12.07.2015 Views

The Development of Intersensory Temporal Perception: An ...

The Development of Intersensory Temporal Perception: An ...

The Development of Intersensory Temporal Perception: An ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

DEVELOPMENT OF INTERSENSORY TEMPORAL PERCEPTION 287and a set <strong>of</strong> developmental principles designed to account for thedevelopmental emergence <strong>of</strong> responsiveness to intersensory temporalrelations.A Taxonomy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Intersensory</strong> <strong>Temporal</strong> Relations and<strong>The</strong>ir <strong>Perception</strong> in Early <strong>Development</strong>As events flow over time, they provide an observer with a steadystream <strong>of</strong> temporally organized and intermodally integrated information(Ashton, 1976; Lashley, 1951; Lewkowicz, 1989; C. L.Miller & Byme, 1984). For example, when infants interact withtheir caretakers they must constantly deal with a wealth <strong>of</strong> multimodal,temporally organized information. That is, each time acaregiver talks and interacts with an infant, the multimodal informationspecifying the caretaker can be specified in terms <strong>of</strong> thetemporal synchrony between audible and visible attributes, theirduration, rate, and rhythmic quality. Figure 1 illustrates this fact inschematic form. As can be seen, when the caregiver produces aword, the voice starts and stops at the same time that the lips startand stop moving. In addition, each individual visible and audibleinstantiation <strong>of</strong> a given word has a specific and equal duration. Ona more global level (i.e., at the level <strong>of</strong> the entire utterance), twotypes <strong>of</strong> intermodally integrated, temporal information are available.First, as the caregiver speaks, he or she utters the words at acertain temporal rate, and that rate is the same regardless <strong>of</strong>whether it is heard or seen. Second, the caregiver's utterance isusually imbued with an overall rhythmic quality that also is thesame whether it is heard or seen. One key aspect <strong>of</strong> the temporalinformation characterizing the utterance illustrated in Figure 1 isthat it is embedded. For example, the temporal synchrony betweenthe audible and visible attributes <strong>of</strong> the utterance depicted inFigure 1 is embedded within intersensory duration, rate, andrhythm equivalence. Put differently, in order for the infant toperceive the equivalence <strong>of</strong> the audible and visible components <strong>of</strong>the utterance in terms <strong>of</strong> either their duration, rate, or rhythmicstructure, the infant must be able to perceive their synchronousonsets and <strong>of</strong>fsets. Likewise, in order for the infant to perceiveA-V equivalence based on the overall rhythmic quality <strong>of</strong> theutterance, the infant must be able to perceive the equivalence <strong>of</strong> theaudible and visible components and <strong>of</strong> the intervals separatingthem in terms <strong>of</strong> their duration.Are infants sensitive and responsive to the various types <strong>of</strong>temporal information shown in Figure 1 ? This is a two-part ques-Audible: Boy, you look sooo goood !Visible: ~ ~ ~, ~ '<strong>Temporal</strong> :TimeFigure 1. A typical temporally organized event illustrating the varioustypes <strong>of</strong> intersensory temporal relations.Ption. One part is whether infants are sensitive and responsive totemporal information regardless <strong>of</strong> whether it is uni- or multimodal.<strong>The</strong> other is whether infants are sensitive and responsive tointersensory temporal relations. In general, evidence indicates thatinfants are sensitive and responsive to different types <strong>of</strong> temporalvariations. For example, infants exhibit sensitivity to a change inthe temporal distribution <strong>of</strong> stimulation (Brooks & Berg, 1979;Clifton, 1974; Davies & Berg, 1983; Donohue & Berg, 1991) andcan perform anticipatory responses based on temporal information.<strong>The</strong> latter ability is evident in the fact that they can preform theirhands in anticipation <strong>of</strong> the future position <strong>of</strong> an object as it movesthrough space (Lockman, Ashmead, & Bushnell, 1984; von H<strong>of</strong>sten,1980, 1985) and can learn to anticipate the spatial position <strong>of</strong>an object as evidenced by anticipatory eye movements (Canfield &Halth, 1991). Finally, infants can organize their behavioral actionsin a temporally meaningful way (Stem, Beebe, Jaffe, & Bennet,1977) and to such an extent that their interactions with a caregiverare usually characterized by relatively precise temporal interlocking(Lester, H<strong>of</strong>fman, & Brazelton, 1985). Indeed, it appears thatthese types <strong>of</strong> temporally based perceptual and motor capacities ininfancy may provide an important developmental foundation forthe emergence <strong>of</strong> cognitive abilities later in life. Thus, Haith andhis colleagues (Dougherty & Haith, 1997) have shown that there isa relationship between infant visual reaction time/visual anticipationat 3.5 months <strong>of</strong> age and childhood full-scale IQ on theWechsler Preschool and Primary Scale <strong>of</strong> Intelligence---Revisedat 46 months <strong>of</strong> age.Given that infants can perceive and respond to temporallyorganized information, it might be expected that they also couldperceive the types <strong>of</strong> intersensory equivalence relations illustratedin Figure 1. In general, findings regarding infants' responsivenessto these types <strong>of</strong> relations indicate that infants are, in fact, sensitiveand responsive to some <strong>of</strong> these intersensory temporal relations. Inaddition, the findings indicate that the ability to respond to differentaspects <strong>of</strong> multimodal temporal structure emerges at differenttimes in development. Specifically, it seems that intersensoryresponsiveness to temporal synchrony emerges first, followed bythe emergence <strong>of</strong> responsiveness to duration, rate, and rhythm inthat order.<strong>Perception</strong> <strong>of</strong> A-V <strong>Temporal</strong> Synchrony and the<strong>Intersensory</strong> <strong>Temporal</strong> Contiguity Window<strong>Intersensory</strong> temporal synchrony is a fundamental temporaldimension that is likely to serve as an important foundation forintersensory integration in early development (Edelman, 1992;<strong>The</strong>len & Smith, 1994) because it can provide a simple andrelatively easy basis for the perception <strong>of</strong> intersensory unity.Perhaps it is for this reason that responsiveness to temporal synchronyis one <strong>of</strong> the most intensively studied aspects <strong>of</strong> intersensoryperception in infants. In general, empirical findings haveshown that infants are sensitive and responsive to intersensorytemporal synchrony (Bahrick, 1988; Lewkowicz, 1986, 1992a,1992b, 1996b; Speike, 1988; Speike, Born, & Chu, 1983) and thatresponsiveness to it emerges very early in life. Moreover, Bahrick(1998) has recently reported that A-V temporal synchrony hasdevelopmental priority over other intersensory relations. Sheshowed that infants as young as 4 weeks <strong>of</strong> age respond to thedisruption <strong>of</strong> the synchronous relationship between an object's

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!