23.03.2013 Views

Emotional reactions to scandals: When does moral character make ...

Emotional reactions to scandals: When does moral character make ...

Emotional reactions to scandals: When does moral character make ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 1 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: F9BD5A9C<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52<br />

53<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology (2011) DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2011.01344.x<br />

<strong>Emotional</strong> <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>scandals</strong>: <strong>When</strong> <strong>does</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong><br />

<strong>make</strong> a difference?ajsp_ 1..11<br />

Introduction<br />

Jiang Jiang, 1 Yu Kou, 1 Fang Wang, 1 Ying Wu, 2 Yan-Mei Li, 3 Yuan Li, 4 Yiyin Yang, 4<br />

Hui Cao, 5 Qiuping Wu, 5 Shi-Jie Jing, 6 Bi-Jing Jiang, 7 La-Mei Shen, 8 Ai-Juan Li, 9<br />

Zhongquan Li, 10 Wenjun Gao, 11 Chi-Yue Chiu, 12 Ying-Yi Hong, 12 Shih-Chi Hsu, 13<br />

Lin Zhang, 14 Bao-Yan Yang, 15 Xiao-Li Yang, 15 De-Lei Zhao, 16 Zhimin Zou, 16<br />

Qing-Wang Wei, 17 Xia Chen, 18 Yu-Fang Zhao, 19 Yi-Jin Zhou, 20 Hong Chen, 21<br />

Jianping Feng 21 and Xin Wang 21<br />

1 , Beijing Normal Univerity, , , 2 , Beijing Technology and Business University, , , 3 , Chinese Academy of<br />

Sciences, , , 4 , Chinese Academy of Social Sciences, , , 5 , Chinese University of Hong Kong, , , 6 , East-<br />

China University of Political Science and Law, , , 7 , Fuzhou University, , , 8 , Hangzhou Normal University, ,<br />

, 9 , Inner Mongolia University of Technology, , , 10 , Nanjing University, , , 11 , Nankai University, , , 12 ,<br />

Nanyang Technological University, , , 13 , National Taiwan University, , , 14 , Ning Bo University, , , 15 ,<br />

Northwest Normal University, , , 16 , Peking University, , , 17 , Renmin University of China, , , 18 , Shanghai<br />

Jiao Tong University, , , 19 , Southwest University, , , 20 , Sun Yat-sen University, , , and 21 , Yunnan<br />

Normal University, ,<br />

A scandal can evoke public outrage when it is widely publicized and involves an individual who is perceived <strong>to</strong><br />

have a high level of cultural representativeness. Two experiments showed that when the scandalous target’s <strong>moral</strong><br />

<strong>character</strong> was highlighted, emotional responses <strong>to</strong>wards the target were least negative when the scandal was<br />

widely circulated and when the target was seen as a representative of his culture. These results suggest that when<br />

a cultural symbol is implicated in a widely-circulated scandal, the negative emotions directed <strong>to</strong>wards the<br />

scandalous individual can be alleviated when the public remains confident in his or her commitment <strong>to</strong> <strong>moral</strong>ity.<br />

Key words: cultural representativeness, emotional reaction, <strong>moral</strong>ity, scandal.<br />

In this article, we seek <strong>to</strong> establish the conditions under<br />

which negative <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong>wards a scandalous person<br />

would be alleviated, followed by two experiments that<br />

tested these conditions. We argue that when an individual is<br />

involved in a widely-circulated scandal, and when this individual<br />

is perceived <strong>to</strong> be a representative of his or her<br />

culture, negative emotional <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong>wards this individual<br />

would be alleviated when he or she seen as having<br />

strong commitment <strong>to</strong> <strong>moral</strong>ity.<br />

Social psychology of <strong>scandals</strong><br />

A scandal is an action or event regarded as <strong>moral</strong>ly or<br />

legally wrong and causing general public outrage.<br />

The strong emotional <strong>reactions</strong> that <strong>scandals</strong> evoke can<br />

Correspondence: Chi-Yue Chiu, S3-01c-81, Nanyang Business<br />

School, Nanyang Technological University, Nanyang Avenue,<br />

639798, Singapore. Email: cychiu@ntu.edu.sg<br />

The reported study is a collaboration of the members of the<br />

Advanced Social Psychology Summer School co-organized by<br />

the Chinese Academy of Social Sciences and Yunnan Normal<br />

University.<br />

Received ; revision ; accepted .<br />

undermine the legitimacy of the status quo and shake the<br />

culture’s <strong>moral</strong> foundation. For example, in the USA, the<br />

highly-publicized Enron scandal has led <strong>to</strong> widespread<br />

distrust in business ethics. It has also evoked cynicism,<br />

pessimism, paranoia, and fear that are targeted at <strong>to</strong>p business<br />

leaders (Pelletier & Bligh, 2008).<br />

Despite the social and cultural importance of understanding<br />

people’s emotional <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>scandals</strong>, this <strong>to</strong>pic has<br />

received little empirical attention in cultural psychology.<br />

Only 11 peer-reviewed journal articles were found in a<br />

recent PsycINFO search (18 May 2010) using ‘scandal’ and<br />

‘reaction’ as keywords. Nine of these articles present qualitative<br />

interpretations and commentaries on <strong>scandals</strong> of government<br />

offices (Rowe, 2009), political leaders (Williams &<br />

Carpini, 2004), political entities (Capelos & Wurzer, 2009;<br />

Jacobsson & Löfmarck, 2008; Johansson, 2007), financial<br />

institutions (Jang & Chen, 2009; Sims, 2009), religious<br />

organizations (Clark, 2006), or the mass media (Hindman,<br />

2005). Only two articles (Fischle, 2000; Tiedens, 2001)<br />

reported surveys or experimental findings on some of the<br />

determinants of lay people’s responses <strong>to</strong> the Clin<strong>to</strong>n–<br />

Lewinsky scandal. Results from both studies highlight the<br />

importance of affect in lay people’s <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong> the scandal.<br />

In one study, Tiedens (2001) found that Americans supported<br />

Clin<strong>to</strong>n more when they viewed him as expressing<br />

anger about the scandal than when they saw him expressing<br />

sadness about it, possibly because anger expression created<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

3<br />

54<br />

55<br />

56<br />

57<br />

58<br />

59<br />

60<br />

61<br />

62<br />

63<br />

64<br />

65<br />

66<br />

67<br />

68<br />

69<br />

70<br />

71<br />

72<br />

73<br />

74<br />

75<br />

76<br />

77<br />

78<br />

79<br />

80


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 2 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: 95CC566F<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52<br />

53<br />

4 4<br />

2 Jiang Jiang et al.<br />

the impression that Clin<strong>to</strong>n was a competent leader. In<br />

another study, Fischle) tracked public opinion in the USA in<br />

the wake of the Clin<strong>to</strong>n–Lewinsky affair and found that prior<br />

positive affect for Bill Clin<strong>to</strong>n significantly lowered US<br />

citizens’ appraisal of the credibility and perceived importance<br />

of the allegations against the president. These two<br />

studies showed that lay people’s prior affect <strong>to</strong>wards a<br />

scandalous leader and his post-scandal impression management<br />

could moderate lay people’s <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong> the scandal.<br />

The present research seeks <strong>to</strong> extend this line of work by<br />

focusing on how the perceived cultural representativeness of<br />

a scandalous person, the perceived circulation of the scandal,<br />

and the <strong>moral</strong> image of the scandalous person jointly influence<br />

lay people’s emotional <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong> a scandal.<br />

Cultural significance of scandalous<br />

cultural icons<br />

Some <strong>scandals</strong> (e.g. <strong>scandals</strong> of pop singers, sports stars,<br />

rich people) might attract public attention and evoke<br />

amusement because of the news worthiness of the individuals<br />

implicated in the <strong>scandals</strong>. However, <strong>scandals</strong> that<br />

involve im<strong>moral</strong> or unethical behaviours of individuals who<br />

are perceived <strong>to</strong> be representatives of their culture (e.g.<br />

cultural celebrities; political, business, and religious<br />

leaders) often incite public outrage. Although most people<br />

are not officially ordained <strong>moral</strong> representatives of their<br />

culture, at least some people, at their best, are regarded as<br />

sacred symbols that reflect what is held <strong>to</strong> be the best of the<br />

culture and society (Kesebir, in press). Im<strong>moral</strong> acts committed<br />

by these individuals might tarnish the <strong>moral</strong> lustre of<br />

their society. Thus, the society has relatively low <strong>to</strong>lerance<br />

of the <strong>moral</strong> transgressions of these individuals.<br />

In addition, according <strong>to</strong> the intersubjective theory of<br />

cultural processes (Chiu, Gelfand, Yamagishi, Shteynberg,<br />

& Wan, 2010; Wan et al., 2007), every society has a need <strong>to</strong><br />

preserve its core values. A core value in a society is one that<br />

most people in the culture are expected <strong>to</strong> endorse. For<br />

example, filial piety is a core value in Chinese culture, and<br />

most Chinese are expected <strong>to</strong> endorse it. Core values are<br />

instituted in many cultural practices and embodied in many<br />

external media, including the public images of the individuals<br />

who represent the culture. Specifically, the society<br />

expects the behaviours of its cultural representatives <strong>to</strong><br />

exemplify its core values, and a cultural representative is<br />

more positively evaluated if his or her behaviours are consistent<br />

(vs inconsistent) with the society’s core cultural<br />

values. In line with this idea, a recent study (Wan, Torelli, &<br />

Chiu, 2010) showed that US undergraduates who regarded<br />

Tom Hanks as an American cultural symbol liked him more<br />

after reading a widely-circulated, allegedly fictitious s<strong>to</strong>ry<br />

that depicted how he insisted on pursuing his own career<br />

goal despite objections from his parents (behaviours that<br />

are consistent with the US cultural ideal of pursuing one’s<br />

own goals; see Wan et al., 2007). Such effect was not found<br />

among participants who did not regard Tom Hanks as a<br />

cultural symbol, or when the s<strong>to</strong>ry depicted positive but<br />

culturally irrelevant behaviours (conscientiousness or<br />

modesty) of Tom Hanks. In short, when a cultural representative<br />

is implicated in a widely-publicized scandal, the<br />

scandal might evoke public outrage. The present research<br />

concerns the conditions under which emotional <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong><br />

a scandalous cultural representative will be alleviated when<br />

he or she is involved in a widely-circulated scandal.<br />

Present research<br />

Cultural representatives are identity-defining symbols of<br />

their culture. Accordingly, they carry the burden of preserving<br />

the <strong>moral</strong> purity of their culture. That is, motivated by the<br />

desire <strong>to</strong> preserve the <strong>moral</strong> lustre of their culture, the public<br />

will not <strong>to</strong>lerate <strong>moral</strong> transgressions from symbols of their<br />

culture that would contaminate its <strong>moral</strong> purity. Thus, the<br />

involvement of a cultural representative in a widelypublicized<br />

<strong>moral</strong> scandal, if not managed properly, can lead<br />

<strong>to</strong> negative emotions <strong>to</strong>wards the cultural representative.<br />

How can a widely-publicized scandal that involves a<br />

cultural representative be managed? Because cultural representatives<br />

are expected <strong>to</strong> be the bearers of <strong>moral</strong> values<br />

in their culture, we predict that when a cultural representative<br />

is implicated in a widely-circulated scandal, the negative<br />

emotions directed <strong>to</strong>wards him or her can be alleviated<br />

when the public still have confidence in his or her <strong>moral</strong><br />

<strong>character</strong>. Accordingly, a cultural representative who is<br />

involved in a widely-publicized scandal can reduce public<br />

outrage by reassuring the public of his or her commitment<br />

<strong>to</strong> <strong>moral</strong> values. To test this hypothesis, in two experiments,<br />

we asked participants <strong>to</strong> indicate their emotional <strong>reactions</strong><br />

<strong>to</strong> a scandalous man. We manipulated the participants’<br />

impression of this person by reminding the participants in<br />

the experimental condition that he was someone with<br />

strong <strong>moral</strong> values. We hypothesize that when the scandalous<br />

person is seen as a cultural symbol and when the<br />

scandal has become widely circulated and deemed <strong>to</strong> be<br />

believable, the scandalous person can pacify the perceivers<br />

by displaying evidence of his commitment <strong>to</strong> <strong>moral</strong> values.<br />

However, this impression-management strategy would not<br />

be particularly effective when the scandalous person is not<br />

regarded as a cultural representative or when the scandal is<br />

not widely circulated. Thus, in both experiments, we<br />

expected that reminding the participants of the scandalous<br />

person’s <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> would be more effective in alleviating<br />

negative emotions <strong>to</strong>wards the scandalous person<br />

when he was seen as a cultural symbol and when the<br />

scandal was widely known and deemed <strong>to</strong> be believable<br />

than when these conditions were not met.<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

54<br />

55<br />

56<br />

57<br />

58<br />

59<br />

60<br />

61<br />

62<br />

63<br />

64<br />

65<br />

66<br />

67<br />

68<br />

69<br />

70<br />

71<br />

72<br />

73<br />

74<br />

75<br />

76<br />

77<br />

78<br />

79<br />

80<br />

81<br />

82<br />

83<br />

84<br />

85<br />

86<br />

87<br />

88<br />

89<br />

90<br />

91<br />

92<br />

93<br />

94<br />

95<br />

96<br />

97<br />

98<br />

99<br />

100<br />

101<br />

102<br />

103<br />

104<br />

105<br />

106


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 3 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: 910F26DD<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52<br />

53<br />

54<br />

5 5<br />

<strong>Emotional</strong> responses <strong>to</strong> <strong>scandals</strong> 3<br />

Study 1<br />

Method<br />

Design and overview. Participants from China responded<br />

<strong>to</strong> a scandal that involved Ge You ( ), a famous Chinese<br />

ac<strong>to</strong>r. Before presenting the scandal <strong>to</strong> the participants, we<br />

measured their perceptions of Ge You’s cultural representativeness.<br />

Because Ge You is one of the most famous<br />

Chinese ac<strong>to</strong>rs and is known in China as ‘the people’s<br />

movie king’, we expected that a sizable percentage of the<br />

participants would regard him as a representative of<br />

Chinese culture. We manipulated the impression of Ge You<br />

as: (i) someone with strong <strong>moral</strong> values; (ii) a person who<br />

felt misunders<strong>to</strong>od by others; or (iii) a humble person. This<br />

was the first between-subjects fac<strong>to</strong>r. We also manipulated<br />

the circulation of the scandal and its believability. These<br />

manipulations constituted the second and third betweensubjects<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>r, respectively. The dependent measure of<br />

interest in the present study was the participants’ positive<br />

and negative emotional responses <strong>to</strong> Ge You. We chose <strong>to</strong><br />

use a real scandal instead of a fictitious one <strong>to</strong> protect the<br />

ecological validity of our results. Real <strong>scandals</strong> are known<br />

<strong>to</strong> evoke <strong>moral</strong> outrage from the public, whereas fictitious<br />

<strong>scandals</strong> are not.<br />

Participants. Given the high level of cultural heterogeneity<br />

in China, we aimed <strong>to</strong> sample participants from different<br />

regions of China. The participants were 527 undergraduates<br />

from 29 different provinces or regions of China (Shanghai,<br />

Yunnan, inner Mongolia, Beijing, Jilin, Sichuan, Tianjin,<br />

Ningxia, Anhui, Shandong, Shanxi, Guangdong, Guangxi,<br />

Xinjiang, Jiangsu, Jiangxi, Hebei, Henan, Zhejiang,<br />

Hainan, Hubei, Hunan, Gansu, Fujian, Guizhou, Liaoning,<br />

Shanxi, Qinghai, Heilongjiang, Hong Kong). The sample<br />

consisted of 258 men and 269 women, with an average age<br />

of 22.44 years (SD = 6.39).<br />

Stimulus materials, manipulations, and procedures. At the<br />

beginning of the study, <strong>to</strong> refresh the participants’ memory<br />

of Ge You, we presented the following biographical information<br />

of Ge You <strong>to</strong> the participants:<br />

Ge You was born in Raoyang, Hebei. His fac<strong>to</strong>r, Ge Cunzhuang<br />

( ), is a famous ac<strong>to</strong>r. In 1985, Ge You made<br />

his movie debut in Cheng Xia and Her Fiancé. The movies<br />

he has starred in include Wan zhu, To Live, A World Without<br />

Thieves, The Banquet, and If You Were The One.<br />

Next, the participants responded <strong>to</strong> a measure of perceived<br />

cultural representativeness of Ge You, adapted from<br />

Wan et al. (2010). The measure consisted of six items (e.g.<br />

‘Ge You is a symbol of Chinese culture’, ‘Ge You can<br />

represent Chinese cultural values’, and ‘Ge You is a repre-<br />

sentative of Chinese culture’; a=0.85), and the participants<br />

indicated their responses <strong>to</strong> each item on a sevenpoint<br />

scale (1 = ‘strongly disagree’ <strong>to</strong> 7 = ’strongly agree’).<br />

As an alternative <strong>to</strong> measuring the perceived cultural representativeness<br />

of the target, we could have manipulated it<br />

by using celebrities of different levels of cultural representativeness<br />

as stimulus persons. However, because celebrities<br />

differed on many aspects, it would be difficult, if not<br />

impossible, <strong>to</strong> find two celebrities who differ on the level of<br />

cultural representativeness only. Furthermore, given the<br />

fame of the celebrities, the participants might have strong<br />

prior attitudes <strong>to</strong>wards the celebrities based on aspects of<br />

the celebrities that are unrelated <strong>to</strong> cultural representativeness.<br />

The participants then read a scandal that involved Ge<br />

You:<br />

The Beijing Police Department held Yilin Forest responsible<br />

for ‘the biggest illegal direct selling case in the recent years’,<br />

and Ge You was the company’s endorser. In just 2 years,<br />

Yilin Forest has ex<strong>to</strong>rted 16.8 billion Yuan from the public<br />

by selling forestland under the guise of ‘Trustee of Forestland.’<br />

Over 20 000 people from the whole country were<br />

victims in this explosive case, and most of them were<br />

Beijing citizens. Many victims trusted Yilin Forest because<br />

Ge You was the endorser in its advertisements. Ge You had<br />

returned <strong>to</strong> the Beijing Police Department all the endorsement<br />

fees he received from the Yilin Group.<br />

Next, <strong>to</strong> manipulate the perceived <strong>moral</strong>ity of GeYou, we<br />

reminded the participants of a famous quote of Ge You. In<br />

the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> condition, we created the impression<br />

that Ge You valued <strong>moral</strong>ity. In this condition, we <strong>to</strong>ld the<br />

participants: ‘Ge You once said openly, “People should<br />

conduct themselves with benevolence”.’ To evaluate the<br />

effect of <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> manipulation, we included two<br />

control conditions in the experiment. In the misunders<strong>to</strong>od<br />

condition, we mentioned <strong>to</strong> the participants that Ge You<br />

once recited a Chinese idiom expressing the feelings of<br />

being misunders<strong>to</strong>od and being treated unfairly. Specifically,<br />

the participants were reminded that ‘Ge You once<br />

said: “I reveal my heart <strong>to</strong> the moon, but the moon shines on<br />

the ditch”.’ Finally, in the humble person condition, the<br />

participants were reminded of a famous Ge You quote that<br />

expressed his humility: ‘Ge You once said openly: “If I<br />

really get the award, that it would be an undeserved fortune.<br />

This is what I think”.’ All the quotes were real quotes. It is<br />

important <strong>to</strong> note that in both control conditions, the quote<br />

conveyed positive impressions of GeYou, but did not implicate<br />

his <strong>moral</strong>ity.<br />

To assess the effectiveness of the manipulation without<br />

sensitizing the participants <strong>to</strong> its purpose, we recruited a<br />

new sample of 67 Chinese participants (31 men, 36 women;<br />

mean age = 20.94 years, SD = 1.69 years) from the same<br />

population, and asked them <strong>to</strong> read the Ge You news s<strong>to</strong>ry,<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

55<br />

56<br />

57<br />

58<br />

59<br />

60<br />

61<br />

62<br />

63<br />

64<br />

65<br />

66<br />

67<br />

68<br />

69<br />

70<br />

71<br />

72<br />

73<br />

74<br />

75<br />

76<br />

77<br />

78<br />

79<br />

80<br />

81<br />

82<br />

83<br />

84<br />

85<br />

86<br />

87<br />

88<br />

89<br />

90<br />

91<br />

92<br />

93<br />

94<br />

95<br />

96<br />

97<br />

98<br />

99<br />

100<br />

101<br />

102<br />

103<br />

104<br />

105<br />

106<br />

107<br />

108


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 4 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: 88DE4DC6<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52<br />

53<br />

4 Jiang Jiang et al.<br />

followed by one of the three quotes (randomly selected).<br />

After reading the news s<strong>to</strong>ry and the quote, the participants<br />

responded <strong>to</strong> three items that tapped how reading the s<strong>to</strong>ry<br />

affected their perception of Ge You’s <strong>moral</strong>ity: (i) ‘Reading<br />

the s<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>make</strong>s me feel that Ge You is a <strong>moral</strong> person’; (ii)<br />

‘Reading the s<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>make</strong>s me feel that Ge You is a person<br />

with a good conscience’; and (iii) ‘Reading the s<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>make</strong>s<br />

me feel that Ge You was remorseful’. The participants<br />

responded <strong>to</strong> each item on an eight-point scale<br />

(1 = ‘strongly disagree’; 8 = ‘strongly agree’). We <strong>to</strong>ok the<br />

average of the three items <strong>to</strong> form a measure of perceived<br />

<strong>moral</strong>ity (a=0.69).<br />

Next, in the main study, <strong>to</strong> provide a context for the<br />

circulation and believability manipulations, we asked the<br />

participants whether they had heard of the s<strong>to</strong>ry (‘yes’ or<br />

‘no’), and whether they thought the s<strong>to</strong>ry was a real one<br />

(1 = ‘definitely not a real s<strong>to</strong>ry’, 2 = ‘unsure’, and 3 = ‘definitely<br />

a real s<strong>to</strong>ry’).<br />

The circulation and believability manipulations were<br />

introduced immediately after these questions. Specifically,<br />

we <strong>to</strong>ld the participants that before they proceeded <strong>to</strong> the<br />

next section of the survey, they would learn how other<br />

participants responded <strong>to</strong> the previous questions. At this<br />

point, the participants were randomly assigned <strong>to</strong> one of the<br />

circulation ¥ believability conditions. Participants in the<br />

high (low) circulation condition were <strong>to</strong>ld that among 400<br />

participants, 82% (12%) had heard of the s<strong>to</strong>ry; participants<br />

in the high (low) believability condition learned that among<br />

400 participants, 78% (18%) believed that the s<strong>to</strong>ry was a<br />

real one, 16% (42%) were unsure, and 6% (40%) thought<br />

that it was not a real s<strong>to</strong>ry.<br />

Dependent measures. To measure participants’ emotional<br />

responses <strong>to</strong> Ge You, after the participants had read the<br />

stimulus s<strong>to</strong>ry and studied the ‘past results’, we had the<br />

participants indicate the extent <strong>to</strong> which they agreed that Ge<br />

You evoked positive emotions (‘I admire Ge You’, ‘I envy<br />

Ge You’; a=0.66) and negative emotions (‘Ge You scares<br />

me’, ‘Ge You angers me’; a=0.71). We chose these emotions<br />

because the target person was a high-status celebrity,<br />

and positive emotions (admiration, envy) are emotions<br />

evoked by high status (warm targets), whereas negative<br />

emotions (fear and anger) are emotions evoked by high<br />

status (cold targets) (Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002). 1<br />

The participants indicated their extent of agreement with<br />

each item on a seven-point scale (1 = ‘strongly disagree’ <strong>to</strong><br />

7 = ‘strongly agree’).<br />

Results<br />

Manipulation check. Manipulation check results confirmed<br />

that the impression manipulation was successful. To<br />

assess the effectiveness of the impression manipulation, we<br />

performed a one-way, between-subjects ANOVA on the<br />

perceived <strong>moral</strong>ity of Ge You, using the data from the<br />

manipulation check study. The effect of the manipulation<br />

was significant [F(2, 64) = 3.57, p = 0.03, h 2 p = 0.104]. Participants<br />

in the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> condition attributed a marginally<br />

higher level of <strong>moral</strong>ity <strong>to</strong> Ge You (M = 5.20,<br />

SD = 1.29) than those in the misunders<strong>to</strong>od condition<br />

(M = 4.56, SD = 1.18) [t(43) = 1.73, p = 0.09], and a significantly<br />

higher level of <strong>moral</strong>ity than those in the humble<br />

person condition (M = 4.23, SD = 1.26) [t(43) = 2.62,<br />

p = 0.01]. More importantly, the perceived <strong>moral</strong>ity of Ge<br />

You was significantly above the mid-point (4.50) in the<br />

<strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> condition only [t(22) = 2.61, p < 0.05].<br />

Effects of <strong>moral</strong>ity, cultural representativeness, and<br />

circulation. Preliminary analysis results revealed highly<br />

variable perceptions of Ge You’s level of cultural representativeness<br />

(M = 3.32, SD = 1.17, range = 1.00–6.50).<br />

Approximately 30% (30.7%) of the participants had heard<br />

of the s<strong>to</strong>ry before. Most participants (72.7%) were unsure<br />

whether the s<strong>to</strong>ry was real; 21.8% believed that the s<strong>to</strong>ry<br />

was real, and only 5.5% believed it was faked.<br />

We hypothesize that making salient that Ge You valued<br />

<strong>moral</strong>ity would evoke stronger positive emotions and<br />

weaker negative emotions when Ge You was perceived <strong>to</strong><br />

be a cultural symbol and when the scandal was widely<br />

circulated. The measures of positive and negative emotions<br />

were negatively correlated (r =-0.25, p < 0.001). To simplify<br />

our analysis, we subtracted the measure of negative<br />

emotions from that of positive emotions <strong>to</strong> form a measure<br />

of affect balance, with higher scores on the scale indicating<br />

expression of stronger positive and weaker negative emotions<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards Ge You. To test our hypothesis, we performed<br />

an impression ¥ circulation ¥ believability ¥ perceived cultural<br />

representativeness general linear model (GLM) on the<br />

affect balance measure. Impression, circulation, and believability<br />

were between-subjects fac<strong>to</strong>rs in this model. Perceived<br />

cultural representativeness was treated as a<br />

continuous predic<strong>to</strong>r. We centred perceived cultural representativeness<br />

at its grand mean <strong>to</strong> minimize the potential<br />

impact of multicolinearity. We predicted a significant interaction<br />

of impression, circulation, and perceived cultural<br />

representativeness.<br />

The predicted three-way interaction was significant [F(2,<br />

504) = 3.42, p = 0.03, h 2 p = 0.013], but the four-way interaction<br />

was not [F(2, 504) = 1.82, p = 0.16], indicating that<br />

believability did not moderate the three-way interaction. 2<br />

Next, we performed a separate circulation ¥ perceived cultural<br />

representativeness GLM for each impression condition.<br />

In all impression conditions, there was a significant<br />

main effect of perceived cultural representativeness: F(1,<br />

172) = 14.96, p < 0.001, h 2 p = 0.080 in the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong><br />

condition; F(1, 172) = 36.30, p < 0.001, h 2 p = 0.173 in the<br />

misunders<strong>to</strong>od condition; and F(1, 172) = 20.48, p < 0.001,<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

54<br />

55<br />

56<br />

57<br />

58<br />

59<br />

60<br />

61<br />

62<br />

63<br />

64<br />

65<br />

66<br />

67<br />

68<br />

69<br />

70<br />

71<br />

72<br />

73<br />

74<br />

75<br />

76<br />

77<br />

78<br />

79<br />

80<br />

81<br />

82<br />

83<br />

84<br />

85<br />

86<br />

87<br />

88<br />

89<br />

90<br />

91<br />

92<br />

93<br />

94<br />

95<br />

96<br />

97<br />

98<br />

99<br />

100<br />

101<br />

102<br />

103<br />

104<br />

105<br />

106<br />

107


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 5 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: 82979859<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

<strong>Emotional</strong> responses <strong>to</strong> <strong>scandals</strong> 5<br />

h 2 p = 0.106 in the humble person condition. In all impression<br />

conditions, participants reported more positive and<br />

fewer negative emotions <strong>to</strong>wards Ge You when they<br />

regarded him as more representative of Chinese culture (r<br />

between 0.27 and 0.34, p < 0.001). The main effect of circulation<br />

was not significant in any of the three impression<br />

conditions: F 1.90, p 0.17. More importantly, the<br />

circulation ¥ perceived cultural representativeness interaction<br />

was significant in the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> condition only:<br />

F(1, 172) = 5.65, p = 0.02, h 2 p = 0.032 in the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong><br />

condition; F(1, 172) = 0.07, p = 0.80 in the misunders<strong>to</strong>od<br />

condition; and F(1, 172) = 0.05, p = 0.82 in the<br />

humble person condition. 3<br />

In Figure 1, we plot the predicted value of positive (vs<br />

negative) emotional responses when cultural representativeness<br />

was one standard deviation above (below) the mean and<br />

when the scandal was widely circulated or not in the <strong>moral</strong><br />

<strong>character</strong> condition. As predicted, when the impression that<br />

Ge You valued <strong>moral</strong>ity was highlighted, the participants’<br />

emotional responses were most positive when the scandal<br />

was widely circulated and when Ge You was seen as a<br />

representative of Chinese culture. <strong>When</strong> the scandal was<br />

widely circulated, emotional responses <strong>to</strong> GeYou were more<br />

positive when he was seen as more representative of Chinese<br />

culture (r = 0.47, p < 0.001). In addition, when the perceived<br />

cultural representativeness of Ge You was relatively high<br />

(one standard deviation above the mean), emotional<br />

responses <strong>to</strong>wards Ge You were more positive when the<br />

scandal was widely circulated than when it was not, although<br />

the difference was not significant [F(1, 172) = 2.94,<br />

p = 0.09]. In summary, consistent with our hypothesis,<br />

reminding the participants of Ge You’s <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> was<br />

Figure 1 Affect balance <strong>to</strong>wards Ge You in the <strong>moral</strong><br />

value condition when the scandal was widely circulated<br />

or not, and when Ge You was perceived <strong>to</strong> be relatively<br />

representative of Chinese culture (one standard deviation<br />

above the mean) or not (one standard deviation<br />

below the mean). Higher scores on the measure indicate<br />

stronger positive and weaker negative emotions<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards Ge You. ( .... ) low circulation; (—) high<br />

circulation.<br />

more effective in alleviating negative emotions <strong>to</strong>wards him<br />

when he was seen as a cultural symbol and when the scandal<br />

was widely known than when either condition was not met.<br />

Discussion<br />

In the present study, we found support for the idea that<br />

reminding people of a scandalous individual’s <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong><br />

is a particularly effective strategy for reducing negative<br />

emotions <strong>to</strong>wards the scandalous individual when the<br />

scandalous individual is seen as a cultural symbol and<br />

when the scandal has spread widely. Specifically, when<br />

the participants were reminded of Ge You’s previous<br />

public statement about his benevolent values, the participants’<br />

emotional responses were most positive when the<br />

scandal was widely circulated and when he was seen as a<br />

symbol of Chinese culture. This pattern of result is consistent<br />

with the idea that a widely-circulated scandal that<br />

involves a cultural representative can evoke negative emotional<br />

<strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong>wards the cultural representative.<br />

However, these emotional <strong>reactions</strong> can be alleviated if<br />

the public still has confidence in the cultural representative’s<br />

<strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong>.<br />

The believability manipulation did not moderate the predicted<br />

three-way interaction. In fact, this manipulation did<br />

not have any effect on the emotional responses <strong>to</strong>wards Ge<br />

You. This finding suggests that emotional responses<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards a scandalous cultural representative are driven<br />

more by whether the scandal has become public knowledge<br />

than by its perceived truth value. This interpretation is<br />

consistent with the shared reality theory (Echterhoff,<br />

Higgins, & Groll, 2005), which posits that people: (i) construe<br />

an intersubjective reality based on shared knowledge<br />

that might or might not correspond <strong>to</strong> the actual reality; and<br />

(ii) allow the shared knowledge <strong>to</strong> predicate their own judgments<br />

and actions. The shared reality theory has shed light<br />

on the social influence process in interpersonal and small<br />

group contexts (Hausmann, Levine, & Higgins, 2008). The<br />

present study extends its relevance <strong>to</strong> understanding cultural<br />

processes.<br />

Because we used a real scandal in the present investigation,<br />

our results can be generalized <strong>to</strong> emotional <strong>reactions</strong><br />

<strong>to</strong> real (vs hypothetical) <strong>scandals</strong>. However, this research<br />

strategy has a drawback: because some participants had<br />

prior knowledge of the scandal, they might also possess<br />

strong prior attitudes <strong>to</strong>wards Ge You’s involvement in the<br />

scandal. This is not a serious concern, because such prior<br />

knowledge works against our hypothesis; it should wash<br />

out the effects of our subtle experimental manipulations.<br />

Moreover, our results show that prior knowledge of the<br />

scandal did not alter our results. 3 To rule out prior knowledge<br />

of a scandal as a confounding fac<strong>to</strong>r, we sought <strong>to</strong><br />

replicate our major findings with a sample that had no prior<br />

knowledge of the scandal in the next study.<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52<br />

53<br />

54<br />

55<br />

56<br />

57<br />

58<br />

59<br />

60<br />

61<br />

62<br />

63<br />

64<br />

65<br />

66<br />

67<br />

68<br />

69<br />

70<br />

71<br />

72<br />

73<br />

74<br />

75<br />

76<br />

77<br />

78<br />

79<br />

80<br />

81<br />

82<br />

83<br />

84<br />

85<br />

86<br />

87<br />

88<br />

89<br />

90<br />

91<br />

92<br />

93<br />

94<br />

95


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 6 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: 9FECBF3D<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52<br />

6 Jiang Jiang et al.<br />

However, given that our stimulus materials are restricted<br />

<strong>to</strong> a relatively mild scandal of a Chinese movie star with a<br />

moderate level of cultural symbolism, we need <strong>to</strong> further<br />

establish the generality of our conclusions. Specifically, Ge<br />

You was implicated in a scandal only through his mere<br />

association with a scandalous company. In addition, GeYou<br />

returned the endorsement fees he received after the scandal<br />

was made known <strong>to</strong> the public. Thus, it would be interesting<br />

<strong>to</strong> examine whether highlighting the scandalous person’s<br />

commitment <strong>to</strong> core <strong>moral</strong> values in the culture can pacify<br />

the public in more severe <strong>scandals</strong>. To extend the generality<br />

of our results, we examined the joint effects of <strong>moral</strong>ity,<br />

cultural representativeness, and circulation on the Chinese<br />

participants’ <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong> another scandal.<br />

Study 2<br />

There are two major objectives in the present study. First,<br />

we sought <strong>to</strong> replicate the Study 1 results with a scandal<br />

of a different target person. To extend the generality of<br />

our results, we chose Jackie Chan ( ), an internationally<br />

famous celebrity who is widely perceived <strong>to</strong> be a<br />

symbol of Chinese culture. In addition, in this scandal,<br />

Jackie Chan’s personal judgment and decisions were the<br />

direct causes of the scandalous behaviour. Second, <strong>to</strong><br />

further control for the potential confounding effect of<br />

prior knowledge of the scandal on the participants’ judgment,<br />

we recruited participants who had no prior knowledge<br />

of the scandal. Again, we chose a real scandal as the<br />

stimulus material in the present study because it is known<br />

<strong>to</strong> have evoked public outrage among those who were<br />

exposed <strong>to</strong> the scandal.<br />

Method<br />

Participants. The participants were 714 undergraduates<br />

from 31 different provinces or regions of China (Shanghai,<br />

Yunnan, inner Mongolia, Beijing, Jilin, Sichuan,<br />

Tianjin, Anhui, Shandong, Shanxi, Guangdong, Guangxi,<br />

Jiangsu, Jiangxi, Hebei, Henan, Zhejiang, Hainan, Hubei,<br />

Hunan, Macau, Gansu, Fujian, Tibet, Guizhou, Liaoning,<br />

Shanxi, Qinghai, Heilongjiang, Hong Kong). The sample<br />

consisted of 362 men and 352 women, with an average<br />

age of 22.44 years (SD = 6.39). None of these participants<br />

had heard of the Jackie Chan scandal used in the present<br />

study. 4<br />

Stimulus materials, manipulations, and procedures. We<br />

replaced the Ge You scandal in Study 1 with a Jackie<br />

Chan scandal. Despite this major change, the design and<br />

procedures of the present study were similar <strong>to</strong> those in<br />

Study 1. As in Study 1, at the beginning of the study, <strong>to</strong><br />

refresh the participants’ memory of Jackie Chan, we presented<br />

the following biographical information of him <strong>to</strong><br />

the participants:<br />

Fang Shi-Long ( ), also known as Cheng-long or<br />

Jackie Chan, is a famous and influential ac<strong>to</strong>r and direc<strong>to</strong>r in<br />

Chinese societies, as well as a celebrity in greater China. He<br />

is influential and reputable in mainland China and Hong<br />

Kong.<br />

Next, the participants rated the perceived cultural representativeness<br />

of Jackie Chan on the same six-item measure<br />

of perceived cultural representativeness used in Study 1<br />

(e.g. ‘Jackie Chan is a symbol of Chinese culture’;<br />

a=0.86).<br />

After having responded <strong>to</strong> this measure, the participants<br />

read a scandal that involved Jackie Chan:<br />

Jackie Chan was married <strong>to</strong> Lin Feng-Jiao ( ), a<br />

retired Taiwanese actress, and they had a son (Fang<br />

Zu-Ming, ). In 1999, Wu Yi-Li ( ), an actress,<br />

claimed that she was pregnant with Jackie Chan’s child. On<br />

19 November 1999, Wu gave birth <strong>to</strong> her daughter (Wu<br />

Zhuo-Lin, , aka ‘little dragon girl’).<br />

The s<strong>to</strong>ry was followed by one of the following actual<br />

responses from Jackie Chan <strong>to</strong> the scandal, which constituted<br />

the impression manipulation in the present study: (i)<br />

<strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> condition. Jackie Chan later admitted that<br />

‘he was a shameful father’, ‘he has been unfair <strong>to</strong> his<br />

daughter’, and he would carry the responsibility for the<br />

well-being of little dragon girl as long as he is alive; (ii)<br />

denial of im<strong>moral</strong>ity condition: ‘Although I am promiscuous,<br />

I am not im<strong>moral</strong>. I have made the mistake all men<br />

would <strong>make</strong> in their life’; and (iii) denial of responsibility<br />

condition: Jackie Chan accused Wu Yi-Li of setting him<br />

up. He was drunk when he had the affair with Wu. He<br />

asked Wu and her daughter not <strong>to</strong> interfere with his life in<br />

the future. The description in the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> condition<br />

was designed <strong>to</strong> evoke a positive <strong>moral</strong> evaluation of<br />

Jackie Chan. We selected the descriptions in the two<br />

control conditions because they were common denial<br />

strategies for impression management in scandalous situations,<br />

and Jackie Chan used these strategies <strong>to</strong> reduce the<br />

scandal’s negative impact on his reputation.<br />

In the current study, instead of manipulating the perceived<br />

circulation of the scandal, we measured with it the following<br />

item: ‘Among 100 Chinese people, how many of them do<br />

you think have heard of this scandal?’The possible range of<br />

response on this item was from 0 <strong>to</strong> 100. Aside from extending<br />

the results <strong>to</strong> a different operation of perceived circulation<br />

of the scandal, measuring perceived circulation versus<br />

manipulating it through bogus feedback also eliminated<br />

possible unintended effects of surprise or expectancy<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

53<br />

54<br />

55<br />

56<br />

57<br />

58<br />

59<br />

60<br />

61<br />

62<br />

63<br />

64<br />

65<br />

66<br />

67<br />

68<br />

69<br />

70<br />

71<br />

72<br />

73<br />

74<br />

75<br />

76<br />

77<br />

78<br />

79<br />

80<br />

81<br />

82<br />

83<br />

84<br />

85<br />

86<br />

87<br />

88<br />

89<br />

90<br />

91<br />

92<br />

93<br />

94<br />

95<br />

96<br />

97<br />

98<br />

99<br />

100<br />

101<br />

102<br />

103<br />

104<br />

105<br />

106


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 7 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: 82F6234A<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52<br />

53<br />

<strong>Emotional</strong> responses <strong>to</strong> <strong>scandals</strong> 7<br />

disconfirmation when the participants’ estimations of<br />

perceived circulation differed from the feedback provided<br />

by the experimenter. Because perceived believability did not<br />

have an effect on emotional response <strong>to</strong> the scandal, we did<br />

not consider this fac<strong>to</strong>r in the present study.<br />

Measures. After responding <strong>to</strong> the perceived circulation<br />

measure, the participants completed the measures of emotional<br />

<strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong> the scandal. Given the nature of the<br />

scandal, the participants might find it strange <strong>to</strong> report how<br />

much the scandal would evoke admiration and envy<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards Jackie Chan. Therefore, we considered negative<br />

emotional responses <strong>to</strong> the scandal only. Regarding the<br />

negative emotion measure, in addition <strong>to</strong> the two original<br />

negative emotion items (fear, angry), we added disgust,<br />

which is another major negative emotion <strong>to</strong>wards <strong>moral</strong><br />

indignity (Fiske et al., 2002). The reliability of the negative<br />

emotional response measure was 0.81. Finally, <strong>to</strong> evaluate<br />

the effectiveness of the impression manipulation, we had<br />

the participants respond <strong>to</strong> the manipulation check items<br />

used in the manipulation check study in Study 1.<br />

Results and discussion<br />

Manipulation check. The impression manipulation was<br />

successful. We performed a one-way ANOVA <strong>to</strong> evaluate<br />

the effect of the impression manipulation on the perceived<br />

<strong>moral</strong>ity of Jackie Chan. The effect of the manipulation was<br />

significant [F(2, 711) = 6.68, p = 0.001, h 2 p = 0.018].<br />

Jackie Chan was perceived <strong>to</strong> be more <strong>moral</strong> in the <strong>moral</strong><br />

<strong>character</strong> condition (M = 4.89, SD = 1.30) than he was in<br />

the denial of im<strong>moral</strong>ity condition (M = 4.52, SD = 1.29)<br />

[t(460) = 3.14, p = 0.002] or the denial of responsibility<br />

condition (M = 4.53, SD = 1.25) [t(497) = 3.17, p = 0.002].<br />

Furthermore, the perceived <strong>moral</strong>ity of Jackie Chan was<br />

significantly above the mid-point (4.50) in the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong><br />

condition only [t(297) = 4.76, p < 0.001].<br />

Preliminary analyses. The mean level of perceived cultural<br />

representativeness of Jackie Chan was 4.06 (SD = 1.26 on a<br />

scale of 1–7), which was significantly higher than that of<br />

Ge You in Study 1 [t(1239) = 10.66, p < 0.001]. The<br />

impression manipulation, which was introduced after the<br />

perceived cultural representativeness measure, did not have<br />

any impact on perceived cultural representativeness [F(2,<br />

711) = 0.21, p = 0.82].<br />

The mean level of perceived circulation was 29.95%<br />

(SD = 23.61%), which was lower than the actual percentage<br />

of participants who had heard of the scandal (38.7% 4 )<br />

[t(712) = 9.92, p < 0.001]. The impression manipulation<br />

did not have an effect on the perceived circulation of the<br />

scandal [F(2, 711) = 0.17, p = 0.84]. Perceived circulation<br />

of the scandal also did not correlate with the perceived<br />

cultural representativeness of Jackie Chan (r =-0.03).<br />

Taken <strong>to</strong>gether, the preliminary analysis results indicated<br />

that the impression manipulation, perceived circulation,<br />

and perceived cultural representativeness were orthogonal<br />

predic<strong>to</strong>rs in the current study.<br />

Effects of <strong>moral</strong>ity, cultural representativeness, and<br />

circulation. We hypothesize that making salient Jackie<br />

Chan’s concern with <strong>moral</strong>ity would be more effective for<br />

reducing negative emotions, only when Jackie Chan was<br />

perceived <strong>to</strong> be a cultural symbol and when the scandal was<br />

widely circulated. To test this hypothesis, we performed an<br />

impression ¥ perceived circulation ¥ perceived cultural<br />

representativeness GLM on negative emotions and predicted<br />

a significant three-way interaction. We mean-centred<br />

perceived circulation and perceived cultural representativeness<br />

<strong>to</strong> reduce the impact of multicolinearity.<br />

The predicted that the impression ¥ perceived<br />

circulation ¥ perceived cultural representativeness interaction<br />

was significant [F(2, 698) = 3.01, p < 0.05,<br />

h 2 p = 0.009]. To understand the nature of this interaction,<br />

we performed a separate circulation ¥ perceived cultural<br />

representativeness GLM in each impression condition. As<br />

in Study 1, in all impression conditions, there was a significant<br />

main effect of perceived cultural representativeness:<br />

F(1, 210) = 21.72, p < 0.001, h 2 p = 0.096 in the denial<br />

of im<strong>moral</strong>ity condition; F(1, 248) = 4.58, p < 0.001,<br />

h 2 p = 0.173 in the denial of responsibility; and F(1,<br />

240) = 9.06, p = 0.003, h 2 p = 0.036 in the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong><br />

condition. In all impression conditions, participants<br />

reported fewer negative emotions <strong>to</strong>wards Jackie Chan<br />

when they regarded him as more representative of Chinese<br />

culture (r between -0.15 and -0.31, p < 0.05). The main<br />

effect of circulation was not significant in any of the three<br />

impression conditions: F 1.03, p 0.31. More importantly,<br />

the circulation ¥ perceived cultural representativeness<br />

interaction was significant in the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong><br />

condition only: f(1, 240) = 6.84, p = 0.009, h 2 p = 0.028, and<br />

was f(1, 210) = 0.63, p = 0.43 in the denial of Im<strong>moral</strong>ity<br />

Condition and F(1, 248) = 1.47, p = 0.23 in the denial of<br />

responsibility condition.<br />

In Figure 2, we plot the predicted value of negative emotional<br />

responses when cultural representativeness was one<br />

standard deviation above (below) the mean and when the<br />

scandal was widely circulated or not in the <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong><br />

condition. As in Study 1, when the impression that Jackie<br />

Chan valued <strong>moral</strong>ity was highlighted, the participants’<br />

emotional responses were least negative when the scandal<br />

was widely circulated and when Jackie Chan was seen as a<br />

representative of Chinese culture. <strong>When</strong> the scandal was<br />

widely circulated, emotional responses <strong>to</strong> Jackie Chan were<br />

less negative when he was seen as more representative of<br />

Chinese culture (B =-0.30, p < 0.001). In contrast, when<br />

the scandal was perceived <strong>to</strong> have low circulation, negative<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

54<br />

55<br />

56<br />

57<br />

58<br />

59<br />

60<br />

61<br />

62<br />

63<br />

64<br />

65<br />

66<br />

67<br />

68<br />

69<br />

70<br />

71<br />

72<br />

73<br />

74<br />

75<br />

76<br />

77<br />

78<br />

79<br />

80<br />

81<br />

82<br />

83<br />

84<br />

85<br />

86<br />

87<br />

88<br />

89<br />

90<br />

91<br />

92<br />

93<br />

94<br />

95<br />

96<br />

97<br />

98<br />

99<br />

100<br />

101<br />

102<br />

103<br />

104<br />

105<br />

106<br />

107


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 8 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: 839D3B83<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

8 Jiang Jiang et al.<br />

Figure 2 Negative emotions <strong>to</strong>wards Jackie Chan in<br />

the <strong>moral</strong> value condition as a function of his level of<br />

cultural representativeness and the perceived circulation<br />

of the scandal. Data are estimated value when the<br />

predic<strong>to</strong>r variables were centred at one standard deviation<br />

above (high) or below (low) their respective mean.<br />

( .... ) low circulation; (—) high circulation.<br />

emotional responses <strong>to</strong> Jackie Chan were unrelated <strong>to</strong> his<br />

perceived representativeness of Chinese culture (B =-0.03,<br />

p = 0.60). In addition, when the perceived cultural representativeness<br />

of Jackie Chan was relatively high (one standard<br />

deviation above the mean), emotional responses<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards him were less negative when the scandal was perceived<br />

<strong>to</strong> be more widely circulated, although the association<br />

was not significant (B =-0.12, p = 0.09). In summary,<br />

replicating Study 1 results, reminding the participants of<br />

Jackie Chan’s <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> was more effective in alleviating<br />

negative emotions <strong>to</strong>wards him when he was seen as<br />

a cultural symbol and when the scandal was widely known<br />

than when either condition was not met.<br />

General discussion<br />

Evidence from the two studies show that when there is an<br />

indication that a scandalous individual is personally committed<br />

<strong>to</strong> the core <strong>moral</strong> values in the culture, people react<br />

less negatively <strong>to</strong> the scandal, particularly when it is widely<br />

known and when the protagonist in the scandal is regarded<br />

as a cultural representative. This is the case when the scandalous<br />

individual is indirectly implicated in a scandal (as in<br />

the Ge You scandal) and when the scandalous individual is<br />

personally responsible for the scandal (as in the Jackie<br />

Chan scandal). These results are consistent with the previous<br />

finding that cultural representatives are expected <strong>to</strong><br />

exemplify the core values of their culture (Wan et al.,<br />

2010). Thus, when a cultural representative is implicated in<br />

a widely-circulated scandal, the scandal can evoke strong<br />

negative <strong>reactions</strong> from the public. <strong>When</strong> this happens, the<br />

scandalous cultural representative can alleviate public<br />

<strong>moral</strong> outrage against him or her by exhibiting evidence of<br />

his or her commitment <strong>to</strong> <strong>moral</strong> values. Such evidence<br />

helps <strong>to</strong> cleanse the <strong>moral</strong> image of the cultural representative<br />

and res<strong>to</strong>re public confidence in the cultural representative<br />

as a carrier of the culture’s <strong>moral</strong> values.<br />

These results <strong>make</strong> important theoretical and empirical<br />

contributions <strong>to</strong> the research literature. First, public <strong>reactions</strong><br />

<strong>to</strong> <strong>scandals</strong> have important ramifications for trust in the<br />

society’s established institutions, including political, financial,<br />

and religious implications. The <strong>moral</strong> outrage incited<br />

by widely-circulated <strong>scandals</strong> can undermine the perceived<br />

legitimacy of the status quo and promote social cynicism<br />

(Pelletier & Bligh, 2008). Despite the apparent significance<br />

of this social psychological phenomenon, surprisingly, only<br />

a couple of studies have systematically examined the determinants<br />

of how the general public reacts <strong>to</strong> <strong>scandals</strong>. Results<br />

from these studies (Fischle, 2000; Tiedens, 2001) highlight<br />

the importance of affect and post-scandal impression management<br />

in alleviating negative <strong>reactions</strong>. We extend these<br />

results by showing how the property of the scandal (i.e. how<br />

widely circulated it is), the quality of the scandalous individual<br />

(i.e. how much he or she is perceived <strong>to</strong> be a representative<br />

of her or his culture), and the public image of the<br />

scandalous individual (the perceived commitment of the<br />

scandalous individual <strong>to</strong> conventional <strong>moral</strong>ity) <strong>to</strong>gether<br />

predict public <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong>wards scandalous individuals.<br />

Furthermore, whereas previous studies have focused on<br />

public <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong> political <strong>scandals</strong> (Fischle; Tiedens), our<br />

studies are the first <strong>to</strong> examine public <strong>reactions</strong> <strong>to</strong> scandalous<br />

celebrities in the entertainment business.<br />

Our results also shed new light on the emerging intersubjective<br />

theory of cultural processes (Chiu et al., 2010;<br />

Wan et al., 2007). According <strong>to</strong> this theory, a value<br />

becomes a core value in the culture, because people in the<br />

culture collectively expect others in the culture <strong>to</strong> endorse<br />

it. For example, filial piety is a core value in Chinese culture<br />

because most Chinese expect other Chinese <strong>to</strong> endorse it.<br />

Core values are embodied in external media, including the<br />

public image of cultural representatives. Previous studies<br />

have shown that people like a cultural representative more<br />

when he or she is widely known <strong>to</strong> have exhibited behaviours<br />

that are consistent with the culture’s core values (Wan<br />

et al., 2010). The current studies provide further support for<br />

the intersubjective theory by showing that people dislike a<br />

scandalous public figure less when he or she remains committed<br />

<strong>to</strong> the culture’s <strong>moral</strong> values, despite his or her<br />

scandalous behaviours.<br />

Aside from emphasizing the scandalous individual’s<br />

<strong>moral</strong> values, as shown in Study 1, a scandalous individual<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52<br />

53<br />

54<br />

55<br />

56<br />

57<br />

58<br />

59<br />

60<br />

61<br />

62<br />

63<br />

64<br />

65<br />

66<br />

67<br />

68<br />

69<br />

70<br />

71<br />

72<br />

73<br />

74<br />

75<br />

76<br />

77<br />

78<br />

79<br />

80<br />

81<br />

82<br />

83<br />

84<br />

85<br />

86<br />

87<br />

88<br />

89


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 9 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: B06429AB<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52<br />

53<br />

54<br />

<strong>Emotional</strong> responses <strong>to</strong> <strong>scandals</strong> 9<br />

might also manage the public’s emotional responses by<br />

highlighting his or her feelings of being misunders<strong>to</strong>od or<br />

desirable personal qualities (humility). Indeed, these two<br />

strategies do not differ from the strategy of emphasizing the<br />

scandalous individual’s <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> in terms of their<br />

overall effectiveness in alleviating negative emotions<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards the scandalous individual. What is unique about<br />

highlighting the scandalous individual’s <strong>moral</strong> <strong>character</strong> is<br />

that this strategy is most effective when the scandal is<br />

widely known and the scandalous individual is seen as a<br />

cultural representative.<br />

Although people are motivated <strong>to</strong> maintain positive perceptions<br />

of the <strong>moral</strong>ity of their cultural symbols, it is<br />

possible that severe <strong>scandals</strong> or ones that involve highly<br />

iconic cultural representatives in the society (e.g. Bill<br />

Clin<strong>to</strong>n of the United States or Chen Shui-bian of Taiwan)<br />

would evoke very strong <strong>moral</strong> outrage, which when<br />

released could lead <strong>to</strong> public denouncement of the cultural<br />

representatives and collective actions <strong>to</strong> dissociate the cultural<br />

representatives from the culture. Nonetheless, we<br />

believe that perceived commitment of the scandalous cultural<br />

representatives <strong>to</strong> <strong>moral</strong> values could still alleviate<br />

public hostility in such severe <strong>scandals</strong>. Take the Lewinsky<br />

scandal that broke on January 17, 1998 as an example. As<br />

the scandal raged on, Bill Clin<strong>to</strong>n, an icon of American<br />

culture, admitted on August 17, 1998 that he had had an<br />

‘improper physical relationship’ with Lewinsky. In a<br />

nationally televised statement, Clin<strong>to</strong>n tried <strong>to</strong> manage<br />

public outrage by accepting personal responsibility for his<br />

actions, attributing the scandal partly <strong>to</strong> right-wing conspiracy<br />

against him, and reaffirming his commitment <strong>to</strong><br />

American family values, claiming that nothing is more<br />

important than his families <strong>to</strong> him personally. Would<br />

Clin<strong>to</strong>n face impeachment and conviction as Chen Shuibian<br />

did if Clin<strong>to</strong>n had not expressed his commitment <strong>to</strong><br />

family values in his admission statement? Nonetheless, a<br />

limitation in the current research is that the scandalous<br />

individual in both studies are cultural celebrities in the<br />

entertainment business. We need future research <strong>to</strong> verify<br />

whether our effects can be generalized <strong>to</strong> different types of<br />

individuals (e.g. political leaders, religious leaders) who are<br />

implicated in the scandal.<br />

In conclusion, when it becomes widely known that a<br />

cultural representative is implicated in <strong>moral</strong>ly questionable<br />

behaviours, it is possible for scandalous cultural representatives<br />

<strong>to</strong> reduce the scandal’s damage by highlighting<br />

References<br />

Becker, E. (1973). The Denial of Death. New<br />

York: Free Press.<br />

Capelos, T. & Wurzer, J. (2009). United front:<br />

Blame management and scandal response<br />

their personal commitment <strong>to</strong> important <strong>moral</strong> values in the<br />

culture.<br />

Acknowledgements<br />

The research reported in this article was funded by research<br />

grants awarded by the National Science Foundation (No.<br />

BCS 0743119) and the Nanyang Technological University<br />

<strong>to</strong> Chi-yue Chiu.<br />

End notes<br />

tactics of the United Nations. Journal of<br />

Contingencies and Crisis Management, 17,<br />

75–94.<br />

Chiu, C.–Y., Gelfand, M., Yamagishi, T.,<br />

Shteynberg, G. & Wan, C. (2010). Intersubjective<br />

culture: The role of intersubjective<br />

1. To confirm whether the participants regarded Ge You as a<br />

high-status target, we also asked the participants <strong>to</strong> indicate the<br />

extent <strong>to</strong> which they agreed that Ge You was: (i) a respectable<br />

ac<strong>to</strong>r; (ii) a despicable fac<strong>to</strong>r (reverse scoring); (iii) a respectable<br />

Chinese; and (iv) a despicable Chinese (reverse scoring).<br />

The participants made their ratings on a seven-point scale<br />

(1 = ‘strongly disagree’ <strong>to</strong> 7 = ‘strongly agree’). The reliability<br />

of this measure was 0.77. The mean respectability rating was<br />

5.40 (SD = 0.99), indicating that Ge You was regarded as a<br />

respectable Chinese ac<strong>to</strong>r.<br />

2. Other significant effects in this analysis were the main effect of<br />

perceived cultural representativeness [F(1, 504) = 43.97,<br />

p < 0.001, h 2 p = 0.080], the main effect of emotion [F(1,<br />

504) = 838.35, p < 0.001, h 2 p = 0.625], and the interaction of<br />

emotion and perceived cultural representativeness [F(2,<br />

504) = 61.12, p < 0.001, h 2 p = 0.108]. We interpreted these<br />

effects in the context of the three-way interaction.<br />

3. We also tested whether participants’ sex moderated the threeway<br />

interaction, by including sex as a between-subjects fac<strong>to</strong>r<br />

in the GLM. The only significant effect involving participant<br />

sex was the main effect of participant sex: men (vs women)<br />

reported more positive emotions <strong>to</strong>wards Ge You (Mmen = 2.21,<br />

SD = 1.74; Mwomen = 1.92, SD = 1.71) [F(1, 480) = 4.27,<br />

p < 0.039, h 2 p = 0.009]. <strong>When</strong> we included whether the participants<br />

knew the scandal as a predic<strong>to</strong>r in the GLM, previous<br />

knowledge of the scandal also did not moderate the three-way<br />

interaction [F(1, 480) = 2.16, p = 0.12]. In summary, both participant<br />

sex and prior knowledge of the scandal did not moderate<br />

the joint effect of impression, circulation, and perceived<br />

cultural representativeness on emotional responses <strong>to</strong> Ge You.<br />

4. To ensure that we would have enough participants who had not<br />

heard of the scandal before, we recruited a <strong>to</strong>tal of 1190 participants<br />

(554 men and 636 women). Among these, 38.7% had<br />

heard of the scandal before. Data from these participants were<br />

not included in the analysis.<br />

perceptions in cross-cultural research. Perspectives<br />

on Psychological Science, 5, 481–<br />

493.<br />

Clark, S. J. (2006). Gay priests and other<br />

bogeymen. Journal of Homosexuality, 51,<br />

1–13.<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

55<br />

56<br />

57<br />

58<br />

59<br />

60<br />

61<br />

62<br />

63<br />

64<br />

65<br />

66<br />

67<br />

68<br />

69<br />

70<br />

71<br />

72<br />

73<br />

74<br />

75<br />

76<br />

77<br />

78<br />

79<br />

80<br />

81<br />

82<br />

83<br />

84<br />

85<br />

86<br />

87<br />

88<br />

89<br />

90<br />

91<br />

92<br />

93<br />

94<br />

95<br />

96<br />

97<br />

98<br />

99<br />

100<br />

101<br />

102<br />

103<br />

104<br />

105<br />

106<br />

107<br />

108


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 10 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: E1BF3310<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

10 Jiang Jiang et al.<br />

Echterhoff, G., Higgins, E. T. & Groll, S.<br />

(2005). Audience-tuning effects on memory:<br />

The role of shared reality. Journal of Personality<br />

and Social Psychology, 89, 257–276.<br />

Fischle, M. (2000). Mass response <strong>to</strong> the<br />

Lewinsky scandal: Motivated reasoning or<br />

Bayesian updating? Political Psychology,<br />

21, 135–159.<br />

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P. & Xu, J.<br />

(2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype<br />

content: Competence and warmth respectively<br />

follow from perceived status and competition.<br />

Journal of Personality and Social<br />

Psychology, 82, 878–902.<br />

Hausmann, L. R. M., Levine, J. M. & Higgins,<br />

E. T. (2008). Communication and group perception:<br />

Extending the ‘saying is believing’<br />

effect. Group Processes and Intergroup<br />

Relations, 11, 539–554.<br />

Hindman, E. B. (2005). Jayson Blair, The New<br />

York Times, and paradigm repair. Journal of<br />

Communication, 55, 225–241.<br />

Jacobsson, K. & Löfmarck, E. (2008). A sociology<br />

of scandal and <strong>moral</strong> transgression:<br />

The Swedish ‘Nannygate’ scandal. Acta<br />

Sociologica, 51, 203–216.<br />

Jang, W.-Y. & Chen, C.-T. (2009). Defendant<br />

firms and response <strong>to</strong> legal crises: Effect on<br />

shareholder value. Journal of Contingencies<br />

and Crisis Management, 17, 108–117.<br />

Johansson, P. Z. (2007). Legitimacy lost and<br />

back <strong>to</strong> normality: Scandals in the public<br />

sec<strong>to</strong>r – The Swedish case. Electronic<br />

Journal of Business Ethics and Organization<br />

Studies, 12, 24–30.<br />

Kesebir, P. (in press). Existential functions of<br />

culture: The monumental immortality<br />

projecy. In: A. K. Leung, C.-Y. Chiu &<br />

Y.-Y. Hong, eds. Cultural Processes: A<br />

Social Psychological Perspectives, pp.<br />

– . Cambridge: Cambridge University<br />

Press.<br />

Pelletier, K. L. & Bligh, M. C. (2008). The<br />

aftermath of organizational corruption:<br />

Employee attributions and emotional <strong>reactions</strong>.<br />

Journal of Business Ethics, 80,<br />

823–844.<br />

Rowe, M. (2009). Notes on a scandal: The official<br />

enquiry in<strong>to</strong> deviance and corruption in<br />

New Zealand police. Australian and New<br />

Zealand Journal of Criminology, 42, 123–<br />

138.<br />

Sims, R. (2009). Toward a better understanding<br />

of organizational efforts <strong>to</strong> rebuild<br />

reputation following an ethical scandal.<br />

Journal of Business Ethics, 90, 453–<br />

472.<br />

Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Anger and advancement<br />

versus sadness and subjugation: The effect<br />

of negative emotion expressions on social<br />

status conferral. Journal of Personality and<br />

Social Psychology, 80, 86–94.<br />

Wan, C., Chiu, C.-Y., Tam, K.-P., Lee, S.-L.,<br />

Lau, I. Y.-M. & Peng, S.-Q. (2007). Perceived<br />

cultural importance and actual selfimportance<br />

of values in cultural<br />

identification. Journal of Personality and<br />

Social Psychology, 92, 337–354.<br />

Wan, C., Torelli, C. & Chiu, C.-Y. (2010).<br />

<strong>When</strong> familiarity breeds admiration and<br />

saying is disbelieving: The cultural implications<br />

of shared reality. Social Cognition, 28,<br />

422–446.<br />

Williams, B. A. & Carpini, D. (2004). Monica<br />

and Bill all the time and everywhere: The<br />

collapse of gatekeeping and agenda setting<br />

in the new media environment. American<br />

Behavioral Scientist, 47, 1208–1230.<br />

© 2011 The Authors<br />

Asian Journal of Social Psychology © 2011 Blackwell Publishing Asia Pty Ltd with the Asian Association of Social Psychology and<br />

the Japanese Group Dynamics Association<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

34<br />

35<br />

36<br />

37<br />

38<br />

39<br />

40<br />

41<br />

42<br />

43<br />

44<br />

45<br />

46<br />

47<br />

48<br />

49<br />

50<br />

51<br />

52


JOBNAME: No Job Name PAGE: 11 SESS: 12 OUTPUT: Wed May 4 18:23:16 2011 SUM: 2755CBEB<br />

/v2501/blackwell/A_journals/ajsp_v0_i0/ajsp_1344<br />

Toppan Best-set Premedia Limited<br />

Journal Code: AJSP Proofreader: Mony<br />

Article No: 1344 Delivery date: 4 May 2011<br />

Page Extent: 10<br />

AUTHOR QUERY FORM<br />

Dear Author<br />

During the preparation of your manuscript, the questions listed below have arisen. Please answer all the<br />

queries (marking any other corrections on the proof enclosed) and return this form with your proofs.<br />

Query no. Query Reply<br />

1 WILEY-BLACKWELL: Please provide the date of submission, revision,<br />

acceptance.<br />

2 AUTHOR: Please provide city.<br />

3 AUTHOR: Please provide the department, city and country for each address.<br />

4 AUTHOR: Please provide updated year or this will need <strong>to</strong> be changed <strong>to</strong> ‘<br />

(title) (forename) Kesebir, pers.comm., ’ (year).<br />

5 AUTHOR: Should this be ‘father’? Please double check.<br />

6 AUTHOR: Becker, 1973 has not been cited in the text. Please indicate where it<br />

should be cited; or delete from the Reference List.<br />

7 AUTHOR: Please provide the page range for this reference.<br />

8 AUTHOR: Please provide year of publication.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!