24.03.2013 Views

The Impact of School Transitions and Transfers on Pupil Progress and Attainment

The Impact of School Transitions and Transfers on Pupil Progress and Attainment

The Impact of School Transitions and Transfers on Pupil Progress and Attainment

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

A different transiti<strong>on</strong> issue<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Transiti<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Transfers</str<strong>on</strong>g>: A Review<br />

So far we have looked at transiti<strong>on</strong> in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pupils moving from year to year but there is another<br />

situati<strong>on</strong> which needs attenti<strong>on</strong>: how pupils manage the change from being a ‘dosser’ (in their words) to<br />

being a ‘worker’. Interviews with young people in sec<strong>on</strong>dary schools (see Rudduck et al, 1997) described<br />

how they c<strong>on</strong>tinually ‘messed about’ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ‘had a laugh’ in class <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> how they wanted now to settle down to<br />

work. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y explained how difficult it was to cast <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f the old image because both their mates <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

their teachers saw them as ‘trouble-makers’ or ‘clowns’ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> also because they had no strategies for<br />

changing those views. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> pers<strong>on</strong>al struggle involved in negotiating such a change - <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> percepti<strong>on</strong> as well<br />

as habit - should not be under-estimated.<br />

<strong>Pupil</strong>s who have established a reputati<strong>on</strong> at primary school for mucking around <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> distracting others have<br />

a chance to reform their image <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> commitment to learning when they move to the new school; older<br />

pupils have no comparable opportunity. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y realise that if they are made to change forms - or even<br />

change schools - then the pupils <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> teachers in the new setting know - as <strong>on</strong>e pupil said - that ‘<strong>on</strong>ly the<br />

baddies’ are made to move in this way; their reputati<strong>on</strong> will go before them (Rudduck et al, 1997).<br />

C: ‘VULNERABLE’ GROUPS AND SUBJECTS<br />

‘Vulnerable’ groups<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Transiti<strong>on</strong>s</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> transfers have different effects <strong>on</strong> different pupils. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> QCA follow-up<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pupils into years 3, 4 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 5 showed that certain groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pupils made less progress <strong>on</strong><br />

the ‘opti<strong>on</strong>al’ tests than others (Minnis et al, 1998). Background factors by themselves do<br />

not, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> course, explain pupils’ progress or lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> it. N<strong>on</strong>etheless, there are signs in this<br />

research that patterns <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance which are later well-established begin to become<br />

more obvious at this stage. This is a cumulative process - existing gaps begin to widen. In<br />

short, some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the seeds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> social exclusi<strong>on</strong> in the latter stages <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the sec<strong>on</strong>dary school are<br />

potentially being planted.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> NFER study shows that am<strong>on</strong>gst those who seemed vulnerable at this stage were a<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> groups whose difficulties have been highlighted in other policy initiatives:<br />

boys, for example, in relati<strong>on</strong> to progress in reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> spelling <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> girls in relati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

written <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mental maths. <strong>Pupil</strong>s from certain social backgrounds were also less likely to<br />

make progress. Groups ‘at risk’ included those <strong>on</strong> free school meals, pupils with special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al needs, pupils who were less fluent in English <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pupils from some ethnic<br />

groups (which <strong>on</strong>es depended <strong>on</strong> the particular subject being assessed).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> North American meta-analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> summer vacati<strong>on</strong> effects provides some additi<strong>on</strong>al support for these<br />

findings (Cooper et al, 1996: 264). As it reported, ‘the summer break has roughly equal negative effects <strong>on</strong><br />

the math skills <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students from middle- <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lower-income families but greater negative effects <strong>on</strong> the<br />

reading skills <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lower-income students’. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y also found that the effects were larger am<strong>on</strong>gst the older<br />

age-groups. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y speculate that part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the reas<strong>on</strong> for these differential effects may be because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

different opportunities to practice various forms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> academic material (with practice in reading more<br />

available than practice in maths) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to ‘differences in the material’s susceptibility to memory decay (with<br />

factual <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> procedural knowledge more easily forgotten than c<strong>on</strong>ceptual knowledge)’. As in the NFER<br />

study, background factors may also be c<strong>on</strong>tributing. ‘<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se income differences (middle vs. low)’, they<br />

suggest, ‘may also be related to differences in opportunities to practice <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learn’ (ibid: 265).<br />

‘Vulnerable’ subjects at Key Stage 3<br />

22

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!