10.02.2014 Views

A Review of the Current Research on Vocabulary Instruction - U.S. ...

A Review of the Current Research on Vocabulary Instruction - U.S. ...

A Review of the Current Research on Vocabulary Instruction - U.S. ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure to targeted vocabulary words<br />

Higher frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure to targeted vocabulary words will increase <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> likelihood that young<br />

children will understand and remember <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> meanings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new words and use <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>m more frequently.<br />

In a multiple study research design, Biemiller and Boote (2006) found that repeated reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a storybook<br />

resulted in greater average gains in word knowledge by young children. The researchers found that students made<br />

an average gain <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 12% compared with <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol group (children who <strong>on</strong>ly heard <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> story read <strong>on</strong>ce), as<br />

measured by a vocabulary test that assessed <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words within c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

These results duplicate findings by Coyne, Simm<strong>on</strong>s, Kame`enui, and Stoolmiller (2004), who researched<br />

how instructi<strong>on</strong>al time should be allocated to meet <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> intensive needs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> children at-risk for reading difficulties.<br />

Although rereading stories and text demand additi<strong>on</strong>al instructi<strong>on</strong>al time, <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> increase in word learning for at-risk<br />

children makes rereading an effective use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> time. A study by Justice, Meier, and Walpole (2005) that investigated<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> effectiveness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> rereading text to enhance word learning also provided evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> positive impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

exposure to targeted words through repeated readings.<br />

Ano<str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>r study, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> third graders, found that semantic and lexical knowledge accrues over time. Greater gains<br />

were made in semantic (meaning-based) knowledge when students had greater frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure to <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

targeted words. The authors found a more gradual effect <strong>on</strong> lexical knowledge (McGregor, Sheng, & Ball, 2007).<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>, Snowling, and Clarke (2006) studied a group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> eight- and nine-year-olds to determine individual<br />

differences in vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in children who have impaired reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. The findings indicate<br />

that poor comprehenders needed as many trials as <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol group (children without comprehensi<strong>on</strong> deficits) to<br />

learn <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>ological form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four n<strong>on</strong>sense words. It was learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning, or definiti<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> “words”<br />

that clearly separated <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> children who struggled with comprehending text from those who did not have<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> difficulties. The findings indicate that <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> source <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> poor comprehenders’ difficulties with lexical<br />

learning may be rooted in semantic, ra<str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>r than ph<strong>on</strong>ological, learning differences.<br />

Explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> targeted vocabulary words<br />

Explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words and <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir meanings increases <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> likelihood that young children will<br />

understand and remember <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> meanings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> new words.<br />

Biemiller and Boote (2006) found that while rereading stories improved students’ understanding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word<br />

meanings by 12%, an additi<strong>on</strong>al 10% gain occurred when word explanati<strong>on</strong>s were taught directly during <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> storybook. Biemiller and Boote suggest that teachers introduce more ra<str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>r than fewer word<br />

meanings during read-alouds, stating that increasing <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> oral vocabulary <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> K–2 students by 400 word meanings<br />

per year is a reas<strong>on</strong>able goal. A similar study in Ipswich, England (Cain, 2007), with third grade students,<br />

investigated whe<str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>r or not <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word explanati<strong>on</strong>s (definiti<strong>on</strong>s) facilitated students’ word learning. The<br />

investigator found that although students made gains when explanati<strong>on</strong>s were provided for unfamiliar words, <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>y<br />

made <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> greatest increases when <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>y explained <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir own definiti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> targeted words.<br />

Although <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>re is str<strong>on</strong>g evidence supporting explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary, a questi<strong>on</strong> remains regarding<br />

which aspect or model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructi<strong>on</strong> is best. Investigating approaches to explicit vocabulary instructi<strong>on</strong>, Nash and<br />

Snowling (2006) found that using a c<strong>on</strong>textual approach to instructi<strong>on</strong> provided greater vocabulary gains<br />

compared with less<strong>on</strong>s that emphasized learning word definiti<strong>on</strong>s. Their findings also indicated that recalling <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

pr<strong>on</strong>unciati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g> unfamiliar words proved more difficult than learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>the</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir definiti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

4

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!