20.07.2015 Views

Vocabulary use in the FCE Listening test - Cambridge English Exams

Vocabulary use in the FCE Listening test - Cambridge English Exams

Vocabulary use in the FCE Listening test - Cambridge English Exams

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

4 | CAMBRIDGE ESOL : RESEARCH NOTES : ISSUE 32 / MAY 2008Figure 1: Extract from Costa RicaRight now <strong>the</strong>re’s a big problem with deforestation <strong>in</strong> Costa Ricaand one of <strong>the</strong> th<strong>in</strong>gs that we need to do is to provide educationand we have a great opportunity here. We’ve got an educationprogramme <strong>in</strong> place where we will br<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>in</strong>, free ofcharge and tell <strong>the</strong>m about er <strong>the</strong> canopy and why it should besaved…Figure 2: Extract from Cable Car… need to do to stop that is to provide education. We’ve got aprogramme <strong>in</strong> place where we will br<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>in</strong> from all over<strong>the</strong> world and tell <strong>the</strong>m about <strong>the</strong> forest and <strong>the</strong>y can see for<strong>the</strong>mselves why it should be saved.Creat<strong>in</strong>g word frequency listsWordlists were made by runn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> two corpora throughWordSmith Tools (Stubbs 1996). The result<strong>in</strong>g exam textswordlist and radio texts wordlist were compared topublished British National Corpus (BNC) lists for spokenand written language (Leech, Rayson, Wilson 2001).The wordlists were <strong>the</strong>n <strong>use</strong>d to make key word lists <strong>in</strong>WordSmith Tools, us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> larger composite radio text as areference text. The KeyWord tool f<strong>in</strong>ds words which aresignificantly more frequent <strong>in</strong> one text than ano<strong>the</strong>r. If aword is unusually <strong>in</strong>frequent <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> smaller corpus (<strong>the</strong>exam texts here), it is said to be a negative key word andwill appear at <strong>the</strong> end of <strong>the</strong> list.Concordances were <strong>the</strong>n run on selected words, so that<strong>the</strong>ir usage <strong>in</strong> both sets of texts could be studied <strong>in</strong> moredetail.Results: lexical densityAs can be seen from Figure 3, all texts ranged from 30% to44% lexical density. This is lower at <strong>the</strong> bottom and top of<strong>the</strong> range than Stubbs’ (1996) f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g for spoken texts(34% to 58%). At <strong>the</strong> upper end, this difference can beaccounted for, as most of <strong>the</strong> texts studied here aredialogues and would not be expected to have particularlyhigh lexical densities. The presence of results which arelower than 34% could, however, suggest that <strong>the</strong> methodfor calculat<strong>in</strong>g lexical density <strong>use</strong>d <strong>in</strong> this study createddifferent results from Stubbs’ method.None of <strong>the</strong> texts analysed, whe<strong>the</strong>r exam or radio texts,have a lexical density greater than 44%, even though someof <strong>the</strong>m are monologues where <strong>the</strong>re is no feedback. Thiswould suggest that all <strong>the</strong>se radio texts are dialogic <strong>in</strong> someway, with speakers regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> listeners as <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction to some extent, even though <strong>the</strong>re is nooption for actual feedback.It is hard to see a particular pattern when compar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>exam texts to <strong>the</strong> radio texts; some exam texts have higherlexical density than <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g radio texts (fivetexts) and some radio texts have higher lexical density thanexam texts (four texts). Overall though, <strong>the</strong> exam texts havea slightly higher lexical density. The average is 37.5% asopposed to 36.8% for <strong>the</strong> radio texts.An <strong>in</strong>dependent t-<strong>test</strong> for significance was carried outus<strong>in</strong>g SPSS©. There was no significant difference foundbetween <strong>the</strong> conditions (t=.443, df= 16, p=.663, twotailed). This shows that <strong>the</strong> difference between <strong>the</strong> meanlexical densities of <strong>the</strong> exam texts and <strong>the</strong> radio texts is notsignificant to 95% probability. That is to say, it isreasonable to assume that <strong>the</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> mean areattributable to chance.What is noticeable, however, is <strong>the</strong> range of densities <strong>in</strong><strong>the</strong> texts. The difference between <strong>the</strong> highest and lowestdensities on <strong>the</strong> radio texts is 13.5%. On <strong>the</strong> exam textsthis difference is only 6.5%, so it seems <strong>the</strong>re is a tendencyfor <strong>the</strong> radio texts to have more variation <strong>in</strong> lexical densityand <strong>the</strong> exam texts to conform to an average density.Results: word frequencyTable 2 shows <strong>the</strong> top 50 words <strong>in</strong> radio texts, exam textsand, for comparison, <strong>the</strong> BNC spoken corpus and BNCwritten corpus.Figure 3:Lexical density of radioand exam textsPercentage lexical density45.0040.0035.0030.00Exam textRadio text25.0020.00Urban WildlifeJanet EllisCosta RicaJames DysonParakeetsPatricia RoutledgeVictoria BeckhamBonesLara HartTexts©UCLES 2008 – The contents of this publication may not be reproduced without <strong>the</strong> written permission of <strong>the</strong> copyright holder.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!