04.11.2014 Views

Women's Scores on the Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation Scales ...

Women's Scores on the Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation Scales ...

Women's Scores on the Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation Scales ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

WOMEN’S SIS=SES SCORES<br />

Janssen and colleagues (2002a) designed <strong>the</strong> to assess<br />

<strong>the</strong> tendency to resp<strong>on</strong>d sexually to a variety of erotic<br />

stimuli. The items <strong>on</strong> this measure c<strong>on</strong>sist primarily of<br />

‘‘if ...<strong>the</strong>n’’ statements (i.e., ‘‘If I am <strong>on</strong> my own watching<br />

a sexual scene in a film, I quickly become sexually<br />

aroused’’). Exploratory factor analysis of data provided<br />

by 408 male university undergraduates unexpectedly<br />

yielded three factors ra<strong>the</strong>r than two: <strong>on</strong>e related to sexual<br />

excitati<strong>on</strong> and two associated with sexual inhibiti<strong>on</strong><br />

(Janssen et al., 2002a). The excitati<strong>on</strong> factor (SES)<br />

describes sexual arousal stemming from social interacti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

visual stimuli, fantasies, and n<strong>on</strong>sexual situati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(such as bathing or lying in <strong>the</strong> sun). SIS1 focuses <strong>on</strong><br />

sexual inhibiti<strong>on</strong> due to threat of performance failure<br />

(e.g., difficulty getting aroused, losing arousal easily,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cern about pleasing a partner, etc.). SIS2 assesses<br />

sexual inhibiti<strong>on</strong> due to potential c<strong>on</strong>sequences of sex<br />

(such as <strong>the</strong> risk of being caught=intruded up<strong>on</strong>, sexually<br />

transmitted diseases [STDs], unwanted pregnancy,<br />

and pain) (Janssen et al., 2002a). The three-factor structure<br />

was c<strong>on</strong>firmed in a sample of 459 undergraduate<br />

men, as well as a university-based sample of 313 male<br />

faculty and staff (Janssen et al., 2002a). Intercorrelati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

indicated that <strong>the</strong> excitati<strong>on</strong> factor (SES) and <strong>the</strong><br />

two inhibiti<strong>on</strong> factors (SIS1 and SIS2) were relatively<br />

independent (Janssen et al., 2002a, 2002b). SIS1 and<br />

SIS2 were related but not highly correlated (r ¼þ.26),<br />

suggesting that <strong>the</strong>y do not measure substantially overlapping<br />

c<strong>on</strong>structs.<br />

Results also suggested that SIS=SES scores showed<br />

acceptable test–retest reliability (r ¼þ.67 and þ.76 for<br />

<strong>the</strong> two samples) and good c<strong>on</strong>vergent=discriminant validity<br />

(Janssen et al., 2002a, 2002b). SES scores were modestly<br />

positively associated with scores <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Behavioral<br />

Inhibiti<strong>on</strong>=Behavioral Activati<strong>on</strong> <strong>Scales</strong> (BIS=BAS;<br />

Carver & White, 1994), <strong>the</strong> Neuroticism subscale of <strong>the</strong><br />

Eysenck Pers<strong>on</strong>ality Questi<strong>on</strong>naire (EPQ; Eysenck &<br />

Eysenck, 1975), and <strong>the</strong> Sociosexual Orientati<strong>on</strong> Inventory<br />

(SOI; Simps<strong>on</strong> & Gangestad, 1991), and more<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly related to <strong>Sexual</strong> Opini<strong>on</strong> Survey (SOS) scores<br />

(Fisher, Byrne, White, & Kelley, 1988). See <strong>the</strong> Method<br />

secti<strong>on</strong> for a more detailed descripti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong>se measures.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Scores</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> SIS1 and SIS2 showed some overlap: both<br />

were modestly positively associated with scores <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Harm Avoidance Scale (MPQ-H). Only SIS2 scores,<br />

however, showed moderate negative correlati<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

SOS and SOI scores, and moderate positive relati<strong>on</strong>ships<br />

with scores <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Social Desirability Scale and <strong>the</strong><br />

BIS=BAS Behavioral Inhibiti<strong>on</strong> subscale.<br />

There are a number of reas<strong>on</strong>s to expect women and<br />

men to show different propensities for sexual inhibiti<strong>on</strong><br />

and excitati<strong>on</strong>. Bjorklund and Kipp (1996) suggest that<br />

gender differences in parental investment may have<br />

rendered sexual inhibiti<strong>on</strong> of particular importance to<br />

our female ancestors. Because reproducti<strong>on</strong> is associated<br />

with greater biological costs for females than for males<br />

(e.g., pregnancy), females especially would benefit from<br />

any mechanism that aids in c<strong>on</strong>trolling sexual desire and<br />

arousal (Wallen, 1995) and avoiding casual or opportunistic<br />

sex (Bjorklund & Kipp, 1996; Sym<strong>on</strong>s, 1979).<br />

In c<strong>on</strong>trast, our male ancestors may have benefited from<br />

higher levels of sexual excitati<strong>on</strong>. As <strong>the</strong> cost associated<br />

with fertilizati<strong>on</strong> is low, males may have increased<br />

reproductive potential by engaging in relatively indiscriminate,<br />

short-term mating with multiple partners<br />

(Buss, 1998; Knoth, Boyd, & Singer, 1988).<br />

Many reports of sexuality-related gender differences<br />

appear relevant to dual c<strong>on</strong>trol model processes.<br />

Research has found that males report earlier and more<br />

intense sexual arousal than females (Knoth, Boyd, &<br />

Singer, 1988), more frequent sexual desire (Beck,<br />

Bozman, & Qualtrough, 1991), and earlier, more<br />

frequent, and more varied sexual fantasies (Leitenberg<br />

& Henning, 1995). Men also spend more m<strong>on</strong>ey than<br />

women <strong>on</strong> sex, sexual products, and sexual entertainment<br />

(Baumeister, Catanese, & Vohs, 2001; Wallen,<br />

1995). In c<strong>on</strong>trast, women report less permissive attitudes<br />

regarding casual, premarital, and extramarital<br />

sex in comparis<strong>on</strong> with men (Clark & Hatfield, 1989;<br />

Cohen & Shotland, 1996; Oliver Hyde, 1993; Wuethrich,<br />

1993), as well as fewer desired (Buss & Schmitt, 1993)<br />

and actual lifetime sexual partners (Kinsey, Pomeroy,<br />

Martin, & Gebhard, 1953; Laumann, Gagn<strong>on</strong>, Michael,<br />

& Michaels, 1994; Wiederman, 1997). In a recent review,<br />

Baumeister and colleagues (2001) c<strong>on</strong>cluded that <strong>the</strong><br />

evidence for str<strong>on</strong>ger sex drive in men than women is ‘‘extensive,<br />

methodologically diverse, and c<strong>on</strong>sistent (p. 263).’’<br />

<strong>Sexual</strong> interest and expressi<strong>on</strong> clearly also are resp<strong>on</strong>sive<br />

to social influences (Hogben & Byrne, 1998). <strong>Sexual</strong><br />

attitudes and behaviors are related to a number of sociocultural<br />

factors, including educati<strong>on</strong>, religious affiliati<strong>on</strong><br />

and activity, marital status, and socioec<strong>on</strong>omic status<br />

(Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948; Laumann et al.,<br />

1994). More social c<strong>on</strong>trol of sexuality may be directed<br />

toward women than toward men (Crawford & Popp,<br />

2003), however, resulting in a sexual double standard.<br />

Female undergraduates report significantly more guilt<br />

associated with first intercourse than do males (Sprecher<br />

& Barbee, 1995), and female virgins report more positive<br />

and fewer negative emoti<strong>on</strong>s associated with <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

virginity than do males (Sprecher & Regan, 1996). In<br />

summary, <strong>the</strong> origins of gender differences in sexual<br />

behavior appear multifaceted, and <strong>the</strong> potential c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of social c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> and biological factors<br />

deserve shared c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> when interpreting data<br />

<strong>on</strong> sexual interest, behavior, and resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

Given extensive evidence for sexuality-related gender<br />

differences, Graham, Sanders, Milhausen, and McBride<br />

(2004) questi<strong>on</strong>ed whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> SIS=SES would be<br />

equally suited for use with women. Their specific c<strong>on</strong>cerns<br />

included <strong>the</strong> possibility that important influences<br />

<strong>on</strong> women’s arousal (such as reputati<strong>on</strong>, body image,<br />

37

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!