29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Friday Afternoon Papers 103–109<br />

answer, regardless <strong>of</strong> whe<strong>the</strong>r that answer is correct or incorrect. This<br />

suggests that familiarity serves to focus attention on potential answers<br />

to guide retrieval attempts.<br />

4:50–5:05 (103)<br />

on <strong>the</strong> Resilience <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Standard interpretation <strong>of</strong> processing<br />

Fluency. DEANNE L. WESTERMAN, JUSTIN M. OLDS, & BRIAN P.<br />

KURILLA, Binghamton University<strong>—</strong>Stimuli that are processed fluently<br />

tend to be regarded as familiar and are more likely to be classified as<br />

old on a recognition test. Recently, it has been shown that <strong>the</strong> relationship<br />

between fluency and positive recognition judgments can be easily<br />

reversed (Unkelbach, 2006, 2007). When participants are given a brief<br />

training phase in which previously studied stimuli are associated with<br />

lower levels <strong>of</strong> fluency, and feedback is given during training, <strong>the</strong>re is a<br />

reversal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> standard fluency effect, and fluent stimuli are more likely<br />

to be classified as new. The present study investigated <strong>the</strong> resilience<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> reversed fluency effect by determining whe<strong>the</strong>r it persists across<br />

a delay between training and test or a change in context. The results<br />

show that <strong>the</strong> reversal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fluency heuristic is short-lived, and that<br />

participants revert to <strong>the</strong> standard interpretation <strong>of</strong> enhanced fluency as<br />

familiarity quite readily.<br />

5:10–5:25 (104)<br />

Context Effects in Episodic Recognition <strong>of</strong> Famous Versus Nonfamous<br />

Faces. LYNNE M. REDER, Carnegie Mellon University<strong>—</strong>Subjects<br />

viewed celebrities and unfamiliar faces that were superimposed on identifiable<br />

scenic locations (e.g., Mount Rushmore) and judged <strong>the</strong> likelihood<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> person actually visiting <strong>the</strong> location. Backgrounds were randomly<br />

assigned to be seen (1) with famous or unfamiliar faces and (2) with many<br />

faces (“Hi Fan”) or only one (“Low Fan”) during encoding. The surprise<br />

posttest asked subjects to recognize whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> face was rated earlier,<br />

regardless <strong>of</strong> whe<strong>the</strong>r or not <strong>the</strong> background was <strong>the</strong> same as before. Half<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> backgrounds were reinstated and half were swapped with ano<strong>the</strong>r<br />

face’s background. Recognition was better when <strong>the</strong> background was a<br />

reinstatement, but this advantage was modulated by <strong>the</strong> fan <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> background<br />

and by <strong>the</strong> fame <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face. An explanation for this pattern is<br />

<strong>of</strong>fered on <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>sis that familiarity stimuli are easier to associate<br />

with context. Supporting evidence is also provided.<br />

Selective Attention iii<br />

Constitution Ballroom, Friday Afternoon, 3:10–5:25<br />

Chaired by Stephen R. Mitr<strong>of</strong>f, Duke University<br />

3:10–3:25 (105)<br />

Generalized “Satisfaction <strong>of</strong> Search”: Adverse influences on Dual-<br />

Target Search Accuracy. MATHIAS S. FLECK, EHSAN SAMEI,<br />

& STEPHEN R. MITROFF, Duke University (read by Stephen R.<br />

Mitr<strong>of</strong>f)<strong>—</strong>The successful detection <strong>of</strong> a target in a radiological search<br />

can reduce <strong>the</strong> detectability <strong>of</strong> a second target, a phenomenon termed<br />

“satisfaction <strong>of</strong> search” (SOS). Here, we investigate <strong>the</strong> generality <strong>of</strong><br />

SOS to simultaneously inform radiology, cognitive psychology, and nonmedical,<br />

real-world searches. Our experiments, utilizing nonmedical<br />

searches and untrained searchers, suggest that SOS is affected by several<br />

factors, including (1) relative salience and frequency <strong>of</strong> different target<br />

types, (2) time pressure, (3) perceptual set, (4) search instructions, and<br />

(5) reward pressure. Collectively, SOS arises when searchers have an<br />

expectation about <strong>the</strong> likelihood <strong>of</strong> a target type and when <strong>the</strong>y are under<br />

pressure to perform quickly. This first demonstration <strong>of</strong> SOS outside <strong>of</strong><br />

radiology might implicate a default heuristic that could affect search<br />

broadly. For example, <strong>the</strong> present data suggest that <strong>the</strong> detection <strong>of</strong> easyto-spot<br />

targets in baggage screening (e.g., water bottles) might reduce<br />

detection <strong>of</strong> hard-to-spot targets (e.g., box cutters).<br />

3:30–3:45 (106)<br />

perceptual Load Theory: much Ado About Nothing. YEHOSHUA<br />

TSAL & HANNA BENONI, Tel Aviv University<strong>—</strong>In a critical discussion<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory, we conclude that perceptual load is not a major factor<br />

in attentional selection. We will focus on <strong>the</strong> following problems:<br />

16<br />

(1) “Perceptual load” has never been defined and its manipulations have<br />

been guided by intuitions. (2) The hypo<strong>the</strong>tical construct “perceptual<br />

load” and its operationalization, “display size,” have <strong>of</strong>ten been confused,<br />

leading to puzzling interpretations. (3) The major tenets <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>ory are based on incoherent assumptions ra<strong>the</strong>r than testable hypo<strong>the</strong>ses.<br />

(4) The <strong>the</strong>ory makes opposite predictions for perceptual load manipulations<br />

and sensory (or cognitive) load manipulations. Given that<br />

<strong>the</strong> distinctions between <strong>the</strong> above concepts are <strong>of</strong>ten fuzzy, any possible<br />

result could “support” <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory by assigning a particular load to<br />

a particular pattern <strong>of</strong> results obtained. (5) In <strong>the</strong> major manipulation,<br />

display size, perceptual load is completely confounded with dilution.<br />

The latter factor is responsible for <strong>the</strong> effects traditionally attributed to<br />

perceptual load.<br />

3:50–4:05 (107)<br />

Target–Distractor Competition influences <strong>the</strong> Compatibility Effect<br />

under Low perceptual Load. YEI-YU YEH, YEN-HO CHEN, &<br />

CHENG-TA YANG, National Taiwan University<strong>—</strong>Three experiments<br />

were conducted to investigate <strong>the</strong> context in which a task-irrelevant distractor<br />

is processed to <strong>the</strong> late stage <strong>of</strong> response selection under low<br />

perceptual load. The results <strong>of</strong> Experiment 1 showed that <strong>the</strong> compatibility<br />

effect was reduced with an increasing number <strong>of</strong> redundant targets<br />

and was eliminated with five targets. The results <strong>of</strong> Experiments 2<br />

and 3 showed that four redundant targets eliminated <strong>the</strong> compatibility<br />

effect caused by three redundant distractors only when <strong>the</strong> distractors<br />

were not perceptually grouped and <strong>the</strong> focus <strong>of</strong> attention could hence be<br />

widened. A hierarchical diffusion modeling approach provided converging<br />

evidence, showing that distractor compatibility influenced how fast<br />

information accumulated toward a correct decision. This compatibility<br />

effect was eliminated when five redundant targets won over a singleton<br />

distractor or when four redundant targets won over three grouped redundant<br />

distractors. The results suggested that target–distractor competition<br />

for recognition and selection in visual short-term memory underlies <strong>the</strong><br />

compatibility effect.<br />

4:10–4:25 (108)<br />

Stimulus-Driven Capture by Abrupt onsets: Evidence Against<br />

Nonspatial Filtering. JAN THEEUWES, DANIEL SCHREIJ, &<br />

CHRISTIAN N. L. OLIVERS, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam<strong>—</strong>Whe<strong>the</strong>r<br />

attentional capture is contingent on top-down control settings or is involuntarily<br />

driven by salient stimuli is still widely debated. Schreij, Owens,<br />

and Theeuwes (2008) demonstrated that an onset distractor caused a<br />

response delay even when participants adopted an attentional set for<br />

a color feature. This contradicts <strong>the</strong> contingent capture hypo<strong>the</strong>sis and<br />

is consistent with stimulus-driven attentional capture. However, Folk,<br />

Remington, and Wu (2009) claimed that this delay does not reflect capture,<br />

but ra<strong>the</strong>r nonspatial filtering costs. The present study shows that<br />

<strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> abrupt onsets is in fact spatially modulated, in that <strong>the</strong> onset<br />

(1) interferes more when presented close to a target, (2) generates inhibition<br />

<strong>of</strong> return specific for its location, and (3) speeds up performance<br />

when coinciding with <strong>the</strong> target location. Moreover, adding onsets to<br />

<strong>the</strong> display has similar effects as does adding attentional-set-contingent<br />

distractors. We conclude that onsets capture attention independently <strong>of</strong><br />

attentional set.<br />

4:30–4:45 (109)<br />

Does Feature Detection Require Focal Attention? HOWARD E.<br />

EGETH & JEFFREY MOHER, Johns Hopkins University<strong>—</strong>Feature integration<br />

<strong>the</strong>ory suggests that a feature can be detected preattentively on<br />

<strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> activity in a hypo<strong>the</strong>tical feature map. Subjects responded<br />

to <strong>the</strong> presence or absence <strong>of</strong> a single red letter in a circle <strong>of</strong> gray letters<br />

surrounding fixation. Subjects were faster to indicate <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> a<br />

red letter when that letter’s identity was repeated from <strong>the</strong> previous trial,<br />

even though identity was task irrelevant. This suggests that focal attention<br />

was directed to <strong>the</strong> letter, even in a simple feature detection task (cf.<br />

Theeuwes, Van der Burg, & Belopolsky, 2008). However, we found no<br />

repetition priming in a similar task with unfamiliar characters instead<br />

<strong>of</strong> English letters, or with heterogeneous displays in which <strong>the</strong> target<br />

color was not a singleton. These and related data suggest that a simple

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!