08.07.2015 Views

Development of a questionnaire for the assessment of ... - PsychWiki

Development of a questionnaire for the assessment of ... - PsychWiki

Development of a questionnaire for the assessment of ... - PsychWiki

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport<strong>Development</strong> <strong>of</strong> a <strong>questionnaire</strong> <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> copingstrategies employed by athletes in competitive sport settingsP. Gaudreau * , J.-P. BlondinDépartement de psychologie, Université de Montréal, P.O. Box 6128, Station Centre-ville, Montreal, Québec,Canada H3C 3J7Received 3 October 2000; received in revised <strong>for</strong>m 22 March 2001; accepted 24 May 2001AbstractObjectives: To develop an original self-report instrument <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> athletes’ coping strategiesin competitive sport settings and to provide preliminary evidence <strong>for</strong> its factorial, convergent, concurrent,and differential validity.Method: French–Canadian athletes (N=316; M age=17.4 years) completed l’Inventaire des Stratégies deCoping en Compétition Sportive (ISCCS) along with three measures <strong>of</strong> cognitive appraisal (i.e. perceivedrelevance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> competition, sense <strong>of</strong> control, and perceived goal attainment), ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> PANAS or <strong>the</strong>CSAI-2, and ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> WOCQ, <strong>the</strong> MCOPE, or a social desirability scale. All scales were administeredwithin six hours <strong>of</strong> completion <strong>of</strong> a sport competition.Results: A sequential four-stage confirmatory factor analysis revealed an adequate fit <strong>for</strong> a 10-factormodel and its superiority over a two- and a three-factor model. The subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS correlatedmeaningfully with appraisal variables, positive and negative affect (PANAS), cognitive and somatic anxiety(CSAI-2), and <strong>the</strong> coping strategies <strong>of</strong> both WOCQ and MCOPE <strong>questionnaire</strong>s, thus providing evidence<strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir concurrent and convergent validity. A multivariate factorial analysis <strong>of</strong> variance lent partial support<strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> differential validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS as ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure, mental imagery, relaxation, and venting <strong>of</strong>unpleasant emotion varied significantly across athletes’ level <strong>of</strong> expertise whereas venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasantemotion and ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure differed significantly between genders.Conclusions: The results revealed promising features <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS. Future research should assesswhe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> factorial structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS is invariant across athletes’ genders and expertise levels aswell as across <strong>the</strong> different phases <strong>of</strong> a sport competition. © 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.Keywords: ISCCS; Coping; Emotion; Per<strong>for</strong>mance; Validity* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-514-343-6508; fax: +1-514-343-2285.E-mail address: gaudreau-pat@sympatico.ca (P. Gaudreau).1469-0292/01/$ - see front matter © 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.PII: S1469-0292(01)00017-6


2 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34IntroductionIn recent years, several researchers began devoting empirical attention to <strong>the</strong> strategies usedby athletes in order to manage <strong>the</strong> stressful demands encountered in sport settings. A first line <strong>of</strong>inquiry was aimed at delineating <strong>the</strong> coping actions <strong>of</strong> athletes in various sport settings (Eklund,1996; Gould, Eklund, & Jackson, 1993; Gould, Finch, & Jackson, 1993; Prapavessis & Grove,1995; Wingate, 1993). Along with providing in-depth descriptions <strong>of</strong> athletes’ coping actions,<strong>the</strong>se works have led to <strong>the</strong> identification <strong>of</strong> numerous clusters <strong>of</strong> homogeneous and distinctcoping strategies. Based on systemic models <strong>of</strong> coping (Finch, 1994; Hardy, Jones, & Gould,1996; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), a second line <strong>of</strong> inquiry examined <strong>the</strong> determinants and outcomes<strong>of</strong> coping in order to identify which specific strategies are best <strong>for</strong> managing <strong>the</strong> internaland external demands encountered in sport settings. Coping strategies <strong>of</strong> athletes have been linkedto variables such as perceived control (Anshel & Kaissidis, 1997; Haney & Long, 1995), selfefficacy(Haney & Long, 1995), trait-anxiety (Finch, 1994; Giacobbi & Weinberg, 2000), traitconfidenceand trait-optimism (Grove & Heard, 1997), state-anxiety (Ntoumanis & Biddle, 2000),goal orientation (Kim & Duda, 1999; Ntoumanis, Biddle, & Haddock, 1999), perceived motivationalclimate (Ntoumanis et al., 1999), positive and negative affect (Crocker & Graham, 1995;Gaudreau, Blondin, & Lapierre, 2001; Ntoumanis et al., 1999), normative measures <strong>of</strong> per<strong>for</strong>mance(Finch, 1994; Haney & Long, 1995), and per<strong>for</strong>mance–goal discrepancy (Gaudreau et al.,2001). Despite <strong>the</strong>se significant advances in both quantitative and qualitative research, insufficientef<strong>for</strong>t has been devoted to <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> quantitative measures <strong>of</strong> coping in sport. Thispaper provides a critical overview <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> self-report coping <strong>questionnaire</strong>s that have been used insport psychology research. Then, it addresses <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> an original measure <strong>for</strong> assessingcoping strategies <strong>of</strong> athletes in competitive sport events.Conceptual and <strong>the</strong>oretical foundations <strong>of</strong> coping instrumentsThe construct <strong>of</strong> coping has been defined as <strong>the</strong> behavioral and cognitive ef<strong>for</strong>ts <strong>of</strong> an individualto manage <strong>the</strong> internal and external demands encountered during a specific stressful situation(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). This definition outlines <strong>the</strong> multivariate nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> construct andassumes <strong>the</strong> necessity <strong>of</strong> aggregating coping actions into meaningful clusters. Thus, a plethora <strong>of</strong>coping typologies has been developed (Parker & Endler, 1992; Zeidner & Endler, 1996) andnumerous researchers borrowed from Lazarus and Folkman’s (1984) distinction between two functionalhigher-order dimensions <strong>of</strong> coping. The first dimension, <strong>the</strong> task-oriented coping (TOC),refers to <strong>the</strong> actions that are employed in order to change or master some aspects <strong>of</strong> a situationthat is perceived as stressful. This dimension subsumes specific coping strategies such as increasedef<strong>for</strong>t, planning, and logical analysis. The second dimension, <strong>the</strong> emotion-oriented coping (EOC),represents <strong>the</strong> actions that are employed in order to change <strong>the</strong> meaning <strong>of</strong> a stressful situationas well as to regulate <strong>the</strong> resulting negative emotions. This dimension subsumes specific copingstrategies such as humor, venting <strong>of</strong> emotion, and acceptance. Despite <strong>the</strong> heuristic value <strong>of</strong> thisconsensual typology, <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> a third functional dimension <strong>of</strong> coping has been suggestedby several authors (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989; Endler & Parker, 1994) and supportedby <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> second-order factor analyses (Hudek-Knezevic, Kardum, & Vukmirovic, 1999;Zautra, Sheets, & Sandler, 1997). This third dimension, <strong>the</strong> avoidance-oriented coping, represents


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–343<strong>the</strong> actions that are employed in order to disengage oneself from <strong>the</strong> task and to redirect one’sattention on task-irrelevant cues. This dimension includes specific strategies such as behavioraldisengagement, denial, and use <strong>of</strong> alcohol/drug.A second <strong>the</strong>oretical issue in coping research stems from its definition as ei<strong>the</strong>r a trait or asituation-specific response. According to <strong>the</strong> coping trait paradigm, coping represents <strong>the</strong> actionsthat people usually per<strong>for</strong>m under stressful circumstances. Tenants <strong>of</strong> this approach (Anshel &Kaissidis, 1997; Bolger, 1990; Carver et al., 1989) assume that coping responses should remainstable, as people employ a preferred set <strong>of</strong> responses when confronted with stressful events. Sucha perspective contrasts with <strong>the</strong> process-oriented approach, which defines coping as contextuallydependent responses (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Tenants <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> latter approach (Compas &Epping, 1993; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) explicitly assume that coping responses should changeacross stressful situations and across <strong>the</strong> distinctive phases <strong>of</strong> a stressful situation.Empirical evidence in mainstream and sport psychology tends to support <strong>the</strong> notion that copingactions change across situations (Frydenberg & Lewis, 1994; Sellers, 1995), across <strong>the</strong> samesituation over time (Stewart & Schwarzer, 1996; Crocker & Isaak, 1997), and across <strong>the</strong> distinctivephases <strong>of</strong> a given situation (Carver & Scheier, 1994; Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Gaudreau et al.,2001; Gaudreau, Lapierre, & Blondin, in press). Based on <strong>the</strong>se findings, many sport scientistshave recently embraced <strong>the</strong> process-oriented approach <strong>of</strong> coping (Cerin, Szabo, Hunt, & Williams,2000; Crocker, Kowalski, & Graham, 1998; Hardy et al., 1996). Thus, self-report measures suchas <strong>the</strong> Ways <strong>of</strong> Coping Questionnaire (WOCQ; Folkman & Lazarus, 1985) and <strong>the</strong> COPE Inventory(COPE; Carver et al., 1989) were adapted <strong>for</strong> sport settings in order to assess athletes’ copingactions under specific stressful situations.Sport versions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQThe WOCQ (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985) measures coping strategies that are supposedly applicablein numerous stressful settings. Despite <strong>the</strong> widespread utilization <strong>of</strong> this <strong>questionnaire</strong> inmainstream psychology, <strong>the</strong> clarity, conceptual specificity, and applicability <strong>of</strong> its items have beencriticized abundantly in recent years (Ben-Porath, Waller, & Butcher, 1991; Stone, Greenberg,Kennedy-Moore, & Newman, 1991). Moreover, empirical evidence has lent credence to <strong>the</strong>secritics by showing <strong>the</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> internal consistency and <strong>the</strong> factorial instability <strong>of</strong> this <strong>questionnaire</strong>(Clark, Bormann, Cropanzano, & James, 1995; Parker, Endler, & Bagby, 1993).Regardless <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se limitations, <strong>the</strong> Ways <strong>of</strong> Coping <strong>for</strong> Sport (WOCS; Madden, Kirkby, &McDonald, 1989) and <strong>the</strong> Modified Ways <strong>of</strong> Coping Questionnaire (MWOCQ; Crocker, 1992)were developed by modifying <strong>the</strong> 66 items <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> original WOCQ in order to make <strong>the</strong>m relevantin sport settings. As expected, <strong>the</strong>se versions possess similar conceptual and psychometrical weaknessesas <strong>the</strong> original <strong>questionnaire</strong>. Firstly, <strong>the</strong> factorial structure is inconsistent across <strong>the</strong> versions<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>questionnaire</strong>. For instance, factor labels <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCS differed from those <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>MWOCQ (Crocker, 1992). The lack <strong>of</strong> conceptual specificity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subscales, developed solelyon <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> exploratory factor analysis, might explain this inconsistency. Also, several itemsloaded on different factors across <strong>the</strong> various versions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>questionnaire</strong>. As such, <strong>the</strong> ambiguity<strong>of</strong> items and <strong>the</strong>ir lack <strong>of</strong> conceptual specificity might have caused <strong>the</strong>m to represent more thanone coping strategy. Secondly, <strong>the</strong> factorial structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCS (Madden et al., 1989) isinconsistent across samples. Whereas <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> a principal components analysis, per<strong>for</strong>med


4 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34with a sample <strong>of</strong> 130 Australian basketball players, yielded an eight-factor model (Madden et al.,1989), those <strong>of</strong> a confirmatory factor analysis, per<strong>for</strong>med with a sample <strong>of</strong> 630 Australian athletes,indicated <strong>the</strong> inadequate fit <strong>of</strong> this model and <strong>the</strong> more adequate fit <strong>of</strong> an alternative four-factormodel (Grove, Eklund, & Heard, 1997). In sum, <strong>the</strong> factorial instability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se <strong>questionnaire</strong>sand <strong>the</strong>ir incapacity to replicate <strong>the</strong> factorial model <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> original WOCQ (Folkman & Lazarus,1985) might hinder <strong>the</strong> comparison <strong>of</strong> empirical data across research settings and also might raiseconcerns about <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> data collected with <strong>the</strong>se measures.Utilization <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> COPE inventory in sport settingsReacting to <strong>the</strong> relative lack <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical relevance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQ, Carver and his collaborators(1989) developed <strong>the</strong> COPE Inventory, a <strong>the</strong>oretically based measure assessing 15 coping strategiesthat are applicable across numerous stressful settings. Results <strong>of</strong> a principal axis factoranalysis conducted with a sample <strong>of</strong> 978 American college students lent partial support to <strong>the</strong>irmodel. However, items from <strong>the</strong> two social support scales loaded on a single factor, and itemsfrom <strong>the</strong> active coping and from <strong>the</strong> planning scales also loaded on a single factor. Althoughunexpected, this 13-factor model was corroborated by <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> a confirmatory factor analysis(Clark et al., 1995) which indicated its superiority over <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretically derived 15-factor model.Sport scientists soon acknowledged <strong>the</strong> qualities <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> COPE. Based on <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>irqualitative studies, Gould and his collaborators (1993) asserted that <strong>the</strong> COPE was <strong>the</strong> best quantitativeinstrument to assess coping actions in sport settings, and numerous sport scientists havesince employed <strong>the</strong> COPE (Eubank & Collins, 2000; Finch, 1994). As such, <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> aconfirmatory factor analysis, conducted with a sample <strong>of</strong> 870 Australian athletes, confirmed <strong>the</strong>factorial validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> COPE in sport settings and also showed <strong>the</strong> superiority <strong>of</strong> a 14-factormodel (i.e. aggregation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two social support scales) over a 15-factor model (Eklund, Grove, &Heard, 1998).Recognizing <strong>the</strong>se psychometric qualities, Crocker and Graham (1995) have developed a sportversion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> COPE (Carver et al., 1989). The Modified-COPE (MCOPE) is comprised <strong>of</strong> ninescales from <strong>the</strong> original COPE (i.e. active coping, planning, seeking instrumental social support,seeking emotional social support, suppression <strong>of</strong> competing activities, denial, humor, venting <strong>of</strong>emotion, and behavioral disengagement) and three scales from <strong>the</strong> sport versions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQ(i.e. wishful thinking, self-blame, and increased ef<strong>for</strong>t). Results <strong>of</strong> both exploratory (Ntoumaniset al., 1999) and confirmatory factor analyses (Eklund et al., 1998) provided reasonable support<strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> factorial structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE.Athletic coping skills inventory-28Accounting <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> necessity <strong>of</strong> developing reliable and valid sport-specific <strong>questionnaire</strong>s <strong>for</strong><strong>the</strong> <strong>assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> basic psychological skills, <strong>the</strong> Athletic Coping Skills Inventory-28 (ACSI-28;Smith, Schutz, Smoll, & Ptacek, 1995) was created. Results <strong>of</strong> a principal components analysisbased on responses from 637 athletes on an 87-item instrument yielded an eighth-factor model(Smith, Smoll, & Ptacek, 1990). However, a confirmatory factor analysis failed to support thismodel and revealed <strong>the</strong> superiority <strong>of</strong> a 28-item seven-factor model (Smith et al., 1995). Evidenceprovided support <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> test–retest reliability and <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> concurrent, convergent (Smith et al.,


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3451995), and differential validity (Smith & Christensen, 1995) <strong>of</strong> this measure. Despite its psychometricqualities and its sport specificity, several issues threaten <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ASCI-28. Firstly,its development was based nei<strong>the</strong>r on a psychological skills training <strong>the</strong>ory (Murphy & Tammen,1998) nor on <strong>the</strong> assumptions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> coping paradigm (Crocker et al., 1998). Consequently, <strong>the</strong>conceptual clarity <strong>of</strong> several subscales could be improved as <strong>the</strong> heterogeneity among <strong>the</strong>ir itemsmakes it difficult to ascertain that it represents a particular coping skill (Murphy & Tammen,1998). Secondly, several items seem to measure coping efficiency ra<strong>the</strong>r than coping utilization.Confounding <strong>the</strong> utilization <strong>of</strong> coping with its efficiency into a single instrument might enhanceartificially its statistical association with positive outcomes (Coyne & Gottlieb, 1996; Oakland &Ostell, 1996) and might lead to <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> unreliable guidelines <strong>for</strong> per<strong>for</strong>mance enhancementinterventions. As such, <strong>the</strong> ASCI-28 overlooks <strong>the</strong> assumption that coping and its potentialoutcomes should be measured independently (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985). Thirdly, severalresearchers (Hardy et al., 1996; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) have outlined <strong>the</strong> need <strong>for</strong> assessingcoping as a contextual-dependent response. Un<strong>for</strong>tunately, <strong>the</strong> trait-like orientation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ASCI-28 prevents <strong>the</strong> <strong>assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> athletes’ coping actions during specific stressful situations andmight inhibit <strong>the</strong> investigation <strong>of</strong> coping consistency across situations and across <strong>the</strong> phases <strong>of</strong> agiven situation.Conceptual limitationsDespite <strong>the</strong>ir respective psychometric strengths and weaknesses, several coping measures usedin sport settings share a common limitation in that <strong>the</strong>y were developed based on <strong>questionnaire</strong>screated to address problems and issues in general psychology. Because coping actions mightchange across situations (Frydenberg & Lewis, 1994; Sellers, 1995), <strong>the</strong> content relevance <strong>of</strong><strong>questionnaire</strong>s designed primarily <strong>for</strong> use in community, clinical, or healthcare settings is debatable.As such, some important facets <strong>of</strong> athletes’ coping might have been omitted in <strong>the</strong>se <strong>questionnaire</strong>swhereas coping actions outside <strong>the</strong> sport-specific domains <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> construct might havebeen included. The inclusion <strong>of</strong> contextually irrelevant coping actions has <strong>the</strong> potential to rendersome items inapplicable and to create psychometrical problems such as low internal consistencyand factorial instability (Ben-Porath et al., 1991). In addition, all coping instruments used in sportsettings were developed in <strong>the</strong> early 1990s. Because numerous in-depth descriptions <strong>of</strong> athletes’coping actions have been provided during <strong>the</strong> last decade, content relevance and representativeness<strong>of</strong> <strong>questionnaire</strong>s could be enhanced. Considering that <strong>the</strong> content validation <strong>of</strong> psychologicalinstruments is a dynamic process, coping instruments should integrate <strong>the</strong> relevant data that haveemerged during <strong>the</strong> last decade.Scale developmentBorrowing from <strong>the</strong> sport-specific qualitative literature <strong>of</strong> coping, <strong>the</strong> main goal <strong>of</strong> this researchwas to create a self-report <strong>questionnaire</strong> with both <strong>the</strong>oretical and conceptual relevance in sportsettings. In accordance with previous research in this area (Crocker et al., 1998; Hardy et al.,1996), it was decided to rely upon Lazarus and Folkman’s (1984) assumptions and to definecoping as <strong>the</strong> behavioral and cognitive actions <strong>of</strong> an individual to manage <strong>the</strong> internal and externaldemands <strong>of</strong> a specific situation. Based on substantial empirical evidence (Carver, Pozo, Harris,


6 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34Noriega, Schier, Robinson, M<strong>of</strong>fat, & Clark, 1993; Carver & Scheier, 1994; Frydenberg & Lewis,1994; Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Gaudreau et al., in press; Sellers, 1995; Stewart & Schwarzer,1996), it was decided to define coping as contextually dependent responses that change acrossstressful situations and across <strong>the</strong> distinctive phases <strong>of</strong> a stressful situation. From this decisionarose <strong>the</strong> necessity <strong>of</strong> assessing coping during a specific stressful sport situation. Because competitivesport events have <strong>the</strong> potential to render stress, it was decided to measure <strong>the</strong> numerouscoping actions used by athletes during <strong>the</strong> pre-competitive, competitive, and post-competitivephases <strong>of</strong> competitive sport events.In order to ensure <strong>the</strong> sport-specificity and <strong>the</strong> content validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> measure, a two-stepprocedure was followed. Firstly, a broad review <strong>of</strong> mainstream and sport psychology copingliterature was conducted in order to cover <strong>the</strong> relevant domain <strong>of</strong> coping actions that could beused by athletes in competitive events. Secondly, 17 qualitative sport psychology studies wereexamined. The published research pertaining specifically to coping was reviewed (Eklund, 1996;Gould, Eklund, & Jackson, 1992a, 1992b, 1993; Gould, Finch, & Jackson, 1993; Prapavessis &Grove, 1995; Wingate, 1993). Also, o<strong>the</strong>r studies investigating factors associated with burnout(Gould, Tuffey, Udry, & Loehr, 1995), facilitative anxiety (Hanton & Jones, 1999), peak per<strong>for</strong>mance(Beauchamp, 1995; Dagrou, Gauvin, & Halliwell, 1991; George, 1988; McCaffrey & Orlick,1989; Orlick & Partington, 1988; Stru<strong>the</strong>rs, 1990), and flow experience (Jackson, 1992, 1995)were reviewed.The breadth <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> review <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> literature maximized <strong>the</strong> content broadness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> measureand outlined <strong>the</strong> similarities between coping and <strong>the</strong> basic psychological skills construct. As withcoping, basic psychological skills have been conceived as a process encompassing specific actionsdirected toward <strong>the</strong> self-regulation <strong>of</strong> behavior, emotion, and cognition (Murphy & Tammen,1998). Although some sport scientists distinguished coping strategies from basic psychologicalskills (Crocker et al., 1998; Hardy et al., 1996; Murphy & Tammen, 1998), constructs that weredefined previously as basic psychological skills (i.e. imagery, relaxation, thought control, positiveself-talk, search <strong>for</strong> improvement, and emotional control) have emerged as coping actions inqualitative research (Gould et al., 1993a). Clearly, <strong>the</strong>se constructs should be included within asport-specific coping instrument as <strong>the</strong>y are frequently employed by athletes to manage <strong>the</strong> stressfuldemands encountered during competitive sport events.Also, <strong>the</strong> broadness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> review was useful in delineating <strong>the</strong> conceptual boundaries <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>measure. Several constructs (e.g. achievement motivation, self-esteem, self-awareness, confidence,self-efficacy, coachability, and concentration) that were defined previously as basic psychologicalskills (Murphy & Tammen, 1998) were excluded from our measure <strong>of</strong> coping. As Thomas, Murphy,and Hardy (1999) noted, <strong>the</strong>se constructs represent traits and states ra<strong>the</strong>r than skills orspecific set <strong>of</strong> actions that individuals can employ under specific situations. Moreover, systemicmodels <strong>of</strong> coping developed in mainstream (Lazarus, 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Moos,1993) and sport psychology (Cerin et al., 2000; Finch, 1994; Hardy et al., 1996) have consideredpersonality traits as well as cognitive, motivational, and affective states as determinants <strong>of</strong> copingbehaviors. Thus, constructs such as self-efficacy (Haney & Long, 1995), self-esteem, confidence(Grove & Heard, 1997), and achievement motivation (Mantzicopoulos, 1997) were seen as potentialdeterminants <strong>of</strong> coping actions and not as coping per se.Based on <strong>the</strong> above rationale, a <strong>questionnaire</strong> encompassing several strategies that fitted <strong>the</strong>parameters <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> coping construct was developed. Using a <strong>the</strong>oretically based approach to scale


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–347development (Clark & Watson, 1995; DeVellis, 1991), 10 sport-relevant, homogeneous, and conceptuallydistinct coping strategies applicable be<strong>for</strong>e, during, and after sport competitions wereidentified and <strong>for</strong>med <strong>the</strong> core <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>questionnaire</strong>. These coping strategies were selected torepresent two functional higher-order dimensions <strong>of</strong> coping: TOC and EOC. The 10 coping strategiesand <strong>the</strong>ir respective definitions are presented in Table 1. We will refer to this <strong>questionnaire</strong>as <strong>the</strong> Inventaire des Stratégies de Coping en Compétition Sportive (ISCCS; Coping Strategiesin Sport Competition Inventory).Goals and hypo<strong>the</strong>sesThis paper had four goals. The first goal was to develop <strong>the</strong> ISCCS, to refine it, and to test itsstructural validity with a four-stage sequential confirmatory factor analysis (Jöreskog, 1993). Thesecond goal was to examine <strong>the</strong> convergent validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS by assessing whe<strong>the</strong>r its subscalescorrelated meaningfully with o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>questionnaire</strong>s measuring similar coping constructs(Kaplan & Saccuzzo, 1993; Kline, 1998). As such, it was hypo<strong>the</strong>sized that <strong>the</strong> TOC subscalesTable 1Definition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 10 coping subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCSSubscalesTOC subscalesThought controlMental imageryRelaxationEf<strong>for</strong>t expenditureLogical analysisSeeking supportEOC subscalesVenting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotionMental distractionDisengagement/resignationSocial withdrawalDefinitionCognitive actions used in order to restructure one’s cognitions byemphasizing positive aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> self and <strong>of</strong> past, actual, and futurestressful sport situationsCognitive actions used in order to rehearse or to practice mentally <strong>the</strong>tactical, <strong>the</strong> technical, and <strong>the</strong> mental aspects associated with past, actual,and future stressful sport situationsBehavioral actions used in order to reduce one’s level <strong>of</strong> physiological,muscular, and mental tensionBehavioral actions used in order to mobilize its physical and mentalresources in order to act directly on stressful situations encountered in sportsettingsCognitive actions used in order to identify and/or assess <strong>the</strong> internal andexternal factors associated with past, actual, and future stressful situationsencountered in sport settingsBehavioral actions used in order to obtain advice, feedback, emotionalsupport, and instrumental supportBehavioral actions used in order to express and ventilate unpleasantemotional tensions experienced in stressful sport situationsCognitive and behavioral actions used in order to focus voluntarily onthings that are irrelevant or unrelated to sport per<strong>for</strong>manceCognitive and behavioral actions used in order to avoid doing <strong>the</strong> actionsthat are necessary to <strong>the</strong> attainment <strong>of</strong> one’s per<strong>for</strong>mance goalBehavioral actions used in order to reduce or eliminate social relationshipsmomentarilyNote: TOC=task-oriented coping, EOC=emotion-oriented coping.


8 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS would exhibit a positive association with those <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE and <strong>the</strong> WOCQ. Also,it was hypo<strong>the</strong>sized that <strong>the</strong> EOC subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS would exhibit a positive association withthose <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE and <strong>the</strong> WOCQ. The third goal was to examine <strong>the</strong> concurrent validity <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> ISCCS by assessing its relationships with external criteria administrated at <strong>the</strong> same point intime (Kaplan & Saccuzzo, 1993; Kline, 1998). Based on systemic <strong>the</strong>ories <strong>of</strong> coping (Lazarus &Folkman, 1984; Hardy et al., 1996) and on mainstream and sport psychology literature, it washypo<strong>the</strong>sized that <strong>the</strong> TOC subscales would correlate positively with perceived goal attainment(Gaudreau et al., 2001), perceived relevance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> competition (Lazarus, 1991), perceived control(Haney & Long, 1995), positive affect (Crocker & Graham, 1995), and somatic anxiety(Ntoumanis & Biddle, 2000). Also, it was expected that <strong>the</strong> EOC subscales would correlate positivelywith negative affect (Crocker & Graham, 1995) and cognitive anxiety (Ntoumanis & Biddle,2000). The fourth goal was to test <strong>the</strong> differential validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS by showing whe<strong>the</strong>rathletes’ use <strong>of</strong> coping strategies differed as function <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir expertise level (Kaplan & Saccuzzo,1993). Based on <strong>the</strong> basic psychological skills literature (Thomas et al., 1999), it was hypo<strong>the</strong>sizedthat international and national athletes would use more TOC and less EOC than <strong>the</strong>ir provincialand regional counterparts.MethodCreation <strong>of</strong> itemsUsing <strong>the</strong> raw data <strong>the</strong>mes and quotes from qualitative studies along with <strong>the</strong> items from quantitativemeasures <strong>of</strong> coping and basic psychological skills, a corpus <strong>of</strong> sport-relevant items wasdeveloped <strong>for</strong> 10 coping strategies. Guidelines <strong>for</strong> items wording (Clark & Watson, 1995) wereclosely followed to maximize <strong>the</strong>ir clarity, <strong>the</strong>ir specificity, and <strong>the</strong>ir shortness. Each item waswritten so that athletes <strong>of</strong> 14 years <strong>of</strong> age and older would understand it. Because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>rutilization <strong>of</strong> this measure in North American and European French-speaking athletic populations,colloquialisms were avoided. Also, expressions pertaining to a specific sport were avoided tobroaden <strong>the</strong> applicability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>questionnaire</strong> across sports. Finally, items were worded so as tobe applicable <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> coping strategies used be<strong>for</strong>e, during, and after competitions.Assessment <strong>of</strong> items’ clarity, applicability, and content relevanceAn initial pool <strong>of</strong> 111 items was administrated to five female and five male athletes from 14to 21 years <strong>of</strong> age (M=16.8; SD=2.5). Using a five-point Likert-type scale (1=totally unclear;5=totally clear), participants were instructed to rate <strong>the</strong> clarity <strong>of</strong> each item and to give commentsalong with alternative <strong>for</strong>mulations <strong>for</strong> items that were not totally clear. Also, items were presentedto 10 coaches working in one <strong>of</strong> five individual sports (i.e. golf, tennis, badminton, alpine skiing,and figure skating) or five team sports (i.e. hockey, baseball, volleyball, basketball, and soccer).Using a dichotomous scale (applicable versus inapplicable), coaches were instructed to assess<strong>the</strong> applicability <strong>of</strong> each item in <strong>the</strong>ir respective sport at each phase <strong>of</strong> a competition (i.e. precompetition,competition, and post-competition). Accounting on <strong>the</strong> ratings provided by athletes


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–349and coaches, and on <strong>the</strong>ir numerous comments, several items were rewritten in order to improve<strong>the</strong>ir clarity and to broaden <strong>the</strong>ir applicability across sports and phases <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> competition.The resulting bank <strong>of</strong> items was reviewed by two graduate students and by one senior researcherinvolved in previous coping investigations. These experts were asked to assess <strong>the</strong> content relevanceand <strong>the</strong> homogeneity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 10 scales. Using a written definition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 10 coping strategies(see Table 1), experts were advised to place each item in one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 10 scales. Also, <strong>the</strong>y wereinstructed to insert irrelevant items in a category labeled ‘irrelevant’ and to include ambiguousitems in more than one category. Sixteen items judged as irrelevant or as fitting within more thanone coping strategy by at least two experts were deleted from <strong>the</strong> bank <strong>of</strong> items.Main researchParticipantsThree hundred and sixteen French–canadian athletes (54% male, 46% female) from 14 to 28years <strong>of</strong> age (M=17.4; SD=2.15) participated in this study. These athletes were competing ininternational (17%), national (20%), provincial (28%), and regional (35%) events at <strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> study. As a group, participants trained 12 hours weekly (SD=8.15) <strong>for</strong> a period <strong>of</strong> 41 weeksannually (SD=10.9). Their competitive experience ranged from 1 to 18 years (M=6.58; SD=3.66).Participants represented individual sports such as golf (13.4%), badminton (5%), figure skating(14%), gymnastic (2.5%), alpine skiing (4%), swimming (10.4%) and team sports such as icehockey(14.5%), deck-hockey (1.5%), baseball (11.7%), basketball (17.1%), and volleyball(5.9%).MeasuresData were collected using a pen-and-paper self-report <strong>questionnaire</strong> divided into four sections.The first section included general background in<strong>for</strong>mation. Also, it included three items assessingperceived goal attainment (e.g. my per<strong>for</strong>mance was better than my expectations) and five itemsassessing perceived relevance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> competition (e.g. this competition was very important to me).All items were rated, respectively, on a Likert-type scale <strong>of</strong> seven or five points. Both scalesexhibited good internal consistency (a0.80; Kline, 1998) and factorial validity. In addition, <strong>the</strong>first section contained an ad hoc French translation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> control scale from <strong>the</strong> FlowState Scale (Jackson & Marsh, 1996). This scale contained four items rated on a five-point Likerttypescale (1=strongly disagree; 5=strongly agree) and <strong>for</strong>med a single factor with good internalconsistency (a0.80; Kline, 1998).The second section contained <strong>the</strong> ISCCS <strong>questionnaire</strong>. The ISCCS comprised 95 items ratedon a five-point Likert-type scale (1=not used at all; 5=used very much). The third section wascomprised <strong>of</strong> variables that should correlate with <strong>the</strong> scales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS. Fifty percent <strong>of</strong> participantscompleted a French–Canadian translation (Gaudreau, 2000) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Positive and NegativeAffective Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), while o<strong>the</strong>r participants completeda French translation (Debois & Fleurance, 1998) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2(CSAI-2; Martens, Burton, Vealey, Bump, & Smith, 1990). The PANAS, a two-scale adjectivechecklist, assesses 10 positive and 10 negative affects on a five-point Likert-type scale (1=notat all; 5=very much). The PANAS has been translated using a double back-translation procedure.Preliminary data indicated that <strong>the</strong> items distribution, inter-scales correlation, internal consistency,


10 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34and factorial structure were similar to those from <strong>the</strong> original PANAS (Gaudreau, 2000). TheCSAI-2, a multidimensional measure <strong>of</strong> competitive state anxiety, includes three scales <strong>of</strong> nineitems measuring cognitive state-anxiety, somatic state-anxiety, and state-confidence on a fourpointLikert-type scale (1=not at all; 4=very much so). The CSAI-2 was translated using a backtranslationprocedure. Results <strong>of</strong> a principal components analysis conducted with 68 French gymnastscorroborated <strong>the</strong> three-factor solution <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> original CSAI-2 (Debois & Fleurance, 1998).As in <strong>the</strong> third section, <strong>the</strong> fourth section assessed variables that should correlate with <strong>the</strong> scales<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS. Participants completed one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> following <strong>questionnaire</strong>s: a group <strong>of</strong> eight selectedsubscales from <strong>the</strong> French–Canadian translation (Gaudreau et al., in press) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE(Crocker & Graham, 1995), <strong>the</strong> French–Canadian translation (Bouchard, Sabourin, Lussier,Wright, & Richer, 1997) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQ (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985), or <strong>the</strong> social desirabilityscale from <strong>the</strong> Questionnaire de Personnalité des Sportifs (QPS; Thill, 1979). The MCOPE hasbeen translated by three researchers using a committee procedure. The eight selected subscales<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE (i.e. seeking instrumental social support, behavioral disengagement, suppression,venting <strong>of</strong> emotion, increased ef<strong>for</strong>t, positive reappraisal, planning, and mental disengagement)contained four items assessed on a five-point Likert-type scale (1=not used; 5=used very much).All subscales exhibited acceptable internal consistency in previous research (Gaudreau et al.,2001, in press), as well as in <strong>the</strong> present study (see Table 6). The French–Canadian WOCQwas comprised <strong>of</strong> 35 items representing four coping strategies: distancing/avoidance,confrontation/seeking social support, problem-focused coping, and denial. Each item was ratedon a four-point Likert-type scale (0=not used at all; 3=used a great deal). A confirmatory factoranalysis showed <strong>the</strong> reasonable goodness-<strong>of</strong>-fit <strong>for</strong> a four-factor model (e.g. GFI=0.92; CFI=0.89;RMSEA=0.07) with a sample <strong>of</strong> 1014 French–Canadian community participants (Bouchard et al.,1997). Finally, <strong>the</strong> social desirability scale from <strong>the</strong> QPS contained 16 items rated on a dichotomousscale (true versus false). The scale <strong>for</strong>med a single scale with good internal consistencyin a previous research conducted with French athletes (Thill & Brenot, 1982).ProcedureThirty coaches were contacted by <strong>the</strong> first author who provided <strong>the</strong>m with an explanation <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> purposes and procedures <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> research project. Twenty-seven coaches authorized us to meet<strong>the</strong>ir athletes under <strong>the</strong>ir supervision at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> a training session. During <strong>the</strong>se meetings,athletes were asked if <strong>the</strong>y would volunteer to participate in <strong>the</strong> study. They were told <strong>the</strong> exactpurpose <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> study and <strong>the</strong>y were made aware that <strong>the</strong> <strong>questionnaire</strong> needed to be completedwithin six hours after <strong>the</strong> completion <strong>of</strong> a competitive event. Also, <strong>the</strong>y were told that <strong>the</strong> <strong>questionnaire</strong>would take 40–60 min to complete. A strong emphasis was put on confidentiality <strong>of</strong> dataand athletes were instructed not to write <strong>the</strong>ir names on <strong>the</strong> <strong>questionnaire</strong> and to put it in anunmarked envelope be<strong>for</strong>e returning it to <strong>the</strong>ir coach. Also, <strong>the</strong> first author <strong>of</strong> this paper met <strong>the</strong>participants and <strong>the</strong>ir coaches at <strong>the</strong> practice site during <strong>the</strong> week following <strong>the</strong> competition inorder to debrief <strong>the</strong>m and to recuperate <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>questionnaire</strong>.Following <strong>the</strong> completion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> competition, coaches gave a <strong>questionnaire</strong>, a pen, and anenvelope to <strong>the</strong> athletes. Participants were instructed to be sincere, serious, and to complete <strong>the</strong>ir<strong>questionnaire</strong> individually in a quiet environment within six hours after <strong>the</strong> competition. Whencompleting <strong>the</strong> ISCCS, <strong>the</strong> selected scales from <strong>the</strong> MCOPE, and <strong>the</strong> WOCQ, participants wereinstructed to indicate to which extent <strong>the</strong> items represented <strong>the</strong> things that <strong>the</strong>y had done or


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3411thought during <strong>the</strong> competition <strong>the</strong>y had just completed. As <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> control scale, <strong>the</strong>PANAS, and <strong>the</strong> CSAI-2, participants were instructed to indicate to which extent each item representedhow <strong>the</strong>y had felt during <strong>the</strong> competition.Analytical strategy and goodness-<strong>of</strong>-fit criteriaBecause <strong>the</strong> ISCCS was developed based on <strong>the</strong>oretical grounds, specific hypo<strong>the</strong>ses regardingwhich items should load significantly on which scale were suitable <strong>for</strong> confirmatory factor analysis.However, <strong>the</strong> weak variables-to-participants (Kline, 1998) and parameters-to-participantsratios (Tanaka, 1987) as well as <strong>the</strong> model generating purposes <strong>of</strong> this study hindered <strong>the</strong> accurateestimation <strong>of</strong> a full-factorial model. Based on recent research conducted in sport (Jackson &Marsh, 1996; Markland & Ingledew, 1997; Mullan, Markland, & Ingledew, 1997) and mainstreampsychology (Zautra et al., 1997), <strong>the</strong> factorial structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS was tested with a four-stagesequential confirmatory factor analysis (Jöreskog, 1993).In <strong>the</strong> first stage, each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 10 proposed subscale was tested separately in order to eliminateitems that were poor indicators <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir underlying latent construct. Items with a low standardizedfactor loading (0.40) or a large standardized residuals (±1.5) were deleted. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, anitem was deleted if a large modification index suggested that its residual could correlate withthose <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r items. In <strong>the</strong> second stage, each subscale was paired with every o<strong>the</strong>r subscale inorder to eliminate items that could load significantly on a non-intended latent construct. A total<strong>of</strong> 45 models were tested at this stage. Latent constructs were free to correlate in all models.Alike <strong>the</strong> first stage, items with low standardized factor loadings and with large standardizedresiduals were eliminated, along with items <strong>for</strong> which large modification indices suggested that<strong>the</strong>y could load on an non-intended factor. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, an item was deleted if a large modificationindex suggested that its residual could correlate with those <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r items. In <strong>the</strong> third stage, <strong>the</strong>structural validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS was tested by grouping <strong>the</strong> subscales within <strong>the</strong>ir respectivefunctional dimension <strong>of</strong> coping. Two models were tested. Firstly, all TOC subscales (see Table1) were combined into a six-factor model. Secondly, all EOC subscales were combined into afour-factor model (see Table 1). Latent constructs were free to correlate in both models. Alike<strong>the</strong> second stage, residuals were not allowed to correlate with those <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r items. Finally, <strong>the</strong>fourth stage tested <strong>the</strong> full-factorial ISCCS model. Because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> heterogeneity <strong>of</strong> copingtypologies (Parker & Endler, 1992), it was relevant to assess whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> coping items couldsubsume into alternative models obtained in previous coping research. As suggested by <strong>the</strong> classicaldistinction between TOC and EOC (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), it was decided to test <strong>the</strong>tenability <strong>of</strong> a two-factor model (i.e. all TOC items constrained on <strong>the</strong> first factor and all EOCitems constrained on <strong>the</strong> second factor). Also, moving by recent claims concerning <strong>the</strong> existence<strong>of</strong> a third functional dimension <strong>of</strong> coping (i.e. avoidance-oriented coping), a three-factor modelwas tested (i.e all TOC items constrained on <strong>the</strong> first factor; mental distraction,disengagement/resignation, and social withdrawal items constrained on <strong>the</strong> second factor; venting<strong>of</strong> emotion items constrained on <strong>the</strong> third factor). In assessing all models, latent constructs werefree to correlate and it was hypo<strong>the</strong>sized that <strong>the</strong> 10-factor model would fit <strong>the</strong> data better thanany <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> alternative models.In order to assess whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> specified models matched <strong>the</strong> observed data, several fit indiceswere employed. Based on Hu and Bentler’s (1995) recommendations, model fit was assessedusing <strong>the</strong> chi-square statistic, <strong>the</strong> confirmatory fit index (CFI), <strong>the</strong> Tucker Lewis fit index (TLI),


12 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34and <strong>the</strong> root mean square error <strong>of</strong> approximation (RMSEA). The CFI and <strong>the</strong> TLI values rangefrom 0 to 1 with values <strong>of</strong> 0.90 or greater showing acceptable fit <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model (e.g. Hu & Bentler,1995; Jöreskog, 1993) and values <strong>of</strong> 0.95 or greater showing good fit <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model (Hu & Bentler,1999). For RMSEA, values <strong>of</strong> 0.08 or less indicate a reasonable fit <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> data whereas values<strong>of</strong> 0.05 or less indicate a good fit <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> data (Browne & Cudeck, 1993).ResultsResponse rateFour hundred and fifteen <strong>questionnaire</strong>s were distributed to athletes during <strong>the</strong> course <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>experimentation. Of <strong>the</strong>se <strong>questionnaire</strong>s, 316 (76%) were returned to <strong>the</strong> researchers. Six participantswere excluded from fur<strong>the</strong>r analyses because <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>questionnaire</strong>s had been completedimproperly (i.e. <strong>the</strong> same score on an entire page and/or systematic response pattern). Also, fourparticipants were excluded from fur<strong>the</strong>r analyses because <strong>the</strong>ir multivariate response pattern on<strong>the</strong> ISCCS differed significantly from <strong>the</strong> rest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sample (Mahalanobis distance p0.001; seeTabachnick & Fidell, 1996). Fur<strong>the</strong>r analyses were conducted on a sample <strong>of</strong> 306 athletes.Distribution <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS itemsThe univariate skewness values <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS items ranged from 0.91 to 3.58 and <strong>the</strong>ir univariatekurtosis values ranged from 1.27 to 13.61, thus suggesting that some items deviated severelyfrom a normal distribution. Consequently, seven items with a skewness greater than ±3 and akurtosis greater than ±8 were deleted from fur<strong>the</strong>r analyses (Chou & Bentler, 1995; West, Finch, &Curran, 1995). As <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> remaining 88 items, multivariate normality <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> data was assessedusing Mardia’s coefficient <strong>of</strong> kurtosis. Results revealed that <strong>the</strong> multivariate joint distribution <strong>of</strong>kurtosis was significant (Mardia’s kurtosis=277.01). To counter violations <strong>of</strong> multivariate normality,Schumacker and Lomax (1996) recommended <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> asymptotic distribution free estimationprocedures. However, <strong>the</strong>se estimation procedures require a sample size <strong>of</strong> at least threetimes larger than <strong>the</strong> one available <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> current study (Hu, Bentler, & Kano, 1992). Thus, itwas decided to use a maximum likelihood estimation procedure and to adjust <strong>the</strong> p-value <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>chi-square statistic with <strong>the</strong> bootstrapping procedure <strong>of</strong> Bollen and Stine (1993) provided in <strong>the</strong>amos 4.0 s<strong>of</strong>tware (Arbuckle, 1999). Results <strong>of</strong> recent simulation studies (Fouladi, 1997; Nevitt &Hancock, 1997) with non-normal data lent support to this decision by revealing that <strong>the</strong> Bollen–Stine corrected chi-square (BS chi-square) per<strong>for</strong>med better than <strong>the</strong> Satorra–Bentler rescaled chisquarestatistic (Bentler, 1995) with small samples.Four-stage sequential confirmatory factor analysisStage 1: single-subscale analysisBased on <strong>the</strong> examination <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> standardized factor loadings, standardized residuals, and modificationindices, six items were deleted from <strong>the</strong> thought control subscale, two from mental imagery,four from relaxation, three from seeking support, five from logical analysis, four from ef<strong>for</strong>t


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3413expenditure, two from social withdrawal, five from venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion, five frommental distraction, and two from disengagement/resignation. Following <strong>the</strong> deletion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se items,nine one-factor models exhibited a non-significant BS chi-square as well as CFI, and TLI greaterthan 0.95, and RMSEA below 0.05. However, <strong>the</strong> relaxation model was still significant and <strong>the</strong>modification indices suggested that allowing a correlation between <strong>the</strong> residual <strong>of</strong> item 22 anditem 36 might improve <strong>the</strong> model significantly. Because <strong>of</strong> our desire to keep at least four itemsper subscale to ensure identification <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model upon subsequent deletions <strong>of</strong> items, it wasdecided to allow those residuals to correlate freely. Freeing up this constraint, <strong>the</strong> one-factorrelaxation model became non-significant and all indices suggested its good fit. Results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>initial and final models are presented in Table 2.Stage 2: scale-pairing analysisThe second stage involved <strong>the</strong> pairing <strong>of</strong> each subscale with every o<strong>the</strong>r subscale in order toeliminate items that could load significantly on an non-intended latent construct. A total <strong>of</strong> 45two-factor models were tested at this stage. In summary, results indicated that <strong>the</strong> fit <strong>of</strong> 30 modelswas satisfactory whereas <strong>the</strong> fit <strong>of</strong> 15 models could be improved upon. Based on <strong>the</strong> standardizedfactor loadings, <strong>the</strong> standardized residuals, and <strong>the</strong> modification indices, two items were deletedfrom <strong>the</strong> thought control subscale, two from ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure, two from mental imagery, onefrom logical analysis, two from disengagement/resignation, one from social withdrawal, and onefrom venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion. Following deletion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se items, 44 models exhibited anon-significant BS chi-square as well as CFI, and TLI greater than 0.95, and RMSEA below 0.05.However, <strong>the</strong> relaxation-ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure model remained significant (BS c 2 (12) =13.50,p0.05) and modification indices suggested that allowing <strong>the</strong> residuals <strong>of</strong> item 11 (i.e. relaxation)and item 92 (i.e. ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure) to correlate freely would improve <strong>the</strong> model significantly.Because <strong>the</strong> ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure consisted <strong>of</strong> only three items, it was decided to allow <strong>the</strong>seresiduals to correlate freely, as a two-item ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure subscale led to a non-identifiedmodel. Freeing up this constraint, <strong>the</strong> model became non-significant (BS c 2 (11) =12.56, p0.05)with all indices suggesting its good fit.Stage 3: functional dimensions <strong>of</strong> coping modelsAt this stage, it was decided to group <strong>the</strong> coping subscales that represent ei<strong>the</strong>r TOC (i.e.thought control, ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure, logical analysis, seeking support, relaxation, and mentalimagery) or EOC (i.e. mental distraction, disengagement/resignation, venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion,and social withdrawal) into distinctive models that were tested separately. Firstly, a 23-itemsix-factor TOC model was tested. Secondly, a 16-item four-factor EOC model was tested. Bothmodels fitted <strong>the</strong> data well as exhibited by <strong>the</strong>ir non-significant BS chi-square, <strong>the</strong>ir CFI, and TLIvalues greater than 0.95 and <strong>the</strong>ir RMSEA below 0.05.Stage 4: full-factorial modelThe fourth stage was aimed at testing <strong>the</strong> fit <strong>of</strong> a 39-item 10-factor model and comparing itsfit to alternative two- and three-factor models. Although <strong>the</strong> BS chi-square <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 10-factor modelwas significant, acceptable fit indices were obtained (CFI and TLI0.90; RMSEA0.05) (Tables3 and 4). Modification indices suggested <strong>the</strong> possibility <strong>of</strong> improving <strong>the</strong> model by allowing someitems’ residuals to correlate freely. Taking into consideration <strong>the</strong> conceptual distinction between


14 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34Table 2Fit indices <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> one-factor models be<strong>for</strong>e (initial) and following (final) <strong>the</strong> deletion <strong>of</strong> itemsCoping subscales c 2 d.f. p BS c 2 d.f. p CFI TLI RMSEAThought controlInitial model (12) 135.76 54 0.000* 62.29 54 0.001* 0.910 0.890 0.070Final model (6) 21.97 13 0.056 10.74 13 0.088 0.985 0.976 0.048Mental imageryInitial model (8) 60.74 20 0.000* 24.24 20 0.001* 0.947 0.926 0.082Final model (6) 10.96 9 0.279 11.29 9 0.459 0.996 0.993 0.027RelaxationInitial model (8) 90.57 20 0.000* 24.34 20 0.000* 0.888 0.843 0.108Final model 1 (4) 15.76 2 0.000* 3.16 2 0.006* 0.965 0.895 0.150Final model 2 (4) 1.01 1 0.314 1.31 1 0.380 1.00 1.00 0.007Ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditureInitial model (9) 105.04 27 0.000* 34.47 27 0.000* 0.929 0.905 0.097Final model (5) 6.86 5 0.231 6.79 5 0.405 0.997 0.994 0.035Logical analysisInitial model (10) 188.63 35 0.000* 40.62 35 0.000* 0.768 0.701 0.120Final model (5) 2.87 5 0.719 5.60 5 0.750 1.00 1.00 0.000Seeking supportInitial model (7) 45.70 14 0.000* 16.93 14 0.001* 0.930 0.895 0.086Final model (4) 0.27 2 0.871 2.18 2 0.884 1.00 1.00 0.000Venting <strong>of</strong> emotionInitial model (10) 170.39 35 0.000* 54.72 35 0.000* 0.872 0.835 0.113Final model (5) 1.85 5 0.872 6.68 5 0.911 1.00 1.00 0.000Mental distractionInitial model (9) 71.67 27 0.000* 37.87 27 0.009* 0.931 0.908 0.074Final model (4) 0.63 2 0.729 3.54 2 0.839 1.00 1.00 0.000DisengagementInitial model (8) 108.65 20 0.000 34.20 20 0.000* 0.854 0.796 0.121Final model (6) 11.70 9 0.231 15.42 9 0.631 0.993 0.988 0.031Social withdrawalInitial model (7) 51.69 14 0.000 19.80 14 0.004* 0.943 0.915 0.094Final model (5) 8.50 5 0.131 6.26 5 0.237 0.991 0.983 0.048Note: c 2 =chi-square statistic, BS c 2 =Bollen–Stine corrected chi-square statistic, CFI=Confirmatory fit index,TLI=Tucker Lewis index, RMSEA=Root mean square error <strong>of</strong> approximation. Numbers in <strong>the</strong> paren<strong>the</strong>ses represent<strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> item <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subscale. *p0.05.<strong>the</strong> TOC and EOC subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS, it was decided not to allow free correlations between<strong>the</strong>ir items’ residuals in order to protect against overfitting <strong>the</strong> model (Cliff, 1983). It is worthnoting that alternative models both provided poor fit to <strong>the</strong> data as suggested by <strong>the</strong>ir significantBS chi-square and <strong>the</strong>ir extremely low fit indices (CFI and TLI0.70; RMSEA0.07).


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3415Table 3Fit indices <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> submodels tested at <strong>the</strong> third stage and <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> full-factorial models tested at <strong>the</strong> fourth stage <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> analysisModels c 2 d.f. p BS c 2 d.f. p CFI TLI RMSEAStage 3Task-orientedSix-factor model 299.65 213 0.000* 240.61 213 0.056 0.958 0.957 0.037Emotion-orientedFour-factor model 134.58 98 0.008* 117.18 98 0.201 0.975 0.969 0.035Stage 4Ten-factor model 910.27 652 0.000* 748.15 652 0.030* 0.931 0.921 0.036Alternate modelsTwo-factor model 2250.39 695 0.000* 799.89 695 0.000* 0.583 0.556 0.086Three-factor model 1943.50 693 0.000* 795.39 696 0.000* 0.665 0.642 0.077Note: c 2 =chi-square statistic, BS c 2 =Bollen–Stine corrected chi-square statistic, CFI=Confirmatory fit index,TLI=Tucker Lewis index, RMSEA=Root mean square error <strong>of</strong> approximation. *p0.05.Means, standard deviations and internal consistencyBased on <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> four-stage sequential confirmatory factor analysis, means, standarddeviations, and Cronbach’s index <strong>of</strong> internal consistency were calculated <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> 10 subscales <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> ISCCS (see Table 5). All scores were summed unweighted and <strong>the</strong>y are presented as per itemscores (i.e. totals divided by <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> items) to facilitate comparison across variables. Results<strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure, mental imagery, relaxation, logical analysis, and thought control subscalesshowed that <strong>the</strong>se strategies were used more frequently than strategies such as seekingsupport, venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion, social withdrawal, disengagement/resignation, and mentaldistraction. Cronbach’s index <strong>of</strong> internal consistency ranged from 0.67 to 0.87, with eight subscalesshowing acceptable alpha coefficient (a0.70; Kline, 1998) and two subscales showingmoderately low alpha coefficient (0.60a0.70; Kline, 1998).Inter-scales correlationsInter-scales correlations and <strong>the</strong>ir standard errors <strong>of</strong> estimation were provided as part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>fourth stage <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sequential confirmatory factor analysis (see Table 5). Significant positive correlationswere observed between thought control, mental imagery, relaxation, ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure,seeking social support, and logical analysis. Also, significant positive correlations were observedbetween social withdrawal, disengagement/resignation, mental distraction, and venting <strong>of</strong>unpleasant emotion.Convergent validityBivariate Pearson correlations were used to assess <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses bearing on <strong>the</strong> convergentrelationship between <strong>the</strong> ISCCS, MCOPE, and WOCQ subscales. Levels <strong>of</strong> significance were


16 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34Table 4Items wording, standardized factor loadings (SL), and <strong>the</strong>ir standard errors (SE) at <strong>the</strong> fourth stage <strong>of</strong> model testingCoping items SL SEThought control (contrôle des pensées)J’ai essayé de ne pas me laisser intimider par les autres athlètes (19). 0.486 —I tried not to be intimidated by o<strong>the</strong>r athletes.J’ai tenté d’éliminer mes doutes en pensant à des choses positives (45) 0.691 0.168I tried to block out my doubts by thinking positively.J’ai remplacé mes pensées négatives par des pensées positives (58) 0.710 0.173I replaced my negative thoughts by positive ones.J’ai pensé à mes bons coups plutôt qu’à mes erreurs (89) 0.614 0.161I tried not to think about my mistakes.Mental imagery (imagerie mentale)J’ai visualisé que j’étais en plein contrôle de la situation (6) 0.653 —I visualized that I was in total control <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> situation.J’ai répété mentalement l’exécution de mes mouvements (35) 0.627 0.114I mentally rehearsed <strong>the</strong> execution <strong>of</strong> my movements.Je me suis imaginé en train de faire une bonne per<strong>for</strong>mance (42) 0.686 0.107I imagined that I was doing a good per<strong>for</strong>mance.J’ai visualisé ma meilleure per<strong>for</strong>mance à vie (86) 0.616 0.125I visualized my all-time best per<strong>for</strong>mance.Relaxation (relaxation)J’ai tenté de détendre mon corps (4) 0.698 —I tried to relax my body.J’ai essayé de réduire ma tension musculaire (11) 0.810 0.109I tried to reduce <strong>the</strong> tension in my muscles.J’ai fait des exercices de relaxation (22) 0.554 0.093I did some relaxation exercises.J’ai relaxé les muscles de mon corps (36) 0.733 0.101I relaxed my muscles.Ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure (déploiement des ef<strong>for</strong>ts)Je me suis appliqué en fournissant un ef<strong>for</strong>t constant (7) 0.671 —I applied myself by giving a consistent ef<strong>for</strong>t.J’ai fourni un ef<strong>for</strong>t acharné (50) 0.835 0.143I gave a relentless ef<strong>for</strong>tJ’ai fourni mon maximum d’ef<strong>for</strong>t (92) 0.734 0.119I gave my best ef<strong>for</strong>t.Logical analysis (analyse logique)J’ai analysé mes per<strong>for</strong>mances antérieures (48) 0.517 —I analyzed my past per<strong>for</strong>mances.J’ai pensé à des solutions possibles pour gérer la situation (66) 0.660 0.161I tried to find solutions in order to manage <strong>the</strong> situationJ’ai analysé les faiblesses de mes adversaires (74) 0.437 0.152I analyzed <strong>the</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> my opponents.(continued on next page)


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3417Table 4 (continued)Coping items SL SEJ’ai analysé les exigences de la compétition (82) 0.675 0.167I analyzed <strong>the</strong> demands <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> competition.Seeking support (recherche de soutien)J’ai demandé des conseils concernant ma préparation mentale (16) 0.535 —I asked someone <strong>for</strong> advice concerning my mental preparation.J’ai demandé conseil à d’autres athlètes (67) 0.628 0.190I asked o<strong>the</strong>r athletes <strong>for</strong> advice.Je me suis confié à une personne digne de confiance (78) 0.619 0.206I talked to a trustworthy person.J’ai parlé à une personne qui est capable de me motiver (91) 0.664 0.221I talked to someone who is able to motivate me.Venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion (ventilation des émotions déplaisantes)J’ai sacré (jurons, blasphèmes, etc) dans ma tête ou à haute voix pour passer ma colère 0.596 —(25)I used swear-words loudly or in my head in order to vent my anger.J’ai exprimé mon mécontentement (43) 0.779 0.110I expressed my discontentJe me suis fâché (64) 0.920 0.112I got angryJ’ai exprimé mes frustrations (73) 0.859 0.110I expressed my frustrations.Mental distraction (distraction mentale)Je me suis occupé l’esprit pour penser à autre chose que la compétition (46) 0.702 —I occupied my mind in order to think about o<strong>the</strong>r things than <strong>the</strong> competition.J’ai pensé à mes loisirs favoris pour ne pas penser à la compétition (59) 0.686 0.097I thought about my favorite leisure in order not to think about <strong>the</strong> competition.J’ai fait des choses divertissantes pour ne pas penser à la compétition (70) 0.636 0.109I entertained myself in order not to think about <strong>the</strong> competition.J’ai pensé à ma famille ou à mes amis pour me distraire (72) 0.642 0.110I thought about my family or about my friends to distract my mind.Disengagement/resignation (désengagement/résignation)Je me suis laisser-aller au découragement (10) 0.616 —I let myself feel hopeless and discouraged.J’ai souhaité que la compétition se termine immédiatement (39) 0.517 0.143I wished that <strong>the</strong> competition would end immediately.J’ai cessé de croire en ma capacité d’atteindre mon but (60) 0.523 0.123I stopped believing in my ability to reach my goal.J’ai perdu tout espoir de pouvoir atteindre mon but (93) 0.683 0.148I lost all hope <strong>of</strong> attaining my goal.Social withdrawal (isolement social)Je me suis éloigné des autres athlètes (3) 0.481 —I took my distance from o<strong>the</strong>r athletes.(continued on next page)


18 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34Table 4 (continued)Coping items SL SEJe me suis éloigné dans un endroit favorable à la réflexion (49) 0.683 0.248I retreated where it was easy to think.J’ai recherché le silence (81) 0.726 0.241I searched <strong>for</strong> calmness and quietness.J’ai fait le vide autour de moi (83) 0.578 0.225I kept all people at a distance.Note: English wording <strong>of</strong> items has not been subjected to a back-translation procedure and should not beinterpreted as valid <strong>for</strong> use in research. Number in <strong>the</strong> paren<strong>the</strong>ses represents <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> an item in <strong>the</strong><strong>questionnaire</strong>. Dashes indicate that <strong>the</strong> parameters were not estimated. All standardized factor loadings aresignificant at p0.05.corrected with a Bonferroni procedure (p=a/10=0.005) in order to prevent <strong>the</strong> occurrence <strong>of</strong> Type-I error (Stevens, 1996). Prior to <strong>the</strong>se analyses, means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s index<strong>of</strong> internal consistency were calculated <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQ and MCOPE subscales (see Table 6).Six MCOPE subscales exhibited good internal consistency (0.79a0.90; Kline, 1998) whereasbehavioral disengagement and suppression <strong>of</strong> competing activities exhibited a moderately lowinternal consistency (0.60a0.70; Kline, 1998). All subscales were retained <strong>for</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>r analyses.Concerning <strong>the</strong> WOCQ, <strong>the</strong> confrontation/seeking social support and problem-focused subscales’internal consistency were within reasonable range (0.70a0.80; Kline, 1998) whereas <strong>the</strong>distancing/avoidance subscale’ internal consistency was moderately low (0.60a0.70; Kline,1998). However, <strong>the</strong> denial subscale (a=0.33) exhibited a very low alpha coefficient and wasdiscarded from fur<strong>the</strong>r analyses.Two sets <strong>of</strong> analyses examined <strong>the</strong> relationships between <strong>the</strong> ISCCS and <strong>the</strong> MCOPE subscales,and between <strong>the</strong> ISCCS and <strong>the</strong> WOCQ subscales, respectively. Several ISCCS subscales correlatedsignificantly with <strong>the</strong> subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE and <strong>the</strong> WOCQ. Results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se analysesare presented in Table 7.Concurrent validityThe relationship between <strong>the</strong> ISCCS subscales and appraisal variables (i.e. perceived goalattainment, perceived relevance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> competition, and sense <strong>of</strong> control), affective variables (i.e.positive affect, negative affect, cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and confidence), and socialdesirability was assessed with bivariate Pearson correlations using a Bonferroni correction. Priorto <strong>the</strong>se analyses, means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s index <strong>of</strong> internal consistency werecalculated <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> nine selected variables (see Table 6). All variables exhibited good internalconsistency (a0.80; Kline, 1998), with <strong>the</strong> exception <strong>of</strong> social desirability, which exhibited amoderately low alpha coefficient. All variables were retained <strong>for</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>r analyses.A first set <strong>of</strong> analyses bore on <strong>the</strong> relationship between ISCCS subscales and each appraisalvariable whereas a second set <strong>of</strong> analyses assessed <strong>the</strong> relationship between ISCCS subscales andeach affective variable. Several subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS correlated with perceived relevance <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> competition, sense <strong>of</strong> control, perceived goal attainment, positive affect, negative affect, cogni-


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3419Table 5Means, standard deviations, Cronbach’s index <strong>of</strong> internal consistency, and correlations between <strong>the</strong> subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCSCoping strategies M SD a 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 91 Thought control 3.15 0.90 0.722 Mental imagery 3.03 0.94 0.74 0.79(0.07)3 Relaxation 2.54 0.93 0.80 0.48 0.50(0.05) (0.06)4 Ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure 3.96 0.83 0.79 0.50 0.43 0.30(0.04) (0.04) (0.04)5 Logical analysis 2.58 0.86 0.67 0.73 0.56 0.26 0.26(0.06) (0.06) (0.03) (0.03)6 Seeking social support 1.95 0.81 0.70 0.55 0.42 0.21 0.21 0.56(0.04) (0.04) (0.02) (0.02) (0.04)7 Social withdrawal 1.93 0.81 0.71 0.39 0.30 0.27 0.01 0.42 0.24(0.03) (0.03) (0.03) (0.02) (0.03) (0.02)8 Mental distraction 1.74 0.76 0.76 0.21 0.12 0. 19 0.12 0.25 0.46 0.37(0.04) (0.04) (0.04) (0.03) (0.04) (0.03) (0.03)9 Disengagement/resignation 1.65 0.73 0.68 0.29 0.19 0.15 0.52 0.01 0.16 0.18 0.41(0.03) (0.04) (0.04) (0.03) (0.03) (0.03) (0.02) (0.04)10 Venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant 2.08 0.99 0.87 0.04 0.00 0.05 0.12 0.21 0.18 0.22 0.26 0.48emotion (0.04) (0.04) (0.04) (0.03) (0.04) (0.03) (0.03) (0.04) (0.04)Note: means and standard deviations are presented as per-items scores to facilitate comparison across variables. Error terms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> correlation arepresented in paren<strong>the</strong>ses.


20 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34Table 6Means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s index <strong>of</strong> internal consistency <strong>for</strong> appraisal, affective, coping, and socialdesirability variablesVariables M SD aAppraisal variablesPerceived relevance <strong>of</strong> competition a 17.09 4.75 0.797Sense <strong>of</strong> control a 14.39 3.14 0.826Perceived goal attainment b 11.66 3.62 0.893Affective variablesPositive affect c 37.09 6.69 0.867Negative affect c 19.76 6.07 0.799Cognitive anxiety-state d 16.48 4.83 0.817Somatic anxiety-state d 16.76 4.85 0.811Confidence-state d 24.05 5.56 0.848MCOPE subscales eInstrumental social support 2.26 0.94 0.786Behavioral disengagement 1.48 0.60 0.621Planning 2.83 0.92 0.815Suppression <strong>of</strong> competing activities 2.81 0.85 0.683Venting <strong>of</strong> emotions 1.76 0.86 0.896Increased ef<strong>for</strong>t 3.63 0.83 0.866Positive reappraisal 3.28 0.88 0.875Mental disengagement 2.12 0.84 0.807WOCQ subscales fDistancing/avoidance 1.73 0.51 0.607Confrontation/seeking social support 2.53 0.54 0.743Problem-focused 1.72 0.47 0.735Denial 1.90 0.48 0.331Social desirability g 13.36 3.67 0.669Note: means and standard deviations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE and WOCQ subscales are presented as per item scores t<strong>of</strong>acilitate comparison across variables. a N=306, b N=301, c N=147, d N=157, e N=108, f N=97, g N=96.tive state-anxiety, and somatic state-anxiety. These results are presented in Table 8. A final set<strong>of</strong> analyses concerned <strong>the</strong> relationship between ISCCS subscales and social desirability. Copingstrategies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS did not correlate significantly with social desirability.Differential validityThe purpose <strong>of</strong> this analysis was to examine <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> athletes’ level <strong>of</strong> expertise (i.e.international, national, provincial, or regional) and gender on <strong>the</strong>ir use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS coping strategies.A factorial multivariate analysis <strong>of</strong> variance (MANOVA), using <strong>the</strong> 10 coping subscalesas dependent variables and both <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong> expertise and gender as independent variables, wasper<strong>for</strong>med. Because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> small number <strong>of</strong> males in <strong>the</strong> international athletes subgroup, nationaland international athletes were combined in a subgroup labeled ‘elite athletes’. There<strong>for</strong>e, anExpertise (3)×Gender (2) analysis was conducted.


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3421Table 7Correlations between <strong>the</strong> subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS and <strong>the</strong> subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE and WOCQCoping strategies MCOPE WOCQSIS PLAN EFF SUPP PR VENT BD MD DIS CON PFCThought control 0.265** 0.472*** 0.582*** 0.618*** 0.713*** 0.112 0.164 0.074 0.263** 0.665*** 0.323***Mental imagery 0.340*** 0.512*** 0.487*** 0.520*** 0.621*** 0.021 0.020 0.049 0.237* 0.529*** 0.152Relaxation 0.299*** 0.356*** 0.348*** 0.435*** 0.483*** 0.003 0.103 0.175 0.071 0.276** 0.184Ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure 0.128 0.260** 0.679*** 0.189* 0.377*** 0.237* 0.464*** 0.073 0.117 0.336*** 0.003Logical analysis 0.450*** 0.541*** 0.436*** 0.467*** 0.400*** 0.115 0.107 0.032 0.312*** 0.581*** 0.340***Seeking support 0.687*** 0.438*** 0.236* 0.183 0.216* 0.156 0.288*** 0.370*** 0.213* 0.449*** 0.617***Social withdrawal 0.196* 0.159 0.089 0.320*** 0.211* 0.135 0.253** 0.037 0.408*** 0.125 0.173*Mental distraction 0.278*** 0.065 0.087 0.119 0.022 0.190* 0.298*** 0.671*** 0.366*** 0.115 0.352***Disengagement/ 0.038 0.144 0.401*** 0.149 0.431*** 0.411*** 0.517*** 0.063 0.436*** 0.101 0.295***resignationVenting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant 0.004 0.056 0.028 0.006 0.150 0.801*** 0.285*** 0.166 0.431*** 0.099 0.443***emotionNote: SIS=seeking instrumental support, PLAN=planning, EFF=increased ef<strong>for</strong>t, SUPP=suppression <strong>of</strong> competing activities, PR=positivereappraisal, VENT=venting <strong>of</strong> emotion, BD=behavioral disengagement, MD=mental disengagement, DIS=distancing/avoidance,CON=confrontation/seeking social support, PFC=problem-focused coping. *p0.05; **p0.01; ***p0.005 (Bonferroni correction a/10).


22 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34Table 8Correlation between subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS, appraisal variables, affective variables, and social desirabilityCoping Appraisal variables Affective variablesstrategiesPRC SOC PGA PA NA SCOG SSOM SCON SDThought control 0.23*** 0.21*** 0.19*** 0.42*** 0.06 0.13 0.05 0.29*** 0.21*Mental imagery 0.31*** 0.26*** 0.20*** 0.46*** 0.10 0.03 0.13 0.29*** 0.08Relaxation 0.28*** 0.15** 0.11 0.35*** 0.02 0.06 0.14 0.06 0.08Ef<strong>for</strong>t 0.35*** 0.35*** 0.38*** 0.58*** 0.05 0.19* 0.09 0.15 0.18expenditureLogical analysis 0.29*** 0.11 0.12* 0.43*** 0.20* 0.27*** 0.21** 0.05 0.04Seeking support 0.08 0.10* 0.16*** 0.28*** 0.14 0.21** 0.12 0.04 0.23*Social 0.11 0.06 0.05 0.10 0.29*** 0.07 0.06 0.14 0.07withdrawalMental 0.12* 0.05 0.01 0.08 0.10 0.27*** 0.06 0.03 0.17distractionDisengagement/ 0.17*** 0.45*** 0.39*** 0.44*** 0.35*** 0.50*** 0.19* 0.24*** 0.11resignationVenting <strong>of</strong> 0.09 0.25*** 0.26*** 0.05 0.43*** 0.18* 0.18* 0.07 0.14unpleasantemotionNote: PRC=perceived relevance <strong>of</strong> competition, SOC=sense <strong>of</strong> control, PGA=perceived goal attainment,PA=positive affect, NA=negative affect, SCOG=cognitive anxiety-state, SSOM=somatic anxiety-state, SCON=stateconfidence, SD=social desirability. *p0.05; **p0.01; ***p0.005 (Bonferroni correction a/10).Whereas <strong>the</strong> multivariate Expertise×Gender effect was non-significant (Wilk’s l=0.93, F (20,580) =1.14, p0.05, h 2 =0.04), <strong>the</strong> Expertise (Wilk’s l=0.81, F (20, 580) =6.80, p0.001,h 2 =0.10) and Gender (Wilk’s l=0.87, F (20, 580) =5.62, p0.001, h 2 =0.14) multivariate maineffects reached significance. Correcting <strong>the</strong> levels <strong>of</strong> significance with a Bonferroni procedure(p=a/10=0.005), <strong>the</strong> univariate main effects <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> expertise and gender factors were examinedwith univariate ANOVAs. Mental imagery (F (2, 299) =6.30, p0.005, h 2 =0.02), relaxation (F(2, 299) =12.23, p0.005, h 2 =0.01), venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion (F (2, 299) =6.80, p0.005,h 2 =0.01), and ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure (F (2, 299) =7.22, p0.005, h 2 =0.01) differed significantly acrossathletes’ expertise whereas venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion was more frequent among males thanfemales (F (1, 299) =16.80, p0.005, h 2 =0.05) and ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure was more frequent amongfemales than males (F (1, 299) =11.57, p0.005, h 2 =0.03). Follow-ups were conducted withTukey a test <strong>for</strong> unequal subgroup size (Stevens, 1996). Results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se analyses revealed thatelite athletes used more mental imagery and ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure than regional athletes. Also, eliteathletes used more relaxation and less venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion than both provincial andregional athletes.


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3423DiscussionFactorial structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCSA first aim <strong>of</strong> this research was to develop a sport-specific <strong>questionnaire</strong> <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong>athletes’ coping actions in competitive sport settings, while providing preliminary evidence <strong>for</strong>its factorial validity. Because <strong>the</strong> <strong>questionnaire</strong> was created using a <strong>the</strong>oretical approach to scaledevelopment (Clark & Watson, 1995; DeVellis, 1991), a sequential four-stage confirmatory factoranalysis was employed in order to refine it without departing from an hypo<strong>the</strong>sis-testing approach(Jöreskog, 1993; Markland & Ingledew, 1997). The first and second stages resulted in <strong>the</strong> deletion<strong>of</strong> 49 items that were unreliable indicators <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir underlying latent construct. Several items weredeleted because <strong>the</strong>ir residuals were associated with <strong>the</strong> residual <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r items. Fewer itemswould have been deleted if residuals had been allowed to correlate freely. However, such a procedurewould have enhanced <strong>the</strong> risk <strong>of</strong> capitalizing on chance while threatening <strong>the</strong> externalvalidity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model (Cliff, 1983). As suggested by several researchers (Byrne, 2001; Jöreskog,1993), correlated residuals <strong>of</strong>ten signify high content overlap or redundancy between items. Theelimination <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se items was appropriate as it minimized content redundancy and shortened <strong>the</strong><strong>questionnaire</strong> significantly, a most convenient advantage in competitive sport settings, withoutaffecting its content broadness and relevance.The third and fourth stages were aimed at providing preliminary evidence <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> factorialvalidity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS. As such, results provided reasonable support by showing <strong>the</strong> good fit <strong>of</strong>two <strong>the</strong>oretically derived submodels (i.e <strong>the</strong> six-factor TOC model and <strong>the</strong> four-factor EOC model)and <strong>the</strong> acceptable fit <strong>of</strong> a full 10-factor model. The 10-factor model was fur<strong>the</strong>r compared withalternate two- and three-factor models. As hypo<strong>the</strong>sized, a 10-factor model fitted <strong>the</strong> data betterthan both alternate models. This finding converged with <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> qualitative studies(Eklund, 1996; Gould et al., 1993a; Prapavessis & Grove, 1995; Wingate, 1993) upon which <strong>the</strong>ISCCS was created. These studies showed that individuals can use a plethora <strong>of</strong> coping actionsto manage <strong>the</strong> demands <strong>of</strong> specific stressful situations. For instance, Gould and his collaborators(1993) reported 158 first-order <strong>the</strong>mes, corresponding to specific coping actions used by UnitedStates National champion figure skaters. Similarly, Kondo (1997) reported 80 specific copingactions used by college students during episode <strong>of</strong> examination stress. The superiority <strong>of</strong> a 10-first-order-factor model over a two- or a three-first-order-factor model seems tenable as <strong>the</strong> <strong>for</strong>mermight better capture <strong>the</strong> complexity and uniqueness <strong>of</strong> athletes’ coping actions in competitivesport settings.Regardless <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> above findings portraying that each coping strategy corresponds to a set <strong>of</strong>similar, homogeneous, and well delineating coping actions, a possibility remains that a hierarchicalsecond-order solution, representing higher-order functional dimensions <strong>of</strong> coping, might exist <strong>for</strong><strong>the</strong> ISCCS. Considering <strong>the</strong> model generating purpose <strong>of</strong> this study as well as its weak parametersto-participantsratio (Tanaka, 1987), it was decided not to test <strong>the</strong> hierarchical factor structure <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> ISCCS. In <strong>the</strong> absence <strong>of</strong> direct evidence, <strong>the</strong> inter-scales correlation matrix never<strong>the</strong>lessprovided indirect evidences <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> a second-order factorial structure <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCSas its subscales were moderately inter-correlated. The first set <strong>of</strong> associations seems to representa cluster <strong>of</strong> TOC strategies (i.e. thought control, mental imagery, relaxation, ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure,logical analysis, and seeking support) whereas <strong>the</strong> second set seems to represent a mixture <strong>of</strong>


24 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34emotion (i.e. social withdrawal and venting <strong>of</strong> emotion) and avoidance-oriented coping strategies(i.e. mental distraction and disengagement/resignation). The <strong>for</strong>mer cluster might encompass constructsthat are similar to basic psychological skills (Murphy & Tammen, 1998), as <strong>the</strong>y representactions <strong>of</strong> athletes aimed at changing or mastering actively <strong>the</strong> external and internal demands <strong>of</strong>a sport competition. The latter cluster might correspond to indirect actions <strong>of</strong> athletes aimed atreducing internal demands and pressure or to avoid <strong>the</strong> external demands associated with a givencompetition. Overall, this finding is in accordance with our review <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> recent literature and,more specifically, it tends to corroborate that <strong>the</strong> ISCCS in fact integrates basic psychologicalskills (Murphy & Tammen, 1998) with <strong>the</strong> EOC strategies reported in recent qualitative studiesconducted in sport settings (Eklund, 1996; Gould et al., 1993a; Prapavessis & Grove, 1995).Despite <strong>the</strong> attractiveness <strong>of</strong> hierarchical solutions, <strong>the</strong>re is <strong>the</strong> possibility that <strong>the</strong> assumptions<strong>of</strong> classical psychometric <strong>the</strong>ory (Nunnally, 1978) could be violated in second-order factor analysis<strong>of</strong> coping <strong>questionnaire</strong>s. In view <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> complexity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> coping construct, it might beunrealistic to assume that each coping strategy should load only on one functional dimension.Because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir situation-driven nature, <strong>the</strong>ir high inter-individual variability, and <strong>the</strong>ir multipledeterminants, coping strategies might serve different functions <strong>for</strong> different individuals in differentstressful situations (Coyne & Gottlieb, 1996; Oakland & Ostell, 1996). Results <strong>of</strong> second-orderfactor analyses conducted on <strong>the</strong> COPE subscales (Carver et al., 1989) lent credence to thisassertion by indicating that venting <strong>of</strong> emotion loaded simultaneously on an avoidance- and onan emotion-oriented second-order factor (Stewart & Schwarzer, 1996) whereas positive reappraisalloaded on both a task- and an emotion-oriented second-order factor (Hudek-Knezevic et al., 1999).A second limitation <strong>for</strong> hierarchical models <strong>of</strong> coping pertains to <strong>the</strong> absence <strong>of</strong> clear <strong>the</strong>oreticalguidelines addressing <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> functional dimensions <strong>of</strong> coping. As such, whe<strong>the</strong>r a two-(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), a three- (Endler & Parker, 1994; Amirkhan, 1990), or a four-secondorder-factormodel (Moos, 1993; Phelps & Jarvis, 1994; Zautra et al., 1997) should account <strong>for</strong><strong>the</strong> variance <strong>of</strong> coping strategies might depend on <strong>the</strong> magnitude <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir inter-scales correlations,on <strong>the</strong> content broadness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> coping <strong>questionnaire</strong>, as well as on <strong>the</strong> situation in which copingactions are employed. Despite <strong>the</strong>se limitations, future research should test whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> copingstrategies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS can be organized in a hierarchical model with a simplex structure thatreplicates across samples <strong>of</strong> athletes (i.e. between-group invariance) as well as across <strong>the</strong> distinctivephases <strong>of</strong> a given competition (i.e. longitudinal invariance).Convergent validityAlong with providing preliminary evidence <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> factorial validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS, <strong>the</strong> secondgoal <strong>of</strong> this study was to assess its convergent association with subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE and <strong>the</strong>WOCQ. The first set <strong>of</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses was guided by <strong>the</strong> moderate conceptual overlap betweensome <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS (i.e. ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure, thought control, seeking support, mental distraction,disengagement/resignation, and venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion) and MCOPE subscales (i.e.increased ef<strong>for</strong>t, positive reappraisal, seeking social support, mental disengagement, behavioraldisengagement, and venting <strong>of</strong> emotion). As expected, <strong>the</strong>se subscales correlated meaningfullyand shared from 27 to 64% <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir variance. These moderately high associations brought support<strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> convergent validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se ISCCS subscales. Moreover, <strong>the</strong>ir originality, sport relevance,


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3425and content broadness might have accounted <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> moderate and interesting part <strong>of</strong> unexplainedvariance in <strong>the</strong>ir relationships with <strong>the</strong> MCOPE subscales.The second set <strong>of</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses bore on <strong>the</strong> relationships between <strong>the</strong> ISCCS and MCOPE subscalescorresponding to ei<strong>the</strong>r a TOC or an EOC functional dimension <strong>of</strong> coping. As <strong>the</strong>y represent<strong>the</strong> same functional dimension <strong>of</strong> coping, TOC subscales from both <strong>questionnaire</strong>s, on one hand,and EOC subscales from both <strong>questionnaire</strong>s, on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, were expected to covary. Resultsprovided some evidence <strong>for</strong> this hypo<strong>the</strong>sis as several subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS correlated meaningfullywith <strong>the</strong>ir MCOPE counterparts. Whereas <strong>the</strong> TOC subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS shared from 7to 38% <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir variance with those <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE, <strong>the</strong> EOC subscales shared from 6 to 17% <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>ir respective variance. These moderate relationships between several MCOPE and ISCCS subscalesprovided support <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> latter as its subscales tapped original and distinctivecoping strategies that none<strong>the</strong>less share a similar functionality with those <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE.Despite <strong>the</strong> compelling evidence <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> convergent validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS, some associationsbetween <strong>the</strong> ISCCS and MCOPE unexpectedly failed to correlate meaningfully. The first concernwas <strong>the</strong> positive association between <strong>the</strong> seeking support subscale with both <strong>the</strong> TOC and EOCsubscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> MCOPE. As noted by Carver and his collaborators (1989), seeking support canserve more than one function in a given stressful situation. In competitive sport settings, seekingout advice from athletes and coaches might represent a task-oriented strategy in some circumstances,as it helps an athlete to solve his technical, tactical, or mental problems. However, itmight constitute an emotion-oriented strategy under o<strong>the</strong>r conditions, as athletes use it to avoidan unpleasant situation, to create a mental diversion, or to express <strong>the</strong>ir negative feelings. Thekey in understanding <strong>the</strong> functional role <strong>of</strong> this strategy might be to assess <strong>the</strong> social environmentin which it is used. For instance, using this strategy in <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> competent individuals whocan provide useful advice might help <strong>the</strong> per<strong>for</strong>mance <strong>of</strong> an athlete whereas using it in a socialenvironment <strong>of</strong>fering irrelevant feedback might entail only disengagement and resignation. Thesecond concern was <strong>the</strong> positive association <strong>of</strong> social withdrawal with some TOC subscales <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> MCOPE. On one hand, <strong>the</strong> ef<strong>for</strong>t to withdraw from o<strong>the</strong>r individuals and to limit socialcontacts in order to think, appraise, and rehearse <strong>the</strong> course <strong>of</strong> actions involved in a stressfulsituation might encourage <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> task-oriented strategies such as relaxation, mental imagery,and positive reappraisal. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> this strategy might also lead to mentaldistraction and avoidance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> stressful situation. Clearly, researchers should investigate <strong>the</strong>conditions in which social withdrawal promotes task-oriented actions as well as those in whichit leads to avoidance <strong>of</strong> stressful events.The final set <strong>of</strong> convergent hypo<strong>the</strong>ses bore on <strong>the</strong> relationship between three reliable subscales<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQ and those <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS. Accounting <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir conceptual similarities, TOC subscales<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS were expected to correlate with <strong>the</strong> confronting/seeking social support scale <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>WOCQ whereas EOC subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS were expected to correlate with <strong>the</strong>distancing/avoidance scale <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQ. As expected, <strong>the</strong> distancing/avoidance scale <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>WOCQ correlated positively with <strong>the</strong> EOC subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS (i.e.disengagement/resignation, mental distraction, venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion, and socialwithdrawal) with which it shared from 11 to 24% <strong>of</strong> variance. Also, <strong>the</strong> confrontation/seekingsocial support scale <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQ correlated positively with <strong>the</strong> TOC subscales <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS(i.e. mental imagery, seeking support, logical analysis, and ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure), with 21–37% <strong>of</strong>shared variance. Even if <strong>the</strong>y support validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS, <strong>the</strong>se results need to be interpreted


26 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34with some caution as <strong>the</strong> four-factor model <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQ was developed solely on empiricalgrounds. As such, it should be mentioned that <strong>the</strong> factorial structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> French–Canadiantranslation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WOCQ might fluctuate across samples (Parker et al., 1993) because it has neverbeen cross-validated on independent samples and because it has never been tested in competitivesport settings. At best, <strong>the</strong> associations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS with <strong>the</strong> WOCQ subscales might facilitate <strong>the</strong>comparison <strong>of</strong> results across research settings as <strong>the</strong> latter <strong>questionnaire</strong> is still widely employed inmainstream psychology.Concurrent validityThe third goal <strong>of</strong> this paper was to assess <strong>the</strong> relationship <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS subscales with cognitiveappraisal and affective variables. Despite <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sized proximal role <strong>of</strong> primary appraisal on<strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> coping strategies (Hardy et al., 1996; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), previous studiesconducted in sport settings have overlooked <strong>the</strong> impact <strong>of</strong> athletes’ perceived relevance <strong>of</strong> acompetition, a primary appraisal variable. However, in <strong>the</strong> present study, in accordance with wellestablished<strong>the</strong>oretical assumptions (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> TOC (i.e. thoughtcontrol, mental imagery, relaxation, ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure, and logical analysis) increased with athletes’perceived relevance <strong>of</strong> a competition. As expected, <strong>the</strong>re seems to be no need <strong>for</strong> TOC incompetitions in which nothing is at stake as well as in competitions in which <strong>the</strong> demands areperceived as non-threatening or as non-challenging. Thus, perceived relevance <strong>of</strong> a competitionmight be necessary <strong>for</strong> any coping strategy to be used by athletes who o<strong>the</strong>rwise might becomepassive, disengaged, and less energized in situations that are perceived as being irrelevant andnon-challenging.The second set <strong>of</strong> analyses tested <strong>the</strong> relationship <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS subscales with perceived sense<strong>of</strong> control and perceived per<strong>for</strong>mance goal attainment. Concerning <strong>the</strong> <strong>for</strong>mer variable, previousresearch has indicated that athletes’ use <strong>of</strong> TOC increased with <strong>the</strong>ir level <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy(Haney & Long, 1995) and perceived control (Anshel & Kaissidis, 1997; Kim & Duda, 1999).As expected and in accordance with <strong>the</strong>se studies, athletes’ sense <strong>of</strong> control increased with <strong>the</strong>iruse <strong>of</strong> thought control, mental imagery, and ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure whereas it decreased with <strong>the</strong>iruse <strong>of</strong> venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotion and disengagement/resignation. Perceiving oneself as beingin control <strong>of</strong> a situation might lessen <strong>the</strong> necessity <strong>of</strong> managing its unpleasantness and mightfavor <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> actions oriented toward direct regulation <strong>of</strong> external demands. As <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> relationshipbetween coping and per<strong>for</strong>mance, it has been found that athletes’ use <strong>of</strong> TOC increased with<strong>the</strong>ir normative per<strong>for</strong>mance (Finch, 1994; Haney & Long, 1995) and with <strong>the</strong>ir per<strong>for</strong>mance goalattainment (Gaudreau et al., 2001). As expected and in accordance with <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se studies,athletes’ level <strong>of</strong> perceived goal attainment was associated positively with <strong>the</strong>ir use <strong>of</strong> thoughtcontrol, mental imagery, ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure, and seeking support. In accordance with <strong>the</strong> basicpsychological skills literature (Hardy et al., 1996; Murphy & Tammen, 1998; Thomas et al.,1999), it might be that using <strong>the</strong>se coping strategies during <strong>the</strong> course <strong>of</strong> a competition actuallypromotes goal attainment. However, an alternative hypo<strong>the</strong>sis should not to be ruled out, as attainment<strong>of</strong> ones’ per<strong>for</strong>mance goal might facilitate <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> TOC in <strong>the</strong> course <strong>of</strong> a competition(Gaudreau et al., 2001). Whe<strong>the</strong>r some coping strategies facilitate goal attainment or whe<strong>the</strong>rperceived goal attainment encourages <strong>the</strong>ir use is an issue that needs clarification in future studies.Over <strong>the</strong> years, several <strong>the</strong>oretical systems (Cerin et al., 2000; Lazarus, 1991; Lazarus & Folk-


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3427man, 1984) have assumed a close relationship between coping and emotions in <strong>the</strong> course <strong>of</strong>stressful encounters. Based on substantive research conducted in sport (Crocker & Graham, 1995;Gaudreau et al., 2001; Ntoumanis et al., 1999), academic (Carver & Scheier, 1994; Clark et al.,1995), child (Crook, Beaver, & Bell, 1998), and health psychology (Billings, Folkman, Acree, &Moskowitz, 2000), athletes’ use <strong>of</strong> TOC was expected to increase with positive affect, whereas<strong>the</strong>ir use <strong>of</strong> EOC was expected to increase with negative affect. In fact, in <strong>the</strong> present study,athletes’ use <strong>of</strong> TOC strategies increased with <strong>the</strong>ir in-competition positive affect whereas <strong>the</strong>iruse <strong>of</strong> EOC strategies increased with <strong>the</strong>ir in-competition negative affect. Although <strong>the</strong>y support<strong>the</strong> concurrent validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS, <strong>the</strong>se results might have been influenced by several interveningvariables such as athletes’ perceived efficacy <strong>of</strong> coping (Ntoumanis & Biddle, 1998), motivationalorientation (Ntoumanis et al., 1999), and goal attainment (Carver & Scheier, 1998; Gaudreauet al., 2001). Future research should explore <strong>the</strong> mediating and/or moderating roles <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>sevariables in <strong>the</strong> relationship <strong>of</strong> coping with affect.Multidimensional anxiety has received a tremendous amount <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical (Hanin, 1989;Hardy, 1990; Jones, 1991; Martens et al., 1990) and empirical (Burton, 1998) attention from sportscientists. Despite <strong>the</strong> advances in research pertaining to <strong>the</strong> anxiety-per<strong>for</strong>mance relationship(Kleine, 1990), little progress has been made in identifying <strong>the</strong> antecedents <strong>of</strong> competitive stateanxiety. Acknowledging <strong>the</strong> centrality <strong>of</strong> state anxiety in <strong>the</strong> sport psychology literature as wellas <strong>the</strong> paucity <strong>of</strong> research linking it to coping, Ntoumanis and Biddle (2000) explored <strong>the</strong> copinganxietyrelationship in a sample <strong>of</strong> 356 athletes. Stemming from <strong>the</strong>ir preliminary results, it wasexpected that TOC strategies would increase with somatic anxiety whereas EOC strategies wouldincrease with cognitive anxiety. In accordance with this hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, our results indicated thatathletes’ use <strong>of</strong> venting <strong>of</strong> emotion, disengagement/resignation, and mental distraction increasedwith <strong>the</strong>ir negative expectations and worries about one’s capabilities and potential <strong>for</strong> failure (i.e.cognitive anxiety). However, results <strong>for</strong> somatic state-anxiety were contrary to expectations, asit was not significantly associated with TOC strategies, while at <strong>the</strong> same time, its relationship withEOC strategies almost reached significance. As suggested by Jones (1991), <strong>the</strong> coping-anxietyrelationship might depend on athletes’ interpretation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir cognitive and somatic symptoms. Inthis perspective, Eubank and Collins (2000) observed that athletes, who perceived <strong>the</strong>ir anxietyas facilitative used more active coping, planning, suppression <strong>of</strong> competing activities, positivereappraisal, and acceptance than athletes who perceived it as debilitative. However, this studyfailed to provide separate data <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> cognitive and somatic components <strong>of</strong> competitive stateanxiety. Clearly, future research should examine whe<strong>the</strong>r perceiving one’s cognitive and somaticstate anxiety as facilitative or debilitative might reflect on some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS coping subscales.Differential validityThe final goal <strong>of</strong> this paper was to explore whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS coping strategiesdiffered across athletes’ levels <strong>of</strong> expertise and genders. Based on <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> numerous qualitative(Orlick & Partington, 1988; McCaffrey & Orlick, 1989) and quantitative investigations(Madden et al., 1989; Thomas et al., 1999), it was expected that elite athletes (i.e. internationaland national) would use more TOC strategies and less EOC strategies than <strong>the</strong>ir provincial andregional counterparts. Results lent partial credence to this hypo<strong>the</strong>sis as elite athletes used more


28 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34mental imagery, relaxation, and ef<strong>for</strong>t expenditure as well as less venting <strong>of</strong> unpleasant emotionthan <strong>the</strong>ir less pr<strong>of</strong>icient counterparts.Although quite compelling by nature, such evidence in favor <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> differential validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ISCCS should be interpreted with extreme caution. The present research was not designed exclusively<strong>for</strong> testing <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> coping strategies across athletes’ levels <strong>of</strong> expertise and genders. Thus,<strong>the</strong> post-hoc nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> research design remains an important concern, as several confoundingvariables were not controlled. Firstly, international level female athletes (n=42) were more representedin our sample than <strong>the</strong>ir male counterparts (n=10). Consequently, <strong>the</strong> greater use <strong>of</strong> ef<strong>for</strong>texpenditure and <strong>the</strong> lesser use <strong>of</strong> venting <strong>of</strong> emotion among female athletes <strong>of</strong> this sample mightbe an artifact <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir superior level <strong>of</strong> expertise. Secondly, international and national level competitionsare relatively rare in Canada, so that a majority <strong>of</strong> elite athletes had to be assessed after<strong>the</strong> completion <strong>of</strong> a provincial competition. Thus, it is debatable whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> challenge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>secompetitions was sufficient to provide a valid portrait <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> coping actions employed by <strong>the</strong>seathletes. As such, future research should counteract this limitation ei<strong>the</strong>r by using a MANCOVA,with athletes’ perceived relevance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> competition as covariate, or by assessing athletes’ copingstrategies after competitions that are congruent with <strong>the</strong>ir own level <strong>of</strong> expertise. Thirdly, results<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong> variance indicated that <strong>the</strong> within-group variance was superior to between-groupvariance <strong>for</strong> some coping strategies. As such, <strong>the</strong> within-group heterogeneity suggested that severalintervening variables need to be accounted <strong>for</strong> in order to develop a valid portrait <strong>of</strong> copingstrategies used by athletes <strong>of</strong> both genders and different levels <strong>of</strong> expertise. Among potentialintervening factors, <strong>the</strong> type <strong>of</strong> sport and <strong>the</strong> athletes’ competitive experience might moderate <strong>the</strong>use <strong>of</strong> coping strategies. For instance, individual sports might facilitate <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> social withdrawal,whereas team sports might encourage <strong>the</strong> seeking <strong>of</strong> support. This interesting possibilityshould be tested using factorial design, <strong>for</strong> example with expertise, gender, and type <strong>of</strong> sport asindependent variables, and years <strong>of</strong> competitive experience as a covariable. Because <strong>of</strong> its posthocnature and considering <strong>the</strong> unequal number <strong>of</strong> participants per subgroups in <strong>the</strong> present study,it was decided not to test this hypo<strong>the</strong>sis as <strong>the</strong> risk <strong>of</strong> capitalizing on chance was too important.ConclusionsThe results presented in this paper provided preliminary evidence <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> reliability and <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong>factorial, convergent, concurrent, and differential validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS. Despite <strong>the</strong>se encouragingfindings, three types <strong>of</strong> limitations must be acknowledged. The first concern pertains to <strong>the</strong> use<strong>of</strong> a sequential confirmatory factor analysis. Although providing a way <strong>of</strong> developing and refining<strong>questionnaire</strong>s without departing from an hypo<strong>the</strong>sis-testing approach, sequential approaches aredriven by <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> a given sample and might enhance <strong>the</strong> possibility <strong>of</strong> developingmodels that are sample specific (MacCallum, Roznowski, & Newcowitz, 1992). Thus, <strong>the</strong> presentresearch should be interpreted as presenting <strong>the</strong> development and <strong>the</strong> calibration <strong>of</strong> a new copingmeasure as well as a preliminary test <strong>of</strong> its validity. Future research should address <strong>the</strong> issue <strong>of</strong>external validity by assessing whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> factorial model <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS can be replicated acrosssamples. Because elite athletes might use more TOC strategies than both provincial and regionalathletes and because <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> some coping strategies may differ between genders, future researchshould test <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> factorial invariance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS across both <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong> expertise and gender


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3429factors. Also, <strong>the</strong> ISCCS should be administered to European as well as North-American Frenchspeaking athletes in order to assess its factorial stability across cultures.The second issue pertains to <strong>the</strong> retrospective and cross-sectional design <strong>of</strong> this research.Because it was decided to assess <strong>the</strong>ir in-competition coping actions, athletes were asked to complete<strong>the</strong> ISCCS within six hours <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> completion <strong>of</strong> a competition. Although <strong>the</strong> retrospectivedelay was short in comparison with previous coping research conducted in sport settings, severalbiases might have affected <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> data (Smith, Leffingwell, & Ptacek, 1999). With<strong>the</strong> passage <strong>of</strong> time, research participants tend to attribute <strong>the</strong>ir successes to internal causes and<strong>the</strong>ir failures to external ones. Thus, conditions <strong>of</strong> success might produce an overestimation <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> TOC reported by athletes whereas conditions <strong>of</strong> failure might produce an underestimation <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>se coping strategies. Because <strong>of</strong> a possible self-enhancement bias, <strong>the</strong> relationship <strong>of</strong> copingwith both appraisal and affective variables should be interpreted with some caution. Futureresearch should be designed so as to minimize <strong>the</strong> post-competition retrospective delay withoutdeparting from <strong>the</strong> practical and ethical considerations embedded with this necessity. Finally,considering that <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> coping strategies might differ across <strong>the</strong> phases <strong>of</strong> a given sportcompetition (Gaudreau et al., in press; Gaudreau et al., 2001), a third problem could arise from<strong>the</strong> cross-sectional design <strong>of</strong> this research. Because <strong>the</strong> ISCCS was developed <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> measurement<strong>of</strong> pre-, in-, and post-competitive coping strategies <strong>of</strong> athletes, future research should assess copingat multiple points in a given competition in order to test <strong>the</strong> factorial stability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ISCCS across<strong>the</strong> phases <strong>of</strong> a sport competition.In spite <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se limitations, this research provided a promising, <strong>the</strong>oretically based sport-specificcoping instruments <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> athletes’ coping strategies in both individual andteam sports. Despite <strong>the</strong> convenience <strong>of</strong> using self-report instruments in coping research, a thoroughunderstanding <strong>of</strong> athletes’ coping actions will necessitate <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> various research methodologies.As such, multiple methods including sport-specific self-report measures, semi-structureinterviews, and daily diaries should not be overlooked by researchers as <strong>the</strong>y might render adeeper and more valuable portrait <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> social, environmental, and cognitive factors that mightencourage or hinder <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> certain coping strategies.AcknowledgementsThis research was supported by doctoral fellowships from <strong>the</strong> Fonds pour la Formation deChercheurs et l’Aide à la Recherche, from la Faculté des Études Supérieures de l’Université deMontréal, and from <strong>the</strong> Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council <strong>of</strong> Canada awarded to<strong>the</strong> first author. The authors would like to thank Geneviève Bouchard, Nadine Debois, PhilippeFleurance, and Edgard Thill <strong>for</strong> making <strong>the</strong>ir respective <strong>questionnaire</strong>s available <strong>for</strong> this project.ReferencesAmirkhan, J. (1990). A factor analytically derived measure <strong>of</strong> coping: <strong>the</strong> coping strategy indicator. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personalityand Social Psychology, 59, 1066–1074.Anshel, M. H., & Kaissidis, A. N. (1997). Coping style and situational appraisals as predictors <strong>of</strong> coping strategiesfollowing stressful events in sport as a function <strong>of</strong> gender and skill level. British Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 88, 263–276.


30 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34Arbuckle, J. L. (1999). amos 4.0 [Computer s<strong>of</strong>tware]. Chicago: Smallwaters.Beauchamp, P. H. (1995). Psychological influences on golfers’ putting per<strong>for</strong>mance. Micr<strong>of</strong>orm publication <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>International Institute <strong>for</strong> Sport and Human Per<strong>for</strong>mance, University <strong>of</strong> Oregon.Ben-Porath, Y. S., Waller, N. G., & Butcher, J. N. (1991). Assessment <strong>of</strong> coping: an empirical illustration <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>problem <strong>of</strong> inapplicable items. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality Assessment, 57, 162–176.Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS: Structural equations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate S<strong>of</strong>tware, Inc.Billings, D. W., Folkman, S., Acree, M., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Coping and physical health during caregiving:<strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong> positive and negative affect. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 131–142.Bolger, N. (1990). Coping a as personality process: a prospective study. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology,59, 525–537.Bollen, K. A., & Stine, R. A. (1993). Bootstrapping goodness-<strong>of</strong>-fit criteria in structural equation modelling. In K. A.Bollen & J. S. Long, Testing structural equation models (pp. 111–135). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Bouchard, G., Sabourin, S., Lussier, Y., Wright, J., & Richer, C. (1997). Testing <strong>the</strong> models underlying <strong>the</strong> ways <strong>of</strong>coping <strong>questionnaire</strong> with couples. Journal <strong>of</strong> Marriage and <strong>the</strong> Family, 59, 409–418.Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways <strong>of</strong> assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long, Testingstructural equation models (pp. 445–455). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Burton, D. (1998). Measuring competitive state anxiety. In J. L. Duda, Advances in sport and exercise psychologymeasurement (pp. 129–148). Morgantown, WV: Fitness In<strong>for</strong>mation Technology.Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modelling with AMOS. Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah,NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Carver, C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D., Noriega, V., Scheier, M. F., Robinson, D. S., Ketcham, A. S., M<strong>of</strong>fat, F. L., &Clark, K. C. (1993). How coping mediates <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> optimism on distress: a study <strong>of</strong> women with early stagebreast cancer. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 375–390.Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On <strong>the</strong> self-regulation <strong>of</strong> behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1994). Situational coping and coping dispositions in a stressful transaction. Journal<strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 184–195.Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: a <strong>the</strong>oretically based approach.Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267–283.Cerin, E., Szabo, A., Hunt, N., & Williams, C. (2000). Temporal patterning <strong>of</strong> competitive emotions: a critical review.Journal <strong>of</strong> Sport Sciences, 18, 605–626.Chou, C. P., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Estimates and tests in structural equation modelling. In R. H. Hoyle, Structuralequation modelling (pp. 37–55). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Clark, K. K., Bormann, C. A., Cropanzano, R. S., & James, K. (1995). Validation evidence <strong>for</strong> three coping measures.Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality Assessment, 65, 434–455.Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: basic issues in objective scale development. PsychologicalAssessment, 7, 309–319.Cliff, N. (1983). Some cautions concerning <strong>the</strong> application <strong>of</strong> causal modelling methods. Multivarariate BehavioralResearch, 18, 115–126.Compas, B. E., & Epping, J. E. (1993). Stress and coping in children and families. In C. F. Saylor, Children anddisasters (pp. 11–28). New York: Plenum Press.Coyne, J. C., & Gottlieb, B. H. (1996). The mismeasure <strong>of</strong> coping by checklist. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality, 64, 959–991.Crocker, P. R. E. (1992). Managing stress by competitive athletes: ways <strong>of</strong> coping. International Journal <strong>of</strong> SportPsychology, 23, 161–175.Crocker, P. R. E., & Graham, T. R. (1995). Coping by competitive athletes with per<strong>for</strong>mance stress: gender differencesand relationships with affect. The Sport Psychologist, 9, 325–338.Crocker, P. R. E., & Isaak, K. (1997). Coping during competitions and training sessions: are youth swimmers consistent?International Journal <strong>of</strong> Sport Psychology, 28, 355–369.Crocker, P. R. E., Kowalski, K. C., & Graham, T. R. (1998). Measurement <strong>of</strong> coping strategies in sport. In J. L. Duda,Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 149–161). Morgantown, WV: Fitness In<strong>for</strong>mationTechnology.Crook, K., Beaver, B. R., & Bell, M. (1998). Anxiety and depression in children: a preliminary examination <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>utility <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> PANAS-C. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 20, 323–350.


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3431Dagrou, E., Gauvin, L., & Halliwell, W. (1991). La préparation mentale des athlètes ivoiriens: Pratiques courantes etperspectives de recherche [Mental preparation <strong>of</strong> athletes from Ivory: Practical and research perspectives]. InternationalJournal <strong>of</strong> Sport Psychology, 22, 15–34.Debois, N., & Fleurance, P. (1998, March). Validation du CSAI-2 sous <strong>for</strong>me rétrospective: questions sur la méthode.[Validation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> CSAI-2 using a retrospective method: Methodological questions]. Paper presented at <strong>the</strong> annualmeeting <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Société Française de Psychologie Sportive, Poitiers, France.DeVellis, R. F. (1991). Scale development: <strong>the</strong>ory and application. London: Sage.Eklund, R. C. (1996). Preparing to compete: a season-long investigation with collegiate wrestlers. The Sport Psychologist,10, 111–131.Eklund, R. C., Grove, R. J., & Heard, P. N. (1998). The measurement <strong>of</strong> slump-related coping: factorial validity <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> COPE and <strong>the</strong> Modified-COPE inventories. Journal <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20, 157–175.Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1994). Assessment <strong>of</strong> multidimensional coping: task, emotion, and avoidance strategies.Psychological Assessment, 6, 50–60.Eubank, M., & Collins, D. (2000). Coping with pre- and in-event fluctuations in competitive state-anxiety: a longitudinalapproach. Journal <strong>of</strong> Sports Sciences, 18, 121–131.Finch, L. M. (1994). The relationships among coping strategies, trait anxiety, and per<strong>for</strong>mance in collegiate s<strong>of</strong>tballplayers. Micr<strong>of</strong>ilm publication <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> International Institute <strong>for</strong> Sport and Human Per<strong>for</strong>mance, University <strong>of</strong> Oregon.Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: Study <strong>of</strong> emotion and coping during threestages <strong>of</strong> a college examination. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 150–170.Fouladi, R. T. (1997, April). Covariance structure analysis techniques under conditions <strong>of</strong> multivariate normality andnonnormality-modified and bootstrap based test statistics. Paper presented at <strong>the</strong> annual meeting <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> AmericanEducational Research Association, San Diego, CA.Frydenberg, E., & Lewis, R. (1994). Coping with different concerns: consistency and variation in coping used byadolescents. Australian Psychologist, 29, 45–48.Gaudreau, P. (2000). Vers une version française du PANAS: Analyses en composantes principales avant, pendant etaprès une compétition sportive. [Towards a French version <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> PANAS: Principal components analyses be<strong>for</strong>e,during, and after a sport competition]. Proceedings <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Second International meeting <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Société Françaisede Psychologie du Sport (pp. 230–231).Gaudreau, P., Blondin, J. P., & Lapierre, A. M. (2001). Athletes’ coping during a competition: relationship <strong>of</strong> copingstrategies with positive affect, negative affect, and per<strong>for</strong>mance-goal discrepancy. Submitted <strong>for</strong> publication.Gaudreau, P., Lapierre, A. M., & Blondin, J. P. (in press). Coping at three phases <strong>of</strong> a competition: comparisonbetween pre-competitive, competitive, and post-competitive utilization <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same strategy. International Journal<strong>of</strong> Sport Psychology.George, T. R. (1988). Mental preparation strategies and peak per<strong>for</strong>mance among intercollegiate baseball players: anexploratory study. Micr<strong>of</strong>orm publication <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> International Institute <strong>for</strong> Sport and Human Per<strong>for</strong>mance, University<strong>of</strong> Oregon.Giacobbi, P. R., & Weinberg, R. S. (2000). An examination <strong>of</strong> coping in sport: individual trait anxiety differences andsituational consistency. The Sport Psychologist, 14, 42–62.Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., & Jackson, S. A. (1993a). Coping strategies used by US Olympic wrestlers. ResearchQuarterly <strong>for</strong> Exercise and Sport, 64, 83–93.Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., & Jackson, S. A. (1992a). 1988 U.S. Olympic wrestlers excellence: I. Mental preparation,precompetitive cognition and affect. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 358–382.Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., & Jackson, S. A. (1992b). 1988 U.S. Olympic wrestlers excellence: II. Thoughts and affectoccuring during competition. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 383–402.Gould, D., Finch, L. M., & Jackson, S. A. (1993b). Coping strategies used by national champion figure skaters. ResearchQuarterly <strong>for</strong> Exercise and Sport, 64, 453–468.Gould, D., Tuffey, S., Udry, E., & Loehr, J. (1995). Burnout in competitive junior tennis players: II. Qualitative analysis.The Sport Psychologist, 10, 341–366.Grove, R. J., Eklund, R. C., & Heard, P. N. (1997). Coping with per<strong>for</strong>mance slumps: factor analysis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ways <strong>of</strong>coping in sport scale. The Australian Journal <strong>of</strong> Science and Medicine in Sport, 29, 199–205.Grove, R. J., & Heard, P. N. (1997). Optimism and sport confidence as correlates <strong>of</strong> slump-related coping amongathletes. The Sport Psychologist, 11, 400–410.


32 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34Haney, C. J., & Long, B. C. (1995). Coping effectiveness: a path analysis <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy, control, coping, and per<strong>for</strong>mancein sport competitions. Journal <strong>of</strong> Applied Social Psychology, 25, 1726–1746.Hanin, Y. L. (1989). Interpersonal and intragroup anxiety in sports. In D. Hack<strong>for</strong>t, & C. D. Spielberger, Anxiety insports: an international perspective (pp. 19–28). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Hanton, S., & Jones, G. (1999). The acquisition and development <strong>of</strong> cognitive skills and strategies: I. Making <strong>the</strong>butterflies fly in <strong>for</strong>mation. The Sport Psychologist, 13, 1–21.Hardy, L. (1990). A catastrophe model <strong>of</strong> per<strong>for</strong>mance in sport. In J. G. Jones, & L. Hardy, Stress and per<strong>for</strong>mancein sport (pp. 81–106). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Hardy, L., Jones, G., & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding psychological preparation <strong>for</strong> sport: <strong>the</strong>ory and practice <strong>of</strong>elite per<strong>for</strong>mers. Chichester, UK: Wiley.Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cut<strong>of</strong>f criteria <strong>for</strong> fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteriaversus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modelling, 6, 1–55.Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle, Structural equation modeling: concepts, issues,and applications (pp. 76–99). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Hu, L., Bentler, P. M., & Kano, Y. (1992). Can test statistics in covariance structure analysis be trusted? PsychologicalBulletin, 112, 351–362.Hudek-Knezevic, J., Kardum, I., & Vukmirovic, Z. (1999). The structure <strong>of</strong> coping styles: a comparative study <strong>of</strong>Croatian sample. European Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality, 13, 149–161.Jackson, S. (1995). Factors influencing <strong>the</strong> occurrence <strong>of</strong> flow state in elite athletes. Journal <strong>of</strong> Applied Sport Psychology,7, 138–166.Jackson, S. (1992). Athletes in flow: A qualitative investigation <strong>of</strong> flow states in elite figure skaters. Journal <strong>of</strong> AppliedSport Psychology, 4, 161–180.Jackson, S., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). <strong>Development</strong> and validation <strong>of</strong> a scale to measure optimal experience: <strong>the</strong> flowstate scale. Journal <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 17–35.Jones (1991). Recent developments and current issues in competitive state anxiety research. The Psychologist, 4,152–155.Jöreskog, K. G. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Scott. Testing structural equationmodels (pp. 294–316). London, UK: Sage.Kaplan, P. M., & Saccuzzo, D. P. (1993). Psychological testing: principles, application, and issues. Pacific Grove,CA: Brooks/Cole.Kim, M. S., & Duda, J. L. (1999). Predicting coping responses: An integration <strong>of</strong> Lazarus’ transactional <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong>psychological stress and coping and goal perspective <strong>the</strong>ory. Proceedings <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> annual meeting <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Association<strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> Advancement <strong>of</strong> Applied Sport Psychology, 41.Kleine, D. (1990). Anxiety and sports per<strong>for</strong>mance: A meta-analysis. Anxiety Research, 2, 113–131.Kline, R. (1998). Principles and practice <strong>of</strong> structural equation modeling. New York: Guil<strong>for</strong>d.Kondo, D. S. (1997). Strategies <strong>for</strong> coping with test anxiety. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 10, 203–215.Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Ox<strong>for</strong>d University Press.Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.MacCallum, R. C., Roznowski, M., & Necowitz, L. B. (1992). Model modifications in covariance structure analysis:<strong>the</strong> problem <strong>of</strong> capitalization on chance. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 490–504.Madden, C. C., Kirkby, R. J., & McDonald, D. (1989). Coping styles <strong>of</strong> competitive middle distance runners. InternationalJournal <strong>of</strong> Sport Psychology, 20, 287–296.Mantzicopoulos, P. (1997). How do children cope with school failure? A study <strong>of</strong> social/emotional factors related tochildren’s coping strategies. Psychology in <strong>the</strong> Schools, 34, 229–237.Markland, D., & Ingledew, D. K. (1997). The measurement <strong>of</strong> exercise motives: Factorial validity and invariance acrossgender <strong>of</strong> a revised exercise motivations inventory. British Journal <strong>of</strong> Health Psychology, 2, 361–376.Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R. S., Bump, L. A., & Smith, D. E. (1990). <strong>Development</strong> and validation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 (CSAI-2). In R. Martens, R. S. Vealey, & D. Burton, Competitive anxietyin sport (pp. 117–190). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.McCaffrey, N., & Orlick, T. (1989). Mental factors related to excellence among top pr<strong>of</strong>essional golfers. InternationalJournal <strong>of</strong> Sport Psychology, 20, 256–278.


P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–3433Moos, R. H. (1993). Coping responses inventory: CRI-adult <strong>for</strong>m. Pr<strong>of</strong>essional manual. Odessa, FL: PsychologicalAssessment Resources, Inc.Mullan, E., Markland, D., & Ingledew, D. K. (1997). A graded conceptualisation <strong>of</strong> self-determination in <strong>the</strong> regulation<strong>of</strong> exercise behaviour: development <strong>of</strong> a measure using confirmatory factor analytical procedures. Personality andIndividual Differences, 23, 745–752.Murphy, S., & Tammen, V. (1998). In search <strong>of</strong> psychological skills. In J. L. Duda, Advances in sport and exercisepsychology measurement (pp. 195–209). Morgantown, WV: Fitness In<strong>for</strong>mation Technology.Nevitt, J., & Hancock, G. R. (1997). Relative per<strong>for</strong>mance <strong>of</strong> rescaling and resampling approaches to model chi-squareand parameter standard error estimation in structural equation modeling. Paper presented at <strong>the</strong> annual meeting<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.Ntoumanis, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2000). The relationship <strong>of</strong> intensity and direction <strong>of</strong> competitive anxiety with copingstrategies. The Sport Psychologist, 14, 360–371.Ntoumanis, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (1998). The relationship <strong>of</strong> coping and its perceived effectiveness to positive andnegative affect in sport. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 773–788.Ntoumanis, N., Biddle, S. J. H., & Haddock, G. (1999). The mediating role <strong>of</strong> coping strategies on <strong>the</strong> relationshipbetween achievement motivation and affect in sport. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 12, 299–327.Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric <strong>the</strong>ory. New York: McGraw-Hill.Oakland, S., & Ostell, A. (1996). Measuring coping: a review and critique. Human Relations, 49, 133–155.Orlick, T., & Partington, J. (1988). Mental links to excellence. The Sport Psychologist, 2, 105–130.Parker, J. D. A., & Endler, N. S. (1992). Coping with coping <strong>assessment</strong>: a critical review. European Journal <strong>of</strong>Personality, 6, 321–344.Parker, J. D. A., Endler, N. S., & Bagby, M. R. (1993). If it changes, it might be unstable: examining <strong>the</strong> factorstructure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ways <strong>of</strong> coping <strong>questionnaire</strong>. Psychological Assessment, 5, 361–368.Phelps, S. B., & Jarvis, P. A. (1994). Coping in adolescence: empirical evidence <strong>for</strong> a <strong>the</strong>oretically based approach toassessing coping. Journal <strong>of</strong> Youth and Adolescence, 23, 359–371.Prapavessis, H., & Grove, R. J. (1995). Ending batting slumps in baseball: a qualitative investigation. The AustralianJournal <strong>of</strong> Science and Medicine in Sport, 27, 14–19.Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. (1996). A beginner’s guide to structural equation modelling. Mahwah, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum.Sellers, R. M. (1995). Situational differences in <strong>the</strong> coping processes <strong>of</strong> student-athletes. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping,8, 325–336.Smith, R. E., & Christensen, D. S. (1995). Psychological skills as predictors <strong>of</strong> per<strong>for</strong>mance and survival in pr<strong>of</strong>essionalbaseball. Journal <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17, 399–415.Smith, R. E., Leffingwell, T. R., & Ptacek, J. T. (1999). Can people remember how <strong>the</strong>y coped? Factors associatedwith discordance between same-day and retrospective reports. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 76,1050–1061.Smith, R. E., Schutz, R. W., Smoll, F. L., & Ptacek, J. T. (1995). <strong>Development</strong> and validation <strong>of</strong> a multidimensionalmeasure <strong>of</strong> sport-specific psychological skills: <strong>the</strong> athletic coping skills inventory. Journal <strong>of</strong> Sport and ExercisePsychology, 17, 379–398.Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., & Ptacek, J. T. (1990). Conjunctive moderator variables in vulnerability and resiliencyresearch: life stress, social support, and coping skills, and adolescent sport injuries. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality andSocial Psychology, 58, 360–370.Stevens, J. (1996). Applied multivariate statistics <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> social sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Stewart, S. M., & Schwarzer, R. (1996). Stability <strong>of</strong> coping in Hong Kong medical students: a longitudinal study.Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 245–255.Stone, A. A., Greenberg, M. A., Kennedy-Moore, E., & Newman, M. G. (1991). Self-reported, situation-specific coping<strong>questionnaire</strong>s: what are <strong>the</strong>y measuring? Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 648–658.Stru<strong>the</strong>rs, G. (1990). Une analyse de la préparation mentale chez des joueurs de badminton d’élite au Québec [An<strong>assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> mental preparation in Quebec’s elite badminton players]. Unpublished masters <strong>the</strong>sis, University<strong>of</strong> Montreal.Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics. New York: Harper Collins.


34 P. Gaudreau, J.-P. Blondin / Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise 3 (2002) 1–34Tanaka, J. S. (1987). “How big is big enough?” Sample size and goodness <strong>of</strong> fit in structural equation models withlatent variables. Child <strong>Development</strong>, 58, 134–146.Thill, E. (1979). Questionnaire de Personnalité pour Sportifs (QPS) [Personality Questionnaire <strong>for</strong> athletes]. Paris:Institut National du Sport et de l’ Éducation Physique.Thill, E., & Brenot, J. (1982). Procédures d’analyse de la consistance d’un <strong>questionnaire</strong> de personnalité [Analyticalprocedures <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>assessment</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> consistency <strong>of</strong> a personality <strong>questionnaire</strong>]. Le Travail Humain, 45, 269–283.Thomas, P. R., Murphy, S. M., & Hardy, L. (1999). Test <strong>of</strong> per<strong>for</strong>mance strategies: <strong>Development</strong> and preliminaryvalidation <strong>of</strong> a comprehensive measure <strong>of</strong> athletes’ psychological skills. Journal <strong>of</strong> Sports Sciences, 17, 697–711.Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). <strong>Development</strong> and validation <strong>of</strong> brief measures <strong>of</strong> positive and negativeaffects. The PANAS scales. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation modelling with non-normal variables: problemsand remedies. In R. H. Hoyle, Structural equation modelling (pp. 56–75). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Wingate, C. F. (1993). Exploring <strong>the</strong> karate way <strong>of</strong> life: Coping, commitment, and psychological well-being amongtraditional karate participants. Micr<strong>of</strong>orm publication <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> International Institute <strong>for</strong> Sport and Human Per<strong>for</strong>mance,Univerisity <strong>of</strong> Oregon.Zautra, A. J., Sheets, V. L., & Sandler, I. N. (1997). An examination <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> construct validity <strong>of</strong> coping dispositions<strong>for</strong> a sample <strong>of</strong> recently divorced mo<strong>the</strong>rs. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 256–264.Zeidner, M., & Endler, N. S. (1996). Handbook <strong>of</strong> coping: <strong>the</strong>ory, research, and applications. New York: Wiley.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!