01.03.2018 Views

final_thp_5thedition

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

activity indicated that low-income students would be<br />

unable to learn as efficiently as their high-income<br />

counterparts. If people continue to draw erroneous,<br />

unsubstantiated conclusions from this type of misrepresented,<br />

media-blasted science, the general populace<br />

will ignore the golden rule of observational sciences:<br />

correlation is not causation. This core message loses<br />

its way to the general media-consuming public in light<br />

of dramatic, sensationalist headlines.<br />

mis•com•mu•ni•ca•tion<br />

During the transition from scientific discovery to<br />

public dissemination, when did people forget to mention<br />

that correlation does not mean causation in<br />

science? Regarded as one of the most important points<br />

of research dissemination, this reminder is repeatedly<br />

ignored in media portrayal of headline-worthy scientific<br />

discoveries. The Erika Hayasaki article featuring my<br />

principal investigator’s research even discussed such<br />

headlines, including “Poverty Shrinks Brains from<br />

Birth” and “Why Poor People Seem to Make Bad Decisions.”<br />

Interestingly, in his Atlantic article “The Point<br />

When Science Becomes Publicity,” James Hamblin<br />

writes that this responsibility lies with the original<br />

researchers themselves, rather than reporters and<br />

news outlets sharing the information.<br />

Hamblin interviews Petroc Sumner, a psychology<br />

professor at Cardiff University in Wales and the lead<br />

investigator of the concerned study. Sumner discusses<br />

the correlations (and does not imply causation) of<br />

well-conducted research being poorly represented in<br />

the media and exaggeration in press releases issued by<br />

researchers and universities. Researchers, Sumner told<br />

The Atlantic, have a responsibility to not sensationalize<br />

their own research in order to make it more consumer-friendly,<br />

but rather to present the research and its<br />

implications as faithfully as possible. Misinterpretations<br />

of correlation-based research implying causation<br />

not only distorts implications of observational<br />

research, but often leads to questionable medical<br />

advice or seemingly scientifically supported falsehoods<br />

PAGE | 9<br />

—things like “Sleeping on your stomach will litter your<br />

visage with angry red pustules” or “People are choosing<br />

marijuana over OxyContin for their recreational -<br />

high.”<br />

While intriguing, the idea that scientists sensationalize<br />

their own work and purposefully cause public misunderstanding<br />

is particularly disturbing. This is<br />

because once we claim things<br />

.<br />

like “Poor brains are not<br />

as smart,” we are paving the way for a “nouveau eugenics,”<br />

a term used by Matthew Hughey, a University of<br />

Connecticut professor of sociology, in Hayasaki’s cover<br />

story. Social neuroscience is the field of study dedicated<br />

to understanding the intersectionality between the<br />

brain and human social interaction. The implications<br />

of a misinformed social neuroscience nouveau eugenics<br />

movement are grand: they potentially support<br />

discrimination against already disadvantaged people,<br />

and they do so with an arsenal of scientific research<br />

which, to the layperson, could be misconstrued as<br />

evidence for their claims about inferiority of said<br />

disadvantaged people‘s brains.<br />

neu•ral dis•crim•i•na•tion<br />

In neuroscience, the “wiring” of the brain is a rather<br />

finicky process. Many analyses are made through the<br />

use of functional magnetic resonance images (fMRIs)<br />

which essentially highlight areas of higher blood flow<br />

in the brain, and consequently brain activity, of someone<br />

in a large brain scanner. This activity can be analyzed<br />

in real time and provides a visual approximation<br />

of participants’ brain activity.<br />

Neuroscientists believe synaptic connections are<br />

forged, pruned, and weeded over long-term exposure<br />

to certain people, places, and situations. In Immordino-Yang’s<br />

research, for example, fMRIs of younger adolescents’<br />

brains appear different from their own brains<br />

two years later, but in minutiae. Potentially significant<br />

minutiae—minutiae my lab certainly finds worth<br />

exploring, but minutiae nonetheless.<br />

When researchers’ press releases and media outlets

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!