19.09.2015 Views

An Interview with Dr Maryanne Wolf

An Interview with Dr. Maryanne Wolf

An Interview with Dr. Maryanne Wolf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

1<br />

<strong>An</strong> <strong>Interview</strong> <strong>with</strong> <strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong><br />

By David Boulton<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong> is the Director of the Center for Reading and Language Research at Tufts University,<br />

where she is an Associate Professor of Child Development. She is the author of Proust and the Squid<br />

and the editor of Dyslexia, Fluency and the Brain, and has also written/designed three empirically proven<br />

instructional programs on thinking skills for middle school students, on reading and writing for elementary<br />

school students, and on linguistic awareness for emergent readers. <strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Wolf</strong> has published hundreds of<br />

articles on reading and learning disabilities.<br />

Our interview <strong>with</strong> <strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Wolf</strong> is a rich wide ranging dialogue that explores, among other things, rapid<br />

naming, the double-deficit hypothesis, processing speed, and dyslexia. <strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Wolf</strong> is brilliant, passionate,<br />

and full of positive energy which she focuses toward helping struggling children. She was a delight to talk<br />

<strong>with</strong>.<br />

Personal Background:<br />

David Boulton: Thank you for taking the time to talk <strong>with</strong> us.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Of course.<br />

David Boulton: How is it that you've come to the focus you have? Tell me about the passion that's driving your<br />

research and your work to help kids?<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: I'll give a brief version of a very long story. It began many years ago when I was in a Peace<br />

Corps-like mission, if you can call it that. I was to go to a Native American reservation and everything fell<br />

through, and suddenly I landed in a Filipino mission school in Hawaii. First I think I'm going to North Dakota and<br />

next I'm ending up in tropical Hawaii. It was not respectable in terms of political cache, but it was beautiful. On<br />

the other hand the poverty, the needs were just extraordinary.<br />

When I began teaching I was assigned to a third grade class <strong>with</strong> ten different languages in it, and kids <strong>with</strong><br />

every possible need: fetal alcohol syndrome, certainly bi-lingualism was a big factor, one child was retarded.<br />

Many kids had various kinds of literacy special needs and they were all mine. I just had a year that transformed<br />

my life. I became so aware of the consequences, the sequel of what not learning how to read would mean for<br />

these kids in terms of the rest of their lives.<br />

The little village that we had had these single rocking chairs and I said, ‘Why are there all these single rocking<br />

chairs?’ They were for the Filipino men who could never earn enough money to bring their families over, as<br />

promised, from the Philippine Islands. They were like the living testimony to all the failed hopes and<br />

exploitation, absolute exploitation of these people. <strong>An</strong>d they couldn't read.<br />

Their children were having all kinds of struggles and I knew if I couldn't do it, their lives were not going to be<br />

changed . I also knew if I could help them, I wasn't going to prevent everything else from going wrong, but that<br />

I would have eliminated a very key factor in their not meeting their potential.<br />

I was an English major and I had a Master's degree. I had my Ph.D. set up to become a Rilke scholar in poetry.<br />

<strong>An</strong>d it just snapped every decision I had made before.


2<br />

I came back to the mainland and almost immediately went into a program that prepared me to switch<br />

completely. The following year I went to Harvard to the reading laboratory and I just was determined from that<br />

moment on that I was going to use my life to help kids through learning how to read. That was my vehicle to<br />

ensure that they're going to at least have a shot at their potential. That that [reading failure] was not going to<br />

hold them back.<br />

That experience at Harvard's reading lab introduced me to all kinds of different approaches to the problem. I<br />

had been thinking about it, more or less, from almost a sociological viewpoint. When I got to Harvard it was a<br />

surprise to me. Here I am an English major and I became a scientist. I became totally involved in neuroscience<br />

because it really seemed to me that we were going to get answers the likes of which we had never had before<br />

in education from understanding what is going on in the reading brain. So, that became my quest, if you will, to<br />

understand what it is the brain does when we read and how we can translate that knowledge, that<br />

fundamental, theoretical knowledge into very practical applications in terms of diagnosis, assessment, and<br />

most important to me, intervention.<br />

So, twenty-plus years later I now have become very involved in the design and creation and testing through the<br />

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD)of the best forms of intervention for<br />

different kinds of children <strong>with</strong> their different kinds of reading issues. It’s been a long journey and it’s been a<br />

theoretically exciting one. I have to say, intellectually, I have never ever had one day of boredom. It’s made my<br />

life very satisfied, if you will, in terms of feeling like everything I do and learn has something that makes the<br />

lives of some kids better. It feels like a great way to live a life. So, that's my story.<br />

David Boulton: That’s a great story. Thank you for sharing it. I like how you connected the dots. Maybe it's not<br />

surprising to you, but a lot of the people I talk <strong>with</strong> have had some experience in their life that brought home<br />

the incredible importance of this in a way that highlights the social lack of understanding at the core of it.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yeah. Well, mine began actually in the social political realm and then it just didn't let go of<br />

me. At that moment I so wanted to study poetry. Instead, I became a scholar who knows what happens when a<br />

poet retrieves a word. It's still intellectually a lot of fun but it is very different from where I began.<br />

History of Reading Disabilities and Dyslexia:<br />

David Boulton: So, what would you say are the most important findings or the most important things that have<br />

come out of your research into the neuroscience of reading that are not generally well understood?<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: The history of reading disabilities, (I'll use the word dyslexia - some people use it, some<br />

people don't), is such a fascinating one because it's like a case study in science patterns, the desire for<br />

parsimony among scientists and the refusal of the human brain to be typed in one way. What you see in this<br />

history is one researcher after another seizing on what is in front of them and saying, "Ah, that's what dyslexia<br />

is. That's what causes it." <strong>An</strong>d it's really the most over-worked and even platitudinous analogy in the world. But<br />

the blind men and the elephant describe the history of dyslexia research. The Blind Men and the Elephant: In<br />

various versions of the tale, a group of blind men (or men in the dark) touch an elephant to learn what it is like.<br />

Each one feels a different part, but only one part, such as the side or the tusk. They then compare notes and<br />

learn that they are in complete disagreement.<br />

David Boulton: <strong>An</strong>d the Sufi key Story.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Exactly. If you look and put together even only the names of dyslexia, you'll see one<br />

hypothesis is visual, one is memory, one is verbal, one is auditory. You just go down the list. Well, if you put<br />

them all together, or if, as I'm doing this in a book now, I just put those names on the brain and you see a crude<br />

cartography of reading. In other words, if it can go wrong it does. <strong>An</strong>d at one point in the history of dyslexia


3<br />

each has been called the major explanation for dyslexia. Now, the modern history has been punctuated by<br />

really different, very technologically sophisticated approaches including neurosciences and also including a lot<br />

of wonderful work done in an area called psycholinguistics.<br />

In the 1970’s there was a great set of researchers at Haskins Lab at Yale and they were really beginning a whole<br />

new approach to understanding dyslexia by looking at the linguistic foundations of reading breakdown. That<br />

began one of the single best hypotheses we've ever had which is that the phonological system in language, that<br />

is, our ability to hear, to discriminate the smallest sounds called phonemes in words is a fundamental necessity<br />

in learning to read and a fundamental source of why some children can't learn to read. That began what is<br />

called the phonological deficit hypothesis, which has really been the most successful of explanations to date.<br />

Rapid Naming, Phonemic Awareness and Speed of Processing:<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: My research began while that hypothesis was in its zenith. At the same time I was equally<br />

influenced by neurosciences, which was then called neurological or neuropsychological studies. We were<br />

beginning to see that there was this one very odd phenomenon that children who were going to become<br />

dyslexic were always exhibiting, whether they were five or six or seven, and that was a failure to be able to<br />

name, it’s so simple, to name things they saw at the same speed that other children could.<br />

Well, naming seems very simple but it's actually a very difficult set of underlying processes. So, my mentor,<br />

Martha Denckla and her mentor, a neurologist, Norman Geschwind, were responsible for really getting the<br />

field to think differently, if you will. In the beginning, people said, "Well, naming speed is just another kind of<br />

phonology. You need to be able to retrieve a phonological label." <strong>An</strong>d, for a while that satisfied me. Then, I<br />

began to see kids who had no phoneme issues in other areas and yet they had this....


4<br />

David Boulton: You mean in terms of their ability to articulate themselves on the fly they would demonstrate<br />

that they had good phoneme processing but they couldn't name, which has an association component?<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Well, that's really close. I'll just give you a slightly more technical explanation by saying<br />

that when we did all our tests we had explicit measures of these phoneme awareness skills that everybody says<br />

were the most important ones and we were seeing that some kids didn't have that but they had naming speed<br />

issues. Well, if they're both the same, they should have both. <strong>An</strong>d they weren't exhibiting that and that began<br />

us thinking that there are so many issues beyond the phoneme, which includes the visual system and the<br />

retrieval system. It includes the speed <strong>with</strong> which the brain puts its systems together.<br />

That was what we got fixated on. That's not the same as phoneme awareness. So, we then began to really get<br />

in-depth understandings of naming speed and the speed <strong>with</strong> which not only that you name but the speed <strong>with</strong><br />

which you read and how that fluency in reading is really important not for speed as speed, but for the brain's<br />

ability to do those easy processes fast enough to allocate time to comprehension.<br />

David Boulton: Right. So, those lower levels are operating efficiently and there's sufficient bandwidth to be<br />

reflective and comprehensive.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Perfect. That's what we were beginning to understand. So, this tiny little innocuous naming<br />

speed test opened up a world of understanding about how important all of these individual processes are that<br />

go beyond the phoneme and how important reading fluency is for comprehension. So, that puts you into,<br />

literally, a different ball park from the implication of the phonological deficit, which is that you work on words<br />

and phonemes and you get the kids to be able to recognize words and read, decode them and everything else<br />

is going to happen naturally. Well, it isn't that simple.<br />

Double-Deficit Hypothesis and Interventions:<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: My colleague, Pat Bowers, and I, and others, (we weren't the only two), advanced our<br />

hypothesis in the early nineties. Back then we were kind of John the Baptist-types. It was a little hard going<br />

there for a while. Then people started thinking, we know they're right. We still believe it's phonology but there<br />

is no doubt that there are these kids. Here is where what we call the double-deficit hypothesis comes in: there<br />

are these kids who have single deficits in phoneme awareness, single deficits in naming speed <strong>with</strong>out<br />

phoneme, and then double-deficit kids who have both. The kids who have both reading fluency and<br />

comprehension issues have different reasons for reading failure than the kids who have only phoneme<br />

awareness issues.<br />

David Boulton: <strong>An</strong>d therefore, need different interventions to differentiate their way through what’s obstructing<br />

their processing.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Perfect. That was the whole point. If you believe that everything is phonology then there’s<br />

only one intervention needed, if you believe that there could be different reasons for this naming speed then<br />

you have to really make sure your intervention is going after all those different things and putting it in the<br />

intervention plans.<br />

So, my work in the mid-nineties was funded by Reid Lyon's group at NICHD. They were funding us to begin to<br />

design whole new interventions based on our knowledge of both the reading brain and our knowledge of<br />

linguistics; all the things that go into what we do when we try to retrieve words in time, both at the phoneme<br />

level, the word level, and the whole connected-text level.<br />

We began an intervention called RAVE-O, which really emphasizes all that we knew and our evolving<br />

knowledge about how the brain processes words. We've got now considerable data that support us showing


5<br />

that the kids who have these naming speed and fluency issues are very much better served by this RAVE-O<br />

fluency program. <strong>An</strong>d the kids who have only a phonology issue are well-served by only phonology-oriented<br />

programs. So, our data is showing both the efficacy of our work and also the needs that these other kids have<br />

that go beyond the conventional phonological programs.<br />

Orthographic Representation: [Orthographic – related to the study of spelling]<br />

David Boulton: Excellent. Let's go into rapid naming for a moment.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Sure.<br />

David Boulton: Most children seem to be able to name the things that are around their world relatively easy<br />

compared to the challenge of reading.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yes, definitely.<br />

David Boulton: All right. But there's a difference in reading that comes from the need to be able to name or to<br />

deal <strong>with</strong> letters and their letter sounds and stick them together fast.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: That's right.<br />

David Boulton: To bundle them together in…<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: To put all of that together.<br />

David Boulton: Right.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. Maryann <strong>Wolf</strong>: You're quite correct. There's something called orthographic representation. We know a lot<br />

about phoneme representation but we don't know as much about what we're doing when we rapidly make<br />

patterns of letters that are specific to our language versus another. For example, in German you would see s-ch-r<br />

very frequently. In English, you would never see it. The German child makes an orthographic<br />

representation, has a pattern...<br />

David Boulton: As a structural unit.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yes, as a unit. Some children <strong>with</strong> reading problems don't have that unit. In English you<br />

have units that are specific to our language. Part of RAVE-O is teaching those units explicitly and trying to make<br />

them faster and faster.<br />

David Boulton: So, you're trying to make sub-unit visual recognition happen fast.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Perfect. So, one piece of RAVE-O is devoted to making orthographic chunks automatic in<br />

the kids. <strong>An</strong>other piece is devoted to the experience of entertaining different meanings of a word. A lot of our<br />

kids are almost penalized because not only are they slower at putting those letters together, they're slower at<br />

retrieving the meanings. So, a lot of them only have one meaning and they’re missing the point or missing the<br />

joke or missing the poem because they don't have that rapid retrieval of multiple meanings. Nor did their<br />

system in the beginning allow them to.<br />

David Boulton: Are you connecting that to the overall ecology of processing and bandwidth again?


6<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. Maryann <strong>Wolf</strong>: Well, it is very similar. At the same time we're teaching orthographic patterns, we're<br />

teaching them the search for multiple meanings of words. We're teaching them how words can be used<br />

differently in syntax by adding on morphemes that are in patterns, -ed, -ing, -s, -ly, or pre-fixes: pre-, re-, all of<br />

those things. We want them to see them, but we also want them to know how they change the meaning. So,<br />

it's all in there.<br />

Processing Bottleneck Relative to Rapid Naming Difficulties:<br />

David Boulton: Back to rapid naming for a moment. Have you found where you think the processing bottleneck<br />

is relative to difficulties <strong>with</strong> rapid naming?<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Great question. I wish. I really wish. We have a lot of very high-falutin’ experiments. Some<br />

of them are literally speech-synthesis experiments where we computerize the speech stream and we look at<br />

exactly where the differences are. Is it in, like the visual, if you remember those rapid naming tests, there's five<br />

lines of ten symbols, like ten letters...<br />

David Boulton: Yes.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Each of those letters are repeated two times on a line and there's only five letters being<br />

repeated ten times in a group of fifty letters, so it's not like a big deal of knowledge. But at the end of a line if<br />

the visual system is slow you're going to be able to detect it in the amount of time that a student takes from<br />

one line to the next. That would be one logical hypothesis. <strong>An</strong>other is articulation. So, we would measure<br />

articulation. There are all these different hypotheses.<br />

David Boulton: Right. You could use Keith Rayner's work to differentiate what part is visual from what part is<br />

stuttering up in the articulatory circuitry.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Right. We're trying to eliminate where we can, and in fact, we are not seeing gross visual<br />

differences at the end of the line. We think that there are some differences in some kids, but not all kids. But<br />

what we do see is that the bottleneck is the time in between the articulation of one symbol and the beginning<br />

of the processing of the other. So, everything that's in that gap, not the visual line, not articulation, but<br />

everything that's in that gap...<br />

David Boulton: Kind of recovery and reset from articulation.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yes, all that that comes before and after disinhibition from the previous stimulus setting it<br />

up. Here's where visual processes can come in, but also hooking it up then to the phonological label. All those<br />

kinds of processes that go into access and retrieval and perception, those are in there. The time and the speech<br />

stream research shows us that's where your money is. But that includes many different areas, so then you have<br />

to go into each one of those. <strong>An</strong>d here's where the field is, rightly, reluctant to get that complex. We aren't<br />

reluctant. We are going to pursue it until we feel like we know which child has which area of problem but in<br />

fact it means that you have to have a pretty sophisticated set of tests to be able to say, "Well, this child has all<br />

of those areas slower." Or, "This child has only one area that's slower."<br />

So, what you have, if you will, it's like we've narrowed it down to a set of processes. We've eliminated some<br />

things like articulation and gross eye movement. But in that subset we still don't know which child has which or<br />

whether they have all of the above. That's where I think the field is right now.<br />

Dyslexia and Environmental Influences:


7<br />

David Boulton: Excellent. Let's climb back up for a minute. You used <strong>with</strong> some hesitancy <strong>with</strong> the word<br />

'dyslexia'.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. Maryann <strong>Wolf</strong>: I use it always. But I think the world sometimes has more difficulty. Not my world, but I'm<br />

thinking this is more for the public.<br />

David Boulton: Yes, it’s such an over-generalized term. I mean, it's a term that's like the Holy Grail, pick your<br />

definition and apply it.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yeah.<br />

David Boulton: You also use the term developmental dyslexia. What do we know about neurobiologically innate<br />

dyslexia versus dyslexia that is a consequence of the learning environment that children are growing in?<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Well, that's one of the interesting things about naming speed. I have to say, by and large,<br />

that our kids who have a lot of environmental issues, they do not have differences on naming speed on things<br />

like color or numbers, as long as they know their numbers and colors. That's not the case for all kids. There<br />

really are extraordinary differences when you walk into kindergarten. But let's say in second grade you find the<br />

kids who have some of the phoneme awareness issues that can certainly go <strong>with</strong> environmental causes, but<br />

naming speed seems more hardwired. You've got some real issues that you're picking up here that are there<br />

forever. In the most compensated dyslexic adult that we've studied you still see these differences. <strong>An</strong>d these<br />

are some Nobel Prize winners.<br />

David Boulton: Are you familiar <strong>with</strong> Hart and Risley's research?<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Oh, gosh. There's the Hart-Risley studies on the thirty million word gap. There's no<br />

question that makes such a huge difference.<br />

David Boulton: I would think that the other side of that is that the greater the number of articulations the<br />

children make, the better - the more that you do it, the faster you get at it.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yes. In kindergarten I think you still have that, but let's say the child's been in the system<br />

for two years. At the level of naming a color or number you really are getting much closer, no matter what your<br />

environmental input is, unless it's just disaster. So, we have this suspicion based on data that if you look at<br />

what we, I just hate this word, but the field historically has used terms like garden-variety poor reader or nondiscrepant<br />

poor reader to describe children whose environment and intellectual quotient on an intelligence<br />

test or something like that is commensurate <strong>with</strong> their reading. Well, I don't like those terms. But let's just say<br />

environmental reasons for lack of a better word. Those kids on our tests look like every other kid on naming<br />

speed in grade two. In grade one, grade kindergarten, they're a little down but they're really getting as close as<br />

can be by grade two, even grade one. But the kids among them who are environmental and dyslexic, in my<br />

terms, their naming speed is going to be depressed down through eighth grade, and now we know into<br />

adulthood. It is a fundamental difference.<br />

Percentage of Population <strong>with</strong> Innate Dyslexia:<br />

David Boulton: Okay. Can you tell us what percentage of the population has innate dyslexia?<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Sure.


8<br />

David Boulton: <strong>An</strong> overlay if possible. We've got a consensus between Reid Lyon, James Wendorf and Sally<br />

Shaywitz, and others that seem to say that we're talking about five or six percent that have some kind of innate<br />

or neurobiological, structural level difficulty. <strong>An</strong>d that for the larger population of struggling readers, the<br />

primary difference for them is the learning environments they’ve been in and the kind of instruction they're<br />

receiving.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: You mean the balance of the percentages that people have would be more environmental.<br />

Is that what you mean?<br />

David Boulton: I mean, Reid Lyon's says ninety-five percent of the kids that have trouble <strong>with</strong> reading are<br />

instructional casualties.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Well, I really respect Reid so much. It’s just that sometimes he may exaggerate. Let's just<br />

say that. <strong>An</strong>d I love his exaggerations because they always help us and sometimes they help us by having to<br />

clarify things he’s said.<br />

David Boulton: Or James Wendorf, to whatever degree he's independent, says that the National Center for<br />

Learning Disabilities research on the amount of innate neuro-biological learning disabilities happens to be at<br />

the same number.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yes, well, I think that most people would say between five and ten percent. Closer to five,<br />

but between five and ten percent of a given population would have some kind of neuro-biological foundation<br />

for the dyslexia. We know increasingly about the genetic components. We're not there yet, but there's such<br />

rich genetic knowledge going on. I think in any given place you've really got a lot of different factors going on<br />

and environmental is a big word that I would prefer to parse.<br />

David Boulton: Yes, I agree. I totally agree.<br />

Bilingual, ELL and African American Vernacular English Learners:<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: So, the parsing that I would do includes what are the children who are bi-lingual or Englishlanguage<br />

learners? What effect does that have? This is an issue that a social linguist colleague and I are working<br />

on, his name is Chip Gidney. He’s here at Tufts and is working on African American children who speak African<br />

American Vernacular English. You know we have Standard English as a dialect and Vernacular English as<br />

another dialect. Well, what are some of the issues that you have when the two dialects are so close and yet<br />

you are learning to read in one of those?<br />

David Boulton: Which has vocabulary and nuances that are quite different.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yes, but also the phoneme level. So, we're really looking at some of the phonological<br />

difficulties of some of the kids who speak African American Vernacular English and we're hoping like heck that<br />

some of that research will point the way to understanding what's going on <strong>with</strong> that particular percentage.<br />

Then you have your children of poverty who, like your Todd Risley study, have not had the same deck of cards<br />

that the other kids have. They're walking into kindergarten totally different linguistically.<br />

David Boulton: Well, my understanding of Risley’s work was that it really came down to the amount of talking<br />

going on.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: That's right.


9<br />

David Boulton: <strong>An</strong>d that it didn't matter whether it was somebody that was rich and taciturn or poor and<br />

talkative, that the language exposure had two influences. One, in the field of linguistic exercise, developing<br />

vocabulary, developing processing speed, developing phonemic awareness differentiation, all that...<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yep.<br />

David Boulton: <strong>An</strong>d at the same time that the greater number of words tended to be of a more positive affect.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: It's amazing what all that work means. <strong>An</strong>d Catherine Snow and David Dickinson and<br />

others also have shown that dinner talk is important. How we talk, how much we speak, what do we do <strong>with</strong><br />

our language around children? This is so important for our pre-school teachers to know. I had one wonderful<br />

dissertation on that and there are so many things that we should be doing that we can do easily but that are<br />

not happening and that's one of them: the development of language in our children.<br />

I think the pediatricians can play a role here, too. My hope has always been, and some people are doing this,<br />

that when you have your well visits you also have a little set of books or other things that the mother or father<br />

or caretaker can take home and use <strong>with</strong> their children at each juncture of each year, those first five years. So,<br />

that's a really important potential asset for our society that we don't use enough.<br />

David Boulton: Right. There's a lot of different programs that are moving in that direction.<br />

<strong>Dr</strong>. <strong>Maryanne</strong> <strong>Wolf</strong>: Yes, and that's great. Everything we can do.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!