11.07.2015 Views

New Drawing on the Right Side of the Brain

New Drawing on the Right Side of the Brain

New Drawing on the Right Side of the Brain

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong>


Also by <strong>the</strong> author:<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Artist Within


The <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong>Betty EdwardsJeremy P. Tarcher/Putnama member <strong>of</strong>Penguin Putnam Inc.<str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York


Most Tarcher/Putnam books are available at special quantity discounts for bulkpurchase for sales promoti<strong>on</strong>s, premiums, fund-raising, and educati<strong>on</strong>al needs.Special books or book excerpts also can be created to fit specific needs. For details,write Putnam Special Markets, 375 Huds<strong>on</strong> Street, <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York, NY 10014.Jeremy P. Tarcher/Putnama member <strong>of</strong>Penguin Putnam Inc.375 Huds<strong>on</strong> Street<str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York, NY 10014www.penguinputnam.comCopyright © 1979,1989,1999 by Betty EdwardsAll rights reserved. This book, or parts <strong>the</strong>re<strong>of</strong>, may not be reproduced in any formwithout permissi<strong>on</strong>.Published simultaneously in CanadaLibrary <strong>of</strong> C<strong>on</strong>gress Cataloging-in-Publicati<strong>on</strong> DataEdwards, Betty.The new drawing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> right side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain / Betty Edwards.—Rev. and expanded ed.p. cm.Rev. and expanded ed. <strong>of</strong>: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> right side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain.Includes bibliographical references.ISBN 0-87477-419-5 (hardcover). — ISBN 0-87477-424-1 (pbk.)1. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>—Technique. 2. Visual percepti<strong>on</strong>. 3. Cerebral dominance.I. Edwards, Betty. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> right side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain. II. Title. III. Title:<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> right side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain.NC.730.E34 199999-35809 CIP741.2—dc2iCover drawing: Betty EdwardsInstructi<strong>on</strong>al drawings: Betty Edwards and Brian BomeislerDesign:Joe MolloyTypeset in M<strong>on</strong>otype Jans<strong>on</strong> by M<strong>on</strong>do Typo, Inc.Printed in <strong>the</strong> United States <strong>of</strong> America40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33This book is printed <strong>on</strong> acid-free paper. ®


To <strong>the</strong> memory <strong>of</strong> my fa<strong>the</strong>r,who sharpened my drawing pencilswith his pocketknifewhen I was a child


C<strong>on</strong>tentsPrefaceXIntroducti<strong>on</strong>XVII1. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> and <strong>the</strong> Art <strong>of</strong> Bicycle RidingI2: The <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> Exercises: One Step at a TimeII3. Your <strong>Brain</strong>: The <strong>Right</strong> and Left <strong>of</strong> It274. Crossing Over: Experiencing <strong>the</strong> Shift from Left to <strong>Right</strong>495. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> Memories: Your History as an Artist676. Getting Around Your Symbol System: Meeting Edges and C<strong>on</strong>tours877. Perceiving <strong>the</strong> Shape <strong>of</strong> a Space: The Positive Aspects <strong>of</strong> Negative Space115


8. Relati<strong>on</strong>ships in a <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> Mode: Putting Sighting in Perspective1379. Facing Forward: Portrait <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> with Ease16110. The Value <strong>of</strong> Logical Lights and Shadows19311. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Beauty <strong>of</strong> Color22912. The Zen <strong>of</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> Out <strong>the</strong> Artist Within247Afterword: Is Beautiful Handwriting a Lost Art?253Postscript267Glossary275Bibliography279 .Index 283


PrefaceTwenty years have passed since <strong>the</strong> first publicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> <strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong> in July 1979. Ten years ago, in 1989,I revised <strong>the</strong> book and published a sec<strong>on</strong>d editi<strong>on</strong>, bringing it upto date with what I had learned during that decade. Now, in 1999,I am revising <strong>the</strong> book <strong>on</strong>e more time. This latest revisi<strong>on</strong> representsa culminati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> my lifel<strong>on</strong>g engrossment in drawing as aquintessentially human activity.How I came to write this bookOver <strong>the</strong> years, many people have asked me how I came to writethis book. As <strong>of</strong>ten happens, it was <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> numerous chanceevents and seemingly random choices. First, my training andbackground were in fine arts—drawing and painting, not in arteducati<strong>on</strong>. This point is important, I think, because I came toteaching with a different set <strong>of</strong> expectati<strong>on</strong>s.After a modest try at living <strong>the</strong> artist's life, I began giving privateless<strong>on</strong>s in painting and drawing in my studio to help pay <strong>the</strong>bills. Then, needing a steadier source <strong>of</strong> income, I returned toUCLA to earn a teaching credential. On completi<strong>on</strong>, I beganteaching at Venice High School in Los Angeles. It was a marvelousjob. We had a small art department <strong>of</strong> five teachers andlively, bright, challenging, and difficult students. Art was <strong>the</strong>irfavorite subject, it seemed, and our students <strong>of</strong>ten swept up manyawards in <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>n-popular citywide art c<strong>on</strong>tests.At Venice High, we tried to reach students in <strong>the</strong>ir first year,quickly teach <strong>the</strong>m to draw well, and <strong>the</strong>n train <strong>the</strong>m up, almostlike athletes, for <strong>the</strong> art competiti<strong>on</strong>s during <strong>the</strong>ir junior andsenior years. (I now have serious reservati<strong>on</strong>s about student c<strong>on</strong>-XPREFACE


tests, but at <strong>the</strong> time <strong>the</strong>y provided great motivati<strong>on</strong> and, perhapsbecause <strong>the</strong>re were so many winners, apparently caused littleharm.)Those five years at Venice High started my puzzlement aboutdrawing. As <strong>the</strong> newest teacher <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> group, I was assigned <strong>the</strong>job <strong>of</strong> bringing <strong>the</strong> students up to speed in drawing. Unlike manyart educators who believe that ability to draw well is dependent<strong>on</strong> inborn talent, I expected that all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> students would learn todraw. I was ast<strong>on</strong>ished by how difficult <strong>the</strong>y found drawing, nomatter how hard I tried to teach <strong>the</strong>m and <strong>the</strong>y tried to learn.I would <strong>of</strong>ten ask myself, "Why is it that <strong>the</strong>se students, whoI know are learning o<strong>the</strong>r skills, have so much trouble learning todraw something that is right in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir eyes?" I would sometimesquiz <strong>the</strong>m, asking a student who was having difficulty drawinga still-life setup, "Can you see in <strong>the</strong> still-life here <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> tablethat <strong>the</strong> orange is in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> vase?" "Yes," replied <strong>the</strong> student,"I see that." "Well," I said, "in your drawing, you have <strong>the</strong> orangeand <strong>the</strong> vase occupying <strong>the</strong> same space." The student answered,"Yes, I know. I didn't know how to draw that." "Well," I would saycarefully, "you look at <strong>the</strong> still-life and you draw it as you see it.""I was looking at it," <strong>the</strong> student replied. "I just didn't know howto draw that." "Well," I would say, voice rising, "you just look atit..." The resp<strong>on</strong>se would come, "I am looking at it," and so <strong>on</strong>.Ano<strong>the</strong>r puzzlement was that students <strong>of</strong>ten seemed to "get"how to draw suddenly ra<strong>the</strong>r than acquiring skills gradually.Again, I questi<strong>on</strong>ed <strong>the</strong>m: "How come you can draw this weekwhen you couldn't draw last week?" Often <strong>the</strong> reply would be, "Id<strong>on</strong>'t know. I'm just seeing things differently." "In what way differently?"I would ask. "I can't say—just differently." I would pursue<strong>the</strong> point, urging students to put it into words, without success.Usually students ended by saying, "I just can't describe it."In frustrati<strong>on</strong>, I began to observe myself: What was I doingwhen I was drawing? Some things quickly showed up—that Icouldn't talk and draw at <strong>the</strong> same time, for example, and thatI lost track <strong>of</strong> time while drawing. My puzzlement c<strong>on</strong>tinued.PREFACEXI


One day, <strong>on</strong> impulse, I asked <strong>the</strong> students to copy a Picassodrawing upside down. That small experiment, more than anythingelse I had tried, showed that something very different isgoing <strong>on</strong> during <strong>the</strong> act <strong>of</strong> drawing. To my surprise, and to <strong>the</strong>students' surprise, <strong>the</strong> finished drawings were so extremely welld<strong>on</strong>e that I asked <strong>the</strong> class, "How come you can draw upsidedown when you can't draw right-side up?" The studentsresp<strong>on</strong>ded, "Upside down, we didn't know what we were drawing."This was <strong>the</strong> greatest puzzlement <strong>of</strong> all and left me simplybaffled.During <strong>the</strong> following year, 1968, first reports <strong>of</strong> psychobiologistRoger W. Sperry's research <strong>on</strong> human brain-hemispherefuncti<strong>on</strong>s, for which he later received a Nobel Prize, appeared in<strong>the</strong> press. Reading Sperry's work caused in me something <strong>of</strong> anAh-ha! experience. His stunning finding, that <strong>the</strong> human brainuses two fundamentally different modes <strong>of</strong> thinking, <strong>on</strong>e verbal,analytic, and sequential and <strong>on</strong>e visual, perceptual, and simultaneous,seemed to cast light <strong>on</strong> my questi<strong>on</strong>s about drawing. Theidea that <strong>on</strong>e is shifting to a different-from-usual way <strong>of</strong> thinking/seeingfitted my own experience <strong>of</strong> drawing and illuminatedmy observati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> my students.Avidly, I read everything I could find about Sperry's work anddid my best to explain to my students its possible relati<strong>on</strong>ship todrawing. They too became interested in <strong>the</strong> problems <strong>of</strong> drawingand so<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>y were achieving great advances in <strong>the</strong>ir drawingskills.I was working <strong>on</strong> my master's degree in Art at <strong>the</strong> time andrealized that if I wanted to seriously search for an educati<strong>on</strong>alapplicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> Sperry's work in <strong>the</strong> field <strong>of</strong> drawing, I would needfur<strong>the</strong>r study. Even though by that time I was teaching full time atLos Angeles Trade Technical College, I decided to return yetagain to UCLA for a doctoral degree. For <strong>the</strong> following threeyears, I attended evening classes that combined <strong>the</strong> fields <strong>of</strong> art,psychology, and educati<strong>on</strong>. The subject <strong>of</strong> my doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>was "Perceptual Skills in <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>," using upside-downdrawing as an experimental variable. After receiving my doctoraldegree in 1976, I began teaching drawing in <strong>the</strong> art department otXIIPREFACE


California State University, L<strong>on</strong>g Beach. I needed a drawing texthookthat included Sperry's research. During <strong>the</strong> next three yearsI wrote <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong>.Since <strong>the</strong> book was first published in 1979, <strong>the</strong> ideas I expressedabout learning to draw have become surprisingly widespread,much to my amazement and delight. I feel h<strong>on</strong>ored by <strong>the</strong> manyforeign language translati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><strong>Brain</strong>. Even more surprising, individuals and groups working infields not remotely c<strong>on</strong>nected with drawing have found ways touse <strong>the</strong> ideas in my book. A few examples will indicate <strong>the</strong> diversity:nursing schools, drama workshops, corporate training seminars,sports-coaching schools, real-estate marketing associati<strong>on</strong>s,psychologists, counselors <strong>of</strong> delinquent youths, writers, hair stylists,even a school for training private investigators. College anduniversity art teachers across <strong>the</strong> nati<strong>on</strong> also have incorporatedmany <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> techniques into <strong>the</strong>ir teaching repertoires.Public-school teachers are also using my book. After twentyfiveyears <strong>of</strong> budget cuts in schools' arts programs, I am happy toreport that state departments <strong>of</strong> educati<strong>on</strong> and public schoolboards <strong>of</strong> educati<strong>on</strong> are starting to turn to <strong>the</strong> arts as <strong>on</strong>e way tohelp repair our failing educati<strong>on</strong>al systems. Educati<strong>on</strong>al administrators,however, tend to be ambivalent about <strong>the</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong>including <strong>the</strong> arts, <strong>of</strong>ten still relegating arts educati<strong>on</strong> to "enrichment."This term's hidden meaning is "valuable but not essential."My view, in c<strong>on</strong>trast, is that <strong>the</strong> arts are essential for trainingspecific, visual, perceptual ways <strong>of</strong> thinking, just as <strong>the</strong> "3 R's" areessential for training specific, verbal, numerical, analytical ways<strong>of</strong> thinking. I believe that both thinking modes—<strong>on</strong>e to comprehend<strong>the</strong> details and <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r to "see" <strong>the</strong> whole picture, forexample, are vital for critical-thinking skills, extrapolati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>meaning, and problem solving.To help public-school administrators see <strong>the</strong> utility <strong>of</strong> artseducati<strong>on</strong>, I believe we must find new ways to teach students howto transfer skills learned through <strong>the</strong> arts to academic subjectsand problem solving. Transfer <strong>of</strong> learning is traditi<strong>on</strong>allyregarded as a most difficult kind <strong>of</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong> and, unfortunately,transfer is <strong>of</strong>ten left to chance. Teachers hope that students willSUGGESTED DESIGNFOR U.S. CAPITOL3. WORKINC DRAWING FROMWHICH THE ORIGINALPHONOCRAPH WAS BUILTIn <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> inventi<strong>on</strong>s, manycreative ideas began with smallsketches. The examples above areby Galileo, Jeffers<strong>on</strong>, Faraday, andEdis<strong>on</strong>.Henning Nelms, Thinking With aPencil, <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Ten Speed Press,1981, p. xiv.PREFACEXIII


"get" <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>, say, between learning to draw and "seeing"soluti<strong>on</strong>s to problems, or between learning English grammar andlogical, sequential thinking.Corporate training seminars"Analog" drawings are purelyexpressive drawings, with no namableobjects depicted, using <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong>expressive quality <strong>of</strong> line—or lines.Unexpectedly, pers<strong>on</strong>s untrained inart are able to use this language—that is, produce expressive drawings—andare also able to read <strong>the</strong>drawings for meaning. The drawingless<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> seminar's first segmentare used mainly to increaseartistic self-c<strong>on</strong>fidence and c<strong>on</strong>fidencein <strong>the</strong> efficacy <strong>of</strong> analogdrawing.My work with various corporati<strong>on</strong>s represents, I believe, <strong>on</strong>easpect <strong>of</strong> transfer <strong>of</strong> learning, in this instance, from drawing skillsto a specific kind <strong>of</strong> problem solving sought by corporate executives.Depending <strong>on</strong> how much corporate time is available, atypical seminar takes three days: a day and a half focused <strong>on</strong>developing drawing skills and <strong>the</strong> remaining time devoted tousing drawing for problem solving.Groups vary in size but most <strong>of</strong>ten number about twenty-five.Problems can be very specific ("What is_________________?"—a specific chemical problem that had troubled a particular companyfor several years) or very general ("What is our relati<strong>on</strong>shipwith our customers?") or something in between specific and general("How can members <strong>of</strong> our special unit work toge<strong>the</strong>r moreproductively?").The first day and a half <strong>of</strong> drawing exercises includes <strong>the</strong>less<strong>on</strong>s in this book through <strong>the</strong> drawing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hand. The tw<strong>of</strong>oldobjective <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing less<strong>on</strong>s is to present <strong>the</strong> five perceptualstrategies emphasized in <strong>the</strong> book and to dem<strong>on</strong>strate eachparticipant's potential artistic capabilities, given effective instructi<strong>on</strong>.The problem-solving segment begins with exercises in usingdrawing to think with. These exercises, called analog drawings,are described in my book <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Artist Within. Participantsuse <strong>the</strong> so-called "language <strong>of</strong> line," first to draw out <strong>the</strong> problemand <strong>the</strong>n to make visible possible soluti<strong>on</strong>s. These expressivedrawings become <strong>the</strong> vehicle for group discussi<strong>on</strong> and analysis,guided, but not led, by me. Participants use <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>of</strong> edges(boundaries), negative spaces (<strong>of</strong>ten called "white spaces" in businessparlance), relati<strong>on</strong>ships (parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> problem viewed proporti<strong>on</strong>allyand "in perspective"), lights and shadows (extrapolati<strong>on</strong>from <strong>the</strong> known to <strong>the</strong> as-yet unknown), and <strong>the</strong> gestaltXIVPREFACE


<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> problem (how <strong>the</strong> parts fit—or d<strong>on</strong>'t fit—toge<strong>the</strong>r).The problem-solving segment c<strong>on</strong>cludes with an extendedsmall drawing <strong>of</strong> an object, different for each participant, whichhas been chosen as somehow related to <strong>the</strong> problem at hand. Thisdrawing, combining perceptual skills with problem solving,evokes an extended shift to an alternate mode <strong>of</strong> thinking which Ihave termed "R-mode," during which <strong>the</strong> participant focuses <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> problem under discussi<strong>on</strong> while also c<strong>on</strong>centrating <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>drawing. The group <strong>the</strong>n explores insights derived from thisprocess.The results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> seminars have been sometimes startling,sometimes almost amusing in terms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> obviousness <strong>of</strong> engenderedsoluti<strong>on</strong>s. An example <strong>of</strong> a startling result was a surprisingrevelati<strong>on</strong> experienced by <strong>the</strong> group working <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> chemicalproblem. It turned out that <strong>the</strong> group had so enjoyed <strong>the</strong>ir specialstatus and favored positi<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong>y were so intrigued by <strong>the</strong> fascinatingproblem that <strong>the</strong>y were in no hurry to solve it. Also, solving<strong>the</strong> problem would mean breaking up <strong>the</strong> group andreturning to more humdrum work. All <strong>of</strong> this showed up clearlyin <strong>the</strong>ir drawings. The curious thing was that <strong>the</strong> group leaderexclaimed, "I thought that might be what was going <strong>on</strong>, but I justdidn't believe it!" The soluti<strong>on</strong>? The group realized that <strong>the</strong>yneeded—and welcomed—a serious deadline and assurance thato<strong>the</strong>r, equally interesting problems awaited <strong>the</strong>m.Ano<strong>the</strong>r surprising result came in resp<strong>on</strong>se to <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>about customer relati<strong>on</strong>s. Participants' drawings in that seminarwere c<strong>on</strong>sistently complex and detailed. Nearly every drawingrepresented customers as small objects floating in large emptyspaces. Areas <strong>of</strong> great complexity excluded <strong>the</strong>se small objects.The ensuing discussi<strong>on</strong> clarified <strong>the</strong> group's (unc<strong>on</strong>scious) indifferencetoward and inattenti<strong>on</strong> to customers. That raised o<strong>the</strong>rquesti<strong>on</strong>s: What was in all <strong>of</strong> that empty negative space, and howcould <strong>the</strong> complex areas (identified in discussi<strong>on</strong> as aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>work that were more interesting to <strong>the</strong> group) make c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>with customer c<strong>on</strong>cerns? This group planned to explore <strong>the</strong>problem fur<strong>the</strong>r.PREFACExv


Krishnamurti: "So where doessilence begin? Does it begin whenthought ends? Have you ever triedto end thought?"Questi<strong>on</strong>er: "How do you do it?"Krishnamurti: "I d<strong>on</strong>'t know, buthave you ever tried it? First <strong>of</strong> all,who is <strong>the</strong> entity who is trying tostop thought?"Questi<strong>on</strong>er: "The thinker."Krishnamurti: "It's ano<strong>the</strong>r thought,isn't it? Thought is trying to stopitself, so <strong>the</strong>re is a battle between<strong>the</strong> thinker and <strong>the</strong> thought....Thought says, 'I must stop thinkingbecause <strong>the</strong>n I shall experience amarvelous state.'... One thought istrying to suppress ano<strong>the</strong>r thought,so <strong>the</strong>re is c<strong>on</strong>flict. When I see thisas a fact, see it totally, understandit completely, have an insight intoit... <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> mind is quiet. Thiscomes about naturally and easilywhen <strong>the</strong> mind is quiet to watch, tolook, to see."—J. KrishnamurtiYou Are <strong>the</strong> World, 1972The group seeking more productive ways <strong>of</strong> workingtoge<strong>the</strong>r came to a c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that was so obvious <strong>the</strong> groupactually laughed about it. Their c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> was that <strong>the</strong>yneeded to improve communicati<strong>on</strong> within <strong>the</strong> group. Memberswere nearly all scientists holding advanced degrees in chemistryand physics. Apparently, each pers<strong>on</strong> had a specificassignment for <strong>on</strong>e part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole task, but <strong>the</strong>y worked indifferent buildings with different groups <strong>of</strong> associates and <strong>on</strong>individual time schedules. For more than twenty-five years<strong>the</strong>y had never met toge<strong>the</strong>r as a group until we held ourthree-day seminar.I hope <strong>the</strong>se examples give-at least some flavor <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> corporateseminars. Participants, <strong>of</strong> course, are highly educated,successful pr<strong>of</strong>essi<strong>on</strong>als. Working as I do with a different way <strong>of</strong>thinking, <strong>the</strong> seminars seem to enable <strong>the</strong>se highly trainedpeople to see things differently. Because <strong>the</strong> participants <strong>the</strong>mselvesgenerate <strong>the</strong> drawings, <strong>the</strong>y provide real evidence torefer to. Thus, insights are hard to dismiss and <strong>the</strong> discussi<strong>on</strong>sstay very focused.I can <strong>on</strong>ly speculate why this process works effectively toget at informati<strong>on</strong> that is <strong>of</strong>ten hidden or ignored or "explainedaway" by <strong>the</strong> language mode <strong>of</strong> thinking. I think it's possiblethat <strong>the</strong> language system (L-mode, in my terminology) regardsdrawing—especially analog drawing—as unimportant, even asjust a form <strong>of</strong> doodling. Perhaps, L-mode drops out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> task,putting its censoring functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> hold. Apparently, what <strong>the</strong>pers<strong>on</strong> knows but doesn't know at a verbal, c<strong>on</strong>scious level<strong>the</strong>refore comes pouring out in <strong>the</strong> drawings. Traditi<strong>on</strong>al executives,<strong>of</strong> course, may regard this informati<strong>on</strong> as "s<strong>of</strong>t," butI suspect that <strong>the</strong>se unspoken reacti<strong>on</strong>s do have some effect <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> ultimate success and failure <strong>of</strong> corporati<strong>on</strong>s. Broadlyspeaking, a glimpse <strong>of</strong> underlying affective dynamics probablyhelps more than it hinders.XVIPREFACE


Introducti<strong>on</strong>The subject <strong>of</strong> how people learn to draw has never lost its charmand fascinati<strong>on</strong> for me. Just when I begin to think I have a grasp<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> subject, a whole new vista or puzzlement opens up. Thisbook, <strong>the</strong>refore, is a work in progress, documenting my understandingat this time.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong>, I believe, was <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>first practical educati<strong>on</strong>al applicati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> Roger Sperry's pi<strong>on</strong>eeringinsight into <strong>the</strong> dual nature <strong>of</strong> human thinking—verbal, analyticthinking mainly located in <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere, and visual,perceptual thinking mainly located in <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere.Since 1979, many writers in o<strong>the</strong>r fields have proposed applicati<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> research, each in turn suggesting new ways toenhance both thinking modes, <strong>the</strong>reby increasing potential forpers<strong>on</strong>al growth.During <strong>the</strong> past ten years, my colleagues and I have polishedand expanded <strong>the</strong> techniques described in <strong>the</strong> original book. Wehave changed some procedures, added some, and deleted some.My main purpose in revising <strong>the</strong> book and presenting this thirdediti<strong>on</strong> is to bring <strong>the</strong> work up-to-date again for my readers.As you will see, much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> original work is retained, havingwithstood <strong>the</strong> test <strong>of</strong> time. But <strong>on</strong>e important organizing principlewas missing in <strong>the</strong> original text, for <strong>the</strong> curious reas<strong>on</strong> thatI couldn't see it until after <strong>the</strong> book was published. I want toreemphasize it here, because it forms <strong>the</strong> overall structure withinwhich <strong>the</strong> reader can see how <strong>the</strong> parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> book fit toge<strong>the</strong>r t<strong>of</strong>orm a whole. This key principle is: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is a global or"whole" skill requiring <strong>on</strong>ly a limited set <strong>of</strong> basic comp<strong>on</strong>ents.This insight came to me about six m<strong>on</strong>ths after <strong>the</strong> book waspublished, right in <strong>the</strong> middle <strong>of</strong> a sentence while teaching aINTRODUCTIONXVII


Please note that I am referring to<strong>the</strong> learning stage <strong>of</strong> basic realisticdrawing <strong>of</strong> a perceived image.There are many o<strong>the</strong>r kinds <strong>of</strong>drawing: abstracti<strong>on</strong>, n<strong>on</strong>objectivedrawing, imaginative drawing,mechanical drawing, and so forth.Also, drawing can be defined inmany o<strong>the</strong>r ways—by mediums,historic styles, or <strong>the</strong> artist's intent.group <strong>of</strong> students. It was <strong>the</strong> classic Ah-ha! experience, with <strong>the</strong>strange physical sensati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> rapid heartbeat, caught breath, anda sense <strong>of</strong> joyful excitement at seeing everything fall into place. Ihad been reviewing with <strong>the</strong> students <strong>the</strong> set <strong>of</strong> skills described inmy book when it hit me that this was it, <strong>the</strong>re were no more, andthat <strong>the</strong> book had a hidden c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>of</strong> which I had been unaware.I checked <strong>the</strong> insight with my colleagues and drawing experts.They agreed.Like o<strong>the</strong>r global skills—for example, reading, driving, skiing,and walking—drawing is made up <strong>of</strong> comp<strong>on</strong>ent skills thatbecome integrated into a whole skill. Once you have learned <strong>the</strong>comp<strong>on</strong>ents and have integrated <strong>the</strong>m, you can draw—just as<strong>on</strong>ce you have learned to read, you know how to read for life;<strong>on</strong>ce you have learned to walk, you know how to walk for life. Youd<strong>on</strong>'t have to go <strong>on</strong> forever adding additi<strong>on</strong>al basic skills. Progresstakes <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> practice, refinement <strong>of</strong> technique, and learningwhat to use <strong>the</strong> skills for.This was an exciting discovery because it meant that a pers<strong>on</strong>can learn to draw within a reas<strong>on</strong>ably short time. And, in fact, mycolleagues and I now teach a five-day seminar, f<strong>on</strong>dly known asour "Killer Class," which enables students to acquire <strong>the</strong> basiccomp<strong>on</strong>ent skills <strong>of</strong> realistic drawing in five days <strong>of</strong> intense learning.Five basic skills <strong>of</strong> drawingThe global skill <strong>of</strong> drawing a perceived object, pers<strong>on</strong>, landscape(something that you see "out <strong>the</strong>re") requires <strong>on</strong>ly five basic comp<strong>on</strong>entskills, no more. These skills are not drawing skills. Theyare perceptual skills, listed as follows:One:Two:<strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edges<strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> spacesThree: <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>shipsFour: <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> lights and shadowsFive:<strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole, or gestaltXVIIIINTRODUCTION


I am aware, <strong>of</strong> course, that additi<strong>on</strong>al basic skills are requiredfor imaginative, expressive drawing leading to "Art with a capitalA." Of <strong>the</strong>se, I have found two and <strong>on</strong>ly two additi<strong>on</strong>al skills:drawing from memory and drawing from imaginati<strong>on</strong>. And <strong>the</strong>reremain, naturally, many techniques <strong>of</strong> drawing—many ways <strong>of</strong>manipulating drawing mediums and endless subject matter, forexample. But, to repeat, for skillful realistic drawing <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e's percepti<strong>on</strong>s,using pencil <strong>on</strong> paper, <strong>the</strong> five skills I will teach you inthis book provide <strong>the</strong> required perceptual training.Those five basic skills are <strong>the</strong> prerequisites for effective use <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> two additi<strong>on</strong>al "advanced" skills, and <strong>the</strong> set <strong>of</strong> seven mayc<strong>on</strong>stitute <strong>the</strong> entire basic global skill <strong>of</strong> drawing. Many books <strong>on</strong>drawing actually focus mainly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> two advanced skills. Therefore,after you complete <strong>the</strong> less<strong>on</strong>s in this book, you will findample instructi<strong>on</strong> available to c<strong>on</strong>tinue learning.I need to emphasize a fur<strong>the</strong>r point: Global or whole skills,such as reading, driving, and drawing, in time become automatic.As I menti<strong>on</strong>ed above, basic comp<strong>on</strong>ent skills become completelyintegrated into <strong>the</strong> smooth flow <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> global skill. But in acquiringany new global skill, <strong>the</strong> initial learning is <strong>of</strong>ten a struggle,first with each comp<strong>on</strong>ent skill, <strong>the</strong>n with <strong>the</strong> smooth integrati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> comp<strong>on</strong>ents. Each <strong>of</strong> my students goes through this process,and so will you. As each new skill is learned, you will merge itwith those previously learned until, <strong>on</strong>e day, you are simplydrawing—just as, <strong>on</strong>e day, you found yourself simply drivingwithout thinking about how to do it. Later, <strong>on</strong>e almost forgetsabout having learned to read, learned to drive, learned to draw.In order to attain this smooth integrati<strong>on</strong> in drawing, all fivecomp<strong>on</strong>ent skills must be in place. I'm happy to say that <strong>the</strong> fifthskill, <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole, or gestalt, is nei<strong>the</strong>r taught norlearned but instead seems to emerge as a result <strong>of</strong> acquiring <strong>the</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r four skills. But <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first four, n<strong>on</strong>e can be omitted, just as •learning how to brake or steer cannot be omitted when learningto drive.In <strong>the</strong> original book, I believe I explained sufficiently well <strong>the</strong>first two skills, <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edges and <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>spaces. The importance <strong>of</strong> sighting (<strong>the</strong> third skill <strong>of</strong> perceivingThe global skill <strong>of</strong> drawingINTRODUCTIONXIX


elati<strong>on</strong>ships) however, needed greater emphasis and clearerexplanati<strong>on</strong>, because students <strong>of</strong>ten tend to give up too quickly<strong>on</strong> this complicated skill. And <strong>the</strong> fourth skill, <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>lights and shadows, also needed expanding. Most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tentchanges for this new editi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong>refore, are in <strong>the</strong> last chapters."You have two brains: a left and aright. Modern brain scientists nowknow that your left brain is yourverbal and rati<strong>on</strong>al brain; it thinksserially and reduces its thoughts t<strong>on</strong>umbers, letters, and words....Your right brain is your n<strong>on</strong>-verbaland intuitive brain; it thinks in patterns,or pictures, composed <strong>of</strong>'whole things,' and does not comprehendreducti<strong>on</strong>s, ei<strong>the</strong>r numbers,letters, or words."From The Fabric <strong>of</strong> Mind, by <strong>the</strong>eminent scientist and neurosurge<strong>on</strong>Richard Bergland. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Viking Penguin, Inc., 1985, p. 1.A basic strategy for accessing R-modeIn this editi<strong>on</strong>, I again reiterate a basic strategy for gaining accessat c<strong>on</strong>scious level to R-mode, my term for <strong>the</strong> visual, perceptualmode <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain. I c<strong>on</strong>tinue to believe that this strategy is probablymy main c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to educati<strong>on</strong>al aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> "righ<strong>the</strong>mispherestory" that began with Roger Sperry's celebratedscientific work. The strategy is stated as follows:In order to gain access to <strong>the</strong> subdominant visual, perceptualR-mode <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain, it is necessary to present <strong>the</strong> brain with a jobthat <strong>the</strong> verbal, analytic L-mode will turn down.For most <strong>of</strong> us, L-mode thinking seems easy, normal, andfamiliar (though perhaps not for many children and dyslexicindividuals). The perverse R-mode strategy, in c<strong>on</strong>trast, mayseem difficult and unfamiliar—even "<strong>of</strong>f-<strong>the</strong>-wall." It must belearned in oppositi<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> "natural" tendency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain t<strong>of</strong>avor L-mode because, in general, language dominates. By learningto c<strong>on</strong>trol this tendency for specific tasks, <strong>on</strong>e gains access topowerful brain functi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>ten obscured by language.All <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> exercises in this book, <strong>the</strong>refore, are based <strong>on</strong> twoorganizing principles and major aims. First, to teach <strong>the</strong> readerfive basic comp<strong>on</strong>ent skills <strong>of</strong> drawing and, sec<strong>on</strong>d, to providec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s that facilitate making cognitive shifts to R-mode, <strong>the</strong>thinking/seeing mode specialized for drawing.In short, in <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> learning to draw, <strong>on</strong>e also learns toc<strong>on</strong>trol (at least to some degree) <strong>the</strong> mode by which <strong>on</strong>e's ownbrain handles informati<strong>on</strong>. Perhaps this explains in part why mybook appeals to individuals from such diverse fields. Intuitively,<strong>the</strong>y see <strong>the</strong> link to o<strong>the</strong>r activities and <strong>the</strong> possibility <strong>of</strong> seeingthings differently by learning to access R-mode at c<strong>on</strong>sciouslevel.xxINTRODUCTION


Color in drawingChapter Eleven, "<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Beauty <strong>of</strong> Color," was a newchapter in <strong>the</strong> 1989 editi<strong>on</strong>, written in resp<strong>on</strong>se to many requestsfrom my readers. The chapter focuses <strong>on</strong> using color in drawing—afine transiti<strong>on</strong>al step toward painting. Over <strong>the</strong> pastdecade, my teaching staff and I have developed a five-day intensivecourse <strong>on</strong> basic color <strong>the</strong>ory, a course that is still a "work inprogress." I am still using <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cepts in <strong>the</strong> chapter <strong>on</strong> color, soI have not revised it for this editi<strong>on</strong>.I believe <strong>the</strong> logical progressi<strong>on</strong> for a pers<strong>on</strong> starting out inartistic expressi<strong>on</strong> should be as follows:From Line to Value to Color to PaintingFirst, a pers<strong>on</strong> learns <strong>the</strong> basic skills <strong>of</strong> drawing, which provideknowledge <strong>of</strong> line (learned through c<strong>on</strong>tour drawing <strong>of</strong>edges, spaces, and relati<strong>on</strong>ships) and knowledge <strong>of</strong> value (learnedthrough rendering lights and shadows). Skillful use <strong>of</strong> colorrequires first <strong>of</strong> all <strong>the</strong> ability to perceive color as value. This abilityis difficult, perhaps impossible, to acquire unless <strong>on</strong>e haslearned to perceive <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> lights and shadowsthrough drawing. I hope that my chapter introducing color indrawing will provide an effective bridge for those who want toprogress from drawing to painting.HandwritingFinally, I am retaining <strong>the</strong> brief secti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> handwriting. In manycultures, writing is regarded as an art form. Americans <strong>of</strong>tendeplore <strong>the</strong>ir handwriting but are at a loss as to how to improve it.Handwriting, however, is a form <strong>of</strong> drawing and can be improved.I regret to say that many California schools are still usinghandwriting-instructi<strong>on</strong>al methods that were failing in 1989 andare still failing today. My suggesti<strong>on</strong>s in this regard appear in <strong>the</strong>Afterword.INTRODUCTIONXXI


An empirical basis for my <strong>the</strong>oryThe underlying <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> this revised editi<strong>on</strong> remains <strong>the</strong> same:to explain in basic terms <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship <strong>of</strong> drawing to visual,perceptual brain processes and to provide methods <strong>of</strong> accessingand c<strong>on</strong>trolling <strong>the</strong>se processes. As a number <strong>of</strong> scientists havenoted, research <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> human brain is complicated by <strong>the</strong> factthat <strong>the</strong> brain is struggling to understand itself. This three-poundorgan is perhaps <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly bit <strong>of</strong> matter in <strong>the</strong> universe—at least asfar as we know—that is observing itself, w<strong>on</strong>dering about itself,trying to analyze itself, and attempting to gain better c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>of</strong>its own capabilities. This paradoxical situati<strong>on</strong> no doubt c<strong>on</strong>tributes—atleast in part—to <strong>the</strong> deep mysteries that still remain,despite rapidly expanding scientific knowledge about <strong>the</strong> brain.One questi<strong>on</strong> scientists are studying intensely is where <strong>the</strong>two major thinking modes are specifically located in <strong>the</strong> humanbrain and how <strong>the</strong> organizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> modes can vary from individualto individual. While <strong>the</strong> so-called locati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>troversy c<strong>on</strong>tinuesto engage scientists, al<strong>on</strong>g with myriad o<strong>the</strong>r areas <strong>of</strong> brainresearch, <strong>the</strong> existence in every brain <strong>of</strong> two fundamentally differentcognitive modes is no l<strong>on</strong>ger c<strong>on</strong>troversial. Corroboratingresearch since Sperry's original work is overwhelming. Moreover,even in <strong>the</strong> midst <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> argument about locati<strong>on</strong>, most scientistsagree that for a majority <strong>of</strong> individuals, informati<strong>on</strong>-processingbased primarily <strong>on</strong> linear, sequential data is mainly located in <strong>the</strong>left hemisphere, while global, perceptual data is mainlyprocessedin <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere.Clearly, for educators like myself, <strong>the</strong> precise locati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>se modes in <strong>the</strong> individual brain is not an important issue.What is important is that incoming informati<strong>on</strong> can be handled intwo fundamentally different ways and that <strong>the</strong> two modes canapparently work toge<strong>the</strong>r in a vast array <strong>of</strong> combinati<strong>on</strong>s. Since<strong>the</strong> late 1970s, I have used <strong>the</strong> terms L-mode and R-mode to try toavoid <strong>the</strong> locati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>troversy. The terms are intended to differentiate<strong>the</strong> major modes <strong>of</strong> cogniti<strong>on</strong>, regardless <strong>of</strong> where <strong>the</strong>yare located in <strong>the</strong> individual brain.Over <strong>the</strong> past decade or so, a new interdisciplinary field <strong>of</strong>XXIIINTRODUCTION


ain-functi<strong>on</strong> study has become formally known as cognitiveneuroscience. In additi<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> traditi<strong>on</strong>al discipline <strong>of</strong> neurology,cognitive neuroscience encompasses study <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r highercognitive processes such as language, memory, and percepti<strong>on</strong>.Computer scientists, linguists, neuroimaging scientists, cognitivepsychologists, and neurobiologists are all c<strong>on</strong>tributing to a growingunderstanding <strong>of</strong> how <strong>the</strong> human brain functi<strong>on</strong>s.Interest in "right brain, left brain" research has subsidedsomewhat am<strong>on</strong>g educators and <strong>the</strong> general public since RogerSperry first published his research findings. Never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong> fact<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ound asymmetry <strong>of</strong> human brain functi<strong>on</strong>s remains,becoming ever more central, for example, am<strong>on</strong>g computer scientiststrying to emulate human mental processes. Facial recogniti<strong>on</strong>,a functi<strong>on</strong> ascribed to <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere, has been soughtfor decades and is still bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> capabilities <strong>of</strong> most computers.Ray Kurzweil, in his recent book The Age <strong>of</strong> Spiritual Machines(Viking, 1999) c<strong>on</strong>trasted human and computer capability in patternseeking (as in facial recogniti<strong>on</strong>) and sequential processing(as in calculati<strong>on</strong>):The human brain has about 100 billi<strong>on</strong> neur<strong>on</strong>s. With an estimatedaverage <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e thousand c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between each neur<strong>on</strong> and itsneighbors, we have about 100 trilli<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s, each capable <strong>of</strong> asimultaneous calculati<strong>on</strong>. That's ra<strong>the</strong>r massive parallel processing,and <strong>on</strong>e key to <strong>the</strong> strength <strong>of</strong> human thinking. A pr<strong>of</strong>ound weakness,however, is <strong>the</strong> excruciatingly slow speed <strong>of</strong> neural circuitry, <strong>on</strong>ly 200calculati<strong>on</strong>s per sec<strong>on</strong>d. For problems that benefit from massive parallelism,such a neural-net-based pattern recogniti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> humanbrain does a great job. For problems that require extensive sequentialthinking, <strong>the</strong> human brain is <strong>on</strong>ly mediocre, (p. 103)In 1979, I proposed that drawing required a cognitive shift toR-mode, now postulated to be a massively parallel mode <strong>of</strong> processing,and away from L-mode, postulated to be a sequentialprocessing mode. I had no hard evidence to support my proposal,<strong>on</strong>ly my experience as an artist and a teacher. Over <strong>the</strong> years, Ihave been criticized occasi<strong>on</strong>ally by various neuroscientists foroverstepping <strong>the</strong> boundaries <strong>of</strong> my own field—though not byIn a c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> with his friendAndre Marchand, <strong>the</strong> French artistHenri Matisse described <strong>the</strong>process <strong>of</strong> passing percepti<strong>on</strong>sfrom <strong>on</strong>e way <strong>of</strong> looking toano<strong>the</strong>r:"Do you know that a man has <strong>on</strong>ly<strong>on</strong>e eye which sees and registerseverything; this eye, like a superbcamera which takes minute pictures,very sharp, tiny—and withthat picture man tells himself:'This time I know <strong>the</strong> reality <strong>of</strong>things,' and he is calm for amoment. Then, slowly superimposingitself <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture,ano<strong>the</strong>r eye makes its appearance,invisibly, which makes an entirelydifferent picture for him."Then our man no l<strong>on</strong>ger seesclearly, a struggle begins between<strong>the</strong> first and sec<strong>on</strong>d eye, <strong>the</strong> fight isfierce, finally <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d eye has<strong>the</strong> upper hand, takes over andthat's <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> it. Now it hascommand <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>deye can <strong>the</strong>n c<strong>on</strong>tinue its workal<strong>on</strong>e and elaborate its own pictureaccording to <strong>the</strong> laws <strong>of</strong> interiorvisi<strong>on</strong>. This very special eye isfound here," says Matisse, pointingto his brain.Marchand didn't menti<strong>on</strong> whichside <strong>of</strong> his brain Matisse pointedto.—J. FlamMatisse <strong>on</strong> Art, 1973INTRODUCTIONXXIII


A recent article in an educati<strong>on</strong>aljournal summarizes neuroscientists'objecti<strong>on</strong>s to "brain-basededucati<strong>on</strong>.""The fundamental problem with <strong>the</strong>right-brain versus left-brain claimsthat <strong>on</strong>e finds in educati<strong>on</strong>al literatureis that <strong>the</strong>y rely <strong>on</strong> our intuiti<strong>on</strong>sand folk <strong>the</strong>ories about <strong>the</strong>brain, ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>on</strong> what brainscience is actually able to tell us.Our folk <strong>the</strong>ories are too crudeand imprecise to have any scientificpredictive or instructi<strong>on</strong>alvalue. What modern brain scienceis telling us—and what brain-basededucators fail to appreciate—isthat it makes no scientific sense tomap gross, unanalyzed behaviorsand skills—reading, arithmetic,spatial reas<strong>on</strong>ing—<strong>on</strong>to <strong>on</strong>e brainhemisphere or ano<strong>the</strong>r."But <strong>the</strong> author also states:"Whe<strong>the</strong>r or not [brain-based]educati<strong>on</strong>al practices should beadopted must be determined <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> impact <strong>on</strong> studentlearning."—John T. Bruer"In Search <strong>of</strong>...<strong>Brain</strong>-Based Educati<strong>on</strong>,"Phi Delta Kappan, May1999, p. 603Roger Sperry, who believed that my applicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> his researchwas reas<strong>on</strong>able.What kept me working at my "folk" <strong>the</strong>ory (see <strong>the</strong> marginexcerpt) was that, when put into practice, <strong>the</strong> results were inspiring.Students <strong>of</strong> all ages made significant gains in drawing abilityand, by extensi<strong>on</strong>, in perceptual abilities, since drawing welldepends <strong>on</strong> seeing well. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> ability has always beenregarded as difficult to acquire, and has nearly always been additi<strong>on</strong>allyburdened by <strong>the</strong> noti<strong>on</strong> that it is an extraordinary, not anordinary, skill. If my method <strong>of</strong> teaching enables people to gain askill <strong>the</strong>y previously thought closed <strong>of</strong>f to <strong>the</strong>m, is it <strong>the</strong> neurologicalexplanati<strong>on</strong> that makes <strong>the</strong> method work, or is it somethingelse that I may not be aware <strong>of</strong>?I know that it is not simply my style <strong>of</strong> teaching that causes<strong>the</strong> method to work, since <strong>the</strong> hundreds <strong>of</strong> teachers who havereported equal success using my methods obviously have widelydiffering teaching styles. Would <strong>the</strong> exercises work without <strong>the</strong>neurological rati<strong>on</strong>ale? It's possible, but it would be very difficultto persuade people to accede to such unlikely exercises asupside-down drawing without some reas<strong>on</strong>able explanati<strong>on</strong>. Is it,<strong>the</strong>n, just <strong>the</strong> fact <strong>of</strong> giving people a rati<strong>on</strong>ale—that any rati<strong>on</strong>alewould do? Perhaps, but I have always been struck by <strong>the</strong> fact thatmy explanati<strong>on</strong> seems to make sense to people at a subjectivelevel. The <strong>the</strong>ory seems to fit <strong>the</strong>ir experience, and certainly <strong>the</strong>ideas derive from my own subjective experience with drawing.In each editi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> this book I have made <strong>the</strong> following statement:The <strong>the</strong>ory and methods presented in my book have provenempirically successful. In short, <strong>the</strong> method works, regardless <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> extent to which future science may eventually determineexact locati<strong>on</strong> and c<strong>on</strong>firm <strong>the</strong> degree <strong>of</strong> separati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> brain functi<strong>on</strong>sin <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres.I hope that eventually scholars using traditi<strong>on</strong>al researchmethods will help answer <strong>the</strong> many questi<strong>on</strong>s I have myself aboutthis work. It does appear that recent research tends to corroboratemy basic ideas. For example, new findings <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>huge bundle <strong>of</strong> nerve fibers c<strong>on</strong>necting <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres, <strong>the</strong>XXIVINTRODUCTION


corpus callosum, indicate that <strong>the</strong> corpus callosum can inhibit <strong>the</strong>passage <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> from hemisphere to hemisphere when <strong>the</strong>task requires n<strong>on</strong>interference from <strong>on</strong>e or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hemisphere.Meanwhile, <strong>the</strong> work appears to bring a great deal <strong>of</strong> joy tomy students, whe<strong>the</strong>r or not we fully understand <strong>the</strong> underlyingprocess.A fur<strong>the</strong>r complicati<strong>on</strong>One fur<strong>the</strong>r complicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> seeing needs menti<strong>on</strong>ing. The eyesga<strong>the</strong>r visual informati<strong>on</strong> by c<strong>on</strong>stantly scanning <strong>the</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ment.But visual data from "out <strong>the</strong>re," ga<strong>the</strong>red by sight, is not<strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> story. At least part, and perhaps much <strong>of</strong> what wesee is changed, interpreted, or c<strong>on</strong>ceptualized in ways thatdepend <strong>on</strong> a pers<strong>on</strong>'s training, mind-set, and past experiences. Wetend to see what we expect to see or what we decide we have seen.This expectati<strong>on</strong> or decisi<strong>on</strong>, however, <strong>of</strong>ten is not a c<strong>on</strong>sciousprocess. Instead, <strong>the</strong> brain frequently does <strong>the</strong> expecting and <strong>the</strong>deciding, without our c<strong>on</strong>scious awareness, and <strong>the</strong>n alters orrearranges—or even simply disregards—<strong>the</strong> raw data <strong>of</strong> visi<strong>on</strong>that hits <strong>the</strong> retina. Learning percepti<strong>on</strong> through drawing seemsto change this process and to allow a different, more direct kind <strong>of</strong>seeing. The brain's editing is somehow put <strong>on</strong> hold, <strong>the</strong>reby permitting<strong>on</strong>e to see more fully and perhaps more realistically.This experience is <strong>of</strong>ten moving and deeply affecting. Mystudents' most frequent comments after learning to draw are"Life seems so much richer now" and "I didn't realize how much<strong>the</strong>re is to see and how beautiful things are." This new way <strong>of</strong> seeingmay al<strong>on</strong>e be reas<strong>on</strong> enough to learn to draw."The artist is <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>fidant <strong>of</strong>nature. Flowers carry <strong>on</strong> dialogueswith him through <strong>the</strong> gracefulbending <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir stems and <strong>the</strong> harm<strong>on</strong>iouslytinted nuances <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>irblossoms. Every flower has a cordialword which nature directstowards him."— Auguste RodinINTRODUCTIONxxv


<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> and<strong>the</strong> Art <strong>of</strong> BicycleRiding


DRAWING is A CURIOUS PROCESS, so intertwined with seeingthat <strong>the</strong> two can hardly be separated. Ability to drawdepends <strong>on</strong> ability to see <strong>the</strong> way an artist sees, and this kind <strong>of</strong>seeing can marvelously enrich your life.In many ways, teaching drawing is somewhat like teachingsome<strong>on</strong>e to ride a bicycle. It is very difficult to explain in words.In teaching some<strong>on</strong>e to ride a bicycle, you might say, "Well, youjust get <strong>on</strong>, push <strong>the</strong> pedals, balance yourself, and <strong>of</strong>f you'll go."Of course, that doesn't explain it at all, and you are likelyfinally to say, "I'll get <strong>on</strong> and show you how. Watch and see how Ido it."And so it is with drawing. Most art teachers and drawing textbookauthors exhort beginners to "change <strong>the</strong>ir ways <strong>of</strong> looking atthings" and to "learn how to see." The problem is that this differentway <strong>of</strong> seeing is as hard to explain as how to balance a bicycle,and <strong>the</strong> teacher <strong>of</strong>ten ends by saying, in effect, "Look at <strong>the</strong>seexamples and just keep trying. If you practice a lot, eventuallyyou may get it." While nearly every<strong>on</strong>e learns to ride a bicycle,many individuals never solve <strong>the</strong> problems <strong>of</strong> drawing. To put itmore precisely, most people never learn to see well enough todraw.Fig. I-I. Bellowing Bis<strong>on</strong>. Paleolithiccave painting from Altamira, Spain.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> by Brevil. Prehistoricartists were probably thought tohave magic powers.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> as a magical abilityBecause <strong>on</strong>ly a few individuals seem to possess <strong>the</strong> ability to seeand draw, artists are <strong>of</strong>ten regarded as pers<strong>on</strong>s with a rare Godgiventalent. To many people, <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> drawing seems mysteriousand somehow bey<strong>on</strong>d human understanding.Artists <strong>the</strong>mselves <strong>of</strong>ten do little to dispel <strong>the</strong> mystery. If youask an artist (that is, some<strong>on</strong>e who draws well as a result <strong>of</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>rl<strong>on</strong>g training or chance discovery <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist's way <strong>of</strong> seeing),"How do you draw something so that it looks real—say a portraitor a landscape?" <strong>the</strong> artist is likely to reply, "Well, I just have a giftfor it, I guess," or "I really d<strong>on</strong>'t know. I just start in and workthings out as I go al<strong>on</strong>g," or "Well, I just look at <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong> (or <strong>the</strong>landscape) and I draw what I see." The last reply seems like alogical and straightforward answer. Yet, <strong>on</strong> reflecti<strong>on</strong>, it clearly2THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


doesn't explain <strong>the</strong> process at all, and <strong>the</strong> sense that <strong>the</strong> skill <strong>of</strong>drawing is a vaguely magical ability persists (Figure I-I).While this attitude <strong>of</strong> w<strong>on</strong>der at artistic skill causes people toappreciate artists and <strong>the</strong>ir work, it does little to encourage individualsto try to learn to draw, and it doesn't help teachersexplain to students <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> drawing. Often, in fact, peopleeven feel that <strong>the</strong>y shouldn't take a drawing course because <strong>the</strong>yd<strong>on</strong>'t know already how to draw. This is like deciding that youshouldn't take a French class because you d<strong>on</strong>'t already speakFrench, or that you shouldn't sign up for a course in carpentrybecause you d<strong>on</strong>'t know how to build a house.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> as a learnable, teachable skillYou will so<strong>on</strong> discover that drawing is a skill that can be learnedby every normal pers<strong>on</strong> with average eyesight and average eyehandcoordinati<strong>on</strong>—with sufficient ability, for example, to threada needle or catch a baseball. C<strong>on</strong>trary to popular opini<strong>on</strong>, manualskill is not a primary factor in drawing. If your handwriting isreadable, or if you can print legibly, you have ample dexterity todraw well.We need say no more here about hands, but about eyes wecannot say enough. Learning to draw is more than learning <strong>the</strong>skill itself; by studying this book you will learn how to see. That is,you will learn how to process visual informati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> specialway used by artists. That way is differentirom <strong>the</strong> way you usuallyprocess visual informati<strong>on</strong> and seems to require that you use yourbrain in a different way than you ordinarily use it.You will be learning, <strong>the</strong>refore, something about how yourbrain handles visual informati<strong>on</strong>. Recent research has begun tothrow new scientific light <strong>on</strong> that marvel <strong>of</strong> capability and complexity,<strong>the</strong> human brain. And <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> things we are learning ishow <strong>the</strong> special properties <strong>of</strong> our brains enable us to draw pictures<strong>of</strong> our percepti<strong>on</strong>s.Roger N. Shepard, pr<strong>of</strong>essor <strong>of</strong>psychology at Stanford University,recently described his pers<strong>on</strong>almode <strong>of</strong> creative thought duringwhich research ideas emerged inhis mind as unverbalized, essentiallycomplete, l<strong>on</strong>g-sought soluti<strong>on</strong>sto problems."That in all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se sudden illuminati<strong>on</strong>smy ideas took shape in aprimarily visual-spatial form without,so far as I can introspect, anyverbal interventi<strong>on</strong> is in accordancewith what has always beenmy preferred mode <strong>of</strong> thinking....Many <strong>of</strong> my happiest hours havesince childhood been spentabsorbed in drawing, in tinkering,or in exercises <strong>of</strong> purely mentalvisualizati<strong>on</strong>."— Roger N . ShepardVisual Learning, Thinking,and Communicati<strong>on</strong>, 1978"Learning to draw is really amatter <strong>of</strong> learning to see—to seecorrectly—and that means a gooddeal more than merely lookingwith <strong>the</strong> eye."— Kim<strong>on</strong> NicolaidesThe Natural Way to Draw,1941DRAWING AND THE ART OF BICYCLE RIDING 3


Gertrude Stein asked <strong>the</strong> Frenchartist Henri Matisse whe<strong>the</strong>r, wheneating a tomato, he looked at it <strong>the</strong>way an artist would. Matissereplied:"No, when I eat a tomato I look at it<strong>the</strong> way any<strong>on</strong>e else would. Butwhen I paint a tomato, <strong>the</strong>n I see itdifferently."— Gertrude SteinPicasso, 1938"The painter draws with his eyes,not with his hands. Whatever hesees, if he sees it clear, he can putdown. The putting <strong>of</strong> it downrequires, perhaps, much care andlabor, but no more muscular agilitythan it takes for him to write hisname. Seeing clear is <strong>the</strong> importantthing."— Maurice GrosserThe Painter's Eye, 1951"It is in order to really see, to seeever deeper, ever more intensely,hence to be fully aware and alive,that I draw what <strong>the</strong> Chinese call'The Ten Thousand Things'around me. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is <strong>the</strong> disciplineby which I c<strong>on</strong>stantly rediscover<strong>the</strong> world."I have learned that what I have notdrawn, I have never really seen,and that when I start drawing anordinary thing, I realize how extraordinaryit is, sheer miracle."— Frederick FranckThe Zen <strong>of</strong> Seeing, 1973<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> and seeingThe magical mystery <strong>of</strong> drawing ability seems to be, in part atleast, an ability to make a shift in brain state to a different mode <strong>of</strong>seeing/perceiving. When you see in <strong>the</strong> special way in which experiencedartists see, <strong>the</strong>n you can draw. This is not to say that <strong>the</strong> drawings<strong>of</strong> great artists such as Le<strong>on</strong>ardo da Vinci or Rembrandt arenot still w<strong>on</strong>drous because we may know something about <strong>the</strong>cerebral process that went into <strong>the</strong>ir creati<strong>on</strong>. Indeed, scientificresearch makes master drawings seem even more remarkablebecause <strong>the</strong>y seem to cause a viewer to shift to <strong>the</strong> artist's mode <strong>of</strong>perceiving. But <strong>the</strong> basic skill <strong>of</strong> drawing is also accessible toevery<strong>on</strong>e who can learn to make <strong>the</strong> shift to <strong>the</strong> artist's mode andsee in <strong>the</strong> artist's way.The artist's way <strong>of</strong> seeing: A tw<strong>of</strong>old process<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is not really very difficult. Seeing is <strong>the</strong> problem, or, tobe more specific, shifting to a particular way <strong>of</strong> seeing. You maynot believe me at this moment. You may feel that you are seeingthings just fine and that it's <strong>the</strong> drawing that is hard. But <strong>the</strong> oppositeis true, and <strong>the</strong> exercises in this book are designed to help youmake <strong>the</strong> mental shift and gain a tw<strong>of</strong>old advantage. First, to openaccess by c<strong>on</strong>scious voliti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> visual, perceptual mode <strong>of</strong> thinkingin order to experience a focus in your awareness, and sec<strong>on</strong>d,to see things in a different way. Both will enable you to draw well.Many artists have spoken <strong>of</strong> seeing things differently whiledrawing and have <strong>of</strong>ten menti<strong>on</strong>ed that drawing puts <strong>the</strong>m into asomewhat altered state <strong>of</strong> awareness. In that different subjectivestate, artists speak <strong>of</strong> feeling transported, "at <strong>on</strong>e with <strong>the</strong> work,"able to grasp relati<strong>on</strong>ships that <strong>the</strong>y ordinarily cannot grasp.Awareness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> passage <strong>of</strong> time fades away and words recedefrom c<strong>on</strong>sciousness. Artists say that <strong>the</strong>y feel alert and aware yetare relaxed and free <strong>of</strong> anxiety, experiencing a pleasurable,almost mystical activati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mind.4THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> to states <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousnessThe slightly altered c<strong>on</strong>sciousness state <strong>of</strong> feeling transported,which most artists experience while drawing, painting, sculpting,or doing any kind <strong>of</strong> art work, is a state probably not altoge<strong>the</strong>runfamiliar to you. You may have observed in yourself slight shiftsin your state <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness while engaged in much more ordinaryactivities than artwork.For example, most people are aware that <strong>the</strong>y occasi<strong>on</strong>allyslip from ordinary waking c<strong>on</strong>sciousness into <strong>the</strong> slightly alteredstate <strong>of</strong> daydreaming. As ano<strong>the</strong>r example, people <strong>of</strong>ten say thatreading takes <strong>the</strong>m "out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>mselves." And o<strong>the</strong>r kinds <strong>of</strong> activitieswhich apparently produce a shift in c<strong>on</strong>sciousness state aremeditati<strong>on</strong>, jogging, needlework, typing, listening to music, and,<strong>of</strong> course, drawing itself.Also, I believe that driving <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> freeway probably induces aslightly different subjective state that is similar to <strong>the</strong> drawingstate. After all, in freeway driving we deal with visual images,keeping track <strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>al, spatial informati<strong>on</strong>, sensing complexcomp<strong>on</strong>ents <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> overall traffic c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>. Many people findthat <strong>the</strong>y do a lot <strong>of</strong> creative thinking while driving, <strong>of</strong>ten losingtrack <strong>of</strong> time and experiencing a pleasurable sense <strong>of</strong> freedomfrom anxiety. These mental operati<strong>on</strong>s may activate <strong>the</strong> sameparts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain used in drawing. Of course, if driving c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sare difficult, if we are late or if some<strong>on</strong>e sharing <strong>the</strong> ridetalks with us, <strong>the</strong> shift to <strong>the</strong> alternative state doesn't occur. Thereas<strong>on</strong>s for this we'll take up in Chapter Three.The key to learning to draw, <strong>the</strong>refore, is to set up c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sthat cause you to make a mental shift to a different mode <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong>processing—<strong>the</strong> slightly altered state <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness—that enables you to see well. In this drawing mode, you will beable to draw your percepti<strong>on</strong>s even though you may never havestudied drawing. Once <strong>the</strong> drawing mode is familiar to you, youwill be able to c<strong>on</strong>sciously c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>the</strong> mental shift."If a certain kind <strong>of</strong> activity, such aspainting, becomes <strong>the</strong> habitualmode <strong>of</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong>, it may followthat taking up <strong>the</strong> painting materialsand beginning work with <strong>the</strong>mwill act suggestively and sopresently evoke a flight into <strong>the</strong>higher state."— Robert HenriThe Art Spirit, 1923DRAWING AND THE ART OF BICYCLE RIDING5


My students <strong>of</strong>ten report thatlearning to draw makes <strong>the</strong>m feelmore creative. Obviously, manyroads lead to creative endeavor:<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e route. HowardGardner, Harvard pr<strong>of</strong>essor <strong>of</strong>psychology and educati<strong>on</strong>, refersto this linkage:"By a curious twist, <strong>the</strong> words artand creativity have become closelylinked in our society."From Gardner's book CreatingMinds, 1993.Samuel Goldwyn <strong>on</strong>ce said:"D<strong>on</strong>'t pay any attenti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong>critics. D<strong>on</strong>'t even ignore <strong>the</strong>m."Quoted in Being Digital by NicolasNegrop<strong>on</strong>te, 1995.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> your creative selfI see you as an individual with creative potential for expressingyourself through drawing. My aim is to provide <strong>the</strong> means forreleasing that potential, for gaining access at a c<strong>on</strong>scious level toyour inventive, intuitive, imaginative powers that may have beenlargely untapped by our verbal, technological culture and educati<strong>on</strong>alsystem. I am going to teach you how to draw, but drawing is<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> means, not <strong>the</strong> end. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> will tap <strong>the</strong> special abilitiesthat are right for drawing. By learning to draw you will learn tosee differently and, as <strong>the</strong> artist Rodin lyrically states, to become ac<strong>on</strong>fidant <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> natural world, to awaken your eye to <strong>the</strong> lovelylanguage <strong>of</strong> forms, to express yourself in that language.In drawing, you will delve deeply into a part <strong>of</strong> your mind too<strong>of</strong>ten obscured by endless details <strong>of</strong> daily life. From this experienceyou will develop your ability to perceive things freshly in<strong>the</strong>ir totality, to see underlying patterns and possibilities for newcombinati<strong>on</strong>s. Creative soluti<strong>on</strong>s to problems, whe<strong>the</strong>r pers<strong>on</strong>alor pr<strong>of</strong>essi<strong>on</strong>al, will be accessible through new modes <strong>of</strong> thinkingand new ways <strong>of</strong> using <strong>the</strong> power <strong>of</strong> your whole brain.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, pleasurable and rewarding though it is, is but a keyto open <strong>the</strong> door to o<strong>the</strong>r goals. My hope is that <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong> will help you expand your powers as anindividual through increased awareness <strong>of</strong> your own mind and itsworkings. The multiple effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> exercises in this book areintended to enhance your c<strong>on</strong>fidence in decisi<strong>on</strong> making andproblem solving. The potential force <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> creative, imaginativehuman brain seems almost limitless. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> may help you cometo know this power and make it known to o<strong>the</strong>rs. Through drawing,you are made visible. The German artist Albrecht Dürersaid, "From this, <strong>the</strong> treasure secretly ga<strong>the</strong>red in your heart willbecome evident through your creative work."Keeping <strong>the</strong> real goal in mind, let us begin to fashi<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> key.6THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


My approach: A path to creativityThe exercises and instructi<strong>on</strong>s in this book have been designedspecifically for people who cannot draw at all, who may feel that<strong>the</strong>y have little or no talent for drawing, and who may feel doubtfulthat <strong>the</strong>y could ever learn to draw—but who think <strong>the</strong>y mightlike to learn. The approach <strong>of</strong> this book is different from o<strong>the</strong>rdrawing instructi<strong>on</strong> books in that <strong>the</strong> exercises are aimed atopening access to skills you already have but that are simply waitingto be released.Creative pers<strong>on</strong>s from fields o<strong>the</strong>r than art who want to get<strong>the</strong>ir working skills under better c<strong>on</strong>trol and learn to overcomeblocks to creativity will benefit from working with <strong>the</strong> techniquespresented here. Teachers and parents will find <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory andexercises useful in helping children to develop <strong>the</strong>ir creative abilities.At <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> book, I have supplied a brief postscript that<strong>of</strong>fers some general suggesti<strong>on</strong>s for adapting my methods andmaterials to children. A sec<strong>on</strong>d postscript is addressed to art students.This book is based <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> five-day workshop that I have beenteaching for about fifteen years to individuals <strong>of</strong> widely rangingages and occupati<strong>on</strong>s. Nearly all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> students begin <strong>the</strong> coursewith very few drawing skills and with high anxiety about <strong>the</strong>irpotential drawing ability. Almost without excepti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> studentsachieve a high level <strong>of</strong> skill in drawing and gain c<strong>on</strong>fidence to go<strong>on</strong> developing <strong>the</strong>ir expressive drawing skills in fur<strong>the</strong>r artcourses or by practice <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir own.An intriguing aspect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten-remarkable gains most studentsachieve is <strong>the</strong> rapid rate <strong>of</strong> improvement in drawing skills.It's my belief that if pers<strong>on</strong>s untrained in art can learn to make<strong>the</strong> shift to <strong>the</strong> artist's mode <strong>of</strong> seeing—that is, <strong>the</strong> right-hemispheremode—those individuals are <strong>the</strong>n able to draw withoutfur<strong>the</strong>r instructi<strong>on</strong>. To put it ano<strong>the</strong>r way, you already know howto draw, but old habits <strong>of</strong> seeing interfere with that ability andblock it. The exercises in this book are designed to remove <strong>the</strong>interference and unblock <strong>the</strong> ability."To be shaken out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ruts <strong>of</strong>ordinary percepti<strong>on</strong>, to be shownfor a few timeless hours <strong>the</strong> outerand <strong>the</strong> inner world, not as <strong>the</strong>yappear to an animal obsessed withwords and noti<strong>on</strong>s, but as <strong>the</strong>y areapprehended, directly and unc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ally,by Mind at Large—thisis an experience <strong>of</strong> inestimablevalue to every<strong>on</strong>e."— Aldous HuxleyThe Doors <strong>of</strong> Percepti<strong>on</strong>,1954DRAWING AND THE ART OF BICYCLE RIDING7


"When <strong>the</strong> artist is alive in any pers<strong>on</strong>,whatever his kind <strong>of</strong> work maybe, he becomes an inventive,searching, daring, self-expressivecreature. He becomes interestingto o<strong>the</strong>r people. He disturbs,upsets, enlightens, and opens waysfor a better understanding. Wherethose who are not artists are tryingto close <strong>the</strong> book, he opens it andshows <strong>the</strong>re are still more pagespossible."— Robert HenriThe Art Spirit, 1923While you may have no interest whatever in becoming a fulltimeworking artist, <strong>the</strong> exercises will provide insights into <strong>the</strong>way your mind works, or your two minds work—singly, cooperatively,<strong>on</strong>e against <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r. And, as many <strong>of</strong> my students havetold me, <strong>the</strong>ir lives seem richer because <strong>the</strong>y are seeing better andseeing more. It's helpful to remember that we d<strong>on</strong>'t teach readingand writing to produce <strong>on</strong>ly poets and writers, but ra<strong>the</strong>r toimprove thinking.Realism as a means to an endWhy faces?A number <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> exercises and instructi<strong>on</strong>al sequences in thisbook are designed to enable you to draw recognizable portraits.Let me explain why I think portrait drawing is useful as a subjectfor beginners in art. Broadly speaking, except for <strong>the</strong> degree <strong>of</strong>complexity, all drawing is <strong>the</strong> same. One drawing task is no harderthan any o<strong>the</strong>r. The same skills and ways <strong>of</strong> seeing are involved indrawing still-life setups, landscapes, <strong>the</strong> figure, random objects,even imaginary subjects, and portrait drawing. It's all <strong>the</strong> same thing:You see what's out <strong>the</strong>re (imaginary subjects are "seen" in <strong>the</strong>mind's eye) and you draw what you see.Why, <strong>the</strong>n, have I selected portrait drawing for some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>exercises? For three reas<strong>on</strong>s. First, beginning students <strong>of</strong> drawing<strong>of</strong>ten think that drawing human faces is <strong>the</strong> hardest <strong>of</strong> all kinds <strong>of</strong>drawing. Thus, when students see that <strong>the</strong>y can draw portraits,<strong>the</strong>y feel c<strong>on</strong>fident and <strong>the</strong>ir c<strong>on</strong>fidence enhances progress. Asec<strong>on</strong>d, more important, reas<strong>on</strong> is that <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> human brain is specialized for recogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> faces. Since <strong>the</strong>right brain is <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e we will be trying to gain access to, it makessense to choose a subject that <strong>the</strong> right brain is used to workingwith. And third, faces are fascinating! Once you have drawn a pers<strong>on</strong>,you will really have seen that individual's face. As <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> mystudents said, "I d<strong>on</strong>'t think I ever actually looked at any<strong>on</strong>e's facebefore I started drawing. Now, <strong>the</strong> oddest thing is that every<strong>on</strong>elooks beautiful to me."8THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Summing upI have described to you <strong>the</strong> basic premise <strong>of</strong> this book—thatdrawing is a teachable, learnable skill that can provide a tw<strong>of</strong>oldadvantage. By gaining access to <strong>the</strong> part <strong>of</strong> your mind that worksin a style c<strong>on</strong>ducive to creative, intuitive thought, you will learn afundamental skill <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> visual arts: how to put down <strong>on</strong> paperwhat you see in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> your eyes. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, through learning todraw by <strong>the</strong> method presented in this book, you will enhanceyour ability to think more creatively in o<strong>the</strong>r areas <strong>of</strong> your life.How far you go with <strong>the</strong>se skills after you complete <strong>the</strong>course will depend <strong>on</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r traits such as energy and curiosity.But first things first! The potential is <strong>the</strong>re. It's sometimes necessaryto remind ourselves that Shakespeare at some point learnedto write a line <strong>of</strong> prose, Beethoven learned <strong>the</strong> musical scales, andas you see in <strong>the</strong> margin quotati<strong>on</strong>, Vincent Van Gogh learnedhow to draw."... at <strong>the</strong> time when you spoke <strong>of</strong>my becoming a painter, I thought itvery impractical and would no<strong>the</strong>ar <strong>of</strong> it. What made me stopdoubting was reading a clear book<strong>on</strong> perspective, Cassange's Guide to<strong>the</strong> ABC <strong>of</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>: and a weeklater I drew <strong>the</strong> interior <strong>of</strong> akitchen with stove, chair, table andwindow—in <strong>the</strong>ir places and <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong>ir legs—whereas before it hadseemed to me that getting depthand <strong>the</strong> right perspective into adrawing was witchcraft or purechance."— Vincent Van Gogh,in a letter to his bro<strong>the</strong>r,Theo, who had suggestedthat Vincent become apainter. Letter 184, p. 331.DRAWING AND THE ART OF BICYCLE RIDING 9


2The <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>Exercises:One Step at a Time


OVER THE YEARS OF TEACHING, I have experimentedwith various progressi<strong>on</strong>s, sequences, and combnati<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong> exercises. The sequence set out in this book hasproved to be <strong>the</strong> most effective in terms <strong>of</strong> student progress.We'll take <strong>the</strong> first step, <strong>the</strong> all-important preinstructi<strong>on</strong> drawings,in this chapter.When you begin <strong>the</strong> drawing exercises in Chapter Four,you'll have some background in <strong>the</strong> underlying <strong>the</strong>ory, how <strong>the</strong>exercises have been set up, and why <strong>the</strong>y work. The sequence isdesigned to enhance success at every step <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> way and to provideaccess to a new mode <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> processing with as littleupset to <strong>the</strong> old mode as possible. Therefore, I ask you to read<strong>the</strong> chapters in <strong>the</strong> order presented and to do <strong>the</strong> exercises as<strong>the</strong>y appear.I have limited <strong>the</strong> recommended exercises to a minimumnumber, but if time permits, do more drawings than are suggested:Seek your own subjects and devise your own exercises.The more practice you provide for yourself, <strong>the</strong> faster you willprogress. To this end, in additi<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> exercises that appear in<strong>the</strong> text, supplementary exercises <strong>of</strong>ten appear in <strong>the</strong> margin.Doing <strong>the</strong>se exercises will reinforce both your skills and yourc<strong>on</strong>fidence.For most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> exercises, I recommend that you readthrough all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong>s before you start drawing and, wheredirected, view <strong>the</strong> examples <strong>of</strong> students' drawings before beginning.Keep all <strong>of</strong> your drawings toge<strong>the</strong>r in a folder or largeenvelope, so that by <strong>the</strong> time you've come to <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> bookyou can review your own progress.Definiti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> termsA glossary <strong>of</strong> terms appears at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> book. Certain termsare defined fairly extensively in <strong>the</strong> text, and <strong>the</strong> glossary c<strong>on</strong>tainso<strong>the</strong>r terms not so extensively defined. Words that are comm<strong>on</strong>lyused in everyday language, such as "value" and"compositi<strong>on</strong>," have very specific, and <strong>of</strong>ten different, meaningsin art terminology. I suggest that you glance through <strong>the</strong> glossarybefore starting to read <strong>the</strong> chapters.12 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> materialsThe materials list for <strong>the</strong> first two editi<strong>on</strong>s was very simple: someinexpensive b<strong>on</strong>d typing paper or a pad <strong>of</strong> inexpensive drawingpaper, a pencil, and an eraser. I menti<strong>on</strong>ed that a #4B drawingpencil is pleasant to use, as <strong>the</strong> lead is smooth and makes a clear,dark line, but an ordinary number 2 writing pencil is nearly asgood. For this editi<strong>on</strong>, you still need <strong>the</strong>se basic materials, but Iwish to suggest a few additi<strong>on</strong>al aids that will help you learn todraw quickly.• You will need a piece <strong>of</strong> clear plastic, about 8" x 10" and about1/16" thick. A piece <strong>of</strong> glass is fine, but <strong>the</strong> edges must be taped.Use a permanent marker to draw two crosshairs <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic,a horiz<strong>on</strong>tal line and a vertical line crossing at <strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> plane. (See <strong>the</strong> sketch in <strong>the</strong> margin.)• Also, you will need two "viewfinders," made <strong>of</strong> black cardboardabout 8" x 10". From <strong>on</strong>e, cut a rectangular opening <strong>of</strong>4+1/4" x 5+1/4" and from <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r, cut out a larger opening <strong>of</strong> 6" x7+5/8". See Figure 2-1.• A n<strong>on</strong>permanent black felt-tip marker• Two clips to fasten your viewfinders to <strong>the</strong> plastic pictureplane• A "graphite stick," #4B, available at most art supply stores• Some masking tape• A pencil sharpener—a small, hand-held sharpener is fine• An eraser, such a "Pink Pearl" or a white plastic eraserGa<strong>the</strong>ring <strong>the</strong>se materials requires a bit <strong>of</strong> effort, but <strong>the</strong>y willtruly help you to learn rapidly. You can buy <strong>the</strong>m at any art materialsor crafts store. My staff <strong>of</strong> teachers and I no l<strong>on</strong>ger attemptto teach our students without using viewfinders and <strong>the</strong> plasticpicture plane, and <strong>the</strong>y will help you just as much. Because <strong>the</strong>seitems are so essential to students' understanding <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> basicnature <strong>of</strong> drawing, for years now we have put toge<strong>the</strong>r—byhand!—portfolios c<strong>on</strong>taining <strong>the</strong> special learning tools that wehave developed for our five-day intensive workshops. The portfoliosalso c<strong>on</strong>tained all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> necessary drawing materials and alightweight drawing board. Now I have made our Portfolio avail-THE DRAWING EXERCISES: ONE STEP AT A TIME 13


C<strong>on</strong>struct a viewfinder as follows:1. Take a sheet <strong>of</strong> paper or use thincardboard <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same size as <strong>the</strong>paper you use for drawing. Theviewfinder must be <strong>the</strong> same format,that is, <strong>the</strong> same proporti<strong>on</strong>alshape, as <strong>the</strong> paper you are usingto draw <strong>on</strong>.2. Draw diag<strong>on</strong>al lines from oppositecorners, crossing in <strong>the</strong> center.In <strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper, draw asmall rectangle by c<strong>on</strong>necting horiz<strong>on</strong>taland vertical lines at points<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> diag<strong>on</strong>als. The rectangleshould be about 1 x 1/4". (See Figure2-1.) C<strong>on</strong>structed this way, <strong>the</strong>inner rectangle has <strong>the</strong> same proporti<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> length to width as <strong>the</strong>outer edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper.3. Next, cut <strong>the</strong> small rectangle out<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> center with scissors. Hold<strong>the</strong> paper up and compare <strong>the</strong>shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> small opening with <strong>the</strong>shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole format. You cansee that <strong>the</strong> two shapes are <strong>the</strong>same, and <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> size is different.This perceptual aid is called aviewfinder. It will help you to perceivenegative spaces by establishingan edge to <strong>the</strong> space aroundforms.Fig. 2-1.able for purchase. It includes as well a two-hour instructi<strong>on</strong>alvideo <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> less<strong>on</strong>s in this book.If you are interested in purchasing a Portfolio, you will findan order slip at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> book, or you can c<strong>on</strong>tact my websiteat www.drawright.com. But <strong>the</strong> few items listed above will besufficient if you would ra<strong>the</strong>r put toge<strong>the</strong>r your own set <strong>of</strong> materials.Pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> drawings: A valuable record <strong>of</strong> yourart skillsNow, let's get started. First, you need to make a record <strong>of</strong> yourpresent drawing skills. This is important! You d<strong>on</strong>'t want to miss<strong>the</strong> pleasure <strong>of</strong> having a real memento <strong>of</strong> your starting point tocompare with your later drawings. I'm fully aware how difficultthis is, but just do it! As <strong>the</strong> great Dutch artist Vincent Van Goghwrote (in a letter to his bro<strong>the</strong>r, Theo):"Just dash something down if you see a blank canvas staring atyou with a certain imbecility. You do not know how paralyzing itis, that staring <strong>of</strong> a blank canvas which says to a painter, 'You d<strong>on</strong>'tknow anything.'"So<strong>on</strong>, you will "know something," I promise. Just gear yourselfup and do <strong>the</strong>se drawings. Later, you'll be very happy thatyou did. The drawings have proved to be invaluable in aiding studentsto see and recognize <strong>the</strong>ir own progress. A kind <strong>of</strong> amnesiaseems to set in as drawing skills improve. Students forget what<strong>the</strong>ir drawing was like before instructi<strong>on</strong>. Moreover <strong>the</strong> degree <strong>of</strong>criticism keeps pace with progress. Even after c<strong>on</strong>siderableimprovement, students are sometimes critical <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir latestdrawing because it's "not as good as da Vinci's." The before drawingsprovide a realistic gauge <strong>of</strong> progress. After you do <strong>the</strong> drawings,put <strong>the</strong>m away and we will look at <strong>the</strong>m again later <strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong>light <strong>of</strong> your newly acquired skills.14 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


What you'll need:• Paper to draw <strong>on</strong>—plain white b<strong>on</strong>d paper is fine• Your #2 writing pencil• Your pencil sharpener• Your masking tape• A small mirror, about 5" x 7", that could be attached to a wall,or any available wall or door mirror• Something to use as a drawing board—a breadboard or asturdy piece <strong>of</strong> cardboard, about 15" x 18"• An hour to an hour and a quarter <strong>of</strong> uninterrupted timeWhat you'll do:You will do three drawings. This usually takes our students aboutan hour or so, but feel free to take as l<strong>on</strong>g as you wish for each <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>m. I will first list <strong>the</strong> drawing titles. Instructi<strong>on</strong> for each drawingfollows.• "Self-Portrait"• "A Pers<strong>on</strong>, Drawn from Memory"• "My Hand"Pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> drawing #1: Your "Self-Portrait"1. Tape a stack <strong>of</strong> two or three sheets <strong>of</strong> paper to your drawingboard or work in your pad <strong>of</strong> paper. (Stacking <strong>the</strong> sheets providesa "padded" surface to draw <strong>on</strong>—much better than <strong>the</strong>ra<strong>the</strong>r hard surface <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing board.)2. Sit at arm's length (about 2 to 2+1/2 feet) from a mirror. Leanyour board up against <strong>the</strong> wall, resting <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>board <strong>on</strong> your lap.3. Look at <strong>the</strong> reflecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> your head and face in <strong>the</strong> mirror anddraw your "Self-Portrait."4. When you have finished, title, date, and sign <strong>the</strong> drawing in<strong>the</strong> lower right-hand or lower left-hand corner.THE DRAWING EXERCISES: ONE STEP AT A TIME15


Pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> drawing #2: A pers<strong>on</strong>, drawn from memory1. Call up in your mind's eye an image <strong>of</strong> a pers<strong>on</strong>—perhapssome<strong>on</strong>e from <strong>the</strong> past or a pers<strong>on</strong> you know now. Or youmay recall a drawing you did in <strong>the</strong> past or a photograph<strong>of</strong> a pers<strong>on</strong> well known to you.2. To <strong>the</strong> best <strong>of</strong> your ability, make a drawing <strong>of</strong> that pers<strong>on</strong>.You may draw just <strong>the</strong> head, a half-figure, or <strong>the</strong> wholefigure.3. When you have finished, title, sign, and date your drawing.Pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> drawing #3: Your hand1. Seat yourself at a table to draw.2. If you are right-handed, draw your left hand in whateverpositi<strong>on</strong> you choose. If you are left-handed, draw yourright hand.3. Title, date and sign your drawing.When you have finished <strong>the</strong> pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> drawings:Be sure that you have titled, signed, and dated each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>three drawings. Some <strong>of</strong> my students have enjoyed writing afew comments <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> each drawing, noting what ispleasing and what is perhaps displeasing, what seemed easyand what seemed difficult in <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> drawing. You'llfind <strong>the</strong>se comments interesting to read later <strong>on</strong>.Spread <strong>the</strong> three drawings <strong>on</strong> a table and look at <strong>the</strong>mclosely. If I were <strong>the</strong>re with you, I would be looking for smallareas in <strong>the</strong> drawings that show you were observing carefully—perhaps<strong>the</strong> way a collar turns or a beautifullyobserved curve <strong>of</strong> an eyebrow. Once I encounter such signs <strong>of</strong>careful seeing, I know <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong> will learn to draw well. You,<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, may find nothing admirable and perhapsdismiss <strong>the</strong> drawings as "childish" and "amateurish." Pleaseremember that <strong>the</strong>se drawings are made before instructi<strong>on</strong>.Would you expect yourself to solve problems in algebra withl6THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


out any instructi<strong>on</strong>? On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, you may be surprised andpleased with parts <strong>of</strong> your drawings, perhaps especially <strong>the</strong> drawing<strong>of</strong> your own hand.The reas<strong>on</strong> for doing <strong>the</strong> memory drawingI'm sure that drawing a pers<strong>on</strong> from memory was very difficultfor you, and rightfully so. Even a trained artist would find it difficultto draw a pers<strong>on</strong> from memory. Visual informati<strong>on</strong> from <strong>the</strong>real world is rich, complicated, and unique to each thing we see.Visual memory is necessarily simplified, generalized, and abbreviated—frustratinglyso for artists, who <strong>of</strong>ten have <strong>on</strong>ly a limitedrepertoire <strong>of</strong> memorized images. "Then why do it?" you may wellask.The reas<strong>on</strong> is simply this: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> a pers<strong>on</strong> from memorybrings forth a memorized set <strong>of</strong> symbols, practiced over and overduring childhood. While doing <strong>the</strong> drawing from memory, canyou recall that your hand seemed to have a mind <strong>of</strong> its own? Youknew that you weren't making <strong>the</strong> image you wanted to, but youcouldn't keep your hand from making those simplified shapes—perhaps <strong>the</strong> nose shape, for example. This is <strong>the</strong> so-called "symbolsystem" <strong>of</strong> children's drawing, memorized by countlessrepetiti<strong>on</strong>s during early childhood. You'll learn more about thisin Chapter Five.Now, compare your Self-Portrait with your memory drawing.Do you see <strong>the</strong> symbols repeated in both drawings—that is, are<strong>the</strong> eyes (or <strong>the</strong> nose or mouth) similar in shape, or even identical?If so, this indicates that your symbol system was c<strong>on</strong>trollingyour hand even when you were observing <strong>the</strong> actual shapes in <strong>the</strong>mirror.The symbol system <strong>of</strong> childhoodThis "tyranny" <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> symbol system explains in large part whypeople untrained in drawing c<strong>on</strong>tinue to produce "childish"drawings right into adulthood and even old age. What you willlearn from me is how to set your symbol system aside and accuratelydraw what you see. This training in perceptual skills is <strong>the</strong>THE DRAWING EXERCISES: ONE STEP AT A TIME 17


T<strong>on</strong>y SchwartzCynthia M. SkewesYv<strong>on</strong>ne OliveSusan W. Dryfoosrockbottom "ABC" <strong>of</strong> drawing, necessarily—or at least ideally—learned before progressing to imaginative drawing, painting, andsculpture.With this informati<strong>on</strong> about <strong>the</strong> symbol system in mind, youmay want to add a few more notes <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> your drawings.Then, put all three drawings away for safekeeping. Do not look at<strong>the</strong>m again until after you have completed my course and havelearned to see and draw.Student showing: A preview <strong>of</strong> before-and-afterdrawingsNow I would like to show you some drawings d<strong>on</strong>e by my students.The drawings show typical changes in students' drawing18 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Frank FernandezAngie HinckelJohn DavisAlan O'C<strong>on</strong>nellDianneSam Fergus<strong>on</strong>The drawings <strong>on</strong> this page and <strong>the</strong> following page showBefore-and-After drawings <strong>of</strong> an entire five-day class,held in Seattle, August 4,1997, to August 8,1997.THE DRAWING EXERCISES: ONE STEP AT A TIME


Lori BishopChris Fercnis<strong>on</strong>Virginia DavisGay StrobleCarla Di PietroDarci Park<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>s from <strong>the</strong> five-day Seattle class, c<strong>on</strong>tinued.2OTHE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


ability from <strong>the</strong> first less<strong>on</strong> (before instructi<strong>on</strong>) to <strong>the</strong> last less<strong>on</strong>.Most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se students attended five-day workshops, eight hoursa day for <strong>the</strong> five days. Both <strong>the</strong> Pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> and Post-instructi<strong>on</strong>drawings are self-portraits, drawn by students observing<strong>the</strong>ir own images in mirrors. As you can see, <strong>the</strong> Before-and-Afterdrawings in <strong>the</strong> student examples dem<strong>on</strong>strate that <strong>the</strong> studentshave transformed <strong>the</strong>ir ways <strong>of</strong> seeing and drawing. The changesare significant enough that it almost seems as though two differentpers<strong>on</strong>s have d<strong>on</strong>e <strong>the</strong> drawings.Learning to perceive is <strong>the</strong> basic skill that <strong>the</strong> students acquired.The change you see in <strong>the</strong>ir ability to draw possibly reflects anequally significant change in <strong>the</strong>ir ability to see. Regard <strong>the</strong> drawingsfrom that standpoint: as a visible record <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> students'improvement in perceptual skills.On pages 19-20 I present Before-and-After drawings by anentire class, a group <strong>of</strong> adult students in Seattle, Washingt<strong>on</strong>.Looking at <strong>the</strong> "Before" drawings, you will see that students cameto <strong>the</strong> five-day class with different levels <strong>of</strong> existing drawing skillsand backgrounds in art. The "After" drawings, d<strong>on</strong>e five dayslater, however, show a remarkably c<strong>on</strong>sistent high level <strong>of</strong> skills.This overall success rate, I believe, dem<strong>on</strong>strates our goal wi<strong>the</strong>very group: that every student will gain high-level drawing skillsregardless <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir existing (or n<strong>on</strong>-existing) skill level."The art <strong>of</strong> archery is not an athleticability mastered more or lessthrough primarily physical practice,but ra<strong>the</strong>r a skill with its originin mental exercise and with itsobject c<strong>on</strong>sisting in mentally hitting<strong>the</strong> mark."Therefore, <strong>the</strong> archer is basicallyaiming for himself. Through this,perhaps, he will succeed in hitting<strong>the</strong> target—his essential self."— HerrigelExpressing yourself in drawing: The n<strong>on</strong>verballanguage <strong>of</strong> artThe purpose <strong>of</strong> this book is to teach you basic skills in seeing anddrawing. The purpose <strong>of</strong> this book is not teach you to expressyourself, but instead to provide you with <strong>the</strong> skills that will releaseyou from stereotypic expressi<strong>on</strong>. This release in turn will open<strong>the</strong> way for you to express your individuality—your essentialuniqueness—in your own way, using your own particular drawingstyle.If, for a moment, we could regard your handwriting as a form<strong>of</strong> expressive drawing, we could say that you are already expressingyourself with a fundamental element <strong>of</strong> art: line.THE DRAWING EXERCISES: ONE STEP AT A TIME 21


On a sheet <strong>of</strong> paper, right in <strong>the</strong> middle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sheet, writeyour own name <strong>the</strong> way you usually sign your name. Next, regardyour signature from <strong>the</strong> following point <strong>of</strong> view: you are lookingat a drawing which is your original creati<strong>on</strong>—shaped, it is true,by <strong>the</strong> cultural influences <strong>of</strong> your life, but aren't <strong>the</strong> creati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>every artist shaped by such influences?Every time you write your name, you have expressed yourselfthrough <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> line. Your signature, "drawn" many times over,is expressive <strong>of</strong> you, just as Picasso's line is expressive <strong>of</strong> him. Theline can be "read" because, in writing your name, you have used<strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>verbal language <strong>of</strong> art. Let's try reading a line. There aresignatures in <strong>the</strong> margin. All are <strong>the</strong> same name: Lu<strong>the</strong>r Gibs<strong>on</strong>.Tell me, what is <strong>the</strong> first Lu<strong>the</strong>r Gibs<strong>on</strong> like?You would probably agree that Lu<strong>the</strong>r Gibs<strong>on</strong> is more likelyto be extroverted than introverted, more likely to wear bright colorsthan subtle <strong>on</strong>es, and, at least superficially, likely to be outgoing,talkative, even dramatic. Of course, <strong>the</strong>se assumpti<strong>on</strong>s may ormay not be correct, but <strong>the</strong> point is that this is how most peoplewould read <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>verbal expressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> signature, becausethat's what Lu<strong>the</strong>r Gibs<strong>on</strong> is (n<strong>on</strong>verbally) saying.Let's look at <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d Lu<strong>the</strong>r Gibs<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> margin.Now, look at <strong>the</strong> third signature. How would you describehim?And ano<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> fourth signature.And <strong>the</strong> last signature? How would you read that?Now regard your own signature and resp<strong>on</strong>d to <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>verbalmessage <strong>of</strong> its line. Write your name in three different ways, eachtime resp<strong>on</strong>ding to <strong>the</strong> message. Next, think back <strong>on</strong> how youresp<strong>on</strong>ded differently to each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se signatures; recall that <strong>the</strong>name that was formed by <strong>the</strong> "drawings" did not change. What,<strong>the</strong>n, were you resp<strong>on</strong>ding to?22THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


You were seeing and resp<strong>on</strong>ding to <strong>the</strong> felt, individual qualities<strong>of</strong> each "drawn" line or set <strong>of</strong> lines. You resp<strong>on</strong>ded to <strong>the</strong> feltspeed <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> line, <strong>the</strong> size and spacing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> marks, <strong>the</strong> muscletensi<strong>on</strong> or lack <strong>of</strong> tensi<strong>on</strong>. All <strong>of</strong> that is precisely communicatedby <strong>the</strong> line, <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong>al pattern or lack <strong>of</strong> pattern—in o<strong>the</strong>rwords, by <strong>the</strong> whole signatures and all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir parts at <strong>on</strong>ce. Apers<strong>on</strong>'s signature is an individual expressi<strong>on</strong> so unique to <strong>the</strong>writer that it is identified legally as being "owned" by that singlepers<strong>on</strong> and n<strong>on</strong>e o<strong>the</strong>r.Your signature, however, does more than identify you. It alsoexpresses you and your individuality, your creativity. Your signatureis true to yourself. In this sense, you already speak <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>verballanguage <strong>of</strong> art: You are using <strong>the</strong> basic element <strong>of</strong>drawing, line, in an expressive way, unique to yourself.In <strong>the</strong> chapters to follow, <strong>the</strong>refore, we w<strong>on</strong>'t dwell <strong>on</strong> whatyou can do already. Instead, <strong>the</strong> aim is to teach you how to see sothat you can use your expressive, individual line to draw yourpercepti<strong>on</strong>s.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> as a mirror and metaphor for <strong>the</strong> artistThe object <strong>of</strong> drawing is not <strong>on</strong>ly to show what you are trying toportray, but also to show you. To illustrate how much pers<strong>on</strong>alstyle is embedded in drawings, I wish to show you two drawings<strong>on</strong> page 24, d<strong>on</strong>e at <strong>the</strong> same time by two different people—myself and artist/teacher Brian Bomeisler. We sat <strong>on</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>r side<strong>of</strong> our model, Hea<strong>the</strong>r Allan. We were dem<strong>on</strong>strating how todraw a pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait for a group <strong>of</strong> students, <strong>the</strong> same pr<strong>of</strong>ileportrait you will learn to do in Chapter Nine. The materials weused were identical, and we both drew for <strong>the</strong> same length <strong>of</strong>time—30 to 40 minutes. A viewer immediately sees that <strong>the</strong>model is <strong>the</strong> same—that is, both drawings achieve a likeness <strong>of</strong>Hea<strong>the</strong>r. But Brian's portrayal expresses his resp<strong>on</strong>se to Hea<strong>the</strong>rin his more "painterly" style (meaning emphasis <strong>on</strong> shapes), andmy portrayal expresses my resp<strong>on</strong>se in my more "linear" style(emphasis <strong>on</strong> line). By looking at my portrait <strong>of</strong> Hea<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>viewer catches sight <strong>of</strong> me, and Brian's drawing provides anTorii Kiyotada (active 1723-1750),Actor Dancing, and Torii Kiy<strong>on</strong>obu I(1664—1729), Woman Dancer (c. 1708).Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The MetropolitanMuseum <strong>of</strong> Art, Harris BrisbaneDick Fund, 1949-Line expresses two different kinds<strong>of</strong> dances in <strong>the</strong> two Japaneseprints. Try to visualize each dance.Can you hear <strong>the</strong> music in yourimaginati<strong>on</strong>? Try to see how <strong>the</strong>character <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> line c<strong>on</strong>trols yourresp<strong>on</strong>se to <strong>the</strong> drawing.THK DRAWING KXKRCISES: ON'F. STKP AT A TIMF. 23


Hea<strong>the</strong>r by instructor BrianBomeisler.insight into him. Thus, paradoxically, <strong>the</strong> more clearly you canperceive and draw what you see in <strong>the</strong> external world, <strong>the</strong> moreclearly <strong>the</strong> viewer can see you, and <strong>the</strong> more you can know aboutyourself. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> becomes a metaphor for <strong>the</strong> artist.Because <strong>the</strong> exercises in this book focus <strong>on</strong> expanding yourperceptual powers, not <strong>on</strong> techniques <strong>of</strong> drawing, your individualstyle—your unique and valuable manner <strong>of</strong> drawing—willemerge intact. This is true even though <strong>the</strong> exercises c<strong>on</strong>centrate<strong>on</strong> realistic drawings, which tend to "look alike" in a large sense.(This probably is true <strong>on</strong>ly for this century, because we are usedto seeing radically different forms <strong>of</strong> art, both stylistically andculturally.) But a closer look at realistic art reveals subtle differencesin line style, emphasis, and intent. In this age <strong>of</strong> massiveself-expressi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> arts, this more subtle communicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>tengoes unnoticed and unappreciated.As your skills in seeing increase, your ability to draw whatyou see will increase, and you will observe your style forming.Guard it, nurture it, and cherish it, for your style expresses you.As with <strong>the</strong> Zen master-archer, <strong>the</strong> target is yourself.Hea<strong>the</strong>r by <strong>the</strong> author.24THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 2-2. Rembrandt Van Rijn(1606-1669), Winter Landscape(c. 1649). Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The Fogg ArtMuseum, Harvard University.Rembrandt drew this tiny landscapewith a rapid calligraphicline. Through it, we sense Rembrandt'svisual and emoti<strong>on</strong>alresp<strong>on</strong>se to <strong>the</strong> deeply silent winterscene. We see, <strong>the</strong>refore, not<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> landscape; we see through<strong>the</strong> landscape to Rembrandt himself.A "bold" line.A "broken" line.A "pure" line.A "lost-and-found" line.Artists are known by <strong>the</strong>ir uniqueline qualities, and experts in drawing<strong>of</strong>ten base <strong>the</strong>ir au<strong>the</strong>nticati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> drawings <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>se known linequalities. Styles <strong>of</strong> line have actuallybeen put into named categories.There are quite a few: <strong>the</strong>"bold line;" <strong>the</strong> "broken line"(sometimes called "<strong>the</strong> line thatrepeats itself"); <strong>the</strong> "pure line"—thin and precise, sometimes called"<strong>the</strong> Ingres line" after <strong>the</strong> 19th centuryFrench artist Jean-AugusteDominique Ingres; <strong>the</strong> "lost-andfoundline," which starts out dark,fades away, <strong>the</strong>n becomes darkagain. See samples in figure 2-3.Beginning students most <strong>of</strong>tenadmire drawings d<strong>on</strong>e in a rapid,self-c<strong>on</strong>fident style—<strong>the</strong> "bold"line that is ra<strong>the</strong>r like Picasso's, infact. But an important point toremember is that every style <strong>of</strong> line isvalued, <strong>on</strong>e not more than ano<strong>the</strong>r.Fig. 2-3.THE DRAWING EXERCISES: ONE STEP AT A TIME 25


Your <strong>Brain</strong>:The <strong>Right</strong> and Left<strong>of</strong> It


"Few people realize what an ast<strong>on</strong>ishingachievement it is to be able tosee at all. The main c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> new field <strong>of</strong> artificial intelligencehas been not so much to solve<strong>the</strong>se problems <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> handlingas to show what tremendouslydifficult problems <strong>the</strong>y are. When<strong>on</strong>e reflects <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> computati<strong>on</strong>sthat must have to be carriedout before <strong>on</strong>e can recognizeeven such an everyday scene asano<strong>the</strong>r pers<strong>on</strong> crossing <strong>the</strong> street,<strong>on</strong>e is left with a feeling <strong>of</strong> amazementthat such an extraordinaryseries <strong>of</strong> detailed operati<strong>on</strong>s can beaccomplished so effortlessly in sucha short space <strong>of</strong> time."F. H. C. Crick, "Thinking about <strong>the</strong><strong>Brain</strong>," in The <strong>Brain</strong>, San Francisco:A Scientific American Book, W. H.Freeman, 1979, p. 130.How DOES THE HUMAN BRAIN WORK? That remains <strong>the</strong>most baffling and elusive <strong>of</strong> all questi<strong>on</strong>s having to dowith human understanding. Despite centuries <strong>of</strong> study andthought and <strong>the</strong> accelerating rate <strong>of</strong> knowledge in recent years,<strong>the</strong> brain still engenders awe and w<strong>on</strong>der at its marvelous capabilities—many<strong>of</strong> which we simply take for granted.Scientists have targeted visual percepti<strong>on</strong> in particular withhighly precise studies, and yet vast mysteries still exist. The mostordinary activities are awe-inspiring. For example, in a recentc<strong>on</strong>test, people were shown a photograph <strong>of</strong> six mo<strong>the</strong>rs and<strong>the</strong>ir six children, arranged randomly in a group. C<strong>on</strong>testants,strangers to <strong>the</strong> photographed group, were asked to link <strong>the</strong> sixmo<strong>the</strong>r-and-child pairs. Forty people resp<strong>on</strong>ded, and each hadpaired all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs and children correctly.To think <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> complexity <strong>of</strong> that task is to make <strong>on</strong>e's headspin. Our faces are more alike than unlike: two eyes, a nose, amouth, hair, and two ears, all more or less <strong>the</strong> same size and in <strong>the</strong>same places <strong>on</strong> our heads. Telling two people apart requires finediscriminati<strong>on</strong>s bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> capability <strong>of</strong> nearly all computers, asI menti<strong>on</strong>ed in <strong>the</strong> Introducti<strong>on</strong>. In this c<strong>on</strong>test, participants hadto distinguish each adult from all <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs and estimate, usingeven finer discriminati<strong>on</strong>s, which child's features/headshape/expressi<strong>on</strong>best fitted with which adult. The fact that peoplecan accomplish this astounding feat and not realize howastounding it is forms, I think, a measure <strong>of</strong> our underestimati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> our visual abilities.Ano<strong>the</strong>r extraordinary activity is drawing. As far as we know,<strong>of</strong> all <strong>the</strong> creatures <strong>on</strong> this planet, human beings are <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>eswho draw images <strong>of</strong> things and pers<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong>ir envir<strong>on</strong>ment.M<strong>on</strong>keys and elephants have been persuaded to paint and drawand <strong>the</strong>ir artworks have been exhibited and sold. And, indeed,<strong>the</strong>se works do seem to have expressive c<strong>on</strong>tent, but <strong>the</strong>y arenever realistic images <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> animals' percepti<strong>on</strong>s. Animals do notdo still-life, landscape, or portrait drawing. So unless <strong>the</strong>re issome m<strong>on</strong>key that we d<strong>on</strong>'t know about out <strong>the</strong>re in <strong>the</strong> forestdrawing pictures <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r m<strong>on</strong>keys, we can assume that drawing28 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


perceived images is an activity c<strong>on</strong>fined to human beings andmade possible by our human brain.Both sides <strong>of</strong> your brainSeen from above, <strong>the</strong> human brain resembles <strong>the</strong> halves <strong>of</strong> a walnut—twosimilar appearing, c<strong>on</strong>voluted, rounded halves c<strong>on</strong>nectedat <strong>the</strong> center (Figure 3-1). The two halves are called <strong>the</strong>"left hemisphere" and <strong>the</strong> "right hemisphere."The left hemisphere c<strong>on</strong>trols <strong>the</strong> right side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> body; <strong>the</strong>right hemisphere c<strong>on</strong>trols <strong>the</strong> left side. If you suffer a stroke oraccidental brain damage to <strong>the</strong> left half <strong>of</strong> your brain, for example,<strong>the</strong> right half <strong>of</strong> your body will be most seriously affected andvice versa. As part <strong>of</strong> this crossing over <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nerve pathways, <strong>the</strong>left hand is c<strong>on</strong>trolled by <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere; <strong>the</strong> right hand, by<strong>the</strong> left hemisphere, as shown in Figure 3-2.Fig. 3-1.The double brainWith <strong>the</strong> excepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> human beings and possibly s<strong>on</strong>gbirds, <strong>the</strong>greater apes, and certain o<strong>the</strong>r mammals, <strong>the</strong> cerebral hemispheres(<strong>the</strong> two halves <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain) <strong>of</strong> Earth's creatures areFig. 3-2. The crossover c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong> left hand to right hemisphere,right hand to left hemisphere.YOUR BRAIN: THE RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 29


essentially alike, or symmetrical, both in appearance and in functi<strong>on</strong>.Human cerebral hemispheres, and those <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> excepti<strong>on</strong>snoted above, develop asymmetrically in terms <strong>of</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>. Themost noticeable outward effect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> asymmetry <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> humanbrain is handedness, which seems to be unique to human beingsand possibly chimpanzees.For <strong>the</strong> past two hundred years or so, scientists have knownthat language and language-related capabilities are mainlylocated in <strong>the</strong> left hemispheres <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> individuals—approximately 98 percent <strong>of</strong> right-handers and about two-thirds<strong>of</strong> left-handers. Knowledge that <strong>the</strong> left half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain is specializedfor language functi<strong>on</strong>s was largely derived from observati<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> brain injuries. It was apparent, for example,that an injury to <strong>the</strong> left side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain was more likely to causea loss <strong>of</strong> speech capability than an injury <strong>of</strong> equal severity to <strong>the</strong>right side.Because speech and language are such vitally importanthuman capabilities, nineteenth-century scientists named <strong>the</strong> lef<strong>the</strong>misphere <strong>the</strong> "dominant," "leading," or "major" hemisphere.Scientists named <strong>the</strong> right brain <strong>the</strong> "subordinate" or "minor"hemisphere. The general view, which prevailed until fairlyFig. 3-3. A diagram <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e half <strong>of</strong> ahuman brain, showing <strong>the</strong> corpuscallosum and related commissures.30THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


ecently, was that <strong>the</strong> right half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain was less advanced,less evolved than <strong>the</strong> left half—a mute twin with lower-levelcapabilities, directed and carried al<strong>on</strong>g by <strong>the</strong> verbal left hemisphere.Even as late as 1961, neuroscientist. Z. Young could stillw<strong>on</strong>der whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere might be merely a"vestige," though he allowed that he would ra<strong>the</strong>r keep than losehis. [Quoted from The Psychology <strong>of</strong> Left and <strong>Right</strong>, M. Corbalisand Ivan Beale, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1976, p. 101.]A l<strong>on</strong>g-time focus <strong>of</strong> neuroscientific study has been <strong>the</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>s,unknown until fairly recently, <strong>of</strong> a thick nerve cable composed<strong>of</strong> milli<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> fibers that cross-c<strong>on</strong>nect <strong>the</strong> two cerebralhemispheres. This c<strong>on</strong>necting cable, <strong>the</strong> corpus callosum, isshown in <strong>the</strong> diagrammatic drawing <strong>of</strong> half <strong>of</strong> a human brain,Figure 3-3. Because <strong>of</strong> its large size, tremendous number <strong>of</strong> nervefibers, and strategic locati<strong>on</strong> as a c<strong>on</strong>nector <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres,<strong>the</strong> corpus callosum gave all <strong>the</strong> appearances <strong>of</strong> being animportant structure. Yet enigmatically, available evidence indicatedthat <strong>the</strong> corpus callosum could be completely severedwithout observable significant effect. Through a series <strong>of</strong> animalstudies during <strong>the</strong> 1950s, c<strong>on</strong>ducted mainly at <strong>the</strong> CaliforniaInstitute <strong>of</strong> Technology by Roger W. Sperry and his students,R<strong>on</strong>ald Myers, Colwyn Trevar<strong>the</strong>n, and o<strong>the</strong>rs, it was establishedthat a main functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> corpus callosum was to provide communicati<strong>on</strong>between <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres and to allow transmissi<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> memory and learning. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, it was determinedthat if <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>necting cable was severed <strong>the</strong> two brain halves c<strong>on</strong>tinuedto functi<strong>on</strong> independently, thus explaining in part <strong>the</strong>apparent lack <strong>of</strong> effect <strong>on</strong> behavior and functi<strong>on</strong>ing.Then during <strong>the</strong> 1960s, extensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> similar studies to humanneurosurgical patients provided fur<strong>the</strong>r informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> functi<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> corpus callosum and caused scientists to postulate arevised view <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> relative capabilities <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> halves <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>human brain: that both hemispheres are involved in higher cognitivefuncti<strong>on</strong>ing, with each half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain specialized in complementaryfashi<strong>on</strong> for different modes <strong>of</strong> thinking, both highlycomplex.YOUR BRAIN: THE RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 31


As journalist Maya Pines stated inher 1982 book, The <strong>Brain</strong> Changers,"All roads lead to Dr. RogerSperry, a California Institute <strong>of</strong>Technology psychobiology pr<strong>of</strong>essorwho has <strong>the</strong> gift <strong>of</strong> making—orprovoking—important discoveries.""The main <strong>the</strong>me to emerge...is that <strong>the</strong>re appear to be twomodes <strong>of</strong> thinking, verbal andn<strong>on</strong>verbal, represented ra<strong>the</strong>rseparately in left and right hemispheres,respectively, and that oureducati<strong>on</strong>al system, as well as sciencein general, tends to neglect<strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>verbal form <strong>of</strong> intellect.What it comes down to is thatmodern society discriminatesagainst <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere."— Roger W. Sperry"Lateral Specializati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> Cerebral Functi<strong>on</strong> in<strong>the</strong> Surgically SeparatedHemispheres," 1973Because this changed percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain has importantimplicati<strong>on</strong>s for educati<strong>on</strong> in general and for learning to draw inparticular, I'll briefly describe some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> research <strong>of</strong>ten referredto as <strong>the</strong> "split-brain" studies. The research was mainly carriedout at Cal Tech by Sperry and his students Michael Gazzaniga,Jerre Levy, Colwyn Trevar<strong>the</strong>n, Robert Nebes, and o<strong>the</strong>rs.The investigati<strong>on</strong> centered <strong>on</strong> a small group <strong>of</strong> individualswho came to be known as <strong>the</strong> commissurotomy, or "split-brain,"patients. They are pers<strong>on</strong>s who had been greatly disabled by"epileptic seizures that involved both hemispheres. As a last-resortmeasure, after all o<strong>the</strong>r remedies had failed, <strong>the</strong> incapacitatingspread <strong>of</strong> seizures between <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres was c<strong>on</strong>trolledby means <strong>of</strong> an operati<strong>on</strong>, performed by Phillip Vogel and JosephBogen, that severed <strong>the</strong> corpus callosum and <strong>the</strong> related commissures,or cross-c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s, thus isolating <strong>on</strong>e hemisphere from<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r. The operati<strong>on</strong> yielded <strong>the</strong> hoped-for result: Thepatients' seizures were c<strong>on</strong>trolled and <strong>the</strong>y regained health. Inspite <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> radical nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> surgery, <strong>the</strong> patients' outwardappearance, manner, and coordinati<strong>on</strong> were little affected; and tocasual observati<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir ordinary daily behavior seemed littlechanged.The Cal Tech group subsequently worked with <strong>the</strong>se patientsin a series <strong>of</strong> ingenious and subtle tests that revealed <strong>the</strong> separatedfuncti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres. The tests provided surprisingnew evidence that each hemisphere, in a sense, perceivesits own reality—or perhaps better stated, perceives reality in itsown way. The verbal half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain—<strong>the</strong> left half—dominatesmost <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> time in individuals with intact brains as well as in <strong>the</strong>split-brain patients. Using ingenious procedures, however, <strong>the</strong>Cal Tech group tested <strong>the</strong> patients' separated right hemispheresand found evidence that <strong>the</strong> right, n<strong>on</strong>speaking half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brainalso experiences, resp<strong>on</strong>ds with feelings, and processes informati<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong> its own. In our own brains, with intact corpus callosa,communicati<strong>on</strong> between <strong>the</strong> hemispheres melds or rec<strong>on</strong>ciles <strong>the</strong>two percepti<strong>on</strong>s, thus preserving our sense <strong>of</strong> being <strong>on</strong>e pers<strong>on</strong>, aunified being.In additi<strong>on</strong> to studying <strong>the</strong> right/left separati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> inner32THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


mental experience created by <strong>the</strong> surgical procedure, <strong>the</strong> scientistsexamined <strong>the</strong> different ways in which <strong>the</strong> two hemispheresprocess informati<strong>on</strong>. Evidence accumulated showing that <strong>the</strong>mode <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere is verbal and analytic, while that <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> right is n<strong>on</strong>verbal and global. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> evidence found by JerreLevy in her doctoral studies showed that <strong>the</strong> mode <strong>of</strong> processingused by <strong>the</strong> right brain is rapid, complex, whole-pattern, spatial,and perceptual—processing that is not <strong>on</strong>ly different from butcomparable in complexity to <strong>the</strong> left brain's verbal, analyticmode. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, Levy found indicati<strong>on</strong>s that <strong>the</strong> two modes <strong>of</strong>processing tend to interfere with each o<strong>the</strong>r, preventing maximalperformance; and she suggested that this may be a rati<strong>on</strong>ale for<strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary development <strong>of</strong> asymmetry in <strong>the</strong> humanbrain—as a means <strong>of</strong> keeping <strong>the</strong> two different modes <strong>of</strong> processingin two different hemispheres.Based <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> evidence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> split-brain studies, <strong>the</strong> viewcame gradually that both hemispheres use high human-level cognitivemodes which, though different, involve thinking, reas<strong>on</strong>ing,and complex mental functi<strong>on</strong>ing. Over <strong>the</strong> past decade, since <strong>the</strong>first statement in 1968 by Levy and Sperry, scientists have foundextensive supporting evidence for this view, not <strong>on</strong>ly in braininjuredpatients but also in individuals with normal, intact brains.A few examples <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> specially designed tests devised for usewith <strong>the</strong> split-brain patients might illustrate <strong>the</strong> separate realityperceived by each hemisphere and <strong>the</strong> special modes <strong>of</strong> processingemployed. In <strong>on</strong>e test, two different pictures were flashed foran instant <strong>on</strong> a screen, with a split-brain patient's eyes fixed <strong>on</strong> amidpoint so that scanning both images was prevented. Eachhemisphere, <strong>the</strong>n, received different pictures. A picture <strong>of</strong> aspo<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> left side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> screen went to <strong>the</strong> right brain; a picture<strong>of</strong> a knife <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> right side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> screen went to <strong>the</strong> verballeft brain, as in Figure 3-4. When questi<strong>on</strong>ed, <strong>the</strong> patient gavedifferent resp<strong>on</strong>ses. If asked to name what had been flashed <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>screen, <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>fidently articulate left hemisphere caused <strong>the</strong>patient to say, "knife." Then <strong>the</strong> patient was asked to reach behinda curtain with his left hand (right hemisphere) and pick out whathad been flashed <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> screen. The patient <strong>the</strong>n picked out a"The data indicate that <strong>the</strong> mute,minor hemisphere is specializedfor Gestalt percepti<strong>on</strong>, being primarilya syn<strong>the</strong>sist in dealing withinformati<strong>on</strong> input. The speaking,major hemisphere, in c<strong>on</strong>trast,seems to operate in a more logical,analytic, computer-like fashi<strong>on</strong>. Itslanguage is inadequate for <strong>the</strong>rapid complex syn<strong>the</strong>ses achievedby <strong>the</strong> minor hemisphere."—Jerre Levy andR. W. Sperry1968Fig. 3-4. A diagram <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>apparatus used to test visualtactileassociati<strong>on</strong>s by split-brainpatients. Adapted from MichaelS. Gazzaniga, "The Split <strong>Brain</strong>in Man."YOUR BRAIN: THE RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 33


spo<strong>on</strong> from a group <strong>of</strong> objects that included a spo<strong>on</strong> and a knife.If <strong>the</strong> experimenter asked <strong>the</strong> patient to identify what he held inhis hand behind <strong>the</strong> curtain, <strong>the</strong> patient might look c<strong>on</strong>fused for amoment and <strong>the</strong>n say, "A knife." The right hemisphere, knowingthat <strong>the</strong> answer was wr<strong>on</strong>g but not having sufficient words to correct<strong>the</strong> articulate left hemisphere, c<strong>on</strong>tinued <strong>the</strong> dialogue bycausing <strong>the</strong> patient to mutely shake his head. At that, <strong>the</strong> verballeft hemisphere w<strong>on</strong>dered aloud, "Why am I shaking my head?"In ano<strong>the</strong>r test that dem<strong>on</strong>strated <strong>the</strong> right brain to be betterat spatial problems, a male patient was given several woodenshapes to arrange to match a certain design. His attempts with hisright hand (left hemisphere) failed again and again. His left handkept trying to help. The right hand would knock <strong>the</strong> left handaway; and finally, <strong>the</strong> man had to sit <strong>on</strong> his left hand to keep itaway from <strong>the</strong> puzzle. When <strong>the</strong> scientists finally suggested tha<strong>the</strong> use both hands, <strong>the</strong> spatially "smart" left hand had to shove <strong>the</strong>spatially "dumb" right hand away to keep it from interfering.As a result <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se extraordinary findings over <strong>the</strong> past fifteenyears, we now know that despite our normal feeling that we are<strong>on</strong>e pers<strong>on</strong>—a single being—our brains are double, each halfwith its own way <strong>of</strong> knowing, its own way <strong>of</strong> perceiving externalreality. In a manner <strong>of</strong> speaking, each <strong>of</strong> us has two minds, twoc<strong>on</strong>sciousnesses, mediated and integrated by <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>nectingcable <strong>of</strong> nerve fibers between <strong>the</strong> hemispheres.We have learned that <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres can work toge<strong>the</strong>rin a number <strong>of</strong> ways. Sometimes <strong>the</strong>y cooperate with each halfc<strong>on</strong>tributing its special abilities and taking <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> particular part<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> task that is suited to its mode <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> processing. Ato<strong>the</strong>r times, <strong>the</strong> hemispheres can work singly, with <strong>on</strong>e modemore or less "leading," <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r more or less "following." And itseems that <strong>the</strong> hemispheres may also c<strong>on</strong>flict, <strong>on</strong>e half attemptingto do what <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r half "knows" it can do better. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, itmay be that each hemisphere has a way <strong>of</strong> keeping knowledgefrom <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hemisphere. It may be, as <strong>the</strong> saying goes, that <strong>the</strong>right hand truly does not know what <strong>the</strong> left hand is doing.34 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


The double reality <strong>of</strong> split-brain patientsBut what, you might ask, does all this have to do with learninghow to draw? Research <strong>on</strong> brain-hemisphere aspects <strong>of</strong> visualpercepti<strong>on</strong> indicates that ability to draw may depend <strong>on</strong> whe<strong>the</strong>ryou can access at c<strong>on</strong>scious level <strong>the</strong> "minor," or subdominant, R-mode. How does this help a pers<strong>on</strong> to draw? It appears that <strong>the</strong>right brain perceives—processes visual informati<strong>on</strong>—in a modesuitable for drawing, and that <strong>the</strong> left-brain mode <strong>of</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>ingmay be inappropriate for complex realistic drawing <strong>of</strong> perceivedforms.Nasrudin was sitting with a friendas dusk fell. "Light a candle," <strong>the</strong>man said, "because it is dark now.There is <strong>on</strong>e just by your left side.""How can I tell my right from myleft in <strong>the</strong> dark, you fool?" asked<strong>the</strong> Mulla.— Indries ShahThe Exploits <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Incomparable MullaNasrudinLanguage cluesIn hindsight, we realize that human beings must have had somesense <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> differences between <strong>the</strong> halves <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain. Languagesworldwide c<strong>on</strong>tain numerous words and phrases suggestingthat <strong>the</strong> left side <strong>of</strong> a pers<strong>on</strong> has different characteristics from<strong>the</strong> right side. These terms indicate not just differences in locati<strong>on</strong>but differences in fundamental traits or qualities. For example,if we want to compare unlike ideas, we say, "On <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e hand... <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand..." "A left-handed compliment," meaning asly dig, indicates <strong>the</strong> differing qualities we assign to left and right.Keep in mind, however, that <strong>the</strong>se phrases generally speak <strong>of</strong>hands, but because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> crossover c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> hands andhemispheres, <strong>the</strong> terms can be inferred also to mean <strong>the</strong> hemispheresthat c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>the</strong> hands. Therefore, <strong>the</strong> examples <strong>of</strong> familiarterms in <strong>the</strong> next secti<strong>on</strong> refer specifically to <strong>the</strong> left and righthands but in reality also refer inferentially to <strong>the</strong> opposite brainhalves—<strong>the</strong> left hand c<strong>on</strong>trolled by <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere, <strong>the</strong>right hand by <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere.The bias <strong>of</strong> language and customsWords and phrases c<strong>on</strong>cerning c<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>of</strong> left and right permeateour language and thinking. The right hand (meaning also <strong>the</strong>left hemisphere) is str<strong>on</strong>gly c<strong>on</strong>nected with what is good, just,moral, and proper. The left hand (<strong>the</strong>refore <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere)YOUR BRAIN: THE RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 35


is str<strong>on</strong>gly linked with c<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>of</strong> anarchy and feelings that areout <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>scious c<strong>on</strong>trol—somehow bad, immoral, and dangerous.Until very recently, <strong>the</strong> ancient bias against <strong>the</strong> left hand/right hemisphere sometimes even led parents and teachers <strong>of</strong>left-handed children to try to force <strong>the</strong> children to use <strong>the</strong>ir righthands for writing, eating, and so <strong>on</strong>—a practice that <strong>of</strong>ten causedproblems lasting into adulthood.Throughout human history, terms with c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> goodfor <strong>the</strong> right hand/left hemisphere and c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> bad for<strong>the</strong> left hand/right hemisphere appear in most languages around<strong>the</strong> world. The Latin word for left is sinister, meaning "bad," "ominous,""treacherous." The Latin word for right is dexter, fromwhich comes our word "dexterity," meaning "skill" or "adroitness."The French word for left—remember that <strong>the</strong> left hand isc<strong>on</strong>nected to <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere—is gauche, meaning "awkward,"from which comes our word "gawky." The French word forright is droit, meaning "good," "just," or "proper."In English, left comes from <strong>the</strong> Anglo-Sax<strong>on</strong> lyft, meaning"weak" or "worthless." The left hand <strong>of</strong> most right-handed peopleis in fact weaker than <strong>the</strong> right, but <strong>the</strong> original word also impliedlack <strong>of</strong> moral strength. The derogatory meaning <strong>of</strong> left may reflecta prejudice <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> right-handed majority against a minority<strong>of</strong> people who were different, that is, left-handed. Reinforcingthis bias, <strong>the</strong> Anglo-Sax<strong>on</strong> word for right, reht (or riht), meant"straight" or "just." From reht and its Latin cognate rectus wederived our words "correct" and "rectitude."These ideas are also reflected in our political vocabulary. Thepolitical right, for instance, admires nati<strong>on</strong>al power, is c<strong>on</strong>servative,and resists change. The political left, c<strong>on</strong>versely, admiresindividual aut<strong>on</strong>omy and promotes change, even radical change.At <strong>the</strong>ir extremes, <strong>the</strong> political right is fascist, <strong>the</strong> political left isanarchist.In <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>of</strong> cultural customs, <strong>the</strong> place <strong>of</strong> h<strong>on</strong>or at a formaldinner is <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> host's right-hand side. The groom stands <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> right in <strong>the</strong> marriage cerem<strong>on</strong>y, <strong>the</strong> bride <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> left—a n<strong>on</strong>-36THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


verbal message <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> relative status <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two participants. Weshake hands with our right hands; it seems somehow wr<strong>on</strong>g toshake hands with our left hands.Under "left-handed," <strong>the</strong> dicti<strong>on</strong>ary lists as syn<strong>on</strong>yms"clumsy," "awkward," "insincere," "malicious." Syn<strong>on</strong>yms for"right-handed," however, are "correct," "indispensable," and"reliable." Now, it's important to remember that <strong>the</strong>se terms wereall made up, when languages began, by some pers<strong>on</strong>s' left hemispheres—<strong>the</strong>left brain calling <strong>the</strong> right bad names! And <strong>the</strong> rightbrain—labeled, pinpointed, and butt<strong>on</strong>holed—was without alanguage <strong>of</strong> its own to defend itself.Two ways <strong>of</strong> knowingAl<strong>on</strong>g with <strong>the</strong> opposite c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> left and right in our language,c<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> duality, or two-sidedness, <strong>of</strong> human natureand thought have been postulated by philosophers, teachers, andscientists from many different times and cultures. The key idea isthat <strong>the</strong>re are two parallel "ways <strong>of</strong> knowing."You probably are familiar with <strong>the</strong>se ideas. As with <strong>the</strong>left/right terms, <strong>the</strong>y are embedded in our languages and cultures.The main divisi<strong>on</strong>s are, for example, between thinking andfeeling, intellect and intuiti<strong>on</strong>, objective analysis and subjectiveinsight. Political writers say that people generally analyze <strong>the</strong>good and bad points <strong>of</strong> an issue and <strong>the</strong>n vote <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir "gut" feelings.The history <strong>of</strong> science is replete with anecdotes aboutresearchers who try repeatedly to figure out a problem and <strong>the</strong>nhave a dream in which <strong>the</strong> answer presents itself as a metaphorintuitively comprehended by <strong>the</strong> scientist. The statement <strong>on</strong> page39 by Henri Poincare is a vivid example <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> process.In ano<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>text, people occasi<strong>on</strong>ally say about some<strong>on</strong>e,"The words sound okay, but something tells me not to trust him(or her)." Or "I can't tell you in words exactly what it is, but <strong>the</strong>reis something about that pers<strong>on</strong> that I like (or dislike)." Thesestatements are intuitive observati<strong>on</strong>s that both sides <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brainare at work, processing <strong>the</strong> same informati<strong>on</strong> in two differentways.Parallel Ways <strong>of</strong> Knowingintellectc<strong>on</strong>vergentdigitalsec<strong>on</strong>daryabstractdirectedpropositi<strong>on</strong>alanalyticlinealrati<strong>on</strong>alsequentialanalyticobjectivesuccessiveintuiti<strong>on</strong>divergentanalogicprimaryc<strong>on</strong>cretefreeimaginativerelati<strong>on</strong>aln<strong>on</strong>linealintuitivemultipleholisticsubjectivesimultaneous—J. E. Bogen"Some Educati<strong>on</strong>alAspects <strong>of</strong> HemisphereSpecializati<strong>on</strong>" in UCLAEducator, 1972The Duality <strong>of</strong> Yin and YangYinfemininenegativemo<strong>on</strong>darknessyieldingleft sidecoldautumnwinterunc<strong>on</strong>sciousright brainemoti<strong>on</strong>Yangmasculinepositivesunlightaggressiveright sidewarmspringsummerc<strong>on</strong>sciousleft brainreas<strong>on</strong>— I Ching or Book <strong>of</strong> Changes,a Chinese Taoist workYOUR BRAIN: THE RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 37


Dr.J. William Bergquist, a ma<strong>the</strong>maticianand specialist in <strong>the</strong> computerlanguage known as APL,proposed in a paper given at Snowmass,Colorado, in 1977 that we canlook forward to computers thatcombine digital and analog functi<strong>on</strong>sin <strong>on</strong>e machine. Dr. Bergquistdubbed his machine "The BifurcatedComputer." He stated thatsuch a computer would functi<strong>on</strong>similarly to <strong>the</strong> two halves <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>human brain."The left hemisphere analyzes overtime, whereas <strong>the</strong> right hemispheresyn<strong>the</strong>sizes over space."—Jerre Levy"PsychobiologicalImplicati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> BilateralAsymmetry," 1974"Every creative act involves ...a new innocence <strong>of</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>,liberated from <strong>the</strong> cataract <strong>of</strong>accepted belief."— Arthur KoestlerThe Sleepwalkers, 1959The two modes <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> processingInside each <strong>of</strong> our skulls, <strong>the</strong>refore, we have a double brain withtwo ways <strong>of</strong> knowing. The dualities and differing characteristics<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two halves <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain and body, intuitively expressed inour language, have a real basis in <strong>the</strong> physiology <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> humanbrain. Because <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>necting fibers are intact in normal brains,we rarely experience at a c<strong>on</strong>scious level c<strong>on</strong>flicts revealed by <strong>the</strong>tests <strong>on</strong> split-brain patients.Never<strong>the</strong>less, as each <strong>of</strong> our hemispheres ga<strong>the</strong>rs in <strong>the</strong> samesensory informati<strong>on</strong>, each half <strong>of</strong> our brains may handle <strong>the</strong>informati<strong>on</strong> in different ways: The task may be divided between<strong>the</strong> hemispheres, each handling <strong>the</strong> part suited to its style. Or <strong>on</strong>ehemisphere, <strong>of</strong>ten <strong>the</strong> dominant left, will "take over" and inhibit<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r half. The left hemisphere analyzes, abstracts, counts,marks time, plans step-by-step procedures, verbalizes, and makesrati<strong>on</strong>al statements based <strong>on</strong> logic. For example, "Given numbersa, b, and c—we can say that if a is greater than b, and b is greaterthan c, <strong>the</strong>n a is necessarily greater than c." This statement illustrates<strong>the</strong> left-hemisphere mode: <strong>the</strong> analytic, verbal, figuringout,sequential, symbolic, linear, objective mode.On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, we have a sec<strong>on</strong>d way <strong>of</strong> knowing: <strong>the</strong>right-hemisphere mode. We "see" things in this mode that may beimaginary—existing <strong>on</strong>ly in <strong>the</strong> mind's eye. In <strong>the</strong> example givenjust above, did you perhaps visualize <strong>the</strong> "a, b, c" relati<strong>on</strong>ship? Invisual mode, we see how things exist in space and how <strong>the</strong> partsgo toge<strong>the</strong>r to make up <strong>the</strong> whole. Using <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere, weunderstand metaphors, we dream, we create new combinati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>ideas. When something is too complex to describe, we can makegestures that communicate. Psychologist David Galin has afavorite example: try to describe a spiral staircase without makinga spiral gesture. And using <strong>the</strong> right-hemisphere mode, we areable to draw pictures <strong>of</strong> our percepti<strong>on</strong>s.My students report that learning to draw makes <strong>the</strong>m feelmore "artistic" and <strong>the</strong>refore more creative. One definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> acreative pers<strong>on</strong> is some<strong>on</strong>e who can process in new ways informati<strong>on</strong>directly at hand—<strong>the</strong> ordinary sensory data available to38THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


all <strong>of</strong> us. A writer uses words, a musician notes, an artist visualpercepti<strong>on</strong>s, and all need some knowledge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> techniques <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>ir crafts. But a creative individual intuitively sees possibilitiesfor transforming ordinary data into a new creati<strong>on</strong>, transcendentover <strong>the</strong> mere raw materials.Time and again, creative individuals have recognized <strong>the</strong>differences between <strong>the</strong> two processes <strong>of</strong> ga<strong>the</strong>ring data andtransforming those data creatively. Neuroscience is now illuminatingthat dual process. I propose that getting to know both sides<strong>of</strong> your brain is an important step in liberating your creativepotential.The Ah-ha! resp<strong>on</strong>seIn <strong>the</strong> right-hemisphere mode <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> processing, we useintuiti<strong>on</strong> and have leaps <strong>of</strong> insight—moments when "everythingseems to fall into place" without figuring things out in a logicalorder. When this occurs, people <strong>of</strong>ten sp<strong>on</strong>taneously exclaim,"I've got it" or "Ah, yes, now I see <strong>the</strong> picture." The classic example<strong>of</strong> this kind <strong>of</strong> exclamati<strong>on</strong> is <strong>the</strong> exultant cry, "Eureka!" (Ihave found it!) attributed to Archimedes. According to <strong>the</strong> story,Archimedes experienced a flash <strong>of</strong> insight while bathing thatenabled him to use <strong>the</strong> weight <strong>of</strong> displaced water to determinewhe<strong>the</strong>r a certain crown was pure gold or alloyed with silver.This, <strong>the</strong>n, is <strong>the</strong> right-hemisphere mode: <strong>the</strong> intuitive, subjective,relati<strong>on</strong>al, holistic, time-free mode. This is also <strong>the</strong> disdained,weak, left-handed mode that in our culture has beengenerally ignored. For example, most <strong>of</strong> our educati<strong>on</strong>al systemhas been designed to cultivate <strong>the</strong> verbal, rati<strong>on</strong>al, <strong>on</strong>-time lef<strong>the</strong>misphere, while half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain <strong>of</strong> every student is virtuallyneglected.The nineteenth-century ma<strong>the</strong>maticianHenri Poincare describeda sudden intuiti<strong>on</strong> that gave him<strong>the</strong> soluti<strong>on</strong> to a difficult problem:"One evening, c<strong>on</strong>trary to mycustom, I drank black c<strong>of</strong>fee andcould not sleep. Ideas rose incrowds; I felt <strong>the</strong>m collide untilpairs interlocked, so to speak,making a stable combinati<strong>on</strong>."[That strange phenomen<strong>on</strong> provided<strong>the</strong> intuiti<strong>on</strong> that solved <strong>the</strong>troublesome problem. Poincarec<strong>on</strong>tinued,] "It seems, in suchcases, that <strong>on</strong>e is present at his ownunc<strong>on</strong>scious work, made partiallyperceptible to <strong>the</strong> overexcited c<strong>on</strong>sciousness,yet without havingchanged its nature. Then wevaguely comprehend what distinguishes<strong>the</strong> two mechanisms or, ifyou wish, <strong>the</strong> working methods <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> two egos."Half a brain is better than n<strong>on</strong>e: A whole brain wouldbe betterWith <strong>the</strong>ir sequenced verbal and numerical classes, <strong>the</strong> schoolsyou and I attended were not equipped to teach <strong>the</strong> right-hemispheremode. The right hemisphere is not, after all, under veryYOUR BRAIN: TI1K RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 39


"Approaching forty, I had a singulardream in which I almost grasped<strong>the</strong> meaning and understood <strong>the</strong>nature <strong>of</strong> what it is that wastes inwasted time."— Cyril C<strong>on</strong>nollyThe Unquiet Grave: A WordCycle by Palinuris, 1945Many creative people seem to haveintuitive awareness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> separate-sidedbrain. For example,Rudyard Kipling wrote <strong>the</strong> followingpoem, entitled "The Two-<strong>Side</strong>d Man," more than fifty yearsago.Much I owe to <strong>the</strong> lands that grew-More to <strong>the</strong> Lives that fed-But most to <strong>the</strong> Allah Who gave meTwoSeparate sides to my head.Much I reflect <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Good and <strong>the</strong>TrueIn <strong>the</strong> faiths beneath <strong>the</strong> sunBut most up<strong>on</strong> Allah Who gave meTwo<strong>Side</strong>s to my head, not <strong>on</strong>e.I would go without shirt or shoe,Friend, tobacco or bread,So<strong>on</strong>er than lose for a minute <strong>the</strong>twoSeparate sides <strong>of</strong> my head!— Rudyard Kiplinggood verbal c<strong>on</strong>trol. You can't reas<strong>on</strong> with it. You can't get it tomake logical propositi<strong>on</strong>s such as "This is good and that is bad,for a, b, and c reas<strong>on</strong>s." It is metaphorically left-handed, with all<strong>the</strong> ancient c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> that characteristic. The right hemisphereis not good at sequencing—doing <strong>the</strong> first thing first, taking<strong>the</strong> next step, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> next. It may start anywhere, or takeeverything at <strong>on</strong>ce. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere hasn't agood sense <strong>of</strong> time and doesn't seem to comprehend what ismeant by <strong>the</strong> term "wasting time," as does <strong>the</strong> good, sensible lef<strong>the</strong>misphere. The right brain is not good at categorizing and naming.It seems to regard <strong>the</strong> thing as-it-is, at <strong>the</strong> present moment <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> present; seeing things for what <strong>the</strong>y simply are, in all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>irawesome, fascinating complexity. It is not good at analyzing andabstracting salient characteristics.Today, educators are increasingly c<strong>on</strong>cerned with <strong>the</strong> importance<strong>of</strong> intuitive and creative thought. Never<strong>the</strong>less, school systemsin general are still structured in <strong>the</strong> left-hemisphere mode.Teaching is sequenced: Students progress through grades <strong>on</strong>e,two, three, etc., in a linear directi<strong>on</strong>. The main subjects learnersstudy are verbal and numerical: reading, writing, arithmetic.Nowadays, however, seats <strong>of</strong>ten are set circles ra<strong>the</strong>r than in rows.Time schedules are more flexible. But learners still c<strong>on</strong>verge<strong>on</strong> "correct" answers to <strong>of</strong>ten-ambiguous questi<strong>on</strong>s. Teachers stillgive out grades that <strong>of</strong>ten are tied to <strong>the</strong> "bell curve," which guaranteesthat <strong>on</strong>e-third <strong>of</strong> every group will be judged "belowaverage," regardless <strong>of</strong> achievement. And every<strong>on</strong>e senses thatsomething is amiss.The right brain—<strong>the</strong> dreamer, <strong>the</strong> artificer, and <strong>the</strong> artist—islost in our school system and goes largely untaught. We mightfind a few art classes, a few shop classes, something called "creativewriting," and perhaps courses in music; but it's unlikely thatwe would find courses in imaginati<strong>on</strong>, in visualizati<strong>on</strong>, in perceptualor spatial skills, in creativity as a separate subject, in intuiti<strong>on</strong>,in inventiveness. Yet educators value <strong>the</strong>se skills and haveapparently hoped that students would develop imaginati<strong>on</strong>, percepti<strong>on</strong>,and intuiti<strong>on</strong> as natural c<strong>on</strong>sequences <strong>of</strong> training in verbal,analytic skills.40 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fortunately, such development <strong>of</strong>ten does occur almost inspite <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> school system—a tribute to <strong>the</strong> survival capacity <strong>of</strong>creative abilities. But <strong>the</strong> emphasis <strong>of</strong> our culture is so str<strong>on</strong>glyslanted toward rewarding left-brain skills that we are surely losinga very large proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> potential ability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rhalves <strong>of</strong> our children's brains. Scientist Jerre Levy has said—<strong>on</strong>ly partly humorously—that American scientific trainingthrough graduate school may entirely destroy <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere.We certainly are aware <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> inadequate trainingin verbal, computati<strong>on</strong>al skills. The verbal left hemispherenever seems to recover fully, and <strong>the</strong> effects may handicap studentsfor life. What happens, <strong>the</strong>n, to <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere that ishardly trained at all?Perhaps now that neuroscientists have provided a c<strong>on</strong>ceptualbase for right-brain training, we can begin to build a school systemthat will teach <strong>the</strong> whole brain. Such a system will surelyinclude training in drawing skills—an efficient, effective way toteach thinking strategies suited to <strong>the</strong> right brain.Handedness, left or rightStudents ask many questi<strong>on</strong>s about left- and right-handedness.This is a good place to address <strong>the</strong> subject, before we begininstructi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> basic skills <strong>of</strong> drawing. I will attempt to clarify<strong>on</strong>ly a few points, because <strong>the</strong> extensive research <strong>on</strong> handednessis difficult and complicated.First, classifying people as strictly left-handed or righthandedis not quite accurate. People range from being completelyleft-handed or completely right-handed to being completelyambidextrous—that is, able to do many things with ei<strong>the</strong>r hand,without a decided preference. Most <strong>of</strong> us fall somewhere <strong>on</strong> ac<strong>on</strong>tinuum, with about 90 percent <strong>of</strong> humans preferring, more orless str<strong>on</strong>gly, <strong>the</strong> right hand, and 10 percent preferring <strong>the</strong> left.The percentage <strong>of</strong> individuals with left-hand preference forhandwriting seems to be rising, from about 2 percent in 1932 toabout 11 percent in <strong>the</strong> 1980s. The main reas<strong>on</strong> for this rise is probablythat teachers and parents have learned to tolerate left-"To make biological survivalpossible, Mind at Large has to befunneled through <strong>the</strong> reducingvalve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain and nervoussystem. What comes out <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rend is a measly trickle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> kind<strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness which will help usto stay alive <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> surface <strong>of</strong> thisparticular planet. To formulateand express <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tents <strong>of</strong> thisreduced awareness, man hasinvented and endlessly elaboratedthose symbol-systems and implicitphilosophies which we calllanguages."— Aldous HuxleyThe Doors <strong>of</strong> Percepti<strong>on</strong>Some famous individuals usuallyclassified as left-handers:Charlie ChaplinJudy GarlandTed WilliamsRobert McNamaraGeorge BurnsLewis CarrollKing George VI <strong>of</strong> BritainW. C. FieldsAlbert EinsteinBilly <strong>the</strong> KidQueen VictoriaHarry S. TrumanCasey StengelCharlemagnePaul McCartneyPharoah Rameses IICole PorterGerald FordCary GrantRingo StarrPrince CharlesBenjamin FranklinJulius CaesarMarilyn M<strong>on</strong>roeGeorge BushYOUR BRAIN: THE RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 41


Mirror writing reverses <strong>the</strong> shape<strong>of</strong> every letter and is written fromright to left—that is, backwards.Only when held up to a mirrordoes it become legible for mostreaders:The most famous mirror-writer inhistory is <strong>the</strong> Italian artist, inventor,and left-hander Le<strong>on</strong>ardo daVinci. Ano<strong>the</strong>r is Lewis Carroll,left-handed author <strong>of</strong> Alice's Adventuresin W<strong>on</strong>derland and its sequel,Through <strong>the</strong> tooking-Glass and WhatAlice Found There, whose mirrorwrittenpoem is shown above.Most right-handers find mirrorwriting difficult, but it is quite easyfor many left-handers.Try writing your signature inmirror writing.handed writing and no l<strong>on</strong>ger force children to use <strong>the</strong> righthand. This relatively new tolerance is fortunate, because forciblechange can cause a child to have serious problems, such as stuttering,right/left directi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong>, and difficulty in learningto read.A useful way to regard handedness is to recognize that handpreference is <strong>the</strong> most visible outward sign <strong>of</strong> how an individual'sbrain is organized. There are o<strong>the</strong>r outward signs: eyedness(every<strong>on</strong>e has a dominant eye, used in sighting al<strong>on</strong>g an edge, for.example) and footedness (<strong>the</strong> foot used to step <strong>of</strong>f a curb or tostart a dance step). The key reas<strong>on</strong> for not forcing a child to use<strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>preferred hand is that brain organizati<strong>on</strong> is probablygenetically determined, and forcing a change works against thisnatural organizati<strong>on</strong>. Natural preference is so str<strong>on</strong>g that pastefforts to change left-handers <strong>of</strong>ten resulted in ambidexterity:children capitulated to pressure (in <strong>the</strong> old days, even punishment)and learned to use <strong>the</strong> right hand for writing but c<strong>on</strong>tinuedto use <strong>the</strong> left for everything else.Moreover, <strong>the</strong>re is no acceptable reas<strong>on</strong> for teachers or parentsto force a change. Reas<strong>on</strong>s pr<strong>of</strong>fered run from "Writing with<strong>the</strong> left hand looks so uncomfortable," to "The world is set up forright-handers and my left-handed child would be at a disadvantage."These are not good reas<strong>on</strong>s, and I believe <strong>the</strong>y <strong>of</strong>ten maskan inherent prejudice against left-handedness—a prejudice nowrapidly disappearing, I'm happy to report.Putting prejudice aside, <strong>the</strong>re are important differencesbetween left-handers and right-handers. Left-handers are generallyless lateralized than right-handers. Lateralizati<strong>on</strong> means <strong>the</strong>degree to which specific functi<strong>on</strong>s are carried out almost exclusivelyby <strong>on</strong>e hemisphere. For example, left-handers more frequentlyprocess language in both hemispheres and process spatialinformati<strong>on</strong> in both hemispheres than do right-handers. Specifically,language is mediated in <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere in 90 percent <strong>of</strong>right-handers and 70 percent <strong>of</strong> left-handers. Of <strong>the</strong> remaining 10percent <strong>of</strong> right-handers, about 2 percent have language locatedin <strong>the</strong> right brain, and about 8 percent mediate language in bothhemispheres. Of <strong>the</strong> remaining 30 percent <strong>of</strong> left-handers, about41THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


15 percent have language located in <strong>the</strong> right brain, and about 15percent mediate language in both hemispheres. Note that individualswith right-hemisphere language locati<strong>on</strong>—termed righ<strong>the</strong>mispheredominance, since language dominates—<strong>of</strong>ten writein <strong>the</strong> "hooked" positi<strong>on</strong> that seems to cause teachers so muchdismay. Scientist Jerre Levy has proposed that hand positi<strong>on</strong> inwriting is ano<strong>the</strong>r outward sign <strong>of</strong> brain organizati<strong>on</strong>.Do <strong>the</strong>se differences matter? Individuals vary so much thatgeneralizati<strong>on</strong>s are risky. Never<strong>the</strong>less, experts agree in generalthat a mixture <strong>of</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>s in both hemispheres (that is, a lesserdegree <strong>of</strong> lateralizati<strong>on</strong>) creates <strong>the</strong> potential for c<strong>on</strong>flict or interference.It is true that left-handers statistically are more pr<strong>on</strong>e tostutter and to experience <strong>the</strong> reading difficulty called dyslexia.However, o<strong>the</strong>r experts suggest that bilateral distributi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>smay produce superior mental abilities. Left-handers excelin ma<strong>the</strong>matics, music, and chess. And <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> art certainlygives evidence <strong>of</strong> an advantage for left-handedness: Le<strong>on</strong>ardo daVinci, Michelangelo, and Raphael were all left-handed.Former United States Vice PresidentNels<strong>on</strong> Rockefeller, a changedleft-hander, had difficulty readingprepared speeches because <strong>of</strong> atendency to read backward fromright to left. The cause <strong>of</strong> this difficultymay have been his fa<strong>the</strong>r'sunrelenting effort to change hiss<strong>on</strong>'s left-handedness."Around <strong>the</strong> family dinner table, <strong>the</strong>elder Mr. Rockefeller would put arubber band around his s<strong>on</strong>'s leftwrist, tie a l<strong>on</strong>g string <strong>on</strong> it andjerk <strong>the</strong> string whenever Nels<strong>on</strong>started to eat with his left hand, <strong>the</strong><strong>on</strong>e he naturally favored."— Quoted in The Left-Handers' Handbookby J. Bliss and J. Morella,1980Eventually, young Nels<strong>on</strong>capitulated and achieved a ra<strong>the</strong>rawkward ambidextrouscompromise, but he suffered <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>sequences <strong>of</strong> his fa<strong>the</strong>r'srigidity throughout his lifetime.Aztecs in early Mexico used <strong>the</strong>left hand for medicine for kidneytrouble, <strong>the</strong> right when curing <strong>the</strong>liver.From The Left-Handers' Handbook,by J. Bliss and J. Morella.The Incas <strong>of</strong> ancient Peruc<strong>on</strong>sidered left-handedness a sign<strong>of</strong> good fortune.Mayan Indians were pro-right:<strong>the</strong> twitching <strong>of</strong> a soothsayer's leftleg foretold disaster.YOUR BRAIN: THE RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 43


A comparis<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> left-mode and right-mode characteristicsIVerbal Using words to name, describe, define. N<strong>on</strong>verbal Using n<strong>on</strong>-verbal cogniti<strong>on</strong> to processpercepti<strong>on</strong>s.Analytic Figuring things out step-by-step and partby-part.Syn<strong>the</strong>tic Putting things toge<strong>the</strong>r to form wholes.Symbolic Using a symbol to stand for something. Forexample, <strong>the</strong> drawn form stands for eye,<strong>the</strong> sign + stands for <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> additi<strong>on</strong>.Actual, realRelating to things as <strong>the</strong>y are, at <strong>the</strong> presentmoment.Abstract Taking out a small bit <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> andusing it to represent <strong>the</strong> whole thing.AnalogicSeeing likenesses am<strong>on</strong>g things; understandingmetaphoric relati<strong>on</strong>ships.Temporal Keeping track <strong>of</strong> time, sequencing <strong>on</strong>e thingafter ano<strong>the</strong>r: Doing first things first, sec<strong>on</strong>dthings sec<strong>on</strong>d, etc.N<strong>on</strong>temporal Without a sense <strong>of</strong> time.Rati<strong>on</strong>al<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> andfacts.N<strong>on</strong>rati<strong>on</strong>al Not requiring a basis <strong>of</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> or facts; willingnessto suspend judgment.Digital Using numbers as in counting. Spatial Seeing where things are in relati<strong>on</strong> to o<strong>the</strong>rthings and how parts go toge<strong>the</strong>r to form awhole.Logical <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong> logic: <strong>on</strong>ething following ano<strong>the</strong>r in logical order—for example, a ma<strong>the</strong>matical <strong>the</strong>orem or awell-stated argument.Intuitive Making leaps <strong>of</strong> insight, <strong>of</strong>ten based <strong>on</strong>incomplete patterns, hunches, feelings, orvisual images.Linear Thinking in terms <strong>of</strong> linked ideas, <strong>on</strong>ethought directly following ano<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>of</strong>tenleading to a c<strong>on</strong>vergent c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>.Holistic(meaning "wholistic") Seeing whole thingsall at <strong>on</strong>ce; perceiving <strong>the</strong> overall patternsand structures, <strong>of</strong>ten leading to divergentc<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s.44 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Handedness and drawingDoes left-handedness, <strong>the</strong>n, improve a pers<strong>on</strong>'s ability to gainaccess to right-hemisphere functi<strong>on</strong>s such as drawing? From myobservati<strong>on</strong>s as a teacher, I can't say that I have noticed muchdifference in ease <strong>of</strong> learning to draw between left- and righthanders.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> came easily to me, for example, and I amextremely right-handed—though, like many people, I have someright/left c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong>, perhaps indicating bilateral functi<strong>on</strong>s. (Apers<strong>on</strong> with right/left c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong> is <strong>on</strong>e who says "Turn left,"while pointing right.) But <strong>the</strong>re is a point to be made here. Theprocess <strong>of</strong> learning to draw creates quite a lot <strong>of</strong> mental c<strong>on</strong>flict.It's possible that left-handers are more used to that kind <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flictand are <strong>the</strong>refore better able to cope with <strong>the</strong> discomfort itcreates than are fully lateralized right-handers. Clearly, muchresearch is needed in this area.Some art teachers recommend that right-handers shift <strong>the</strong>pencil to <strong>the</strong> left hand, presumably to have more direct access toR-mode. I do not agree. The problems with seeing that preventindividuals from being able to draw do not disappear simply bychanging hands; <strong>the</strong> drawing is just more awkward. Awkwardness,I regret to say, is viewed by some art teachers as being more creativeor more interesting. I think this attitude does a disservice to<strong>the</strong> student and is demeaning to art itself. We do not view awkwardlanguage, for instance, or awkward science as being morecreative and somehow better.A small percentage <strong>of</strong> students do discover by trying to drawwith <strong>the</strong> left hand that <strong>the</strong>y actually draw more pr<strong>of</strong>iciently thatway. On questi<strong>on</strong>ing, however, it almost always comes to light that<strong>the</strong> student has some ambidexterity or was a left-hander who hadbeen pressured to change. It would not even occur to a true righthanderlike myself (or to a true left-hander) to draw with <strong>the</strong> lessusedhand. But <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> chance that a few <strong>of</strong> you may discoversome previously hidden ambidexterity, I encourage you to tryboth hands at drawing, <strong>the</strong>n settle <strong>on</strong> whichever hand feels <strong>the</strong>most comfortable.Sigmund Freud, Hermann v<strong>on</strong>Helmholtz, and <strong>the</strong> German poetSchiller were afflicted withright/left c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong>. Freud wroteto a friend:"I do not know whe<strong>the</strong>r it is obviousto o<strong>the</strong>r people which is <strong>the</strong>ir ownor o<strong>the</strong>r's right or left. In my case,I had to think which was my right;no organic feeling told me. Tomake sure which was my right handI used quickly to make a few writingmovements."— Sigmund FreudThe Origins <strong>of</strong> PsychoanalysisA less august pers<strong>on</strong>age had <strong>the</strong>same problem:Pooh looked at his two paws. Heknew that <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m was <strong>the</strong>right, and he knew that when youhad decided which <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m was<strong>the</strong> right, that <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong>e was<strong>the</strong> left, but he never couldremember how to begin. "Well," hesaid slowly...— A. A. MilneThe House at Pooh CornerYOUR BRAIN: THE RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 45


Psychologist Charles T. Tart,discussing alternate states <strong>of</strong>c<strong>on</strong>sciousness, has said, "Manymeditative disciplines take <strong>the</strong>view that... <strong>on</strong>e possesses (or candevelop) an Observer that is highlyobjective with respect to <strong>the</strong>ordinary pers<strong>on</strong>ality. Because itis an Observer that is essentiallypure attenti<strong>on</strong>/awareness, it hasno characteristics <strong>of</strong> its own."Pr<strong>of</strong>essor Tart goes <strong>on</strong> to say thatsome pers<strong>on</strong>s who feel that <strong>the</strong>yhave a fairly well-developedObserver "feel that this Observercan make essentially c<strong>on</strong>tinuousobservati<strong>on</strong>s not <strong>on</strong>ly within aparticular d-SoC (discrete state<strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness) but also during<strong>the</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong> between two or morediscrete states."— "Putting <strong>the</strong> PiecesToge<strong>the</strong>r," 1977In <strong>the</strong> chapters to follow, I will address <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s toright-handers and thus avoid tedious repetiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>sspecifically for left-handers, with no intenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> "handism"that left-handers know so well.Setting up <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong> L->R shiftThe exercises in <strong>the</strong> next chapter are specifically designed tocause a (hypo<strong>the</strong>sized) mental shift from L-mode to R-mode.The basic assumpti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> exercises is that <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>task can influence which mode will "take up" <strong>the</strong> job whileinhibiting <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hemisphere. But <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> is what factorsdetermine which mode will predominate?Through studies with animals, split-brain patients, and individualswith intact brains, scientists believe that <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol questi<strong>on</strong>may be decided mainly in two ways. One way is speed:Which hemisphere gets to <strong>the</strong> job <strong>the</strong> quickest? A sec<strong>on</strong>d way ismotivati<strong>on</strong>: Which hemisphere cares most or likes <strong>the</strong> task <strong>the</strong>best? And c<strong>on</strong>versely: Which hemisphere cares least and likes <strong>the</strong>job <strong>the</strong> least?Since drawing a perceived form is largely an R-mode functi<strong>on</strong>,it helps to reduce L-mode interference as much as possible.The problem is that <strong>the</strong> left brain is dominant and speedy and isvery pr<strong>on</strong>e to rush in with words and symbols, even taking overjobs which it is not good at. The split brain studies indicated thatdominant L-mode prefers not to relinquish tasks to its mute partnerunless it really dislikes <strong>the</strong> job—ei<strong>the</strong>r because <strong>the</strong> job takestoo much time, is too detailed or slow or because <strong>the</strong> left brain issimply unable to accomplish <strong>the</strong> task. That's exactly what weneed—tasks that <strong>the</strong> dominant left brain will turn down. Theexercises that follow are designed to present <strong>the</strong> brain with a taskthat <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere ei<strong>the</strong>r can't or w<strong>on</strong>'t do.46 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


And now if e'er by chance I putMy fingers into glueOr madly squeeze a right-hand footInto a left-hand shoe...— Lewis CarrollUp<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> L<strong>on</strong>ely Moor, 1856YOUR BRAIN: THE RIGHT AND LEFT OF IT 47


Crossing Over:Experiencing <strong>the</strong> Shiftfrom Left to <strong>Right</strong>


A puzzle: "If <strong>on</strong>e picture is wortha thousand words, can a thousandwords explicate <strong>on</strong>e picture?"— Michael StephanA Transformati<strong>on</strong>alTheory <strong>of</strong> Aes<strong>the</strong>tics,L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Routledge, 1990Vases and faces: An exercise for <strong>the</strong> double brainThe exercises that follow are specifically designed to help youunderstand <strong>the</strong> shift from dominant left-hemisphere mode tosubdominant R-mode. I could go <strong>on</strong> describing <strong>the</strong> process overand over in words, but <strong>on</strong>ly you can experience for yourself thiscognitive shift, this slight change in subjective state. As FatsWaller <strong>on</strong>ce said, "If you gotta ask what jazz is, you ain't neverg<strong>on</strong>na know." So it is with R-mode state: You must experience <strong>the</strong>L- to R-mode shift, observe <strong>the</strong> R-mode state, and in this waycome to know it. As a first step, <strong>the</strong> exercise below is designed tocause c<strong>on</strong>flict between <strong>the</strong> two modes.Following is a quick exercise designed to induce mental c<strong>on</strong>flict.What you'll need:• <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> paper• Your #2 writing pencil• Your pencil sharpener• Your drawing board and masking tapeFigure 4-1 is a famous optical-illusi<strong>on</strong> drawing, called "Vase/Faces" because it can be seen as ei<strong>the</strong>r:• two facing pr<strong>of</strong>ilesor• a symmetrical vase in <strong>the</strong> center.What you'll do:Fig. 4-1.Your job, <strong>of</strong> course, is to complete <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d pr<strong>of</strong>ile, which willinadvertently complete <strong>the</strong> symmetrical vase in <strong>the</strong> center.Before you begin: Read all <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong> exercise.1. Copy <strong>the</strong> pattern (ei<strong>the</strong>r Figure 4-2 or 4-3). If you are righthanded,copy <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> left side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper, facingtoward <strong>the</strong> center. If you are left-handed, draw <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> right side, facing toward <strong>the</strong> center. Examples are shown<strong>of</strong> both <strong>the</strong> right-handed and left-handed drawings. Make up50THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


your own versi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile if you wish.2. Next, draw horiz<strong>on</strong>tal lines at <strong>the</strong> top and bottom <strong>of</strong> yourpr<strong>of</strong>ile, forming top and bottom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> vase (Figures 4-2 and4-3).3. Now, redraw <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>on</strong> your "Vase/Faces" pattern. Justtake your pencil and go over <strong>the</strong> lines, naming <strong>the</strong> parts as yougo, like this: "Forehead ... nose ... upper lip ... lower lip ...chin . . . neck." You might even do that a sec<strong>on</strong>d time, redrawing<strong>on</strong>e more time and really thinking to yourself whatthose terms mean.4. Then, go to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r side and start to draw <strong>the</strong> missingpr<strong>of</strong>ile that will complete <strong>the</strong> symmetrical vase.5. When you get to somewhere around <strong>the</strong> forehead or nose,you may begin perhaps to experience some c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong> orc<strong>on</strong>flict. Observe this as it happens.6. The purpose <strong>of</strong> this exercise is for you to self-observe: "Howdo I solve <strong>the</strong> problem?"Begin <strong>the</strong> exercise now. It should take you about five or six minutes.Fig. 4-2. For left-handers.Why you did this exercise:Nearly all <strong>of</strong> my students experience some c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong> or c<strong>on</strong>flictwhile doing this exercise. A few people experience a great deal <strong>of</strong>c<strong>on</strong>flict, even a moment <strong>of</strong> paralysis. If this happened to you, youmay have come to a point where you needed to change directi<strong>on</strong>in <strong>the</strong> drawing, but didn't know which way to go. The c<strong>on</strong>flictmay have been so great that you could not make your hand move<strong>the</strong> pencil to <strong>the</strong> right or <strong>the</strong> left.That is <strong>the</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> exercise: to create c<strong>on</strong>flict so thateach pers<strong>on</strong> can experience in <strong>the</strong>ir own minds <strong>the</strong> mental"crunch" that can occur when instructi<strong>on</strong>s are inappropriate to<strong>the</strong> task at hand. I believe that <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flict can be explained as follows:I gave you instructi<strong>on</strong>s that str<strong>on</strong>gly "plugged in" <strong>the</strong> verbalsystem in <strong>the</strong> brain. Remember that I insisted that you name eachFig. 4-3. For right-handers.CROSSING OVER: EXPERIENCING THE SHIFT FROM LEFT TO RIGHT 51


By <strong>the</strong> way, I must menti<strong>on</strong> that <strong>the</strong>eraser is just as important a toolfor drawing as <strong>the</strong> pencil. I'm notexactly sure where <strong>the</strong> noti<strong>on</strong>"erasing is bad" came from. Theeraser allows you to correct yourdrawings. My students certainlysee me erasing when I do dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>drawings in our workshops.part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile and I said, "Now, really think what those termsmean."Then, I gave you a task (to complete <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d pr<strong>of</strong>ile and,simultaneously, <strong>the</strong> vase) that can <strong>on</strong>ly be d<strong>on</strong>e by shifting to <strong>the</strong>visual, spatial mode <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain. This is <strong>the</strong> part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain thatcan perceive and n<strong>on</strong>verbally assess relati<strong>on</strong>ship <strong>of</strong> sizes, curves,angles, and shapes.The difficulty <strong>of</strong> making that mental shift causes a feeling <strong>of</strong>c<strong>on</strong>flict and c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong>—and even a momentary mental paralysis.You may have found a way to solve <strong>the</strong> problem, <strong>the</strong>rebyenabling yourself to complete <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d pr<strong>of</strong>ile and <strong>the</strong>refore<strong>the</strong> symmetrical vase.How did you solve it?• By deciding not to think <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> names <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> features?• By shifting your focus from <strong>the</strong> face-shapes to <strong>the</strong> vaseshapes?• By using a grid (drawing vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal lines to helpyou see relati<strong>on</strong>ships)? Or perhaps by marking points where<strong>the</strong> outermost and innermost curves occurred?• By drawing from <strong>the</strong> bottom up ra<strong>the</strong>r than from <strong>the</strong> topdown?• By deciding that you didn't care whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> vase was symmetricalor not and drawing any old memorized pr<strong>of</strong>ile just t<strong>of</strong>inish with <strong>the</strong> exercise? (With this last decisi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> verbalsystem "w<strong>on</strong>" and <strong>the</strong> visual system "lost.")Let me ask you a few more questi<strong>on</strong>s. Did you use your eraser to"fix up" your drawing? If so, did you feel guilty? If so, why? (Theverbal system has a set <strong>of</strong> memorized rules, <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> which may be,"You can't use an eraser unless <strong>the</strong> teacher says it's okay.") Thevisual system, which is largely without language, just keeps lookingfor ways to solve <strong>the</strong> problem according to ano<strong>the</strong>r kind <strong>of</strong>logic—visual logic.To sum up, <strong>the</strong> point <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> seemingly simple "Vase/Faces"exercise is this:In order to draw a perceived object or pers<strong>on</strong>—somethingthat you see with your eyes—you must make a mental shift to a52THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


ain-mode that is specialized for this visual, perceptual task.The difficulty <strong>of</strong> making this shift from verbal to visual mode<strong>of</strong>ten causes c<strong>on</strong>flict. Didn't you feel it? To reduce <strong>the</strong> discomfort<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flict, you stopped (do you remember feeling stoppedshort?) and made a new start. That's what you were doing whenyou gave yourself instructi<strong>on</strong>s—that is, gave your brain instructi<strong>on</strong>s—to"shift gears," or "change strategy," or "d<strong>on</strong>'t do this; dothat," or whatever terms you may have used to cause a cognitiveshift.There are numerous soluti<strong>on</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> mental "crunch" <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>"Vase/Faces" Exercise. Perhaps you found a unique or unusualsoluti<strong>on</strong>. To capture your pers<strong>on</strong>al soluti<strong>on</strong> in words, you mightwant to write down what happened <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> your drawing.Thomas Gladwin, an anthropologist,c<strong>on</strong>trasted <strong>the</strong> ways that aEuropean and a native Trukesesailor navigated small boatsbetween tiny islands in <strong>the</strong> vastPacific Ocean.Before setting sail, <strong>the</strong> Europeanbegins with a plan that can be writtenin terms <strong>of</strong> directi<strong>on</strong>s, degrees<strong>of</strong> l<strong>on</strong>gitude and latitude, estimatedtime <strong>of</strong> arrival at separate points <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> journey. Once <strong>the</strong> plan is c<strong>on</strong>ceivedand completed, <strong>the</strong> sailorhas <strong>on</strong>ly to carry out each step c<strong>on</strong>secutively,<strong>on</strong>e after ano<strong>the</strong>r, to beassured <strong>of</strong> arriving <strong>on</strong> time at <strong>the</strong>planned destinati<strong>on</strong>. The sailoruses all available tools, such as acompass, a sextant, a map, etc., andif asked, can describe exactly howhe got where he was going.The European navigator uses <strong>the</strong> lef<strong>the</strong>mispheremode.In c<strong>on</strong>trast, <strong>the</strong> native Trukesesailor starts his voyage by imaging<strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> his destinati<strong>on</strong> relativeto <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r islands. As hesails al<strong>on</strong>g, he c<strong>on</strong>stantly adjusts hisdirecti<strong>on</strong> according to his awareness<strong>of</strong> his positi<strong>on</strong> thus far. Hisdecisi<strong>on</strong>s are improvised c<strong>on</strong>tinuallyby checking relative positi<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong> landmarks, sun, wind directi<strong>on</strong>,etc. He navigates with reference towhere he started, where he is going,and <strong>the</strong> space between his destinati<strong>on</strong>and <strong>the</strong> point where he is at <strong>the</strong>moment. If asked how he navigatesso well without instruments or awritten plan, he cannot possiblyput it into words. This is notbecause <strong>the</strong> Trukese are unaccustomedto describing things inwords, but ra<strong>the</strong>r because <strong>the</strong>process is too complex and fluid tobe put into words.The Trukese navigator uses <strong>the</strong> righ<strong>the</strong>mispheremode.—J. A. Paredes and M. J. Hepburn"The Split-<strong>Brain</strong> and <strong>the</strong> Culture-Cogniti<strong>on</strong> Paradox," 1976CROSSING OVER: EXPERIENCING THE SHIFT FROM LEFT TO RIGHT 53


Charles Tart, pr<strong>of</strong>essor <strong>of</strong> psychologyat <strong>the</strong> University <strong>of</strong> California,Davis, states: "We beginwith a c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> some kind <strong>of</strong>basic awareness, some kind <strong>of</strong> basicability to 'know' or 'sense' or 'cognize'or 'recognize' that somethingis happening. This is a fundamental<strong>the</strong>oretical and experiential given.We do not know scientifically what<strong>the</strong> ultimate nature <strong>of</strong> awareness is,but it is our starting point."— Charles T. TartAlternate States <strong>of</strong>C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, 1975Navigating a drawing in right-hemisphere modeWhen you did your drawing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Vase/Faces, you drew <strong>the</strong> firstpr<strong>of</strong>ile in <strong>the</strong> left-hemisphere mode, like <strong>the</strong> European navigator,taking <strong>on</strong>e part at a time and naming <strong>the</strong> parts <strong>on</strong>e by <strong>on</strong>e. Thesec<strong>on</strong>d pr<strong>of</strong>ile was drawn in <strong>the</strong> right-hemisphere mode. Like <strong>the</strong>navigator from <strong>the</strong> South Sea Island <strong>of</strong> Truk, you c<strong>on</strong>stantlyscanned to adjust <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> line. You probably foundthat naming <strong>the</strong> parts such as forehead, nose, or mouth seemed toc<strong>on</strong>fuse you. It was better not to think <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing as a face. Itwas easier to use <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> space between <strong>the</strong> two pr<strong>of</strong>ilesas your guide. Stated differently, it was easiest not to think at all—that is, in words. In right-hemisphere-mode drawing, <strong>the</strong> mode <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> artist, if you do use words to think, ask yourself <strong>on</strong>ly suchthings as:"Where does that curve start?""How deep is that curve?""What is that angle relative to <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper?""How l<strong>on</strong>g is that line relative to <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e I've just drawn?""Where is that point as I scan across to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r side—whereis that point relative to <strong>the</strong> distance from <strong>the</strong> top (or bottom) edge<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper?"These are R-mode questi<strong>on</strong>s: spatial, relati<strong>on</strong>al, and comparative.Notice that no parts are named. No statements are made,no c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s drawn, such as, "The chin must come out as far as<strong>the</strong> nose," or "Noses are curved."A brief review: What is learned in "learning to draw"?Realistic drawing <strong>of</strong> a perceived image requires <strong>the</strong> visual mode<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain, most <strong>of</strong>ten mainly located in <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere.This visual mode <strong>of</strong> thinking is fundamentally different from <strong>the</strong>brain's verbal system—<strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e we largely rely <strong>on</strong> nearly all <strong>of</strong>our waking hours.For most tasks, <strong>the</strong> two modes are combined. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> a perceivedobject or pers<strong>on</strong> may be <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> few tasks that requiresmainly <strong>on</strong>e mode: <strong>the</strong> visual mode largely unassisted by <strong>the</strong> ver-54THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


al mode. There are o<strong>the</strong>r examples. Athletes and dancers, forinstance, seem to perform best by quieting <strong>the</strong> verbal system duringperformances. Moreover, a pers<strong>on</strong> who needs to shift in <strong>the</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r directi<strong>on</strong>, from visual to verbal mode, can also experiencec<strong>on</strong>flict. A surge<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ce told me that while operating <strong>on</strong> a patient(mainly a visual task, <strong>on</strong>ce a surge<strong>on</strong> has acquired <strong>the</strong> knowledgeand experience needed) he would find himself unable to name<strong>the</strong> instruments. He would hear himself saying to an attendant,"Give me <strong>the</strong>... <strong>the</strong>... you know, <strong>the</strong>... thingamajig!"Learning to draw, <strong>the</strong>refore, turns out not to be "learning todraw." Paradoxically, learning to draw means learning to access atwill that system in <strong>the</strong> brain that is <strong>the</strong> appropriate <strong>on</strong>e for drawing.Putting it ano<strong>the</strong>r way, accessing <strong>the</strong> visual mode <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>brain—<strong>the</strong> appropriate mode for drawings—causes you to see in<strong>the</strong> special way an artist sees. The artist's way <strong>of</strong> seeing is differentfrom ordinary seeing and requires an ability to make mentalshifts at c<strong>on</strong>scious level. Put ano<strong>the</strong>r way and perhaps more accurately,<strong>the</strong> artist is able to set up c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s that cause a cognitiveshift to "happen." That is what a pers<strong>on</strong> trained in drawing does,and that is what you are about to learn.Again, this ability to see things differently has many uses inlife aside from drawing—not <strong>the</strong> least <strong>of</strong> which is creative problemsolving.Keeping <strong>the</strong> "Vase/Faces" less<strong>on</strong> in mind, <strong>the</strong>n, try <strong>the</strong> nextexercise, <strong>on</strong>e that I designed to reduce c<strong>on</strong>flict between <strong>the</strong> twobrain-modes. The purpose <strong>of</strong> this exercise is just <strong>the</strong> reverse <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> previous <strong>on</strong>e."The object <strong>of</strong> painting a picture isnot to make a picture—howeverunreas<strong>on</strong>able that may sound ...The object, which is back <strong>of</strong> everytrue work <strong>of</strong> art, is <strong>the</strong> attainment<strong>of</strong> a state <strong>of</strong> being [Henri's emphasis],a state <strong>of</strong> high functi<strong>on</strong>ing, amore than ordinary moment <strong>of</strong>existence. [The picture] is but aby-product <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> state, a trace, <strong>the</strong>footprint <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> state."From The Art Spirit by Americanartist and teacher Robert Henri,B. Lippincott Company, 1923.Upside-down drawing: Making <strong>the</strong> shift to R-modeFamiliar things do not look <strong>the</strong> same when <strong>the</strong>y are upside down.We automatically assign a top, a bottom, and sides to <strong>the</strong> thingswe perceive, and we expect to see things oriented in <strong>the</strong> usualway—that is, right side up. For, in upright orientati<strong>on</strong>, we canrecognize familiar things, name <strong>the</strong>m, and categorize <strong>the</strong>m bymatching what we see with our stored memories and c<strong>on</strong>cepts.When an image is upside down, <strong>the</strong> visual clues d<strong>on</strong>'t match.CROSSING OVER: EXPERIENCING THE SHIFT FROM LEFT TO RIGHT 55


The message is strange, and <strong>the</strong> brain becomes c<strong>on</strong>fused. We see<strong>the</strong> shapes and <strong>the</strong> areas <strong>of</strong> light and shadow. We d<strong>on</strong>'t particularlyobject to looking at upside-down pictures unless we arecalled <strong>on</strong> to name <strong>the</strong> image. Then <strong>the</strong> task becomes exasperating.Seen upside down, even well-known faces are difficult to recognizeand name. For example, <strong>the</strong> photograph in Figure 4-4 is <strong>of</strong>a famous pers<strong>on</strong>. Do you recognize who it is?You may have had to turn <strong>the</strong> photograph right side up to seethat it is Albert Einstein, <strong>the</strong> famous scientist. Even after youFig. 4-4. Photograph by Philippe Halsman.56THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


know who <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong> is, <strong>the</strong> upside-down image probably c<strong>on</strong>tinuesto look strange.Inverted orientati<strong>on</strong> causes recogniti<strong>on</strong> problems with o<strong>the</strong>rimages (see Figure 4-5). Your own handwriting, turned upsidedown, is probably difficult for you to figure out, although you'vebeen reading it for years. To test this, find an old shopping list orletter in your handwriting and try to read it upside down.A complex drawing, such as <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e shown upside down in<strong>the</strong> Tiepolo drawing, Figure 4-6, is almost indecipherable. The(left) mind just gives up <strong>on</strong> it.Fig. 4-5. In copying signatures,forgers turn <strong>the</strong> originals upsidedown to see <strong>the</strong> exact shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>letters more clearly—to see, infact, in <strong>the</strong> artist's mode.Upside-down drawingAn exercise that reduces mental c<strong>on</strong>flictWe shall use this gap in <strong>the</strong> abilities <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere toallow R-mode to have a chance to take over for a while.Figure 4-7 is a reproducti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a line drawing by Picasso <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> composer Igor Stravinsky. The image is upside down. You willbe copying <strong>the</strong> upside-down image. Your drawing, <strong>the</strong>refore, willbe d<strong>on</strong>e also upside down. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, you will copy <strong>the</strong>Picasso drawing just as you see it. See Figures 4-8 and 4-9.What you'll need:• The reproducti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Picasso drawing, Fig. 4-7, p. 58.• Your #2 writing pencil, sharpened.• Your drawing board and masking tape.• Forty minutes to an hour <strong>of</strong> uninterrupted time.Fig. 4-6. Giovanni Battista Tiepolo(1696-1770), The Death <strong>of</strong> Seneca.Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The Art Institute <strong>of</strong>Chicago, Joseph and Helen RegensteinCollecti<strong>on</strong>.What you'll do:Before you begin: Read all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> following instructi<strong>on</strong>s.1. Play music if you like. As you shift into R-mode, you may findthat <strong>the</strong> music fades out. Finish <strong>the</strong> drawing in <strong>on</strong>e sitting,allowing yourself at least forty minutes—more if possible.And more important, do not turn <strong>the</strong> drawing right side upuntil you have finished. Turning <strong>the</strong> drawing would cause ashift back to L-mode, which we want to avoid while you areCROSSING OVER: EXPERIENCING THE SHIFT FROM LEFT TO RIGHT 57


learning to experience <strong>the</strong> focused R-mode state <strong>of</strong> awareness.2. You may start anywhere you wish—bottom, ei<strong>the</strong>r side, or<strong>the</strong> top. Most people tend to start at <strong>the</strong> top. Try not to figureout what you are looking at in <strong>the</strong> upside-down image. It isbetter not to know. Simply start copying <strong>the</strong> lines. Butremember: d<strong>on</strong>'t turn <strong>the</strong> drawing right side up!Photograph by Philippe Haisman,1947. © Yv<strong>on</strong>ne Haisman, 1989. Thisis <strong>the</strong> full photograph shownupside down <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> page 56. Weare indebted to Yv<strong>on</strong>ne Haismanfor allowing this unorthodox presentati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> Philippe Halsman'sfamous image <strong>of</strong> Einstein.Fig. 4-7. Pablo Picasso (1881-1973), Portrait <strong>of</strong> Igor Stravinsky. Paris, May 21,1920 (dated). Privately owned.58THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


3. I recommend that you not try to draw <strong>the</strong> entire outline <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> form and <strong>the</strong>n "fill in" <strong>the</strong> parts. The reas<strong>on</strong> is that if youmake any small error in <strong>the</strong> outline, <strong>the</strong> parts inside w<strong>on</strong>'t fit.One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> great joys <strong>of</strong> drawing is <strong>the</strong> discovery <strong>of</strong> how <strong>the</strong>parts fit toge<strong>the</strong>r. Therefore, I recommend that you movefrom line to adjacent line, space to adjacent shape, workingyour way through <strong>the</strong> drawing, fitting <strong>the</strong> parts toge<strong>the</strong>r asyou go.4. If you talk to yourself at all, use <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong> visi<strong>on</strong>,such as: "This line bends this way," or, "That shape has a curve<strong>the</strong>re," or "Compared to <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper (vertical orhoriz<strong>on</strong>tal), this line angles like that," and so <strong>on</strong>. What you d<strong>on</strong>ot want to do is to name <strong>the</strong> parts.5. When you come to parts that seem to force <strong>the</strong>ir names <strong>on</strong>you—<strong>the</strong> H-A-N-D-S and <strong>the</strong> F-A-C-E—try to focus <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong>se parts just as shapes. You might even cover up with <strong>on</strong>ehand or finger all but <strong>the</strong> specific line you are drawing and<strong>the</strong>n uncover each adjacent line. Alternatively, you might shiftto ano<strong>the</strong>r part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing.6. At some point, <strong>the</strong> drawing may begin to seem like an interesting,even fascinating, puzzle. When this happens, you willbe "really drawing," meaning that you have successfullyshifted to R-mode and you are seeing clearly. This state iseasily broken. For example, if some<strong>on</strong>e were to come into <strong>the</strong>room and ask, "How are you doing?" your verbal systemwould be reactivated and your focus and c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> wouldbe over.7. You may even want to cover most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> reproduced drawingwith ano<strong>the</strong>r piece <strong>of</strong> paper, slowly uncovering new areas asyou work your way down through <strong>the</strong> drawing. A note <strong>of</strong> cauti<strong>on</strong>,however: Some <strong>of</strong> my students find this ploy helpful,while some find it distracting and unhelpful.8. Remember that everything you need to know in order todraw <strong>the</strong> image is right in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> your eyes. All <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong>is right <strong>the</strong>re, making it easy for you. D<strong>on</strong>'t make itcomplicated. It really is as simple as that.Begin your Upside-Down <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> now.Figs. 4-8,4-9. Inverted drawing.Forcing <strong>the</strong> cognitive shift from <strong>the</strong>dominant left-hemisphere mode to<strong>the</strong> subdominant right-hemispheremode.CROSSING OVER: EXPERIENCING THE SHIFT FROM LEFT TO RIGHT 59


Fig. 4-10. "I Want You for U S.Army" by James M<strong>on</strong>tgomeryFlagg, 1917 poster. Permissi<strong>on</strong>:Trustees <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Imperial WarMuseum, L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>, England.Uncle Sam's arm and hand are"foreshortened" in this Armyposter. Foreshortening is an artterm. It means that, in order togive <strong>the</strong> illusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> forms advancingor receding in space, <strong>the</strong> formsmust be drawn just as <strong>the</strong>y appearin that positi<strong>on</strong>, not depicting whatwe know about <strong>the</strong>ir actual length.Learning to "foreshorten" is <strong>of</strong>tendifficult for beginners in drawing.After you have finished:Turn both <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawings—<strong>the</strong> reproducti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> book andyour copy—right side up. I can c<strong>on</strong>fidently predict that you willbe pleased with your drawing, especially if you have thought in<strong>the</strong> past that you would never be able to draw.I can also c<strong>on</strong>fidently predict that <strong>the</strong> most "difficult" parts,<strong>the</strong> "foreshortened" areas, are beautifully drawn, creating a spatialillusi<strong>on</strong>.Yet, see what you have accomplished, drawing upside down.If you used Picasso's drawing <strong>of</strong> Igor Stravinsky seated in a chair,you drew <strong>the</strong> crossed legs beautifully in foreshortened view. Formost <strong>of</strong> my students, this is <strong>the</strong> finest part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir drawing,despite <strong>the</strong> foreshortening. How could <strong>the</strong>y draw this "difficult"part so well? Because <strong>the</strong>y didn't know what <strong>the</strong>y were drawing!They simply drew what <strong>the</strong>y saw, just as <strong>the</strong>y saw it—<strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>most important keys to drawing well. The same applies to <strong>the</strong>foreshortened horse in <strong>the</strong> German drawing, Figure 4-13.A logical box for L-modeFigure 4-11 and Figure 4-12 show two drawings by <strong>the</strong> same universitystudent. This student had misunderstood my instructi<strong>on</strong>sto <strong>the</strong> class and did <strong>the</strong> drawing right side up. When he came toclass <strong>the</strong> next day, he showed me his drawing and said, "I misunderstood.I just drew it <strong>the</strong> regular way." I asked him to do ano<strong>the</strong>rdrawing, this time upside down. He did, and Fig. 4-12 was <strong>the</strong>result.It goes against comm<strong>on</strong> sense that <strong>the</strong> upside-down drawingFig. 4-11; near right: The Picassodrawing mistakenly copied rightside up by a university student.Fig. 4-12; far right: The Picassodrawing copied upside down <strong>the</strong>next day by <strong>the</strong> same student.60 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


is so far superior to <strong>the</strong> drawing d<strong>on</strong>e right side up. The studenthimself was ast<strong>on</strong>ished.This puzzle puts L-mode into a logical box: how to accountfor this sudden ability to draw well, when <strong>the</strong> verbal mode hasbeen eased out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> task. The left brain, which admires a jobwell d<strong>on</strong>e, must now c<strong>on</strong>sider <strong>the</strong> possibility that <strong>the</strong> disdainedright brain is good at drawingFor reas<strong>on</strong>s that are still unclear, <strong>the</strong> verbal system immediatelyrejects <strong>the</strong> task <strong>of</strong> "reading" and naming upside-downimages. L-mode seems to say, in effect, "I d<strong>on</strong>'t do upside down.It's too hard to name things seen this way, and, besides, <strong>the</strong> worldisn't upside down. Why should I bo<strong>the</strong>r with such stuff?"Well, that's just what we want! On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong> visualsystem seems not to care. <strong>Right</strong> side up, upside down, it's allinteresting, perhaps even more interesting upside down becauseR-mode is free <strong>of</strong> interference from its verbal partner, which is<strong>of</strong>ten in a "rush to judgment" or, at least, a rush to recognize andname."Our normal waking c<strong>on</strong>sciousness,rati<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>sciousness, as we callit, is but <strong>on</strong>e special type <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness,whilst all about it,parted from it by <strong>the</strong> filmiest <strong>of</strong>screens, <strong>the</strong>re lie potential forms <strong>of</strong>c<strong>on</strong>sciousness entirely different.We may go through life withoutsuspecting <strong>the</strong>ir existence; butapply <strong>the</strong> requisite stimulus, and ata touch <strong>the</strong>y are <strong>the</strong>re in all <strong>the</strong>ircompleteness, definite types <strong>of</strong>mentality which probably somewherehave <strong>the</strong>ir field <strong>of</strong> applicati<strong>on</strong>and adaptati<strong>on</strong>."— William JamesThe Varieties <strong>of</strong> ReligiousExperience, 1902Why you did this exercise:The reas<strong>on</strong> you did this exercise, <strong>the</strong>refore, is to experienceescaping <strong>the</strong> clash <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flicting modes—<strong>the</strong> kind <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flict andeven mental paralysis that <strong>the</strong> "Vase/Faces" exercise caused.When L-mode drops out voluntarily, c<strong>on</strong>flict is avoided and R-mode quickly takes up <strong>the</strong> task that is appropriate for it: drawinga perceived image.Getting to know <strong>the</strong> L->R shiftTwo important points <strong>of</strong> progress emerge from <strong>the</strong> upside-downexercise. The first is your c<strong>on</strong>scious recall <strong>of</strong> how you felt afteryou made <strong>the</strong> L->R cognitive shift. The quality <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> R-modestate <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness is different from <strong>the</strong> L-mode. One candetect those differences and begin to recognize when <strong>the</strong> cognitiveshift has occurred. Oddly, <strong>the</strong> moment <strong>of</strong> shifting betweenstates <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness always remains out <strong>of</strong> awareness. Forexample, <strong>on</strong>e can be aware <strong>of</strong> being alert and <strong>the</strong>n <strong>of</strong> being in aL-mode is <strong>the</strong> "right-handed,"left-hemisphere mode. The L isfoursquare, upright, sensible,direct, true, hard-edged, unfanciful,forceful.R-mode is <strong>the</strong> "left-handed,"right-hemisphere mode. The R iscurvy, flexible, more playful in itsunexpected twists and turns, morecomplex, diag<strong>on</strong>al, fanciful.CROSSING OVER: EXPERIENCING THE SHIFT FROM LEFT TO RIGHT 61


"I have supposed a Human Being tobe capable <strong>of</strong> various physicalstates, and varying degrees <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness,as follows:"(a) <strong>the</strong> ordinary state, with no c<strong>on</strong>sciousness<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong>Fairies;"(b) <strong>the</strong> 'eerie' state, in which, whilec<strong>on</strong>scious <strong>of</strong> actual surroundings,he is also c<strong>on</strong>scious <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> presence<strong>of</strong> Fairies;"(c) a form <strong>of</strong> trance, in which,while unc<strong>on</strong>scious <strong>of</strong> actual surrounding,and apparently asleep,he (i.e., his immaterial essence)migrates to o<strong>the</strong>r scenes, in <strong>the</strong>actual world, or in Fairyland, andis c<strong>on</strong>scious <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong>Fairies."— Lewis CarrollPreface to Sylvie andBrunodaydream, but <strong>the</strong> moment <strong>of</strong> shifting between <strong>the</strong> two statesremains elusive. Similarly, <strong>the</strong> moment <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cognitive shift fromL->R remains out <strong>of</strong> awareness, but <strong>on</strong>ce you have made <strong>the</strong>shift, <strong>the</strong> difference in <strong>the</strong> two states is accessible to knowing.This knowing will help to bring <strong>the</strong> shift under c<strong>on</strong>scious c<strong>on</strong>trol—amain goal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se less<strong>on</strong>s.The sec<strong>on</strong>d insight gained from <strong>the</strong> exercise is your awarenessthat shifting to <strong>the</strong> R-mode enables you to see in <strong>the</strong> way atrained artist sees, and <strong>the</strong>refore to draw what you perceive.Now, it's obvious that we can't always be turning thingsupside down. Your models are not going to stand <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir headsfor you, nor is <strong>the</strong> landscape going to turn itself upside down orinside out. Our goal, <strong>the</strong>n, is to teach you how to make <strong>the</strong> cognitiveshift when perceiving things in <strong>the</strong>ir normal right-side-uppositi<strong>on</strong>s. You will learn <strong>the</strong> artist's "gambit": to direct your attenti<strong>on</strong>toward visual informati<strong>on</strong> that L-mode cannot or will notprocess. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, you will always try to present your brainwith a task <strong>the</strong> language system will refuse, thus allowing R-mode to use its capability for drawing. Exercises in <strong>the</strong> comingchapters will show you some ways to do this.A review <strong>of</strong> R-modeIt might be helpful to review what R-mode feels like. Think back.You have made <strong>the</strong> shift several times now—slightly, perhaps,while doing <strong>the</strong> Vase/Faces drawings and more intensely justnow while drawing <strong>the</strong> "Stravinsky."In <strong>the</strong> R-mode state, did you notice that you were somewhatunaware <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> passage <strong>of</strong> time—that <strong>the</strong> time you spent drawingmay have been l<strong>on</strong>g or short, but you couldn't have known untilyou checked it afterward? If <strong>the</strong>re were people near, did younotice that you couldn't listen to what <strong>the</strong>y said—in fact, that youdidn't want to hear? You may have heard sounds, but you probablydidn't care about figuring out <strong>the</strong> meaning <strong>of</strong> what was beingsaid. And were you aware <strong>of</strong> feeling alert, but relaxed—c<strong>on</strong>fident,interested, absorbed in <strong>the</strong> drawing and clear in your mind?Most <strong>of</strong> my students have characterized <strong>the</strong> R-mode state <strong>of</strong>62 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


c<strong>on</strong>sciousness in <strong>the</strong>se terms, and <strong>the</strong> terms coincide with myown experience and accounts related to me <strong>of</strong> artists' experiences.One artist told me, "When I'm really working well, it's likenothing else I've ever experienced. I feel at <strong>on</strong>e with <strong>the</strong> work: <strong>the</strong>painter, <strong>the</strong> painting, it's all <strong>on</strong>e. I feel excited, but calm—exhilarated,but in full c<strong>on</strong>trol. It's not exactly happiness; it's more likebliss. I think it's what keeps me coming back and back to paintingand drawing."R-mode state is indeed pleasurable, and in that mode you candraw well. But <strong>the</strong>re is an additi<strong>on</strong>al advantage: Shifting to R-mode releases you for a time from <strong>the</strong> verbal, symbolic dominati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> L-mode, and that's a welcome relief. The pleasure maycome from resting <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere, stopping its chatter, keepingit quiet for a change. This yearning to quiet L-mode may partiallyexplain centuries-old practices such as meditati<strong>on</strong> andself-induced altered states <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness achieved throughfasting, drugs, chanting, and alcohol. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> induces a focused,alert state <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness that can last for hours, bringingsignificant satisfacti<strong>on</strong>.Before you read fur<strong>the</strong>r, do at least <strong>on</strong>e or two more drawingsupside down. Use ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> reproducti<strong>on</strong> in Figure 4-13, or findo<strong>the</strong>r line drawings to copy. Each time you draw, try c<strong>on</strong>sciouslyto experience <strong>the</strong> R-mode shift, so that you become familiar withhow it feels to be in that mode."I know perfectly well that <strong>on</strong>ly inhappy instants am I lucky enoughto lose myself in my work. Thepainter-poet feels that his trueimmutable essence comes fromthat invisible realm that <strong>of</strong>fers himan image <strong>of</strong> eternal reality....I feel that I do not exist in time,but that time exists in me. I canalso realize that it is not given tome to solve <strong>the</strong> mystery <strong>of</strong> art inan absolute fashi<strong>on</strong>. N<strong>on</strong>e<strong>the</strong>less,I am almost brought to believe thatI am about to get my hands <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>divine."— Carlo Carra"The Quadrant <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Spirit," 1919Recalling <strong>the</strong> art <strong>of</strong> your childhoodIn <strong>the</strong> next chapter we'll review your childhood development asan artist. The developmental sequence <strong>of</strong> children's art is linkedto development changes in <strong>the</strong> brain. In <strong>the</strong> early stages, infants'brain hemispheres are not clearly specialized for separate functi<strong>on</strong>s.Lateralizati<strong>on</strong>—<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>solidati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> specific functi<strong>on</strong>s into<strong>on</strong>e hemisphere or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r—progresses gradually through <strong>the</strong>childhood years, paralleling <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> language skills and<strong>the</strong> symbols <strong>of</strong> childhood art.Lateralizati<strong>on</strong> is usually complete by around age ten, and thiscoincides with <strong>the</strong> period <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flict in children's art, when <strong>the</strong>CROSSING OVER: EXPERIENCING THE SHIFT FROM LEFT TO RIGHT63


This sixteenth-century drawing byan unknown German artist <strong>of</strong>fers aw<strong>on</strong>derful opportunity to practiceupside-down drawing.Fig. 4-13. Line drawing copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> German horse and rider.64 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


symbol system seems to override percepti<strong>on</strong>s and to interferewith accurate drawing <strong>of</strong> those percepti<strong>on</strong>s. One could speculatethat c<strong>on</strong>flict arises because children may be using <strong>the</strong> "wr<strong>on</strong>g"brain mode—L-mode—to accomplish a task best suited for R-mode. Perhaps <strong>the</strong>y simply cannot work out a way to shift to <strong>the</strong>visual mode. Also, by age ten, language dominates, adding fur<strong>the</strong>rcomplicati<strong>on</strong> as names and symbols overpower spatial, holisticpercepti<strong>on</strong>s.Reviewing your childhood art is important for several reas<strong>on</strong>s:to look back as an adult at how your set <strong>of</strong> drawing symbolsdeveloped from infancy <strong>on</strong>ward; to reexperience <strong>the</strong> increasingcomplexity <strong>of</strong> your drawing as you approached adolescence; torecall <strong>the</strong> discrepancy between your percepti<strong>on</strong>s and your drawingskills; to view your childhood drawings with a less critical eyethan you were able to manage at <strong>the</strong> time; and finally, to set yourchildhood symbol system aside and move <strong>on</strong> to an adult level <strong>of</strong>visual expressi<strong>on</strong> by using <strong>the</strong> appropriate brain mode—<strong>the</strong> rightmode—for <strong>the</strong> task <strong>of</strong> drawing.CROSSING OVER: EXPERIENCING THE SHIFT FROM LEFT TO RIGHT 65


<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>Memories: YourHistory as an Artist


"When I was a child, I spake as achild, I understood as a child,I thought as a child: but when Ibecame a man, I put away childishthings."Fig. 5-1.Fig. 5-2.— 1 Cor. 13:11THE MAJORITY OF ADULTS IN THE WESTERN WORLD do notprogress in art skills much bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong> development<strong>the</strong>y reached at age nine or ten. In most mental and physicalactivities, individuals' skills change and develop as <strong>the</strong>y grow toadulthood: Speech is <strong>on</strong>e example, handwriting ano<strong>the</strong>r. Thedevelopment <strong>of</strong> drawing skills, however, seems to halt unaccountablyat an early age for most people. In our culture, children,<strong>of</strong> course, draw like children, but most adults also draw likechildren, no matter what level <strong>the</strong>y may have achieved in o<strong>the</strong>rareas <strong>of</strong> life. For example, Figures 5-1 and 5-2 illustrate <strong>the</strong> persistence<strong>of</strong> childlike forms in drawings that were d<strong>on</strong>e recently by abrilliant young pr<strong>of</strong>essi<strong>on</strong>al man who was just completing a doctoraldegree at a major university.I watched <strong>the</strong> man as he did <strong>the</strong> drawings, watched him as heregarded <strong>the</strong> models, drew a bit, erased and drew again, for abouttwenty minutes. During this time, he became restless and seemedtense and frustrated. Later he told me that he hated his drawingsand that he hated drawing, period.If we were to attach a label to this disability in <strong>the</strong> way thateducators have attached <strong>the</strong> label dyslexia to reading problems,we might call <strong>the</strong> problem dyspictoria or dysartistica or somesuch term. But no <strong>on</strong>e has d<strong>on</strong>e so because drawing is not a vitalskill for survival in our culture, whereas speech and reading are.Therefore, hardly any<strong>on</strong>e seems to notice that many adults drawchildlike drawings and many children give up drawing at age nineor ten. These children grow up to become <strong>the</strong> adults who say that<strong>the</strong>y never could draw and can't even draw a straight line. Thesame adults, however, if questi<strong>on</strong>ed, <strong>of</strong>ten say that <strong>the</strong>y wouldhave liked to learn to draw well, just for <strong>the</strong>ir own satisfacti<strong>on</strong> atsolving <strong>the</strong> drawing problems that plagued <strong>the</strong>m as children. But<strong>the</strong>y feel that <strong>the</strong>y had to stop drawing because <strong>the</strong>y simplycouldn't learn how to draw.A c<strong>on</strong>sequence <strong>of</strong> this early cutting <strong>of</strong>f <strong>of</strong> artistic developmentis that fully competent and self-c<strong>on</strong>fident adults <strong>of</strong>tenbecome suddenly self-c<strong>on</strong>scious, embarrassed, and anxious if<strong>the</strong>y are asked to draw a picture <strong>of</strong> a human face or figure. In thissituati<strong>on</strong>, individuals <strong>of</strong>ten say such things as "No, I can't! What-68 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN 1


ever I draw is always terrible. It looks like a kid's drawing." Or, "Id<strong>on</strong>'t like to draw. It makes me feel so stupid." You yourself mayhave felt a twinge or two <strong>of</strong> those feelings when you did <strong>the</strong> Preinstructi<strong>on</strong>drawings.The crisis periodThe beginning <strong>of</strong> adolescence seems to mark <strong>the</strong> abrupt end <strong>of</strong>artistic development in terms <strong>of</strong> drawing skills for many adults.As children, <strong>the</strong>y c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ted an artistic crisis, a c<strong>on</strong>flict between<strong>the</strong>ir increasingly complex percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> world around <strong>the</strong>mand <strong>the</strong>ir current level <strong>of</strong> art skills.Most children between <strong>the</strong> ages <strong>of</strong> about nine and elevenhave a passi<strong>on</strong> for realistic drawing. They become sharply critical<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir childhood drawings and begin to draw certain favoritesubjects over and over again, attempting to perfect <strong>the</strong> image.Anything short <strong>of</strong> perfect realism may be regarded as failure.Perhaps you can remember your own attempts at that age tomake things "look right" in your drawings, and your feeling <strong>of</strong>disappointment with <strong>the</strong> results. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>s you might have beenproud <strong>of</strong> at an earlier age probably seemed hopelessly wr<strong>on</strong>g andembarrassing. Looking at your drawings, you may have said, asmany adolescents say, "This is terrible! I have no talent for art.I never liked it anyway, so I'm not doing it anymore."Children <strong>of</strong>ten aband<strong>on</strong> art as an expressive activity forano<strong>the</strong>r unfortunately frequent reas<strong>on</strong>. Unthinking people sometimesmake sarcastic or derogatory remarks about children's art.The thoughtless pers<strong>on</strong> may be a teacher, a parent, ano<strong>the</strong>r child,or perhaps an admired older bro<strong>the</strong>r or sister. Many adults haverelated to me <strong>the</strong>ir painfully clear memories <strong>of</strong> some<strong>on</strong>e ridiculing<strong>the</strong>ir attempts at drawing. Sadly, children <strong>of</strong>ten blame <strong>the</strong>drawing for causing <strong>the</strong> hurt, ra<strong>the</strong>r than blaming <strong>the</strong> carelesscritic. Therefore, to protect <strong>the</strong> ego from fur<strong>the</strong>r damage, childrenreact defensively, and understandably so: They seldom everattempt to draw again.As an expert <strong>on</strong> children's art,Miriam Lindstrom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> SanFrancisco Art Museum, described<strong>the</strong> adolescent art student:"Disc<strong>on</strong>tented with his own accomplishmentsand extremely anxiousto please o<strong>the</strong>rs with his art, hetends to give up original creati<strong>on</strong>and pers<strong>on</strong>al expressi<strong>on</strong>.... Fur<strong>the</strong>rdevelopment <strong>of</strong> his visualizingpowers and even his capacity fororiginal thought and for relatinghimself through pers<strong>on</strong>al feelingsto his envir<strong>on</strong>ment may be blockedat this point. It is a crucial stagebey<strong>on</strong>d which many adults have notadvanced."— Miriam LindstromChildren's Art, 1957DRAWING ON MEMORIES: YOUR HISTORY AS AN ARTIST 69


Art in schoolEven sympa<strong>the</strong>tic art teachers, who may feel dismayed by unfaircriticism <strong>of</strong> children's art and who want to help, become discouragedby <strong>the</strong> style <strong>of</strong> drawing that young adolescents prefer—complex, detailed scenes, labored attempts at realistic drawing,endless repetiti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> favorite <strong>the</strong>mes such as racing cars, and so<strong>on</strong>. Teachers recall <strong>the</strong> beguiling freedom and charm <strong>of</strong> youngerchildren's work and w<strong>on</strong>der what happened. They deplore what<strong>the</strong>y see as "tightness" and "lack <strong>of</strong> creativity" in students' drawings.The children <strong>the</strong>mselves <strong>of</strong>ten become <strong>the</strong>ir own mostunrelenting critics. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, teachers frequently resort tocrafts projects because <strong>the</strong>y seem safer and cause less anguish—projects such as paper mosaics, string painting, drip painting, ando<strong>the</strong>r manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> materials.As a result, most students do not learn how to draw in <strong>the</strong>early and middle grades. Their self-criticism becomes permanent,and <strong>the</strong>y very rarely try to learn how to draw later in life.Like <strong>the</strong> doctoral candidate menti<strong>on</strong>ed earlier, <strong>the</strong>y might growup to be highly skilled in a number <strong>of</strong> areas, but if asked to draw ahuman being, <strong>the</strong>y will produce <strong>the</strong> same childlike image <strong>the</strong>ywere drawing at age ten.From infancy to adolescence"The scribblings <strong>of</strong> any... childclearly indicate how thoroughlyimmersed he is in <strong>the</strong> sensati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> moving his hand and cray<strong>on</strong>aimlessly over a surface, depositinga line in his path. There must besome quantity <strong>of</strong> magic in thisal<strong>on</strong>e."— Edward HillThe Language <strong>of</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>,1966For most <strong>of</strong> my students, it has proved beneficial to go back intime to try to understand how <strong>the</strong>ir visual imagery in drawingdeveloped from infancy to adolescence. With a firm grasp <strong>on</strong> how<strong>the</strong> symbol system <strong>of</strong> childhood drawing has developed, studentsseem to "unstick" <strong>the</strong>ir artistic development more easily in orderto move <strong>on</strong> to adult skills.The scribbling stageMaking marks <strong>on</strong> paper begins at about age <strong>on</strong>e and a half, whenyou as an infant were given a pencil or cray<strong>on</strong>, and you, by yourself,made a mark. It's hard for us to imagine <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> w<strong>on</strong>dera child experiences <strong>on</strong> seeing a black line emerge from <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong>70THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


a stick, a line <strong>the</strong> child c<strong>on</strong>trols. You and I, all <strong>of</strong> us, had thatexperience.After a tentative start, you probably scribbled with delight <strong>on</strong>every available surface, perhaps including your parents' bestbooks and <strong>the</strong> walls <strong>of</strong> a bedroom or two. Your scribbles wereseemingly quite random at first, like <strong>the</strong> example in Figure 5-3,but very quickly began to take <strong>on</strong> definite shapes. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> basicscribbling movements is a circular <strong>on</strong>e, probably arising simplyfrom <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong> shoulder, arm, wrist, hand, and fingers worktoge<strong>the</strong>r. A circular movement is a natural movement—more so,for instance, than <strong>the</strong> arm movements required to draw a square.(Try both <strong>on</strong> a piece <strong>of</strong> paper, and you'll see what I mean.)The stage <strong>of</strong> symbolsAfter some days or weeks <strong>of</strong> scribbling, infants—and apparentlyall human children—make <strong>the</strong> basic discovery <strong>of</strong> art: A drawnsymbol can stand for something out <strong>the</strong>re in <strong>the</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ment.The child makes a circular mark, looks at it, adds two marks foreyes, points to <strong>the</strong> drawing, and says, "Mommy," or "Daddy," or"That's me," or "My dog," or whatever. Thus, we all made <strong>the</strong>uniquely human leap <strong>of</strong> insight that is <strong>the</strong> foundati<strong>on</strong> for art,from <strong>the</strong> prehistoric cave paintings all <strong>the</strong> way up through <strong>the</strong>centuries to <strong>the</strong> art <strong>of</strong> Le<strong>on</strong>ardo, Rembrandt, and Picasso.With great delight, infants draw circles with eyes, mouth, andlines sticking out to represent arms and legs, as in Figure 5-4.This form, a symmetrical, circular form, is a basic form universallydrawn by infants. The circular form can be used for almostanything: With slight variati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>the</strong> basic pattern can stand for ahuman being, a cat, a sun, a jellyfish, an elephant, a crocodile, aflower, or a germ. For you as a child, <strong>the</strong> picture was whatever yousaid it was, although you probably made subtle and charmingadjustments <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> basic form to get <strong>the</strong> idea across.By <strong>the</strong> time children are about three and a half, <strong>the</strong> imagery<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir art becomes more complex, reflecting growing awarenessand percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> world. A body is attached to <strong>the</strong> head,though it may be smaller than <strong>the</strong> head. Arms may still grow outFig. 5-3. Scribble drawing by a twoand-a-half-year-old.Fig. 5-4. Figure-image drawing bya three-and-a-half-year-old.DRAWING ON MEMORIES: YOUR HISTORY AS AN ARTIST 71


Children's repeated imagesbecome known to fellow studentsand teachers, as shown in this w<strong>on</strong>derfulcarto<strong>on</strong> by Brenda Burbank.<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head, but more <strong>of</strong>ten <strong>the</strong>y emerge from <strong>the</strong> body—sometimesfrom below <strong>the</strong> waist. Legs are attached to <strong>the</strong> body.By age four, children are keenly aware <strong>of</strong> details <strong>of</strong> clothing—butt<strong>on</strong>sand zippers, for example, appear as details <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>drawings. Fingers appear at <strong>the</strong> ends <strong>of</strong> arms and hands, and toesat <strong>the</strong> ends <strong>of</strong> legs and feet. Numbers <strong>of</strong> fingers and toes varyimaginatively. I have counted as many as thirty-<strong>on</strong>e fingers <strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong>e hand and as few as <strong>on</strong>e toe per foot (Figure 5-4).Although children's drawings <strong>of</strong> figures resemble each o<strong>the</strong>rin many ways, each child works out through trial and error afavorite image, which becomes refined through repetiti<strong>on</strong>. Childrendraw <strong>the</strong>ir special images over and over, memorizing <strong>the</strong>mand adding details as time goes <strong>on</strong>. These favorite ways to drawvarious parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> image eventually become embedded in <strong>the</strong>memory and are remarkably stable over time (Figure 5-5).Fig. 5-5. Notice that <strong>the</strong> features are <strong>the</strong> same in each figure—including<strong>the</strong> cat—and that <strong>the</strong> little hand symbol is also used for <strong>the</strong> cat's paws.Pictures that tell storiesFig. 5-6.Around age four or five, children begin to use drawings to tell storiesand to work out problems, using small or gross adjustments <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> basic forms to express <strong>the</strong>ir intended meaning. For example,in Figure 5-6, <strong>the</strong> young artist has made <strong>the</strong> arm that holds <strong>the</strong>72THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Using his basic figure symbol, hefirst drew himself.He <strong>the</strong>n added his mo<strong>the</strong>r, using <strong>the</strong>same basic figure c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong> withadjustments—l<strong>on</strong>g hair, a dress.He <strong>the</strong>n added his fa<strong>the</strong>r, who wasbald and wore glasses.umbrella huge in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r arm, because <strong>the</strong> arm thatholds <strong>the</strong> umbrella is <strong>the</strong> important point <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing.Ano<strong>the</strong>r instance <strong>of</strong> using drawing to portray feelings is afamily portrait, drawn by a shy five-year-old whose every wakingmoment apparently was dominated by his older sister.Even Picasso could hardly have expressed a feeling withgreater power than that. Once <strong>the</strong> feeling was drawn, giving formto formless emoti<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>the</strong> child who drew <strong>the</strong> family portrait mayhave been better able to cope with his overwhelming sister.He <strong>the</strong>n added his sister, withteeth.The landscapeBy around age five or six, children have developed a set <strong>of</strong> symbolsto create a landscape. Again, by a process <strong>of</strong> trial and error,children usually settle <strong>on</strong> a single versi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a symbolic landscape,which is endlessly repeated. Perhaps you can remember<strong>the</strong> landscape you drew around age five or six.What were <strong>the</strong> comp<strong>on</strong>ents <strong>of</strong> that landscape? First, <strong>the</strong>ground and sky. Thinking symbolically, a child knows that <strong>the</strong>ground is at <strong>the</strong> bottom and <strong>the</strong> sky is at <strong>the</strong> top. Therefore, <strong>the</strong>ground is <strong>the</strong> bottom edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper, and <strong>the</strong> sky is <strong>the</strong> topedge, as in Figure 5-7. Children emphasize this point, if <strong>the</strong>y areworking with color, by painting a green stripe across <strong>the</strong> bottom,blue across <strong>the</strong> top.Most children's landscapes c<strong>on</strong>tain some versi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a house.Try to call up in your mind's eye an image <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> house you drew.Did it have windows? With curtains? And what else? A door?DRAWING ON MEMORIES: YOUR HISTORY AS AN ARTIST 73


Fig. 5-7. Landscape drawing by asix-year-old. This house is veryclose to <strong>the</strong> viewer. The bottomedge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper functi<strong>on</strong>s as <strong>the</strong>ground. To a child it seems thatevery part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing surfacehas symbolic meaning, <strong>the</strong> emptyspaces <strong>of</strong> this surface functi<strong>on</strong>ingas air through which smoke rises,<strong>the</strong> sun's rays shine, and birds fly.Fig. 5-8. Landscape drawing by asix-year-old. This house is far<strong>the</strong>raway from <strong>the</strong> viewer and has aw<strong>on</strong>derfully self-satisfied expressi<strong>on</strong>,enclosed as it is under <strong>the</strong> arc<strong>of</strong> a rainbow.What was <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> door? A doorknob, <strong>of</strong> course, because that's howyou get in. I have never seen an au<strong>the</strong>ntic, child-drawn housewith a missing doorknob.You may begin to remember <strong>the</strong> rest <strong>of</strong> your landscape: <strong>the</strong>sun (did you use a corner sun or a circle with radiating rays?), <strong>the</strong>clouds, <strong>the</strong> chimney, <strong>the</strong> flowers, <strong>the</strong> trees (did yours have a c<strong>on</strong>venientlimb sticking out for a swing?), <strong>the</strong> mountains (wereyours like upside-down ice cream c<strong>on</strong>es?). And what else? A roadgoing back? A fence? Birds?At this point, before you read any fur<strong>the</strong>r, please take a sheet<strong>of</strong> paper and draw <strong>the</strong> landscape that you drew as a child. Labelyour drawing "Recalled Childhood Landscape." You mayremember this image with surprising clarity as a whole image,complete in all its parts; or it may come back to you more graduallyas you begin to draw.While you are drawing <strong>the</strong> landscape, try also to recall <strong>the</strong>pleasure drawing gave you as a child, <strong>the</strong> satisfacti<strong>on</strong> with whicheach symbol was drawn, and <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> Tightness about <strong>the</strong>placement <strong>of</strong> each symbol within <strong>the</strong> drawing. Recall <strong>the</strong> sensethat nothing must be left out and, when all <strong>the</strong> symbols were inplace, your sense that <strong>the</strong> drawing was complete.If you can't recall <strong>the</strong> drawing at this point, d<strong>on</strong>'t be c<strong>on</strong>cerned.You may recall it later. If not, it may simply indicate thatyou've blocked it out for some reas<strong>on</strong>. Usually about ten percent<strong>of</strong> my adult students are unable to recall <strong>the</strong>ir childhood drawings.Before we go <strong>on</strong>, let's take a minute to look at some recalledchildhood landscapes drawn by adults. First, you will observethat <strong>the</strong> landscapes are pers<strong>on</strong>alized images, each different from<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r. Observe also that in every case <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>—<strong>the</strong>way <strong>the</strong> elements <strong>of</strong> each drawing are composed or distributedwithin <strong>the</strong> four edges—seems exactly right, in <strong>the</strong> sense that not asingle element could be added or removed without disturbing <strong>the</strong>Tightness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole (Figure 5-9). Let me dem<strong>on</strong>strate that byshowing you what happens in Figure 5-10 when <strong>on</strong>e form (<strong>the</strong>tree) is removed. Test this c<strong>on</strong>cept in your own recalled landscapeby covering <strong>on</strong>e form at a time. You will find that removing74 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


any single form throws <strong>of</strong>f <strong>the</strong> balance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole picture. Figures5-9.and 5-10 show examples <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r characteristics<strong>of</strong> childhood landscape drawings.After you have looked at <strong>the</strong> examples, observe your owndrawing. Observe <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong> (<strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong> forms arearranged and balanced within <strong>the</strong> four edges). Observe distanceas a factor in <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>. Try to characterize <strong>the</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> house, at first wordlessly and <strong>the</strong>n in words. Cover <strong>on</strong>e elementand see what effect that has <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>. Think back<strong>on</strong> how you did <strong>the</strong> drawing. Did you do it with a sense <strong>of</strong> sureness,knowing where each part was to go? For each part, did youfind that you had an exact symbol that was perfect in itself and fitperfectly with <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r symbols? You may have been aware <strong>of</strong>feeling <strong>the</strong> same sense <strong>of</strong> satisfacti<strong>on</strong> that you felt as a child when<strong>the</strong> forms were in place and <strong>the</strong> image completed.Fig. 5-9.The stage <strong>of</strong> complexityNow, like <strong>the</strong> ghosts in Dickens's A Christmas Carol, we'll move you<strong>on</strong> to observe yourself at a slightly later age, at nine or ten. Possiblyyou may remember some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawings you did at thatage—in <strong>the</strong> fifth, sixth, or seventh grade.During this period, children try. for more detail in <strong>the</strong>ir artwork,hoping by this means to achieve greater realism, which is aprized goal. C<strong>on</strong>cern for compositi<strong>on</strong> diminishes, <strong>the</strong> forms <strong>of</strong>tenbeing placed almost at random <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> page. Seemingly, children'sc<strong>on</strong>cern for where things are in <strong>the</strong> drawing is replaced with c<strong>on</strong>cernfor how things look, particularly <strong>the</strong> details <strong>of</strong> forms. Overall,drawings by older children show greater complexity and, at<strong>the</strong> same time, less assurance than do <strong>the</strong> landscapes <strong>of</strong> earlychildhood.Also around this time, children's drawings become differentiatedby sex, probably because <strong>of</strong> cultural factors. Boys begin todraw automobiles—hot rods and racing cars; war scenes withdive bombers, submarines, tanks, and rockets. They draw legendaryfigures and heroes—bearded pirates, Viking crewmen and<strong>the</strong>ir ships, televisi<strong>on</strong> stars, mountain climbers, and deep-seaFig. 5-10.Children seem to start out with anearly perfect sense <strong>of</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>,which <strong>the</strong>y <strong>of</strong>ten lose duringadolescence and regain <strong>on</strong>lythrough laborious study. I believethat <strong>the</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> may be that olderchildren c<strong>on</strong>centrate <strong>the</strong>ir percepti<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong> separate objects existingin an undifferentiated space,whereas young children c<strong>on</strong>structa self-c<strong>on</strong>tained c<strong>on</strong>ceptual worldbounded by <strong>the</strong> paper's edges.For older children, however, <strong>the</strong>edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper seem almostn<strong>on</strong>existent, just as edges aren<strong>on</strong>existent in open, real space.DRAWING ON MEMORIES: YOUR HISTORY AS AN ARTIST 75


divers. They are fascinated by block letters, especially m<strong>on</strong>ograms;and some odd images such as (my favorite) an eyeballcomplete with piercing dagger and pools <strong>of</strong> blood.Meanwhile, girls are drawing tamer things—flowers in vases,waterfalls, mountains reflected in still lakes, pretty girls runningor sitting <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> grass, fashi<strong>on</strong> models with incredible eyelashes,elaborate hairstyles, tiny waists and feet, and hands held behind<strong>the</strong> back because hands are "hard to draw."Figures 5-11 through 5-14 are some examples <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se earlyadolescent drawings. I've included a carto<strong>on</strong> drawing: Carto<strong>on</strong>sare drawn by both boys and girls and are much admired. I believethat carto<strong>on</strong>ing appeals to children at this age because carto<strong>on</strong>semploy familiar symbolic forms but are used in a more sophisticatedway, thus enabling adolescents to avoid feeling that <strong>the</strong>irdrawing is "babyish."Fig. 5-11. Gruesome eyeballs are afavorite <strong>the</strong>me <strong>of</strong> adolescent boys.Meanwhile, girls are drawingtamer subjects such as this bride.76 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 5-12. Complex drawing byNaveen Molloy, <strong>the</strong>n ten years old.This is an example <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> kind <strong>of</strong>drawing by adolescents that teachers<strong>of</strong>ten deplore as "tight" anduncreative. Young artists work veryhard to perfect images like this <strong>on</strong>e<strong>of</strong> electr<strong>on</strong>ic equipment. Note <strong>the</strong>keyboard and mouse. The childwill so<strong>on</strong> reject this image, however,as hopelessly inadequate.Fig. 5-13. Complex drawing by anine-year-old girl. Transparency isa recurrent <strong>the</strong>me in <strong>the</strong> drawings<strong>of</strong> children at this stage. Thingsseen under water, through glasswindows, or in transparent vases—as in this drawing—are all favorite<strong>the</strong>mes. Though <strong>on</strong>e could guess ata psychological meaning, it is quitelikely that young artists are simplytrying this idea to see if <strong>the</strong>y canmake <strong>the</strong> drawings "look right."Fig. 5-14. Complex drawing by aten-year-old boy. Carto<strong>on</strong>ing is afavorite form <strong>of</strong> art in <strong>the</strong> earlyadolescent years. As art educatorMiriam Lindstrom notes inChildren's Art, <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong> taste atthis age is at an all-time low.DRAWING ON MEMORIES: YOUR HISTORY AS AN ARTIST 77


The stage <strong>of</strong> realismBy around age ten or eleven, children's passi<strong>on</strong> for realism is infull bloom (Figures 5-15 and 5-16). When <strong>the</strong>ir drawings d<strong>on</strong>'tcome out "right"—meaning that <strong>the</strong>y d<strong>on</strong>'t look realistic—children<strong>of</strong>ten become discouraged and ask <strong>the</strong>ir teachers for help.The teacher may say, "You must look more carefully," but thisdoesn't help, because <strong>the</strong> child doesn't know what to look morecarefully for. Let me illustrate that with an example.Fig. 5-15. Realistic drawing by a twelve-year-old.Children aged ten to twelve are searching for ways tomake things "look real." Figure drawing in particularfascinates adolescents. In this drawing, symbols froman earlier stage are fitted into new percepti<strong>on</strong>s: Note<strong>the</strong> fr<strong>on</strong>t-view eye in this pr<strong>of</strong>ile drawing. Note alsothat <strong>the</strong> child's knowledge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chair back has beensubstituted for <strong>the</strong> purely visual appearance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chair seen from <strong>the</strong> side.Fig. 5-16. Realistic drawing by a twelve-year-old. Atthis stage, children's main effort is toward achievingrealism. Awareness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing surfacefades and attenti<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>centrated <strong>on</strong> individual,unrelated forms randomly distributed about <strong>the</strong> page.Each segment functi<strong>on</strong>s as an individual elementwithout regard for unified compositi<strong>on</strong>.78THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Say that a ten-year-old wants to draw a picture <strong>of</strong> a cube, perhapsa three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al block <strong>of</strong> wood. Wanting <strong>the</strong> drawing tolook "real," <strong>the</strong> child tries to draw <strong>the</strong> cube from an angle thatshows two or three planes—not just a straight-<strong>on</strong> side view thatwould show <strong>on</strong>ly a single plane, and thus would not reveal <strong>the</strong>true shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cube.To do this, <strong>the</strong> child must draw <strong>the</strong> oddly angled shapes justas <strong>the</strong>y appear—that is, just like <strong>the</strong> image that falls <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> retina<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> perceiving eye. Those shapes are not square. In fact, <strong>the</strong>child must suppress knowing that <strong>the</strong> cube is square and drawshapes that are "funny." The drawn cube will look like a cube<strong>on</strong>ly if it is comprised <strong>of</strong> oddly angled shapes. Put ano<strong>the</strong>r way,<strong>the</strong> child must draw unsquare shapes to draw a square cube. Thechild must accept this paradox, this illogical process, which c<strong>on</strong>flictswith verbal, c<strong>on</strong>ceptual knowledge. (Perhaps this is <strong>on</strong>emeaning <strong>of</strong> Picasso's statement that "Painting is a lie that tells <strong>the</strong>truth.")Fig. 5-17. Children's unsuccessful attempts to draw a cube that "looks real."Fig. 5-18. Realistic depicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a cube requires drawing uncubelike shapes.DRAWING ON MEMORIES: YOUR HISTORY AS AN ARTIST 79


"The painter who strives to representreality must transcend his ownpercepti<strong>on</strong>. He must ignore oroverride <strong>the</strong> very mechanisms inhis mind that create objects out <strong>of</strong>images.... The artist, like <strong>the</strong> eye,must provide true images and <strong>the</strong>clues <strong>of</strong> distance to tell his magiclies."— Colin BlakemoreMechanics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mind, 1977From childhood <strong>on</strong>ward, we havelearned to see things in terms <strong>of</strong>words: We name things, and weknow facts about <strong>the</strong>m. The dominantleft verbal system doesn'twant too much informati<strong>on</strong> aboutthings it perceives—just enough torecognize and to categorize. Itseems that <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> its functi<strong>on</strong>s is toscreen out a large proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>c<strong>on</strong>textual percepti<strong>on</strong>s. This is anecessary process and <strong>on</strong>e thatworks very well for us most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>time, enabling us to focus ourattenti<strong>on</strong>. The left brain, in thissense, learns to take a quick lookand says, "<strong>Right</strong>, that's a chair (oran umbrella, bird, tree, dog, etc.)."But drawing requires that youlook at something for a l<strong>on</strong>g time,perceiving lots <strong>of</strong> details andhow <strong>the</strong>y fit toge<strong>the</strong>r, registeringas much informati<strong>on</strong> as possible—ideally, everything, as AlbrechtDürer apparently tried to do inFigure 5-19.If verbal knowledge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cube's real shape overwhelms <strong>the</strong>student's purely visual percepti<strong>on</strong>, "incorrect" drawing results—drawing with <strong>the</strong> kinds <strong>of</strong> problems that make adolescentsdespair (see Figure 5-17). Knowing that cubes have square corners,students usually start a drawing <strong>of</strong> a cube with a square corner.Knowing that a cube rests <strong>on</strong> a flat surface, students drawstraight lines across <strong>the</strong> bottom. Their errors compound <strong>the</strong>mselvesas <strong>the</strong> drawing proceeds, and <strong>the</strong> students become moreand more c<strong>on</strong>fused.Though a sophisticated viewer, familiar with <strong>the</strong> art <strong>of</strong>cubism and abstracti<strong>on</strong>, might find <strong>the</strong> "incorrect" drawings inFigure 5-17 more interesting than <strong>the</strong> "correct" drawings in Figure5-18, young students find praise <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir wr<strong>on</strong>g forms incomprehensible.In this case, <strong>the</strong> child's intent was to make <strong>the</strong> cubelook "real." Therefore, to <strong>the</strong> child, <strong>the</strong> drawing is a failure. Tosay o<strong>the</strong>rwise seems as absurd to students as telling <strong>the</strong>m that"two plus two equals five" is a creative and praiseworthy soluti<strong>on</strong>.On <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> "incorrect" drawings such as <strong>the</strong> cube drawings,students may decide that <strong>the</strong>y "can't draw." But <strong>the</strong>y candraw; that is, <strong>the</strong> forms indicate that manually <strong>the</strong>y are perfectlyable to draw. The dilemma is that previously stored knowledge—which is useful in o<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>texts—prevents <strong>the</strong>ir seeing <strong>the</strong>thing-as-it-is, right <strong>the</strong>re in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir eyes.Sometimes <strong>the</strong> teacher solves <strong>the</strong> problem by showing <strong>the</strong>students how—that is, by dem<strong>on</strong>strating <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> drawing.Learning by dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> is a time-h<strong>on</strong>ored method <strong>of</strong> teachingart, and it works if <strong>the</strong> teacher can draw well and has c<strong>on</strong>fidenceenough to dem<strong>on</strong>strate realistic drawing in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> aclass. Unfortunately, most teachers at <strong>the</strong> crucial elementarylevel are <strong>the</strong>mselves not trained in perceptual skills in drawing.Therefore, teachers <strong>of</strong>ten have <strong>the</strong> same feelings <strong>of</strong> inadequacyc<strong>on</strong>cerning <strong>the</strong>ir own ability to draw realistically as <strong>the</strong> children<strong>the</strong>y wish to teach.Many teachers wish children at this age would be freer, lessc<strong>on</strong>cerned about realism in <strong>the</strong>ir artwork. But however muchsome teachers may deplore <strong>the</strong>ir students' insistence <strong>on</strong> realism,<strong>the</strong> children <strong>the</strong>mselves are relentless. They will have realism, or80 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


<strong>the</strong>y will give up art forever. They want <strong>the</strong>ir drawings to matchwhat <strong>the</strong>y see, and <strong>the</strong>y want to know how to do that.I believe that children at this age love realism because <strong>the</strong>yare trying to learn how to see. They are willing to put greatenergy and effort into <strong>the</strong> task if <strong>the</strong> results are encouraging. Afew children are lucky enough to accidentally discover <strong>the</strong> secret:how to see things in a different (R-mode) way. I think I was <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong>those children who, by chance, stumbles <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> process. But <strong>the</strong>majority <strong>of</strong> children need to be taught how to make that cognitiveshift. Fortunately, we are now developing new instructi<strong>on</strong>almethods, based <strong>on</strong> recent brain research, which will enableteachers to help satisfy children's yearning for seeing and drawingskills.How <strong>the</strong> symbol system, developed in childhood,influences seeingNow we are coming closer to <strong>the</strong> problem and its soluti<strong>on</strong>. First,what prevents a pers<strong>on</strong> from seeing things clearly enough to draw<strong>the</strong>m?The left hemisphere has no patience with this detailed percepti<strong>on</strong>and says, in effect, "It's a chair, I tell you. That's enough toknow. In fact, d<strong>on</strong>'t bo<strong>the</strong>r to look at it, because I've got a readymadesymbol for you. Here it is; add a few details if you want, butd<strong>on</strong>'t bo<strong>the</strong>r me with this looking business."And where do <strong>the</strong> symbols come from? From <strong>the</strong> years <strong>of</strong>childhood drawing during which every pers<strong>on</strong> develops a system<strong>of</strong> symbols. The symbol system becomes embedded in <strong>the</strong> memory,and <strong>the</strong> symbols are ready to be called out, just as you called<strong>the</strong>m out to draw your childhood landscape.The symbols are also ready to be called out when you draw aface, for example. The efficient left brain says, "Oh yes, eyes.Here's a symbol for eyes, <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e you've always used. And a nose?Yes, here's <strong>the</strong> way to do it." Mouth? Hair? Eyelashes? There's asymbol for each. There are also symbols for chairs, tables, andhands.To sum up, adult students beginning in art generally do not"By <strong>the</strong> time <strong>the</strong> child can drawmore than a scribble, by age threeor four years, an already wellformedbody <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual knowledgeformulated in languagedominates his memory and c<strong>on</strong>trolshis graphic work.... <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>sare graphic accounts <strong>of</strong>essentially verbal processes. As anessentially verbal educati<strong>on</strong> gainsc<strong>on</strong>trol, <strong>the</strong> child aband<strong>on</strong>s hisgraphic efforts and relies almostentirely <strong>on</strong> words. Language hasfirst spoilt drawing and <strong>the</strong>n swallowedit up completely."— Written in 1930 bypsychologist Karl BuhlerDRAWING ON MEMORIES: YOUR HISTORY AS AN ARTIST8l


"I must begin, not with hypo<strong>the</strong>sis,but with specific instances, no matterhow minute."— Paul Kleereally see what is in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir eyes—that is, <strong>the</strong>y do not perceivein <strong>the</strong> special way required for drawing. They take note <strong>of</strong>what's <strong>the</strong>re, and quickly translate <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> into words andsymbols mainly based <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> symbol system developed throughoutchildhood and <strong>on</strong> what <strong>the</strong>y know about <strong>the</strong> perceived object.What is <strong>the</strong> soluti<strong>on</strong> to this dilemma? Psychologist RobertOrnstein suggests that in order to draw, <strong>the</strong> artist must "mirror"things or perceive <strong>the</strong>m exactly as <strong>the</strong>y are. Thus, you must setaside your usual verbal categorizing and turn your full visualattenti<strong>on</strong> to what you are perceiving—to all <strong>of</strong> its details and howeach detail fits into <strong>the</strong> whole c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>. In short, you mustsee <strong>the</strong> way an artist sees.Fig. 5-19. Albrecht Dürer, Study for<strong>the</strong> Saint Jerome (1521). One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>L-mode functi<strong>on</strong>s is to screen out alarge proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> incoming percepti<strong>on</strong>s.This is a necessaryprocess to enable us to focus ourthinking and <strong>on</strong>e that works verywell for us most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> time. Butdrawing requires that you look atsomething for a l<strong>on</strong>g time, perceivinglots <strong>of</strong> details, registering asmuch informati<strong>on</strong> as possible—ideally, everything, as AlbrechtDiirer tried to do here.82 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Given proper instructi<strong>on</strong>, youngchildren can easliy learn to draw.These examples are by third-gradechildren, age eight.DRAWING ON MEMORIES: YOUR HISTORY AS AN ARTIST83


"Art is a form <strong>of</strong> supremely delicateawareness... meaning at-<strong>on</strong>eness,<strong>the</strong> state <strong>of</strong> being at <strong>on</strong>e with <strong>the</strong>object.... The picture must allcome out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist's inside.... Itis <strong>the</strong> image that lives in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness,alive like a visi<strong>on</strong>, butunknown."— D. H. Lawrence, <strong>the</strong>English writer, speakingabout his paintingsAgain, <strong>the</strong> key questi<strong>on</strong> is how to accomplish that cognitiveL->R shift. As I said in Chapter Four, <strong>the</strong> most efficient wayseems to be to present <strong>the</strong> brain with a task <strong>the</strong> left brain ei<strong>the</strong>rcan't or w<strong>on</strong>'t handle. You have already experienced a few <strong>of</strong>those tasks: <strong>the</strong> Vase/Faces drawings and <strong>the</strong> upside-down drawing.And to some extent, you have already begun to experienceand recognize <strong>the</strong> alternate state <strong>of</strong> right-hemisphere mode. Youare beginning to know that while you are in that slightly differentsubjective state <strong>of</strong> mind, you slow down so that you can see moreclearly.As you think back over experiences with drawing since youstarted this book and over experiences <strong>of</strong> alternative states <strong>of</strong>c<strong>on</strong>sciousness you may have had in c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> with o<strong>the</strong>r activities(freeway driving, reading, etc., menti<strong>on</strong>ed in Chapter One),think again about <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> that slightly altered state.It is important that you c<strong>on</strong>tinue to develop your awareness andrecogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> R-mode state.Lewis Carroll described an analogousshift in Alice's adventures inThrough <strong>the</strong> Looking Glass:"Oh, Kitty, how nice it would be ifwe could <strong>on</strong>ly get through intoLooking Glass House! I'm sure it'sgot, oh! such beautiful things in it!Let's pretend <strong>the</strong>re's a way <strong>of</strong> gettingthrough into it, somehow,Kitty. Let's pretend <strong>the</strong> glass hasgot all s<strong>of</strong>t like gauze, so that wecan get through. Why, it's turningto a mist now, I declare! It'll beeasy enough to get through...."84THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Let's review <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> R-mode <strong>on</strong>e more time.First, <strong>the</strong>re is a seeming suspensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> time. You are not aware <strong>of</strong>time in <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> marking time. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, you pay no attenti<strong>on</strong>to spoken words. You may hear <strong>the</strong> sounds <strong>of</strong> speech, but you d<strong>on</strong>ot decode <strong>the</strong> sounds into meaningful words. If some<strong>on</strong>e speaksto you, it seems as though it would take a great effort to crossback, think again in words, and answer. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, whateveryou are doing seems immensely interesting. You are attentive andc<strong>on</strong>centrated and feel "at <strong>on</strong>e" with <strong>the</strong> thing you are c<strong>on</strong>centrating<strong>on</strong>. You feel energized but calm, active without anxiety. Youfeel self-c<strong>on</strong>fident and capable <strong>of</strong> doing <strong>the</strong> task at hand. Yourthinking is not in words but in images and, particularly whiledrawing, your thinking is "locked <strong>on</strong>" to <strong>the</strong> object you are perceiving.On leaving R-mode state, you do not feel tired, butrefreshed.Our job now is to bring this state into clearer focus and undergreater c<strong>on</strong>scious c<strong>on</strong>trol, in order to take advantage <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> righ<strong>the</strong>misphere's superior ability to process visual informati<strong>on</strong> and toincrease your ability to make <strong>the</strong> cognitive shift to R-mode atwill."The development <strong>of</strong> an Observercan allow a pers<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>siderableaccess to observing different identitystates, and an outside observermay <strong>of</strong>ten clearly infer differentidentity states, but a pers<strong>on</strong> himselfwho has not developed <strong>the</strong>Observer functi<strong>on</strong> very well maynever notice <strong>the</strong> many transiti<strong>on</strong>sfrom <strong>on</strong>e identity state to ano<strong>the</strong>r."— Charles T. TartAlternate States <strong>of</strong>C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, 1977DRAWING ON MEMORIES: YOUR HISTORY AS AN ARTIST 85


Getting AroundYour Symbol System:Meeting Edges andC<strong>on</strong>tours


"To empty <strong>on</strong>e's mind <strong>of</strong> all thoughtand refill <strong>the</strong> void with a spiritgreater than <strong>on</strong>eself is to extend<strong>the</strong> mind into a realm not accessibleby c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al processes <strong>of</strong>reas<strong>on</strong>."— Edward HillThe Language <strong>of</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>,1966WE HAVE REVIEWED YOUR CHILDHOOD ART and <strong>the</strong>development <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> set <strong>of</strong> symbols that formed yourchildhood language <strong>of</strong> drawing. This process paralleled <strong>the</strong>development <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r symbol systems: speech, reading, writing,and arithmetic. Whereas <strong>the</strong>se o<strong>the</strong>r symbol systems formed usefulfoundati<strong>on</strong>s for later development <strong>of</strong> verbal and computati<strong>on</strong>alskills, childhood drawing symbols seem to interfere withlater stages <strong>of</strong> art.Thus, <strong>the</strong> central problem <strong>of</strong> teaching realistic drawing toindividuals from age ten or so <strong>on</strong>ward is <strong>the</strong> persistence <strong>of</strong> memorized,stored drawing symbols when <strong>the</strong>y are no l<strong>on</strong>ger appropriateto <strong>the</strong> task. In a sense, L-mode unfortunately c<strong>on</strong>tinues to"think" it can draw l<strong>on</strong>g after <strong>the</strong> ability to process spatial, relati<strong>on</strong>alinformati<strong>on</strong> has been lateralized to <strong>the</strong> right brain. Whenc<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ted with a drawing task, <strong>the</strong> language mode comes rushingin with its verbally linked symbols. Then afterward, ir<strong>on</strong>ically,<strong>the</strong> left brain is all too ready to supply derogatory words <strong>of</strong> judgmentif <strong>the</strong> drawing looks childlike or naive.In <strong>the</strong> last chapter I said that an effective way to "set aside"<strong>the</strong> dominant left verbal hemisphere and to "bring forward" yourn<strong>on</strong>dominant right brain, with its visual, spatial, relati<strong>on</strong>al style,is to present your brain with a task that <strong>the</strong> left brain ei<strong>the</strong>r can'tor w<strong>on</strong>'t work at. We have used <strong>the</strong> Vase/Faces drawings andupside-down drawings to illustrate this process. Now we'll tryano<strong>the</strong>r, more drastic strategy that will force a str<strong>on</strong>ger cognitiveshift and set aside your L-mode more completely.Nicolaides's c<strong>on</strong>tour drawingI've called <strong>the</strong> method <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> next exercise "Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>,"and your left hemisphere is probably not going to enjoy it.Introduced by a revered art teacher, Kim<strong>on</strong> Nicolaides, in his 1941book, The Natural Way to Draw, <strong>the</strong> method has been widely usedby art teachers. I believe that our new knowledge about how <strong>the</strong>brain divides its workload provides a c<strong>on</strong>ceptual basis for understandingwhy Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is effective as a teachingmethod. At <strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong> writing his book, Nicolaides apparently88THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


felt that <strong>the</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tour method improved students' drawingwas that it caused students to use both senses <strong>of</strong> sight andtouch. Nicolaides recommended that students imagine that <strong>the</strong>ywere touching <strong>the</strong> form as <strong>the</strong>y drew. I suggest an alternate possibility:L-mode rejects <strong>the</strong> meticulous, complex percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>spatial, relati<strong>on</strong>al informati<strong>on</strong>, thus allowing access to R-modeprocessing. In short, Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> doesn't suit <strong>the</strong> leftbrain's style. It suits <strong>the</strong> style <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> right brain—again, just whatwe want.Using Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> to bypass your symbolsystemIn my classes, I dem<strong>on</strong>strate Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, describinghow to use <strong>the</strong> method as I draw—if I can manage to keep talking(an L-mode functi<strong>on</strong>) while I'm drawing. Usually, I start out allright but begin trailing <strong>of</strong>f in mid-sentence after a minute or so.By that time, however, my students have <strong>the</strong> idea.Following <strong>the</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>, I show examples <strong>of</strong> previousstudents' Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>s. See examples <strong>of</strong> students'drawings <strong>on</strong> page 95.What you'll need:• Several pieces <strong>of</strong> drawing paper. You will draw <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> topsheet and use two or three additi<strong>on</strong>al sheets to pad <strong>the</strong> drawing.• Your #2 writing pencil, sharpened• Masking tape to tape your drawing paper to your drawingboard• An alarm clock or kitchen timer• About thirty minutes <strong>of</strong> uninterrupted timeWoman in a Hat, Kim<strong>on</strong> Nicolaides.Collecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> author."Merely to see, <strong>the</strong>refore, is notenough. It is necessary to have afresh, vivid, physical c<strong>on</strong>tact with<strong>the</strong> object you draw through asmany <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> senses as possible—and especially through <strong>the</strong> sense<strong>of</strong> touch."— Kim<strong>on</strong> NicolaidesThe Natural Way to Draw,1941What you'll do:Please read through <strong>the</strong> following instructi<strong>on</strong>s before you beginyour drawing.1. Look at <strong>the</strong> palm <strong>of</strong> your hand—<strong>the</strong> left hand if you areGETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURS 89


Fig. 6-1.right-handed and <strong>the</strong> right if you are left-handed. Bring yourfingers and thumb toge<strong>the</strong>r to create a mass <strong>of</strong> wrinkles inyour palm. Those wrinkles are what you are going to draw—all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m. I can almost hear you saying, "Are you joking?" or"Forget that!"2. Sit in a comfortable positi<strong>on</strong> with your drawing hand <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>drawing paper, holding <strong>the</strong> pencil and ready to draw. Then,put <strong>the</strong> pencil down and tape <strong>the</strong> paper in that prearrangedpositi<strong>on</strong> so it w<strong>on</strong>'t shift around while you are drawing.3. Set <strong>the</strong> timer for 5 minutes. In this way, you w<strong>on</strong>'t have tokeep track <strong>of</strong> time, an L-mode functi<strong>on</strong>.4. Then, face all <strong>the</strong> way around in <strong>the</strong> opposite directi<strong>on</strong>, keepingyour hand with <strong>the</strong> pencil <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing paper, and gazeat <strong>the</strong> palm <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand. Be sure to rest that hand <strong>on</strong>some support—<strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> a chair or perhaps <strong>on</strong> your knee—because you will be holding this ra<strong>the</strong>r awkward positi<strong>on</strong> for<strong>the</strong> allotted 5 minutes. Remember, <strong>on</strong>ce you start to draw, youwill not turn to look at <strong>the</strong> drawing until <strong>the</strong> timer goes <strong>of</strong>f.See figure 6-1.5. Gaze at a single wrinkle in your palm. Place your pencil <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> paper and begin to draw just that edge. As your eyes track<strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> edge very slowly, <strong>on</strong>e millimeter at atime, your pencil will record your percepti<strong>on</strong>s. If <strong>the</strong> edgechanges directi<strong>on</strong>, so does your pencil. If <strong>the</strong> edge intersectswith ano<strong>the</strong>r edge, follow that new informati<strong>on</strong> slowly withyour eyes, while your pencil simultaneously records everydetail. An important point: Your pencil can record <strong>on</strong>ly whatyou see—nothing more, nothing less—at <strong>the</strong> moment <strong>of</strong> seeing.Your hand and pencil functi<strong>on</strong> like a seismograph,resp<strong>on</strong>ding <strong>on</strong>ly to your actual percepti<strong>on</strong>s.The temptati<strong>on</strong> to turn and look at <strong>the</strong> drawing will be verystr<strong>on</strong>g. Resist <strong>the</strong> impulse! D<strong>on</strong>'t do it! Keep your eyes focussed<strong>on</strong> your hand.Match <strong>the</strong> movement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pencil exactly with your eyemovement. One or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r may begin to speed up, but d<strong>on</strong>'t letthat happen. You must record everything at <strong>the</strong> very instant that90 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


you see each point <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tour. Do not pause in <strong>the</strong> drawing,but c<strong>on</strong>tinue at a slow, even pace. At first you may feel uneasy oruncomfortable: Some students even report sudden headaches or asense <strong>of</strong> panic.6. Do not turn around to see what <strong>the</strong> drawing looks like untilyour timer signals <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> 5 minutes.7. Most important, you must c<strong>on</strong>tinue to draw until <strong>the</strong> timersignals you to stop.8. If you experience painful objecti<strong>on</strong>s from your verbal mode("What am I doing this for? This is really stupid! It w<strong>on</strong>'t evenbe a good drawing because I can't see what I'm doing," and s<strong>of</strong>orth), try your best to keep <strong>on</strong> drawing. The protests from<strong>the</strong> left will fade out and your mind will become quiet. Youwill find yourself becoming fascinated with <strong>the</strong> w<strong>on</strong>drouscomplexity <strong>of</strong> what you are seeing, and you will feel that youcould go deeper and deeper into <strong>the</strong> complexity. Allow this tohappen. You have nothing to fear or be uneasy about. Yourdrawing will be a beautiful record <strong>of</strong> your deep percepti<strong>on</strong>.We are not c<strong>on</strong>cerned about whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> drawing looks like ahand. We want <strong>the</strong> record <strong>of</strong> your percepti<strong>on</strong>s.9. So<strong>on</strong>, this mental chatter will cease, and you will find yourselfbecoming intensely interested in <strong>the</strong> complexity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> edgesyou see in your palm and intensely aware <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> beauty <strong>of</strong>that complex percepti<strong>on</strong>. When that change takes place, youwill have shifted to <strong>the</strong> visual mode and again you will be"really drawing."10. When <strong>the</strong> timer signals <strong>the</strong> allotted time, turn and look atyour drawing."Blind swimmer, I have mademyself see. I have seen. And I wassurprised and enamored <strong>of</strong> whatI saw, wishing to identify myselfwith it...."— Max Ernst, 1948After you have finished:Think back now <strong>on</strong> how you felt at <strong>the</strong> beginning <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> PureC<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> compared to how you felt later, when you weredeeply into <strong>the</strong> drawing. What did that later state feel like? Didyou lose awareness <strong>of</strong> time passing? Like Max Ernst, did youbecome enamored <strong>of</strong> what you saw? When you return to <strong>the</strong>alternative state you were in, will you recognize it?GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURS 91


Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is soeffective at producing this str<strong>on</strong>gshift that many artists routinelybegin drawing with at least a shortsessi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> method, in orderto start <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> shirting toR-mode.Looking at your drawing, a tangled mass <strong>of</strong> pencil marks, perhapsyou will say, "What a mess!" But look more closely and youwill see that <strong>the</strong>se marks are strangely beautiful. Of course, <strong>the</strong>ydo not represent <strong>the</strong> hand, <strong>on</strong>ly its details, and details withindetails. You have drawn complex edges from actual percepti<strong>on</strong>s.These are not quick, abstract, symbolic representati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>wrinkles in your palm. They are painstakingly accurate, excruciatinglyintricate, entangled, descriptive, and specific marks—justwhat we want at this point. I believe that <strong>the</strong>se drawings are visualrecords <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> R-mode state <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness. As a witty friend <strong>of</strong>mine, writer Judi Marks, remarked <strong>on</strong> viewing a Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> for <strong>the</strong> first time, "No <strong>on</strong>e in <strong>the</strong>ir left mind would do adrawing like that!"Why you did this exerciseThe most important reas<strong>on</strong> for this exercise is that Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> apparently causes L-mode to "reject <strong>the</strong> task," enablingyou to shift to R-mode. Perhaps <strong>the</strong> lengthy, minute observati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> severely limited, "n<strong>on</strong>-useful," and "boring" informati<strong>on</strong>—informati<strong>on</strong> that defies verbal descripti<strong>on</strong>—is incompatible withL-mode's thinking style.Note that:• Your verbal mode may object and object, but eventually will"bow out," leaving you "free" to draw. This is why I asked youto c<strong>on</strong>tinue drawing until <strong>the</strong> timer sounds.• The marks you make in R-mode are different from and <strong>of</strong>tenmore beautiful than marks made in your more usual L-modestate <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness.• Anything can be a subject for a Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour drawing: afea<strong>the</strong>r, a piece <strong>of</strong> shredded bark, a lock <strong>of</strong> hair. Once youhave shifted to R-mode, <strong>the</strong> most ordinary things becomeinordinately beautiful and interesting. Can you remember <strong>the</strong>sense <strong>of</strong> w<strong>on</strong>der you had as a child, poring over some tinyinsect or a dandeli<strong>on</strong>?92THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


The paradox <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> exerciseFor reas<strong>on</strong>s that are still unclear, Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> key exercises in learning to draw. But it's a paradox: PureC<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, which doesn't produce a "good" drawing (instudents' estimati<strong>on</strong>s), is <strong>the</strong> best exercise for effectively and efficientlycausing students later to achieve good drawing. Even moreimportant, though, this is <strong>the</strong> exercise that revives our childhoodw<strong>on</strong>der and <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> beauty found in ordinary things.A possible explanati<strong>on</strong>Apparently, in our habitual use <strong>of</strong> brain modes, L-mode seeksquickly to recognize (and name and categorize) by picking outdetails, while R-mode wordlessly perceives whole c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>sand seeks how <strong>the</strong> parts fit toge<strong>the</strong>r—or perhaps whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>parts fit toge<strong>the</strong>r.In regarding a hand, for example, <strong>the</strong> nails, <strong>the</strong> wrinkles andcreases are details and <strong>the</strong> hand itself is <strong>the</strong> whole c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>.This "divisi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> labor" works fine in ordinary life. In drawing ahand, however, <strong>on</strong>e must give equal attenti<strong>on</strong>—visual attenti<strong>on</strong>—toboth <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong> details and how <strong>the</strong>y fittoge<strong>the</strong>r into <strong>the</strong> whole. Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> may functi<strong>on</strong> asa sort <strong>of</strong> "shock treatment" for <strong>the</strong> brain, forcing it to do thingsdifferently.Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, I believe, causes L-mode to "dropout," perhaps, as I menti<strong>on</strong>ed before, through simple boredom.("I've already named it—it's a wrinkle, I tell you. They're all alike.Why bo<strong>the</strong>r with all this looking.") Once L-mode has "droppedout," it seems possible that R-mode <strong>the</strong>n perceives each wrinkle—normallyregarded as a detail—as a whole c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>,made up <strong>of</strong> even smaller details. Then each detail <strong>of</strong> each wrinklebecomes a fur<strong>the</strong>r whole, made up <strong>of</strong> ever-smaller parts, and so<strong>on</strong>, going deeper and deeper into ever expanding complexity.There is some similarity, I believe, to <strong>the</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> fractals,in which whole patterns are c<strong>on</strong>structed <strong>of</strong> smaller detailedwhole patterns, which are c<strong>on</strong>structed <strong>of</strong> ever smaller, detailedwhole patterns."In prose, <strong>the</strong> worst thing <strong>on</strong>e cando with words is to surrender to<strong>the</strong>m. When you think <strong>of</strong> a c<strong>on</strong>creteobject, you think wordlessly,and <strong>the</strong>n, if you want to describe<strong>the</strong> thing you have been visualizing,you probably hunt about tillyou find <strong>the</strong> exact words that seemto fit it. When you think <strong>of</strong> somethingabstract you are moreinclined to use words from <strong>the</strong>start, and unless you make a c<strong>on</strong>sciouseffort to prevent it, <strong>the</strong>existing dialect will come rushingin and do <strong>the</strong> job for you, at <strong>the</strong>expense <strong>of</strong> blurring or even changingyour meaning. Probably it isbetter to put <strong>of</strong>f using words asl<strong>on</strong>g as possible and get <strong>on</strong>e'smeaning clear as <strong>on</strong>e can throughpictures or sensati<strong>on</strong>s."— George Orwell"Politics and <strong>the</strong> EnglishLanguage," 1968GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURS 93


If perhaps you did not attain a shiftto R-mode in your first Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, please be patientwith yourself. You may have a verydetermined verbal system. I suggestthat you try again. You mighttry using a crumpled piece <strong>of</strong>paper, a flower, or any complexobject that appeals to you. My studentssometimes have to make twoor even three tries in order to "winout" against <strong>the</strong>ir str<strong>on</strong>g verbalmodes.Set a timer, perhaps for eight oreven ten minutes. In <strong>the</strong> beginning,it takes time to cause a shift to R-mode. Later <strong>on</strong>, as American artistRobert Henri proposed in <strong>the</strong>sidebar quotati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> page 5, <strong>the</strong>shift "to <strong>the</strong> higher state" willoccur just by starting to draw.These strange marks <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> wall<strong>of</strong> a cave were made by Paleolithichumans. In <strong>the</strong>ir intensity, <strong>the</strong>marks seem to resemble PureC<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>.— Shamans <strong>of</strong> Prehistory,J. Clottes and D. Lewis-Williams. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Harry N. Abrams, Inc.,1996Why Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is importantWhatever <strong>the</strong> actual reas<strong>on</strong> may be, I can assure you that PureC<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> will permanently change your ability to perceive.From this point <strong>on</strong>ward, you will start to see in <strong>the</strong> way anartist sees and your skills in seeing and drawing will progressrapidly.Look at <strong>the</strong> Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>of</strong> your hand <strong>on</strong>e moretime and appreciate <strong>the</strong> quality <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> marks you made in R-mode. Again, <strong>the</strong>se are not <strong>the</strong> quick, glib, stereotypic marks <strong>of</strong>symbolic L-mode. These marks are true records <strong>of</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>.The next exercise will pull toge<strong>the</strong>r everything learned so farand you will be doing a w<strong>on</strong>derful "real" drawing.Student showing: A record <strong>of</strong> an alternative stateFollowing is a Student Showing <strong>of</strong> some Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>s.What strange and marvelous markings are <strong>the</strong>se! Never mind that<strong>the</strong> drawings d<strong>on</strong>'t resemble greatly <strong>the</strong> overall c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> ahand—that's to be expected. We will attend to <strong>the</strong> overall c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>in <strong>the</strong> next exercise, "Modified C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>."In Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, it is <strong>the</strong> quality <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> marks and<strong>the</strong>ir character that we care about. The marks, <strong>the</strong>se living hieroglyphs,are records <strong>of</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>s. To be found nowhere in <strong>the</strong>drawings are <strong>the</strong> thin, glib, stereotypic marks <strong>of</strong> casual, rapid L-mode symbolic processing. Instead, we see rich, deep, intuitivemarks made in resp<strong>on</strong>se to <strong>the</strong> thing-as-it-is, <strong>the</strong> thing as it existsout <strong>the</strong>re, marks that delineate <strong>the</strong> is-ness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> object. Blindswimmers have seen! And seeing, <strong>the</strong>y have drawn.Before moving <strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> next step, Modified C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>,let's review <strong>the</strong> important c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> edges in art.The first comp<strong>on</strong>ent skill: The percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edgesPure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> has introduced you to <strong>the</strong> first comp<strong>on</strong>entskill <strong>of</strong> drawing: <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edges. In drawing, <strong>the</strong>term edge has a special meaning, different from its ordinary definiti<strong>on</strong>as a border or outline.94 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


In drawing, an edge is where two things come toge<strong>the</strong>r. In <strong>the</strong>Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> you just finished, for example, <strong>the</strong> edgeyou drew' was <strong>the</strong> place (<strong>the</strong> wrinkle) where two parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> flesh<strong>of</strong> your palm came toge<strong>the</strong>r to form a single boundary for bothparts. That shared boundary, in drawing, is described by a linethat is called a c<strong>on</strong>tour line. In drawing, <strong>the</strong>refore, a line (a c<strong>on</strong>tourline or, more simply, a c<strong>on</strong>tour) is always <strong>the</strong> border <strong>of</strong> twothings simultaneously—that is, a shared edge. The Vase/Facesexercise illustrates this c<strong>on</strong>cept. The line you drew was simultaneously<strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile and <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> vase.To sum up this c<strong>on</strong>cept: In drawing, an edge is always ashared boundary.GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURS 95


Fig. 6-2.A good definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> "pictureplane" from The Art Pack, KeyDefiniti<strong>on</strong>s/Key Styles, 1992."Picture plane: Often used—err<strong>on</strong>eously—to describe <strong>the</strong>physical surface <strong>of</strong> a painting, <strong>the</strong>picture plane is in fact a mentalc<strong>on</strong>struct—like an imaginaryplane <strong>of</strong> glass... Alberti (<strong>the</strong>Italian Renaissance artist) called ita 'window' separating <strong>the</strong> viewerfrom <strong>the</strong> picture itself... "John Elsum, in his 1704 book TheArt <strong>of</strong> Painting After <strong>the</strong> ItalianManner, gave instructi<strong>on</strong>s for making"a handy device":"Take a Square Frame <strong>of</strong> Woodabout <strong>on</strong>e foot large, and <strong>on</strong> thismake a little grate [grid] <strong>of</strong>Threads, so that crossing <strong>on</strong>eano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y may fall into perfectSquares about a Dozen at least,<strong>the</strong>n place [it] between your Eyeand <strong>the</strong> Object, and by this grateimitate up<strong>on</strong> your Table [drawingsurface] <strong>the</strong> true Posture it keeps,and this will prevent you from runninginto Errors. The more Work isto be [fore] shortened <strong>the</strong> smallerare to be <strong>the</strong> Squares."Quoted in A Miscellany <strong>of</strong> Artists'Wisdom, compiled by Diana Craig,Philadelphia: Running Press, 1993,p. 79.The child's jigsaw puzzle, Figure 6-2, illustrates this importantpoint. The edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> boat is shared with <strong>the</strong> water. Theedge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sail is shared with <strong>the</strong> sky and <strong>the</strong> water. Put ano<strong>the</strong>rway, <strong>the</strong> water stop where <strong>the</strong> boat begins—a shared edge. Thewater and <strong>the</strong> sky stop where <strong>the</strong> sail begins—shared edges.Note also that <strong>the</strong> outer edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> puzzle—its frame orformat, meaning <strong>the</strong> bounding edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>—is also<strong>the</strong> outer edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sky-shape, <strong>the</strong> land-shapes, and <strong>the</strong> watershape.A quick review <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> five perceptual skills <strong>of</strong> drawingIn this less<strong>on</strong>, we are working <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edges as <strong>on</strong>e<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> comp<strong>on</strong>ent skills <strong>of</strong> drawing. Recall that <strong>the</strong>re are four o<strong>the</strong>rsand toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>se five comp<strong>on</strong>ents make up <strong>the</strong> whole skill <strong>of</strong>drawing:1. The percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edges (<strong>the</strong> "shared" edges <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tourdrawing).2. The percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> spaces (in drawing called negative spaces).3. The percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships (known as perspective andproporti<strong>on</strong>).4. The percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> lights and shadows (<strong>of</strong>ten called "shading").5. The percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole (<strong>the</strong> gestalt, <strong>the</strong> "thingness" <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> thing).Modified C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>: First, drawing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>picture planeWhat you'll need:• Your clear plastic Picture Plane• Your felt-tip marker• Both <strong>of</strong> your viewfindersBefore you begin: Please read through all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>sbefore starting your drawing. In <strong>the</strong> next secti<strong>on</strong> I will define andfully explain <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane. For now, you will be simply usingit. Just follow <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s.96 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


What you'll do:1. Rest your hand <strong>on</strong> a desk or table in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> you (<strong>the</strong> lefthand if you are right-handed, and <strong>the</strong> right, if you are lefthanded)with <strong>the</strong> fingers and thumb curved upward, pointingtoward your face. This is a foreshortened view <strong>of</strong> your hand.Imagine now that you are about to draw that foreshortenedform.If you are like most <strong>of</strong> my students, you would simply not knowhow to go about doing that. It seems far too difficult to draw thisthree-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al form, with its parts moving toward you inspace. You would hardly know where to start. The viewfindersand plastic Picture Plane will help you get started.2. Try out each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Viewfinders to decide which size fits mostcomfortably over your hand, which you should be holding ina foreshortened positi<strong>on</strong> with <strong>the</strong> fingers coming toward you.Men <strong>of</strong>ten need <strong>the</strong> larger, women <strong>the</strong> smaller-sized Viewfinder.Choose <strong>on</strong>e or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r.3. Clip <strong>the</strong> Viewfinder you have chosen <strong>on</strong> top <strong>of</strong> your clearplasticPicture Plane.4. Use your felt-tip marker to draw a "format" line <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plasticPicture Plane, running your marker around <strong>the</strong> inside <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>opening <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Viewfinder. A format line forms <strong>the</strong> outerboundary <strong>of</strong> your drawing. See Figure 6-4.5. Now, holding you hand in <strong>the</strong> same foreshortened positi<strong>on</strong> asbefore, balance <strong>the</strong> Viewfinder/plastic Picture Plane <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>tips <strong>of</strong> your fingers and thumb. Move it about a bit until <strong>the</strong>picture-plane seems balanced comfortably.6. Pick up your uncapped marking pen, gaze at <strong>the</strong> hand under<strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane and close <strong>on</strong>e eye. (I'll explain in <strong>the</strong>next segment why it is necessary to close <strong>on</strong>e eye. For now,please just do it.) See Figure 6-5.7. Choose an edge to start your drawing. Any edge will do. With<strong>the</strong> marking pen, begin to draw <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane<strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> shapes just as you see <strong>the</strong>m. D<strong>on</strong>'t try to "sec<strong>on</strong>dguess" any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> edges. Do not name <strong>the</strong> parts. Do notw<strong>on</strong>der why <strong>the</strong> edges are <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>y are. Your job, just as inFig. 6-3.Fig. 6-4.Fig. 6-5.GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURS 97


Upside-Down <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> and in Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, is todraw exactly what you see, with as much detail as you canmanage with <strong>the</strong> marking pen (which is not as precise as apencil).8. Be sure to keep your head in <strong>the</strong> same place and keep <strong>on</strong>e eyeclosed. D<strong>on</strong>'t move your head to try to "see around" <strong>the</strong> form.Keep it still. (Again, I'll explain why in <strong>the</strong> next segment.)9. Correct any lines you wish by just wiping <strong>the</strong>m <strong>of</strong>f with amoistened tissue <strong>on</strong> your forefinger. It is very easy to redraw<strong>the</strong>m more precisely.Fig. 6-6. Albrecht Dürer(1471—1528), Hands in Adorati<strong>on</strong>.Black and white tempera <strong>on</strong> bluepaper. Albertina Museum, Vienna.Fig. 6-7. Vincent Van Gogh,Sketches with Two Sowers. St. Remy,1890.After you have finished:Place <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane <strong>on</strong> a plain sheet <strong>of</strong> paper so thatyou can clearly see what you have drawn. I can predict withc<strong>on</strong>fidence that you will be amazed. With relatively little effort,you have accomplished <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> truly difficult tasks in drawing—drawing<strong>the</strong> human hand in foreshortened view. Greatartists in <strong>the</strong> past have practiced drawing hands over and over.Observe <strong>the</strong> examples by Albrecht Dürer and Vincent Van Gogh,Figures 6-6 and 6-7.How did you accomplish this so easily? The answer, <strong>of</strong>course, is that you did what a trained artist does: You "copied"what you saw <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture-plane—in this instance, an actualplastic plane. I fully define and explain <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane in <strong>the</strong>next secti<strong>on</strong>. For now, you are simply using it. I have found that<strong>the</strong> explanati<strong>on</strong> makes more sense after students have used <strong>the</strong>plastic plane.For fur<strong>the</strong>r practice: I suggest that you erase your felt-tip pendrawing from <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane with a damp tissue and do severalmore, with your hand in a different positi<strong>on</strong> each time. Try for<strong>the</strong> really "hard" views—<strong>the</strong> more complicated <strong>the</strong> better. Oddlyenough, <strong>the</strong> flat hand is <strong>the</strong> hardest to draw; a complex positi<strong>on</strong> isactually easier. Therefore, arrange your hand with <strong>the</strong> fingerscurved, entwined, crossed, fist clenched, whatever. Try to includesome foreshortening. Remember, <strong>the</strong> more you practice each <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>se exercises, <strong>the</strong> faster you will progress. Save your-last (or98 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


est) drawing for <strong>the</strong> next exercise.This brings us to a crucial questi<strong>on</strong>—that is, an all-importantquesti<strong>on</strong> in terms <strong>of</strong> your understanding: What is drawing?The quick answer: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is "copying" what you see <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>picture-plane. In <strong>the</strong> drawing you did just now, your own hand inforeshortened view, you "copied" <strong>the</strong> "flattened" image <strong>of</strong> yourhand that you "saw" <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane.And now, a more complete answer to <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>, "What isdrawing?"In art, <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> "<strong>the</strong> picture plane" is extremely abstractand difficult to explain, and even more difficult to comprehend.But this c<strong>on</strong>cept is <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> most important keys to learning todraw, so stay with me. I'll try to be clear.The picture plane is a mental c<strong>on</strong>cept. See this in your"mind's eye": <strong>the</strong> picture plane is an imaginary transparent plane,like a framed window, that is always hanging out in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>artist's face, always parallel to <strong>the</strong> "plane" <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist's two eyes.If <strong>the</strong> artist turns, <strong>the</strong> plane also turns. What <strong>the</strong> artist sees "<strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> plane" actually extends back into <strong>the</strong> distance. But <strong>the</strong> planeenables <strong>the</strong> artist to "see" <strong>the</strong> scene as though it were magicallysmashed flat <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> clear glass plane—like a photograph,in a sense. Put ano<strong>the</strong>r way, <strong>the</strong> 3-D image behind <strong>the</strong>framed "window" is c<strong>on</strong>verted to a 2-D (flat) image. The artist<strong>the</strong>n "copies" what is seen "<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane" <strong>on</strong>to <strong>the</strong> flat drawingpaper.This trick <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist's mind, so difficult to describe, is evenmore difficult for beginning students to discover <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir own. Inthis course, <strong>the</strong>refore, you need an actual picture plane (yourplastic Picture Plane) and actual window frames (<strong>the</strong> Viewfinders).These devices seem to work like magic in causing students to"get" what drawing is—that is, to understand <strong>the</strong> fundamentalnature <strong>of</strong> drawing perceived objects or pers<strong>on</strong>s.To fur<strong>the</strong>r help beginners in drawing, I asked you to drawcrosshairs <strong>on</strong> your sheet <strong>of</strong> plastic (<strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane).These two "grid" lines represent vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal, <strong>the</strong> twoc<strong>on</strong>stants that <strong>the</strong> artist absolutely depends <strong>on</strong> to assess relati<strong>on</strong>-It might help your understanding<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture plane to realize thatphotography grew out <strong>of</strong> drawing.In <strong>the</strong> years before photographywas invented, artists generallyunderstood and used <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture-plane. You canimagine <strong>the</strong> artists' excitement(and, perhaps, dismay) to see thata photograph could, in an instant,capture <strong>the</strong> image <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> pictureplane—animage that would havetaken an artist hours, days, or evenweeks to render in a drawing.Artists, deposed from realisticdepicti<strong>on</strong>, began exploring o<strong>the</strong>raspects <strong>of</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>, such as <strong>the</strong>effects <strong>of</strong> light (Impressi<strong>on</strong>ism).After photography became comm<strong>on</strong>,<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pictureplane was less necessary and beganto fade away.GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURS 99


The picture plane is an imaginaryvertical surface, like a window,through which you look at yoursubject. In this way, you copy yourthree-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al view <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>world to your two-dimensi<strong>on</strong>alsurface <strong>on</strong>to your drawing paper.Dozens <strong>of</strong> picture planes and perspectivedevices are recorded in<strong>the</strong> U.S. Patent Office. Here are twoexamples.1OOTHE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


ships. Early <strong>on</strong> in my classes, I used a grid <strong>of</strong> many lines, but Ifound that students were counting up—"two spaces over andthree down." This is just <strong>the</strong> kind <strong>of</strong> L-mode activity we didn'twant. I <strong>the</strong>n reduced <strong>the</strong> "grid" lines to <strong>on</strong>e vertical and <strong>on</strong>e horiz<strong>on</strong>taland found that was sufficient.Later <strong>on</strong>, you will need nei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane withits gridlines nor <strong>the</strong> Viewfinders. You will replace <strong>the</strong>se technicaldevices with <strong>the</strong> imaginary, internalized mental picture-planethat every artist uses, whe<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>sciously or subc<strong>on</strong>sciously. Theactual plane (your plastic Picture Plane) and <strong>the</strong> actual Viewfindersare simply very effective aids during <strong>the</strong> time you are learninghow to draw.Try this: Fasten your Viewfinder, <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e with <strong>the</strong> largestopening, <strong>on</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane, using your clips. Close <strong>on</strong>eeye and hold <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane/Viewfinder toge<strong>the</strong>r up in fr<strong>on</strong>t<strong>of</strong> your face. See Figure 6-8.Look at <strong>the</strong> "framed" image, whatever is in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> your eye(singular). You can change <strong>the</strong> "compositi<strong>on</strong>" by bringing <strong>the</strong>Viewfinder closer to or far<strong>the</strong>r away from your face, much as acamera viewfinder works. Check out <strong>the</strong> angles <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> ceiling, or perhaps <strong>of</strong> a table, relative to <strong>the</strong> crosshairs—thatis, relative to vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal. These angles may surpriseyou. Next, imagine that you are drawing with your felt-tipmarker what you see <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane, just as you did in drawing yourhand. See Figure 6-9.Then turn to see ano<strong>the</strong>r view, and <strong>the</strong>n ano<strong>the</strong>r, alwayskeeping <strong>the</strong> picture-plane parallel to <strong>the</strong> fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> your face. D<strong>on</strong>'tslant it in any directi<strong>on</strong>! One way to practice not slanting <strong>the</strong>plane is to bring <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane right up to your face,<strong>the</strong>n quickly extend your arms straight out toge<strong>the</strong>r.Next, choose a view that you like, framed by your Viewfinder<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane. Imagine that you are "copying" whatyou see <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane <strong>on</strong>to a piece <strong>of</strong> drawing paper. Remember,all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> angles, sizes, spaces, and relati<strong>on</strong>ships will be just whatyou see <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane. See Figure 6-10.These two images, your (imagined) drawing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper and<strong>the</strong> image <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane will be (approximately)Fig. 6-8.Fig. 6-9.Fig. 6-10.GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURSIOI


"Dear Theo,In my last letter you will havefound a little sketch <strong>of</strong> that perspectiveframe I menti<strong>on</strong>ed. I justcame back from <strong>the</strong> blacksmith,who made ir<strong>on</strong> points for <strong>the</strong> sticksand ir<strong>on</strong> corners for <strong>the</strong> frame. Itc<strong>on</strong>sists <strong>of</strong> two l<strong>on</strong>g stakes; <strong>the</strong>frame can be attached to <strong>the</strong>mei<strong>the</strong>r way with str<strong>on</strong>g woodenpegs."So <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> shore or in <strong>the</strong> meadowsor in <strong>the</strong> fields <strong>on</strong>e can lookthrough it like a window [<strong>the</strong> artist'semphasis]. The vertical lines and<strong>the</strong> horiz<strong>on</strong>tal line <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> frameand <strong>the</strong> diag<strong>on</strong>al lines and <strong>the</strong>intersecti<strong>on</strong>, or else <strong>the</strong> divisi<strong>on</strong> insquares, certainly give a few pointerswhich help <strong>on</strong>e make a soliddrawing and which indicate <strong>the</strong>main lines and proporti<strong>on</strong> ... <strong>of</strong>why and how <strong>the</strong> perspectivecauses an apparent change <strong>of</strong>directi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> lines and change <strong>of</strong>size in <strong>the</strong> planes and in <strong>the</strong> wholemass."L<strong>on</strong>g and c<strong>on</strong>tinuous practicewith it enables <strong>on</strong>e to draw quickas lightning—and <strong>on</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> drawingis d<strong>on</strong>e firmly, to paint quick aslightning, too."From Letter 223, The CompleteLetters <strong>of</strong> Vincent Van Gogh, Greenwich,C<strong>on</strong>n.: The <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> YorkGraphic Society, 1954, p. 432-33.<strong>the</strong> same. If perfectly drawn—very hard to do!—<strong>the</strong>y will beidentical. At its most basic level, that is what drawing is. To reiterate,basic realistic drawing is copying what is seen <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> pictureplane."If that is so," you may object, "why not just take a photograph?"I believe <strong>on</strong>e answer is that <strong>the</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> realistic drawingis not simply to record data, but ra<strong>the</strong>r to record your uniquepercepti<strong>on</strong>—how you pers<strong>on</strong>ally see something—and, moreover,how you understand <strong>the</strong> thing you are drawing. By slowing downand closely observing something, pers<strong>on</strong>al expressi<strong>on</strong> and comprehensi<strong>on</strong>occur in ways that cannot occur simply by taking asnapshot. (I am referring, <strong>of</strong> course, to casual photography, not<strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong> artist-photographers.)Also, your style <strong>of</strong> line, choices for emphasis, and subc<strong>on</strong>sciousmental processes—your pers<strong>on</strong>ality, so to speak—enters<strong>the</strong> drawing. In this way, again paradoxically, your careful observati<strong>on</strong>and depicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> your subject give <strong>the</strong> viewer both <strong>the</strong>image <strong>of</strong> your subject and an insight into you. In <strong>the</strong> best sense,you have expressed yourself.Use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture-plane has a l<strong>on</strong>g traditi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong>art. The great Renaissance artist Le<strong>on</strong>e Battista Alberti discoveredthat he could draw in perspective <strong>the</strong> cityscape bey<strong>on</strong>d hiswindow by drawing directly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> glass pane <strong>the</strong> view he sawbehind <strong>the</strong> pane. Inspired by Le<strong>on</strong>ardo da Vinci's writing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>subject, German artist Albrecht Dürer developed <strong>the</strong> pictureplanec<strong>on</strong>cept fur<strong>the</strong>r, building actual picture-plane devices.Dürer's writings and drawings inspired Vincent Van Gogh toc<strong>on</strong>struct his own "perspective device," as he called it, when hewas laboriously teaching himself to draw (see Figure 6-11). Later<strong>on</strong>, after Van Gogh had mastered basic drawing, he discarded hisdevice, just as you will.Note that Van Gogh's device must have weighed twentypounds or more. I can picture him in my mind's eye laboriouslydismantling <strong>the</strong> parts, tying <strong>the</strong>m up, carrying <strong>the</strong> bundle—al<strong>on</strong>gwith his painting materials—<strong>on</strong> his l<strong>on</strong>g walk to <strong>the</strong> seashore,unbundling and setting <strong>the</strong> device up, and <strong>the</strong>n repeating <strong>the</strong>whole sequence to get home at night. This gives us some insightIO2THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 6-12. The artist using his device at <strong>the</strong> seashore.From The Complete Letters <strong>of</strong> Vincent Van Gogh.Greenwich: The <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York Graphic Society, 1954.The drawings are reproduced by permissi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>The <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York Graphic Society.Fig. 6-n. Vincent Van Gogh's perspective device.into how resolutely Van Gogh labored to improve his drawingskills (see Figure 6-12).Ano<strong>the</strong>r renowned artist, <strong>the</strong> 16th-century Dutch masterHans Holbein, who had no need for help with his drawing, alsoused an actual Picture Plane. Art historians recently discoveredthat Holbein used a glass pane <strong>on</strong> which he directly drew images<strong>of</strong> his sitters for <strong>the</strong> overwhelming number <strong>of</strong> portrait drawingsrequired <strong>of</strong> him when he lived in <strong>the</strong> English court <strong>of</strong> Henry VIII.Art historians speculate that Holbein, <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> great draughtsmen<strong>of</strong> art history, did this to save time—<strong>the</strong> overworked artistcould <strong>the</strong>n quickly transfer <strong>the</strong> drawing <strong>on</strong> glass to paper and get<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> next portrait.One more important point: "<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>" means drawing a singleview.Recall that when you drew your hand directly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plasticPicture Plane, I asked you to keep your hand still and your headstill in order to see <strong>on</strong>e view <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane. Even aslight movement <strong>of</strong> your hand or a slight change in <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>your head will give you a different view <strong>of</strong> your hand. I some-GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURSI03


Pr<strong>of</strong>essor Elliot Elgart <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> University<strong>of</strong> California at Los AngelesArt Department told me in c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>that he has <strong>of</strong>tenobserved beginning drawing students,presented for <strong>the</strong> first timewith a reclining model, tilt <strong>the</strong>irheads far to <strong>on</strong>e side while drawing<strong>the</strong> model. Why? To see <strong>the</strong> modelin <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>y are used to,which is standing up!Seeing perspective drawing asdepicting three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al spaceis apparently a learned percept,culturally determined. Individualsfrom remote cultures sometimesdo not decipher photographs orrealistic drawings.times see students bend <strong>the</strong>ir heads around to see something <strong>the</strong>ycouldn't see with <strong>the</strong>ir head in <strong>the</strong> original positi<strong>on</strong>. D<strong>on</strong>'t do it!If you can't see that fourth finger, you d<strong>on</strong>'t draw it. To repeat:Keep your hand and your head in an unchanged positi<strong>on</strong> anddraw just what you see.For <strong>the</strong> same reas<strong>on</strong>—to see <strong>on</strong>e view <strong>on</strong>ly—you kept <strong>on</strong>eeye closed. By closing <strong>on</strong>e eye, you removed binocular visi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong>slight variance in images, called "binocular disparity," that occurswhen we view an object with both eyes open.Binocular visi<strong>on</strong> allows us to see <strong>the</strong> world as three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al.This ability is sometimes called "depth percepti<strong>on</strong>." Whenyou close <strong>on</strong>e eye, <strong>the</strong> single image is two-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al—that is, itis flat, like a photograph. The paper we draw <strong>on</strong> is also twodimensi<strong>on</strong>alor flat.Here is yet ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paradoxes <strong>of</strong> drawing:The flat, two-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al image you see (with <strong>on</strong>e eyeclosed) <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture-plane, when copied <strong>on</strong>to your drawingpaper, miraculously "looks" three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al to <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong> whoviews your drawing. One necessary step in learning to draw is tobelieve that this miracle will happen. Often, students strugglingwith a drawing will ask, "How can I make this table look like it'sgoing back in space?" or "How do I make this arm look like it'scoming toward me?" The answer, <strong>of</strong> course, is to draw—tocopy!—just what you see flattened <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture-plane. Only<strong>the</strong>n will <strong>the</strong> drawing c<strong>on</strong>vincingly depict <strong>the</strong>se "movements"through three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al space (see Figure 6-13).You may be w<strong>on</strong>dering, "Is it always necessary to close <strong>on</strong>eeye while drawing?" Not always, but most artists do quite a lot <strong>of</strong><strong>on</strong>e-eye closing while drawing. The closer <strong>the</strong> viewed object, <strong>the</strong>more eye-closing. The far<strong>the</strong>r away <strong>the</strong> object, <strong>the</strong> less eye closing,because <strong>the</strong> binocular disparity referred to above diminisheswith distance.In this next exercise, you will use your technical aids (yourplastic Picture Plane and your Viewfinders) to enable you to do arealistic drawing <strong>of</strong> your own hand—a "real" drawing depicting athree-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al form <strong>on</strong> a flat sheet <strong>of</strong> paper.104 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Students <strong>of</strong>ten become very frustratedat <strong>the</strong> start <strong>of</strong> a drawing—perhaps because <strong>the</strong> starting <strong>of</strong> adrawing is always difficult. Also Ithink students beginning in drawingbelieve that drawings just "flowout." They d<strong>on</strong>'t. You will be makingnumerous intense relati<strong>on</strong>alcalculati<strong>on</strong>s at <strong>the</strong> start, and it's<strong>on</strong>ly after <strong>the</strong> drawing is wellstarted—in fact, nearing completi<strong>on</strong>—thatit begins to "flow."Fig. 6-13. Brian Harking.Modified C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>of</strong> your handWhat you'll need:• Several sheets <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> smaller drawing paper• Your graphite stick and some paper napkins or paper towels• Your #2 writing pencil or your #4B drawing pencil, sharpened• Your eraser• Your plastic Picture Plane• Your felt-tip marker• The Viewfinder you used for your drawing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> PicturePlane• An hour <strong>of</strong> uninterrupted timeWhat you'll do:Before you begin: Please read through all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s.In this drawing, we are modifying <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s for Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>. You will sit in a normal positi<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong>refore beable to glance at your drawing to m<strong>on</strong>itor its progress (see Figure6-14). Never<strong>the</strong>less, I hope you will use <strong>the</strong> same focused c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>that you used in Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>.GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURS105


Fig. 6-14. The positi<strong>on</strong> for ModifiedCountour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is <strong>the</strong> usualdrawing positi<strong>on</strong>.Most <strong>of</strong> my students greatly enjoythis process <strong>of</strong> t<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>the</strong>ir paper,and <strong>the</strong> physical acti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> "workingup" <strong>the</strong> t<strong>on</strong>e seems to help<strong>the</strong>m get started with a drawing.A possible reas<strong>on</strong> is that, havingmarked <strong>the</strong> paper and made it<strong>the</strong>ir own, so to speak, <strong>the</strong>y escape<strong>the</strong> intimidati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> that blank sheet<strong>of</strong> white paper staring at <strong>the</strong>m.1. Tape a stack <strong>of</strong> several sheets <strong>of</strong> paper to your drawing board.Tape all four corners securely, so that <strong>the</strong> paper will not shiftaround. One <strong>of</strong> your hands will be "posing" and must remainstill. The o<strong>the</strong>r will be drawing and perhaps erasing. If <strong>the</strong>paper shifts under your hand while you are drawing or erasing,it is very distracting.2. Draw a format <strong>on</strong> your drawing paper, using <strong>the</strong> inside edge<strong>of</strong> your Viewfinder.3. The next step is to t<strong>on</strong>e your paper. Make sure you have astack <strong>of</strong> several sheets <strong>of</strong> paper to pad your drawing. Begin tot<strong>on</strong>e your paper by rubbing <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> graphite stickvery lightly over <strong>the</strong> paper, staying inside <strong>the</strong> format. Youwant to achieve a pale, even t<strong>on</strong>e—d<strong>on</strong>'t worry too muchabout staying within <strong>the</strong> lines. You can clean up <strong>the</strong> edges at alater time. Figure 6-15.4. Once you have covered <strong>the</strong> paper with a light applicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>graphite, begin to rub <strong>the</strong> graphite into <strong>the</strong> paper with your106 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 6-15. Fig. 6-16. Fig. 6-17.paper towels. Rub with a circular moti<strong>on</strong>, applying even pressureright up to <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format. You want to achieve avery smooth, silvery t<strong>on</strong>e. Figure 6-16.Next, lightly draw horiz<strong>on</strong>tal and vertical crosshairs <strong>on</strong> yourt<strong>on</strong>ed paper. The lines will cross in <strong>the</strong> center just as <strong>the</strong>y do<strong>on</strong> your plastic Picture Plane. Use <strong>the</strong> crosshairs <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plasticplane to mark <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> crosshairs <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> format<strong>of</strong> your t<strong>on</strong>ed paper. A cauti<strong>on</strong>: D<strong>on</strong>'t draw <strong>the</strong> lines too dark.They are <strong>on</strong>ly guidelines and later you may want to eliminate<strong>the</strong>m. Figure 6-17.Retrieve your Picture Plane with <strong>the</strong> felt-tip drawing thatyou did at <strong>the</strong> start <strong>of</strong> this chapter, or, if you wish, you can doa new drawing (Figure 6-18). Place <strong>the</strong> plane down <strong>on</strong> a lightsurface, perhaps a sheet <strong>of</strong> paper, so that you can clearly see<strong>the</strong> drawing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic. This image will act as a guide foryou when you next draw your hand without <strong>the</strong> actual plane.Figure 6-19.Fig. 6-18. Pose your hand under <strong>the</strong>Picture Plane.Fig. 6-19. Draw <strong>the</strong> edges as you see<strong>the</strong>m <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane.GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURSI07


Fig. 6-20. Transfer <strong>the</strong> main pointsfrom your drawing or plastic toyour t<strong>on</strong>ed drawing paper.Fig. 6-21.Try to observe <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>lights and shadows. I realize youhaven't yet had any instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> fourth drawing skill, <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> lights and shadows. I'vefound, however, that students dowell just plunging in and <strong>the</strong>y <strong>of</strong>tenenjoy it very much.7. An important step: Now, you will transfer <strong>the</strong> main pointsand edges <strong>of</strong> your drawing <strong>on</strong> plastic to your drawing paper(Figure 6-20). The formats are <strong>the</strong> same size, so it is a <strong>on</strong>e-to<strong>on</strong>escale transfer. Using <strong>the</strong> crosshairs, place <strong>the</strong> point wherean edge <strong>of</strong> your hand c<strong>on</strong>tacts <strong>the</strong> format. Transfer several <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>se points. Then, begin to c<strong>on</strong>nect <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> your hand,fingers, thumb, palm, wrinkles, and so <strong>on</strong> with <strong>the</strong> points youhave established. This is just a light sketch to help you place<strong>the</strong> hand within <strong>the</strong> format. Recall that drawing is copyingwhat you see <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture plane. For this drawing, you willtake this actual step, to get used to <strong>the</strong> process. D<strong>on</strong>'t worryabout erasing <strong>the</strong> ground if you have to change a line. Erase,<strong>the</strong>n just rub <strong>the</strong> erased area with your finger or a paper napkinand <strong>the</strong> erasure disappears.8. Once this rough, light sketch is <strong>on</strong> your paper, you are readyto start drawing.9. Repositi<strong>on</strong> your "posing" hand, using <strong>the</strong> drawing-<strong>on</strong>-plasticto guide <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>ing. Then, set aside your drawing-<strong>on</strong>plastic,but place it where you can still refer to it.10. Then, closing <strong>on</strong>e eye, focus <strong>on</strong> a point <strong>on</strong> some edge in yourposing hand. Any edge will do to make a start. Place yourpencil point <strong>on</strong> this same point in <strong>the</strong> drawing. Then, gazeagain at this point <strong>on</strong> your hand in preparati<strong>on</strong> to draw. Thiswill start <strong>the</strong> mental shift to R-mode and help to quiet anymutterings from L-mode.11. When you begin to draw, your eyes—or ra<strong>the</strong>r, eye—willmove slowly al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tour and your pencil will recordyour percepti<strong>on</strong>s at <strong>the</strong> same slow speed that your eye is moving.Just as you did in Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, try to perceiveand record all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> slight undulati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> each edge (Figure6-21). Use your eraser whenever needed, even to make tinyadjustments in <strong>the</strong> line. Looking at your hand (with <strong>on</strong>e eyeclosed, remember), you can estimate <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> any edge bycomparing it to <strong>the</strong> crosshairs. Check <strong>the</strong>se angles in yourdrawing-<strong>on</strong>-plastic that you did earlier, but also try to see<strong>the</strong>se relati<strong>on</strong>ships by imagining a picture-plane hovering108 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


over your hand, with its helpful crosshairs and <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>format to guide you.12. About 90 percent <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> time, you should be looking at yourhand. That is where you will find <strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> that youneed. In fact, all <strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> you need to do a w<strong>on</strong>derfuldrawing <strong>of</strong> your hand is right in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> your eyes. Glance at<strong>the</strong> drawing <strong>on</strong>ly to m<strong>on</strong>itor <strong>the</strong> pencil's recording <strong>of</strong> yourpercepti<strong>on</strong>s, to check for relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> sizes and angles, orto pick a point to start a new c<strong>on</strong>tour. C<strong>on</strong>centrate <strong>on</strong> whatyou see, wordlessly sensing to yourself, "How wide is this partcompared to that? How steep is this angle compared to that?"And so <strong>on</strong>.13. Move from c<strong>on</strong>tour to adjacent c<strong>on</strong>tour. If you see spacesbetween <strong>the</strong> fingers, use that informati<strong>on</strong> as well: "How wideis that space compared to <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> that shape?" (Remember,we are not naming things—fingernails, fingers, thumb,palm. They are all just edges, spaces, shapes, relati<strong>on</strong>ships.)Be sure to keep <strong>on</strong>e eye closed at least a good porti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>time. Your hand is quite close in proximity to your eyes, and<strong>the</strong> binocular disparity can c<strong>on</strong>fuse you with two images.When you come to parts that impose <strong>the</strong>ir names <strong>on</strong> you—fingernails, for example—try to escape <strong>the</strong> words. One goodstrategy is to focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> flesh around <strong>the</strong> fingernails.These shapes share edges with <strong>the</strong> fingernails. Therefore, ifyou draw <strong>the</strong> shapes around <strong>the</strong> nails, you will have also drawn<strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fingernails—but you'll get both right! In fact, ifmental c<strong>on</strong>flict sets in over any part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing, move to <strong>the</strong>next adjacent space or shape, remembering <strong>the</strong> "shared edge"c<strong>on</strong>cept. Then, return later with "new eyes" to <strong>the</strong> part thatseemed difficult. (Figure 6-22)14. You may want to erase out <strong>the</strong> spaces around your hand. Thismakes <strong>the</strong> hand "stand out" from <strong>the</strong> negative spaces.You can work up <strong>the</strong> drawing with a little shading by observingwhere you see areas <strong>of</strong> light (highlights) and areas <strong>of</strong> shadowappear <strong>on</strong> your posing hand. Erase out <strong>the</strong> highlights and draw in<strong>the</strong> shadows.Fig. 6-22. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> hand byusing shapes and spaces.GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURSIO9


Suggesti<strong>on</strong>s for practice: Using <strong>the</strong>method for Modified C<strong>on</strong>tour<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, draw a preliminary"copy" <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> object first <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>Picture Plane. Then draw <strong>the</strong>object itself, using <strong>the</strong> drawing-<strong>on</strong>plasticas a guide. Try <strong>the</strong>se subjects:A shoe, or shoesA pair <strong>of</strong> eyeglassesA kitchen whisk or winebottle openerA flower<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> takes many forms. In thiscourse, you are acquiring <strong>the</strong> basicperceptual skills <strong>of</strong> drawing, comparableto learning <strong>the</strong> basicABC's <strong>of</strong> reading and writing.15. Finally, when <strong>the</strong> drawing begins to become intensely interesting,like a complicated and beautiful jigsaw puzzle graduallytaking shape under your pencil, you will be reallydrawing.After you have finished: This is your first "real" drawing and I canassume with some c<strong>on</strong>fidence that you are pleased with <strong>the</strong>results. I hope you now see what I meant about <strong>the</strong> miracle <strong>of</strong>drawing. Because you drew what you saw <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> flat pictureplane,your drawing appears au<strong>the</strong>ntically three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al.Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, some very subtle qualities will show in yourdrawing. For example, a sense <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> volume—<strong>the</strong> three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>althickness—<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hand will be <strong>the</strong>re, as well as <strong>the</strong> precisetensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> certain muscles or <strong>the</strong> pressure <strong>of</strong> a finger <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>thumb. And all <strong>of</strong> this comes from simply drawing what you see<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane.In <strong>the</strong> following group <strong>of</strong> drawings, <strong>the</strong> hands are threedimensi<strong>on</strong>al,believable, and au<strong>the</strong>ntic. They seem to be made <strong>of</strong>flesh, muscles, skin, and b<strong>on</strong>es. Even very subtle qualities aredepicted, such as <strong>the</strong> pressure <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e finger <strong>on</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> tensi<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> certain muscles, or <strong>the</strong> precise texture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> skin.I've included some dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawings by me and o<strong>the</strong>rs<strong>of</strong> our teaching staff. As you see, <strong>the</strong>se drawings are d<strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong> at<strong>on</strong>ed ground, which we'll also be using in <strong>the</strong> next exercise.Before we move <strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> next step, think back <strong>on</strong> your mentalstate during <strong>the</strong> drawing <strong>of</strong> your hand. Did you lose track <strong>of</strong>time? Did <strong>the</strong> drawing at some point become interesting, evenfascinating? Did you experience any distracti<strong>on</strong> from your verbalmode? If so, how did you escape it?Also, think back <strong>on</strong> this basic c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture-planeand our working definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> drawing: "copying" what you see <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> picture-plane. From now <strong>on</strong>, each time you pick up <strong>the</strong> pencilto draw, <strong>the</strong> strategies learned in this drawing will become betterintegrated and more "automatic."You might want to do a sec<strong>on</strong>d Modified C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>of</strong>your hand, perhaps this time holding some complex object: atwisted handkerchief, a flower, a pinec<strong>on</strong>e, a pair <strong>of</strong> eyeglasses.For this drawing, you can again work <strong>on</strong> a lightly "t<strong>on</strong>ed" ground.110THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MEETING EDGES AND CONTOURSIII


112 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


The next step: Tricking L-mode with empty spaceSo far, we-have located some gaps in <strong>the</strong> abilities <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere.It has problems with mirror images (as in <strong>the</strong> Vase/Facesdrawing). It can't deal with upside-down perceptual informati<strong>on</strong>(as in <strong>the</strong> upside-down Stravinsky drawing). It refuses to processslow, complex percepti<strong>on</strong>s (as in <strong>the</strong> Pure and Modified C<strong>on</strong>tourdrawings). We used those gaps to give R-mode a chance toprocess visual informati<strong>on</strong> without interference from L-mode.The next less<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> negative space is designed to reestablishyour grasp <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> unity <strong>of</strong> spaces and forms in compositi<strong>on</strong>, whichyou had as a child.GETTING AROUND YOUR SYMBOL SYSTEM: MF.KTING EDGES AND CONTOURS 113


Perceiving <strong>the</strong>Shape <strong>of</strong> a Space:The Positive Aspects<strong>of</strong> Negative Space


IN THIS CHAPTER, we'll take up <strong>the</strong> next comp<strong>on</strong>ent skill <strong>of</strong>drawing—<strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> negative spaces. You will use yournew skills <strong>of</strong> seeing and drawing complicated edges in order todraw <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> negative spaces.This exercise will be a stretch for some, a joy for o<strong>the</strong>rs.There is an antic or whimsical quality to seeing negative spaces.In a sense, you are seeing what is not <strong>the</strong>re. In American life, it is<strong>of</strong>ten a new experience to realize that spaces are important. Wetend to focus <strong>on</strong> objects; we are an objective culture. In o<strong>the</strong>r cultures,working "within <strong>the</strong> space <strong>of</strong> a problem." is comm<strong>on</strong> practice.My aim is to make spaces become "real" for you and toprovide a new experience in seeing.In this chapter, you will also learn to find and use a "BasicUnit" that will enable you to correctly size <strong>the</strong> first shape youdraw. And you will dip into lights and shadows by working <strong>on</strong> at<strong>on</strong>ed ground.46THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Let's quickly review <strong>the</strong> five basic skills <strong>of</strong> drawing. Remember,<strong>the</strong>se are perceptual skills: The percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>• Edges (line <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tour drawing)• Spaces (negative spaces)• Relati<strong>on</strong>ships (proporti<strong>on</strong> and perspective)• Lights and shadows (shading)• The gestalt (<strong>the</strong> "thingness" <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> thing)What are negative spaces and positive forms?Two terms traditi<strong>on</strong>ally used in art are "negative spaces" and"positive forms." In <strong>the</strong> drawings <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> bighorn sheep, for example,<strong>the</strong> sheep is <strong>the</strong> positive form and <strong>the</strong> sky behind and groundbelow <strong>the</strong> animal are <strong>the</strong> negative spaces.The word "negative" in negative spaces is a bit unfortunatebecause it carries, well, a negative c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>. I have searched invain for a better term, so we'll stick with this <strong>on</strong>e. The terms negativespaces and positive forms have <strong>the</strong> advantage <strong>of</strong> being easy toPERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACEII7


emember and <strong>the</strong>y are, after all, comm<strong>on</strong>ly used in <strong>the</strong> wholefield <strong>of</strong> art and design. The main point is that negative spaces arejust as important as <strong>the</strong> positive forms. For <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong> just learningto draw, <strong>the</strong>y are perhaps more important!Why is learning to see and draw negative spaces so important?Fig. 7-1. Jeanne O'Neil."Expressi<strong>on</strong> to my way <strong>of</strong> thinkingdoes not c<strong>on</strong>sist <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> passi<strong>on</strong> mirroredup<strong>on</strong> a human face orbetrayed by a violent gesture. Thewhole arrangement <strong>of</strong> my pictureis expressive. The place occupiedby <strong>the</strong> figures or objects, <strong>the</strong> emptyspaces around <strong>the</strong>m, <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s,everything plays a part."— Henri Matisse"Notes d'un peintre,"1908When a pers<strong>on</strong> just beginning in drawing tries to draw a chair,that pers<strong>on</strong> knows too much, in an L-mode sense, about chairs.For example, seats have to be big enough to hold a pers<strong>on</strong>; all fourchair legs are usually all <strong>the</strong> same length; chair legs sit <strong>on</strong> a flatsurface, and so forth. This knowledge does not help, and in factcan greatly hinder, drawing a chair. The reas<strong>on</strong> is that, when seenfrom different angles, <strong>the</strong> visual informati<strong>on</strong> may not c<strong>on</strong>form towhat we know.Visually—that is, as seen <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane—a chair seat mayappear as a narrow strip, not nearly wide enough to sit <strong>on</strong>. Thelegs may appear to be all <strong>of</strong> different lengths. The curve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>back <strong>of</strong> a chair may appear to be entirely different from what weknow it to be (Figure 7-1).What are we to do? An answer: D<strong>on</strong>'t draw <strong>the</strong> chair at all!Instead, draw <strong>the</strong> spaces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chair.Why does using negative space make drawing easier? Ibelieve that it's because you d<strong>on</strong>'t know anything, in a verbalsense, about <strong>the</strong>se spaces. Because you have no pre-existingmemorized symbols for space-shapes, you can see <strong>the</strong>m clearlyand draw <strong>the</strong>m correctly. Also, by focusing <strong>on</strong> negative spaces,you can cause L-mode again to drop out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> task, perhaps aftera bit <strong>of</strong> protest: "Why are you looking at nothing? I do not dealwith nothing! I can't name it. It's <strong>of</strong> no use ..." So<strong>on</strong>, this chatterwill cease—again, just what we want.An analogy to clarify <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> negative spacesIn drawing, negative space-shapes are real. They are not justempty "air."The following analogy may help you to see that. Imagine thatyou are watching a Bugs Bunny carto<strong>on</strong>. Imagine that Bugs118 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Bunny is running at top speed down a l<strong>on</strong>g hallway, at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong>which is a closed door. He smashes through <strong>the</strong> door, leaving aBugs-Bunny shaped hole in <strong>the</strong> door. What's left <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> door isnegative space. Note that <strong>the</strong> door has an outside edge (its format).This edge is <strong>the</strong> outside edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> negative space-shape.In this analogy, <strong>the</strong> hole in <strong>the</strong> door is <strong>the</strong> positive form (BugsBunny) g<strong>on</strong>e po<strong>of</strong>!Now, take your Viewfinder/plastic Picture Plane and look ata chair. Close <strong>on</strong>e eye and move <strong>the</strong> Viewfinder backward andforward, up and down, as though framing a snapshot. When youhave found a compositi<strong>on</strong> that pleases you, hold <strong>the</strong> Viewfindervery still. Now, gazing at a space in <strong>the</strong> chair, perhaps <strong>the</strong> spacebetween two back slats, imagine that <strong>the</strong> chair is magically pulverizedand—like Bugs Bunny, in a po<strong>of</strong>!—disappears, leaving<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> negative spaces, <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e you are gazing at and all <strong>the</strong> rest<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> spaces. They are real. They have real shapes, just like <strong>the</strong>remains <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> door in <strong>the</strong> analogy above. These negative spaceshapesare what you are going to draw. In short, you will draw <strong>the</strong>spaces, not <strong>the</strong> chair.The reas<strong>on</strong>? Recall our definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edges: All edges areshared edges where two things come toge<strong>the</strong>r. The negativespaces share edges with <strong>the</strong> (now absent) chair. If you draw <strong>the</strong>edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> spaces, you also will have drawn <strong>the</strong> chair, because itshares edges with <strong>the</strong> spaces. But <strong>the</strong> chair will "look right,"because you will be able to see and draw <strong>the</strong> spaces accurately.(See <strong>the</strong> examples <strong>of</strong> negative-space drawings <strong>of</strong> chairs.)Note that <strong>the</strong> format is also <strong>the</strong> outer edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chair's negativespaces (ano<strong>the</strong>r shared edge) and toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> chair-formand <strong>the</strong> space-shapes fill <strong>the</strong> format completely. Technicallyspeaking, <strong>the</strong> whole image, made up <strong>of</strong> positive forms and negativespace-shapes, is called <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>. The artist composes<strong>the</strong> forms and <strong>the</strong> spaces within <strong>the</strong> format, arranging <strong>the</strong>maccording to certain "rules" called <strong>the</strong> Principles <strong>of</strong> Art.Art teachers <strong>of</strong>ten laboriously try to teach <strong>the</strong>ir students "<strong>the</strong>rules <strong>of</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>," but I have discovered that if students payclose attenti<strong>on</strong> to negative spaces in <strong>the</strong>ir drawings, many compositi<strong>on</strong>alproblems are automatically solved.Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawing byinstructor Brian Bomeisler.PERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACE 119


Unity: A most important principle<strong>of</strong> art.If negative spaces are given equalimportance to <strong>the</strong> positive forms,all parts <strong>of</strong> a drawing seem interestingand all work toge<strong>the</strong>r tocreate a unified image. If, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong> focus is almostentirely <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> positive forms, <strong>the</strong>drawing may seem uninterestingand disunified—even boring—nomatter how beautifully rendered<strong>the</strong> positive form may be. A str<strong>on</strong>gfocus <strong>on</strong> negative spaces will make<strong>the</strong>se basic instructi<strong>on</strong>al drawingsstr<strong>on</strong>g in compositi<strong>on</strong> and beautifulto look at.Fig. 7-2. A variety <strong>of</strong> formats.Defining compositi<strong>on</strong>In drawing, <strong>the</strong> term compositi<strong>on</strong> means <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong> comp<strong>on</strong>ents<strong>of</strong> a drawing are arranged by <strong>the</strong> artist. Some key comp<strong>on</strong>ents <strong>of</strong>a compositi<strong>on</strong> are positive shapes (<strong>the</strong> objects or pers<strong>on</strong>s), negativespaces (<strong>the</strong> empty areas), and <strong>the</strong> format (<strong>the</strong> relative lengthand width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> bounding edges <strong>of</strong> a surface). To compose adrawing, <strong>the</strong>refore, <strong>the</strong> artist places and fits toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> positiveshapes and <strong>the</strong> negative spaces within <strong>the</strong> format with <strong>the</strong> goal <strong>of</strong>unifying <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>.The format c<strong>on</strong>trols compositi<strong>on</strong>. Put ano<strong>the</strong>r way, <strong>the</strong> shape<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing surface (usually rectangular paper) will greatlyinfluence how an artist distributes <strong>the</strong> shapes and spaces within<strong>the</strong> bounding edges <strong>of</strong> that surface. To clarify this, use your R-mode ability to image a tree, perhaps an elm or a pine. Now fit<strong>the</strong> same tree into each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> formats in Figure 7-2. You will findthat—to "fill <strong>the</strong> space"—you have to change <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>tree and <strong>the</strong> spaces around <strong>the</strong> tree for each format. Then testagain by imaging exactly <strong>the</strong> same tree in all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> formats. Youwill find that a shape that fits <strong>on</strong>e format is all wr<strong>on</strong>g for ano<strong>the</strong>r.Experienced artists fully comprehend <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format. Beginning students in drawing, however, arecuriously oblivious to <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper and <strong>the</strong> boundaries<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper. Because <strong>the</strong>ir attenti<strong>on</strong> is directed almost exclusivelytoward <strong>the</strong> objects or pers<strong>on</strong>s <strong>the</strong>y are drawing, <strong>the</strong>y seemto regard <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper almost as n<strong>on</strong>existent, almost like<strong>the</strong> real space that surrounds objects and has no bounds.This obliviousness to <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper, which boundboth <strong>the</strong> negative spaces and positive shapes, causes problemswith compositi<strong>on</strong> for nearly all beginning art students. The mostserious problem is <strong>the</strong> failure to unify <strong>the</strong> spaces and <strong>the</strong>shapes—a basic requirement for good compositi<strong>on</strong>.The importance <strong>of</strong> composing within <strong>the</strong> formatIn Chapter Five, we saw that young children have a str<strong>on</strong>g grasp<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format. Children's c<strong>on</strong>sciousness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>bounding edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format c<strong>on</strong>trols <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>y distribute <strong>the</strong>I2OTHE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 7-4.Fig. 7-3. Joan Miro, Pers<strong>on</strong>ages with Star (1933). Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The Art Institute<strong>of</strong> Chicago.forms and spaces, and young children <strong>of</strong>ten produce nearly flawlesscompositi<strong>on</strong>s. The compositi<strong>on</strong> by a six-year-old in Figure 7-4 compares favorably with <strong>the</strong> Spanish artist Miro's compositi<strong>on</strong>in Figure 7-3.Unfortunately, as you have seen, this ability lapses as childrenapproach adolescence, perhaps due to lateralizati<strong>on</strong>, increasingdominance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> language system, and <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere'spenchant for recognizing, naming, and categorizing objects. C<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong> things seems to supersede <strong>the</strong> young child's moreholistic or global view <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> world, where everything is important,including <strong>the</strong> negative spaces <strong>of</strong> sky, ground, and air. Usuallyit takes years <strong>of</strong> training to c<strong>on</strong>vince students, in <strong>the</strong> way experiencedartists are c<strong>on</strong>vinced, that <strong>the</strong> negative spaces, bounded by<strong>the</strong> format, require <strong>the</strong> same degree <strong>of</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> and care that <strong>the</strong>positive forms require. Beginning students generally lavish all<strong>the</strong>ir attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> objects, pers<strong>on</strong>s, or forms in <strong>the</strong>ir drawings,and <strong>the</strong>n more or less "fill in <strong>the</strong> background." It may seem hardto believe at this moment, but if care and attenti<strong>on</strong> are lavishedPERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACE 121


<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> negative spaces, <strong>the</strong> forms will take care <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>mselves. I'llbe showing you specific examples <strong>of</strong> that.The quotati<strong>on</strong>s by <strong>the</strong> playwright Samuel Beckett and <strong>the</strong>Zen philosopher Alan Watts (<strong>on</strong> page 123) state this c<strong>on</strong>cept c<strong>on</strong>cisely.In art, as Beckett says, nothing (in <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> emptyspace) is real. And as Alan Watts says, <strong>the</strong> inside and outside are<strong>on</strong>e. You saw in <strong>the</strong> last chapter that in drawing, <strong>the</strong> objects and<strong>the</strong> spaces around <strong>the</strong>m fit toge<strong>the</strong>r like <strong>the</strong> pieces <strong>of</strong> a puzzle.Every piece is important and <strong>the</strong>y share edges. Toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y fillup all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> area within <strong>the</strong> four edges—that is, within <strong>the</strong> format.Look at <strong>the</strong> example <strong>of</strong> this fitting toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> spaces andshapes in <strong>the</strong> still-life painting by Paul Cezanne (Figure 7-6) and<strong>the</strong> figure drawing by Dürer (Figure 7-5). Notice how varied andFig. 7-5. Albrecht Dürer (1471-1528),Nude Woman with a Staff(1508).Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The Nati<strong>on</strong>al Gallery<strong>of</strong> Canada, Ottawa. The negativeshapes surrounding <strong>the</strong> figure arebeautifully varied in size and c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>.Fig. 7-6. Paul Cezanne (1839—1936),The Vase <strong>of</strong> Tulips. Courtesy <strong>of</strong> TheArt Institute <strong>of</strong> Chicago. By making<strong>the</strong> positive forms touch <strong>the</strong>edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format in severalplaces, Cezanne enclosed and separated<strong>the</strong> negative shapes, whichc<strong>on</strong>tribute as much to <strong>the</strong> interestand balance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong> asdo <strong>the</strong> positive forms.122THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


interesting <strong>the</strong> negative spaces are. Even in <strong>the</strong> Dürer, which isalmost symmetrical, <strong>the</strong> negative spaces are beautifully varied.Now, back to <strong>the</strong> drawing less<strong>on</strong>s.Summing up,, <strong>the</strong>n, negative spaces have two important functi<strong>on</strong>s:1. Negative spaces make "difficult" drawing tasks easy—forexample, areas <strong>of</strong> foreshortening or complicated forms orforms that d<strong>on</strong>'t "look like" what we know about <strong>the</strong>m,become easy to draw by using negative space. The chairdrawings in <strong>the</strong> margin and <strong>the</strong> horns <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sheep <strong>on</strong> page 116are good examples.2. Emphasis <strong>on</strong> negative spaces unifies your drawing andstreng<strong>the</strong>ns compositi<strong>on</strong> and—perhaps most important,improves your perceptual abilities.I realize that it is counter-intuitive—that is, it goes against comm<strong>on</strong>sense—to think that focusing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> spaces around objectswill improve your drawing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> objects. But this is simplyano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paradoxes <strong>of</strong> drawing and may help to explain whyit is so difficult to teach <strong>on</strong>eself to draw. So many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> strategies<strong>of</strong> drawing—using negative space, for example—would neveroccur to any<strong>on</strong>e "in <strong>the</strong>ir left mind."Our next bit <strong>of</strong> preparati<strong>on</strong> is to define <strong>the</strong> "Basic Unit." Whatis it and how does it help with drawing?"Nothing is more real thannothing."— Samuel Beckett"You can never have <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>inside <strong>of</strong> a cup without <strong>the</strong> outside.The inside and <strong>the</strong> outside gotoge<strong>the</strong>r. They're <strong>on</strong>e."— Alan WattsChoosing a Basic UnitOn looking at a finished drawing, students just beginning to draw<strong>of</strong>ten w<strong>on</strong>der how <strong>the</strong> artist decided where to start. This is <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> most serious problems that plague students. They ask, "AfterI've decided what to draw, how do I know where to start?" or"What happens if I start too large or too small?" Using a BasicUnit to start a drawing answers both <strong>the</strong>se questi<strong>on</strong>s, and ensuresthat you will end with <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong> you so carefully chosebefore you started a drawing.After years <strong>of</strong> teaching classes and workshops, struggling t<strong>of</strong>ind words to explain how to start a drawing, I and my fellowteachers finally worked out a method that helped us to communi-PERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACE 123


cate how a trained artist does this. We had to carefully introspectwhat we were doing when starting a drawing and <strong>the</strong>n figure outhow to teach <strong>the</strong> process, which is fundamentally n<strong>on</strong>-verbal,extremely rapid, and "<strong>on</strong> automatic." I have called this method,"Choosing a Basic Unit." This Basic Unit becomes <strong>the</strong> key thatunlocks all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships within a chosen compositi<strong>on</strong>: Allproporti<strong>on</strong>s are found by comparing everything to <strong>the</strong> Basic Unit.The Basic Unit—A definiti<strong>on</strong>Fig-7-7.In Chapter Six, I stated that all parts <strong>of</strong> a compositi<strong>on</strong> (negativespaces and positive forms) are locked into a relati<strong>on</strong>ship that isbounded by <strong>the</strong> outside edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format. For realistic drawing,<strong>the</strong> artist is bound to that relati<strong>on</strong>ship in which all <strong>the</strong> parts fittoge<strong>the</strong>r: The artist is not at liberty to change <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>alrelati<strong>on</strong>ships. I'm sure you can see that if you change <strong>on</strong>e part,something else necessarily gets changed. In Chapter Six I used achild's jigsaw puzzle to illustrate <strong>the</strong> important c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> sharededges. I'll use <strong>the</strong> same puzzle to illustrate <strong>the</strong> Basic Unit (Figure7-7).The Basic Unit is a "starting shape" or "starting unit" that youchoose from within <strong>the</strong> scene you are looking at through <strong>the</strong>Viewfinder (<strong>the</strong> sailboat <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> water). You need to choose a BasicUnit <strong>of</strong> medium size—nei<strong>the</strong>r very small nor very large, relativeto <strong>the</strong> format. In this instance, you could choose <strong>the</strong> straight edge<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sail. A Basic Unit can be a whole shape (<strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> a windowor <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> a negative space) or it can be just a singleedge from point to point (<strong>the</strong> top edge <strong>of</strong> a window, for example).The choice depends <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> what is easiest to see and easiest touse as your Basic Unit <strong>of</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>.In <strong>the</strong> jigsaw puzzle, I chose to use <strong>the</strong> straight edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sailas my Basic Unit.Once chosen, all o<strong>the</strong>r proporti<strong>on</strong>s are determined relative toyour Basic Unit. The Basic Unit is always called "One." You canlay your pencil down <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> puzzle to compare <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships.For example, you can now ask yourself, "How wide is <strong>the</strong> boatcompared to my Basic Unit, <strong>the</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sail?" (One to124 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


<strong>on</strong>e and <strong>on</strong>e-third.) "How wide is <strong>the</strong> sail relative to my BasicUnit?" (One to two-thirds.) "Where is <strong>the</strong> sea/sky edge from <strong>the</strong>bottom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format?" (One to <strong>on</strong>e and <strong>on</strong>e-quarter.) Note thatfor each proporti<strong>on</strong>, you go back to your Basic Unit to measure it<strong>on</strong> your pencil and <strong>the</strong>n you make <strong>the</strong> comparis<strong>on</strong> with ano<strong>the</strong>rpart <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>. I'm sure you can see <strong>the</strong> logic <strong>of</strong> thismethod and how it will enable you to draw in proporti<strong>on</strong>.As I teach you how to find and use a Basic Unit, this method<strong>of</strong> starting may seem a bit tedious and mechanical at first. But itresolves many problems, including problems <strong>of</strong> starting and <strong>of</strong>compositi<strong>on</strong> as well as problems <strong>of</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships. Itso<strong>on</strong> becomes quite automatic. In fact, this is <strong>the</strong> method mostexperienced artists use, but <strong>the</strong>y do it so rapidly that some<strong>on</strong>ewatching would think that an artist "just starts drawing."An anecdote about French artist Henri Matisse illustrates thispoint and also illustrates <strong>the</strong> almost subc<strong>on</strong>scious process <strong>of</strong> findinga Basic Unit. John Elderfield, curator <strong>of</strong> drawings at <strong>the</strong>Museum <strong>of</strong> Modern Art in <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York, in his w<strong>on</strong>derful catalog <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> Matisse Retrospective Exhibiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> 1992, states: "There is a1946 film <strong>of</strong> Matisse painting Young Woman in White, Red Background[see Figure 7-8].... When Matisse saw <strong>the</strong> slow-moti<strong>on</strong> sequence<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> film, he felt 'suddenly naked,' he said, because he saw howhis hand 'made a strange journey <strong>of</strong> its own' in <strong>the</strong> air beforedrawing <strong>the</strong> model's features. It was not hesitati<strong>on</strong>, he insisted: 'Iwas unc<strong>on</strong>sciously establishing <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship between <strong>the</strong> subjectI was about to draw and <strong>the</strong> size <strong>of</strong> my paper.' " Elderfieldgoes <strong>on</strong> to say, "This can be taken to mean that he had to be aware<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> entire area he was composing before he could mark a particularsecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> it."Clearly, Matisse was finding his "starting shape," <strong>the</strong> head <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> model, to make sure he would have it <strong>the</strong> right size to show<strong>the</strong> whole figure in his painting. The curious thing aboutMatisse's remark, I think, is that he felt "suddenly naked" whenhe saw himself apparently figuring out how big to make that firstshape. I think this indicates <strong>the</strong> almost entirely subc<strong>on</strong>sciousnature <strong>of</strong> this process.Later <strong>on</strong>, you too will rapidly find a starting shape or a BasicFig. 7-8. Henri Matisse, YoungWoman in White, Red Background,1946.PERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACE 125


Unit or your "One"—or whatever you may eventually call it. Andsome<strong>on</strong>e watching you will think that you "just started drawing."Fig. 7-9.Getting <strong>of</strong>f to a good startI hope that you will become used to quickly choosing a BasicUnit to ensure a good compositi<strong>on</strong>. I imagine that you havealready grasped <strong>the</strong> (visual) logic <strong>of</strong> starting your drawing thisway, but allow me to put it into words <strong>on</strong>ce more.When students are first learning to draw, <strong>the</strong>y almost desperatelywant to get something down <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper. Often, <strong>the</strong>y justplunge in, drawing some object in <strong>the</strong> scene in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>mwithout paying attenti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> size <strong>of</strong> that first shape in relati<strong>on</strong>to <strong>the</strong> size <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format.The size <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first shape that you draw c<strong>on</strong>trols <strong>the</strong> subsequentsize <strong>of</strong> everything in <strong>the</strong> drawing. If that first shape is inadvertentlydrawn too small or too large, <strong>the</strong> resulting drawing maybe an entirely different compositi<strong>on</strong> from <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e you intended todepict.Students find this frustrating, because it <strong>of</strong>ten happens that<strong>the</strong> very thing that interested <strong>the</strong>m in <strong>the</strong> scene turns out to be"<strong>of</strong>f <strong>the</strong> edge" <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper. They d<strong>on</strong>'t get to draw that part at allsimply because <strong>the</strong> first shape <strong>the</strong>y drew was too large. C<strong>on</strong>versely,if <strong>the</strong> first shape is too small, students find that <strong>the</strong>y mustinclude much that is <strong>of</strong> no interest to <strong>the</strong>m in order to "fill out"<strong>the</strong> format.The method I am recommending to you, <strong>of</strong> correctly sizing<strong>the</strong> first shape (your Basic Unit) that you set down, prevents thisinadvertent problem and becomes quite automatic with a bit <strong>of</strong>practice. Later <strong>on</strong>, when you have discarded all <strong>of</strong> your drawingaids—<strong>the</strong> Viewfinders and plastic Picture Plane, you will useyour hands to form a rough "viewfinder" (as in Figure 7-9), andyou will still size <strong>the</strong> first shape (which, in <strong>the</strong>se less<strong>on</strong>s, we arecalling your Basic Unit) correctly for your chosen compositi<strong>on</strong>.126 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Your Negative Space drawing <strong>of</strong> a chairWhat you'll need:• Your Viewfinder with <strong>the</strong> larger opening• Your Picture Plane• Your felt-tip marker• Your masking tape• Several sheets <strong>of</strong> drawing paper• Your drawing board• Your pencils, sharpened• Your eraser• Your graphite stick and several dry paper towels or papernapkins• About an hour <strong>of</strong> interrupted time—more, if possible, but atleast an hourGetting set up to drawYou'll be taking some preliminary steps, so please read all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>instructi<strong>on</strong>s before you start. The following are <strong>the</strong> preliminarysteps for every drawing and take <strong>on</strong>ly a few minutes, <strong>on</strong>ce youhave learned <strong>the</strong> process.• choosing a format and drawing it <strong>on</strong> your paper• t<strong>on</strong>ing your paper (if you choose to work <strong>on</strong> a t<strong>on</strong>ed ground)• drawing your crosshairs• composing your drawing• choosing a Basic Unit• drawing <strong>the</strong> chosen Basic Unit <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane with afelt-tip marker• transferring <strong>the</strong> Basic Unit to your paper• <strong>the</strong>n, starting <strong>the</strong> drawingI'll describe each step.1. The first step is to draw a format <strong>on</strong> your drawing paper. Foryour Negative Space drawing <strong>of</strong> a chair, use <strong>the</strong> outside edge<strong>of</strong> your Viewfinder or <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane. The drawingwill be larger than <strong>the</strong> opening <strong>of</strong> your Viewfinder.PERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACE 127


As in any field, <strong>the</strong> "rules" <strong>of</strong> artare made to be broken by artistsworking at advanced levels. Whileacquiring basic drawing skills,however, I think it is best to staywith <strong>the</strong> task at hand—learninghow to see and draw. Once youhave mastered basic drawing, youcan take it as far as you want andbreak <strong>the</strong> rules intenti<strong>on</strong>ally, notby accident.2. The sec<strong>on</strong>d step is to t<strong>on</strong>e your paper. Make sure you have astack <strong>of</strong> several sheets <strong>of</strong> paper to pad your drawing. Begin tot<strong>on</strong>e your paper by rubbing <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> graphite stickvery lightly over <strong>the</strong> paper, staying inside <strong>the</strong> format.3. Once you have covered <strong>the</strong> paper with a light applicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>graphite, begin to rub <strong>the</strong> graphite into <strong>the</strong> paper with yourpaper towels. Rub with a circular moti<strong>on</strong>, applying pressureevenly and going right up to <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format. You wantto achieve a very smooth, silvery t<strong>on</strong>e.4. Next, lightly draw horiz<strong>on</strong>tal and vertical crosshairs <strong>on</strong> yourt<strong>on</strong>ed paper. The lines will cross in <strong>the</strong> center, just as <strong>the</strong>y do<strong>on</strong> your plastic Picture Plane. Use <strong>the</strong> crosshairs <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plasticplane to mark <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> crosshairs <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> format<strong>of</strong> your t<strong>on</strong>ed paper. A cauti<strong>on</strong>: D<strong>on</strong>'t make <strong>the</strong> lines too dark.They are <strong>on</strong>ly guidelines, and later you may want to eliminate<strong>the</strong>m.5. The next step is to choose a chair to use as <strong>the</strong> subject <strong>of</strong> yourdrawing. Any chair will do—an <strong>of</strong>fice chair, a plain straightchair, a stool, a cafeteria chair, whatever. If you are lucky, youmay find a rocking chair or a bentwood chair or somethingelse very complicated and interesting. But <strong>the</strong> simplest kind<strong>of</strong> chair will be fine for your drawing.6. Place <strong>the</strong> chair against a fairly simple background, perhaps aroom corner or a wall with a door. A blank wall is just fine andwill make a beautiful, simple drawing, but <strong>the</strong> choice <strong>of</strong> settingis entirely up to you. A lamp placed nearby may throw a128 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OK THE BRAIN


w<strong>on</strong>derful shadow <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chair <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> wall or floor—a shadowthat can become part <strong>of</strong> your compositi<strong>on</strong>.7. Sit in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> your "still life"—<strong>the</strong> chair and setting you havechosen—at a comfortable distance <strong>of</strong> about eight to ten feet.Take <strong>the</strong> cap <strong>of</strong>f your felt-tip marker and place it close besideyou.8. Next, use your Viewfinder to compose your drawing. Fasten<strong>the</strong> Viewfinder <strong>on</strong>to your clear plastic Picture Plane. Hold<strong>the</strong> Viewfinder/ Picture Plane in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> your face, close <strong>on</strong>eeye, and, moving <strong>the</strong> device forward or backward, "frame" <strong>the</strong>chair in a compositi<strong>on</strong> that you like. (Students are very goodat this. They seem to have an intuitive "feel" for compositi<strong>on</strong>.)If you wish, <strong>the</strong> chair can nearly touch <strong>the</strong> format so that <strong>the</strong>chair pretty much "fills <strong>the</strong> space."9. Hold <strong>the</strong> Viewfinder very still. Now, gazing at a space in <strong>the</strong>chair, perhaps between two back slats, imagine that <strong>the</strong> chairis magically pulverized and—like Bugs Bunny, in a po<strong>of</strong>!—disappears. What is left are <strong>the</strong> negative spaces. They are real.They have real shapes, just like <strong>the</strong> remains <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> door in <strong>the</strong>analogy above. These negative space-shapes are what you aregoing to draw. I repeat: You will draw <strong>the</strong> spaces, not <strong>the</strong> chair.See Figure 7-10.Fig. 7-10Choosing a Basic Unit1. When you have found a compositi<strong>on</strong> you like, hold <strong>the</strong>Viewfinder/plastic Picture Plane in that positi<strong>on</strong>. Pick up <strong>the</strong>felt-tip marker. Next, choose a negative space within <strong>the</strong>drawing—perhaps a space-shape between two rungs orbetween two back-slats. This space-shape should be fairlysimple, if possible, and nei<strong>the</strong>r too small nor too large. Youare looking for a manageable unit that you can clearly see forits shape and size. This is your Basic Unit, your "startingshape," your "One." See Figure 7-10 for an example.2. With <strong>on</strong>e eye closed, focus <strong>on</strong> that particular negativespace—your Basic Unit. Keep your eye focused <strong>on</strong> your BasicUnit until it "pops" into focus as a shape. (This always takes amoment—perhaps it is L-mode's protesting time!)Fig. 7-11.PERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACE129


Note that:•The t<strong>on</strong>ed format <strong>on</strong> your paper islarger in size than <strong>the</strong> format <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>opening <strong>of</strong> your Viewfinder.Though <strong>the</strong> sizes are different, <strong>the</strong>proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two formats—meaning <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship <strong>of</strong> widthto length—is <strong>the</strong> same.•Your felt-tip drawing <strong>of</strong> your BasicUnit <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Planeand your drawing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> t<strong>on</strong>edpaper will be <strong>the</strong> same, but <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e<strong>on</strong> your paper will be larger.•Stated ano<strong>the</strong>r way, <strong>the</strong> images are<strong>the</strong> same, but <strong>the</strong> scale is different.Note that in this instance, you"scale up." At o<strong>the</strong>r times, you may"scale down."3. With your felt-tip marker, carefully draw your Basic Unit <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane. This shape will be <strong>the</strong> start <strong>of</strong> yournegative space drawing <strong>on</strong> your t<strong>on</strong>ed paper (Figure. 7-11).4. The next step is to transfer your Basic Unit <strong>on</strong>to <strong>the</strong> paperyou have t<strong>on</strong>ed. You will use your crosshairs to place it andsize it correctly. (This is called "scaling up." See <strong>the</strong> sidebarfor an explanati<strong>on</strong>.) Looking at your drawing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plasticplane, say to yourself: "Relative to <strong>the</strong> format and to <strong>the</strong>crosshairs, where does that edge start? How far over from thatside? From <strong>the</strong> crosshair? From <strong>the</strong> bottom?" These assessmentswill help you draw your Basic Unit correctly. Check itthree ways: The shape <strong>on</strong> your t<strong>on</strong>ed paper, <strong>the</strong> actual spaceshapein <strong>the</strong> chair-model, and <strong>the</strong> shape in <strong>the</strong> Picture Planedrawing should all be proporti<strong>on</strong>ally <strong>the</strong> same.5. Check each angle in your Basic Unit <strong>the</strong> same way, by comparingthree ways as above. To determine an angle, say toyourself, "Relative to <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format (vertical or horiz<strong>on</strong>tal),what is that angle?" You can also use <strong>the</strong> crosshairs(vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal) to assess any angles in your BasicUnit. Then, draw <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> space at an angle just as yousee it. (Simultaneously, <strong>of</strong> course, you are drawing <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> chair.)6. One more time, check your drawing <strong>of</strong> your Basic Unit, firstwith <strong>the</strong> actual chair-model and <strong>the</strong>n with <strong>the</strong> rough sketch<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane. Even though <strong>the</strong> scale is differentin each, <strong>the</strong> relative proporti<strong>on</strong>s and angles will be <strong>the</strong>same.It is worth taking time to make sure your Basic Unit is correct.Once you have this first negative space-shape correctly sized andplaced within <strong>the</strong> format in your drawing, all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> rest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>drawing will be in relati<strong>on</strong>ship to that first shape. You will experience<strong>the</strong> beautiful logic <strong>of</strong> drawing and you will end with <strong>the</strong>compositi<strong>on</strong> you so carefully chose at <strong>the</strong> start.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> rest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> negative spaces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chair1. Remember to focus <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> negative spaces.130 THK NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Try to c<strong>on</strong>vince yourself that <strong>the</strong> chair is g<strong>on</strong>e, pulverized,absent. Only <strong>the</strong> spaces are real. Try also to avoid talking toyourself or questi<strong>on</strong>ing why things are <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>y are—forexample, why any space-shape is <strong>the</strong> way it is. Draw it just asyou see it. Try not to "think" at all, in terms <strong>of</strong> L-mode logic.Remember that everything you need is right <strong>the</strong>re in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong>your eye and you need not "figure it out." Remember also thatyou can check out any problem area by returning to yourplastic Picture Plane and, remembering to close <strong>on</strong>e eye,drawing <strong>the</strong> troublesome part directly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic plane.2. Draw <strong>the</strong> spaces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chair <strong>on</strong>e after ano<strong>the</strong>r. Working outwardfrom your Basic Unit, all <strong>the</strong> shapes will fit toge<strong>the</strong>r likea jigsaw puzzle. You d<strong>on</strong>'t have to figure out anything about<strong>the</strong> chair. In fact, you d<strong>on</strong>'t have to think about <strong>the</strong> chair at all.And d<strong>on</strong>'t questi<strong>on</strong> why <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> a space goes this way orthat. Just draw it as you see it. See Figure 7-12.3. Again, if an edge is at an angle, say to yourself, "What is thatangle compared to vertical?" Then, draw <strong>the</strong> edge at <strong>the</strong> angleyou see it.4. Gauge horiz<strong>on</strong>tals in <strong>the</strong> same way: What is <strong>the</strong> angle, comparedto horiz<strong>on</strong>tal (that is, <strong>the</strong> top or bottom edges <strong>of</strong> yourformat)?5. As you draw, try to take c<strong>on</strong>scious note <strong>of</strong> what <strong>the</strong> mentalmode <strong>of</strong> drawing feels like—<strong>the</strong> loss <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> time, <strong>the</strong>feeling <strong>of</strong> "locking <strong>on</strong>" to <strong>the</strong> image, and <strong>the</strong> w<strong>on</strong>derful sense<strong>of</strong> amazement at <strong>the</strong> beauty <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>s. During <strong>the</strong>process, you will find that <strong>the</strong> negative spaces will begin toseem interesting in <strong>the</strong>ir strangeness and complexity. If youhave a problem with any part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing, remind yourselfthat everything you need to know in order to do this drawingis right <strong>the</strong>re, perfectly available to you.6. C<strong>on</strong>tinue working your way through <strong>the</strong> drawing, searchingout relati<strong>on</strong>ships, both angles (relative to vertical or horiz<strong>on</strong>tal)and proporti<strong>on</strong>s (relative to each o<strong>the</strong>r). If you talk toyourself at all during <strong>the</strong> drawing, use <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong>relati<strong>on</strong>ships: "How wide is this space compared to <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e Ihave just drawn?" "What is this angle compared to horiz<strong>on</strong>tal?"Fig. 7-12.Fig. 7-13.PERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACE 131


Fig. 7-14.Fig. 7-15."How far does that space extend relative to that whole edge <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> format?" So<strong>on</strong>, you will be "really drawing." The drawingwill begin to seem like a fascinating puzzle, <strong>the</strong> parts fittingtoge<strong>the</strong>r in an entirely satisfying way (Figure 7-14).7. When you have finished drawing <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> spaces, youmay want to "work up" <strong>the</strong> drawing a bit by using your eraserto remove <strong>the</strong> t<strong>on</strong>e in some areas, perhaps erasing <strong>the</strong> negativespaces and leaving <strong>the</strong> chair in t<strong>on</strong>e (Figure 7-15). If yousee shadows <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> floor or <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> wall behind, you may wantto add <strong>the</strong>m to your drawing, perhaps adding in some t<strong>on</strong>ewith your pencil, or erasing out <strong>the</strong> negative spaces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>shadows. You may also want to "work up" <strong>the</strong> positive form <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> chair itself, adding some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> interior c<strong>on</strong>tours.After you have finished:I feel c<strong>on</strong>fident that your drawing will please you. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>most striking characteristics <strong>of</strong> negative-space drawings is that nomatter how mundane <strong>the</strong> subject—a chair, an eggbeater, a canopener—<strong>the</strong> drawing will seem beautiful.Perhaps negative-space drawings remind us <strong>of</strong> our l<strong>on</strong>gingfor unity, or perhaps <strong>of</strong> our actual unity with <strong>the</strong> world around us.No matter what <strong>the</strong> explanati<strong>on</strong>, we simply like to look at negative-spacedrawings. D<strong>on</strong>'t you agree?With <strong>on</strong>ly this brief less<strong>on</strong>, you will begin to see negativespaces everywhere. My students <strong>of</strong>ten regard this as a great andjoyful discovery. Practice seeing negative spaces as you gothrough your everyday routine and imagine yourself drawingthose beautiful spaces. This mental practice at odd moments isextremely helpful in putting perceptual skills "<strong>on</strong> automatic,"ready to be integrated into a learned skill that you own.What follows is <strong>on</strong>e last example <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> usefulness <strong>of</strong> negativespaces.The cognitive battle <strong>of</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>Figures 7-16 and 7-17 show an interesting graphic record <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>struggle and its resoluti<strong>on</strong> in two drawings by a student <strong>of</strong> a cart132THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


and slide projector. In Figure 7-16, <strong>the</strong> first drawing, <strong>the</strong> studenthad great difficulty rec<strong>on</strong>ciling his stored knowledge <strong>of</strong> what <strong>the</strong>objects were "supposed to look like" with what he saw. Notice in<strong>the</strong> drawing that <strong>the</strong> legs <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cart are all <strong>the</strong> same length, and asymbol is used for <strong>the</strong> wheels. When he shifted to using a viewfinderand drawing <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> negative spaces, he was farmore successful (Figure 7-17). The visual informati<strong>on</strong> apparentlycame through clearly; <strong>the</strong> drawing looks c<strong>on</strong>fident and as thoughit were d<strong>on</strong>e with ease. And, in fact, it was d<strong>on</strong>e with ease,because using negative space enables <strong>on</strong>e to escape <strong>the</strong> mentalcrunch that occurs when percepti<strong>on</strong>s d<strong>on</strong>'t match c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s.It's not that <strong>the</strong> visual informati<strong>on</strong> ga<strong>the</strong>red by regardingspaces ra<strong>the</strong>r than objects is really less complex or is in any wayeasier to draw. The spaces, after all, share edges with <strong>the</strong> form.But by looking at <strong>the</strong> spaces, we free R-mode from <strong>the</strong> dominati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> L-mode. Put ano<strong>the</strong>r way, by focusing <strong>on</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> thatdoes not suit <strong>the</strong> style <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> verbal system, we cause <strong>the</strong> job to beshifted to <strong>the</strong> mode appropriate for drawing. Thus, <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flictends, and in R-mode, <strong>the</strong> brain processes spatial, relati<strong>on</strong>al informati<strong>on</strong>with ease.Showing all manner <strong>of</strong> negative spacesThese drawings are intriguingly pleasurable to look at, evenwhen <strong>the</strong> positive forms are as mundane as schoolroom chairs.One could speculate that <strong>the</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> is that <strong>the</strong> method <strong>of</strong> drawingraises to a c<strong>on</strong>scious level <strong>the</strong> unity <strong>of</strong> positive and negativeshapes and spaces. Ano<strong>the</strong>r reas<strong>on</strong> may be that <strong>the</strong> techniqueresults in excellent compositi<strong>on</strong>s with particularly interestingdivisi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> shapes and spaces within <strong>the</strong> format.Learning to see clearly through drawing can surely enhanceyour capacity to take a clear look at problems and to be betterable to see things in perspective. In <strong>the</strong> next chapter, we'll take up<strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships, a skill you can put to use in asmany directi<strong>on</strong>s as your mind can take you.Fig. 7-16.Fig. 7-17.PERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACE 133


Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawing byinstructor Lisbeth Firmin.Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawing by <strong>the</strong>author.Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawing by <strong>the</strong>author.Student drawing.Student drawing by Sandy DePhillippo.134 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Winslow Homer (1836-1910), Child Seated in a WickerChair (1874). Courtesy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Sterling and FrancineClark Art Institute.Observe how Winslow Homer used negative space inhis drawing <strong>of</strong> a child in a chair. Try copying thisdrawing.Peter Paul Rubens (1577-1640). Studies <strong>of</strong> Arms and Legs.Courtesy <strong>of</strong> Museum Boymans-Van Beuningen,Rotterdam.Copy this drawing. Turn <strong>the</strong> original upside down anddraw <strong>the</strong> negative spaces. Then turn <strong>the</strong> drawing rightside up and complete <strong>the</strong> details inside <strong>the</strong> forms. These"difficult" foreshortened forms become easy to draw ifattenti<strong>on</strong> is focused <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> spaces around <strong>the</strong> forms.PERCEIVING THE SHAPE OF A SPACE: THE POSITIVE ASPECTS OF NEGATIVE SPACE 65


Relati<strong>on</strong>shipsin a <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> Mode:Putting Sightingin Perspective


Lupe Ramirez.In this chapter, you will learn <strong>the</strong> third basic skill <strong>of</strong> drawing,how to see and draw relati<strong>on</strong>ships. You will learn how todraw "in perspective" and "in proporti<strong>on</strong>." Ano<strong>the</strong>r term foracquiring this skill is "learning how to sight."Learning this skill is perhaps comparable to learning <strong>the</strong>rules <strong>of</strong> grammar in reading and writing. Just as good grammarcauses words and phrases to hang toge<strong>the</strong>r logically and to communicateideas clearly, skillful sighting <strong>of</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s and perspectivecauses edges, spaces, relati<strong>on</strong>ships, lights and shadows tocome toge<strong>the</strong>r with visual logic. Clear percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>shipsenables us to depict <strong>on</strong> a flat surface <strong>the</strong> world we seearound us. Moreover, just as learning how to use grammar skillfullygives us power with words, learning how to draw in perspectiveand in proporti<strong>on</strong> will give your drawings power through <strong>the</strong>illusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> space.In speaking <strong>of</strong> grammar, I am referring to <strong>the</strong> mechanics <strong>of</strong>language, not <strong>the</strong> tedious naming <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> parts <strong>of</strong> speech. Bymechanics, I mean getting <strong>the</strong> subject and verb to agree, using <strong>the</strong>rules <strong>of</strong> word order and sentence structure, and so <strong>on</strong>. I couldn'tparse a complicated sentence today if I tried my best (which probablyindicates its usefulness or lack <strong>the</strong>re<strong>of</strong>), but I've learned andpracticed <strong>the</strong> mechanics <strong>of</strong> language for so l<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>y are <strong>on</strong> automatic.This is what we are aiming for in this chapter: You will learnto use perspective and proporti<strong>on</strong> in your drawing. You will notlearn tedious and cumbersome terminology <strong>of</strong> vanishing points,c<strong>on</strong>verging parallel lines, and perspective <strong>of</strong> ellipses. You willlearn <strong>the</strong> mechanics <strong>of</strong> sighting, which is what most artists use.Some <strong>of</strong> my students, never<strong>the</strong>less, still complain that learningto sight seems so "left-brained" after <strong>the</strong> R-mode joy <strong>of</strong> drawingedges and negative spaces. Indeed, <strong>the</strong>re are lots <strong>of</strong> little stepsand instructi<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> beginning. But almost every skill requiresa comp<strong>on</strong>ent similar to sighting in drawing. For example, learningto drive a car requires that at some point you learn <strong>the</strong> rules <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> road. Tedious? Yes, but without <strong>the</strong>m, you are very likely tobe arrested or to have an accident. Significantly, <strong>on</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>se rulesare learned and "<strong>on</strong> automatic," you drive a car by <strong>the</strong> rules withouteven thinking <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m.138THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


It is <strong>the</strong> same with drawing. Once you have worked your waythrough <strong>the</strong> next exercise, you will have learned <strong>the</strong> "rules <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>road" <strong>of</strong> drawing. With a bit <strong>of</strong> practice, sighting goes <strong>on</strong> automaticand you will hardly be aware <strong>of</strong> taking sights and comparingproporti<strong>on</strong>s. Best <strong>of</strong> all, you will have achieved <strong>the</strong> power todepict three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al space in your drawings.Students <strong>of</strong> drawing who learn everything except how tosight relati<strong>on</strong>ships greatly handicap <strong>the</strong>ir drawing and find <strong>the</strong>mselvesc<strong>on</strong>stantly making baffling mistakes in proporti<strong>on</strong> and perspective.This problem plagues students new to drawing and, Imight add, some ra<strong>the</strong>r advanced students as well.Why does this skill seem so difficult? First, it is a two-partskill. The first part is sighting angles relative to vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal,and <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d part is sighting proporti<strong>on</strong>s relative to eacho<strong>the</strong>r. In additi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> skill requires that <strong>on</strong>e deal with ratios andcomparis<strong>on</strong>s that seem quite "left-brained." And, finally, itrequires that <strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ts and deals with paradoxes. For example,we can know that a ceiling is flat and <strong>the</strong> corner is a rightangle. But <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture plane, <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ceiling are nothoriz<strong>on</strong>tal and <strong>the</strong> corner angles are not right angles at all. Theyare oblique angles. As you can imagine, we'll have to carefullyoutmaneuver your L-mode, which will so<strong>on</strong> be saying, "Thisdoesn't make sense!" Or, "This is too complicated! I'll never getit!" Or, "This stuff is stupid!"On my word, learning how to sight relati<strong>on</strong>ships is not boring;it is powerful—it unlocks space. I agree, <strong>the</strong> skill is complicated,but you've learned o<strong>the</strong>r complicated things beforethis—how to read and write, for example. And sighting isdefinitely not stupid; it is intellectually fascinating—witness <strong>the</strong>many great thinkers <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Renaissance who grappled with <strong>the</strong>problem <strong>of</strong> how to depict space <strong>on</strong> a flat surface.Once L-mode complaints are set aside, I believe you willactually enjoy learning this skill. I'm sure you can see <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>between learning to see and draw what is right <strong>the</strong>re in fr<strong>on</strong>t<strong>of</strong> your eyes and learning to be a more "clear-sighted" pers<strong>on</strong>,able to deal with c<strong>on</strong>tradictory informati<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong> many paradoxes<strong>of</strong> our world. Be prepared for all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> objecti<strong>on</strong>s. YourDem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawingby Grace Kennedy.Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawingby <strong>the</strong> author.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE 139


L-mode will have a field day, but stay with me! I'll try to be asclear as possible.Fig. 8-1. Roll up a tube <strong>of</strong> paper andcheck <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship <strong>of</strong> sizes <strong>of</strong> anearby object (some<strong>on</strong>e's head, forexample) and a similar object far<strong>the</strong>raway. You will be surprised at<strong>the</strong> apparent change in size.Fig. 8-2. Laurie Kuroyama.Notice <strong>the</strong> great change in headsize from near to far.Fig. 8-3.On dealing with <strong>the</strong> two-part skill <strong>of</strong> sighting anglesand proporti<strong>on</strong>sThe term sighting really means seeing, but seeing in <strong>the</strong> artist'sspecial way—seeing relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture plane (See Figures8-1 and 8-2). All <strong>of</strong> sighting is comparis<strong>on</strong>: What is this anglecompared to vertical? How big is <strong>the</strong> apple compared to <strong>the</strong>mel<strong>on</strong>? How wide is <strong>the</strong> table compared to its length? All comparis<strong>on</strong>sare made relative to c<strong>on</strong>stants: Angles are compared to<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stants vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal. Sizes (proporti<strong>on</strong>s) are alsocompared to a c<strong>on</strong>stant—our Basic Unit.On dealing with ratios: The root <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word "relati<strong>on</strong>ship" isratio. In ma<strong>the</strong>matics, ratios are expressed as numbers—1:2 means<strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> this to two <strong>of</strong> that. Ratios seem like a left-brained c<strong>on</strong>ceptbecause <strong>the</strong>y are str<strong>on</strong>gly c<strong>on</strong>nected in our minds with ma<strong>the</strong>matics.But we use ratios in many ordinary activities. In cooking,for example, candy is <strong>on</strong>e part liquid to two parts sugar—that is,1:2. In map reading, city X is three times as far as city Y—<strong>the</strong> ratiois 3:1. In drawing, ratios become handy tags to assess proporti<strong>on</strong>alrelati<strong>on</strong>ships am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> parts <strong>of</strong> a compositi<strong>on</strong>. The artistchooses something to be "One," our Basic Unit, and that unit israti<strong>on</strong>alized or proporti<strong>on</strong>alized with all o<strong>the</strong>r parts.To illustrate, <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> a window can be called "One," <strong>the</strong>Basic Unit. In comparis<strong>on</strong>, let's say that <strong>the</strong> window is twice asl<strong>on</strong>g as it is wide. The ratio is 1:2. The artist draws <strong>the</strong> width, callsit "One," measures it as "One" and <strong>the</strong>n measures <strong>of</strong>f two BasicUnits, counting "One to <strong>on</strong>e, two." The ratio is 1:2. It's an easy wayto tag and remember a proporti<strong>on</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g enough to transfer it intoyour drawing.On dealing with paradox: Seen flattened <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane, a tablemay appear (by taking a sight) to be narrower than you know it tobe (see Figure 8-3). The sighted ratio might be 1:8, for example.You must learn to "swallow" this visual paradox and draw whatyou have seen <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane. Only <strong>the</strong>n will <strong>the</strong> table, in your140THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Sighting can be used to determine<strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship <strong>of</strong> lengths andwidths <strong>of</strong> forms. When drawing atable viewed from an obliqueangle, for example, an artist firstdetermines angles <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> edges relativeto horiz<strong>on</strong>tal and vertical bysighting, as in Figure 8-4.Fig. 8-4.The next percepti<strong>on</strong> required ishow wide <strong>the</strong> table is (from thisviewpoint) in relati<strong>on</strong> to its length.This apparent width relative tolength will vary from viewpoint toviewpoint, depending <strong>on</strong> where <strong>the</strong>viewer's eye level happens to be.1. Holding <strong>the</strong> pencil <strong>on</strong> a planeparallel to your eyes and at arm'slength, with <strong>the</strong> elbow locked tokeep <strong>the</strong> scale c<strong>on</strong>stant, measure<strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> table. Place <strong>the</strong>eraser <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pencil so it coincideswith <strong>on</strong>e corner <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> table andplace your thumb at <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r corner.This is your Basic Unit (Figure8-5).2. Still keeping your elbow lockedand with <strong>the</strong> pencil still parallel toyour eyes, carry that measurementto <strong>the</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> table (Figure8-5). How l<strong>on</strong>g is <strong>the</strong> table, relativeto its width? In this instance, <strong>the</strong>ratio is <strong>on</strong>e to <strong>on</strong>e and a half (1:1+1/2)(Figure 8-6).3. Next, you will take a sight <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>table legs by holding your pencilvertically, taking note <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> angle<strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e leg relative to vertical. Are<strong>the</strong> table legs perfectly vertical orare <strong>the</strong>y at an angle? Draw <strong>the</strong> legclosest to you. You can take a sight<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> leg relative(again) to <strong>the</strong> width, your BasicUnit (Figure 8-7).Fig. 8-6.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE141


"Point <strong>of</strong> view is worth eighty points— Alan Kay, computerscientist and DisneyFellowdrawing appear, paradoxically, to be <strong>the</strong> size and shape you knowitto be. Moreover, <strong>the</strong> angles <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tabletop may appear to bedifferent from what you know to be right angles. You must "swallow"this paradox as well.Perspective and proporti<strong>on</strong>Learning to draw in perspective requires that we see things as<strong>the</strong>y are out <strong>the</strong>re in <strong>the</strong> external world. We must put aside ourprejudgments, our stored and memorized stereotypes and habits<strong>of</strong> thinking. We must overcome false interpretati<strong>on</strong>s, which are<strong>of</strong>ten based <strong>on</strong> what we think must be out <strong>the</strong>re even though wemay never have taken a really clear look at what is right in fr<strong>on</strong>t<strong>of</strong> our eyes.I'm sure you can see <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> to problem solving. One<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first steps in solving problems is to scan <strong>the</strong> relevant factorsand to put things "into perspective" and "into proporti<strong>on</strong>." Thisprocess requires <strong>the</strong> capacity to see <strong>the</strong> various parts <strong>of</strong> a problemin <strong>the</strong>ir true relati<strong>on</strong>ship.Defining perspectiveThe term "perspective" comes from <strong>the</strong> Latin word "prospectus,"meaning "to look forward." Linear perspective, <strong>the</strong> system mostfamiliar to us, was perfected during <strong>the</strong> Renaissance by Europeanartists. Linear perspective enabled artists to reproduce visualchanges <strong>of</strong> lines and forms as <strong>the</strong>y appear in three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>alspace.Various cultures have developed different c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>s orperspective systems. Egyptian and Oriental artists, for example,developed a kind <strong>of</strong> stair-step or tiered perspective, in whichplacement from bottom to top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format indicated positi<strong>on</strong> inspace. In this system, which is <strong>of</strong>ten used intuitively by children,<strong>the</strong> forms at <strong>the</strong> very top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> page—regardless <strong>of</strong> size—arec<strong>on</strong>sidered to be <strong>the</strong> far<strong>the</strong>st away. More recently, artists haverebelled against rigid c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> perspective and haveinvented new systems employing abstract spatial qualities <strong>of</strong> colors,textures, lines, and shapes.142 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OK THE BRAIN


Traditi<strong>on</strong>al Renaissance perspective c<strong>on</strong>forms most closelyto <strong>the</strong> way people in our Western culture perceive objects inspace. In our percepti<strong>on</strong>s, parallel lines appear to c<strong>on</strong>verge atvanishing points <strong>on</strong> a horiz<strong>on</strong> line (<strong>the</strong> viewer's eye level) andforms appear to become smaller as distance from <strong>the</strong> viewerincreases. For this reas<strong>on</strong>, realistic drawing depends heavily <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong>se principles. The Dürer etching (Figure 8-8) illustrates thisperceptual system.Fig. 8-8. Albrecht Dürer, DraughtsmanMaking a Perspective <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>of</strong> a Woman (1525). Courtesy <strong>of</strong>The Metropolitan Museum <strong>of</strong> Art,<str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York. Gift <strong>of</strong> Felix M.Warburg, 1918.Dürer's deviceThe great sixteenth-century Renaissance artist, Albrecht Dürer,invented a device to help him draw in proporti<strong>on</strong> and in perspective.Your plastic Picture Plane is a simplified versi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> Dürer'sdevice. Let's look at <strong>the</strong> artist's depicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> his device in Figure8-8. Dürer's draughtsman, holding his head in a stati<strong>on</strong>ary positi<strong>on</strong>(note <strong>the</strong> vertical marker for his viewpoint), looks through anupright wire grid. The artist peers at his model from a viewpointthat foreshortens his visual image <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model—that is, a viewpointin which <strong>the</strong> main axis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> woman's figure from head t<strong>of</strong>oot coincides with <strong>the</strong> artist's line <strong>of</strong> sight. This view causes <strong>the</strong>more distant parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> figure (<strong>the</strong> head and shoulders) toappear to be smaller than <strong>the</strong>y actually are, and <strong>the</strong> nearby parts(<strong>the</strong> knees and lower legs) to appear to be larger.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE 143


Fig. 8-9. What Dürer saw: Sightingparts <strong>on</strong>e by <strong>on</strong>e.A foreshortened view <strong>of</strong> a legand foot, as seen flattened <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>Picture Plane.In fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> Dürer's draughtsman <strong>on</strong> his drawing table is apaper <strong>the</strong> same size as <strong>the</strong> wire grid, marked <strong>of</strong>f with an identicalgrid <strong>of</strong> lines. The artist draws <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper what he perceivesthrough <strong>the</strong> grid, matching in his drawing <strong>the</strong> exact angles andcurves and lengths <strong>of</strong> lines compared to <strong>the</strong> verticals and horiz<strong>on</strong>tals<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> grid. In effect, he is copying what he sees flattened<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture plane. If he copies just what he sees, he will produce<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper a foreshortened view <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model. The proporti<strong>on</strong>s,shapes, and sizes will be c<strong>on</strong>trary to what <strong>the</strong> artistknows about <strong>the</strong> actual proporti<strong>on</strong>s, shapes, and sizes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>human body; but <strong>on</strong>ly if he draws <strong>the</strong> untrue proporti<strong>on</strong>s he perceiveswill <strong>the</strong> drawing look true to life.What did Dürer see through his grid? (See Figure 8-9.) Dürersights point <strong>on</strong>e, <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> left knee, and marks that point <strong>on</strong>his gridded paper. Next, he sights point two, <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> lefthand, and <strong>the</strong>n point three, <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> left knee. Bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong>sepoints he sights <strong>the</strong> torso and <strong>the</strong> head. He c<strong>on</strong>nects all <strong>the</strong> pointsand ends with a foreshortened drawing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> entire figure.The problem with foreshortening in drawing is that what weknow about <strong>the</strong> subject <strong>of</strong> a drawing somehow intrudes into <strong>the</strong>drawing, and we draw what we know ra<strong>the</strong>r than what we see.The purpose <strong>of</strong> Dürer's device, using <strong>the</strong> grid and <strong>the</strong> fixed viewpoint,was to force himself to draw <strong>the</strong> form exactly as he saw it,with all <strong>of</strong> its "wr<strong>on</strong>g" proporti<strong>on</strong>s. Then, paradoxically, <strong>the</strong>drawing "looked right." A viewer <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing, <strong>the</strong>n, might144 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


w<strong>on</strong>der how <strong>the</strong> draughtsman was able to make <strong>the</strong> drawing look"so real."The achievement, <strong>the</strong>refore, <strong>of</strong> Renaissance perspective wasto codify and systematize a method <strong>of</strong> bypassing artists' knowledgeabout shapes and forms. The science <strong>of</strong> "formal" perspectiveprovided a means by which <strong>the</strong>y could draw forms just as <strong>the</strong>yappeared to <strong>the</strong> eye—including distorti<strong>on</strong>s created optically by aform's positi<strong>on</strong> in space relative to <strong>the</strong> viewer's eye.The system worked beautifully and solved <strong>the</strong> problem<strong>of</strong> how to create an illusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> deep space <strong>on</strong> a flat surface, <strong>of</strong>re-creating <strong>the</strong> visible world. Dürer's simple device evolved intoa complicated ma<strong>the</strong>matical system, enabling artists from <strong>the</strong>Renaissance <strong>on</strong>ward to overcome <strong>the</strong>ir mental resistance tooptical distorti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> true shapes <strong>of</strong> things and to drawrealistically.Formal perspective versus "informal" perspectiveBut <strong>the</strong> system <strong>of</strong> formal perspective is not without problems.Followed to <strong>the</strong> letter, strictly applied perspective rules can resultin ra<strong>the</strong>r dry and rigid drawings. Perhaps <strong>the</strong> most serious problemwith <strong>the</strong> formal perspective system is that it is so "leftbrained."It employs <strong>the</strong> style <strong>of</strong> left-hemisphere processing:analysis, sequential logical cogitati<strong>on</strong>, and mental calculati<strong>on</strong>swithin a pre-prescribed system. There are vanishing points, horiz<strong>on</strong>lines, perspective <strong>of</strong> circles and ellipses, and so <strong>on</strong>. The systemis detailed and cumbersome, <strong>the</strong> anti<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>of</strong> R-mode stylewith its antic/serious, pleasurable quality. For example, in anythingbut <strong>the</strong> simplest <strong>on</strong>e-point perspective setup (Figure 8-10),vanishing points may be several feet bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>drawing paper, requiring pins and strings to mark <strong>the</strong>m.Fortunately, <strong>on</strong>ce you understand "informal" perspective(sighting), you d<strong>on</strong>'t really need to know formal perspective at all.That's not to say <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> perspective is not useful and interesting.In my view, knowledge never hurts! But sighting is sufficientfor basic drawing skills.Graham Collier, pr<strong>of</strong>essor <strong>of</strong> art,states that in <strong>the</strong> early days <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>incepti<strong>on</strong> and development <strong>of</strong>Renaissance perspective it wasused creatively and imaginativelyto impart what must have been athrilling sense <strong>of</strong> space to art."Effective as perspective is, however,it becomes a deadeninginfluence <strong>on</strong> an artist's natural way<strong>of</strong> seeing things <strong>on</strong>ce it is acceptedas a system—as a mechanicalformula."— Graham CollierForm, Space, and Visi<strong>on</strong>,1963Fig. 8-10. The classic perspectiveillustrati<strong>on</strong>. Note that vertical linesremain vertical; horiz<strong>on</strong>tal edgesc<strong>on</strong>verge at a vanishing point (orpoints) <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> horiz<strong>on</strong> line (whichis always at <strong>the</strong> artist's eye level).That's <strong>on</strong>e-point perspective in anutshell. Two-point and threepointperspective are complex systems,involving multiple vanishingpoints that <strong>of</strong>ten extend far bey<strong>on</strong>d<strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing paper andrequiring a large drawing table,T-squares, straight-edges, etc., todraw. Informal sighting is mucheasier and is sufficiently accuratefor most drawing.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE 145


Fig. 8-11. Draw <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> doorway<strong>on</strong> your plastic Picture Plane.This is your Basic Unit.Fig. 8-12. Transfer your Basic Unitto your t<strong>on</strong>ed drawing paper. Since<strong>the</strong> paper is larger than <strong>the</strong> PicturePlane, you need to scale up (proporti<strong>on</strong>allyenlarge) your BasicUnit.A brief practice in sighting before you do a "real"perspective drawingWhat you'll need:• Your drawing board• Several sheets <strong>of</strong> scratch paper• Your drawing pencils, sharpened, and your eraser• Your plastic Picture Plane and your felt-tip marker• Your larger ViewfinderWhat you'll do:First, you will practice sighting proporti<strong>on</strong>s and angles, usingyour pencil as a sighting device. Once you've practiced a bit, <strong>the</strong>nyou'll do your "real" sighting drawing. Begin by seating yourselfin fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> a doorway, at about ten feet away.Hold up your Viewfinder/Picture Plane and compose yourdrawing so that you can see <strong>the</strong> whole doorway. Hold <strong>the</strong> PicturePlane very still and use your felt-tip marker to draw <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> doorway <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic plane. See Figure 8-n. (The line willbe somewhat shaky.) This is your Basic Unit. Transfer this unit toa piece <strong>of</strong> paper, estimating <strong>the</strong> size and positi<strong>on</strong> so that it is <strong>the</strong>same as <strong>on</strong> your Picture Plane. Set <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane aside. SeeFigure 8-12.Now, pick up your pencil. Hold it at arm's length toward <strong>the</strong>top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> doorway with <strong>the</strong> flat (eraser) end out and with yourelbow locked. Close <strong>on</strong>e eye and move <strong>the</strong> pencil so that <strong>the</strong> endcoincides with <strong>on</strong>e side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> doorway. (Choose ei<strong>the</strong>r<strong>the</strong> outside <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> molding or <strong>the</strong> inside edge.) Then, with <strong>on</strong>eeye still closed, move your thumb al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> pencil untilyour thumbnail coincides with <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> doorway.Hold that measure. You have "taken a sight" <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>doorway.A test: What happens if you open both eyes or if you relax your elbKeep your thumb at <strong>the</strong> same positi<strong>on</strong> and try bending yourelbow just slightly, just barely pulling <strong>the</strong> pencil toward you.146 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


What happens? The "measurement" has changed, hasn't it?Therefore, <strong>the</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> you must lock your elbow when sightingproporti<strong>on</strong>s is to maintain <strong>the</strong> same scale. When your elbow islocked, you are always taking sights using <strong>the</strong> same positi<strong>on</strong>.Then, relock your elbow, and resight <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> doorway<strong>on</strong> your pencil (Figure 8-13). We'll call this your Basic Unit,or your "One." Now, keeping your thumb in <strong>the</strong> same positi<strong>on</strong>,turn your pencil vertically and find <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship (<strong>the</strong> ratio orproporti<strong>on</strong>) <strong>of</strong> width to length.Still holding <strong>the</strong> pencil at arm's length, and still with <strong>on</strong>e eyeclosed and your elbow locked, measure from <strong>the</strong> top corner: "One(width), to <strong>on</strong>e (height)" (Figure 8-14), <strong>the</strong>n drop down, measure"One to two" (Figure 8-15), drop it again and measure <strong>the</strong> remainder,"One to two and two-thirds" (Figure 8-16). You have now"taken a sight" <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> width relative to <strong>the</strong>height <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> doorway. This proporti<strong>on</strong> is expressed as a ratio:1:2+2/3, or, in words, "One to two and two-thirds."Fig. 8-15. Measure "One..."Now, turn back to your sketchBy sighting <strong>the</strong> doorway, you determined that <strong>the</strong> width-toheightproporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> doorway was 1:2+2/3. That is <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> doorway "out <strong>the</strong>re" in <strong>the</strong> real world. Your job is totransfer that proporti<strong>on</strong> from "out <strong>the</strong>re" into your drawing.Fig. 8-14. "... to <strong>on</strong>e.. Fig. 8-15. "... two..." Fig. 8-16. "... and two-thirds."RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE 147


Fig. 8-17. Measure "One..."Obviously, <strong>the</strong> door in your drawing will be smaller—muchsmaller—than <strong>the</strong> real doorway. But it must be proporti<strong>on</strong>ally <strong>the</strong>same, width to length.Now, <strong>the</strong>refore, use your pencil and thumb to take a newmeasure: <strong>the</strong> width you have drawn <strong>on</strong> your paper (Figure 8-17).Then turn <strong>the</strong> pencil to vertical <strong>on</strong> your paper and measure <strong>of</strong>f"One to <strong>on</strong>e, two, and two-thirds" (Figures 8-18, 8-19, and 8-20).Make a mark and draw in <strong>the</strong> two sides <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> doorway. The doorwayyou have just drawn has <strong>the</strong> same proporti<strong>on</strong>—width toheight—as <strong>the</strong> real doorway you were looking at.To set this idea, draw a new "One," smaller than <strong>the</strong> first <strong>on</strong>e.Now, measure that width with your pencil and again mark <strong>of</strong>f <strong>the</strong>proporti<strong>on</strong>al height. This doorway will be smaller, but it will beproporti<strong>on</strong>ally <strong>the</strong> same as your first drawing and <strong>the</strong> real doorway.Summing up: In sighting proporti<strong>on</strong>s, you find out what <strong>the</strong>proporti<strong>on</strong>s are "out <strong>the</strong>re" in <strong>the</strong> real world and <strong>the</strong>n, holding<strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> in your mind as a ratio (your Basic Unit or"One"—in relati<strong>on</strong> to something else), remeasure in <strong>the</strong> drawingto transfer <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> drawing. Obviously, in drawings,sizes are almost always <strong>on</strong> a different scale (smaller or larger) thanwhat we see "out <strong>the</strong>re," but <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s are <strong>the</strong> same.Fig. 8-18. "... to <strong>on</strong>e..." Fig. 8-19. "... two ..." Fig. 8-20. "... and two-thirds.I48THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


As a clever student <strong>of</strong> mine put it: "You use your pencil to find<strong>the</strong> ratio 'out <strong>the</strong>re.' You remember it, wipe <strong>the</strong> measure <strong>of</strong>f <strong>the</strong>pencil, and remeasure with your pencil in <strong>the</strong> drawing."The next step: Sighting anglesRemember, sighting is a two-part skill. You have just learned<strong>the</strong> first part: sighting proporti<strong>on</strong>s. Your pencil, used as a sightingdevice, enables you to see "How big is this compared to that?""How wide is that compared to my Basic Unit?" And so <strong>on</strong>. Proporti<strong>on</strong>sare sighted relative to each o<strong>the</strong>r and to your Basic Unit.Sighting angles is different. Angles are sighted relative to verticaland horiz<strong>on</strong>tal. Remember, both angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>smust be sighted <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane.Take up your Viewfinder/Picture Plane and your felt-tipmarker again and seat yourself in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r corner <strong>of</strong> aroom. Hold up <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane and look at <strong>the</strong> angle formedwhere <strong>the</strong> ceiling meets <strong>the</strong> two walls. Be sure to keep <strong>the</strong> PicturePlane vertical in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> your face, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> same plane as <strong>the</strong> plane<strong>of</strong> your two eyes. D<strong>on</strong>'t tilt <strong>the</strong> plane in any directi<strong>on</strong>.Again, compose your view, and use your marker <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> PicturePlane to draw <strong>the</strong> corner (a vertical line). Then, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane,draw <strong>the</strong> edges where <strong>the</strong> ceiling meets <strong>the</strong> two walls, and, if possible,<strong>the</strong> edges where <strong>the</strong> floor meets <strong>the</strong> walls.Then, put your Picture Plane down <strong>on</strong> a piece <strong>of</strong> paper soyou can see <strong>the</strong> drawing and transfer those lines to a piece <strong>of</strong>drawing paper.You have just drawn a corner in perspective. Now, let's do thatwithout <strong>the</strong> aid <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane.Move to a different corner or a different positi<strong>on</strong>. Tape apiece <strong>of</strong> paper to your drawing board. Now, take a sight <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>vertical corner. Close <strong>on</strong>e eye and hold your pencil perfectly verticallyat <strong>the</strong> corner. Having checked, you can now draw a verticalline for <strong>the</strong> corner.Next, hold up your pencil perfectly horiz<strong>on</strong>tally, staying <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> plane, to see what <strong>the</strong> angles <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ceiling are relative to horiz<strong>on</strong>tal(Figure 8-21). You will see <strong>the</strong>m as angles between <strong>the</strong>pencil and <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ceiling. Remember <strong>the</strong>se angles asFig. 8-21.Fig. 8-22.Verticals in human-built structuresremain vertical. Horiz<strong>on</strong>tals—thatis, edges parallel to <strong>the</strong> face <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>earth—appear to change and c<strong>on</strong>vergeand must be sighted. But youcan pretty much count <strong>on</strong> verticalsremaining vertical. In your drawing,<strong>the</strong>y will be parallel to <strong>the</strong>edges <strong>of</strong> your paper. There areexcepti<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>of</strong> course. If you standat street level, looking up, to drawa tall building, those vertical edgeswill c<strong>on</strong>verge and must be sighted.This situati<strong>on</strong>, however, is fairlyrare in drawing.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE 149


shapes. Then, again estimating, draw <strong>the</strong> angles into your drawing.Use <strong>the</strong> same procedure for <strong>the</strong> floor angles (Figure 8-22).These fundamental sighting movements or measuring gesturesin drawing are not difficult to master, <strong>on</strong>ce you have a realunderstanding <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> movements.• The purpose <strong>of</strong> closing <strong>on</strong>e eye, as I explained earlier, is tosee a 2-D image <strong>on</strong>ly, not a 3-D binocular image.• The purpose <strong>of</strong> locking <strong>the</strong> elbow is to ensure using a singlescale in sighting proporti<strong>on</strong>s. Relaxing <strong>the</strong> elbow evenslightly can cause errors by changing <strong>the</strong> scale <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sights.In sighting angles, it is not necessary to take <strong>the</strong> sights at arm'slength, but you must stay <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane.• The purpose <strong>of</strong> comparing angles to vertical or horiz<strong>on</strong>tal isobvious. Angles can vary infinitely around 360 degrees. Onlytrue vertical and true horiz<strong>on</strong>tal are c<strong>on</strong>stant and reliable.And since <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper (and <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> formatyou have drawn) also represent vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal, anyangle can be assessed <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane and transferred into <strong>the</strong>drawing in relati<strong>on</strong> to those c<strong>on</strong>stants.Some important points about sighting anglesFig. 8-23. Later, when you havelearned to sight and have discarded<strong>the</strong> actual Picture Plane, you muststill remember to sight <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> planeand be careful to not "pokethrough" <strong>the</strong> imaginary plane.• All angles are sighted relative to <strong>the</strong> two c<strong>on</strong>stants: verticaland horiz<strong>on</strong>tal.• In your drawing, <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> your format represent <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stantsvertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal. Once you have determined anangle "out <strong>the</strong>re" in <strong>the</strong> real world, you will draw it into <strong>the</strong>drawing relative to <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> your format.• All angles are sighted <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture-plane. This is a solidplane. You cannot "poke through" it to align your pencil withan edge as it moves through space. You determine <strong>the</strong> angle asit appears <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane (Figure 8-23).• You can sight angles by holding your pencil ei<strong>the</strong>r verticallyor horiz<strong>on</strong>tally and comparing <strong>the</strong> angle with <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>pencil. You can also use <strong>the</strong> crosshairs <strong>on</strong> your clear plasticPicture Plane or even <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Viewfinders. Youjust need some edge that you can hold up in a vertical or hori-150 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


z<strong>on</strong>tal positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane to compare <strong>the</strong> angle you intendto draw. The pencil is simply <strong>the</strong> easiest to use and doesn'tinterrupt your drawing.• Visual informati<strong>on</strong> seen <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane is nearly always differentfrom what you know about things. Say you are facing acorner <strong>of</strong> a room. You know that <strong>the</strong> ceiling is flat—that is,horiz<strong>on</strong>tal—and that it meets <strong>the</strong> wall at right angles. But ifyou hold up your pencil perfectly horiz<strong>on</strong>tally, close <strong>on</strong>e eye,and, staying <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane, line up <strong>the</strong> corner so that it touches<strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong> your horiz<strong>on</strong>tal pencil, you will find that <strong>the</strong>edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ceiling go <strong>of</strong>f at odd angles. Perhaps <strong>on</strong>e angle issteeper than <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r. See Figure 8-22, page 149.• You must draw <strong>the</strong>se angles just as you see <strong>the</strong>m. Only <strong>the</strong>nwill <strong>the</strong> ceiling look flat and <strong>the</strong> right angles <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> wallsappear to be correct in your finished drawing. This is <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> great paradoxes <strong>of</strong> drawing.• You must put <strong>the</strong>se paradoxical angles into your drawing justas you perceive <strong>the</strong>m. To do this, you remember <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong><strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> triangles made by <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ceiling and yourhoriz<strong>on</strong>tal pencil. Then, imagining a horiz<strong>on</strong>tal line in yourdrawing (parallel to <strong>the</strong> top and bottom edges <strong>of</strong> your format),draw <strong>the</strong> same triangle. Use <strong>the</strong> same process to draw<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r angled edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ceiling. See Figure 8-21, page 149,for an illustrati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> this.I realize that sighting sounds very"left-brained" at this point. Butremember we are searching outrelati<strong>on</strong>ships. The right hemisphereis specialized for <strong>the</strong>percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships—howthingscompare. As I said before,<strong>the</strong> "counting up" <strong>of</strong> sighting is justa simple way <strong>of</strong> "tagging" our percepti<strong>on</strong>s.The Basic Unit is always"One," because it is <strong>the</strong> first part <strong>of</strong>a comparis<strong>on</strong>. After you practicesighting a bit, you are hardly aware<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> process and it is very rapid.Also, with practice in drawing, youwill be doing a lot <strong>of</strong>"eyeballing," meaning estimatingra<strong>the</strong>r than needing to sighteverything. But for any difficultpercepti<strong>on</strong>, as in foreshortening,an experienced artist gladlyuses sighting. Like negative space,sighting helps to make drawingeasy.I usually recommend that students not try to designate an angleby degrees: a 45-degree angle; a 30-degree angle; etc. It really isbest to simply remember <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>the</strong> angle makes when comparedto vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal and carry that visual shape inyour mind to draw it. You may have to double-check angles a fewtimes at first, but my students learn this skill very quickly.The decisi<strong>on</strong> whe<strong>the</strong>r to use vertical or horiz<strong>on</strong>tal as <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stantagainst which to see a particular angle occasi<strong>on</strong>ally puzzlesstudents. I recommend that you choose whichever will produce<strong>the</strong> smaller angle.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE 151


Try to remember that drawingalways produces an approximateversi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subject, even for apers<strong>on</strong> highly skilled in drawing.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is not photography. Thepers<strong>on</strong> who is drawing c<strong>on</strong>sciouslyor subc<strong>on</strong>sciously edits, emphasizes(or minimizes), or o<strong>the</strong>rwiseslightly changes various aspects <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> subject. Students are <strong>of</strong>ten verycritical <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir work because it isnot an exact renditi<strong>on</strong>, but <strong>the</strong> subc<strong>on</strong>sciouschoices made duringdrawing are part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> expressiveness<strong>of</strong> drawings.Please note that in public placesyou will attract an audience <strong>of</strong>viewers who will very likely want totalk with you—not a good situati<strong>on</strong>for maintaining an R-mode, wordlessstate <strong>of</strong> mind. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rhand, if you would like to makesome new friends, drawing in apublic spot will work every time.For some reas<strong>on</strong>, people who ordinarilywould not approach astranger do not hesitate to talkwith some<strong>on</strong>e who is drawing.A "real" perspective drawingWhat you'll need:• Your drawing board• Several sheets <strong>of</strong> drawing paper, in a stack for padding• Your masking tape• Your drawing pencils, sharpened, and your eraser• Your graphite stick and several paper towels or paper napkins• Your plastic Picture Plane and your felt-tip marker• Your larger ViewfinderBefore you start:Tape a stack <strong>of</strong> several sheets <strong>of</strong> drawing paper to your drawingboard. Draw a format <strong>on</strong> your drawing paper and t<strong>on</strong>e <strong>the</strong> paperwithin <strong>the</strong> format to a medium gray t<strong>on</strong>e. Draw <strong>the</strong> crosshairs <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> t<strong>on</strong>ed paper.1. Choose your subject. Learning how to draw "in proporti<strong>on</strong>"and "in perspective" are <strong>the</strong> two great challenges—<strong>the</strong>Waterloo, even—<strong>of</strong> most drawing students in art schools. Youwill want to prove to yourself that you can achieve this skill.Therefore, pick your subject with that objective in mind:Choose a view or a site that you think would be really hard todraw—<strong>on</strong>e with lots <strong>of</strong> angles or a complicated ceiling or al<strong>on</strong>g view down a hall. See <strong>the</strong> student drawing <strong>on</strong> page 153.The best way to choose a site is to walk around, using yourViewfinder to find a compositi<strong>on</strong> that pleases you—much in<strong>the</strong> same way as composing with a camera's viewfinder.Possible sites:• A kitchen corner• A hallway• A view through an open doorway• A corner <strong>of</strong> any room in your house• A porch or balc<strong>on</strong>y• Any street corner where you can sit in your car or <strong>on</strong> a benchand draw• An entrance to any public building, inside or out152THE NKW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Set yourself up to draw at your chosen site. You will need twochairs, <strong>on</strong>e for sitting <strong>on</strong> and <strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong> which to lean your drawingboard. If you are drawing outside, folding chairs are c<strong>on</strong>venient.Make sure that you are directly facing your chosenview.2. Clip your larger Viewfinder and <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Planetoge<strong>the</strong>r. Draw a format edge <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic plane by running<strong>the</strong> felt-tip marker around <strong>the</strong> inside edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Viewfinderopening. Closing <strong>on</strong>e eye, move <strong>the</strong> Viewfinder/plastic PicturePlane backward and forward to find <strong>the</strong> best compositi<strong>on</strong>—<strong>the</strong><strong>on</strong>e you like best.3. Having found a compositi<strong>on</strong> you like, choose your Basic Unit.Your Basic Unit should be <strong>of</strong> medium size and <strong>of</strong> a shape thatis not too complicated. It might be a window or a picture <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> wall or a doorway. It can be a positive form or a negativespace. It can be a single line or a shape. Draw your Basic Unitdirectly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic with your felt-tip marker.A student's drawing <strong>of</strong> an interestingand challenging view.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE153


After you have drawn your BasicUnit <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane,you may also wish to draw <strong>on</strong>e ortwo <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> more important edges<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Plane, but beaware that <strong>the</strong> line will be veryshaky and uncertain. The essentialpiece <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> is your BasicUnit, and that is really all youneed.A perspective drawing by CindyBall-Kingst<strong>on</strong>. You will find interestingcompositi<strong>on</strong>s in unexpectedplaces.4. Set aside your Viewfinder/plastic Picture Plane <strong>on</strong> a piece <strong>of</strong>white paper so that you can see what you have drawn <strong>on</strong> it.You will next draw your Basic Unit <strong>on</strong> your paper. It will be<strong>the</strong> same shape but larger, just as your t<strong>on</strong>ed format is largerthan <strong>the</strong> Viewfinder opening.5. Transfer your Basic Unit <strong>on</strong>to <strong>the</strong> t<strong>on</strong>ed paper using yourcrosshairs as a guide. On both <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane and <strong>on</strong> yourt<strong>on</strong>ed paper, <strong>the</strong> crosshairs divide <strong>the</strong> drawing area into fourquadrants. Refer to Figures 8-11 and 8-12 <strong>on</strong> page 146 for howto transfer your Basic Unit from your Picture Plane to yourt<strong>on</strong>ed paper by using <strong>the</strong>se quadrants.How to re-find your compositi<strong>on</strong>: Sometimes it is useful to goback to <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane to check <strong>on</strong> an angle or proporti<strong>on</strong>. Tore-find your compositi<strong>on</strong>, simply hold up your Viewfinder/plasticPicture Plane, close <strong>on</strong>e eye and move <strong>the</strong> plane forward orbackward until your Basic Unit "out <strong>the</strong>re" lines up with <strong>the</strong> felttipdrawing <strong>of</strong> Basic Unit <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic plane. Then check outany angle or proporti<strong>on</strong> that may be puzzling you.For most people just learning to draw, <strong>the</strong> hardest part <strong>of</strong>drawing is believing <strong>the</strong>ir own sights <strong>of</strong> both angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>s.Many times I have watched students take a sight, shake<strong>the</strong>ir heads, take <strong>the</strong> sight again, again shake <strong>the</strong>ir heads, even sayout loud, "It [an angle] can't be that steep," or, "It [a proporti<strong>on</strong>]can't be that small."With a little more experience in drawing, students are able toaccept <strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>y obtain by sighting. You just have toswallow it whole, so to speak, and make a decisi<strong>on</strong> not to sec<strong>on</strong>dguessyour sights. I say to my students, "If you see it so, you drawit so. D<strong>on</strong>'t argue with yourself about it."Of course, <strong>the</strong> sights have to be taken as correctly and carefullyas possible. When I dem<strong>on</strong>strate drawing in a workshop, studentssee me making a very careful, deliberate movement toextend my arm, lock my elbow, and close <strong>on</strong>e eye in order tocarefully check a proporti<strong>on</strong> or an angle <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane. But <strong>the</strong>semovements become quite automatic very quickly, just as <strong>on</strong>equickly learns to brake a car to a smooth stop.154 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


To complete your perspective drawing:1. Again, you will fit <strong>the</strong> pieces <strong>of</strong> your drawing toge<strong>the</strong>r like afascinating puzzle. Work from part to adjacent part, alwayschecking <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> each new part to <strong>the</strong> partsalready drawn. Also, remember <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> edges asshared edges, with <strong>the</strong> positive forms and negative spacesfitted into <strong>the</strong> format to create a compositi<strong>on</strong>. Remember thatall <strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> you need for this drawing is right <strong>the</strong>rebefore your eyes. You now know <strong>the</strong> strategies artists use to"unlock" that visual informati<strong>on</strong> and you have <strong>the</strong> correctdevices to help you.2. Be sure to use negative spaces as an important part <strong>of</strong> yourdrawing as in Figure 8-24. You will add strength to yourdrawing if you use negative space to see and draw small itemssuch as lamps, tables, signs with lettering, and so <strong>on</strong>. If you d<strong>on</strong>ot, and focus <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> positive shapes, <strong>the</strong>y will tend toweaken your drawing. If you are drawing a landscape, treesand foliage in particular are much str<strong>on</strong>ger when <strong>the</strong>ir negativespaces are emphasized.3. Once you have completed <strong>the</strong> main parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing, youcan focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> lights and shadows. "Squinting" your eyes abit will blur <strong>the</strong> details and allow you to see large shapes <strong>of</strong>lighted areas and shadowed areas. Again using your newsighting skills, you can erase out <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> lights and useyour pencil to darken in <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> shadows. These shapesare sighted in exactly <strong>the</strong> same way as you have sighted <strong>the</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing: "What is <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> that shadowrelative to horiz<strong>on</strong>tal? How wide is that streak <strong>of</strong> light relativeto <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> window?"4. If any part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing seems "<strong>of</strong>f" or "out <strong>of</strong> drawing," assuch errors are called, check out <strong>the</strong> troublesome area withyour clear plastic Picture Plane. Look at <strong>the</strong> image <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>plane (with <strong>on</strong>e eye closed, <strong>of</strong> course) and alternately glancedown at your drawing to double-check angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>s.Make any correcti<strong>on</strong>s that seem reas<strong>on</strong>ably easy tomake.Fig. 8-24. Remember to emphasizenegative spaces in your drawing.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NKW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE 155


Artist/teacher Robert Henri sendsa stern warning to his students:"If in your drawing you habituallydisregard proporti<strong>on</strong>s you becomeaccustomed to <strong>the</strong> sight <strong>of</strong> distorti<strong>on</strong>and lose critical ability. A pers<strong>on</strong>living in squalor eventuallygets used to it."— The Art Spirit, 1923.After you have finished:C<strong>on</strong>gratulati<strong>on</strong>s! You have just accomplished a task that manyuniversity art students would find daunting if not impossible.Sighting is an aptly named skill. You take a sight and you seethings as <strong>the</strong>y really appear <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> picture-plane. This skill willenable you to draw anything you can see with your own eyes. Youneed not search for "easy" subjects. You will be able to draw anythingat all.The skill <strong>of</strong> sighting takes some practice to master, but veryso<strong>on</strong> you will find yourself "just drawing," taking sights automatically,at times even without needing to measure proporti<strong>on</strong>s orassess angles. I think it's significant that this is called "eyeballing."Also, when you come to <strong>the</strong> difficult foreshortened parts, you willhave just <strong>the</strong> skills needed to make <strong>the</strong> drawing seem easy.Fig. 8-25. Charles White, Preacher(1952). Courtesy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> WhitneyMuseum.This drawing by Charles Whitedem<strong>on</strong>strates a foreshortenedview. Study it. Copy it, turning <strong>the</strong>drawing upside down if necessary.You might use <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>man's left hand from <strong>the</strong> wrist to<strong>the</strong> tip <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pointing finger asyour Basic Unit. Perhaps you'll besurprised to find that <strong>the</strong> ratio <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> head to <strong>the</strong> model's left hand is1:1+2/3.Each time you experience <strong>the</strong> factthat drawing just what you seeworks <strong>the</strong> w<strong>on</strong>der <strong>of</strong> creating <strong>the</strong>illusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> space and volume <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>flat surface <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper, <strong>the</strong> methodswill become more securelyintegrated as your way <strong>of</strong> seeing—<strong>the</strong> artist's way <strong>of</strong> seeing.156 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 8-26. Edgar Degas (1834-1917),Dancer Adjusting Her Slipper (1873).Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The MetropolitanMuseum <strong>of</strong> Art, bequest <strong>of</strong> Mrs.H. O. Havemeyer, 1929. The H. O.Havemeyer Collecti<strong>on</strong>The Use <strong>of</strong> Sighting in Figure<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>This technique <strong>of</strong> using <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stants,vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal,against which to gauge angles is animportant basic skill in drawingfigures as well as objects. Manyartists' sketches still show traces<strong>of</strong> sight lines drawn in by <strong>the</strong> artist,as in <strong>the</strong> Edgar Degas drawingentitled Dancer Adjusting Her Slipper(Figure 8-26). Degas was probablysighting such points as <strong>the</strong>locati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> left toe in relati<strong>on</strong>to <strong>the</strong> ear and <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> armcompared to vertical.Note that Degas's Basic Unit wasfrom <strong>the</strong> topmost edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hairto <strong>the</strong> neckband. The artist used<strong>the</strong> same Basic Unit in Figure 11-6,shown in <strong>the</strong> chapter <strong>on</strong> color.The visible world is replete with foreshortened views <strong>of</strong> people,streets, trees, and flowers. Beginning students sometimesavoid <strong>the</strong>se "difficult" views and search instead for "easy" views.With <strong>the</strong> skills you now have, this limiting <strong>of</strong> subject matter foryour drawing is unnecessary. Edges, negative spaces, and sightings<strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships work toge<strong>the</strong>r to make drawing foreshortenedforms not just possible—<strong>the</strong>y become downright enjoyable.As in learning any skill, learning <strong>the</strong> "hard parts" is challengingand exhilarating.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE 157


Looking aheadThe technique I have just taught you, "informal perspective,"relies <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> sights taken <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane. Most artists use informalperspective, even though <strong>the</strong>y may have complete knowledge <strong>of</strong>formal perspective. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> advantages <strong>of</strong> learning informalsighting is that it can be used for any subject matter, as you willsee in <strong>the</strong> next exercise. You will be drawing a pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait,putting to use your skills <strong>of</strong> perceiving edges, spaces, and proporti<strong>on</strong>alrelati<strong>on</strong>ships in drawing <strong>the</strong> human head.Remember that realistic drawings <strong>of</strong> perceived subjectsalways require <strong>the</strong> same basic perceptual skills—<strong>the</strong> skills you arelearning right now. Of course, this is true <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r R-mode globalskills. For example, <strong>on</strong>ce you have learned to drive, you can verylikely drive any make <strong>of</strong> automobile.In your next drawing, you will enjoy drawing <strong>the</strong> humanhead, a most intriguing and challenging subject.Randa Cardwell158THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Instructor Dana Crowe.RELATIONSHIPS IN A NEW MODE: PUTTING SIGHTING IN PERSPECTIVE 159


Facing Forward:Portrait <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>with Ease


HUMAN FACES HAVE ALWAYS FASCINATED ARTISTS. To Catcha likeness, to show <strong>the</strong> exterior in such a way that <strong>the</strong> innerpers<strong>on</strong> can be seen, is a challenging, inviting prospect. Moreover,a portrait can reveal not <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> appearance and pers<strong>on</strong>ality (<strong>the</strong>gestalt) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sitter but also <strong>the</strong> soul <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist. Paradoxically,<strong>the</strong> more clearly <strong>the</strong> artist sees <strong>the</strong> sitter, <strong>the</strong> more clearly <strong>the</strong>viewer can see through <strong>the</strong> likeness to perceive <strong>the</strong> artist. Theserevelati<strong>on</strong>s bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> likeness are not intenti<strong>on</strong>al. They are simply<strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> close, sustained R-mode observati<strong>on</strong>. .Therefore, because we are searching for you through <strong>the</strong>images you draw, you will be drawing human faces in <strong>the</strong> next set<strong>of</strong> exercises. The more clearly you see, <strong>the</strong> better you will draw,and <strong>the</strong> more you will express yourself to yourself and to o<strong>the</strong>rs.Since portrait drawing requires very fine percepti<strong>on</strong>s in orderto produce a likeness, faces are effective for training beginners inseeing and drawing. The feedback <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> correctness <strong>of</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>is immediate and certain, because we all know when a drawing<strong>of</strong> a human head is correct in its general proporti<strong>on</strong>s. And ifwe know <strong>the</strong> sitter, we can make even more precise judgmentsabout <strong>the</strong> accuracy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>s.But perhaps more important for our purposes, drawing <strong>the</strong>human head has a special advantage for us in our quest for ways togain c<strong>on</strong>scious access to our right-hemisphere functi<strong>on</strong>s. Theright hemisphere <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> human brain is specialized for <strong>the</strong> recogniti<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> faces. People with right-hemisphere injury caused by astroke or accident <strong>of</strong>ten have difficulty recognizing <strong>the</strong>ir friendsor even recognizing <strong>the</strong>ir own faces in <strong>the</strong> mirror. Left-hemisphere-injuredpatients usually do not experience this deficit.Beginners <strong>of</strong>ten think that drawing people is <strong>the</strong> hardest <strong>of</strong> allkinds <strong>of</strong> drawing. It isn't, actually. As with any o<strong>the</strong>r subject matter,<strong>the</strong> visual informati<strong>on</strong> is right <strong>the</strong>re, ready and available.Again, <strong>the</strong> problem is seeing. To restate a major premise <strong>of</strong> thisbook, drawing is always <strong>the</strong> same task—that is, every drawingrequires <strong>the</strong> basic perceptual skills you are learning. Aside fromcomplexity, <strong>on</strong>e subject is not harder or easier than ano<strong>the</strong>r.However, certain subjects <strong>of</strong>ten seem harder than o<strong>the</strong>rs, probablybecause embedded symbol systems, which interfere with162 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


clear percepti<strong>on</strong>s, are str<strong>on</strong>ger for some subjects than for o<strong>the</strong>rs.Most people have a very str<strong>on</strong>g, persistent symbol system fordrawing <strong>the</strong> human head. For example, a comm<strong>on</strong> symbol for aneye is made <strong>of</strong> two curved lines enclosing a small circle (<strong>the</strong> iris).Your own unique set <strong>of</strong> symbols, as we discussed in Chapter Five,was developed and memorized during childhood and is remarkablystable and resistant to change. These symbols actually seemto override seeing, and <strong>the</strong>refore few people can draw a realistichuman head. Even fewer can draw recognizable portraits.Summing up, <strong>the</strong>n, portrait drawing is useful to our goals for<strong>the</strong>se reas<strong>on</strong>s: First, it is a suitable subject for accessing <strong>the</strong> righ<strong>the</strong>misphere, which is specialized for recogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> human facesand for making <strong>the</strong> fine visual discriminati<strong>on</strong>s necessary toachieve a likeness. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, drawing faces will help you tostreng<strong>the</strong>n your ability to perceive proporti<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships,since proporti<strong>on</strong> is integral to portraiture. Third, drawing faces isexcellent practice in bypassing embedded symbol systems. Andfourth, <strong>the</strong> ability to draw portraits with credible likenesses is ac<strong>on</strong>vincing dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> to your ever-critical left hemispherethat you have—dare we say it?—talent for drawing. And you'llfind that drawing portraits is not difficult <strong>on</strong>ce you can shift to <strong>the</strong>artist's way <strong>of</strong> seeing.In drawing your pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait, you will be using all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>skills you have learned so far:• How to perceive and draw edges• How to perceive and draw spaces• How to perceive and draw relati<strong>on</strong>ships• How to perceive and draw (a bit <strong>of</strong>) lights and shadows(I will present more in-depth instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> lights and shadowsin Chapter Ten.)• And in additi<strong>on</strong>, you will acquire a new skill, how to perceiveand draw <strong>the</strong> gestalt <strong>of</strong> your model—<strong>the</strong> character and pers<strong>on</strong>alitybehind <strong>the</strong> drawn image—by focussing intently <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> first four skills.Our main strategy for accessing R-mode remains <strong>the</strong> same: topresent <strong>the</strong> brain with a task that L-mode will turn down.A reminder: The global skill <strong>of</strong>drawing has five comp<strong>on</strong>ent skills.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE163


Fig. 9-1. The four figures are <strong>the</strong>The importance <strong>of</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> in portrait drawingAll drawing involves proporti<strong>on</strong>, whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> subject is still life,landscape, figure drawing, or portrait drawing. Proporti<strong>on</strong> isimportant whe<strong>the</strong>r an artwork's style is realistic, abstract, or completelyn<strong>on</strong>objective (that is, without recognizable forms from <strong>the</strong>external world). Realistic drawing in particular depends heavily<strong>on</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>al correctness. Therefore, realistic drawing is especiallyeffective in training <strong>the</strong> eye to see <strong>the</strong> thing-as-it-is in itsrelati<strong>on</strong>al proporti<strong>on</strong>s. Individuals whose jobs require close estimati<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong> size relati<strong>on</strong>ships—carpenters, dentists, dressmakers,carpet-layers, and surge<strong>on</strong>s—develop great facility in perceivingproporti<strong>on</strong>. Creative thinkers in all fields benefit from enhancedawareness <strong>of</strong> part-to-whole relati<strong>on</strong>ships—from seeing both <strong>the</strong>trees and <strong>the</strong> forest.On believing what you think you seeFig. 9-2. Mark <strong>the</strong> size <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e figure<strong>on</strong> a piece <strong>of</strong> paper.Fig. 9-3. Cut out a notch <strong>the</strong> size <strong>of</strong><strong>on</strong>e figure and measure each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>figures by fitting it into <strong>the</strong> cut-outnotch.One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> problems <strong>of</strong> seeing comes from <strong>the</strong> brain's ability tochange visual informati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> fitting incominginformati<strong>on</strong> to pre-existing c<strong>on</strong>cepts or beliefs. The parts that areimportant (that is, provide key informati<strong>on</strong>), or <strong>the</strong> parts that wedecide are larger, or <strong>the</strong> parts that we think should be larger, wesee as larger than <strong>the</strong>y actually are. C<strong>on</strong>versely, parts that areunimportant, or that we decide are smaller, or that we thinkshould be smaller, we see as being smaller than <strong>the</strong>y actually are.Let me give you a couple <strong>of</strong> examples <strong>of</strong> this perceptual phenomen<strong>on</strong>.Figure 9-1 shows a diagrammatic landscape with fourmen. The man at <strong>the</strong> far right appears to be <strong>the</strong> largest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> four.But all four figures are exactly <strong>the</strong> same size. Lay a pencil al<strong>on</strong>gsidefirst <strong>the</strong> left-hand man and <strong>the</strong> right-hand man to measureand test <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> that statement. Even after measuring andproving to yourself that <strong>the</strong> figures are <strong>the</strong> same size, however,you will probably find that <strong>the</strong> man <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> right will still looklarger (Figure 9-2,9-3).The reas<strong>on</strong> for this mispercepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>ate size probablyderives from our past knowledge and experience <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> effect164 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


<strong>of</strong> distance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> apparent size <strong>of</strong> forms: Given two objects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>same size, <strong>on</strong>e nearby and <strong>on</strong>e at a distance away, <strong>the</strong> distantobject will appear to be smaller. If <strong>the</strong>y look <strong>the</strong> same size, <strong>the</strong> farobject must be a great deal larger than <strong>the</strong> near object. This makessense, and we d<strong>on</strong>'t quarrel with <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept. But coming back to<strong>the</strong> drawing, apparently <strong>the</strong> brain enlarges <strong>the</strong> far object to make<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept truer than true. This is overdoing it! And this is precisely<strong>the</strong> kind <strong>of</strong> overdoing—<strong>of</strong> overlaying memorized verbalc<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>on</strong>to visual percepti<strong>on</strong>s—that causes problems withproporti<strong>on</strong> for beginning drawing students.Even after we have measured <strong>the</strong> men in <strong>the</strong> drawing andhave determined with irrefutable evidence that <strong>the</strong>y are <strong>the</strong> samesize, we still wr<strong>on</strong>gly see <strong>the</strong> right-hand man as being larger than<strong>the</strong> left. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, if you turn this book upside down andview <strong>the</strong> drawing in <strong>the</strong> inverted orientati<strong>on</strong> that <strong>the</strong> verbal, c<strong>on</strong>ceptualmode apparently rejects, you will find that you can moreeasily see that <strong>the</strong> two men are <strong>the</strong> same size. The same visualinformati<strong>on</strong> triggers a different resp<strong>on</strong>se. The brain, apparentlynow less influenced by <strong>the</strong> verbal c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> diminishing size indistant forms, allows us to see <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> correctly.For an even more striking example <strong>of</strong> perceptual illusi<strong>on</strong>,look at <strong>the</strong> drawing <strong>of</strong> two tables, Figure 9-4. Will you believe methat <strong>the</strong> two tabletops are exactly <strong>the</strong> same shape and size? Youmay have to use your plastic Picture Plane and trace <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>tabletops, <strong>the</strong>n slide <strong>the</strong> Plane over <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r tabletop to believethis. This w<strong>on</strong>derfully original illusi<strong>on</strong> drawing is by Roger N.Shepard, a renowned psychologist <strong>of</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> and cogniti<strong>on</strong>.On not believing what you seeOne more example: Stand in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> a mirror at about arm'sdistance away. How large would you say is <strong>the</strong> image <strong>of</strong> your headin <strong>the</strong> mirror? About <strong>the</strong> same size as your head? Using a felt-tippen or a cray<strong>on</strong>, extend your arm and make two marks <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>mirror—<strong>on</strong>e at <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> reflected image (<strong>the</strong> outside c<strong>on</strong>tour<strong>of</strong> your head) and <strong>on</strong>e at <strong>the</strong> bottom c<strong>on</strong>tour <strong>of</strong> your chin(Figure 9-5). Step to <strong>on</strong>e side to see how l<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> image is inFig. 9-4. From Mind Sights byRoger N. Shepard, 1990. Reproducedby permissi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> author.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASF 165


Fig. 9-5.inches. You'll find it's about four and <strong>on</strong>e half to five inches, or<strong>on</strong>e-half <strong>the</strong> true size <strong>of</strong> your head. Yet, when you remove <strong>the</strong>marks and look again at yourself in <strong>the</strong> mirror, it seems that <strong>the</strong>image must be life-size! Again, you are seeing what you believe,not believing what you see.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> closer to realityOnce we have accepted that <strong>the</strong> brain is changing informati<strong>on</strong>and not telling us that it has d<strong>on</strong>e so, some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> problems <strong>of</strong>drawing become clearer, and learning to see what is actually "out<strong>the</strong>re" in <strong>the</strong> real world becomes very interesting. Note that thisperceptual phenomen<strong>on</strong> is probably essential to ordinary life. Itreduces <strong>the</strong> complexity <strong>of</strong> incoming data and enables us to havestable c<strong>on</strong>cepts. The problems start when we try to see what is166 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


eally "out <strong>the</strong>re," for purposes <strong>of</strong> checking reality, solving realproblems, or drawing realistically. To accomplish that, we shalltry to prove in a logical way that certain proporti<strong>on</strong>s are what<strong>the</strong>y are.The mystery <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chopped-<strong>of</strong>f skullMost people find it quite difficult to perceive <strong>the</strong> relative proporti<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> features and <strong>the</strong> skull.In this introducti<strong>on</strong> to pr<strong>of</strong>ile-portrait drawing, I'll c<strong>on</strong>centrate<strong>on</strong> two critical relati<strong>on</strong>ships that are persistently difficult forbeginning drawing students to correctly perceive: <strong>the</strong> locati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>eye level in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole head; and <strong>the</strong> locati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear in <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile view. I believe <strong>the</strong>se are two examples<strong>of</strong> perceptual errors caused by <strong>the</strong> brain's propensity tochange visual informati<strong>on</strong> to better fit its c<strong>on</strong>cepts.Let me explain. To most people, <strong>the</strong> eye level line (an imaginaryhoriz<strong>on</strong>tal line that passes through <strong>the</strong> inside corners <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>eyes) appears to be about <strong>on</strong>e-third <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> way down from <strong>the</strong> top<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head. The actual measure is <strong>on</strong>e-half. I think this mispercepti<strong>on</strong>occurs because we tend to see that <strong>the</strong> important visualinformati<strong>on</strong> is in <strong>the</strong> features, not in foreheads and hair areas.Apparently, <strong>the</strong> top half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head seems less compelling than<strong>the</strong> features, and <strong>the</strong>refore is perceived as smaller. This error inpercepti<strong>on</strong> results in what I've called <strong>the</strong> "chopped-<strong>of</strong>f-skullerror," my term for <strong>the</strong> most comm<strong>on</strong> perceptual error made bybeginning drawing students (Figures 9-6, 9-7).I stumbled <strong>on</strong> this problem <strong>on</strong>e day while teaching a group <strong>of</strong>beginning drawing students at <strong>the</strong> university. They were working<strong>on</strong> portrait drawings and <strong>on</strong>e after ano<strong>the</strong>r had "chopped <strong>of</strong>f" <strong>the</strong>skull <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model. I went through my "Can't you see that <strong>the</strong> eyelevel line is halfway between <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chin and <strong>the</strong> topedge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hair?" queries. The students said, "No. We can't seethat." I asked <strong>the</strong>m to measure <strong>the</strong> model's head, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong>ir ownheads, and <strong>the</strong>n each o<strong>the</strong>rs' heads. "Was <strong>the</strong> measure <strong>on</strong>e to<strong>on</strong>e?" I asked. "Yes," <strong>the</strong>y said. "Well," I said, "now you can see <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> model's head that <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ship is <strong>on</strong>e to <strong>on</strong>e,Fig. 9-6. A student drawing illustrating<strong>the</strong> chopped-<strong>of</strong>f-skullFig. 9-7. The same facial featurestraced from <strong>the</strong> student's drawingwith two correcti<strong>on</strong>s: <strong>the</strong> size <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> skull and placement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> right-hand side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 167


isn't that true?" "No," <strong>the</strong>y said, "we still can't see it." One studenteven said, "We'll see it when we can believe it."This went <strong>on</strong> for a while until finally <strong>the</strong> light dawned and Isaid, "Are you telling me that you really can't see that relati<strong>on</strong>ship?""Yes," <strong>the</strong>y said, "we really can't see it." At that point I realizedthat brain processes were actually preventing accuratepercepti<strong>on</strong> and causing <strong>the</strong> "chopped-<strong>of</strong>f-skull" error. Once weall agreed <strong>on</strong> this phenomen<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> students were able to accept<strong>the</strong>ir sightings <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>, and so<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> problem wassolved.Now we must put your own brain into a logical box (by showingit irrefutable evidence) that will help it accept your sightings<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head.Fig. 9-8. Central axis.Fig 9-9<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> a blank to see better than ever1. Draw a "blank," an oval shape used by artists to represent <strong>the</strong>human skull in diagrams. The shape is shown in Figure 9-8.Draw a vertical line through <strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> blank, dividing<strong>the</strong> shape in half. This is called <strong>the</strong> central axis.2. Next, you will locate <strong>the</strong> horiz<strong>on</strong>tal "eye level line," whichcrosses <strong>the</strong> central axis at a right angle. Use your pencil tomeasure <strong>on</strong> your own head <strong>the</strong> distance from <strong>the</strong> inside corner<strong>of</strong> your eye to <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong> your chin. Do this by placing<strong>the</strong> eraser end (to protect your eye) at <strong>the</strong> inside corner <strong>of</strong>your eye and marking with your thumb where your chin hits<strong>the</strong> pencil, as in Figure 9-9. Now, holding that measurement,raise <strong>the</strong> pencil, as in Figure 9-10, and compare <strong>the</strong> first distance(eye level to chin) with <strong>the</strong> distance from your eye levelto <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> your head (feel across from <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pencilto <strong>the</strong> topmost part <strong>of</strong> your head). You will find that those twodistances are approximately <strong>the</strong> same.3. Repeat <strong>the</strong> measurement in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> a mirror. Regard <strong>the</strong> reflecti<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> your head. Without measuring, visually compare<strong>the</strong> bottom half with <strong>the</strong> top half <strong>of</strong> your head. Then use yourpencil to repeat <strong>the</strong> measurement <strong>of</strong> eye level <strong>on</strong>e more time.168 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


4. If you have newspapers or magazines handy, check this proporti<strong>on</strong>in photographs <strong>of</strong> people, or use <strong>the</strong> photo <strong>of</strong> Englishwriter George Orwell, Figure 9-11. Use your pencil to measure.You will find that:Eye level to chin equals eye level to <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> skull.This is an almost invariant proporti<strong>on</strong>.5. Check <strong>the</strong> photographs again. In each head, is <strong>the</strong> eye level atabout <strong>the</strong> middle, dividing <strong>the</strong> whole shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head aboutin half? Can you clearly see that proporti<strong>on</strong>? If not, turn <strong>on</strong>televisi<strong>on</strong> to a news program and measure heads right <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>televisi<strong>on</strong> screen by placing your pencil flat <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> screen,measuring first eye level to chin, <strong>the</strong>n eye level to <strong>the</strong> topedge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head. Now, take <strong>the</strong> pencil away and look again.Can you see <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> clearly now?Fig. 9-10.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 169


Fig. 9-12.When you finally believe what you see, you will find that <strong>on</strong>nearly every head you observe, <strong>the</strong> eye level is at about <strong>the</strong>halfway mark. The eye level is almost never less than half—thatis, almost never nearer to <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> skull than to <strong>the</strong> bottom<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chin (See Figure 9-12). And if <strong>the</strong> hair is thick, <strong>the</strong> top half<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head—eye level to <strong>the</strong> topmost edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hair—is biggerthan <strong>the</strong> bottom half.The "chopped-<strong>of</strong>f skull" creates <strong>the</strong> masklike effect so <strong>of</strong>tenseen in children's drawings, abstract or expressi<strong>on</strong>istic art, and inso-called "primitive" or "ethnic" art. This masklike effect <strong>of</strong>enlarging <strong>the</strong> features relative to <strong>the</strong> skull size, <strong>of</strong> course, canhave tremendous expressive power, as seen, for example, in worksby Picasso, Matisse, and Modigliani, and great works <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rcultures. The point is that master artists, especially <strong>of</strong> our owntime and culture, use <strong>the</strong> device by choice and not by mistake.Let me dem<strong>on</strong>strate <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> mispercepti<strong>on</strong>.Many "police artists" are <strong>of</strong>ficersmore or less assigned to puttingtoge<strong>the</strong>r composite drawings <strong>of</strong>suspects from witnesses' descripti<strong>on</strong>s.The resulting drawings <strong>of</strong>tendisplay <strong>the</strong> perceptual error I'vedescribed in this secti<strong>on</strong>.Irrefutable evidence that <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head isimportant after allFirst, I have drawn <strong>the</strong> lower part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> faces <strong>of</strong> two models, <strong>on</strong>ein pr<strong>of</strong>ile and <strong>on</strong>e in three-quarter view (see Figure 9-13). C<strong>on</strong>traryto what <strong>on</strong>e would expect, most students have few seriousproblems in learning to see and draw <strong>the</strong> features. The problem isnot <strong>the</strong> features; it's in perceiving <strong>the</strong> skull that things go wr<strong>on</strong>g.What I want to dem<strong>on</strong>strate is how important it is to provide <strong>the</strong>full skull for <strong>the</strong> features—not to cut <strong>of</strong>f <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> headbecause your brain is less interested and makes you see it assmaller.In Figure 9-13 are two sets <strong>of</strong> three drawings: First, <strong>the</strong> features<strong>on</strong>ly, without <strong>the</strong> rest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> skull; sec<strong>on</strong>d, <strong>the</strong> identical featureswith <strong>the</strong> cut-<strong>of</strong>f-skull error; and third, <strong>the</strong> identical features,this time with <strong>the</strong> full skull, which complements and supports <strong>the</strong>features.You can see that it's not <strong>the</strong> features that cause <strong>the</strong> problem <strong>of</strong>wr<strong>on</strong>g proporti<strong>on</strong>; it's <strong>the</strong> skull. Now turn to Figure 9-14 and seethat Van Gogh in his 1880 drawing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> carpenter apparently170 THE: NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 9-13. The features <strong>on</strong>ly. The cut-<strong>of</strong>f-skull error, using <strong>the</strong> The same features again, this timesame features.with <strong>the</strong> full skull.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 171


Fig. 9-14. Vincent Van Gogh(1853-1890), Carpenter (1880).Courtesy <strong>of</strong> Rijksmuseum Kroller-Miiller, Otterlo.Van Gogh worked as an artist <strong>on</strong>lyduring <strong>the</strong> last ten years <strong>of</strong> his life,from <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 27 until he died at37. During <strong>the</strong> first two years <strong>of</strong>that decade, Van Gogh did drawings<strong>on</strong>ly, teaching himself how todraw. As you can see in <strong>the</strong> drawing<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> carpenter, he struggled withproblems <strong>of</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> and placement<strong>of</strong> forms. By 1882, however—two years later—in his WomanMourning, Van Gogh had overcomehis difficulties with drawing andincreased <strong>the</strong> expressive quality <strong>of</strong>his work.made <strong>the</strong> "chopped-<strong>of</strong>f skull" error in <strong>the</strong> carpenter's head. Also,see <strong>the</strong> Dürer etching in Figure 9-16 in which <strong>the</strong> artist dem<strong>on</strong>strates<strong>the</strong> effort <strong>of</strong> diminishing <strong>the</strong> relative proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>skull to <strong>the</strong> features. Are you c<strong>on</strong>vinced? Is your logical left hemispherec<strong>on</strong>vinced? Good. You will save yourself innumerablehours <strong>of</strong> baffling mistakes in drawing.172 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 9-15. Vincent Van Gogh,Woman Mourning(1882). Courtesy<strong>of</strong> Rijksmuseum Kroller-Müller,Otterlo.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> ano<strong>the</strong>r blank and getting a line <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>pr<strong>of</strong>ileDraw ano<strong>the</strong>r blank now, this time for a pr<strong>of</strong>ile. The pr<strong>of</strong>ile blankis a somewhat different shape—like an oddly shaped egg. This isbecause <strong>the</strong> human skull (Figure 9-17), seen from <strong>the</strong> side, is adifferent shape than <strong>the</strong> skull seen from <strong>the</strong> fr<strong>on</strong>t. It's easier toFACING I'ORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 173


Fig. 9-16. Albrecht Dürer, FourHeads (1513 or 1515). Courtesy<strong>of</strong> The Nels<strong>on</strong> Gallery-AtkinsMuseum. Kansas City, Missouri(Nels<strong>on</strong> Fund).draw <strong>the</strong> blank if you look at <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> negative spacesaround <strong>the</strong> blank in Figure 9-17. Notice that <strong>the</strong> negative spacesare different in each corner.If it helps you to see, draw in some symbolic shapes for nose,eye, mouth, and chin, making sure that you have first drawn <strong>the</strong>eye level line at <strong>the</strong> halfway point <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> blank.Fig. 9-17. The side-view blank.Note that (a) eye level to chinequals (b) eye level to highest part<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> skull.174 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Placing <strong>the</strong> ear in a pr<strong>of</strong>ile portraitThe next measurement is extremely important in helpingyou perceive correctly <strong>the</strong> placement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear, which in turnwill help you perceive correctly <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head in pr<strong>of</strong>ileand prevent chopping <strong>of</strong>f <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> skull.On almost every head, <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear doesn't varymuch. On your own face, use your pencil again to measure <strong>the</strong>length from <strong>the</strong> inside corner <strong>of</strong> your eye to <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong> yourchin (Figure 9-18). Now, holding that measurement, lay <strong>the</strong> pencilhoriz<strong>on</strong>tally al<strong>on</strong>g your eye level line (Figure 9-19) with <strong>the</strong>eraser end at <strong>the</strong> outside corner <strong>of</strong> your eye. That measurementcoincides with <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> your ear. /Putting that ano<strong>the</strong>r way, <strong>the</strong> length from eye level to chinequals <strong>the</strong> distance from <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye to <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear.Make a mark for <strong>the</strong> ear placement <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye level line <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>blank, as in Figure 9-20. This proporti<strong>on</strong> may seem a little complex,but if you will learn <strong>the</strong> measurement, it will save you fromano<strong>the</strong>r stubborn problem in drawing <strong>the</strong> human head: Mostbeginning students draw <strong>the</strong> ear too close to <strong>the</strong> features whendrawing a pr<strong>of</strong>ile. When <strong>the</strong> ear is placed too close to <strong>the</strong> features,<strong>the</strong> skull is <strong>on</strong>ce more chopped <strong>of</strong>f, this time at <strong>the</strong> back. Again,<strong>the</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> problem may be that <strong>the</strong> expanse <strong>of</strong> cheek and ,jaw is uninteresting and boring, and <strong>the</strong>refore beginning studentsfail to perceive <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> space correctly.You can memorize this important measurement as a saying ormnem<strong>on</strong>ic, similar to "i" before "e," except after "c." To place <strong>the</strong>ear in a pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait, memorize this mnem<strong>on</strong>ic: eye level-tochinequals back-<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-eye to <strong>the</strong> back-<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-ear.Note that enlarging <strong>the</strong> features and diminishing <strong>the</strong> skullproduce str<strong>on</strong>g, expressive, symbolic effects, a device you canalways use later if you wish. <strong>Right</strong> now, for this "basic training,"we want you to be able to see things as <strong>the</strong>y really are in <strong>the</strong>ir correctproporti<strong>on</strong>.Visualizing is ano<strong>the</strong>r useful technique for teaching <strong>the</strong> correctplacement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear. Since you now know that two measurementsare equal—from eye level to chin, and from <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 175


Fig. 9-22. Locate also <strong>the</strong> pointwhere <strong>the</strong> neck joins <strong>the</strong> skull (<strong>the</strong>place that bends) relative to <strong>the</strong>upper lip.Correct point in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> facialfeatures.A comm<strong>on</strong> error: misplacement <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> point where <strong>the</strong> neck joins <strong>the</strong>skull.Fig. 9-21. Check <strong>the</strong> locati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>bottom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear relative to <strong>the</strong>upper lip.eye to <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear—you can visualize an equal-sidedright-angle triangle (an isosceles triangle) c<strong>on</strong>necting <strong>the</strong>se threepoints, as shown in <strong>the</strong> drawing in Figure 9-12, page 170. This is aneasy way to place <strong>the</strong> ear correctly. The isosceles triangle can bevisualized <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> model. See Figure 9-20, page 175.Practice seeing proporti<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships now by looking atphotographs or drawings <strong>of</strong> people in <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile view and visualizing<strong>the</strong> isosceles triangle, as in Figure 9-12. This technique willsave you from a lot <strong>of</strong> problems and errors in your pr<strong>of</strong>ile drawings.We still need to make two more measurements <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ileblank. First, holding your pencil horiz<strong>on</strong>tally, just under your ear,slide <strong>the</strong> pencil forward as in Figure 9-21. You come to <strong>the</strong> spacebetween your nose and mouth. This is <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong>your ear. Make a mark <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> blank.Again, holding your pencil horiz<strong>on</strong>tally just under your ear,slide <strong>the</strong> pencil backward this time. You will come to <strong>the</strong> placewhere your skull and neck c<strong>on</strong>nect—<strong>the</strong> place that bends, as inFigure 9-22. Mark this point <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> blank. The point is higherthan you think. In symbolic drawing, <strong>the</strong> neck is usually placedbelow <strong>the</strong> circle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head, with <strong>the</strong> point that bends <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>level <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chin. This will cause problems in your drawing: Theneck will be too narrow. Make sure that you perceive <strong>on</strong> yourmodel <strong>the</strong> correct place where <strong>the</strong> neck begins at <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>skull.176 THE NEW DRAW ING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Now you will need to practice <strong>the</strong>se percepti<strong>on</strong>s. Look atpeople. Practice perceiving faces, observing relati<strong>on</strong>ships, seeing<strong>the</strong> unique forms <strong>of</strong> each individual face.You are ready now to draw a pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait. You will be usingall <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> skills you have learned so far:• Focusing, <strong>on</strong> complex edges and negative spaces until you feel<strong>the</strong> shift to an alternative state <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness, <strong>on</strong>e in whichyour right hemisphere leads and your left hemisphere isquiet. Remember that this process requires an uninterruptedblock <strong>of</strong> time.• Estimating angles in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>taledges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper• <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> just what you see without trying to identify or attachverbal labels to forms (you learned <strong>the</strong> value <strong>of</strong> this in <strong>the</strong>upside-down drawing)• <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> just what you see without relying <strong>on</strong> old stored-andmemorizedsymbols from your childhood drawing• Estimating relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> sizes—how big is this form comparedto that <strong>on</strong>e?And finally:• Perceiving proporti<strong>on</strong>s as <strong>the</strong>y really are, without changingor revising visual informati<strong>on</strong> to fit prec<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s aboutwhat parts are important. They are all important, and eachpart must be given its full proporti<strong>on</strong> in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rparts. This requires bypassing <strong>the</strong> brain's propensity tochange incoming informati<strong>on</strong> without "telling" you what ithas d<strong>on</strong>e. Your sighting tool—your pencil—will enable youto "get at" <strong>the</strong> true proporti<strong>on</strong>s.If you feel that you need to review any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> techniques at thispoint, turn to <strong>the</strong> previous chapters to refresh your memory.Reviewing some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> exercises, in fact, will help to streng<strong>the</strong>nyour new skills. Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is particularly useful instreng<strong>the</strong>ning your newfound method <strong>of</strong> gaining access to yourright hemisphere and quieting <strong>the</strong> left.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 177


If, as occasi<strong>on</strong>ally happens, yourL-mode remains active as you startto draw, <strong>the</strong> best remedy is to do ashort sessi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, drawing any complexobject—a crumpled piece <strong>of</strong> paperis fine. Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>seems to force <strong>the</strong> shift to R-modeand <strong>the</strong>refore is a good warm-upexercise for drawing any subject.A warm-up exerciseTo illuminate for yourself <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edges, spaces, andrelati<strong>on</strong>ships in portrait drawing, I suggest that you copy (make adrawing <strong>of</strong>) John Singer Sargent's beautiful pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait <strong>of</strong>Mme. Pierre Gautreau, which Sargent drew in 1883 (Figure 9-23).You may wish turn it upside down.For <strong>the</strong> past forty years or so, most art teachers have not recommendedcopying masterworks as an aid to learning to draw.With <strong>the</strong> advent <strong>of</strong> modern art, many art schools rejected traditi<strong>on</strong>alteaching methods and copying master drawings went out<strong>of</strong> favor. Now, copying drawings and paintings is coming backinto favor as an effective means <strong>of</strong> training <strong>the</strong> eye in art.I believe that copying great drawings is very instructive forbeginning students. Copying forces <strong>on</strong>e to slow down and reallysee what <strong>the</strong> artist saw. I can practically guarantee that carefullycopying any masterwork <strong>of</strong> drawing will forever imprint <strong>the</strong>image in your memory. Therefore, because copied drawingsbecome an almost permanent file <strong>of</strong> memorized images, I recommendthat you copy <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong> major and minor masters <strong>of</strong>drawing. We are fortunate <strong>the</strong>se days to have reproducti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>great works readily and inexpensively available.For how to do an exercise copy <strong>of</strong> Sargent's pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait <strong>of</strong>Mme. Pierre Gautreau, also known as "Madame X," please read all<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s before you begin.What you'll need:• Your drawing paper• Your #2B writing pencil and #4B drawing pencils, sharpened,and your eraser• Your plastic Picture Plane• An hour <strong>of</strong> uninterrupted timeWhat you'll do:These instructi<strong>on</strong>s will be appropriate for ei<strong>the</strong>r right-side-up orupside-down drawing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Sargent portrait.178 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


1. As always, in starting a drawing, you will first draw a format.Center <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Viewfinders <strong>on</strong> your drawing paper and useyour pencil to draw around <strong>the</strong> outside edges. Then, lightlydraw crosshairs <strong>on</strong> your paper.2. You will be using your new skills <strong>of</strong> seeing edges, spaces, andrelati<strong>on</strong>ships in this drawing. Since <strong>the</strong> original is a line drawing,lights and shadows are not relevant in this exercise.3. Lay your clear plastic Picture Plane directly <strong>on</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Sargent and note where <strong>the</strong> crosshairs fall <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> portraitdrawing. You will immediately see how this will help you inFig. 9-23. John Singer Sargent.Mme. Pierre Gautreau, 1883FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASK 179


"People have many illusi<strong>on</strong>s whichblock <strong>the</strong>m from acting in <strong>the</strong>irown best interest as a species, aswell as individuals. In dealing with<strong>the</strong> present problems <strong>of</strong> life, wemust first be able to see <strong>the</strong> realities<strong>of</strong> our lives."Fig. 9-24.—J<strong>on</strong>as SalkThe Anatomy <strong>of</strong> Reality,1983deciding <strong>on</strong> your Basic Unit and starting your copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>drawing. You can check proporti<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships right <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> original drawing and transfer <strong>the</strong>m to your copy.Ask yourself <strong>the</strong> following series <strong>of</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s. (Note that I mustname <strong>the</strong> features in order to give <strong>the</strong>se verbal instructi<strong>on</strong>s, butwhen you are drawing, try to clear your mind <strong>of</strong> words.) Lookingat <strong>the</strong> Sargent drawing and using <strong>the</strong> crosshairs as in Figure 9-24,ask yourself <strong>the</strong> following:1. Where is <strong>the</strong> point where <strong>the</strong> forehead meets <strong>the</strong> hairline?2. Where is <strong>the</strong> outermost curve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tip <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose?What are <strong>the</strong> angles <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> forehead?3. What is <strong>the</strong> negative shape that lies between those two points?4. If you draw a line between <strong>the</strong> tip <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose and <strong>the</strong> outermostcurve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chin, what is <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> that line relativeto vertical (or horiz<strong>on</strong>tal)?5. What is <strong>the</strong> negative shape defined by that line?6. Relative to <strong>the</strong> crosshairs, where is <strong>the</strong> curve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> neck?7. What is <strong>the</strong> negative space made by <strong>the</strong> chin and neck?8., 9., and 10. Check <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear, bend <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> neck, and <strong>the</strong> slant <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> back.C<strong>on</strong>tinue in this fashi<strong>on</strong>, putting <strong>the</strong> drawing toge<strong>the</strong>r like a jigsawpuzzle: Where is <strong>the</strong> ear? How big is it relative to <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ileyou have just drawn? What is <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> neck?What is <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> negative space made by <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>neck and <strong>the</strong> hair? And so <strong>on</strong>. Draw just what you see, nothingmore. Notice how small <strong>the</strong> eye is relative to <strong>the</strong> nose, and notice<strong>the</strong> size <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mouth relative to <strong>the</strong> eye. When you haveunlocked <strong>the</strong> true proporti<strong>on</strong> by sighting, you will be surprised, Ifeel quite sure. In fact, if you lay <strong>on</strong>e finger over <strong>the</strong> features inSargent's drawing, you will see what a small proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>whole form is occupied by <strong>the</strong> main features. This is <strong>of</strong>ten quitesurprising to beginning drawing students.180 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Now, <strong>the</strong> real thing: A pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait <strong>of</strong> a pers<strong>on</strong>Now you are ready to draw a real portrait <strong>of</strong> a pers<strong>on</strong>. You'll beseeing <strong>the</strong> w<strong>on</strong>drous complexity <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tours, watching yourdrawing evolve from <strong>the</strong> line that is your unique, creative inventi<strong>on</strong>,and observing yourself integrating your skills into <strong>the</strong> drawingprocess. You will be seeing, in <strong>the</strong> artist's mode <strong>of</strong> seeing, <strong>the</strong>astounding thing-as-it-is, not a pale, symbolized, categorized,analyzed, memorized shell <strong>of</strong> itself. Opening <strong>the</strong> door to seeclearly that which is before you, you will draw <strong>the</strong> image bywhich you make yourself known to us.If I were pers<strong>on</strong>ally dem<strong>on</strong>strating <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> drawing aportrait pr<strong>of</strong>ile, I would not be naming parts. I would point to <strong>the</strong>various areas and refer to features, for example, as "this form, thisc<strong>on</strong>tour, this angle, <strong>the</strong> curve <strong>of</strong> this form," and so <strong>on</strong>. For <strong>the</strong> sake<strong>of</strong> clarity in writing, unfortunately, I'll have to name <strong>the</strong> parts. Ifear that <strong>the</strong> process may seem cumbersome and detailed whenwritten out as verbal instructi<strong>on</strong>s. The truth is that your drawingwill seem like a wordless, antic dance, an exhilarating investigati<strong>on</strong>,with each new percepti<strong>on</strong> miraculously linked to <strong>the</strong> lastand to <strong>the</strong> next.With that cauti<strong>on</strong> in mind, read through all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>sbefore you start and <strong>the</strong>n try to do <strong>the</strong> drawing withoutinterrupti<strong>on</strong>.What you'll need:1. Most important, you'll need a model—some<strong>on</strong>e who willpose for you in pr<strong>of</strong>ile view. Finding a model is not easy.Many people strenuously object to sitting perfectly still forany period <strong>of</strong> time. One soluti<strong>on</strong> is to draw some<strong>on</strong>e who iswatching televisi<strong>on</strong>. Ano<strong>the</strong>r possibility is to catch some<strong>on</strong>esleeping—preferably upright in a chair, though that doesn'tseem to happen too <strong>of</strong>ten!2. Your clear plastic Picture Plane and your felt-tip markingpen3. Two or three sheets <strong>of</strong> your drawing paper, taped in a stack<strong>on</strong>to your drawing boardFACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASEl8l


4. Your drawing pencils and eraser5. Two chairs, <strong>on</strong>e to sit <strong>on</strong> and <strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong> which to lean your drawingboard. See Figure 9-25 for setting up to draw. Note that it'salso helpful to have a small table or a stool or even ano<strong>the</strong>rchair <strong>on</strong> which to put your pencils, erasers, and o<strong>the</strong>r gear.6. An hour or more <strong>of</strong> uninterrupted timeWhat you'll do:Fig. 9-25.Fig. 9-26.1. As always, start by drawing a format. You may use <strong>the</strong> outsideedge <strong>of</strong> your Picture Plane as a template.2. Lightly t<strong>on</strong>e your paper. This will allow you to erase outlighted areas and to add graphite for shadowed areas. I'll givecomplete instructi<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong> fourth perceptual skill, perceivinglights and shadows, in <strong>the</strong> next chapter. You have alreadyhad some experience with "shading," however, and I find thatmy students greatly enjoy adding at least some lights andshadows to this exercise. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, you may preferto do a line drawing without t<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>the</strong> paper, as John SingerSargent did in his pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait <strong>of</strong> Mme. Gautreau.Whe<strong>the</strong>r you t<strong>on</strong>e <strong>the</strong> paper or not, be sure to add <strong>the</strong>crosshairs.3. Pose your model. The model can be facing ei<strong>the</strong>r right or left,but in this first pr<strong>of</strong>ile drawing, I suggest that you place yourmodel facing to your left if you are right-handed, and to <strong>the</strong>right if you are left-handed. With this arrangement, you willnot be covering up <strong>the</strong> features as you draw <strong>the</strong> skull, hair,neck and shoulders.4. Sit as close to your model as possible. Two to four feet isabout ideal, and this distance can be managed even with <strong>the</strong>intervening chair for propping up your drawing board. Check<strong>the</strong> setup again in Figure 9-25.5. Next, use your plastic Picture Plane to compose your drawing.Close <strong>on</strong>e eye and hold up <strong>the</strong> Picture Plane with aclipped-<strong>on</strong> Viewfinder; move it backward and forward until<strong>the</strong> head <strong>of</strong> your model is placed pleasingly within <strong>the</strong> format—thatis, not too crowded <strong>on</strong> any edge and with enough<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> neck and shoulders to provide "support" for <strong>the</strong> head.182 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


A compositi<strong>on</strong> you certainly d<strong>on</strong>'t want is <strong>on</strong>e in which <strong>the</strong>model's chin is resting <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> bottom edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format.6. When you have decided <strong>on</strong> your compositi<strong>on</strong>, hold <strong>the</strong> plasticPicture Plane as steadily as possible. You will next choosea Basic Unit—a c<strong>on</strong>venient size and shape to guide proporti<strong>on</strong>sas you draw. I usually use <strong>the</strong> span from <strong>the</strong> model's eyelevel to <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chin. You, however, might prefer touse ano<strong>the</strong>r Basic Unit—perhaps <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose or <strong>the</strong>span from <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose to <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chin(Figure 9-27).7. When you have chosen your Basic Unit, mark <strong>the</strong> unit withyour felt-tip marker directly <strong>on</strong> your plastic Picture Plane.Then, transfer <strong>the</strong> Basic Unit to your drawing paper, using<strong>the</strong> same procedure that you have learned in previous exercises.You may need to review <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> pages126-130 and Figures 8-11 and 8-12, page 146. You may want toalso mark <strong>the</strong> topmost edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hair and <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>head at <strong>the</strong> point opposite eye level. You can transfer <strong>the</strong>semarks to your paper as a rough guide for <strong>the</strong> drawing (Figure9-28).8. At this point, you can begin to draw, c<strong>on</strong>fident that you willend up with <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong> you have so carefully chosen.Again, I must remind you that although this process seems cumbersomenow, later <strong>on</strong> it becomes so automatic and so rapid thatyou will hardly be aware <strong>of</strong> how you start a drawing. Allow yourmind to roam over <strong>the</strong> many complicated processes you accomplishwithout thinking <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> step-by-step methods: making a U-turn <strong>on</strong> a two-way street; cracking and separating an egg yolkfrom <strong>the</strong> white; crossing a busy intersecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> foot where <strong>the</strong>reis no stoplight; making a ph<strong>on</strong>e call from a pay ph<strong>on</strong>e. Imaginehow many steps you would need to put instructi<strong>on</strong>s into wordsfor any <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> those skills.In time, and with practice, starting a drawing becomes almostcompletely automatic, allowing you to c<strong>on</strong>centrate <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> modeland <strong>on</strong> composing your drawing. You will hardly be aware <strong>of</strong>choosing a Basic Unit, sizing it and placing it <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawingpaper. I recall an incident when <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> my students realized thatFig. 9-27.Fig. 9-28.FACING FOREWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 183


Fig. 9-29.she was "just drawing." She exclaimed, "I'm doing it!" The samething will happen to you—in time, and with practice.9. Gaze at <strong>the</strong> negative space in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile and begin todraw that negative shape. Check <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose relativeto vertical. It may help to hold up your pencil vertically tocheck that shape, or you may want to use <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> yourViewfinders. Remember that <strong>the</strong> outside edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> negativeshape is <strong>the</strong> outer edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> format, but to make a negativespace easier to see, you may want to make a new, closer edge.See Figure 9-29 for how to check <strong>the</strong> angle formed by holdingyour pencil <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane against <strong>the</strong> tip <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose and <strong>the</strong>outermost curve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chin.10. You may choose to erase out <strong>the</strong> negative space around <strong>the</strong>head. This will enable you to see <strong>the</strong> head as a whole, separatedfrom <strong>the</strong> ground. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, you may decide todarken <strong>the</strong> negative spaces around <strong>the</strong> head or to leave <strong>the</strong>t<strong>on</strong>e as it is, working <strong>on</strong>ly within <strong>the</strong> head. See <strong>the</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>drawings at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chpter for examples. Theseare aes<strong>the</strong>tic choices—some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> many that you'll make inthis drawing.11. If your model wears glasses, use <strong>the</strong> negative shapes around<strong>the</strong> outside edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> glasses (remembering to close <strong>on</strong>eeye to see a 2-D image <strong>of</strong> your model). See Figure 9-30.12. Place <strong>the</strong> eye in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> innermost curve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> bridge<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose. Check <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eyelid relative to horiz<strong>on</strong>tal.13. Use <strong>the</strong> shape under <strong>the</strong> nostril as a negative shape (Figure9-31).14. Check <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> centerline <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mouth. This is <strong>the</strong><strong>on</strong>ly true edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mouth—<strong>the</strong> upper and lower c<strong>on</strong>tours<strong>on</strong>ly mark a color change. It's usually best to draw this colorchangeboundary lightly, especially in portraits <strong>of</strong> males.Note that, in pr<strong>of</strong>ile, <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> centerline <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>mouth—<strong>the</strong> true edge—<strong>of</strong>ten descends relative to horiz<strong>on</strong>tal.D<strong>on</strong>'t hesitate to draw this angle just as you see it. See Figure9-32.15. Using your pencil to measure (Figure 9-33), you can check <strong>the</strong>184 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 9-31. Look for <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>space under <strong>the</strong> nostril. This shapewill vary from model to model andshould be specifically observed <strong>on</strong>each individual.positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear (if it is visible). To place <strong>the</strong> ear in pr<strong>of</strong>ileportrait, recall our mnem<strong>on</strong>ic: Eye level-to-chin equals back<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-eyeto <strong>the</strong> back-<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-ear. Remember also that thismeasure forms an isosceles triangle, which can be visualized<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> model. See Figure 9-34.16. Check <strong>the</strong> length and width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear. Ears are nearly alwaysbigger than you expect <strong>the</strong>m to be. Check <strong>the</strong> size against <strong>the</strong>features <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile.17. Check <strong>the</strong> height <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> topmost curve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head—that is,<strong>the</strong> topmost edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hair or <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> skull if your modelhappens to have a shaved head or thin hair. See Figure 9-35.18. In drawing <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head, sight as follows:• Close <strong>on</strong>e eye, extend your arm holding your pencil perfectlyvertically, lock your elbow, and take a sight <strong>on</strong> eye level tochin.• Then, holding that measure, turn your pencil to horiz<strong>on</strong>taland check how far it is to <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head. It will be 1 (to<strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear) and something more—perhaps 1:1+1/2 oreven 1:2 if <strong>the</strong> hair is very thick. Keep that ratio in your mind.• Then, turn back to your drawing to transfer <strong>the</strong> ratio. Using<strong>the</strong> pencil, re-measure eye level to chin in <strong>the</strong> drawing. Holdingthat measure with your thumb, turning your pencil to <strong>the</strong>horiz<strong>on</strong>tal positi<strong>on</strong>, measure from <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye to <strong>the</strong>back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear, <strong>the</strong>n to <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head (or hair). Make amark. Perhaps you will not believe your own sights. If carefullytaken, <strong>the</strong>y are true, and your job is to believe what youreyes tell you. Learning to have faith in <strong>on</strong>e's percepti<strong>on</strong>s is<strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> principal keys to drawing well. I'm sure you canextrapolate <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> this to o<strong>the</strong>r areas <strong>of</strong> life.Fig. 9-32.Fig. 9-33.Fig. 9-34.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 185


Fig. 9-35.19. In drawing your model's hair, what you want not to do is todraw hairs. Students <strong>of</strong>ten ask me, "How do you draw hair?" Ithink <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> really means, "Give me a quick and easyway to draw hair that looks good and doesn't take too l<strong>on</strong>g."But <strong>the</strong> answer to <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> is, "Look carefully at <strong>the</strong>model's (unique) hair and draw what you see." If <strong>the</strong> model'shair is a complicated mass <strong>of</strong> curls, <strong>the</strong> student is likely toanswer, "You can't be serious! Draw all <strong>of</strong> that?"But it really isn't necessary to draw every hair and every curl.What your viewer wants is for you to express <strong>the</strong> character <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>hair, particularly <strong>the</strong> hair closest to <strong>the</strong> face. Look for <strong>the</strong> darkareas where <strong>the</strong> hair separates and use those areas as negativespaces. Look for <strong>the</strong> major directi<strong>on</strong>al movements, <strong>the</strong> exact turn<strong>of</strong> a strand or wave. The right hemisphere, loving complexity, canbecome entranced with <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> hair, and <strong>the</strong> record <strong>of</strong>your percepti<strong>on</strong>s in this part <strong>of</strong> a portrait can have great impact,as in <strong>the</strong> portrait <strong>of</strong> Proud Maisie (Figure 9-36). To be avoided are<strong>the</strong> thin, glib, symbolic marks that spell out h-a-i-r <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> samelevel as if you lettered <strong>the</strong> word across <strong>the</strong> skull <strong>of</strong> your portrait.Given enough clues, <strong>the</strong> viewer can extrapolate and, in fact,enjoys extrapolating <strong>the</strong> general texture and nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hair.See <strong>the</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawings at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> this chapter forexamples.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> hair is largely a light-shadow process. In <strong>the</strong> nextchapter, we'll take up <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> lights and shadows indepth. For now, I'll set down some brief suggesti<strong>on</strong>s. To drawyour model's hair, gaze at it with your eyes squinted to obscuredetails and to see where <strong>the</strong> larger highlights lie and where <strong>the</strong>larger shadows fall. Notice particularly <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>hair (wordlessly, <strong>of</strong> course, though I must use words for <strong>the</strong> sake<strong>of</strong> clarity). Is <strong>the</strong> hair crinkly and dense, smooth and shiny, randomlycurled, short and stiff? Take notice <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> overall shape <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> hair and make sure that you have matched that shape in yourdrawing. Begin to draw <strong>the</strong> hair in some detail where <strong>the</strong> hairmeets <strong>the</strong> face, transcribing <strong>the</strong> light-shadow patterns and <strong>the</strong>directi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> angles and curves in various segments <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hair.186 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 9-37. Note <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ear. The placement fits ourmnem<strong>on</strong>ic for locating <strong>the</strong> ear inpr<strong>of</strong>ile view: Eye level-to-chinequals back-<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-eye to <strong>the</strong>back-<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-ear.Fig. 9-36. Anth<strong>on</strong>y FrederickAugustus Sandys (1832-1904), ProudMaisie. Courtesy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Victoriaand Albert Museum, L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>.20. Finally, to complete your pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait, draw in <strong>the</strong> neckand shoulders, which provide a support for <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile head.The amount <strong>of</strong> detail <strong>of</strong> clothing is ano<strong>the</strong>r aes<strong>the</strong>tic choicewith no strict guidelines. The major aims are to provideenough detail to fit—that is, to be c<strong>on</strong>gruent with—<strong>the</strong> drawing<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head, and to make sure that <strong>the</strong> drawing <strong>of</strong> details<strong>of</strong> clothing adds to, and does not detract from, your drawing<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head. See Figure 9-36 for an example.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 187


Fig. 9-38.Fig. 9-39.Fig. 9-40.Fig. 9-41.Some fur<strong>the</strong>r tipsEyes: Observe that <strong>the</strong> eyelids have thickness. The eyeball isbehind <strong>the</strong> lids (Figure 9-38). To draw <strong>the</strong> iris (<strong>the</strong> colored part <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> eye)—d<strong>on</strong>'t draw it. Draw <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> white (Figure 9-39). The white can be regarded as negative space, sharing edgeswith <strong>the</strong> iris. By drawing <strong>the</strong> (negative) shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> white part,you'll get <strong>the</strong> iris right because you'll bypass your memorizedsymbol for iris. Note that this bypassing technique works foreverything that you might find "hard to draw." The technique isto shift to <strong>the</strong> next adjacent shape or space and draw that instead.Observe that <strong>the</strong> upper lashes grow first downward and <strong>the</strong>n(sometimes) curve upward. Observe that <strong>the</strong> whole shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>eye slants back at an angle from <strong>the</strong> fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile (Figure9-38). This is because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong> eyeball is set in <strong>the</strong> surroundingb<strong>on</strong>y structure. Observe this angle <strong>on</strong> your model'seye—this is an important detail.Neck: Use <strong>the</strong> negative space in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> neck in order toperceive <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tour under <strong>the</strong> chin and <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> neck(Figure 9-40). Check <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> neck in relati<strong>on</strong>to vertical. Make sure to check <strong>the</strong> point where <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>neck joins <strong>the</strong> skull. This is <strong>of</strong>ten at about <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose ormouth (Figure 9-22).Collar: D<strong>on</strong>'t draw <strong>the</strong> collar. Collars, too, are str<strong>on</strong>gly symbolic.Instead, use <strong>the</strong> neck as negative space to draw <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>collar, and use negative spaces to draw collar points, open necks<strong>of</strong> shirts, and <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> back below <strong>the</strong> neck, as inFigures 9-40 and 9-41. (This bypassing technique works, <strong>of</strong>course, because shapes such as <strong>the</strong> spaces around collars cannotbe easily named and have generated no preexisting symbols todistort percepti<strong>on</strong>.)After you have finished:C<strong>on</strong>gratulati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> drawing your first pr<strong>of</strong>ile portrait. You arenow using <strong>the</strong> perceptual skills <strong>of</strong> drawing with some c<strong>on</strong>fidence,I feel sure. D<strong>on</strong>'t forget to practice seeing <strong>the</strong> angles and propor-188 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE: RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


ti<strong>on</strong>s you have just sighted. Televisi<strong>on</strong> is w<strong>on</strong>derful for supplyingmodels for practice, and <strong>the</strong> televisi<strong>on</strong> screen is, after all, a "picture-plane."Even if you can't draw <strong>the</strong>se free models because<strong>the</strong>y rarely stay still, you can practice eyeballing edges, spaces,angles, and proporti<strong>on</strong>s. So<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong>se percepti<strong>on</strong>s will occur automatically,and you will be really seeing.Showing <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile portraitsStudy <strong>the</strong> drawings <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> following pages. Notice <strong>the</strong> variati<strong>on</strong>sin styles <strong>of</strong> drawing. Check <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s by measuring withyour pencil.In <strong>the</strong> next chapter, you will learn <strong>the</strong> fourth skill <strong>of</strong> drawing,<strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> lights and shadows. The main exercise will be afully modeled, t<strong>on</strong>al, volumetric self-portrait and will bring usfull-circle to your "Before Instructi<strong>on</strong>" self-portrait for comparis<strong>on</strong>.Your "After Instructi<strong>on</strong>" self-portrait will be ei<strong>the</strong>r a "threequarter"view or a "full-face" view. I'll define <strong>the</strong> three portraitviews for you before we turn to lights and shadows.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 189


Ano<strong>the</strong>r example <strong>of</strong> two styles <strong>of</strong> drawing. InstructorBrian Bomeisler and I sat <strong>on</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>r side <strong>of</strong> GraceKennedy, who is also <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> our instructors, and drew<strong>the</strong>se dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawings for our students. Wewere using <strong>the</strong> same materials, <strong>the</strong> same model, and<strong>the</strong> same lighting.Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawing by <strong>the</strong> author.Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawing by instructor Brian Bomeisler.I9OTHE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


"Portrait <strong>of</strong> Joy" by student JeromeBroekhuijsen.A student drawing by Hea<strong>the</strong>r Tappen.Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawing by <strong>the</strong> author."Portrait <strong>of</strong> Scott" dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawing byinstructor Beth Firmin.FACING FORWARD: PORTRAIT DRAWING WITH EASE 191


The Value<strong>of</strong> Logical Lightsand Shadows


Fig. IO-I. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> by studentElizabeth Arnold.Light logic. Light falls <strong>on</strong> objectsand (logically) results in <strong>the</strong> fouraspects <strong>of</strong> light/shadow:1. Highlight: The brightest light,where light from <strong>the</strong> source fallsmost directly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> object.2. Cast shadow: The darkest shadow,caused by <strong>the</strong> object's blocking <strong>of</strong>light from <strong>the</strong> source.3. Reflected light: A dim light,bounced back <strong>on</strong>to <strong>the</strong> object bylight falling <strong>on</strong> surfaces around <strong>the</strong>object.4. Crest shadow: A shadow that lies<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> crest <strong>of</strong> a rounded form,between <strong>the</strong> highlight and <strong>the</strong>reflected light. Crest shadows andreflected lights are difficult to seeat first, but are <strong>the</strong> key to "roundingup" forms for <strong>the</strong> illusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>3-D <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> flat paper.Now that you have gained experience with <strong>the</strong> first threeperceptual skills <strong>of</strong> drawing—<strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edges,spaces, and relati<strong>on</strong>ships—you are ready to put <strong>the</strong>m toge<strong>the</strong>rwith <strong>the</strong> fourth skill, <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> lights and shadows. After<strong>the</strong> mental stretch and effort <strong>of</strong> sighting relati<strong>on</strong>ships, you willfind that drawing lights and shadows is especially joyful. This is<strong>the</strong> skill most desired by drawing students. It enables <strong>the</strong>m tomake things look three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al through <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> a techniquestudents <strong>of</strong>ten call "shading," but which in art terminologyis called "light logic."This term means just what it says: Light falling <strong>on</strong> forms createslights and shadows in a logical way. Look for a moment atHenry Fuseli's self-portrait (Figure 10-2). Clearly, <strong>the</strong>re is asource <strong>of</strong> light, perhaps from a lamp. This light strikes <strong>the</strong> side <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> head nearest <strong>the</strong> light source (<strong>the</strong> side <strong>on</strong> your left, as Fuselifaces you). Shadows are logically formed where <strong>the</strong> light isblocked, for example, by <strong>the</strong> nose. We c<strong>on</strong>stantly use this R-modevisual informati<strong>on</strong> in our everyday percepti<strong>on</strong>s because it enablesus to know <strong>the</strong> three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al shapes <strong>of</strong> objects we seearound us. But, like much R-mode processing, seeing lights andshadows remains below <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>scious level; we use <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>swithout "knowing" what we see.Learning to draw requires learning c<strong>on</strong>sciously to see lightsand shadows and to draw <strong>the</strong>m with all <strong>the</strong>ir inherent logic. Thisis new learning for most students, just as learning to see complexedges, negative spaces, and <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>sare newly acquired skills.Seeing valuesLight logic also requires that you learn to see differences in t<strong>on</strong>es<strong>of</strong> light and dark. These t<strong>on</strong>al differences are called "values."Pale, light t<strong>on</strong>es are called "high" in value, dark t<strong>on</strong>es "low" invalue. A complete value scale goes from pure white to pure blackwith literally thousands <strong>of</strong> minute gradati<strong>on</strong>s between <strong>the</strong> twoextremes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> scale. An abbreviated scale with twelve t<strong>on</strong>es inevenly graduated steps between light and dark is shown in Figure194 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 10-2. Henry Fuseli (1741—1825),Portrait <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Artist. Courtesy <strong>of</strong>The Victoria and Albert Museum,L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>.Find <strong>the</strong> four aspects <strong>of</strong> light logicin Fuseli's self-portrait.1. Highlights: Forehead, cheeks, etc.2. Cast shadows: Cast by <strong>the</strong> nose,lips, hands.3. Reflected lights: <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose,side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cheek.4. Crest shadows: Crest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose,crest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cheek, temple.11-4 in <strong>the</strong> color secti<strong>on</strong> following page 210.In pencil drawing, <strong>the</strong> lightest possible light is <strong>the</strong> white <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> paper. (See <strong>the</strong> white areas <strong>on</strong> Fuseli's forehead, cheeks, andnose.) The darkest dark appears where <strong>the</strong> pencil lines are packedtoge<strong>the</strong>r in a t<strong>on</strong>e as dark as <strong>the</strong> graphite will allow. (See <strong>the</strong> darkshadows cast by Fuseli's nose and hand.) Fuseli achieved <strong>the</strong>many t<strong>on</strong>es between <strong>the</strong> lightest light and <strong>the</strong> darkest dark byvarious methods <strong>of</strong> using <strong>the</strong> pencil: solid shading, crosshatching,THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 195


and combinati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> techniques. Many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> white shapes heactually erased out, using an eraser as a drawing tool. (See <strong>the</strong>highlights <strong>on</strong> Fuseli's forehead.)In this chapter, I'll show you how to see and draw lights andshadows as shapes and how to perceive value relati<strong>on</strong>ships toachieve "depth" or three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>ality in your drawings. Theseskills lead directly to color and subsequently to painting, as I outlinedin <strong>the</strong> Preface.As we proceed, keep in mind <strong>the</strong> following: The percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>edges (line) leads to <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> shapes (negative spaces andpositive shapes), drawn in correct proporti<strong>on</strong> and perspective(sighting). These skills lead to <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> values (lightlogic), which leads to <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> colors as values, whichleads to painting.j"Shadows are capricious. Theychange c<strong>on</strong>stantly—with time <strong>of</strong>day, wattage <strong>of</strong> light bulbs, placement<strong>of</strong> lamps, and changes in yourown locati<strong>on</strong>. Although youdepend <strong>on</strong> shadow for visual informati<strong>on</strong>about <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> an object,you are not usually aware <strong>of</strong> it as aquality separate from <strong>the</strong> objectitself. You usually discount <strong>the</strong>shadow and exclude it from c<strong>on</strong>sciouspercepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> object.After all, shadows change, butobjects do not."— Carolyn M. BloomerPrinciples <strong>of</strong> VisualPercepti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Van Nostrand Reinhold,1976The role <strong>of</strong> R-mode in perceiving shadowsIn <strong>the</strong> same curious way that L-mode apparently will pay almostno attenti<strong>on</strong> to negative space or upside-down informati<strong>on</strong>, itseems also to ignore lights and shadows. L-mode, after all, may beunaware that R-mode percepti<strong>on</strong>s help with naming and categorizing.You will <strong>the</strong>refore need to learn to see lights and shadows at ac<strong>on</strong>scious level. To illustrate for yourself how we interpret ra<strong>the</strong>rthan see lights and shadows, turn this book upside down and lookat Gustave Courbet's Self-portrait, Figure 10-3. Upside down, <strong>the</strong>drawing looks entirely different—simply a pattern <strong>of</strong> dark areasand light areas.Now, turn <strong>the</strong> book right side up. You will see that <strong>the</strong> dark/light pattern seems to change and, in a sense, disappear into <strong>the</strong>three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head. This is ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> manyparadoxes <strong>of</strong> drawing: If you draw <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> lighted areas andshadowed areas just as you perceive <strong>the</strong>m, a viewer <strong>of</strong> your drawingwill not notice those shapes. Instead, <strong>the</strong> viewer will w<strong>on</strong>derhow you were able to make your subject so "real," meaning threedimensi<strong>on</strong>al.196 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


These special percepti<strong>on</strong>s, like all drawing skills, are easy toattain <strong>on</strong>ce you have made a cognitive shift to <strong>the</strong> artist's mode <strong>of</strong>seeing. Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain indicates that <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere,as well as being able to perceive <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> particular shadows,is also specialized for deriving meaning from patterns <strong>of</strong> shadows.Apparently, this derived meaning is <strong>the</strong>n communicated to <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>scious verbal system, which names it.Fig. 10-3. Self-portrait, GustaveCourbet, 1897. Courtesy <strong>of</strong> TheWadsworth A<strong>the</strong>neum, Hartford,C<strong>on</strong>necticut.THE VAI.UE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 197


Fig. 10-4.How does R-mode accomplish <strong>the</strong> leap <strong>of</strong> insight required toknow what <strong>the</strong>se patterns <strong>of</strong> light and dark areas mean? ApparentlyR-mode is able to extrapolate from incomplete informati<strong>on</strong>to envisi<strong>on</strong> a complete image. The right brain seems undeterredby missing pieces <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> and appears to delight in "getting"<strong>the</strong> picture, despite its incompleteness.Look, for example, at <strong>the</strong> patterns in Figure 10-4. In each <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> drawings, notice that you first see <strong>the</strong> pattern, <strong>the</strong>n you perceiveit as a gestalt, and <strong>the</strong>n you name it.Patients with right-hemisphere injuries <strong>of</strong>ten have great difficultymaking sense <strong>of</strong> complex, fragmentary shadow patternssuch as those in Figure 10-4. They see <strong>on</strong>ly random light and darkshapes. Try turning <strong>the</strong> book upside down to approximate seeing<strong>the</strong> patterns as <strong>the</strong>se patients do—as unnamable shapes. Your taskin drawing is to see <strong>the</strong> shadow-shapes in this way even when <strong>the</strong>image is right side up, while holding at arm's length, so to speak,knowledge <strong>of</strong> what <strong>the</strong> shapes mean.This "trick <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist" is great fun. I'm sure you will enjoy<strong>the</strong>se last exercises in which you will put toge<strong>the</strong>r all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> basicskills—edges, spaces, relati<strong>on</strong>ships, lights and shadows, and,finally, expressing your unique resp<strong>on</strong>se to <strong>the</strong> gestalt—<strong>the</strong>"thingness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> thing." In this chapter, we'll work with <strong>the</strong>remaining two <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> three basic portrait poses.The three basic portrait posesIn portrait drawing, artists have traditi<strong>on</strong>ally posed <strong>the</strong>ir models(or <strong>the</strong>mselves in self-portraits) in <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> three views:• Full face: The model faces <strong>the</strong> artist directly with both sides198 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model's face fully visible to <strong>the</strong> artist.• Pr<strong>of</strong>ile: The view you drew in <strong>the</strong> last exercises. The modelfaces toward <strong>the</strong> artist's left or right and <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e side (<strong>on</strong>ehalf) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model's face is visible to <strong>the</strong> artist.• Three-quarter view: The model makes a half-turn toward <strong>the</strong>artist's left or right, making visible to <strong>the</strong> artist three-quarters<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model's face—<strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile (<strong>on</strong>e half) plus <strong>on</strong>e quarter <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> remaining half-face.Note that <strong>the</strong> full-face and pr<strong>of</strong>ile views are relatively invariant,while <strong>the</strong> three-quarter view can vary from an almost pr<strong>of</strong>ile toan almost full-face pose and still be called a "three-quarter view."John Singer Sargent, 1856-1925.Study for "Madame X." Courtesy <strong>of</strong>The Metropolitan Museum <strong>of</strong>Art, Gift <strong>of</strong> Mrs. Frances Orm<strong>on</strong>dand Miss Emily Sargent, 1931John Singer Sargent, 1856-1925. Olimpio Fusco, c.1905-15? Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The Corcoran Gallery <strong>of</strong>Art, Washingt<strong>on</strong>.Dante Gabriel Rossetti, 1828-82. Jane Burden, LaterMrs. William Morris, as Queen Guinevere. Courtesy<strong>of</strong> The Nati<strong>on</strong>al Gallery <strong>of</strong> Ireland, Dublin.THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 199


A warm-up exercise: A copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Courbet selfportraitImagine that you are h<strong>on</strong>ored by a visit from <strong>the</strong> nineteenth-centuryFrench artist, Gustave Courbet (pr<strong>on</strong>ounced goos-tav koorbay),and that he has agreed to sit for a portrait drawing, wearinghis jaunty hat and smoking his pipe. The artist is in a ra<strong>the</strong>r seriousmood, quiet and thoughtful. See Figure. 10-3, page 197.Imagine fur<strong>the</strong>r that you have arranged a spotlight so that itshines from above and in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> Courbet, illuminating <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong>his face but leaving <strong>the</strong> eyes and much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face and neck inra<strong>the</strong>r deep shadow. Take a moment to c<strong>on</strong>sciously see how <strong>the</strong>lights and shadows logically fall relative to <strong>the</strong> source <strong>of</strong> light.Then turn <strong>the</strong> book upside-down to see <strong>the</strong> shadows as a pattern<strong>of</strong> shapes. The wall behind is dark, silhouetting your model.What you'll need:Fig. 10-5.Fig. 10-6.1. Your #4B drawing pencil2. Your eraser3. Your clear plastic Picture Plane4. A stack <strong>of</strong> three or four sheets <strong>of</strong> drawing paper5. Your graphite stick and some paper napkinsWhat you'll do:Please read through all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s before starting.1. As always, draw a format edge <strong>on</strong> your drawing paper, using<strong>the</strong> outside edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> your Viewfinders. This format is in<strong>the</strong> same proporti<strong>on</strong>, width to height, as <strong>the</strong> reproducti<strong>on</strong>.2. T<strong>on</strong>e your paper with a rubbed graphite ground to amedium-dark silvery gray—about <strong>the</strong> t<strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> wall behindCourbet. Lightly draw <strong>the</strong> crosshairs as shown in Figure 10-5.You may wish to copy this drawing upside down.3. Set your Picture Plane <strong>on</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> reproducti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Courbet drawing. The crosshairs <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plastic Picture Planewill instantly show you where to locate <strong>the</strong> essential points <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> drawing. I suggest that you work upside down for at least200 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


<strong>the</strong> first "blocking in" <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> lights and shadows (Figure. 10-6).4. Decide <strong>on</strong> a Basic Unit, perhaps <strong>the</strong> light-shape from <strong>the</strong> center<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hat brim to <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> upper lip, or perhaps <strong>the</strong>pipe stem, or you may decide <strong>on</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r Basic Unit. Rememberthat everything in Courbet's drawing is locked into a relati<strong>on</strong>ship.For this reas<strong>on</strong>, you can start with any Basic Unitand end up with <strong>the</strong> correct relati<strong>on</strong>ships. Then, transfer yourBasic Unit to <strong>the</strong> drawing paper, following <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>page 130 and in Figures 8-11 and 8-12, page 146.Note: The step-by-step procedure I <strong>of</strong>fer below is <strong>on</strong>ly a suggesti<strong>on</strong>about how to proceed. You may wish to use anentirely different sequence. Also note that I am naming parts<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing <strong>on</strong>ly for instructi<strong>on</strong>al purposes. As you draw,try your best to see <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> lights and darks wordlessly. Irealize that this is like trying not to think <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word "elephant,"but as you c<strong>on</strong>tinue to draw, thinking wordlesslybecomes sec<strong>on</strong>d nature.5. You will be "drawing" with an eraser. Sharpen your eraserinto a drawing tool by cutting <strong>on</strong>e end into a wedge shape asshown in Figure 10-7.Begin by erasing out <strong>the</strong> major shapes <strong>of</strong> light, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> face,hat, and shirtfr<strong>on</strong>t, always checking <strong>the</strong> size and positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>those shapes against your Basic Unit. You might think <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>se light-shapes as negative shapes that share edges with<strong>the</strong> dark forms. By correctly seeing and erasing <strong>the</strong> lightshapes, you'll have <strong>the</strong> dark shapes "for free."6. Next, carefully erase <strong>the</strong> lightest parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hat, <strong>the</strong> side <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> neck, and <strong>the</strong> coat. Your t<strong>on</strong>ed ground supplies <strong>the</strong> middlevalue <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hat and coat (Figure. 10-8).7. Using your #4B pencil, darken in <strong>the</strong> area around <strong>the</strong> head,<strong>the</strong> shadow under <strong>the</strong> hat brim, <strong>the</strong> shadows below <strong>the</strong> eyebrows,under <strong>the</strong> nose, under <strong>the</strong> lower lip, <strong>the</strong> beard, <strong>the</strong>shadow <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> beard, and <strong>the</strong> shadows under <strong>the</strong> shirt collarand <strong>the</strong> coat collar. Carefully observe <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se shadows.Keep your t<strong>on</strong>es quite smooth, ei<strong>the</strong>r crosshatching orworking a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous t<strong>on</strong>e or combining <strong>the</strong> two. Ask yourself:Where is <strong>the</strong> darkest dark? Where is <strong>the</strong> lightest light?Fig. 10-7. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> with an eraser.a. A rubbed graphite ground <strong>of</strong>middle valueb. An eraser trimmed for preciseerasing <strong>of</strong> light areas. Then use a#4B or #6B pencil to darken shadowedareasTHE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS2OI


Fig. 10-8.Fig. 10-9.Notice also that <strong>the</strong>re is almost no informati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> shadowedareas. They are nearly uniform t<strong>on</strong>es. Yet, when youturn <strong>the</strong> book right side up, <strong>the</strong> face and features emerge out<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> shadows. These percepti<strong>on</strong>s are occurring in your ownbrain, imaging and extrapolating from incomplete informati<strong>on</strong>.The hardest part <strong>of</strong> this drawing will be resisting <strong>the</strong>temptati<strong>on</strong> to give too much informati<strong>on</strong>! Let <strong>the</strong> shadowsstay shadowy, and have faith that your viewer will extrapolate<strong>the</strong> features, <strong>the</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> eyes, <strong>the</strong> beard, everything(Figure. 10-9).8. At this point you have <strong>the</strong> drawing "blocked in." The rest isall refinement, called "working up" <strong>the</strong> drawing to a finish.Note that, because <strong>the</strong> original drawing was d<strong>on</strong>e in charcoaland you are working in pencil, <strong>the</strong> exact roughness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>charcoal medium is difficult to reproduce in pencil. But also,even though you are copying Courbet's self-portrait, yourdrawing is your drawing. Your unique line quality and choice<strong>of</strong> emphasis will differ from Courbet's.9. At each step, pull back a little from <strong>the</strong> drawing, squint youreyes a bit, and move your head from side to side slightly to seeif <strong>the</strong> image is beginning to emerge. Try to see (that is, toimage) what you have not yet drawn. Use this emerging,imagined image to add to, change, reinforce what is <strong>the</strong>re in<strong>the</strong> drawing. You will find yourself shifting back and forth:drawing, imaging, drawing again. Be parsim<strong>on</strong>ious! Provide<strong>on</strong>ly enough informati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> viewer to allow <strong>the</strong> correctimage to occur in <strong>the</strong> viewer's imagined percepti<strong>on</strong>. Do notoverdraw.At this point, I hope you will be really seeing, really drawing,really experiencing <strong>the</strong> joy <strong>of</strong> drawing. Later, when drawing apers<strong>on</strong> from life, you will find yourself w<strong>on</strong>dering why younever noticed how beautiful <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong> is, noticing perhapsfor <strong>the</strong> first time <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose or <strong>the</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>eyes (Figure. 10-10).10. As you are working up <strong>the</strong> drawing, try to focus your attenti<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> original. For any problem that you encounter, <strong>the</strong>answer is in <strong>the</strong> original. For example, you will want to202 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 10-10. A copy in pencil <strong>of</strong>Courbet's Self-portraitby instructorBrian Bomeisler.achieve <strong>the</strong> same facial expressi<strong>on</strong>: <strong>the</strong> way to accomplish thatis to pay careful attenti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> exact shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> lights and<strong>the</strong> shadows. For example, notice <strong>the</strong> exact angle (relative tovertical or horiz<strong>on</strong>tal) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> shadow in <strong>the</strong> corner <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>mouth. Notice <strong>the</strong> exact curve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> shadow under Courbet'sright eye and <strong>the</strong> exact shape <strong>of</strong> that small shadow under <strong>the</strong>right cheekb<strong>on</strong>e. Try not to talk to yourself about <strong>the</strong> facialexpressi<strong>on</strong>.THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS2O1


11. Draw just what you see, no more, no less. You'll notice that<strong>the</strong> whites <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eyes are barely lighter than <strong>the</strong> dark shadowsurrounding <strong>the</strong> eye. You will be tempted to erase out <strong>the</strong>whites because, well, you know <strong>the</strong>y are called "whites <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>eyes." D<strong>on</strong>'t do it! Allow <strong>the</strong> viewer <strong>of</strong> your drawing to "play<strong>the</strong> game" <strong>of</strong> "seeing" what is not <strong>the</strong>re. Your job is to barelysuggest, just as Courbet did.After you have finished:In drawing <strong>the</strong> Courbet portrait, you were bound to be impressedby this work, its subtlety and strength, and how <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>alityand character <strong>of</strong> Courbet emerge from <strong>the</strong> shadows. I'm sure thatthis exercise has provided you with a taste for <strong>the</strong> power <strong>of</strong> light/shadow drawing. An even greater satisfacti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>of</strong> course, willcome from doing your own self-portrait.Taking <strong>the</strong> next stepFig. IO-II. Ber<strong>the</strong> Morisot(1841-1895). Self-Portrait, c. 1885.Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The Art Institute <strong>of</strong>Chicago.I'm sure you are aware that we have moved from seeing anddrawing every detailed edge, as in Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, to preciselyseeing and drawing negative space, to seeing exact proporti<strong>on</strong>alrelati<strong>on</strong>ships, to accurately seeing and drawing <strong>the</strong> largeand small shapes <strong>of</strong> lights and shadows. As you c<strong>on</strong>tinue to drawafter completing <strong>the</strong>se less<strong>on</strong>s, you will begin to find your ownunique style <strong>of</strong> using <strong>the</strong>se fundamental comp<strong>on</strong>ents. Your pers<strong>on</strong>alstyle may evolve into a rapid, vigorous calligraphy (as in <strong>the</strong>Morisot Self-Portrait, Figure 10-11), a beautifully pale, delicatestyle <strong>of</strong> drawing, or a str<strong>on</strong>g, dense style. Or your style maybecome more and more precise, as in <strong>the</strong> Sheeler drawing, Figure10-12. Remember, you are always searching for your way <strong>of</strong> seeingand drawing. No matter how your style evolves, however, youwill always be using edges, spaces, relati<strong>on</strong>ships, and (usually)lights and shadows, and you will depict <strong>the</strong> thing itself (<strong>the</strong>gestalt) in your own way.In this less<strong>on</strong>, we are relying <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> skills you've developedwith <strong>the</strong> first three comp<strong>on</strong>ents to learn <strong>the</strong> fourth, lights andshadows, so <strong>the</strong> viewer can correctly see what you have left out.204 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 10-12. Charles Sheelcr(1883-1965), Feline Felicity, 1934.C<strong>on</strong>te cray<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> white paper.Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The Fogg ArtMuseum, Harvard University.Purchase-Louise E. Bettens Fund.For this process to work, it is helpful to see <strong>the</strong> exact shapes <strong>of</strong>lights and shadows as positive and negative shapes, and to correctlysee <strong>the</strong> angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> lights and shadows.More than <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r comp<strong>on</strong>ents, this fourth skill apparentlystr<strong>on</strong>gly triggers <strong>the</strong> brain's ability to envisi<strong>on</strong> a complete formfrom incomplete informati<strong>on</strong>. By suggesting a form with light/shadow shapes, you cause to viewer to see something that is notactually <strong>the</strong>re. And <strong>the</strong> viewer's brain apparently always gets itright. If you provide <strong>the</strong> right clues, your viewers will see marvelousthings that you d<strong>on</strong>'t even have to draw! For examples, see<strong>the</strong> self-portrait by Edward Hopper, Figure 10-13.The truth is, you can cause yourself to see what is actuallyTHE VALUF. OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS


Fig. 10-13. Edward Hopper (1882-1967), Self-Portrait, 1903. C<strong>on</strong>tecray<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> white paper. Courtesy<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>al Portrait Gallery,Smiths<strong>on</strong>ian Instituti<strong>on</strong>. The artistshadowed <strong>the</strong> left side <strong>of</strong> his headin an almost even t<strong>on</strong>e. Yet <strong>the</strong>viewer "sees" <strong>the</strong> eye that is bareleysuggested.not <strong>the</strong>re, and you should strive for this phenomen<strong>on</strong>. Learningthis "trick <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist" is quite intriguing. As you are drawing,c<strong>on</strong>stantly squint you eyes to see if you can yet "see" <strong>the</strong> form youintend. And when you "see" it—that is, <strong>the</strong> envisi<strong>on</strong>ed image is<strong>the</strong>re—stop! So many times in workshops, watching a beginningstudent draw, I find myself urgently saying, "Stop! It's <strong>the</strong>re.You've got it. D<strong>on</strong>'t overwork it!" There is an amusing saying inart circles that every artist needs some<strong>on</strong>e standing right behindwith a sledgehammer to let <strong>the</strong> artist know when <strong>the</strong> artwork isfinished.206 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Crosshatching a lighter shadowBefore we advance to <strong>the</strong> next drawing, your self-portrait, I wantto show you how to "crosshatch." This is a technical term for creatinga variety <strong>of</strong> t<strong>on</strong>es or values in a drawing by laying down asort <strong>of</strong> "carpet" <strong>of</strong> pencil strokes, <strong>of</strong>ten crossing <strong>the</strong> strokes atangles. Figure 10-14 is an example <strong>of</strong> a t<strong>on</strong>al drawing built almostentirely <strong>of</strong> crosshatches. I'll also review <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>head in fr<strong>on</strong>tal view and in three-quarter view.In former years, I thought that crosshatching was a naturalactivity, not requiring teaching. Apparently, this is not <strong>the</strong> case.The technique must be taught and must be learned. In fact, I nowbelieve that <strong>the</strong> ability to Crosshatch is a mark <strong>of</strong> a trained artist.If you glance through this book at <strong>the</strong> many reproducti<strong>on</strong>s, youwill see that almost every drawing has some area <strong>of</strong> hatching. Youwill also notice that crosshatching has almost as many forms as<strong>the</strong>re are artists to use <strong>the</strong>m. Each artist, it seems, develops a pers<strong>on</strong>alstyle <strong>of</strong> hatching, almost a "signature," and, very quickly, sowill you.At this point, I will show you <strong>the</strong> technique and a few <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>traditi<strong>on</strong>al styles <strong>of</strong> hatching. You will need paper and a carefullysharpened pencil.1. Hold your pencil firmly and make a group <strong>of</strong> parallel marks,called a "set" (shown in Figure 10-15), by placing <strong>the</strong> pencilpoint down firmly, fingers extended. Swing <strong>of</strong>f each mark bymoving <strong>the</strong> whole hand from <strong>the</strong> wrist. The wrist remains stati<strong>on</strong>aryand <strong>the</strong> fingers pull <strong>the</strong> pencil back just a bit for eachsuccessive hatch. When you have finished <strong>on</strong>e "set" <strong>of</strong> eight toten hatch marks, move your hand and wrist to a new positi<strong>on</strong>and hatch a new set. Try swinging <strong>the</strong> mark toward you, andalso try swinging it away from you in an outward movementto see which seems more natural for you. Try changing <strong>the</strong>angle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> marks.2. Practice making sets until you have found <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong>, spacing,and length <strong>of</strong> marks that seem right for you.3. The next step is to make <strong>the</strong> "cross" sets. In classical hatching,<strong>the</strong> cross set is made at an angle <strong>on</strong>ly slightly different fromFig. 10-14.THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND .SHADOWS2O7


Fig. 10-15. Fig. 10-16.Fig. 10-18. Some examples <strong>of</strong> variousstyles <strong>of</strong> crosshatching.Fig. 10-19. Alph<strong>on</strong>se Legros, red chalk <strong>on</strong> paper.Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The Metropolitan Museum <strong>of</strong> Art, <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York.208 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


<strong>the</strong> original set, as shown in Figure 10-16. This slight angleproduces a very pretty moire pattern that causes a drawing toseem to shimmer with light and air. Try this. Figure 10-17shows how to use crosshatching to create a three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>alform.4. By increasing <strong>the</strong> angle <strong>of</strong> crossing, a different style <strong>of</strong> crosshatchis achieved. In Figure 10-18, see various examples <strong>of</strong>styles <strong>of</strong> hatching: full cross (hatch marks crossing at rightangles), cross-c<strong>on</strong>tour (usually curved hatches), and hookedhatches (where a slight hook inadvertently occurs at <strong>the</strong> end<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hatch), as in <strong>the</strong> topmost example <strong>of</strong> hatching styles inFigure 10-18. There are myriad styles <strong>of</strong> hatching.5. To increase <strong>the</strong> darkness <strong>of</strong> t<strong>on</strong>e, simply pile up <strong>on</strong>e set <strong>of</strong>hatches <strong>on</strong>to o<strong>the</strong>rs, as shown in <strong>the</strong> left arm <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> figuredrawing by Alph<strong>on</strong>se Legros, Figure 10-19.6. Practice, practice, practice. Instead <strong>of</strong> doodling while talking<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> teleph<strong>on</strong>e, practice crosshatching—perhaps shadinggeometric forms such as spheres, or cylinders. (See <strong>the</strong> examplesin Figure 10-20.) As I menti<strong>on</strong>ed, crosshatching is not anaturally occurring skill for most individuals, but it can berapidly developed with practice. I assure you that skillful,individualized use <strong>of</strong> hatching in your drawings will be gratifyingto you and much admired by your viewers.Fig. 10-20.Shading into a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous t<strong>on</strong>eAreas <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuous t<strong>on</strong>e are created without using <strong>the</strong> separatestrokes <strong>of</strong> crosshatching. The pencil is applied in ei<strong>the</strong>r short,overlapping movements or in elliptical movements, going fromdark areas to light and back again, if necessary, to create a smootht<strong>on</strong>e. Most students have little trouble with c<strong>on</strong>tinuous t<strong>on</strong>e,although practice is usually needed for smoothly modulatedt<strong>on</strong>es. Charles Sheeler's complex light/shadow drawing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> catsleeping <strong>on</strong> a chair (Figure 10-12) superbly illustrates this technique.So<strong>on</strong> you will bring toge<strong>the</strong>r all <strong>of</strong> your new skills, <strong>the</strong> basiccomp<strong>on</strong>ent skills <strong>of</strong> drawing: percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> edges, spaces, andTHE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS2O9


shapes, relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>s, lights and shadows,<strong>the</strong> gestalt <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> thing drawn, and <strong>the</strong> skills <strong>of</strong> crosshatchingand c<strong>on</strong>tinuous t<strong>on</strong>e.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> logic <strong>of</strong> light for a fully modeled,t<strong>on</strong>al, volumetric self-portraitIn <strong>the</strong>se less<strong>on</strong>s, we began with line drawing and we end with afully realized drawing. The terms in <strong>the</strong> subhead above are <strong>the</strong>technical terms that describe <strong>the</strong> drawing you will do next. Fromthis exercise <strong>on</strong>ward, you will practice <strong>the</strong> five perceptual skills <strong>of</strong>drawing with c<strong>on</strong>stantly changing subject matter. The basic skillswill so<strong>on</strong> become integrated into a global skill, and you will findyourself "just drawing." You will shift flexibly from edges tospaces to angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>s, lights and shadows. So<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong>skills will be <strong>on</strong> automatic and some<strong>on</strong>e watching you draw willbe baffled by how you do it. I feel sure that you will find yourselfseeing things differently, and I hope that, for you, as for many <strong>of</strong>my students, life will seem much richer by having learned to seeand draw.Before you start your drawing, we need to review briefly <strong>the</strong>proporti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fr<strong>on</strong>tal or full-face view and <strong>the</strong> three-quarterview. You will use <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se views for your Self-Portrait.I believe that this training for precisepercepti<strong>on</strong>s is <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> greatb<strong>on</strong>uses <strong>of</strong> learning to draw. Onereally does learn to see betterthrough drawing, to see withgreater precisi<strong>on</strong> and finer discriminati<strong>on</strong>.I feel sure that youcan extrapolate <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong>this to general thinking skills. We<strong>of</strong>ten describe creative intelligenceas "<strong>the</strong> ability to see thingsclearly."The fr<strong>on</strong>tal viewKeeping this book open to <strong>the</strong> diagram <strong>on</strong> page 212, sit in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong>a mirror with <strong>the</strong> book, a piece <strong>of</strong> paper, and a pencil. You aregoing to observe and diagram <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> various parts <strong>of</strong>your own head, as you go step by step through <strong>the</strong> exercise.1. First, draw a blank (an oval shape) <strong>on</strong> your paper and draw<strong>the</strong> central axis dividing <strong>the</strong> diagram. Then, observe andmeasure <strong>on</strong> your own head <strong>the</strong> eye level line. It will behalfway. On <strong>the</strong> blank, draw in an eye level line. Be sure tomeasure to make sure you make this placement accurately.2IOTHE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig. 10-21. Vincent Van Gogh(etching, 1890), B-10, 283. Courtesy<strong>of</strong> The Nati<strong>on</strong>al Gallery <strong>of</strong> Art,Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C., RosenwaldCollecti<strong>on</strong>. An interesting example<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> expressive effect <strong>of</strong> skewedfeatures.Fig. 10-22.2. Now, looking at your own face in <strong>the</strong> mirror, visualize a centralaxis that divides your face and an eye level line at a rightangle to <strong>the</strong> central axis. Tip your head to <strong>on</strong>e side, as in Figure10-23. Notice that <strong>the</strong> central axis and <strong>the</strong> eye level lineremain at a right angle no matter what directi<strong>on</strong> you tip yourhead. (This is <strong>on</strong>ly logical, I know, but many beginners ignorethis fact and skew <strong>the</strong> features as in <strong>the</strong> example in Figure10-22.)3. Observe in <strong>the</strong> mirror: What is <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> distancebetween your eyes, compared to <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e eye? Yes, it's<strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e eye. Divide <strong>the</strong> eye level in fifths, as shown inFigure 10-24. Mark <strong>the</strong> outside corners <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eyes.4. Observe your face in <strong>the</strong> mirror. Between eye level and chin,where is <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose? This is <strong>the</strong> most variable <strong>of</strong> all<strong>the</strong> features <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> human head. You can visualize an invertedtriangle <strong>on</strong> your own face, with <strong>the</strong> wide points at <strong>the</strong> outsidecorners <strong>of</strong> your eyes and <strong>the</strong> center point at <strong>the</strong> bottom edge<strong>of</strong> your nose. This method is quite reliable. Mark <strong>the</strong> bottomedge <strong>of</strong> your nose <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> blank. See Figure 10-24.Fig. 10-23.THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS


Fig. 10-24. The full-face view diagram.Note that this diagram is<strong>on</strong>ly a general guide to proporti<strong>on</strong>sthat vary from head to head.The differences, however, are <strong>of</strong>tenvery slight and must be carefullyperceived and drawn to achieve alikeness.Where is <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> centerline <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mouth? About athird between <strong>the</strong> nose and chin. Make a mark <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> blank.Again, observe in <strong>the</strong> mirror: If you drop a straight line downfrom <strong>the</strong> inside corners <strong>of</strong> your eyes, what do you come to?The edges <strong>of</strong> your nostrils. Noses are wider than you think.Mark <strong>the</strong> blank.If you drop a line straight down from <strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pupils<strong>of</strong> your eyes, what do you come to? The outside corners <strong>of</strong>your mouth. Mouths are wider than you think. Mark <strong>the</strong>blank.212 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


8. If you move your pencil al<strong>on</strong>g a horiz<strong>on</strong>tal line <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong>your eyes, what do you come to? The tops <strong>of</strong> your ears. Mark<strong>the</strong> blank.9. Coming back from <strong>the</strong> bottoms <strong>of</strong> your ears, in a horiz<strong>on</strong>talline, what do you come to? In most faces, <strong>the</strong> space betweenyour nose and mouth. Ears are bigger than you think. Mark<strong>the</strong> blank.10. Feel <strong>on</strong> your own face and neck: How wide is your neck comparedto <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> your jaw just in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> your ears? You'llsee that your neck is almost as wide—in some men, it's aswide or wider. Mark <strong>the</strong> blank. Note that necks are widerthan you think.11. Now test each <strong>of</strong> your percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> people, photographs <strong>of</strong>people, images <strong>of</strong> people <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> televisi<strong>on</strong> screen. Practice<strong>of</strong>ten, observing—first without measuring, <strong>the</strong>n if necessarycorroborating by measuring—perceiving relati<strong>on</strong>ships betweenthis feature and that, perceiving <strong>the</strong> unique, minutedifferences between faces; seeing, seeing, seeing. Eventually,you will have memorized <strong>the</strong> general measurements givenabove and you w<strong>on</strong>'t have to analyze in <strong>the</strong> left-hemispheremode as we have been doing. But for now it's best to practiceobserving <strong>the</strong> specific proporti<strong>on</strong>s."When drawing a face, any face, itis as if curtain after curtain, maskafter mask, falls away... until afinal mask remains, <strong>on</strong>e that canno l<strong>on</strong>ger be removed, reduced.By <strong>the</strong> time <strong>the</strong> drawing is finished,I know a great deal about that face,for no face can hide itself for l<strong>on</strong>g.But although nothing escapes <strong>the</strong>eye, all is forgiven beforehand.The eye does not judge, moralize,criticize. It accepts <strong>the</strong> masks ingratitude as it does <strong>the</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g bamboosbeing l<strong>on</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> goldenrodbeing yellow."— Frederick FranckThe Zen <strong>of</strong> Seeing, 1973Now we'll turn to <strong>the</strong> three-quarter viewRecall our previous definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> three-quarter view: <strong>on</strong>e-half<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head plus <strong>on</strong>e-quarter. Still sitting in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> a mirror, poseyour head in this view by starting with a full, fr<strong>on</strong>tal view and<strong>the</strong>n turning (ei<strong>the</strong>r left or right) so that you can <strong>on</strong>ly partly see<strong>on</strong>e side <strong>of</strong> your head. You are now seeing <strong>on</strong>e full side plus <strong>on</strong>equarter—ino<strong>the</strong>r words a three-quarter view.Artists <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Renaissance loved <strong>the</strong> three-quarter view, <strong>on</strong>ce<strong>the</strong>y had finally worked through <strong>the</strong> problems <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s.I hope you will choose this view for your self-portrait. It's somewhatcomplicated, but fascinating to draw.Young children rarely draw people with heads turned to <strong>the</strong>three-quarter view. Children generally draw ei<strong>the</strong>r pr<strong>of</strong>iles or <strong>the</strong>THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 213


Fig. 10-25. A sketch by <strong>the</strong> authorfrom a three-quarter view portraitby <strong>the</strong> German artist LucasCranach (1472-1553), Head <strong>of</strong> a Youthwith a Red Cap.full, fr<strong>on</strong>tal view. Around age ten or so, children begin to attemptthree-quarter view drawings, perhaps because this view can beparticularly expressive <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>ality <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model. Theproblems young artists encounter with this view are <strong>the</strong> same oldproblems: <strong>the</strong> three-quarter view brings visual percepti<strong>on</strong>s intoc<strong>on</strong>flict with <strong>the</strong> symbolic forms developed throughout childhoodfor pr<strong>of</strong>ile and full-face views, which by age ten are embeddedin <strong>the</strong> memory.What are those c<strong>on</strong>flicts? First, as you see in Figure 10-15,<strong>the</strong>nose is not <strong>the</strong> same as a nose seen in pr<strong>of</strong>ile. In three-quarterview, you see <strong>the</strong> top and <strong>the</strong> side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose, making it seemvery wide. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, <strong>the</strong> two sides <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face are differentwidths—<strong>on</strong>e side narrow, <strong>on</strong>e side wide. Third, <strong>the</strong> eye <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>turned side is narrower and shaped differently from <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r eye.Fourth, <strong>the</strong> mouth from its center to <strong>the</strong> corner is shorter <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>turned side and shaped differently from <strong>the</strong> mouth <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rside <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> centerline. These percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> n<strong>on</strong>matching featuresc<strong>on</strong>flict with <strong>the</strong> memorized symbols for features that are214 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


usually more symmetrical.The soluti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flict is <strong>of</strong> course to draw just what yousee without questi<strong>on</strong>ing why it is thus or so and without changing<strong>the</strong> perceived forms to fit with a memorized-and-stored set <strong>of</strong>symbols for features. To see <strong>the</strong> thing-as-it-is in all <strong>of</strong> its uniqueand marvelous complexity—that is always <strong>the</strong> key.My students have found it helpful if I point out some specificaidsto seeing <strong>the</strong> three-quarter proporti<strong>on</strong>s. Let me again takeyou through <strong>the</strong> process step by step, giving you some methodsfor keeping your percepti<strong>on</strong>s clear. Again note that if I weredem<strong>on</strong>strating <strong>the</strong> three-quarter-view drawing, I would not benaming any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> parts, <strong>on</strong>ly pointing to each area. When youare drawing, do not name <strong>the</strong> parts to yourself. In fact, try not totalk to yourself at all while drawing.1. Again, sit in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> a mirror with paper and a pencil. Now,close <strong>on</strong>e eye and pose in <strong>the</strong> three-quarter view so that <strong>the</strong>tip <strong>of</strong> your nose nearly coincides with <strong>the</strong> outer c<strong>on</strong>tour <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> turned cheek, as in Figure 10-25. You can see that thisforms an enclosed shape (see Figure 10-26).2. Observe your head. Perceive <strong>the</strong> central axis—that is, animaginary line that passes through <strong>the</strong> very center <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face.In three-quarter view, <strong>the</strong> central axis passes through twopoints: a point at <strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> bridge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose and apoint at <strong>the</strong> middle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> upper lip. Image this as a thin wirethat passes right through <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose (Figure 10-27).By holding your pencil vertically at arm's length toward yourreflecti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> mirror, check <strong>the</strong> angle or tilt <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> centralaxis <strong>of</strong> your head. Each pers<strong>on</strong> may have a different characteristictilt to <strong>the</strong> head, or <strong>the</strong> axis may be perfectly vertical.3. Next, observe that <strong>the</strong> eye level line is at right angles to <strong>the</strong>central axis. This observati<strong>on</strong> will help you to avoid skewing<strong>the</strong> features as I menti<strong>on</strong>ed <strong>on</strong> page 212. Next measure <strong>on</strong>your head to observe that <strong>the</strong> eye level line is at half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>whole form.5. Now, practice making a line drawing <strong>of</strong> a three-quarter view<strong>on</strong> your scratch paper. You will be using <strong>the</strong> method <strong>of</strong>modified c<strong>on</strong>tour drawing: drawing slowly, directing yourFig. 10-26. First, see this whole areaas a shape.Fig. 10-27. Observe <strong>the</strong> tilt <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>central axis compared to vertical(your pencil). The eye level line isat a right angle compared to <strong>the</strong>central axis.THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS2I5


Fig. 10-28. Fig. 10-29.A three-quarter portrait, Kandalu,by <strong>the</strong> author. Note <strong>the</strong> tilt <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>central axis.gaze at edges, and perceiving relati<strong>on</strong>al sizes, angles, etc.Again, you can start anywhere you wish. I tend to start withthat shape between <strong>the</strong> nose and <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> turnedside <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cheek because that shape is easy to see, as in Figure10-25. Note that this shape can be used as an "interior"negative shape—a shape you have no name for. I'll describe adefinite order for <strong>the</strong> drawing, but you may prefer a differentorder.6. Direct your eyes at <strong>the</strong> shape and wait until you can see itclearly. Draw <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> that shape. Because <strong>the</strong> edges areshared, you will have also drawn <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose. Inside<strong>the</strong> shape you have drawn is <strong>the</strong> eye with <strong>the</strong> odd c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> three-quarter eye. To draw <strong>the</strong> eye, d<strong>on</strong>'t draw <strong>the</strong>eye. Draw <strong>the</strong> shapes around <strong>the</strong> eye. You may want to use <strong>the</strong>order 1, 2, 3, 4 as shown in Figure 10-28, but any order willwork as well. First <strong>the</strong> shape over <strong>the</strong> eye (1), <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> shapenext to it (2), <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> white part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye (3),216 THE NEW DRAW ING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


<strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> shape under <strong>the</strong> eye (4). Try not to think about whatyou are drawing.Just draw each shape, always shifting to <strong>the</strong>next adjacent shape.7. Next, locate <strong>the</strong> correct placement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> side <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> head closest to you. Observe <strong>on</strong> your model that <strong>the</strong>inside corner lies <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye level line. Note especially howfar away from <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose this eye is. This distance isnearly always a distance equal to <strong>the</strong> full width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> near side <strong>of</strong> your head. Be sure to look at Figure 10-28 forthis proporti<strong>on</strong>. The most comm<strong>on</strong> error beginning studentsmake in this view <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model is to place <strong>the</strong> eye too close to<strong>the</strong> nose. This error throws all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> remaining percepti<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong>f and can spoil <strong>the</strong> drawing. Make sure that you see (bysighting) <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> that space and draw it as you see it.Incidentally, it took <strong>the</strong> early Renaissance artists half acentury to work out this particular proporti<strong>on</strong>. We benefit,<strong>of</strong> course, from <strong>the</strong>ir hard-w<strong>on</strong> insights (Figures 10-28 and10-29).8. Next, <strong>the</strong> nose. Check <strong>on</strong> your reflecti<strong>on</strong> where <strong>the</strong> edge <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> nostril is in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> inside corner <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eye: Drop aline straight down, following (that is, parallel to) <strong>the</strong> centralaxis (Figure 10-29). Remember that noses are bigger than youthink.9. Observe where <strong>the</strong> corner <strong>of</strong> your mouth lies in relati<strong>on</strong> to<strong>the</strong> eye (Figure 10-29). Then observe <strong>the</strong> centerline <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>mouth and <strong>the</strong> exact curve. This curve is important in catching<strong>the</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model. D<strong>on</strong>'t talk to yourself aboutthis. The visual percepti<strong>on</strong>s are <strong>the</strong>re to be seen. By seeingclearly and drawing exactly what you see—exact angles,edges, spaces, proporti<strong>on</strong>s, lights, and shadows. In R-mode,you do resp<strong>on</strong>d—but not in words.10. Observe <strong>the</strong> upper and lower edges <strong>of</strong> your lips, rememberingthat <strong>the</strong> line is usually light because <strong>the</strong>se are not trueedges or str<strong>on</strong>g c<strong>on</strong>tours.11. On <strong>the</strong> turned side <strong>of</strong> your head, observe <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>spaces around <strong>the</strong> mouth. Again, note <strong>the</strong> exact curve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>centerline <strong>on</strong> this side.THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 217


12. The ear. The mnem<strong>on</strong>ic for placing <strong>the</strong> ear in pr<strong>of</strong>ile viewmust be slightly changed to account for <strong>the</strong> added quarter inthree-quarter view.Pr<strong>of</strong>ile:Eye level-to-chin = back-<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-eye to <strong>the</strong> back-<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-earbecomesThree-quarter:Eye level-to-chin = fr<strong>on</strong>t-<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-eye to <strong>the</strong> back-<strong>of</strong>-<strong>the</strong>-earYou can perceive this relati<strong>on</strong>ship by measuring it <strong>on</strong> your reflecti<strong>on</strong>in <strong>the</strong> mirror. Then note where <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ear is, and <strong>the</strong>n<strong>the</strong> bottom. See Figure 10-30.Fig. 10-30. A three-quarter selfportraitby instructor BrianBomeisler.Ready to draw!Now that we've reviewed crosshatching and <strong>the</strong> general proporti<strong>on</strong>sfor <strong>the</strong> fr<strong>on</strong>tal and three-quarter views, you are ready for<strong>the</strong> last drawing exercise, your Self-Portrait in fully articulatedlights and shadows.What you'll need:• Your drawing paper—three or four sheets (for padding),taped to your drawing board.• Your pencils, sharpened, and your eraser• A mirror and tape for attaching <strong>the</strong> mirror to a wall, or youmay want to sit in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> a bathroom mirror or dressing tablemirror• Your felt-tip marker• Your graphite stick• A paper tissue or towel for rubbing in a ground• A dampened tissue for correcting marker-pen marks <strong>on</strong>plastic• A floor lamp or a table lamp to illuminate <strong>on</strong>e side <strong>of</strong> yourhead (Figure 10-31 shows an inexpensive spot lamp)• A hat, scarf, or headdress, if that idea appeals to youFig. 10-31.218 THE NEW DRAW ING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


What you'll do:1. First, prepare your drawing paper with a ground. You maychoose any level <strong>of</strong> t<strong>on</strong>e. You may want to do a "high-key"(meaning light) drawing by starting with a pale ground, oryou may decide to use <strong>the</strong> drama <strong>of</strong> a dark ground for a "lowkey"(meaning dark) drawing. Or, perhaps you prefer a middlevalue. Be sure to lightly draw in <strong>the</strong> crosshairs.Note that in this drawing, you will not need your plastic PicturePlane. The mirror itself becomes <strong>the</strong> picture-plane. Tryto think that through—I'm sure you will see <strong>the</strong> logic <strong>of</strong> it.2. Once your ground is prepared, set yourself up to draw. Check<strong>the</strong> setup in Figure 10-32. You will need <strong>on</strong>e chair to sit in and<strong>on</strong>e chair or small table to hold your drawing tools. As yousee in <strong>the</strong> diagram, you will lean your drawing board against<strong>the</strong> wall. Once you are seated, adjust <strong>the</strong> mirror <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> wall sothat you can comfortably see your image. Also, <strong>the</strong> mirrorshould be just at arm's length from where you are sitting. Youwant to be able to take sightings directly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> mirror as wellas directly <strong>on</strong> your face and skull as you observe <strong>the</strong> measurementsin <strong>the</strong> mirror.3. Adjust <strong>the</strong> lamp and test out various poses by turning yourhead, raising or lowering your chin, and adjusting your hat orheaddress, until you see in <strong>the</strong> mirror a compositi<strong>on</strong> in lightsand shadows that you like. Decide whe<strong>the</strong>r to draw a full-faceview or a three-quarter view, and decide which way you willturn, left or right, if you choose <strong>the</strong> three-quarter view.4. Once you have carefully chosen your compositi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> mirrorand <strong>the</strong> pose is "set," try to keep all <strong>of</strong> your gear in <strong>the</strong>same places until <strong>the</strong> drawing is finished. If you stand up totake a break, for example, try not to move your chair or <strong>the</strong>lamp. Students <strong>of</strong>ten find it very frustrating if <strong>the</strong>y can'trecapture exactly <strong>the</strong> same view when <strong>the</strong>y sit down again.5. You are now ready to draw. The instructi<strong>on</strong>s that follow arereally <strong>on</strong>ly a suggesti<strong>on</strong> for <strong>on</strong>e procedure am<strong>on</strong>g myriadpossible procedures. I suggest that you read through all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>remaining instructi<strong>on</strong>s and <strong>the</strong>n begin to draw following <strong>the</strong>Fig. 10-32.THE VALUE OF LOGICAL. LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 219


suggested procedure. Later <strong>on</strong>, you'll find your own way toproceed.Fig. 10-33.A self-portrait in pencil1. Gaze at your reflecti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> mirror, searching for negativespaces, interesting edges, and <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>of</strong> lights and shadows.Try to suppress language entirely, particularly verbalcriticism <strong>of</strong> your face or features. This is not easy to do,because this is a new use <strong>of</strong> a mirror—not for checking orcorrecting, but to reflect an image in an almost impers<strong>on</strong>alway. Try to regard yourself <strong>the</strong> way you would regard a stilllifesetup or a photograph <strong>of</strong> a stranger.2. Choose a Basic Unit. This is entirely up to you. I generallyuse eye level to chin, and I <strong>of</strong>ten draw in a central axis (a linethat vertically bisects <strong>the</strong> head, running through <strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> bridge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose and <strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mouth). Next,draw in <strong>the</strong> eye level line.These two guidelines, <strong>the</strong> central axis and <strong>the</strong> eye level line,always cross at right angles, whe<strong>the</strong>r in full-face view or threequarterand whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>'s head is tilted relative to verticalor is held perfectly upright. I suggest drawing <strong>the</strong> central axis andeye level line directly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> mirror with your felt-tip pen. (Youmay prefer to start your drawing ano<strong>the</strong>r way, perhaps relying<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> crosshairs printed <strong>on</strong> your mirror. Please feel free todo so.) You must, however, be sure to mark <strong>the</strong> top and bottom <strong>of</strong>your Basic Unit directly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> mirror.3. The next step, <strong>of</strong> course, is to transfer your Basic Unit to yourdrawing paper with its crosshairs and t<strong>on</strong>ed ground. Justmake marks at <strong>the</strong> top and bottom <strong>of</strong> your Basic Unit. Youmay wish to add marks for <strong>the</strong> top edge and side edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>image in <strong>the</strong> mirror. Transfer <strong>the</strong>se marks to your drawing.4. Next, squint your eye a bit to mask out some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> detail inyour mirror image and find <strong>the</strong> large lighted shapes. Notewhere <strong>the</strong>y are located relative to your Basic Unit and to <strong>the</strong>crosshairs <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> mirror and in your drawing and to <strong>the</strong> centralaxis/eye level lines, if you are using <strong>the</strong>m.220 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


5. Begin your drawing by erasing out <strong>the</strong> largest lighted shapes,as in Figure 10-34. Try to avoid any small forms or edges.<strong>Right</strong> now you are trying to see <strong>the</strong> large lights and shadows.6. You may wish to erase out <strong>the</strong> ground around <strong>the</strong> head, leaving<strong>the</strong> t<strong>on</strong>ed ground as <strong>the</strong> middle value <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head. Youmay, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, want to lower <strong>the</strong> value (darken) <strong>the</strong>negative spaces. These are aes<strong>the</strong>tic choices. Figure 10-34shows both.7. You may want to add some graphite to <strong>the</strong> shadowed side<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face. For this, I recommend your #4B pencil, not <strong>the</strong>graphite stick, which is somewhat hard to c<strong>on</strong>trol andbecomes ra<strong>the</strong>r greasy if pressed hard <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper.8. I'm sure you've noticed that I have said nothing about eyes,nose, or mouth up to this point. If you can resist <strong>the</strong> impulseto draw <strong>the</strong> features first, and allow <strong>the</strong>m to "come out" <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>light/shadow pattern, as I describe in <strong>the</strong> margin, you will beable to exploit <strong>the</strong> full power <strong>of</strong> this kind <strong>of</strong> drawing.9. Ra<strong>the</strong>r than drawing <strong>the</strong> eyes, for example, I recommend thatyou rub your #4B pencil point <strong>on</strong> a scrap <strong>of</strong> paper, rub yourforefinger over <strong>the</strong> graphite, and, checking back in <strong>the</strong> mirrorfor <strong>the</strong> locati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eyes, rub your graphited finger where<strong>the</strong> eyes should be. Suddenly you will be able to "see" <strong>the</strong>eyes, and you need <strong>on</strong>ly to reinforce that ghostly percepti<strong>on</strong>.10. Once you have <strong>the</strong> large shapes <strong>of</strong> lights and shadows drawn,begin to look for some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> smaller shapes. For example,you may find a shadowed shape under <strong>the</strong> lower lip or under<strong>the</strong> chin or under <strong>the</strong> nose. You may see a shadow-shape <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose or under <strong>the</strong> lower lid. You can slightlyt<strong>on</strong>e <strong>the</strong> shadow-shape with your pencil, using crosshatching,or, if you wish, rub <strong>the</strong> t<strong>on</strong>e in with your finger to smooth it.Be sure that you place and t<strong>on</strong>e <strong>the</strong> shadow-shapes exactly asyou see <strong>the</strong>m. They are <strong>the</strong> shapes <strong>the</strong>y are because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>b<strong>on</strong>e structure and <strong>the</strong> particular light that falls <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> shape.11. At this point, you are ready to decide whe<strong>the</strong>r you want toleave <strong>the</strong> drawing at this somewhat rough or "unfinished"stage, or whe<strong>the</strong>r you want to work <strong>the</strong> drawing up to a "highFig. 10-34.This less<strong>on</strong> leads to <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> twoadditi<strong>on</strong>al basic skills I menti<strong>on</strong>edin <strong>the</strong> Introducti<strong>on</strong>: <strong>the</strong> "dialogue"that goes <strong>on</strong> in drawing from <strong>the</strong>imaginati<strong>on</strong>. This is drawing at amore advanced level. You check<strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> "out <strong>the</strong>re" or inyour imaginati<strong>on</strong> and just barelyindicate placement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> firstmarks. This causes an imaginedimage in <strong>the</strong> mind <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist, who<strong>the</strong>n draws what he or she hasalready "seen." Thus drawingbecomes a kind <strong>of</strong> dialoguebetween <strong>the</strong> artist's intent andwhat develops <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper. Theartist makes a mark. That markgenerates a fur<strong>the</strong>r image. Theartist reinforces <strong>the</strong> imaginedadditi<strong>on</strong>, which triggers moreimaging, and so <strong>on</strong>.THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 221


Fig. 10-35. A diagram <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> threequarterself-portrait shown in Fig.10-37.Fig. 10-36. The full-face view diagram.finish." Throughout this book, you will find numerous examples<strong>of</strong> drawings at various degrees <strong>of</strong> "finish."12. I will briefly list again some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> main proporti<strong>on</strong>s to watchfor. Remember that your brain may not be helping you to seewhat is really "out <strong>the</strong>re," and <strong>the</strong>se reminders may encourageyou to take sights <strong>on</strong> everything!• For a full-face self-portrait: Eye level to chin = eye level to<strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> skull.• If <strong>the</strong> hair is thick, <strong>the</strong> upper part will be greater than half.• The space between <strong>the</strong> eyes is approximately <strong>on</strong>e eye-width.• Determine <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose by imaging an inverted trianglewith <strong>the</strong> outside points at <strong>the</strong> outside corners <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>eyes and <strong>the</strong> point at <strong>the</strong> bottom edge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose. This is avariable proporti<strong>on</strong>. The inverted triangle is a particularshape for each particular model.• The outside edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nostrils <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nose are usuallydirectly under <strong>the</strong> inside corners <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eyes. This proporti<strong>on</strong>also varies.• The outside corners <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mouth fall under <strong>the</strong> pupils <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>eyes. This proporti<strong>on</strong> varies. Note with special care <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>and shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> outer corners <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mouth, wheremuch <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subtle expressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a face is located.• The tops <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ears fall approximately at or slightly aboveeye level line.• The bottoms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ears are approximately at (or slightlyabove or below) <strong>the</strong> upper lip. Note that if <strong>the</strong> head is tiltedup or down, <strong>the</strong> locati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ears—as seen <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> pictureplane—relative to <strong>the</strong> eye level line will change.• Observe <strong>the</strong> neck, collar, and shoulders relative to <strong>the</strong> head.Make sure that <strong>the</strong> neck is wide enough by checking <strong>the</strong>width in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face. Use negative spacefor <strong>the</strong> collar (draw <strong>the</strong> spaces under and around <strong>the</strong> collar).Notice how wide <strong>the</strong> shoulders are. A frequent, student erroris making <strong>the</strong> shoulders too narrow. Sight <strong>the</strong> width relativeto your Basic Unit.• In drawing <strong>the</strong> hair, look for <strong>the</strong> largest lights and shadows <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> hair first and <strong>the</strong>n work down to <strong>the</strong> finer details later.222 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Note <strong>the</strong> major directi<strong>on</strong>s in which <strong>the</strong> hair grows and <strong>the</strong>places where it parts to shows a darker t<strong>on</strong>e underneath. Noteand draw details <strong>of</strong> how <strong>the</strong> hair grows and what its texture isclose to <strong>the</strong> face. Give your viewer enough informati<strong>on</strong> about<strong>the</strong> hair to know what it is like.The portrait drawings throughout this book will dem<strong>on</strong>stratevarious ways <strong>of</strong> drawing different types <strong>of</strong> hair. There is obviouslyno <strong>on</strong>e way <strong>of</strong> drawing hair just as <strong>the</strong>re is no <strong>on</strong>e way todraw eyes, noses, or mouths. As always, <strong>the</strong> answer to any drawingproblem is to draw what you see.If you have decided <strong>on</strong> a three-quarter view, please review<strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s for that view that I provided earlier in this chapter.Also, see Figure 10-35. One cauti<strong>on</strong>: Beginning students sometimesbegin to widen <strong>the</strong> narrow side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face and <strong>the</strong>n,because that makes <strong>the</strong> face seem too wide, <strong>the</strong>y narrow <strong>the</strong> nearside <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face. Often, <strong>the</strong> drawing ends up a fr<strong>on</strong>tal view, eventhough <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong> was posing in three-quarter view. This is veryfrustrating for students, because <strong>the</strong>y <strong>of</strong>ten can't figure out whathappened. The key is to accept you percepti<strong>on</strong>s. Draw just whatyou see! D<strong>on</strong>'t sec<strong>on</strong>d-guess your sightings.Now that you have read all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s, you are readyto begin. I hope you will find yourself quickly shifting into R-mode.Fig. 10-37. The completed drawing:A self-portrait by instructor BrianBomeisler.After you have finished:When <strong>the</strong> drawing is finished, observe in yourself that you sitback and regard <strong>the</strong> drawing in a different way from <strong>the</strong> way youregarded <strong>the</strong> drawing while working <strong>on</strong> it. Afterward, you regard<strong>the</strong> drawing more critically, more analytically, perhaps notingslight errors, slight discrepancies between your drawing and <strong>the</strong>model. This is <strong>the</strong> artist's way. Shifting out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> working R-mode and back to L-mode, <strong>the</strong> artist assesses <strong>the</strong> next move, tests<strong>the</strong> drawing against <strong>the</strong> critical left brain's standards, plans <strong>the</strong>required correcti<strong>on</strong>s, notes where areas must be reworked. Then,by taking up <strong>the</strong> brush or pencil and starting in again, <strong>the</strong> artistshifts back into <strong>the</strong> working R-mode. This <strong>on</strong>-<strong>of</strong>f procedure c<strong>on</strong>-THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 223


"One <strong>of</strong> life's most fulfillingmoments occurs in that split sec<strong>on</strong>dwhen <strong>the</strong> familiar is suddenlytransformed into <strong>the</strong> dazzling aura<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>oundly new.... Thesebreakthroughs are too infrequent,more uncomm<strong>on</strong> than comm<strong>on</strong>;and we are mired most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> timein <strong>the</strong> mundane and <strong>the</strong> trivial.The shocker: what seems mundaneand trivial is <strong>the</strong> very stuff that discoveryis made <strong>of</strong>. The <strong>on</strong>ly differenceis our perspective, ourreadiness to put <strong>the</strong> piecestoge<strong>the</strong>r in an entirely new wayand to see patterns where <strong>on</strong>lyshadows appeared just a momentbefore."— Edward B. LindamanThinking in Future Tense,1978tinues until <strong>the</strong> work is d<strong>on</strong>e—that is, until <strong>the</strong> artist decides thatno fur<strong>the</strong>r work is needed.Before and after: A pers<strong>on</strong>al comparis<strong>on</strong>This is a good time to retrieve your pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> drawings andcompare <strong>the</strong>m with <strong>the</strong> drawing you have just completed. Pleaselay out <strong>the</strong> drawings for review.I fully expect that you are looking at a transformati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> yourdrawing skills. Often my students are amazed, even incredulous,that <strong>the</strong>y could actually have d<strong>on</strong>e <strong>the</strong> pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> drawings<strong>the</strong>y now find in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m. The errors in percepti<strong>on</strong> seem soobvious, so childish, that it even seems that some<strong>on</strong>e else musthave d<strong>on</strong>e <strong>the</strong> drawing. And in a way, I suppose, this is true. L-mode, in drawing, sees what is "out <strong>the</strong>re" in its own way—linkedc<strong>on</strong>ceptually and symbolically to ways <strong>of</strong> seeing and drawingdeveloped during childhood. These drawings are generalized.Your recent R-mode drawings, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, are morecomplex, more linked to actual perceptual informati<strong>on</strong> from "out<strong>the</strong>re," drawn from <strong>the</strong> present moment, not from memorizedsymbols <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> past. These drawings are <strong>the</strong>refore more realistic.A friend might remark up<strong>on</strong> looking at your drawings that youhad uncovered a hidden talent. In a way, I believe this is true,although I am c<strong>on</strong>vinced that this talent is not c<strong>on</strong>fined to a few,but instead is as widespread as, say, talent for reading.Your recent drawings aren't necessarily more expressive thanyour "Before-Instructi<strong>on</strong>" drawings. C<strong>on</strong>ceptual L-mode drawingscan be powerfully expressive. Your "After-Instructi<strong>on</strong>" drawingsare expressive as well, but in a different way: They are morespecific, more complicated, and more true to life. They are <strong>the</strong>result <strong>of</strong> newfound skills for seeing things differently, <strong>of</strong> drawingfrom a different point <strong>of</strong> view. The true and more subtle expressi<strong>on</strong>is in your unique line and your unique "take" <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>model—in this instance yourself.At some future time, you may wish to partly reintegrate simplified,c<strong>on</strong>ceptual forms into your drawings. But you will do soby design, not by mistake or inability to draw realistically. For224 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


now, I hope you are proud <strong>of</strong> your drawings as signs <strong>of</strong> victory in<strong>the</strong> struggle to learn basic perceptual skills and to c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>the</strong>processes <strong>of</strong> your brain.Now that you have, with great care, seen and drawn your ownface and <strong>the</strong> faces <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r human beings, surely you understandwhat artists mean when <strong>the</strong>y say that every human face isbeautiful.A showing <strong>of</strong> portraitsAs you look at <strong>the</strong> portraits <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> following pages, try to mentallyreview how each drawing developed from start to finish.Go through <strong>the</strong> measurement process yourself. This will helpto reinforce your skill and train your eye. Three <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawingsare instructi<strong>on</strong>al dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> drawings from our five-dayworkshop."The object, which is back <strong>of</strong> everytrue work <strong>of</strong> art, is <strong>the</strong> attainment <strong>of</strong>a state <strong>of</strong> being, a state <strong>of</strong> high functi<strong>on</strong>ing,a more than ordinarymoment <strong>of</strong> existence.... We makeour discoveries while in <strong>the</strong> statebecause <strong>the</strong>n we are clearsighted."— Robert HenriThe Art Spirit, 1913A suggesti<strong>on</strong> for a next drawingA drawing suggesti<strong>on</strong> that has proven to be amusing and interestingis a self-portrait as a character from art history. A few suchexamples might include "Self-Portrait as <strong>the</strong> M<strong>on</strong>a Lisa"; "Self-Portrait as a Renaissance Youth"; "Self-Portrait as Venus Risingfrom <strong>the</strong> Sea."THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 125


Two additi<strong>on</strong>al self-portraits byinstructor Brian Bomeisler. Notehow <strong>the</strong>y differ <strong>on</strong>e from ano<strong>the</strong>r.You will find that your selfportraitswill differ, reflecting <strong>the</strong>mood, feeling, and surroundings <strong>of</strong>each sitting. Remember, drawing isnot photography.A beautiful self-portrait inlight/shadow by instructor GraceKennedy.226 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


THE VALUE OF LOGICAL LIGHTS AND SHADOWS 227A three-quarter self-portrait bystudent Mauro Imamoto. Thecompositi<strong>on</strong> is especially fine.


<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Beauty<strong>of</strong> Color


"No <strong>on</strong>e knows how far back in time<strong>the</strong> human passi<strong>on</strong> for colorevolved, but... its transmigrati<strong>on</strong>from <strong>on</strong>e culture to ano<strong>the</strong>r can betraced from archaeological fragmentsas old as recorded history."— Enid VerityColor Observed, 1980Miss Helen Keller, who was bothblind and deaf, writes <strong>of</strong> color:"I understand how scarlet can differfrom crims<strong>on</strong> because I knowthat <strong>the</strong> smell <strong>of</strong> an orange is not<strong>the</strong> smell <strong>of</strong> a grapefruit... Withoutcolor or its equivalent, life tome would be dark, barren, a vastblackness.... Therefore, I habituallythink <strong>of</strong> things as colored andres<strong>on</strong>ant. Habit accounts for part.The soul sense accounts forano<strong>the</strong>r part. The brain with itsfive-sensed c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> asserts itsright and accounts for <strong>the</strong> rest.The unity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> world demandsthat color be kept in it whe<strong>the</strong>r Ihave cognizance <strong>of</strong> it or not.Ra<strong>the</strong>r than be shut out, I take partin it by discussing it, happy in <strong>the</strong>happiness <strong>of</strong> those near me whogaze at <strong>the</strong> lovely hues <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sunsetor <strong>the</strong> rainbow."— Helen KellerThe World I Live In, 1908IN AN AGE LIKE OURS, color is not <strong>the</strong> luxury it was in pastcenturies. We are inundated by manufactured color—surrounded,immersed, swimming in a sea <strong>of</strong> color. Because <strong>of</strong> sheerquantity, color is perhaps in danger <strong>of</strong> losing some <strong>of</strong> its magic. Ibelieve that using color in drawing and painting helps us torecapture <strong>the</strong> beauty <strong>of</strong> color and to experience <strong>on</strong>ce again <strong>the</strong>almost hypnotic fascinati<strong>on</strong> it <strong>on</strong>ce had for us.Human beings have made colored objects from earliest times,but never in such great quantity as now. In past centuries, coloredobjects were most <strong>of</strong>ten owned by <strong>on</strong>ly a few wealthy or powerfulpers<strong>on</strong>s. For ordinary people, color was not available, except asfound in <strong>the</strong> natural world and as seen in churches and ca<strong>the</strong>drals.Cottages and <strong>the</strong>ir furnishings were made <strong>of</strong> natural materials—mud,wood, and st<strong>on</strong>e. Homespun cloth usually retained<strong>the</strong> neutral colors <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> original fibers or, if dyed with vegetabledyes, was <strong>of</strong>ten quick to s<strong>of</strong>ten and fade. For most people, a bit <strong>of</strong>bright ribb<strong>on</strong>, a beaded hatband, or a brightly embroidered beltwas a treasure to guard and cherish.C<strong>on</strong>trast this with <strong>the</strong> fluorescent world we live in today.Everywhere we turn, we encounter human-made color: televisi<strong>on</strong>and movies in color, buildings painted brilliant colors insideand out, flashing colored lights, highway billboards, magazinesand books in full color, even newspapers with full-page color displays.Intensely colored fabrics that would have been valued likejewels and reserved for royalty in times past are now available t<strong>on</strong>early every<strong>on</strong>e, wealthy or not. Thus, we have largely lost ourformer sense <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> w<strong>on</strong>drous specialness <strong>of</strong> color. Never<strong>the</strong>less,as humans, we can't seem to get enough color. No amount seemstoo much—at least not yet. True, quite a few individuals objectedto <strong>the</strong> "colorizati<strong>on</strong>" <strong>of</strong> vintage black-and-white films. Thesearguments, however, were lost to commerce; most people preferred<strong>the</strong> colorized versi<strong>on</strong>s.But what is all this color for? In <strong>the</strong> natural world <strong>of</strong> animals,birds, and plants, color always has a purpose—to attract, repel,c<strong>on</strong>ceal, communicate, warn, or assure survival. For present-dayhumans, has color even begun to lose its purpose and meaning?Now that we have this huge bulk <strong>of</strong> manufactured color, is its use230 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Fig 11-3. Color Wheel. Complements are directlyopposite each o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> wheel. The complement<strong>of</strong> each primary color (yellow, red, and blue) is asec<strong>on</strong>dary color (violet, green, and orange). Thecomplement <strong>of</strong> each tertiary color is ano<strong>the</strong>rtertiary color.Because any complementary pair alwaysc<strong>on</strong>tains, between <strong>the</strong> two hues, all three primarycolors, complements completely cancel colorwhen mixed toge<strong>the</strong>r in equal quantities. Thischaracteristic is <strong>the</strong> key to c<strong>on</strong>trolling intensity<strong>of</strong> hues.Exercise: The pattern for making your own colorwheel is <strong>on</strong> page 234.Fig. 11-4. Value scale. A scale in even stepsbetween <strong>the</strong> opposites, <strong>the</strong> white <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper and<strong>the</strong> darkest dark <strong>the</strong> pencil will make.The inset strip is <strong>the</strong> same value throughout. Theapparent change in value is a perceptual illusi<strong>on</strong>,caused by <strong>the</strong> differences in c<strong>on</strong>trast between <strong>the</strong>light-to-dark t<strong>on</strong>es <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> scale and <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stantvalue <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> central strip.Exercise: Make a value scale <strong>of</strong> twelve steps, usingpencil.Fig. 11-5. Hea<strong>the</strong>r Heilman, age 6, The Park, 12x18".Courtesy <strong>of</strong> The Internati<strong>on</strong>al Child Art Collecti<strong>on</strong>,Junior Arts Center, Los Angeles, California.Children tend to use symbolic color as well assymbolic forms. These symbol systems are linked tolanguage acquisiti<strong>on</strong>: "Trees have green leaves andbrown trunks." Learning perceptual skills helps olderchildren to see bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong>se symbolic systems.Exercise: Review Chapter Five <strong>on</strong> childhood drawing,<strong>the</strong>n redraw your own childhood landscape,this time in color.


Fig. 11-6. Edgar Degas, Ballet Dancer in Positi<strong>on</strong> Facing 3/4 Fr<strong>on</strong>t (1872). S<strong>of</strong>t black graphiteaccented with black cray<strong>on</strong>, heightened with white <strong>on</strong> pink paper. 16+1/8 x 11+1/4. The Fogg ArtMuseum, Harvard. Bequest <strong>of</strong> Meta and Paul J. Sachs.Exercise: To experience <strong>the</strong> impact <strong>of</strong> color <strong>on</strong> drawing, compare this drawing with ano<strong>the</strong>rDegas dancer <strong>on</strong> page 157. See page 237 for a drawing exercise.


Fig. 11-7. Ka<strong>the</strong> Kollwitz, Self-Portrait (c. 1891/92).Pen and black ink with brush and gray wash,heightened with white gouache, <strong>on</strong> brown wovepaper. 15+13/16 x 12+11/16". The Art Institute <strong>of</strong> Chicago.Gift <strong>of</strong> Margaret Day Blake, Mr. and Mrs. Alan Press,and Prints and <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>s Purchase, 1980.Over her lifetime, <strong>the</strong> German artist Ka<strong>the</strong> Kollwitzproduced more than fifty probing images <strong>of</strong> herself.This serious, c<strong>on</strong>templative self-portrait was drawnwhen <strong>the</strong> artist was about twenty-five and reflectsher early training in engraving.Exercise: Try a heightened self-portrait, using <strong>the</strong>procedure described below.The artist sits in fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong> a mirror, cheek resting <strong>on</strong>hand. The light, as you see, comes from above and to<strong>the</strong> left <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sitter (note <strong>the</strong> shadow cast by <strong>the</strong> noseand <strong>the</strong> crest shadow al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> wrist).Working <strong>on</strong> brown paper, quickly paint a darknegative space around <strong>the</strong> head, using a brush andblack ink mixed with water. The brown <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> papersupplies <strong>the</strong> middle value for <strong>the</strong> face.Use a tiny brush to draw in <strong>the</strong> details <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> facein black ink, and <strong>the</strong> same tiny brush to heighten <strong>the</strong>drawing with white gouache. The heightening linesfollow <strong>the</strong> curve <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> surface <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face, almost asthough you are feeling your way across <strong>the</strong> forms.Fig.11-8. HenriToulouse Lautrec, At <strong>the</strong> Circus:Work in <strong>the</strong> Ring (1899). Colored pencil with pastel andblack cray<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> ivory wove paper. 21.8 x 31.6 cm.The Art Institute <strong>of</strong> Chicago. Gift <strong>of</strong> Mr. and Mrs. B. E.Bensinger.Exercise: For practice with color, negative space,and sighting, copy this drawing using colored penciland pastels, but change <strong>the</strong> colors to those <strong>of</strong> yourown choice to see <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> color <strong>on</strong> drawing.


Fig. 11-9. Jean-Baptiste-Sime<strong>on</strong> Chardin (French, 1699-1779),Self Portrait with a Visor (c.1776|. Pastel <strong>on</strong> blue laid papermounted <strong>on</strong> canvas. 18 x 14+13/16 (457 x 374 mm). The Art Institute<strong>of</strong> Chicago, Clarence Buckingham Collecti<strong>on</strong> and Harold JoachimMemorial Fund.Fig.11-10. Jean-Baptiste-Sime<strong>on</strong> Chardin, Portrait <strong>of</strong> MadameChardin (c. 1776). Pastel <strong>on</strong> blue laid paper mounted <strong>on</strong> canvas.18 x 14+15/16 (457 x 378 mm). The Art Institute <strong>of</strong> Chicago,Helen Regerstein Collecti<strong>on</strong>.Toward <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> a l<strong>on</strong>g career as a successful painter <strong>of</strong> still lifes and scenes <strong>of</strong> everydaylife, Jean-Baptiste-Sime<strong>on</strong> Chardin turned to pastels, a new medium for him, and toportraiture, an unexplored subject. Only twelve Chardin pastels are known to exist today,foremost am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>m <strong>the</strong> two masterpieces shown above. These portraits illustrate a pointmade in <strong>the</strong> text: rich and pr<strong>of</strong>ound color can be achieved by using very few hues. The basichues in each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> portraits are <strong>the</strong> complements blue and orange, each transformed into acomplex harm<strong>on</strong>ious medley <strong>of</strong> balanced values and intensities.Exercise: Try a portrait or self-portrait <strong>on</strong> colored paper using <strong>on</strong>ly two complementary huesplus white and black. The masterworks above can guide your efforts to gain c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>of</strong> color.


Fig, 11-11. Elizabeth Layt<strong>on</strong>, Self-Portrait in a Mirror. Colored pencil <strong>on</strong> paper. Reproducedwith kind permissi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist.Elizabeth Layt<strong>on</strong> first began drawing at age 68 with <strong>the</strong> hope <strong>of</strong> finding relief from severedepressi<strong>on</strong> following a stroke. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> proved <strong>the</strong>rapeutic (she calls it "cure by c<strong>on</strong>tour")and she c<strong>on</strong>tinued to draw. Since <strong>the</strong>n, herwork has been exhibited nati<strong>on</strong>wide and is greatlyadmired. She believes that every<strong>on</strong>e can learn to draw and that children in particular shouldbe taught to draw at an early age.Fig. 11-12. Photograph <strong>of</strong> Elizabeth Layt<strong>on</strong>. Reproduced with kind permissi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist.Exercise: Try a colored-pencil self-portrait in a mirror, including your hands.


Fig. 11-13. Richard Diebenkorn, Untitled(Ocean Park) (1977). Acrylic, gouache,cut-and-pasted paper. 18+1/4 x 32+3/4"(47.6 x 83.2 cm). Collecti<strong>on</strong>, The Museum <strong>of</strong>Modern Art, <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York. Purchase.Exercise: Working within an unusual format(tall and narrow, short and wide, circular,oval), divide <strong>the</strong> space and manipulate <strong>the</strong>quantities <strong>of</strong> hues to achieve a pleasing,harm<strong>on</strong>ious balance and tensi<strong>on</strong> (a sense <strong>of</strong>c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> or "pull") between color areas.Fig. 11-14. Brian Bomeisler, Adam and Eve.1984. Mixed media <strong>on</strong> paper. 10 x 9".Collecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist.This <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York artist explores color, light,and scale through <strong>the</strong>mes from mythologyand literature.Exercise: Experiment with scale by usingc<strong>on</strong>trasting sizes-very large to very small.Experiment with light by changing <strong>the</strong> values<strong>of</strong> a hue to achieve luminosity in color.Observe how <strong>the</strong> artist achieved a w<strong>on</strong>derfulsense <strong>of</strong> luminous color in Adam and Eve.


Fig. 11-15. Odil<strong>on</strong> Red<strong>on</strong> (1840-19161, Head <strong>of</strong> a Young Girl. Pastel <strong>on</strong> blue-gray laid paper. 20+5/8 x 14+7/8. The FoggArt Museum, Harvard.Exercise: See page 243 for an exercise based <strong>on</strong> this excepti<strong>on</strong>al drawing.


Fig. 11-16. Student Gary Berberet, Sell-Portrait. Pastel <strong>on</strong> gray paper. 18 x 24".Exercise: Try an intense, close-up self-portrait in pastel <strong>on</strong> colored paper. Remember that you alwayshave an available model-yourself. The additi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> props such as hats can stimulate interest in eachnew self-portrait.


Fig. 11-17. Student Laura Wright, UmbrellaStill Lite. A m<strong>on</strong>ochromatic color harm<strong>on</strong>ybased <strong>on</strong> varying values and intensities <strong>of</strong>orange.Exercise: C<strong>on</strong>struct a still life with somerandomly chosen objects. Do a negativespacedrawing <strong>on</strong> colored paper (or do apreliminary drawing and transfer it tocolored paper, using carb<strong>on</strong> paper).Choose colored pencils that are variati<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e hue, <strong>the</strong> hue <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> colored paper.Fig. 11-18. Student Ken Ludwig, Large StuffedEagle. Rubbed pastel <strong>on</strong> white paper withpen and black ink. 18 x 24".A few analogous colors can produce asurprising range <strong>of</strong> harm<strong>on</strong>ious hues. Str<strong>on</strong>gc<strong>on</strong>trast is supplied by <strong>the</strong> black ink andwhite paper.Exercise: Draw an animal or bird from life,if possible, or from photographs. (Habitatgroups at natural history museums arew<strong>on</strong>derful as models-<strong>the</strong>y hold very still.)Rub analogous hues <strong>of</strong> colored chalk intowhite paper and draw with pen and ink.


Fig. 11-19. Piet M<strong>on</strong>drian, RedAmarylis with Blue Background (c. 1907). Watercolor. 18+3/8x 13". The Museum <strong>of</strong>Modern Art, <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York. The Sidney and Harriet Janis Collecti<strong>on</strong>.Exercise: Prismacolor watercolor pencils c<strong>on</strong>vert to watercolor when dampened with a wet brush. Using<strong>the</strong>se pencils, try a "portrait" <strong>of</strong> a flower or plant, paying attenti<strong>on</strong> to negative space and using c<strong>on</strong>trastingcolors, guided by <strong>the</strong> superb drawing above.


Fig. 11-20. David Hockney, Celia in a Black Dress with White Flowers, 1972. Cray<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> paper. 17 x 14". Collecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist.Exercise: Try a half-length or full-length portrait or self-portrait in colored pencil <strong>on</strong> white paper. Place an object or objects in fr<strong>on</strong>t<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> figure and use negative space to delineate <strong>the</strong> space between. Three distances are described: from <strong>the</strong> artist's eyes to <strong>the</strong>objects to <strong>the</strong> figure.


Fig. 11-21. Paul Gauguin, Tahitian Woman. Pastel <strong>on</strong> paper.21+5/8x19+1/2. The Brooklyn Museum, <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York.Exercise: Combine warm and cool hues in a pastel drawing.Fig. 11-22. Student Thu Ha Huyung, Girl in a Flowered Hat.Colored pencil <strong>on</strong> yellow paper. 18 x 24".Exercise: For a colorful drawing, try a portrait using two sets<strong>of</strong> complements and black and white <strong>on</strong> colored paper.Fig. 11-23. Hans Baldung Grien, Self-Portrait(1502).Offentliche Kunstsammlung, Kupferstichkabinett Basel.Exercise: This drawing combines three-quarter view andfull-face in <strong>on</strong>e drawing, with strangely intriguing results.You might deliberately try this distorti<strong>on</strong> as a step into moreabstract portraiture.Fig. 11-24. Student drawing. The Arrow Hotel. Negative spaceand c<strong>on</strong>trasting colors transform an urban scene.Exercise: See page 240 for suggesti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> drawing an urbanlandscape.


mostly indiscriminate? Or is purpose and meaning still subliminallyinherent in color as a remnant <strong>of</strong> our biological heritage? Is<strong>the</strong> pencil I write with painted yellow for a purpose? Did I chooseto wear blue today for a reas<strong>on</strong>?And what is color? Is it merely, as scientists tell us, a subjectiveexperience, a mental sensati<strong>on</strong> that can <strong>on</strong>ly occur if threerequirements are fulfilled: that <strong>the</strong>re is an observer, an object, andsufficient light in <strong>the</strong> narrow band <strong>of</strong> wavelengths called <strong>the</strong> "visiblespectrum"? It certainly is true that at twilight <strong>the</strong> world turnsto shades <strong>of</strong> gray. Is <strong>the</strong> world really colorless, <strong>on</strong>ly seeming tobecome full <strong>of</strong> color again when we turn <strong>the</strong> lights <strong>on</strong>?If color is a mental sensati<strong>on</strong>, how does it happen? Scientiststell us that when light falls <strong>on</strong> an object—for example, anorange—<strong>the</strong> surface <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> orange has <strong>the</strong> particular property <strong>of</strong>absorbing all <strong>the</strong> wavelengths <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> spectrum except that which,when reflected back to our eyes and processed through <strong>the</strong> visualsystem, causes <strong>the</strong> mental sensati<strong>on</strong> we have named <strong>the</strong> color"orange." My writing pencil is coated with a chemical substance(paint) that absorbs all wavelengths except that which, when reflectedback to my eyes, is "yellow." Is <strong>the</strong> orange really orange? Is<strong>the</strong> pencil really yellow? We cannot know, because we cannot getoutside <strong>of</strong> our own eye/brain/mind system to find out. What wedo know is that when <strong>the</strong> sun goes down, color disappears.Placing color in <strong>the</strong> brainGiven sufficient light to perceive colors, scientists also tell us that<strong>the</strong> brain's reacti<strong>on</strong> to colors seems to depend <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> differencesin thinking modes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> various secti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain.Very bright, intense colors (and colors that shine and glitter)draw a resp<strong>on</strong>se from <strong>the</strong> so-called "primitive" brain, <strong>the</strong> limbicsystem. This resp<strong>on</strong>se is an emoti<strong>on</strong>al <strong>on</strong>e, perhaps c<strong>on</strong>nected toour biological heritage <strong>of</strong> color as communicati<strong>on</strong>. For example,many people say, "When I get mad, I see red!" The inverse <strong>of</strong> thisexclamati<strong>on</strong> perhaps describes <strong>the</strong> situati<strong>on</strong> whereby an intensered elicits an emoti<strong>on</strong>al, aggressive resp<strong>on</strong>se.The main role <strong>of</strong> L-mode, generally located in <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere,is to tag colors with names and attributes, such as "brightIn <strong>the</strong> Middle Ages, color was usedin heraldry, <strong>the</strong> practice <strong>of</strong> designing<strong>the</strong> insignia for armor that"heralded" or announced <strong>the</strong>wearer's status, family c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s,and history as a warrior.Color helped to carry <strong>the</strong> message<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> design:WhiteGoldRedBlue= fate and purity= h<strong>on</strong>or= courage and zeal= purity and sincerityGreen = youth and fertilityBlack= grief and penitenceOrange = strength and endurancePurple = royalty and high birthThe limbic system is a group <strong>of</strong>structures, as yet incompletelydefined, that generally includesareas deep in <strong>the</strong> brain. Theseareas are transiti<strong>on</strong>al in structurebetween <strong>the</strong> "new" cortex andolder porti<strong>on</strong>s such as <strong>the</strong> olfactorybrain. Scientists believe that<strong>the</strong> limbic system is involved inpatterns <strong>of</strong> str<strong>on</strong>g emoti<strong>on</strong>s.— H. B. English andAva C. EnglishA Comprehensive Dicti<strong>on</strong>ary<strong>of</strong> Psychological andPsychoanalytical Terms,1974DRAWING ON THE BEAUTY OF COLOR 231


"In my own case <strong>the</strong> sight <strong>of</strong> vividblue has always been accompaniedby an emoti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> vague delight.And in <strong>on</strong>e experience <strong>of</strong> travel,this feeling rose into ecstasy. It waswhen I beheld for <strong>the</strong> first time <strong>the</strong>grandest visi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> blue in thisworld <strong>the</strong> glory <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Gulf Stream;a magical splendor that made medoubt my senses—a flaming azurethat looked as if a milli<strong>on</strong> summerskies had been c<strong>on</strong>densed intopure fluid color for <strong>the</strong> making <strong>of</strong>it."— Lafcadio HearnExotics and Retrospectives,1968"He knows all about art, but hedoesn't know what he likes."© 1943james Thurber, © 1971 HelenW. Thurber and RosemaryThurber Sauers.blue," "lem<strong>on</strong> yellow," or "burnt umber," and to translate intowords our emoti<strong>on</strong>al reacti<strong>on</strong>s to colors. (As an example, read in<strong>the</strong> marginal note how <strong>the</strong> Irish-Greek writer Lafcadio Hearntranslated into words his emoti<strong>on</strong>al reacti<strong>on</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> color blue.)Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, L-mode is specialized for designating sequencedsteps in mixing colors—for example, "to mix orange, add yellowto red," or "to darken blue, add black."The right hemisphere (or R-mode) is specialized for <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> hues, particularly for subtle linkages <strong>of</strong><strong>on</strong>e hue to ano<strong>the</strong>r. R-mode is biased toward discovering patterns<strong>of</strong> coherence, specifically toward combinati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> hues that balanceopposites—for example, red/green, blue/orange, dark/light, dull/bright.In his 1976 essay "The Dialectics <strong>of</strong> Color," Dr. Peter Smithstates: "Since <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere has a str<strong>on</strong>g interest in <strong>the</strong>way things fit toge<strong>the</strong>r to form a closed system, it may be said tobe a decisive factor in <strong>the</strong> es<strong>the</strong>tic resp<strong>on</strong>se." This closed systemmay be what artists speak <strong>of</strong> as unified, harm<strong>on</strong>ious color—thatis, color in relati<strong>on</strong>ships that are locked into balance. Perhaps R-mode recognizes <strong>the</strong> satisfying wholeness <strong>of</strong> properly unifiedcolor and reacts with a pleasurable sense <strong>of</strong> "Yes. That's it. That'sright."The c<strong>on</strong>verse is also true: R-mode recognizes unbalanced ordisunified color arrangements and perhaps l<strong>on</strong>gs for unity and<strong>the</strong> missing parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> closed system. An individual may experiencethis l<strong>on</strong>ging as vague dislike—a sense that something ismissing or out <strong>of</strong> place.R-mode has ano<strong>the</strong>r important role in color: seeing whichcombinati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> colors has produced a particular color. Given arange <strong>of</strong> grays, for example, R-mode sees which <strong>on</strong>e is warmedwith red, which is cooled with blue.Learning <strong>the</strong> basics <strong>of</strong> colorNearly every<strong>on</strong>e is interested in color, yet most people have surprisinglylittle comprehensive knowledge about it. We <strong>of</strong>ten takeit for granted that we know enough about color to know what we232 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


like, and we feel that's sufficient. Yet knowing something <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>enormous body <strong>of</strong> knowledge about color increases pleasure incolor, as in almost every subject. In <strong>the</strong> following pages, you willadd a few color skills to your newly acquired basic perceptualskills <strong>of</strong> drawing.Something odd happens when a student <strong>of</strong> drawing begins toadd color to <strong>the</strong> gray, black, and white <strong>of</strong> drawing. No matter howsatiated by our modern color-loaded surroundings, studentsfocus <strong>on</strong> color as though seeing it for <strong>the</strong> first time, almost with<strong>the</strong> naive pleasure <strong>of</strong> children. And color in drawing does indeedadd a tremendous emoti<strong>on</strong>al charge to drawing. For an example<strong>of</strong> this, compare Edgar Degas's drawing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ballet dancer <strong>on</strong>pink paper (Figure 11-6) with <strong>the</strong> almost identical Degas drawing<strong>on</strong> page 157 <strong>of</strong> Chapter Eight. But I must cauti<strong>on</strong> you: I am notsaying that color makes a drawing better. It doesn't. Colorchanges drawing, adding an element <strong>of</strong> drama and verve thatmoves it closer to painting.For <strong>the</strong> basic exercises described in this chapter, you willneed to buy a few new drawing supplies. I will add to <strong>the</strong> list <strong>of</strong>supplies as each technique is introduced.First, buy a set <strong>of</strong> colored pencils. "Prismacolor" is a goodbrand, but <strong>the</strong>re are many o<strong>the</strong>rs. Prismacolor <strong>of</strong>fers a completeset <strong>of</strong> sixty pencils, or you can buy individual colors. I suggest <strong>the</strong>following:blackwhiteultramarine blueCopenhagen bluedark greencanary yellowscarlet redmagentasienna browndark brownsepiaburnt umberyellow ochrelem<strong>on</strong> yellowflesholive greenvermili<strong>on</strong>violetslate graysandwarm gray lightwarm gray mediumcreamorangeAlso, buy six sheets <strong>of</strong> colored paper at least 9" x 12" or larger.C<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> paper is fine, or you may prefer ano<strong>the</strong>r type <strong>of</strong>paper. Any colored paper that is not too smooth or shiny will do.Avoid bright, intense colors. Choose instead s<strong>of</strong>t green, gray,"Perhaps <strong>the</strong> most important point Ican make is that you are not tothink <strong>of</strong> painting as something separatefrom drawing."— Kim<strong>on</strong> NicolaidesThe Natural Way to Draw,1941Some basic informati<strong>on</strong> aboutcolor:The three main attributes <strong>of</strong> colorare:huevalueintensityHue is simply <strong>the</strong> name <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> color.This is <strong>the</strong> L-mode attribute.Value is <strong>the</strong> lightness or darkness <strong>of</strong>a hue, relative to <strong>the</strong> value scale.Value is an R-mode attribute.Intensity is <strong>the</strong> brightness or dullness<strong>of</strong> a hue, relative to <strong>the</strong> utmostbrightness available in pigments—generally color straight out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>tube. Intensity is an R-modeattribute.To balance color, remember <strong>the</strong>following:Every hue has its complement.For every hue <strong>of</strong> a given intensity,<strong>the</strong>re is <strong>the</strong> same hue at <strong>the</strong> oppositeintensity.For every hue <strong>of</strong> a given value,<strong>the</strong>re is <strong>the</strong> same hue at <strong>the</strong> oppositevalue.DRAWING ON THE BEAUTY OF COLOR 233


sand, blue, brown, or, as in Degas's dancer, s<strong>of</strong>t pink. You willneed a plastic eraser and a kneaded eraser. Buy a hand-held pencilsharpener, or a small knife if you prefer to hand-sharpen yourpencils.A wheel <strong>of</strong> colorStarting with rock-bottom basics, make a color wheel. Thethought <strong>of</strong> this probably takes you right back to sixth grade, butlet me assure you that some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> best minds in human historyhave delved into color wheels—for example, <strong>the</strong> great EnglishFig. 11-1. For <strong>the</strong> arrangement<strong>of</strong> colors, see Fig. 11-2.Fig. 11-2.234THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


physicist and ma<strong>the</strong>matician Isaac <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g>t<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong> German poetand scholar Johann Goe<strong>the</strong>.What is <strong>the</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>structing a color wheel? Simplyput, to set in your mind <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> color. The three primaryhues—yellow, red, and blue—are <strong>the</strong> basic building blocks <strong>of</strong>color. Theoretically, all o<strong>the</strong>r colors are derived from <strong>the</strong>se three.Next come <strong>the</strong> three sec<strong>on</strong>dary hues—orange, violet, andgreen—born <strong>of</strong> primary parents. And <strong>the</strong>n follows <strong>the</strong> third generati<strong>on</strong>,<strong>the</strong> six tertiary (third-level) hues—yellow-orange, redorange,red-violet, blue-violet, blue-green, and yellow-green.The color wheel has a total <strong>of</strong> twelve hues, arranged like <strong>the</strong>numbers <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> face <strong>of</strong> a clock.Use your colored pencils to match <strong>the</strong> color wheel (Figure 11-3) in <strong>the</strong> color secti<strong>on</strong>. You can trace <strong>the</strong> pattern in Figure 11-1<strong>on</strong>to a piece <strong>of</strong> b<strong>on</strong>d paper, or you can color directly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> patternin <strong>the</strong> book. Bear down hard with your colored pencils toproduce <strong>the</strong> most intense hues possible."Hues which approach red havealmost universally been c<strong>on</strong>sideredas warm colors and those whichtend toward blue as cool. Fire andsunlight and <strong>the</strong> glow <strong>of</strong> brisk circulati<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> blood are all associatedwith warmth."The colors <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sky and distantmountains and cool waters aregenerally bluish. When <strong>the</strong> body ischilled its color tends toward abluish hue. These reas<strong>on</strong>s naturallymake us associate red,orange, and yellow with warmth,and blue, blue-green, and bluevioletwith coolness."— Walter SargeantThe Enjoyment and Use <strong>of</strong>Color, 1923DRAWING ON THE BEAUTY OF COLOR 235


Psychologist Guy T. Buswell, in his1935 study, How People Look at Pictures,noted that although initialfixati<strong>on</strong> tends to be roughly in <strong>the</strong>center <strong>of</strong> a painting, <strong>the</strong> eye generallymoves first to <strong>the</strong> left and <strong>the</strong>nto <strong>the</strong> right. Dr. Buswell speculatedthat this is a carry-over from reading.Russian artist Wassily Kandinskyagreed with Buswell about centerto-left-to-rightscanning, but disagreedabout <strong>the</strong> reas<strong>on</strong>.Kandinsky's explanati<strong>on</strong>:"The picture is facing us, <strong>the</strong>reforeits sides are reversed. Just as whenwe meet some<strong>on</strong>e, we shake <strong>the</strong>irright hand—which is <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> left aswe face each o<strong>the</strong>r."Kandinsky c<strong>on</strong>tinued: "The leftside <strong>of</strong> an image is dominant,<strong>the</strong>refore, just as our right hand is(usually) <strong>the</strong> leading or str<strong>on</strong>gesthand."— W. KandinskyPoint and Line to Plane,1945Those <strong>of</strong> you familiar with color wheels will notice that Ihave used <strong>the</strong> usual order for colors <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> wheel: yellow at <strong>the</strong>top, violet at <strong>the</strong> bottom; <strong>the</strong> cool colors <strong>of</strong> green, blue-green,blue, and blue-violet <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> right side; <strong>the</strong> warm hues <strong>of</strong> yellow,yellow-orange, orange, red-orange, and red <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> left (see Figure11-2).I believe that this is <strong>the</strong> correct placement in terms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>complicated crossover system <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain, <strong>the</strong> visual system, and<strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong> art. The left side <strong>of</strong> an image is addressed by <strong>the</strong>(usually) dominant right eye, which is c<strong>on</strong>trolled by <strong>the</strong> lef<strong>the</strong>misphere (stay with me; it is complicated!). In <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong>art, <strong>the</strong> left side <strong>of</strong> an image carries <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> dominance,aggressi<strong>on</strong>, and forward movement. The right side,scanned after <strong>the</strong> left side, is addressed by <strong>the</strong> left eye, c<strong>on</strong>trolledby <strong>the</strong> right brain. The right side <strong>of</strong> an image, in <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong>art, carries <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> passivity, defensiveness, andblocked movement.In this zigzag fashi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere, right eye, and <strong>the</strong>left side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> color wheel are linked to <strong>the</strong> sun, daylight, andwarmth—and also to dominance, aggressi<strong>on</strong>, and forward movement.C<strong>on</strong>versely, <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere, left eye, and right side <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> wheel are linked to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>on</strong>, nighttime, and coolness—andthus also to passivity, defensiveness, and distance. Most colorwheels are oriented in this fashi<strong>on</strong>, apparently purely <strong>on</strong> intuiti<strong>on</strong>.Russian artist Wassily Kandinsky, <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> great colorists<strong>of</strong> art history, put his intuiti<strong>on</strong>s into words in <strong>the</strong> margin quotati<strong>on</strong>.The purpose, <strong>the</strong>n, <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>structing <strong>the</strong> color wheel is to set inyour mind which colors are opposite each o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> wheel.Blue is opposite orange, red is opposite green, yellow-green isopposite red-violet. These opposites are called complements.The root <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word "complement" is "complete." This meansthat complements form <strong>the</strong> closed system previously, proposed byDr. Peter Smith as a requirement for an es<strong>the</strong>tic resp<strong>on</strong>se. Perceivedtoge<strong>the</strong>r in proper relati<strong>on</strong>ship, complements seem to satisfy<strong>the</strong> needs <strong>of</strong> R-mode and <strong>the</strong> visual system for completi<strong>on</strong>.You can use your color wheel to practice determining which236 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


hues are complements. This knowledge should be learned sothoroughly that it becomes as automatic as 2 + 2 = 4.Taking <strong>the</strong> first steps in color drawingBefore you begin, please read all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s.I will use <strong>the</strong> Degas drawing <strong>on</strong> pink paper (Figure 11-6) as<strong>the</strong> basis for instructi<strong>on</strong>s, but please choose any subject thatappeals to you: a group <strong>of</strong> objects for a still-life drawing, a pers<strong>on</strong>who will pose for a figure drawing or a portrait, ano<strong>the</strong>r reproducti<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> a master drawing, a photograph that appeals to you, ora self-portrait (<strong>the</strong> artist always has <strong>on</strong>e available model!).1. Choose a sheet <strong>of</strong> colored paper, not necessarily pink.2. The original Degas drawing measures 16+1/8" x 11+1/4". Measureand lightly draw with pencil a format <strong>of</strong> that size.3. Choose two colored pencils, <strong>on</strong>e dark and <strong>on</strong>e light, in colorsyou feel harm<strong>on</strong>ize with <strong>the</strong> color <strong>of</strong> your paper.Some suggesti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> this point: If your paper is s<strong>of</strong>t blue,for example, choose pencils <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> opposite (that is, <strong>the</strong> complementary)hue—in this case, orange. Your choice, <strong>the</strong>n,could be flesh (pale orange) and dark brown (which is actuallya dark orange). If your paper is s<strong>of</strong>t violet, your choices couldbe cream (pale yellow) and dark purple (or burnt umber,which has a slightly violet cast). Degas used "s<strong>of</strong>t blackgraphite" (which has a slightly greenish cast) for his darkt<strong>on</strong>es, which he accented with black cray<strong>on</strong>, and a cool whiteto complement his (warm) pink paper.The brain's "need" for <strong>the</strong> complementis most clearly dem<strong>on</strong>stratedby <strong>the</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong> called "afterimage,"which is still not entirelyunderstood.To cause an after-image, color acircle <strong>of</strong> intense red about an inchor so in diameter. Make a tinyblack dot in <strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> red.Make a similar dot in <strong>the</strong> center <strong>of</strong>a sec<strong>on</strong>d, blank sheet <strong>of</strong> paper.Holding <strong>the</strong> two sheets side-byside,gaze at <strong>the</strong> red-hued circlefor about a minute. Then quicklyshift your gaze to <strong>the</strong> dot <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>sec<strong>on</strong>d, blank sheet. You will "see"<strong>the</strong> complement to red (green)emerge <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> blank paper <strong>the</strong>same shape, <strong>the</strong> same size as <strong>the</strong>original red circle.You can experiment with any hue,and your mind/brain/visual systemwill produce <strong>the</strong> exact complement<strong>of</strong> any hue. This is termed<strong>the</strong> negative after-image. If youexperiment with two hues, bothcomplements will appear. In someinstances, <strong>the</strong> original hue (called apositive after-image) will appear asan after-image, but in <strong>the</strong> negativespaces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> original shapes,which appear empty <strong>of</strong> color.An asideAn important point: have c<strong>on</strong>fidence in your color choices!Guided by some basic L-mode knowledge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong>color (for example, <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> complements), your R-mode willknow when color is right. Within <strong>the</strong> guidelines, follow your intuiti<strong>on</strong>.Try out <strong>the</strong> hues <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> back <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper. Then say toyourself, "Does that feel right?" and listen to what you feel. D<strong>on</strong>'targue with yourself—I should say, with your L-mode. We havelimited your choices to three: <strong>the</strong> paper and two pencils. GivenDRAWING ON THE BEAUTY OF COLOR 237


"Color can overwhelm.... Onemust understand that when itcomes to color less is <strong>of</strong>ten more—a less<strong>on</strong> taught us by <strong>the</strong> mastersbut ignored by many artists."—Joe SingerHovi to Paint in Pastels, 1976In his 1926 work, <strong>the</strong> color <strong>the</strong>oristAlbert Munsell stressed <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept<strong>of</strong> balance to create color harm<strong>on</strong>iesand established anumerical code which is still <strong>the</strong>most widely used system for identifyingcolor.Munsell recommended balancinghues with <strong>the</strong>ir complements, valueswith <strong>the</strong>ir opposite values,intensities with opposite intensities,areas <strong>of</strong> str<strong>on</strong>g color balancedby weak (low-intensity) color, largeareas balanced by small, warm colorsbalanced by cool colors.— Albert MunsellA Color Notati<strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong>se limits, you are sure to produce harm<strong>on</strong>ious color.Bear in mind that color most <strong>of</strong>ten "goes wr<strong>on</strong>g" when studentswithout knowledge <strong>of</strong> color use too many hues. They <strong>of</strong>tenthrow toge<strong>the</strong>r a variety <strong>of</strong> hues, chosen at random from <strong>the</strong> colorwheel. Such combinati<strong>on</strong>s are difficult—<strong>of</strong>ten impossible—tobalance and unify, and even beginning students sense that somethingisn't working. This is <strong>the</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> for limiting <strong>the</strong> palette in<strong>the</strong>se first exercises to a few hues and <strong>the</strong>ir related lights anddarks. And I encourage you to c<strong>on</strong>tinue to limit your palette untilyou have wider experience with color.Having said that, I will reverse <strong>the</strong> thought and suggest that atsome point, you may want to go wild with color, throwing everythingtoge<strong>the</strong>r to see what happens. Buy a sheet <strong>of</strong> brightly coloredpaper and use every color you have <strong>on</strong> it. Create discordantcolor. Then try to pull it toge<strong>the</strong>r, perhaps with dark or dull colors.You may be able to make it work—or you may like it in itsdiscordant state! Much <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>temporary art uses discordant colorin very inventive ways. Let me emphasize, however, that youshould attempt discordant color by design and not by mistake.Your R-mode will always perceive <strong>the</strong> difference, perhaps notimmediately, but over a period <strong>of</strong> time. Ugly color is not <strong>the</strong> sameas discordant color. Discordant color is not <strong>the</strong> same as harm<strong>on</strong>iouscolor. For <strong>the</strong>se first exercises, we shall c<strong>on</strong>centrate <strong>on</strong> creatingharm<strong>on</strong>ious color, because it more readily provides basicknowledge about color.Now, to c<strong>on</strong>tinue:4. Notice that Degas gridded his drawing with evenly spacedhoriz<strong>on</strong>tal and vertical guide lines, just as he gridded hisdancer without color <strong>on</strong> page 157. A grid with squares about2+1/2" will be about right for <strong>the</strong> size <strong>of</strong> your format.Try to follow Degas's thinking in his use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> grid: Whatpoints was he looking for? Note <strong>the</strong> obvious points <strong>of</strong> crossedgrid lines at <strong>the</strong> elbow and at <strong>the</strong> dancer's right toe.Start with <strong>the</strong> grid, using your dark-colored pencil tolightly draw <strong>the</strong> lines. Call up your new skills <strong>of</strong> drawing:edges, spaces, relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>s, and238 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


light logic. Use <strong>the</strong> grid as a boundary for <strong>the</strong> negative spacesaround <strong>the</strong> head, arms, hands, and feet. Use negative space todraw <strong>the</strong> ballet shoes. Carefully work out <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> head: Check <strong>the</strong> eye level and <strong>the</strong> central axis. Noticewhat a small proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole head is occupied by <strong>the</strong>features; do not enlarge <strong>the</strong>se features! Check <strong>the</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> ear (review proporti<strong>on</strong>s in Chapter Eight, if necessary).Complete <strong>the</strong> "dark" drawing before starting <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> "light."5. Now, for <strong>the</strong> fun part—<strong>the</strong> heightening <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing.Heightening is <strong>the</strong> technical term that refers to <strong>the</strong> technique<strong>of</strong> using pale-colored chalk or pencil to depict light falling <strong>on</strong>a subject.First, determine <strong>the</strong> logic <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> light falling <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>dancer. Where is <strong>the</strong> source <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> light? As you can see, thislight source is located just above <strong>the</strong> dancer and slightly <strong>of</strong>f toher left. Light falls <strong>on</strong> her forehead and right cheek. Her headthrows a shadow <strong>on</strong> her right shoulder, and <strong>the</strong> light streamsacross her left shoulder and falls <strong>on</strong> her chest and left breast.Bits <strong>of</strong> light fall <strong>on</strong> her left toe and right heel as well.Now use your light-colored pencil to heighten <strong>the</strong> drawing.You may need to alternately use your dark pencil todeepen <strong>the</strong> shadow-shapes. Grasp with your mind that <strong>the</strong>middle t<strong>on</strong>es are supplied by <strong>the</strong> value <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> colored paper.Try to see <strong>the</strong> color <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper as value. This is difficult.Imagine for a moment that <strong>the</strong> world has turned to shades <strong>of</strong>gray, as though twilight has fallen, draining color from yourpaper but leaving <strong>the</strong> value in <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> a gray. Where <strong>on</strong> avalue scale would that gray be, relative to white and black?Then, relative to that value, where is <strong>the</strong> darkest dark inDegas's drawing? Where is <strong>the</strong> lightest light? Your task is tomatch <strong>the</strong>se values in your drawing.When you have finished: Pin your drawing to a wall, standback, and enjoy your first small step into color. Some studentdrawings using colored pencil are shown in <strong>the</strong> color secti<strong>on</strong>. Asyou see, very few colors were used in each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawings. StudentThu Ha Huyung used <strong>the</strong> largest number <strong>of</strong> colors (fourplus black and white) in her Girl in a Flowered Hat (Figure 11-22).DRAWING ON THE BEAUTY OF COLOR 2 39"To me, painting—all painting—is not so much <strong>the</strong> intelligent use<strong>of</strong> color as <strong>the</strong> intelligent use <strong>of</strong>value. If <strong>the</strong> values are right <strong>the</strong>color cannot help but be right."—Joe SingerHow to Paint in Pastels, 1976Based <strong>on</strong> his teaching at YaleUniversity, <strong>the</strong> great colorist JosefAlbers wrote that <strong>the</strong>re are norules <strong>of</strong> color harm<strong>on</strong>y, <strong>on</strong>ly rules<strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> quantity <strong>of</strong> colors:"Independent <strong>of</strong> harm<strong>on</strong>y rules,any color 'goes' or 'works' with anyo<strong>the</strong>r color, presupposing that<strong>the</strong>ir quantities are appropriate."—Josef AlbersThe Interacti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> Color,1962Ano<strong>the</strong>r view <strong>on</strong> harm<strong>on</strong>y in color:"After learning to see color as value,<strong>the</strong> next step is learning to seecolor as color."— Pr<strong>of</strong>essor D<strong>on</strong> DameCalifornia StateUniversity, L<strong>on</strong>g Beach


A Heightened Self-PortraitA w<strong>on</strong>derful example for this exerciseis found in Figure 11-7, <strong>the</strong> selfportraitby <strong>the</strong> German artistKa<strong>the</strong> Kollwitz.Exercise:1. Set up lights and a mirror.Arrange your drawing materials sothat you can both draw andobserve yourself.2. Take <strong>the</strong> pose and spend a fewmoments studying <strong>the</strong> logic <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>lights and shadows created by yourlighting setup. Where is <strong>the</strong> lightestlight? The darkest dark? Whereare <strong>the</strong> cast shadows and <strong>the</strong> crestshadows? Where are <strong>the</strong> highlightsand <strong>the</strong> reflected lights?3. Lightly sketch your self-portrait<strong>on</strong> colored paper, checking<strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s carefully.4. Quickly paint in <strong>the</strong> negativespace, using black ink thinnedslightly with water and a fairlylarge brush (a <strong>on</strong>e-inch-widehousepainter's brush will do, withink poured in a small bowl).5. Use a dark colored pencil todefine features and shadows.6. Use a white or cream pencil toheighten <strong>the</strong> drawing, usinghatches that follow <strong>the</strong> curves <strong>of</strong>your face and features.The colors she used were canary yellow and ultramarine blue(near complements), magenta and dark green (near complements),and black and white (opposites).Thu Ha's color is harm<strong>on</strong>ious because it is balanced and colorsare repeated from area to area. (See Josef Albers's statementin <strong>the</strong> margin <strong>of</strong> page 239.) The pale magenta <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> lips isrepeated in <strong>the</strong> pink flower. The green <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> leaves reappears in<strong>the</strong> hair. The blue <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> blouse reappears in <strong>the</strong> eyes and hat.The black is used for <strong>the</strong> shadow-shapes, and <strong>the</strong> white heightens<strong>the</strong> lights. And, finally, <strong>the</strong> yellow <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hair is a lighter value <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> ochre paper that forms <strong>the</strong> ground and middle value.If you haven't yet tried a colored-pencil portrait <strong>on</strong> a coloredground, I urge you to find a model or to draw a self-portrait, following<strong>the</strong> suggesti<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> margin. Because <strong>the</strong> colored groundso beautifully supplies <strong>the</strong> middle-value t<strong>on</strong>es, you are sure toenjoy this project. With <strong>the</strong> middle-value ground in place, italmost seems that <strong>the</strong> drawing is half-complete before you start.Recall that in Chapter Ten your rubbed-graphite ground supplied<strong>the</strong> middle-value t<strong>on</strong>es, <strong>the</strong> eraser provided <strong>the</strong> lights, and<strong>the</strong> darkest dark <strong>of</strong> your pencil supplied <strong>the</strong> dark shadows. Thetransiti<strong>on</strong> from that drawing to drawing in color <strong>on</strong> a coloredground is a very short step.Ano<strong>the</strong>r project: An ugly corner as cityscapeYou might also enjoy trying a cityscape similar to <strong>the</strong> studentdrawing The Arrow Hotel in Figure 11-24. This drawing was <strong>the</strong>result <strong>of</strong> an assignment to my students to "Go out and find a trulyugly corner." (Regrettably, ugly corners are all too easy to find inmost <strong>of</strong> our cities.) Using <strong>the</strong> perceptual skills <strong>of</strong> seeing edges,spaces, and relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>s, students weredirected to draw exactly what <strong>the</strong>y saw—including signs, lettering,everything—placing great emphasis <strong>on</strong> negative space. Theproject was completed by following <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong>cityscape provided below.I believe you'll agree that ugliness was transformed intosomething approaching beauty in <strong>the</strong> student's drawing. This is240 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


ano<strong>the</strong>r instance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> transformative power <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist's way <strong>of</strong>seeing. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> great paradoxes <strong>of</strong> art is that subject matter isnot <strong>of</strong> prime importance in creating beauty.Directi<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong> cityscape:1. Find your corner, <strong>the</strong> uglier <strong>the</strong> better.2. Sit in your car to do <strong>the</strong> drawing, or use a folding stool to sit<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> sidewalk.3. You will need an 18" x 24" board to draw <strong>on</strong>, and an 18" x 24"piece <strong>of</strong> ordinary white paper. Draw a format edge about aninch in from <strong>the</strong> edges <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper. Use a pencil to draw <strong>the</strong>cityscape. A viewfinder and a transparent grid will help insighting angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>s.4. Use negative space almost exclusively to c<strong>on</strong>struct <strong>the</strong> drawing.All details, such as teleph<strong>on</strong>e lines, lettering, street signs,and girders, are to be drawn in negative space. This is <strong>the</strong> keyto success in this drawing. (But that is true for almost everybit <strong>of</strong> drawing that you do!) Remember that negative space,clearly observed and drawn, reminds <strong>the</strong> viewer <strong>of</strong> that forwhich we all l<strong>on</strong>g—unity, <strong>the</strong> most basic requirement <strong>of</strong> awork <strong>of</strong> art.5. When you have finished <strong>the</strong> drawing, return home andchoose a piece <strong>of</strong> 18" x 24" colored paper or colored cardboard.Transfer your <strong>on</strong>-site drawing to <strong>the</strong> colored paper,using carb<strong>on</strong> paper or graphite transfer paper, available in artsupply stores. Be sure to transfer your format edge to <strong>the</strong> coloredground.6. If you want to try a simple complementary arrangement asused in The Arrow Hotel, choose two colored pencils thatharm<strong>on</strong>ize with your paper, <strong>on</strong>e dark and <strong>on</strong>e light. The ArrowHotel provides a satisfying color scheme because <strong>the</strong> coloris balanced: <strong>the</strong> yellow-green <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper is balanced by<strong>the</strong> dark, dull red-violet pencil, and <strong>the</strong> light t<strong>on</strong>es aresupplied by <strong>the</strong> cream-colored pencil, which relates to <strong>the</strong>yellow-green ground and acts as a near-complement to <strong>the</strong>red-violet.About cityscapes, Americanabstract artist Stuart Davis said:"I am an American, born inPhiladelphia <strong>of</strong> American stock.I paint what I see in America."Some things that have made mewant to paint... skyscraper architecture,<strong>the</strong> brilliant color <strong>of</strong> gasolinestati<strong>on</strong>s; chain store fr<strong>on</strong>ts andtaxi-cabs; electric signs... EarlHines' hot piano and jazz music ingeneral."— Stuart Davis, 1943A half-serious cauti<strong>on</strong>: If you drawin a public place, you will so<strong>on</strong> bebesieged by spectators w<strong>on</strong>deringwhat in <strong>the</strong> world you are drawing—andwhy. I can't help you withthis problem.One thing is certain: A l<strong>on</strong>ely pers<strong>on</strong>need <strong>on</strong>ly to start drawing inpublic places to be l<strong>on</strong>ely no more.DRAWING ON THE BEAUTY OF COLOR24I


Because most people believe <strong>the</strong>yprefer bright colors, <strong>the</strong> following isa difficult c<strong>on</strong>cept to grasp:Just as negative spaces are equallyimportant as objects, dull colors(low-intensity colors) are equallyimportant as bright (high-intensity)colors.The simplest way to reduce <strong>the</strong>intensity <strong>of</strong> a given hue is to add aneutral gray or black. Thismethod, however, <strong>of</strong>ten seems todrain color from a hue in <strong>the</strong> sameway that twilight dims and weakenscolors.A sec<strong>on</strong>d way is to mix a color withsome <strong>of</strong> its complementary hue.This method seems to leave <strong>the</strong> colorunabated, and richly, str<strong>on</strong>glydull—not weakly dull. Low-intensityhues mixed this way greatlyassist in harm<strong>on</strong>izing colorschemes.Believing that <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d way ispreferable, my friend and colleaguePr<strong>of</strong>essor D<strong>on</strong> Dame, anexpert colorist, frequently refusesto allow his students to even buyblack.Expanding harm<strong>on</strong>ious colorWe have explored complementary color schemes in <strong>the</strong> exercisesabove. Two additi<strong>on</strong>al ways <strong>of</strong> arranging harm<strong>on</strong>ious color arem<strong>on</strong>ochromatic schemes and analogous schemes.M<strong>on</strong>ochromatic color, meaning variati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> a single hue, isan interesting experiment with color. Choose a colored paper anduse all <strong>the</strong> pencils you have in hues related to that color. In herUmbrella Still Life (Figure 11-17), student Laura Wright used variati<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong> a <strong>the</strong>me <strong>of</strong> orange—<strong>the</strong> color orange in all its transformati<strong>on</strong>s,from dark brown to <strong>the</strong> pale orange <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper.Analogous color is an arrangement <strong>of</strong> hues close to <strong>on</strong>eano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> color wheel—red, orange, and yellow; blue, bluegreen,and green, for example. Student Ken Ludwig's drawing,Large Stuffed Eagle (Figure, 11-18), is an analogous arrangement <strong>of</strong>red, red-orange, yellow-orange, and pink chalk rubbed into whitepaper. (Using pastel chalk is explained in <strong>the</strong> next secti<strong>on</strong>.) Kenused pen and black India ink in short, hatched strokes to draw <strong>the</strong>eagle. You might try this combinati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a rubbed chalk ground(which again supplies <strong>the</strong> middle value) and ink lines for a variety<strong>of</strong> subjects—animals, birds, flowers—to practice analogous color.Pressing <strong>on</strong> to a pastel worldYour next purchase should be a set <strong>of</strong> pastels, which are pure pigmentspressed into round or square chalks (sometimes called"pastel cray<strong>on</strong>s") using a minimum <strong>of</strong> binder. You can buy a basicset <strong>of</strong> twelve chalks (ten hues plus black and white) or a larger set<strong>of</strong> up to <strong>on</strong>e hundred hues. But be assured that <strong>the</strong> small basic setis sufficient for <strong>the</strong>se first exercises.I must warn you that pastels have some serious drawbacks.They are quite s<strong>of</strong>t and break easily. They rub <strong>of</strong>f <strong>on</strong> your handsand clo<strong>the</strong>s, spread colored dust wafting through <strong>the</strong> air, and producea drawing that is extremely fragile.But <strong>the</strong>re is a positive side. Pastels are almost pure pigment,and <strong>the</strong> colors are lovely—as clear and brilliant as oil paints. Pastels,in fact, are <strong>the</strong> drawing medium closest to painting. Pasteldrawings are <strong>of</strong>ten referred to as "pastel paintings."242THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Because pastels come in a wide range <strong>of</strong> pure and mixedhues, a student beginning in color can experience something veryclose to painting without <strong>the</strong> difficulties encountered in mixingpaints <strong>on</strong> a palette, c<strong>on</strong>tending with turpentine, stretching canvas,and dealing with o<strong>the</strong>r technical problems <strong>of</strong> painting.For many reas<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>the</strong>refore, pastels are an ideal medium toprovide a transiti<strong>on</strong>al midpoint between drawing and painting.For an example <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> proximity <strong>of</strong> pastels to painting, look at <strong>the</strong>exquisite pastel paintings by <strong>the</strong> eighteenth-century French artistJean-Baptiste-Sime<strong>on</strong> Chardin in Figures 11-9 and 11-10. Chardin,<strong>of</strong>ten called <strong>the</strong> "artists' artist," has portrayed himself in his greeneyeshade and his wife in her demure headdress. ExamineChardin's marvelous use <strong>of</strong> color, bold yet restrained. These twodrawings are masterpieces <strong>of</strong> portraiture and <strong>of</strong> pastel painting.One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> main differences between exercises with coloredpencil and pastel drawing is in <strong>the</strong> quantity <strong>of</strong> applied color relativeto <strong>the</strong> ground. Student Gary Berberet's Self-Portrait (Figure11-16) illustrates expanded use <strong>of</strong> color to c<strong>on</strong>struct <strong>the</strong> entireimage.For <strong>the</strong> exercise that follows, I will use as my model <strong>the</strong> pasteldrawing Head <strong>of</strong> a Young Girl, by <strong>the</strong> French painter Odil<strong>on</strong> Red<strong>on</strong>(Figure 11-15). Red<strong>on</strong>'s free use <strong>of</strong> pastel color in <strong>the</strong> negativespace <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing will inspire you to experiment with thismedium.Red<strong>on</strong>'s mystical and lyrical work spanned <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>nineteenth century and <strong>the</strong> beginning <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> twentieth. His pasteldrawings have been linked to <strong>the</strong> writing <strong>of</strong> Poe, Baudelaire, andMallarme, and all are c<strong>on</strong>nected c<strong>on</strong>ceptually to Surrealism, aperiod in early twentieth-century art that focused <strong>on</strong> dream symbolism.The yellow lizard in Red<strong>on</strong>'s drawing, juxtaposed to <strong>the</strong>dreamlike serenity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> girl's head, is reminiscent <strong>of</strong> Surrealistsymbolism.Before you begin, please read all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>s.1. Find a model or a suitable subject. Arrange a light so that <strong>the</strong>background is illuminated, providing a pale negative spacebehind your model's head.Surrealist artists were fascinatedby psychological meanings <strong>of</strong> colors.Oddly, each hue has both apositive and negative c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>in most cultures. For example, c<strong>on</strong>sider<strong>the</strong> following:White: innocence and ghostlinessBlack:restful strength anddepressi<strong>on</strong>Yellow: nobility and treas<strong>on</strong>Red:Blue:ardent love and sintruth and desp<strong>on</strong>dencyPurple: dignity and griefGreen: growth and jealousyDRAWING ON THE BEAUTY OF COLOR 243


To correct a mistake in pastel,begin by brushing <strong>of</strong>f <strong>the</strong> wr<strong>on</strong>gmarks with a paintbrush. Then usea kneaded eraser (a s<strong>of</strong>t malleableeraser available in craft or art supplystores) to "lift" or blot <strong>the</strong>color without rubbing. You caneven scrape <strong>the</strong> paper carefullywith a small knife, <strong>the</strong>n blot againand draw in your correcti<strong>on</strong>s.2. Choose a piece <strong>of</strong> pastel paper in any s<strong>of</strong>t color. Pastel paperhas a sharp "tooth" to grasp and hold <strong>the</strong> dry pigment. Red<strong>on</strong>used a s<strong>of</strong>t gray-blue paper.3. Choose a medium-dark pastel cray<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> line drawing <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> head. Choose three harm<strong>on</strong>izing light pastels for <strong>the</strong> lightnegative space behind <strong>the</strong> head.4. Pose your model and draw <strong>the</strong> head in semi-pr<strong>of</strong>ile—that is,with <strong>the</strong> model turned very slightly <strong>of</strong>f true pr<strong>of</strong>ile view.5. Calling <strong>on</strong> your five basic drawing skills, draw <strong>the</strong> head using<strong>the</strong> dark pastel you have chosen. (Red<strong>on</strong> used a sepia pastel, adulled violet.) Using your imaginati<strong>on</strong>, or using objects in <strong>the</strong>room, complete your compositi<strong>on</strong> by adding objects or parts<strong>of</strong> objects. (Red<strong>on</strong> added part <strong>of</strong> a clock—a recurrent Surrealistsymbol—and a falling lizard.)6. Using your three pale pastels, work up <strong>the</strong> negative space surrounding<strong>the</strong> head. Use crosshatching ra<strong>the</strong>r than filling <strong>the</strong>area solidly, so that light and air are retained in your drawing.A special point: Look at your three pale pastels and decide whichis <strong>the</strong> darkest (lowest) in value, which is in <strong>the</strong> middle, and whichis <strong>the</strong> lightest. Then use <strong>the</strong> lowest-value chalk for <strong>the</strong> first layer<strong>of</strong> hatches, <strong>the</strong> middle for <strong>the</strong> next, and <strong>the</strong> lightest for <strong>the</strong> lastand final layer <strong>of</strong> hatches. This sequencing <strong>of</strong> colors from darkfirst to light last is <strong>the</strong> sequencing required for most paintingmediums (with <strong>the</strong> excepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> watercolor, which is usuallyworked from light first to dark last). In working with pastels, <strong>the</strong>dark-to-light sequencing helps to keep your colors clear andfresh. Reversing this sequence can result in muddy color. Thispoint will help you to see why practice with pastels eases <strong>the</strong>transiti<strong>on</strong> to painting.7. Complete your drawing with bold colors <strong>of</strong> your choice. Youmay prefer to harm<strong>on</strong>ize your color by staying with complementsor analogous hues, or you may prefer discordant huesthat are anchored in <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong> by repeating or echoingareas <strong>of</strong> each color. (In Red<strong>on</strong>'s drawing, you will notice thateach <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> intense hues is echoed in <strong>on</strong>e or more additi<strong>on</strong>alsmall areas.)244 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Start your drawing now. You will need about an hour and perhapsa bit more to complete <strong>the</strong> drawing. Be sure to give yourmodel a rest at midpoint in <strong>the</strong> hour! Try to work without interrupti<strong>on</strong>,and ask your model not to c<strong>on</strong>verse with you while youare drawing. Your R-mode needs to be completely free <strong>of</strong> distracti<strong>on</strong>.When you have finished: Pin up your drawing, stand back,and regard your work. Check <strong>the</strong> balance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> color. Then turnyour drawing upside down and check <strong>the</strong> color again. If any hueseems to pop out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>, somehow not locked into<strong>the</strong> color arrangement, some slight adjustment needs to be made.The color may need to be repeated somewhere, or it may needdarkening, lightening, or dulling (by lightly hatching a bit <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>complement over <strong>the</strong> hue). Have faith in your judgment and inyour R-mode ability to perceive coherence—and incoherence.When <strong>the</strong> color is right, you will know it!On <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> purpose<strong>of</strong> painting, <strong>the</strong> French nineteenthcenturyartist Eugene Delacroixwrote:"I have told myself a hundred timesthat painting—that is, <strong>the</strong> materialthing called a painting—is no morethan a pretext, <strong>the</strong> bridge between<strong>the</strong> mind <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> painter and that <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> spectator."— Eugene Delacroixin Artists <strong>on</strong> Art, 1967Summing upIn this book, we have covered <strong>the</strong> basic skills <strong>of</strong> drawing: fromedges to negative spaces to relati<strong>on</strong>ships to lights and shadows tocolor in drawing. These skills will lead you directly to <strong>the</strong> world<strong>of</strong> painting and new ways <strong>of</strong> expressing yourself through art.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>s stand <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir own as works <strong>of</strong> art, and paintingsstand <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir own as works <strong>of</strong> art. But drawing also becomes part<strong>of</strong> painting—<strong>the</strong> underpinning, so to speak—just as languageskills become <strong>the</strong> underpinning <strong>of</strong> poetry and literature. So,drawing merges with painting and a new directi<strong>on</strong> beck<strong>on</strong>s. Yourjourney has <strong>on</strong>ly just begun.DRAWING ON THE BEAUTY OF COLOR 245


The Zen <strong>of</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>:<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> Out<strong>the</strong> Artist Within


"In <strong>on</strong>eself lies <strong>the</strong> whole world andif you know how to look and learn,<strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> door is <strong>the</strong>re and <strong>the</strong> keyis in your hand. Nobody <strong>on</strong> earthcan give you ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> key or <strong>the</strong>door to open, except yourself."—J. KrishnamurtiYou Are <strong>the</strong> World"The life <strong>of</strong> Zen begins with <strong>the</strong>opening <strong>of</strong> satori. Satori may bedefined as intuitive looking into, inc<strong>on</strong>tradicti<strong>on</strong> to intellectual andlogical understanding. Whatever<strong>the</strong> definiti<strong>on</strong>, satori means <strong>the</strong>unfolding <strong>of</strong> a new world hi<strong>the</strong>rtounperceived."— D. T. Suzuki, "Satori,"in The Gospel Accordingto ZenAT THE BEGINNING OF THIS BOOK I said that drawing is a. magical process. When your brain is weary <strong>of</strong> its verbalchatter, drawing is a way to quiet <strong>the</strong> chatter and to grasp a fleetingglimpse <strong>of</strong> transcendent reality. By <strong>the</strong> most direct meansyour visual percepti<strong>on</strong>s stream through <strong>the</strong> human system—through retinas, optic pathways, brain hemispheres, motor pathways—tomagically transform an ordinary sheet <strong>of</strong> paper into adirect image <strong>of</strong> your unique resp<strong>on</strong>se, your visi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>.Through your visi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> viewer <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing—no matterwhat <strong>the</strong> subject—can find you, see you.Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, drawing can reveal much about you to yourself,some facets <strong>of</strong> you that might be obscured by your verbal self.Your drawings can show you how you see things and feel aboutthings. First, you draw in R-mode, wordlessly c<strong>on</strong>necting yourselfto <strong>the</strong> drawing. Then shifting back to your verbal mode, youcan interpret your feelings and percepti<strong>on</strong>s by using <strong>the</strong> powerfulskills <strong>of</strong> your left brain—words and logical thought. If <strong>the</strong> patternis incomplete and not amenable to words and rati<strong>on</strong>al logic, ashift back to R-mode can bring intuiti<strong>on</strong> and analogic insight tobear <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> process. Or <strong>the</strong> hemispheres might work cooperativelyin countless possible combinati<strong>on</strong>s.The exercises in this book, <strong>of</strong> course, encompass <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong>very beginning steps toward <strong>the</strong> goal <strong>of</strong> knowing your two mindsand how to use <strong>the</strong>ir capabilities. From here <strong>on</strong>, having caught aglimpse <strong>of</strong> yourself in your drawings, you can c<strong>on</strong>tinue <strong>the</strong> journey<strong>on</strong> your own.Once you have started <strong>on</strong> this path, <strong>the</strong>re is always <strong>the</strong> sensethat in <strong>the</strong> next drawing you will more truly see, more truly grasp<strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> reality, express <strong>the</strong> inexpressible, find <strong>the</strong> secretbey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> secret. As <strong>the</strong> great Japanese artist Hokusai said,learning to draw never ends.Having shifted to a new mode <strong>of</strong> seeing, you may findyourself looking into <strong>the</strong> essence <strong>of</strong> things, a way <strong>of</strong> knowingtending toward <strong>the</strong> Zen c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> satori, as described in <strong>the</strong>quotati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> D. T. Suzuki. As your percepti<strong>on</strong>s unfold, you takenew approaches to problems, correct old mispercepti<strong>on</strong>s, peel248 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


away layers <strong>of</strong> stereotypes that mask reality and keep you fromclear seeing.With <strong>the</strong> power <strong>of</strong> both halves <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain available to youand <strong>the</strong> myriad possible combinati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> separate powers <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> hemispheres, <strong>the</strong> door is open to your becoming moreintensely aware, more capable <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trolling some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> verbalprocesses that can distort thinking—sometimes even to <strong>the</strong>extent <strong>of</strong> causing physical illness. Logical, systematic thinking issurely essential for survival in our culture, but if our culture is tosurvive, understanding how <strong>the</strong> human brain molds behavior isour urgent need.Through introspecti<strong>on</strong>, you can embark <strong>on</strong> that study,becoming an observer and learning, to some degree at least, howyour brain works. In observing your own brain at work, you willwiden your powers <strong>of</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong> and take advantage <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> capabilities<strong>of</strong> both its halves. Presented with a problem, you will have<strong>the</strong> possibility <strong>of</strong> seeing things two ways: abstractly, verbally, logically—butalso holistically, wordlessly, intuitively.Use your tw<strong>of</strong>old ability. Draw everything and anything. Nosubject is too hard or too easy, nothing is unbeautiful. Everythingis your subject—a few square inches <strong>of</strong> weeds, a broken glass, anentire landscape, a human being.C<strong>on</strong>tinue to study. The great masters <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> past and <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>present are readily available at reas<strong>on</strong>able cost in books <strong>of</strong> drawings.Study <strong>the</strong> masters, not to copy <strong>the</strong>ir styles, but to read <strong>the</strong>irminds. Let <strong>the</strong>m teach you how to see in new ways, to see <strong>the</strong>beauty in reality, to invent new forms and open new vistas.Observe your style developing. Guard it and nurture it. Provideyourself with time so that your style can develop and growsure <strong>of</strong> itself. If a drawing goes badly, calm yourself and quietyour mind. End for a time <strong>the</strong> endless talking to yourself. Knowthat what you need to see is right <strong>the</strong>re before you.Put your pencil to paper every day. D<strong>on</strong>'t wait for a specialmoment, an inspirati<strong>on</strong>. As you have learned in this book, youmust set things up, positi<strong>on</strong> yourself, in order to evoke <strong>the</strong> flightto <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r-than-ordinary state in which you can see clearly."Set yourself to practice drawing,drawing <strong>on</strong>ly a little each day, sothat you may not come to lose yourtaste for it, or get tired <strong>of</strong> it....Do not fail, as you go <strong>on</strong>, to drawsomething every day, for no matterhow little it is, it will be well worthwhile, and will do you a world <strong>of</strong>good."— Cennino CenniniIl Libro Dell'Arte, c. 1435THE ZEN OF DRAWING: DRAWING OUT THE ARTIST WITHIN 249


Through practice, your mind will shift ever more easily. Byneglect, <strong>the</strong> pathways can become blocked again.Teach some<strong>on</strong>e else to draw. The review <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> less<strong>on</strong>s willbe invaluable. The less<strong>on</strong>s you give will deepen your insightabout <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> drawing and may open new possibilities forsome<strong>on</strong>e else.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> skills six and sevenIn <strong>the</strong> Introducti<strong>on</strong>, I menti<strong>on</strong>ed that I have proposed two additi<strong>on</strong>alskills bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> five basic comp<strong>on</strong>ent skills <strong>of</strong> seeingedges, spaces, relati<strong>on</strong>ships, lights and shadows, and <strong>the</strong> gestalt.My colleagues and I have not found more than <strong>the</strong>se seven skillsover <strong>the</strong> past decade, and it's possible <strong>the</strong>re are no more. Again,mediums, styles, and subject matter form an endless study, and allseven <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> basic skills benefit by a lifetime <strong>of</strong> practice andrefinement. But for basic understanding <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> perceptualprocesses <strong>of</strong> drawing, <strong>the</strong> seven skills seem sufficient at this time.I'll briefly review skills six and seven.Perceptual skill six: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> from memorySkill six is essentially drawing from memory. Students yearn forthis skill, but it is difficult. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> is a visual task and mostartists have great problems drawing from memory except forthose images <strong>the</strong>y have drawn before. If some<strong>on</strong>e asked me todraw a picture <strong>of</strong> an antique railway engine, for example, I couldnot do that because I d<strong>on</strong>'t know what it looks like. If I could see apicture, or go to view <strong>the</strong> object, <strong>the</strong>n I could draw it. Curiously,this occasi<strong>on</strong>ally comes as a surprise to people who d<strong>on</strong>'t draw.They seem to think that an artist is some<strong>on</strong>e who can draw anything.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> from memory can be trained. The nineteenth-centuryFrench artist Edgar Degas, so <strong>the</strong> story goes, forced his studentsto study <strong>the</strong> model posing in <strong>the</strong> basement <strong>of</strong> a building and<strong>the</strong>n climb to <strong>the</strong> seventh floor to do <strong>the</strong>ir paintings <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> posedmodel. No doubt this was effective visual memory training!250 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


For training yourself in visual memory, <strong>the</strong> key is to decide toremember—in a sense, to take a visual "snapshot" <strong>of</strong> an imageyou want to retain in memory. This means developing your abilityto image—to see something with your mind's eye well enoughthat later you can "look at" <strong>the</strong> image. Then, using <strong>the</strong> first fiveskills, you draw <strong>the</strong> image "seen in <strong>the</strong> mind's eye."Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, whatever you draw will etch itself into yourmemory. Call up those images; see again <strong>the</strong> master drawings youhave studied, <strong>the</strong> faces <strong>of</strong> friends you have drawn. Image alsoscenes that you have never viewed, and draw what you seethrough your mind's eye. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> will give <strong>the</strong> image a life andreality <strong>of</strong> its own.Perceptual skill seven: The "dialogue"Skill seven takes us all <strong>the</strong> way to <strong>the</strong> art <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> museums,I believe. I briefly outlined some main aspects <strong>of</strong> this skill inChapter Ten, page 221. The artist has a vague idea, let's say, todraw a creature that never existed, perhaps a winged drag<strong>on</strong>. Theartist has a vague imagined image and begins to draw, making afew marks that perhaps indicate <strong>the</strong> head <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> drag<strong>on</strong>. Thosemarks trigger an imagined extensi<strong>on</strong> and elaborati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, say, <strong>the</strong>head and neck. The artist "sees" or envisi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>the</strong>se elaborateddetails <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper. The artist <strong>the</strong>n draws in <strong>the</strong> imagined extensi<strong>on</strong>with new marks. That triggers an expanded image, perhaps<strong>the</strong> body and wings, now "seen" <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> drawing. The artist is nowable to draw those parts. And so <strong>the</strong> drawing progresses as a result<strong>of</strong> this "dialogue" between <strong>the</strong> imagined creature in <strong>the</strong> drawingwhich <strong>the</strong> artist makes real with <strong>the</strong> pencil marks. This dialoguec<strong>on</strong>tinues until <strong>the</strong> artwork is finished.You experienced this skill to some extent in your light/shadow drawings, and you can now nurture this beginning. Youwill find it most satisfying, I assure you. One way to practice <strong>the</strong>dialogue is to find or make stained paper, stained perhaps withspilled c<strong>of</strong>fee or smeared paint or even mud. Let <strong>the</strong> paper dryand <strong>the</strong>n try to "see" images in <strong>the</strong> stains. Reinforce <strong>the</strong>se imageswith pencil or pen or colored pencil. This is <strong>the</strong> so-called "daTHE ZEN OF DRAWING: DRAWING OUT THE ARTIST WITHIN 251


"It is not to be despised, in my opini<strong>on</strong>,if, after gazing fixedly at <strong>the</strong>spot <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> wall, <strong>the</strong> coals in <strong>the</strong>grate, <strong>the</strong> clouds, <strong>the</strong> flowingstream, <strong>on</strong>e remembers some <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>ir aspects; and if you look at<strong>the</strong>m carefully you will discoversome quite admirable inventi<strong>on</strong>s.Of <strong>the</strong>se <strong>the</strong> genius <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paintermay take full advantage, to composebattles <strong>of</strong> animals and <strong>of</strong> men,<strong>of</strong> landscapes or m<strong>on</strong>sters, <strong>of</strong> devilsand o<strong>the</strong>r fantastic things."Le<strong>on</strong>ardo da Vinci (1452-1519)Vinci device." Renaissance artist Le<strong>on</strong>ardo da Vinci recommendedthat student artists should practice seeing fancifulimages in <strong>the</strong> stained walls <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> city in order to improve <strong>the</strong>irimaging abilities.Clearly, <strong>the</strong>se skills have o<strong>the</strong>r applicati<strong>on</strong>s. Use your imagingability to solve problems. Look at a problem from severalviewpoints and different perspectives. See <strong>the</strong> parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> problemin <strong>the</strong>ir true proporti<strong>on</strong>. Instruct your brain to work <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>problem while you sleep or take a walk or do a drawing. Scan <strong>the</strong>problem to see all <strong>of</strong> its facets. Image dozens <strong>of</strong> soluti<strong>on</strong>s withoutcensoring or revising. Play with <strong>the</strong> problems in <strong>the</strong> antic/seriousintuitive mode. The soluti<strong>on</strong> is very likely to present itself nicelywhen you least expect it.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> capabilities <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> right side <strong>of</strong> your brain,develop your ability to see ever more deeply into <strong>the</strong> nature<strong>of</strong> things. As you look at people and objects in your world, imaginethat you are drawing <strong>the</strong>m, and <strong>the</strong>n you will see differently.You will see with an awakened eye, with <strong>the</strong> eye <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artistwithin you.252 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Afterword:Is BeautifulHandwritinga Lost Art?TODAY, HANDWRITING IS NO LONGER asubject <strong>of</strong> interest. Like <strong>the</strong> times tables,moral sayings, and polite manners at tea parties,handwriting—if it is thought <strong>of</strong> at all—is relegatedto quaint customs <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> past century. Yetwhen I ask a group <strong>of</strong> people, "How many <strong>of</strong>you would like to improve your handwriting?"nearly all <strong>the</strong> hands go up. If I ask "Why?" <strong>the</strong>answers vary: "I want my handwriting to lookbetter ... to be more readable ... to be goodenough to be proud <strong>of</strong>."This resp<strong>on</strong>se has surprised me. Handwritinghas virtually been discarded as a school subject,at least bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> third or fourth grade.Out <strong>of</strong> curiosity, I scoured my home library <strong>of</strong>books <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> topics <strong>of</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>, school art programs,drawing, painting, art history, <strong>the</strong> brainand brain-hemisphere functi<strong>on</strong>s for indexForgers copy signatures upside down. This trick probablyworks for <strong>the</strong> same reas<strong>on</strong> that upside-down drawingworks. As an exercise, try copying <strong>the</strong> signaturesabove upside down.AFTERWORD: IS BEAUTIFUL HANDWRITING A LOST ART?


The Palmer method drill.entries <strong>on</strong> handwriting. I found nothing, not aword <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> subject.Next, I searched <strong>the</strong> university library,indexes <strong>of</strong> books <strong>on</strong> art educati<strong>on</strong>, drawing, andbrain functi<strong>on</strong>—again, nothing. Early educati<strong>on</strong>books, <strong>of</strong> course, had entries <strong>on</strong> teaching <strong>the</strong>alphabet letters and words, and I found a fewbooks specifically <strong>on</strong> handwriting—most <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>m published in England, where handwritingskills apparently still receive c<strong>on</strong>siderableattenti<strong>on</strong>. When I opened <strong>the</strong>se books andskimmed through <strong>the</strong>m, however, I was struckby my immediate reacti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> sinking dismay at<strong>the</strong> tedium <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> exercises. All <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> worstaspects <strong>of</strong> public educati<strong>on</strong> came flooding backto me as memories <strong>of</strong> boring tasks, boringlytaught, with no possibility <strong>of</strong> escape.And yet I know that handwriting is important,and <strong>the</strong> group resp<strong>on</strong>se I described aboveindicates that o<strong>the</strong>rs feel that way too. In fact, <strong>of</strong>all <strong>the</strong> ways we express ourselves n<strong>on</strong>verbally,n<strong>on</strong>e is quite so pers<strong>on</strong>al as our handwriting—so pers<strong>on</strong>al and important that our signaturesare legally protected as a mark <strong>of</strong> identity.Unlike o<strong>the</strong>r ways we use to express our individuality,we have sole ownership <strong>of</strong> our handwriting.It is a pers<strong>on</strong>al possessi<strong>on</strong> that no o<strong>the</strong>rpers<strong>on</strong> is allowed to use or imitate.In past centuries, handwriting was c<strong>on</strong>sideredan art. Every school had its master or mistress<strong>of</strong> penmanship, and in <strong>the</strong> nineteenthcentury much time and attenti<strong>on</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>sumedin perfecting <strong>the</strong> extravagant loops and swirls <strong>of</strong>Copperplate script. In America in <strong>the</strong> earlydecades <strong>of</strong> this century, our schoolchildrenassiduously studied <strong>the</strong> venerable Palmermethod, derived from a beautiful Spencerian254 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE Of THE BRAIN


script. By <strong>the</strong> late 1930s, however, <strong>the</strong> Palmermethod had given way to an unlovely manuscriptprinting called "ball-and-stick" letteringfor very young children, with a shift from letteringto cursive, or "real," writing by aroundfourth grade. This shift was mainly a matter <strong>of</strong>making joining marks between <strong>the</strong> "ball-andstick"letters.Resp<strong>on</strong>ding to educati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>the</strong>ories in <strong>the</strong>1940s and 1950s about encouraging individualityand avoiding rote learning, teachers encouragedeach child to use <strong>the</strong> style <strong>of</strong> writing that feltcomfortable, within limits <strong>of</strong> legibility and correctness<strong>of</strong> letter forms. Children had a choice<strong>of</strong> size and slant <strong>of</strong> letters, sometimes even <strong>the</strong>choice <strong>of</strong> staying with printing, and teachersexpected that each child's handwriting wouldmore or less settle down to a legible form.Beauty was not an issue. Legibility was sufficient.But writing is an art form. Using line, <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> most basic elements <strong>of</strong> art, handwriting canfuncti<strong>on</strong> as a means <strong>of</strong> artistic self-expressi<strong>on</strong>.Like drawing, handwriting employs certain c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>alforms that have agreed-up<strong>on</strong> meanings.Over centuries, <strong>the</strong> letters <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> alphabethave evolved into shapes <strong>of</strong> great beauty thatcommunicate verbally, yet at <strong>the</strong> same time canc<strong>on</strong>vey subtle n<strong>on</strong>verbal intenti<strong>on</strong>s and reflecti<strong>on</strong>s<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mind <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> writer/artist. This iswhat we have lost. In my opini<strong>on</strong>, legibility isnot enough. Educati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>the</strong>orists have soldhandwriting short.Can we regain this lost art? I think we can,by linking writing <strong>on</strong>ce again to <strong>the</strong> es<strong>the</strong>ticpurposes <strong>of</strong> drawing. There is little differencebetween making a drawing in line and "draw-The Palmer method is joined, looped, and linked.Ball-and-stick letters are round, unjoined, and upright.This is surely <strong>the</strong> low-water mark <strong>of</strong> handwriting—awkward to <strong>the</strong> hand, without flow, and totally unrelatedto <strong>the</strong> historical development <strong>of</strong> handwriting.AFTERWORD: IS BEAUTIFUL HANDWRITING A LOST ART?


Have s<strong>of</strong>t eyes and a gentle manner.Shodo painting by William Reed.ing" a signature, sentence, or paragraph. Thepurpose is <strong>the</strong> same: to c<strong>on</strong>vey informati<strong>on</strong>about <strong>the</strong> subject and to express <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>ality<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> writer/artist. This n<strong>on</strong>verbal expressi<strong>on</strong>is subc<strong>on</strong>sciously perceived and understoodby <strong>the</strong> reader/viewer. C<strong>on</strong>sider what WilliamReed, an expert <strong>on</strong>japanese calligraphy, has tosay:Shodo paintings [cursive calligraphy] are likepictures <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subc<strong>on</strong>scious mind. They are notfinal statements, but ra<strong>the</strong>r instant snapshots <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>ality at <strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong> writing. That pers<strong>on</strong>alitycan be developed and streng<strong>the</strong>nedthrough AY practice. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, carelesscalligraphy is also a form <strong>of</strong> practice, reinforcingbad habits and stunting <strong>the</strong> growth <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>ality.While we may never attain <strong>the</strong> disciplined aes<strong>the</strong>tic<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Oriental mind, surely we can bringbeauty back into handwriting—not <strong>the</strong> ornatebeauty <strong>of</strong> past centuries, but ra<strong>the</strong>r a modernbeauty <strong>of</strong> ease, clarity, and coherence. I will recommenda few general principles and a fewexercises, and I will hope against hope that youw<strong>on</strong>'t get that awful sinking feeling <strong>of</strong> boredom.I urge you, at <strong>the</strong> least, to give <strong>the</strong> exercises atry.The basic perceptual skills <strong>of</strong> writing/drawingWrite your signature three times. First, your usual signature;next, your best "hand"; last, your "odd hand"signature.1. First, review <strong>the</strong> short secti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> handwritingin Chapter Two. Then, <strong>on</strong> a sheet <strong>of</strong>plain paper, write your signature just as youusually sign your name.2. Underneath that signature, write your nameagain, this time using your most beautiful256 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


"hand." Write slowly, drawing <strong>the</strong> letterswith care.3. Last, write your name <strong>on</strong>e more time underneath<strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d versi<strong>on</strong>. This time,however, use <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand: If you areright-handed, use your left hand, and if lefthanded,use your right hand.Now, compare <strong>the</strong>se three "drawings." The lineexpresses everything, and <strong>the</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong> isvery clear. All you have to do is ask yourself, "Ifthree people <strong>of</strong> equal qualificati<strong>on</strong>s were toapply for a positi<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong>se were <strong>the</strong>ir signatures,who would get <strong>the</strong> job?"To improve your handwriting, <strong>the</strong>refore, <strong>the</strong>first step is to decide that it does matter; yourwriting sends a distinct message. The next stepis to think about what message you want to c<strong>on</strong>vey.Reliability? Intelligence? Masculinity?Femininity? Humor? Sophisticati<strong>on</strong>? Clarity?(These, <strong>of</strong> course, are all positive messages.Writing can also c<strong>on</strong>vey such negative messagesas carelessness, indifference, deviousness, laziness,instability, and egotism. But I'll assume youw<strong>on</strong>'t choose <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se qualities.)Keeping style in mind as a final goal, let ussee how <strong>the</strong> perceptual skills <strong>of</strong> drawing canhelp your handwriting to become more beautiful.As children grow and change, so will <strong>the</strong>ir handwriting.— Ornella SantoliHow to Read HandwritingChristopher Jarman designed this alphabet with <strong>the</strong>aim <strong>of</strong> using simple, ec<strong>on</strong>omical letters that can bewritten with any type <strong>of</strong> writing implement.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tours <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> alphabet1. The percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> edges: Try a Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>of</strong> your handwriting. Tape apiece <strong>of</strong> paper down. Choose a pen or pencilthat you like, with <strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> line thatfeels comfortable to you. Turn away from<strong>the</strong> taped-down paper, so that it is out <strong>of</strong>The "looped" style, based <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Palmer method.AFTERWORD: IS BEAUTIFUL HANDWRITING A LOST ART?


Student example <strong>of</strong> "pure" or "blind c<strong>on</strong>tour" handwriting.Signature <strong>of</strong> George III, king <strong>of</strong> England.An actual "blind c<strong>on</strong>tour" signature: George III, whenblind.Negative-space letters.Write your signature again. Using graph paper helps tosee <strong>the</strong> negative spaces.sight. Holding <strong>the</strong> pen or pencil, place yourwriting hand <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper and hold thisbook in <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, open to this page.2. Choose <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> alphabets illustrated hereand copy each letter, first <strong>the</strong> lowercase,<strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> capitals. Draw each letter veryslowly, millimeter by millimeter, at <strong>the</strong> sameslow pace that your eyes move al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tours, paying attenti<strong>on</strong> to each detailand observing <strong>the</strong> beauty <strong>of</strong> each form.3. When you have finished <strong>the</strong> alphabets, lowercaseand capitals, write your name threetimes, very slowly, visualizing in yourmind's eye <strong>the</strong> ideal forms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> letters.Then, turn and look at your writing. I thinkyou will be surprised. Even unable to seewhat you were writing, and even with <strong>the</strong>awkward positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>,you will find your handwriting improvedimmediately, becausejow were payingclose attenti<strong>on</strong> to details <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> letterforms. Noticehow beautifully spaced your letters are, andhow you stayed "<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> line," even thoughyou couldn't see what you were doing.4. Next, using <strong>the</strong> technique <strong>of</strong> Modified C<strong>on</strong>tour<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, repeat <strong>the</strong> exercise above.Place your plastic grid or a sheet <strong>of</strong> linedpaper under your writing paper, to provide aguideline. Place this book where you can see<strong>the</strong> examples <strong>of</strong> alphabets. Choose <strong>on</strong>e andcopy it letter by letter, drawing very slowly.Then, write your signature again threetimes, or copy a few sentences from <strong>the</strong> text.When you have finished: Compare your last "drawings"with <strong>the</strong> first. You will have made progressalready, simply by paying attenti<strong>on</strong> and slowingdown.THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Using <strong>the</strong> negative spaces <strong>of</strong>handwritingIn Japanese as well as in European/Americancalligraphy, <strong>the</strong> negative spaces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> letters areas important as <strong>the</strong> lines we generally think <strong>of</strong> asc<strong>on</strong>stituting <strong>the</strong> letters. Examine <strong>the</strong> alphabets,first for enclosed, rounded negative spaces: a, b, d,g, o, p, q.1. Practice <strong>the</strong>se rounded negative spaces. Trynot to think that you are drawing <strong>the</strong> letter o,for example. Think—decide!—that you aredrawing <strong>the</strong> space inside and that it is abeautiful shape, embraced by <strong>the</strong> line withits precise closure. Write your signatureagain, paying special attenti<strong>on</strong> to anyclosed, rounded negative shapes.2. Next search <strong>the</strong> lowercase alphabets forclosed, el<strong>on</strong>gated negative shapes, someabove <strong>the</strong> line, some below: kf,g,j, k, I, q,y, z.Draw <strong>the</strong>se letters now, again focusing <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> negative shapes. Try to make all <strong>the</strong>closed, el<strong>on</strong>gated negative shapes <strong>the</strong> samein size and in shape. Write your signatureagain, paying special attenti<strong>on</strong> to closed,el<strong>on</strong>gated negative shapes.3. C<strong>on</strong>tinue with each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> main shapes <strong>of</strong>spaces—for example, <strong>the</strong> negative shape <strong>of</strong>n, m, h, v, ID, y. These letters have moundednegative spaces. Draw a series <strong>of</strong> m's and n's,really c<strong>on</strong>centrating <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> negativemounds. Make each negative mound <strong>the</strong>same—same size, same shape.4. Try <strong>the</strong> open negative space <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> letters c, k,v, iv, and 2. Check <strong>the</strong> margin model for <strong>the</strong>exact shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se spaces.Fill in your loops to check <strong>the</strong>ir c<strong>on</strong>sistency <strong>of</strong> size.Each letter needs its own negative space.AFTERWORD: IS BEAUTIFUL HANDWRITING A LOST ART? 259


Decide <strong>on</strong> a slant, <strong>the</strong>n use sighting to keep <strong>the</strong> slantc<strong>on</strong>sistent.j. Try <strong>the</strong> pointed negative space <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> lettersi,j, t. Make sure that you place <strong>the</strong> dot over<strong>the</strong> i so that it precisely lines up with <strong>the</strong> tip<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> letter.6. Try <strong>the</strong> negative shapes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> "odd" letterss, r, x. Note that each letter can be visualizedin negative space in two ways:a. The interior negative spaces: <strong>the</strong> spacesinside <strong>the</strong> letters.b. The exterior negative spaces: <strong>the</strong> spacesoutside <strong>the</strong> letters.For exterior negative spaces, imagine a formatdrawn around each letter. For <strong>the</strong> "short" lowercaseletters, <strong>the</strong> basic format is a square. For letterswith ascenders ("tall" letters), imagine arectangle two boxes high, resting <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> line.For letters with descenders (g,y, etc.), <strong>the</strong> twoboxrectangle drops half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> rectangle below<strong>the</strong> line.The key point about <strong>the</strong> exterior negativespaces is that each letter needs its space (its format).Notice how <strong>the</strong> slanted letter fits inside<strong>the</strong> format. To practice exterior negative spaces,obtain a sheet <strong>of</strong> graph paper for ready-madeformats.Sighting a beautiful handIn art, <strong>the</strong> word relati<strong>on</strong>ships expresses a c<strong>on</strong>stant<strong>the</strong>me. As you have learned, art is relati<strong>on</strong>ship—partsbrought into beautiful relati<strong>on</strong>shipswith <strong>on</strong>e ano<strong>the</strong>r and with <strong>the</strong> whole, thus creatingthat most treasured attribute <strong>of</strong> art, unity.The same holds true for <strong>the</strong> art <strong>of</strong> handwriting.Precisely <strong>the</strong> same skills will shape your handwritinginto closely related parts, fitted into arhythmic, coherent, unified whole, thus creating260 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


eauty—beautiful handwriting.Recall that in learning to draw, you learned<strong>the</strong> skill <strong>of</strong> perceiving relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>of</strong> angles(angles relative to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stants, vertical andhoriz<strong>on</strong>tal) and proporti<strong>on</strong>s (in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>on</strong>eano<strong>the</strong>r). Let's apply that skill to handwriting.The main task is first to decide <strong>on</strong> a slant—an angle relative to vertical—and, sec<strong>on</strong>d, touse <strong>the</strong> slant without deviati<strong>on</strong>. This gives yourhandwriting a beautiful rhythm. More than anyo<strong>the</strong>r aspect, c<strong>on</strong>sistent slant will give yourwriting coherence and unity.It doesn't really matter what angle youchoose, but be aware that slant c<strong>on</strong>veys a message,subc<strong>on</strong>sciously understood by your reader.A slightly forward slant c<strong>on</strong>veys energy andmeasured, forward acti<strong>on</strong>. A backward slantc<strong>on</strong>veys cauti<strong>on</strong>, a c<strong>on</strong>servative pace. Anextreme forward slant c<strong>on</strong>veys eagerness, orperhaps a bit <strong>of</strong> recklessness. Perfectly verticalwriting c<strong>on</strong>veys sobriety, a bias toward formality.(Please be assured that <strong>the</strong>se ideas are nottaken from graphology. Graphologists haveg<strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong>f into fanciful <strong>the</strong>ories; for example,"Large loops <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> letter y indicate that <strong>the</strong>writer is greedy, because <strong>the</strong> loops look likem<strong>on</strong>ey bags." This is n<strong>on</strong>sense.)Slant <strong>of</strong> line is part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong> artand, without questi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong> line usedin handwriting is related to <strong>the</strong> principles <strong>of</strong>art—<strong>the</strong> basic precepts <strong>of</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>, balance,movement, rhythm, and placement. Just as artexpresses <strong>the</strong> intent <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist, so does handwriting.To practice c<strong>on</strong>sistent slant, place <strong>on</strong>e sheet <strong>of</strong> ruledpaper over ano<strong>the</strong>r.Decide <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s that are pleasing to you,<strong>the</strong>n use <strong>the</strong>m c<strong>on</strong>sistently.AFTKRWORD: IS BEAUTIFUL HANDWRITING A LOST ART? 261


In drawing, different styles <strong>of</strong> line have names: <strong>the</strong> boldline, <strong>the</strong> pure line, <strong>the</strong> repeated line, <strong>the</strong> lost-and-foundline, <strong>the</strong> nervous line, <strong>the</strong> hard line, <strong>the</strong> s<strong>of</strong>t line, andmany o<strong>the</strong>rs.C<strong>on</strong>sistency is <strong>the</strong> keyTo c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>sistency <strong>of</strong> slant and proporti<strong>on</strong>s,try <strong>the</strong> following exercises:1. Place <strong>on</strong>e sheet <strong>of</strong> lined paper over ano<strong>the</strong>r,with <strong>the</strong> lines <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> bottom sheet runningvertically, at right angles to <strong>the</strong> horiz<strong>on</strong>tallines <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> top sheet. Adjust <strong>the</strong> bottomsheet until <strong>the</strong> angle seems right to you.(You may want to try several differentslants.) Practice writing your signature, orcopying a paragraph <strong>of</strong> text, aligning <strong>the</strong>slant to a perfectly c<strong>on</strong>sistent angle. At <strong>the</strong>same time, focus <strong>on</strong> forming <strong>the</strong> negativespaces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> letters.2. The sec<strong>on</strong>d part <strong>of</strong> sighting relati<strong>on</strong>ships issighting proporti<strong>on</strong>s. In handwriting, thisaspect is sec<strong>on</strong>d in importance <strong>on</strong>ly to c<strong>on</strong>sistentslant. The main task is to decide <strong>on</strong>size relati<strong>on</strong>ships for your writing and to use<strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>sistently.There are several proporti<strong>on</strong>s you will need todecide <strong>on</strong>. First, try out some alternatives, <strong>the</strong>ndecide <strong>on</strong> a proporti<strong>on</strong>al space to leave betweenwords (<strong>the</strong> width <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> letter ois <strong>on</strong>e possiblechoice). Then, use that proporti<strong>on</strong>al spacingc<strong>on</strong>sistently. Decide <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> size relati<strong>on</strong>shipbetween short and tall letters, and use that relati<strong>on</strong>shipc<strong>on</strong>sistently. Decide <strong>on</strong> how far down<strong>the</strong> descenders will drop relative to <strong>the</strong> tall lettersand <strong>the</strong> short letters, and use that relati<strong>on</strong>shipc<strong>on</strong>sistently. The key word, <strong>of</strong> course, isc<strong>on</strong>sistency. But also keep in mind that <strong>the</strong>se relati<strong>on</strong>shipscarry subtle messages, as you can seein <strong>the</strong> examples <strong>on</strong> page 263.3. Practice sighting angles and proporti<strong>on</strong>s.Write your signature and copy a few sen-262 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


tences from <strong>the</strong> text. As you write, allowyour eyes to scan <strong>the</strong> whole picture you arecreating with your "drawing" to check that<strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships are c<strong>on</strong>sistent.Seeing <strong>the</strong> lights and shadows inhandwritingThis aspect <strong>of</strong> handwriting emerges from <strong>the</strong>"value" <strong>of</strong> your hand, <strong>the</strong> lightness or darkness—thatis, fineness or heaviness—<strong>of</strong> yourline, <strong>the</strong> closeness or distance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> individualletters to <strong>on</strong>e ano<strong>the</strong>r.Your writing tool, <strong>of</strong> course, affects <strong>the</strong> line.The most important point here is that youshould use a certain pen or pencil by choice, notby accident.I find it so odd that art students are <strong>of</strong>tenextremely fussy about having just <strong>the</strong> right pencil,perfectly sharpened, for drawing. But whenit comes to writing, <strong>the</strong>y will unthinkingly use<strong>the</strong> dullest, scratchiest pencil or pen. Each activitydeserves <strong>the</strong> same care. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, sketching,handwriting—it's all <strong>the</strong> same. In each, you areexpressing yourself.Therefore, I recommend that you try out<strong>the</strong> lightness or darkness <strong>of</strong> various kinds <strong>of</strong>pencils or pens, <strong>the</strong>n decide <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e that fits yourstyle <strong>of</strong> writing and c<strong>on</strong>veys <strong>the</strong> message youwant to send. A heavy, dark line, for example,c<strong>on</strong>veys power and muscular (or intellectual)strength. A thin, fine, precise line c<strong>on</strong>veys a finesensibility and elegance. A medium line thatvaries in width (from a flexible pen point, forexample) c<strong>on</strong>veys an aes<strong>the</strong>tic, almost poetic,pers<strong>on</strong>ality, a pers<strong>on</strong> aware <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nuances <strong>of</strong>meaning in visual informati<strong>on</strong>. A wide, sturdyC<strong>on</strong>scious choice <strong>of</strong> handwriting style gives youc<strong>on</strong>trol over <strong>the</strong> effect your writing has <strong>on</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs.AFTERWORD: IS BEAUTIFUL HANDWRITING A LOST ART? 263


Handwriting can stand c<strong>on</strong>siderable mauling before itbecomes entirely illegible, but why make it so hard foro<strong>the</strong>rs to read?... gives ease and pleasure to your reader.line c<strong>on</strong>veys a rugged, natural pers<strong>on</strong>ality, closeto <strong>the</strong> earth.Ano<strong>the</strong>r means by which <strong>the</strong> lights andshadows are c<strong>on</strong>veyed in handwriting is through<strong>the</strong> closeness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> letters. If you write <strong>the</strong> letters<strong>of</strong> words very closely, with <strong>the</strong> lines <strong>of</strong> writingspaced closely, your writing will be dark andclose. If you write letterforms that are spacedmore openly and your lines are far apart, yourhandwriting will be full <strong>of</strong> light and air.Dark writing is nei<strong>the</strong>r better nor worsethan light, but it ;> different. Again, <strong>the</strong> point is:What do you want to c<strong>on</strong>vey to <strong>the</strong> viewer <strong>of</strong>your handwriting? Dark writing c<strong>on</strong>veys intensityand passi<strong>on</strong>, like some<strong>on</strong>e whisperingintensely in your ear. Light writing c<strong>on</strong>veysopenness and enthusiasm, like some<strong>on</strong>e calling"Hello!" from across <strong>the</strong> room. The choice isyours, but it should be a c<strong>on</strong>scious choice.264 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


Summing upOnce you have absorbed and practiced <strong>the</strong> basicfundamentals <strong>of</strong> beautiful handwriting, you willbe free to develop your individual style. As yourhandwriting changes to a more artistic style, youwill find it interesting to observe <strong>the</strong> reacti<strong>on</strong>s<strong>the</strong>se changes cause. I think you will be pleasantlysurprised.I hope this brief review <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> expressivequalities <strong>of</strong> handwriting is helpful and inspiring.I believe <strong>the</strong> Japanese are right in <strong>the</strong>ir insistence<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> n<strong>on</strong>verbal messagesand in <strong>the</strong>ir c<strong>on</strong>victi<strong>on</strong> that <strong>the</strong> way we writeaffects our pers<strong>on</strong>alities.I urge my readers who are parents to letteachers know that you are interested in beauty,wherever it might be encouraged. Help teachersto understand that you want your children toexperience handwriting as an art form so that<strong>the</strong>y will know <strong>the</strong> joy <strong>of</strong> creating beauty insimple acts <strong>of</strong> daily life.I believe teachers will welcome your interestin beauty. Teachers, after all, are <strong>the</strong> very<strong>on</strong>es whose eyes and sensibilities are assaultedby ugly handwriting, <strong>the</strong> very <strong>on</strong>es who muststruggle with illegibility and with n<strong>on</strong>verbalmessages <strong>of</strong> disunity, carelessness, and indifference.Making <strong>on</strong>e's handwriting more beautifulmay seem a very small way to increase <strong>the</strong> totalamount <strong>of</strong> beauty in <strong>the</strong> world. But, still, <strong>the</strong>widest ocean is made up <strong>of</strong> very small drops <strong>of</strong>AFTERWORD: IS BEAUTIFUL HANDWRITING A LOST ART? z6 5


PostscriptFor teachers and parentsAs a teacher and a parent, I've had a very pers<strong>on</strong>al interest inseeking new ways <strong>of</strong> teaching. Like most o<strong>the</strong>r teachers and parents,I've been well aware—painfully so, at times—that <strong>the</strong> wholeteaching/learning process is extraordinarily imprecise, most <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> time a hit-and-miss operati<strong>on</strong>. Students may not learn whatwe think we are teaching <strong>the</strong>m and what <strong>the</strong>y do learn may not bewhat we intended to teach <strong>the</strong>m at all.I remember <strong>on</strong>e clear example <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> problem <strong>of</strong> communicatingwhat is to be learned. You may have heard <strong>of</strong> or g<strong>on</strong>ethrough a similar experience with a student or your child. Yearsago, <strong>the</strong> child <strong>of</strong> a friend whom I was visiting arrived home fromhis day at school, all excited about something he had learned. Hewas in <strong>the</strong> first grade and his teacher had started <strong>the</strong> class <strong>on</strong>reading less<strong>on</strong>s. The child, Gary, announced that he had learneda new word. "That's great, Gary," his mo<strong>the</strong>r said. "What is it?"He thought a moment, <strong>the</strong>n said, "I'll write it down for you." On aPOSTSCRIPT 267


little chalkboard <strong>the</strong> child carefully printed, HOUSE. "That'sfine, Gary," his mo<strong>the</strong>r said. "What does it say?" He looked at <strong>the</strong>word, <strong>the</strong>n at his mo<strong>the</strong>r, and said matter-<strong>of</strong>-factly, "I d<strong>on</strong>'tknow."The child apparently had learned what <strong>the</strong> word looked like—he had learned <strong>the</strong> visual shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word perfectly. Theteacher, however, was teaching ano<strong>the</strong>r aspect <strong>of</strong> reading—whatwords mean, what words stand for or symbolize. As <strong>of</strong>ten happens,what <strong>the</strong> teacher had taught and what Gary had learnedwere strangely inc<strong>on</strong>gruent.As it turned out, my friend's s<strong>on</strong> always learned visual materialbest and fastest, a mode <strong>of</strong> learning c<strong>on</strong>sistently preferred by acertain number <strong>of</strong> students. Unfortunately, <strong>the</strong> school world ismainly a verbal, symbolic world, and learners like Gary mustadjust, that is, put aside <strong>the</strong>ir best way <strong>of</strong> learning and learn <strong>the</strong>way <strong>the</strong> school decrees. My friend's child, fortunately, was able tomake this change, but how many o<strong>the</strong>r students are lost al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>way?This forced shift in learning style must be somewhat comparableto a forced change in handedness. It was a comm<strong>on</strong> practicein former times to make individuals who were naturally lefthandedchange over to right-handedness. In <strong>the</strong> future, we maycome to regard forcing children to change <strong>the</strong>ir natural learningmodes with <strong>the</strong> same dismay that we now regard <strong>the</strong> idea <strong>of</strong> forcinga change in handedness. So<strong>on</strong> we may be able to test childrento determine <strong>the</strong>ir best learning styles and choose from a repertoire<strong>of</strong> teaching methods to ensure that children learn both visuallyand verbally.Teachers have always known that children learn in differentways and, for a l<strong>on</strong>g time now, people who have <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>sibilityfor educating youngsters have hoped that <strong>the</strong> advances in brainresearch would shed some light <strong>on</strong> how to teach all studentsequally well. Until about fifteen years ago, new discoveries about<strong>the</strong> brain seemed to be useful mainly to science. But <strong>the</strong>se discoveriesare now being applied to o<strong>the</strong>r fields and <strong>the</strong> recent researchthat I've outlined in this book promises to provide a firm basis forfundamental changes in techniques <strong>of</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>.268 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


David Galin, am<strong>on</strong>g o<strong>the</strong>r researchers, has pointed out thatteachers have three main tasks: first, to train both hemispheres—not <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> verbal, symbolic, logical left hemisphere, which hasalways been trained in traditi<strong>on</strong>al educati<strong>on</strong>, but also <strong>the</strong> spatial,relati<strong>on</strong>al, holistic right hemisphere, which is largely neglected intoday's schools; sec<strong>on</strong>d, to train students to use <strong>the</strong> cognitive stylesuited to <strong>the</strong> task at hand; and third, to train students to be able tobring both styles—both hemispheres—to bear <strong>on</strong> a problem in anintegrated manner.When teachers can pair <strong>the</strong> complementary modes or fit <strong>on</strong>emode to <strong>the</strong> appropriate task, teaching and learning will becomea much more precise process. Ultimately, <strong>the</strong> goal will be todevelop both halves <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain. Both modes are necessary forfull human functi<strong>on</strong>ing and both are necessary for creative work<strong>of</strong> all kinds, whe<strong>the</strong>r writing or painting, developing a new <strong>the</strong>oryin physics, or dealing with envir<strong>on</strong>mental problems.This is a difficult goal to present to teachers, coming as it doesat a time when educati<strong>on</strong> is under attack from many quarters. Butour society is changing rapidly, and <strong>the</strong> difficulties <strong>of</strong> foreseeingwhat kinds <strong>of</strong> skills future generati<strong>on</strong>s will require are increasing.Although we have so far depended <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> rati<strong>on</strong>al, left half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>human brain to plan for our children's future and to solve <strong>the</strong>problems <strong>the</strong>y might encounter <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> way to that future, <strong>the</strong><strong>on</strong>slaught <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ound change is shaking our c<strong>on</strong>fidence in technologicalthinking and in <strong>the</strong> old methods <strong>of</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>. Withoutaband<strong>on</strong>ing training in traditi<strong>on</strong>al verbal and computati<strong>on</strong>alskills, c<strong>on</strong>cerned teachers are looking for teaching techniquesthat will enhance children's intuitive and creative powers, thuspreparing students to meet new challenges with flexibility, inventiveness,and imaginati<strong>on</strong> and with <strong>the</strong> ability to grasp complexarrays <strong>of</strong> interc<strong>on</strong>nected ideas and facts, to perceive underlyingpatterns <strong>of</strong> events, and to see old problems in new ways.What can you, as parents and teachers, hope to accomplishright now in terms <strong>of</strong> teaching both halves <strong>of</strong> children's brains?First, it's important that you know <strong>the</strong> specialized functi<strong>on</strong>s andstyles <strong>of</strong> our hemispheres. Books such as this <strong>on</strong>e can provide youwith a basic understanding <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory and also with <strong>the</strong> expe-POSTSCRIPT 269


ience <strong>of</strong> making cognitive shifts from <strong>on</strong>e mode to ano<strong>the</strong>r.I believe that this pers<strong>on</strong>al experiential knowledge is extremelyimportant, perhaps essential, before teachers try to transmit <strong>the</strong>knowledge to o<strong>the</strong>rs.Sec<strong>on</strong>d, you need to help students to become aware <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>different ways <strong>the</strong>y can resp<strong>on</strong>d to <strong>the</strong> same material. For example,you might have students read <strong>on</strong>e passage for facts and askfor verbal or written resp<strong>on</strong>ses. The same passage might <strong>the</strong>n beread for meaning or underlying c<strong>on</strong>tent accessible throughimagery and metaphoric thought. For this learning mode, youmight require as a resp<strong>on</strong>se a poem, painting, dance, riddle, pun,fable, or s<strong>on</strong>g. As ano<strong>the</strong>r example, certain kinds <strong>of</strong> arithmeticand ma<strong>the</strong>matics problems require linear, logical thought. O<strong>the</strong>rsrequire imaginary rotati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> forms in space or manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>numbers, which are best accomplished by mentally producingpatterned visualizati<strong>on</strong>s. Try to discover—ei<strong>the</strong>r through notingyour own thought processes or observing your students—whichtasks utilize <strong>the</strong> style <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere, which require <strong>the</strong>style <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> left, and which require complementary or simultaneousstyles.Third, you might experiment with varying <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s inyour classroom—at least those c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s over which you havesome c<strong>on</strong>trol. For example, talking am<strong>on</strong>g students or c<strong>on</strong>stanttalking by a teacher probably tends to lock students fairly rigidlyinto left-hemisphere mode. If you can cause your students tomake a str<strong>on</strong>g shift to R-mode, you will have a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> that isvery rare in modern classrooms: silence. Not <strong>on</strong>ly will <strong>the</strong> studentsbe silent, <strong>the</strong>y will be engaged in <strong>the</strong> task at hand, attentiveand c<strong>on</strong>fident, alert and c<strong>on</strong>tent. Learning becomes pleasurable.This aspect al<strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> R-mode is worth striving for. Be sure thatyou yourself encourage and maintain this silence.As additi<strong>on</strong>al suggesti<strong>on</strong>s, you might experiment with rearranging<strong>the</strong> seating or <strong>the</strong> lighting. Physical movement, especiallypatterned movement such as dancing, might help to produce <strong>the</strong>cognitive shift. Music is c<strong>on</strong>ducive to R-mode shifts. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> andpainting, as you have seen in this book, produce str<strong>on</strong>g shifts toR-mode. You might experiment with private languages, perhaps270 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


inventing a pictorial language with which <strong>the</strong> students can communicatein your classroom. I recommend using <strong>the</strong> chalkboardas much as possible—not just to write words but also to draw pictures,diagrams, illustrati<strong>on</strong>s, and patterns. Ideally, all informati<strong>on</strong>should be presented in at least two modes: verbal and pictographic.You might experiment with reducing <strong>the</strong> verbal c<strong>on</strong>tent<strong>of</strong> your teaching by substituting n<strong>on</strong>verbal communicati<strong>on</strong> whenthat mode seems suitable.Last, I hope you will c<strong>on</strong>sciously use your intuitive powers todevelop teaching methods and communicate those methods too<strong>the</strong>r teachers through workshops or teachers' journals. You areprobably already using many techniques—intuitively or by c<strong>on</strong>sciousdesign— that cause cognitive shifts. As teachers, we needto share our discoveries, just as we share <strong>the</strong> goal <strong>of</strong> a balanced,integrated, whole-brain future for our children.As parents, we can do a great deal to fur<strong>the</strong>r this goal by helpingour children develop alternate ways <strong>of</strong> knowing <strong>the</strong> world—verbally/analytically and visually/spatially. During <strong>the</strong> crucialearly years, parents can help to shape a child's life in such a waythat words do not completely mask o<strong>the</strong>r kinds <strong>of</strong> reality. Mymost urgent suggesti<strong>on</strong>s to parents are c<strong>on</strong>cerned with <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong>words, or ra<strong>the</strong>r, not using words.I believe that most <strong>of</strong> us are too quick to name things whenwe are with small children. By simply naming a thing and lettingit go at that when a child asks, "What is that?" we communicatethat <strong>the</strong> name or label is <strong>the</strong> most important thing, that naming issufficient. We deprive our children <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir sense <strong>of</strong> w<strong>on</strong>der anddiscovery by labeling and categorizing things in <strong>the</strong> physicalworld. Instead <strong>of</strong> merely naming a tree, for example, try alsoguiding your child through an explorati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tree both physicallyand mentally. This explorati<strong>on</strong> may include touching,smelling, seeing from various angles, comparing <strong>on</strong>e tree withano<strong>the</strong>r, imagining <strong>the</strong> inside <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tree and <strong>the</strong> parts underground,listening to <strong>the</strong> leaves, viewing <strong>the</strong> tree at different times<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> day or during different seas<strong>on</strong>s, planting its seeds, observinghow o<strong>the</strong>r creatures—birds, moths, bugs—use <strong>the</strong> tree, andso <strong>on</strong>. After discovering that every object is fascinating and com-POSTSCRIPT 271


<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>s by a fourth-gradestudent: Three less<strong>on</strong>s, April 15 toApril 19,1977. Instructi<strong>on</strong>al period:four days.plex, a child will begin to understand that <strong>the</strong> label is <strong>on</strong>ly a smallpart <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> whole. Thus taught, a child's sense <strong>of</strong> w<strong>on</strong>der will survive,even under our modern avalanche <strong>of</strong> words.In terms <strong>of</strong> encouraging your child's artistic abilities, I recommendproviding a very young child with plenty <strong>of</strong> art materialsand <strong>the</strong> kind <strong>of</strong> perceptual experiences described above. Yourchild will progress through <strong>the</strong> developmental sequence <strong>of</strong> childart in a relatively predictable manner, just as children progressthrough o<strong>the</strong>r sequential stages. If your child asks for help with adrawing, your resp<strong>on</strong>se should be, "Let's go look at what you'retrying to draw." <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> percepti<strong>on</strong>s will <strong>the</strong>n become part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>symbolic representati<strong>on</strong>s.Both teachers and parents can help with <strong>the</strong> problems <strong>of</strong> adolescentartists, which I discussed in <strong>the</strong> text. As I menti<strong>on</strong>ed, realisticdrawing is a stage that children need to pass through ataround age ten. Children want to learn to see, and <strong>the</strong>y deserveall <strong>the</strong> help <strong>the</strong>y require. The sequence <strong>of</strong> exercises in thisbook—including <strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> hemisphere functi<strong>on</strong>s insomewhat simplified form—can be used with children as youngas eight or nine. Subjects that suit <strong>the</strong> interests <strong>of</strong> adolescents (forexample, well-drawn realistic carto<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> heroes and heroines inacti<strong>on</strong> poses) can be used for upside-down drawing. Negativespace and c<strong>on</strong>tour drawing also appeal to children at this age, and<strong>the</strong>y readily incorporate <strong>the</strong> techniques into <strong>the</strong>ir drawing. (See<strong>the</strong> illustrati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a ten-year-old fourth-grade student's progressover four days <strong>of</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>.) Portrait drawing has a specialappeal for this age group, and adolescents can do quite accomplisheddrawings <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir friends or family members. Once <strong>the</strong>yovercome <strong>the</strong>ir fear <strong>of</strong> failure at drawing, youngsters will workhard to perfect <strong>the</strong>ir skills, and success enhances <strong>the</strong>ir self-c<strong>on</strong>ceptand self-c<strong>on</strong>fidence.But more important for <strong>the</strong> future, drawing, as you havelearned through <strong>the</strong> exercises in this book, is an effective way <strong>of</strong>gaining access to and c<strong>on</strong>trol over <strong>the</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere.Learning to see through drawing may help children tolater become adults who will put <strong>the</strong> whole brain to use.272 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


For art studentsMany successful c<strong>on</strong>temporary artists believe that realistic drawingskills are not important. It is true, generally speaking, thatc<strong>on</strong>temporary art does not necessarily require drawing skill, andgood art—even great art—has been produced by modern artistswho can't draw. They are able to produce good art, I suspect,because <strong>the</strong>ir aes<strong>the</strong>tic sensibilities have been cultivated bymeans o<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> traditi<strong>on</strong>al, basic teaching methods <strong>of</strong> artschools: drawing and painting from <strong>the</strong> model, <strong>the</strong> still life, and<strong>the</strong> landscape.Since c<strong>on</strong>temporary artists <strong>of</strong>ten dismiss drawing ability asunnecessary, beginning art students are placed in a double bind.Very few students feel secure enough about <strong>the</strong>ir creative abilitiesand about <strong>the</strong>ir chances for success in <strong>the</strong> art world to dispensealtoge<strong>the</strong>r with schooling in art. Yet when <strong>the</strong>y encounter<strong>the</strong> kind <strong>of</strong> modern art shown in galleries and museums—art thatdoesn't appear to require traditi<strong>on</strong>al skills at all—<strong>the</strong>y feel thattraditi<strong>on</strong>al methods <strong>of</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong> d<strong>on</strong>'t apply to <strong>the</strong>ir goals. Tobreak <strong>the</strong> double bind, students <strong>of</strong>ten avoid learning to draw realisticallyand settle as quickly as possible into narrow c<strong>on</strong>ceptualstyles, emulating c<strong>on</strong>temporary artists who <strong>of</strong>ten strive for aunique, repeatable, recognizable "signature" style.The English artist David Hockney calls this narrowing <strong>of</strong>opti<strong>on</strong>s a trap for artists (see <strong>the</strong> quotati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> margin). It issurely a dangerous trap for art students, who too <strong>of</strong>ten force<strong>the</strong>mselves to settle into repetitive motifs. They may try to makestatements with art before <strong>the</strong>y know what <strong>the</strong>y have to say.Based <strong>on</strong> my teaching experience with art students at variousskill levels, I'd like to make several recommendati<strong>on</strong>s to all artstudents, especially beginning art students. First, d<strong>on</strong>'t be afraidto learn to draw realistically. Gaining skills in drawing, <strong>the</strong> basicskill <strong>of</strong> all art, has never blocked <strong>the</strong> sources <strong>of</strong> creativity. Picasso,who could draw like an angel, is a prime illustrati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> this fact,and <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> art is replete with o<strong>the</strong>rs. Artists who learn todraw well d<strong>on</strong>'t always produce boring and pedantic realistic art.The artists who do produce such art would no doubt produce"To me, moving into more naturalismwas a freedom. I thought, if Iwant to I could paint a portrait;this is what I mean by freedom.Tomorrow if I want, I could get up,I could do a drawing <strong>of</strong> some<strong>on</strong>e, Icould draw my mo<strong>the</strong>r from memory,I could even paint a strangelittle abstract picture. It would allfit in to my c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> painting asan art. A lot <strong>of</strong> painters can't dothat—<strong>the</strong>ir c<strong>on</strong>cept is completelydifferent. It's too narrow; <strong>the</strong>ymake it much too narrow. A lot <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>m, like Frank Stella, who toldme so, can't draw at all. But <strong>the</strong>reare probably older painters, Englishabstract painters, who weretrained to draw. Anybody who'dbeen in art school before I hadmust have d<strong>on</strong>e a c<strong>on</strong>siderableamount <strong>of</strong> drawing. To me, a lot <strong>of</strong>painters were trapping <strong>the</strong>mselves;<strong>the</strong>y were picking such a narrowaspect <strong>of</strong> painting and specializingin it. And it's a trap. Now <strong>the</strong>re'snothing wr<strong>on</strong>g with <strong>the</strong> trap if youhave <strong>the</strong> courage to just leave it,but that takes a lot <strong>of</strong> courage."— David HockneyPOSTSCRIPT 273


oring and pedantic abstract or n<strong>on</strong>objective art as well. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>skill will never hinder your work but will certainly help it.Sec<strong>on</strong>d, be clear in your mind about why learning to drawwell is important. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> enables you to see in that special,epiphanous way that artists see, no matter what style you chooseto express your special insight. Your goal in drawing should be toencounter <strong>the</strong> reality <strong>of</strong> experience—to see ever more clearly,ever more deeply. True, you may sharpen your aes<strong>the</strong>tic sensibilitiesin ways o<strong>the</strong>r than drawing, such as meditati<strong>on</strong>, reading, ortravel. But it's my belief that for an artist <strong>the</strong>se o<strong>the</strong>r ways arechancier and less efficient. As an artist you will be most likely touse a visual means <strong>of</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong>, and drawing sharpens <strong>the</strong> visualsenses.And last, draw every day. Carrying a small sketchbook willhelp you remember to draw frequently. Draw anything—an ashtray,a half-eaten apple, a pers<strong>on</strong>, a twig. I repeat this recommendati<strong>on</strong>given in <strong>the</strong> last chapter <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> text because for art studentsit is especially important. In a way, art is like athletics: If you d<strong>on</strong>'tpractice, <strong>the</strong> visual sense quickly gets flabby and out <strong>of</strong> shape.The purpose <strong>of</strong> your daily sketchbook drawing is not to producefinished drawings, just as <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> jogging is not to getsomewhere. You must exercise your visi<strong>on</strong> without caring overlymuch about <strong>the</strong> products <strong>of</strong> your practice. You can periodicallycull <strong>the</strong> best examples from your drawings, throwing out <strong>the</strong> restor even throwing out everything. In your daily drawing sessi<strong>on</strong>s,<strong>the</strong> desired goal should be to see ever more deeply.274 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


GlossaryAbstract Art. A translati<strong>on</strong> into drawing, painting, sculpture,or design <strong>of</strong> a real-life object or experience. Usuallyimplies <strong>the</strong> isolati<strong>on</strong>, emphasis, or exaggerati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> someaspect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist's percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> reality. Should not bec<strong>on</strong>fused with n<strong>on</strong>objective art.Awareness. C<strong>on</strong>sciousness; <strong>the</strong> act <strong>of</strong> "taking account" <strong>of</strong> anobject, pers<strong>on</strong>, or <strong>the</strong> surroundings. Possible syn<strong>on</strong>ymsare seeing or cogniti<strong>on</strong>.Basic Unit. A "starting shape" or "starting unit" chosen fromwithin a compositi<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> maintainingcorrect size relati<strong>on</strong>ships in a drawing. The Basic Unit isalways termed "One" and becomes part <strong>of</strong> a ratio, as in"1:2."Blank. An egg-shaped oval, drawn <strong>on</strong> paper to represent <strong>the</strong>basic shape <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> human head. Because <strong>the</strong> humanskull, seen from <strong>the</strong> side, is a different shape than <strong>the</strong>skull seen from <strong>the</strong> fr<strong>on</strong>t, <strong>the</strong> side-view blank is a somewhatdifferently shaped oval than fr<strong>on</strong>t-view blank.Central Axis. Human features are more or less symmetricaland are bisected by an imaginary vertical line in <strong>the</strong>middle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face. This line is called <strong>the</strong> central axis. Itis used in drawing to determine <strong>the</strong> tilt <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head andto place <strong>the</strong> features.Cerebral Hemispheres. The outermost part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> forebrain,clearly separated into two halves <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> right andleft sides <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain. C<strong>on</strong>sists essentially <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cerebralcortex, corpus callosum, basal ganglia, and limbicsystem.Cerebrum. The main divisi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain in vertebrates,c<strong>on</strong>sisting <strong>of</strong> two hemispheres. It is <strong>the</strong> last part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>brain to evolve and is <strong>of</strong> critical importance in all kinds<strong>of</strong> mental activity.Cognitive Shift. A transformati<strong>on</strong> from <strong>on</strong>e mental state toano<strong>the</strong>r, e.g., from L-mode to R-mode or vice versa.Compositi<strong>on</strong>. An ordered relati<strong>on</strong>ship am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> parts orelements <strong>of</strong> a work <strong>of</strong> art. In drawing, <strong>the</strong> arrangement<strong>of</strong> forms and spaces within <strong>the</strong> format.


C<strong>on</strong>ceptual Images. Imagery from internal sources (<strong>the</strong>"mind's eye") ra<strong>the</strong>r than from external, perceivedsources; usually simplified images; <strong>of</strong>ten abstract ra<strong>the</strong>rthan realistic.C<strong>on</strong>tour Line. In drawing, a line that represents <strong>the</strong> sharededges <strong>of</strong> a form, a group <strong>of</strong> forms, or forms and spaces.Corpus Callosum. A massive, compact bundle <strong>of</strong> ax<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>necting<strong>the</strong> right and left cerebral cortices. The corpuscallosum allows <strong>the</strong> two halves, or hemispheres, <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>cerebral cortex to communicate directly with <strong>on</strong>eano<strong>the</strong>r.Creativity. The ability to find new soluti<strong>on</strong>s to a problem ornew modes <strong>of</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong>; <strong>the</strong> bringing into existence <strong>of</strong>something new to <strong>the</strong> individual and to <strong>the</strong> culture.Writer Arthur Koestler added <strong>the</strong> requirement that <strong>the</strong>new creati<strong>on</strong> should be socially useful.Crosshatching. A series <strong>of</strong> intersecting sets <strong>of</strong> parallel linesused to indicate shading or volume in a drawing.Edge. In drawing, <strong>the</strong> place where two things meet (forexample, where <strong>the</strong> sky meets <strong>the</strong> ground); <strong>the</strong> line <strong>of</strong>separati<strong>on</strong> between two shapes or a space and a shape.Expressive Quality. The slight individual differences in <strong>the</strong>way each <strong>of</strong> us perceives and represents our percepti<strong>on</strong>sin a work <strong>of</strong> art. These differences express an individual'sinner reacti<strong>on</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> perceived stimulus as well as<strong>the</strong> unique "touch" arising from individual physiologicalmotor differences.Eye Level, In perspective drawing, a horiz<strong>on</strong>tal line <strong>on</strong>which lines above and below it in <strong>the</strong> horiz<strong>on</strong>tal planeappear to c<strong>on</strong>verge. In portrait drawing, <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong>alline that divides <strong>the</strong> head in half horiz<strong>on</strong>tally; <strong>the</strong>locati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eyes at this halfway mark <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> head.Foreshortening. A way to portray forms <strong>on</strong> a two-dimensi<strong>on</strong>alsurface so that <strong>the</strong>y appear to project from orrecede behind a flat surface; a means <strong>of</strong> creating <strong>the</strong> illusi<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> spatial depth in figures or forms.Format. The particular shape <strong>of</strong> a drawing or painting surface—rectangular,circular, triangular, etc.; <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> surface, e.g., <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> length to<strong>the</strong> width in a rectangular surface.Grid. Evenly spaced lines, running horiz<strong>on</strong>tally and verticallyat right angles, that divide a drawing or paintinginto small squares or rectangles. Often used to enlarge adrawing or to aid in seeing spatial relati<strong>on</strong>ships.Hemispheric Lateralizati<strong>on</strong>. The differentiati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> twocerebral hemispheres with respect to functi<strong>on</strong> and mode<strong>of</strong> cogniti<strong>on</strong>.Holistic. In terms <strong>of</strong> cognitive functi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>the</strong> simultaneousprocessing <strong>of</strong> an array <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> in a total c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>as opposed to sequential processing <strong>of</strong> its separateparts.Image. Verb: to call up in <strong>the</strong> mind a mental copy <strong>of</strong> somethingnot present to <strong>the</strong> senses; see in <strong>the</strong> "mind's eye."Noun: a retinal image; <strong>the</strong> optical image <strong>of</strong> externalobjects received by <strong>the</strong> visual system and interpreted by<strong>the</strong> brain.Imaginati<strong>on</strong>. A recombinati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> mental images from pastexperiences into a new pattern.Intuiti<strong>on</strong>. Direct and apparently unmediated knowledge; ajudgment, meaning, or idea that occurs to a pers<strong>on</strong>without any known process <strong>of</strong> reflective thinking. Thejudgment is <strong>of</strong>ten reached as a result <strong>of</strong> minimal cuesand seems to "come from nowhere."Key. In drawing, <strong>the</strong> lightness or darkness <strong>of</strong> an image. Ahigh-key drawing is light or pale in value; a low-keydrawing is dark or low in value.Learning. Any relatively permanent change in behavior as aresult <strong>of</strong> experience or practice.Left Hemisphere. The left half (oriented according to yourleft) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cerebrum. For most right-handed individualsand a large proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> left-handed individuals, verbalfuncti<strong>on</strong>s are in <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere.L-Mode. A state <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> processing characterized aslinear, verbal, analytic, and logical.Negative Spaces. The areas around positive forms that shareedges with <strong>the</strong> forms. Negative spaces are bounded by<strong>the</strong> outer edges by <strong>the</strong> format. "Interior" negative spacescanbepart<strong>of</strong> positive forms: For example, <strong>the</strong> whites <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> eyes can be regarded as interior negative spaces usefulfor drawing <strong>the</strong> irises.276 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


N<strong>on</strong>objective Art. Art that makes no attempt to reproduce<strong>the</strong> appearance <strong>of</strong> real-life objects or experiences or toproduce <strong>the</strong> illusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> reality. Also called "n<strong>on</strong>representati<strong>on</strong>alart."Percepti<strong>on</strong>. The awareness, or <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> becomingaware, <strong>of</strong> objects, relati<strong>on</strong>s, or qualities—ei<strong>the</strong>r internalor external to <strong>the</strong> individual—by means <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sensesand under <strong>the</strong> influence <strong>of</strong> previous experiences.Picture Plane. An imaginary c<strong>on</strong>struct <strong>of</strong> a transparentplane, like a framed window, that always remains parallelto <strong>the</strong> vertical plane <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> artist's face. The artistdraws <strong>on</strong> paper what he or she sees bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> plane asthough <strong>the</strong> view were flattened <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> plane. Inventors<strong>of</strong> photography used this c<strong>on</strong>cept to develop <strong>the</strong> firstcameras.Realistic Art. The objective depicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> objects, forms, andfigures attentively perceived. Also called "naturalism."<strong>Right</strong> Hemisphere. The right half (oriented according toyour right) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cerebrum. For most right-handedindividuals and a large proporti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> left-handed individuals,spatial, relati<strong>on</strong>al functi<strong>on</strong>s are in <strong>the</strong> righ<strong>the</strong>misphere.R-Mode. A state <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> processing characterized assimultaneous, holistic, spatial, and relati<strong>on</strong>al.Scanning. In drawing, checking points, distances, degrees <strong>of</strong>angles relative to vertical or horiz<strong>on</strong>tal, relative sizes,etc.Sighting. In drawing, measuring relative sizes by means <strong>of</strong> ac<strong>on</strong>stant measure (<strong>the</strong> pencil held at arm's length is <strong>the</strong>most usual measuring device); determining relativepoints in a drawing—<strong>the</strong> locati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e part relative tosome o<strong>the</strong>r part. Also, determining angles relative to <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>stant's vertical and horiz<strong>on</strong>tal.States <strong>of</strong> C<strong>on</strong>sciousness. A largely unresolved c<strong>on</strong>cept,c<strong>on</strong>sciousness is used in this book to mean <strong>the</strong> awareness,c<strong>on</strong>tinually changing, <strong>of</strong> what passes in <strong>on</strong>e's ownmind. An alternate state <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness is <strong>on</strong>e that isperceived as noticeably different from ordinary, wakingc<strong>on</strong>sciousness. Familiar alternate states are daydreaming,sleep dreaming, and meditati<strong>on</strong>.Symbol System. In drawing, a set <strong>of</strong> symbols that are c<strong>on</strong>sistentlyused toge<strong>the</strong>r to form an image, for example, afigure. The symbols are usually used in sequence, <strong>on</strong>eappearing to call forth ano<strong>the</strong>r, much in <strong>the</strong> manner <strong>of</strong>writing familiar words, in which writing <strong>on</strong>e letter leadsto writing <strong>the</strong> next. Symbol systems in drawn forms areusually set in childhood and <strong>of</strong>ten persist throughoutadulthood unless modified by learning new ways todraw.Value. In art, <strong>the</strong> darkness or lightness <strong>of</strong> t<strong>on</strong>es or colors.White is <strong>the</strong> lightest, or highest, value; black is <strong>the</strong> darkest,or lowest, value.Visual Informati<strong>on</strong> Processing. The use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> visual systemto gain informati<strong>on</strong> from external sources and <strong>the</strong>interpretati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> that sensory data by means <strong>of</strong> cogniti<strong>on</strong>.Zen. A system <strong>of</strong> thought that emphasizes a form <strong>of</strong> meditati<strong>on</strong>called zazen. Zazen begins with c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>,<strong>of</strong>ten <strong>on</strong> puzzles wholly impervious to soluti<strong>on</strong> throughreas<strong>on</strong>. C<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> leads to samadhi, a "state <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>eness"in which <strong>the</strong> meditator gains insight into <strong>the</strong> unity<strong>of</strong> things in <strong>the</strong> world. The meditator strives to movethrough fur<strong>the</strong>r stages to <strong>the</strong> final stage <strong>of</strong> Zen, satori, or"no mind," a brilliantly clear state <strong>of</strong> mind in which <strong>the</strong>details <strong>of</strong> every phenomen<strong>on</strong> are perceived, yet withoutevaluati<strong>on</strong> or attachment.Split-<strong>Brain</strong> Patients. Individuals who had been sufferingfrom intractable epileptic seizures and whose medicalproblems were relieved by a surgical operati<strong>on</strong>. Theprocedure separates <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres by severing<strong>the</strong> corpus callosum. The procedure is rarely d<strong>on</strong>e andsplit-brain patients are few in number.2 77


BibliographyAlbersjosef. The Interacti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> Color. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> Haven: Yale UniversityPress, 1962.Arguelles,J. The Transformative Visi<strong>on</strong>, Berkeley, Calif.:Shambhala Publicati<strong>on</strong>s, 1975.Arnheim, R. Art and Visual Percepti<strong>on</strong>. Berkeley, Calif.: University<strong>of</strong> California Press, 1954.Ayrt<strong>on</strong>, M. Golden Secti<strong>on</strong>s. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Methuen, 1957.Berger, J. Ways <strong>of</strong> Seeing. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Viking Press, 1972.Bergquist,J. W. The Use <strong>of</strong> Computers in EducatingBoth Halves <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong>. Proceedings: Eighth Annual Seminar forDirectors <strong>of</strong> Academic Computati<strong>on</strong>al Services, August1977. P.O. Box 1036, La Canada, Calif.Blakemore, C. Mechanics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mind. Cambridge; At <strong>the</strong> UniversityPress, 1977.BlissJ., andj. Morella. TheLeft-Handers' Handbook. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:A & W Visual Library, 1980.BogenJ. E. "The O<strong>the</strong>r <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong>." Bulletin <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> LosAngeles Neurological Societies34 (1969): 73—105".. "Some Educati<strong>on</strong>al Aspects <strong>of</strong> Hemispheric Specializati<strong>on</strong>."UCLA Educator 17 (1975): 24-32.BrunerJ. S. "The Creative Surprise." In C<strong>on</strong>temporaryApproaches to Creative Educati<strong>on</strong>, edited by H. E. Gruber,G. Terrell, and M. Wer<strong>the</strong>imer. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: A<strong>the</strong>rt<strong>on</strong>Press, 1962.. On Knowing: Essays for <strong>the</strong> Left Hand. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:A<strong>the</strong>neum, ip6>.Buswell, Guy T. Hoiv People Look at Pictures. Chicago: ChicagoUniversity Press, 193$".Bu/.an, T. Use Both <strong>Side</strong>s <strong>of</strong> Your <strong>Brain</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: E. P. Durt<strong>on</strong>,1976.Carra, C. "The Quadrant <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Spirit." Valori Plastici 1(April—May 1919): 2.Carroll, L., pseud. See Dodgs<strong>on</strong>, C. L.Collier, G. Form, Space, and Visi<strong>on</strong>. Englewood Cliffs, NJ.:Prentice-Hall, 1963.BIBLIOGRAPHY 279


C<strong>on</strong>nolly, C. The Unquiet Grave: A Word Cycle by Palinurus.<str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Harper and Bro<strong>the</strong>rs, 194J.Corballis, M., and I. Beale. The Psychology <strong>of</strong> Left and <strong>Right</strong>.Hillsdale, NJ.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1976.Cottrell, Sir Alan. "Emergent Properties <strong>of</strong> Complex Systems."In The Encyclopedia <strong>of</strong> Ignorance, edited by R. Duncanand M. West<strong>on</strong>-Smith. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Sim<strong>on</strong> &Schuster, 1977.Critchley, M. Music and <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong>. Springfield, 111.: Charles C.Thomas, 1977.Davis, Stuart. Quoted in "The Cube Root." In Art<str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g>sXLl,'943. 22 ~ 2 3-Delacroix, Eugene. In Artists <strong>on</strong> Art, edited by Robert Goldwaterand Marco Treves. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Pan<strong>the</strong><strong>on</strong> Books, 1945.Descartes, Rene. Fourth Discourse <strong>on</strong> Method, Optics, Geometry,and Meteorology, 1637.Dodgs<strong>on</strong>, C. L. [pseud. Carroll, L.] The Complete Works <strong>of</strong>Lewis Carroll, edited by A. Woollcott. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: ModernLibrary, 1936.Edwards, B. "Anxiety and <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>." Master's <strong>the</strong>sis, CaliforniaState University at Northridge, 1972.. "An Experiment in Perceptual Skills in <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>."Ph.D. dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <strong>of</strong> California, Los Angeles,1976.Edwards, B., and D. Narikawa. Art, Grades Four to Six. LosAngeles: Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Objectives Exchange, 1974.English, H. B., and Ava C. English. A Comprehensive Dicti<strong>on</strong>ary<strong>of</strong> Psychological and Psychoanalytical Terms. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:David McKay Company, Inc., 1974.Ernst, M. Bey<strong>on</strong>d Painting. Translated by D. Tanning. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g>York: Wittenborn, Schultz, 1948.Fergus<strong>on</strong>, M. The <strong>Brain</strong> Revoluti<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Taplinger Publishing,1973.Flam, J. Matisse <strong>on</strong> Art. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Phaid<strong>on</strong>, 1973.Franck, F. The Zen <strong>of</strong> Seeing. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1973.Franco, L., and R. VV. Sperry, "Hemisphere Lateralizati<strong>on</strong> forCognitive Processing <strong>of</strong> Geometry," Neuropsychologia,1977, Vol. 15,107—14.Friend, D. Compositi<strong>on</strong>: A Painter's Guide to Basic Problems andSoluti<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Wats<strong>on</strong>-Guptill Publicati<strong>on</strong>s, 1975-.Gardner, H. The Shattered Mind: The Pers<strong>on</strong> After Damage. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g>York: Alfred A. Knopf, 197J.Gazzaniga, M. "The Split <strong>Brain</strong> in Man." In Percepti<strong>on</strong>: Mechanismsand Models, edited by R. Held and W. Richards, p.33. San Francisco, Calif: W. H. Freeman, 1972.Gladwin, T. "Culture and Logical Process." In Explorati<strong>on</strong>s inCultural Anthropology, edited by W. H. Goodenough, pp.167-77. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: McGraw-Hill, 1964.Goldstein, N. The Art <strong>of</strong> Resp<strong>on</strong>sive <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>. EnglewoodCliffs, NJ.: Prentice-Hall, 1973.Gregory, R. L. Eye and <strong>Brain</strong>: The Psychology <strong>of</strong> Seeing. 2nd ed.,rev. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: McGraw-Hill, 1973.Grosser, M. The Painter's Eye. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Holt, Rinehart andWinst<strong>on</strong>, 1951.Hadamard, J. An Essay <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Psychology <strong>of</strong> Inventi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> Ma<strong>the</strong>maticalField. Princet<strong>on</strong>, N.J.: Princet<strong>on</strong> UniversityPress, 194J.Hall, E. T. The Silent Language. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday,•9J9-Hearn, Lafcadio. Exotics and Retrospectives. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> Jersey: LiteratureHouse, 1968.Henri, R. The Art Spirit. Philadelphia, Pa.:J. B. Lippincott,192;.Herrigel, E. Quoted in The Joy <strong>of</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: The OakTree Press, 1961.Hill, E. The Language <strong>of</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.:Prentice-Hall, 1966.Hockney, D. David Hockney, edited by N. Stangos. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Harry N. Abrams, 1976.H<strong>of</strong>fman, Howard. Visi<strong>on</strong> & <strong>the</strong> Art<strong>of</strong>'<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>. EnglewoodCliffs, NJ.: Prentice-Hall, 1989.Huxley, A. The Art <strong>of</strong> Seeing. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Harper and Bro<strong>the</strong>rs,1942.'954-. The Doors <strong>of</strong> Percepti<strong>on</strong>.<str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Harper and Row,280 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


lames, W. The Varieties <strong>of</strong> Religious Experience. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:L<strong>on</strong>gmans, Green, 1902.[aynes, J. The Origin <strong>of</strong> C<strong>on</strong>sciousness in <strong>the</strong> Breakdown <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Bicameral Mind. Bost<strong>on</strong>: Hought<strong>on</strong> Mifnin, 1976.Jung, C. G. Man and His Symbols. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday,1964.Kandinsky, W. Point and Line to Plane. Chicago: Solom<strong>on</strong>Guggenheim Foundati<strong>on</strong>, 1945.'947-. C<strong>on</strong>cerning <strong>the</strong> Spiritual in Art. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Wittenborn,Keller, Helen. The World I Live In. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: The CenturyCompany, 1908.Kipling, R. RudyardKipling's Verse. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Hodder &Stought<strong>on</strong>, 1927.Koestler, A. The Sleepwalkers. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Hutchins<strong>on</strong>, 1959.Krishnamurti,J. The Awakening <strong>of</strong> Intelligence. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Harper and Row, 1973.. "Self Knowledge." In The First and Last Freedom, p. 43.<str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Harper and Row, 954.. You Are <strong>the</strong> World. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Harper and Row, 1972.LevyJ. "Differential Perceptual Capacities in Major andMinor Hemispheres," Proceedings <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy<strong>of</strong> Science, Vol. 61,1968, IIJI.. "Psychobiological Implicati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> Bilateral Asymmetry."In Hemisphere Functi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> Human <strong>Brain</strong>, editedby S.J. Dim<strong>on</strong>d and J. G. Beaum<strong>on</strong>t. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: JohnWiley and S<strong>on</strong>s, 1974.LevyJ., C. Trevar<strong>the</strong>n, and R. W. Sperry. "Percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>Bilateral Chimeric Figures Following HemisphericDec<strong>on</strong>nexi<strong>on</strong>," <strong>Brain</strong>, 95,1972,61—78.Lindaman, E. B. Thinkingin Future Tense. Nashville: BroadmanPress, 1978.Lindstrom, M. Children's Art: A Study <strong>of</strong> Normal Development inChildren's Modes <strong>of</strong> Visualizati<strong>on</strong>. Berkeley, Calif.: University<strong>of</strong> California Press, 1957.Lord, J. A Giacometti Portrait. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Museum <strong>of</strong> ModernArt, 11)6$.Lowenfeld, V. Creative and Mental Grcnvth. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Macmillan,1947.Matisse, H. Notes d'un peintre. In Lagranderevue. Paris, 1908.McFeeJ. Preparati<strong>on</strong> for Art. San Francisco, Calif: WadsworchPublishing, 1961.McKim, R. Experiences in Visual Thinking. M<strong>on</strong>terey, Calif.:Brooks/Cole Publishing, 1972.Munsell, Albert. A Color Notati<strong>on</strong>. Baltimore: Munsell ColorCo., 1926.<str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g>t<strong>on</strong>, Isaac. Opticks, Book 1, Part 2.1st ed., L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>, 1704.Nicolaides, K. The Natural Way to Draw. Bost<strong>on</strong>: Hought<strong>on</strong>Mifflin, 1941.Ornstein, R. The Psychology <strong>of</strong> C<strong>on</strong>sciousness. 2nd ed., rev. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g>York: Harcourt BraceJovanovich, 1977.Orwell, G. "Politics and <strong>the</strong> English Language." In In Fr<strong>on</strong>t <strong>of</strong>Your Nose, Vol. 4 <strong>of</strong> The Collected Essays <strong>of</strong> George Orwell,edited by S. Orwell and I. Angus, p. 138. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: HarcourtBrace and World, 1968.Paivio, A. Imagery and Verbal Processes.<str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Holt, Rinehartand Winst<strong>on</strong>, 1971.ParedesJ., and M. Hepburn. "The Split-<strong>Brain</strong> and <strong>the</strong> Culture-Cogniti<strong>on</strong>Paradox." Current Anthropology 17 (March1976): 1.Pascal, B. Pensees: Thoughts <strong>on</strong> Religi<strong>on</strong> and O<strong>the</strong>r Objects, editedby H. S. Thayer and E. B. Thayer. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Washingt<strong>on</strong>Square Press, 1965.PiagetJ. The Language and Thought <strong>of</strong> a Child. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Meridian, i95"J.Pirsig, R. Zen and <strong>the</strong> Art <strong>of</strong> Motorcycle Maintenance. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:William Morrow, 1974.P<strong>on</strong>omarev, L. In Quest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Quantum. Trans, by N. Weinstein.Moscow: Mir Publishers, 1973.Reed, William. KLA Practical Guide for Westerners. Tokyo:Japan Publicati<strong>on</strong>s, 1986.Rock, I. "The Percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> Disoriented Figures." In Image,Object, and Illusi<strong>on</strong>, edited by R. M. Held. San Francisco:W. H. Freeman, 1971.BIBLIOGRAPHY 281


Rodin, A. Quoted in The Joy <strong>of</strong> Draining. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: The OakTree Press, 1961.Samples, B. TheMetaphoricMind. Reading, Mass.: Addis<strong>on</strong>-Wesley Publishing, 1976.. The Wholeschool Book. Reading, Mass.: Addis<strong>on</strong>-WesleyPublishing, 1977.Samuels, M., and N. Samuels. Seeing with <strong>the</strong> Mind's Eye. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g>York: Random House, 1975.Sargeant, Walter. The Enjoyment and Use <strong>of</strong> Color. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Charles Scribner's S<strong>on</strong>s, 192;.Shah, I. The Exploits <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Incomparable Mulla Nasrudin. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g>York: E. P. Dutt<strong>on</strong>, 1972.. States <strong>of</strong> C<strong>on</strong>sciousness. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: E. P. Dutt<strong>on</strong>, 1975.Taylor, J. Design and Expressi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> Visual Arts. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Dover Publicati<strong>on</strong>s, 1964.Verity, Enid. Color Observed. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Van Nostrand Reinholdand Company, 1980.Walter, W. G. The Living <strong>Brain</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: W. W. Nort<strong>on</strong>, 1963.Wittrock, M. C, et al. The Human <strong>Brain</strong>. Englewood Cliffs,NJ.: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1977.Zaidel, E., and R. Sperry. "Memory Impairment after Commisurotomyin Man." <strong>Brain</strong> 97 (1974): 263-72.Shepard, R. N. Visual Learning, Thinking, and Communicati<strong>on</strong>,edited by B. S. Randhawa and W. E. C<strong>of</strong>fman. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Academic Press, 1978.Singer, J. How to Paint in Pastels. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Wats<strong>on</strong>-GuptilPublishers, 1976.Smith, Peter. "The Dialectics <strong>of</strong> Color." In Color for Architecture,T. Porter and B. Mikellides. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Van NostrandReinhold, 1976.Sperry, R. W. "Hemisphere Disc<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> and Unity in C<strong>on</strong>sciousAwareness." American Psychologist!^ (1968): 723—33.. "Lateral Specializati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> Cerebral Functi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong>Surgically Separated Hemispheres," in The Psychophysiology<strong>of</strong> Thinking, edited by F. J. McGuigan and R. A.Scho<strong>on</strong>over. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: Academic Press, 1973,209-29.Sperry, R. W., M. S. Gazzaniga, and J. E. Bogen, "InterhemisphericRelati<strong>on</strong>ships: <strong>the</strong> Neocortical Commissures;Syndromes <strong>of</strong> Hemisphere Disc<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>," Handbook <strong>of</strong>Clinical Neurology, edited by P. J. Vinken and G. W.Bruyn. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Co.,1969,273-89.Stein, G. Picasso. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: B. T. Batsford, 1938.Suzuki, D. "Satori." [n The Gospel According to Zen, edited by R.Sohl and A. Carr. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York: <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> American Library,1970.Tart, C. T. "Putting <strong>the</strong> Pieces Toge<strong>the</strong>r." In Alternate States <strong>of</strong>C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, edited by N. E. Zinberg, 204-6. <str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g> York:Macmillan, 1977.282 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


IndexPage numbers in italicsrefer to illustrati<strong>on</strong>s.abstracti<strong>on</strong>, 80,142,170adolescents, 69,70, 75-81, yS, jj>, 121,272after-image, 237Age <strong>of</strong> Spiritual Machines, The (Kurzweil),XXIIIAlbers, Josef, 239,240Alberti, Le<strong>on</strong>e Battista, 96,102Allan, Hea<strong>the</strong>r, 23-24, 24analog drawing, xiv, xviAnglo-Sax<strong>on</strong>, 36animals, 29,30,46Archimedes, 39Army poster, 60Arnold, Elizabeth, 194artists, 2,3,4, 5, 7,54,55, 62, 63,128,162,206drawing as metaphor for, 23-24left-handed, 43police, 170Renaissance, 139,142,143-45, H3< H4-213,217,252Art <strong>of</strong> PaintingAfter <strong>the</strong> Italian Manner,7"fo(Elsum), 96Aztecs, 43Bball-and-stick letters, 255,2jjBall-Kingst<strong>on</strong>, Cindy, 1J4Basic Unit, 140,141,146-49,146,14-/, 14S,151,153,154,180,183in compositi<strong>on</strong>, 116,123—26,124,12J, 126,129—30,131definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 124—26Beckett, Samuel, 122,123Berberet, Gary, 243Bergland, Richard, xxBergquist, J. William, 38Blakemore, Colin, 80blanks, 168-70,16S, 173-74,174,210-13, z "-212,222Bloomer, Carolyn M., 196Bogenjoseph E., 32,37bold line, 2j283


Bomeisler, Brian, 23-24,24, up, 190,203,218, 223, 226brain, hemispheres <strong>of</strong>, xn, XVII, XX,XXII-XXV, 7,8,28-47,^0,81, 82, 151,162,165,197-98,248-50color and, 231-32c<strong>on</strong>flict between, 33,34corpus callosum <strong>of</strong>, xxiv-xxv, 30,crossover c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>, 29,29, 35,236cultural customs and, 36-37developmental changes in, 63as dominant vs. subordinate, 30—31,32,35,38, 46, 80,121, 236educati<strong>on</strong>al system and, xxiv, 31,39-41,268-72informati<strong>on</strong> processing in, 32-31,injuries <strong>of</strong>, 30,162,198language functi<strong>on</strong>s in, 30,32, 42-43,62lateralizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 42-43,63-65,88,121in linguistic terminology, 35-37,38locati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>troversy regarding, XXII,xxivpolitics and, 36reality percepti<strong>on</strong> in, 32,35in split-brain studies, 31—35,38, 46ways <strong>of</strong> knowing in, 37,38jwa/johandedness; L-mode; R-modeBroekhuijsen, Jerome, 191broken line, 2jBruer,John X, xxivBuhler, Karl, 81Burbank, Brenda, 72Buswell, Guy X, 236bypassing technique, 188CCaldwell, Randa, IJSCalifornia Institute <strong>of</strong> Xechnology,31—34calligraphy, 25, 204Japanese, 256,256,259Carpenter (Van Gogh), 170-72,172Carra, Carlo, 63Carroll, Lewis, 41,42,4762, 84carto<strong>on</strong>s, 72,76,77, 232cave painting, 2,94central axis, 168, iifS, 210,211, 211,215, 2ij, 216Cezanne, Paul, 122-23,122Chardin,Jean-Baptiste—Sime<strong>on</strong>, 243children, 7, 68-85, '7°> 267-72adolescent, 69,70,75-81,78,79,111, 272compositi<strong>on</strong> by, 74—75,78,120—21,121infants, 70-71portrait views preferred by, 213-14tiered perspective used by, 142see also symbol system, childhoodChildren's Art (Lindstrom), 6


Compltte Letters <strong>of</strong> Vincent Van Gogh, The,102, tojcomplexity, stage <strong>of</strong>, 7^—77,76, 77compositi<strong>on</strong>, 12,74,75, 113,119-26,140,154,227Basic Unit in, 116,123-26,124,12j, 126,129-30,131by children, 74-75, 78,120-21, mdefiniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 119,120format in, 120-23,121,122,124,126,127,'3°. '33unified, 120,123computers, xxiii, 28,38C<strong>on</strong>nolly, Cyril, 40c<strong>on</strong>sciousness, altered states <strong>of</strong>, 4,5,46,54,61,62-63,84-85,91,177c<strong>on</strong>tour drawing, 88—113, n 7<strong>of</strong> handwriting, 257—58, 2j8see also modified c<strong>on</strong>tour drawing;pure c<strong>on</strong>tour drawingcopying <strong>of</strong> masterworks, 178-80,200—204corporate training seminars, xm,XIV-XVIcorpus callosum, xxiv-xxv.^o, 31-35Courbet, Gustave, 196,197,200-204crafts projects, 70Cranach, Lucas, 214creativity, 3,5, 6-8,9,40definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 38—39,210Crick, F.H.C., 28crosshatching, 195, 207—9, z0 7> 2 °8> z0 9Crowe, Dana, /jycubes, 79-80,79cubism, 80DDame, D<strong>on</strong>, 239, 242Dancer Adjusting Her Slipper (Degas), 1x7,2 33"da Vinci device," 251-52Davis, Stuart, 241Degas, Edgar, 1J7,233,237-39,250Delacroix, Eugene, 245DePhillippo, Sandy, 134"Dialectics <strong>of</strong> Color, The" (Smith), 232Draughtsman Making a Perspective <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>of</strong>a^tfWM(Durer), 143—45,'43-'44drawing, 2-9,45,54-55,248-52altered states <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness in, 4,5by animals, 28by art students, 273—74creativity <strong>of</strong>, 6—8,9definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 99, nodifficulty at start <strong>of</strong>, 105,123-26handedness and, 45—46as learnable skill, 3as magical ability, 2—3as metaphor for artist, 23-24in public places, 152, 241realistic, 8seeing and, 4working up <strong>of</strong>, 132drawing materials, 13—14, y<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Artist Within (Edwards),XIVdriving, xvm, xix, 138freeway, 5,84Diirer, Albrecht, 6,80, 82, 122-23, E2 > >7 Z >•74picture-plane device <strong>of</strong>, 102,143-45,'44. '4Jedges, xiv, xvm, xix, xxi, 14,75,78,116,138,149,150,157,163,177,196in c<strong>on</strong>tour drawing, 90,92,94-96,97—98,108,109,117definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 94-95, "9<strong>of</strong> format, see formatsshared, 95,96,109,119,124educati<strong>on</strong>al system, xxiv, 31,39-41,Egyptians, ancient, 142Einstein, Albert, 56-57, p,$8Elderfieldjohn, 125Elgart, Elliot, 104Elsum, John, 170,211, 224n<strong>on</strong>verbal, 21—23, 256eyeballing, 151,156eyedness, 422&S


Fabric <strong>of</strong> Mind, The (Bergland), XXfacial recogniti<strong>on</strong>, xxm, 8, 162, 165Feline Felicity (Sheeler), 204, 20J, 209figure drawings, 208,209by children, 71-73,7/, 74 73,78sighting in, 1J7Firmin, Lisbeth, 134, ifiiFlagg, James M<strong>on</strong>tgomery, 60FlamJ., xxmflowers, xxvfootedness, 42foreshortening, 60,97-98,123, rjj, 143-45,'44. > ¥•


I/ Ching, 37imaginati<strong>on</strong>, drawing from, xix, 8,221,251-5:Imamoto, Mauro, 227Impressi<strong>on</strong>ism, 99Incas, 43infants, 70-71Ingresjean-Auguste Dominique, 25inventi<strong>on</strong>s, XIIIlames, William, 61Jam Burden, Later Mrs. William Morris, asQueen Guinevere (Rossetti), ipplapanese calligraphy, 256, 256,259Japanese prints, 23Jarman, Christopher, 257KKandinsky, Wassily, 236Kay, Alan, 142Keller, Helen, 230Kennedy, Grace, 13P, ipo, 226Kipling, Rudyard, 40Kiy<strong>on</strong>obu I, Torii, 23Kiyotada, Torii, 23Klee, Paul, 82Koestler, Arthur, 38Kollwitz, Ka<strong>the</strong>, 240KrishnamurtiJ., xvi, 248Kuroyama, Laurie, 140Kurzweil, Ray, xxmlandscapes, K5recalled childhood, 74—75,75stage <strong>of</strong>, 73-75,74language, 30,32,52, 62, 65,81,121<strong>of</strong> art, 236, 261grammar <strong>of</strong>, 138lateralizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 42-43terms for left and right, 35-37,38Large Stuffed£rfg/t'(Ludwig), 242Latin, 36,142Lawrence, D. H., 84learning, transfer <strong>of</strong>, xm-xivLeft-Handers' Handbook, T^(Bliss andMorella), 43Legros, Alph<strong>on</strong>se, 208,209Le<strong>on</strong>ardo da Vinci, 4,14, 42,43,102,251-52Levy, Jerre, 32,33,38,41Lewis-Williams, D., 94lights and shadows, xiv, xvm, xx, XXI, 96,108,109,116,117,128-29,138,155,163,186,189,194-227c<strong>on</strong>tinuous t<strong>on</strong>e in, 209—10crosshatching in, 195, 207—9, 20 7> 20 %>20(1exercise for, 200—204in handwriting, 263—64R-mode in percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 194,196-98,rp!seeing and, 204-7see also self-portraitslimbic system, 231Lindaman, Edward B., 224Lindstrom, Miriam, 69, 77line, xxi, 23,142,196handwriting as, 21—23, 1 55~5&styles <strong>of</strong>, 25,2j, 262L-mode, xvi, xx, xxn, xxm, 46, 6^, 88,113,118,129,163,178, 223—24characteristics <strong>of</strong>, 33,37,38—40,44,80,82cognitive shift from, 50,54—55,57-58,60—62,63colors and, 231—32c<strong>on</strong>tour drawing and, 89, 92,93,94,108formal perspective as, 145navigati<strong>on</strong> by, 53,54sighting as, 138,139—40,145upside-down images rejected by,60—61,113see also brain, hemispheres <strong>of</strong>; R-mode; symbol system,childhoodLudwig, Ken, 242MMarchand, Andre, xxmMarks,Judi, 92masklike effect, 170Matisse, Henri, xxm, 4,118,125, n;, 170Mayas, 45memory, drawing from, xix, 250—51as pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> exercise, 15,16—17Milne, A. A., 45287


Miro.Joan, 121, aimirror handwriting, 42mirror images, 50-54,113Mme. Pierre Gautreau (Sargent), 178-80,iyp, 180, ippmodels, 104,125,143-44,198-99,213,214,243-45. y°for pr<strong>of</strong>ile portraits, 167-68,170, IJI,181,182-89, ipo, ipimodified c<strong>on</strong>tour drawing, 94, 96-112,215-16, 258<strong>on</strong> picture plane, 96-104, py, 100, mipositi<strong>on</strong> for, 105,106t<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> paper in, 106—7, l0 7> IJOMolloy, Naveen, 77Morisot, Ber<strong>the</strong>, 204,204Munsell, Albert, 238Myers, R<strong>on</strong>ald, 31NNatural Way to Draw, T/V(Nicolaides),navigati<strong>on</strong>, 53,54Nebes, Robert, 32negative spaces, xiv, xv, 16,96,116-35,116,7/7,155,/jf, 157,174,177,196cognitive shift and, 132-33,133descripti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 117-19<strong>of</strong> drawn chairs, 118, up, 123, 127—32, np,131,132,134,ifffuncti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>, 123<strong>of</strong> handwriting, 2j8, 259-60, 2jpimportance <strong>of</strong>, 118positive forms vs., 117-19,120,121-22,722,155, 196see also compositi<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>New</str<strong>on</strong>g>t<strong>on</strong>, Isaac, 234—35,23JNicolaides, Kim<strong>on</strong>, 3,88—89, Sp, 233n<strong>on</strong>verbal expressi<strong>on</strong>, 21-23, 2 j6Nude Woman with a Staff(Diiver), 122OObserver, objective, 46Olimpio Fusco (Sargent), ippO'Neil,Jeanne, 11SOrnstein, Robert, 82Orwell, George, 93,169,169Ppainterly style, 23painting, xxi, 196,239, 245Shodo, 256Palmer method handwriting, 255,2jj, 257Paredes, J. A., 53pastels, 242—45perceptual illusi<strong>on</strong>s, 164-67,164, ioj, 166Vase/Faces, 50-54, jo, 61,95,113perceptual skills, xiv-xv, xvm-xx, 17-18,21, 40,96,117"Perceptual Skills in <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>"(Edwards), xnPers<strong>on</strong>ages with Star (Miro), 121perspective, 9,96,102,104,117,138,'4 2 ~J9. 'S3- >S4- '/•?. 196definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 142—43distance in, 75,140,143,164-65,1(4exercise for, 152-56,1J3foreshortening in, 60,97-98,123, IJJ,H3-4J. '44, yi. 'S«, '57formal vs. informal, 145,14J, 158linear, 142Renaissance, 139,142,143—45,143,144tiered, 142vanishing points in, 143,145,14Jperspective devices, mo, 102-3, ">3photography, 99,152,169Picasso, Pablo, 22, 25,57-61, jS, 60,73,79,170, 273picture plane, 96-104,^7, mo, mi, 108, nodescripti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 99historical use <strong>of</strong>, 102-3portraits and, 103sighting and, 139,140,144,150—51, ijo,156single view through, 103-4three-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al space and, 104picture plane, plastic, 13,96-98,104,105,107-8, toy, no, 119,143,146-49,146,148,153,154,155,178,179-80,181,182,183picture-plane devices, 102,143—45,144,9Sportraits, 8,170288 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


asic poses <strong>of</strong>, 198—99copying <strong>of</strong> masterwork, 178-80,200—204picture plane used for, 103proporti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>, see proporti<strong>on</strong>s, inportraitssee also pr<strong>of</strong>ile portraits; self-portraitspositive forms, 117—19,120,121—22,122,155,196Preacber(\\ 7 hite), 156pre-instructi<strong>on</strong> self-portraits, 14—24,69,224-25primitive art, 170Principles <strong>of</strong> Art, 119Prismacolor, 23;problem solving, xm, 6,55,142at corporate training seminars,XIV-XVIpr<strong>of</strong>ile portraits, 23-24, 24, yS, 158,162-91,198,199,213,214,218blanks for, 168-70,169,173-74,1/4childhood symbol systems and,162-63, '77' '^exercise for, 180-89, '&, '83, '84, '85, '86188models for, 167-68,170,lyi, 181,182-89,ipo, ipiproporti<strong>on</strong>s in, 162,163,164-77; see alsoproporti<strong>on</strong>s, in portraitsshowing <strong>of</strong>, 189—91proporti<strong>on</strong>, 96,117,124,131,138,139,140—51, 156,196, 222—23Basic Unit <strong>of</strong>, see Basic Unitexercise for, 146-51perceptual illusi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong>, 164—67, 164,i6s, 166ratios in, 139,140—42,141,147-49, iqj,148, ijfscale in, 130,141,146,147proporti<strong>on</strong>s, in portraits, 162,163,164—77,210—18, 222—23blanks for, 168-70,168, 173-74,174,210—13, 2II > 2 '2,222central axis in, 168,168,210,211, 21s, 215,2ij, 216chopped-<strong>of</strong>fskull error in, 167-74, '^7'168, i6p, 1/0, 171, 172,174,175clothing in, 187,188collar in, 188,188,222diagrammed, iyo, 212,222ear placement in, 167,175-77, '75' '7&184-85, 187, 213, 218, 222eye level in, 167-70,174,174,184-85,210,211, 211,215, 215,216eyes in, 163,184,188,188,212,214, 216-17in full-face view, 210-13, 222-23hair in, 167,170,186, 222—23neck in, 176,176,187,188,188,213, 222nose in, 184, 211, 214, 216, 217,222nostril in, 184, I8J, 222in three-quarter view, 170, lyi, 213—18,223, 227ProudMaisie (Sandys), 186, i8yPsychology <strong>of</strong> Left ana 1 <strong>Right</strong>, The (Corbalisand Beale), 31Pure C<strong>on</strong>tour <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 88—94,113,177,178<strong>of</strong> handwriting, 257—58, 258paradox <strong>of</strong>, 93-94student showing <strong>of</strong>, 94,pypure line, 2jRRamirez, Lupe, IJ8ratios, 139,140—42,141,147—49, i4y, 14S, ijtfrealism, xvm, xix, 8, 24,35,75, 80,124stage <strong>of</strong>, 78-81, y8, yprecalled childhood landscapes, 74-75, y;Red<strong>on</strong>, Odil<strong>on</strong>, 243,244Reed, William, 256,2j


R-mode, cognitive shifts to, xx, 3,4, 5, 7,8, 46, 50-65, 81,84-85,88,92,94,108,178c<strong>on</strong>scious c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>of</strong>, 62,85, 162negative spaces and, 132-33, rjjrecogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>, 61-62,85,91,131upside-down drawing exercise for,55-62, jv? , 63, 64Vase/Faces exercise for, 50-54,/.;, 61Rockefeller, Nels<strong>on</strong>, 43Rodin, Auguste, xxv, 6Rossetti, Dante Gabriel, igpRubens, Peter Paul, IJJSSaint Jerome, Study for <strong>the</strong> (Diirer), &Salk.J<strong>on</strong>as, 180Sandys, Anth<strong>on</strong>y Frederick Augustus,1S7Santoli, Ornella, 257Sargeant, Walter, 235Sargentjohn Singer, 178-80, lyp, JSO, ippscale, 130, 141,146,147<strong>of</strong> value, 194-95scanning, left-to-right, 236scribbling stage, 70-71,7/seeing, xi, xxv, 3, 4,7,8, 9,16, 45,;;,81-82,162-68,204-7self-expressi<strong>on</strong>, 21-24handwriting as, 21—23see also expressivenessSelf-Portrait(Berberet), 243Self-Porrrait(Courbei), 196, ipy, 200-204Self-Portmit (Hopper), 205, 206Self-Portrait(M<strong>on</strong>sot), 204, 204self-portraits, 15,16—21,18, ip-20,194,195,210-27,240blanks for, 210—13, 2II > 2I2 < 222copying <strong>of</strong> masterwork, 200—204exercise for, 218-25full-face, 189,210—13, 2II > 212 > 2I 4> 222 ~ 2 3»222,226pre-instructi<strong>on</strong>, 14—24,69,224—25proporti<strong>on</strong>s in, 210-18,222-23; see alsoproporti<strong>on</strong>s, in portraitsshowing <strong>of</strong>, 225-27,226,227suggesti<strong>on</strong> for, 225three-quarter, 189,210, 213-18, 214,215,216,21S, 222,22}, 227sex differences, 75-76,76Shah, Indries, 35Sheeler, Charles, 204,20J, 209Shepard, Roger N., 3,165,10JShodo paintings, 256sighting, xix-xx, 138-59, ijS, ijp 137, ijp<strong>of</strong> angles, 139,140-42,141,144,146,149-51,149, IJO, 177eyeballing in, 151,156<strong>of</strong> handwriting, 260—63, 260,261paradoxes <strong>of</strong>, 139,140—42picture plane and, 139,140,144,150—51,IJO, 156sec<strong>on</strong>d-guessing <strong>of</strong>, 154see also perspective; proporti<strong>on</strong>Singerjoe, 238,239Sketches with Two Sowers (Van Gogh),pgSmith, Peter, 232,236spaces, xiv, xv, xvm, xix, xxi, 16,75,96,109, log, 113,138,139,163negative, see negative spacesthree-dimensi<strong>on</strong>ality <strong>of</strong>, 104,139,142,194. WSperry, Roger W., xii-xm, xvn, xx, xxn,XXIII-XXIV, 31-34"Split-<strong>Brain</strong> and <strong>the</strong> Culrure-Cogniti<strong>on</strong>Paradox, The" (Paredes andHepburn), 53split-brain studies, 31-35,38,46tests used in, 33-34stained paper, 251-52Stein, Gertrude, 4Stephan, Michael, 50still-lifes, xi, 8,122-23Stravinsky, Igor, 57—6i,/.£ 60,113Studies <strong>of</strong> Arms and tegs (Rubens), ijjStudy fir "Madame X"(SiLTgent), ipt)stuttering, 43Surrealism, 243Suzuki, D. T, 248symbol system, childhood, 17-18, &]-6$,68-85,88,118, 214-15complexity stage <strong>of</strong>, 75-77,76,77landscape stage <strong>of</strong>, 73-75,74in pictures that tell stories, 72-73,72,73in pr<strong>of</strong>ile portraits, 162-63, '77. '88realism stage <strong>of</strong>, 78-81,7S, 7P290 THE NEW DRAWING ON THE RIGHT SIDE OF THE BRAIN


scribbling stage <strong>of</strong>, 70—71,71seeing influenced by, 81—85sex differences in, 75-76, 76TTappen, Hea<strong>the</strong>r, i?iTart, Charles T., 46,54,8jthinking modes, xm-xiv, xvn, xxn, 31,3 2 -3J. 38-39. V-see also L-mode; R-modeThree Studies <strong>of</strong> Hands (Holbein), }SThrough <strong>the</strong> Looking-Glass and What AliceFound r&re(Carroll), 42, 84Thurber, James, 232Tiepolo, Giovanni Battista, yytiered perspective, 142time, sense <strong>of</strong>, 40,44, 62, 63, 85t<strong>on</strong>ing, 106—7, W 7> Ilo > n &, 127—28,128,130,touch, sense <strong>of</strong>, 89Trevar<strong>the</strong>n, Colwyn, 31,32Trukese, 53,54"Two-<strong>Side</strong>d Man, The" (Kipling), 40UUmbrella Still Life (W'right), 242upside-down drawing, xn, xxiv, 55-62,j/jg, 60,63,


Portfolio and Video Ordering<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong>The <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Right</strong><strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong> Portfoliois a complete drawing course.It includes:A two-hour VideoCassettePortfolioBooklet<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>BoardLarge <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>PaperSmall <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>PaperSmall TemplateLarge TemplateExtra-Large TemplateRedGelSelf-PortraitMirrorSmall PicturePlane/ViewFinderLarge PicturePlane/ViewFinderAngleFinderProporti<strong>on</strong>FinderErasableMarkerGraphiteStick4BWritingPencil2<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>PencikB<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g>Pencil6BPencilSharpenerKneadedEraserPlasticEraserTapeTo order <strong>the</strong> complete <str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong> Portfolio or just<strong>the</strong> video, complete this order form and mail or fax it to us at:<str<strong>on</strong>g>Drawing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Right</strong> <strong>Side</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Brain</strong>1158 26th Street, Box 530Santa M<strong>on</strong>ica, CA 90403Fax: 562-856-0909Internet:Or order via <strong>the</strong> internet at www.drawright.comTeleph<strong>on</strong>e: 888 DRAW IOI (888 372-9101)Please allow two to four weeks for delivery.Sales tax will apply for California.OShipping charges will be added—visit our website or call for a quote.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!