong> adjustment of people with<br />

visible differences. Developing a more robust scale would allow <strong>the</strong> measurement of early<br />

avoidance behaviour and <strong>the</strong> opportunity to offer appropriate intervention sooner. It would also<br />

provide an important outcome measure for both psychosocial and surgical interventions for<br />

people with appearance-related concerns. This remains a priority for future research in this<br />

area.<br />

169


170


STUDY 11<br />

WOMEN WITH LIMB PROSTHESES: EXPERIENCES AND<br />

ADJUSTMENT<br />

Study leads: Rob Newell, Andrew Thompson, Sally Clarke<br />

171


SUMMARY<br />

• Semi-structured interviews were conducted with 6 women aged 29 to 67 with limb<br />

pros<strong>the</strong>ses, and analysed using <strong>the</strong>matic analysis.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> following <strong>the</strong>mes were identified from participants’ responses: psychological<br />

adaptation; physical adaptation; independence; <strong>the</strong> role of o<strong>the</strong>rs; emotional impact and<br />

support; social impact; body image & self image.<br />

• In <strong>the</strong> main respondents mostly described adaptation to loss or absence of a limb in<br />

positive terms, and rarely described high levels of distress, despite acknowledging <strong>the</strong><br />

extent of <strong>the</strong> task involved in adaptation. <strong>The</strong>re was an emphasis on <strong>the</strong> need for a<br />

positive mental attitude, involving confronting difficulties and asserting independence<br />

and personhood.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> attitudes and behaviours of members of <strong>the</strong> public and social acquaintances were a<br />

source of discomfort to participants. It was also clear that participants spent<br />

considerable time predicting, observing, preparing for and responding to <strong>the</strong> actual or<br />

assumed reactions of o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

• This is small sample, and so <strong>the</strong> picture of successful adaptation, whilst encouraging, is<br />

not necessarily <strong>the</strong> norm, and understanding of <strong>the</strong> concerns of this group would benefit<br />

from a larger qualitative scale study.<br />

172


INTRODUCTION<br />

People with a limb absent from birth or through trauma or disease are subject to significant<br />

challenges as a result, independent of <strong>the</strong> direct consequences of <strong>the</strong> original source of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

absent limb. Amongst <strong>the</strong>se are phantom pains and o<strong>the</strong>r phantom phenomena (Katz &<br />

Melzack 1990), as well as poorer quality of life and o<strong>the</strong>r psychosocial indicators (Demet et al<br />

2003).<br />

Use of pros<strong>the</strong>ses where a limb is absent or lost is an aspect of development or rehabilitation of<br />

physical function. However, surprisingly, little is known about patterns of pros<strong>the</strong>tic use (Murray<br />

& Fox 2002) or psychological adaptation to <strong>the</strong> loss or absence of a limb, as research is<br />

generally limited to anecdotal <strong>report</strong>s and small surveys (Rybarczyk et al 1995). In particular,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re is little examination of <strong>the</strong> attitudes of <strong>the</strong> person using <strong>the</strong> pros<strong>the</strong>sis or <strong>the</strong>ir experiences<br />

of pros<strong>the</strong>sis use, focusing instead on functional characteristics of <strong>the</strong> pros<strong>the</strong>sis and <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

putative effects on use (e.g. Millstein et al 1986, Balance et al 1989).<br />

A recent study (Saradjian et al 2007) used interviews to focus on <strong>the</strong> experiences of 11 male<br />

amputees, and found that participants <strong>report</strong>ed adjustment to reduce <strong>the</strong>ir sense of difference<br />

using a positive coping style. However, <strong>the</strong>re is some suggestion from <strong>the</strong> results that <strong>the</strong><br />

experiences of men and women of limb absence and pros<strong>the</strong>sis use may be different.<br />

Accordingly, <strong>the</strong> current study aimed to complement <strong>the</strong> work of Saradjian et al by interviewing<br />

women with experience of limb absence and pros<strong>the</strong>tic use.<br />

METHOD<br />

79 women with pros<strong>the</strong>ses were approached from <strong>the</strong> cohort of respondents to <strong>the</strong> ARC survey<br />

who had agreed to be contacted with regard to fur<strong>the</strong>r participation. A group of 15 respondents<br />

was initially sought. Ethics approval was sought and granted from <strong>the</strong> original NRES panel as a<br />

substantial amendment to <strong>the</strong> original study protocol.<br />

Interviews were conducted using a brief semi-structured interview schedule devised by <strong>the</strong><br />

investigators and broadly following <strong>the</strong> issues covered in Saradjian et al (2007).<br />

Respondents were approached via <strong>the</strong> post and consent to participate was sought. Participants<br />

were interviewed at a venue of <strong>the</strong>ir choice, consent was sought for audiotaping, and <strong>the</strong><br />

interviews were taped and transcribed. Data were analysed using <strong>the</strong>matic analysis and<br />

following <strong>the</strong> pragmatic approach of Burnard (1991) to organisation of <strong>the</strong> data.<br />

RESULTS<br />

Six women agreed to participate in <strong>the</strong> study. <strong>The</strong>ir ages ranged from 29 to 67 years. Four had<br />

lower limb pros<strong>the</strong>ses and two had upper limb pros<strong>the</strong>ses.<br />

173


Seven key <strong>the</strong>mes were identified and are <strong>report</strong>ed below. Sub<strong>the</strong>mes are <strong>report</strong>ed where<br />

appropriate. <strong>The</strong>re were clear links between sub<strong>the</strong>mes in different <strong>the</strong>me headings, and<br />

between different key <strong>the</strong>mes, and <strong>the</strong>se are discussed.<br />

1. Independence<br />

This issue was mentioned by all respondents, often extensively. Respondents viewed<br />

independence both as a number of physical tasks to be carried out and as a state of mind:<br />

‘whe<strong>the</strong>r I'm in a wheelchair or whe<strong>the</strong>r I'm walking I'll be fairly independent<br />

because I'm quite an independent minded person, so I shall be able to get on<br />

one way or ano<strong>the</strong>r.’ (Respondent 2)<br />

1a. Doing things for yourself<br />

All respondents regarded this as being very important, and as a desirable and appropriate way<br />

to behave, but also as in some ways inevitable, even in <strong>the</strong> face of difficulty:<br />

“Well I have a little motto that I live by and it's pain if you do and pain if you don’t”<br />

(Respondent 4)<br />

1b. Role of o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

<strong>The</strong> role of o<strong>the</strong>r people crops up repeatedly in <strong>the</strong>se accounts. With regard to independence,<br />

respondents felt that o<strong>the</strong>rs could ei<strong>the</strong>r help or hinder <strong>the</strong>m in being independent (although<br />

often quite inadvertently):<br />

1c. Mobility<br />

“I mean she was a lovely lady but she found it difficult not to keep helping me and<br />

I can remember to this day going rigid when she wanted to take my arm to cross<br />

<strong>the</strong> road, poor lady yeah, and I have difficulty with anybody doing that.”<br />

(Respondent 1)<br />

“Yeah, I ask people at Sainsbury’s to reach things off <strong>the</strong> top shelf because I<br />

can't reach <strong>the</strong>m and I think that doesn’t matter that I'm short but I wouldn’t want<br />

to ask anybody to help me to do something because I've got an artificial arm”<br />

(Respondent 6)<br />

As might be expected, mobility was more of an issue for respondents with lower limb<br />

pros<strong>the</strong>ses, and also emerges under <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>me of physical adaptation (below). Mobility was a<br />

key to independence, and, for this reason, respondents went to considerable lengths to maintain<br />

it.<br />

174


1d. Not giving in<br />

<strong>The</strong> notion of refusing to give in to <strong>the</strong> possible limitations of loss of a limb was also <strong>report</strong>ed.<br />

“But I'm not willing to be a non person, that is just not going to happen, <strong>the</strong><br />

wheelchair is a means of conveying me, just like my legs are, a means of<br />

conveying me so as far as I'm concerned but people do <strong>the</strong>y have, <strong>the</strong>y think oh<br />

that’s it, wheelchair, you can't do this, you can't do that, you can't do <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r but<br />

outside of dancing <strong>the</strong>re isn’t anything I can't do basically because I'm<br />

advantaged because I still have a good leg,” (Respondent 2)<br />

2. Psychological adaptation<br />

Coming to terms with loss is commonly described in <strong>the</strong> literature, particularly in <strong>the</strong> context of<br />

grief. Whilst respondents occasionally expressed regret (see Emotional Impact, below), <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

account of adapting to <strong>the</strong> limb absence did not carry this sense. On <strong>the</strong> contrary, limb absence<br />

was seen as a challenge which, whilst not welcome, had to be faced.<br />

2a. Positive mental outlook<br />

Most respondents describe <strong>the</strong> need for this, although <strong>the</strong> character of <strong>the</strong>ir descriptions was<br />

very varied, from matter-of-fact stoicism, as in this account of difficulties wearing necklaces:<br />

“It doesn’t bo<strong>the</strong>r me that much that I can't wear it, I don’t weep every night<br />

thinking oh I want to wear a diamond.” (Respondent 6)<br />

to highly developed statements of an enduring attitude to life:<br />

“Yes if you’ve got barriers up in your head that’s it you will not move, and you will<br />

be that person in that wheelchair and I just can't do it. Get on your leg and get<br />

walking for heavens sake, you’ve got nothing wrong, <strong>the</strong>re's absolutely nothing<br />

wrong with you, <strong>the</strong>re's nothing wrong with me. Like I say <strong>the</strong> barriers are up in<br />

your head.” (Respondent 4)<br />

2b. Confrontation / Getting on with it<br />

Avoidance of activities was almost always regarded as negative.<br />

“Yeah. I see people at <strong>the</strong> artificial limb centre that have got less off than me,<br />

<strong>the</strong>y’ve just got <strong>the</strong> foot off and <strong>the</strong>y're walking around on crutches, oh I need<br />

help, oh I need, oh I've got to have <strong>the</strong>se, I can't wear <strong>the</strong> leg, it's too painful.<br />

What, pain if you do and pain if you don’t. I can't get my head round that.”<br />

(Respondent 4)<br />

175


“I've heard tell that a lot of people that just sit <strong>the</strong>re and you know moan and<br />

groan and things like that and people that just won’t get on with it, some people<br />

can't but I think it's that what's within you ….” (Respondent 3)<br />

2c. Onset/reaction<br />

We asked specifically about <strong>the</strong> circumstances in which respondents had come to lose a limb.<br />

Several of <strong>the</strong> respondents had amputations in childhood (in one case almost certainly too<br />

young to have any direct memory). <strong>The</strong>se respondents noted that being so young had helped<br />

<strong>the</strong>m to cope with amputation because <strong>the</strong>y had little to compare it with.<br />

3. O<strong>the</strong>r people<br />

<strong>The</strong> reactions of o<strong>the</strong>rs was a specific prompt in our interview schedule, as it is an abiding<br />

feature of research into visible difference since its inception. Respondents spoke of <strong>the</strong><br />

reactions of o<strong>the</strong>r people to <strong>the</strong>ir missing limb and <strong>the</strong>ir beliefs about o<strong>the</strong>r people’s reactions in<br />

connections with many aspects of <strong>the</strong> interview, not just in response to this specific question. It<br />

seemed clear that a considerable amount of <strong>the</strong>ir time was occupied with consideration of <strong>the</strong><br />

reactions, assumed reactions or <strong>the</strong> predicted reactions of o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

3a. O<strong>the</strong>r people’s behaviours<br />

Typically, <strong>the</strong>se reactions were negative, at least from <strong>the</strong> point of view of <strong>the</strong> respondent.<br />

“also in my career, when I've been for interviews, I went for a job interview as an<br />

insurance clerk, collecting insurances and I went into <strong>the</strong> office for <strong>the</strong> interview<br />

and <strong>the</strong> chap just turned round and looked at my legs and said sorry we can't<br />

deal with someone like you.” (Respondent 4)<br />

“I've had people come up to me, not even said hello and said do you go<br />

swimming in your leg and I say no I'd sink like a stone love.” (Respondent 4)<br />

“when I go to <strong>the</strong> Juniors I've got to walk through some classrooms to get to my<br />

base so <strong>the</strong>re will be so many of <strong>the</strong>m ei<strong>the</strong>r looking or pointing or saying<br />

something” (Respondent 6).<br />

Members of <strong>the</strong> public appeared to regard it as acceptable to interact with respondents in ways<br />

which would normally be regarded as impolite, intrusive and harassing. One respondent inferred<br />

from this that it was because having a missing limb meant that <strong>the</strong>y were less of a person:<br />

“Like you're less of a human being as <strong>the</strong>y are,” (Respondent 1)<br />

Respondent 1 did, however, go on to note that: ‘but that’s all in your own head I think, I don’t<br />

think <strong>the</strong>y're thinking that <strong>the</strong>y just think what's <strong>the</strong> matter with her.’ before going on to give a<br />

176


fur<strong>the</strong>r example of how intrusive and unwelcome people’s reactions could be. In this context,<br />

her interpretation may perhaps be regarded as overgenerous, or as a way of protecting herself<br />

from <strong>the</strong> negative self image which might arise from <strong>the</strong> repeated intrusions of o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

It should also be noted that <strong>the</strong>re were positive accounts of <strong>the</strong> actions of o<strong>the</strong>rs, although <strong>the</strong>se<br />

were typically people whom <strong>the</strong> respondent knew well.<br />

3b. Beliefs about o<strong>the</strong>r people’s attitudes<br />

Respondent 1’s comments do, however, raise <strong>the</strong> general issue of <strong>the</strong> difficulty of distinguishing<br />

between <strong>the</strong> actual attitudes of <strong>the</strong> public and one’s own interpretation of <strong>the</strong>ir behaviours.<br />

Certainly respondents <strong>report</strong>ed many such interpretations:<br />

“I always wear <strong>the</strong> artificial limb because I don’t want people to look at me and<br />

think <strong>the</strong>re's <strong>the</strong> lady with one arm.” (Respondent 6)<br />

“I'd feel self conscious that people would notice whereas maybe <strong>the</strong>y don’t,<br />

normally you wouldn’t get a second glance, people would notice, have a look at<br />

you whereas <strong>the</strong>y wouldn’t normally.” (Respondent 5)<br />

“<strong>the</strong>y're ok with <strong>the</strong> wheelchair I think but <strong>the</strong>y're much happier when <strong>the</strong> see me<br />

walking because that is <strong>the</strong>ir image of me.” (Respondent 2)<br />

3c. Being / appearing normal<br />

Appearing ‘normal’ to o<strong>the</strong>rs was mentioned by several respondents, who thus shared <strong>the</strong> views<br />

of our culture about appropriate ways to present <strong>the</strong> self. This idea of normality was usually tied<br />

to <strong>the</strong> idea of concealment and with body image and appearance (see Body Image below).<br />

“Of normality, even though <strong>the</strong>y're not particularly attractive and obviously <strong>the</strong>re's<br />

things that could be done with <strong>the</strong>m and <strong>the</strong>y are uncomfortable sometimes but<br />

outwardly at <strong>the</strong> first glance people think of you as normal, people often say <strong>the</strong>y<br />

don’t notice I've got an artificial arm until several months, weeks later”<br />

(Respondent 6 [describing rationale for wearing pros<strong>the</strong>sis])<br />

“…it is just a bit of camouflage, I think that’s <strong>the</strong> main thing.” (Respondent 5)<br />

“For a good while I didn’t go out, not until I felt that, I wanted to be able to be as<br />

normal as I possibly could” (Respondent 3)<br />

177


4. Physical adaptation<br />

From so small a sample, it was surprising that such a considerable range of everyday issues<br />

arose, and <strong>the</strong> practicalities of pros<strong>the</strong>sis use were clearly of great salience in <strong>the</strong> minds of<br />

respondents.<br />

4a. Pain & discomfort<br />

This issue was <strong>report</strong>ed by all respondents with lower limb pros<strong>the</strong>ses, and, whilst a source of<br />

frustration and distress, was regarded as inevitable.<br />

“and it can be painful, and it can rub because it's right into your groin and it's very<br />

uncomfortable, it's not <strong>the</strong> kind of thing you can kind of forget about like it's at <strong>the</strong><br />

back of your mind, it's always <strong>the</strong>re right at <strong>the</strong> front of your mind” (Respondent<br />

2)<br />

4b. Mobility/access<br />

Issues of mobility affected all aspects of <strong>the</strong> lives of those with lower limb pros<strong>the</strong>ses.<br />

“Yeah obviously like driving, because it's my left leg that’s amputated I can't feel<br />

<strong>the</strong> clutch so I feel safer to drive an automatic car where I don’t have to use <strong>the</strong><br />

clutch. You know things like climbing up steps an things like that to get on to <strong>the</strong><br />

top of <strong>the</strong> units, you can't feel where your foot is so it's judgement more than<br />

anything because you can't feel it” (Respondent 3)<br />

“Well travel, it's made me very nervous about travel. Simple things like if you fly<br />

<strong>the</strong>se days people rush about and especially if it's Rayanair or something people<br />

are rushing to get to seats and I can't do that” (Respondent 1)<br />

4c. Importance of pros<strong>the</strong>sis & fitting service<br />

Because of <strong>the</strong> issues around pain, mobility, function and independence, a well fitting pros<strong>the</strong>sis<br />

was of great importance. A sympa<strong>the</strong>tic pros<strong>the</strong>tist was highly valued.<br />

“Aes<strong>the</strong>tically it was nicer but functionally, practicality no it wasn’t hard wearing<br />

and durable like <strong>the</strong> rest of me, I need something that I can stand on and know<br />

full well that it's going to be reliable and give as much as I can give it.”<br />

(Respondent 4)<br />

“I'm having a very good relationship with <strong>the</strong> clinic at Sheffield. I've got a very<br />

short stump and it's something called an abduction so it looks as thought it goes<br />

one way but you actually have to fit <strong>the</strong> limb <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r way and I do dread having<br />

178


to change pros<strong>the</strong>tists because <strong>the</strong>y have to re-learn but I've been with Rose<br />

ever since I came here 7 years and she's just fantastic.” (Respondent 1)<br />

5. Body Image<br />

5a. Being/appearing normal<br />

“It sounds silly doesn’t it, you know you get women that have had mastectomies and<br />

<strong>the</strong>y wear a false boob, it's just so that outward appearances your normal aren’t you,<br />

that’s what everybody wants.” (Respondent 5)<br />

5b. O<strong>the</strong>r people<br />

“so I know really that’s it's a lot more noticeable to me than it is to anybody else<br />

and it's easy to say that you magnify your flaws don’t you but I always tell myself<br />

that it is a bit lumpy and a bit, it doesn’t look completely natural, it doesn’t move,<br />

it's not <strong>the</strong> best thing, I know in reality that most people aren’t going to notice<br />

that…” (Respondent 5)<br />

“I just wanted to wear something that would cover it and not let people see it”’<br />

(Respondent 3)<br />

5c. Pros<strong>the</strong>sis as part of body/person<br />

“it's like saying what's it like to wear a bra, it's something you just do and you<br />

don’t notice you're wearing it, you know it's just part of me” (Respondent 6)<br />

“If I name my leg it feels more a part of me, it is mine, it's my leg. Some people<br />

think of a false leg as an entity as an alien being that’s got to be strapped to<br />

<strong>the</strong>m, I don’t. It is my leg it is part of me and that is it. That’s who I am.”<br />

(Respondent 4)<br />

“I can never accept it as me although it's something I have got, o<strong>the</strong>rwise I don’t<br />

walk, I've got to use it every day o<strong>the</strong>rwise I don’t walk and I don’t want not to be<br />

able to walk. But it's still not me” (Respondent 3)<br />

5d. Role in self image<br />

“I've not actually lost who I am because it's just a leg, I am <strong>the</strong> same person,<br />

intellectually and emotionally I am <strong>the</strong> same person, I'm not a different person<br />

and to be honest my total image of myself isn’t about whe<strong>the</strong>r I can or cannot<br />

walk, so I have an image of myself which isn’t really about just <strong>the</strong> physicality of it<br />

because that’s just one part” (Respondent 2)<br />

179


6. Emotional impact & Emotional support<br />

6a. Friends and family<br />

Friends and family were mentioned as a frequent source of support, and one respondent noted<br />

<strong>the</strong> importance of people around one in allowing one to be oneself. By contrast, <strong>the</strong> possibility<br />

that friends and family were over-considerate or ‘mollycoddling’ was seen as negative.<br />

“I had a friend called J particularly, that was quite curious because J was a great<br />

support but actually when I got independent I didn’t want to be supported in that<br />

way anymore so that was quite a difficult time for us in <strong>the</strong> third year at university,<br />

she was still looking after me, she found it difficult to change.” (Respondent 1)<br />

6b. Religious faith<br />

Religious faith was mentioned by two respondents as a source of emotional support.<br />

6c. Negative emotional impact<br />

Although all respondents maintained a positive attitude in <strong>the</strong> present, <strong>the</strong>re were times for most<br />

when <strong>the</strong> impact of loss of a limb was felt negatively, including expressions of loss, regret,<br />

frustration.<br />

“You just sort of get frustrated with yourself sometimes because you know <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>ory of how to do things but you can't always put it in to practice.” (Respondent<br />

5)<br />

“<strong>The</strong>re's no o<strong>the</strong>r way of describing it, people say oh you get used to it, no you<br />

don’t, I don’t know who tells <strong>the</strong>m that nonsense that you get used to it, it's<br />

having a great big plastic thing shoved on your leg, and it's a great big heavy<br />

plastic thing and you're shuntering it along like Long John Sliver, it's rubbish, it's<br />

rubbish, that’s what it's like.” (Respondent 2)<br />

7. Social impact<br />

Although <strong>the</strong>re was a range of responses, respondents mostly <strong>report</strong>ed that limb absence had<br />

made little or no difference to <strong>the</strong>ir social lives. Respondent 5 noted that:<br />

“I'm quite a retiring person, I don’t like people looking at me, I don’t think it's<br />

anything to do with my arm, I just think it's my personality”<br />

All <strong>report</strong>ed having dates and boyfriends, and all ei<strong>the</strong>r were or had been married.<br />

7a. Intimacy<br />

180


We asked respondents specifically about intimate relationships, but responses were not<br />

extensive. Typically, no particular issues were described, and respondents did not seem to<br />

regard <strong>the</strong> topic as a critical issue. One responded more directly:<br />

“He touches me <strong>the</strong>re, it's not like it's out of bounds or anything, it's no more out<br />

of bounds than any o<strong>the</strong>r part of my body.” (Respondent 2)<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

Only a small number of women wished to participate in this element of <strong>the</strong> study, and it is<br />

doubtful whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>mes identified here are exhaustive. Moreover, it is entirely possible that<br />

<strong>the</strong> generally positive picture of adjustment presented here is a characteristic only of people<br />

who are willing to engage in detailed discussion of <strong>the</strong>ir experiences. Accordingly, broader<br />

recruitment of people from this group would be an important additional study. It would also be<br />

informative to include participants who choose not to wear pros<strong>the</strong>tic devices.<br />

Regardless of this limitation, <strong>the</strong> issues raised by respondents during <strong>the</strong> interviews offer a<br />

detailed impression of <strong>the</strong>ir experiences. Respondents generally <strong>report</strong>ed a pattern of<br />

successful adjustment to limb absence, even though <strong>the</strong>y acknowledged both that this was a<br />

process requiring effort and also that it was sometimes punctuated with periods of distress.<br />

Several of <strong>the</strong> areas <strong>report</strong>ed in Saradjian et al’s (2007) study were confirmed here (e.g. role of<br />

<strong>the</strong> pros<strong>the</strong>sis; role of support and social interaction, positive mental attitude). <strong>The</strong>re were also<br />

interesting departures from Saradjian et al. First, not all respondents <strong>report</strong>ed <strong>the</strong> use of <strong>the</strong><br />

pros<strong>the</strong>sis as a compensatory device in terms of a role in minimising <strong>the</strong>ir sense of difference<br />

from o<strong>the</strong>rs. In one case where <strong>the</strong> pros<strong>the</strong>sis was used in this way, <strong>the</strong> rationale was highly<br />

specific – it was a temporary measure so that o<strong>the</strong>rs would, on first acquaintance, not see <strong>the</strong><br />

respondent solely in terms of <strong>the</strong>ir limb absence. Second, although <strong>the</strong>re was some variation,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re were indications that <strong>the</strong> pros<strong>the</strong>sis contributed to <strong>the</strong> person’s body image and sense of<br />

self in a positive way. This was related by one respondent to <strong>the</strong> idea of acceptance of <strong>the</strong> self<br />

ra<strong>the</strong>r than acceptance of <strong>the</strong> pros<strong>the</strong>sis.<br />

<strong>The</strong> frequent previous finding in visible difference research with regard to <strong>the</strong> potentially<br />

negative role of o<strong>the</strong>r people was repeated here, as was <strong>the</strong> description of inferences and<br />

predictions about <strong>the</strong> attitudes and behaviours of o<strong>the</strong>rs. Confronting obstacles and regarding<br />

<strong>the</strong>m as challenges were identified as important in adaptation.<br />

<strong>The</strong> accounts by women with pros<strong>the</strong>ses in <strong>the</strong> current study add to <strong>the</strong> weight of evidence<br />

regarding <strong>the</strong> role of active coping strategies in mediating adjustment to visible difference,<br />

particularly in <strong>the</strong> face of often negative behaviours by members of <strong>the</strong> public. This in turn<br />

reinforces <strong>the</strong> importance of promoting both such active coping in people with visible<br />

difference,and education and attitude change in <strong>the</strong> general public.<br />

181


182


STUDY 12<br />

DOES APPEARANCE MATTER IN THE CONTEXT OF<br />

RHEUMATOID ARTHRITIS (RA)?<br />

Study leads: Hayley James, Stanton Newman, Michael Shipley, Abigail Olaleye, Samantha<br />

Moore,<br />

183


SUMMARY<br />

• This study investigates whe<strong>the</strong>r a visible hand disfigurement due to RA has an independent<br />

impact on psychological well being.<br />

• 49 RA patients with, and 32 with no visible hand disfigurement completed a booklet of<br />

psychosocial questionnaires<br />

• Those with an observable hand disfigurement scored higher on measures of functional<br />

disability and feelings of stigmatization. <strong>The</strong>y also rate <strong>the</strong>ir appearance more negatively<br />

than those with no hand disfigurement. When controlling for functional disability, all<br />

significant differences disappeared.<br />

• Participants with an observable hand disfigurement were 2.62 times more likely to be<br />

clinically depressed than those with no observable hand disfigurement. Conversely those<br />

with no observable hand disfigurement were 1.68 times more likely to be clinically anxious.<br />

• Regression analysis revealed that high ideal appearance discrepancy, low optimism and low<br />

satisfaction with social support predict higher levels of anxiety.<br />

• High functional disability and low optimism predict a majority of <strong>the</strong> variance in depression.<br />

• Appearance-related social anxiety and avoidance is predicted by age and living status, as<br />

well as objective hand disfigurement and <strong>the</strong> psychological variables of valence, salience,<br />

functional disability, satisfaction with social support and social acceptance.<br />

• Visible hand disfigurement does not independently affect mood, but may contribute to<br />

feelings of appearance-related social anxiety and avoidance.<br />

• Findings suggest that it is not <strong>the</strong> observable nature of <strong>the</strong> hand disfigurement but <strong>the</strong> visible<br />

disability when moving which may have an effect on feelings of stigmatization and <strong>the</strong><br />

participants’ evaluation of <strong>the</strong>ir own appearance. <strong>The</strong>refore, <strong>the</strong> outward signs of physical<br />

impairment may be regarded as highly salient.<br />

184


INTRODUCTION<br />

Rheumatoid arthritis (RA) is <strong>the</strong> single greatest cause of disability in <strong>the</strong> UK (Goff & Barasi,<br />

1999). At present <strong>the</strong>re is no treatment to ei<strong>the</strong>r cure or prevent <strong>the</strong> disease, so <strong>the</strong> main aim is<br />

to reduce its impact on peoples’ lives by limiting symptoms, reducing disability and improving<br />

quality of life (Pollard, Choy & Scott, 2005). Disfigurement of <strong>the</strong> hands is a common and<br />

significant complication of RA and once <strong>the</strong>se changes occur, <strong>the</strong>y are not reversible medically<br />

and may require surgery.<br />

Despite a large amount of research looking at psychological impact of RA during <strong>the</strong> course of<br />

<strong>the</strong> disease, very little research has focused on <strong>the</strong> psychological effects of body image and<br />

disfigurement. Early research on body image and attractiveness in RA has produced somewhat<br />

mixed results with some suggesting no difference from <strong>the</strong> normal population (Cornwell &<br />

Schmidt, 1990) and o<strong>the</strong>rs suggesting negative impacts on body image (Skevington, Blackwell<br />

& Britton, 1987). Rumsey, Clarke & Musa (2002) <strong>report</strong> higher levels of anxiety and depression<br />

in patients with rheumatic conditions including RA, compared with normative levels, as well as in<br />

comparison to people with burns, head and neck cancer and eye conditions. Those patients<br />

with rheumatic conditions experiencing higher levels of distress spoke of embarrassment, selfconsciousness<br />

and distress in relation to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance, with specific mention of <strong>the</strong><br />

appearance of <strong>the</strong>ir joints. Concerns regarding <strong>the</strong> noticeability of <strong>the</strong> condition were also<br />

<strong>report</strong>ed along with <strong>the</strong> use of avoidant and concealment behaviours. In a study on RA and<br />

lupus patients, Monaghan, Sharpe, Denton, Levy, Schrieber & Sensky (2007) found that<br />

perceived appearance and physical disability were predictive of depression but not anxiety in<br />

RA patients.<br />

Very few studies have focussed on <strong>the</strong> physical and psychological issues surrounding<br />

observable hand disfigurement in RA, despite this being <strong>the</strong> most overtly exposed area and <strong>the</strong><br />

identification that hands are one of <strong>the</strong> areas most affected by <strong>the</strong> illness (Cornwell & Schmidt,<br />

1990). Due to <strong>the</strong> lack of consistency in <strong>the</strong> results of previous research into appearance<br />

concerns this study investigates whe<strong>the</strong>r observer rated hand disfigurement due to RA has an<br />

independent impact on psychological well being.<br />

METHOD<br />

Participants<br />

49 RA patients with hand disfigurement due to RA rated as visible by <strong>the</strong> researcher and 32 RA<br />

patients with no observable hand disfigurement were included in <strong>the</strong> study. <strong>The</strong> mean age was<br />

58.64 years. Respondents were outpatients attending routine rheumatology clinics at University<br />

College London Hospital. Fur<strong>the</strong>r demographics details are displayed in Table 12.1.<br />

185


Table 12.1 Demographic data by group<br />

Group 1 2<br />

n 48 30<br />

Age M(range) 60(23-85) 56.47(29-78)<br />

Gender female n(%) 42(85.7) 25(78.1)<br />

Marital status Married or living with partner n(%) 32(65.3) 22(68.8)<br />

Living alone n(%) 11(22.4) 5(15.6)<br />

Living with friends or relatives n(%) 4(8.2) 4(12.5)<br />

Nationality British/English n(%) 44(89.8) 24(75)<br />

Ethnicity White n(%) 41(83.7) 25(78.1)<br />

Note: Group 1. Observable hand disfigurement; Group 2. No observable hand disfigurement<br />

Measures<br />

Using a checklist participants were asked to tick those areas of <strong>the</strong>ir body about which <strong>the</strong>y felt<br />

most sensitive and to indicate how difficult <strong>the</strong>y felt it was to hide or disguise this area on a<br />

seven point Likert scale ranging from 1 (extremely easy) to 7 (impossible).<br />

Participants completed <strong>the</strong> following battery of psychosocial measures:<br />

<strong>The</strong> Hospital Anxiety & Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond & Snaith, 1983)<br />

Derriford Appearance Scale short form (DAS 24; Moss, Harris & Carr, 2004)<br />

Feelings of social acceptance<br />

Physical Appearance Discrepancy Questionnaire (PADQ)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Life Orientation Test Revised (LOT-R; Scheier and Carver, 1987)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Social Networks Scale (Sarason, Levine, Basham & Sarason, 1983)<br />

CARVAL & CARSAL (Rosser, 2008)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Stanford Health Assessment Questionnaire-II (HAQ-II; Wolfe, Michaud & Pincus, 2004)<br />

– a measure of functional disability<br />

Feelings of Stigmatization questionnaire (FSQ) (Schmid-Ott, Jaeger, Kuensebeck, Ott &<br />

Lamprecht, 1996)<br />

RESULTS<br />

<strong>The</strong> majority (54%) of participants with an observer rated hand disfigurement considered <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

hands to be <strong>the</strong> main area of concern in terms of appearance. For <strong>the</strong> non-disfigured group <strong>the</strong><br />

area of most concern was <strong>the</strong> abdomen (22%).<br />

Participant characteristics<br />

An exploration of group characteristics revealed no significant differences in age between those<br />

with and without an observable hand difference and no significant association between gender,<br />

nationality, ethnicity or marital status and allocation to ei<strong>the</strong>r group.<br />

186


Preliminary analysis<br />

Mean, standard deviations and scale ranges for all variables according to group are presented<br />

in Table 12.2. Analysis reveals that those with an observable hand disfigurement experience<br />

higher levels of functional disability, perceived stigmatization, a more negative view of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance and felt <strong>the</strong>y were able to disguise <strong>the</strong>ir area of concern more than those with no<br />

observable hand disfigurement.<br />

Table 12.2: Means and standard deviations for all variables by group<br />

Group 1 2<br />

Variable Scale range M (SD) n M(SD) n<br />

Depression 0-21 8.78(3.31) 49 7.97(3.26) 32<br />

Anxiety 0-21 7.55(3.48) 49 7.31(3.94) 32<br />

DAS-24 11-96 33.62(11.89) 48 33.71(14.94) 32<br />

HAQ-II 0-30 1.50(0.81) 47 0.71(0.64) 32<br />

Disguisability 1-7 4.87(1.59) 45 3.69(1.67) 26<br />

Social acceptance 2-14 11.84(2.91) 49 12.13(2.86) 30<br />

Ideal PADQ 4-28 13.88(6.09) 49 12.73(6.53) 30<br />

Should PAQD 4-28 12.96(6.27) 49 11.65(6.63) 31<br />

LOT-R 4-20 14.96(3.03) 48 14.28(2.85) 32<br />

CARVAL 6-36 19.79(6.61) 47 16.28(7.73) 32<br />

CARSAL 7-36 29.20(8.26) 49 29.83(8.09) 30<br />

Perceived Stigma 10-60 24.65(7.69) 48 20.47(8.03) 32<br />

Note. Group 1. Observable hand disfigurement; Group 2. No observable hand disfigurement PADQ:<br />

Physical Appearance Discrepancy Questionnaire; LOT-R: Life Orientation Test Revised; CARVAL:<br />

Centre of Appearance Research Valence; CARSAL: Centre of Appearance Research Salience; DAS-24:<br />

Derriford Appearance Scale-24; HAQ-II: Health Assessment Questionnaire-II.<br />

In order to examine whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> differences observed between <strong>the</strong> groups were due to<br />

differences in functional disability, analyses were conducted controlling for functional disability<br />

scores. <strong>The</strong>se indicated that <strong>the</strong>re were no significant differences between <strong>the</strong> two groups on<br />

any of <strong>the</strong> variables except optimism. However, <strong>the</strong> difference in mean scores between <strong>the</strong><br />

group with a visible hand disfigurement and those with no visible hand disfigurement was also<br />

extremely small on this variable.<br />

Of <strong>the</strong> 49 RA patients with visible hand disfigurement, 14.3% were clinically anxious and 32.7%<br />

clinically depressed. Of 32 participants with no observable hand disfigurement 21.9% were<br />

clinically anxious and 15.6% clinically depressed. Results suggest that participants with an<br />

observable hand disfigurement were 2.62 times more likely to be clinically depressed than those<br />

with no observable hand disfigurement. Conversely those with no hand differences were 1.68<br />

times more likely to be clinical anxious.<br />

Predictors of anxious mood<br />

<strong>The</strong> demographic variables and <strong>the</strong> disfigurement grouping variable failed to make any<br />

significant contribution to <strong>the</strong> final model. After entry of <strong>the</strong> remaining variables, 42.7% of <strong>the</strong><br />

187


variance in anxious mood was accounted for by optimism, ideal discrepancy and satisfaction<br />

with social support (F (9,57) = 6.460, p


e significant. This suggests a close association between stigma, appearance valence and<br />

functional ability.<br />

<strong>The</strong> lower levels of appearance satisfaction in <strong>the</strong> disfigured group to some degree reflect <strong>the</strong><br />

work of o<strong>the</strong>r researchers (Skevington, et al., 1987). But <strong>the</strong>se results contrast with o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

studies that have found that body image concerns for RA patients and healthy controls<br />

approximate each o<strong>the</strong>r (Cornwell & Schmidt, 1990) or do not differ (Ben-Tovim & Walker,<br />

1995). <strong>The</strong> variability in <strong>the</strong>se results may reflect <strong>the</strong> differences in measurement techniques.<br />

A participant with an observable hand disfigurement was almost three times more likely to be<br />

clinically depressed than a participant those with no visible hand differences. This greater<br />

likelihood is noteworthy, but may to be due to <strong>the</strong> associated differences in functional disability<br />

ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> disfigurement itself. <strong>The</strong> percentage of RA patients with no observable hand<br />

disfigurement meeting caseness levels of depression are comparable to levels <strong>report</strong>ed in many<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r RA studies (Pincus, et al, 1996; Værøy, et al., 2005). Conversely, those with no<br />

observable hand disfigurement were almost twice as likely to be clinicaly anxious. One may<br />

speculate that this may be associated with <strong>the</strong> unpredictability of RA. Those who have hand<br />

involvement and resulting disability may feel things can get no worse while those with lower<br />

levels of disability (i.e. no hand involvement) may be anxious about future deterioration. Those<br />

with no visible hand disfigurement exhibited ei<strong>the</strong>r lower or comparable levels of clinical anxiety<br />

to that <strong>report</strong>ed in o<strong>the</strong>r studies (Söderlin, et al., 2000; El-Miedany & El-Rasheed, 2002).<br />

RA patients with high levels of ideal discrepancy, low optimism and lower satisfaction with social<br />

support had higher levels of anxious mood. Optimism was <strong>the</strong> most significant independent<br />

predictor of anxiety and <strong>the</strong> same variable was found to be <strong>the</strong> best predictor of depression. <strong>The</strong><br />

significant contribution of satisfaction with social support to anxiety highlights <strong>the</strong> need for<br />

quality social networks which provide <strong>the</strong> resources necessary for successful adaptation. This<br />

has been demonstrated in research for both recently diagnosed and long term RA sufferers<br />

(Evers, Kraaimaat, Geenen & Bijlsma, 1997).<br />

In common with o<strong>the</strong>r studies, <strong>the</strong> multivariate analysis demonstrated that, as well as lower<br />

levels of optimism, higher levels of functional disability were also predictive of depressed mood.<br />

Wright, Parker, Smarr, Schoenfeld-Smith, Buckelew, Slaughter Johnson, and Hewett (2005)<br />

found that a high degree of physical disability was one of <strong>the</strong> 3 best predictors of depression in<br />

RA. Persons with a high degree of disability meet more obstacles in everyday life and are more<br />

likely to be forced to give up valued activities. In addition, Katz and Yelin (1995) have shown<br />

that <strong>the</strong> loss of valued activities is strongly associated with <strong>the</strong> development of depression in<br />

RA. This contributed to <strong>the</strong> suggestion that functional disability is a risk factor for depression.<br />

This may be due to feelings of isolation which may occur due to <strong>the</strong> subsequent restriction in<br />

social activities.<br />

<strong>The</strong> multivariate analysis of social anxiety and avoidance showed a number of <strong>the</strong> variables<br />

studied to be significant. Notably, participants of a younger age were more likely to avoid and be<br />

anxious about social situations, confirming <strong>the</strong> relatively higher importance attached to<br />

189


appearance of younger people (Harris & Carr, 2001). Not surprisingly those with higher<br />

appearance-related salience and valence also engaged in greater social avoidance. Lower<br />

levels of satisfaction with social support and lower levels of acceptance, along with greater<br />

functional disability, were o<strong>the</strong>r predictors. Group differences did not significantly contribute to<br />

<strong>the</strong> explanation of variance in social anxiety and avoidance when it was initially introduced into<br />

<strong>the</strong> equation, although it did gain significance in <strong>the</strong> later stages of <strong>the</strong> analyses; this may<br />

however, be a statistical artefact.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are number of limitations which need to be recognised when interpreting <strong>the</strong>se data.<br />

Generalization of <strong>the</strong>se findings to <strong>the</strong> RA population is limited both by <strong>the</strong> modest sample size<br />

and by <strong>the</strong> potential selectivity bias as only those well enough to attend an outpatient clinic were<br />

included. Due to <strong>the</strong> cross sectional nature of <strong>the</strong> study it is not possible to draw conclusions<br />

about <strong>the</strong> direction of <strong>the</strong> causality. Fur<strong>the</strong>r comprehensive longitudinal studies with adequate<br />

power and <strong>the</strong>refore greater sample sizes are required to fur<strong>the</strong>r understand <strong>the</strong>se<br />

relationships.<br />

CONCLUSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> findings suggest that in <strong>the</strong> presence of a disabling condition such as RA, an objective<br />

judgement of visible hand disfigurement does not independently predict mood, but may<br />

contribute to feelings of appearance-related social anxiety and avoidance. Conversely,<br />

subjective judgements of appearance play an integral role in anxiety (HADS) and appearancerelated<br />

anxiety and avoidance (DAS 24). It is not <strong>the</strong> observable nature of <strong>the</strong> hand<br />

disfigurement, but <strong>the</strong> resultant functional disability and possible visibility of this disability (in not<br />

being able to complete tasks, for example) which has an effect on feelings of stigmatization and<br />

<strong>the</strong> evaluation of <strong>the</strong>ir own appearance. This suggests that disfigurement is not necessarily just<br />

about how a person looks in terms of <strong>the</strong>ir body, skin or facial features, but encompasses how a<br />

person appears as a whole, particularly when <strong>the</strong>y move. Signs of physical impairment may be<br />

considered as a highly salient form of visible difference.<br />

190


STUDY 13<br />

WORKING WITH PATIENTS WITH VISIBLE DIFFERENCES:<br />

GENERAL PRACTITIONERS’ BELIEFS, DECISION MAKING<br />

PROCESSES AND TRAINING NEEDS.<br />

Study leads: Elizabeth Jenkinson and Tim Moss<br />

191


SUMMARY<br />

• This study is recruiting UK based General Practitioners (target n=30) to complete a<br />

questionnaire incorporating questions about previous training and understanding in <strong>the</strong><br />

field and vignettes about hypo<strong>the</strong>tical patients<br />

• Vignettes include systematic variations in variables representing severity and visibility of<br />

<strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>tical patient’s condition and levels of psychosocial distress.<br />

• Data collection is still in progress (n=21 completed returned questionnaires to date) and<br />

is expected to be completed in October 2009.<br />

• Data will be analysed using hierarchical stepwise regression analyses to ascertain which<br />

cues GP’s rely on in <strong>the</strong>ir decision making regarding referral to medical, psychological<br />

and lay led services.<br />

192


AIMS<br />

To investigate general practitioners’ beliefs and assumptions about which factors mediate and<br />

moderate adjustment to disfiguring conditions<br />

To investigate <strong>the</strong> factors which general practitioners use as cues in decision making<br />

regarding referral to psychological services<br />

To identify <strong>the</strong> training needs of general practitioners who work with patients with visible<br />

differences and to make recommendations for future training<br />

BACKGROUND<br />

GPs have been identified as ‘gatekeepers’ to existing psychological support services by<br />

psychologists working in <strong>the</strong> field of appearance (Rumsey and Harcourt 2005). Despite <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

pivotal role, GPs are not routinely trained in dealing with <strong>the</strong> psychosocial needs of those with<br />

appearance concerns and may find it hard to identify appearance-related distress from o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

health concerns or mental illness. When appearance issues are acknowledged, Hopwood and<br />

Maguire (1988) suggest that those working with <strong>the</strong>se patients often underestimate <strong>the</strong><br />

problems of adjusting to <strong>the</strong>ir appearance. This may be due to an over-reliance on <strong>the</strong><br />

biomedical approach to <strong>the</strong> assessment of patient adjustment. A growing evidence base of<br />

psychological research suggests biomedical factors such visibility, severity and type of condition<br />

are, in fact, not reliable predictors of psychological adjustment (Robinson 1997, Rumsey,<br />

Clarke, White and Hooper 2003), and advocates adopting a more biopsychosocial approach<br />

(Engel 1980) which considers psychological factors alongside physical assessment.<br />

Charlton, Rumsey, Partridge, Barlow and Saul (2003) suggested that efforts should be made to<br />

enable GPs to appropriately identify and advise patients with concerns about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance<br />

and noted that more support and training is needed in this area. <strong>The</strong> authors considered that<br />

consultations with patients presenting with appearance issues present a quandary for GPs. <strong>The</strong><br />

decision to refer for surgical intervention may not always be appropriate. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore <strong>the</strong><br />

expectations of patients that <strong>the</strong>ir visible difference will be aes<strong>the</strong>tically improved through<br />

surgery may be raised and lead patients to believe feelings of self-consciousness about <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance may be cured. However referral for surgical or medical treatment may be <strong>the</strong> only<br />

option, as <strong>the</strong> alternative of offering psychological support may not be feasible where services<br />

are not in <strong>the</strong> locality or in cases in which patients are not willing to engage with psychological<br />

input. Charlton and colleagues argue that more research is needed at primary care level to<br />

gauge current levels of GP knowledge or training in working with people with disfiguring<br />

conditions, and to inform education for practitioners.<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore, <strong>the</strong> focus of this research is to understand <strong>the</strong> existing perceptions, beliefs and<br />

referral patterns of GPs in relation to working with patients with disfigurements, and recognises<br />

that this understanding is key to designing adequate training packages.<br />

193


METHOD<br />

Participants<br />

GPs from across <strong>the</strong> UK who come into contact with patients with a wide range of appearance<br />

concerns and visible differences, are currently being sought (n=30) and recruited via<br />

advertisements and word of mouth.<br />

All participants are required to be ei<strong>the</strong>r a GP or GP in training currently working in primary care,<br />

able to converse in English, and able to undertake <strong>the</strong> questionnaire without supervision. All<br />

GPs who expressed interest in <strong>the</strong> research and fitted <strong>the</strong> eligibility criteria are being invited to<br />

take part.<br />

Participants can access information about <strong>the</strong> study by logging onto a site hosted by <strong>the</strong> UWE<br />

network, or by requesting a pack including an information sheet and consent form directly from<br />

<strong>the</strong> researcher via email, post or telephone.<br />

Design<br />

A mixed methodological approach is being adopted. Previous research in related areas of<br />

health psychology and decision making suggests that <strong>the</strong> levels of self-insight health care<br />

professionals have into which factors or ‘cues’ <strong>the</strong>y use when making decisions about patient<br />

care often does not mirror <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>se cues are actually used in practice (Harris, Evans and<br />

Dennis 2000). <strong>The</strong>refore vignettes are being used to increase ecological validity, in addition to a<br />

series of open ended and closed ended questions.<br />

Materials<br />

<strong>The</strong> final questionnaire was designed by <strong>the</strong> researcher in two parts; firstly questions designed<br />

to ga<strong>the</strong>r information about participants’ current role and experience of working with patients<br />

with visible differences and secondly a series of 12 randomised experimental vignettes<br />

designed to extrapolate those cues used by respondents to make decisions about referral<br />

pathways. <strong>The</strong> questionnaire is presented with a consent form, information sheet and reply<br />

envelope.<br />

<strong>The</strong> vignettes were designed by <strong>the</strong> research team and detail hypo<strong>the</strong>tical patients and<br />

scenarios. <strong>The</strong>se vignettes included information about <strong>the</strong> age, gender, condition, severity and<br />

visibility of <strong>the</strong> patients’ condition as well as painting a picture of <strong>the</strong>ir current levels of<br />

psychological distress. A number of variables were also kept constant across vignettes,<br />

including socioeconomic status, sex, age (30-50), relationship status, and living arrangements.<br />

As data from <strong>the</strong> cross sectional study suggest greater numbers of visible differences acquired<br />

during <strong>the</strong> life course compared with those evident at birth, all actors in <strong>the</strong> scenarios had<br />

acquired <strong>the</strong>ir visible differences. All scenarios were also between 95-100 words long.<br />

194


Example Vignette<br />

Sarah, 32 lives locally in a flat with her husband, Nick. She works at a local university as a HR<br />

manager. A recent stroke has left <strong>the</strong> left side of her face in paralysis. Over <strong>the</strong> last few months,<br />

she has been tired every day and more on edge than usual. She feels ‘jumpy’ when on her own<br />

and has been feeling anxious even thinking about going to work. She has been staying at home<br />

whenever possible and trying to wear her hair so it covers <strong>the</strong> affected side of her face in order<br />

to conceal it from o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

Participants are asked to decide whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y would identify <strong>the</strong>se patients as needing fur<strong>the</strong>r<br />

psychosocial support and whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y would refer <strong>the</strong>m on to secondary or tertiary services.<br />

Following coding, <strong>the</strong> factors or ‘cues’ provided within <strong>the</strong> vignettes are designed to be suitable<br />

for inclusion in regression analyses to ascertain which cues are more likely to result in a referral<br />

to psychological services.<br />

Procedure<br />

Participants receive <strong>the</strong> same questionnaire pack via post or face to face from <strong>the</strong> researcher<br />

and questionnaires are taken for participants to complete in <strong>the</strong>ir own time. <strong>The</strong>se are <strong>the</strong>n<br />

posted back directly to <strong>the</strong> researcher in a freepost envelope, along with <strong>the</strong> consent form.<br />

Data collection is still in progress (n=21 completed returned questionnaires) and is expected to<br />

be completed in September 2009.<br />

Data will be analysed using hierarchical stepwise regression analyses to ascertain which cues<br />

GPs rely on in <strong>the</strong>ir decision making regarding referral to medical, psychological and lay led<br />

services.<br />

Ethics<br />

Ethical approval was granted by <strong>the</strong> University of <strong>the</strong> West of England.<br />

195


196


OVERALL SYNTHESIS OF RESULTS<br />

197


<strong>The</strong> results of each study have been discussed throughout this <strong>report</strong>. In <strong>the</strong> syn<strong>the</strong>sis which<br />

follows, <strong>the</strong> main findings of <strong>the</strong> research programme are highlighted. <strong>The</strong> follow-on Studies (5-<br />

13) are in <strong>the</strong> main, small scale and exploratory. In view of this, <strong>the</strong> findings derived from <strong>the</strong>se<br />

studies were explored in less depth in <strong>the</strong> <strong>report</strong>, and at this stage should be taken as<br />

suggestive ra<strong>the</strong>r than indicative. Never<strong>the</strong>less, many of <strong>the</strong> findings resonate with <strong>the</strong> results<br />

of Studies 1-4. In particular, <strong>the</strong>y highlight <strong>the</strong> range and complexity of individual experiences<br />

and are helpful in formulating recommendations relating both to interventions and future<br />

research agendas. Where appropriate, <strong>the</strong> findings from <strong>the</strong> follow-on studies are integrated<br />

with <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> earlier studies in <strong>the</strong> section that follows.<br />

Profiles of adjustment and distress<br />

<strong>The</strong> participants in this programme of research demonstrated very variable profiles of<br />

adjustment and distress. In line with previous research, participants in study 1 showed<br />

significant levels of distress, with 51.7% participants with scores above <strong>the</strong> clinical cut off for<br />

anxiety and 23.6% for depression. <strong>The</strong> results also revealed considerable information about <strong>the</strong><br />

characteristics of those with disfiguring conditions who can be considered to be well adjusted<br />

(Studies 1, 4, 5) Difficulties and distress, which were often extensive, were experienced by<br />

those sampled through out-patient clinics, and from <strong>the</strong> community. It is clear that <strong>the</strong><br />

populations of both those actively seeking treatment, and those who are not contain within <strong>the</strong>m<br />

significant proportions of those with often debilitating levels of unmet psychosocial need.<br />

Adjustment is multi-factorial (all Studies). Some contributory components identified or suggested<br />

in previous research were confirmed and clarified, including dispositional style<br />

(optimism/pessimism) (Studies 1, 4,) and cognitive processes such as levels of satisfaction with<br />

social support (Studies 1, 3, 4, 5 & 11), fear of negative evaluation (Studies 1, 4) and perceived<br />

social acceptance (Studies 1 & 4). <strong>The</strong> interpretation of <strong>the</strong> responses of o<strong>the</strong>rs has been<br />

highlighted as a crucial factor in adjustment in previous research, and is also a recurring feature<br />

of <strong>the</strong> results of this programme (Studies 2 & 11).<br />

One of <strong>the</strong> most striking findings of this research is <strong>the</strong> role played by appearance-specific<br />

cognitions, including salience, valence and appearance-related self discrepancies (Studies 1, 3,<br />

4). <strong>The</strong> role of some factors in adjustment to disfigurement, hi<strong>the</strong>rto neglected in research, have<br />

also been highlighted, including aggression in response to appearance-related threats (Studies<br />

1 & 9), and <strong>the</strong> role of functional disability and <strong>the</strong> desire to function independently (Studies 11<br />

& 12). <strong>The</strong>se appearance-specific issues may cause distress in <strong>the</strong> absence of o<strong>the</strong>r more<br />

general signs of psychological need (for example, anxiety or depression) (Studies 1, 4, 9, 10),<br />

but never<strong>the</strong>less impact negatively on many aspects of <strong>the</strong> lives of those affected. In those<br />

without obvious signs of general psychopathology, <strong>the</strong> existence of appearance-related distress<br />

may be less obvious to clinicians and o<strong>the</strong>rs involved in <strong>the</strong>ir care. However, unhelpful<br />

appearance-specific cognitions and behaviours are likely to be amenable to intervention, and<br />

<strong>the</strong>se are urgently needed to improve <strong>the</strong> quality of life of people with significant levels of<br />

distress.<br />

198


<strong>The</strong> specific nature of appearance-related cognitions and distress also highlights <strong>the</strong> need to<br />

continue to develop appearance-specific measures with relevance to people with disfigurement.<br />

At present, <strong>the</strong> CARSAL (salience), CARVAL (valence) and <strong>the</strong> Derriford Appearance Scales<br />

measuring social anxiety and social avoidance are <strong>the</strong> only ones in existence which are<br />

applicable to people with a wide range of disfiguring conditions. <strong>The</strong> CARSAL & CARVAL are<br />

still under development and <strong>the</strong> DAS is rarely used in screening and assessment of people with<br />

disfiguring conditions as a matter of routine. Researchers from CAR and <strong>The</strong> Royal Free<br />

Hospital, Hampstead will continue to develop <strong>the</strong> intimacy measure <strong>report</strong>ed in Study 10. <strong>The</strong><br />

research community is encouraged to consider <strong>the</strong> development of fur<strong>the</strong>r scales tapping<br />

relevant constructs, including appearance self-efficacy, and culturally sensitive measures of<br />

adjustment and distress.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>rs factors and processes produced contradictory findings within this programme and are in<br />

need fur<strong>the</strong>r research, including <strong>the</strong> issue of <strong>the</strong> visibility of a disfigurement to o<strong>the</strong>rs (Studies 1,<br />

3),and social comparison processes (Studies 1 & 8). <strong>The</strong> nature of longitudinal research of<br />

adjustment over time (Study 3) also needs to be examined fur<strong>the</strong>r, across a longer period than<br />

was possible in <strong>the</strong> current study.<br />

Positive adjustment and resilience<br />

<strong>The</strong> impact of cognitive processes such as dispositional style and appearance-related<br />

cognitions were particularly apparent in those who self <strong>report</strong>ed as positive adjusters. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

people talked of <strong>the</strong> experience of disfigurement as one of personal growth (Studies 5 & 11) and<br />

<strong>report</strong>ed a number of strategies that appeared to work well including pragmatism and ‘getting on<br />

with it’ in <strong>the</strong> face of challenges (Studies 5 & 11), acceptance and determination (Study 2), and<br />

engaging with problems and difficulties ra<strong>the</strong>r than avoiding <strong>the</strong>m (Studies 3, 5 & 7).<br />

In <strong>the</strong> longitudinal study (Study 4), <strong>the</strong>re was a striking lack of concordance between self<br />

classification as a positive adjuster (stable or improved over time) and <strong>the</strong> scores on<br />

standardised measures, which did not necessarily paint <strong>the</strong> same picture of positive adjustment.<br />

Once more, <strong>the</strong> crucial importance of taking into account each person’s subjective beliefs and<br />

assessment about <strong>the</strong>ir level of adjustment is highlighted. It is not sufficient, and may even be<br />

misleading, to rely solely on standardised measures in this regard.<br />

<strong>The</strong> impact of appearance-related distress<br />

<strong>The</strong> impact of appearance-related distress on a wide range of daily activities is striking. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

include negative effects on cognitions about <strong>the</strong> self, social functioning and intimate<br />

relationships (Studies 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, 7 & 10).<br />

<strong>The</strong> multiple nature of appearance concerns<br />

Distress relating to appearance can relate to many different aspects of <strong>the</strong> body (Studies 1 & 2).<br />

Some participants focussed <strong>the</strong>ir concerns clearly and exclusively on <strong>the</strong>ir disfigurement.<br />

199


O<strong>the</strong>rs expressed dissatisfaction with additional issues including weight, shape, and <strong>the</strong> effects<br />

of ageing on appearance. In some cases <strong>the</strong>se latter concerns caused more distress than <strong>the</strong><br />

disfigurement. Interestingly, <strong>the</strong> consequences for <strong>the</strong> individual of concerns about aspects of<br />

appearance which are not normally considered to be disfigurements (such as body shape and<br />

size and <strong>the</strong> effects of ageing) appeared to be strikingly similar to <strong>the</strong> distress caused by<br />

disfigurement (Studies 1, 2). <strong>The</strong> findings stress <strong>the</strong> need for a comprehensive assessment of<br />

each of <strong>the</strong> sources of appearance-related distress in those affected, and a need to tailor<br />

intervention appropriately. <strong>The</strong>re would be little point in focussing a surgical or psychological<br />

intervention on <strong>the</strong> aspect of appearance affected by a disfigurement, when, in fact, <strong>the</strong> person<br />

is equally or more stressed by o<strong>the</strong>r aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir looks.<br />

More research is needed to unpick fur<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> similarities and differences in <strong>the</strong> impact of<br />

concerns about weight, shape and ageing in comparison with <strong>the</strong> consequences of<br />

disfigurement. However, <strong>the</strong> findings add credence to <strong>the</strong> legitimacy of considering <strong>the</strong><br />

appearance concerns of those with and without disfigurements as comprising one continuum.<br />

In efforts to increase public education and understanding of <strong>the</strong> consequences of disfigurement,<br />

this may be a fruitful way of ‘normalising’ <strong>the</strong> distress experienced by those affected,<br />

<strong>The</strong> dynamic nature of adjustment<br />

In contrast to <strong>the</strong> ra<strong>the</strong>r static picture of adjustment and distress painted by previous,<br />

predominantly cross sectional research, and <strong>the</strong> widely held assumption that time is a greater<br />

healer in relation to adjustment to disfigurement, this programme of research has also brought<br />

into sharp relief <strong>the</strong> dynamic nature of adjustment over time. Fluctuations in <strong>the</strong> salience and<br />

impact of appearance concerns are triggered by a variety of events and changes, including life<br />

events, developmental milestones, signs of ageing, and <strong>the</strong> cumulative impact of daily life<br />

stessors such as <strong>the</strong> reactions of o<strong>the</strong>rs (Studies 2 & 4). Even when adjustment is positive over<br />

long periods of time, coping with a visible difference can be an ongoing strain on resources<br />

(Studies 2 & 11), and at times of particular stress and change may be an ‘Achilles heel’,<br />

becoming <strong>the</strong> focus of distress. This research (Studies 1 & 3) suggests that many factors<br />

predicting adjustment at a single point in time also account for a significant proportion of change<br />

in <strong>report</strong>ed distress over time. This offers fur<strong>the</strong>r support for <strong>the</strong> likely utility of an intervention<br />

focusing around <strong>the</strong>se factors.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se findings are a strong endorsement of <strong>the</strong> need for regular screening and easy access to<br />

appropriate intervention at all life stages for those with congenital disfigurements, and at all<br />

stages of rehabilitation following later onset.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Impact of Age, Gender & Ethnicity<br />

<strong>The</strong> principal focus of this research programme has been on those psychological factors and<br />

processes which are amenable to change. However, <strong>the</strong> data from <strong>the</strong> cross-sectional and<br />

longitudinal studies offer findings which contradict some widely held assumptions about <strong>the</strong><br />

200


impact of some demographic factors, including age and gender. In <strong>the</strong> next phase of analyses,<br />

we will also examine <strong>the</strong> effects of socio-economic status, using postcode information.<br />

<strong>The</strong> span of ages of <strong>the</strong> participants was large (18-91 years) (Studies 1, 2, 3, 4). <strong>The</strong>re were<br />

overall decreases in levels of distress with age, but individual variation was considerable, and<br />

large numbers of older people were distressed about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance.<br />

In line with previous research, levels of appearance-related distress were higher in women, but<br />

in this research programme differences between men and women were small, and many men<br />

had significant and in some cases, debilitating concerns about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance. (Studies<br />

1,2,3,4). Study 9 illustrated that men and women may respond differently to appearance-specific<br />

threats in social situations, with men more prone to responses underpinned by aggression and<br />

hostility.<br />

Although someone’s ethnic origin is to all intents and purposes ‘fixed’ and <strong>the</strong>refore not<br />

amenable to change, <strong>the</strong> influence of cultural and religious beliefs was a focus within this<br />

research programme, as this is an area which has been under-researched in <strong>the</strong> past. <strong>The</strong><br />

under representation of people from BMEs in treatment settings has been remarked upon by<br />

researchers from this collaboration in <strong>the</strong> past, and <strong>the</strong>re has been concern that treatment has<br />

been perceived as ei<strong>the</strong>r irrelevant or inappropriate for <strong>the</strong>ir needs. Within <strong>the</strong> ethnic groupings<br />

included in Studies 6 & 7, <strong>the</strong>re was evidence of <strong>the</strong> strong influence on <strong>the</strong> beliefs, attitudes<br />

and personal experience of people with disfigurement resulting from cultural and religious<br />

beliefs. <strong>The</strong>se results indicate a need for support for members of ethnic groups who have<br />

disfigurements (Studies 6 & 7), and also for educational initiatives to dispel myths and promote<br />

positive adjustment in those affected within ethnic communities (Studies 6 & 7).<br />

A Framework of Adjustment to Disfigurement<br />

<strong>The</strong> advantages and drawbacks of diagrammatic representations of adjustment and distress in<br />

relation to disfigurement have been discussed earlier in this <strong>report</strong> (including for example, <strong>the</strong><br />

acknowledged difficulty of designating psychological variables as ‘process’ or ‘outcome’,) and<br />

have been a source of considerable debate within <strong>the</strong> collaboration. Having used diagrams<br />

earlier in this <strong>report</strong> to illustrate aspects of <strong>the</strong> methodology and <strong>the</strong> analyses (Figures 1, 2 and<br />

10), it was considered prudent to fur<strong>the</strong>r modify <strong>the</strong>se in <strong>the</strong> light of <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>the</strong> overall<br />

programme. Consumers of research findings have a penchant for reproducing diagrams of<br />

complex processes such as <strong>the</strong>se, often without <strong>the</strong> associated health warnings about <strong>the</strong><br />

illusion of simplicity implied by boxes and lines. Figure 12 is an attempt to highlight <strong>the</strong> iterative<br />

process of adjustment and <strong>the</strong> need to represent <strong>the</strong> dynamic and fluctuating nature of<br />

wellbeing and distress. This diagram is not intended to be definitive, but hopefully goes some<br />

way to capturing <strong>the</strong> complexities of <strong>the</strong> processes involved, and may be more useful as an<br />

heuristic for those unfamiliar with <strong>the</strong> field than <strong>the</strong> Figures appearing earlier in this <strong>report</strong>.<br />

PREDISPOSING<br />

FACTORS<br />

INTERVENING COGNITIVE<br />

PROCESSES<br />

OUTCOMES<br />

201


Age<br />

Gender<br />

Ethnicity<br />

Relationship status<br />

Parental influence*<br />

Peer influence*<br />

Treatment history<br />

Visibility to o<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

Societal and media<br />

influence*<br />

Dispositional<br />

Style<br />

Optimism / Pessimism<br />

Socio-cognitive processing<br />

Satisfaction with social support<br />

Fear of Negative Evaluation<br />

Social Acceptance<br />

Appearance<br />

Specific cognitions<br />

Social comparison<br />

Salience<br />

Valence<br />

Appearance discrepancy<br />

Subjective visibility (self <strong>report</strong><br />

disguise, noticeability)<br />

Social Anxiety<br />

Social avoidance<br />

Anxiety<br />

Depression<br />

Mood<br />

Aggression<br />

Shame/ Inadequacy*<br />

Intimacy<br />

Figure 12: Resulting proposed framework for understanding adjustment to appearance based on<br />

research programme<br />

* Note: <strong>The</strong>se components of <strong>the</strong> framework were not included in <strong>the</strong> cross sectional study but have been<br />

highlighted in previous research and in studies 5 and 6.<br />

202


THE INTERVENTION STRAND<br />

Lead: Alex Clarke<br />

203


Whilst <strong>the</strong> main thrust of this research programme was one of fur<strong>the</strong>r exploration and enquiry<br />

about <strong>the</strong> predictors of good outcome in disfigurement, <strong>the</strong> collaboration were committed to a<br />

concurrent strand focussed on developing intervention pathways to meet <strong>the</strong> needs of those<br />

affected. This strand of work ran as a ‘wedge’ of activity, with increasing efforts as <strong>the</strong> results of<br />

<strong>the</strong> research became available. During <strong>the</strong> first 18 months of <strong>the</strong> project, existing interventions<br />

were reviewed. <strong>The</strong>se included social skills programmes (Robinson et al, 1996), individual<br />

interventions (Kleve et al, 2002), <strong>the</strong> use of patient information (Newell & Clarke, 2000) and<br />

education packages delivered both to health professionals (Clarke and Cooper, 2001) and<br />

teachers (Frances 2003). Recourse was also made to a <strong>report</strong> on <strong>the</strong> provision of information<br />

for people with disfigurements, funded by <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> and carried out by <strong>the</strong> Picker<br />

Institute, and two systematic reviews of <strong>the</strong> evidence base for interventions with adults (Bessell<br />

& Moss, 2007) and Jenkinson, Moss, Byron-Daniel and Naqvi (unpublished, in preparation).<br />

<strong>The</strong> intervention strand of <strong>the</strong> project also staged a workshop for 17 experts in <strong>the</strong> area held<br />

during <strong>the</strong> first year of <strong>the</strong> project. <strong>The</strong> aims of this workshop were, firstly, to reach a consensus<br />

view of <strong>the</strong> ‘state of <strong>the</strong> art’ (i.e. what we currently know, and do not know) in relation to<br />

interventions for people with disfigurements, and, secondly, to plan <strong>the</strong> way forward for <strong>the</strong><br />

intervention elements of <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> Research Programme. Both aims were<br />

achieved. <strong>The</strong> consensus concerning <strong>the</strong> way forward was to focus on <strong>the</strong> development of a<br />

manual for psychosocial specialists. This would be used as a basis for recommendations for a<br />

subsequent programme of work. As planned, <strong>the</strong> activity relating to <strong>the</strong> interventions strand of<br />

<strong>the</strong> project escalated during 2007 and 2008 culminating in <strong>the</strong> manual included in Appendix 11<br />

Systematic reviews of interventions (Bessell et al, 2008; Jenkinson et al, in preparation) have<br />

indicated that more research is needed, but also that cognitive-behavioural and social<br />

interaction skills training approaches show promise. <strong>The</strong> results of this programme of research<br />

indicate that a cognitive behavior <strong>the</strong>rapy (CBT) approach to intervention, which focuses on<br />

facilitating those with a visible difference to identify and work on <strong>the</strong>ir appearance-related<br />

cognitive processing, is supported by <strong>the</strong> findings of this research programme, although it is<br />

recognized that social skills training (SST) can provide a potentially useful model for lower<br />

intensity intervention, and in cases where social functioning is a particular focus.<br />

.<br />

204


THE IMPLICATION OF THE HEALING FOUNDATION STUDY FOR THE PROVISION OF<br />

INTERVENTIONS<br />

<strong>The</strong> key findings of this research programme stress <strong>the</strong> role of appearance related cognitions<br />

and beliefs in predicting adjustment to disfiguring conditions (studies 1-4, 5, 8-12). <strong>The</strong> headline<br />

findings include <strong>the</strong> high level of unmet need in both community and hospital settings (studies 1-<br />

4, 6, 7, 10, 12). Results from follow-on studies illustrate <strong>the</strong> challenges for people from ethnic<br />

minorities (studies 6, 7) for whom we have had up until now very little information. <strong>The</strong> central<br />

impact of disfigurement for people from ethnic minorities is similar to o<strong>the</strong>r groups: <strong>the</strong> fear of<br />

<strong>the</strong> reaction of o<strong>the</strong>r people and <strong>the</strong> judgements <strong>the</strong>y will make impact on self concept and<br />

promote isolation from <strong>the</strong> very social activities and networks that we now understand to be<br />

protective. For o<strong>the</strong>rs, particularly those with a less well defined self concept, <strong>the</strong> perception that<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs judge <strong>the</strong>m negatively leads to an aggressive response which is again counter productive<br />

in facilitating social inclusion (study 9).<br />

We understand more about <strong>the</strong> day to day ‘work’ of having an appearance concern. Lansdown<br />

et al (1996) described this as an ‘Achilles heel’ and it is clear that in meeting <strong>the</strong> challenges and<br />

demands of everyday life as we all do, excess stress often manifests itself as renewed<br />

preoccupation with appearance for those who perceive <strong>the</strong>mselves to be disfigured. This finding<br />

fits with <strong>the</strong> clinical observation that people often seek surgery or physical solutions at times<br />

when <strong>the</strong>y feel most vulnerable and are at risk of making poor decisions.<br />

Dissatisfaction with appearance may not be confined to one area (studies 1-4, 12). In<br />

understanding its impact, it is clear that for some people <strong>the</strong>re are multiple concerns about<br />

appearance or that a visible difference has an impact because of its interaction with weight<br />

problems or with ageing or o<strong>the</strong>r aspects of appearance. Nor is appearance concern <strong>the</strong><br />

perogative of <strong>the</strong> young; many older people continue to be concerned with <strong>the</strong> impact <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

appearance has on <strong>the</strong> way in which o<strong>the</strong>r people perceive <strong>the</strong>m.<br />

<strong>The</strong> visibility of a disfigurement has produced some interesting findings (studies 1, 3, 10),<br />

suggesting that in <strong>the</strong> same way that social interaction can be difficult because of <strong>the</strong> fear of<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs reactions to a visible disfigurement (e.g. on <strong>the</strong> face), similar problems arise when<br />

conditions are not normally visible, because of <strong>the</strong> fear of disclosure during intimate settings.<br />

Both responses are connected to <strong>the</strong> concern people have to avoid negative evaluation, with <strong>the</strong><br />

fear of negative evaluation acting to reinforce <strong>the</strong> negative appraisal of appearance in those for<br />

whom appearance has a high premium. Once this view of <strong>the</strong> world is established, it is selfreinforcing<br />

through <strong>the</strong> selective attention to and negative appraisal of <strong>the</strong> information that<br />

sustains it.<br />

An important positive aspect of <strong>the</strong>se findings is that appearance cognitions are amenable to<br />

change. CBT approaches target <strong>the</strong> beliefs and behaviours that maintain a maladaptive schema<br />

and promote continued preoccupation with appearance. Using <strong>the</strong> techniques which have such<br />

205


a strong evidence base in o<strong>the</strong>r areas of psychological treatment, we can work with individuals<br />

and groups to develop new, sustainable and positive appearance schemas which will facilitate<br />

social interaction and inclusion and promote positive adjustment.<br />

<strong>The</strong> high levels of individual variation that have been evident in earlier studies of appearance are<br />

clear in this study too (studies 1-4, 5, 8, 10-12). Thus we have needed to develop a tool which<br />

allows us to choose from a range of interventions and packages of support. Whilst one person<br />

may benefit from a detailed psychological input, many o<strong>the</strong>rs respond to less intensive<br />

measures. Indeed <strong>the</strong>re is evidence for brief solution focussed approaches which focus on<br />

managing staring and comments from o<strong>the</strong>rs (Clarke, 1999).<br />

<strong>The</strong> intervention manual has <strong>the</strong>refore been designed both to provide an informative evidence<br />

based background to <strong>the</strong> area, and to describe a range of methods and approaches to<br />

managing appearance concerns. In common with <strong>the</strong> stepped approaches used in <strong>the</strong> delivery<br />

of clinical care in o<strong>the</strong>r conditions, we have devised a stepped care model for <strong>the</strong> management<br />

of appearance.<br />

A stepped care approach to psychosocial intervention<br />

Informed by <strong>the</strong> results of <strong>The</strong> <strong>Healing</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> study, <strong>the</strong> manual provides <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical<br />

basis on which to plan intervention, toge<strong>the</strong>r with an outline of <strong>the</strong> approaches that have been<br />

used so far in disfigurement and in associated problems of appearance anxiety and social<br />

phobia. Clinical examples are used to illustrate <strong>the</strong> range of problems that people encounter.<br />

Stepped care models, now routine practice in cancer care, provide an approach which facilitates<br />

<strong>the</strong> psychosocial care of patients by providing a role for everyone in <strong>the</strong> care team. Level one<br />

intervention refers to <strong>the</strong> provision of a sympa<strong>the</strong>tic and caring environment in which people are<br />

treated with dignity and respect, encouraged to ask questions and provided with relevant<br />

information. Whilst all team members are involved at this level, more complex interventions<br />

(level 4) would be delivered by specialists with additional training. A similar approach is<br />

illustrated by <strong>the</strong> PLISSIT model in sexual health, used here because it offers <strong>the</strong> advantage of<br />

an acronym which aids recall.<br />

<strong>The</strong> PLISSIT model (Annon, 1974):<br />

1. Permission<br />

2. Limited Information<br />

3. Specific Suggestions<br />

4. Intensive Treatment<br />

Level 1: Permission<br />

<strong>The</strong> first level – permission – is a particularly important concept. Observers often worry about<br />

whe<strong>the</strong>r it is appropriate or not to address issues relating to appearance. <strong>The</strong> model provides a<br />

framework in which a direct approach is encouraged. Permission also applies to both sides of<br />

206


<strong>the</strong> interaction, with both patients and health professionals encouraged to ask questions about<br />

psychosocial issues.<br />

We have recommended that all patients should develop a response to answering questions<br />

about <strong>the</strong>ir appearance which manages curiosity by providing a minimal amount of information<br />

(Coughlan & Clarke 2002; Rumsey et al 2002). In practice, this means that all practitioners,<br />

including GPs, are responsible for raising questions about appearance.<br />

Level one training can be delivered as part of a simple study day or via written information<br />

available to health professionals. However, it is also essential that pathways are in place for<br />

referral where it is evident that people are having significant problems in addressing some of<br />

<strong>the</strong>se issues.<br />

Level Two: Limited Information<br />

If people are to ask about appearance <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong>y must have an idea about how to manage <strong>the</strong><br />

responses <strong>the</strong>y get. Perhaps <strong>the</strong> biggest barrier to asking about emotional reactions is <strong>the</strong> lack<br />

of training to deal with <strong>the</strong> response or lack of access to someone else who can help.<br />

Resources such as details of support organisations or website addresses provide a Limited<br />

Information response. Changing Faces leaflets or o<strong>the</strong>r resources produced to specifically<br />

address <strong>the</strong> area of visible difference and appearance concerns can also be provided via a<br />

patient library or via <strong>the</strong> book prescription scheme. As a basic minimum, anyone who has an<br />

unusual facial appearance should be encouraged to have an answer to <strong>the</strong> question:<br />

• “What happened to your face?”<br />

Chapter 5 of <strong>the</strong> manual provides details of examples of how this might be managed, but <strong>the</strong><br />

goal is to try to elicit a response from <strong>the</strong> individual which is personal to <strong>the</strong>m and fits within <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

usual interpersonal style – ra<strong>the</strong>r than to provide a “stock answer”.<br />

It would be reasonable, after minimal training, for health professionals to deliver <strong>the</strong> first two<br />

levels of this stepped approach.<br />

Level Three: Specific Suggestions<br />

At this level of intervention, <strong>the</strong> health professional is providing more guided help towards a<br />

specific problem. This has been framed as a ‘target stressors’ approach – building strategies for<br />

managing commonly <strong>report</strong>ed problems. This is <strong>the</strong> approach used by Changing Faces in <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

direct work with patients and through <strong>the</strong> use of <strong>the</strong>ir information resources.<br />

As a minimum this would require an assessment and identification of <strong>the</strong> goals for change (see<br />

Goal Setting; Chapter 4), <strong>the</strong> design of a strategy to achieve this goal, monitoring of<br />

effectiveness and a fur<strong>the</strong>r assessment to evaluate <strong>the</strong> outcome. <strong>The</strong> section on social skills<br />

training gives examples of working in this way (Chapter 5).<br />

207


Nurses in a counseling role, clinical nurse specialists, occupational <strong>the</strong>rapists and maxillofacial<br />

technicians frequently find <strong>the</strong>mselves in a setting where <strong>the</strong>y spend time with patients as part<br />

of <strong>the</strong>ir physical treatment. <strong>The</strong>y are ideally placed to deliver a psychosocial intervention at this<br />

third level of <strong>the</strong> PLISSIT model. <strong>The</strong> training days for health professionals at Changing Faces<br />

have included this tier for health professionals working in head and neck cancer. Ideally,<br />

supervision from expert staff such as psychologists should be available.<br />

Computer-based interventions such as FaceIT, developed by Bessell and colleagues, are a<br />

recent addition to psychological treatment and <strong>the</strong> evaluation of this approach indicates that it is<br />

extremely effective (Bessell et al, 2008). FaceIT could be part of an intervention provided by a<br />

non-psychologist, but has been evaluated as part of package where it is supervised ra<strong>the</strong>r than<br />

standing alone. It could also be a very useful addition or first step to an intensive treatment<br />

offered by a psychologist or person with a recognised training in CBT. It offers <strong>the</strong> enormous<br />

advantage that <strong>the</strong> intervention can be completed remotely.<br />

Level Four: Intensive Treatment<br />

This final level of intervention relies on specialised training in psychological <strong>the</strong>rapy. <strong>The</strong> CBT<br />

model outlined in Chapter 6 should be delivered by someone with a relevant qualification in<br />

psychology or training in CBT.<br />

This stepped care model suggests that a non-specialist can provide <strong>the</strong> first three levels of<br />

intervention to include behavioural approaches to managing problems of visible difference. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

must be provided with additional training; resources and clinical governance arrangements<br />

should be in place to ensure supervised practice. This level of intervention can be effective<br />

when <strong>the</strong>re is:<br />

• Good agreement between objective and subjective assessment of visibility<br />

• Clear evidence of intrusion from o<strong>the</strong>r people with concrete examples<br />

• Inadequate social skills, particularly poor eye contact<br />

• Visible safety behaviours – baggy clo<strong>the</strong>s, hats and camouflage, unkempt appearance<br />

• Inability to deal comfortably with staring and questions about appearance<br />

• Preoccupation with appearance is situation specific ra<strong>the</strong>r than constant<br />

An intensive intervention provided by a psychologist or o<strong>the</strong>r specialist is more appropriate<br />

when <strong>the</strong>re is:<br />

• A mismatch between subjective and objective visibility of appearance<br />

• Multiple appearance-related concerns<br />

• Past history of or current body image concerns<br />

• External shame is assumed in <strong>the</strong> absence of concrete examples (“no-one actually says<br />

anything but I know what <strong>the</strong>y are thinking”)<br />

• High frequency of checking particularly in <strong>the</strong> form of reassurance elicited from o<strong>the</strong>r people<br />

• Safety behaviours are more internalised<br />

• Preoccupation with appearance is continuous<br />

• High levels of anticipatory anxiety and post event analysis<br />

208


Level of<br />

intervention<br />

Level 1 Permission<br />

Level 2<br />

Level 3<br />

Level 4<br />

Description Example of intervention<br />

Limited<br />

information<br />

Specific<br />

suggestions<br />

Intensive<br />

treatments<br />

Sensitive exploration of<br />

psychosocial concerns<br />

Written information,<br />

recommended websites and<br />

contact details for support<br />

groups. Answering basic<br />

questions about visible difference<br />

Social skills training, dealing with<br />

staring, comments and<br />

questions. Managing social<br />

situations proactively<br />

Cognitive behaviour <strong>the</strong>rapy<br />

aimed at identifying and<br />

modifying maladaptive<br />

appearance schemas<br />

Health professional<br />

background<br />

All health practitioners<br />

including GPs, practice<br />

nurses, NHS direct<br />

All health practitioners<br />

working with target<br />

groups including<br />

doctors and nurses in<br />

relevant specialties<br />

Nurses in a counseling<br />

role, clinical nurse<br />

specialists,<br />

occupational <strong>the</strong>rapists,<br />

maxillofacial<br />

technicians, support<br />

groups<br />

Specialist training in<br />

CBT<br />

Figure 13: Stepped care framework for interventions to promote psychosocial adjustment in appearance<br />

concern<br />

<strong>The</strong> skills for providing this stepped care approach are fully developed in <strong>the</strong> manual, toge<strong>the</strong>r<br />

with worked patient examples, sessional guides and suggested techniques. We anticipate that<br />

in <strong>the</strong> next phase of our work in appearance, we will start to offer training at <strong>the</strong> different levels<br />

outlined, using <strong>the</strong> manual to inform and support this training.<br />

209


210


CONCLUSIONS AND THE WAY FORWARD<br />

211


This series of studies represents <strong>the</strong> largest programme of research in this area to date. <strong>The</strong><br />

quantitative and qualitative results have confirmed <strong>the</strong> considerable distress experienced by<br />

many people with disfiguring conditions. <strong>The</strong> research has also provided a much greater<br />

understanding of <strong>the</strong> factors and processes involved in positive adjustment. <strong>The</strong> on-going effort<br />

of coping with a disfiguring condition, and <strong>the</strong> multi-factorial nature of adjustment and distress<br />

are clear, as is <strong>the</strong> huge range of individual variation in <strong>the</strong> experiences of those affected.<br />

Levels of distress were high and debilitating for many participants, both in those actively seeking<br />

treatment, and those accessed through community settings.<br />

Socio-cognitive and appearance specific cognitions are key in adjustment, and are likely to be<br />

amenable to intervention. Packages of support and intervention tailored to individual need<br />

should be developed, trialled and refined. This process has begun. An intervention manual<br />

designed to be delivered by psychosocial specialists in one-to-one sessions with those with<br />

greatest need has been informed by <strong>the</strong> results of this programme, is included in this <strong>report</strong> and<br />

will be introduced in a training workshop for specialists in November 2009.<br />

In addition, <strong>the</strong> results indicate that adjustment is labile and can be influenced by a myriad of<br />

factors. <strong>The</strong>se fluctuations indicate <strong>the</strong> need for <strong>the</strong> routine availability of support and<br />

intervention. Screening should be offered in health care settings and appropriate referral<br />

pathways for specialist assessment and intervention should be developed. (For a model of how<br />

this might work in <strong>the</strong> context of health care, see Figure 14).<br />

Routine screening<br />

(e.g. when attending<br />

for treatment &/or at<br />

times at transition or<br />

change)<br />

Psychological<br />

Assessment<br />

Surgical / Medical<br />

Intervention<br />

Psychological<br />

Intervention<br />

Psychological<br />

Intervention<br />

Follow up Discharge Easy access<br />

to intervention<br />

as necessary<br />

Figure 14: Discussion framework for <strong>the</strong> routine availability of screening, assessment and intervention.<br />

A comprehensive model of care will require training health professionals about psychological<br />

adjustment to disfigurement and of ways of meeting need. Plans are currently underway to<br />

devise a tiered approach to training, ranging from awareness-raising for all health care team<br />

members through to conveying <strong>the</strong> specialist knowledge needed by professionals taking<br />

particular responsibility for <strong>the</strong> psychological well being of those affected by disfigurement.<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>r funding is needed for both <strong>the</strong> development of interventions and for <strong>the</strong> training of health<br />

care professionals.<br />

212


Although this research has shown that <strong>the</strong> cognitive architecture of each individual and <strong>the</strong><br />

personal interpretation of appearance related information are key in adjustment, it is also<br />

evident that <strong>the</strong> person’s social and cultural context are of considerable importance. For <strong>the</strong><br />

most vulnerable, messages from <strong>the</strong> media and from society are relentless. Even <strong>the</strong> most<br />

resilient can be affected by <strong>the</strong> barrage of appearance information in <strong>the</strong>ir societal context.<br />

Public education is needed to reduce <strong>the</strong> pressure on those vulnerable to appearance concerns,<br />

and interventions should be adapted to suit <strong>the</strong> cultural context.<br />

As is usually <strong>the</strong> case in research, <strong>the</strong> findings of this programme offer clear pointers for <strong>the</strong><br />

research agenda of <strong>the</strong> future. This agenda includes fur<strong>the</strong>r examination of specific issues<br />

raised by this programme, such as <strong>the</strong> impact on adjustment of <strong>the</strong> visibility and disguisability of<br />

a difference. <strong>The</strong> effects of functional disability in <strong>the</strong> context of disfigurement also warrant a<br />

more detailed exploration. Gender differences in manifestations of distress, a better<br />

understanding of <strong>the</strong> similarities and differences in body image and appearance concerns in<br />

those with and without disfigurements and <strong>the</strong> development of culturally sensitive measures<br />

specifically relating to <strong>the</strong> appearance concerns of those with visible differences are also<br />

research priorities. This research programme has focussed on <strong>the</strong> adjustment of adults, and<br />

<strong>the</strong>re is a pressing need to examine <strong>the</strong>se issues in young people. This research will be a<br />

considerable undertaking and will require <strong>the</strong> development of measures appropriate to each<br />

developmental stage.<br />

In addition to tangible benefits for those affected in <strong>the</strong> form of a specialist intervention, and to<br />

plans for fur<strong>the</strong>r packages of information and support, for <strong>the</strong> training of health care<br />

professionals, and for school and community based public education initiatives, this research<br />

programme has addressed an acknowledged need to increase research capacity in this area.<br />

As <strong>the</strong> result of <strong>the</strong>ir experience of working on this project, several of <strong>the</strong> research assistants<br />

are developing <strong>the</strong>ir newly established interest in <strong>the</strong> field. Dr. Sally-Ann Clarke<br />

(Sheffield/Bradford) has begun her training in clinical psychology and remains committed to<br />

research and practice in this area. Hayley James (UCL) has achieved funding to complete a<br />

PhD and is continuing to write papers from this programme for publication. Dr. Antje<br />

Lindenmeyer (Warwick) is continuing to develop her interest in weight and shape issues, and<br />

<strong>the</strong> links of <strong>the</strong>se to disfigurement. Dr. James Byron-Daniel (Bristol) has retained his interest in<br />

appearance research and has established a research programme relating to exercise and<br />

appearance. Emma Williams (Bristol) has been developing a research programme relating to<br />

<strong>the</strong> appearance related consequences of transplants. Eleanor Walsh (Bristol) is currently<br />

analysing data on lay perceptions of disfigurement for her MSc <strong>the</strong>sis and Liz Jenkinson<br />

(Bristol) has been conducting <strong>the</strong> study of <strong>the</strong> training needs of GPs as part of a Professional<br />

Doctorate programme. Liz has recently been appointed as a Research Fellow at CAR on a five<br />

year contract and will lead a programme of research on interventions for people with<br />

appearance related distress. Dr. Alex Clarke (Royal Free) has become a Visiting Professor at<br />

UWE and will be working with <strong>the</strong> team at CAR on a regular basis over <strong>the</strong> next five years.<br />

Krysia Saul (Warwick) is developing her interest in <strong>the</strong> disfigurement related adjustment of<br />

people from BMEs and is staging a series of workshops in <strong>The</strong> Midlands.<br />

213


214


REFERENCES<br />

Abraham, C., Connor, M., Jones, F. and O’Connor, D. (2008). Health Psychology: Topics in<br />

Applied Psychology. London: Hodder Education<br />

Allan, S., and Gilbert, P. (1995). A social comparison scale: psychometric properties and<br />

relationship to psychopathology. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 293-9.<br />

Altabe, M. and Thompson, J.K. (1996). Body image: a cognitive self-schema construct.<br />

Cognitive <strong>The</strong>rapy and Research, 20, 171-93.<br />

Altabe, M. (1996). Issues in <strong>the</strong> assessment and treatment of body image disturbance in<br />

culturally diverse populations. In J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Eating Disorders, Obesity, and Body<br />

Image: A Practical Guide to Assessment and Treatment. Washington, DC: American<br />

Psychological Association Books.<br />

Baker, C., (1992). Factors associated with rehabilitation in head and neck cancer. Cancer<br />

Nursing, 15, 395–400.<br />

Balance, R., Wilson, B., and Harder, J.A. (1989). Factors affecting myoelectric pros<strong>the</strong>tic use<br />

and wearing patterns in <strong>the</strong> juvenile unilateral below elbow amputee. Canadian Journal of<br />

Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy 56, 132-7.<br />

Baldwin, M.W. (1992). Relational schemas and <strong>the</strong> processing of social information.<br />

Psychological Bulletin, 112, 461-84.<br />

Bordo, S. (1993). Unbearable Weight: Feminism, Western Culture, and <strong>the</strong> Body. Berkeley:<br />

University of California Press.<br />

Bargh, J. A., Lombardi, W. J., and Higgins, E. T. (1988). Automaticity of chronically accessible<br />

constructs in person x situation effects on person perception: it's just a matter of time. Journal of<br />

Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 599-605.<br />

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., and van Ijzendoorn, M.H.<br />

(2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non-anxious individuals: A meta-analytic<br />

study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24.<br />

Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., and Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to<br />

violence and aggression: <strong>the</strong> dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5-33.<br />

Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J.(196l). An inventory for<br />

measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-71.<br />

215


Ben-Tovim, D.I. and Walker, M.K. (1995). Body image, disfigurement and disability. Journal of<br />

Psychosomatic Research, 39, 283-91.<br />

Bessell, A. and Moss, T.P. (2007). Evaluating <strong>the</strong> effectiveness of psychosocial interventions for<br />

individuals with visible differences: a systematic review of <strong>the</strong> empirical literature. Body Image,<br />

4, 227-38.<br />

Bessell, A. (2009). Increasing access to intervention for disfigurement through <strong>the</strong> development<br />

and evaluation of computer based interventions. Unpublished PhD <strong>The</strong>sis, University of <strong>the</strong><br />

West of England.<br />

Bjelland, I., Dahl, A.A., Haug, T.T., and Neckelmann D. (2002). <strong>The</strong> validity of <strong>the</strong> Hospital<br />

Anxiety and Depression Scale. An updated literature review. Journal of Psychosomatic<br />

Research. 52, 69–77.<br />

Blakeney, P., Portman, S. and Rutan, R., (1990). Familial values as factors influencing longterm<br />

psychological adjustment of children after severe burn injury. Journal of Burn Care and<br />

Rehabilitation, 11, 472–5<br />

Braun, V. and Clarke, V. (2006). Using <strong>the</strong>matic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in<br />

Psychology, 3, 77-101.<br />

Bryant, F.B., and Smith, B.D. (2001). Refining <strong>the</strong> architecture of aggression: a measurement<br />

model for <strong>the</strong> Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 35,<br />

138-67.<br />

Bryman, A. (2007). Barriers to integrating quantitative and qualitative research. Journal of Mixed<br />

Methods Research, 1, 1-18.<br />

Burnard, P. (1991). A method of analysing interview transcripts in qualitative research. Nurse<br />

Education Today, 11, 461-6.<br />

Burke, P.J., and Stets, J.E. (1999). Trust and commitment through self-verification. Social<br />

Psychology Quarterly, 62, 347-66.<br />

Buss, A. H and Perry, M J. (1992). <strong>The</strong> Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and<br />

Social Psychology, 63, 452-9.<br />

Carr, T., Harris, D. and James, C. (2000). <strong>The</strong> Derriford Appearance Scale: a new scale to<br />

measure individual responses to living with problems of appearance. British Journal of Health<br />

Psychology, 5, 201 – 15.<br />

216


Carr, T., Moss, T. and Harris, D. (2005). <strong>The</strong> DAS24: A Short Form of <strong>the</strong> Derriford Appearance<br />

Scale DAS59 to measure individual responses to living with problems of appearance. British<br />

Journal of Health Psychology, 10, 285 – 98.<br />

Cash, T. F. (1997). <strong>The</strong> Body Image Workbook: An 8-step Program for Learning to Like Your<br />

Looks. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger<br />

Cash, T. F., Melnyk, S. E., and Hrabosky, J. I. (2004). <strong>The</strong> assessment of body image<br />

investment: An extensive revision of <strong>the</strong> Appearance Schemas Inventory. International Journal<br />

of Eating Disorders, 35, 305–16.<br />

Cash, T.F. and Pruzinsky, T. (2002). Body Image: a Handbook of <strong>The</strong>ory, Research And<br />

Clinical Practice. New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Cash, T.F. Winstead, B.A. Janda, L.H. (1986). <strong>The</strong> great American shape-up. Psychology<br />

Today. 20, 30-7.<br />

Charlton, R., Rumsey, N., Partridge, J., Barlow, J. and Saul, K. (2003). Disfigurement-<br />

neglected in primary care? British Journal of General Practice, 53, 6-8<br />

Clarke, A. (1999). Psychosocial aspects of facial disfigurement: problems, management and <strong>the</strong><br />

role of a lay-led organisation. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 4, 127-142.<br />

Clarke, A. and Cooper, C. (2001). Psychological rehabilitation after disfiguring injury or disease:<br />

investigating <strong>the</strong> training needs of specialist nurses. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 34, 18-26.<br />

Clarke, A., Hansen, E., White, P. and Butler, P. (in preparation). <strong>The</strong> appearance concerns of<br />

patients seeking cosmetic surgery in <strong>the</strong> NHS: evidence from one thousand consecutive<br />

patients.<br />

Conner, M.T. and Sparks, P. (2005). <strong>The</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory of planned behaviour and health behaviours. In<br />

M. Conner and P. Norman (Eds), Predicting Health Behaviour: Research and Practice with<br />

Social Cognition Models, (2 nd Ed.). Maidenhead: Open University Press, 170-222.<br />

Cooper, M. (1997). Cognitive <strong>the</strong>ory in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a review.<br />

Behavioural and Cognitive Psycho<strong>the</strong>rapy, 25, 113–45.<br />

Cornwell, C.J. and Schmitt, M.H. (1990). Perceived health status, self-esteem and body image<br />

in women with rheumatoid arthritis or systemic lupus ery<strong>the</strong>matosus. Research in Nursing &<br />

Health, 13, 99-107.<br />

Coughlan, G. and Clarke, A. (2002). Shame and Burns, in P. Gilbert and J. Miles (eds) Body<br />

Shame: Conceptualisation, Research and Treatment. London: Psychology in Press.<br />

217


Crocker, J., Voelkl, K., Testa, M. and Major, B. (1991). Social stigma: <strong>the</strong> affective<br />

consequences of attributional ambiguity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 218-<br />

228.<br />

Culley, L., Hudson, N., and Rapport, F. (2007). Using focus groups with minority ethnic<br />

communities: researching infertility in British South Asian Communities. Qualitative Health<br />

Research, 17, 102-12.<br />

Demet, K., Martinet, N., Francis, G., Paysant, J. and Andre, J. (2003). Health related quality of<br />

life and related factors in 539 persons with amputation of upper and lower limb. Disability<br />

Rehabilitation, 25, 480–6.<br />

Denscombe, M. (2008). Communities of practice: a research paradigm for <strong>the</strong> mixed methods<br />

approach. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 22, 351 – 72.<br />

Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., Rickels, K., Uhlenhuth, E. H., and Covi, L. (1974). <strong>The</strong> Hopkins<br />

Symptom Checklist (HSCL): a self-<strong>report</strong> symptom inventory. Behavioral Science, 19, 1-15.<br />

Dures, E. (2009). An Exploration of <strong>the</strong> Psychosocial Impact of Epidermolysis Bullosa on <strong>the</strong><br />

Daily Lives of Affected Adults and Identification of Associated Support Needs. Unpublished PhD<br />

<strong>The</strong>sis, University of <strong>the</strong> West of England.<br />

Eiserman, W. (2001). Unique outcomes and positive contributions associated with facial<br />

difference: expanding research and practice. Cleft Palate Craniofacial Journal, 38, 236–44.<br />

El-Miedany, Y.M. and El Rasheed, A.H. (2002). Is anxiety a more common disorder than<br />

depression in rheumatoid arthritis? Joint Bone Spine, 69, 300-06.<br />

Endriga, M. and Kapp-Simon, K. (1999). Psychological issues in craniofacial care: state of <strong>the</strong><br />

art. Cleft Palate Craniofacial Journal, 36, 3 – 11.<br />

Engel, G.L. (1980). <strong>The</strong> clinical application of <strong>the</strong> biopsychosocial model. American Journal of<br />

Psychiatry. 137, 535-44.<br />

Evers, A.W.M., Kraaimaat, F.W., Geenen, R. and Bijlsma, J.W.J. (1997). Determinants of<br />

psychological distress and its course in <strong>the</strong> first year after diagnosis in rheumatoid arthritis<br />

patients. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 20, 489-503.<br />

Fagervik-Morton, H. (In preparation). What psychological and social factors exacerbate distress<br />

and/or promote psychosocial adjustment among adults with congenital and acquired visible<br />

difference not related to surgical medical procedures? A systematic review.<br />

Fauerbach, J.A., Heinberg, L.J., Lawrence, J.W. and Bryant, A.G. (2002). Coping with body<br />

image changes following a disfiguring burn injury. Health Psychology, 21, 115-21.<br />

218


Faulkner, L., & Thompson, A. R (2006). Managing intrusive reactions to visible difference. Oral<br />

presentation at <strong>the</strong> Appearance Matters Conference, Bath, 07/06/06<br />

Festinger, L. (1954). A <strong>the</strong>ory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117-40.<br />

Firooz, A., Bouzari, N., Fallah, N., Ghazisaidi, B., Firoozabadi, M. R., and Dowlati, Y. (2004).<br />

What patients with vitiligo believe about <strong>the</strong>ir condition. International Journal of Dermatology,<br />

43, 811-4.<br />

Fortune, D., Richards, H., Griffiths, C. and Main, C. (2005). Adversarial growth in patients<br />

undergoing treatment for psoriasis: a prospective study of <strong>the</strong> ability of patients to construe<br />

benefits from negative events. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 10, 44-56.<br />

Frances, J. (2003). Educating Children with Facial Disfigurement: Creating Inclusive School<br />

Communities. New York: Routledge Falmer.<br />

Frith, H. (2000). Focusing on sex: using focus groups in sex research. Sexualities, 3, 275-97.<br />

Gamba, A., Romano, M., Grosso, I.M., Tamburini, M., Cantu, G., Molinari, R. and Ventafridda,<br />

V. (1992). Psychosocial adjustment of patients surgically treated for <strong>the</strong> head and neck cancer.<br />

Head and Neck, 14, 218-23.<br />

Gibbons, F.X. and Buunk, B.P. (1999). Individual differences in social comparison: development<br />

of a scale of social comparison orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,<br />

129-42.<br />

Gilbert, P., Broomhead, C., Irons, C., McEwan, K., Bellew, R., Mills, A., et al. (2007).<br />

Development of a Striving to Avoid Inferiority Scale. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46,<br />

633-48.<br />

Gilbert, S. and Thompson, J. (2002). Body shame in childhood and adolescence, in P. Gilbert<br />

and J. Miles (eds) Body Shame: Conceptualisation, Research and Treatment. Hove: Brunner-<br />

Routledge.<br />

Goff, L.M. and Barasi, M. (1999). An assessment of <strong>the</strong> diets of people with Rheumatoid<br />

Arthritis. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics, 12, 93–101.<br />

Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on <strong>the</strong> Management of <strong>the</strong> Spoiled Identity. Englewood<br />

Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.<br />

Green, J.D. and Sedikides, C. (2001). When do self-schemas shape social perception? <strong>The</strong> role<br />

of descriptive ambiguity. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 67-83.<br />

219


Greenberg, L. S. and Safran, J. D. (1987). Emotion in Psycho<strong>the</strong>rapy. New York Guilford Press<br />

Halliwell, E. (2002). Socio-cultural influences on body image concern through adulthood.<br />

Unpublished PhD <strong>The</strong>sis, University of Sussex.<br />

Hargreaves, D., and Tiggemann, M. (2002). <strong>The</strong> effect of television commercials on mood and<br />

body dissatisfaction: <strong>the</strong> role of appearance-schema activation. Journal of Social and Clinical<br />

Psychology, 21, 287–308.<br />

Harris, D.L. and Carr, A.T. (2001). Prevalence of concern about physical appearance in <strong>the</strong><br />

general population. British Journal of Plastic Surgery, 54, 223-26.<br />

Harris, C., Evans, J.S.B.T. and Dennis, I. (2000). Measuring doctors’ self-insight into <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

treatment decisions. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 14, 455-77.<br />

Higgins, E.T. and Brendl, C.M. (1995). Accessibility and applicability: some ‘activation rules’<br />

influencing judgement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31, 218-43.<br />

Hopwood, P. and Maguire, G.P. (1988). Body image problems in cancer patients. British Journal<br />

of Psychiatry, 153, 47-50.<br />

Jansen, A., Smeets, T., Boon, B., Nederkoorn, C., Roefs, A. and Mulkens, S. (2007).<br />

Vulnerability to interpretation bias in overweight children. Psychology & Health, 22, 561–74.<br />

Jenkinson, E., Moss, T., Byron-Daniel, J.Z. and Naqvi, H. (In preparation). <strong>The</strong> efficacy of<br />

psychosocial interventions for children and adolescents who are visibly different: a systematic<br />

review of <strong>the</strong> literature.<br />

Johnson, M.R.D. (2007) Researching <strong>the</strong> health of minority ethnic groups, in M. Saks and J.<br />

Allsop, Researching Health. London: Sage.<br />

Johnson, R. B., Onwuegbuzie, A. J., and Turner, L. A. (2007). Toward a definition of<br />

mixed methods research. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 1, 112-133.<br />

Katz, J. and Melzack, R. (1990). Pain `memories’ in phantom limbs: review and clinical<br />

observations. Pain, 43, 319-36.<br />

Katz, P.P. and Yelin, E.H. (1995). <strong>The</strong> Development of depressive symptoms among women<br />

with rheumatoid arthritis: <strong>the</strong> role of function. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 38, 49-56.<br />

Kent, G. (2000). Understanding <strong>the</strong> experiences of people with disfigurements: an integration of<br />

four models of social and psychological functioning. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 5, 117-29.<br />

220


Kent, G. (2002). Testing a model of disfigurement: effects of a skin camouflage service on wellbeing<br />

and appearance anxiety. Psychology and Health, 17, 377-86.<br />

Kent, G. and Keahone, S. (2001). Social anxiety and disfigurement: <strong>the</strong> moderating effects of<br />

fear of negative evaluation and past experience. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 23–<br />

34.<br />

Kent, G. and Thompson, A.R. (2002). <strong>The</strong> development and maintenance of shame in<br />

disfigurement: implications for treatment, in P. Gilbert and J. Miles (eds) Body Shame. Hove:<br />

Brunner-Routledge.<br />

Kernis, M. H., Grannemann, B. D., and Barclay, L. C. (1989). Stability and level of self-esteem<br />

as predictors of anger arousal and hostility. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 56(6),<br />

1013-22.<br />

King, N. (1998). Template analysis, in G.Symon and C.Cassell (eds.) Qualitative Methods and<br />

Analysis in Organizational Research. London: Sage.<br />

Kleve, L., Rumsey, N., Wyn-Williams, M. and White, P. (2002). <strong>The</strong> effectiveness of cognitivebehavioural<br />

interventions provided at Outlook: a disfigurement support unit. Journal of<br />

Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 8, 387-95.<br />

Lansdown, R. Lloyd, J. and Hunter, J. (1991). Facial deformity in childhood: severity and<br />

psychological adjustment. Child: Care, Health & Development, 17, 165–71.<br />

Lansdown, R., Rumsey, R., Bradbury, E., Carr, T. and Partridge. J. (1997). Visibly Different:<br />

Coping with Disfigurement. London: Butterworth Heinemann.<br />

Leary, M.R. (1990). Responses to social exclusion: social anxiety, jealousy, loneliness,<br />

depression and low self-esteem. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 221-9.<br />

Leary, M.R. (1983). A brief version of <strong>the</strong> Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Personality and<br />

Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 371–5.<br />

Liossi, C. (2003). Appearance Related Concerns Across <strong>the</strong> General and Clinical Populations.<br />

Unpublished PhD <strong>The</strong>sis, City University London.<br />

Lovegrove, E. and Rumsey, N. (2005). Ignoring it doesn't make it stop: adolescents,<br />

appearance and anti-bullying strategies. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 42, 33-44.<br />

Luthar, S.S., Cichetti, D. and Becker, B. (2002). Research on resilience: response to<br />

commentaries. Child Development, 71, 573-5.<br />

221


Macgregor, F., Abel, T.M., Brut, A., Lauer, E. and Weissmann, S. (1953). Facial Deformities and<br />

Plastic Surgery: A Psychosocial Study. Springfield, Illinois. Thomas.<br />

Macgregor, F.C. (1979). After Plastic Surgery: Adaptation and Adjustment. New York. Praeger.<br />

Major, B. and Granzow, R.H. (1999). Abortion as stigma; cognitive and emotional implications of<br />

concealment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 735–45.<br />

Markus, H. and Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954-69.<br />

Martin C.R., and Newell R.J. (2004). Factor Structure of <strong>the</strong> Hospital Anxiety and Depression<br />

Scale in individuals with facial disfigurement. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 9, 327-36.<br />

Ma<strong>the</strong>ws, A. (1990). Why worry? <strong>The</strong> cognitive function of anxiety. Behaviour Research and<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy, 28, 455–68.<br />

Matto, S. K., Handa, S., Kaur, I., Gupta, N. and Malhotra, R. (2002). Psychiatric morbidity in<br />

vitiligo: prevalence and correlates in India. Journal of European Academy of Dermatology &<br />

Venereology, 16, 573-8.<br />

Mendelson B.K., Mendelson, M.J. and White, D.R. (2001). Body-Esteem Scale for adolescents<br />

and adults. Journal of Personality Assessment, 76, 90-106.<br />

Meyerson, M.D. (2001). Resiliency and success in adults with Moebius Syndrome. Cleft Palate<br />

Craniofacial Journal, 38, 231–5.<br />

Millstein, S.G., Heger, H., and Hunter, G.A. (1986). Pros<strong>the</strong>tic use in adult and upper limb<br />

amputees: a comparison of <strong>the</strong> body powered and electrically powered pros<strong>the</strong>ses. Pros<strong>the</strong>tics<br />

and Orthotics International, 10, 27-34.<br />

Mogg, K., and Bradley, B.P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behavior<br />

Research <strong>The</strong>rapy, 36, 809–48.<br />

Monaghan, S.M., Sharpe, L., Denton, F., Levy, J., Schrieber, L. and Sensky, T. (2007).<br />

Relationship between appearance and psychological distress in rheumatic diseases. Arthritis &<br />

Rheumatism, 57, 303-09.<br />

Moss, T. (1997). Individual Differences in Psychological Adjustment to Perceived Abnormalities<br />

of Appearance. Unpublished PhD <strong>The</strong>sis. University of Plymouth.<br />

Moss, T. (2005). <strong>The</strong> relationship between objective and subjective ratings of disfigurement<br />

severity, and psychological adjustment. Body Image, 2, 151-9.<br />

Moss, T. and Carr, T. (2004). Understanding adjustment to disfigurement: <strong>the</strong> role of selfconcept.<br />

Psychology and Health, 19, 737 – 48.<br />

222


Moss, T.P., Harris, D. and Carr, T. (2004). Manual for <strong>the</strong> Derriford Appearance Scale 24<br />

(DAS24). Musketeer Press, Bradford on Avon.<br />

Moss, T., Hobbs, E. and Rosser, B. (In preparation) Measurement of constructs within <strong>the</strong><br />

appearance schema: salience and valence of appearance scales.<br />

Murray, C.D., Fox, J. (2002). Body image and pros<strong>the</strong>sis satisfaction in <strong>the</strong> lower limb amputee.<br />

Disability and Rehabilitation, 24, 925 –31.<br />

Newell, R. (1998). Facial Disfigurement and Avoidance: A Cognitive—Behavioural Approach.<br />

Unpublished PhD <strong>the</strong>sis, University of Hull.<br />

Newell, R.J. (2000a). Body Image and Disfigurement Care. London: Routledge.<br />

Newell, R.J. (2000b). Psychological difficulties amongst plastic surgery ex-patients following<br />

surgery to <strong>the</strong> face: a survey. British Journal of Plastic Surgery 53, 386-92.<br />

Newell, R.J. and Clarke, M. (2000). Evaluation of a self-help leaflet in treatment of social<br />

difficulties following surgery to <strong>the</strong> face: A Survey. British Journal of Plastic Surgery, 53, 381-8.<br />

Ongenae, K., Beelaert, L., van Geel, N., and Naeyaert, J. M. (2006). Psychosocial effects of<br />

vitiligo. European Academy of Dermatology & Venereology, 20, 1-8.<br />

Onunu, A. N., and Kubeyinje, E. P. (2003). Vitiligo in Nigerian Africa: A study of 351 patients in<br />

Benin City. International Journal of Dermatology, 42, 800-802.<br />

Padesky, C. A. (1994). Schema change processes in cognitive <strong>the</strong>rapy. Clinical Psychology and<br />

Psycho<strong>the</strong>rapy, 1,267–78.<br />

Papadopoulos, L., Bor, R. and Legg, C. (1999). Psychological factors in cutaneous disease: an<br />

overview of research. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 4, 107-26.<br />

Partridge, J. (1990). Changing faces: <strong>the</strong> challenge of facial disfigurement. London: Penguin<br />

Books<br />

Patrick D.L., Bushnell, D.M., and Rothman M. (2004). Performance of two self-<strong>report</strong> measures<br />

for evaluating obesity and weight loss. Obesity Research. 2004, 12, 48-57.<br />

Petrie, K. J., Buick, D. L., Weinman, J., and Booth, R. J. (1999). Positive effects of illness<br />

<strong>report</strong>ed by myocardial infarction and breast cancer patients. Journal of Psychosomatic<br />

Research, 55, 99.<br />

Pincus, T., Griffiths, S., Pearce, S. and Isenberg, D. (1996). Prevalence of self-<strong>report</strong>ed<br />

depression in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. British Journal of Rheumatology, 35, 879-83.<br />

223


Pollard, L., Choy, E.H. and Scott, D.L. (2005). <strong>The</strong> consequences of rheumatoid arthritis:<br />

Quality of life measures in <strong>the</strong> individual patient. Clinical and Experimental Rheumatology, 23,<br />

S.43-S.52.<br />

Popovic, M. (2005). Intimacy and its relevance in human functioning. Sexual and Relationship<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy, 2, 31-49.<br />

Porter, J. R. and Beuf, A. H. (1991). Racial variation in reaction to physical stigma: a study of<br />

degree of disturbance by vitiligo among black and white patients. Journal of Health and Social<br />

Behavior, 32, 192-204.<br />

Porter, J.R., Beuf, A.H., Lerner, A. and Nordlund, J. (1990). <strong>The</strong> effects of vitiligo on sexual<br />

relationships. Journal of American Academy of Dermatology, 22, 221-2.<br />

Ramsey, B., and O’ Reagan, M. (1988). A survey of <strong>the</strong> social and psychological effects of<br />

psoriasis. British Journal of Dermatology, 118, 195-201.<br />

Raskin, R., and Terry, H. (1988) A principal-component analysis of <strong>the</strong> narcissistic personality<br />

inventory and fur<strong>the</strong>r evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social<br />

Psychology, 54, 5.<br />

Roberts, L.J., Salem, D., Rappaport, J., Toro, P. A., Luke, D. A. and Seidman, E. (1999). Giving<br />

and receiving help: interpersonal transactions in mutual-help meetings and psychosocial<br />

adjustment of members. American Journal of Community Psychology, 27, 841–8.<br />

Robinson, E. (1997). Psychological research on visible differences in adults, in R. Lansdown, N.<br />

Rumsey, E. Bradbury, T. Carr, and J. Partridge. (eds). Visibly Different: Coping with<br />

Disfigurement. London: Butterworth Heinemann.<br />

Robinson, E., Rumsey, N. and Partridge, J. (1996). An evaluation of <strong>the</strong> social skills interaction<br />

skills workshops for people with disfiguring conditions. British Journal of Plastic Surgery, 49:<br />

281-9.<br />

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and <strong>the</strong> Adolscent Self-Image. New Jersey: Princeton University<br />

Press.<br />

Rosser, B. (2008). Cognitive Information Processing Biases and Appearance Adjustment: <strong>The</strong><br />

Role of <strong>the</strong> Appearance Self-Schema. Unpublished PhD <strong>The</strong>sis, University of <strong>the</strong> West of<br />

England.<br />

Rozario, S. (2007) Growing up and living with neurofibromatosis1 (NF1): A British Bangladeshi<br />

case-study. Journal of Genetic Counselling, 16, 551-9.<br />

224


Rumsey, N. (2002). Body image and congenital conditions with visible differences. In T.F. Cash<br />

& T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body image: A Handbook of <strong>The</strong>ory, Research, and Clinical Practice.<br />

New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Rumsey, N. and Harcourt, D. (2004). Body image and disfigurement: issues and interventions.<br />

Body Image, 1, 83 - 97.<br />

Rumsey, N. and Harcourt, D. (2005). <strong>The</strong> Psychology of Appearance. Berkshire: Open<br />

University Press.<br />

Rumsey, N., Clarke, A., White, P., Wyn-Williams, M. and Garlick, W. (2004). Altered body<br />

image: appearance-related concerns of pople with visible disfigurement. Journal of Advanced<br />

Nursing, 48, 443 - 53.<br />

Rumsey, N., Clarke, A., White, P., and Hooper, E. (2003). Exploring <strong>the</strong> psychosocial concerns<br />

of outpatients with disfiguring conditions. Journal of Wound Care, 12, 247-52.<br />

Rumsey, N., Clarke, A. and Musa, M. (2002). Altered body image: <strong>the</strong> psychosocial needs of<br />

patients. British Journal of Community Nursing, 7, 563-6.<br />

Rybarczyk, B.D., Nyenhuis, D.L., Nicholas, J.J., Cash, S.M., and Kaiser, J. (1995). Body image,<br />

perceived social stigma, and <strong>the</strong> prediction of psychosocial adjustment to leg amputation.<br />

Rehabilitation Psychology, 40, 95-110.<br />

Saradjian, A., Thompson, A.R., and Datta, D. (2007). <strong>The</strong> experience of men using an upper<br />

limb pros<strong>the</strong>sis following amputation: positive coping and minimizing feeling different. Disability<br />

and Rehabilitation 30, 871-83.<br />

Sarason, I.G., Levine, H.M., Basham, R.B. and Sarason, B.R. (1983). Assessing social support:<br />

<strong>The</strong> Social Support Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 127-39.<br />

Sarwer, D.B. and Crerand, C.E. (2004). Body image and cosmetic medical treatment. Body<br />

Image, 1, 99-111.<br />

Sarwer, D.B., Wadden, T.A., Pertchuk, M.J. and Whitaker, L.A. (1997). Body image<br />

dissatisfaction and body dysmorphic disorder in 100 cosmetic surgery patients. Plastic and<br />

Reconstructive Surgery, 101, 1644-9.<br />

Schaefer, M.T. and Olson, D.H. (1981). Assessing intimacy: <strong>The</strong> PAIR Inventory. Journal of<br />

Marital and Family <strong>The</strong>rapy, 7, 47-60<br />

Scheier, M.F. and Carver, C.S. (1987). Dispositional optimism and physical well-being: <strong>the</strong><br />

influence of generalized outcome expectancies on health. Journal of Personality, 55, 169-210.<br />

225


Scheier, M.F., Carver, C.S. and Bridges, M.W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism<br />

(and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a re-evaluation of <strong>the</strong> Life Orientation Test.<br />

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063-78.<br />

Schmid-Ott, G., Jaeger, B., Kuensebeck, H.W., Ott, R. and Lamprecht, F. (1996). Dimensions of<br />

stigmatization in patients with psoriasis in a ‘Questionnaire on Experiences with Skin<br />

Complaints’. Dermatology, 193, 304-10.<br />

Schwartz, C.E., Sprangers, M.A.G. (1999). Methodological approaches for assessing response<br />

shift in longitudinal quality of life research. Social Science & Medicine, 48, 1531–48.<br />

Seligman, M.E.P. (1998). Learned Optimism. Second edition. New York: Pocket Books<br />

Skevington, S.M., Blackwell, F. and Britton, N.F. (1987). Self-esteem and perception of<br />

attractiveness: an investigation of early rheumatoid arthritis. British Journal of Medical<br />

Psychology, 60, 45-52.<br />

Smart, L. and Wegner, D.M. (1999). Covering up what can't be seen: Concealable stigma and<br />

mental control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 474<br />

Snell, W.E., Fisher, T.D., and Walters, A.S. (1993). <strong>The</strong> Multidimensional Sexuality<br />

Questionnaire: an objective self-<strong>report</strong> measure of psychological tendencies associated with<br />

human sexuality. Annals of Sex Research, 6, 27-55.<br />

Sodergren, S.C., and Hyland, M.E., (2000). What are <strong>the</strong> positive consequences of illness?<br />

Psychology and Health, 15, 85-97.<br />

Sodergren, S. C., Hyland, M. E., Crawford, A., Partridge, M. R.. (2004). Positivitiy in illness: selfdelusion<br />

or existential growth? British Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 163-74<br />

Söderlin, M.K., Hakala, M. and Nieminen, P. (2000). Anxiety and depression in a communitybased<br />

rheumatoid arthritis population. Scandinavian Journal of Rheumatology, 29, 177-83.<br />

Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., and Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for <strong>the</strong> State-Trait Anxiety<br />

Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.<br />

Strunk, D. R., & Adler, A. D. (2009). Cognitive biases in three prediction tasks: a test of <strong>the</strong><br />

cognitive model of depression. Behaviour Research and <strong>The</strong>rapy, 37, 34-40.<br />

Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A <strong>the</strong>ory of cognitive adaptation.<br />

American Psychologist, 38, 61–1173.<br />

Thompson, A.R. (2005). Coping with chronic skin conditions: factors important in explaining<br />

individual variation in adjustment. In l. Papadopoulos & C. Walker (Ed). Psychodermatology:<br />

<strong>The</strong> Impact of Skin Disorders. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

226


Thompson, A.R., Kent, G., and Smith, J. A. (2002). Living with vitiligo: dealing with difference.<br />

British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 213-25.<br />

Thompson, A.R., and Kent, G. (2001). Adjusting to disfigurement: processes involved in dealing<br />

with being visibly different. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 663-82.<br />

Thompson, J.K., Heinberg, L.J., Altabe, M. and Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting Beauty:<br />

<strong>The</strong>ory, Assessment and Treatment of Body Image Disturbance. Washington, DC: APA.<br />

Thompson, J.K., Heinberg, L.J., and Tantleff, S. (1991). <strong>The</strong> physical appearance comparison<br />

scale (PACS). <strong>The</strong> Behavior <strong>The</strong>rapist, 14, 137.<br />

Tiggemann, M. (2004). Body image across <strong>the</strong> adult lifespan: stability and change. Body Image,<br />

1, 29-41.<br />

Tugade, M.M., and Fredrickson, B.L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to<br />

bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social<br />

Psychology, 86, 320-33.<br />

Værøy, H., Tanum, L., Bruaset, H. Mørkrid, L and Førre, O. (2005). Symptoms of depression<br />

and anxiety in functionally disabled rheumatic pain patients. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 59,<br />

109-13.<br />

Veal, D., Wilson, R. and Clarke, A. (2009). Overcoming Body Image Problems Including Body<br />

Dysmorphic Disorder. England, Robinson<br />

Vincenzi, H. and Grabosky, F. (1987). Measuring <strong>the</strong> emotional/social aspects of loneliness and<br />

isolation. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2, 257-70.<br />

Watson, D., Clark, L.A. and Tellagan, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures<br />

of posiitive and negative affect: <strong>The</strong> PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social<br />

Psychology, 47, 1063-70.<br />

White, C.A. (2002). Body image issues in oncology, in T.F. Cash and T. Pruzinsky (eds) Body<br />

Image: A Handbook of <strong>The</strong>ory, Research and Clinical Practice. London: Guilford Press.<br />

White, J. (2008). <strong>The</strong> development of negative body image and disordered eating in<br />

adolescence. Unpublished PhD <strong>The</strong>sis. University of <strong>the</strong> West of England.<br />

Wolfe, F., Michaud, K. and Pincus, T. (2004). Development and validation of <strong>the</strong> Health<br />

Assessment Questionnaire II: a revised version of <strong>the</strong> Health Assessment Questionnaire.<br />

Arthritis & Rheumatism, 50, 3296–305.<br />

227


Wright, G.E., Parker, J.C., Smarr, K.L., Schoenfeld-Smith, K., Buckelew, S.P., Slaughter, J.R.,<br />

Johnson, J.C. and Hewett, J.E. (2005). Risk factors for depression in rheumatoid Arthritis.<br />

Arthritis Care and Research, 9, 264-72.<br />

Zigmond A.S., and Snaith R.P.(1983). <strong>The</strong> Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta<br />

Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 361–70.<br />

228


APPENDICES<br />

229

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!