02.07.2013 Views

Pinker - Bloom - NATURAL LANGUAGE AND NATURAL SELECTION

Pinker - Bloom - NATURAL LANGUAGE AND NATURAL SELECTION

Pinker - Bloom - NATURAL LANGUAGE AND NATURAL SELECTION

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

PINKER & BLOOM / <strong>NATURAL</strong> <strong>LANGUAGE</strong> <strong>AND</strong> <strong>NATURAL</strong> <strong>SELECTION</strong> 20<br />

Since the mere appearance of some nonoptimal feature is inconclusive, we must examine<br />

specific explanations for why the feature exists. In the case of the nonselectionist position<br />

espoused by Piattelli-Palmarini, there is none: not a hint of how any specific aspect of<br />

grammar might be explained, even in principle, as a specific consequence of some<br />

developmental process or genetic mechanism or constraint on possible brain structure. The<br />

position gains all its support from the supposed lack of an adaptive explanation. In fact, we<br />

will show that there is such an explanation, well-motivated both within evolutionary theory<br />

and within linguistics, so the support disappears.<br />

The idea that natural selection aspires toward perfection has long been discredited within<br />

evolutionary theory (Williams, 1966). As Maynard Smith (1984: 290) has put it, "If there<br />

were no constraints on what is possible, the best phenotype would live forever, would be<br />

impregnable to predators, would lay eggs at an infinite rate, and so on." Tradeoffs among<br />

conflicting adaptive goals are a ubiquitous limitation on optimality in the design of<br />

organisms. It may be adaptive for a male bird to advertise his health to females with gaudy<br />

plumage or a long tail, but not to the extent that predators are attracted or flight is<br />

impossible.<br />

Tradeoffs of utility within language are also unavoidable (Bolinger, 1980; Slobin, 1977).<br />

For example, there is a conflict of interest between speaker and hearer. Speakers want to<br />

minimize articulatory effort and hence tend towards brevity and phonological reduction.<br />

Hearers want to minimize the effort of understanding and hence desire explicitness and<br />

clarity. This conflict of interest is inherent to the communication process and operates at<br />

many levels. Editors badger authors into expanding elliptical passages; parsimonious<br />

headline writers unwittingly produce Squad Helps Dog Bite Victim and Stud Tires Out.<br />

Similarly there is a conflict of interest between speaker and learner. A large vocabulary<br />

allows for concise and precise expression. But it is only useful if every potential listener has<br />

had the opportunity to learn each item. Again, this tradeoff is inherent to communication;<br />

one man's jargon term is another's mot juste.<br />

Clearly, any shared system of communication is going to have to adopt a code that is a<br />

compromise among these demands, and so will appear to be arbitrary from the point of<br />

view of any one criterion. There is always a large range of solutions to the combined<br />

demands of communication that reach slightly different equilibrium points in this<br />

multidimensional space. Slobin (1977) points out that the Serbo-Croatian inflectional<br />

system is "a classic Indo-European synthetic muddle," suffixing each noun with a single<br />

affix from a paradigm full of irregularity, homophony, and zero-morphemes. As a result the<br />

system is perfected late and with considerable difficulty. In contrast the Turkish inflectional<br />

system is semantically transparent, with strings of clearly demarcated regular suffixes, and<br />

is mastered by the age of two. When it comes to production by an adult who has<br />

overlearned the system, however, Serbo-Croatian does have an advantage in minimizing<br />

the sheer number of syllables that must be articulated. Furthermore, Slobin points out that<br />

such tradeoffs can be documented in studies of historical change and borrowing. For<br />

example changes that serve to enhance brevity will proceed until comprehension becomes<br />

impaired, at which point new affixes or distinctions are introduced to restore the balance<br />

(see also Samuels, 1972). A given feature of language may be arbitrary in the sense that

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!