10.03.2015 Views

Cognitive Processes Syllabus - Survey Research Laboratory

Cognitive Processes Syllabus - Survey Research Laboratory

Cognitive Processes Syllabus - Survey Research Laboratory

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Semester: Fall 2003<br />

PA 583: The Psychology of <strong>Survey</strong> Measurement: <strong>Cognitive</strong> and Social <strong>Processes</strong><br />

Instructor: Allyson L. Holbrook<br />

Office Hours: By appointment. Phone: (312)996-0471; E-mail: allyson@uic.edu<br />

Course Credits: 2 hours<br />

Course Overview and Objectives: This course introduces students to one approach to survey<br />

methodology – the examination of the psychological processes through which survey respondents answer<br />

questions. Evidence about these processes from cognitive and social psychology, sociology, and<br />

linguistics is applied to survey research, particularly questionnaire construction and administration.<br />

Required Texts:<br />

Tourangeau, Roger, Lance j. Rips, and Kenneth Rasinski (2000). The Psychology of <strong>Survey</strong><br />

Response. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Schwarz, N. (1996). Cognition and communication: Judgmental biases, research methods, and<br />

the logic of conversation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Assignments: There are two assignments due during the course. You will also be asked to write<br />

discussion question for three classes and to write a final paper. These assignments are designed for you to<br />

(1) demonstrate your understanding and mastery of the concepts discussed in class; (2) apply these<br />

concepts; and (3) prompt your discussion and questions. (Note: Due dates for assignments are listed in<br />

the course schedule.)<br />

Assignment #1: An article will be distributed for discussion. Students will be asked to write a 2-<br />

3 pages summary and critique of the article. The primary findings of the article or chapter should<br />

be briefly summarized and their implications discussed (1-2 paragraphs). In addition, you should<br />

discuss an issue or problem regarding the major findings of the article. For example, the<br />

conclusions drawn by the author might not be warranted given the evidence they reviewed, or<br />

there might be additional factors the authors did not consider that might be relevant to the<br />

question at hand. Finally, you should briefly discuss additional research that could address the<br />

problem or issue you described. This article (and assignment #1) will the basis for class<br />

discussion.<br />

Assignment #2: Two readings about the use of response latencies to measuring cognitive<br />

processes will be distributed. Students will be asked to write2-3 pages answering the following<br />

questions: 1) What cognitive aspects of question answering might influence response latencies?<br />

2) What are the challenges to using response latencies as an indicator of cognitive processes in<br />

surveys? 3) Briefly describe the methodology and expected results of a study using response<br />

latencies in a novel way to assess the cognitive process of answering survey questions.<br />

Discussion Questions: Students are asked to write discussion questions for 3 of the 7 classes.<br />

These weeks, ple ase write 2-3 discussion questions about the readings for that week and send<br />

them to me at allyson@uic.edu by noon on the day of class. You will be graded on the<br />

promptness and completeness of the questions as well as the quality of the questions themselves.<br />

Final Paper. A final paper is due on the Tuesday of finals week (May 3) by 5:00 p.m. The paper<br />

has two components. First, you should review the literature regarding one aspect of the<br />

psychological processes of answering survey questions and how it relates to survey methodology<br />

1


more broadly. Second, you should outline a proposal for research that builds on the literature<br />

review in Part I. The research you propose should add to our understanding of the psychological<br />

processes of answering survey questions and be tie directly into the aspect of these processes that<br />

you review in Part I of this paper (Each part of the paper should be approximately 10 pages so the<br />

paper should be 20 pages).<br />

Course Policies:<br />

Deadlines: All assignments are due at the beginning of class on the day they are due. Students will<br />

lose 10% credit for each day that an assignment is late. In addition, do the assigned readings before<br />

they are discussed in class.<br />

Extensions for assignments: Extensions for assignments are at the discretion of the instructors and<br />

should be requested before the deadline. Emergencies will be considered on a case-by-case basis. If<br />

you have a question about an extension, please talk to the instructor.<br />

Plagiarism or cheating: Don’t do it. It’s not worth it. Guidelines regarding academic integrity at<br />

UIC are available online , and sanctions for academic<br />

dishonesty are also available . Please don’t put me in a<br />

position where I need to learn more about these policies.<br />

Attendance: Fifteen points will be assigned based on attendance and participation. If you must miss<br />

a class, please let the instructors know in advance. Class attendance is strongly encouraged. Active<br />

involvement in class is also expected. If your final grade is on the border, a subjective assessment of<br />

your class participation may be used to make final adjustments.<br />

Grades<br />

Assignment<br />

Points<br />

Percent of Total<br />

Grade<br />

Assignment #1 20 20<br />

Assignment #2 20 20<br />

Discussion Questions 15 15<br />

Class Participation 10 10<br />

Final Paper 30 30<br />

Attendance 5 5<br />

Total 100 points 100 %<br />

Outline:<br />

March 8 (Week 1)<br />

Part I: <strong>Cognitive</strong> Models of the Response Process<br />

Part II: Pre-testing and measuring cognitive processes<br />

Part III: <strong>Cognitive</strong> interview exercise and discussion<br />

2


March 15 (Week2):<br />

Part I: Comprehension and Interpretation of <strong>Survey</strong> Questions<br />

Part II: Autobiographical Memory<br />

Part III: Discussion Questions (set 1)<br />

Distribute Assignment #1<br />

March 22: NO CLASS - SPRING BREAK<br />

March 29 (Week 3):<br />

Part I: Context Effects in <strong>Survey</strong>s<br />

Part II: <strong>Cognitive</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> and Response Effects (Satisficing)<br />

Part III: Assignment #1 (critique and discussion of article)<br />

Assignment #1 due<br />

April 5 (Week 4)<br />

Part I: Memory Errors in <strong>Survey</strong>s<br />

Part II: Temporal Judgments<br />

Part III: Discussion Questions (set 2)<br />

April 12 (Week 5)<br />

Part I: Estimation <strong>Processes</strong><br />

Part II: Probability Judgments<br />

Part III: Discussion Questions (set 3)<br />

April 19 (Week 6)<br />

Part I: Attitudes<br />

Part II: Persuasion, Social Influence, and Nonresponse<br />

Part III: Mapping, Social Norms and Social Desirability<br />

Distribute Assignment #2<br />

April 26 (Week 7)<br />

Part I: Interviewers: Rapport and Social Distance<br />

Part II: Implications and Summing up<br />

Part III: Assignment #2 Discussion<br />

Assignment #2 due<br />

May 3: Final paper due by 5:00 p.m.<br />

******************************************************************************<br />

3


Schedule and Reading Assignments<br />

Week 1<br />

Part I: <strong>Cognitive</strong> Models of the Response Process<br />

Readings:<br />

Chapter 1 of Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinksi, K. (2000). The Psychology of<br />

<strong>Survey</strong> Response.<br />

Part II: Pre-testing<br />

Week2:<br />

Readings: To be distributed<br />

Part I: Comprehension and Interpretation of <strong>Survey</strong> Questions<br />

Readings:<br />

Chapter 2 of Tourangeau et al. (2000)<br />

Fowler, F. (1992). How unclear terms affect survey data. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56,<br />

218-231.<br />

Schwarz, N. (1996). Cognition and communication: Judgmental biases, research<br />

methods, and the logic of conversation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Part II: Autobiographical Memory<br />

Readings<br />

Chapter 3 of Tourangeau et al. (2000)<br />

Shum, M. S., & Rips, L. J. (1999). The Respondent’s Confession: Autobiographical<br />

Memory in the context of <strong>Survey</strong>s. In Sirken, M. G., Herrmann, D. J., Schechter, S.,<br />

Schwarz, N., Tanur, J. M., & Tourangeau, R. (Eds.), Cognition and <strong>Survey</strong> <strong>Research</strong>.<br />

New York: Wiley.<br />

Part III: Discussion Questions (set 1)<br />

4


Week 3:<br />

Week 4<br />

Part I: Context Effects in <strong>Survey</strong>s<br />

Readings<br />

Chapter 7 of Tourangeau et al.<br />

Bishop, G., Oldendick, R., & Tuchfarber, A. (1984). What must my interest in politics be if<br />

I just told you "I don't know"? Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 510-519.<br />

Schwarz, N., Strack, F., & H. Mai. (1991). Assimilation and contrast effects in part-whole<br />

question sequences: A conversational logic analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 3-23.<br />

Part II: <strong>Cognitive</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> and Response Effects<br />

Readings<br />

Krosnick, J. A. (1991). Response strategies for coping with the cognitive demands of<br />

attitude measures in surveys. Applied <strong>Cognitive</strong> Psychology, 5, 213-236.<br />

Krosnick, J., & Alwin, D. (1987). An evaluation of a cognitive theory of response order<br />

effects in survey measurement. Public Opinion Quarterly, 51, 201-219.<br />

Krosnick, J. A., Narayan, S., & Smith, W. R. (1996). Satisficing in <strong>Survey</strong>s: Initial<br />

Evidence. New Directions for Evaluation, 70, 29-44.<br />

Part III: Assignment #1 (critique and discussion of article)<br />

Part I: Memory Errors in <strong>Survey</strong>s<br />

Readings<br />

Loftus, E. F., Smith, K. D., Klinger, M. R. & Fiedler, J. (1992). Memory and mismemory<br />

for health events. In J. M. Tanur (ed), Questions about questions: inquiries into the<br />

cognitive bases of surveys (pp. 102-137). New York: Sage.<br />

Belli, R. F., Traugott, M. W., Young, M., & McGonagle, K. A. (1999). Reducing<br />

Vote Overreporting in <strong>Survey</strong>s: Social Desirability, Memory Failure, and Source<br />

Monitoring. Public Opinion Quarterly, 63, 90-108.<br />

Part II: Temporal Judgments<br />

Readings<br />

Required<br />

Chapters 4 and 5 in Tourangeau, et al. (2000)<br />

Part III: Discussion Questions (set 2)<br />

5


Week 5<br />

Part I: Estimation <strong>Processes</strong><br />

Readings<br />

Required<br />

Schaeffer, N.C., & Bradburn, N. (1989). Respondent behavior in magnitude<br />

estimation. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 84, 402-413.<br />

Burton, S., & Blair, E. (1991). Task conditions, response formulation processes, and<br />

response accuracy for behavioral frequency questions in surveys. Public Opinion<br />

Quarterly, 55, 50-79.<br />

Part II: Probability Judgments<br />

Week 6:<br />

Readings<br />

Part II: Attitudes<br />

Readings<br />

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and<br />

biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131.<br />

Hastie, R. & Park, B. (1986). The relationship between memory and judgment depends<br />

on whether the judgment is memory-based or on-line. Psychological Review, 93, 258-<br />

268.<br />

Chapter 6 of Tourangeau et al. (2000)<br />

Dawes, R., & Smith, T. (1985). Attitude and opinion measurement. In G. Lindzey & E.<br />

Aronson (Eds.), The Handbook of Social Psychology: Vol. 1. (3rd ed.). New York:<br />

Random House, pp. 509-566.<br />

Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude<br />

measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science, 121,<br />

163-170.<br />

Fazio, R., Sanbonmatsu, D., Powell, M., & Kardes, F. (1986). On the automatic<br />

activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 229-238.<br />

Part II: Persuasion, Social Influence, and Nonresponse<br />

Readings<br />

Required<br />

Groves, R., Cialdini, R., & Couper, M. (1992). Understanding the decision to<br />

participate in a survey. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56, 475-495.<br />

6


Petty, R. E., Wheeler, S. C., & Tormala, Z. L. (2002). Persuasion and Attitude<br />

Change. In T. Millong & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of<br />

psychology. New York: John Wiley and Sons.<br />

Part III: Mapping, Social Norms and Social Desirability<br />

Week 7:<br />

Readings<br />

Required<br />

Chapters 8 and 9 of Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinksi, K. (2000). The<br />

Psychology of <strong>Survey</strong> Response.<br />

Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding.<br />

Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 46, 598-609.<br />

Bishop, G., Oldendick, R., & Tuchfarber, A. (1986). Opinions on fictitious issues: The<br />

pressure to answer survey questions. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50, 240-250.<br />

Part I: Interviewers: Rapport and Social Distance<br />

Readings<br />

Required<br />

Chapter 10 of Tourangeau et al. (2000)<br />

Schaeffer, N. (1991). Conversation with a purpose--or conversation? Interaction in the<br />

standardized interview. In P. P. Biemer, R. M. Groves, L. E. Lyberg, N. A. Mathiowetz<br />

& S. Sudman (eds), Measurement errors in surveys. New York: Wiley, pp. 367-391.<br />

van der Zouwen, J., Dijkstra, W., & Smit, J. (1991). Studying respondent interviewer<br />

interaction: The relationship between interviewing style, interviewer behavior, and<br />

response behavior. P. P. Biemer, R. M. Groves, L. E. Lyberg, N. A. Mathiowetz & S.<br />

Sudman (eds), Measurement errors in surveys. New York: Wiley, pp. 419-437.<br />

Schober, M. F. (1999). Making sense of questions: An interactional approach. In<br />

Sirken, M. G., Herrmann, D. J., Schechter, S., Schwarz, N., Tanur, J. M., &<br />

Tourangeau, R. (Eds.), Cognition and <strong>Survey</strong> <strong>Research</strong>. New York: Wiley.<br />

Katz, D. (1942). Do interviewers bias poll results? Public Opinion Quarterly, 6, 248-<br />

268.<br />

Part II: Implications and Summing up<br />

Reading (required)<br />

Chapter 11 of Tourangeau et al. (2000).<br />

Part III: Assignment #2 Discussion<br />

7


Optional readings:<br />

<strong>Cognitive</strong> Models of the Response Process<br />

Chapters 1-3 in Sudman, S., Bradburn, N., & Schwarz, N. (1996), Thinking about Answers: The<br />

Application of <strong>Cognitive</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> to <strong>Survey</strong> Methodology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Bradburn, N., & Danis, C. (1984). Potential contributions of cognitive research to survey<br />

questionnaire design. In T. Jabine et al. (Eds.), <strong>Cognitive</strong> Aspects of <strong>Survey</strong> Design: Building a<br />

Bridge Between Disciplines. Washington: National Academy Press, pp. 101-129.<br />

Strack, F., & Martin, L. (1987). Thinking, judging, and communicating: A process account of context<br />

effects in attitude surveys. In H. Hippler, N. Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Social information<br />

processing and survey methodology (pp. 123-148). New York: Springer-Verlag.<br />

Tourangeau, R. (1984). <strong>Cognitive</strong> science and survey methods. In T. Jabine et al. (Eds.), <strong>Cognitive</strong><br />

Aspects of <strong>Survey</strong> Design: Building a Bridge Between Disciplines. Washington: National Academy<br />

Press, pp.73-100.<br />

Chapter 9 of Groves, R. (1989). <strong>Survey</strong> Costs and <strong>Survey</strong> Errors. New York: Wiley.<br />

Comprehension and Interpretation of <strong>Survey</strong> Questions<br />

Bradburn, N., & Miles, C. (1979). Vague quantifiers. Public Opinion Quarterly, 43, 92-101.<br />

Clark, H. (1985). Language use and language users. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The<br />

Handbook of Social Psychology: Vol. 2. Special fields and applications (3rd ed.). New York:<br />

Random House, pp. 179-231.<br />

Chase, C. (1969). Often is where you find it. American Psychologist, 24, 1043.<br />

Pace, C., & Friedlander, J. (1982). The meaning of response categories: How often is 'Occasionally,'<br />

'Often,' and 'Very Often'? <strong>Research</strong> in Higher Education, 17, 267-281.<br />

Pepper, S., & Prytulak, L. (1974). Sometimes frequently means seldom: Context effects in the<br />

interpretation of quantitative expressions. Journal of <strong>Research</strong> in Personality, 8, 95-101.<br />

Schaeffer, N. C. (1991). Hardly or constantly? Group comparisons using vague quantifiers. Public<br />

Opinion Quarterly, 55, 395-423.<br />

Wallsten, T., Budescu, D., Rapoport, A., Zwick, R., & Forsythe, B. (1986). "Measuring the vague<br />

meaning of probability terms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 348-365.<br />

Grice, H. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & T. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics: Vol.<br />

3 Speech Acts (pp. 41-58). New York: Seminar Press.<br />

Context Effects in <strong>Survey</strong>s<br />

Tourangeau, R. (1999). Context effects on answers to attitude questions. In Sirken, M. G.,<br />

Herrmann, D. J., Schechter, S., Schwarz, N., Tanur, J. M., & Tourangeau, R. (Eds.), Cognition and<br />

<strong>Survey</strong> <strong>Research</strong>. New York: Wiley.<br />

8


Chapters 4 and 5 in Sudman, S., Bradburn, N., & Schwarz, N. (1996), Thinking about Answers: The<br />

Application of <strong>Cognitive</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> to <strong>Survey</strong> Methodology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Tourangeau, R., & Rasinski, K. (1988). "<strong>Cognitive</strong> processes underlying context effects in attitude<br />

measurement." Psychological Bulletin, 103, 229-314.<br />

Schuman, H. (1992). Context effects: State of the art/state of the past. In N. Schwarz & S. Sudman<br />

(Eds.), Context effects in social and psychological research. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 35-47.<br />

Tourangeau, R. (1992). "Context effects on attitude responses: The role of retrieval and memory<br />

structures." In N. Schwarz & S. Sudman (Eds.), Context effects in social and psychological research.<br />

New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 35-47.<br />

Tourangeau, R., Rasinski, K., Bradburn, N., & D'Andrade, R. (1989a). Carryover effects in attitude<br />

surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 53, 495-524.<br />

Bishop, G., Oldendick, R., & Tuchfarber, A. (1982). Political information processing: Question order<br />

and context effects. Political Behavior, 4, 177-200.<br />

Bishop, G., Oldendick, R., & Tuchfarber, A. (1984a). Interest in political campaigns: The influence of<br />

question order and electoral context. Political Behavior, 6, 159-168.<br />

McClendon, M., & O'Brien, D. (1988). Question-order effects on subjective well-being. Public<br />

Opinion Quarterly, 52, 351-364.<br />

Smith, T. W. (1991). Context effects in the General Social <strong>Survey</strong>. In P. P. Biemer, R. M. Groves,<br />

L. E. Lyberg, N. A. Mathiowetz & S. Sudman (eds), Measurement errors in surveys. New York:<br />

Wiley, pp. 367-391.<br />

<strong>Cognitive</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> and Response Effects<br />

Chapter 6 in Sudman, S., Bradburn, N., & Schwarz, N. (1996), Thinking about Answers: The<br />

Application of <strong>Cognitive</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> to <strong>Survey</strong> Methodology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Schwarz, N., & Hippler, H. (1987). What response scales may tell your respondents: Informative<br />

functions of response alternatives. In H. Hippler, N. Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Social<br />

Information Processing and <strong>Survey</strong> Methodology. New York: Springer-Verlag.<br />

Wanke, M., Schwarz, N., & Noelle-Neumann, E. (1995). Asking comparative questions: The impact<br />

of the direction of comparison. Public Opinion Quarterly, 59, 347-372.<br />

Bishop, G., Oldendick, R., & Tuchfarber, A. (1983). Effects of filter questions in public opinion<br />

surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47, 528-546.<br />

Autobiographical Memory<br />

Loftus, E. F., & Fathi, D. C. (1985). Retrieving multiple autobiographical memories. Social<br />

Cognition, 3, 280-295.<br />

Wagenaar, W. (1986). My memory: A study of autobiographical memory over six years. <strong>Cognitive</strong><br />

Psychology, 18, 225-252.<br />

Robinson, J. (1986). Temporal reference systems and autobiographical memory. In D. Rubin (Ed.),<br />

Autobiograpical memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 159-188.<br />

9


Mathiowetz, N. A. (1988). Forgetting events in autobiographical memory: findings from a health care<br />

survey. Proceedings of the Section on <strong>Survey</strong> Methods <strong>Research</strong>, American Statistical Association,<br />

pp. 167-172.<br />

Temporal Judgments<br />

Chapter 8 in Sudman, S., Bradburn, N., & Schwarz, N. (1996), Thinking about Answers: The<br />

Application of <strong>Cognitive</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> to <strong>Survey</strong> Methodology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Friedman, W. (1993). Memory for the time of past events. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 44-66.<br />

Thompson, C. P., Skowronski, J. J. & Lee, D. J. (1988). Telescoping in dating naturally occurring<br />

events. Memory and Cognition, 16, 461-468.<br />

Loftus, E. F. & Marburger, W. (1983). Since the eruption of Mt. St. Helens, has anyone beaten you<br />

up? Improving the accuracy of retrospective reports with landmark events. Memory and Cognition,<br />

11, 114-120.<br />

Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L., & Prohaska, V. (1988). Hierarchical organization in ordered domains:<br />

estimating the dates of events. Psychological Review, 95, 471-484.<br />

Sudman, S. & Bradburn, N. M. (1973). Effects of time and memory factors on response in surveys.<br />

Journal of the American Statistical Association, 68, 805-815.<br />

Baddeley, A. D., Lewis, V. & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1978). When did you last ... ? In M. M. Gruneberg &<br />

R. N. Sykes (eds), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 77-83). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.<br />

Rubin, D.C. & Baddeley, A.D. (1989). Telescoping is not time compression: a model of the dating of<br />

autobiographical events. Memory and Cognition, 17, 653-661.<br />

10


Estimation <strong>Processes</strong><br />

Chapter 9 in Sudman, S., Bradburn, N., & Schwarz, N. (1996), Thinking about Answers: The<br />

Application of <strong>Cognitive</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> to <strong>Survey</strong> Methodology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L., & Bradburn, N. M. (1990). Reports of elapsed time: bounding and<br />

rounding processes in estimation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and<br />

Cognition, 16, 196-213.<br />

Memory Errors in <strong>Survey</strong>s<br />

Chapter 10 in Sudman, S., Bradburn, N., & Schwarz, N. (1996), Thinking about Answers: The<br />

Application of <strong>Cognitive</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> to <strong>Survey</strong> Methodology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Kolers, P. A., & Palef, S. R. (1976). Knowing not. Memory and Cognition, 4, 553-558.<br />

Neter, J., & Waksberg, J. (1964). A study of response errors in expenditures data from household<br />

interviews. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 59, 17-55.<br />

Jobe, J., Tourangeau, R., & Smith, A.F. (1993). "Contributions of survey research to the<br />

understanding of memory." Applied <strong>Cognitive</strong> Psychology, 7, 567-584.<br />

Blair, J., Menon, G., & Bickart, B. (1991). Measurement effects in self vs. proxy responses: An<br />

information-processing perspective. In P. P. Biemer, R. M. Groves, L. E. Lyberg, N. A. Mathiowetz<br />

& S. Sudman (eds), Measurement errors in surveys (pp. 145-166). New York: Wiley.<br />

Cash, W. S. & Moss, A. J. (1972). Optimum recall period for reporting persons injured in motor<br />

vehicle accidents. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 2, No. 50 (DHEW Publication No. HSM 72-<br />

1050). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.<br />

Loftus, E. F., Klinger, M. R., Smith, K. D. & Fiedler, J. (1990). A tale of two questions: benefits of<br />

asking more than one question. Public Opinion Quarterly, 54, 330-345.<br />

Means, B. & Loftus, E. F. (1991). When personal history repeats itself: decomposing memories for<br />

recurring events. Applied <strong>Cognitive</strong> Psychology, 5, 297-318.<br />

Means, B., Swan, G. E., Jobe, J., & Esposito, J. L. (1993). Estimating frequencies for habitual<br />

behaviors: reports of cigarette smoking. In N. Schwarz & S. Sudman (eds), Autobiographical memory<br />

and the validity of retrospective reports. New York: Springer-Verlag.<br />

Means, B., Swan, G. E., Jobe, J. B. & Esposito, J. L. (1991). An alternative approach to obtaining<br />

personal history data. In P. P. Biemer, R. M. Groves, L. E. Lyberg, N. A. Mathiowetz & S. Sudman<br />

(eds), Measurement errors in surveys (pp. 167-183). New York: Wiley.<br />

Menon, G. (1993). Judgments of behavioral frequencies: memory search and retrieval strategies. In<br />

N. Schwarz & S. Sudman (eds), Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports.<br />

New York: Springer-Verlag.<br />

Mingay, D. J. & Greenwell, M. T. (1989). Memory bias and response-order effects. Journal of<br />

Official Statistics, 5, 253-263.<br />

Smith, A. F. & Jobe, J. B. (1993). Reports of long-term dietary memories: data and a model. In N.<br />

Schwarz & S. Sudman (eds), Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports. New<br />

York: Springer-Verlag.<br />

11


Smith, A. F., Jobe, J. B. & Mingay, D. J. (1991). Question-induced cognitive biases in reports of<br />

dietary intake by college men and women. Health Psychology, 10, 244-251.<br />

Sudman, S., Finn, A. & Lannom, L. (1984). The use of bounded recall procedures in single<br />

interviews. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 520-524.<br />

Tourangeau, R. & Rasinski, K. A. (1987). Evaluation of data collection frequency and the use of a<br />

summary in the National Medical Care Utilization and Expenditure <strong>Survey</strong>. National Medical Care<br />

Utilization and Expenditure <strong>Survey</strong>, Series A, No. 2 (DHHS Publication No. PHS 87-20002).<br />

Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.<br />

Glucksberg, S. & McCloskey, M. (1981). Decisions about ignorance: knowing what you don't know.<br />

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 311-325.<br />

Probability Judgments<br />

Gentner, D., & Collins, A. (1981). Studies of inference from lack of knowledge. Memory and<br />

Cognition, 9, 434-443.<br />

Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.<br />

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment.<br />

Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.<br />

Sherman, S., & Corty, E. (1984). <strong>Cognitive</strong> heuristics. In R. Wyer and T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of<br />

Social Cognition (pp. 189-286). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.<br />

Attitudes<br />

Salancik, G., & Conway, M. (1975). Attitude inferences from salient and relevant cognitive content<br />

about behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 829-840.<br />

Schuman, J., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys: Experiments in<br />

question form, wording, and context. New York: Academic Press.<br />

12


Persuasion, Social Influence, and Nonresponse<br />

Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus<br />

message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752-766.<br />

McGuire, W. (1968). The nature of attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.),<br />

The handbook of social psychology, (Vol. 3). Reading, Mass: Addison-Wesley.<br />

Mapping, Social Norms and Social Desirability<br />

DeMaio, Theresa J., "Social Desirability and <strong>Survey</strong> Measurement: A Review," Chapter Nine, pp.<br />

257-282 in Charles F. Turner and Elizabeth Martin, <strong>Survey</strong>ing Subjective Phenomena, Vol. 2, Russell<br />

Sage, 1985.<br />

Interviewers: Rapport and Social Distance<br />

Suchman, L., & Jordan, B. (1990). Interactional troubles in face-to-face survey interviews. Journal of<br />

the American Statistical Association, 85, 232-241.<br />

Fowler, F., & Mangione, T. (1985). The value of interviewer training and supervision. Final Report to<br />

the National Center for Health Services <strong>Research</strong>.<br />

Fowler, F., & Mangione, T. (1989). Standardized survey interviewing: Minimizing interviewerrelated<br />

error. Newbury Park, California: Sage.<br />

Horvitz, D. (1952). Sampling and field procedures in the Pittsburgh Morbidity <strong>Survey</strong>, Public Health<br />

Reports, 67, 1003-1012.<br />

Hyman, H., Cobb, W., Feldman, J., Hart, C., & Stember, C. (1954). Interviewing in social research.<br />

Chicago: University of Chicago Press.<br />

13

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!