The Dynamic Context of Emotion Socialization
The Dynamic Context of Emotion Socialization
The Dynamic Context of Emotion Socialization
You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles
YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.
<strong>The</strong> <strong>Dynamic</strong> <strong>Context</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Emotion</strong> <strong>Socialization</strong><br />
Julie C. Dunsmore and Amy G. Halberstadt<br />
Presented at the 2008 ISCE Summit, Riva san Vitale, Switzerland<br />
Funded by AdvanceVT and R03 53572 from NICHD
Overview I<br />
• Our goal is to consider how culture and<br />
parents’ emotion-related socialization beliefs<br />
and behaviors relate with each other.<br />
• More specifically:<br />
• how parents’ individual beliefs about emotion may<br />
be affected by culture, and, in turn,<br />
• how parents’ beliefs may impact parents’<br />
socialization ation behaviors, thus, mediating relations<br />
between culture and parental behavior.<br />
• Along the way, I will discuss:<br />
2
Overview II<br />
• Significance <strong>of</strong> parents’ emotion<br />
socialization<br />
• What counts as “culture”<br />
• Conceptual foundations<br />
• Heuristic models<br />
• Socioecological approach<br />
• Measurement <strong>of</strong> ethnicity and class<br />
• Our work on culture, parental beliefs, and<br />
parental socialization <strong>of</strong> emotion<br />
3
Significance:<br />
<strong>Emotion</strong> socialization influences . . .<br />
• Experience <strong>of</strong> emotion<br />
• Arousal<br />
• Understanding <strong>of</strong> one’s own emotions<br />
• Regulation <strong>of</strong> one’s own emotions<br />
• Expression <strong>of</strong> emotion<br />
• Regulation <strong>of</strong> one’s expression <strong>of</strong> emotion<br />
• Understanding <strong>of</strong> others’ emotions<br />
• Skill in judging other’s emotions<br />
• Interpretation <strong>of</strong> other’s emotions<br />
Dunsmore & Halberstadt, 1997; Eisenberg et al, 1998<br />
4
In short, emotion socialization impacts<br />
children’s affective social competence<br />
5
What counts as “culture”<br />
• Culture = shared values, beliefs, customs<br />
passed down from generation to generation<br />
• Culture ≠ identity<br />
• Nationality, race, ethnicity, social class may<br />
be related to culture, but not necessarily<br />
• “Culture is to a human collectivity what<br />
personality is to an individual (H<strong>of</strong>stede, 2001, p. 10).”<br />
Cole & Tan, 2007; H<strong>of</strong>stede, 2001; Parke & Buriel, 2006<br />
6
Eisenberg, Spinrad, &<br />
Cumberland, 1998<br />
Child<br />
characteristics<br />
Parent<br />
characteristics<br />
<strong>Emotion</strong>related<br />
parenting<br />
practices<br />
Child<br />
arousal<br />
Child socioemotional<br />
outcomes<br />
Cultural factors<br />
Situational<br />
context<br />
7
Garcia-Coll Coll, et al., 1996<br />
Racism,<br />
prejudice,<br />
oppression<br />
Child<br />
characteristics<br />
Social<br />
position<br />
variables<br />
Promoting/<br />
inhibiting<br />
environments<br />
Adaptive<br />
culture<br />
Developm mental<br />
competen ncies<br />
Segregation<br />
Family<br />
8
Dunsmore & Halberstadt, 1997<br />
Family’s attributions about<br />
emotions and emotional<br />
expressiveness<br />
Family’s typical pattern <strong>of</strong><br />
emotional expressiveness<br />
Child’s Schemas: <strong>Emotion</strong>s,<br />
Self, World<br />
Child characteristics<br />
Culture (e.g., appraisal <strong>of</strong><br />
situations, ti cultural l scripts)<br />
<strong>The</strong>se four factors are highly interactive with one another; also match between<br />
child, family, and culture is important in children’s formation <strong>of</strong> schemas.<br />
9
Cole & Tan, 2007<br />
• Child temperament<br />
• Attachment<br />
• Parents’ emotional expression<br />
• Positive emotions<br />
• Negative emotions<br />
• Parents’ communication about children’s<br />
emotions<br />
• Encouragement <strong>of</strong> child’s expression<br />
• Conversations related to emotions<br />
10
Universal and culture-specific<br />
emotion socialization<br />
• Universal:<br />
• Goals:<br />
• <strong>Emotion</strong>al well-being<br />
• Self-regulation; socially appropriate behavior<br />
• As a child, feeling <strong>of</strong> security<br />
• As an adult, emotional competence<br />
• Sequence <strong>of</strong> developing emotional skills<br />
• Cultural variation:<br />
• How universal goals are achieved<br />
• Specific socialization goals<br />
• Interpretation <strong>of</strong> children’s behaviors<br />
11<br />
Cole & Tan, 2007
Socioecological approach<br />
• Bruner, Harkness & Super, LeVine,<br />
Rog<strong>of</strong>f, Valsiner, Whiting & Whiting<br />
• Emphases on:<br />
• Active role <strong>of</strong> child<br />
• Nested settings<br />
• Meaning <strong>of</strong> fbehavior<br />
Rothbaum & Trommsdorff (2007)<br />
12
General issues<br />
• Ethnocentrism; “derived etic” approach<br />
• Other factors influence emotional experiences<br />
• Within-group variation<br />
• Different specific criteria may relate to child<br />
outcomes across groups, whereas broad<br />
criteria may relate similarly to child outcomes<br />
across groups<br />
• Indices may interact in unexpected ways<br />
Berry, 1990; Bradley & Corwyn, 2006; Cole & Tan, 2007; Parke & Buriel, 2006<br />
13
Measurement <strong>of</strong> social class<br />
• Three common indicators:<br />
• Education<br />
• Income<br />
• Occupational status<br />
• More nuanced indicators:<br />
• Net assets (wealth)<br />
• Perceived status & income inequality<br />
• Neighborhood conditions: violence, perceived<br />
school quality, neighborhood income,<br />
person:household space density, police records<br />
Adler, Castellazzo, & Ickovics, 2000; Ceballo & McLoyd, 2002; Conger & Dogan,<br />
2007; Conley, 1999; Darity & Nicholson, 2005; Duncan & Magnuson, 2003<br />
14
Culture within nationality<br />
• Acculturation and assimilation<br />
• Ethnic identity<br />
• Centrality <strong>of</strong> ethnic identity<br />
• Ethnic pride<br />
• Experiences with discrimination<br />
• Racial/ethnic i socialization<br />
• Ethnic pride<br />
• Preparation for bias<br />
• Promoting mistrust vs promoting egalitarianism<br />
• Group density<br />
15
Individualism & Collectivism<br />
• Also independence/interdependence, eg<strong>of</strong>ocused<br />
vs other-focused, needs <strong>of</strong> self vs<br />
needs <strong>of</strong> group<br />
• Some measures assume opposition (e.g.,<br />
H<strong>of</strong>stede, 2001)<br />
• Loose ties to others vs strong ties to others<br />
• Associated with national wealth, cooler climate<br />
• Orthogonal dimensions <strong>of</strong> self-construal<br />
16
H<strong>of</strong>stede’s s other 4 dimensions<br />
17<br />
• Power distance<br />
• Hierarchical organization, acceptance <strong>of</strong> inequality<br />
• Predicted by warmer climate, greater population, lower<br />
national wealth<br />
• Uncertainty avoidance<br />
• Feeling threatened by unpredictable, ambiguous situations<br />
• Modestly related to lower national wealth<br />
• Masculinity/femininity<br />
• Distinct gender roles (assertiveness in opposition to<br />
nurturance) vs overlapping gender roles<br />
• Modestly related to greater population<br />
• Long-term vs short-term orientation<br />
• Virtues that promote future rewards vs virtues oriented<br />
towards past and present<br />
• Predicts nation’s future economic growth
Our national scores on<br />
H<strong>of</strong>stede’s (2001) 5 dimensions<br />
100<br />
90 Canada<br />
80<br />
(English)<br />
70 Canada<br />
60<br />
50<br />
(French)<br />
Great Britain<br />
40<br />
30<br />
Ireland<br />
20<br />
10<br />
USA<br />
0<br />
PD UA I/C M/F LT/ST<br />
18
Parents’ beliefs<br />
• Participants:<br />
• 385 African-American parents (58% female)<br />
• 398 European-American parents (54% female)<br />
• 297 Lumbee Native American parents (56% female)<br />
• Measures:<br />
• Education, Occupational status<br />
• Parents’ Beliefs about Children’s <strong>Emotion</strong>s (PBACE,<br />
Halberstadt et al., 2008)<br />
• Two questionnaires assessing parent socialization<br />
behavior<br />
19
Development <strong>of</strong> PBACE<br />
• Dimensions derived from theory<br />
• Control <strong>of</strong> emotion<br />
• Parents’ role as socializers<br />
• Value/danger <strong>of</strong> emotion<br />
• 12 focus group sessions with mothers and<br />
fathers from our three target ethnicities<br />
• Additional dimensions<br />
• Developmental processes<br />
• Relational aspects<br />
• Item revision; back-translation<br />
20
PBACE subscales<br />
• Contempt<br />
• LA > AA > EA<br />
• Lower education > higher education<br />
• <strong>Emotion</strong>s are dangerous<br />
• LA > AA > EA<br />
• Lower education > higher education<br />
• Positive emotions are valuable<br />
• EA > AA & LA<br />
• Higher education > lower education<br />
21
Parenting behavior measures<br />
• Coping with Children’s Negative <strong>Emotion</strong>s<br />
Scale (CCNES, Fabes et al., 2002)<br />
• Supportive: <strong>Emotion</strong>-focused, problemfocused<br />
• Discouraging: Minimization, punitive<br />
• <strong>Emotion</strong> Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ,<br />
Gross & John, 2003)<br />
• Appraisal<br />
• Suppression<br />
22
Cultural <strong>Context</strong> and Parent<br />
Behaviors<br />
23<br />
• First, did cultural context predict parent<br />
behaviors<br />
• Yes for parents’ discouraging responses to<br />
children’s negative emotions<br />
• No for parents’ supportive responses to children’s<br />
negative emotions<br />
• No for parents’ own use <strong>of</strong> appraisal to regulate<br />
emotions<br />
• Yes for parents’ own suppression <strong>of</strong> emotions<br />
• Second, when cultural context did predict<br />
parent behaviors, did beliefs mediate the<br />
effects
Discouraging g<br />
responses:<br />
1. Predicted by Ethnicity:<br />
• F (2, 245) = 7.94, p < .001, R 2 = .06<br />
• AA > LA > EA<br />
2. Predicted by Education:<br />
• F (3, 242) = 10.40, p < .0001, R 2 = .11<br />
• Lower Education > Higher Education<br />
3. With beliefs, Ethnicity & Education drop out:<br />
• F (6, 118) = 10.05, p < .0001, R 2 = .34<br />
• Contempt β = .28, p < .01<br />
• Danger β = .16, p < .10<br />
• Positive good β = -.29, p < .001<br />
24
Suppression:<br />
1. Predicted by Ethnicity:<br />
• F (2, 134) = 4.21, p < .05, R 2 = .06<br />
• EA < AA; LA = EA and AA<br />
2. No additional effect <strong>of</strong> Education:<br />
• F (3, 131) = 3.53, p < .05, R 2 = .07<br />
3. With beliefs, effect for Ethnicity drops out:<br />
• F (6, 61) = 2.57, p < .05, R 2 = .20<br />
• Contempt t ns<br />
• Danger β = .27, p < .01<br />
• Positive good ns<br />
25
<strong>The</strong> <strong>Dynamic</strong> <strong>Context</strong><br />
• Children change over time: stage-salient<br />
salient<br />
developmental tasks, competencies<br />
• Parents and families change over time and<br />
across situations<br />
• Social class and culture are dynamic<br />
• Salience <strong>of</strong> culture, social class, identity vary<br />
across situations<br />
Calkins & Bell, 1999; Cole & Tan, 2007; Parke & Buriel, 2006<br />
26
Acknowledgements<br />
• Alfred J. Bryant, Jr.<br />
• Graduate students:<br />
• Karen Beale<br />
• Pa Her<br />
• Alison Parker<br />
• Marie Perez-Rivera<br />
• Rebecca Stelter<br />
• Julie Thompson<br />
27