25.03.2013 Views

Friendships and Adaptation in the Life Course - Psychology ...

Friendships and Adaptation in the Life Course - Psychology ...

Friendships and Adaptation in the Life Course - Psychology ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

356 HARTUP AND STEVENS<br />

als trigger changes <strong>in</strong> relationships <strong>and</strong> that <strong>the</strong>se changes extend<br />

through <strong>the</strong> life course. For example, (a) among very young<br />

children, new coord<strong>in</strong>ations <strong>and</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ations <strong>in</strong> memory<br />

emerge at about 2 years of age, appear<strong>in</strong>g to serve as a basis for<br />

an <strong>in</strong>crease of coord<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>and</strong> collaboration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

between toddlers <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir friends (Brownell, 1986); (b)<br />

changes associated with puberty trigger <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>timacy<br />

between opposite-gender friends but not same-gender friends<br />

(Sharabany, Gershoni, & Hofman, 1981 ); <strong>and</strong> (c) when strength<br />

<strong>and</strong> mobility decl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> old age, one' s friends must <strong>in</strong>itiate social<br />

contact <strong>and</strong> activities, <strong>the</strong>reby chang<strong>in</strong>g relationship reciproci-<br />

ties (O'Connor, 1993; Rawl<strong>in</strong>s, 1992).<br />

To consider friendships across <strong>the</strong> life course requires <strong>the</strong><br />

differentiation of deep structure, which characterizes <strong>the</strong>se rela-<br />

tionships, from surface structure. We use deep structure to refer<br />

to <strong>the</strong> social mean<strong>in</strong>g (essence) of relationships <strong>and</strong> surface<br />

structure to refer to <strong>the</strong> social exchanges that characterize <strong>the</strong>m<br />

at any given moment or <strong>in</strong> any given situation--a convention<br />

that is similar to <strong>the</strong> one used <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistics (Chomsky, 1965).<br />

The friendship deep structure can be identified by researchers<br />

ask<strong>in</strong>g participants to describe a friend or friends--especially<br />

an ideal or hypo<strong>the</strong>tical friend--<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n content analyz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

results. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, among young children, friendship expecta-<br />

tions have been found to center ma<strong>in</strong>ly on common activities<br />

<strong>and</strong> concrete reciprocities ("We play." "And I give <strong>the</strong>m food,<br />

so <strong>the</strong>y give me food back"; Goodnow & Burns, 1985, p. 120).<br />

School-aged children describe friends as underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, loyal,<br />

<strong>and</strong> trustworthy; children expect to spend time with <strong>the</strong>ir friends,<br />

share <strong>in</strong>terests, <strong>and</strong> engage <strong>in</strong> self-disclosure with <strong>the</strong>m ("A<br />

good friend is someone who likes you <strong>and</strong> spends time with<br />

you <strong>and</strong> forgives you <strong>and</strong> doesn't actually bash you up"; p.<br />

120). Children do not use words like <strong>in</strong>timate to describe <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

friends, but such constructs beg<strong>in</strong> to differentiate <strong>the</strong>se relation-<br />

ships shortly before adolescence (Bigelow, 1977; Selman,<br />

1980). Older <strong>in</strong>dividuals describe an ideal friend ma<strong>in</strong>ly as<br />

be<strong>in</strong>g supportive (dependable, underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> accept<strong>in</strong>g), a<br />

confidant, <strong>and</strong> trustworthy. Most important, high school seniors,<br />

newlyweds, middle-aged parents, <strong>and</strong> soon-to-be retirees differ<br />

relatively little from one ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir emphasis on <strong>the</strong>se<br />

reciprocities when <strong>the</strong>y describe an ideal friend. Similarity be-<br />

tween friends (shar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terests, experiences, <strong>and</strong> activities as<br />

well as communicative compatibility) is regarded, however, as<br />

an important friendship attribute among adolescents but de-<br />

creases subsequently (L. Weiss & Lowenthal, 1975).<br />

Close exam<strong>in</strong>ation of <strong>the</strong>se results reveals differences as well<br />

as similarities <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> manner <strong>in</strong> which friends are described by<br />

younger <strong>and</strong> older <strong>in</strong>dividuals. Differences occur ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

number of psychological constructs used, complexity <strong>and</strong> orga-<br />

nization of <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>and</strong> ideas, <strong>and</strong> flexibility with which<br />

this <strong>in</strong>formation is used to describe a friend, These age differ-<br />

ences are similar to those recorded <strong>in</strong> person perceptions (Lives-<br />

ley & Bromley, 1973), which probably reflect general cognitive<br />

development. Similarities across age occur <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> extent to which<br />

reciprocity <strong>and</strong> mutuality emerge <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g structure. Reci-<br />

procity does not have narrow connotations <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se descriptions;<br />

most <strong>in</strong>dividuals do not describe friendships as exchange rela-<br />

tionships <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>puts <strong>and</strong> resource exchanges must be equiv-<br />

alent or one <strong>in</strong>dividual's behavior must exactly match ano<strong>the</strong>r's.<br />

Never<strong>the</strong>less, children <strong>and</strong> adults of all ages consider <strong>the</strong>se rela-<br />

tionships to be marked by reciprocation, that is, mutuality--<br />

<strong>the</strong> giv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> tak<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> return<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>d or degree. On this<br />

basis, <strong>the</strong>n, we argue that <strong>the</strong> friendship deep structure <strong>in</strong> West-<br />

ern cultures is best described as "symmetrical reciprocity" (see<br />

H<strong>in</strong>de, 1979; <strong>and</strong> Youniss, 1980).<br />

Because reciprocity constitutes <strong>the</strong> friendship deep structure<br />

across <strong>the</strong> life course, certa<strong>in</strong> outcomes or consequences should<br />

be evident at all ages. Social reciprocities should be significant<br />

sources of security (Bowlby, t969), self-worth (Sullivan,<br />

1953), <strong>and</strong> well-be<strong>in</strong>g (R. S. Weiss, 1973). These conditions,<br />

<strong>in</strong> turn, should support successful cop<strong>in</strong>g, especially with devel-<br />

opmental transitions such as school entrance, puberty, workforce<br />

entrance, marriage, child bear<strong>in</strong>g, spouse' s death, <strong>and</strong> retirement<br />

(Magnusson, Statt<strong>in</strong>, & Allen, 1985). On <strong>the</strong> basis that friend-<br />

ship reciprocities support cop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this fashion, one expects<br />

<strong>the</strong>se relationships to promote good outcomes regardless of age.<br />

Surface structures--<strong>the</strong> actual exchanges that occur between<br />

friends--differ from situation to situation <strong>and</strong> from early child-<br />

hood to old age. Social reciprocities between two toddlers, for<br />

example, are manifested ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>in</strong> time spent toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>and</strong> con-<br />

nected <strong>in</strong>teractions (Howes, 1989). Reciprocities between k<strong>in</strong>-<br />

dergarten friends are more elaborate socially but rema<strong>in</strong> basi-<br />

cally concrete, consist<strong>in</strong>g of play <strong>and</strong> shar<strong>in</strong>g (Howes, 1983).<br />

Reciprocities among adolescent friends consist of common ac-<br />

tivities (especially socializ<strong>in</strong>g), augmented by self-disclosure<br />

<strong>and</strong> expectations of loyalty <strong>and</strong> trust (Berndt, 1989). Reciproci-<br />

ties among adult friends are centered on work activities, <strong>and</strong><br />

many friendships become "fused" or "blended" with work<br />

(Hess, 1972; W<strong>in</strong>stead, Derlega, & Montgomery, 1995). O<strong>the</strong>r<br />

fusions occur with marriage; husb<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> wives frequently<br />

share friends with whom issues <strong>in</strong> marriage <strong>and</strong> family relations<br />

are stressed (Hess, 1972; Rawl<strong>in</strong>s, 1992). Reciprocities among<br />

older persons are more separated from family <strong>and</strong> work but<br />

concern support issues <strong>and</strong> companionship. Surface structures<br />

at this time <strong>in</strong>clude friends exchang<strong>in</strong>g letters <strong>and</strong> gifts, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

on <strong>the</strong> telephone, do<strong>in</strong>g favors for one ano<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>and</strong> express<strong>in</strong>g<br />

affection <strong>and</strong> mutual respect (Rawl<strong>in</strong>s, 1992; Shea, Thomp-<br />

son, & Blieszner, 1988). What friends talk about <strong>and</strong> what <strong>the</strong>y<br />

do with one ano<strong>the</strong>r thus change with age <strong>and</strong> circumstance.<br />

We believe that <strong>the</strong>se changes <strong>in</strong> surface structure ma<strong>in</strong>ly<br />

reflect changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> developmental tasks (Havighurst, 1953)<br />

confront<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong>volved. So, among young children,<br />

friendship reciprocities support <strong>the</strong> acquisition of new social<br />

skills, especially expertise <strong>in</strong> cooperation <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r socially co-<br />

ord<strong>in</strong>ated skills (e.g., conflict resolution). Among older children<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents, <strong>the</strong> emphasis shifts to <strong>in</strong>timacy <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r ex-<br />

changes that support a sense of self-identity, sensitivity with<br />

respect to <strong>the</strong> needs of o<strong>the</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of mutually<br />

oriented relationships with "agemates"--<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g members<br />

of both <strong>the</strong> same <strong>and</strong> opposite genders (Sullivan, 1953). Among<br />

young adults, friendshiPS center on a new collection of develop-<br />

mental tasks--work <strong>and</strong> family issues (Hess, 1972). F<strong>in</strong>ally,<br />

when older <strong>in</strong>dividuals confront major life transitions that re-<br />

quire reorganization of one's lifestyle <strong>and</strong> expectations, friend-<br />

ship content changes once aga<strong>in</strong>. Clearly, <strong>the</strong> socializ<strong>in</strong>g func-<br />

tions of <strong>the</strong>se relationships are closely tied to <strong>the</strong> developmental<br />

challenges that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong>volved face, whatever <strong>the</strong>ir age.<br />

Some researchers suggested that friendship surface structures<br />

are ma<strong>in</strong>ly socializ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> childhood <strong>and</strong> adolescence because<br />

<strong>the</strong>y support <strong>in</strong>novation <strong>in</strong> both social underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> social<br />

skill, but <strong>the</strong>y are ma<strong>in</strong>ly susta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> old age, through which

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!