Mindfulness at work (Glomb, Duffy et al, 2012) - Human Resources
Mindfulness at work (Glomb, Duffy et al, 2012) - Human Resources
Mindfulness at work (Glomb, Duffy et al, 2012) - Human Resources
Create successful ePaper yourself
Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.
MINDFULNESS AT WORKTheresa M. <strong>Glomb</strong>, Michelle K. <strong>Duffy</strong>,Joyce E. Bono and Tao YangABSTRACTIn this chapter, we argue th<strong>at</strong> st<strong>at</strong>e and trait mindfulness andmindfulness-based practices in the <strong>work</strong>place should enhance employeeoutcomes. First, we review the existing liter<strong>at</strong>ure on mindfulness, providea brief history and definition of the construct, and discuss its benefici<strong>al</strong>effects on physic<strong>al</strong> and psychologic<strong>al</strong> he<strong>al</strong>th. Second, we deline<strong>at</strong>e amodel of the ment<strong>al</strong> and neurobiologic<strong>al</strong> processes by which mindfulnessand mindfulness-based practices improve self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion of thoughts,emotions, and behaviors, linking them to both performance and employeewell-being in the <strong>work</strong>place. We especi<strong>al</strong>ly focus on the power ofmindfulness, via improved self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion, to enhance soci<strong>al</strong> rel<strong>at</strong>ionshipsin the <strong>work</strong>place, make employees more resilient in the face of ch<strong>al</strong>lenges,and increase task performance. Third, we outline controversies, questions,and ch<strong>al</strong>lenges th<strong>at</strong> surround the study of mindfulness, paying speci<strong>al</strong><strong>at</strong>tention to the implic<strong>at</strong>ions of unresolved issues for understanding theeffects of mindfulness <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>. We conclude with a discussion of theimplic<strong>at</strong>ions of our propositions for organiz<strong>at</strong>ions and employees andoffer some recommend<strong>at</strong>ions for future research on mindfulness in the<strong>work</strong>place.Research in Personnel and <strong>Human</strong> <strong>Resources</strong> Management, Volume 30, 115–157Copyright r 2011 by Emer<strong>al</strong>d Group Publishing LimitedAll rights of reproduction in any form reservedISSN: 0742-7301/doi:10.1108/S0742-7301(2011)0000030005115
116THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.INTRODUCTIONThe concept of mindfulness – awareness and observ<strong>at</strong>ion of the presentmoment without reactivity or judgment – has gone mainstream. A Googled<strong>at</strong>abase search on the term mindfulness yielded more than six million links;mindfulness and <strong>work</strong> gener<strong>at</strong>ed 1.4 million links. Amazon.com lists mor<strong>et</strong>han 2,000 books with mindfulness in the title or as a keyword. A PsycInfod<strong>at</strong>abase search produced 2,221 articles, books, and dissert<strong>at</strong>ions withmindfulness as a keyword; Medline yielded 640. As the mindfulness concepthas grown in popularity, claims about its broad-reaching benefici<strong>al</strong> effectshave increased; y<strong>et</strong> its meaning has become hazier. <strong>Mindfulness</strong> could beeasily dismissed as nothing more than the l<strong>at</strong>est panacea for a stressedsoci<strong>et</strong>y (Altman, 2010; Lehrer, Woolfolk, & Sime, 2007) or the newest fad inorganiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> development (e.g., Carroll, 2006; Duerr, 2004a) if there werenot <strong>al</strong>so a growing body of scientific research suggesting th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnessand the practices associ<strong>at</strong>ed with it significantly benefit both he<strong>al</strong>thyindividu<strong>al</strong>s (including <strong>work</strong>ers), as well as those suffering from physic<strong>al</strong> andpsychologic<strong>al</strong> problems.The time is ripe to carefully examine the role th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness might playin the performance and well-being of individu<strong>al</strong>s <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>. Accordingly, thepurpose of this chapter is to assess the expected effects of mindfulness onemployees’ task and rel<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> functioning on the job. We review theliter<strong>at</strong>ure on mindfulness and discuss its roots, definition, and associ<strong>at</strong>ionwith critic<strong>al</strong> psychologic<strong>al</strong>, physic<strong>al</strong>, and neurologic<strong>al</strong> processes. Othershave linked mindfulness and <strong>work</strong> (Dane, 2010; Davidson <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2003;Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008; Giluk, 2010), but existingresearch has lacked a coherent theor<strong>et</strong>ic<strong>al</strong> frame<strong>work</strong> th<strong>at</strong> explains why andhow mindfulness might impact employee performance and well-being. Weaim to fill th<strong>at</strong> void.This chapter has four major sections. First, we present a brief history ofmindfulness and a <strong>work</strong>ing definition. Second, we provide a broad overviewof the liter<strong>at</strong>ure regarding outcomes th<strong>at</strong> have been associ<strong>at</strong>ed withmindfulness. Third, we introduce a process model linking mindfulness andmindfulness-based practices with three core and seven secondary processesth<strong>at</strong> explain its effects. In the process section, we explicitly link each processto <strong>work</strong> variables, including job performance and employee well-being. Ourgo<strong>al</strong> is not to link mindfulness to organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> functioning (Weick,Sutcliffe, & Obstfeld, 1999), but r<strong>at</strong>her to show how mindfulness and rel<strong>at</strong>edpractices might affect employees directly, in both task and rel<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong>functioning. Fourth, we focus on three areas where we expect mindfulness to
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 117most strongly affect employees: improved soci<strong>al</strong> rel<strong>at</strong>ionships, resilience,and task performance and decision making. Fin<strong>al</strong>ly, we conclude with adiscussion of the controversy and confusion surrounding mindfulness,suggestions for future research, and practic<strong>al</strong> implic<strong>at</strong>ions for organiz<strong>at</strong>ions.History of <strong>Mindfulness</strong>Rooted in Buddhist philosophy, the concept of mindfulness is the liter<strong>al</strong>transl<strong>at</strong>ion of the Buddhist word s<strong>at</strong>i – ‘‘intentness of mind,’’ ‘‘wakefulnessof mind,’’ and ‘‘lucidity of mind’’ (Davids & Stede, 1959, p. 672) –highlighting intention, awareness, and <strong>at</strong>tention as key constituents ofmindfulness. <strong>Mindfulness</strong> medit<strong>at</strong>ion is <strong>at</strong> the heart of Buddhist traditionand its aim is to deepen conscious awareness of the present moment(Nyanaponika, 1998). Despite these roots, the concept of mindfulness, perse, has no religious connot<strong>at</strong>ion (see Hagen, 2003, for a discussion ofwh<strong>et</strong>her Buddhism is a religion), and mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ive practices arebecoming increasingly popular, not only in Eastern countries but throughoutthe world (Mitchell, 2002).The public has become more aware of mindfulness largely becausepsychologists and medic<strong>al</strong> practitioners have turned to therapeutic use ofmindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion. Over the past three decades, researchers havefrequently examined mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion for its role in <strong>al</strong>levi<strong>at</strong>ingsymptoms of physic<strong>al</strong> and psychologic<strong>al</strong> disorders in clinic<strong>al</strong> popul<strong>at</strong>ions,and as a stress reduction technique in nonclinic<strong>al</strong> popul<strong>at</strong>ions (Chiesa &Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2010; Delmonte, 1990). P<strong>at</strong>ients are trained in mindfulnessmedit<strong>at</strong>ion to heighten their awareness and <strong>at</strong>tention to the present byintention<strong>al</strong>ly orienting them to <strong>at</strong>tend to moment-to-moment stimuli, and toaccept those stimuli without judgment, elabor<strong>at</strong>ion, or <strong>at</strong>tempts to controlthem (e.g., Baer, 2003; Chambers, Gullone, & Allen, 2009).Among the most prominent of the therapeutic mindfulness-basedinterventions is <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR; Kab<strong>at</strong>-Zinn,1990), initi<strong>al</strong>ly developed to assist medic<strong>al</strong> p<strong>at</strong>ients. More than 18,000medic<strong>al</strong> and nonmedic<strong>al</strong> p<strong>at</strong>ients have particip<strong>at</strong>ed in the MBSR program <strong>at</strong>the University of Massachus<strong>et</strong>ts <strong>al</strong>one (Center for <strong>Mindfulness</strong> in Medicine,He<strong>al</strong>th Care, and Soci<strong>et</strong>y, 2010). Clinic<strong>al</strong> ev<strong>al</strong>u<strong>at</strong>ion and academic researchhave established the he<strong>al</strong>th benefits of MBSR in clinic<strong>al</strong> and nonclinic<strong>al</strong>popul<strong>at</strong>ions (for qu<strong>al</strong>it<strong>at</strong>ive reviews, see Baer, 2003; Bishop, 2002; Chiesa &Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2010; for m<strong>et</strong>a-an<strong>al</strong>yses, see Chiesa & Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2009; Grossman,Niemann, Schmidt, & W<strong>al</strong>ach, 2004). Furthermore, thousands of he<strong>al</strong>th-care
118THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.profession<strong>al</strong>s have been trained to teach MBSR techniques (Duerr, 2004b).<strong>Mindfulness</strong> has <strong>al</strong>so emerged as a therapeutic practice in psychology (i.e.,<strong>Mindfulness</strong>-Based Cognitive Therapy [MBCT]; Seg<strong>al</strong>, Williams, &Teasd<strong>al</strong>e, 2002). As a variant of MBSR, MBCT has integr<strong>at</strong>ed componentsof cognitive-behavior<strong>al</strong> therapy (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979) withmindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion. It has been shown effective in reducing clinic<strong>al</strong>symptoms and relapses in p<strong>at</strong>ients with psychi<strong>at</strong>ric disorders (for review, seeBaer, 2003; Chiesa & Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2010). Clinic<strong>al</strong> psychologists have foundmindfulness practices to benefit both clinic<strong>al</strong> popul<strong>at</strong>ions and therapists;practices such as medit<strong>at</strong>ion may lead therapists to feel more emp<strong>at</strong>hytoward p<strong>at</strong>ients (Delmonte, 1990) and ‘‘enjoy their <strong>work</strong> more fully’’(Germer, 2005, p. 11). Preliminary evidence shows th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnessmedit<strong>at</strong>ion training helped psychotherapists achieve significantly b<strong>et</strong>tertre<strong>at</strong>ment results for their p<strong>at</strong>ients (Grepmair, Mitterlehner, Rother, &Nickel, 2006).DefinitionsThe popular press and the scholarly liter<strong>at</strong>ure have both used the termmindfulness to refer to a vari<strong>et</strong>y of rel<strong>at</strong>ed constructs, traits, practices, andprocesses (e.g., Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Kri<strong>et</strong>emeyer, & Toney, 2006; Bishop<strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2004; Brown & Ryan, 2003; Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007; Chiesa &Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2010; Grossman, 2008). Grossman (2008) noted: ‘‘<strong>Mindfulness</strong> is adifficult concept to define, l<strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>one oper<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong>ize’’ (p. 405). Despitesurface-level confusion about the n<strong>at</strong>ure and meaning of mindfulness,agreement exists on its fundament<strong>al</strong> n<strong>at</strong>ure, defined by Brown andcolleagues as ‘‘a receptive <strong>at</strong>tention to and awareness of present events andexperience’’ (Brown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007, p. 212, it<strong>al</strong>ics in origin<strong>al</strong>; see <strong>al</strong>so Brown &Ryan, 2003). Put simply, mindfulness is the process of paying <strong>at</strong>tention towh<strong>at</strong> is happening in the moment – both intern<strong>al</strong> (thoughts, bodilysens<strong>at</strong>ions) and extern<strong>al</strong> stimuli (physic<strong>al</strong> and soci<strong>al</strong> environment) – andobserving those stimuli without judgment or ev<strong>al</strong>u<strong>at</strong>ion, and withoutassigning meaning to them. Basic-level examples of mindfulness includeexperiences such as noticing ‘‘the positions of our hands and the sens<strong>at</strong>ionsof holding a knife and bagel,’’ being aware of ‘‘our bodies sitting in the carwhen we drive,’’ and noticing the traffic, the road, and the passing scenery(Siegel, Germer, & Olendzki, 2009, p. 21). Thus, awareness and <strong>at</strong>tentionare <strong>at</strong> the heart of mindfulness, but mindfulness <strong>al</strong>so involves <strong>at</strong>tendingto stimuli without imposing judgments, memories, or other self-relevant
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 119cognitive manipul<strong>at</strong>ions on them (Brown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007). In the drivingexample, mindfulness involves noticing heavy traffic but refraining fromev<strong>al</strong>u<strong>at</strong>ing it neg<strong>at</strong>ively when it is tied up or moving slowly and fromrumin<strong>at</strong>ing about wh<strong>at</strong> traffic might be like on another route. Our <strong>work</strong>ingdefinition of mindfulness draws heavily from Brown and colleagues (seeBrown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007; Brown & Ryan, 2003). We define mindfulness as ast<strong>at</strong>e of consciousness characterized by receptive <strong>at</strong>tention to and awarenessof present events and experiences, without ev<strong>al</strong>u<strong>at</strong>ion, judgment, andcognitive filters. Our definition clearly establishes mindfulness as a st<strong>at</strong>e ofconsciousness, given empiric<strong>al</strong> evidence of considerable within-individu<strong>al</strong>vari<strong>at</strong>ion in mindfulness over time (Brown & Ryan, 2003) and evidence th<strong>at</strong>mindfulness can be cultiv<strong>at</strong>ed or enhanced through practices and trainingsuch as mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion (see Brown & Ryan, 2003), loving-kindnessmedit<strong>at</strong>ion (see Fredrickson <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2008), and MBSR and MBCT (seeGiluk, 2010).Although we define mindfulness as a st<strong>at</strong>e of consciousness, we <strong>al</strong>sorecognize th<strong>at</strong> the average frequency with which individu<strong>al</strong>s experiencest<strong>at</strong>es of mindfulness may vary from person to person, suggesting th<strong>at</strong>people may have trait-like tendencies toward mindfulness (see Brown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>.,2007; Brown & Cordon, 2009; Brown & Ryan, 2003); indeed one line ofresearch tre<strong>at</strong>s mindfulness as a stable individu<strong>al</strong> difference (i.e., traitmindfulness) similar to other person<strong>al</strong>ity traits (e.g., Brown & Ryan, 2003;Cardaciotto, Herbert, Forman, Moitra, & Farrow, 2008; Lakey, Campbell,Brown, & Goodie, 2007; W<strong>al</strong>sh, B<strong>al</strong>int, Smolira SJ, Fredericksen, &Madsen, 2009; Way, Creswell, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2010).The liter<strong>at</strong>ure has <strong>al</strong>so examined mindfulness training as a therapeutictechnique (e.g., MBSR; Kab<strong>at</strong>-Zinn, 1990, or MBCT; Seg<strong>al</strong> <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2002) th<strong>at</strong>aims to improve the capacity to cre<strong>at</strong>e more mindful st<strong>at</strong>es. One centr<strong>al</strong>element of these programs is mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion. Our definitionsuggests th<strong>at</strong> none of these programs nor the mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion theyincorpor<strong>at</strong>e are mindfulness, but r<strong>at</strong>her medit<strong>at</strong>ion is a technique used todevelop mindfulness (see <strong>al</strong>so Brown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007; Grossman, 2008).Moreover, we note th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion is just one specific type ofmedit<strong>at</strong>ion and differs from other practices such as concentr<strong>at</strong>ive medit<strong>at</strong>ionth<strong>at</strong> requires focused <strong>at</strong>tention on a single stimulus such as a word, sound,or candle (see Goleman, 1977; but see <strong>al</strong>so Cahn & Polich, 2006; Germer,2005; Lutz, Slagter, Dunne, & Davidson, 2008, for the counterargumentth<strong>at</strong> concentr<strong>at</strong>ive medit<strong>at</strong>ion may <strong>al</strong>so develop mindfulness). Thus,mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion, but perhaps not <strong>al</strong>l medit<strong>at</strong>ion, develops themindfulness st<strong>at</strong>e of nonjudgment<strong>al</strong> awareness of and <strong>at</strong>tention to intern<strong>al</strong>
120THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.and extern<strong>al</strong> stimuli. In our review of the liter<strong>at</strong>ure, we use the termmindfulness tre<strong>at</strong>ment to refer to mindfulness-based therapeutic programsdesigned to develop the ability to achieve mindful st<strong>at</strong>es (e.g., MBSR), th<strong>et</strong>erm mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion to refer to mindfulness-based medit<strong>at</strong>ionpractices, and the term trait mindfulness to refer to stable individu<strong>al</strong>differences in mindfulness.Despite our efforts to clarify the mindfulness liter<strong>at</strong>ure, we acknowledg<strong>et</strong>he difficulty of making clean distinctions. For example, it is not clearwh<strong>et</strong>her studies of the brain, which document changes in brain activitiesduring mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion, are documenting the effects of mindfulnessmedit<strong>at</strong>ion practices or the st<strong>at</strong>e of mindfulness achieved during medit<strong>at</strong>ion,or both.There is <strong>al</strong>so an important stream of mindfulness research in thepsychologic<strong>al</strong> and organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> liter<strong>at</strong>ure th<strong>at</strong> does not explicitly haveroots in Buddhist philosophy. Langer (1989a) defined mindfulness as an‘‘active inform<strong>at</strong>ion processing’’ mode (p. 138). Although research based onLanger’s <strong>work</strong> uses the term mindfulness, her concept, <strong>al</strong>though rel<strong>at</strong>ed,appears to be a distinct phenomenon from our definition of mindfulness asnonjudgment<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tention to and awareness of intern<strong>al</strong> and extern<strong>al</strong> stimuli.Both approaches focus on ongoing awareness of and <strong>at</strong>tention to stimuli butdiverge in considering wh<strong>at</strong> individu<strong>al</strong>s do with observ<strong>at</strong>ions. R<strong>at</strong>her thanobserving without judgment, Langer’s conceptu<strong>al</strong>iz<strong>at</strong>ion explicitly includesdeliber<strong>at</strong>e cognitive c<strong>at</strong>egoriz<strong>at</strong>ion, gener<strong>at</strong>ing new distinctions, andadapting to changing situ<strong>at</strong>ions (see Brown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007; Brown & Ryan,2003; Langer, 1989b, for a discussion of overlap and distinction of the twomindfulness concepts).Langer’s <strong>work</strong> on mindfulness is germane because it parti<strong>al</strong>ly forms thebasis of Weick and colleagues’ theorizing about collective mindfulness inhigh-reliability organiz<strong>at</strong>ions (Weick <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 1999). These authors draw onboth Langer (1989a) and Buddhist mindfulness, and define collectivemindfulness as an organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> level <strong>at</strong>tribute th<strong>at</strong> involves ‘‘a richawareness of discrimin<strong>at</strong>ory d<strong>et</strong>ail and a capacity for action’’ (Weick <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>.,1999, p. 88; see <strong>al</strong>so Weick & Putnam, 2006). Collective mindfulness isconstrued as the result of a s<strong>et</strong> of organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> practices and processesaimed <strong>at</strong> observing, c<strong>at</strong>egorizing, and responding to unexpected events anderrors and it is fundament<strong>al</strong> to high-reliability organiz<strong>at</strong>ions; (Weick <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>.,1999). Recent <strong>work</strong> on collective mindfulness (Weick & Sutcliffe, 2006,2007) has shifted somewh<strong>at</strong> from a focus on action capacities to awareness.Although there are touchpoints with our conceptu<strong>al</strong>iz<strong>at</strong>ion of mindfulness,Weick and colleagues’ <strong>work</strong> on collective mindfulness oper<strong>at</strong>es <strong>at</strong> the
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 121organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> level and is distinct from the individu<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>e levelphenomenon of interest here.Positive Effects of <strong>Mindfulness</strong>Wh<strong>at</strong> benefits have been established regarding mindfulness and mindfulness-basedpractices and therapies? First, a burgeoning body of researchhas reported clear links b<strong>et</strong>ween mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion, mindfulnesstre<strong>at</strong>ment, and improved physic<strong>al</strong> he<strong>al</strong>th. Most of this research has focusedon reducing symptoms or distress caused by physic<strong>al</strong> disease. Research hasshown th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness tre<strong>at</strong>ment (i.e., MBSR, MBCT, and their variants)can reduce pain, decrease symptoms (e.g., Carmody & Baer, 2008; Ljo´tsson<strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010), and increase over<strong>al</strong>l physic<strong>al</strong> he<strong>al</strong>th in clinic<strong>al</strong> popul<strong>at</strong>ions withvarious he<strong>al</strong>th ch<strong>al</strong>lenges (for qu<strong>al</strong>it<strong>at</strong>ive review, see Baer, 2003; for m<strong>et</strong>aan<strong>al</strong>ysis,see Grossman <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2004). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> tre<strong>at</strong>ments have <strong>al</strong>so beenlinked to higher mel<strong>at</strong>onin levels (an indic<strong>at</strong>or of immune function) innonclinic<strong>al</strong> popul<strong>at</strong>ions (for review, see Baer, 2003). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> medit<strong>at</strong>ionhas been associ<strong>at</strong>ed with decreased som<strong>at</strong>ic he<strong>al</strong>th complaints (Delgado<strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010) and improvements in an array of physiologic<strong>al</strong> markers includingincreased cardiac respir<strong>at</strong>ory sinus arrhythmia (RSA; Ditto, Eclache, &Goldman, 2006), increased cardiac output (Ditto <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2006), loweredrespir<strong>at</strong>ory r<strong>at</strong>e (Delgado <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010), and decreased blood pressure (forqu<strong>al</strong>it<strong>at</strong>ive review, see Chiesa & Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2010) across clinic<strong>al</strong> and he<strong>al</strong>thypopul<strong>at</strong>ions. Evidence from labor<strong>at</strong>ory s<strong>et</strong>tings has suggested th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnessis associ<strong>at</strong>ed with decreased unpleasantness and sensitivity to painfulstimuli (Grant & Rainville, 2009; Perlman, S<strong>al</strong>omons, Davidson, & Lutz,2010).Second, mindfulness and mindfulness-based practices have been clearlylinked to reduced symptoms of ment<strong>al</strong>, psychologic<strong>al</strong>, and psychi<strong>at</strong>ricconditions. Ment<strong>al</strong> he<strong>al</strong>th benefits include decreased anxi<strong>et</strong>y (e.g., Biegel,Brown, Shapiro, & Schubert, 2009), depression (e.g., Foley, Baillie, Huxter,Price, & Sinclair, 2010), stress (e.g., Bra¨nstro¨m, Kvillemo, Brandberg, &Moskowitz, 2010), psychologic<strong>al</strong> distress (e.g., Foley <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010), and over<strong>al</strong>lpsychologic<strong>al</strong> symptoms (e.g., Carmody & Baer, 2008). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>tre<strong>at</strong>ments have <strong>al</strong>so been associ<strong>at</strong>ed with reduced anxi<strong>et</strong>y and depressionin individu<strong>al</strong>s with chronic conditions such as pain disorders, cancer,diab<strong>et</strong>es, rheum<strong>at</strong>oid arthritis, and heart disease (for qu<strong>al</strong>it<strong>at</strong>ive review, seeBaer, 2003; Chiesa & Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2010; for m<strong>et</strong>a-an<strong>al</strong>ysis, see Bohlmeijer,Prenger, Ta<strong>al</strong>, & Cuijpers, 2010; Grossman <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2004; Hofmann, Sawyer,
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 123affective st<strong>at</strong>es, and the absence of <strong>al</strong>pha-blocking habitu<strong>at</strong>ion, whichindic<strong>at</strong>es mindful awareness of stimuli (for review, see Cahn & Polich, 2006;Chiesa & Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2010; Treadway & Lazar, 2009).Other research has used function<strong>al</strong> magn<strong>et</strong>ic resonance imaging (fMRI)techniques to examine activ<strong>at</strong>ion of specific brain regions during mindfulnessmedit<strong>at</strong>ion and enduring brain structure changes in experiencedmedit<strong>at</strong>ors. Changes observed during mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion includeactiv<strong>at</strong>ion in the areas of the brain associ<strong>at</strong>ed with emotion<strong>al</strong> regul<strong>at</strong>ion,<strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> regul<strong>at</strong>ion, enhanced <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> focus, and heightened awarenessof intern<strong>al</strong> bodily sens<strong>at</strong>ions (for review, see Cahn & Polich, 2006;Chiesa & Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2010; Treadway & Lazar, 2009). Researchers have foundth<strong>at</strong> long-term medit<strong>at</strong>ors show increased thickness of brain regions (e.g.,middle prefront<strong>al</strong> cortex [mPFC]) associ<strong>at</strong>ed with intern<strong>al</strong> awareness and<strong>at</strong>tention (for review, see Chiesa & Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, 2010; Treadway & Lazar, 2009)and areas associ<strong>at</strong>ed with reduced pain sensitivity (Grant, Courtemanche,Duerden, Duncan, & Rainville, 2010). Trait mindfulness has been linked tothe brain’s neur<strong>al</strong> activities. Recent fMRI studies found trait mindfulness isassoci<strong>at</strong>ed with increased prefront<strong>al</strong> cortex (PFC) activity (Creswell, Way,Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007; Frewen <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010) and decreasedamygd<strong>al</strong>a activity (Creswell <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007; cf. Frewen <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010) duringaffect-rel<strong>at</strong>ed tasks, suggesting b<strong>et</strong>ter affective regul<strong>at</strong>ion among individu<strong>al</strong>shigh in disposition<strong>al</strong> mindfulness, which may explain why such individu<strong>al</strong>sexperience less depression (Way <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010). Thus, neurobiology researchsuggests mindfulness-rel<strong>at</strong>ed changes in brain activities and structures arerel<strong>at</strong>ed to heightened awareness, positive ment<strong>al</strong> experiences, and <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong>,affective, and physiologic<strong>al</strong> regul<strong>at</strong>ion.CORE PROCESSES LINKING MINDFULNESS ANDSELF-REGULATIONAs discussed in the preceding text, a large and growing body of liter<strong>at</strong>ureaffirms th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness and mindfulness-based practices have benefici<strong>al</strong>effects for a vari<strong>et</strong>y of outcomes in clinic<strong>al</strong> and nonclinic<strong>al</strong> popul<strong>at</strong>ions. Y<strong>et</strong>scholars and researchers have only recently begun to carefully examine theprocess and mechanisms behind these effects. Reviews of the mindfulnessliter<strong>at</strong>ure converge in identifying the centr<strong>al</strong> outcome of mindfulness:improved self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion of thoughts, emotions, behaviors, and physiologic<strong>al</strong>reactions. As our go<strong>al</strong> here is to explore the potenti<strong>al</strong> effects of mindfulness
124THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.on employees’ functioning <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>, we draw from neuroscience, psychology,and medicine to develop propositions about how mindfulness and mindfulness-basedpractices will influence <strong>work</strong> behaviors, performance, andwell-being. Although there is considerable agreement th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnessimproves self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion, our go<strong>al</strong> is to dig more deeply into the processes bywhich th<strong>at</strong> occurs.In Fig. 1, we present a model linking mindfulness to its outcomes, via a seriesof core and secondary ment<strong>al</strong> and neurobiologic<strong>al</strong> processes. We identify twocore ment<strong>al</strong> processes and one core neurobiologic<strong>al</strong> process th<strong>at</strong> are affectedby mindfulness: (a) a decoupling of the self (i.e., ego) from events, experiences,thoughts, and emotions; (b) a decrease in autom<strong>at</strong>icity of ment<strong>al</strong> processes inwhich past experiences, schemas, and cognitive habits constrain thinking; and(c) increased awareness and regul<strong>at</strong>ion of physiologic<strong>al</strong> systems. In addition tothese three core processes, we identify seven addition<strong>al</strong>, secondary processesby which mindfulness-based practices are expected to improve employeefunctioning: (a) decreased rumin<strong>at</strong>ion, (b) gre<strong>at</strong>er emp<strong>at</strong>hy, (c) increasedresponse flexibility, (d) improved affective regul<strong>at</strong>ion, (e) increased selfd<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ionand gre<strong>at</strong>er persistence (f) enhanced <strong>work</strong>ing memory, and (g)gre<strong>at</strong>er accuracy in affective forecasting. We suggest th<strong>at</strong>, in concert, thes<strong>et</strong>hree core and seven secondary processes form a series of p<strong>at</strong>hways by whichmindfulness and mindfulness-based practices lead to improved self-regul<strong>at</strong>ionand, ultim<strong>at</strong>ely, higher functioning.As we describe the secondary processes, we provide evidence from theliter<strong>at</strong>ure linking them to mindfulness, as well as outlining their expected<strong>work</strong>-rel<strong>at</strong>ed outcomes. We <strong>al</strong>so draw on interviews the first three authorsconducted as part of a larger mindfulness research project. Through a loc<strong>al</strong><strong>Mindfulness</strong> & <strong>Mindfulness</strong>‐based PracticesDecoupling of Self from Experiences & EmotionsDecreased Use of Autom<strong>at</strong>ic Ment<strong>al</strong> ProcessesAwareness of Physiologic<strong>al</strong> Regul<strong>at</strong>ionResponse Flexibility. . . Decreased Rumin<strong>at</strong>ion . . .Emp<strong>at</strong>hy . . . Affective Regul<strong>at</strong>ion . . .Increased Self‐d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion and PersistenceIncreased Working Memory. . . More accur<strong>at</strong>e affective forecastingImproved Self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion of Thoughts\Emotions\BehaviorFig. 1.Core and Secondary Processes Linking <strong>Mindfulness</strong> to Self-Regul<strong>at</strong>ion.
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 125medit<strong>at</strong>ion center, we recruited 20 employed individu<strong>al</strong>s who had beenregularly practicing mindfulness-based practices for <strong>at</strong> least 1 year. In onehourinterviews, we asked participants how their mindfulness-basedpractices influenced their <strong>at</strong>titudes, emotions, thoughts, and behaviors onthe job. We use observ<strong>at</strong>ions from three of these interviews to illustr<strong>at</strong>e thesecondary processes in action.Decoupling of the Self from Experiences, Events, and Ment<strong>al</strong> ProcessesOne of the key mechanisms by which mindfulness affects self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion is bycre<strong>at</strong>ing a separ<strong>at</strong>ion b<strong>et</strong>ween the self (e.g., ego, self-esteem, self-concept) andevents, emotions, and experiences. <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-based practices aim to trainindividu<strong>al</strong>s to observe intern<strong>al</strong> and extern<strong>al</strong> stimuli objectively, cre<strong>at</strong>ing m<strong>et</strong>aawareness.As such, most mindfulness training involves noticing, observing,and naming stimuli without ev<strong>al</strong>u<strong>at</strong>ing or assigning meaning to them. Thisleads individu<strong>al</strong>s to cre<strong>at</strong>e distance b<strong>et</strong>ween themselves (and their self-worth)and their thoughts, emotions, and experiences. This process has <strong>al</strong>so beendescribed as ‘‘de-centering’’ in which one ‘‘view[s] thoughts as events in themind r<strong>at</strong>her than necessarily being reflections of re<strong>al</strong>ity or accur<strong>at</strong>e self-view’’(Feldman, Greeson, & Senville, 2010, p. 1002).When ego involvement in a neg<strong>at</strong>ive situ<strong>at</strong>ion is high, people feel their selfworthis under <strong>at</strong>tack (Kernis, Paradise, Whitaker, Whe<strong>at</strong>man, & Goldman,2000), but when the ego is separ<strong>at</strong>ed from events, neg<strong>at</strong>ive events aredecoupled from the self and become less thre<strong>at</strong>ening. For example, before animportant s<strong>al</strong>es present<strong>at</strong>ion, a s<strong>al</strong>esperson might interpr<strong>et</strong> thoughts aboutwh<strong>at</strong> could go wrong or potenti<strong>al</strong> failure as ‘‘just those nerves t<strong>al</strong>king,’’ r<strong>at</strong>herthan as a v<strong>al</strong>id indic<strong>at</strong>ion of inadequacy. The liter<strong>at</strong>ure has supported the ide<strong>at</strong>h<strong>at</strong> mindfulness and mindfulness-based practices are associ<strong>at</strong>ed with adecoupling of the self from experiences. Hargus, Crane, Barnhofer, andWilliams (2010) found th<strong>at</strong> 8 weeks of mindfulness training was associ<strong>at</strong>edwith a significant improvement in m<strong>et</strong>a-awareness, demonstr<strong>at</strong>ing th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnesscan help people ‘‘learn to uncouple the sensory, directly experienced selffrom the ‘narr<strong>at</strong>ive’ self’’ (Williams, 2010, p. 1). Preliminary evidence hasindic<strong>at</strong>ed th<strong>at</strong> even temporarily heightened mindfulness (via experiment<strong>al</strong>manipul<strong>at</strong>ion) increases separ<strong>at</strong>ion b<strong>et</strong>ween self-worth and experiencessuch as interperson<strong>al</strong> rejection (Heppner & Kernis, 2007). Althoughexisting neurobiologic<strong>al</strong> research has not y<strong>et</strong> explicitly tested the decouplinghypothesis, Davidson (2010) suggested th<strong>at</strong> the brains of mindfulnesspractitioners might reve<strong>al</strong> ‘‘decreased connectivity b<strong>et</strong>ween emotion
126THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.processing and self-relevant processing’’ (p. 10) areas of the brain. Forexample, one study found th<strong>at</strong> when subjects were exposed to a manipul<strong>at</strong>iondesigned to induce sadness, those who were trained in mindfulness techniquesreported just as much sadness as others, but their fMRI scans showed lessactivity in the brain regions associ<strong>at</strong>ed with self-referenti<strong>al</strong> processing. Thesedifferences in neur<strong>al</strong> p<strong>at</strong>terns ‘‘may stem from the objectific<strong>at</strong>ion of emotionsas innocuous sensory inform<strong>at</strong>ion r<strong>at</strong>her than as affect laden thre<strong>at</strong>s to theself’’ (Farb <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010, p. 31).Decreased Use of Autom<strong>at</strong>ic Ment<strong>al</strong> ProcessesA second core process by which mindfulness leads to improved selfregul<strong>at</strong>ionis via decreased autom<strong>at</strong>icity of ment<strong>al</strong> processes. Through theprocess of autom<strong>at</strong>icity, engrained brain st<strong>at</strong>es mold awareness of presentdayexperiences (Siegel, 2007). Autom<strong>at</strong>icity of thinking can arise fromdifferent sources including prior experiences, entrenched ment<strong>al</strong> models, andbodily responses based on prior experiences. Offering individu<strong>al</strong>s a clearsurviv<strong>al</strong> benefit (i.e., quick inform<strong>at</strong>ion processing and responses),autom<strong>at</strong>icity has the unfortun<strong>at</strong>e consequence of restricting individu<strong>al</strong>s’perceptions and experiences of the present moment. In a sense, autom<strong>at</strong>icityhijacks the ability to fully experience the present moment. Althoughautom<strong>at</strong>icity provides ment<strong>al</strong> efficiency, it diminishes present-momentawareness, control, and intent (Bargh, 1994). For example, we may findourselves responding to colleagues without re<strong>al</strong>ly listening to the convers<strong>at</strong>ionbecause we believe we <strong>al</strong>ready know where the convers<strong>at</strong>ion is going.We may compl<strong>et</strong>e a task without rec<strong>al</strong>ling actu<strong>al</strong>ly doing it or head to thestore to g<strong>et</strong> groceries and end up in the parking lot <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>, <strong>al</strong>l because ofdeeply ingrained, autom<strong>at</strong>ic responses.Engaging in mindfulness disrupts autom<strong>at</strong>icity of thought p<strong>at</strong>terns andhabits as individu<strong>al</strong>s move from heuristic modes of inform<strong>at</strong>ion processingto more system<strong>at</strong>ic modes (Chaiken, 1980). Through the simple act ofobserving thoughts arising in the mind, mindful nonjudging awareness<strong>al</strong>lows one to disengage from: (a) autom<strong>at</strong>ic thought p<strong>at</strong>terns and (b)perceptu<strong>al</strong> filtering driven by emotions and schemas from the past (Siegel,2007). As Shapiro and colleagues noted, ‘‘We experience wh<strong>at</strong> is instead of acommentary or story about wh<strong>at</strong> is’’ (Shapiro, Carlson, Astin, & Freedman,2006, p. 379, it<strong>al</strong>ics in origin<strong>al</strong>).As a result, the range of responses is increased because responses are nolonger constrained by autom<strong>at</strong>icity. Mindful awareness <strong>al</strong>lows individu<strong>al</strong>s to
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 127consciously sense and shape their thoughts and to have gre<strong>at</strong>er cognitiveflexibility in response to thoughts (Siegel, 2010). The flipside of reducedautom<strong>at</strong>icity is th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness may promote ‘‘rel<strong>at</strong>ively more thorough<strong>at</strong>tention to the extern<strong>al</strong> environment’’ (Herndon, 2008, p. 33). For example,Radel, Sarrazin, Legrain, and Gobance (2009) showed th<strong>at</strong> students higherin trait mindfulness were immune to a motiv<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> manipul<strong>at</strong>ion usingunconscious priming; they were less influenced by autom<strong>at</strong>ic processes.Awareness of Physiologic<strong>al</strong> Regul<strong>at</strong>ionThe third major process through which mindfulness influences self-regul<strong>at</strong>ionis through gener<strong>at</strong>ing bodily awareness and concomitant physiologic<strong>al</strong>regul<strong>at</strong>ion (Siegel, 2010). Much like the process of ‘‘thought observ<strong>at</strong>ion’’described in the preceding text, present-moment nonjudgment<strong>al</strong> awarenessof one’s physiologic<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>e promotes a more b<strong>al</strong>anced regul<strong>at</strong>ion of thebody’s physiologic<strong>al</strong> response systems (e.g., approach–avoidance, fight–flight,inhibition–activ<strong>at</strong>ion systems). Coordin<strong>at</strong>ed by the brain’s mPFC via thesymp<strong>at</strong>h<strong>et</strong>ic (activ<strong>at</strong>ion) and parasymp<strong>at</strong>h<strong>et</strong>ic (inhibition) nervous system,these physiologic<strong>al</strong> response systems are designed to <strong>work</strong> in b<strong>al</strong>ance andcoordin<strong>at</strong>ion with another. When physiologic<strong>al</strong> activ<strong>at</strong>ion reaches too high <strong>at</strong>hreshold, the mPFC is overloaded and unable to modul<strong>at</strong>e the firing of thelimbic system, which leads to myriad undesirable affective (i.e., anger, anxi<strong>et</strong>y)and physic<strong>al</strong> consequences (i.e., heart p<strong>al</strong>pit<strong>at</strong>ions, gastrointestin<strong>al</strong> distress)(Siegel, 2007). As Siegel (2010) noted, without the coordin<strong>at</strong>ion of theactiv<strong>at</strong>ion and inhibition systems people are likely to ‘‘burn out, revving up’’when they need to slow down (p. 27). When b<strong>al</strong>anced, however, these brainsystems can gener<strong>at</strong>e feelings of c<strong>al</strong>m, connection, and physic<strong>al</strong> well-being(Cozolino, 2006). Thus, increased <strong>at</strong>tention to and awareness of the body’sphysiologic<strong>al</strong> response systems can help individu<strong>al</strong>s b<strong>et</strong>ter interpr<strong>et</strong> andrespond to messages from the body.SECONDARY MINDFULNESS PROCESSESAs presented in Fig. 1, the ment<strong>al</strong> and neurobiologic<strong>al</strong> processes associ<strong>at</strong>edwith mindfulness and mindfulness-based practices lead to more dist<strong>al</strong>processes th<strong>at</strong> influence employees’ ability to effectively regul<strong>at</strong>e theirthoughts, behaviors, and emotions <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>. In this section, we turn ourfocus explicitly to these processes, with a speci<strong>al</strong> emphasis on how these
128THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.processes might be expected to affect employee performance and well-being<strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>. We explicitly link each process to one or more of the core processesdescribed in the preceding section. We provide empiric<strong>al</strong> evidence for eachprocess, suggest ways in which the process would affect employees’functioning <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>, and, where available, provide illustr<strong>at</strong>ions of theseprocesses from our interviews. In Table 1, we provide a summary of theTable 1.Potenti<strong>al</strong> Effects of Secondary Processes of <strong>Mindfulness</strong> onEmployee Performance and Well-Being.<strong>Mindfulness</strong>-Based ProcessPossible Work-Rel<strong>at</strong>ed EffectsResponse flexibility Improved decision making Improved communic<strong>at</strong>ionDecreased rumin<strong>at</strong>ion Improved coping with stressful events Faster recovery from neg<strong>at</strong>ive events Increased confidence and self-efficacy B<strong>et</strong>ter problem solving Improved concentr<strong>at</strong>ions More effective use of soci<strong>al</strong> supportEmp<strong>at</strong>hy Increased interaction<strong>al</strong> and inform<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> justice Reduced antisoci<strong>al</strong> behavior Increased organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> citizenship behaviors Positive leadership behaviorsAffective regul<strong>at</strong>ion Improved communic<strong>at</strong>ion Improved coping with stressful events Faster recovery from neg<strong>at</strong>ive events Fewer accidentsIncreased self-d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion andpersistence Increased go<strong>al</strong>-directed effort Improved task performance Gre<strong>at</strong>er learning Increased job s<strong>at</strong>isfaction Increased organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> commitment Increased performance on cre<strong>at</strong>ive tasksIncreased <strong>work</strong>ing memory Reduced neg<strong>at</strong>ive affect Improved ability to handle multiple demands Ability to perform under stressMore accur<strong>at</strong>e affective forecasting Less biased decision making More accur<strong>at</strong>e expect<strong>at</strong>ions Less frustr<strong>at</strong>ion and neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotion
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 129cognitive and emotion<strong>al</strong> process rel<strong>at</strong>ed to mindfulness and their proposed<strong>work</strong>-rel<strong>at</strong>ed effects.Response Flexibility[During a medit<strong>at</strong>ion r<strong>et</strong>re<strong>at</strong>] my teacher was t<strong>al</strong>king about a kind of reptilian inheritedkind of restlessness – jump! jump! jump! jump! – th<strong>at</strong> we probably inherited because weneeded to, and I tend not to respond to th<strong>at</strong> y which I think is wise. Now when someonecomes to me with som<strong>et</strong>hing [<strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>], instead of giving a fast glance I find myselfstaring a lot <strong>at</strong> people y just kind of slowing down, you know y and I try to come tosome wisdom before I answer y and if th<strong>at</strong> turns out not to be wise, or not wisdom, thenswitching it. But I don’t think I’m nearly as impulsive as I would be if I didn’t practice,th<strong>at</strong>’s for sure.– <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Medit<strong>at</strong>or (Participant #3)Response flexibility can be defined as the ability to pause before takingverb<strong>al</strong> or physic<strong>al</strong> action (Siegel, 2007). Response flexibility occurs when oneis able to pause before responding to an environment<strong>al</strong> stimulus. In thewords of our study participant, response flexibility is characterized by a‘‘slowing down’’ and deeper consider<strong>at</strong>ion of the situ<strong>at</strong>ion (‘‘come to somewisdom’’) before responding to <strong>work</strong>place events and interactions. Allowingtime and space to reflect and consider multiple, nonautom<strong>at</strong>ic ways ofresponding offers more opportunities for optim<strong>al</strong> outcomes and functioning.R<strong>at</strong>her than responding to <strong>work</strong>place events habitu<strong>al</strong>ly and invariantly,response flexibility <strong>al</strong>lows one the power to act in <strong>al</strong>ignment with one’sgo<strong>al</strong>s, needs, and v<strong>al</strong>ues (Brown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007). As our interviewee indic<strong>at</strong>ed,mindfulness promotes a slowing down of one’s response and mor<strong>et</strong>houghtful consider<strong>at</strong>ion of how (and wh<strong>et</strong>her) to react to <strong>work</strong> eventsr<strong>at</strong>her than ‘‘jump’’ impulsively and reactively.A growing body of evidence suggests th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness plays a significantrole in heightened response flexibility across a vari<strong>et</strong>y of situ<strong>at</strong>ions rangingfrom gambling to interperson<strong>al</strong> communic<strong>at</strong>ion (e.g., Bishop <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2004;Ch<strong>at</strong>zisarantis & Hagger, 2007; Lakey <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007; Wenk-Sormaz, 2005).Responding in a flexible manner requires not only a delay in response but<strong>al</strong>so a careful assessment of the situ<strong>at</strong>ion, the available response options, aswell as an ultim<strong>at</strong>e initi<strong>at</strong>ion of action (Siegel, 2007). This type of executiveself-control is initi<strong>at</strong>ed in large part by the mPFC, which, as noted in thepreceding text, is activ<strong>at</strong>ed by mindfulness. All three core processesdeline<strong>at</strong>ed in the preceding text appear to play a role in gener<strong>at</strong>ing acapacity for flexible responding. Physiologic<strong>al</strong> regul<strong>at</strong>ion and awareness<strong>al</strong>lows one to assess environment<strong>al</strong> stimuli without experiencing
130THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.physiologic<strong>al</strong> activ<strong>at</strong>ion of the fight-or-flight response system in which highlevels of physiologic<strong>al</strong> arous<strong>al</strong> overload the mPFC and override the abilityto think and to choose reactions (Cozolino, 2006). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>al</strong>sofacilit<strong>at</strong>es response flexibility through the nonreactive, nonjudging aspectsth<strong>at</strong> characterize two of our other core processes – decoupling and decreasedautom<strong>at</strong>icity. Through decoupling and decreased autom<strong>at</strong>icity, individu<strong>al</strong>srecognize th<strong>at</strong> thoughts and reactions to an event are not an objective re<strong>al</strong>ityrequiring immedi<strong>at</strong>e <strong>al</strong>ter<strong>at</strong>ion or response (Chambers <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2009).As such, the range and optimiz<strong>at</strong>ion of possible behavior<strong>al</strong> responsesgrows. In the <strong>work</strong>place, we suspect th<strong>at</strong> increased response flexibility wouldcontribute to a more productive environment in a vari<strong>et</strong>y of ways includingfewer instances of esc<strong>al</strong><strong>at</strong>ing conflict and displaced aggression in respons<strong>et</strong>o perceived thre<strong>at</strong>s and disagreements and improved decision makingbecause reactive decision making would be less likely (i.e., esc<strong>al</strong><strong>at</strong>ion ofcommitment).Decreased Rumin<strong>at</strong>ionI find th<strong>at</strong> medit<strong>at</strong>ion l<strong>et</strong>s you just have an emotion, and it’s so hard to not g<strong>et</strong> caught upin them. But you can take a bre<strong>at</strong>h and step back and say ‘‘Oh! I’m feeling re<strong>al</strong>ly angry!’’And a lot of times th<strong>at</strong> l<strong>et</strong>s you do som<strong>et</strong>hing different and not just do your habitu<strong>al</strong>response. It’s helped with my emotions quite a bit y But it’s <strong>al</strong>so helped me be aware ofthought p<strong>at</strong>terns th<strong>at</strong> keep occurring y th<strong>at</strong> you re<strong>al</strong>ly g<strong>et</strong> trapped in. And you canspend a lot of time there if you can’t step back and say ‘‘Oh! Here I am having thisargument with myself again!’’ And I find with both the emotion<strong>al</strong> kind of habits andthought habits y I think it re<strong>al</strong>ly helps me to just stop, step back and see it, you know?– <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Medit<strong>at</strong>or (Participant #1)When individu<strong>al</strong>s are confronted with events th<strong>at</strong> would norm<strong>al</strong>lyprovoke neg<strong>at</strong>ive thought p<strong>at</strong>terns, a mindful orient<strong>at</strong>ion makes them lesslikely to engage in rumin<strong>at</strong>ion – a rep<strong>et</strong>itive and passive focus on symptoms,causes, and consequences of distress (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). Becausemindfulness leads to a separ<strong>at</strong>ion of the self from the experience or emotion,and because it reduces autom<strong>at</strong>ic responding, individu<strong>al</strong>s who practicemindfulness engage in less rumin<strong>at</strong>ion, leading them to b<strong>et</strong>ter cope withstressful events (Broderick, 2005).Absent the power of mindfulness to decouple and reduce autom<strong>at</strong>icity,rumin<strong>at</strong>ion will follow certain stimuli as individu<strong>al</strong>s <strong>at</strong>tempt to make senseof and resolve discrepancies b<strong>et</strong>ween wh<strong>at</strong> is happening and wh<strong>at</strong> they desir<strong>et</strong>o happen. As our interviewee noted, rumin<strong>at</strong>ion can ‘‘trap’’ one in a spir<strong>al</strong>of neg<strong>at</strong>ive and unproductive thoughts. In a mindful st<strong>at</strong>e, individu<strong>al</strong>s are
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 131aware of their thoughts, but can separ<strong>at</strong>e them from their self-view andavoid ev<strong>al</strong>u<strong>at</strong>ing their thoughts as good or bad. Indeed, more adaptivecoping with change or with adverse experiences <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong> was one of thecommon themes th<strong>at</strong> emerged in our interviews.In terms of the empiric<strong>al</strong> evidence, a clear link exists b<strong>et</strong>ween mindfulnessand mindfulness-based techniques and decreased rumin<strong>at</strong>ion among clinic<strong>al</strong>and nonclinic<strong>al</strong> popul<strong>at</strong>ions. Research has suggested th<strong>at</strong> individu<strong>al</strong>s whoparticip<strong>at</strong>ed in mindfulness programs reported less rumin<strong>at</strong>ive thinking inresponse to life events (Ramel, Goldin, Carmona, & McQuaid, 2004), evenwhen the events were similar to those experienced by others who had nomindfulness training (Goldin & Gross, 2010). Trait mindfulness <strong>al</strong>so hasbeen associ<strong>at</strong>ed with less rumin<strong>at</strong>ion. Frewen, Evans, Maraj, Dozois, andPartridge (2008) found associ<strong>at</strong>ions b<strong>et</strong>ween trait mindfulness [as measuredby the Mindful Attention and Awareness Sc<strong>al</strong>e (MAAS; Brown & Ryan,2003) and the Kentucky Inventory of <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Skills (KIMS; Baer,Smith, & Allen, 2004)] and less difficulty ‘‘l<strong>et</strong>ting go’’ of neg<strong>at</strong>ive autom<strong>at</strong>icthinking (depressive, worry, or soci<strong>al</strong> fear cognitions). Verplanken, Friborg,Wang, Trafimow, and Woolf (2007) found moder<strong>at</strong>e neg<strong>at</strong>ive correl<strong>at</strong>ionsb<strong>et</strong>ween the MAAS and a sc<strong>al</strong>e of habitu<strong>al</strong> neg<strong>at</strong>ive thinking as well asrumin<strong>at</strong>ion.Individu<strong>al</strong>s who engage in rumin<strong>at</strong>ing thought p<strong>at</strong>terns are <strong>at</strong> gre<strong>at</strong>er riskfor poor concentr<strong>at</strong>ion (Ingram & Smith, 1984), depressed mood (Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991), low self-efficacy (Brockner & Hulton, 1978)and are more likely to <strong>al</strong>ien<strong>at</strong>e those who might provide soci<strong>al</strong> support(Nolen-Hoeksema & Davis, 1999). Conversely, individu<strong>al</strong>s who are lessprone to rumin<strong>at</strong>ion after stressful events report fewer <strong>work</strong>-rel<strong>at</strong>ed he<strong>al</strong>thcomplaints. Accordingly, we suggest th<strong>at</strong> a reduction in rumin<strong>at</strong>ionresulting from mindfulness will have broad ranging effects on employees’performance and well-being, via improved confidence, b<strong>et</strong>ter problemsolving, more effective use of soci<strong>al</strong> support mechanisms, and b<strong>et</strong>terconcentr<strong>at</strong>ion. In addition, a reduction in rumin<strong>at</strong>ion will lead to fasterrecovery from neg<strong>at</strong>ive <strong>work</strong>place events.Emp<strong>at</strong>hyThe other part was y <strong>work</strong>ing with compassion outside of class, like re<strong>al</strong>ly looking <strong>at</strong>how damaged my students are and how y how many holes they have in their life andhow wounded they are y re<strong>al</strong>ly looking <strong>at</strong> their pain and their confusion and theirtraumas and dramas and y re<strong>al</strong>ly feeling how hard it is to be a teenager in this world,and re<strong>al</strong>ly just feeling it, like how inherently shitty their circumstances are y Once you
132THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.feel th<strong>at</strong>, then you’re not so angry because y you feel like, well, just try to help them.You know, just try to y make them smile and just p<strong>at</strong> them on the back, and try tomake their life a little less hard. Th<strong>at</strong> becomes the go<strong>al</strong> y being friendly and being kindand just understanding th<strong>at</strong> this is re<strong>al</strong>ly hard for them y kind of coming <strong>at</strong> it from theirpoint of view.– <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Medit<strong>at</strong>or (Participant #3)Emp<strong>at</strong>hy is the ability to see life from another’s perspective. Emp<strong>at</strong>hy<strong>al</strong>lows us to be <strong>at</strong>tuned to others, to reson<strong>at</strong>e with them, and to havecompassion (Cozolino, 2006). As suggested in the quote above, emp<strong>at</strong>hyenabled our interviewee to see how deeply wounded the students were. Ourinterviewee <strong>al</strong>tered his/her behavior to b<strong>et</strong>ter connect with them, offeringkindness and compassion r<strong>at</strong>her than judgment. Through emp<strong>at</strong>hy,individu<strong>al</strong>s are able to consider the larger soci<strong>al</strong> picture, moving out of‘‘surviv<strong>al</strong> mode’’ by considering wh<strong>at</strong> actions are best for others (Siegel,2007). How is mindfulness rel<strong>at</strong>ed to emp<strong>at</strong>hy? Building on a growing bodyof <strong>work</strong>, we see sever<strong>al</strong> links b<strong>et</strong>ween mindful awareness and emp<strong>at</strong>hy via itslinks to decreased autom<strong>at</strong>icity and decoupling and increased physiologic<strong>al</strong>awareness and regul<strong>at</strong>ion (Block-Lerner, Adair, Plumb, Rh<strong>at</strong>igan, &Orsillo, 2007; Brems, Fromme, & Johnson, 1992; Brown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007;Cozolino, 2006; Hughes, Tingle, & Sawin, 1981; Shapiro, Schwartz, &Bonner, 1998; Siegel, 2010; Tipsord, 2009).As the D<strong>al</strong>ai Lama (2002, p. 67) noted, ‘‘Ultim<strong>at</strong>ely, how we act andbehave in rel<strong>at</strong>ion to our fellow humans and the world, depends on how weperceive ourselves.’’ With emp<strong>at</strong>hy, one must be able to simultaneously‘‘hold one’s own perspective in mind while simultaneously imagining wh<strong>at</strong> itis like to be the other’’ (Cozolino, 2006, p. 203). The ability to perceive theself as it is without the constraints of autom<strong>at</strong>icity is a key fe<strong>at</strong>ure ofmindfulness. Indeed, it is difficult for individu<strong>al</strong>s to be aware of others’perspectives if they are unaware of their own. In essence, nonjudgment<strong>al</strong>,present-moment awareness (i.e., mindfulness) of one’s own intern<strong>al</strong>thoughts facilit<strong>at</strong>es emp<strong>at</strong>hy for the intern<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>es of others (Block-Lerner<strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007). Through mindfulness gener<strong>at</strong>ed m<strong>et</strong>a-cognitive awareness,individu<strong>al</strong>s can develop the capacity to understand their own intern<strong>al</strong>emotion<strong>al</strong> processes, which can help them b<strong>et</strong>ter understand the emotion<strong>al</strong>processes of others (Teasd<strong>al</strong>e <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2002). A cycle of mutu<strong>al</strong> reinforcementdevelops where intraperson<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tunement promotes interperson<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tunement(Siegel, 2007). A growing body of soci<strong>al</strong> neurobiology researchindic<strong>at</strong>es th<strong>at</strong> our capacity to be <strong>at</strong>tuned to others depends, in part, on ourknowledge of our own mind and intern<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>e (Siegel, 2010).
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 133Second, a growing body of research indic<strong>at</strong>es th<strong>at</strong> physiologic<strong>al</strong> awarenessand regul<strong>at</strong>ion promotes emp<strong>at</strong>hy (e.g., Cozolino, 2006). In the process of<strong>at</strong>tuning and reson<strong>at</strong>ing with others, individu<strong>al</strong>s use subcortic<strong>al</strong> d<strong>at</strong>a (i.e.,heart r<strong>at</strong>e, limbic system) to guide their responses. The act of emp<strong>at</strong>hyrequires individu<strong>al</strong>s to experience, emotion<strong>al</strong>ly and physiologic<strong>al</strong>ly, theinner experience of others. As part of the emp<strong>at</strong>hy process, the physiologic<strong>al</strong>and limbic systems send sign<strong>al</strong>s to the body and brain <strong>al</strong>lowing individu<strong>al</strong>sto liter<strong>al</strong>ly feel wh<strong>at</strong> the other person is experiencing.Third, mindfulness increases our ability to toler<strong>at</strong>e neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotions inourselves and others (Tipsord, 2009). Having true emp<strong>at</strong>hy requires <strong>at</strong>olerance for and regul<strong>at</strong>ion of the neg<strong>at</strong>ive intern<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>es of others andthose th<strong>at</strong> arise in ourselves as a consequence. By observing and not reacting toour own neg<strong>at</strong>ive st<strong>at</strong>es (i.e., decoupling) we can b<strong>et</strong>ter toler<strong>at</strong>e our ownneg<strong>at</strong>ive st<strong>at</strong>es and the neg<strong>at</strong>ive st<strong>at</strong>es of others. Without the ability to regul<strong>at</strong>eour own neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotion<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>es, we can become flooded with the neg<strong>at</strong>iveemotions of others, limiting our ability to remain <strong>at</strong>tuned and compassion<strong>at</strong>e.Higher levels of emp<strong>at</strong>hy are clearly desirable for organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> members<strong>at</strong> <strong>al</strong>l levels (Kamdar, McAllister, & Turban, 2006; P<strong>at</strong>ient & Skarlicki,2010). Organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> members who have higher levels of emp<strong>at</strong>hy for theircolleagues demonstr<strong>at</strong>e higher levels of interaction<strong>al</strong> justice (i.e., lower levelsof sexu<strong>al</strong> harassment and antisoci<strong>al</strong> behavior; Douglas & Martinko, 2001;O’Leary-Kelly, Bowes-Sperry, B<strong>at</strong>es, & Lean, 2009), inform<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> justice(P<strong>at</strong>ient & Skarlicki, 2010), organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> citizenship behaviors (Kamdar <strong>et</strong><strong>al</strong>., 2006; Kidder, 2002), and positive leadership behaviors (Kell<strong>et</strong>t,Humphrey, & Sle<strong>et</strong>h, 2002; Scott, Colquitt, Paddock, & Judge, 2010).Emp<strong>at</strong>hy may be particularly important in certain occup<strong>at</strong>ions th<strong>at</strong> requiregre<strong>at</strong>er compassion. For example, in one study of medic<strong>al</strong> students, anMBSR training program increased self-reported emp<strong>at</strong>hy over preprogramlevels (Shapiro <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 1998). Thus, if mindfulness can imbue employees withemp<strong>at</strong>hy, we would expect positive organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> and individu<strong>al</strong> outcomes.Affective Regul<strong>at</strong>iony just even being in a positive st<strong>at</strong>e of mind y you know, I’ve only been here two yearsafter being gone for ten and I h<strong>at</strong>e winter and, just using a practice while w<strong>al</strong>king to thebus of dis-identific<strong>at</strong>ion with the experience of having it be twenty below, freezing, andit’s 5:30 in the morning. You know, I’d instantly just go to agit<strong>at</strong>ion. But the practice hastaught me just to w<strong>at</strong>ch it and even have a sense of humor about it y . If I go to the buswith agit<strong>at</strong>ion and I got to <strong>work</strong> with agit<strong>at</strong>ion, I’m not going to be very productive.– <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Medit<strong>at</strong>or (Participant #2)
134THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.Affective regul<strong>at</strong>ion comprises the reduction of neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotions as wellas the gener<strong>at</strong>ion and maintenance of positive emotion. Our intervieweecaptured the essence of affective regul<strong>at</strong>ion and its benefits. R<strong>at</strong>her than bedistressed by the cold, by using mindfulness practices this medit<strong>at</strong>orelimin<strong>at</strong>ed ‘‘agit<strong>at</strong>ion’’ about extern<strong>al</strong> conditions beyond their control.<strong>Mindfulness</strong> (st<strong>at</strong>e and trait) has been linked to both fac<strong>et</strong>s of affectregul<strong>at</strong>ion (gener<strong>at</strong>ing positive emotions, down-regul<strong>at</strong>ing neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotionswhen they arise; e.g., Fredrickson <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2008; Giluk, 2009, 2010) inlarge part because mindfulness enhances the brain circuits responsible foremotion<strong>al</strong> regul<strong>at</strong>ion (Davidson, 2000; Siegel, 2007). Specific<strong>al</strong>ly, enhancedleft prefront<strong>al</strong> activ<strong>at</strong>ion seems to be a critic<strong>al</strong> trigger of positive emotion,approach motiv<strong>at</strong>ion, and increased ability to modul<strong>at</strong>e neg<strong>at</strong>ive moodsarising from the firing of the limbic system. Building on the growingliter<strong>at</strong>ure in this area (e.g., Urry <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2004), we assert th<strong>at</strong> two coreprocesses – awareness and regul<strong>at</strong>ion of one’s physiologic<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>es combinedwith decoupling of the self from experiences and emotions – play a key rolein affect regul<strong>at</strong>ion.In terms of the influence of mindfulness on positive emotions, m<strong>et</strong>aan<strong>al</strong>yticevidence indic<strong>at</strong>es a positive associ<strong>at</strong>ion b<strong>et</strong>ween mindfulness andpositive mood st<strong>at</strong>es (i.e., PA; Giluk, 2009). Although ch<strong>al</strong>lenging situ<strong>at</strong>ionsdepl<strong>et</strong>e important self-regul<strong>at</strong>ory resources (Tice, Baumeister, & Zhang,2004), positive mood st<strong>at</strong>es ‘‘restore and replenish’’ these resources, <strong>al</strong>lowingindividu<strong>al</strong>s to persist (Giluk, 2010, p. 55). Gre<strong>at</strong>er awareness promoted bymindfulness may enhance the experience of and engagement with positiveemotions (Erisman & Roemer, 2010; Tomarken, Davidson, & Henriques,1990). In other words, a cycle of positivity may develop throughmindfulness, as individu<strong>al</strong>s are more likely to notice positive events in theirlives and thus experience more positive moods.According to Brown and his colleagues (2007), mindfulness is <strong>al</strong>soassoci<strong>at</strong>ed with acceptance of emotion<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>es and enhanced ability torepair neg<strong>at</strong>ive st<strong>at</strong>es. Further evidence of the role of mindfulness inregul<strong>at</strong>ing neg<strong>at</strong>ive affect comes from a study by Hariri, Bookheimer, andMazziotta (2000) in which the simple act of observing a neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotionand labeling it without judgment decreased limbic system activ<strong>at</strong>ion, whichsubsequently reduced felt and expressed neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotions. It appears th<strong>at</strong>mindfulness driven mPFC activ<strong>at</strong>ion modul<strong>at</strong>es limbic system activ<strong>at</strong>ion inresponse to neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotions (Siegel, 2007).In terms of the <strong>work</strong>place, a significant body of extant <strong>work</strong> hasdocumented the benefits of increased positive mood and decreased orregul<strong>at</strong>ed neg<strong>at</strong>ive affective experiences (Bono & Ilies, 2006; Isen, 1987;
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 135Losada, 1999; Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005; Miner, <strong>Glomb</strong>, &Hulin, 2005). For example, Lyubomirsky and her colleagues (2005)demonstr<strong>at</strong>ed th<strong>at</strong> positive affect gener<strong>at</strong>es success in multiple life domains.Likewise, Fredrickson and her colleagues (Cohn, Fredrickson, Brown,Mikels, & Conway, 2009; Fredrickson, 1998; Fredrickson <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2008)suggested th<strong>at</strong> positive emotions enable individu<strong>al</strong>s to build importantcognitive, physic<strong>al</strong>, and soci<strong>al</strong> resources such as resilience. Moreover,employees who tend to experience more positive moods are more sensitive tothe reward sign<strong>al</strong>s in the environment such as pay raises and other forms ofrecognition (e.g., Shaw, <strong>Duffy</strong>, Mitra, Lockhart, & Bowler, 2003).The regul<strong>at</strong>ion and reduction of neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotion <strong>al</strong>so has clearimplic<strong>at</strong>ions for employee functioning (see Brief & Weiss, 2002). Forexample, individu<strong>al</strong>s who experience chronic<strong>al</strong>ly neg<strong>at</strong>ive mood st<strong>at</strong>es aremore likely to be victimized <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong> and to be perp<strong>et</strong>r<strong>at</strong>ors of <strong>work</strong>placeaggression (e.g., Aquino, Grover, Bradfield, & Allen, 1999; Tepper, <strong>Duffy</strong>,Henle, & Lambert, 2006). Leaders’ neg<strong>at</strong>ive mood st<strong>at</strong>es have been linked tofollowers’ moods and group processing effects as well (Sy, Coˆ té, &Saavedra, 2005).Increased Self-D<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion and PersistenceI do this job because y it’s <strong>work</strong>ing with humans, but <strong>al</strong>so it’s essenti<strong>al</strong> to me th<strong>at</strong> I have aright livelihood you know y th<strong>at</strong>’s very important to me. I mean, I don’t want a job th<strong>at</strong>wouldn’t f<strong>al</strong>l under the c<strong>at</strong>egory of right livelihood or direct contact with people in need.– <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Medit<strong>at</strong>or (Participant #2)Brown and Ryan (2003) argued th<strong>at</strong> individu<strong>al</strong>s acting mindfully behavein accord with their underlying v<strong>al</strong>ues and interests. They reported a positiveassoci<strong>at</strong>ion b<strong>et</strong>ween the experience of mindful st<strong>at</strong>es and feelings ofautonomy, a key component of self-d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion. Addition<strong>al</strong>ly, Shapiro<strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>. (2006) suggested th<strong>at</strong> the d<strong>et</strong>ached observ<strong>at</strong>ion developed inmindfulness training <strong>al</strong>lows gre<strong>at</strong>er recognition of wh<strong>at</strong> is v<strong>al</strong>ued, andincreased likelihood th<strong>at</strong> individu<strong>al</strong>s will choose behaviors in <strong>al</strong>ignment withthose v<strong>al</strong>ues. The reduced autom<strong>at</strong>icity associ<strong>at</strong>ed with mindfulness leadsindividu<strong>al</strong>s to ‘‘reflectively choose wh<strong>at</strong> has previously been reflexivelyadopted or conditioned’’ (Shapiro <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2006, p. 380), ultim<strong>at</strong>ely cre<strong>at</strong>inggre<strong>at</strong>er congruence b<strong>et</strong>ween v<strong>al</strong>ues and actions, which is <strong>at</strong> the heart of selfd<strong>et</strong>erminedbehavior. Because mindful individu<strong>al</strong>s b<strong>et</strong>ter understand theirgo<strong>al</strong>s and v<strong>al</strong>ues, and act more congruently with them, their intentions areb<strong>et</strong>ter predictors of their behavior. This proposition is consistent with the
136THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.predictions of self-d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion theory (Ryan & Deci, 2000) and wassupported empiric<strong>al</strong>ly by Ch<strong>at</strong>zisarantis and Hagger (2007), who found th<strong>at</strong>intentions for physic<strong>al</strong> activity in leisure time predicted actu<strong>al</strong> physic<strong>al</strong>activity among individu<strong>al</strong>s high in trait mindfulness.Gre<strong>at</strong>er <strong>al</strong>ignment b<strong>et</strong>ween go<strong>al</strong>s and v<strong>al</strong>ues is <strong>al</strong>so associ<strong>at</strong>ed withpersistence toward go<strong>al</strong> accomplishment. Self-d<strong>et</strong>ermined go<strong>al</strong>s elicit moreeffort (Bono & Judge, 2003; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999) and <strong>al</strong>so lead to gre<strong>at</strong>erpersistence, even in the face of ch<strong>al</strong>lenges. <strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>al</strong>so reduces the extentto which people see barriers to go<strong>al</strong> accomplishment, or obstacles in go<strong>al</strong>pursuit, as indic<strong>at</strong>ions of their comp<strong>et</strong>ency (e.g., decoupling of self fromexperiences). Ch<strong>al</strong>lenges often trigger derailing neg<strong>at</strong>ive, self-critic<strong>al</strong>, reactive,and judgment<strong>al</strong> thoughts. As individu<strong>al</strong>s <strong>at</strong>tempt to avoid de<strong>al</strong>ing with thesech<strong>al</strong>lenging thre<strong>at</strong>s to self, persistence lags (Teasd<strong>al</strong>e, Seg<strong>al</strong>, & Williams, 1995).By <strong>al</strong>lowing neg<strong>at</strong>ive thoughts to occur without judgment and reaction, th<strong>et</strong>houghts and concomitant frustr<strong>at</strong>ion dissip<strong>at</strong>e, <strong>al</strong>lowing successful go<strong>al</strong>pursuit (Brown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2007). Empiric<strong>al</strong> evidence has supported the notion th<strong>at</strong>mindfulness plays a significant role in persistence (e.g., Evans, Baer, &Segerstrom, 2009). R<strong>at</strong>her than being absorbed in a dysfunction<strong>al</strong> cycle ofrumin<strong>at</strong>ion, mindfulness helps people maintain cognitive focus (Chambers<strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2009; Chambers, Lo, & Allen, 2008). We suggest this occurs becausemindful people are pursuing go<strong>al</strong>s th<strong>at</strong> are important to them, andmindfulness helps them cope more effectively with obstacles.The implic<strong>at</strong>ions for increased self-d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion and persistence <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>are broad reaching. Existing research links go<strong>al</strong> self-concordance directly tojob s<strong>at</strong>isfaction and organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> commitment (Bono & Judge, 2003),increased job s<strong>at</strong>isfaction (Judge, Bono, Erez, & Locke, 2005), and increasedeffort and performance on cre<strong>at</strong>ive tasks (Bono & Judge, 2003). Addition<strong>al</strong>ly,Sheldon, Turban, Brown, Barrick, and Judge (2003) suggested th<strong>at</strong> selfd<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ionincreases go<strong>al</strong>-commitment and learning efforts, and ultim<strong>at</strong>elyincreases learning. A series of studies <strong>al</strong>so link go<strong>al</strong> self-concordance toincreased effort, improved go<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tainment, and gre<strong>at</strong>er s<strong>at</strong>isfaction with go<strong>al</strong><strong>at</strong>tainment (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). In summary, we expect th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness,primarily via reduced autom<strong>at</strong>icity of thought, will lead employees to both bemore productive and to experience gre<strong>at</strong>er s<strong>at</strong>isfaction from their <strong>work</strong>.Other Secondary ProcessesSo far we have identified major processes we believe to be centr<strong>al</strong> in theoper<strong>at</strong>ion of mindfulness. However, the liter<strong>at</strong>ure has provided suggestive
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 137evidence for addition<strong>al</strong> processes. In the following text, we d<strong>et</strong>ail two suchprocesses: increased <strong>work</strong>ing memory capacity and more accur<strong>at</strong>e affectiveforecasting. Although these issues did not arise directly in our interviews,empiric<strong>al</strong> evidence suggests th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness may increase <strong>work</strong>ing memoryand improve affective forecasting.Increased Working MemoryWorking memory or ‘‘the cognitive mechanism th<strong>at</strong> <strong>al</strong>lows us to keep <strong>al</strong>imited amount of inform<strong>at</strong>ion active for a limited period of time’’ (Elzinga &Roelofs, 2005, p. 98), plays a key role in self-regul<strong>at</strong>ory processes because itis used to manage cognitive demands and to regul<strong>at</strong>e emotions (e.g.,Schmeichel, Volokhov, & Demaree, 2008). The existing liter<strong>at</strong>ure hasprovided considerable evidence th<strong>at</strong> highly stressful or demanding situ<strong>at</strong>ionsdepl<strong>et</strong>e <strong>work</strong>ing memory capacity, partly because stressful or otherphysiologic<strong>al</strong>ly and emotion<strong>al</strong>ly activ<strong>at</strong>ed situ<strong>at</strong>ions cause the adren<strong>al</strong>glands to release stress hormones (e.g., cortisol) to me<strong>et</strong> situ<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> demands(e.g., Roozenda<strong>al</strong>, 2002). Although helpful in activ<strong>at</strong>ing response systems,elev<strong>at</strong>ed cortisol levels in stressful situ<strong>at</strong>ions have the unfortun<strong>at</strong>econsequence of inhibiting <strong>work</strong>ing memory (Oei, Everaerd, Elzinga, VanWell, & Bermond, 2006). Indeed, <strong>work</strong>ing memory is considered to be oneof the memory functions most affected by cortisol (Elzinga & Roelofs,2005).Given the link b<strong>et</strong>ween mindfulness and physiologic<strong>al</strong> b<strong>al</strong>ance andawareness, and building on the growing research in this area (e.g., Jha,Stanley, Kiyonaga, Wong, & Gelfand, 2010), we propose th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnessmedit<strong>at</strong>ion will improve <strong>work</strong>ing memory in <strong>work</strong> s<strong>et</strong>tings. In a recent studyof military employees, Jha and her colleagues (2010) hypothesized andfound th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness practices protected <strong>work</strong>ing memory fromdegrad<strong>at</strong>ion during a stressful predeployment phase for soldiers whocompl<strong>et</strong>ed an 8-week mindfulness training program. Results confirmed anincrease in <strong>work</strong>ing memory capacity for soldiers who were trained and whopracticed and degrad<strong>at</strong>ions in <strong>work</strong>ing memory capacity for those who wer<strong>et</strong>rained but did not practice, consistent with expect<strong>at</strong>ions for individu<strong>al</strong>s instressful environments. Jha and her colleagues proposed th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnessmay help cultiv<strong>at</strong>e a ‘‘<strong>work</strong>ing memory reserve’’ (p. 62). In addition, theyfound th<strong>at</strong> the increase in <strong>work</strong>ing memory capacity associ<strong>at</strong>ed withmindfulness training led to reduced neg<strong>at</strong>ive affect.Although not a focus of their study, this research is <strong>al</strong>so consistent withthe notion th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness is associ<strong>at</strong>ed with improved self-regul<strong>at</strong>ionbecause it affects the brain’s capacity for b<strong>al</strong>anced physiologic<strong>al</strong> regul<strong>at</strong>ion.
138THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.As noted in the preceding text, when individu<strong>al</strong>s become more aware of theirbodily st<strong>at</strong>es, they are more able to regul<strong>at</strong>e their levels of physiologic<strong>al</strong>activ<strong>at</strong>ion and responses to neg<strong>at</strong>ive thoughts and emotions. Consequently,unhe<strong>al</strong>thy stress hormone production (i.e., cortisol) is reduced, <strong>al</strong>lowing<strong>work</strong>ing memory to function more effectively, which suggests th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnessmay be especi<strong>al</strong>ly important for effective performance in the<strong>work</strong>place when multiple demands or stress-inducing conditions prevail.Improved Accuracy in Affective ForecastingAffective forecasting refers to an individu<strong>al</strong>’s ability to accur<strong>at</strong>ely predicttheir emotion<strong>al</strong> responses to future events (Wilson & Gilbert, 2003). It hasbeen well established th<strong>at</strong> people are gener<strong>al</strong>ly poor <strong>at</strong> anticip<strong>at</strong>ing futureemotions; they are unable to accur<strong>at</strong>ely predict how they will feel followingemotion<strong>al</strong>ly charged events (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Whe<strong>at</strong>ley,1998). They tend to predict they will be happier than they actu<strong>al</strong>ly are afterpositive events and to predict th<strong>at</strong> they will be unhappier than they actu<strong>al</strong>lyare following neg<strong>at</strong>ive events. <strong>Mindfulness</strong> may lead to improvements inaffective forecasting by reducing the impact bias (i.e., overestim<strong>at</strong>ing theemotion<strong>al</strong> impact of a future event), because mindfulness <strong>al</strong>lows people toconsider emotions and emotion<strong>al</strong> experiences as separ<strong>at</strong>e from the self, andbecause it reduces autom<strong>at</strong>icity of thought. In one study of a sample of 188young adults who forecasted their emotions for the weeks following the2008 presidenti<strong>al</strong> election, Emanuel, Updegraff, K<strong>al</strong>mbach, and Ciesla(2010) found th<strong>at</strong> trait mindfulness was associ<strong>at</strong>ed with more moder<strong>at</strong>eaffective forecasts and decreased impact bias.Errors in affective forecasting are important in the <strong>work</strong>place becaus<strong>et</strong>hey introduce bias into decision-making processes when individu<strong>al</strong>soverweight their own or others’ reactions to future occurrences. Moreaccur<strong>at</strong>e affective forecasting is expected not only to improve employeeperformance via improved decision making, but is <strong>al</strong>so expected to improvewell-being because of gre<strong>at</strong>er <strong>al</strong>ignment b<strong>et</strong>ween expect<strong>at</strong>ions and re<strong>al</strong>ity,which elimin<strong>at</strong>es the disappointment, neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotions, and frustr<strong>at</strong>ionth<strong>at</strong> follow from unm<strong>et</strong> expect<strong>at</strong>ions.KEY WORK-RELATED BENEFITS OF MINDFULNESSOur centr<strong>al</strong> purpose in this manuscript was to link mindfulness andmindfulness-based processes to employees’ performance and well-being <strong>at</strong><strong>work</strong>. In examining the cognitive, emotion<strong>al</strong>, and neurobiologic<strong>al</strong> processes
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 139linking mindfulness with improved self-regul<strong>at</strong>ory capacity in the <strong>work</strong>place,three centr<strong>al</strong> themes emerge. First, mindfulness is associ<strong>at</strong>ed withfactors expected to influence rel<strong>at</strong>ionship qu<strong>al</strong>ity. Second, mindfulness islinked to processes indic<strong>at</strong>ive of resiliency. Third, mindfulness is linked withprocesses expected to improve task performance and decision making. Inthe following sections, we focus explicitly on the ways mindfulness andmindfulness-based practices lead to more positive rel<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> functioning <strong>at</strong><strong>work</strong>, how they build resiliency, and how they improve task performanceand decision making. The three outcomes we focus on are inextricablylinked, but we discuss them independently because they represent distinct<strong>work</strong>-rel<strong>at</strong>ed outcomes.Improved Soci<strong>al</strong> Rel<strong>at</strong>ionshipsThere are two things I think th<strong>at</strong> I’m re<strong>al</strong>ly aware of how [mindfulness] helped me, andone of them is rel<strong>at</strong>ionships. It’s th<strong>at</strong> pause y I mean I can think of instances where Iwas having a disagreement with either a co-<strong>work</strong>er or a student, and just being able to,like, just come back to myself and re<strong>al</strong>ize y a lot of times wh<strong>at</strong> I actu<strong>al</strong>ly re<strong>al</strong>ize isthey’re ups<strong>et</strong> but I don’t think I have to be (laughter). And boy, th<strong>at</strong> can be so helpful.Many times th<strong>at</strong> either just s<strong>et</strong>tles it down or <strong>at</strong> least y <strong>at</strong> least I’m not <strong>al</strong>l ups<strong>et</strong> about it.I mean I certainly do g<strong>et</strong> ups<strong>et</strong> <strong>at</strong> times y there’s things th<strong>at</strong> happen but y th<strong>at</strong>’s on<strong>et</strong>hing with rel<strong>at</strong>ionships th<strong>at</strong> helps tremendously.– <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Medit<strong>at</strong>or (Participant #1)A fundament<strong>al</strong> finding of soci<strong>al</strong> psychologic<strong>al</strong> research is th<strong>at</strong> individu<strong>al</strong>sthrive through positive soci<strong>al</strong> connections with others (Baumeister & Leary,1995). Positive soci<strong>al</strong> connections in a <strong>work</strong> s<strong>et</strong>ting are no exception. Agrowing body of <strong>work</strong> indic<strong>at</strong>es th<strong>at</strong> positive <strong>work</strong>place rel<strong>at</strong>ionships buildcritic<strong>al</strong> resources th<strong>at</strong> protect individu<strong>al</strong>s from <strong>work</strong>place stressors, andfoster employee thriving, communic<strong>at</strong>ion, cre<strong>at</strong>ivity, and citizenshipbehaviors (e.g., Dutton & Heaphy, 2003; Harter, Schmidt, & Hayes, 2002;Thau, Aquino, & Poortvli<strong>et</strong>, 2007). We submit th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness willpromote these positive soci<strong>al</strong> connections.How does mindfulness foster positive soci<strong>al</strong> connections in the <strong>work</strong>place?Many processes may contribute to improved <strong>work</strong>place rel<strong>at</strong>ionships,but we note the speci<strong>al</strong> importance of emp<strong>at</strong>hy and response flexibility, bothmarkers of the intern<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tunement associ<strong>at</strong>ed with mindfulness in whichone is distinctly aware of one’s own physic<strong>al</strong> and emotion<strong>al</strong> sign<strong>al</strong>s, which<strong>al</strong>lows enhanced sensitivity to others’ sign<strong>al</strong>s without subsequent reactivity(Davidson, 2000; Siegel, 2007). Thus, individu<strong>al</strong>s who practice mindfulnessshould be b<strong>et</strong>ter poised to respond to colleagues with gre<strong>at</strong>er acceptance and
140THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.without unskillful reactivity. In essence, mindfulness promotes he<strong>al</strong>thy waysof rel<strong>at</strong>ing to others in the <strong>work</strong>place (Giluk, 2010), which include takinganother’s perspective and reducing habitu<strong>al</strong> reactions th<strong>at</strong> may bedysfunction<strong>al</strong> or promote esc<strong>al</strong><strong>at</strong>ion. As indic<strong>at</strong>ed in the preceding quote,the practice has <strong>al</strong>lowed the mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>or to communic<strong>at</strong>e moreopenly and to resolve conflict without neg<strong>at</strong>ive contagion and esc<strong>al</strong><strong>at</strong>ingp<strong>at</strong>terns of tit-for-t<strong>at</strong> behaviors, instead being more accepting of others andtheir imperfections.Recent research has turned to the role of mindfulness in facilit<strong>at</strong>ing thequ<strong>al</strong>ity of interperson<strong>al</strong> connections as well (e.g., Hutcherson <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2008),and studies suggest th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness training may be rel<strong>at</strong>ed to gre<strong>at</strong>ersoci<strong>al</strong> connectedness (e.g., Cohen & Miller, 2009). This sense of connectionmay be important for the <strong>work</strong>place as individu<strong>al</strong>s higher in soci<strong>al</strong>connectedness tend to display more desirable interperson<strong>al</strong> behaviors (e.g.,OCBs, received and perceived soci<strong>al</strong> support) than those lower in soci<strong>al</strong>connectedness (Fredrickson <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2008). In many ways, positive interperson<strong>al</strong>rel<strong>at</strong>ionships are a critic<strong>al</strong> d<strong>et</strong>erminant of optim<strong>al</strong> organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong>functioning (<strong>Duffy</strong>, Ganster, & Pagon, 2002), thereby underscoring theimportant role of mindfulness in <strong>work</strong> rel<strong>at</strong>ionships.Increased Resiliencyyyou g<strong>et</strong> re<strong>al</strong>ly sensitive, like, to people’s energy, like y my [boss name], she’s the exactopposite of me, she’s like re<strong>al</strong>ly tight y she’s r<strong>et</strong>iring this year, she’s very old, she’s beendoing it for way too long and she’s burnt out, she’s re<strong>al</strong>ly whipped up and she’s verytight. And I notice I respond to her with c<strong>al</strong>mness ‘‘Be <strong>at</strong> ease, be <strong>at</strong> ease’’ like this, and Iwant to w<strong>al</strong>k away because she’s so intense! Like her body, and her language. At firstwhen I was re<strong>al</strong>ly sensitive I just wanted to, like, g<strong>et</strong> out of there! I just couldn’t de<strong>al</strong> withher energy, y but now I’ve just learned how to, like, just kind of be with it, and it feelsgood th<strong>at</strong> I can re<strong>al</strong>ly just listen to her go off and just y and y just stay with her. Butnot feel drained by her. It was draining listening to her <strong>at</strong> one point, and now it’s just like‘‘Just l<strong>et</strong> it move through me, don’t resist and don’t kind of like react, respond, just takeit <strong>al</strong>l in.’’ And then she feels b<strong>et</strong>ter because I’m not reacting, I’m not g<strong>et</strong>ting caught up inher drama, ‘cause th<strong>at</strong>’s wh<strong>at</strong> som<strong>et</strong>imes happens, you g<strong>et</strong> caught up in each others’dramas and just kind of whip it up. When you don’t, and they vent, you still feel c<strong>al</strong>m,and th<strong>at</strong> way you don’t have to avoid. I used to avoid people because I didn’t want tode<strong>al</strong> with their shit. But now I don’t have to, it’s like ‘‘Alright, give me your shit, wh<strong>at</strong>’swrong?’’– <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Medit<strong>at</strong>or (Participant #3)The desire to avoid unpleasant and ch<strong>al</strong>lenging situ<strong>at</strong>ions, such as the onedescribed in the preceding text by our study participant, is common.
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 141Although offering temporary respite from adversari<strong>al</strong> conditions, in thelong-term such avoidance behavior is m<strong>al</strong>adaptive. Withdraw<strong>al</strong> tendencies(e.g., ‘‘I wanted to g<strong>et</strong> out of there!’’) deprive individu<strong>al</strong>s of theopportunities to achieve go<strong>al</strong>-relevant behaviors and activities necessaryfor thriving (Urry <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2004). Conversely, a convincing body of evidencesuggests th<strong>at</strong> approach tendencies (e.g., engaged thinking and interactingwith others and one’s environment) are associ<strong>at</strong>ed with well-being andthriving (Urry <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2004). To engage in approach behaviors and experienc<strong>et</strong>heir associ<strong>at</strong>ed well-being effects, individu<strong>al</strong>s must be resilient in the face ofch<strong>al</strong>lenge and difficulties.How does mindfulness gener<strong>at</strong>e resilience? Although resilience is likely tobe fostered through sever<strong>al</strong> of the mechanisms posited in the preceding text,we highlight the role of two processes associ<strong>at</strong>ed with mindfulness –affective regul<strong>at</strong>ion and persistence. Approach behavior requires persistenceas well as the maintenance of positive affect and well-being in the face ofadversity (Chambers <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2009; Davidson, 2000). Another centr<strong>al</strong> fe<strong>at</strong>ureof resilience is the capacity to be nonreactive to one’s thoughts and emotionsand to accept them (i.e., decoupling and reduced autom<strong>at</strong>icity). Thecapacity to regul<strong>at</strong>e neg<strong>at</strong>ive thoughts and emotions, particularly once theysurface, is a key fe<strong>at</strong>ure of resilience. Davis (2009) wrote, ‘‘The capacity toharness positive emotion in daily life may be a key ingredient to resilience,helping individu<strong>al</strong>s to persevere in the face of ch<strong>al</strong>lenge, speeding recoveryfrom transient life difficulties, and sustaining qu<strong>al</strong>ity of life in the face ofmore chronic stressors’’ (p. 62). As our medit<strong>at</strong>or so eloquently illustr<strong>at</strong>ed,mindfulness not only <strong>al</strong>lows us to approach others positively (in this case, aperson who is known to be difficult) it <strong>al</strong>so protects us from other’s neg<strong>at</strong>iveemotion<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>es and agit<strong>at</strong>ion through appropri<strong>at</strong>e regul<strong>at</strong>ion of affect anddecreased reactivity. In a <strong>work</strong> s<strong>et</strong>ting, remaining resilient to <strong>work</strong>ch<strong>al</strong>lenges and stressors, be they interperson<strong>al</strong> or task rel<strong>at</strong>ed, is critic<strong>al</strong> tooptim<strong>al</strong> <strong>work</strong> functioning.Improved Task PerformanceI’ve learned th<strong>at</strong> when your energy is concentr<strong>at</strong>ed on y when you’re absorbed in a tasklike planning, it re<strong>al</strong>ly feels good. It re<strong>al</strong>ly feels good to [<strong>work</strong> task] because you’reabsorbed in a task, your mind is focusing on one thing, so those are re<strong>al</strong>ly pleasurablebecause you’re not sc<strong>at</strong>tered, your mind’s not sc<strong>at</strong>teredAnd I <strong>al</strong>so do try and w<strong>al</strong>k re<strong>al</strong>ly slowly som<strong>et</strong>imes, or just tell myself ‘‘I’m going to dothis re<strong>al</strong>ly slowly’’ instead of <strong>al</strong>ways feeling like I’m being re<strong>al</strong>ly pushed to rush which we<strong>al</strong>ways gener<strong>al</strong>ly are, but y and th<strong>at</strong> actu<strong>al</strong>ly does help, you know, it kinda
142THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.just y som<strong>et</strong>hing still g<strong>et</strong>s done (laughter), you know? And y and a lot of times if youdo it slowly it actu<strong>al</strong>ly g<strong>et</strong>s done well!– <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Medit<strong>at</strong>or (Participant #3)Many processes described in this chapter are likely to have downstreameffects on task performance, but the way th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness affectsperformance is likely to depend heavily on the type of tasks required of ajob. For example, for jobs with hefty interperson<strong>al</strong> interactions, we mightexpect emp<strong>at</strong>hy to play a major role. For occup<strong>at</strong>ions with high emotion<strong>al</strong>content, we might expect decreased rumin<strong>at</strong>ion and improved affectiveregul<strong>at</strong>ion to be the critic<strong>al</strong> p<strong>at</strong>hways to performance. For jobs th<strong>at</strong> are notroutinized and have high task complexity, response flexibility may be key.Although some processes (e.g., increased self-d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion and persistence)can be expected to benefici<strong>al</strong>ly affect many job types, we believe th<strong>at</strong> the roleof mindfulness in performance largely depends on the task and contextu<strong>al</strong>fe<strong>at</strong>ures of the <strong>work</strong>.The effects of the <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> component of mindfulness on taskperformance have been thoughtfully considered by Dane (2010). Danesuggested th<strong>at</strong> wide <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> breadth, such as th<strong>at</strong> present in a st<strong>at</strong>e ofmindfulness, may inhibit or promote task performance depending on th<strong>et</strong>ask environment and level of expertise. Specific<strong>al</strong>ly, maintaining a wideextern<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> breadth is useful in dynamic task environments as it<strong>al</strong>lows for <strong>at</strong>tention to a wide range of stimuli. However, in fairly st<strong>at</strong>icenvironments, wide extern<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> breadth might inhibit performanceas one loses focus on their tasks. Herndon (2008) found th<strong>at</strong> traitmindfulness was associ<strong>at</strong>ed with fewer cognitive failures (i.e., forg<strong>et</strong>ting,distraction, blunders), which suggests th<strong>at</strong> if mindfulness is associ<strong>at</strong>ed withgre<strong>at</strong>er <strong>at</strong>tention to extern<strong>al</strong> stimuli, and therefore, fewer cognitive failures,then a vari<strong>et</strong>y of favorable <strong>work</strong> outcomes are likely to follow includingincreased performance and fewer accidents.<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>al</strong>so <strong>at</strong>tunes individu<strong>al</strong>s to intern<strong>al</strong> processes such th<strong>at</strong> anindividu<strong>al</strong> is more <strong>at</strong>tentive to their nonconscious or autom<strong>at</strong>ic thoughts,feelings, and perceptions, often in the form of gut feelings or reactions. Dane(2010) argued th<strong>at</strong> these intuitions may promote task performance whenexpertise is high. <strong>Mindfulness</strong> is <strong>al</strong>so expected to impact job performanc<strong>et</strong>hrough improved decision making. When heuristic processing is reducedand <strong>at</strong>tention to intern<strong>al</strong> and extern<strong>al</strong> stimuli is increased, decision biasessuch as anchoring and fundament<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tribution error should be decreased(Hammond, Keeney, & Raiffa, 2006).As we consider the links of mindfulness to task performance, we recogniz<strong>et</strong>h<strong>at</strong> mindfulness might be antith<strong>et</strong>ic<strong>al</strong> to the evolutionary development and
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 143efficient functioning of the human brain, which is designed to rapidlyprocess and c<strong>at</strong>egorize a large volume of stimuli. Autom<strong>at</strong>icity is function<strong>al</strong>and <strong>at</strong> first blush, it might seem as if mindfulness may make one less efficientand less productive, as individu<strong>al</strong>s are no longer able to rapidly processstimuli. However, we might reconcile these seemingly contradictory ideas byconsidering th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness may help tune our minds so th<strong>at</strong> autom<strong>at</strong>icitybecomes more function<strong>al</strong> and redirects <strong>at</strong>tention to the appropri<strong>at</strong>eenvironment<strong>al</strong> stimuli, <strong>al</strong>lowing individu<strong>al</strong>s to respond more skillfullyr<strong>at</strong>her than autom<strong>at</strong>ic<strong>al</strong>ly. There is much room for future research testingour propositions about the effects of mindfulness on performance <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>.CONTROVERSIES, QUESTIONS, AND CHALLENGESAlthough this chapter focuses on carefully examining the processes by whichmindfulness and mindfulness-based practices might influence employees’functioning <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>, we would be remiss if we did not address sever<strong>al</strong>controversies, questions, and ch<strong>al</strong>lenges in the gener<strong>al</strong> mindfulness liter<strong>at</strong>ur<strong>et</strong>o d<strong>et</strong>ermine how seriously they ch<strong>al</strong>lenge the link b<strong>et</strong>ween mindfulnessand <strong>work</strong>.<strong>Mindfulness</strong> vs. <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-Based Practices and ProgramsIn conjunction with our foray into the mindfulness liter<strong>at</strong>ure, the first threeauthors compl<strong>et</strong>ed an 8-week mindfulness training program modeled afterthe Kab<strong>at</strong>-Zinn MBSR program. Our reasons were twofold. As scholars, wewanted to b<strong>et</strong>ter understand the ideas of mindfulness and the MBSRapproach to cultiv<strong>at</strong>ing mindfulness. As individu<strong>al</strong>s, we were <strong>at</strong>tracted tothe possibilities of stress reduction and other benefits associ<strong>at</strong>ed with MBSRprograms. Although each had different experiences, we <strong>al</strong>l found th<strong>at</strong> theprocesses described in the preceding text and illustr<strong>at</strong>ed by our intervieweesreson<strong>at</strong>ed with our experiences in mindfulness training. We felt th<strong>at</strong>cultiv<strong>at</strong>ing mindfulness via MBSR training benefitted our <strong>work</strong> andperson<strong>al</strong> lives in many ways. Despite these experiences, we believe muchremains to be known about the ‘‘active ingredients’’ th<strong>at</strong> led to thesebenefits, especi<strong>al</strong>ly wh<strong>et</strong>her they were <strong>at</strong>tributable to mindfulness.<strong>Mindfulness</strong>-based stress reduction curriculums include, as one aspect,practices designed to develop participant mindfulness. However, especi<strong>al</strong>lyin a <strong>work</strong>-rel<strong>at</strong>ed discussion, we must recognize th<strong>at</strong> these programs are,
144THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.first and foremost, for reducing stress. Indeed, the practices are designed tohelp individu<strong>al</strong>s relax ment<strong>al</strong>ly (guided imagery) and release tensionphysic<strong>al</strong>ly (yoga and Qigong). Some, but not <strong>al</strong>l, of these techniques areexpected to improve mindfulness. Moreover, because they are conducted insm<strong>al</strong>l group sessions with regular sharing among participants, they <strong>al</strong>soprovide soci<strong>al</strong> support and som<strong>et</strong>imes lead to ongoing soci<strong>al</strong> rel<strong>at</strong>ionshipswith the concomitant benefits. Participants may <strong>al</strong>so change variousbehaviors (e.g., reducing commitments, b<strong>et</strong>ter time management) over thecourse of an MBSR program (Kab<strong>at</strong>-Zinn, 1990, 1994). Although thesepractices (i.e., ment<strong>al</strong> relax<strong>at</strong>ion, reduced physic<strong>al</strong> tensions, soci<strong>al</strong> support,b<strong>et</strong>ter time management) and their direct benefits are not necessarily rel<strong>at</strong>edto mindfulness, they would be expected to benefit <strong>work</strong> outcomes. It isbeyond our scope here to an<strong>al</strong>yze which MBSR benefits are due tomindfulness and which result from other processes such as relax<strong>at</strong>ion orsoci<strong>al</strong> support (see Bishop, 2002; Dimidjian & Linehan, 2003; Dobkin, 2008;Roemer & Orsillo, 2003, for a deb<strong>at</strong>e of wh<strong>et</strong>her mindfulness is the keyingredient of mindfulness-based practices), and we caution againstunthinkingly choosing MBSR-type programs <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong> if the ultim<strong>at</strong>e go<strong>al</strong>is to increase mindfulness. As Shapiro <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>. (2006) noted, ‘‘Dismantlestudies are necessary to separ<strong>at</strong>e and compare the various active ingredientsin mindfulness-based interventions such as soci<strong>al</strong> support, relax<strong>at</strong>ion, andcognitive behavior<strong>al</strong> elements’’ (p. 374).Furthermore, questions surface about distinguishing mindfulness fromexisting self-regul<strong>at</strong>ory concepts. Masicampo and Baumeister (2010) questionedwh<strong>et</strong>her mindfulness is substantively different from self-control. Giventhe difficulty of measuring mindfulness itself (as a st<strong>at</strong>e of consciousness),most research has focused on the benefici<strong>al</strong> effects of mindfulness-basedpractices or therapies. These effects are gener<strong>al</strong>ly <strong>at</strong>tributed to improved selfregul<strong>at</strong>ionof emotions, thoughts, and behaviors. Brown <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>. (2007) arguedth<strong>at</strong> mindfulness improves autonomous self-control, which is associ<strong>at</strong>edwith more effective self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion. Masicampo and Baumeister (2010) <strong>al</strong>soquestioned wh<strong>et</strong>her there is anything unique about mindfulness as <strong>at</strong>echnique for developing self-control.We suggest this is a question for neuroscience. Neuroimaging studies th<strong>at</strong>link brain activity to self-reported st<strong>at</strong>es of mindfulness may be one way tov<strong>al</strong>id<strong>at</strong>e self-reports (e.g., Davidson, 2010) and differenti<strong>at</strong>e mindfulnessfrom other self-regul<strong>at</strong>ory str<strong>at</strong>egies. We do not know wh<strong>et</strong>her brainactivities or structur<strong>al</strong> changes are the same for <strong>al</strong>l practices th<strong>at</strong> improveself-regul<strong>at</strong>ion. Perhaps mindfulness-based techniques are unique; perhapsthey are not.
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 145From the standpoint of basic research, it is critic<strong>al</strong> to the advancement ofknowledge th<strong>at</strong> we continue efforts to separ<strong>at</strong>e mindfulness and rel<strong>at</strong>edst<strong>at</strong>es, traits, medit<strong>at</strong>ion, and programs. But, from the standpoint of <strong>work</strong>, itmay be more v<strong>al</strong>uable to focus on the outcomes (and medi<strong>at</strong>ing processes)by which mindfulness-based practices lead to improved self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion ofthoughts, emotions, and behaviors. An underlying assumption here is th<strong>at</strong>organiz<strong>at</strong>ions might implement mindfulness-based programs as part of theirwellness initi<strong>at</strong>ives. Thus, the focus of organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> research might be tounderstand the effects of mindfulness training r<strong>at</strong>her than mindfulness itself,and to identify fe<strong>at</strong>ures of the <strong>work</strong> environment th<strong>at</strong> support mindfulness.Bringing <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Training into Work Organiz<strong>at</strong>ionsGiven the impressive outcomes associ<strong>at</strong>ed with mindfulness and mindfulness-basedpractices, especi<strong>al</strong>ly in the clinic<strong>al</strong> domain, it is easy to think ofmindfulness practices as a cure-<strong>al</strong>l. Indeed, <strong>al</strong>ready we see research (Giluk,2010; Tipsord, 2009) – with mixed results – linking MBSR to a vari<strong>et</strong>y of<strong>work</strong>-rel<strong>at</strong>ed outcomes including experienced emp<strong>at</strong>hy, affect, citizenshipbehavior, rel<strong>at</strong>ionship qu<strong>al</strong>ity, and job performance. Although we warnagainst the tendency to view mindfulness and mindfulness-based practices asa panacea, we <strong>al</strong>so recognize its demonstr<strong>at</strong>ed efficacy for a number ofimportant <strong>work</strong>-rel<strong>at</strong>ed outcomes, particularly for stress reduction. Accordingto the N<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> Institute for Occup<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> Saf<strong>et</strong>y and He<strong>al</strong>th (1999),about one-third of <strong>work</strong>ers experience high levels of stress. Work stress hasbeen identified as a major cause of employee turnover and burnout and hasbeen linked to many physic<strong>al</strong> and psychologic<strong>al</strong> complaints, includingheadaches, higher incidence of cardiovascular disease, and increased he<strong>al</strong>thinsurance claims (Schn<strong>al</strong>l, Dobson, & Rosskam, 2009). Along with emergingneurobiologic<strong>al</strong> studies linking mindfulness-based practices to changes inthe activity and structure of the brain, research presents compelling evidenc<strong>et</strong>h<strong>at</strong> mindfulness-based practices may be a fruitful addition to organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong>wellness programs.Although the evidence for mindfulness is compelling, we <strong>al</strong>so recogniz<strong>et</strong>h<strong>at</strong> much of the existing liter<strong>at</strong>ure has been conducted outside the <strong>work</strong>environment, with little <strong>at</strong>tention to the contextu<strong>al</strong> fe<strong>at</strong>ures of <strong>work</strong>. Manyexamin<strong>at</strong>ions have used student samples or p<strong>at</strong>ient popul<strong>at</strong>ions seekingtre<strong>at</strong>ment for medic<strong>al</strong> or psychologic<strong>al</strong> symptoms. Thus, the gener<strong>al</strong>izabilityof current research findings to employees in organiz<strong>at</strong>ions is uncertain.Nevertheless, initi<strong>al</strong> research ev<strong>al</strong>u<strong>at</strong>ing the efficacy of mindfulness-based
146THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.training programs for leaders is promising (Shambh<strong>al</strong>a Sun Found<strong>at</strong>ion,2010a). For instance, leaders particip<strong>at</strong>ing in the mindfulness trainingprogram in Gener<strong>al</strong> Mills have experienced dram<strong>at</strong>ic improvement oflistening <strong>at</strong>tentiveness and decision-making qu<strong>al</strong>ity (Gener<strong>al</strong> Mills, 2010).Addition<strong>al</strong> research using rigorous m<strong>et</strong>hodologies is needed.We <strong>al</strong>so considered the possibility th<strong>at</strong> organiz<strong>at</strong>ions might experienceunintended consequences from mindfulness. Given th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnessincreases individu<strong>al</strong>s’ ability to control their thoughts, emotions, andbehaviors, to be more aware of their person<strong>al</strong> go<strong>al</strong>s and v<strong>al</strong>ues, and to bemore <strong>at</strong>tuned to others’ needs, a more mindful employee may act in waysth<strong>at</strong> are counter to the organiz<strong>at</strong>ion’s best interests, by favoring family,person<strong>al</strong> connections, or a slower <strong>work</strong> pace. For example, in the case ofcitizenship behaviors, an organiz<strong>at</strong>ion might benefit because a mindfulemployee is more likely to notice th<strong>at</strong> a co<strong>work</strong>er needs help (via gre<strong>at</strong>er<strong>at</strong>tention to the employee and increased emp<strong>at</strong>hy), but the mindfulemployee may <strong>al</strong>so be more <strong>at</strong>tuned to costs associ<strong>at</strong>ed with helping (e.g.,less time for family) or more aware of task go<strong>al</strong>s and, as a result, choose notto help. This sm<strong>al</strong>l example raises an important issue: The behaviors of moremindful employees will be more intention<strong>al</strong>, but they may not <strong>al</strong>ways lead toself-regul<strong>at</strong>ion th<strong>at</strong> is consistent with organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> go<strong>al</strong>s.We <strong>al</strong>so suspect th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness is easier to cultiv<strong>at</strong>e in certainoccup<strong>at</strong>ions or organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> contexts. One might argue th<strong>at</strong> mindfulnessis diam<strong>et</strong>ric<strong>al</strong>ly opposed to organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> cultures th<strong>at</strong> v<strong>al</strong>ue <strong>work</strong>ing fast,multitasking, and being hyper busy. Perhaps mindfulness would improve<strong>work</strong> qu<strong>al</strong>ity but decrease quantity of <strong>work</strong>. There are a number ofinteresting questions to ask: Wh<strong>at</strong> would an organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> culture th<strong>at</strong>promotes mindfulness look like? How do people enter into mindful st<strong>at</strong>es <strong>at</strong><strong>work</strong>? Do certain conditions in the <strong>work</strong> environment make a mindful st<strong>at</strong>emore likely?We <strong>al</strong>so believe th<strong>at</strong> there may be certain conditions where the effects ofmindfulness will be particularly strong, including situ<strong>at</strong>ions when employeesare de<strong>al</strong>ing with ch<strong>al</strong>lenging roles, when emotion<strong>al</strong> regul<strong>at</strong>ion is required, orduring times of transition. For example, mindfulness might be helpful incross cultur<strong>al</strong> situ<strong>at</strong>ions when response flexibility and affective regul<strong>at</strong>ionare critic<strong>al</strong>. Mindful, nonjudgment<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tention to cultur<strong>al</strong> differences mightimprove the odds of successful exp<strong>at</strong>ri<strong>at</strong>e adjustment, because consciousawareness of differences should result in b<strong>et</strong>ter adapt<strong>at</strong>ion to cultur<strong>al</strong>norms, and because of the increased resilience and improved soci<strong>al</strong>rel<strong>at</strong>ionships th<strong>at</strong> we expect to result from a more mindful orient<strong>at</strong>ion. Asimilar r<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong>e would apply to employees in new roles, such as moving
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 147into a new organiz<strong>at</strong>ion, department, or transitioning into management.Cultiv<strong>at</strong>ing mindfulness might <strong>al</strong>so be benefici<strong>al</strong> for organiz<strong>at</strong>ions duringperiods of large-sc<strong>al</strong>e change. Consider the benefits of increased emp<strong>at</strong>hy,response flexibility, and affective regul<strong>at</strong>ion <strong>al</strong>ong with decreased rumin<strong>at</strong>ionin times of uncertainty and stress such as during a downsizing,restructuring, or merger. The advantages of mindfulness programs might beheightened in such contexts.Before organiz<strong>at</strong>ions adopt existing mindfulness training programs, theyshould carefully consider their go<strong>al</strong>s. If the go<strong>al</strong> is to develop mindfulnessamong employees, they should consider multiple techniques for trainingemployees to be more mindful. Kab<strong>at</strong>-Zinn (2005) discussed MBSR as‘‘scaffolding,’’ but other techniques to cultiv<strong>at</strong>e mindfulness are <strong>al</strong>sopossible. If the go<strong>al</strong> is to develop improved self-regul<strong>at</strong>ory capabilities,results might be obtained via self-control building exercises, or via increasesin self-d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong> (e.g., Bono & Judge, 2003). Focusing directlyon self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion would elimin<strong>at</strong>e uneasiness th<strong>at</strong> might exist in organiz<strong>at</strong>ionsabout the Buddhist roots of mindfulness. Deciding on the ultim<strong>at</strong>ego<strong>al</strong> of an organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> intervention is critic<strong>al</strong>, as there is no reason tobelieve th<strong>at</strong> self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion exercises will lead to the same benefits asmindfulness training, especi<strong>al</strong>ly those associ<strong>at</strong>ed with emp<strong>at</strong>hy, or those th<strong>at</strong>result from changes in the structure of the brain. Further, research hassuggested th<strong>at</strong> other relax<strong>at</strong>ion or medit<strong>at</strong>ion exercises (i.e., loving-kindnessmedit<strong>at</strong>ion designed to increase feelings of soci<strong>al</strong> connection and progressivemuscle relax<strong>at</strong>ion designed for physic<strong>al</strong> relax<strong>at</strong>ion) do not evidence thedecentering th<strong>at</strong> occurs in mindful exercises (Feldman <strong>et</strong> <strong>al</strong>., 2010). Clearly,there is a need for research on optim<strong>al</strong> ways to increase mindfulness <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>and to deline<strong>at</strong>e the effects of different interventions. It may <strong>al</strong>so be th<strong>at</strong>optim<strong>al</strong> practices for each of these outcomes vary for different individu<strong>al</strong>s.It is <strong>al</strong>so important to note th<strong>at</strong> in some organiz<strong>at</strong>ions, core tenants oforigin<strong>al</strong> mindfulness training programs are being removed for theorganiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> audience. For example, programs are not touted as stressreduction programs, given th<strong>at</strong> stress has become a way of life in mostorganiz<strong>at</strong>ions and a badge of honor in many. Nor do some <strong>work</strong>-rel<strong>at</strong>edprograms r<strong>et</strong>ain a link to Buddhism or any other philosophic<strong>al</strong> underpinnings.Thus, it seems reasonable to assume th<strong>at</strong> programs must b<strong>et</strong>ailored to <strong>work</strong> s<strong>et</strong>tings, and adoption of any program must carefullyconsider the elements of the program and the organiz<strong>at</strong>ion’s specific go<strong>al</strong>s.Google’s ‘‘Search Inside Yourself’’ program provides one example of as<strong>et</strong>ting where mindfulness has been positioned not as a stress reductionprogram or one with Buddhist links, but as a program designed to promote
148THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.autonomy, cre<strong>at</strong>ivity, and joy of <strong>work</strong> – <strong>al</strong>l in <strong>al</strong>ignment with organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong>go<strong>al</strong>s and v<strong>al</strong>ues. Although Google’s program includes the same medit<strong>at</strong>iveand contempl<strong>at</strong>ive techniques present in other mindfulness programs, thereare <strong>al</strong>so topics tailored to organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> s<strong>et</strong>tings such as mindful emailing,mindful listening, and de<strong>al</strong>ing with difficult convers<strong>at</strong>ions (Shambh<strong>al</strong>a SunFound<strong>at</strong>ion, 2010b). Indeed, customized mindfulness programs andpractices have been increasingly popular in a vari<strong>et</strong>y of companies such asApple, McKinsey, and Deutsche Bank (Mindfuln<strong>et</strong>, 2011). However, it ishard to know wh<strong>et</strong>her such tailoring to suit organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> purposesundermines the core principles of mindfulness or its outcomes. In the wordsof one of our interviewees:wh<strong>at</strong> I notice is y the seculariz<strong>at</strong>ion of mindfulness, I mean, there’s some good thingsabout th<strong>at</strong> as people are g<strong>et</strong>ting interested and it’s helping them reduce stress, and it’smaking them more effective. Th<strong>at</strong>’s <strong>al</strong>l good. But I’m a little bit worried about thelongevity, if they see it as a tool, if it’s seen as like another tactic, str<strong>at</strong>egy y a lot oftimes people have a very short span of <strong>at</strong>tention for th<strong>at</strong> because it takes years ofpractice. When it’s [mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion] connected to more of a spiritu<strong>al</strong> y religiousteachings, like the Dharma, then it’s much more transform<strong>at</strong>ive, like it’s more of aperson<strong>al</strong> transform<strong>at</strong>ion, it’s much deeper. And then it’s more sustainable because youhave th<strong>at</strong> y it’s not just about being effective <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>, it’s about your life, it’s about howto be happy in your life, so then it’s much deeper and it’s much more transform<strong>at</strong>ive.Then I think there’s a chance for it to be sustainable in the <strong>work</strong>place. But, in the<strong>work</strong>place you have different religions, different creeds, and you could never pull th<strong>at</strong>off. You’d have to secularize the practice. And you lose som<strong>et</strong>hing when you secularizeit, you know, you lose som<strong>et</strong>hing. y it just seems light, and fluffy. It’s not, it doesn’tpen<strong>et</strong>r<strong>at</strong>e y it just feels re<strong>al</strong>ly y surface. <strong>Mindfulness</strong> is re<strong>al</strong>ly powerful.– <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Medit<strong>at</strong>or (Participant #3)CONCLUSIONBoth the words of our participants and the research liter<strong>at</strong>ure suggest th<strong>at</strong>mindfulness is powerful and considering the ways th<strong>at</strong> mindfulness mightimprove people’s <strong>work</strong> lives is exciting. In this chapter, we reviewed theexisting liter<strong>at</strong>ure on mindfulness and explicitly link it to a vari<strong>et</strong>y of core andsecondary processes th<strong>at</strong> are expected to improve <strong>work</strong> outcomes, includingimproved soci<strong>al</strong> rel<strong>at</strong>ionships, resilience, and performance. We see thepotenti<strong>al</strong> for many positive outcomes associ<strong>at</strong>ed with mindfulness <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>,but note th<strong>at</strong> few studies directly test our propositions in <strong>work</strong> s<strong>et</strong>tings or withemployee samples. Building on the strong found<strong>at</strong>ion of existing research, it istime to test the efficacy of mindfulness-based practices in a series of carefullydesigned field experiments or quasi-experiments in <strong>work</strong> s<strong>et</strong>tings.
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 149REFERENCESAltman, D. (2010). The mindfulness code: Keys for overcoming stress, anxi<strong>et</strong>y, fear, andunhappiness. Nov<strong>at</strong>o, CA: New World Library.Aquino, K., Grover, S. L., Bradfield, M., & Allen, D. G. (1999). The effects of neg<strong>at</strong>iveaffectivity, hierarchic<strong>al</strong> st<strong>at</strong>us, and self-d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion on <strong>work</strong>place victimiz<strong>at</strong>ion.Academy of Management Journ<strong>al</strong>, 42, 260–272.Baer, R. A. (2003). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> training as a clinic<strong>al</strong> intervention: A conceptu<strong>al</strong> and empiric<strong>al</strong>review. Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125–143.Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: TheKentucky Inventory of <strong>Mindfulness</strong> Skills. Assessment, 11, 191–206.Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Kri<strong>et</strong>emeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-reportassessment m<strong>et</strong>hods to explore fac<strong>et</strong>s of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45.Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of autom<strong>at</strong>icity: Awareness, efficiency, intention, andcontrol in soci<strong>al</strong> cognition. In: R. S. Wyer, Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds), Handbook of soci<strong>al</strong>cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–40). Hillsd<strong>al</strong>e, NJ: Erlbaum.Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interperson<strong>al</strong><strong>at</strong>tachments as a fundament<strong>al</strong> human motiv<strong>at</strong>ion. Psychologic<strong>al</strong> Bull<strong>et</strong>in, 117, 497–529.Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. NewYork: Guilford Press.Biegel, G. M., Brown, K. W., Shapiro, S. L., & Schubert, C. M. (2009). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-basedstress reduction for the tre<strong>at</strong>ment of adolescent psychi<strong>at</strong>ric outp<strong>at</strong>ients: A randomizedclinic<strong>al</strong> tri<strong>al</strong>. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Consulting and Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 77, 855–866.Bishop, S. R. (2002). Wh<strong>at</strong> do we re<strong>al</strong>ly know about mindfulness-based stress reduction?Psychosom<strong>at</strong>ic Medicine, 64, 71–83.Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Seg<strong>al</strong>, Z. V.,Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Devins, G. (2004). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>: A proposedoper<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> definition. Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230–241.Block-Lerner, J., Adair, C., Plumb, J. C., Rh<strong>at</strong>igan, D. L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2007). The case formindfulness-based approaches in the cultiv<strong>at</strong>ion of emp<strong>at</strong>hy: Does nonjudgment<strong>al</strong>,present-moment awareness increase capacity for perspective-taking and emp<strong>at</strong>hicconcern? Journ<strong>al</strong> of Marit<strong>al</strong> and Family Therapy, 33, 501–516.Bohlmeijer, E., Prenger, R., Ta<strong>al</strong>, E., & Cuijpers, P. (2010). The effects of mindfulness-basedstress reduction therapy on ment<strong>al</strong> he<strong>al</strong>th of adults with a chronic medic<strong>al</strong> disease: Am<strong>et</strong>a-an<strong>al</strong>ysis. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Psychosom<strong>at</strong>ic Research, 68, 539–544.Bono, J. E., & Ilies, R. (2006). Charisma, positive emotions and mood contagion. LeadershipQuarterly, 17, 317–334.Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Self-concordance <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>: Toward understanding themotiv<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> effects of transform<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> leaders. Academy of Management Journ<strong>al</strong>, 46,554–571.Bra¨nström, R., Kvillemo, P., Brandberg, Y., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2010). Self-report mindfulnessas a medi<strong>at</strong>or of psychologic<strong>al</strong> well-being in a stress reduction intervention for cancerp<strong>at</strong>ients – A randomized study. Ann<strong>al</strong>s of Behavior<strong>al</strong> Medicine, 39, 151–161.Brems, C., Fromme, D. K., & Johnson, M. E. (1992). Group modific<strong>at</strong>ion of emp<strong>at</strong>hicverb<strong>al</strong>iz<strong>at</strong>ions and self-disclosure. The Journ<strong>al</strong> of Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 132, 189–200.Brief, A. P., & Weiss, H. M. (2002). Organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> behavior: Affect in the <strong>work</strong>place. Annu<strong>al</strong>Review of Psychology, 53, 279–307.
150THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.Brockner, J., & Hulton, A. B. (1978). How to reverse the vicious cycle of low self-esteem:The importance of <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> focus. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Experiment<strong>al</strong> Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 14,564–578.Broderick, P. C. (2005). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> and coping with dysphoric mood: Contrasts withrumin<strong>at</strong>ion and distraction. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 29, 501–510.Brown, K. W., & Cordon, S. (2009). Toward a phenomenology of mindfulness: Subjectiveexperience and emotion<strong>al</strong> correl<strong>at</strong>es. In: F. Didonna (Ed.), Clinic<strong>al</strong> handbook ofmindfulness (pp. 59–81). New York: Springer.Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: <strong>Mindfulness</strong> and its role inpsychologic<strong>al</strong> well-being. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 84, 822–848.Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>: Theor<strong>et</strong>ic<strong>al</strong> found<strong>at</strong>ionsand evidence for its s<strong>al</strong>utary effects. Psychologic<strong>al</strong> Inquiry, 18, 211–237.Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Medit<strong>at</strong>ion st<strong>at</strong>es and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimagingstudies. Psychologic<strong>al</strong> Bull<strong>et</strong>in, 132, 180–211.Cardaciotto, L., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Moitra, E., & Farrow, V. (2008). Theassessment of present-moment awareness and acceptance: The Philadelphia <strong>Mindfulness</strong>Sc<strong>al</strong>e. Assessment, 15, 204–223.Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2008). Rel<strong>at</strong>ionships b<strong>et</strong>ween mindfulness practice and levels ofmindfulness, medic<strong>al</strong> and psychologic<strong>al</strong> symptoms and well-being in a mindfulnessbasedstress reduction program. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Behavior<strong>al</strong> Medicine, 31, 23–33.Carroll, M. (2006). Awake <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>: 35 practic<strong>al</strong> Buddhist principles for discovering clarity andb<strong>al</strong>ance in the midst of <strong>work</strong>’s chaos (2nd ed.). Boston: Shambh<strong>al</strong>a.Center for <strong>Mindfulness</strong> in Medicine, He<strong>al</strong>th Care, and Soci<strong>et</strong>y, University of Massachus<strong>et</strong>tsMedic<strong>al</strong> School. (2010). Stress reduction program. Available <strong>at</strong> http://www.umassmed.edu/Content.aspx?id=41254&LinkIdentifier=id. R<strong>et</strong>rieved on December 30, 2010.Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus system<strong>at</strong>ic inform<strong>at</strong>ion processing and the use of sourceversus message cues in persuasion. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 39, 752–766.Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regul<strong>at</strong>ion: An integr<strong>at</strong>ivereview. Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology Review, 29, 560–572.Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness trainingon <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32,303–322.Ch<strong>at</strong>zisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2007). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> and the intention-behaviorrel<strong>at</strong>ionship within the theory of planned behavior. Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong> PsychologyBull<strong>et</strong>in, 33, 663–676.Chiesa, A., & Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, A. (2009). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-based stress reduction for stress management inhe<strong>al</strong>thy people: A review and m<strong>et</strong>a-an<strong>al</strong>ysis. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Altern<strong>at</strong>ive and ComplementaryMedicine, 15, 593–600.Chiesa, A., & Serr<strong>et</strong>ti, A. (2010). A system<strong>at</strong>ic review of neurobiologic<strong>al</strong> and clinic<strong>al</strong> fe<strong>at</strong>ures ofmindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ions. Psychologic<strong>al</strong> Medicine: A Journ<strong>al</strong> of Research in Psychi<strong>at</strong>ryand the Allied Sciences, 40, 1239–1252.Cohen, J. S., & Miller, L. J. (2009). Interperson<strong>al</strong> mindfulness training for well-being: A pilotstudy with psychology gradu<strong>at</strong>e students. Teachers College Record, 111, 2760–2774.Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., & Conway, A. M. (2009).Happiness unpacked: Positive emotions increase life s<strong>at</strong>isfaction by building resilience.Emotion, 9, 361–368.
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 151Cozolino, L. (2006). The neuroscience of human rel<strong>at</strong>ionships: Attachment and the developingsoci<strong>al</strong> brain. New York: Norton.Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neur<strong>al</strong> correl<strong>at</strong>esof disposition<strong>al</strong> mindfulness during affect labeling. Psychosom<strong>at</strong>ic Medicine, 69, 560–565.D<strong>al</strong>ai Lama. (2002). Live in a b<strong>et</strong>ter way: Reflections on truth, love, and happiness. New York:Penguin Compass.Dane, E. (2010). Reconsidering the trade-off b<strong>et</strong>ween expertise and flexibility: A cognitiveentrenchment perspective. Academy of Management Review, 35, 579–603.Davids, T. W. R., & Stede, W. (Eds). (1959). The P<strong>al</strong>i Text Soci<strong>et</strong>y’s P<strong>al</strong>i-English dictionary.London: Luzac & Company, Ltd.Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychop<strong>at</strong>hology, and resilience: Brain mechanisms andplasticity. American Psychologist, 55, 1196–1214.Davidson, R. J. (2010). Empiric<strong>al</strong> explor<strong>at</strong>ions of mindfulness: Conceptu<strong>al</strong> and m<strong>et</strong>hodologic<strong>al</strong>conundrums. Emotion, 10, 8–11.Davidson, R. J., Kab<strong>at</strong>-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F.,Urbanowski, F., Harrington, A., Bonus, K., & Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alter<strong>at</strong>ions inbrain and immune function produced by mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion. Psychosom<strong>at</strong>icMedicine, 65, 564–570.Davis, M. C. (2009). Building emotion<strong>al</strong> resilience to promote he<strong>al</strong>th. American Journ<strong>al</strong> ofLifestyle Medicine, 3, 60–63.Delgado, L. C., Guerra, P., Perakakis, P., Vera, M. N., del Paso, G. R., & Vila, J. (2010).Tre<strong>at</strong>ing chronic worry: Psychologic<strong>al</strong> and physiologic<strong>al</strong> effects of a training programmebased on mindfulness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 873–882.Delmonte, M. M. (1990). The relevance of medit<strong>at</strong>ion to clinic<strong>al</strong> practice: An overview. AppliedPsychology: An Intern<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> Review, 39, 331–354.Dimidjian, S., & Linehan, M. M. (2003). Defining an agenda for future research on the clinic<strong>al</strong>applic<strong>at</strong>ion of mindfulness practice. Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 166–171.Ditto, B., Eclache, M., & Goldman, N. (2006). Short-term autonomic and cardiovasculareffects of mindfulness body scan medit<strong>at</strong>ion. Ann<strong>al</strong>s of Behavior<strong>al</strong> Medicine, 32, 227–234.Dobkin, P. L. (2008). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-based stress reduction: Wh<strong>at</strong> processes are <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>?Complementary Therapies in Clinic<strong>al</strong> Practice, 14, 8–16.Douglas, S. C., & Martinko, M. J. (2001). Exploring the role of individu<strong>al</strong> differences in theprediction of <strong>work</strong>place aggression. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Applied Psychology, 86, 547–559.Duerr, M. (2004a). The contempl<strong>at</strong>ive organiz<strong>at</strong>ion. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> ChangeManagement, 17, 43–61.Duerr, M. (2004b). A powerful silence: The role of medit<strong>at</strong>ion and other contempl<strong>at</strong>ivepractices in American life and <strong>work</strong>. R<strong>et</strong>rieved from the Center for Contempl<strong>at</strong>ive Mindin Soci<strong>et</strong>y (http://www.contempl<strong>at</strong>ivemind.org).<strong>Duffy</strong>, M. K., Ganster, D., & Pagon, M. (2002). Soci<strong>al</strong> undermining in the <strong>work</strong>place. Academyof Management Journ<strong>al</strong>, 45, 331–351.Dutton, J. E., & Heaphy, E. D. (2003). The power of high-qu<strong>al</strong>ity connections. In:K.S. Cameron, J. E. Dutton & R. E. Quinn (Eds), Positive organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> scholarship:Found<strong>at</strong>ions of a new discipline (pp. 263–278). San Francisco: Berr<strong>et</strong>t-Koehler Publishers.Elzinga, B. M., & Roelofs, K. (2005). Cortisol-induced impairments of <strong>work</strong>ing memory requireacute symp<strong>at</strong>h<strong>et</strong>ic activ<strong>at</strong>ion. Behavior<strong>al</strong> Neuroscience, 119, 98–103.
152THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.Emanuel, A. S., Updegraff, J. A., K<strong>al</strong>mbach, D. A., & Ciesla, J. A. (2010). The role ofmindfulness fac<strong>et</strong>s in affective forecasting. Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Individu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 49,815–818.Erisman, S. M., & Roemer, L. (2010). A preliminary investig<strong>at</strong>ion of the effects ofexperiment<strong>al</strong>ly induced mindfulness on emotion<strong>al</strong> responding to film clips. Emotion,10, 72–82.Evans, D. R., Baer, R. A., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2009). The effects of mindfulness and selfconsciousnesson persistence. Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Individu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 47, 379–382.F<strong>al</strong>kenström, F. (2010). Studying mindfulness in experienced medit<strong>at</strong>ors: A quasi-experiment<strong>al</strong>approach. Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Individu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 48, 305–310.Farb, N. A. S., Anderson, A. K., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D., & Seg<strong>al</strong>, Z. V. (2010).Minding one’s emotions: <strong>Mindfulness</strong> training <strong>al</strong>ters the neur<strong>al</strong> expression of sadness.Emotion, 10, 25–33.Feldman, G., Greeson, J., & Senville, J. (2010). Differenti<strong>al</strong> effects of mindful bre<strong>at</strong>hing,progressive muscle relax<strong>at</strong>ion, and loving-kindness medit<strong>at</strong>ion on decentering andneg<strong>at</strong>ive reactions to rep<strong>et</strong>itive thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 1002–1011.Foley, E., Baillie, A., Huxter, M., Price, M., & Sinclair, E. (2010). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-based cognitiv<strong>et</strong>herapy for individu<strong>al</strong>s whose lives have been affected by cancer: A randomizedcontrolled tri<strong>al</strong>. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Consulting and Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 78, 72–79.Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). Wh<strong>at</strong> good are positive emotions? Review of Gener<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 2,300–319.Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open heartsbuild lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness medit<strong>at</strong>ion, buildconsequenti<strong>al</strong> person<strong>al</strong> resources. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 95,1045–1062.Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J. A., Neufeld, R. W. J., Lane, R. D., Densmore, M., Stevens, T. K.,& Lanius, R. A. (2010). Individu<strong>al</strong> differences in trait mindfulness predict dorsomedi<strong>al</strong>prefront<strong>al</strong> and amygd<strong>al</strong>a response during emotion<strong>al</strong> imagery: An fMRI study.Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Individu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 49, 479–484.Frewen, P. A., Evans, E. M., Maraj, N., Dozois, D. J. A., & Partridge, K. (2008). L<strong>et</strong>ting go:<strong>Mindfulness</strong> and neg<strong>at</strong>ive autom<strong>at</strong>ic thinking. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 758–774.Gener<strong>al</strong> Mills. (2010). Leadership program helps train the mind. Available <strong>at</strong> http://www.gener<strong>al</strong>mills.com/Home/Media/Inside_Gener<strong>al</strong>_Mills_archive/leadership_6_8_2010.R<strong>et</strong>rieved on February 22, 2011.Germer, C. K. (2005). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>: Wh<strong>at</strong> is it? Wh<strong>at</strong> does it m<strong>at</strong>ter? In: C. K. Germer,R.D. Siegel & P. R. Fulton (Eds), <strong>Mindfulness</strong> and psychotherapy (pp. 3–27). New York,NY: Guilford Press.Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Whe<strong>at</strong>ley, T. P. (1998). Immuneneglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity andSoci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 75, 617–638.Giluk, T. L. (2009). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>, Big Five person<strong>al</strong>ity, and affect: A m<strong>et</strong>a-an<strong>al</strong>ysis. Person<strong>al</strong>ityand Individu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 47, 805–811.Giluk, T. L. (2010). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-based stress reduction: Facilit<strong>at</strong>ing <strong>work</strong> outcomes throughexperienced affect and high-qu<strong>al</strong>ity rel<strong>at</strong>ionships. Doctor<strong>al</strong> dissert<strong>at</strong>ion. Available <strong>at</strong> IowaResearch Online.
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 153Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) onemotion regul<strong>at</strong>ion in soci<strong>al</strong> anxi<strong>et</strong>y disorder. Emotion, 10, 83–91.Goleman, D. (1977). The vari<strong>et</strong>ies of the medit<strong>at</strong>ive experience. New York: Dutton.Grant, J. A., Courtemanche, J., Duerden, E. G., Duncan, G. H., & Rainville, P. (2010). Cortic<strong>al</strong>thickness and pain sensitivity in Zen medit<strong>at</strong>ors. Emotion, 10, 43–53.Grant, J. A., & Rainville, P. (2009). Pain sensitivity and an<strong>al</strong>gesic effects of mindful st<strong>at</strong>es inZen medit<strong>at</strong>ors: A cross-section<strong>al</strong> study. Psychosom<strong>at</strong>ic Medicine, 71, 106–114.Grepmair, L., Mitterlehner, F., Rother, W., & Nickel, M. (2006). Promotion of mindfulness inpsychotherapists in training and tre<strong>at</strong>ment results of their p<strong>at</strong>ients. Journ<strong>al</strong> ofPsychosom<strong>at</strong>ic Research, 60, 649–650.Grossman, P. (2008). On measuring mindfulness in psychosom<strong>at</strong>ic and psychologic<strong>al</strong> research.Journ<strong>al</strong> of Psychosom<strong>at</strong>ic Research, 64, 405–408.Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & W<strong>al</strong>ach, H. (2004). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-based stressreduction and he<strong>al</strong>th benefits: A m<strong>et</strong>a-an<strong>al</strong>ysis. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Psychosom<strong>at</strong>ic Research, 57,35–43.Hagen, S. (2003). Buddhism is not wh<strong>at</strong> you think: Finding freedom beyond beliefs. New York:HarperCollins Publishers.Hammond, J. S., Keeney, R. L., & Raiffa, H. (2006). The hidden traps in decision making.Harvard Business Review, 84, 118–126.Hargus, E., Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., & Williams, J. M. G. (2010). Effects of mindfulness onm<strong>et</strong>a-awareness and specificity of describing prodrom<strong>al</strong> symptoms in suicid<strong>al</strong> depression.Emotion, 10, 34–42.Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2000). Modul<strong>at</strong>ing emotion<strong>al</strong> responses:Effects of a neocortic<strong>al</strong> n<strong>et</strong><strong>work</strong> on the limbic system. NeuroReport: For RapidCommunic<strong>at</strong>ion of Neuroscience Research, 11, 43–48.Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., & Hayes, T. L. (2002). Business-unit-level rel<strong>at</strong>ionship b<strong>et</strong>weenemployee s<strong>at</strong>isfaction, employee engagement, and business outcomes: A m<strong>et</strong>a-an<strong>al</strong>ysis.Journ<strong>al</strong> of Applied Psychology, 87, 268–279.Heppner, W. L., & Kernis, M. H. (2007). ‘‘Qui<strong>et</strong> ego’’ functioning: The complementary roles ofmindfulness, authenticity, and secure high self-esteem. Psychologic<strong>al</strong> Inquiry, 18, 248–251.Heppner, W. L., Kernis, M. H., Lakey, C. E., Campbell, W. K., Goldman, B. M., Davis, P. J., &Cascio, E. V. (2008). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> as a means of reducing aggressive behavior:Disposition<strong>al</strong> and situ<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> evidence. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 486–496.Herndon, F. (2008). Testing mindfulness with perceptu<strong>al</strong> and cognitive factors: Extern<strong>al</strong> vs.intern<strong>al</strong> encoding, and the cognitive failures questionnaire. Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Individu<strong>al</strong>Differences, 44, 32–41.Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. (2010). The effect of mindfulness-basedtherapy on anxi<strong>et</strong>y and depression: A m<strong>et</strong>a-an<strong>al</strong>ytic review. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Consulting andClinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 78, 169–183.Howell, A. J., Digdon, N. L., & Buro, K. (2010). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> predicts sleep-rel<strong>at</strong>ed selfregul<strong>at</strong>ionand well-being. Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Individu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 48, 419–424.Howell, A. J., Digdon, N. L., Buro, K., & Sheptycki, A. R. (2008). Rel<strong>at</strong>ions amongmindfulness, well-being, and sleep. Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Individu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 45, 773–777.Hughes, R., Tingle, B. A., & Sawin, D. B. (1981). Development of emp<strong>at</strong>hic understanding inchildren. Child Development, 52, 122–128.Hutcherson, C. A., Sepp<strong>al</strong>a, E. M., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Loving-kindness medit<strong>at</strong>ion increasessoci<strong>al</strong> connectedness. Emotion, 8, 720–724.
154THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.Ingram, R. E., & Smith, T. W. (1984). Depression and intern<strong>al</strong> versus extern<strong>al</strong> focus of<strong>at</strong>tention. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 139–151.Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and soci<strong>al</strong> behavior. Advances inExperiment<strong>al</strong> Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 20, 203–253.Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining theprotective effects of mindfulness training on <strong>work</strong>ing memory capacity and affectiveexperience. Emotion, 10, 54–64.Jimenez, S. S., Niles, B. L., & Park, C. L. (2010). A mindfulness model of affect regul<strong>at</strong>ion anddepressive symptoms: Positive emotions, mood regul<strong>at</strong>ion expectancies, and selfacceptanceas regul<strong>at</strong>ory mechanisms. Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Individu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 49, 645–650.Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Erez, A., & Locke, E. A. (2005). Core self-ev<strong>al</strong>u<strong>at</strong>ions and job and lifes<strong>at</strong>isfaction: The role of self-concordance and go<strong>al</strong> <strong>at</strong>tainment. Journ<strong>al</strong> of AppliedPsychology, 90, 257–268.Kab<strong>at</strong>-Zinn, J. (1990). Full c<strong>at</strong>astrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to facestress, pain, and illness. New York, NY: Delacorte Press.Kab<strong>at</strong>-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: <strong>Mindfulness</strong> medit<strong>at</strong>ion in everyday life.New York, NY: Hyperion.Kab<strong>at</strong>-Zinn, J. (2005). Coming to our senses: He<strong>al</strong>ing ourselves and the world throughmindfulness. New York, NY: Hyperion.Kamdar, D., McAllister, D. J., & Turban, D. B. (2006). ‘‘All in a day’s <strong>work</strong>’’: How followerindividu<strong>al</strong> differences and justice perceptions predict OCB role definitions and behavior.Journ<strong>al</strong> of Applied Psychology, 91, 841–855.Kell<strong>et</strong>t, J. B., Humphrey, R. H., & Sle<strong>et</strong>h, R. G. (2002). Emp<strong>at</strong>hy and complex taskperformance: Two routes to leadership. Leadership Quarterly, 13, 523–544.Kernis, M. H., Paradise, A. W., Whitaker, D. J., Whe<strong>at</strong>man, S. R., & Goldman, B. N. (2000).Master of one’s psychologic<strong>al</strong> domain? Not likely if one’s self-esteem is unstable.Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology Bull<strong>et</strong>in, 26, 1297–1305.Kidder, D. L. (2002). The influence of gender on the performance of organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> citizenshipbehaviors. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Management, 28, 629–648.Lakey, C. E., Campbell, W. K., Brown, K. W., & Goodie, A. S. (2007). Disposition<strong>al</strong>mindfulness as a predictor of the severity of gambling outcomes. Person<strong>al</strong>ity andIndividu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 43, 1698–1710.Langer, E. J. (1989a). Minding m<strong>at</strong>ters: The consequences of mindlessness-mindfulness.Advances in Experiment<strong>al</strong> Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 22, 137–173.Langer, E. J. (1989b). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Lehrer, P. M., Woolfolk, R. L., & Sime, W. E. (Eds). (2007). Principles and practice of stressmanagement (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Ljo´tsson, B., F<strong>al</strong>k, L., Vesterlund, A. W., Hedman, E., Lindfors, P., Ru¨ck, C., Hursti, T.,Andréewitch, S., Jansson, L., Lindefors, N., & Andersson, G. (2010). Intern<strong>et</strong>-deliveredexposure and mindfulness based therapy for irritable bowel syndrome – A randomizedcontrolled tri<strong>al</strong>. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 531–539.Losada, M. (1999). The complex dynamics of high performance teams. M<strong>at</strong>hem<strong>at</strong>ic<strong>al</strong> andComputer Modelling, 30, 179–192.Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regul<strong>at</strong>ion andmonitoring in medit<strong>at</strong>ion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163–169.Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Doeshappiness lead to success? Psychologic<strong>al</strong> Bull<strong>et</strong>in, 131, 803–855.
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 155Masicampo, E. J. & Baumeister, R. F. (2010). Unfulfilled go<strong>al</strong>s interfere with tasks th<strong>at</strong> requireexecutive functions. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Experiment<strong>al</strong> Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.011.Mindfuln<strong>et</strong>. (2011). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> in the <strong>work</strong>place. Available <strong>at</strong> http://www.mindfuln<strong>et</strong>.org/page9.htm. R<strong>et</strong>rieved on February 23, 2011.Miner, A. G., <strong>Glomb</strong>, T. M., & Hulin, C. (2005). Experience sampling mood and its correl<strong>at</strong>es<strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Occup<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> and Organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 78, 171–193.Mitchell, D. W. (2002). Buddhism: Introducing the Buddhist experience. New York: OxfordUniversity Press.N<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> Institute for Occup<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> Saf<strong>et</strong>y and He<strong>al</strong>th. (1999). Stress <strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>. DHHS [NIOSH]Public<strong>at</strong>ion no. 99-101. Cincinn<strong>at</strong>i, OH: Public<strong>at</strong>ions Dissemin<strong>at</strong>ion, EIDNolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the dur<strong>at</strong>ion ofdepressive episodes. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Abnorm<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 100, 569–582.Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Davis, C. G. (1999). ‘‘Thanks for sharing th<strong>at</strong>’’: Rumin<strong>at</strong>ors and theirsoci<strong>al</strong> support n<strong>et</strong><strong>work</strong>s. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 77, 801–814.Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression andposttraum<strong>at</strong>ic stress symptoms after a n<strong>at</strong>ur<strong>al</strong> disaster: The 1989 Loma Pri<strong>et</strong>aearthquake. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 61, 115–121.Nyanaponika, T. (1998). In: B. Bodhi (Ed.), Abhidhamma studies: Buddhist explor<strong>at</strong>ions ofconsciousness and time (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Wisdom Public<strong>at</strong>ions.Oei, N. Y. L., Everaerd, W. T. A. M., Elzinga, B. M., Van Well, S., & Bermond, B. (2006).Psychosoci<strong>al</strong> stress impairs <strong>work</strong>ing memory <strong>at</strong> high loads: An associ<strong>at</strong>ion with cortisollevels and memory r<strong>et</strong>riev<strong>al</strong>. Stress: The Intern<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> Journ<strong>al</strong> on the Biology of Stress, 9,133–141.O’Leary-Kelly, A. M., Bowes-Sperry, L., B<strong>at</strong>es, C. A., & Lean, E. R. (2009). Sexu<strong>al</strong> harassment<strong>at</strong> <strong>work</strong>: A decade (plus) of progress. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Management, 35, 503–536.P<strong>at</strong>ient, D. L., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2010). Increasing interperson<strong>al</strong> and inform<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> justicewhen communic<strong>at</strong>ing neg<strong>at</strong>ive news: The role of the manager’s emp<strong>at</strong>hic concern andmor<strong>al</strong> development. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Management, 36, 555–578.Perlman, D. M., S<strong>al</strong>omons, T. V., Davidson, R. J., & Lutz, A. (2010). Differenti<strong>al</strong> effects onpain intensity and unpleasantness of two medit<strong>at</strong>ion practices. Emotion, 10, 65–71.Radel, R., Sarrazin, P., Legrain, P., & Gobance, L. (2009). Sublimin<strong>al</strong> priming of motiv<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong>orient<strong>at</strong>ion in educ<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> s<strong>et</strong>tings: Effect on academic performance moder<strong>at</strong>ed bymindfulness. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Research in Person<strong>al</strong>ity, 43, 695–698.Ramel, W., Goldin, P. R., Carmona, P. E., & McQuaid, J. R. (2004). The effects of mindfulnessmedit<strong>at</strong>ion on cognitive processes and affect in p<strong>at</strong>ients with past depression. CognitiveTherapy and Research, 28, 433–455.Roemer, L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2003). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>: A promising intervention str<strong>at</strong>egy in need offurther study. Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 172–178.Roozenda<strong>al</strong>, B. (2002). Stress and memory: Opposing effects of glucocorticoids onmemory consolid<strong>at</strong>ion and memory r<strong>et</strong>riev<strong>al</strong>. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory,78, 578–595.Roth, B., & Robbins, D. (2004). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-based stress reduction and he<strong>al</strong>th-rel<strong>at</strong>ed qu<strong>al</strong>ityof life: Findings from a bilingu<strong>al</strong> inner-city p<strong>at</strong>ient popul<strong>at</strong>ion. Psychosom<strong>at</strong>ic Medicine,66, 113–123.Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion theory and the facilit<strong>at</strong>ion of intrinsicmotiv<strong>at</strong>ion, soci<strong>al</strong> development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
156THERESA M. GLOMB ET AL.Saavedra, M. C., Chapman, K. E., & Rogge, R. D. (2010). Clarifying links b<strong>et</strong>ween <strong>at</strong>tachmentand rel<strong>at</strong>ionship qu<strong>al</strong>ity: Hostile conflict and mindfulness as moder<strong>at</strong>ors. Journ<strong>al</strong> ofFamily Psychology, 24, 380–390.Schmeichel, B. J., Volokhov, R. N., & Demaree, H. A. (2008). Working memory capacity andthe self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion of emotion<strong>al</strong> expression and experience. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity andSoci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 95, 1526–1540.Schn<strong>al</strong>l, P. L., Dobson, M., & Rosskam, E. (Eds). (2009). Unhe<strong>al</strong>thy <strong>work</strong>: Causes,consequences, cures. Amityville, NY: Baywood Publishing.Scott, B. A., Colquitt, J. A., Paddock, E. L., & Judge, T. A. (2010). A daily investig<strong>at</strong>ion of therole of manager emp<strong>at</strong>hy on employee well-being. Organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> Behavior and <strong>Human</strong>Decision Processes, 113, 127–140.Sears, S., & Kraus, S. (2009). I think therefore I am: Cognitive distortions and coping style asmedi<strong>at</strong>ors for the effects of mindfulness medit<strong>at</strong>ion on anxi<strong>et</strong>y, positive and neg<strong>at</strong>iveaffect, and hope. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 65, 561–573.Seg<strong>al</strong>, Z. V., Williams, M. G., & Teasd<strong>al</strong>e, J. D. (2002). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>-based cognitive therapy fordepression: A new approach to preventing relapse. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Shambh<strong>al</strong>a Sun Found<strong>at</strong>ion. (2010a). Finding the space to lead. Available <strong>at</strong> http://mindful.org/<strong>at</strong>-<strong>work</strong>/leadership/finding-the-space-to-lead. R<strong>et</strong>rieved on December 31,2010.Shambh<strong>al</strong>a Sun Found<strong>at</strong>ion. (2010b). Google searches. Available <strong>at</strong> http://mindful.org/<strong>at</strong><strong>work</strong>/in-the-<strong>work</strong>place/google-searches.R<strong>et</strong>rieved on December 31, 2010.Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness.Journ<strong>al</strong> of Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 62, 373–386.Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G. E., & Bonner, G. (1998). Effects of mindfulness-based stressreduction on medic<strong>al</strong> and premedic<strong>al</strong> students. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Behavior<strong>al</strong> Medicine, 21,581–599.Shaw, J. D., <strong>Duffy</strong>, M. K., Mitra, A., Lockhart, D. E., & Bowler, M. (2003). Reactions to meritpay increases: A longitudin<strong>al</strong> test of a sign<strong>al</strong> sensitivity perspective. Journ<strong>al</strong> of AppliedPsychology, 88, 538–544.Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Go<strong>al</strong> striving, need s<strong>at</strong>isfaction, and longitudin<strong>al</strong> wellbeing:The self-concordance model. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 76,482–497.Sheldon, K. M., Turban, D. B., Brown, K. G., Barrick, M. R., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Applyingself-d<strong>et</strong>ermin<strong>at</strong>ion theory to organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> research. Research in Personnel and <strong>Human</strong><strong>Resources</strong> Management, 22, 357–393.Siegel, D. J. (2007). The mindful brain: Reflection and <strong>at</strong>tunement in the cultiv<strong>at</strong>ion of well-being.New York: Norton.Siegel, D. J. (2010). Mindsight: The new science of person<strong>al</strong> transform<strong>at</strong>ion. New York: Bantam.Siegel, R. D., Germer, C. K., & Olendzki, A. (2009). <strong>Mindfulness</strong>: Wh<strong>at</strong> is it? Where did it comefrom? In: F. Didonna (Ed.), Clinic<strong>al</strong> handbook of mindfulness (pp. 17–35). New York:Springer.Sy, T., Coˆ té, S., & Saavedra, R. (2005). The contagious leader: Impact of the leader’s mood onthe mood of group members, group affective tone, and group processes. Journ<strong>al</strong> ofApplied Psychology, 90, 295–305.Teasd<strong>al</strong>e, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Seg<strong>al</strong>, Z. V. (2002).M<strong>et</strong>acognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empiric<strong>al</strong> evidence.Journ<strong>al</strong> of Consulting and Clinic<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 70, 275–287.
<strong>Mindfulness</strong> <strong>at</strong> Work 157Teasd<strong>al</strong>e, J. D., Seg<strong>al</strong>, Z., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does cognitive therapy preventdepressive relapse and why should <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> control (mindfulness) training help?Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 25–39.Tepper, B. J., <strong>Duffy</strong>, M. K., Henle, C. A., & Lambert, L. S. (2006). Procedur<strong>al</strong> injustice, victimprecipit<strong>at</strong>ion, and abusive supervision. Personnel Psychology, 59, 101–123.Thau, S., Aquino, K., & Poortvli<strong>et</strong>, P. M. (2007). Self-defe<strong>at</strong>ing behaviors in organiz<strong>at</strong>ions: Therel<strong>at</strong>ionship b<strong>et</strong>ween thwarted belonging and interperson<strong>al</strong> <strong>work</strong> behaviors. Journ<strong>al</strong> ofApplied Psychology, 92, 840–847.Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., & Zhang, L. (2004). The role of emotion in self-regul<strong>at</strong>ion:Differing roles of positive and neg<strong>at</strong>ive emotion. In: P. Philippot & R. S. Feldman (Eds),The regul<strong>at</strong>ion of emotion (pp. 213–226). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Tipsord, J. (2009). The effects of mindfulness training and individu<strong>al</strong> difference in mindfulness onsoci<strong>al</strong> perception and emp<strong>at</strong>hy. Unpublished doctor<strong>al</strong> dissert<strong>at</strong>ion, University of Oregon,Portland, OR.Tomarken, A. J., Davidson, R. J., & Henriques, J. B. (1990). Resting front<strong>al</strong> brain asymm<strong>et</strong>rypredicts affective responses to films. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong> Psychology, 59,791–801.Treadway, M. T., & Lazar, S. W. (2009). The neurobiology of mindfulness. In: F. Didonna(Ed.), Clinic<strong>al</strong> handbook of mindfulness (pp. 45–57). New York: Springer.Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., Jackson, D. C., D<strong>al</strong>ton, K. M., Mueller, C. J.,Rosenkranz, M. A., Ryff, C. D., Singer, B. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Making a lifeworth living: Neur<strong>al</strong> correl<strong>at</strong>es of well-being. Psychologic<strong>al</strong> Science, 15, 367–372.Verplanken, B., Friborg, O., Wang, C. E., Trafimow, D., & Woolf, K. (2007). Ment<strong>al</strong> habits:M<strong>et</strong>acognitive reflection on neg<strong>at</strong>ive self-thinking. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Soci<strong>al</strong>Psychology, 92, 526–541.W<strong>al</strong>sh, J. J., B<strong>al</strong>int, M. G., Smolira SJ, D. R., Fredericksen, L. K., & Madsen, S. (2009).Predicting individu<strong>al</strong> differences in mindfulness: The role of trait anxi<strong>et</strong>y, <strong>at</strong>tachmentanxi<strong>et</strong>y and <strong>at</strong>tention<strong>al</strong> control. Person<strong>al</strong>ity and Individu<strong>al</strong> Differences, 46, 94–99.Way, B. M., Creswell, J. D., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Disposition<strong>al</strong>mindfulness and depressive symptom<strong>at</strong>ology: Correl<strong>at</strong>ions with limbic and selfreferenti<strong>al</strong>neur<strong>al</strong> activity during rest. Emotion, 10, 12–24.Weick, K. E., & Putnam, T. (2006). Organizing for mindfulness: Eastern wisdom and Westernknowledge. Journ<strong>al</strong> of Management Inquiry, 15, 275–287.Weick, K. E., & Sutcliffe, K. M. (2006). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> and the qu<strong>al</strong>ity of organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong><strong>at</strong>tention. Organiz<strong>at</strong>ion Science, 17, 514–524.Weick, K. E., & Sutcliffe, K. M. (2007). Managing the unexpected: Resilient performance in anage of uncertainty (2nd ed.). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Weick, K. E., Sutcliffe, K. M., & Obstfeld, D. (1999). Organizing for high reliability: Processesof collective mindfulness. Research in Organiz<strong>at</strong>ion<strong>al</strong> Behavior, 21, 81–123.Weinstein, N., Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). A multi-m<strong>et</strong>hod examin<strong>at</strong>ion of the effectsof mindfulness on stress <strong>at</strong>tribution, coping, and emotion<strong>al</strong> well-being. Journ<strong>al</strong> ofResearch in Person<strong>al</strong>ity, 43, 374–385.Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Medit<strong>at</strong>ion can reduce habitu<strong>al</strong> responding. Altern<strong>at</strong>ive Therapies inHe<strong>al</strong>th and Medicine, 11, 42–58.Williams, J. M. G. (2010). <strong>Mindfulness</strong> and psychologic<strong>al</strong> process. Emotion, 10, 1–7.Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. Advances in Experiment<strong>al</strong> Soci<strong>al</strong>Psychology, 35, 345–411.