Loyola College in Maryland
Loyola College in Maryland
Loyola College in Maryland
Transform your PDFs into Flipbooks and boost your revenue!
Leverage SEO-optimized Flipbooks, powerful backlinks, and multimedia content to professionally showcase your products and significantly increase your reach.
<strong>Loyola</strong> <strong>College</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Maryland</strong><br />
The dissertation of ___Kathryn L. Savitz__________________entitled<br />
_________Narcissism and Overcontrolled-Hostility:_______________<br />
_________Personality Characteristics of Priests with Pedophilia____<br />
submitted to the department of ___Psychology_______________<strong>in</strong> partial<br />
fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Psychology_______<br />
<strong>in</strong> the <strong>College</strong> of Arts and Sciences has been read and approved by the Committee:
Runn<strong>in</strong>g head: NARCISSISM AND PRIESTS WITH PEDOPHILIA<br />
Narcissism and Overcontrolled Hostility:<br />
Personality Characteristics of Priests with Pedophilia<br />
A Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the<br />
Graduate School of <strong>Loyola</strong> <strong>College</strong> <strong>in</strong> Partial Fulfillment of the<br />
Requirements for the Degree of<br />
Doctor of Psychology<br />
by<br />
Kathryn L. Savitz, M.A.<br />
2000<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia
ABSTRACT<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
Individuals who commit sexual crimes aga<strong>in</strong>st children are among the most scorned <strong>in</strong><br />
society. Those who possess a sexual preference for children are diagnosed with<br />
Pedophilia and typically are men. This study focused on determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g hallmark<br />
personality traits (narcissism and overcontrolled-hostility) of a subpopulation of<br />
pedophilia, specifically, priest pedophiles which, if identified, could serve as a foundation<br />
for risk assessment and treatment <strong>in</strong>terventions. Meloy (1986) theorized that priest<br />
pedophiles exhibit narcissistic traits <strong>in</strong> a dichotomous relationship to their self-perception<br />
as be<strong>in</strong>g conveyors of God's word. Consistent with this theory, this study used W<strong>in</strong>k's<br />
(1991) categorization of narcissism: Grandiosity Exhibitionism (GE: extroverted, self-<br />
centered, and assertive) and Vulnerability Sensitivity (VS: <strong>in</strong>troverted, anxious, and<br />
defensive) to compare priest pedophiles and non-priest pedophiles. Statistical analyses<br />
failed to confirm Hypothesis 1 that priest pedophiles would demonstrate higher levels of<br />
GE narcissism than other pedophiles. Although not significant, priest pedophiles had<br />
lower levels than non-priest pedophiles, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that this personality trait may have<br />
dim<strong>in</strong>ished by the identification of their crime as well as the loss of their identity as an<br />
active messenger of God. On the other hand, as predicted, pedophiles demonstrated<br />
higher levels of VS than priest pedophiles, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that these traits could dist<strong>in</strong>guish<br />
the two subpopulations of pedophilia. Results failed to support the hypothesis that priest<br />
pedophiles would evidence higher levels of overcontrolled-hostility than pedophiles.<br />
Another significant f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g which was not predicted was that pedophiles presented<br />
i
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
themselves as hav<strong>in</strong>g greater psychological problems than priest pedophiles. This<br />
suggests pedophiles and priest pedophiles may differ <strong>in</strong> this respect as well. Overall, this<br />
study <strong>in</strong>troduces the likelihood that, with ref<strong>in</strong>ed methodology, particular personality<br />
characteristics with<strong>in</strong> subpopulations of pedophiles, as subtle as they may be, could be<br />
highlighted and considered <strong>in</strong> preventative and treatment <strong>in</strong>terventions.<br />
ii
COMMITTEE IN CHARGE OF CANDIDACY:<br />
Associate Professor Ruth Stemberger, Chairperson<br />
Assistant Professor Deborah Hask<strong>in</strong>s<br />
Associate Professor Charles LoPresto<br />
iii<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia
DEDICATION<br />
Many <strong>in</strong>dividuals have impacted the personal and professional paths I have<br />
chosen dur<strong>in</strong>g my life. My parents have always provided me unconditional love,<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
encouragement, and devotion and, although there have been times when my appreciation<br />
for them has not been evident, I reflect on their <strong>in</strong>valuable qualities, which have<br />
ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed my motivation to accomplish my most challeng<strong>in</strong>g goals. My brother and<br />
sister-<strong>in</strong>-law have also been essential to my happ<strong>in</strong>ess and serenity, particularly my<br />
brother who has cont<strong>in</strong>uously rema<strong>in</strong>ed a magnificent friend, advisor, and educator. My<br />
adorable nephew, although unknow<strong>in</strong>gly, has made me perceive and approach new<br />
experiences, not as obstacles, but as excit<strong>in</strong>g explorations of myself and my environment.<br />
In addition to my immediate family, I must mention the <strong>in</strong>f<strong>in</strong>ite worth of my dearest and<br />
closest friends who have always shared the highlights <strong>in</strong> my life and have helped smooth<br />
out the bumps along the way.<br />
This manuscript represents one of my greatest accomplishments. I dedicate it to<br />
my grandmother, whom I have admired all my life. Her wisdom, <strong>in</strong>dependence,<br />
generosity, s<strong>in</strong>cerity, and sense of style, which I observed as a young girl and now as an<br />
adult, have made me aware of the strides women can make <strong>in</strong> this world. She has also<br />
demonstrated that through perseverance anyth<strong>in</strong>g is possible. I will forever remember<br />
her mortal lessons and will treasure them as gifts for a lifetime.<br />
iv
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
The author wishes to express the utmost gratitude to my major reader, Ruth<br />
Stemberger, Ph.D., for her <strong>in</strong>credible knowledge and ideas as well as her enthusiasm,<br />
encouragement, and persistence which were critical elements <strong>in</strong> the development and<br />
completion of this research project. F<strong>in</strong>ally, it cannot go without mention that her<br />
cont<strong>in</strong>ued assistance when personal matters arose, which went way beyond the call of<br />
duty, was immensely appreciated. Special thanks are also <strong>in</strong> order for my other<br />
committee members, Drs. Hask<strong>in</strong>s and LoPresto, who have been outstand<strong>in</strong>g mentors.<br />
Their advice, suggestions, and support were essential to the successful completion of this<br />
manuscript.<br />
In addition, without the will<strong>in</strong>gness of St. Luke's Institute to support this research,<br />
this study would have been dramatically different and not as nearly as <strong>in</strong>trigu<strong>in</strong>g. Special<br />
acknowledgements go to Steve Montana, Ph.D. and Alka Broome, B.A. for represent<strong>in</strong>g<br />
my project at St. Luke's. This author also extends her thanks to Fred Berl<strong>in</strong>, M.D.,<br />
Ph.D., of the National Institute for the Study, Prevention, and Treatment of Sexual<br />
Trauma, who permitted this writer to request patient volunteers at his cl<strong>in</strong>ic, and to<br />
Denise Sawyer who organized that endeavor. In addition, Bob G<strong>in</strong>gerich, M.A., of T. W.<br />
Ponessa & Associates Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Services, played a critical role <strong>in</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g research<br />
participants for this project. Another important acknowledgement goes to a good friend,<br />
Michael Tiburzi, for shar<strong>in</strong>g his impressive statistical knowledge. F<strong>in</strong>ally, thanks to all<br />
research participants who volunteered their time and energy to assist <strong>in</strong> this project.<br />
v
TABLE 1.<br />
TABLE 2.<br />
TABLE 3.<br />
TABLE 4.<br />
TABLE 5.<br />
TABLE 6.<br />
TABLE 7.<br />
TABLE 8.<br />
TABLE 9.<br />
LIST OF TABLES<br />
Means and Standard Deviations for Pedophiles and Ephebophiles<br />
on the Significant Scales of the MMPI-2, the MCMI-II, and the<br />
Rorschach ..............................................................................................<br />
Hypothesized Group Differences...........................................................<br />
Summary of Independent t-Tests between Priest Pedophiles and<br />
Priest Ephebophiles................................................................................<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
Demographic Data for Control Pedophiles............................................. 64<br />
Demographic Data for Control Priests...................................................<br />
Summary of Statistical Analyses for Age and Education by Group......<br />
Summary of Statistical Analyses for MMPI-2 L, F, and K Validity<br />
Scales by Group .....................................................................................<br />
Intercorrelations Between Narcissism Scales.........................................<br />
Summary of Statistical Analyses for Vulnerability-Sensitivity (VS)<br />
Narcissism, Grandiosity-Exhibitionism Narcissism, and<br />
Overcontrolled-Hostility by Group......................................................... 72<br />
vi<br />
Page<br />
36<br />
48<br />
62<br />
65<br />
67<br />
69<br />
70
TABLE OF CONTENTS<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
ABSTRACT............................................................................................................. i<br />
COMMITTEE IN CHARGE OF CANDIDACY....................................................<br />
DEDICATION.........................................................................................................<br />
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS.....................................................................................<br />
LIST OF TABLES...................................................................................................<br />
CHAPTER I -Introduction.......................................................................................<br />
Review of the Literature..............................................................................<br />
Epidemiology of Pedophilia........................................................................<br />
Typology of Pedophilia...............................................................................<br />
Theories of Pedophilia.................................................................................<br />
Psychoanalytic Theory.....................................................................<br />
Behavioral Theory...........................................................................<br />
Social Learn<strong>in</strong>g Theory...................................................................<br />
Cognitive Theory.............................................................................<br />
Biological Theory............................................................................<br />
Four Factor Model. .........................................................................<br />
Personality Characteristics of Pedophiles.................................................... 19<br />
Personality Characteristics of Ephebophiles...............................................<br />
Child Molestation <strong>in</strong> the Priesthood............................................................<br />
vii<br />
Page<br />
iii<br />
iv<br />
v<br />
vi<br />
1<br />
3<br />
3<br />
4<br />
8<br />
8<br />
11<br />
11<br />
12<br />
14<br />
16<br />
23<br />
24
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
The Catholic Priest Profile...........................................................................<br />
Narcissism Tendencies <strong>in</strong> Priests.................................................................<br />
The Priest Pedophile.....................................................................................<br />
Statement of the Problem.............................................................................<br />
Statement of the Hypotheses .......................................................................<br />
CHAPTER II -Method.............................................................................................<br />
Participants...................................................................................................<br />
Experimental Groups........................................................................<br />
Control Groups................................................................................. 49<br />
Materials.......................................................................................................<br />
Demographic Variables....................................................................<br />
M<strong>in</strong>nesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2..............................<br />
Morey, Waugh, and Blashfield Narcissism Scale............................<br />
Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek Narcissism Scale.............................................<br />
W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough Narcissism Scale..................................................<br />
Narcissism-Hypersensitivity Scale ..................................................<br />
Narcissistic Personality Disorder Scale............................................<br />
Procedure. ....................................................................................................<br />
Design and Analyses.................................................................................... 59<br />
viii<br />
Page<br />
27<br />
30<br />
34<br />
41<br />
46<br />
49<br />
49<br />
49<br />
50<br />
50<br />
50<br />
53<br />
53<br />
54<br />
55<br />
56<br />
57
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
CHAPTER III - Results...........................................................................................<br />
Demographic Data.......................................................................................<br />
Control Pedophiles...........................................................................<br />
Control Priests..................................................................................<br />
Priest Pedophiles, Priest Ephebophiles, and Psychiatric Priests......<br />
Descriptive Data..........................................................................................<br />
Comparison of Age and Education................................................... 66<br />
Primary Analyses.........................................................................................<br />
Statistical Analyses for the MMPI-2 Validity Scales.......................<br />
Statistical Analyses for the MMPI-2 Narcissism Scales..................<br />
Statistical Analyses for Grandiosity-Exhibitionism (GE) and<br />
Vulnerability-Sensitivity (VS) Narcissism.......................................<br />
Statistical Analyses for Overcontrolled-Hostility (OH)...................<br />
CHAPTER IV -Discussion......................................................................................<br />
F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs Related to Demographic Information...........................................<br />
Other F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs Related to Psychological Function<strong>in</strong>g................................ 77<br />
F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs From Hypothesis Tests.................................................................<br />
Summary and Conclusions.......................................................................... 87<br />
REFERENCES......................................................................................................... 89<br />
APPENDIXES.........................................................................................................<br />
APPENDIX A - Human Subject Review Letter of Consent for Study.......<br />
ix<br />
Page<br />
61<br />
61<br />
61<br />
63<br />
66<br />
66<br />
68<br />
68<br />
68<br />
71<br />
72<br />
74<br />
74<br />
79<br />
99<br />
99
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
APPENDIX B - Demographic Information for Control Pedophiles ...........<br />
APPENDIX C - Demographic Information for Control Priests ..................<br />
APPENDIX D -<br />
APPENDIX E -<br />
APPENDIX F -<br />
APPENDIX G -<br />
APPENDIX H -<br />
APPENDIX I -<br />
APPENDIX J -<br />
APPENDIX K -<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the Morey, Waugh, and<br />
Blashfield Scale................................................................<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the Narcissism-<br />
Hypersensitivity Scale......................................................<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the Narcissistic<br />
Personality Disorder<br />
Scale..................................................................................<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek<br />
Scale..................................................................................<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough<br />
Scale..................................................................................<br />
Excluded MMPI Items for the Grandiosity-<br />
Exhibitionism Narcissism<br />
Scales ...............................................................................<br />
Excluded MMPI Items for the Vulnerability-Sensitivity<br />
Narcissism Scales.............................................................<br />
Rephrased MMPI Items for the RN and WG Narcissism<br />
Scales................................................................................<br />
Page<br />
APPENDIX L - Morey, Waugh, and Blashfield Narcissism Scale.............. 121<br />
APPENDIX M -Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek Narcissism Scale..............................<br />
APPENDIX N - W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough Narcissism Scale...................................<br />
APPENDIX O - Narcissism-Hypersensitivity Narcissism Scale.................<br />
APPENDIX P - Narcissistic Personality Disorder Narcissism Scale........... 131<br />
x<br />
101<br />
103<br />
105<br />
107<br />
109<br />
111<br />
113<br />
115<br />
117<br />
119<br />
123<br />
126<br />
129<br />
Page
APPENDIX Q -<br />
APPENDIX R -<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia<br />
Informed Consent Document for the Control Pedophile<br />
Group................................................................................ 133<br />
Informed Consent Document for the Control Priest<br />
Group ............................................................................... 135<br />
xi
CHAPTER I<br />
Introduction<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia 1<br />
Throughout this century, society has been forced to take a more liberal approach to<br />
controversial issues regard<strong>in</strong>g sexuality. Regardless of this change <strong>in</strong> attitude about<br />
sexuality, however, there are those <strong>in</strong>dividuals who engage <strong>in</strong> sexual practices outside of<br />
ethical and legal standards. That is, some <strong>in</strong>dividuals experience sexual arousal through<br />
crim<strong>in</strong>al acts such as rape. These <strong>in</strong>dividuals are generally viewed as hav<strong>in</strong>g antisocial<br />
personality traits (Kalichman, 1991). However, there are other deviant and illegal sexual<br />
practices which are <strong>in</strong>fluenced more from a psychological disorder than from a<br />
psychopathic predisposition. These sexual behaviors have been labeled as paraphilias<br />
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Rosenhan & Seligman, 1989): 'philia' mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />
love of and 'para' mean<strong>in</strong>g what is beyond (Rosenhan & Seligman, 1989). Despite their<br />
clear labels and diagnostic criteria, <strong>in</strong>dividuals who suffer from a paraphilia present a very<br />
confus<strong>in</strong>g picture for behavioral researchers, cl<strong>in</strong>icians, and the legal system.<br />
One particularly challeng<strong>in</strong>g category of paraphilia is pedophilia. The Diagnostic<br />
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th Ed) (DSM-IV; APA, 1994) characterizes<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividuals diagnosed with pedophilia as hav<strong>in</strong>g "recurrent, <strong>in</strong>tense sexually arous<strong>in</strong>g<br />
fantasies, sexual urges, or behaviors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g sexual activity with a prepubescent child or<br />
children, generally age 13 years or younger" (APA, 1994, p. 528). Another form of<br />
paraphilia, which often is generalized as pedophilia, is ephebophilia. Ephebophilia is<br />
typified by an <strong>in</strong>dividual's sexual preference for an adolescent or postpubescent child,<br />
typically age 13 to 16 (Rob<strong>in</strong>son, 1994; Rossetti, 1990); however, this specification is not<br />
recognized <strong>in</strong> the DSM-IV as a separate diagnosis. Because the theoretical literature<br />
generally does not dist<strong>in</strong>guish ephebophilia from pedophilia, the explanation of such<br />
throughout this manuscript will not either. Therefore, unless otherwise stated, both
Narcissism and Pedophilia 2<br />
categories of paraphilia will be referred to <strong>in</strong>terchangeably as pedophilia. Dist<strong>in</strong>ctions<br />
among the two pathologies <strong>in</strong> terms of personality characteristics will, however, be made<br />
later where it is appropriate for discussion.<br />
One important area <strong>in</strong> research on pedophilia is identification of <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are<br />
most vulnerable to sexual <strong>in</strong>teractions with children. That is, there have been many reports<br />
(K<strong>in</strong>near, 1995) of child molestation by <strong>in</strong>dividuals who frequently come <strong>in</strong>to contact with<br />
children (i.e., teachers, coaches, bus drivers). Such cases are disturb<strong>in</strong>g for a number of<br />
reasons. First, these <strong>in</strong>dividuals are typically viewed by children as role models and even<br />
parental figures. Second, the question arises as to whether there is a relationship between<br />
serv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> those roles and molest<strong>in</strong>g children. F<strong>in</strong>ally, it is critical to identify the m<strong>in</strong>ority<br />
of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> those groups (i.e., the small number of teachers) who actually abuse<br />
children and differentiate them from those who do not. One such group which has received<br />
<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g attention over the last decade, and which will be the focal po<strong>in</strong>t of this paper, is<br />
priests who sexually abuse children.<br />
Parents typically utilize the services of their priest to care for, give advice to, and<br />
encourage the healthy and moral development of their children (Sipe, 1995).<br />
Unfortunately, "those priests who use their positions of trust and the presumption of moral<br />
<strong>in</strong>tegrity as a cover for sexual activity with children destroy and pervert the mean<strong>in</strong>g and<br />
reality of celibacy and m<strong>in</strong>istry" (Sipe, 1995, p. 9). Therefore, the purpose of this paper is<br />
to discuss the epidemiological, theoretical, and characterological issues of pedophilia, <strong>in</strong><br />
general, and to utilize that discussion <strong>in</strong> exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g priests who molest children as a specific<br />
subpopulation of pedophiles as well as of priests <strong>in</strong> general.
Epidemiology of Pedophilia<br />
Review of the Literature<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia 3<br />
At the present time, the prevalence and epidemiology of pedophilia is unknown.<br />
However, research studies have <strong>in</strong>cluded demographic <strong>in</strong>formation (e.g., age of<br />
perpetrator, ethnicity, gender of child victim, victim age) of pedophiles which has been<br />
found to be similar across studies. This suggests that, for the most part, pedophiles are a<br />
homogenous population with regard to those demographic variables. Thus, results from<br />
several recent studies will be considered as a reflection of the pedophile population as a<br />
whole.<br />
Extrafamilial child molesters have been found to be <strong>in</strong> their twenties at the time of<br />
their first sexual abuse of a child, whereas <strong>in</strong>cestuous child molesters typically are <strong>in</strong> their<br />
thirties (Hayash<strong>in</strong>o, Wurtele, & Klebe, 1995). However, other research has <strong>in</strong>dicated that<br />
there are clusters of molesters who range <strong>in</strong> age from late teens to early 70s (Simon, Sales,<br />
Kaszniak, & Kahn, 1992). More Caucasians have been identified as molesters than<br />
m<strong>in</strong>ority groups such as Hispanics or African Americans (Hayash<strong>in</strong>o et al., 1995;<br />
Kalichman, 1991; Simon et al., 1992). A study conducted by Erickson, Walbek, and<br />
Seely (1988) used <strong>in</strong>formation gathered from evaluations of 229 convicted child molesters<br />
who were referred to an Intensive Treatment Program for Sexual Aggressives <strong>in</strong><br />
M<strong>in</strong>nesota. Based on these data, these researchers concluded that 70% of the child victims<br />
were female (n = 259) whereas 26% were male (n = 102). Sex offenders who molested<br />
children of both sexes were rare (4%). Twenty-two percent of the female and 25% of the<br />
male victims were under the age of six years old, while 27% of female and 24% of male<br />
victims <strong>in</strong> this study were between the ages of six and ten. F<strong>in</strong>ally, 51 % of both the female<br />
and male victims were between 11 and 13 years old. Results of this study also <strong>in</strong>dicated<br />
that few children were physically harmed. In fact, bribery rather than threat was the
Narcissism and Pedophilia 4<br />
primary method of seduc<strong>in</strong>g the child. F<strong>in</strong>ally, eighty-six percent of molesters of male<br />
children considered themselves as be<strong>in</strong>g either homosexual or bisexual.<br />
In addition to these data, most children are abused by adults with whom they are<br />
familiar (K<strong>in</strong>near, 1995), although this may <strong>in</strong>clude a spectrum of <strong>in</strong>dividuals rang<strong>in</strong>g from<br />
a stranger or mere acqua<strong>in</strong>tance to someone much more significant <strong>in</strong> the child's life such<br />
as a parent or family member. Nonetheless, children seem to f<strong>in</strong>d it difficult to report their<br />
abuse due to their fear of the possible consequences of reveal<strong>in</strong>g their perpetrator who,<br />
mostly likely, is known to other adults close to the child (Howitt, 1995; K<strong>in</strong>near, 1995).<br />
In addition, it may be embarrass<strong>in</strong>g for the child to describe details of their victimization<br />
and they often consider themselves at fault (Howitt, 1995). A survey conducted by the<br />
National Committee for Prevention of Child Abuse revealed that, between 1989 and 1993,<br />
approximately 3 million cases of child abuse or neglect were reported with approximately<br />
15% of those <strong>in</strong>cidences <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g sexual abuse (McCurdy & Daro, 1994). Unfortunately,<br />
the prevalence of child molestation is probably higher than that which has been reported<br />
due to the factors previously mentioned.<br />
Typology of Pedophilia<br />
Groth and Birnbaum (1978) have dist<strong>in</strong>guished two typologies of pedophiles,<br />
fixated and regressed, which have been developed through psychodynamic theory.<br />
Accord<strong>in</strong>g to these researchers:<br />
Fixation is def<strong>in</strong>ed as a temporary or permanent arrestment of psychological<br />
maturation result<strong>in</strong>g from unresolved formative issues which persist and underlie<br />
the organization of subsequent phases of development. A fixated offender has from<br />
adolescence been sexually attracted primarily or exclusively to significantly younger<br />
persons. Sexual <strong>in</strong>volvement with peer-age or older persons, where this has
Narcissism and Pedophilia 5<br />
occurred, has been situational <strong>in</strong> nature and has never replaced the primary sexual<br />
attraction to and preference for underage persons (p. 176).<br />
Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Simon et al.,'s (1992) <strong>in</strong>terpretation of this explanation, fixated offenders fail<br />
to experience a healthy sociosexual development, mak<strong>in</strong>g them vulnerable to sexual<br />
attraction to children. In fact, any sexual activity with adults is circumstantial and does not<br />
substitute their preferred sexual <strong>in</strong>timacy with children. In addition, accord<strong>in</strong>g to these<br />
researchers' theoretical conceptualization, fixated child molesters tend to choose male<br />
victims based on a "narcissistic identification with them" (p. 212). That is, they can relate<br />
to the male child's egocentric needs which are characteristic of that phase of psychosexual<br />
development.<br />
Socarides (1991), based on his own cl<strong>in</strong>ical experiences with pedophilic patients,<br />
considered this sexual preference or fixation for children or adolescents to be a<br />
"perversion" such that, without sexual activity with a child, the <strong>in</strong>dividual experiences<br />
much anxiety and emotional turmoil. Furthermore, his patients rarely described any sexual<br />
or <strong>in</strong>timate relationships with adults as pleasurable. These cl<strong>in</strong>ical f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs support other<br />
research which suggested that child molesters generally are never married (Groth &<br />
Birnbaum, 1978). Socarides theorized that, as a result of their "perversion," fixated<br />
pedophiles must cont<strong>in</strong>uously seek out their specified partners <strong>in</strong> order to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> stable<br />
psychological function<strong>in</strong>g. In other words, the victims of fixated pedophiles, typically<br />
strangers or casual acqua<strong>in</strong>tances, are deliberately sought through premeditation (Groth &<br />
Birnbaum, 1978).<br />
The other type of pedophile described by Groth and Birnbaum (1978) is the<br />
regressed pedophile. As described by these authors:<br />
Regression is def<strong>in</strong>ed as a temporary or permanent appearance of primitive behavior<br />
after more mature forms of expression had been atta<strong>in</strong>ed, regardless of whether the
Narcissism and Pedophilia 6<br />
immature behavior was actually manifested earlier <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual's development.<br />
A regressed offender has not exhibited any predom<strong>in</strong>ant sexual attraction to<br />
significantly younger persons dur<strong>in</strong>g his sexual development - if any such<br />
<strong>in</strong>volvement did occur dur<strong>in</strong>g adolescence, it was situational or experimental <strong>in</strong><br />
nature. Instead, this <strong>in</strong>dividual's sociosexual <strong>in</strong>terests have focused on peer-age or<br />
adult persons primarily or exclusively (p. 177).<br />
Like the fixated offender, Simon et al. (1992) summarized that regressed child offenders<br />
engage <strong>in</strong> sexual activities with children as a "temporary or permanent departure from his<br />
more characteristic attraction to adults" (p. 212). Further f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from Groth and<br />
Birnbaum's study demonstrated that regressed child molesters tend to be flexible <strong>in</strong> their<br />
choice of child victim (i.e., acqua<strong>in</strong>tance or relative) and act more on impulsive and<br />
opportunistic factors rather than on a predatory compulsion. These researchers also<br />
revealed that these molesters engage <strong>in</strong> sexual activities with children due to fluctuations of<br />
stress related to situations <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g other adults. Regressed pedophiles also tend to prefer<br />
female victims based on a history of <strong>in</strong>timate and prolonged relationships with other female<br />
adults and the majority are married.<br />
A more recent study of pedophilia typology by Johnston and Johnston (1997)<br />
utilized <strong>in</strong>terview and questionnaire <strong>in</strong>formation gathered from cl<strong>in</strong>ician/<strong>in</strong>formants, child<br />
molesters, and child molester counselors to further evaluate the fixated-regressed<br />
contention. These researchers found fixated child molesters to have impaired social<br />
adjustment, have a greater preoccupation with children, molest male children, and have<br />
come from dysfunctional families, whereas regressed offenders demonstrated better<br />
adjustment, a preference for female children, and tended to have had more "normal"<br />
childhoods. In addition, regressed molesters were found to abuse alcohol much more<br />
frequently than fixated molesters. This was expla<strong>in</strong>ed by the presumption that regressed
Narcissism and Pedophilia 7<br />
offenders are uncomfortable with their attraction to children and will use alcohol to<br />
dis<strong>in</strong>hibit their impulses. On the other hand, fixated molesters accept their attraction for<br />
children and, thus, do not require "external dis<strong>in</strong>hibition to <strong>in</strong>dulge [their] sexual deviance"<br />
(p. 367).<br />
Although Groth and Birnbaum's typology possesses much validity, there are also<br />
criticisms to their theory. Simon et al. (1992) conducted an archival study of 136 child<br />
molestation cases <strong>in</strong>itially to empirically validate Groth and Birnbaum's fixated-regressed<br />
typology. However, despite agreement with most of Groth and Birnbaum's po<strong>in</strong>t, Simon<br />
et al. suggested that child molesters should be considered on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum of attributes<br />
rather than dichotomously. Specifically, the relationship to the victim (i.e., relative or<br />
stranger), the perpetrator's prior nonsexual crim<strong>in</strong>al record, and age of the offender were<br />
found to be related to the molester's level of regression. In addition, Simon et al. criticized<br />
Groth and Birnbaum's theory <strong>in</strong> their exclusive reliance on factors related to psychological<br />
and psychosexual development <strong>in</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>g the nature of the pedophilic behavior. Simon<br />
et al. argued that elements such as prior crim<strong>in</strong>ality as well as alcohol and substance abuse<br />
should also be considered as significant factors <strong>in</strong> molestation recidivism.<br />
Simon and his colleagues also focused on an <strong>in</strong>dividual's potential to molest based<br />
on various age groups as well as situational and opportunistic variables, rather than the<br />
limited variables <strong>in</strong>cluded with<strong>in</strong> the regressed-fixated typology. In specific, these<br />
researchers proposed one group could consist of adolescents <strong>in</strong> their late teens or early<br />
twenties. Although these <strong>in</strong>dividuals may demonstrate social immaturity and an attraction<br />
toward children, it is possible they deliberately decrease their <strong>in</strong>teractions with children to<br />
avoid the sexual urges experienced while <strong>in</strong> the presence of children. This active avoidance<br />
of children might promote more appropriate peer relationships. Another group might<br />
consist of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> their twenties and thirties who obta<strong>in</strong> connections to children
Narcissism and Pedophilia 8<br />
through family or friends. Such offenders most likely would be divorced or, at the time of<br />
the child molestation, would be <strong>in</strong> a dysfunctional relationship. This group, <strong>in</strong> essence,<br />
may turn to children for <strong>in</strong>timacy due to their troubled adult relationships. The f<strong>in</strong>al group<br />
could consist of men age 50 or older who typically may have discont<strong>in</strong>ued social or sexual<br />
relationships with adults. Therefore, they may tend to rely on and prefer children to fulfill<br />
their sexual needs.<br />
This latter description of pedophilia has yet to be validated empirically, although it,<br />
nonetheless, weakens the fixated-regressed typology of pedophilia by depict<strong>in</strong>g its<br />
dichotomous framework as <strong>in</strong>complete. However, this also holds true for Simon et al.'s<br />
(1992) age group classification. That is, they did not consider that adult relationships may<br />
be disrupted because of the pedophilic behaviors <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> the perpetrator. Thus, the<br />
child molester may have hidden their 'sexual identity' <strong>in</strong> the same manner and for the same<br />
reasons as some homosexual <strong>in</strong>dividuals have, to protect their reputation and status as an<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividual. It may be their <strong>in</strong>ability to further <strong>in</strong>hibit their sexual desires for children which<br />
breaks up the adult relationship.<br />
Theories of Pedophilia<br />
Psychoanalytic theory.<br />
The psychoanalytic model concentrates primarily on <strong>in</strong>secure attachment and<br />
childhood sexual abuse or trauma as contributions to pedophilic behaviors, each of which<br />
will be discussed, respectively. Bowlby (1969) was the first to exam<strong>in</strong>e the effects of<br />
attachment on child development. He believed that, through attachment experiences,<br />
especially with the mother, <strong>in</strong>fants and children develop a model of beliefs, expectations,<br />
and attitudes regard<strong>in</strong>g relationships (Ward, Hudson, and Marshall, 1996). Without a<br />
secure bond, the child is vulnerable to emotional and <strong>in</strong>terpersonal difficulties <strong>in</strong> adulthood<br />
(Ward et al., 1996). Socarides (1991), based on his experiences with pedophilic patients,
Narcissism and Pedophilia 9<br />
believed that unhealthy attachment is a primary factor <strong>in</strong> the sexual deviancy of these<br />
patients. In particular, he found that child molesters experienced childhood trauma caused<br />
by a cruel mother and an emotionally detached or physically absent father. He proposed<br />
that because of this, the child demonstrates a splitt<strong>in</strong>g of the ego and of the object which<br />
symbolizes the distorted or nonexistent attachment with the mother. Subsequently, <strong>in</strong><br />
adulthood, the molester's primary focus becomes the "self and their child victim is then<br />
utilized to establish a self-object relationship. Overall, it is assumed that this lack of<br />
bond<strong>in</strong>g impairs psychosexual development, arrest<strong>in</strong>g the maturational growth of the adult<br />
pedophiles, mak<strong>in</strong>g them appear functionally similar to their child victim (Hall, Graham, &<br />
Shepherd, 1991).<br />
Marshall (1989) elaborated on this psychoanalytic approach and suggested that<br />
difficulties with attachment can lead to <strong>in</strong>timacy deficits. In other words, sex offenders<br />
typically fail to have a secure attachment experience as children and, as a result, they lack<br />
the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal skills and self-confidence necessary to form emotional relationships with<br />
peers <strong>in</strong> adulthood (Hall et al., 1991; Marshall, 1989). This theory was supported by<br />
Seidman, Marshall, Hudson, and Robertson (1994) who compared rapists, child<br />
molesters, violent non-sex offenders (i.e., wife batterers) and a community control group.<br />
Based on responses from various assessment measures of <strong>in</strong>timacy, desirability, and<br />
lonel<strong>in</strong>ess, sex offenders evidenced greater deficiencies <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>timacy and were more lonely<br />
than the non-sex offenders. It is important to note that Marshall's theory failed to consider<br />
that there are other, more specific categories of attachment styles, rather than the broad<br />
classifications <strong>in</strong>troduced by Bowlby which better represent how childhood experiences<br />
<strong>in</strong>fluence adult <strong>in</strong>terpersonal <strong>in</strong>teractions (Ward et al., 1996). Such dist<strong>in</strong>ctions have, <strong>in</strong><br />
fact, been <strong>in</strong>vestigated (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Ward et al., 1996); however, a<br />
discussion of those classifications goes beyond the scope of this paper.
Narcissism and Pedophilia 10<br />
Although these developmental experiences may be common among <strong>in</strong>dividuals who<br />
are later identified as pedophiles, there are many other <strong>in</strong>dividuals who do not have a sexual<br />
preference for children yet who have encountered emotional deficits <strong>in</strong> their life.<br />
Therefore, it is premature to consider impoverished psychosexual development and<br />
attachment difficulties as the primary factor predispos<strong>in</strong>g pedophilia. There must be other<br />
variables <strong>in</strong>volved which contribute to a vulnerability to commit sexually deviant crimes.<br />
In addition to attachment deficits, it is thought that sexual deviance may develop<br />
from psychological trauma, particularly sexual and/or physical abuse occurr<strong>in</strong>g between the<br />
ages of 2 and 5 (Rossetti, 1990; Socarides, 1991). Sexually abused children often<br />
experience confusion, separation anxiety, overstimulation, and rage (Howitt, 1995;<br />
Rossetti, 1990). In an effort to overcome feel<strong>in</strong>gs of helplessness and lack of control, the<br />
victim may reenact their own sexual abuse by replac<strong>in</strong>g their previous powerless role to that<br />
of dom<strong>in</strong>ator <strong>in</strong> an effort to dim<strong>in</strong>ish their anxiety about their own abuse (Rossetti, 1990;<br />
Howitt, 1995). In addition, it is likely that the anger associated with hav<strong>in</strong>g been abused is<br />
acted out aga<strong>in</strong>st younger people (Howitt, 1995). Unfortunately, the resolution of anxiety,<br />
anger, and stress is only temporary and the abuser must cont<strong>in</strong>ue victimiz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> order to<br />
subdue those feel<strong>in</strong>gs (Rossetti, 1990). This reenactment process has been termed<br />
"repetition compulsion" which is considered the "addictive process <strong>in</strong> pedophilia"<br />
(Rossetti, 1990). In other words, similar to an alcohol or drug addict us<strong>in</strong>g substances as a<br />
means to subdue feel<strong>in</strong>gs of anxiety, the pedophile engages <strong>in</strong> repetitive sexual behavior<br />
with children to cope with the distress related to their own abuse (Rosetti, 1990). Thus,<br />
accord<strong>in</strong>g to this model, adults who were abused as children are vulnerable to becom<strong>in</strong>g<br />
abusers themselves.
Behavioral theory.<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia 11<br />
Behaviorists believe that pedophilia is a learned behavior <strong>in</strong> which a sexually<br />
fulfill<strong>in</strong>g experience, typically masturbation, is preceded by stimuli associated with children<br />
(McGuire, Carlisle, & Young, 1965). That is, children become the 'object' or stimulus to<br />
which perpetrators masturbate and about whom they have sexual fantasies (McGuire et al.,<br />
1965). This may <strong>in</strong>itially become established by pair<strong>in</strong>g physiological sexual arousal with<br />
a deviant stimulus such as pornography of children (Hall & Andersen, 1993). The sexual<br />
impulses are re<strong>in</strong>forced by the cont<strong>in</strong>uation of the sexual act which, <strong>in</strong> turn, strengthens the<br />
conditioned relationship between the object of desire (i.e., the child) and sexual satisfaction<br />
(Hall & Anderson, 1993; Rosenhan & Seligman, 1989). This stimulus becomes<br />
<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly sexually arous<strong>in</strong>g and, subsequently, the masturbatory activity is transformed<br />
<strong>in</strong>to sexual relations with the child (McGuire et al., 1965).<br />
This theoretical approach focuses on conditioned behavior dur<strong>in</strong>g childhood.<br />
However, it does not address the regressed molester who for his entire sexual life has<br />
engaged <strong>in</strong> heterosexual, peer-age relationships. In other words, this conceptualization<br />
suggests that the vulnerability to molest only occurs through condition<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />
childhood, not anytime afterwards. In addition, behaviorists seem to claim that<br />
masturbation to an unacceptable stimulus lends itself to deviant sexual activity. In fact,<br />
some <strong>in</strong>dividuals engage <strong>in</strong> such private sexual activities yet they are never acted out on<br />
others. Therefore, it is possible that additional factors are <strong>in</strong>volved which, <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation<br />
to sexual condition<strong>in</strong>g, make sexual offend<strong>in</strong>g more conceivable.<br />
Social learn<strong>in</strong>g theory.<br />
A further extension of the behavioral theory is the role that social learn<strong>in</strong>g plays on<br />
an <strong>in</strong>dividual's sexual identity and desires. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Abel, Becker, & Cunn<strong>in</strong>gham-<br />
Rathner (1984), the social learn<strong>in</strong>g model contends that, dur<strong>in</strong>g the socialization process,
Narcissism and Pedophilia 12<br />
children learn which sexual activities are socially acceptable and which are not. Therefore,<br />
the child learns to <strong>in</strong>hibit overt expression of <strong>in</strong>appropriate sexual fantasies <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g<br />
particular stimuli. This process cont<strong>in</strong>ues <strong>in</strong>to adulthood where the <strong>in</strong>dividual has firmly<br />
dist<strong>in</strong>guished which fantasies to ignore. However, <strong>in</strong>hibition of sexual arousal does not<br />
always occur when children, ma<strong>in</strong>ly boys, engage <strong>in</strong> masturbatory activities. When<br />
fantasized stimuli considered <strong>in</strong>appropriate by society are coupled with masturbation, the<br />
entire fantasy element becomes erotic to the child and, consequently, provokes a sexual<br />
arousal response <strong>in</strong> the child. Typically such sexual activities are kept secret and, thus, the<br />
lack of negative consequences associated with the scorned act (i.e., punishment) facilitates<br />
cont<strong>in</strong>uation of the deviant sexual activity. As a result, learned <strong>in</strong>hibition of socially<br />
unacceptable sexual practices does not take place. With regard to child molesters, the<br />
fantasized stimuli eventually becomes young children and, due to the absence of negative<br />
consequences, this sexual attraction or arousal persists <strong>in</strong>to adulthood.<br />
Social learn<strong>in</strong>g theory <strong>in</strong>corporates the condition<strong>in</strong>g and re<strong>in</strong>forcement tenets<br />
associated with behavioral theory with<strong>in</strong> its framework; however, it <strong>in</strong>cludes model<strong>in</strong>g of<br />
socially acceptable behaviors as an additional component which behaviorists do not<br />
address.<br />
Cognitive theory.<br />
In addition to the above-mentioned explanations, cognitive theory suggests that<br />
paraphilics, <strong>in</strong> general, engage <strong>in</strong> distorted th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g which justifies their behavior as well as<br />
the cont<strong>in</strong>uation of their actions (Hall & Andersen, 1993). In fact, Abel et al. (1984)<br />
described seven cognitive distortions typically utilized by pedophiles:<br />
1) A child who does not physically resist my sexual advances really wants to have<br />
sex with me; 2) Hav<strong>in</strong>g sex with a child is a good way for an adult to teach the child<br />
about sex; 3) Children do not tell others about hav<strong>in</strong>g sex with a parent because
Narcissism and Pedophilia 13<br />
they really enjoy the sexual activity and want it to cont<strong>in</strong>ue; 4) Sometime <strong>in</strong> the<br />
future our society will realize that sex between a child and an adult is alright; 5) An<br />
adult who only feels a child's body or feels the child's genitals is not really be<strong>in</strong>g<br />
sexual with the child so no harm is be<strong>in</strong>g done; 6) When a child asks an adult a<br />
question about sex it means that the child wants to see the adult's sex organs or<br />
have sex with the adult; and 7) My relationship with my daughter or son or other<br />
child is enhanced by my hav<strong>in</strong>g sex with them (pp. 98-101).<br />
These <strong>in</strong>dividuals believe that certa<strong>in</strong> conduct is common to or acceptable by others (Hall &<br />
Andersen, 1993) and; therefore, they believe these cognitions <strong>in</strong> order to substantiate their<br />
behavior (Abel et al., 1984). In other words, they use denial and m<strong>in</strong>imization to decrease<br />
their level of emotional <strong>in</strong>volvement with their distressed victim (Ward et al., 1995). Thus,<br />
the perpetrator can fulfill his fantasies and desires without the associated feel<strong>in</strong>gs of guilt<br />
(Ward et al., 1995).<br />
Although social learn<strong>in</strong>g theory and cognitive theory appear similar based on the<br />
way <strong>in</strong> which the <strong>in</strong>dividual perceives the sexual activity, social learn<strong>in</strong>g theory<br />
concentrates more on the socialization process and whether the behavior rema<strong>in</strong>s undetected<br />
or punished. The former consequence (i.e., lack of detection), as dist<strong>in</strong>guished as part of<br />
the social learn<strong>in</strong>g model, suggests that the deviancy will cont<strong>in</strong>ue; however, the latter<br />
consequence (i.e., punishment), most likely, will lead to suppression of the behavior.<br />
Cognitive theory, on the other hand, focuses solely on the role of distorted beliefs<br />
associated with the sexual molestation. That is, the way <strong>in</strong> which pedophiles perceive the<br />
sexual activity determ<strong>in</strong>es their level of motivation <strong>in</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g child victimization. This<br />
model does not, however, account for the orig<strong>in</strong>s of these beliefs. One possibility is that<br />
by engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> pedophilic behaviors, the <strong>in</strong>dividual develops distorted cognitions to
Narcissism and Pedophilia 14<br />
m<strong>in</strong>imize the effects the abuse may have on their victim, which results <strong>in</strong> a cyclical pattern<br />
between deviant sexual behaviors and distorted thoughts.<br />
In support of these assumptions, Hayash<strong>in</strong>o et al. (1995) compared the level of<br />
cognitive distortions of <strong>in</strong>carcerated males convicted of <strong>in</strong>cestuous child molestation,<br />
extrafamilial child molestation, rape, and nonsex offense crimes. As was predicted, child<br />
molesters as a group exhibited different cognitions than a control group of laypersons<br />
without a history of sex offend<strong>in</strong>g. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, the extrafamilial child molesters had<br />
higher levels of cognitive distortions than all other groups and, <strong>in</strong> addition, appeared to<br />
m<strong>in</strong>imize and justify their behaviors more than the <strong>in</strong>cestuous offenders. These results are<br />
consistent with the notion that child molesters, especially extrafamilial, utilize cognitive<br />
distortions more than other sex offender groups to justify their actions. This is not to say<br />
that other offenders do not, but extrafamilial molesters of children may rely more heavily<br />
on those distortions to avoid emotional consequences of abus<strong>in</strong>g a child they do not know.<br />
Biological theory.<br />
Aside from the above-mentioned theoretical models of pedophilia, research on the<br />
biological l<strong>in</strong>ks to this disorder has been conducted, but only to a limited extent.<br />
Furthermore, the majority of the studies have evaluated paraphilias, <strong>in</strong> general, rather than<br />
solely pedophilia. That which has been specific to pedophilia has suggested that this group<br />
of sexual offenders, compared to all other paraphilics, evidence more bra<strong>in</strong> damage and<br />
dysfunction (Langev<strong>in</strong>, 1992). For <strong>in</strong>stance, one of the first studies of this phenomenon<br />
was conducted by Kolarsky, Freund, Machek, and Polak (1967) who revealed early<br />
temporal lobe damage as a causal factor <strong>in</strong> the deviant sexual activity of 86 epileptic men.<br />
Pedophiles also have evidenced left hemisphere dysfunction as assessed by the Halstead-<br />
Reitan Battery (Hucker, Langev<strong>in</strong>, Wortzman, Ba<strong>in</strong>, Handy, Chambers, & Wright, 1986).<br />
Furthermore, compared to controls, they have shown less dense and th<strong>in</strong>ner skulls, lower
Narcissism and Pedophilia 15<br />
levels of regional cerebral blood flow (Hendrick, Fitzpatrick, Hartman, Quaife, Statbucker,<br />
& Graber, 1988), as well as smaller left frontal and temporal bra<strong>in</strong> regions as compared to<br />
other sex offenders (Wright, Nobrega, Langev<strong>in</strong>, & Wortzman, 1990). Frontal lobe<br />
dysfunction has been found to impede one's ability to <strong>in</strong>hibit socially unacceptable<br />
impulses (Kalat, 1995). In addition, it may result <strong>in</strong> "dis<strong>in</strong>hibition, poor judgment,<br />
anxiety, low frustration tolerance, and impulsivity" (Rossetti, 1990). Temporal lobe<br />
impairment, on the other hand, affects emotional and motivational behavior (Kalat, 1995).<br />
That is, it could lead to "deviant fantasiz<strong>in</strong>g, compulsive th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about sexuality, and<br />
hypersexuality" (Rossetti, 1990).<br />
These above-described consequences of regional bra<strong>in</strong> impairment are similar to<br />
personality and behavioral patterns common to pedophiles. Despite the suggested<br />
<strong>in</strong>volvement of bra<strong>in</strong> abnormalities <strong>in</strong> pedophilia, these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs should be considered <strong>in</strong><br />
light of small sample size typically <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g homosexual males as well as the imperfect<br />
psychometric properties of certa<strong>in</strong> neuropsychological <strong>in</strong>struments (Langev<strong>in</strong>, 1992).<br />
Furthermore, this type of research makes it unclear whether the pedophilia preceded the<br />
bra<strong>in</strong> dysfunction or whether the cortical impairment contributed to the pedophilia.<br />
Along with neurological <strong>in</strong>vestigations, research also has concentrated on the<br />
function of the male hormone, testosterone, which is <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the regulation of the male<br />
sex drive and aggression (Rossetti, 1990). In fact, Berl<strong>in</strong> and Me<strong>in</strong>ecke (1981) treated 20<br />
paraphilic patients, 12 of whom were pedophiles, with medoxyprogesterone acetate, a drug<br />
which has been found to reduce levels of testosterone. This drug also is known to prevent<br />
elevations of follicle-stimulat<strong>in</strong>g hormone (FSH) and lute<strong>in</strong>iz<strong>in</strong>g hormone (LH) which<br />
compensate for lowered levels of testosterone. Results of this study revealed that only 2<br />
pedophiles relapsed dur<strong>in</strong>g treatment (which lasted anywhere from 3 months to almost 6<br />
years). In contrast, 5 of the 8 pedophiles, who did not rema<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> treatment, relapsed.
Narcissism and Pedophilia 16<br />
Although this sample size was too small to make def<strong>in</strong>ite conclusions, it is evident that<br />
decreas<strong>in</strong>g testosterone levels <strong>in</strong> pedophiles seems to deter sexually deviant behaviors.<br />
A later study by Gaffney and Berl<strong>in</strong> (1984) discovered <strong>in</strong>creased levels of<br />
lute<strong>in</strong>is<strong>in</strong>g hormone (LH) <strong>in</strong> pedophiles, as compared to basel<strong>in</strong>e levels <strong>in</strong> normal male<br />
controls, follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>jection of synthetic lute<strong>in</strong>iz<strong>in</strong>g hormone-releas<strong>in</strong>g hormone (LHRH)<br />
<strong>in</strong>to the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) cortical region. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs supported<br />
the correlation between testosterone levels and deviant behaviors <strong>in</strong> pedophiles. In<br />
addition, the data suggested an HPG dysfunction <strong>in</strong> these sex offenders. Based on the<br />
results of these studies, it is possible that hormone levels are associated with the<br />
development of pedophilia (Rossetti, 1990).<br />
Four factor model.<br />
Although these s<strong>in</strong>gle-factor theories of sexual abuse demonstrate specific variables<br />
thought to <strong>in</strong>fluence or predispose an <strong>in</strong>dividual to engage <strong>in</strong> such behaviors, it is felt that<br />
multi-factorial models are much more accurate <strong>in</strong> their account of pedophilia (Fisher,<br />
1994). One such model was developed by F<strong>in</strong>kelhor and Araji (1986) who identified four<br />
complementary factors under which theories of pedophilia can be subsumed: emotional<br />
congruence, sexual arousal, blockage, and dis<strong>in</strong>hibition.<br />
The first factor, emotional congruence, contends that the sexual abuser identifies his<br />
own emotional needs with those of the child. This is based on the fact that pedophiles have<br />
historically demonstrated an arrested psychosexual development which, subsequently,<br />
results <strong>in</strong> an immature and even child-like approach to their environment (Groth &<br />
Birnbaum, 1978; Hall et al., 1991). It also has been suggested that pedophiles have a<br />
narcissistic self-identification, similar to that of the child, which allows them to further<br />
identify with the child emotionally (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986; Simon et al., 1992).
Narcissism and Pedophilia 17<br />
In addition to identification with the child, it is believed that pedophiles attempt to<br />
avoid feel<strong>in</strong>gs of low self-esteem and low self efficacy that they usually experience when<br />
<strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g with peers by engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> sexual relations with children (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji,<br />
1986). That is, pedophiles may ga<strong>in</strong> a sense of control and power with a child who, by<br />
def<strong>in</strong>ition, is vulnerable and nonthreaten<strong>in</strong>g, compared to an adult (Loss & Glancy, 1983).<br />
The emotional congruence factor also applies to sexual abusers who were abused<br />
themselves as children. In other words, pedophiles not only identify with their victims,<br />
they also identify with their own abuser by engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> sexual activities with the child<br />
(F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986; Fisher, 1994; Howitt, 1995). In essence, it is assumed that the<br />
molester is able to overcome traumatic feel<strong>in</strong>gs of shame, humiliation, and helplessness<br />
they experienced dur<strong>in</strong>g their own sexual abuse (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986). This also<br />
assists the abuser to understand the emotional feel<strong>in</strong>gs expressed as the victimizer<br />
compared to the victim. Compared to previously discussed theories, emotional congruence<br />
is conceptually similar to the reenactment theory which contends that the perpetration helps<br />
resolve conflict and anxiety associated with the abuse experienced <strong>in</strong> childhood (Rossetti,<br />
1990).<br />
Factor two, sexual arousal, relates to the physiological sensations the molester<br />
experiences <strong>in</strong> response to children themselves, fantasies about children, or stimuli<br />
associated with children (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986). This factor is related to emotional<br />
congruence where<strong>in</strong>, if the pedophile responds to affective connections with his child<br />
victim, he must become sexually aroused by the child as well (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986).<br />
This assumption, however, is too simplistic as it does not account for those <strong>in</strong>dividuals<br />
who have engaged <strong>in</strong> sexual activities with children, yet are not "true" pedophiles who have<br />
an emotional attachment to children (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986). It is proposed that it is<br />
those <strong>in</strong>dividuals who experienced a significant sense of fulfillment or frustration (i.e.,
Narcissism and Pedophilia 18<br />
physiological arousal) associated with the sexual act with the child who are most vulnerable<br />
to pedophilia (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986).<br />
The third factor, blockage, entails the pedophile's <strong>in</strong>ability to f<strong>in</strong>d sexual pleasure<br />
from <strong>in</strong>timate relationships with peers (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986). Blockage has been<br />
<strong>in</strong>troduced by s<strong>in</strong>gle-factor theories such as psychodynamic and family dynamic models.<br />
Based on these accounts, however, blockage has been divided <strong>in</strong>to two categories,<br />
developmental and situational (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986). Developmental blockage<br />
<strong>in</strong>volves Oedipal conflicts where the <strong>in</strong>dividual is thought to be "prevented from mov<strong>in</strong>g<br />
<strong>in</strong>to the adult heterosexual stage of development" (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986, p. 154).<br />
Situational blockage <strong>in</strong>volves more <strong>in</strong>cestual types of molestation (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji,<br />
1986). In other words, these <strong>in</strong>dividuals are unable to engage <strong>in</strong> peer-appropriate sexual<br />
relations for reasons which preclude a preference for adult sexual relationships (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor<br />
& Araji, 1986).<br />
The fourth factor, dis<strong>in</strong>hibition, refers to the abuser's <strong>in</strong>creased acceptability of his<br />
sexual behavior (F<strong>in</strong>kelhor & Araji, 1986). That is, if pedophiles fully believe their actions<br />
are wrong and punishable, they probably would discont<strong>in</strong>ue their predatory behavior. As<br />
cognitive theorists suggest, however, child molesters distort their beliefs regard<strong>in</strong>g their<br />
sexual behavior which allows them to cont<strong>in</strong>ue gratify<strong>in</strong>g their sexual needs without feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />
remorse.<br />
As comprehensive as this theory may be, Howitt (1995) criticized it for its use of<br />
weak supportive evidence. For <strong>in</strong>stance, sample populations were almost exclusively<br />
prison or offender populations who were not compared to control groups on specified<br />
variables. Thus, this model may be biased <strong>in</strong> its presentation of pedophiles. Nonetheless,<br />
the theoretical notions <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> this multifactoral model depict the complexity of this
Narcissism and Pedophilia 19<br />
pattern of behavior. This may <strong>in</strong>crease cl<strong>in</strong>icians' awareness of vary<strong>in</strong>g factors so that<br />
case-specific treatment can be implemented.<br />
The various theoretical models of pedophilia presented <strong>in</strong> this manuscript provide a<br />
basic foundation for understand<strong>in</strong>g how sexual preferences for children may emerge. What<br />
follows is a review of the literature on personality characteristics of pedophilia. This will<br />
provide a more comprehensive description of this phenomenon so that comparisons of<br />
particular subpopulations of pedophilia can be made.<br />
Personality Characteristics of Pedophiles<br />
Much of the research on personality correlates of sexual offenders of children, as<br />
well as of adults, has utilized the M<strong>in</strong>nesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI).<br />
The MMPI is the most widely used, self-report measure of personality function<strong>in</strong>g and of<br />
psychopathology (Anastasi, 1988; Graham, 1987). Interpretations of the MMPI <strong>in</strong> this<br />
context have focused primarily on specific scale elevations as well as 2-po<strong>in</strong>t code profiles<br />
(Graham, 1987; Greene, 1991). The most common elevation for these sexual offenders<br />
has been on the Psychopathic Deviate (Pd) scale (Scale 4: Erickson, Luxemberg, Walbek,<br />
& Seely, 1987; Hall et al., 1991; Kalichman, 1991; Mann, Stenn<strong>in</strong>g, & Borman, 1992).<br />
The Pd scale was designed to detect <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are rebellious, impulsive, immature,<br />
self-centered, aggressive, and unemotional (Graham, 1993). Child molesters also have<br />
produced elevations on the Schizophrenia scale (Sc: Scale 8), together with the Pd scale,<br />
result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a 4-8/8-4 MMPI profile (Erickson et al., 1987). Individuals with this type of<br />
profile are considered bizarre, impulsive, defiant of authority, <strong>in</strong>secure, socially withdrawn<br />
and <strong>in</strong>ept, and are overly concerned about their sexual adequacy (Graham, 1993).<br />
Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, McCreary (1975) revealed that first offense child molesters showed<br />
MMPI profiles typical of normals whereas those with prior convictions showed a 4-8<br />
profile. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that child sex offenders who repeat their sexual crimes or
Narcissism and Pedophilia 20<br />
additional and potentially related crimes might have a more pathological motivation to<br />
ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> their deviant behaviors than do first time child victimizers. Thus, it might be that<br />
multiple offenses are associated more with severe pathology than are first time offenses.<br />
Although research has implicated the 4-8 profile type as typical of child molesters,<br />
this profile also has been revealed among other crim<strong>in</strong>al populations such as rapists<br />
(Erickson et al., 1987; Graham, 1993; Hall et al., 1991) and forensic psychiatric patients<br />
who committed murder of family and nonfamily members, property offenses, and arson<br />
(Qu<strong>in</strong>sey, Arnold, & Pruesse, 1980). In addition, it was demonstrated <strong>in</strong> profiles of<br />
<strong>in</strong>patients (1) with a history of sexual and drug abuse, (2) who prefer solitary activities, (3)<br />
who have difficulties with concentration, and (4) who are likely to be irritable and<br />
irresponsible (Archer, Griff<strong>in</strong>, & Aiduk, 1995). Based on these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, therefore,<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividuals who produce a 4-8 code profile are a heterogeneous population and, thus, this<br />
profile specificity cannot be established for the pedophile population.<br />
Despite the attention given to the 4-8 profile, pedophiles also have revealed 2-po<strong>in</strong>t<br />
code profiles consist<strong>in</strong>g of the Depression (D) and Pd scales, or 2-4/4-2 profiles (Erickson<br />
et al., 1987). Individuals with 2-4/4-2 comb<strong>in</strong>ations are typically described as impulsive,<br />
unable to delay gratification, remorseful about their actions, socially <strong>in</strong>ept, self-conscious,<br />
and passive-dependent (Graham, 1993). Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, an 8-2-4 code profile was found to<br />
be highly consistent with child sexual offenders who had an <strong>in</strong>tense and unsatisfied need<br />
for affection and fear and distrust of significant, emotional relationships (Hall et al., 1991).<br />
These characteristics correspond to the developmental patterns of pedophiles as suggested<br />
by psychoanalytic theory.<br />
The fact that pedophiles may have multiple scale elevations which vary with<strong>in</strong><br />
group suggests that profil<strong>in</strong>g us<strong>in</strong>g MMPI 2-po<strong>in</strong>t codes cannot establish solid personality<br />
factors associated with pedophilia. Kalichman (1991) supported this contention through
Narcissism and Pedophilia 21<br />
his <strong>in</strong>vestigation of personality characteristics of 144 <strong>in</strong>carcerated sex offenders whose<br />
victims <strong>in</strong>cluded prepubescent children, postpubescent adolescents, and adults. He found<br />
that sex offenders of children appeared to be more emotionally disturbed and exhibited<br />
greater psychopathology than sex offenders of adults, as demonstrated by higher elevations<br />
on the Hypochondriasis (Hs), D, Paranoia (Pa), Sc, and Social Introversion (Si) scales of<br />
the MMPI-2. Thus, although personality similarities exist among sex offenders of children<br />
and adults, it is possible that the degree of psychological disturbance, as <strong>in</strong>dicated by level<br />
of scale elevation, could dist<strong>in</strong>guish these two types of crim<strong>in</strong>als, rather than solely<br />
consider<strong>in</strong>g code type profiles .<br />
Scale elevations also were exam<strong>in</strong>ed by Heers<strong>in</strong>k and Strassberg (1995) who<br />
exam<strong>in</strong>ed 122 men <strong>in</strong> an outpatient, sex offender treatment program for molest<strong>in</strong>g children<br />
under the age of 15. These men were divided <strong>in</strong>to three clusters based on their MMPI<br />
scores. Cluster 1 (n = 87) consisted of profiles with<strong>in</strong> normal limits with only the Pd scale<br />
elevated (M = 67.7). Cluster 2 (n = 23) consisted of profiles with 8 out of the 10 cl<strong>in</strong>ical<br />
scales elevated. Those cl<strong>in</strong>ical scales with the highest scores were D (M = 86.0),<br />
Psychasthenia (Pt: M= 81.7), and Sc (M= 81.4), with Hs, Hysteria (Hy), Pd,<br />
Mascul<strong>in</strong>ity-Fem<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ity (Mf), and Pa scales also significantly elevated. Cluster 3 <strong>in</strong>cluded<br />
profiles with highest mean elevations on the Sc (M = 85.4), Pt (M= 76.1), and Mania (Ma:<br />
M = 73.5) scales.<br />
Based on these clusters alone, it is evident that sexual offenders of children are a<br />
heterogeneous population by themselves, let alone from sexual offenders of adults. The<br />
only demographic variable that could account for the different clusters was the "nature of<br />
the sex act." That is, Cluster 3 offenders engaged <strong>in</strong> some form of oral sex, whereas there<br />
was no <strong>in</strong>dication of this <strong>in</strong> the other groups. This variable was not further <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong><br />
this particular study. It is clear, however, that there were those molesters who had a
Narcissism and Pedophilia 22<br />
chronic pattern of psychological difficulties <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g anxiety, depression, and low self-<br />
esteem (Cluster 2) and those who demonstrated more cognitive disorganization and<br />
confusion (Cluster 3). Consider<strong>in</strong>g the results of this study with that of Kalichman's<br />
(1991), child molesters not only differ from other sex offenders <strong>in</strong> terms of degree of scale<br />
elevations and level of pathology, but they also differ among each other <strong>in</strong> the same<br />
manner. This perhaps verifies Simon et al.'s (1992) contention that child molesters cannot<br />
simply be characterized dichotomously but <strong>in</strong>stead along a cont<strong>in</strong>uum of variables.<br />
Aside from research focused on MMPI code profiles, other research has<br />
concentrated on other variables thought to dist<strong>in</strong>guish pedophiles from other sex offenders<br />
as well as from control groups. For <strong>in</strong>stance, Marshall, Jones, Hudson, and McDonald<br />
(1993) exam<strong>in</strong>ed generalized empathy <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>carcerated and outpatient child molesters and<br />
found the latter group as exhibit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>creased deficits <strong>in</strong> empathy when compared to the<br />
former group. Although there was the possibility of outliers <strong>in</strong> the outpatient group which<br />
may have distorted the data, these results suggest that the environment the offender is <strong>in</strong><br />
might affect their ability to empathize with their victims. More specifically, it is possible<br />
that, because the outpatient group may have had access to potential victims, they may<br />
cont<strong>in</strong>ue to rationalize their behavior as be<strong>in</strong>g acceptable and nontraumatic for the child. In<br />
contrast, the <strong>in</strong>carcerated offenders are strictly isolated from potential victims which might<br />
force them to acknowledge the detrimental effects their behavior had on their victim.<br />
Another study by Marshall, Barbaree, and Fernandez (1995) compared outpatient<br />
rapists, child molesters, nonoffend<strong>in</strong>g university students, and nonoffend<strong>in</strong>g community<br />
members on various levels of social function<strong>in</strong>g. Results <strong>in</strong>dicated that child molesters and<br />
community nonoffenders were lower <strong>in</strong> social self-confidence, more socially anxious, and<br />
demonstrated less assertiveness than the rapists and the university nonoffenders. Child
Narcissism and Pedophilia 23<br />
molesters also were found to have a different perception of what appropriate social<br />
behavior entails <strong>in</strong> contrast to what is generally accepted by others.<br />
Denial of hostility and psychopathology were other variables <strong>in</strong>vestigated by<br />
Wasyliw, Grossman, and Haywood (1994). Eighty-two male patients at a sexual<br />
behaviors cl<strong>in</strong>ic who were accused of child molestation were adm<strong>in</strong>istered the MMPI-2 and<br />
the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (BDHI: Buss & Durkee, 1957). Those patients who<br />
admitted to molestation (n = 45) scored significantly higher on the BDHI than those that<br />
denied molestation (n = 37). Out of the admitter group, 28% (n = 10) of the patients<br />
exaggerated their pathology on the MMPI-2 as compared to only 5% (n = 2) of the denial<br />
group (p < .01). Despite the fact that measures of hostility such as the BDHI are<br />
vulnerable to response bias, which may distort the results, these researchers found a<br />
correlation between acknowledgment of hostility and psychopathology. In particular, it<br />
was suggested that the admitters exaggerated their symptoms <strong>in</strong> order to blame their<br />
behaviors on a mental disorder. On the other hand, deniers, despite their guilt or<br />
<strong>in</strong>nocence, may have thought that any evidence of psychopathology would make them<br />
<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly suspect of deviant behaviors. Aside from the confound<strong>in</strong>g variables <strong>in</strong> this<br />
study, it still is worth not<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>in</strong>dividuals accused of molestation may have a particular<br />
response set <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g hostility and psychopathology and it is this pattern of impression<br />
management which must be considered <strong>in</strong> terms of diagnosis and treatment <strong>in</strong>tervention.<br />
Personality Characteristics of Ephebophiles<br />
Although it was previously <strong>in</strong>dicated that ephebophiles have not been explicitly and<br />
consistently dist<strong>in</strong>guished from pedophiles, it is essential to describe at this po<strong>in</strong>t what<br />
differences do, <strong>in</strong> fact, exist between these paraphilias. To reiterate, ephebophiles are those<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividuals who engage <strong>in</strong> sexual activity with pubertal children. Based on cl<strong>in</strong>ical<br />
observation, Rossetti (1990) found that ephebophiles generally demonstrate greater social
Narcissism and Pedophilia 24<br />
and psychosexual development than pedophiles. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, Greenberg, Bradford, and<br />
Curry (1993) compared pedophiles and ephebophiles who were sexually abused as<br />
children and found a strong correlation between age of their own abuse and age of their<br />
child victim. That is, pedophiles reported be<strong>in</strong>g molested at a younger age than did<br />
ephebophiles. Although self-report is not always reliable and memories of the onset of<br />
abuse may be distorted, these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs support social learn<strong>in</strong>g theory <strong>in</strong> that the sexual<br />
activity is patterned after experiences occurr<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g a particular phase of their childhood.<br />
This also might justify the developmental dist<strong>in</strong>ctions of these groups of molesters.<br />
Ephebophiles also tend to believe that the adolescent enjoyed the sexual <strong>in</strong>teraction based<br />
on the perpetrator's assumption that the adolescent had the ability to choose to engage <strong>in</strong> the<br />
sexual act. In addition, these sexual offenders usually do not believe that sexual coercion<br />
or exploitation has occurred. It is essential to po<strong>in</strong>t out that pedophiles have similar<br />
explanations for their behavior. It is possible then that these cognitive distortions are more<br />
prom<strong>in</strong>ent for ephebophiles.<br />
Pedophiles and ephebophiles also have been found to differ with regard to degree<br />
of psychopathology. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Kalichman's (1991) analysis, as was previously cited,<br />
offenders of adolescents demonstrated less psychological disturbances than offenders of<br />
children, although ephebophiles were found to be more pathological than their adult<br />
offender counterparts as <strong>in</strong>dicated by elevations on the MMPI-2. In addition, ephebophiles<br />
tended to be more self-confident and less emotionally expressive and self-effac<strong>in</strong>g than<br />
pedophiles. Ephebophiles, aga<strong>in</strong>, fell between adult offenders and pedophiles on these<br />
personality dimensions. Considerations have been made that offenders of adolescents<br />
might exhibit moderate symptoms of both psychological disturbance and sociopathy.<br />
Overall, offenders of children, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g adolescents, are thought to exhibit more
Narcissism and Pedophilia 25<br />
psychopathology and emotional regression than sexual offenders of adults who tend to be<br />
more antisocial, sociopathic, and defensive.<br />
Child Molestation <strong>in</strong> the Priesthood<br />
The purpose of this study is to identify personality characteristics present <strong>in</strong><br />
pedophiles who are also priests. The previous discussion described general characteristics,<br />
theories, and personality traits of pedophiles, <strong>in</strong> general, <strong>in</strong> order to contrast this<br />
<strong>in</strong>formation with what limited knowledge we have about sexually abus<strong>in</strong>g priests. At this<br />
po<strong>in</strong>t, a discussion of Catholic priests who are sex offenders as well as a review of the<br />
characteristics of priests <strong>in</strong> general is warranted. This discussion will allow for a clearer<br />
discussion of the m<strong>in</strong>ority of priests who are also pedophiles. In addition, it will attempt to<br />
demonstrate that if pedophiles are exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> terms of specific subpopulations (i.e.,<br />
priests with pedophilia) they may not appear as heterogeneous <strong>in</strong> personality traits as the<br />
literature has portrayed. The follow<strong>in</strong>g sections, therefore, will concentrate on<br />
characteristics of priests who do not molest children as compared to those priests who do.<br />
This will suggest that priests who are pedophiles have unique characteristics, despite<br />
similarities, from both nonpedophile priests and nonpriest pedophiles, which may<br />
predispose them to engage <strong>in</strong> sexual activities with children.<br />
With<strong>in</strong> the past twenty years, <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g reports have identified clergymembers and,<br />
<strong>in</strong> specific, Catholic priests who have sexually abused children (Plante, 1996; Sipe, 1990).<br />
Specifically, Jason Berry, a Catholic journalist, documented cases of child molestation by<br />
priests and, accord<strong>in</strong>g to his (1989) article, 200 priests or religious brothers were reported<br />
for child molestation between the years of 1983 and 1987. This averaged to approximately<br />
one case per week. Sipe (1990) also acknowledged, based on 25 years of <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g and<br />
treat<strong>in</strong>g priests with pedophilia, that pedophilic priests often enter the clergy with an already<br />
exist<strong>in</strong>g sexual preference for children. He believed that this orientation exists regardless
Narcissism and Pedophilia 26<br />
of the age the priest entered the sem<strong>in</strong>ary. However, he proposed that most of these men<br />
may not have been aware of their pedophilic tendencies until after their ord<strong>in</strong>ation.<br />
Despite reports of molestation, most cases of abuse, particularly <strong>in</strong> the past, have<br />
been concealed from public knowledge <strong>in</strong> order to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> the sanctity of the church<br />
(Plante, 1996). Sipe (1990) provided two reasons for this lack of awareness. First,<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the psychological and medical fields as well as the entire population have<br />
been ignorant of such a problem. Secondly, the Catholic Church has a "system of secrecy"<br />
<strong>in</strong> that issues regard<strong>in</strong>g sexuality of the clergy are avoided, overlooked, and actively<br />
suppressed. This is evidenced by the fact that only m<strong>in</strong>imal attention has been given to this<br />
issue <strong>in</strong> religious professional publications (Isely & Isely, 1990).<br />
Although there is a reluctance to disclose a priest or any other clergyperson as a<br />
child molester, society demonstrates a certa<strong>in</strong> degree of <strong>in</strong>trigue with this offender<br />
population (Plante, 1996). In fact, Plante (1996) suggested that this <strong>in</strong>terest occurs<br />
because clergy are assumed to be more ethical and scrupulous <strong>in</strong> their behaviors than the<br />
lay population. In addition, he po<strong>in</strong>ted out that children are believed to be safe <strong>in</strong> the<br />
presence of clergy. Furthermore, priests are sworn to celibacy and to break that vow with<br />
another adult, let alone a child, is aga<strong>in</strong>st all sworn and moral pr<strong>in</strong>ciples.<br />
It is obvious from the above discussion that religious figures are probably one of<br />
the most respected and trusted groups <strong>in</strong> society. They represent sanctity and spirituality<br />
and are considered to be unconditionally accept<strong>in</strong>g of all people. Unfortunately, those<br />
clergy members who use their powerful position to manipulate vulnerable children to<br />
gratify their sexual needs are viewed by many <strong>in</strong>dividuals as unforgivable. Therefore, the<br />
follow<strong>in</strong>g discussion will describe typical personality traits of nonoffend<strong>in</strong>g priests, as<br />
reviewed <strong>in</strong> the literature, and will attempt to identify potential risk factors related to priests
Narcissism and Pedophilia 27<br />
who molest children. This may shed additional light on this legal, moral, and ethical<br />
problem and might provide <strong>in</strong>sight as to how this problem may be solved.<br />
The Catholic Priest Profile<br />
In terms of typical personality commonalities among clergymen without any<br />
recognized pathologies, Kennedy, Heckler, Kobler, and Walker (1977) were the first to<br />
attempt to answer the question, "What is the Catholic priest like as a person?" (p. 120).<br />
These researchers <strong>in</strong>vestigated 271 priests who were randomly selected and later stratified<br />
accord<strong>in</strong>g to geography from United States Roman Catholic dioceses and religious<br />
communities. Data were primarily obta<strong>in</strong>ed via cl<strong>in</strong>ical <strong>in</strong>terviews, although psychological<br />
tests also were utilized. Us<strong>in</strong>g a stepwise discrim<strong>in</strong>ant analysis, researchers were able to<br />
categorize those priests who completed all cl<strong>in</strong>ical procedures (N = 218) <strong>in</strong> terms of their<br />
psychological development. These <strong>in</strong>cluded underdeveloped (57%), develop<strong>in</strong>g (29%),<br />
maldeveloped (8%), and developed (6%).<br />
The largest group consisted of the underdeveloped priests. These <strong>in</strong>dividuals<br />
identified themselves more with their religious duties than with their own personal qualities<br />
and responsibilities. Their lives tended to be structured by the expectations of others and,<br />
thus, uphold<strong>in</strong>g a particular status, which was be<strong>in</strong>g fulfilled through their church position,<br />
was particularly important to them. In addition, they had few <strong>in</strong>timate or platonic<br />
relationships and avoided display<strong>in</strong>g their emotions. They coped with distress through<br />
repression and <strong>in</strong>tellectualization. In general, although these priests were successful with<strong>in</strong><br />
the priesthood, they tended to be dissatisfied with their personal life.<br />
Develop<strong>in</strong>g priests, on the other hand, were <strong>in</strong> the process of rejuvenat<strong>in</strong>g their life,<br />
which had previously been emotionally and socially isolative. They encountered certa<strong>in</strong><br />
experiences <strong>in</strong> their lives (i.e., new job, death of a parent, or a significant religious or<br />
social experience) which provided motivation for personal growth. It is this experience
Narcissism and Pedophilia 28<br />
which is thought to allow the priest to rel<strong>in</strong>quish his defenses and enjoy new aspects of<br />
life.<br />
The maldeveloped group and the developed group comprised the smallest<br />
proportion of priests and, <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>gly, were characteristically opposite on a variety of<br />
dimensions. For <strong>in</strong>stance, the maldeveloped group evidenced long-term and serious<br />
psychological difficulties, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g covert hostility, low self-esteem, and disruptive sexual<br />
conflict. In contrast, the latter group was described as personally <strong>in</strong>tegrated, mature, and<br />
autonomous yet appropriately dependent. In addition, they could <strong>in</strong>vest emotional feel<strong>in</strong>gs<br />
<strong>in</strong> others without concern. They also were sexually secure and confident <strong>in</strong> their celibacy<br />
oath and were productive <strong>in</strong> their career.<br />
It was the underdeveloped and the maldeveloped priests that were the focus of<br />
Keddy, Erdberg, and Sammon's (1990) study. These researchers <strong>in</strong>vestigated personality<br />
characteristics via psychological assessment of priests or brothers (N = 29) and nuns (N =<br />
13) admitted to a residential treatment program. Typical present<strong>in</strong>g problems were<br />
<strong>in</strong>terpersonal difficulties, depression, sexual concerns, and vocational <strong>in</strong>decision.<br />
Performance on the WAIS-R revealed that these clergy functioned at an average <strong>in</strong>telligence<br />
level, with highest subtest scores be<strong>in</strong>g on those most <strong>in</strong>dicative of <strong>in</strong>tellectualization<br />
processes (e.g., Vocabulary and Comprehension). The MMPI revealed normal scale<br />
elevations overall; however, 78% of the clergy had at least one cl<strong>in</strong>ically significant scale<br />
elevation (T > 70). The Mf scale mean for all participants was 75.9. This suggested that<br />
male clergy with high elevations on this scale typically did not exemplify traditional male<br />
characteristics. In fact, psychiatric males with elevations on this scale tend to be passive,<br />
dependent, <strong>in</strong>secure about their mascul<strong>in</strong>e role, and <strong>in</strong>ner-directed (Greene, 1991).<br />
Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, 44% (n = 12) of the male clergy were considered to have significant<br />
difficulties with sexual issues as evidenced by <strong>in</strong>formation on the face sheet of their
Narcissism and Pedophilia 29<br />
evaluation. Thirty percent (n = 8) of those cases <strong>in</strong>volved distress or confusion regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />
sexual orientation. In addition, the Rorschach demonstrated that these clergy relied on<br />
<strong>in</strong>tellectualization as a way of suppress<strong>in</strong>g and avoid<strong>in</strong>g emotional complexity. As a result,<br />
these clergy were prone to emotional outbursts and loss of control when confronted with<br />
situations of an emotional nature. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were similar to those of the<br />
underdeveloped priests described by Kennedy et al. (1977).<br />
More recently, Plante, Manuel, and Tandez (1996a) evaluated personality<br />
characteristics of 21 successful candidates to the Catholic priesthood. Elevated scores on<br />
the MMPI-2 were revealed on the Lie (L; M = 57.24, SD = 8.57), K (M = 60.48, SD =<br />
9.03), Mf (M = 58.86, SD = 6.89), Gender Role-Fem<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>e (Gf; M = 60.30, SD = 11.39),<br />
Overcontrolled-Hostility (O-H; M = 60.71, SD = 8.06), and Social Responsibility scales<br />
(RE; M = 57.52, SD = 9.70). Lowered scores on the MMPI-2 were found on the F (M =<br />
43.91, SD = 4.81), D (M = 43.86, SD = 7.65), Anxiety (ANX; M = 44.95, SD = 8.02),<br />
as well as several other supplementary scales <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the Anger (ANG), Antisocial<br />
Behavior (ASP), Type A Behavior (TPA), and Obsessions (OBS) scales. Based on these<br />
results as well as those from the 16PF (Cattell, Cattell, & Cattell, 1993) and cl<strong>in</strong>ical<br />
<strong>in</strong>terviews, these priests demonstrated adequate and stable personality adjustment and were<br />
found to be bright and <strong>in</strong>terpersonally sensitive. Results also <strong>in</strong>dicated, however, that<br />
these priests were defensive and tended to be concerned about controll<strong>in</strong>g hostile impulses.<br />
To be more specific, those priests scor<strong>in</strong>g high on the O-H scale exemplify <strong>in</strong>dividuals who<br />
attempt to control their hostility even under circumstances which ord<strong>in</strong>arily would provoke<br />
an emotional reaction (Greene, 1991). Unfortunately, there comes a po<strong>in</strong>t when these<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividuals experience an overload of this controlled emotion, forc<strong>in</strong>g them to exhibit their<br />
aggression (Greene, 1991).
Narcissism and Pedophilia 30<br />
It should be noted that these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were based on priest candidates from only one<br />
religious order which limits generalizability (Plante et al., 1996). In addition, the chance of<br />
committ<strong>in</strong>g a Type I error was <strong>in</strong>creased given the large number of statistical analyses<br />
performed on these data (Plante et al., 1996). F<strong>in</strong>ally, due to the defensive response style<br />
of these priests, <strong>in</strong>terpretation should be considered with caution (Plante et al., 1996).<br />
Narcissistic Tendencies <strong>in</strong> Priests<br />
Narcissistic characteristics also have been proposed as be<strong>in</strong>g prevalent <strong>in</strong> clergy<br />
members. Because this variable will be the focus of this study, theories of narcissism will<br />
be expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> this section prior to discussion of its relevance to priests.<br />
Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Kernberg (1976):<br />
The ma<strong>in</strong> characteristics of these narcissistic personalities are grandiosity, extreme<br />
self-centeredness, and a remarkable absence of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> and empathy for others <strong>in</strong><br />
spite of the fact that they are eager to obta<strong>in</strong> admiration and approval from other<br />
people. These patients experience a remarkable <strong>in</strong>tense envy of other people who<br />
seem to have th<strong>in</strong>gs they do not have or who simply seem to enjoy their lives.<br />
These patients not only lack emotional depth and fail to understand complex<br />
emotions <strong>in</strong> other people, but their own feel<strong>in</strong>gs lack differentiation, with quick<br />
flare-ups and subsequent dispersal of emotion. They are especially deficient <strong>in</strong><br />
genu<strong>in</strong>e feel<strong>in</strong>gs of sadness and mournful long<strong>in</strong>g; their <strong>in</strong>capacity for experienc<strong>in</strong>g<br />
depressive reactions is a basic feature of their personalities. When abandoned or<br />
disappo<strong>in</strong>ted by other people, they may show what on the surface may look like<br />
depression, but which on further exam<strong>in</strong>ation emerges as anger and resentment,<br />
loaded with revengeful wishes, rather than sadness for the loss of a person whom<br />
they appreciate (pp. 228-229).
Narcissism and Pedophilia 31<br />
Based on a psychoanalytic approach to this type of personality, Kernberg (1975)<br />
contends that narcissism is manifested by a contradiction between the ego and superego<br />
where<strong>in</strong> the narcissist may at one moment feel <strong>in</strong>secure and <strong>in</strong>ferior while the next moment<br />
believe he or she is omnipotent. This theorist believes that the narcissist is able to cope<br />
with this struggle by <strong>in</strong>vest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> defense mechanisms such as splitt<strong>in</strong>g and denial. More<br />
fundamentally, Kernberg proposes that, for the narcissist, their ideal self, ideal object, and<br />
actual self images are bound together so that focus is placed on their ideal self which serves<br />
to overshadow the weaknesses <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> their actual self. It is this ideal self focus which<br />
perpetuates the grandiosity and enhanced self-esteem typical of these <strong>in</strong>dividuals.<br />
Kernberg ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>s through his cl<strong>in</strong>ical experiences that this entire process is often<br />
<strong>in</strong>fluenced by "chronically cold parental figures with covert but <strong>in</strong>tense aggression"<br />
(p.234). The child must, therefore, learn to defend him or herself at an early age aga<strong>in</strong>st<br />
hate, envy, and devaluation and, <strong>in</strong> turn, feels that he or she can only love and trust<br />
themselves.<br />
Both Kernberg (1975, 1986) and Kohut (1977) ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> that narcissists can be<br />
categorized as either overt or covert with regard to expression of their personality<br />
characteristics. In other words, the overt narcissist outwardly displays his or her sense of<br />
self-importance and strong desire for admiration and attention. Conversely, the covert<br />
narcissist typically is unaware of their feel<strong>in</strong>gs of grandeur and, <strong>in</strong> fact, appears <strong>in</strong>secure,<br />
hypersensitive, and lacks self-confidence. Despite these differences, both categories of<br />
narcissists possess those qualities (i.e., manipulativeness, exploitativeness, and sense of<br />
entitlement) <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> and most typical of this type of personality.<br />
In contrast to the psychoanalytic model, Millon (1981) and Millon and Everly<br />
(1985) based the emergence of narcissism on social learn<strong>in</strong>g theory. A hallmark of this<br />
model is that narcissistic development results from children be<strong>in</strong>g overvalued, as opposed
Narcissism and Pedophilia 32<br />
to devalued, by their parents. Due to parental over<strong>in</strong>dulgence, the child becomes egotistical<br />
and learns how to exploit and manipulate others so as to receive the attention and special<br />
treatment to which they have been accustomed and which they feel they deserve. This<br />
process leads to a perception that others are <strong>in</strong>ferior, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, re<strong>in</strong>forces the<br />
grandiose and exploitive nature of the narcissist. It must be noted that this pattern is<br />
presumably common <strong>in</strong> "only-children" because they are often valued by their parents as<br />
'special.' They rarely experience restrictions nor are they given responsibilities that<br />
typically would be required if there were other children <strong>in</strong> the family.<br />
With regard to priests, Meloy (1986) hypothesized that narcissistic qualities<br />
exhibited by clergy members prior to ord<strong>in</strong>ation are strengthened largely due to the nature<br />
of their profession and the constant reverence from their parishioners. Meloy uses both<br />
Kernberg's (1975) and Kohut's (1971) contention of normal and pathological narcissism<br />
with regard to characterological qualities <strong>in</strong> priests. Meloy states that:<br />
Narcissistically balanced <strong>in</strong>dividuals experience themselves as the biological and<br />
psychological center of their universe. They have the capacity and motivation to<br />
care for themselves at least as much as they do others. Loved ones are perceived as<br />
whole and separate <strong>in</strong>dividuals with their own needs and desires. They easily<br />
recognize rather than deny the magical quality of their own transient fantasies of<br />
entitlement and grandiosity <strong>in</strong> a world that is quite forgetful and not very forgiv<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
Narcissistically balanced clergy will reflect on personal experience and behavior,<br />
and allow a nurturant blend of private time alone, <strong>in</strong>timate time with family, and<br />
public time with parishioners (p. 50).<br />
In contrast, the pathological narcissist demonstrates such extreme grandiosity which<br />
precludes empathic and genu<strong>in</strong>e feel<strong>in</strong>gs towards the welfare of others. To reiterate<br />
theoretical contentions, splitt<strong>in</strong>g is utilized both as an <strong>in</strong>herent defense mechanism for the
Narcissism and Pedophilia 33<br />
narcissist as well as an aspect of clergy members' duty to the church. More specifically,<br />
clergy are supposed to preach ethical and moral standards for their congregation. With the<br />
existence of ambivalence regard<strong>in</strong>g those preach<strong>in</strong>gs, they engage <strong>in</strong> splitt<strong>in</strong>g to reduce any<br />
anxiety associated with the ambivalence. In essence then, the world is considered either<br />
good or bad, and it is this dichotomous perception that <strong>in</strong>hibits the clergy person from<br />
experienc<strong>in</strong>g vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees of emotion. As a result, the narcissistic clergy member is<br />
vulnerable to engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> unstable and <strong>in</strong>significant relationships, both <strong>in</strong>timate and<br />
professional.<br />
Associated with the pathological narcissistic personality are themes of omnipotence<br />
and entitlement. To be more specific, this type of clergy person tends to feel omnipotent<br />
and omniscient <strong>in</strong> their role of relay<strong>in</strong>g the word of God. They also feel entitled to the<br />
admiration and attention they receive from others based on their spiritual importance and<br />
contributions to the lives of their parishioners. As such, "for the narcissistically disordered<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividual respond<strong>in</strong>g to a "call<strong>in</strong>g" from God, the grandiose self is fortified by the passive<br />
acceptance of the ideal self as "chosen" and its psychodynamic fusion with the deified ideal<br />
object, consciously valued as a special relationship to God" (p. 51).<br />
The previous section explored the personality characteristics of priests as found <strong>in</strong><br />
the literature. That which was most outstand<strong>in</strong>g was that priests, for the most part, are<br />
quite dedicated to their religious responsibilities and are concerned with fulfill<strong>in</strong>g those<br />
duties as well as the expectations of their parishioners. Overall, they demonstrate adequate<br />
personality adjustment, typically avoid displays of emotion, and rely on <strong>in</strong>tellectualization<br />
to cope with distress.<br />
The issue of narcissism was discussed as a focal po<strong>in</strong>t of this study. This<br />
personality characteristic has been theorized as an <strong>in</strong>herent component of the personality of<br />
some priests. More specifically, it has been viewed as both a precursor and result of those
Narcissism and Pedophilia 34<br />
priests and their "call<strong>in</strong>g" to their spiritual obligations. This variable of narcissism, <strong>in</strong><br />
particular, will be addressed later <strong>in</strong> regard to priests with pedophilia.<br />
The Priest Pedophile<br />
As was previously mentioned, the literature regard<strong>in</strong>g sexually molest<strong>in</strong>g priests is<br />
quite limited and; thus, little <strong>in</strong>formation and <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to this population of priests can be<br />
given. One extensive exam<strong>in</strong>ation of 150 Catholic clergy members who were admitted to<br />
an <strong>in</strong>patient treatment facility was conducted by Rob<strong>in</strong>son (1994). These clergy were<br />
categorized <strong>in</strong>to five diagnostic groups: 1) Paraphilia Not Otherwise Specified-<br />
Ephebophilia, 2) Pedophilia, 3) Sexual Disorder Not Otherwise Specified-Compulsive<br />
Sexuality, 4) Alcohol Dependence or Alcohol Abuse, and 5) a Control consist<strong>in</strong>g of priests<br />
diagnosed with Occupational Problem, Other Interpersonal Problem, No Diagnosis on Axis<br />
I, Adjustment Disorder with Depressed Mood, and Adjustment Disorder with Anxious<br />
Mood. All groups were compared on personality dimensions assessed by the MMPI-2,<br />
MCMI-II, and the Rorschach.<br />
Descriptive statistical analyses of 2-po<strong>in</strong>t elevations on the MMPI-2 suggested that<br />
all groups were heterogeneous <strong>in</strong> that no group demonstrated a specific 2-po<strong>in</strong>t profile nor<br />
did they reflect cl<strong>in</strong>ically elevated scores. The MCMI-II also revealed similar results;<br />
however, the most common elevations across groups was a compulsive-dependent<br />
comb<strong>in</strong>ation. Rob<strong>in</strong>son found this to suggest that clergy undergo<strong>in</strong>g a psychological<br />
evaluation exhibit significant compulsive and dependent personality traits. He was unable<br />
to assess whether these traits are typical of clergy who were not seek<strong>in</strong>g treatment.<br />
In terms of the Rorschach, significant differences were found on a variety of<br />
variables. First, the control group scored significantly lower on Food Content (.27) than<br />
the pedophile (.83) and sexual compulsive groups (1.07). This suggests that the latter two<br />
groups (i.e., pedophiles, and sexual compulsives) have higher levels of dependency than
Narcissism and Pedophilia 35<br />
the control group. Second, all groups scored significantly below average on the<br />
Conventional Form Ratio (F+%). In specific, the pedophile group (0.47) scored<br />
significantly lower than the ephebophile group (0.60) and the control group (0.59) on this<br />
measure. This <strong>in</strong>dicates that pedophiles may have more of a conventional th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g style<br />
than these other two groups (i.e., ephebophiles and controls). Third, alcoholics (.44) and<br />
pedophiles (0.46) scored lowest on Perceptual Accuracy (X+%) while the ephebophiles<br />
(0.55) and the controls (0.58) scored significantly higher. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that the<br />
former two groups (i.e., alcoholics and pedophiles) may have a more distorted th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g<br />
pattern and impaired problem solv<strong>in</strong>g abilities. Fourth, the control group (1.19) scored<br />
lowest on the Disordered Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g (Sum6) variable. These scores were found to be<br />
significantly lower than that of the sexual compulsives (5.00). F<strong>in</strong>ally, on the Achromatic<br />
Color (C') variable, the control group scored the lowest (1.27) while the pedophile group<br />
scored the highest (2.57). This <strong>in</strong>dicated that pedophiles may have experienced more<br />
trauma <strong>in</strong> their lives than the other groups.<br />
Rob<strong>in</strong>son summarized the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of this Rorschach data <strong>in</strong> that the control group<br />
demonstrated significantly lower levels of pathology than the other groups. In addition,<br />
pedophiles were more impaired than the ephebophiles on various dimensions which<br />
supports Kalichman's (1991) contention that victim age may determ<strong>in</strong>e degree of<br />
psychological disturbance.<br />
Rob<strong>in</strong>son also performed discrim<strong>in</strong>ant analyses on all of the above-mentioned<br />
measures (i.e., MMPI-2, MCMI-II, and the Rorschach) to discrim<strong>in</strong>ate ephebophiles and<br />
pedophiles (see Table 1). Significant group differences were <strong>in</strong>dicated for the Mascul<strong>in</strong>ity-<br />
Fem<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ity, Depression, Hypochondriasis, and Schizophrenia scales of the MMPI-2. For<br />
MMPI-2 scale and subscale T-scores, Social Alienation, PTSD (Keane), Psychomotor<br />
Retardation, Hypochondriasis, Addiction Potential, and PTSD (Schlenger) were
Table 1<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia 36<br />
Means and Standard Deviations for Pedophiles and Ephebophiles on the Significant Scales of<br />
the MMPI-2, the MCMI-II, and the Rorschach<br />
Pedophiles<br />
Ephebophiles<br />
Measure Mean SD Mean SD<br />
MMPI-2:<br />
Hypochondriasis 52.50 9.10 55.57 8.75<br />
Depression 59.50 11.31 56.63 10.08<br />
Mascul<strong>in</strong>ity-Fem<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ity 55.47 9.08 59.17 8.58<br />
Schizophrenia 57.27 12.51 54.67 9.29<br />
MCMI-II:<br />
Histrionic 46.77 27.49 49.27 25.17<br />
Antisocial 33.33 23.72 42.23 17.27<br />
Schizotypal 54.00 17.15 48.37 13.59<br />
Anxiety 42.37 29.42 44.17 28.63<br />
Somatoform 48.30 18.61 57.40 15.98<br />
Alcohol Dependence 31.17 23.30 35.83 20.81<br />
Thought Disorder 24.73 24.99 31.07 24.38<br />
Rorschach:<br />
Pure Form (F+%) 0.42 0.21 0.60 0.24<br />
Active (a) 6.13 2.97 8.00 3.98<br />
Passive (p) 5.90 1.67 4.80 3.63<br />
Achromatic Color (Sum C')<br />
Complexity (Blends)<br />
2.57<br />
4.70<br />
2.74<br />
3.26<br />
1.47<br />
5.43<br />
1.60<br />
4.32<br />
Color/Form (CF)<br />
Animal Detail (Ad)<br />
Mythical Animal Detail ((Ad))<br />
1.83<br />
3.13<br />
0.23<br />
1.68<br />
1.57<br />
0.50<br />
1.53<br />
2.13<br />
0.03<br />
1.61<br />
1.70<br />
0.18<br />
Color Nam<strong>in</strong>g (Cn) 0.00 0.00 0.10 0.55<br />
Conventional Form (X+%)<br />
Incongruous-2 (INC2)<br />
0.46<br />
0.03<br />
0.17<br />
0.18<br />
0.55<br />
0.13<br />
0.16<br />
0.43<br />
Note. Adapted from "Shadows of the Lantern Bearers: A Study of Sexually Troubled<br />
Clergy," by E. A. Rob<strong>in</strong>son, 1994, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, pp. 90-184.
Narcissism and Pedophilia 37<br />
significant. For the MCMI-II, only the Anti-Social personality trait scale was found to be<br />
significant when only analyz<strong>in</strong>g the personality scales. However, when us<strong>in</strong>g all scores,<br />
there were significant differences on the Somatoform, Antisocial, Schizotypal, Histrionic,<br />
Anxiety, Thought-Disorder, and Alcohol Dependence scales. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the Rorschach<br />
revealed significant differences on the Pure Form, the Active-Passive Ratio, Passive<br />
Movement, Achromatic Color, Complexity, Incongruous Special Scores, and Obsessive<br />
style when analyz<strong>in</strong>g only structural summary scores. However, for all scores there was<br />
statistical significance for Pure Form, the Active-Passive Ratio, Form/Achromatic Color,<br />
Complexity, Color/Form, Animal Detail, Mythical Animal Detail, Color Nam<strong>in</strong>g,<br />
Conventional Form Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, and Incongruous Special Scores-Level 2.<br />
Rob<strong>in</strong>son's work clearly provided a wealth of <strong>in</strong>formation about sexual disorders <strong>in</strong><br />
clergy. There were, however, several limitations to his study which need to be addressed.<br />
First, the large number of statistical analyses may have resulted <strong>in</strong> a Type I error; thus,<br />
there may not be true differences between the priest groups on those variables outl<strong>in</strong>ed<br />
(Rob<strong>in</strong>son, 1994). Second, the Rorschach data may have been biased based on the limited<br />
range of scores produced for certa<strong>in</strong> variables (Rob<strong>in</strong>son, 1994). Third, these results<br />
cannot be generalized to the control sexually disordered nor the nonpsychiatric priest<br />
populations. Therefore, it is unclear whether these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs would hold true for these<br />
groups. In fact, it is possible that there are traits specific to priests, due to the nature of<br />
their vocation, as well as those typical of sexually disordered <strong>in</strong>dividuals, specifically<br />
pedophiles, which may demonstrate, psychodiagnostically, differences among personality<br />
characteristics from sexually disordered priests.<br />
Another study by Stumpf (1995) focused on personality pathology and traits of 167<br />
<strong>in</strong>patient priests and religious men and women with sexual disorders and nonsexual<br />
psychiatric disorders. Assessment measures utilized were the MCMI-II, the NEO-
Narcissism and Pedophilia 38<br />
Personality Inventory, as well as other measures to assess treatment response. Accord<strong>in</strong>g<br />
to results, approximately 37% of the participants (n = 62) were diagnosed with Paraphilia<br />
Not Otherwise Specified, Ephebophilia, 38% with Sexual Disorder Not Otherwise<br />
Specified (n = 63), and only 11% were diagnosed with Pedophilia (n = 19). Contrary to<br />
prediction, results <strong>in</strong>dicated that the existence of a personality disorder enhanced treatment<br />
effectiveness <strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>dividuals. It is possible, therefore, that personality traits are not as<br />
negatively correlated with treatment response <strong>in</strong> this particular population of patients as<br />
with other groups of patients. In terms of personality differences among groups, no<br />
significant differences were found except on the MCMI-II Bipolar scale (p < .025),<br />
however, no between group specifications were made regard<strong>in</strong>g where those differences<br />
were found.<br />
Further <strong>in</strong>vestigation of priest sexual offenders was conducted by Plante, Manuel,<br />
and Bryant (1996b) who exam<strong>in</strong>ed 160 Roman Catholic priests aged 22 to 74 who were<br />
hospitalized <strong>in</strong> a private psychiatric facility specific to clergymembers. They compared<br />
personality and cognitive variables of sexually offend<strong>in</strong>g priests (n = 80) to nonsexually<br />
offend<strong>in</strong>g priests (n = 80) who were primarily diagnosed with affective, adjustment, or<br />
eat<strong>in</strong>g disorders, as assessed by the WAIS-R, the Halstead-Reitan, and the MMPI-2.<br />
Results from Plante et al.'s (1996b) primary analyses <strong>in</strong>dicated that the sex offend<strong>in</strong>g<br />
group was significantly older (M = 52.49, SD = 9.86) than the nonsex offend<strong>in</strong>g group (M =<br />
45.06, SD = 11.09). In addition, the sexually abus<strong>in</strong>g priests scored significantly higher on<br />
the Overcontrolled-Hostility MMPI scale (O-H: M = 56.08, SD = 10.18) than did their<br />
nonabus<strong>in</strong>g counterparts (M = 52.98, SD = 9.30). In fact, twenty of the total sexually<br />
abus<strong>in</strong>g group had cl<strong>in</strong>ically significant elevations on this scale as compared to only eleven of<br />
the total nonsexually abus<strong>in</strong>g group. Based on these results, sex offend<strong>in</strong>g priests evidenced<br />
more passivity and compliance toward others, especially authority figures, than their
Narcissism and Pedophilia 39<br />
counterparts. It is the passivity which was thought to represent a denial of emotional<br />
disturbance, specifically, hostility. In fact, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with this type of personality style tend<br />
to avoid conflict, are unassertive, are dependent on others, and usually are aware of hostility<br />
and rejection by others yet they prefer to ignore it. It is the comb<strong>in</strong>ation of these traits along<br />
with a fear of rejection and abandonment which lead to <strong>in</strong>security and vulnerability to act out<br />
and blame others. These researchers suggest that the denial of hostility and aggressive<br />
impulses comb<strong>in</strong>ed with the <strong>in</strong>ability to appropriately cope with these feel<strong>in</strong>gs, might be<br />
expressed through the abuse of a m<strong>in</strong>or.<br />
In reference to the cognitive measures utilized <strong>in</strong> Plante et al.'s (1996b) study, there<br />
were no significant differences among hospitalized priest groups. More specifically, mean<br />
Full Scale IQ scores on the WAIS-R were <strong>in</strong> the above average range of <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />
function<strong>in</strong>g for both the sexual abus<strong>in</strong>g group (M= 120) and the nonsexual abus<strong>in</strong>g group<br />
(M= 117). Furthermore, these groups both evidenced low cognitive impairment on the<br />
Halstead Impairment Index with scores of .26 for both groups. F<strong>in</strong>ally, about 67% of the<br />
sexual offend<strong>in</strong>g priests were diagnosed with a personality disorder compared to 58% of<br />
the nonsexual offend<strong>in</strong>g priests. Although these statistics were not found to be significant<br />
(χ 2 = 82.96, p > .05), it is possible that personality deficits may exist and even make some<br />
priests vulnerable to cognitive and <strong>in</strong>terpersonal difficulties.<br />
Despite the significant results of this study, there were some methodological issues to<br />
consider. First, neither an outpatient nor a "normal" control group of priests were <strong>in</strong>cluded<br />
which greatly affects generalizability and external validity (Plante et al., 1996b). Second,<br />
demographic variables such as crim<strong>in</strong>al history, number of victims, prior history of sexual<br />
and/or physical abuse, duration and frequency of abuse were not available (Plante et al.,<br />
1996). It is unclear, therefore, whether that demographic <strong>in</strong>formation would have affected<br />
the outcome of the analysis. That is, significant differences among groups on demographic
Narcissism and Pedophilia 40<br />
variables may have confounded the study. Without collect<strong>in</strong>g such data, these researchers<br />
were unable to control for confound<strong>in</strong>g effects which may have existed.<br />
In addition to these above-listed limitations, these researchers suggested that the<br />
statistically significant f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of this study may not necessarily suggest cl<strong>in</strong>ical<br />
significance. In other words, just because sexually abus<strong>in</strong>g priests scored generally higher<br />
than the non-abus<strong>in</strong>g priests on the Overcontrolled-Hostility scale, this does not <strong>in</strong>dicate that<br />
they possess the personality traits described above at the cl<strong>in</strong>ical level. Nonetheless, these<br />
f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs implicate differences among hospitalized priest groups who do and do not sexually<br />
abuse children along this overcontrolled-hostility dimension. In fact, denial and repression<br />
were previously described by various theories as a way for the pedophile to rationalize his<br />
behavior. Utilization of these strategies have been postulated by psychoanalysts as<br />
prevent<strong>in</strong>g feel<strong>in</strong>gs of guilt and empathy for the victim which, <strong>in</strong> turn, leads to further<br />
victimization.<br />
Priests with pedophilia seem to have similar personality characteristics as their control<br />
pedophile counterparts. These <strong>in</strong>clude passivity, dependency, deficits <strong>in</strong> emotion, <strong>in</strong>security,<br />
as well as other traits. However, the literature has only compared sexually disordered priests<br />
to priests with nonsexual psychiatric disorders. Therefore, it is difficult to reliably assume<br />
that priest pedophiles and generic pedophiles are homogenous based solely on their sexual<br />
behavior. It appears that there are variables which may dist<strong>in</strong>guish the priest pedophiles from<br />
the generic pedophiles, specifically, narcissism and overcontrolled-hostility. Although, the<br />
research has not identified narcissism as a salient characteristic of priest pedophiles, it seems<br />
possible that this feature exists <strong>in</strong> this population based on f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of similar characteristics<br />
among priesthood members. A more detailed argument regard<strong>in</strong>g these issues will be<br />
presented <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g section.
Statement of the Problem<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia 41<br />
Children who have been sexually victimized suffer lifelong emotional consequences.<br />
This devastat<strong>in</strong>g experience leaves them susceptible to numerous difficulties such as post-<br />
traumatic stress disorder, eat<strong>in</strong>g disorders, substance abuse, and depression (K<strong>in</strong>near,<br />
1995). These emotional ramifications of sexual abuse, among many others, highlight the<br />
importance of <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the phenomenon of pedophilia. If personality correlates of<br />
pedophilia could be identified as risk factors, the potential for identify<strong>in</strong>g and prevent<strong>in</strong>g<br />
abuse will be greater. Unfortunately, pedophiles have been found to be a heterogeneous<br />
population when compared to other sex offenders (Erickson et al., 1987; Hall et al., 1991;<br />
Heers<strong>in</strong>k & Strassberg, 1995; McCreary, 1975). Therefore, it might be more helpful to<br />
exam<strong>in</strong>e particular subgroups of pedophilia rather than to assume that <strong>in</strong>vestigations of<br />
pedophiles as a whole will be fruitful <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g risk factors. One group that is poorly<br />
understood and may be unique from other groups with pedophilia is priests with a history of<br />
pedophilia. More specifically, this research attempted to identify those personality<br />
characteristics of priest pedophiles which have not yet been specifically <strong>in</strong>vestigated and<br />
which have been found to be associated with both the overall pedophile and priest<br />
populations, namely narcissism and overcontrolled-hostility.<br />
As Meloy (1986) described, priests seem to possess at least some degree of<br />
narcissism due largely to their position <strong>in</strong> the church and from the reverence they receive<br />
from society. As a result of the narcissism, they, theoretically, separate themselves from<br />
their emotions via splitt<strong>in</strong>g which, <strong>in</strong> turn, protects their grandiose self-image. This also<br />
seems to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> the sense of entitlement they feel toward others. As such, these priests<br />
would be classified more as 'balanced' narcissists as opposed to their pedophile counterpart.<br />
In other words, if priests, <strong>in</strong> general, have a tendency toward narcissism, it seems that priests<br />
who are pedophiles would exhibit an even greater degree of self-value and worth. Because
Narcissism and Pedophilia 42<br />
of splitt<strong>in</strong>g as a defense mechanism, they are able to distance themselves from the guilt<br />
associated with the sexual activity with the child. This allows them to protect their self-image<br />
and to validate their sense of entitlement <strong>in</strong> receiv<strong>in</strong>g immediate gratification. Further, they<br />
succumb to their own grandiose needs over the emotional needs of others, particularly<br />
children. Because pedophile priests, presumably, engage <strong>in</strong> splitt<strong>in</strong>g to a greater degree than<br />
priests <strong>in</strong> general (i.e., to justify their molest<strong>in</strong>g behavior), priest pedophiles should be even<br />
higher along the 'pathological' cont<strong>in</strong>uum of narcissism than their priest counterparts.<br />
It is noteworthy to po<strong>in</strong>t out that control pedophiles also exhibit this same sense of<br />
entitlement and emotional dissociation <strong>in</strong> their relationships with children (Abel et al., 1984;<br />
Socarides, 1991; Ward et al., 1995). However, these personality characteristics appear<br />
specific only with regard to sexual gratification. For pedophile priests, on the other hand, the<br />
attitude that they are worthy of admiration and their accentuated self-value appears to be more<br />
global <strong>in</strong> that it encompasses sexual, professional, and social doma<strong>in</strong>s of their lives. This,<br />
therefore, might account for a higher degree of narcissism among pedophile priests.<br />
Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, despite theoretical contentions regard<strong>in</strong>g the prevalence of narcissism <strong>in</strong><br />
priests and pedophile priests, Rob<strong>in</strong>son (1994) was unable to identify this as an outstand<strong>in</strong>g<br />
characteristic among pedophile, ephebophile, compulsive sexual disorder, or psychiatric<br />
priests. Aside from the fact that he used neither a non-psychiatric priest control group nor a<br />
control pedophile group to determ<strong>in</strong>e differences among these populations, it is possible that<br />
the measure used (MCMI-II Narcissism Scale) did not tap <strong>in</strong>to the relevant subconstructs of<br />
narcissism present <strong>in</strong> priests and pedophile priests. Specifically, W<strong>in</strong>k (1991) demonstrated<br />
that narcissism could be manifested along two dimensions: Vulnerability-Sensitivity and<br />
Grandiosity-Exhibitionism. This researcher exam<strong>in</strong>ed correlations among the Rask<strong>in</strong> and<br />
Novacek Narcissism Scale (RN: Rask<strong>in</strong> & Novacek, 1989), the Morey, Waugh, and<br />
Blashfield Narcissism Scale (MWB: Morey, Waugh, & Blashfield, 1985), the W<strong>in</strong>k and
Narcissism and Pedophilia 43<br />
Gough Narcissism Scale (WG: W<strong>in</strong>k & Gough, 1990), the Narcissistic Personality Disorder<br />
Scale (NPDS: Ashby, Lee, & Duke, 1979), the Ego-Sensitivity Scale (E-S: Pepper &<br />
Strong, 1958), and the Narcissism-Hypersensitivity Scale (NH: Serkownek, 1975) for 350<br />
male and female participants from the Institute of Personality Assessment and Research.<br />
Utiliz<strong>in</strong>g a pr<strong>in</strong>cipal components analyses with varimax rotation, the latter three scales; NPD,<br />
E-S, and NH loaded on the first factor, Vulnerability-Sensitivity, with respective correlations<br />
of .78, .89, and .78, whereas the RN, MWB, and the WG loaded on the second factor,<br />
Grandiosity-Sensitivity, with respective correlations of .92, .88, and .83.<br />
Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the results of his study, W<strong>in</strong>k (1991) was able to characterize the<br />
vulnerability-sensitivity and grandiosity-exhibitionism factors based on similarities of covert<br />
and overt narcissists as described by Kernberg's (1975) and Kohut's (1971) classifications.<br />
Specifically, W<strong>in</strong>k (1991) reported that "like the covert narcissists, high scorers on<br />
Vulnerability-Sensitivity appeared to be defensive, hypersensitive, anxious, and socially<br />
reticent <strong>in</strong>dividuals whose personal relations, however, were marked by self-<strong>in</strong>dulgence,<br />
conceit and arrogance, and an <strong>in</strong>sistence on hav<strong>in</strong>g their own way" (p. 596). W<strong>in</strong>k further<br />
dist<strong>in</strong>guished the vulnerability-sensitivity narcissist as pessimistic, lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> fulfillment, and<br />
vulnerable to distress. Conversely, "high scorers on Grandiosity-Exhibitionism, similar to<br />
overt narcissism, showed a consistent behavioral pattern of self-assuredness,<br />
aggressiveness, exhibitionism, self-<strong>in</strong>dulgence, and disrespect for the needs of others" (p.<br />
596). This type of narcissist also was described by W<strong>in</strong>k as extroverted, aggressive, self-<br />
confident, and need<strong>in</strong>g of admiration.<br />
In light of these typologies of narcissism, this study attempted to demonstrate that<br />
pedophiles, priests, and pedophile priests will differ along these dimensions. More<br />
specifically, due to the fact that pedophiles have been found to be more passive, dependent,<br />
self-conscious, and socially <strong>in</strong>troverted (Erickson et al., 1987; Kalichman, 1991), it was
Narcissism and Pedophilia 44<br />
hypothesized that they would tend to exhibit characteristics typical of vulnerability-<br />
sensitivity. Conversely, priests who evidence more social eptness, self-confidence, and<br />
feel<strong>in</strong>gs of omnipotence and entitlement (Kennedy et al., 1977; Meloy, 1988) should be best<br />
described as hav<strong>in</strong>g grandiosity-exhibitionism traits. Pedophile priests who demonstrate<br />
both types of these traits present an even more complex picture. Despite the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that<br />
pedophile priests are prone to passivity, unassertiveness, and <strong>in</strong>security (Plante et al.,<br />
1996b), it was assumed that the grandiosity and <strong>in</strong>flated self-perception (Meloy, 1988)<br />
would constitute more outstand<strong>in</strong>g traits. Thus, pedophile priests would be more apt to have<br />
a grandiosity-exhibitionism personality style, even when compared to their nonpedophile<br />
priest counterparts. In terms of overall differences, it was predicted that pedophile priests<br />
would demonstrate more narcissistic qualities than control pedophiles and nonpedophile<br />
priests.<br />
Narcissism is one variable that may be common among pedophile priests. The<br />
literature also suggests that overcontrolled-hostility may be a risk factor <strong>in</strong> this population.<br />
First, priests, <strong>in</strong> general, appear to be vulnerable to overcontrolled-hostility (Keddy et al.,<br />
1990). Keddy et al. (1990) argue that overcontrolled-hostility results from the tendency to<br />
<strong>in</strong>tellectualize their problems and to avoid the emotional representations and, presumably, the<br />
process<strong>in</strong>g, of their distress. Theoretically, pedophiles also should have elevated levels of<br />
overcontrolled-hostility. Their over-utilization of cognitive distortions to m<strong>in</strong>imize and avoid<br />
the emotional consequences of their molest<strong>in</strong>g behavior probably puts them at risk for this<br />
elevated pattern.<br />
Given the predicted existence of overcontrolled-hostility <strong>in</strong> both priests and<br />
pedophiles, it also might be predicted that priests with pedophilia will have even higher levels<br />
of overcontrolled-hostility than either of the former two populations. Indeed, Plante et al.<br />
(1996b) suggested this when they said that sexually abus<strong>in</strong>g priests rely on <strong>in</strong>tellectualization
Narcissism and Pedophilia 45<br />
and denial processes to assist them <strong>in</strong> protect<strong>in</strong>g their sense of stability and their religious<br />
reputation. Thus, the eventual act<strong>in</strong>g out behavior associated with overcontrolled-hostility is<br />
directed toward a child who poses no apparent threat to this desired image. Thus, for the<br />
priests with pedophilia, overcontrolled-hostility may be either a precipitant and/or result of<br />
the molest<strong>in</strong>g behavior. In either case, it was predicted that priests with pedophilia would<br />
demonstrate higher levels of overcontrolled-hostility than either priests or pedophiles.<br />
Although narcissism and overcontrolled hostility have been discussed as separate<br />
qualities of priests and pedophile priests, it is possible that these two variables are related.<br />
As Meloy (1988) theorized, narcissistic priests are unable to express or experience genu<strong>in</strong>e<br />
emotions due to their reliance on splitt<strong>in</strong>g (i.e., which preserves the <strong>in</strong>flated self-image). As<br />
a result, it may be difficult for them to recognize feel<strong>in</strong>gs of hostility prior to an aggressive<br />
act. Theoretical and empirical evidence has demonstrated that this narcissistic strategy<br />
<strong>in</strong>fluences an ignorance of emotions. As has been expla<strong>in</strong>ed, overcontrolled-hostility also<br />
<strong>in</strong>volves avoidance of emotional displays. The similarity of these processes provides support<br />
for the correlation between these traits. Further, Hoppe and S<strong>in</strong>ger (1976) found<br />
overcontrolled hostility to be related to egocentric balance, more specifically, the excessive<br />
self-focus that is a hallmark trait of narcissism.<br />
In light of the above discussion, levels of narcissism, overcontrolled-hostility, or a<br />
comb<strong>in</strong>ation of these traits, may present as risk factors for priests who are pedophiles. Of<br />
course, not all priests who exhibit narcissism and overcontrolled-hostility are pedophiles.<br />
However, priests that do possess these characteristics to a greater degree than others might<br />
warrant some concern as to their activities outside their religious duties. The rationale for this<br />
personality type identification is to recognize that pedophiles do, <strong>in</strong> fact, exist <strong>in</strong> the<br />
priesthood and that there may be variables specific to priest pedophiles that may help further<br />
account for their deviant sexuality. If this were to be the case, then proper screen<strong>in</strong>g
Narcissism and Pedophilia 46<br />
measures could be <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to sem<strong>in</strong>ary tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g so as to identify priests at potential<br />
risk for molest<strong>in</strong>g children.<br />
Statement of the Hypotheses<br />
After review<strong>in</strong>g the literature, the follow<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses were generated regard<strong>in</strong>g<br />
personality characteristics of control priests, control pedophiles, priest molesters (priest<br />
pedophiles and priest ephebophiles), and psychiatric priests (see Table 2):<br />
1. There will be a significant relationship between group membership (control<br />
priest, control pedophile, priest molester, and psychiatric priest) and degree of<br />
Grandiosity-Exhibitionism (GE) narcissism as assessed by mean raw scores on the<br />
Morey, Waugh, and Blashfield Narcissism Scale (MWB), Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek<br />
Narcissism Scale (RN), and the W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough Narcissism Scale (WG).<br />
Specifically:<br />
a. Priest molesters will exhibit a significantly greater level of grandiosity-<br />
exhibitionism than control pedophiles, control priests, and psychiatric<br />
priests.<br />
b. Control priests and psychiatric priests will demonstrate significantly<br />
greater levels of grandiosity-exhibitionism than control pedophiles.<br />
2. There will be a significant relationship between group membership (control<br />
pedophile, control priest, priest molester, and psychiatric priest) and degree of<br />
Vulnerability-Sensitivity (VS) narcissism as assessed by mean raw scores on the<br />
Narcissism-Hypersensitivity Scale (NH) and the Narcissistic Personality Disorder<br />
Scale (NPD). Specifically, control pedophiles will exhibit a significantly greater<br />
level of vulnerability-sensitivity than control priests, priest molesters, and<br />
psychiatric priests.<br />
3. There will be a significant relationship between group membership
Table 2<br />
Hypothesized Group Differences<br />
Variable<br />
1. Grandiosity-Exhibitionism<br />
a. Morey, Waugh,<br />
& Blashfield (MWB)<br />
b. Rask<strong>in</strong> & Novacek (RN)<br />
c. W<strong>in</strong>k & Gough (WG)<br />
2. Vulnerability-Sensitivity<br />
a. Narcissism-<br />
Hypersensitivity (NH)<br />
b. Narcissistic Personality<br />
Disorder (NPD)<br />
3. Overcontrolled Hostility<br />
a. MMPI-2 O-H Scale<br />
Note. Mean scores = a > b > c.<br />
Control<br />
Pedophile Control<br />
Priest<br />
c<br />
a<br />
b<br />
b<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia 47<br />
Group<br />
Priest<br />
Pedophile<br />
a<br />
b<br />
Priest<br />
Ephebophile<br />
b b a a<br />
a<br />
b<br />
Psychiatric<br />
Priest<br />
b<br />
b<br />
b
Narcissism and Pedophilia 48<br />
(control pedophile, control priest, priest molester, and psychiatric priest) and level<br />
of overcontrolled-hostility as assessed by mean t scores on the MMPI-2<br />
Overcontrolled-Hostility Scale (OH). Specifically, priest molesters will reveal a<br />
significantly higher level of overcontrolled-hostility than control pedophiles, control<br />
priests, and psychiatric priests.
Participants<br />
CHAPTER II<br />
Method<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia 49<br />
Experimental Groups. There were five groups of participants <strong>in</strong> this study: 1) priest<br />
pedophiles, 2) priest ephebophiles, 3) psychiatric priests, 4) control priests, and 5) control<br />
pedophiles. The first three groups were composed of 110 priests (30 pedophile, 40<br />
ephebophile, and 40 psychiatric) who were referred to a Catholic <strong>in</strong>patient psychiatric<br />
facility <strong>in</strong> <strong>Maryland</strong> for a psychological evaluation. Based on <strong>in</strong>formation gathered from<br />
psychiatric, psychological, neuropsychological, psychosocial, spiritual, and medical<br />
assessment measures as well as cl<strong>in</strong>ical <strong>in</strong>terviews, <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the first two groups were<br />
diagnosed with Pedophilia and Paraphilia Not Otherwise Specified-Ephebophilia,<br />
respectively. None of the participants <strong>in</strong> these two groups had a dual diagnosis. The<br />
psychiatric priest group presented with V-code diagnoses as well as other psychological<br />
disorders such as Occupational Problems, Other Interpersonal Problem, Adjustment<br />
Disorder with Depressed Mood, Adjustment Disorder with Anxious Mood, or No<br />
Diagnosis on Axis I. These <strong>in</strong>dividuals were not diagnosed with a sexual disorder or with<br />
alcohol or substance abuse/dependence. All diagnoses for these three groups were based<br />
on DSM-III-R or DSM-IV criteria depend<strong>in</strong>g on the date the evaluation was conducted.<br />
Control Groups. The control priest group consisted of 30 active priests of the<br />
Archdiocese <strong>in</strong> Baltimore, <strong>Maryland</strong>, who were not receiv<strong>in</strong>g any known psychological<br />
treatment for a paraphilia at the time of their participation. For these control priests, the<br />
mean number of years s<strong>in</strong>ce ord<strong>in</strong>ation was 23.45 (SD = 13.39, n = 30). In addition, type<br />
of sem<strong>in</strong>ary tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g for these participants was as follows: 11 religious order, 18 diocesan,<br />
and 1 religious order and diocesan.
Narcissism and Pedophilia 50<br />
The control pedophile group was composed of 28 <strong>in</strong>dividuals who met the DSM-IV<br />
criteria for Pedophilia. They also met the criteria for fixated pedophilia (i.e., sexual<br />
preference for children) as outl<strong>in</strong>ed by Groth and Birnbaum (1978). The twenty-eight<br />
participants were actively attend<strong>in</strong>g group psychotherapy sessions. Frequency of<br />
attendance ranged from weekly to one session every 3 months. Fifteen of the control<br />
pedophile participants were obta<strong>in</strong>ed at a national center for treatment of sexual disorders <strong>in</strong><br />
<strong>Maryland</strong>. Two of those participants were verbally adm<strong>in</strong>istered the MMPI-2 by the<br />
<strong>in</strong>vestigator due to illiteracy and bl<strong>in</strong>dness. Two participants, despite their diagnosis, had<br />
not directly acted upon their sexual impulses, although their fantasies and behaviors were<br />
associated with sexual <strong>in</strong>tentions with a child (i.e., <strong>in</strong>ternet child pornography). All<br />
diagnoses were obta<strong>in</strong>ed through review of medical records by the exam<strong>in</strong>er.<br />
Thirteen control pedophile participants were obta<strong>in</strong>ed at an outpatient sexual<br />
disorders cl<strong>in</strong>ic <strong>in</strong> Pennsylvania. Two of the control pedophile participants did not<br />
complete the demographic <strong>in</strong>formation sheet; however, one of those participants <strong>in</strong>cluded<br />
his age while the other did not. In order for the test material to be analyzed, a mean age of<br />
all control pedophile participants was computed and utilized for that particular protocol.<br />
Those two protocols, however, could not be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> any statistical analyses of<br />
descriptive data other than those <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g age.<br />
All participants were treated <strong>in</strong> accordance with the "Ethical Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of<br />
Psychologists and Code of Conduct" (APA, 1992) as well as with<strong>in</strong> the provisions<br />
established by <strong>Loyola</strong> <strong>College</strong>'s Human Subjects Review Board.<br />
Materials<br />
Demographic Variables. Demographic variables for the control pedophiles<br />
<strong>in</strong>cluded: age, race, education, occupation, number of victims, gender of victim (s), age of<br />
onset of sexual disorder, length of <strong>in</strong>carceration for any crime committed as a result of their
Narcissism and Pedophilia 51<br />
sexual disorder, and prior psychiatric history/treatment for their sexual disorder or other<br />
psychological problems (Appendix B). The follow<strong>in</strong>g demographic <strong>in</strong>formation for the<br />
control priests was collected: age, race, education, type of sem<strong>in</strong>ary tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g (i.e., religious<br />
order or diocesan), number of years s<strong>in</strong>ce ord<strong>in</strong>ation, and prior psychiatric<br />
history/treatment (Appendix C).<br />
M<strong>in</strong>nesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2. The MMPI was developed <strong>in</strong> 1943<br />
by Hathaway and McK<strong>in</strong>ley and was a widely used self-report measure of personality until<br />
its revision <strong>in</strong> 1989 (Graham, 1993). The MMPI-2 was constructed to <strong>in</strong>clude a much<br />
larger and more heterogeneous normative sample, to reconstruct the orig<strong>in</strong>al components of<br />
the MMPI scales, and to <strong>in</strong>clude additional content scales to measure personality features<br />
(Mann et al., 1992). The MMPI-2 conta<strong>in</strong>s 567 items <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g 3 validity scales: Lie (L),<br />
Infrequency (F), and Correction (K); and 10 cl<strong>in</strong>ical scales: 1) Hypochondriasis (Hs), 2)<br />
Depression (D), 3) Hysteria (Hy), 4) Psychopathic Deviate (Pd), 5) Paranoia (Pa), 6)<br />
Mascul<strong>in</strong>ity-Fem<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ity (Mf), 7) Psychasthenia (Pt), 8) Schizophrenia (Sc), 9) Mania (Ma),<br />
and 0) Social Introversion (Si). This measure also <strong>in</strong>cludes supplementary scales such as<br />
Anxiety (A), Repression (R), Ego Strength (Es), MacAndrews Alcoholism Scale, Revised<br />
(MAC-R), Overcontrolled Hostility (O-H), Dom<strong>in</strong>ance (Do), Social Responsibility (Re),<br />
<strong>College</strong> Maladjustment (Mt), Gender Role-Mascul<strong>in</strong>e (GM), Gender Role-Fem<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>e (GF),<br />
Post Traumatic Stress Disorder-Keane (PK), and Post Traumatic Stress Disorder-<br />
Schlenger (PS). In addition, there are 15 content scales which <strong>in</strong>clude Anxiety (ANX),<br />
Fears (FRS), Obsessiveness (OBS), Depression (DEP), Health Concerns (HEA), Bizarre<br />
Mentation (BIZ), Anger (ANG), Cynicism (CYN), Antisocial Practices (ASP), Type A<br />
(TPA), Low Self-Esteem (LSE), Social Discomfort (SOD), Family Problems (FAM),<br />
Work Interference (WRK), and Negative Treatment Indicators (TRT). Raw scores based<br />
upon true-false responses are converted <strong>in</strong>to T scores with a mean of 50 and a standard
Narcissism and Pedophilia 52<br />
deviation of 10. T scores ≥ 65 on the MMPI-2 cl<strong>in</strong>ical scales are usually <strong>in</strong>dicative of<br />
significant elevations (Greene, 1991) and will be considered as such <strong>in</strong> this study.<br />
The MMPI-2 has demonstrated average test-retest reliability over a one-week period<br />
of .79 for normal men and women. For normal males, test-retest coefficients have ranged<br />
from .72 to .89 on the <strong>in</strong>dividual cl<strong>in</strong>ical scales (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, &<br />
Kaemmer, 1989). Internal consistency reliability estimates for normal males have been<br />
found to range from .34 to .85 on the <strong>in</strong>dividual cl<strong>in</strong>ical scales (Butcher et al., 1989).<br />
Furthermore, <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency for psychiatric <strong>in</strong>patients has ranged from .60 to .92<br />
(Boone, 1994).<br />
The MMPI-2 is considered a valid measure such that raw scores for both normal<br />
and psychiatric subjects have been found to be strongly correlated with the orig<strong>in</strong>al version<br />
of the MMPI (r > .98; Graham, 1988). In terms of 2-po<strong>in</strong>t scale configuration, average<br />
agreement between the MMPI and the MMPI-2 have been found to be 86% for normal<br />
males and 76% for psychiatric males (Graham, Timbrook, Ben-Porath, & Butcher, 1991).<br />
Criterion-related validity as <strong>in</strong>dicated by correlations with counterpart scales on the SCL-<br />
90-R was found to be somewhat weak, however, with the highest correlation be<strong>in</strong>g .52 on<br />
the Somatization counterpart scales (Graham et al., 1991). The correlation between the<br />
Depression scale on the MMPI-2 and its SCL-90-R counterpart has been found to be only<br />
.16 and that of the Schizophrenia counterparts was .11 (Graham et al., 1991). Overall,<br />
psychometric data on the MMPI-2 is fairly limited (Graham, 1993), especially with regard<br />
to the target populations of this study. It is of importance to note that the weaknesses <strong>in</strong><br />
these psychometric properties may be due to the fact that the MMPI-2 and the SCL-90-R<br />
may not address those psychological constructs <strong>in</strong> a similar manner. Furthermore, the<br />
overwhelm<strong>in</strong>g popularity of this <strong>in</strong>strument with cl<strong>in</strong>icians world-wide provides evidence<br />
for its utility.
Narcissism and Pedophilia 53<br />
Because overcontrolled-hostility is a focus of this study, it is relevant to describe<br />
the MMPI-2 O-H scale. It was developed by Megargee, Cook, and Mendelsohn (1967)<br />
who contrasted item responses of extremely assaultive prisoners, moderately assaultive<br />
prisoners, nonassaultive prisoners, and normals (Greene, 1991). This scale consists of 31<br />
items and, although Megargee et al. did not specify a cut-off po<strong>in</strong>t, high scorers tend to<br />
evidence extreme control of hostile impulses and typically are socially <strong>in</strong>troverted, reluctant<br />
to admit to any psychological difficulties, and unable to express feel<strong>in</strong>gs of anxiety.<br />
Morey, Waugh, and Blashfield Narcissism Scale (MWB). The 22-item MMPI-<br />
derived MWB scale (Morey et al., 1985) was <strong>in</strong>itially rationally constructed accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />
MMPI item responses chosen by cl<strong>in</strong>ical psychologists as typify<strong>in</strong>g narcissism. Empirical<br />
validation was performed with t-test analyses for high and low scorers on particular items.<br />
There were seven items dropped from the Narcissism scale (NAR) due to failure to meet<br />
the .05 criteria. Psychometric analyses of responses from 475 psychiatric patients revealed<br />
an <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency of .78 (KR-20); however, this statistic was slightly lower (KR-20 =<br />
.71) when overlapp<strong>in</strong>g items were elim<strong>in</strong>ated. There was a significant effect for gender for<br />
the full MWB scale where males (M = 15.85, SD = 4.78) scored higher than females (M =<br />
14.10, SD = 5.14). As was found with other MMPI-derived Narcissism scales, the MWB<br />
scale had a correlation with the MA cl<strong>in</strong>ical scale of .44.<br />
Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek Narcissism Scale (RN). The MMPI-derived RN scale<br />
consists of 42 items. Based on a correlational analysis of the MMPI and the Narcissistic<br />
Personality Inventory (NPI; Rask<strong>in</strong> & Hall, 1979) scores for undergraduates, Rask<strong>in</strong> and<br />
Novacek (1989) found that the NPI was positively correlated with the Mania scale (r = .29,<br />
p < .01) and negatively correlated with the Depression (r = -.36, p < .01), Psychasthenia (r<br />
= -.34, p < .01), Social Introversion (r = -.40, p
Narcissism and Pedophilia 54<br />
analyses correlated 42 MMPI items with the 40-item full scale of the NPI (r = .79). Those<br />
42 items reflected an alpha <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency coefficient of .65, suggest<strong>in</strong>g homogeneity<br />
among items. In fact, 64% of both the men and women who were identified as highly<br />
narcissistic based on the NPI 90th percentile cutoff po<strong>in</strong>t had the Ma scale as their highest<br />
scale elevation. Furthermore, 55% and 36% of the male and female group cases,<br />
respectively, had the Sc scale as the first or second highest score. Typically, a 9/8-8/9<br />
profile exemplifies <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are grandiose, self-centered, demand attention from<br />
others, and are prone to hostility and resentment toward others (Graham, 1988).<br />
In terms of NPI first order component scales, the NPI Entitlement scale was<br />
positively correlated with the MA (r = .29, p < .001), Pd (r = .19, p < .001) and F (r =<br />
.25, p < .01) scales with the F scale also be<strong>in</strong>g correlated with the Exploitativeness scale (r<br />
= .15, p < .05). Both the Exploitative and Authority scales were positively correlated with<br />
the Ma scale (r = .39, p < .001; r = . 15, p < .05, respectively). Most of these correlations<br />
appear to be related to MMPI imperturbability, self-aggrandizement, and aggressive<br />
content. Furthermore, the significant relationships between the NPI and the MMPI Ma and<br />
Pd content scales were specific to ego <strong>in</strong>flation, problems with authority, manifest hostility,<br />
psychoticism, and amorality.<br />
W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough Narcissism Scale (WG). This 39-item scale was rationally<br />
derived from those MMPI items which most reflected narcissism doma<strong>in</strong>s of exaggerated<br />
self-esteem, devaluation of others, feel<strong>in</strong>gs of entitlement, and dissatisfaction with current<br />
status (W<strong>in</strong>k & Gough, 1990). These items were correlated with seem<strong>in</strong>gly<br />
complementary items from the California Personality Inventory (CPI; item sets with a<br />
correlation below .20 were elim<strong>in</strong>ated). Thirteen analyses were conducted until the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />
39-item scale was developed.
Narcissism and Pedophilia 55<br />
Both MMPI and CPI Narcissism scales were adm<strong>in</strong>istered to 350 male and female<br />
community residents and 198 males and female undergraduates. Results revealed that<br />
college students (M = 20.21, SD = 5.02) scored significantly higher on both of these scales<br />
than the adults (M = 18.90, SD = 5.02). Both adult (M = 20.50, SD = 4.88) and<br />
undergraduate males (M = 21.03, SD = 5.16) scored significantly higher on the MMPI<br />
Narcissism Scale than did their respective female counterparts (M = 17.30, SD = 5.16; M =<br />
19.39, SD = 4.88). Alpha <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency was .72 for this scale. Correlation between<br />
full scale CPI and MMPI Narcissism scales was .81; however, this decreased to .71 upon<br />
elim<strong>in</strong>ation of 14 overlapp<strong>in</strong>g items for each scale. This scale was most highly correlated<br />
with the MA scale (r = .57, p < .01) suggest<strong>in</strong>g that items tap <strong>in</strong>to characteristics of high<br />
energy level, expressive emotionality, and impatience.<br />
Narcissism-Hypersensitivity Scale (NH). Serkownek (1975) developed this<br />
MMPI-derived 18-item scale on a factor analysis of MMPI Mascul<strong>in</strong>ity-Fem<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ity scale<br />
scores of 422 psychiatric <strong>in</strong>patients, outpatients, and nonpatients performed by Graham,<br />
Schroeder, and Lilly (1971). Mull<strong>in</strong>s and Kopelman (1988) revealed moderate correlations<br />
between the Margolis-Thomas Measure of Narcissism (MT; Margolis, & Thomas, 1980),<br />
the Narcissistic Personality Disorder Scale (NPD), and the Narcissism-Hypersensitivity<br />
Scale (NH; rs = .42, .40, and .44, respectively). In addition, they found the NH scale to<br />
be positively correlated with Machiavellianism (r = .44, p < .001), operationally def<strong>in</strong>ed as<br />
the manipulation of others for personal ga<strong>in</strong>, and Need for Autonomy (r = .18, p < .01),<br />
described as one's need for <strong>in</strong>dependence and self-direction, as assessed by the 20-item<br />
Mach IV (Christie & Geis, 1970) and the Manifest Needs Questionnaire (Steers &<br />
Braunste<strong>in</strong>, 1976), respectively. The NH scale also was found to be negatively correlated<br />
with Social Desirability Scale (r = -.38, p < .001) as assessed by the Marlowe-Crowne<br />
Social Desirability Scale (Reynolds, 1982). Internal consistency for the NH scale was .52
Narcissism and Pedophilia 56<br />
(KR-20). It must be noted that, because the MMPI Mf subscale attends to hypersensitivity,<br />
six items on the NH scale were elim<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>in</strong> this study based on their seem<strong>in</strong>g irrelevance<br />
to this construct.<br />
Narcissistic Personality Disorder Scale (NPD). The NPD scale (Ashby, Lee, &<br />
Duke, 1979) is a 19-item subscale of the MMPI derived by compar<strong>in</strong>g MMPI responses of<br />
20 patients diagnosed by psychoanalysts as narcissistic and controls consist<strong>in</strong>g of<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> psychotherapy and <strong>in</strong>dividuals not <strong>in</strong> treatment. Mean scores for these<br />
patients was 10.00 (SD = 3.80) whereas normals obta<strong>in</strong>ed mean scores of 3.66 (SD =<br />
1.10: Ashby, Lee, and Duke, 1979). In a study by Solomon (1982), the NPD scale was<br />
found to be negatively correlated with self-concept (r = -.61, p < .001) and <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong><br />
satisfy<strong>in</strong>g love relationship (r = -.42, p < .001) as assessed by the Tennessee Self-concept<br />
Scale (Fitts, 1965) and a Profile Questionnaire compris<strong>in</strong>g demographic <strong>in</strong>formation which<br />
was designed by the author, respectively.<br />
Mull<strong>in</strong>s and Kopelman (1988) elim<strong>in</strong>ated seven NPD items <strong>in</strong> their study that<br />
overlapped with the MMPI Sc scale as well as two more items found to be "archaic <strong>in</strong><br />
content." Based on their n<strong>in</strong>e-item version of the NPD scale, <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency for this<br />
subscale was fairly low with a reliability estimate of .42 (KR-20). It was predicted that,<br />
for all subscales <strong>in</strong> their study, reliability would have been greater with <strong>in</strong>creased item<br />
number. The NPD scale was found to be moderately correlated with the MT and the NH<br />
scale. It was positively correlated with Machiavellianism (r = .38, p < .001) which was<br />
operationally def<strong>in</strong>ed as the manipulation of others for personal ga<strong>in</strong> as well as Need for<br />
Autonomy (r = .18, p < .01) which was described as one's need for <strong>in</strong>dependence and<br />
self-direction. The NPD scale was negatively correlated, however, with Social Desirability<br />
(r = -.29, p < .001), described as a tendency for an <strong>in</strong>dividual to present himself/herself <strong>in</strong>
Narcissism and Pedophilia 57<br />
a more favorable light. This was assessed via the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability<br />
Scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964).<br />
F<strong>in</strong>k (1991) revealed the NPD scale to be positively correlated with the NH scale (r<br />
= .50, p < .01), the Ego-Sensitivity Scale (r = .45, p < .01), and the WG scale (r = .17, p<br />
< .01). On the other hand, it was negatively correlated with the RN scale (r = -.18, p <<br />
.01) and the MWB scale (r = -.26, p < .01).<br />
MMPI-2 counterpart items of the MMPI were utilized for analysis and those items<br />
that did not correspond with the relative mean<strong>in</strong>g of the orig<strong>in</strong>al item were excluded from<br />
the scale. A list of correspond<strong>in</strong>g MMPI and MMPI-2 item numbers from each narcissism<br />
scale and those items excluded are found <strong>in</strong> Appendixes D through H. Actual item<br />
statements for the excluded items Grandiosity-Exhibitionism and Vulnerability-Sensitivity<br />
scales are listed <strong>in</strong> Appendixes I and J, respectively. Item statements which were rephrased<br />
from the orig<strong>in</strong>al MMPI item statement were still <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the respective narcissism scale<br />
and are <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>in</strong> Appendix K.<br />
In order to construct the narcissism scales, each item conta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the specific scale<br />
was extracted from the MMPI-2 protocol and comb<strong>in</strong>ed with the other scale items <strong>in</strong> order<br />
to establish the completed scale. All narcissism scales are provided <strong>in</strong> Appendixes L<br />
through P.<br />
Procedure<br />
MMPI-2 data for the priest pedophile, priest ephebophile, and psychiatric priest<br />
groups were collected from the Catholic psychiatric facility's computer database. Thirty<br />
protocols for the priest pedophile group, 40 protocols for the ephebophile group, and 40<br />
protocols for the psychiatric priest group were utilized for statistical analyses. T-scores for<br />
the MMPI-2 validity scales (L, F, and K) for each of these groups were obta<strong>in</strong>ed to ensure<br />
<strong>in</strong>clusion of valid protocols. In addition, MMPI-2 T scores for the O-H scale as well as
Narcissism and Pedophilia 58<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividual item responses for all protocols were obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Item responses <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> each<br />
particular MMPI-2 derived narcissism scale were extracted accord<strong>in</strong>g to the guidel<strong>in</strong>es of<br />
each scale. Age and education for each protocol also was used for analysis of descriptive<br />
data.<br />
In order to preserve confidentiality, all above-mentioned data were provided by a<br />
staff member of the psychiatric facility. Patient profiles <strong>in</strong>itially were referred by their<br />
hospital number; however, once the f<strong>in</strong>al list of protocols was completed, protocols were<br />
randomly assigned a new case code accord<strong>in</strong>g to group membership.<br />
Participants for the control priest group were solicited on a voluntary basis as listed<br />
<strong>in</strong> the telephone directory. They were <strong>in</strong>formed that the purpose of the research project was<br />
to obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation relevant to the treatment of priests with psychological difficulties.<br />
Data for the control pedophiles were collected through two outpatient sexual<br />
disorders treatment facilities <strong>in</strong> <strong>Maryland</strong> and Pennsylvania. One protocol was collected<br />
from a private cl<strong>in</strong>ician <strong>in</strong> <strong>Maryland</strong>. All procedures followed regulations of each particular<br />
facility. Control pedophile participants were solicited on a voluntary basis and were<br />
<strong>in</strong>formed that their participation would provide additional <strong>in</strong>formation as to the assessment<br />
and treatment of sexual disorders.<br />
Both the control pedophile and control priest participants were given two <strong>in</strong>formed<br />
consent documents to sign (see Appendixes Q and R), one copy the participant reta<strong>in</strong>ed for<br />
their records, the other copy was placed <strong>in</strong> an envelope separate from their test material for<br />
the record of the <strong>in</strong>vestigator. Control priest and control pedophile participants then<br />
completed a demographic <strong>in</strong>formation sheet followed by the MMPI-2. Collected protocols<br />
were only referred to by their randomly assigned case code <strong>in</strong> order to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><br />
confidentiality.
Design and Analyses<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia 59<br />
Group membership (control pedophile, control priest, priest molester, and<br />
psychiatric priest) was the predictor variable and degree of narcissism (grandiosity-<br />
exhibitionism and vulnerability-sensitivity measure) and overcontrolled-hostility were the<br />
criterion variables.<br />
Because groups were significantly different with regard to age and education, these<br />
variables were held constant for all statistical analyses. Therefore, a multivariate analysis<br />
of covariance (MANCOVA) and planned contrast t tests were performed to address<br />
hypotheses la, 1b, and 2. Degree of Grandiosity-Exhibitionism (GE) narcissism for each<br />
group was assessed by the Morey, Waugh, and Blashfield Narcissism Scale (MWB), the<br />
Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek Narcissism Scale (RN), and the W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough Narcissism Scale<br />
(WG: hypotheses 1a and 1b). Degree of Vulnerability-Sensitivity (VS) narcissism for each<br />
group was assessed by the Narcissism-Hypersensitivity Scale (NH) and the Narcissistic<br />
Personality Disorder Scale (NPD: hypothesis 2). A MANCOVA used the shared variance<br />
among the dependent variables (i.e., MMPI-2 narcissism scales) to create two composite<br />
variables. F<strong>in</strong>ally, an Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted to assess<br />
hypothesis 3. Group membership and level of overcontrolled-hostility was evaluated by<br />
the MMPI-2 O-H scale.<br />
Priest pedophiles and priest ephebophiles were not expected to differ significantly<br />
on any given measure. However, comparisons still were made via the above-mentioned<br />
statistical analyses. These groups were comb<strong>in</strong>ed and referred to as priest molesters for<br />
those statistical measures where no differences between groups were found.<br />
An alpha level of .05 was used to determ<strong>in</strong>e statistical significance for all statistical<br />
analyses. Statistical analyses used mean T scores for the MMPI-2 validity scales and the
Narcissism and Pedophilia 60<br />
MMPI-2 O-H scale, and used mean raw total scores for each of the MMPI-2 derived<br />
narcissism scales.
Prelim<strong>in</strong>ary Analyses<br />
CHAPTER III<br />
Results<br />
Narcissism and Pedophilia 61<br />
Because previous research (Rob<strong>in</strong>son, 1994) has not found significant differences<br />
between the priest pedophile and priest ephebophile groups, <strong>in</strong>dependent t-tests were<br />
performed between those two groups on age, education, and each of the MMPI-2 validity<br />
and cl<strong>in</strong>ical scales <strong>in</strong> order to determ<strong>in</strong>e whether they could be comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>to a s<strong>in</strong>gle priest<br />
molester group. Table 3 reveals the results of the <strong>in</strong>dependent t-test analyses. A significant<br />
difference was <strong>in</strong>dicated for age, t (49.49) = 2.31, p (two-tailed) = .03, d = .59, such that<br />
priest pedophiles (M = 56.73, SD = 11.70, n = 30) were significantly older than priest<br />
ephebophiles (M = 50.95, SD = 8.24, n = 40). An <strong>in</strong>dependent t-test also revealed a<br />
significant relationship between group and grandiosity-exhibitionism (MWB scale), t<br />
(64.25) = -2.09, p (two-tailed) = .04, d = -1.00, such that priest pedophiles (M = 13.27,<br />
SD = 3.12, n = 30) scored significantly lower on the MWB scale than priest ephebophiles<br />
(M = 14.87, SD = 3.28, n = 40). Therefore, for the rema<strong>in</strong>der of the analyses, all<br />
statistical analyses <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the variables of age and the MWB scale compared the orig<strong>in</strong>al<br />
5 groups (i.e., control pedophiles, control priests, priest pedophiles, priest ephebophiles,<br />
and psychiatric priests); however, all other statistical analyses comb<strong>in</strong>ed priest pedophiles<br />
and priest ephebophiles <strong>in</strong>to a priest molester group.<br />
Demographic Data<br />
Control pedophiles.<br />
Demographic data for the control pedophile group was collected via each<br />
participants' voluntary disclosure of <strong>in</strong>formation on the demographics form which was<br />
provided with the research materials. All miss<strong>in</strong>g data from participants at the sexual<br />
disorders cl<strong>in</strong>ic <strong>in</strong> <strong>Maryland</strong> were obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> their medical records. However, charts of
Table 3<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 62<br />
Summary of Independent t-Tests between Priest Pedophiles and Priest Ephebophiles<br />
Variable<br />
Age<br />
t<br />
5.72<br />
df<br />
49.49*<br />
t prob. (two-tailed)<br />
Education -.41 68 .69<br />
MMPI-2 L Scale -.91 68 .37<br />
MMPI-2 F Scale 1.26 68 .21<br />
MMPI-2 K Scale -1.33 68 .19<br />
MMPI-2 Overcontrolled-<br />
Hostility Scale<br />
Morey, Waugh, and<br />
Blashfield Scale<br />
-.24<br />
-2.09<br />
68<br />
64.25*<br />
Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek Scale -1.11 68 .27<br />
W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough Scale<br />
Narcissism-Hypersensitivity<br />
Scale<br />
Narcissistic Personality<br />
Disorder Scale<br />
.03<br />
.81<br />
.04<br />
.43 68 .67<br />
-.72<br />
1.73<br />
Note. * <strong>in</strong>dicates degrees of freedom represent<strong>in</strong>g unequal group variance (p < .05).<br />
68<br />
68<br />
.48<br />
.09
Narcissism and pedophilia 63<br />
those participants from the facility <strong>in</strong> Pennsylvania were not available. Therefore, the<br />
miss<strong>in</strong>g data for those participants could not be retrieved.<br />
Table 4 describes demographic data collected for the control pedophile group. All<br />
control pedophile participants were male (n = 28) with a mean age of 46.00 (SD = 13.10, n<br />
= 28). Mean education level was 13.10 years (SD = 2.92, n = 25). Mean age of onset<br />
was 20.84 (SD = 12.70, n = 25). Occupation was categorized as follows: White Collar (n<br />
= 5), Blue Collar (n = 11), Disability (n = 4), Retired (n = 6). The majority of control<br />
pedophiles were Caucasian (n = 24), although two participants were African American.<br />
Mean number of victims was 13.28 (SD = 15.32, n = 25) with ten participants preferr<strong>in</strong>g<br />
male victims, six participants preferr<strong>in</strong>g female victims, and n<strong>in</strong>e participants preferr<strong>in</strong>g<br />
both male and female victims. Sixteen participants (n = 26) were previously <strong>in</strong>carcerated<br />
for child molestation with a mean length of <strong>in</strong>carceration of 4.00 years (SD = 4.25, n =<br />
16). Mean length of current group treatment was 4.30 years (SD = 5.03, n = 26) and 16 of<br />
26 participants received previous psychiatric treatment for their sexual disorder and/or other<br />
psychological problems. Seven participants met the criteria for a dual diagnosis. Eight of<br />
13 participants were diagnosed with a personality disorder.<br />
Control priests.<br />
Table 5 describes demographic data for the control priest group. Participants' mean<br />
age was 51.87 (SD = 11.66, n = 30) and mean number of years of education was 18.83<br />
(SD = 1.63, n = 29). Eleven participants received religious order tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, eighteen<br />
received diocesan tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, and one participant received both. Mean number of years s<strong>in</strong>ce<br />
ord<strong>in</strong>ation was 23.45 (SD = 13.39, n = 30). Twenty-eight participants were Caucasian<br />
and two were Asian. Approximately fifty percent of control priests (n = 14) received<br />
psychiatric treatment prior to their participation.
Table 4<br />
Demographic Data for Control Pedophiles<br />
Variable<br />
Age<br />
Years of Education<br />
Race<br />
Caucasian<br />
African American<br />
Occupation<br />
White Collar<br />
Blue Collar<br />
Disability<br />
Retired<br />
Age of Onset<br />
Victim Number<br />
Gender Preference<br />
Male<br />
Female<br />
Both<br />
Length of Incarceration<br />
Previous psychiatric treatment<br />
Yes<br />
No<br />
Length of Treatment<br />
Dual Diagnosis<br />
Yes<br />
No<br />
Personality Disorder<br />
Yes<br />
No<br />
n<br />
28<br />
28<br />
24 2<br />
5<br />
11<br />
4<br />
6<br />
25<br />
25<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 64<br />
M<br />
46.00<br />
13.10<br />
20.84<br />
13.28<br />
SD<br />
13.10<br />
2.92<br />
12.70<br />
15.32<br />
10<br />
6<br />
9<br />
15 4.00 4.25<br />
16<br />
10<br />
26 4.30 5.03<br />
7<br />
6<br />
8<br />
5
Table 5<br />
Demographic Data for Control Priests<br />
Variable<br />
Age<br />
n<br />
30<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 65<br />
M<br />
51.87<br />
SD<br />
11.66<br />
Years of Education 30 18.83 1.63<br />
Race<br />
Caucasian<br />
Asian<br />
Type of Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />
Diocesan<br />
Religious Order<br />
Both<br />
Previous Psychiatric Treatment<br />
Yes<br />
No<br />
28<br />
2<br />
18<br />
11<br />
1<br />
14<br />
16
Narcissism and pedophilia 66<br />
Priest pedophiles, priest ephebophiles, and psychiatric priests.<br />
Table 5 outl<strong>in</strong>es demographic data for the priest pedophile, priest ephebophile, and<br />
psychiatric groups. The mean age for the priest pedophile and priest ephebophile<br />
participants was 56.73 (SD = 11.70, n = 30) and 50.95 (SD = 8.24, n = 40), respectively.<br />
For years of education, priest pedophiles averaged 19.37 (SD = 2.1, n = 30) and priest<br />
ephebophiles averaged 19.58 (SD = 2.17, n = 40). For the psychiatric priests, average age<br />
was 43.23 (SD = 10.13) and mean years of education was 19.42 (SD = 2.51). All of these<br />
priest groups were Caucasian.<br />
Descriptive Data<br />
Comparison of age and education for all groups.<br />
Table 6 provides a summary of statistical analyses for age and level of education for<br />
all participant groups. A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference for age, F (4,<br />
163) = 7.94, p (two-tailed) = .00, η 2 = .16. Tukey HSD post-hoc analyses <strong>in</strong>dicated that<br />
control priests (M = 51.87, SD = 11.66, n = 30) were significantly older than psychiatric<br />
priests (M = 43.23, SD = 10.13, n = 40). Priest pedophiles (M = 56.73, SD = 11.70, n =<br />
30) were significantly older than control pedophiles (M = 46.00, SD = 13.10, n = 28) and<br />
psychiatric priests. F<strong>in</strong>ally, priest ephebophiles (M = 50.95, SD = 8.24, n = 40) were<br />
significantly older than psychiatric priests. A one-way ANOVA also <strong>in</strong>dicated a significant<br />
difference for education, F (3, 160) = 53.00, p (two-tailed) = .00, η 2 = .50. Tukey HSD<br />
post-hoc analyses <strong>in</strong>dicated that control pedophiles (M = 13.10, SD = 2.92, n = 25) had<br />
significantly fewer years of education than control priests (M = 18.83, SD = 1.63, n = 29),<br />
priest molesters (M = 19.49, SD = 2.13, n = 70), and psychiatric priests (M = 19.42, SD<br />
= 2.51, n = 40). Specifically, control pedophiles tended to have one year of college level<br />
education while all priest groups tended to have five years of graduate level education.
Table 6<br />
Statistical Analyses for Age and Education by Group<br />
Control<br />
Pedophile<br />
Control<br />
Priest<br />
Group<br />
Priest<br />
Pedophile<br />
Age<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 67<br />
Priest<br />
Ephebophile<br />
Priest<br />
Molester<br />
Psychiatric<br />
Priest<br />
M 46.00 b 51.87 a 56.73 bc 50.95 d 53.43 43.23 acd *<br />
SD<br />
13.10<br />
11.66<br />
11.70<br />
Education<br />
8.24<br />
10.21<br />
10.13<br />
M 13.10 abc 18.83 a 19.37 19.58 19.49 b 19.42 c *<br />
SD<br />
2.92<br />
1.63<br />
2.11<br />
2.17<br />
2.13<br />
Note. * <strong>in</strong>dicates significant Univariate F, p < .05. Different superscripts <strong>in</strong>dicate<br />
significant group mean differences as revealed by Tukey post-hoc tests.<br />
2.51
Narcissism and pedophilia 68<br />
Because age and education were found to be significantly different among groups, these<br />
variables were held constant for all subsequent statistical analyses.<br />
Primary Analyses<br />
Statistical analyses for the MMPI-2 validity scales.<br />
A MANCOVA was conducted to determ<strong>in</strong>e whether there were differences between the<br />
groups on the MMPI Validity scales (L, F, and K). Table 7 provides a summary of<br />
these statistical analyses. The MANCOVA revealed a significant difference between the<br />
groups on the MMPI-2 validity scales, Wilks' Λ=.85, F (9, 377.38) = 2.93, p = .00.<br />
Univariate F tests <strong>in</strong>dicated a significant effect for only the MMPI-2 F scale, F (3, 157) =<br />
7.78, p (two-tailed) = .00, η 2 = .13. Tukey HSD post-hoc analyses revealed a significant<br />
difference between control pedophiles (M = 65.79, SD = 24.89, n = 28), control priests<br />
(M = 44.03, SD = 4.44, n = 30), priest molesters (M = 44.14, SD = 6.11, n = 70), and<br />
psychiatric priests (M = 43.60, SD = 6.34, n = 40), such that control pedophiles presented<br />
themselves as hav<strong>in</strong>g more psychological difficulties than all priest groups.<br />
Statistical Analyses for the MMPI-2 Narcissism Scales.<br />
A correlation analysis was conducted to exam<strong>in</strong>e the relationship among the<br />
narcissism scales (see Table 8) and to assess the construct validity among the measures.<br />
Results from this data analysis showed the Morey, Waugh, and Blashfield (MWB) scale<br />
was significantly related to the Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek (RN) scale, r (168)=.73, p (two-tailed)<br />
= .00, suggest<strong>in</strong>g good construct validity for these measures of Grandiosity-Exhibitionism<br />
(GE) narcissism. A moderate relationship was revealed between the MWB and W<strong>in</strong>k and<br />
Gough (WG) scale, r (168)= .35, p (two-tailed) = .00, as well as the RN and WG scales, r<br />
(168) = .42, p = .00, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a relationship between these scales and GE narcissism.<br />
With regard to Vulnerability-Sensitivity (VS) narcissism, a significant relationship was
Table 7<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 69<br />
Summary of Statistical Analyses for MMPI-2 L, F, and K Validity Scales by Group<br />
Validity Scale<br />
MMPI-2 L<br />
Control<br />
Pedophile<br />
55.22 (13.14)<br />
Control<br />
Priest<br />
53.30 (8.61)<br />
Priest<br />
Molester<br />
50.56 (10.24)<br />
Psychiatric<br />
Priest<br />
51.85 (7.90)<br />
MMPI-2 F 65.79 (24.89) b 44.03 (4.44) a 44.14 (6.11) a 43.60 (6.34) a *<br />
MMPI-2 K<br />
47.63 (11.76)<br />
57.40 (8.39)<br />
55.77 (9.14)<br />
56.21 (9.34)<br />
Note. Values not enclosed <strong>in</strong> parentheses represent mean T scores. Values enclosed <strong>in</strong><br />
parentheses represent standard deviations. * <strong>in</strong>dicates significant Univariate F, p < .00.<br />
Different superscripts <strong>in</strong>dicate significant group mean differences as revealed by Tukey<br />
post-hoc tests.
Table 8<br />
Intercorrelations Between Narcissism Scales<br />
Narcissism Scale<br />
Grandiosity-Exhibitionism<br />
1. Morey, Waugh, and<br />
Blashfield<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 70<br />
2<br />
73**<br />
3<br />
.35**<br />
4<br />
-.17*<br />
5<br />
-.32**<br />
2. Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek .42** .03 -.16*<br />
3. W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough .42** .31**<br />
Vulnerability-Sensitivity<br />
4. Narcissism-Hypersensitivity .59**<br />
5. Narcissistic Personality Disorder<br />
Note. *p < .05, two-tailed, **p < .01, two-tailed.
Narcissism and pedophilia 71<br />
revealed between the Narcissism-Hypersensitivity (NH) scales and the Narcissistic<br />
Personality Disorder (NPD), r (168) = .59, p (two-tailed) = .00, suggest<strong>in</strong>g good construct<br />
validity for these measures of VS narcissism.<br />
The MWB scale was found to be negatively correlated with both the NH scale, r<br />
(168) = -.17, p (two-tailed) = .03 and the NPD scale, r (168) = -.32, p (two-tailed) = .00,<br />
<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a dist<strong>in</strong>ction between the two types of narcissism. However, a relationship was<br />
<strong>in</strong>dicated between the NPD and WG scales, r (168) = .31, p (two-tailed) = .00, as well as<br />
the NH and WG scales, r (168) = .42, p (two-tailed) = .00, suggest<strong>in</strong>g an overlap between<br />
the scales on VS and GE narcissism, which was expected given similarities <strong>in</strong> personality<br />
characteristics of each type of narcissism. Overall, the correlation analyses demonstrated<br />
good construct validity for each narcissism type, despite some relationship between GE and<br />
VS subscales.<br />
Statistical Analyses for Grandiosity-Exhibitionism (GE) and Vulnerability-<br />
Sensitivity (VS) Narcissism.<br />
Table 9 represents statistical analyses conducted on Grandiosity-Exhibitionism<br />
(GE) and Vulnerability-Sensitivity (VS) narcissism. A MANCOVA, us<strong>in</strong>g the MWB, RN,<br />
and WG scales to form a composite variable for Grandiosity-Exhibitionism (GE)<br />
narcissism, failed to support the prediction (hypothesis 1) that there would be a significant<br />
difference between group membership and GE narcissism (p > .05). It is important to<br />
note; however, that Univariate F tests revealed a significant difference among groups for<br />
the MWB scale, F (4, 157) = 4.28, p (one-tailed) = .00, η 2 = .10, but not for the other GE<br />
subscales. Contrast t-tests further <strong>in</strong>dicated that, converse to prediction (hypothesis 1a),<br />
priest pedophiles (M = 13.27, SD = 3.12, n = 30) and priest ephebophiles (M = 14.87, SD<br />
= 3.28, n = 40) scored significantly lower on the MWB scale than control pedophiles (M =<br />
15.61, SD = 3.29, n = 28), control priests (M = 16.57, SD = 3.11, n = 30), and
Table 9<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 72<br />
Summary of Statistical Analyses for Grandiosity-Exhibitionism (GE) Narcissism,<br />
Vulnerability-Sensitivity (VS) Narcissism, and Overcontrolled-Hostility by Group<br />
MWB<br />
RN<br />
WG<br />
NH<br />
NPD<br />
Control<br />
Pedophile<br />
n=28<br />
15.61 a<br />
(3.29)<br />
15.93<br />
(3.29)<br />
14.93<br />
(5.41)<br />
7.64 b<br />
(2.77)<br />
5.50 b<br />
(3.66)<br />
51.78<br />
(10.52)<br />
Control<br />
Priest<br />
n=30<br />
16.57 a<br />
(3.11)<br />
15.87<br />
(4.08)<br />
13.57<br />
(3.72)<br />
5.60 a<br />
(2.01)<br />
2.40 a<br />
(1.45)<br />
57.07<br />
(7.76)<br />
Group<br />
Priest<br />
Pedophile<br />
n=30<br />
13.27 b<br />
(3.12)<br />
13.43<br />
(3.93)<br />
12.50<br />
(4.24)<br />
5.70<br />
(2.15)<br />
3.87<br />
(2.24)<br />
Priest<br />
Ephebophile<br />
n=40<br />
GE Narcissism<br />
14.87 b<br />
(3.28)<br />
14.43<br />
(3.51)<br />
12.03<br />
(4.77)<br />
VS Narcissism<br />
6.10<br />
(2.43)<br />
3.05<br />
(1.72)<br />
Overcontrolled-Hostility<br />
54.27<br />
(8.38)<br />
54.85<br />
(11.27)<br />
Priest<br />
Molester<br />
n=70<br />
14.19<br />
(3.29)<br />
14.00<br />
(3.70)<br />
12.23<br />
(4.53)<br />
5.93 a<br />
(2.31)<br />
3.40 a<br />
(1.99)<br />
54.60<br />
(10.07)<br />
Psychiatric<br />
Priest<br />
n=40<br />
16.03 a *<br />
(3.52)<br />
15.27<br />
(4.10)<br />
13.33<br />
(4.44)<br />
5.48 a *<br />
(2.61)<br />
2.35 a *<br />
(1.67)<br />
54.90<br />
(8.84)<br />
Note. Values not enclosed <strong>in</strong> parentheses represent mean raw scores. Values enclosed <strong>in</strong><br />
parentheses represent standard deviations. * <strong>in</strong>dicates significant Univariate F, p < .00.<br />
Different superscripts <strong>in</strong>dicate significant group mean differences as revealed by Contrast t<br />
tests.
Narcissism and pedophilia 73<br />
psychiatric priests (M = 16.03, SD = 3.52, n = 40). Hypothesis 1b also was not<br />
confirmed such that control priests and psychiatric priests did not score significantly higher<br />
on the MWB scale than control pedophiles (p > .05).<br />
Us<strong>in</strong>g the NH and NPD scales to establish a composite variable for VS narcissism,<br />
a MANCOVA supported the prediction (hypothesis 2) that there would be a significant<br />
difference between group membership and VS narcissism, Wilks' Λ = .89, F (6, 314) =<br />
3.07, p (one-tailed) = .01. Univariate F tests <strong>in</strong>dicated a significant effect for the NH<br />
scale, F (3, 158) = 3.21, p (one-tailed) = .02, η 2 = .06 as well as the NPD scale, F (3,<br />
158) = 5.50, p (one-tailed) = .00, η 2 = .09. Contrast t-tests for both the NH and NPD<br />
scales, respectively, supported the prediction (hypothesis 2) that control pedophiles [(NH:<br />
M = 7.64, SD = 2.77, n = 28), (NPD: M = 5.50, SD = 3.66)] would exhibit significantly<br />
greater levels of VS narcissism than control priests [(NH: M = 5.60, SD = 2.01), (NPD: M<br />
= 2.40, SD = 1.45), n = 30], priest molesters [(NH: M = 5.93, SD = 2.31), (NPD: M =<br />
3.40, SD = 1.99), n = 70] and psychiatric priests [(NH: M = 5.48, SD = 2.61), (NPD: M<br />
= 2.35, SD 1.67), n = 40].<br />
Statistical Analyses for Overcontrolled-Hostility (OH).<br />
An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was utilized to compare overcontrolled-<br />
hostility by group; however, statistical analyses failed to support hypothesis 3 (p > .05),<br />
which predicted priest molesters would reveal significantly higher levels of overcontrolled-<br />
hostility than control priests, control pedophiles, and psychiatric priests (see Table 9).
CHAPTER IV<br />
Discussion<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 74<br />
The purpose of this project was to determ<strong>in</strong>e if subpopulations of pedophiles could<br />
be dist<strong>in</strong>guished by specific personality characteristics. In particular, it was proposed that<br />
pedophiles who are priests would differ <strong>in</strong> the degree to which they display certa<strong>in</strong><br />
narcissism traits as compared to pedophiles who are not priests (who will be referred to as<br />
"generic pedophiles"). That is, priest molesters were predicted to exhibit greater symptoms<br />
of Grandiosity-Exhibitionism (GE) narcissism than their generic pedophile counterparts, as<br />
well as two other priest groups: psychiatric controls and healthy, nonpsychiatric controls.<br />
Furthermore, these nonpedophile priests (who will be referred to as "healthy priests") and<br />
priests <strong>in</strong> psychiatric treatment for psychological problems (who will be referred to as<br />
psychiatric priests) were proposed to have higher levels of GE narcissism than generic<br />
pedophiles. On the other hand, generic pedophiles were expected to possess higher levels<br />
of Vulnerability-Sensitivity (VS) narcissism than their priest molester counterparts, as well<br />
as healthy priests and psychiatric priests. F<strong>in</strong>ally, overcontrolled-hostility, which bears an<br />
important theoretical relationship to narcissistic qualities, was postulated as be<strong>in</strong>g a more<br />
prom<strong>in</strong>ent personality feature of priest molesters (priest pedophiles and priest<br />
ephebophiles) than all other groups.<br />
F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs Related to Demographic Information<br />
Prior to <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the primary statistical analyses, the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from prelim<strong>in</strong>ary<br />
analyses should be addressed. Significant differences were revealed among the different<br />
populations <strong>in</strong> this study with regard to age of the perpetrators at the time of the<br />
assessment. First, these analyses identified that priest pedophiles were markedly older than<br />
priest ephebophiles. If one considers the victim age categories for these perpetrators, it is<br />
possible that the younger child victims of the priest pedophiles may have been more fearful
Narcissism and pedophilia 75<br />
of disclos<strong>in</strong>g their abuse than the adolescent victims of the priest ephebophiles. Thus,<br />
priest pedophiles may have been identified after more years of abuse transpired. In<br />
addition, it is possible that the child victims experienced more confusion dur<strong>in</strong>g their abuse<br />
as to the appropriateness of the sexual perpetration, whereas the adolescent was more<br />
knowledgeable of the <strong>in</strong>appropriate behavior and, thus, revealed the abuse sooner. This<br />
difference might be due to the fact that priest ephebophiles who, theoretically and<br />
empirically, are thought to have greater problem solv<strong>in</strong>g abilities and less impaired<br />
cognitive processes than priest pedophiles (Kalichman, 1991; Rob<strong>in</strong>son, 1994), identified<br />
their need for treatment at an earlier age than the priest pedophiles who denied or lacked<br />
<strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to their problem until they were older.<br />
Second, it was found that priest pedophiles and priest ephebophiles were<br />
significantly older than psychiatric priests. It is possible that there is a relationship between<br />
the nature of their treatment referral (i.e., pedophilia/ephebophilia and other DSM-IV<br />
diagnoses, respectively) and their age. In particular, it is likely that the psychological and<br />
behavioral problems of priest child molesters were kept secret and not publicly identified<br />
until the priest molesters were much older. That is, their sexual perpetration may not have<br />
been disclosed to their superior or another adult (i.e., parent, police) until after many years<br />
of cover<strong>in</strong>g it up and/or the child not disclos<strong>in</strong>g their victimization. Typically with other<br />
psychological problems (i.e., adjustment disorder, depression, anxiety), symptoms and<br />
behaviors related to the emotional impairment are more obvious and can be more readily<br />
addressed.<br />
In addition, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with those types of diagnosable disorders tend to evidence a<br />
marked decrease <strong>in</strong> social and occupational function<strong>in</strong>g, as opposed to priest molesters,<br />
whose external function<strong>in</strong>g can rema<strong>in</strong>, or appear, relatively consistent. Another factor<br />
might <strong>in</strong>volve voluntary treatment. Priest molesters who <strong>in</strong>dependently hospitalized
Narcissism and pedophilia 76<br />
themselves may have rema<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> denial of their problem much longer due to the legal,<br />
social, and personal scrut<strong>in</strong>ization they predicted would occur. This contrasts with<br />
psychiatric priests who might be motivated sooner to seek treatment due to the emotional<br />
distress from which they have suffered.<br />
Third, priest pedophiles were significantly older than generic pedophiles. It is<br />
likely that because of their high societal status, priest pedophiles could hide their sexual<br />
behaviors longer than their generic pedophile counterparts whose behaviors may have been<br />
more conceivable and readily admonished. It is noted that generic pedophiles were not<br />
significantly younger than priest ephebophiles, likely due to the comb<strong>in</strong>ation of generic<br />
pedophiles be<strong>in</strong>g more easily detected and priest ephebophiles hav<strong>in</strong>g victims which<br />
possess greater maturity and confidence to reveal the abuse.<br />
Fourth, healthy priests were substantially older than psychiatric priests. It is<br />
difficult to expla<strong>in</strong> the age differential given that their participation occurred at different<br />
facilities and under different circumstances (i.e., voluntarily vs. unknow<strong>in</strong>gly,<br />
respectively). However, because the populations of priests <strong>in</strong> general is negatively skewed<br />
(i.e., the majority of priests are older) along with the fact that younger priests may be more<br />
accept<strong>in</strong>g of the need to seek treatment for psychiatric problems, this f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g is not<br />
surpris<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
In addition to age, the number of years participants were formally educated was<br />
exam<strong>in</strong>ed. Generic pedophiles were found to average about one year of college education,<br />
which was markedly less than the graduate and theological tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g all of the priest groups<br />
received. Although this was not <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> this study, it is possible that there is a<br />
relationship between education and age at which different subpopulations of pedophiles are<br />
identified. As researchers demonstrated, generic pedophiles tend to be socially <strong>in</strong>adequate<br />
(Erickson et al., 1987; Hall et al., 1991; Kalichman, 1991; Mann et al., 1992) and evidence
Narcissism and pedophilia 77<br />
a greater degree of pathology than ephebophiles (Kalichman, 1991) which could be related<br />
to education. With this <strong>in</strong> consideration, level of education might be positively related to<br />
cognitive capacity, problem solv<strong>in</strong>g ability, and reality test<strong>in</strong>g which all presumably are<br />
related to the decisions <strong>in</strong>dividuals make. That is, priest molesters, because they have more<br />
years of education, might possess higher <strong>in</strong>tellectual capabilities, and, ultimately, a more<br />
advanced ability to be discrete <strong>in</strong> their sexual behaviors. On the other hand, generic<br />
pedophiles who have less education might have less sophisticated decision mak<strong>in</strong>g skills<br />
and decreased <strong>in</strong>sight about the consequences of their sexual behaviors, mak<strong>in</strong>g them<br />
vulnerable to act<strong>in</strong>g out their sexual impulses <strong>in</strong> less discrete ways and be<strong>in</strong>g caught<br />
sooner.<br />
Although education level has not been found to be significantly different between<br />
generic pedophiles and ephebophiles (Kalichman, 1991), it might be that type of education<br />
(i.e., liberal arts vs. sem<strong>in</strong>ary) is an essential factor. Evidence of this relationship <strong>in</strong> future<br />
<strong>in</strong>vestigations would lend more credence to the idea that mean<strong>in</strong>gful subpopulations of<br />
pedophiles exist.<br />
Other F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs Related to Psychological Function<strong>in</strong>g<br />
One additional f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g from the prelim<strong>in</strong>ary analyses was that priest ephebophiles<br />
scored significantly higher on the MWB scale (GE narcissism) than priest pedophiles.<br />
Although def<strong>in</strong>ite conclusions cannot be made regard<strong>in</strong>g narcissism traits from just one<br />
measure, this f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g provides more weight to the description of priest ephebophiles as<br />
be<strong>in</strong>g more socially capable, extroverted, and self-confident than priest pedophiles.<br />
Of the many <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, one revealed that generic pedophiles presented<br />
themselves as experienc<strong>in</strong>g more psychological problems than all the other groups, as<br />
<strong>in</strong>dicated by the MMPI-2 validity scales. As Wasyliw et al. (1994) suggested, generic<br />
pedophiles tend to exaggerate psychological symptoms to justify and blame their behaviors
Narcissism and pedophilia 78<br />
on a mental disorder rather than on deliberate judgment. In do<strong>in</strong>g so, generic pedophiles<br />
decrease their anxiety, guilt, and overall emotional distress related to their offense (s).<br />
Without this cop<strong>in</strong>g mechanism to deter their responsibility, the generic pedophile may not<br />
be able to withstand those <strong>in</strong>tense emotions. However, it is also possible that generic<br />
pedophiles genu<strong>in</strong>ely believe they have greater pathology due to the nature of their offense.<br />
This argument is supported by Kalichman (1991) who noted that pedophiles have greater<br />
psychological disturbance than ephebophiles and other perpetrators of sexual crimes.<br />
None of the priest groups, on the other hand, produced elevations on this variable<br />
as compared to generic pedophiles. As the literature suggests (Keddy et al., 1990;<br />
Kennedy et al., 1977; Plante et al., 1996a; Rob<strong>in</strong>son, 1994), priests, <strong>in</strong> general, tend to<br />
<strong>in</strong>tellectualize their problems, thus, disconnect<strong>in</strong>g from any <strong>in</strong>ternal emotional distress they<br />
may experience. Therefore, it is perceivable that the priest molester may not acknowledge<br />
their affect. This is quite problematic because, without treatment, they could be at high risk<br />
for future offend<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
Another explanation for the disparity between generic pedophiles and priest<br />
molesters, is that it is unlikely that priest molesters would deliberately create a more<br />
pathological profile of themselves, particularly given that their reputation has been ru<strong>in</strong>ed<br />
from the identification of their sexual activities. In other words, by provok<strong>in</strong>g additional<br />
scrut<strong>in</strong>y to their professional and personal <strong>in</strong>tegrity, the priest molester would likely attempt<br />
to present himself as psychologically functional as possible. In regard to healthy priests, it<br />
is understandable that they would not perceive themselves as hav<strong>in</strong>g emotional problems<br />
s<strong>in</strong>ce they seem healthy enough to be active <strong>in</strong> the community. F<strong>in</strong>ally, it cannot be<br />
overlooked that all priests submit to a psychological evaluation and, thus, they must have<br />
been deemed emotionally and cognitively adjusted prior to ord<strong>in</strong>ation. If this is the case, it
Narcissism and pedophilia 79<br />
is understandable that priests would not view themselves as hav<strong>in</strong>g significant emotional<br />
problems.<br />
F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs From Hypothesis Tests<br />
Statistical analyses failed to confirm that priest molesters would demonstrate<br />
significantly more Grandiosity-Exhibitionism (GE) narcissism traits than generic<br />
pedophiles, healthy priests, and psychiatric priests (hypothesis 1a). This may suggest that<br />
priest molesters do not possess the level of assertiveness, self-centeredness, impulsivity,<br />
and stubbornness (W<strong>in</strong>k, 1991) assumed <strong>in</strong> earlier works address<strong>in</strong>g this population.<br />
Perhaps, rather than the priest qualities (i.e., extroversion, omnipotence, self-assuredness)<br />
highlight<strong>in</strong>g the personality dynamics of priest pedophiles, as was suggested, it might be<br />
that the pedophile components (i.e., passivity, dependence, <strong>in</strong>security) <strong>in</strong>stead decrease<br />
any exist<strong>in</strong>g overt, GE narcissism traits.<br />
Another possibility is that those priest pedophiles caught for their sexual<br />
<strong>in</strong>discretions may, <strong>in</strong> fact, have possessed GE traits to a greater degree than generic<br />
pedophiles prior to their detection. However, upon identification of their sexual behaviors,<br />
the overt narcissistic qualities (i.e., self-confidence, extroversion, grandiosity) may have<br />
been subdued by the embarrassment and shame of discovery. Furthermore, priest<br />
molesters have, <strong>in</strong> essence, lost their identity as they will no longer be respected and trusted<br />
as <strong>in</strong>dividuals, but most importantly to them, as messengers and symbols of God. This<br />
loss of sense of self could easily leave the priest molester experienc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tense emotional<br />
distress, which was previously speculated as be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tellectualized, but which nonetheless<br />
could adversely alter their previous exaggerated positive self-perception.<br />
The results of this study also rejected the prediction that healthy priests and<br />
psychiatric priests would demonstrate significantly higher levels of GE narcissism than<br />
generic pedophiles (hypothesis 1b). It is possible that healthy priests who volunteered to
Narcissism and pedophilia 80<br />
participate <strong>in</strong> the study do not possess the extent of GE narcissism expected, compared to<br />
those priests who decl<strong>in</strong>ed to volunteer. In addition, psychiatric priests may have<br />
emotional vulnerabilities that supercede any narcissistic tendencies. The variety of cl<strong>in</strong>ical<br />
presentations with<strong>in</strong> this group also might dilute their more demonstrative and typical<br />
personality dynamics. Of course, those priests vulnerable to serious psychological<br />
problems may, <strong>in</strong> fact, not possess the narcissistic traits predicted due to their <strong>in</strong>ability to<br />
cope effectively and <strong>in</strong>dependently with their problems. It also is possible that generic<br />
pedophiles possess more GE traits than expected, and, although they may not exist as<br />
primary characteristics, they are comparable to the level exhibited <strong>in</strong> healthy priests and<br />
psychiatric priests.<br />
One f<strong>in</strong>al thought about GE narcissism is that <strong>in</strong>stead of utiliz<strong>in</strong>g the psychological<br />
processes of GE narcissism to cope with stressors related to their sexual behavior, priest<br />
molesters might rely more heavily on <strong>in</strong>tellectualization as a compensatory mechanism.<br />
Through their education they may have developed this more sophisticated cop<strong>in</strong>g technique<br />
and, therefore, may not have needed to rely on GE narcissism, which is a more primitive<br />
defense mechanism.<br />
With regard to Vulnerability-Sensitivity (VS) narcissism, generic pedophiles, as<br />
proposed, exhibited markedly greater levels of VS narcissism than healthy priests, priest<br />
molesters, and psychiatric priests (hypothesis 2). Therefore, it appears that generic<br />
pedophiles tend to possess more traits of emotional vulnerability, dissatisfaction,<br />
dependency, and immaturity <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation with feel<strong>in</strong>gs of entitlement and grandiosity<br />
(W<strong>in</strong>k, 1991) when compared to their priest molester counterparts, as well as healthy<br />
priests and psychiatric priests. Thus, there appears to be an important difference between<br />
priests who molest and their non-priest counterparts.
Narcissism and pedophilia 81<br />
Psychodynamic theory supports the notion that pedophiles should have elevated VS<br />
narcissism, compared to the non-pedophiles (Groth & Birnbaum, 1978). Specifically, the<br />
"arrested sociosexual development of pedophiles leaves them susceptible to impaired<br />
<strong>in</strong>terpersonal function<strong>in</strong>g and problems with autonomy and assertiveness, while their<br />
egocentric needs" (Simon et al. 1991, p. 212) contribute to the constitution and<br />
ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of their narcissistic traits. In addition, other theorists and researchers have<br />
proclaimed that attachment deficits (Bowlby, 1969; Marshall, 1989; Socarides, 1991; Ward<br />
et al., 1996) impair healthy maturation which significantly contributes to the decreased<br />
cognitive and emotional function<strong>in</strong>g of pedophiles. Thus, pedophiles experience <strong>in</strong>security<br />
and social <strong>in</strong>troversion but also utilize splitt<strong>in</strong>g as a representation of the impaired<br />
attachment and as a way to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> their narcissistic drive for self-satisfaction. This<br />
duality <strong>in</strong> personality presentation fits with the description of VS narcissism.<br />
Social learn<strong>in</strong>g theory and cognitive theory also support the notion that the<br />
pedophiles should have elevated VS narcissism scores. These two conceptualizations,<br />
considered <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation, suggest that the socialization experience of <strong>in</strong>dividuals guides<br />
their thoughts, feel<strong>in</strong>gs, and behaviors. If one considers the relationship between<br />
emotional maturity level and age of victim, pedophiles would likely function at less<br />
advanced levels than priest molesters based on their limited or <strong>in</strong>sufficient model<strong>in</strong>g<br />
experience. This deficiency could lead to dim<strong>in</strong>ished confidence and assertiveness.<br />
A more complex question, however, is why the priest pedophiles have lower VS<br />
narcissism levels than the non-priest pedophiles. It was proposed that, because the priest<br />
role of priest pedophiles requires a more extroverted and confident character and facilitates<br />
a sense of omnipotence, priest pedophiles would not demonstrate the noted <strong>in</strong>security,<br />
withdrawal, and dependency of generic pedophiles (i.e., which are hallmark traits of VS<br />
narcissism). When this f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g is taken <strong>in</strong> concert with the fact that the priest pedophiles
Narcissism and pedophilia 82<br />
were not more pathological than the non-pedophile groups with respect to GE narcissism or<br />
the MMPI-2 F scale, it seems as if the priests are generally less disturbed than the non-<br />
priest pedophiles. Therefore, two critical questions rema<strong>in</strong>: Why do men with such<br />
pathological sexual patterns exhibit so little pathology <strong>in</strong> this area? and What other<br />
differences might be identifiable between priests who are pedophiles compared to priests<br />
who are not pedophiles?<br />
With respect to the first question, it is possible that priest molesters, compared to<br />
generic pedophiles, have had more opportunities to adopt skills required to function at a<br />
higher level, leav<strong>in</strong>g them more autonomy and confidence <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationships. It<br />
also is possible that, those (Catholic) pedophiles who demonstrate greater <strong>in</strong>terpersonal and<br />
emotional function<strong>in</strong>g are drawn to the priesthood where they hope to f<strong>in</strong>d either relief or<br />
distraction from their sexual disorder or, possibly, greater access to children. It also is<br />
possible that <strong>in</strong> the process of becom<strong>in</strong>g a priest and function<strong>in</strong>g as a priest, they may have<br />
<strong>in</strong>creased the skills necessary for higher psychological function<strong>in</strong>g compared to their non-<br />
priest counterparts. In any case, this study clearly supported the notion that generic<br />
pedophiles and priest pedophiles differ from each other; specifically, priest pedophiles<br />
seem to function at a higher level and are less dist<strong>in</strong>guishable from non-pedophiles than<br />
generic pedophiles are. Further exam<strong>in</strong>ation of these issues is clearly necessary as one of<br />
the goals of the study, to target traits that might identify priests at risk for molest<strong>in</strong>g<br />
children, has not yet been met.<br />
Despite the differences found <strong>in</strong> narcissism, overcontrolled-hostility was not found<br />
to be a prom<strong>in</strong>ent characteristic of priest molesters compared to other groups as was<br />
predicted (hypothesis 3). In contrast to Plante et al. (1996) and Keddy et al.'s (1990)<br />
identification of overcontrolled-hostility <strong>in</strong> priests, priest molesters may not utilize<br />
<strong>in</strong>tellectualization processes to control emotional distress to the extent this study assumed.
Narcissism and pedophilia 83<br />
It is plausible that this trait cannot be generalized to priest molesters and, <strong>in</strong> fact, because<br />
neither healthy priests nor psychiatric priests evidenced higher levels of overcontrolled-<br />
hostility, it might be that sample differences <strong>in</strong> these studies account for the dist<strong>in</strong>ction.<br />
Furthermore, Rob<strong>in</strong>son (1994) did not f<strong>in</strong>d MMPI O-H scale elevations for priest<br />
pedophiles or priest ephebophiles, suggest<strong>in</strong>g this trait is not outstand<strong>in</strong>g for these<br />
populations. Specifically, it is possible that these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are a function of environment.<br />
Certa<strong>in</strong>ly, it is still possible that the circumstances under which priest molesters were<br />
assessed (i.e., after their religious duties were revoked) may have altered their cognitive<br />
and emotional processes. Because they were <strong>in</strong> a treatment facility rather than their usual<br />
church community, the stressors they typically experienced may have been different,<br />
possibly reduc<strong>in</strong>g any hostile feel<strong>in</strong>gs. It is, therefore, possible that assessment dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />
their regular rout<strong>in</strong>e may have shown higher levels of this trait.<br />
The rejection of hypotheses related to two of the predicted prom<strong>in</strong>ent personality<br />
characteristics of priest molesters (i.e., GE narcissism and overcontrolled-hostility) raises<br />
important issues. For <strong>in</strong>stance, narcissism may not be as salient a personality characteristic<br />
<strong>in</strong> priests as Meloy (1988) theorized. In fact, Patrick (1990) argued aga<strong>in</strong>st Meloy's stance<br />
that narcissism is prevalent among clergy members. Specifically, she suggested that<br />
Meloy's theory of pathological narcissism <strong>in</strong> clergy was based on his presumption that<br />
because it could exist, it <strong>in</strong> fact does. Thus, she proposed there is a limited foundation to<br />
his argument.<br />
In further exam<strong>in</strong>ation of this issue, Patrick <strong>in</strong>vestigated United Church candidates<br />
us<strong>in</strong>g the Narcissism-Hypersensitivity (NH) Scale, the Narcissistic Personality Disorder<br />
(NPD) scale, the Millon Narcissistic Personality (MNP) scale, the Ego Inflation Scale, and<br />
the Edwards Personality Preference Schedule (EPPS; Edwards, 1957). The latter scale,<br />
EPPS, has been found to identify narcissistic symptoms by high scores on the
Narcissism and pedophilia 84<br />
Achievement (ACH), Exhibitionism (EXH), Succorance (SUC), Dom<strong>in</strong>ance (DOM), and<br />
Aggression (AGG) subscales and by low scores on the Deference (DEF), Intraception<br />
(INT), Abasement (ABA), and Nurturance (NUR) subscales. Despite <strong>in</strong>corporation of<br />
various scales tapp<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to narcissism, Patrick's results argued aga<strong>in</strong>st a high prevalence of<br />
pathological narcissism <strong>in</strong> clergy members. Thus, it is possible that pathological<br />
narcissism exists among rare clergy, rather than among a greater proportion of the clergy<br />
population. It also needs to be noted that Patrick did not <strong>in</strong>vestigate Catholic priests <strong>in</strong><br />
specific and, therefore, generalizations of her results should be made with caution.<br />
Another explanation arises from a review of Francis and Turner's (1995) study that<br />
<strong>in</strong>vestigated sexual misconduct <strong>in</strong> the Christian Church. These researchers outl<strong>in</strong>ed two<br />
reasons for this problem. One explanation focused on the premise of this project, that the<br />
existence of a personality disorder, typically narcissism, <strong>in</strong>fluenced clergy members to<br />
expect certa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals to fulfill their needs. Francis and Turner also proposed that<br />
sexual misconduct is exhibited by "naive and un<strong>in</strong>formed pastors who have failed to<br />
establish boundaries for themselves and others" (p.220). That is, such clergy dismiss or<br />
m<strong>in</strong>imize tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g and education related to ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of appropriate boundaries. They do<br />
not understand appropriate versus <strong>in</strong>appropriate behavior and do not possess <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to<br />
the dist<strong>in</strong>ction between their professional and personal roles. These writers suggested, that<br />
with education, the 'naive pastor' can learn acceptable behavior. They emphasized that,<br />
unlike the 'personality disordered pastor,' the 'naive pastor' does not possess a grandiose<br />
self image which <strong>in</strong>fluences the pastor to exploit other <strong>in</strong>dividuals for his own gratification,<br />
as might the 'personality disordered pastor.' This argument br<strong>in</strong>gs up the question of<br />
whether priest pedophiles may possess more naivete than narcissism, which could expla<strong>in</strong><br />
the rejection of hypotheses for this group. Aga<strong>in</strong>, although Catholic priests were not the
Narcissism and pedophilia 85<br />
focus of Francis and Turner's research, it is possible that their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs could generalize to<br />
that population.<br />
Although naivete was proposed as a possible factor <strong>in</strong> priests who molest children,<br />
it may also be that these priests possess a sense of entitlement which may play a more<br />
prom<strong>in</strong>ent role <strong>in</strong> their behavior. That is, because narcissism <strong>in</strong>volves many complex and<br />
<strong>in</strong>terrelated constructs, it might be that the priest pedophile, because he has received a<br />
'call<strong>in</strong>g from God,' which is unique to clergy members, might possess a stronger and more<br />
profound sense of entitlement <strong>in</strong> their relationships with others, particularly children.<br />
Future research should exam<strong>in</strong>e this particular subsconstruct of narcissism <strong>in</strong> its potential<br />
role <strong>in</strong> priest with pedophilia. Specifically, it would be predicted that priest pedophiles<br />
would demonstrate a greater level of this variable than their pedophile counterparts.<br />
As with any research study, design limitations must be addressed. In order to<br />
assess GE and VS narcissism, the MMPI-derived narcissism scales utilized <strong>in</strong> W<strong>in</strong>k's<br />
(1991) study were chosen for this research project. These scales were selected based on<br />
W<strong>in</strong>k's (1991) utilization <strong>in</strong> his study as represent<strong>in</strong>g the dist<strong>in</strong>ct types of narcissism. His<br />
study presented moderate to strong correlations between the MWB, RN, and WG scales<br />
(<strong>in</strong>ternal consistency coefficients ranged from .54 to .81) and between the NH and NPD<br />
scales (<strong>in</strong>ternal consistency of .57). Although these dist<strong>in</strong>ctions were made, his study and<br />
this current project revealed correlations between the GE and VS scales which may lead one<br />
to question about the clear dist<strong>in</strong>ction between the "types" of narcissism. In particular, the<br />
moderate correlation between the NH and NPD scales (VS) with the WG scale (GE) may<br />
have created difficulties filter<strong>in</strong>g out the dist<strong>in</strong>ct characteristics of the narcissism types,<br />
particularly GE narcissism. Because of this, the level of GE narcissism as exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the<br />
multivariate analyses may have also <strong>in</strong>cluded traits of VS narcissism. Thus, it also cannot
Narcissism and pedophilia 86<br />
be ruled out that the level of GE narcissism which was not found to be significantly<br />
different among groups could have conta<strong>in</strong>ed VS components and, thus, not be a true<br />
<strong>in</strong>dicator of GE narcissism. However, it is important to note that the univariate analyses<br />
were reported, and therefore, limit the problems that may exist with the scale overlap <strong>in</strong> the<br />
multivariate analysis.<br />
These caveats above demonstrate the difficulty <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g an absolute difference<br />
between these two types of narcissism, given that they have similar traits. W<strong>in</strong>k (1991)<br />
outl<strong>in</strong>ed those traits that are common to both GE and VS narcissism. These <strong>in</strong>cluded:<br />
"boss<strong>in</strong>ess, <strong>in</strong>tolerance, cruelty, argumentativeness, dishonesty, opportunism,<br />
rebelliousness, conceit, arrogance, be<strong>in</strong>g demand<strong>in</strong>g, be<strong>in</strong>g temperamental, and loudness"<br />
(p. 595). Future research might exclude the WG scale to elim<strong>in</strong>ate any confound<strong>in</strong>g<br />
variables it may conta<strong>in</strong> as well as overlap among scale types. In addition, although the<br />
NH and NPD scales are strongly correlated with one another, they do not clearly represent<br />
the dist<strong>in</strong>ct characteristics of VS narcissism. Thus, because there are not other known<br />
scales which are specific to VS narcissism, further research should establish measures<br />
which identify specific VS traits without the confound of GE traits. Empirical efforts<br />
should cont<strong>in</strong>ue to focus on filter<strong>in</strong>g out extraneous variables that might be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong><br />
certa<strong>in</strong> MMPI-2 items.<br />
Another po<strong>in</strong>t to address is that a healthy, non-priest sample was not <strong>in</strong>corporated<br />
<strong>in</strong>to this study as a control group. As the literature has found (McCreary, 1975),<br />
pedophiles have demonstrated personality traits comparable to normals, highlight<strong>in</strong>g the<br />
difficulty <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g characteristics particular to child molesters. Utiliz<strong>in</strong>g a healthy,<br />
non-priest sample might have served to better clarify the traits common and dist<strong>in</strong>ct to<br />
priests who are pedophiles.
Narcissism and pedophilia 87<br />
The fact that most of the participants volunteered was addressed earlier <strong>in</strong> this<br />
section; however, it is important to devote more discussion to this issue. It is likely that<br />
generic pedophiles and healthy priest volunteers possess personality dynamics that differ<br />
from those of persons unwill<strong>in</strong>g to volunteer. Certa<strong>in</strong>ly there exists factors of time, literacy<br />
level, right to privacy, and general dis<strong>in</strong>terest, but issues such as <strong>in</strong>troversion, lack of<br />
altruism, secrecy, and defensiveness cannot be ignored. It is possible that those<br />
<strong>in</strong>dividuals unwill<strong>in</strong>g to participate would have presented slightly differently (i.e., higher<br />
elevations on the MMPI-2 K scale). For the pedophiles, specifically, it could be postulated<br />
that those who decl<strong>in</strong>ed to volunteer for reasons related to defensiveness might be at greater<br />
risk for reoffend<strong>in</strong>g than those who were more will<strong>in</strong>g to expose their vulnerabilities.<br />
The lack of demographic <strong>in</strong>formation, aside from age and education, for the priest<br />
pedophile, priest ephebophile, and psychiatric priest participants created some difficulties <strong>in</strong><br />
this project. In particular, the number of victims for the priest molesters would have<br />
enhanced the analysis of the data. That is, generic pedophiles were found to have many<br />
victims. Given that priests, <strong>in</strong> general, were older than generic pedophiles, it is possible<br />
that the priest molesters had even more victims because their victimization period<br />
presumably is longer. Conversely, they may have had fewer victims because of greater<br />
cognitive dissonance. As the research has <strong>in</strong>dicated (Kalichman, 1991; Heers<strong>in</strong>k &<br />
Strassberg, 1995; McCreary, 1975), those child molesters who commit multiple offenses<br />
tend to suffer from greater pathology than first time offenders. Tak<strong>in</strong>g these statistics <strong>in</strong>to<br />
consideration, if priest molesters averaged even more child victims than generic pedophiles,<br />
and do not demonstrate higher elevations on any of the measures utilized <strong>in</strong> this study, then<br />
it is critical to identify the mechanisms (i.e., psychological, physical) which ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> their<br />
apparent function<strong>in</strong>g. On the other hand, if they have fewer victims, results show<strong>in</strong>g that<br />
they possess significantly less psychological disturbance would make sense.
Narcissism and pedophilia 88<br />
In addition to number of victims, other variables, such as gender of victims and age<br />
of onset, also could shed more light on the possible dist<strong>in</strong>ctions between priest pedophiles<br />
and generic pedophiles. Because these potentially important variables were not available<br />
for this study, it will be important for future studies to exam<strong>in</strong>e their impact.<br />
Summary and Conclusions<br />
The cl<strong>in</strong>ical phenomenon of pedophilia is both <strong>in</strong>trigu<strong>in</strong>g with regard to the<br />
development and ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of their sexual patterns, yet petrify<strong>in</strong>g to a society which<br />
must constantly be vigilant <strong>in</strong> regard to potential perpetrators. Priests who sexually<br />
victimize children are of even greater concern because they easily blend <strong>in</strong> with their<br />
healthy, non-pedophile cohorts who possess genu<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>tegrity and altruistic qualities <strong>in</strong><br />
uphold<strong>in</strong>g the hol<strong>in</strong>ess and safety of humanity. It is hoped that, with cont<strong>in</strong>ued cl<strong>in</strong>ical and<br />
empirical efforts, personality structures of these subpopulations of pedophiles can be<br />
identified so as to direct preventative and treatment strategies for pedophile groups<br />
accord<strong>in</strong>gly.<br />
This project sheds new theoretical and empirical light regard<strong>in</strong>g the existence of<br />
greater perceived (or real) psychological disturbance and vulnerability-sensitivity<br />
narcissism among generic pedophiles when compared to priest molesters. In addition,<br />
priest molesters did not possess grandiosity-exhibitionism narcissism to the degree<br />
speculated, thus rais<strong>in</strong>g questions about additional aspects of the priest molester personality<br />
profile that may differentiate them from generic pedophiles. This <strong>in</strong>formation could be<br />
useful <strong>in</strong> screen<strong>in</strong>g and risk assessment as well as treatment <strong>in</strong>terventions.<br />
Despite these important f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, there rema<strong>in</strong> multiple, unexam<strong>in</strong>ed variables<br />
related to the issue of subpopulations of pedophilia. Perhaps by target<strong>in</strong>g other adult<br />
perpetrator groups (i.e., teachers, coaches), the range and nature of the heterogeneity will<br />
become clearer. Unfortunately, until those aspects are determ<strong>in</strong>ed, children will cont<strong>in</strong>ue to
Narcissism and pedophilia 89<br />
be victimized sexually by adults whom they respect and trust to harbor nurturance and<br />
security. However, cont<strong>in</strong>ued diligence by society and the professional psychological<br />
community to research, expose, and provide education and prevention regard<strong>in</strong>g profiles of<br />
sexually offend<strong>in</strong>g adults may reduce the prevalence of pedophilia and child victims.
REFERENCES<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 90<br />
Abel, G. G., Becker, J. V., & Cunn<strong>in</strong>gham-Rathner, J. (1984). Complications,<br />
consent, and cognitions <strong>in</strong> sex between children and adults. International Journal of Law<br />
and Psychiatry, 7, 89-103.<br />
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of<br />
mental disorders (4th ed.). Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC: Author.<br />
American Psychological Association. (1992). Ethical pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of psychologists<br />
and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 47, 1597-1611.<br />
Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological test<strong>in</strong>g (6th ed.). New York: MacMillan.<br />
Archer, R. P., Griff<strong>in</strong>, R., Aiduk, R. (1995). MMPI-2 cl<strong>in</strong>ical correlates for ten<br />
common codes. Journal of Personality Assessment, 65, 391-407.<br />
Ashby, H. U., Lee, R. R., & Duke, E. H. (1979, September). A Narcissistic<br />
Personality Disorder MMPI scale. Paper presented at the annual meet<strong>in</strong>g of the American<br />
Psychological Association, New York.<br />
Auerbach, J. S. (1984). Validation of two scales for narcissistic personality<br />
disorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 649-653.<br />
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young<br />
adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61,<br />
226-244.<br />
Berl<strong>in</strong>, F. S., & Me<strong>in</strong>ecke, C. F. (1981). Treatment of sex offenders with<br />
antiandrogenic medication: Conceptualization, review of treatment modalities, and<br />
prelim<strong>in</strong>ary f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 601-607.<br />
Berry, J. (1989). Pedophilia. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton Post, 16 and 17 September.<br />
Boone, D. (1994). Reliability of the MMPI-2 subtle and obvious scales with<br />
psychiatric <strong>in</strong>patients. Journal of Personality Assessment, 62, 346-351.
Narcissism and pedophilia 91<br />
Bowlby, J. (1977). The mak<strong>in</strong>g and break<strong>in</strong>g of affectional bonds. British<br />
Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 201-210.<br />
Buss, A. H., & Durkee, A. (1957). An <strong>in</strong>ventory for assess<strong>in</strong>g different k<strong>in</strong>ds of<br />
hostility. Journal of Consult<strong>in</strong>g Psychology, 4, 343-349.<br />
Butcher, J. N. (1990). The MMPI-2 <strong>in</strong> psychological treatment. New York:<br />
Oxford University Press.<br />
Davis, R. D., Wenger, A., & Guzman, A. (1997). Validation of the MCMI-III.<br />
In T. Millon (Ed.), The Millon Inventories: Cl<strong>in</strong>ical and personality assessment (pp. 327-<br />
359). New York: Guilford Press.<br />
Erickson, W. D., Walbek, N. H., & Seely, R. K. (1988). Behavior patterns of<br />
child molesters. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 17, 77-86.<br />
Erickson, W. D., Luxemberg, M. G., Walbek, N. H., & Seely, R. K. (1987).<br />
Frequency of MMPI two-po<strong>in</strong>t code types among sex offenders. Journal of Consult<strong>in</strong>g<br />
and Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology, 55, 566-570.<br />
F<strong>in</strong>kelhor, D., & Araji, S. (1986). Explanations of pedophilia: A four factor<br />
model. Journal of Sex Research, 22, 145-161.<br />
Fisher, D. (1994). Adult sexual offenders: Who are they? Why and how do they<br />
do it? In T. Morrison, M. Erooga, & R.C. Bennett (Eds.) Sexual offend<strong>in</strong>g aga<strong>in</strong>st<br />
children: Assessment and treatment of male abusers (pp. 1-24). London: Routledge.<br />
Francis, P. C., & Turner, N. R. (1995). Sexual misconduct with<strong>in</strong> the Christian<br />
Church: Who are the perpetrators and those they victimize? Counsel<strong>in</strong>g and Values, 39,<br />
218-227.<br />
Gaffney, G. S., & Berl<strong>in</strong>, F. S. (1984). Is there a hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal<br />
dysfunction <strong>in</strong> paedophilia? British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 657-660.
Narcissism and pedophilia 92<br />
Graham, J. R. (1988). The MMPI: A practical guide (2nd ed.). New York:<br />
Oxford University Press.<br />
Graham, J. R. (1993). MMPI-2: Assess<strong>in</strong>g personality and psychopathology<br />
(2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.<br />
Graham, J. R., Timbrook, R. E., Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Butcher, J. N. (1991).<br />
Code-type congruence between the MMPI and the MMPI-2: Separat<strong>in</strong>g fact from artifact.<br />
Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 264-277.<br />
Greene, R. L. (1991). The MMPI-2/MMPI: An <strong>in</strong>terpretive manual. Boston:<br />
Allyn & Bacon.<br />
Greenburg, D. M., Bradford, J. M. W., & Curry, S. (1993). A comparison of<br />
sexual victimization <strong>in</strong> the childhoods of pedophiles and hebephiles. Journal of Forensic<br />
Sciences, 38, 432-436.<br />
Groth, A. N., & Birnbaum, H. J. (1978). Adult sexual orientation and attraction<br />
to underage persons. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 7, 175-181.<br />
Hall, G. C. N., Graham, J. R., & Shepherd, J. B. (1991). Three methods of<br />
develop<strong>in</strong>g MMPI- taxonomies of sexual offenders. Journal of Personality Assessment,<br />
56, 2-13.<br />
Hall, G. C. N., & Anderson, B. L. (1993). Sexual dysfunction and deviation. In<br />
A. S. Bellack & M. Hersen (Eds.). Psychopathology <strong>in</strong> adulthood (pp. 295-318).<br />
Boston: Allyn & Bacon.<br />
Hayash<strong>in</strong>o, D. S., Wurtele, S. K., & Klebe, K. J. (1995). Child molesters: An<br />
exam<strong>in</strong>ation of cognitive factors. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 106-116.<br />
Heers<strong>in</strong>k, N., & Strassberg, D. S. (1995). A normative and descriptive study of<br />
the MMPI with acknowledged child molesters. Journal of Psychopathology and<br />
Behavioral Assessment, 17, 377-391.
Narcissism and pedophilia 93<br />
Heilbrun, K. (1992). The role of psychological test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> forensic assessment.<br />
Law and Human Behavior, 16, 257-272.<br />
Hendrick, S. E., Fitzpatrick, D. R., Hartman, K., Quaife, M. A., Statbucker, R.<br />
A., & Graber, B. (1988). Bra<strong>in</strong> structure and function <strong>in</strong> sexual molesters of children and<br />
adolescents. Journal of Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychiatry, 49, 108-112.<br />
Hoppe, C. M., & S<strong>in</strong>ger, R. D. (1976). Overcontrolled hostility, empathy, and<br />
egocentric balance <strong>in</strong> violent and nonviolent psychiatric offenders. Psychological Reports,<br />
39, 1303-1308.<br />
Howitt, D. (1995). Paedophiles and sexual offenses aga<strong>in</strong>st children. New York:<br />
John Wiley & Sons.<br />
Hucker, S., Langev<strong>in</strong>, R., Wortzman, G., Dickey, R., Ba<strong>in</strong>, J., Handy, L.,<br />
Chambers, J., & Wright, S. (1986). Neuropsychological impairment <strong>in</strong> pedophiles.<br />
Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 18, 440-448.<br />
Isely, P. J., & Isely, P. (1990). The sexual abuse of male children by church<br />
personnel: Intervention and prevention. Pastoral Psychology, 39, 85-99.<br />
Johnston, F. A., & Johnston, S. A. (1997). A cognitive approach to validation of<br />
the fixated-regressed typology of child molesters. Journal of Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology, 53,<br />
361-368.<br />
Kalat, J. W. (1995). Biological psychology (5th ed.). New York: Brooks/Cole<br />
Publish<strong>in</strong>g Company.<br />
Kalichman, S. C. (1991). Psychopathology and personality characteristics of<br />
crim<strong>in</strong>al sexual offenders as a function of victim age. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 20,<br />
187-197.
Narcissism and pedophilia 94<br />
Keddy, P. J., Erdberg, P., & Sammon, S. D. (1990). The psychological<br />
assessment of Catholic clergy and religious referred for residential treatment. Pastoral<br />
Psychology, 38, 147-159.<br />
Kennedy, E. C., Heckler, V. J., Kobler, F. J., & Walker, R. E. (1977). Cl<strong>in</strong>ical<br />
assessment of a profession: Roman Catholic clergymen. Journal of Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology,<br />
33, 120-128.<br />
Kernberg, O. F. (1975). Borderl<strong>in</strong>e conditions and pathological narcissism.<br />
Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.<br />
Kernberg, O. F. (1986). Narcissistic personality disorder. In A. A. Cooper, A.<br />
J. Frances, & M. H. Sachs (Eds.), The personality disorder and neuroses (Vol. 1, pp.<br />
219-213). New York: Basic Books.<br />
K<strong>in</strong>near, K. L. (1995). Childhood sexual abuse: A reference handbook. Santa<br />
Barbara: ABC-CLIO.<br />
Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self: A systematic approach to the<br />
psychoanalytic treatment of narcissistic personality disorder. New York: International<br />
Universities Press, Inc.<br />
Kolarsky, A., Freund, K., Machek, J., & Polak, O. (1967). Male sexual<br />
deviation: Association with early temporal lobe damage. Archives of General Psychiatry,<br />
17, 735-743.<br />
Kurtz, J. E., Morey, L. C., & Tomarken, A. J. (1993). The concurrent validity of<br />
three self-report measures of borderl<strong>in</strong>e personality. Journal of Psychopathology and<br />
Behavioral Assessment, 13, 255-266.<br />
Langev<strong>in</strong>, R. (1992). Biological factors contribut<strong>in</strong>g to paraphilic behavior.<br />
Psychiatric Annals, 22, 307-314.
Narcissism and pedophilia 95<br />
Lehne, G. K. (1994). The NEO-PI and the MCMI <strong>in</strong> the forensic evaluation of sex<br />
offenders. In P. T. Costa and T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-<br />
factor model of personality (pp. 175-188). Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC: American Psychological<br />
Association.<br />
Loss, P., & Glancy, E. (1983). Men who sexually abuse their children. Medical<br />
Aspects of Human Sexuality, 17, 328-329.<br />
Mann, J., Stenn<strong>in</strong>g, W., Borman, C. (1992). The utility of the MMPI-2 with<br />
pedophiles. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 18, 59-74.<br />
Marshall, W. L. (1989). Invited essay: Intimacy, lonel<strong>in</strong>ess, and sexual<br />
offenders. Behavior Research and Therapy, 27, 491-503.<br />
Marshall, W. L., Barbaree, H. E., & Fernandez, Y. M. (1995). Some aspects of<br />
social competence <strong>in</strong> sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and<br />
Treatment, 7, 113-127.<br />
Marshall, Jones, Hudson, & McDonald (1993). Generalized empathy <strong>in</strong> child<br />
molesters. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 2, 61-69.<br />
McCreary, C. P. (1975). Personality differences among child molesters. Journal<br />
of Personality Assessment, 39, 591-593.<br />
McGuire, R. J., Carlisle, J. M., & Young, B. G. (1964). Sexual deviations as<br />
conditioned behavior: A hypothesis. Behavior Research and Therapy, 2, 185-190.<br />
Megargee, E. I., Cook, P. E., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1967). Development and<br />
validation of an MMPI scale of assaultiveness <strong>in</strong> overcontrolled <strong>in</strong>dividuals. Journal of<br />
Abnormal Psychology, 72, 519-528.<br />
Meloy, J.R. (1986). Narcissistic psychopathology and the clergy. Pastoral<br />
Psychology, 35, 50-55.
Narcissism and pedophilia 96<br />
Meyer, R. G., & Deitsch, S. E. (1996). The cl<strong>in</strong>ician's handbook: Integrated<br />
diagnostics, assessment and <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> adult and adolescent psychopathology (4th ed.,<br />
pp. 153-183). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.<br />
Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality. New York: John Wiley & Sons.<br />
Millon, T. (1987). Millon Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Multiaxial Inventory-II: Manual for the MCMI-<br />
II. M<strong>in</strong>neapolis: National Computer Systems.<br />
Millon, T. (1994). Millon Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Multiaxial Inventory-III manual. M<strong>in</strong>neapolis:<br />
National Computer Systems.<br />
Millon, T., & Everly, G. S. (1985). Personality and its disorders: A biosocial<br />
learn<strong>in</strong>g approach. New York: John Wiley & Sons.<br />
Millon, T., & Millon, C. M. (1997). History, theory, and validation of the<br />
MCMI. In T. Millon (Ed.), The Millon Inventories: Cl<strong>in</strong>ical and personality assessment<br />
(pp. 3-58). New York: Guilford Press.<br />
Money, J. (1984). Paraphilias: Phenomenology and classification. American Journal of<br />
Psychotherapy, 38, 164-179.<br />
Morey, L. C., Waugh, M. H., & Blashfield, R. K. (1985). MMPI scales for<br />
DSM-III personality disorders: Their derivation and correlates. Journal of Personality<br />
Assessment, 49, 245-251.<br />
Mull<strong>in</strong>s, L. S., & Kopelman, R. E. (1988). Toward an assessment of the<br />
construct validity of four measures of narcissism. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52,<br />
610-625.<br />
Murphy, W. D., & Peters, J. M. (1992). Profil<strong>in</strong>g child sexual abusers. Crim<strong>in</strong>al<br />
Justice and Behavior, 19, 24-37.
Narcissism and pedophilia 97<br />
Nagayama, G. C., Graham, J. R., & Shepherd, J. B. (1991). Three methods of<br />
develop<strong>in</strong>g MMPI taxonomies of sexual offenders. Journal of Personality Assessment,<br />
56, 2-13.<br />
Okami, P., & Goldberg, A. (1992). Personality correlates of pedophiles: Are<br />
they reliable <strong>in</strong>dicators? The Journal of Sex Research, 29, 297-328.<br />
Patrick, J. (1990). Assessment of narcissistic psychopathology <strong>in</strong> the clergy.<br />
Pastoral Psychology, 38, 173-180.<br />
Plante, T. G. (1996). Catholic priests who sexually abuse m<strong>in</strong>ors: Why do we<br />
hear so much yet know so little? Pastoral Psychology, 44, 305-310<br />
Plante, T. G., Manuel, G., & Bryant, C. (1996b). Personality and cognitive<br />
function<strong>in</strong>g among hospitalized sexual offend<strong>in</strong>g Roman Catholic priests. Pastoral<br />
Psychology, 45, 129-139.<br />
Plante, T. G., Manuel, G., & Tandez, J. (1996a). Personality characteristics of<br />
successful applicants to the priesthood. Pastoral Psychology, 45, 29-40.<br />
Qu<strong>in</strong>sey, V. L., Arnold, L. S., & Pruesse, M. G. (1980). MMPI profiles of men<br />
referred for a pretrial psychiatric assessment as a function of assessment type. Journal of<br />
Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology, 36, 410-417.<br />
Rask<strong>in</strong>, R. & Novacek, J. (1989). An MMPI description of the Narcissistic<br />
Personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 66-80.<br />
Rob<strong>in</strong>son, E. A. (1994). Shadows of the lantern bearers: A study of sexually<br />
troubled clergy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, <strong>Loyola</strong> <strong>College</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Maryland</strong>.<br />
Rosenhan, D. L., & Seligman, M. E. (1989). Abnormal psychology (2nd ed.,<br />
pp. 411-449). New York: W. W. Norton and Company.<br />
Rossetti, S. J. (1990). Slayer of the soul: Child sexual abuse and the Catholic<br />
church. Mystic, CT: Twenty-Third Publications.
Narcissism and pedophilia 98<br />
Seidman, B. T., Marshall, W. L., Hudson, S. M., & Robertson, P. J. (1994).<br />
An exam<strong>in</strong>ation of <strong>in</strong>timacy and lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>in</strong> sex offenders. Journal of Interpersonal<br />
Violence, 9, 518-534.<br />
Serkownek, K. (1975). Subscales for Scales 5 and 9 of the M<strong>in</strong>nesota<br />
Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Unpublished manuscript.<br />
Shealy, L., Kalichman, S. C., Henderson, M. C., Szymanowski, D., & McKee,<br />
G. (1991). MMPI profile subtypes of <strong>in</strong>carcerated sex offenders aga<strong>in</strong>st children.<br />
Violence and Victims, 6, 201-212.<br />
Simon, L. M., Sales, B., Kasniak, A., & Kahn, M. (1992). Characteristics of<br />
child molesters: Implications for the fixated-regressed dichotomy. Journal of Interpersonal<br />
Violence, 7, 211-225.<br />
Sipe, A. W. R. (1990). A secret world: Sexuality and the search for celibacy.<br />
New York: Brunner/Mazel.<br />
Sipe, A. W. R. (1995). Sex, priests, and power: Anatomy of a crisis. New York:<br />
Brunner/Mazel.<br />
Socarides, C. W. (1991). Adult-child sexual pairs: Psychoanalytic f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. The<br />
Journal of Psychohistory, 19, 185-189.<br />
Solomon, R. S. (1982). Validity of the MMPI Narcissistic Personality Disorder<br />
Scale. Psychological Reports, 50, 463-466.<br />
Stumpf, W. F. (1995). The effect of personality pathology on the treatment<br />
response of clergy with sexual disorders. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, <strong>Loyola</strong><br />
<strong>College</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Maryland</strong>.<br />
Ward, T., Hudson, S. M., & Marshall, W. L. (1995). Cognitive distortions and<br />
affective deficits <strong>in</strong> sex offenders: A cognitive deconstructionist <strong>in</strong>terpretation. Sexual<br />
Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 7, 67-83.
Narcissism and pedophilia 99<br />
Wasyliw, O. E., Grossman, L. S., & Haywood, T. W. (1994). Denial of<br />
hostility and psychopathology <strong>in</strong> the evaluation of child molestation. Journal of Personality<br />
Assessment, 63, 185-190.<br />
West, D. J. (1991). The effects of sex offenses. In C.R. Holl<strong>in</strong> & K. Howells<br />
(Eds.) Cl<strong>in</strong>ical approaches to sex offenders and their victims (pp. 55-73). New York: John<br />
Wiley & Sons.<br />
W<strong>in</strong>k, P. (1991). Two faces of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social<br />
Psychology, 6, 590-597.<br />
W<strong>in</strong>k, P., & Gough, H. G. (1990). New Narcissism scales for the California<br />
Psychological Inventory and the MMPI. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, 446-462.<br />
Wright, P., Nobrega, J., Langev<strong>in</strong>, R., Wortzman, G. (1990). Bra<strong>in</strong> density and<br />
symmetry <strong>in</strong> pedophilic and sexually aggressive men. Annals of Sex Research, 3, 319-<br />
328.
APPENDIX A<br />
Human Subject Review Letter of Consent for Study<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 100
MEMORANDUM - Human Subjects Review Committee<br />
Date: June 18, 1998<br />
To:<br />
Fr:<br />
Subj:<br />
Kathryn Savitz<br />
Stewart P. Craig<br />
Assistant Director for Grant Services, Member HSRC<br />
Protocol Approval<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 101<br />
Your protocol entitled "Narcissism and Overcontrolled Hostility: Personality<br />
Characteristics of Priests with Pedophilia" has been reviewed and approved<br />
by the Human Subjects Review Committee. This approval is valid for one<br />
year from the date of approval and is subject to annual review as required by<br />
federal regulations.<br />
Log Number: 891<br />
Date received: 6/10/98<br />
Date Approved: 6/16/98<br />
Title of Protocol: "Narcissism and Overcontrolled..."<br />
Pr<strong>in</strong>cipal Investigator: Kathryn Savitz<br />
Project Period: 6/1/98 to 6/1/99<br />
Reviewers: Faith Gilroy<br />
Comments:<br />
c. HSRC files<br />
4501 North Charles Street • Baltimore, <strong>Maryland</strong> 21210-2699 • (410) 617-2000
APPENDIX B<br />
Demographic Information for Control Pedophiles<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 102
Narcissism and pedophilia 103<br />
Age _________ Race ______________<br />
Education ______________<br />
Occupation ________________________<br />
Your approximate age at the onset of your sexual disorder __________<br />
Approximately how many <strong>in</strong>dividuals have been directly affected by your<br />
sexual disorder? ___________<br />
Gender of those <strong>in</strong>dividuals directly affected by your sexual disorder ________<br />
Age of those <strong>in</strong>dividuals who have directly been affected by your<br />
sexual disorder _________<br />
Have you ever been <strong>in</strong>carcerated as a result of your sexual disorder? YES NO<br />
If yes, for how long? __________<br />
How long have you been <strong>in</strong> treatment at this cl<strong>in</strong>ic? __________<br />
Have you had any other psychiatric treatment? YES NO<br />
If yes, please expla<strong>in</strong> (i.e., disorders, type and length of treatment, hospitalizations, etc.):
APPENDIX C<br />
Demographic Information for Control Priests<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 104
Age__________<br />
Race____________<br />
Education __________<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 105<br />
Type of sem<strong>in</strong>ary tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g (please circle): Religious order Diocesan<br />
Number of years s<strong>in</strong>ce ord<strong>in</strong>ation ________<br />
Have you received any psychiatric treatment? YES NO<br />
If yes, please expla<strong>in</strong> (i.e., outpatient, hospitalizations, etc.):
APPENDIX D<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 106<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the Morey, Waugh, and Blashfield Narcissism Scale
Narcissism and pedophilia 107<br />
MMPI<br />
19<br />
73<br />
86<br />
91<br />
22<br />
124<br />
126<br />
142<br />
165<br />
171<br />
180<br />
267<br />
218<br />
250<br />
257<br />
264<br />
271<br />
280<br />
304<br />
319<br />
321<br />
353<br />
394<br />
400<br />
415<br />
450<br />
469<br />
517<br />
520<br />
521<br />
547<br />
MMPI-2<br />
19<br />
61<br />
73<br />
791<br />
109<br />
110<br />
112<br />
130<br />
153<br />
158<br />
167<br />
243<br />
270<br />
227<br />
318<br />
239<br />
248<br />
254<br />
275<br />
286<br />
289<br />
321<br />
365<br />
385<br />
**<br />
388<br />
392<br />
450<br />
452<br />
398<br />
399<br />
**excluded item
APPENDIX E<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 108<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the Narcissism-Hypersensitivity Scale
**excluded item<br />
MMPI<br />
25<br />
79<br />
89<br />
133<br />
117<br />
179<br />
187<br />
198<br />
214<br />
262<br />
217<br />
226<br />
239<br />
264<br />
278<br />
282<br />
297<br />
299<br />
MMPI-2<br />
25<br />
63<br />
76<br />
121<br />
104<br />
**<br />
177<br />
184<br />
194<br />
237<br />
196<br />
205<br />
219<br />
239<br />
**<br />
**<br />
268<br />
271<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 109
APPENDIX F<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 110<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the Narcissistic Personality Disorder Scale
**excluded item<br />
MMPI<br />
12<br />
20<br />
142<br />
53<br />
70<br />
137<br />
148<br />
168<br />
175<br />
249<br />
276<br />
301<br />
310<br />
314<br />
335<br />
338<br />
345<br />
422<br />
431<br />
MMPI-2<br />
14<br />
12<br />
130<br />
**<br />
**<br />
125<br />
136<br />
180<br />
164<br />
**<br />
343<br />
273<br />
20<br />
16<br />
299<br />
305<br />
311<br />
**<br />
415<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 111
APPENDIX G<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek Scale<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 112
Narcissism and pedophilia 113<br />
MMPI<br />
29<br />
73<br />
118<br />
133<br />
142<br />
165<br />
171<br />
180<br />
181<br />
201<br />
208<br />
232<br />
264<br />
265<br />
266<br />
269<br />
304<br />
321<br />
322<br />
323<br />
328<br />
352<br />
353<br />
MMPI-2<br />
28<br />
61<br />
105<br />
121<br />
130<br />
153<br />
158<br />
167<br />
169<br />
185<br />
189<br />
211<br />
239<br />
241<br />
242<br />
**<br />
275<br />
289<br />
290<br />
**<br />
299<br />
320<br />
321<br />
MMPI<br />
365<br />
377<br />
381<br />
387<br />
393<br />
400<br />
415<br />
416<br />
417<br />
434<br />
447<br />
460<br />
469<br />
503<br />
506<br />
509<br />
520<br />
521<br />
529<br />
MMPI-2<br />
334<br />
350<br />
344<br />
**<br />
364<br />
385<br />
**<br />
409<br />
410<br />
417<br />
423<br />
**<br />
392<br />
**<br />
444<br />
446<br />
452<br />
398<br />
456<br />
**excluded item
APPENDIX H<br />
MMPI and MMPI-2 Items for the W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough Scale<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 114
Narcissism and pedophilia 115<br />
MMPI-2<br />
4<br />
73<br />
75<br />
82<br />
89<br />
111<br />
109<br />
112<br />
117<br />
124<br />
165<br />
208<br />
233<br />
248<br />
261<br />
271<br />
276<br />
277<br />
299<br />
348<br />
355<br />
380<br />
400<br />
406<br />
MMPI<br />
4<br />
61<br />
102<br />
70<br />
76<br />
100<br />
98<br />
120<br />
104<br />
110<br />
153<br />
189<br />
212<br />
226<br />
236<br />
248<br />
343<br />
250<br />
271<br />
315<br />
323<br />
354<br />
385<br />
386<br />
MMPI-2<br />
410<br />
426<br />
434<br />
438<br />
440<br />
444<br />
447<br />
452<br />
456<br />
459<br />
463<br />
503<br />
504<br />
520<br />
529<br />
MMPI<br />
406<br />
414<br />
417<br />
419<br />
**<br />
**<br />
423<br />
**<br />
**<br />
**<br />
426<br />
**<br />
443<br />
452<br />
456<br />
**excluded item
APPENDIX I<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 116<br />
Excluded MMPI Items for the Grandiosity-Exhibitionism Narcissism Scales
MWB SCALE<br />
415 (T). I hate to have to rush when work<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
RN SCALE<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 117<br />
269 (T). I can easily make other people afraid of me, and sometimes do for the fun of it.<br />
387 (T). I have had no difficulty start<strong>in</strong>g or hold<strong>in</strong>g my ur<strong>in</strong>e.<br />
415 (T). I hate to rush when work<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
460 (F). I have used alcohol moderately (or not at all).<br />
503 (F). It is unusual for me to express strong approval or disapproval of the actions of<br />
others.<br />
WG SCALE<br />
440 (T). When I am feel<strong>in</strong>g very happy and active, someone who is blue or low will<br />
spoil it all.<br />
452 (T). I like to poke fun at people.<br />
456 (T). A person shouldn't be punished for break<strong>in</strong>g a law that he th<strong>in</strong>ks is unreasonable.<br />
459 (T). I have one or more bad habits which are so strong that it is no use fight<strong>in</strong>g aga<strong>in</strong>st them.<br />
444 (F). I do not try to correct people who express an ignorant belief.<br />
503 (F). It is unusual for me to express strong approval or disapproval of the actions of<br />
others.
APPENDIX J<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 118<br />
Excluded MMPI Items for the Vulnerability-Sensitivity Narcissism Scales
NH SCALE<br />
179 (T). I am worried about sex matters.<br />
278 (T). I have often felt that strangers were look<strong>in</strong>g at me critically.<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 119<br />
282 (T). Once <strong>in</strong> awhile I feel hate toward members of my family who I usually love.<br />
NPD SCALE<br />
422 (T). I have felt embarrassed over the type of work that one or more members of my<br />
family have done.<br />
53 (F). A m<strong>in</strong>ister can cure disease by pray<strong>in</strong>g and putt<strong>in</strong>g his hand on your hand.<br />
70 (F). I use to like drop-the handkerchief.<br />
249 (T). I believe there is a Devil and Hell <strong>in</strong> afterlife.
APPENDIX K<br />
Rephrased MMPI Items for the RN and WG Narcissism Scales<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 120
RN SCALE<br />
28. I am bothered by acid stomach several times a week.<br />
118. In school I was sometimes sent to the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal for cutt<strong>in</strong>g up.<br />
304. In school I found it very hard to talk before the class.<br />
299. I f<strong>in</strong>d it hard to keep my m<strong>in</strong>d on a task or job.<br />
WG SCALE<br />
463. I used to like hopscotch.<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 121
APPENDIX L<br />
Morey, Waugh, and Blashfield Narcissism Scale<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 122
Narcissism and pedophilia 123<br />
Items scored 'true.'<br />
____ 19. When I take a new job, I like to f<strong>in</strong>d out who it is important to be nice to.<br />
____ 61. I am an important person.<br />
____ 109. I seem to be about as capable and smart as most others around me.<br />
____ 110. Most people will use somewhat unfair means to ga<strong>in</strong> profit or an<br />
advantage rather than to lose it.<br />
____ 112. I like dramatics.<br />
____ 153. I like to know some important people because it makes me feel important.<br />
____ 227. I don't blame people for try<strong>in</strong>g to grab everyth<strong>in</strong>g they can get <strong>in</strong> this world.<br />
____ 239. I am entirely self-confident.<br />
____ 248. I do not blame a person for tak<strong>in</strong>g advantage of people who leave<br />
themselves open to it.<br />
____ 254. Most people make friends because friends are likely to be useful to them.<br />
____ 270. It does not bother me particularly to see animals suffer.<br />
____ 286. Most people <strong>in</strong>wardly dislike putt<strong>in</strong>g themselves out to help other people.<br />
____ 318. I usually expect to succeed <strong>in</strong> th<strong>in</strong>gs I do.<br />
____ 321. I have no dread of go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to a room by myself where other people have<br />
already gathered and are talk<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
____ 365. I like to let people know where I stand on th<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />
____ 385. I am not afraid of fire.<br />
____ 388. I very seldom have spells of the blues.<br />
____ 392. Lighten<strong>in</strong>g is one of my fears.<br />
____ 398. I frequently ask people for advice.<br />
____ 399. The future is too uncerta<strong>in</strong> for a person to make serious plans.<br />
____ 452. I strongly defend my op<strong>in</strong>ions as a rule.<br />
Items scored 'false.'<br />
____ 73. I certa<strong>in</strong>ly am lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> self-confidence.<br />
____ 79. I do not m<strong>in</strong>d be<strong>in</strong>g made fun of.<br />
____ 130. I certa<strong>in</strong>ly feel useless at times.<br />
____ 158. It makes me uncomfortable to put on a stunt at a party even when others<br />
are do<strong>in</strong>g the same sort of th<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />
____ 167. I f<strong>in</strong>d it hard to make talk when I meet new people.<br />
____ 243. When <strong>in</strong> a group of people I have trouble th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g of the right th<strong>in</strong>gs to talk about.<br />
____ 275. In school I found it very hard to talk <strong>in</strong> front of the class.<br />
____ 289. I am easily embarrassed.<br />
____ 450. I cannot do anyth<strong>in</strong>g well.<br />
____ TOTAL (Grandiosity-Exhibitionism)
APPENDIX M<br />
Rask<strong>in</strong> and Novacek Narcissism Scale<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 124
Narcissism and pedophilia 125<br />
Items scored 'true.'<br />
____ 28. I am bothered by an upset stomach several times a week.<br />
____ 61. I am an important person.<br />
____ 105. In school I was sometimes sent to the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal for bad behavior.<br />
____ 153. I like to know some important people because it makes me feel important.<br />
____ 169. When I get bored I like to stir up some excitement.<br />
____ 189. I like to flirt.<br />
____ 211. I have been <strong>in</strong>spired to a program of life based on duty which I have s<strong>in</strong>ce<br />
carefully followed.<br />
____ 239. I am entirely self-confident.<br />
____ 241. It is safer to trust nobody.<br />
____ 242. Once a week or oftener I become very excited.<br />
____ 290. I worry over money and bus<strong>in</strong>ess.<br />
____ 321. I have no dread of go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to a room by myself where other people have<br />
already gathered and are talk<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
____ 334. I feel uneasy <strong>in</strong>doors.<br />
____ 344. I enjoy gambl<strong>in</strong>g at small stakes.<br />
____ 364. I feel like giv<strong>in</strong>g up quickly when th<strong>in</strong>gs go wrong.<br />
____ 385. I am not afraid of fire.<br />
____ 392. Lighten<strong>in</strong>g is one of my fears.<br />
____ 398. I frequently ask people for advice.<br />
____ 410. I am often so annoyed when someone tries to get ahead of me <strong>in</strong> a l<strong>in</strong>e of<br />
people that I speak to that person about it.<br />
____ 417. I would like to be an auto racer.<br />
____ 423. I am often <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed to go out of my way to w<strong>in</strong> a po<strong>in</strong>t with someone who<br />
has opposed me.<br />
____ 444. I am a high-strung person.<br />
____ 452. I strongly defend my own op<strong>in</strong>ions as a rule.<br />
____ 456. I would like to wear expensive clothes.<br />
Items scored 'false.'<br />
____ 121. I have never <strong>in</strong>dulged <strong>in</strong> any unusual sex practices.<br />
____ 130. I certa<strong>in</strong>ly feel useless at times.<br />
____ 158. It makes me uncomfortable to put on a stunt at a party even when others<br />
are do<strong>in</strong>g the same sort of th<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
____ 167. I f<strong>in</strong>d it hard to take talk when I meet new people.<br />
____ 185. I wish I were not so shy.<br />
____ 275. In school I found it very hard to talk <strong>in</strong> front of the class.<br />
____ 289. I am easily embarrassed.
____<br />
____<br />
____<br />
____<br />
____<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 126<br />
299. I cannot keep my m<strong>in</strong>d on one th<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
320. I have been afraid of th<strong>in</strong>gs or people that I knew could not hurt me.<br />
350. If given a chance I would make a good leader of people.<br />
409. It bothers me to have someone watch me at work even though I<br />
know I can do it well.<br />
446. I sometimes f<strong>in</strong>d it hard to stick up for my rights because I am so<br />
reserved.<br />
____ TOTAL (Grandiosity-Exhibitionism)
APPENDIX N<br />
W<strong>in</strong>k and Gough Narcissism Scale<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 127
Items scored 'true.'<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
_____<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 128<br />
61. I am an important person.<br />
76. It takes a lot of argument to conv<strong>in</strong>ce most people of the truth.<br />
98. Some people are so bossy that I feel like do<strong>in</strong>g the opposite of what<br />
they request, even though I know they are right.<br />
104. Most people are honest chiefly because they are afraid of be<strong>in</strong>g caught.<br />
110. Most people will use somewhat unfair means to ga<strong>in</strong> profit or an<br />
advantage rather than to lose it.<br />
120. I frequently f<strong>in</strong>d it necessary to stand up for what I th<strong>in</strong>k is right.<br />
153. I like to know some important people because it makes me feel important.<br />
189. I like to flirt.<br />
212. I have at times stood <strong>in</strong> the way of people who are try<strong>in</strong>g to do someth<strong>in</strong>g,<br />
not because it amounted to much but because of the pr<strong>in</strong>ciple of the th<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
226. Sometimes without any reason or even when th<strong>in</strong>gs are go<strong>in</strong>g wrong I<br />
feel excitedly happy, "on top of the world."<br />
248. I do not blame a person for tak<strong>in</strong>g advantage of people who leave<br />
themselves open to it.<br />
250. At times I have been so enterta<strong>in</strong>ed by the cleverness of some crim<strong>in</strong>als<br />
that I have hoped they would get away with it.<br />
271. I th<strong>in</strong>k that I feel more <strong>in</strong>tensely than most people do.<br />
315. I tend to be on my guard with people who are somewhat more friendly<br />
than I had expected.<br />
323. Sometimes I enjoy hurt<strong>in</strong>g persons I love.<br />
354. I try to remember good stories to pass them on to other people.<br />
385. I am not afraid of fire.<br />
386. I have sometimes stayed away from another person because I feared<br />
do<strong>in</strong>g or say<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g that I might regret afterwards.<br />
406. I would certa<strong>in</strong>ly enjoy beat<strong>in</strong>g crim<strong>in</strong>als at their own game.<br />
414. I have at times had to be rough with people who were rude and annoy<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
417. I would like to be an auto racer.<br />
419. There are certa<strong>in</strong> people whom I dislike so much that I am <strong>in</strong>wardly<br />
pleased when they are catch<strong>in</strong>g it for someth<strong>in</strong>g they have done.<br />
423. I am often <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed to go out of my way to w<strong>in</strong> a po<strong>in</strong>t with someone<br />
who has opposed me.<br />
443. I do not try to cover up my poor op<strong>in</strong>ion or pity people so that they<br />
won't know how I feel.<br />
452. I strongly defend my own op<strong>in</strong>ions as a rule.<br />
456. I would like to wear expensive clothes.<br />
Items scored 'false.'<br />
_____ 4. I th<strong>in</strong>k I would like the work of a librarian.
____ 70. I am easily downed <strong>in</strong> an argument.<br />
____ 100. I have never done anyth<strong>in</strong>g dangerous for the thrill of it.<br />
____ 236. If I were an artist I would like to draw flowers.<br />
____ 343. I enjoy children.<br />
____ 426. I used to like to play hopscotch and jump rope.*<br />
____ TOTAL (Grandiosity-Exhibitionism)<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 129
APPENDIX O<br />
Narcissism-Hypersensitivity Narcissism Scale<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 130
Items scored 'true.'<br />
________<br />
________<br />
________<br />
________<br />
________<br />
________<br />
________<br />
________<br />
________<br />
Items scored 'false.'<br />
_______<br />
_______<br />
_______<br />
_______<br />
_______<br />
_______<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 131<br />
25. I would like to be a s<strong>in</strong>ger.<br />
76. It takes a lot of argument to conv<strong>in</strong>ce most people of the truth.<br />
104. Most people are honest chiefly because they are afraid of be<strong>in</strong>g caught.<br />
194. I have never had any break<strong>in</strong>g out on my sk<strong>in</strong> that has worried me.<br />
196. I frequently f<strong>in</strong>d myself worry<strong>in</strong>g about someth<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
205. Some of my family have habits that bother and annoy me very much.<br />
219. I have been disappo<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong> love.<br />
268. I wish I were not bothered by thoughts about sex.<br />
271. I th<strong>in</strong>k that I feel more <strong>in</strong>tensely than most people do.<br />
63. My feel<strong>in</strong>gs are not easily hurt.<br />
121. I have never <strong>in</strong>dulged <strong>in</strong> any unusual practices.<br />
177. My hands have not become clumsy and awkward.<br />
184. I daydream very little.<br />
237. It does not bother me that I am not better look<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
239. I am entirely self-confident.<br />
_______ TOTAL (Vulnerability-Sensitivity
APPENDIX P<br />
Narcissistic Personality Disorder Narcissism Scale<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 132
Narcissism and pedophilia 133<br />
Items scored 'true.'<br />
_______ 16. Once <strong>in</strong> awhile I th<strong>in</strong>k of th<strong>in</strong>gs too bad to talk about.<br />
_______ 130. I certa<strong>in</strong>ly feel useless at times.<br />
_______ 136. It makes me <strong>in</strong>patient to have people ask my advice or otherwise <strong>in</strong>terrupt<br />
me when I am work<strong>in</strong>g on someth<strong>in</strong>g important.<br />
_______ 180. There is someth<strong>in</strong>g wrong with my m<strong>in</strong>d.<br />
_______ 273. Life is a stra<strong>in</strong> for me much of the time.<br />
_______ 299. I cannot keep my m<strong>in</strong>d on one th<strong>in</strong>g.<br />
_______ 305. I often feel as if th<strong>in</strong>gs were not real.<br />
_______ 415. I worry quite a bit over possibly misfortunes.<br />
Items scored 'false.'<br />
_______ 12. My sex life is satisfactory.<br />
_______ 14. I enjoy detective or mystery stories.<br />
_______ 125. I believe that my home life is as pleasant as that of most people I know.<br />
_______ 164. I seldom or never have dizzy spells.<br />
_______ 343. I enjoy children<br />
_______ TOTAL (Vulnerability-Sensitivity)
APPENDIX Q<br />
Informed Consent Document for the Control Pedophile Group<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 134
Narcissism and pedophilia 135<br />
I, ________________________, agree to voluntarily participate <strong>in</strong> this<br />
research project. I have been <strong>in</strong>formed that the purpose of this study is to ga<strong>in</strong> further<br />
<strong>in</strong>formation as to the assessment and treatment of sexual disorders.<br />
I understand that I will be required to complete a demographic <strong>in</strong>formation sheet and<br />
one (1) true/false questionnaire. I agree to answer all questions as truthfully as possible. I<br />
acknowledge that most people can complete the questionnaire <strong>in</strong> approximately two hours;<br />
however, some <strong>in</strong>dividuals may require a shorter or longer period of time. I have been<br />
<strong>in</strong>formed that any document <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g my agreement to participate <strong>in</strong> this study will be kept<br />
separate from all test<strong>in</strong>g material. Therefore, my responses will rema<strong>in</strong> anonymous. I am<br />
aware that all of my completed material will be given a randomly assigned code number to<br />
ensure confidentiality. I agree that any <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed from this research project may<br />
be used <strong>in</strong> any way thought best for publication or education, provided that the procedures<br />
for confidentiality are upheld.<br />
I understand that there is no personal risk or discomfort directly <strong>in</strong>volved with this<br />
research and that I may choose to withdraw my consent and discont<strong>in</strong>ue participation <strong>in</strong><br />
this project at any time.<br />
If I have any questions <strong>in</strong> connection with my participation <strong>in</strong> this study, I should<br />
contact Kathryn Savitz or Dr. Ruth Stemberger <strong>in</strong> the <strong>Loyola</strong> <strong>College</strong> Psychology<br />
Department at (410) 617-2969.<br />
Date<br />
Signature of Participant<br />
Date Signature of Investigator
APPENDIX R<br />
Informed Consent Document for the Control Priest Group<br />
Narcissism and pedophilia 136
Narcissism and pedophilia 137<br />
I, __________________, agree to voluntarily participate <strong>in</strong> this research<br />
project. I have been <strong>in</strong>formed that the purpose of this study is to compare personality<br />
characteristics of active priests to priests who are be<strong>in</strong>g treated for psychiatric disorders. I<br />
acknowledge that my assistance could benefit <strong>in</strong>patient treatment programs and could identify<br />
potential risk factors associated with psychological difficulties among certa<strong>in</strong> priests.<br />
I understand that I will be required to complete a demographic <strong>in</strong>formation sheet and<br />
one (1) true/false questionnaire. I agree to answer all questions as truthfully as possible. I<br />
acknowledge that most people can complete the questionnaire <strong>in</strong> approximately two hours;<br />
however, some <strong>in</strong>dividuals may require a shorter or longer period of time. I have been<br />
<strong>in</strong>formed that any document <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g my agreement to participate <strong>in</strong> this study will be kept<br />
separate from all test<strong>in</strong>g material. Therefore, my responses will rema<strong>in</strong> anonymous. I am<br />
aware that all of my completed material will be given a randomly assigned code number to<br />
ensure confidentiality. I agree that any <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed from this research project may<br />
be used <strong>in</strong> any way thought best for publication or education, provided that the procedures<br />
for confidentiality are upheld.<br />
I understand that there is no personal risk or discomfort directly <strong>in</strong>volved with this<br />
research and that I may choose to withdraw my consent and discont<strong>in</strong>ue participation <strong>in</strong> this<br />
project at any time.<br />
If I have any questions that arise <strong>in</strong> connection with my participation <strong>in</strong> this study, I<br />
should contact Kathryn Savitz or Dr. Ruth Stemberger <strong>in</strong> the <strong>Loyola</strong> <strong>College</strong> Psychology<br />
Department at (410) 617- 2696.<br />
Date<br />
Signature of Participant<br />
Date Signature of Investigator