25.03.2013 Views

Friendship and Friendship Quality in Middle Childhood ... - Psychology

Friendship and Friendship Quality in Middle Childhood ... - Psychology

Friendship and Friendship Quality in Middle Childhood ... - Psychology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Developmental <strong>Psychology</strong><br />

1993, Vol. 29, No. 4,611-621<br />

Copyright 1993 bv the American Psychological Association, Inc.<br />

O012-1649/93/$3.O0<br />

<strong>Friendship</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Friendship</strong> <strong>Quality</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Middle</strong> <strong>Childhood</strong>: L<strong>in</strong>ks With Peer<br />

Group Acceptance <strong>and</strong> Feel<strong>in</strong>gs of Lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> Social Dissatisfaction<br />

Jeffrey G. Parker <strong>and</strong> Steven R. Asher<br />

The dist<strong>in</strong>ction between friendship adjustment <strong>and</strong> acceptance by the peer group was exam<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

Third- through 5th-grade children (N= 881) completed sociometric measures of acceptance <strong>and</strong><br />

friendship, a measure of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess, a questionnaire on the features of their very best friendships,<br />

<strong>and</strong> a measure of their friendship satisfaction. Results <strong>in</strong>dicated that many low-accepted children<br />

had best friends <strong>and</strong> were satisfied with these friendships. However, these children's friendships<br />

were lower than those of other children on most dimensions of quality. Hav<strong>in</strong>g a friend, friendship<br />

quality, <strong>and</strong> group acceptance made separate contributions to the prediction of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess. Results<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicate the utility of the new friendship quality measure <strong>and</strong> the value of dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g children's<br />

friendship adjustment from their general peer acceptance.<br />

Researchers have made considerable progress <strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

the emergence, ma<strong>in</strong>tenance, <strong>and</strong> consequences of acceptance<br />

versus rejection by the peer group (see Asher & Coie,<br />

1990, for reviews). Although group acceptance is an important<br />

facet of children's successful adaptation to peers, greater attention<br />

is needed to children's ability to form <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> satisfy<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>and</strong> supportive specific dyadic friendships as dist<strong>in</strong>ct from<br />

their ability to ga<strong>in</strong> acceptance <strong>in</strong> the classroom more generally.<br />

Indeed, several authors (e.g., Blyth, 1983; Bukowski & Hoza,<br />

1989; Furman & Robb<strong>in</strong>s, 1985) argued that problems <strong>in</strong> group<br />

acceptance do not necessarily preclude satisfactory friendship<br />

adjustment, <strong>and</strong> they po<strong>in</strong>ted out that concerns about the emotional<br />

well-be<strong>in</strong>g of low-accepted children might be attenuated<br />

if it could be established that low-accepted children have satisfy<strong>in</strong>g<br />

one-to-one friendships. To date, however, children's friendship<br />

adjustment has been studied far less frequently <strong>and</strong> less<br />

systematically than children's group acceptance, <strong>and</strong> the l<strong>in</strong>ks<br />

Jeffrey G. Parker, Department of <strong>Psychology</strong>, University of Ill<strong>in</strong>ois<br />

at Urbana-Champaign; Steven R. Asher, Bureau of Educational Research,<br />

University of Ill<strong>in</strong>ois at Urbana-Champaign.<br />

This article is based on a doctoral dissertation carried out by Jeffrey<br />

G. Parker <strong>in</strong> the Department of <strong>Psychology</strong> at the University of Ill<strong>in</strong>ois<br />

at Urbana-Champaign. Portions of this article were presented at the<br />

annual meet<strong>in</strong>g of the American Educational Research Association,<br />

Boston, <strong>in</strong> April 1990.<br />

The research reported <strong>in</strong> this article was supported by National Institute<br />

of Child Health <strong>and</strong> Human Development Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Grant<br />

HD07205, by an American Psychological Association Dissertation<br />

Fellowship to Jeffrey G. Parker, <strong>and</strong> by a research grant to Steven R.<br />

Asher from the W T. Grant Foundation.<br />

We express our deepest appreciation to the students, teachers, <strong>and</strong><br />

pr<strong>in</strong>cipals of Bottenfield, Carrie Busey, Ben Frankl<strong>in</strong>, Dr. Howard,<br />

Thomas Pa<strong>in</strong>e, <strong>and</strong> Robeson Elementary Schools. Without the considerable<br />

cooperation <strong>and</strong> support of everyone <strong>in</strong>volved, this research<br />

would not have been possible. We also thank Carol Rockhill <strong>and</strong> Kathleen<br />

Zelis for their help with data collection <strong>and</strong> Ben Wallace for develop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a program to calculate reciprocal sociometric nom<strong>in</strong>ations.<br />

Correspondence concern<strong>in</strong>g this article should be addressed to Jeffrey<br />

G. Parker, who is now at Department of <strong>Psychology</strong>, University of<br />

Michigan, 3433 Mason Hall, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109-1027.<br />

611<br />

between these two forms of peer adjustment are poorly understood.<br />

Although little consensus exists as yet regard<strong>in</strong>g the most<br />

appropriate means of assess<strong>in</strong>g friendship success or adjustment,<br />

several reviews (e.g., Berndt, 1984; Bukowski & Hoza,<br />

1989) suggested that two different elements of friendship adjustment<br />

could be exam<strong>in</strong>ed. The first is the extent of a child's<br />

participation <strong>in</strong> friendship, that is, whether or not the child has<br />

an acknowledged, mutual friendship with another child. The<br />

second is the quality of a child's best friendship, that is, the<br />

degree of companionship the relationship provides, its supportiveness,<br />

<strong>and</strong> its level of conflict.<br />

One can gauge the first element—children's participation <strong>in</strong><br />

friendship—<strong>in</strong> school sett<strong>in</strong>gs by identify<strong>in</strong>g reciprocal choices<br />

among the patterns of nom<strong>in</strong>ations children give <strong>in</strong> response to<br />

questions about who their best friends are (see Berndt, 1984).<br />

Studies of this type have documented differences <strong>in</strong> participation<br />

<strong>in</strong> friendship as a function of gender, classroom <strong>and</strong> school<br />

structure, age, <strong>and</strong> several other variables (see Epste<strong>in</strong>, 1986, for<br />

a review). Moreover, recent research has documented the existence<br />

of important behavioral <strong>and</strong> social-cognitive differences<br />

between children <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> mutual friendships <strong>and</strong> children<br />

without mutual friendships (Carlson-Jones & Bowl<strong>in</strong>g, 1988;<br />

Howes, 1988; Mannar<strong>in</strong>o, 1976; McGuire & Weisz, 1982; Roopnar<strong>in</strong>e&<br />

Field, 1984).<br />

To date, however, it is unclear how participation <strong>in</strong> friendship<br />

varies as a function of group acceptance. In study<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

relationship between friendship <strong>and</strong> acceptance, one must have<br />

clearly dist<strong>in</strong>ct measures of each dimension. As Bukowski <strong>and</strong><br />

Hoza (1989) po<strong>in</strong>ted out, certa<strong>in</strong> commonly used sociometric<br />

measures of acceptance actually confound acceptance with<br />

friendship. This occurs particularly when group acceptance is<br />

assessed with sociometric nom<strong>in</strong>ation procedures, which typically<br />

require children to <strong>in</strong>dicate their best friends or children<br />

they like most with<strong>in</strong> a particular group (often only three<br />

choices are allowed). The number of nom<strong>in</strong>ations children receive<br />

is then taken as their level of acceptance, or these positive<br />

nom<strong>in</strong>ations are used <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation with negative nom<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

to identify poorly-accepted children. Although nom<strong>in</strong>a-


612 JEFFREY G. PARKER AND STEVEN R. ASHER<br />

tion measures are highly valid for certa<strong>in</strong> research questions,<br />

when limited-choice positive nom<strong>in</strong>ations are used to assess<br />

peer acceptance, certa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretative problems arise. When<br />

friendship is the explicit criterion (e.g., "Name your three best<br />

friends"), error is likely to be <strong>in</strong>troduced, because children will<br />

fail to nom<strong>in</strong>ate other children who they actually like or accept<br />

but do not view as a best friend. Even when friendship is not the<br />

explicit criterion (e.g., "Name three children you like most"),<br />

there is still a problem. Because children are restricted <strong>in</strong> the<br />

number of choices they can make, they are likely to limit their<br />

choices to their best friends. Moreover, by allow<strong>in</strong>g only a limited<br />

number of choices, <strong>in</strong>formation is miss<strong>in</strong>g about how children<br />

feel about most of the children <strong>in</strong> their classroom.<br />

A conceptually clearer approach would be for one to use<br />

reciprocal friendship-nom<strong>in</strong>ation sociometric measures to assess<br />

friendship <strong>and</strong> to use a "roster-<strong>and</strong>-rat<strong>in</strong>g scale" sociometric<br />

measure of lik<strong>in</strong>g to assess children's overall acceptance by<br />

their peers (for early steps <strong>in</strong> this direction, see Asher, S<strong>in</strong>gleton,<br />

& Taylor, 1982; Oden & Asher. 1977). Reciprocal friendship<br />

nom<strong>in</strong>ations provide face-valid <strong>in</strong>dicators of friendship (see<br />

Bukowski & Hoza, 1989), <strong>and</strong> rat<strong>in</strong>g-scale measures of acceptance<br />

provide complete <strong>in</strong>formation about how much each<br />

child likes every other child <strong>in</strong> the group (Asher & Hymel,<br />

1981). For rat<strong>in</strong>g-scale measures to serve this purpose, it is important<br />

that children be asked to rate how much they like or<br />

like to play with every other classmate, rather than be asked to<br />

rate how much they view every other child as a friend.<br />

One aim of the present research was to evaluate differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> the prevalence of friendship among low- average- <strong>and</strong> highaccepted<br />

children. Of particular <strong>in</strong>terest was whether many<br />

low-accepted children, despite their low overall status, nonetheless<br />

have best friends. Although several studies conta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

about this issue (Berghout-Aust<strong>in</strong>, 1985; Bukowski,<br />

Hoza, & Newcomb, 1987; Buzzelli, 1988; Drewry & Clark,<br />

1984; Feltham, Doyle, Schwartzman, Serb<strong>in</strong>, & Led<strong>in</strong>gham,<br />

1985; Howes, 1988; Mannar<strong>in</strong>o, 1976; Rizzo, 1988; Roopnar<strong>in</strong>e<br />

& Field, 1984), none of the research comb<strong>in</strong>es reciprocal nom<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

measures of friendship with rat<strong>in</strong>g-scale measures of<br />

acceptance.<br />

A second aim of the present research was to develop an <strong>in</strong>strument<br />

for assess<strong>in</strong>g children's perceptions of the qualitative<br />

features of their best friendships. A third, related aim was to<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>e how the qualities of accepted children's friendships<br />

differ from the qualities of low-accepted children's friendships.<br />

Several studies have exam<strong>in</strong>ed age <strong>and</strong> gender differences <strong>in</strong><br />

the quality of children's friendships by obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g from children<br />

estimates of the extent to which their friendships meet certa<strong>in</strong><br />

relationship needs (Berndt & Perry, 1986; Buhrmester & Furman,<br />

1987; Bukowski et al., 1987; Furman & Buhrmester, 1985;<br />

Sharabany, Gershoni, & Hofman, 1981). In this research, there<br />

is general consistency across studies concern<strong>in</strong>g the qualitative<br />

aspects of friendship that are important to consider (see Asher<br />

& Parker, 1989). These <strong>in</strong>clude (a) the extent to which the relationship<br />

offers children opportunities for play, companionship,<br />

<strong>and</strong> recreation; (b) the degree of <strong>in</strong>timate disclosure <strong>and</strong> exchange<br />

that characterizes the relationship; (c) the extent to<br />

which the friends share, help, <strong>and</strong> guide one another; <strong>and</strong> (d) the<br />

extent to which children f<strong>in</strong>d the relationship validat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><br />

enhanc<strong>in</strong>g of self-worth.<br />

In exist<strong>in</strong>g research on <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> friendship<br />

quality, researchers have sometimes also considered the types<br />

of stress as well as support present <strong>in</strong> children's friendships (e.g.,<br />

Berndt & Perry, 1986; Bukowski et al., 1987). For example,<br />

Berndt <strong>and</strong> Perry found that conflict <strong>and</strong> disagreement are<br />

common <strong>in</strong> children's close friendships (see also Gottman,<br />

1983) <strong>and</strong>, at least among older children, are <strong>in</strong>dependent of<br />

the level of positive, supportive aspects of friendship. A dist<strong>in</strong>ction<br />

needs to be made, however, between the level of conflict<br />

children experience <strong>and</strong> the ease <strong>and</strong> manner with which their<br />

conflicts are resolved. Several <strong>in</strong>vestigators have found that it is<br />

the ability to resolve conflicts quickly <strong>and</strong> amicably, not the<br />

ability to avoid conflict altogether, that dist<strong>in</strong>guishes close peer<br />

relationships from other peer relationships <strong>in</strong> early childhood<br />

(Gottman & Parkhurst, 1980; Hartup & Laursen, 1989; Vespo<br />

&Caplan, 1988). Other authors (e.g., Carlson-Jones, 1985; Hartup<br />

& Laursen, 1989) have reported similar f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs with respect<br />

to the friendships of older, school-age children. The<br />

amount of conflict children experience <strong>and</strong> the ease <strong>and</strong> read<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

with which conflict is resolved represent, therefore, somewhat<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>ct dimensions.<br />

The nature of the l<strong>in</strong>k between acceptance by the peer group<br />

<strong>and</strong> the quality of children's friendships is virtually unexplored.<br />

Scholars of personal relationships caution aga<strong>in</strong>st be<strong>in</strong>g too<br />

ready to make <strong>in</strong>ferences about the properties of dyadic relationships<br />

from measures of an <strong>in</strong>dividual's group function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>and</strong> behavior. H<strong>in</strong>de (1979), Hartup (1986), Sroufe (e.g., Sroufe<br />

& Fleeson, 1986), Furman (1984a, 1984b), <strong>and</strong> others have<br />

noted that behavior <strong>in</strong> dyadic relationships is always the product<br />

of a complex, idiosyncratic <strong>in</strong>teraction among the personal<br />

characteristics of an <strong>in</strong>dividual, the personal characteristics of<br />

his or her partner, <strong>and</strong> the relationship's history <strong>and</strong> circumstances.<br />

Furthermore, friendships, like all other personal relationships,<br />

have emergent properties, such as role expectations<br />

<strong>and</strong> obligations, that can mute, alter, or amplify an <strong>in</strong>dividual's<br />

personality characteristics or typical patterns of behavior.<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, a number of behavioral <strong>and</strong> social skills<br />

deficits have been shown to dist<strong>in</strong>guish children who are rejected<br />

by their peer group from better-accepted children (see<br />

Asher & Coie, 1990); these deficits could be expected to contribute<br />

to problems <strong>in</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g between low-accepted children<br />

<strong>and</strong> their friends. Furthermore, several studies (e.g., Buzzelli,<br />

1988; Gottman, Gonso, & Rasmussen, 1975; Kurdek & Krile,<br />

1982) have shown that unpopular children reason <strong>in</strong> less sophisticated<br />

ways about friendship <strong>and</strong> friendship-related issues<br />

than other children (but see Bichard, Alden, Walker, & McMahon,<br />

1988, for an exception).<br />

In the present study, we compared high- average- <strong>and</strong> lowaccepted<br />

children on the qualities of their best friendships. Specifically,<br />

we exam<strong>in</strong>ed whether the friendships of poorly-accepted<br />

children differed from better-accepted children with<br />

respect to six qualitative aspects: validation <strong>and</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g (i.e., the<br />

degree to which the relationship is characterized by car<strong>in</strong>g, support,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terest); conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal (i.e., the extent to which<br />

the relationship is typified by argument, disagreement, annoyance,<br />

<strong>and</strong> mistrust); companionship <strong>and</strong> recreation (i.e., the extent<br />

to which the friends spend enjoyable time together <strong>in</strong>side<br />

or outside of school); help <strong>and</strong> guidance (i.e., the extent of the<br />

friends' efforts to assist one another with rout<strong>in</strong>e or challeng<strong>in</strong>g


FRIENDSHIP AND FRIENDSHIP QUALITY<br />

tasks); <strong>in</strong>timate exchange (i.e., the extent to which the relationship<br />

is characterized by disclosure of personal <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>and</strong><br />

feel<strong>in</strong>gs); <strong>and</strong> conflict resolution (i.e., the degree to which disagreements<br />

<strong>in</strong> the relationship are resolved efficiently <strong>and</strong><br />

fairly). In addition, we compared high-, average-, <strong>and</strong> low-accepted<br />

children with respect to the level of satisfaction they<br />

expressed with their best friendship. We <strong>in</strong>cluded a satisfaction<br />

measure, because children of different levels of acceptance<br />

might report similar amounts of satisfaction even though there<br />

are differences <strong>in</strong> the specific qualities they perceive <strong>in</strong> their<br />

friendships. 1<br />

A fourth aim of the present research was to exam<strong>in</strong>e children's<br />

friendship adjustment <strong>in</strong> relation to children's feel<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction. The l<strong>in</strong>k between friendship<br />

adjustment <strong>and</strong> lonel<strong>in</strong>ess has been the subject of considerable<br />

theoriz<strong>in</strong>g but little empirical <strong>in</strong>quiry. Sullivan (1953) drew<br />

specific attention to the putative l<strong>in</strong>ks between friendship <strong>and</strong><br />

lonel<strong>in</strong>ess, ascrib<strong>in</strong>g special significance to preadolescent<br />

friendship as a means of stav<strong>in</strong>g off feel<strong>in</strong>gs of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong><br />

isolation. Over the past several years, considerable progress has<br />

been made <strong>in</strong> the reliable measurement of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>in</strong> children<br />

(see Asher, Parkhurst, Hymel, & Williams, 1990), <strong>and</strong><br />

lonel<strong>in</strong>ess has been found to be correlated with low group acceptance<br />

or rejection as measured by sociometric rat<strong>in</strong>gs or a<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ation of positive <strong>and</strong> negative sociometric nom<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

(e.g., Asher, Hymel, & Renshaw, 1984; Asher & Wheeler, 1985;<br />

Cassidy & Asher, 1992; Crick & Ladd, 1993). Dyadic friendships<br />

were not assessed <strong>in</strong> these studies, however. Therefore, it<br />

rema<strong>in</strong>s to be seen whether friendship bears a similarly strong<br />

relationship to lonel<strong>in</strong>ess or whether the apparent relation between<br />

lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> acceptance might be accounted for by<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> the friendship adjustment of accepted <strong>and</strong> unaccepted<br />

children. Furthermore, it is of theoretical <strong>and</strong> practical<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest to learn whether hav<strong>in</strong>g a friend has positive emotional<br />

benefits for low-accepted children who might otherwise report<br />

high levels of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess. Such a possibility is suggested by Bukowski<br />

et al. (1987), who focused on friendship adjustment,<br />

acceptance, <strong>and</strong> children's feel<strong>in</strong>gs of general self-worth <strong>and</strong><br />

perceived cognitive <strong>and</strong> social competence.<br />

In summary, the specific aims of the present research were (a)<br />

to exam<strong>in</strong>e the prevalence of mutual friendship among children<br />

<strong>in</strong> general <strong>and</strong> among low-accepted children <strong>in</strong> particular;<br />

(b) to develop a new measure of friendship quality; (c) to compare<br />

friendships of accepted <strong>and</strong> low-accepted children <strong>in</strong><br />

terms of specific qualitative features <strong>and</strong> with respect to children's<br />

satisfaction with their friendships; <strong>and</strong> (d) to exam<strong>in</strong>e<br />

how acceptance, hav<strong>in</strong>g a friend, <strong>and</strong> the quality of one's best<br />

friendship are related to the degree of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> social<br />

dissatisfaction <strong>in</strong> middle childhood.<br />

Subjects<br />

Method<br />

Eight hundred <strong>and</strong> eighty-one children were recruited from 36 thirdthrough<br />

fifth-grade classrooms of five public elementary schools located<br />

<strong>in</strong> a mid-sized, midwestern community <strong>in</strong> the United States. The<br />

participants represented 98.5% of all children enrolled <strong>in</strong> these<br />

classrooms (13 children were excluded at parental request). Two<br />

hundred <strong>and</strong> n<strong>in</strong>ety-six children (163 boys <strong>and</strong> 133 girls) were <strong>in</strong> the<br />

613<br />

third grade, 251 children (135 boys <strong>and</strong> 116 girls) were <strong>in</strong> the fourth<br />

grade, <strong>and</strong> 334 children (174 boys <strong>and</strong> 160 girls) were <strong>in</strong> the fifth grade.<br />

The sample was 73.2% White, 23.4% Black, <strong>and</strong> 3.4% Asian or Hispanic.<br />

All children completed all measures. However, for certa<strong>in</strong> analyses<br />

(see the follow<strong>in</strong>g sections) only children (« = 484) with at least one<br />

mutual, very best friend were of <strong>in</strong>terest. The distribution of gender<br />

<strong>and</strong> grade for this subsample of friended children was as follows: 154<br />

third graders (71 boys <strong>and</strong> 83 girls), 141 fourth graders (68 boys <strong>and</strong> 73<br />

girls), <strong>and</strong> 189 fifth graders (95 boys <strong>and</strong> 94 girls).<br />

Measures<br />

Level of acceptance. We used a "roster-<strong>and</strong>-rat<strong>in</strong>g" sociometric<br />

procedure (S<strong>in</strong>gleton & Asher, 1977) to assess children's level of<br />

classroom acceptance. Children were provided with rosters of all classmates<br />

<strong>and</strong> were asked to <strong>in</strong>dicate on a l-to-5 rat<strong>in</strong>g scale how much<br />

they liked to play with each of their classmates. A child's level of acceptance<br />

was determ<strong>in</strong>ed from the average rat<strong>in</strong>g received from his or her<br />

classmates, st<strong>and</strong>ardized with<strong>in</strong> gender with<strong>in</strong> each classroom. Children<br />

were classified as high-accepted (n = 65 boys <strong>and</strong> 63 girls) if their<br />

received rat<strong>in</strong>g z score was greater than or equal to 1. Children were<br />

classified as low-accepted (n = 74 boys <strong>and</strong> 76 girls) if their z score was<br />

less than or equal to -1. The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g children (n = 333 boys <strong>and</strong> 269<br />

girls) were classified as average-accepted. The high-accepted <strong>and</strong> lowaccepted<br />

groups represented the highest 14.5% <strong>and</strong> lowest 17% of the<br />

sample <strong>in</strong> terms of acceptance, respectively.<br />

<strong>Friendship</strong> assessments. Children's friendships were identified by<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g data from a two-step sociometric nom<strong>in</strong>ation procedure. In the<br />

first step, children were asked to <strong>in</strong>dicate their three "best friends"<br />

from a roster of the names of all other children <strong>in</strong> their class. In the<br />

second step, children were then asked to review their three choices <strong>and</strong><br />

to <strong>in</strong>dicate which of the three choices represented their s<strong>in</strong>gle, "very<br />

best friend." We then exam<strong>in</strong>ed the choice matrix with<strong>in</strong> each<br />

classroom to identify children who nom<strong>in</strong>ated each other. 2 Children<br />

were considered to have a very best friend if the classmate they designated<br />

as their very best friend <strong>in</strong> turn <strong>in</strong>cluded them among his or her<br />

' We are <strong>in</strong>debted to Carol Dweck for suggest<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dependent<br />

assessment of children's friendship satisfaction.<br />

2 An issue that arises concerns the appropriateness of restrict<strong>in</strong>g<br />

friendship choices to with<strong>in</strong> classrooms. It is possible that children<br />

who differ <strong>in</strong> acceptance also differ <strong>in</strong> their tendency to have a friend<br />

outside the classroom <strong>and</strong> that restrict<strong>in</strong>g friendship nom<strong>in</strong>ations to<br />

with<strong>in</strong> the class could underestimate the friendship status of certa<strong>in</strong><br />

groups. In a previous study, we reported the results of a direct appraisal<br />

of this possibility us<strong>in</strong>g a sample of 278 elementary school-age children<br />

(Parker & Asher, 1989). In that study, before children completed<br />

any other sociometric measure, they were asked to write the names of<br />

their three very best friends. The children were told that these friends<br />

could live anywhere but should be other children <strong>and</strong> not adults. The<br />

number of friends listed who were not class members was tallied for<br />

each child. On average, children <strong>in</strong>cluded just under one (M = 0.90)<br />

nonclassmate on this list of friends. A Gender X Level of Acceptance<br />

analysis of variance <strong>in</strong>dicated no significant ma<strong>in</strong> effects or <strong>in</strong>teraction.<br />

Thus, high-accepted, average-accepted, <strong>and</strong> low-accepted children<br />

did not differ <strong>in</strong> the number of nonclassmates they <strong>in</strong>cluded.<br />

Because these nom<strong>in</strong>ations were of children outside the classroom, it<br />

was not possible to verify whether friendships were actually reciprocal<br />

relationships. But the important f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g is that whatever underestimation<br />

of friendship does take place by restrict<strong>in</strong>g friendship assessment<br />

to the classroom seems to be limited <strong>and</strong> not particularly biased toward<br />

one acceptance group or another.


614 JEFFREY G. PARKER AND STEVEN R. ASHER<br />

list of three best-friend choices. In other words, it was not necessary<br />

that the chosen classmate designate the choos<strong>in</strong>g child as a very best<br />

friend (as opposed to a best friend) <strong>in</strong> order for the choos<strong>in</strong>g child to be<br />

considered to have a very best friend, although this strict reciprocity<br />

held for 266 (55%) of the 484 cases of children who were identified as<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g a very best friend. 3<br />

In addition to this primary operationalization of friendship, a second,<br />

more <strong>in</strong>clusive criterion for friendship was established. For this<br />

second operationalization, children's "very best" designations were<br />

ignored. Thus, best (as opposed to very best) friendship was def<strong>in</strong>ed as a<br />

reciprocity across children for any of their three best-friend nom<strong>in</strong>ations.<br />

Unlike the more restrictive primary criterion, then, children<br />

could have more than one (but no more than three) best friendships by<br />

this criterion. We felt that this supplemental operationalization of<br />

friendship was essential to a comprehensive appraisal of children's participation<br />

<strong>in</strong> friendship. It also was of <strong>in</strong>terest by virtue of be<strong>in</strong>g more<br />

directly comparable with operationalizations used <strong>in</strong> past studies.<br />

<strong>Friendship</strong> quality We exam<strong>in</strong>ed the quality of children's very best<br />

friendship by us<strong>in</strong>g a questionnaire designed to assess children's perceptions<br />

of various qualitative aspects of their very best friendship.<br />

This questionnaire, the <strong>Friendship</strong> <strong>Quality</strong> Questionnaire (FQQ). conta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

40 primary items <strong>and</strong> an <strong>in</strong>itial "warm-up" item. The items<br />

asked children to <strong>in</strong>dicate on a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t scale how true a particular<br />

quality is of their relationship with a specific friend (e.g., "Jamie <strong>and</strong> I<br />

loan each other th<strong>in</strong>gs all the time"). The scale ranged from not at all<br />

true (0) to a little true (1) to somewhat true (2) to pretty true (3) to really<br />

true (4). The child completed the questionnaire with reference to a<br />

specific friend, whose name was <strong>in</strong>serted at the top of the questionnaire<br />

<strong>and</strong> embedded <strong>in</strong> each <strong>in</strong>dividual item by us<strong>in</strong>g a personal computer<br />

<strong>and</strong> word-process<strong>in</strong>g software with data-merg<strong>in</strong>g capabilities.<br />

This reference to a specific friend was done to discourage children<br />

from complet<strong>in</strong>g the questionnaire on the basis of an <strong>in</strong>ternal representation<br />

of a stereotypic or idealized friendship or based on a mental<br />

composite of several different friendships. The target friend was the<br />

child's very best mutual friend as determ<strong>in</strong>ed from the sociometric<br />

assessment described earlier. 4 The questionnaire was group adm<strong>in</strong>istered<br />

<strong>in</strong> the child's classroom. We began adm<strong>in</strong>istration by giv<strong>in</strong>g children<br />

<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g the scale. After this, children completed the<br />

questionnaire <strong>in</strong>dividually. Each item was read aloud to make as few<br />

dem<strong>and</strong>s as possible on children's read<strong>in</strong>g ability.<br />

The FQQ evolved over time through two adm<strong>in</strong>istrations <strong>and</strong> appraisals<br />

before the present study. An <strong>in</strong>itial pool of potential items was<br />

derived from a similar questionnaire developed by Bukowski et al.<br />

(1987). 5 We made some changes to the word<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> response format of<br />

these <strong>in</strong>itial items to clarify potential ambiguities <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation. In<br />

addition, the practice of customiz<strong>in</strong>g each questionnaire with the<br />

name of a specific friend was <strong>in</strong>troduced. The FQQ was adm<strong>in</strong>istered<br />

<strong>in</strong>itially to 278 third- through sixth-grade children (see Parker &Asher,<br />

1989) <strong>and</strong> subsequently to 153 third- through fifth-grade children. We<br />

used each evaluation to identify <strong>and</strong> replace weak or ambiguous items<br />

<strong>and</strong> to clarify the underly<strong>in</strong>g factor structure. These evaluations resulted<br />

<strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al pool of 40 items used <strong>in</strong> the present study <strong>and</strong> are<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> Table 1. The scor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the derivation of subscales are described<br />

<strong>in</strong> the Results section.<br />

<strong>Friendship</strong> satisfaction. Two questions were used to assess children's<br />

satisfaction with their friendship. These questions were adm<strong>in</strong>istered<br />

separately from the FQQ. The first question asked, "How is this<br />

friendship go<strong>in</strong>g?" The second question asked, "How happy are you<br />

with this friendship?" For both questions, children <strong>in</strong>dicated theirsatisfaction<br />

along a cont<strong>in</strong>uum anchored at the low end by a stylized l<strong>in</strong>edraw<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of a frown<strong>in</strong>g, unhappy face <strong>and</strong> at the opposite, high end by a<br />

complementary stylized l<strong>in</strong>e-draw<strong>in</strong>g of a smil<strong>in</strong>g, happy face. Fifteen<br />

evenly spaced ticks were marked along the cont<strong>in</strong>uum to assist with<br />

measurement, <strong>and</strong> a child's score was based on which tick was circled,<br />

with higher scores <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g more satisfaction. To familiarize children<br />

with the use of the scale, we first guided the children through<br />

several examples <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g school activities <strong>and</strong> hobbies. The two<br />

friendship satisfaction questions were then read aloud. The responses<br />

of children with very best friends to the two questions were highly<br />

related (r = .85), <strong>and</strong> rat<strong>in</strong>gs were therefore averaged <strong>in</strong>to a s<strong>in</strong>gle satisfaction<br />

score for each child.<br />

Lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction. Children's feel<strong>in</strong>gs of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

<strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction were assessed by us<strong>in</strong>g Asher <strong>and</strong><br />

Wheeler's (1985) modification of a 24-item self-report questionnaire<br />

developed by Asher et al. (1984). This questionnaire conta<strong>in</strong>s 16 items<br />

focused on feel<strong>in</strong>gs of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction <strong>in</strong> school<br />

<strong>and</strong> 8 filler items. The 16 primary items <strong>in</strong>clude four different k<strong>in</strong>ds of<br />

items. These items assessed (a) children's feel<strong>in</strong>gs of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess (e.g.,<br />

"I'm lonely at school"), (b) children's appraisal of their current peer<br />

relationships (e.g., "I don't have any friends <strong>in</strong> class"), (c) children's<br />

perceptions of the degree to which important relationship needs are<br />

be<strong>in</strong>g met (e.g., "There're no other kids I can go to when I need help at<br />

school"), <strong>and</strong> (d) children's perceptions of their social competence (e.g.,<br />

"I'm good at work<strong>in</strong>g with other children <strong>in</strong> my class"). Children responded<br />

to each item on a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t scale, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g the degree to which<br />

each statement is a true description of themselves. Total scores could<br />

range from 16 to 80, with greater scores <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g greater lonel<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

<strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction. This questionnaire has been used <strong>in</strong> several<br />

studies with elementary school-age children's <strong>and</strong> has proved to have<br />

excellent <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency (Cronbach a > .90; see Asher et al., 1990).<br />

The <strong>in</strong>ternal reliability <strong>in</strong> the present sample was comparable (a = .91).<br />

One limitation of the Asher <strong>and</strong> Wheeler (1985) measure <strong>in</strong> the context<br />

of the present study is that a number of its items overlap <strong>in</strong> content<br />

with items that assess the quality of children's best friendships. This<br />

means that significant correlations could be found between this measure<br />

<strong>and</strong> items assess<strong>in</strong>g the quality of children's friendships simply<br />

because of the partially overlapp<strong>in</strong>g content (see Nicholls, Licht, &<br />

Pearl, 1982, for a discussion of this issue). Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, we constructed<br />

a secondary measure of children's lonel<strong>in</strong>ess us<strong>in</strong>g a subset of three of<br />

the Asher <strong>and</strong> Wheeler questionnaire items: "I feel alone at school," "I<br />

feel left out of th<strong>in</strong>gs at school," <strong>and</strong> "I'm lonely at school ."This secondary<br />

measure, then, represents a "pure" assessment of children's feel-<br />

3 A small m<strong>in</strong>ority of children (n = 3, or less than 1%) failed or refused<br />

to designate one or more friends at the <strong>in</strong>itial step. These children were<br />

excluded from analyses. Another small m<strong>in</strong>ority (n = 4, also less than<br />

1 %) <strong>in</strong>dicated one or more friendship choices but failed or refused at the<br />

second step to designate a "very best friend" among the friendship<br />

choices. These children could not be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> analyses requir<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knowledge of their very best friendship. Their data could be used <strong>in</strong><br />

other analyses, however, <strong>and</strong> their choices could be used to exam<strong>in</strong>e<br />

the reciprocity of other children's choices. In three additional cases,<br />

children ignored <strong>in</strong>structions <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicated more than one "very best<br />

friendship" from among the three choices. In these cases, one choice<br />

was r<strong>and</strong>omly designated the very best friendship, with<strong>in</strong> the constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

that a nonmutual choice could not take precedence over a mutual<br />

choice (one case).<br />

4 For those children without a mutual very best friend, one of the<br />

child's mutual best friendship choices (top three) was substituted. For<br />

children without either type of friendship, the name of the child's unilateral<br />

(i.e., nonreciprocated) very best friend choice was substituted.<br />

These substitutions were necessary to avoid draw<strong>in</strong>g undue attention<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g adm<strong>in</strong>istration to children without mutual very best friends.<br />

Data from subjects without mutual very best friends were excluded<br />

from data analyses concern<strong>in</strong>g the qualities of children's best friendships.<br />

5 We are <strong>in</strong>debted to William Bukowski <strong>and</strong> his colleagues for provid<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation about the reliability of their items.


Table 1<br />

Items <strong>and</strong> Subscales of the <strong>Friendship</strong> <strong>Quality</strong> Questionnaire<br />

Structure<br />

Subscale/item" coefficient<br />

Validation <strong>and</strong> Car<strong>in</strong>g (a = .90)<br />

15. Makes me feel good about my ideas<br />

4. Tells me I am good at th<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

6. Make each other feel important <strong>and</strong> special<br />

13. Tells me I am pretty smart<br />

8. Says "I'm sorry" if [he/she] hurts my<br />

feel<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

5. Sticks up for me if others talk beh<strong>in</strong>d my<br />

back<br />

10. Has good ideas about games to play<br />

41. Cares about my feel<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

12. Would like me even if others didn't<br />

30. Does not tell others my secrets<br />

Conflict Resolution (a = .73)<br />

26. Make up easily when we have a fight<br />

35. Get over our arguments really quickly<br />

11. Talk about how to get over be<strong>in</strong>g mad at<br />

each other<br />

Conflict <strong>and</strong> Betrayal (a = .84)<br />

20. Argue a lot<br />

27. Fight a lot<br />

3. Get mad a lot<br />

37. Doesn't listen to me<br />

31. Bug each other a lot<br />

9. Sometimes says mean th<strong>in</strong>gs about me to<br />

other kids<br />

21. Can count on to keep promises<br />

Help <strong>and</strong> Guidance (a = .90)<br />

34. Helps me so I can get done quicker<br />

39. Help each other with schoolwork a lot<br />

24. Gives advice with figur<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs out<br />

36. Count on each other for good ideas on how<br />

to get th<strong>in</strong>gs done<br />

32. Come up with good ideas on ways to do<br />

th<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

33. Loan each other th<strong>in</strong>gs all the time<br />

28. Share th<strong>in</strong>gs with each other<br />

18. Do special favors for each other<br />

17. Help each other with chores a lot<br />

Companionship <strong>and</strong> Recreation (a = .75)<br />

2. Always sit together at lunch<br />

7. Always pick each other as partners for th<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

23. Always play together at recess<br />

19. Do fun th<strong>in</strong>gs together a lot<br />

22. Go to each others' houses<br />

Intimate Exchange (a = .86)<br />

14. Always tell each other our problems<br />

25. Talk about the th<strong>in</strong>gs that make us sad<br />

16. Talk to her when I'm mad about someth<strong>in</strong>g<br />

that happened to me<br />

40. Tell each other secrets<br />

38. Tell each other private th<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

29. Talk about how to make ourselves feel<br />

better if we are mad at each other<br />

FRIENDSHIP AND FRIENDSHIP QUALITY<br />

.783<br />

.780<br />

.729<br />

.703<br />

.695<br />

.662<br />

.644<br />

.635<br />

.595<br />

.547<br />

.880<br />

.840<br />

.573<br />

.844<br />

.822<br />

.782<br />

.696<br />

.652<br />

.638<br />

.635<br />

.823<br />

.768<br />

.757<br />

.744<br />

.702<br />

.640<br />

.595<br />

.576<br />

.567<br />

.802<br />

.728<br />

.690<br />

.660<br />

.571<br />

.755<br />

.740<br />

.709<br />

.690<br />

.674<br />

.653<br />

a Numbers are item numbers from the <strong>Friendship</strong> <strong>Quality</strong> Question-<br />

naire.<br />

<strong>in</strong>gs of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction, uncontam<strong>in</strong>ated by their<br />

appraisals of the quality of their friendships, their perception of their<br />

social competence, or their estimate of the state of their current peer<br />

relationships. The <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency of this 3-item pure scale was a<br />

615<br />

= .11. The correlation between the pure lonel<strong>in</strong>ess scale <strong>and</strong> the larger,<br />

16-item scale was .84. Whenever lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction<br />

was analyzed <strong>in</strong> relation to measures of friendship quality, the 3-item<br />

pure lonel<strong>in</strong>ess scale was substituted for the 16-item measure, <strong>and</strong> this<br />

fact is noted accord<strong>in</strong>gly. For the sake of cont<strong>in</strong>uity with other research,<br />

scores for the larger scale were used <strong>in</strong> analyses <strong>in</strong> which the possibility<br />

of overlapp<strong>in</strong>g content was not an issue (i.e., <strong>in</strong> analyses that did not<br />

<strong>in</strong>volve friendship quality or friendship satisfaction).<br />

Procedure<br />

In the late fall, the parents of all children <strong>in</strong> the third through fifth<br />

grades of each participat<strong>in</strong>g school were mailed first-class letters describ<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the study. Parents were asked to contact either the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal or<br />

the experimenters if they did not want their child to participate or if<br />

they required more <strong>in</strong>formation about the study before decid<strong>in</strong>g. Data<br />

collection took place <strong>in</strong> three sessions <strong>in</strong> the w<strong>in</strong>ter <strong>and</strong> spr<strong>in</strong>g of the<br />

school year. Each session lasted about 1 hr. The first session began with<br />

an <strong>in</strong>troduction to the project <strong>and</strong> project staff. This <strong>in</strong>troduction<br />

stressed that participation was voluntary <strong>and</strong> confidential. After this,<br />

we adm<strong>in</strong>istered sociometric measures to assess peer acceptance <strong>and</strong><br />

to identify best friendships. Other measures were also adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong><br />

this session that are not relevant to the present study. In the second<br />

session, approximately 1 month later, the lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction<br />

questionnaire was adm<strong>in</strong>istered. In the third session, several<br />

weeks later, children's perception of the quality of their best friendship<br />

was assessed, as was their satisfaction with their friendship. All measures<br />

were group adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong> class, <strong>and</strong> each session was conducted<br />

by a different <strong>in</strong>vestigator to m<strong>in</strong>imize reactivity across sessions.<br />

Prelim<strong>in</strong>ary Analyses<br />

Results<br />

Prelim<strong>in</strong>ary analyses <strong>in</strong>dicated that children's grade level<br />

was not significantly related to any of the <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong><br />

dependent variables under consideration. Furthermore, there<br />

were no <strong>in</strong>stances <strong>in</strong> which it was necessary to qualify any of the<br />

f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs on the basis of grade. However, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g grade did<br />

adversely affect the size of some cells <strong>in</strong> several analyses. For<br />

this reason, the analyses reported here are derived after collaps<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the data across grades.<br />

Prevalence of <strong>Friendship</strong><br />

Us<strong>in</strong>g the criterion of reciprocated best friendship, 77.6% of<br />

the children <strong>in</strong> this sample had at least one friend. However, as<br />

noted earlier, 484, or 54.9%, of the 881 children <strong>in</strong> the sample<br />

had very best friends. Thus, the narrow<strong>in</strong>g of the focus from<br />

best friendship to very best friendship had the impact of doubl<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the proportion of friendless children identified (45.1% vs.<br />

22.4%).<br />

Of primary <strong>in</strong>terest was the prevalence of best friendship<br />

among groups of children differ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> level of acceptance. We<br />

expected that the likelihood of friendship would <strong>in</strong>crease as<br />

level of acceptance <strong>in</strong>creased but that many low-accepted children<br />

would nevertheless have friends. Logit analysis was used<br />

to model the odds of hav<strong>in</strong>g versus not hav<strong>in</strong>g a mutual best<br />

friend as a function of children's gender <strong>and</strong> level of acceptance.<br />

We evaluated the significance of <strong>in</strong>dividual terms (ma<strong>in</strong> effects


616 JEFFREY G. PARKER AND STEVEN R. ASHER<br />

<strong>and</strong> their <strong>in</strong>teraction) by compar<strong>in</strong>g the goodness of fit of the<br />

model that <strong>in</strong>cluded the term to the goodness of fit of the model<br />

without the term. A backward elim<strong>in</strong>ation procedure was used<br />

to arrive at a parsimonious model that fit the data. For cases <strong>in</strong><br />

which significant effects were found, we exam<strong>in</strong>ed adjusted<br />

residuals to assess the effects of membership <strong>in</strong> particular categories<br />

of the <strong>in</strong>dependent variables (i.e., low- vs. average- vs.<br />

high-acceptance). We ran separate analyses for the two dist<strong>in</strong>ct<br />

operationalizations of friendship to learn how estimates of the<br />

prevalence of friendship, particularly among low-accepted children,<br />

are affected by changes <strong>in</strong> the str<strong>in</strong>gency of the criterion.<br />

The first logit analysis, us<strong>in</strong>g the looser, best friendship criterion<br />

found a significant effect for children's level of acceptance,<br />

likelihood ratio x 2 (2, N= 881) = 103.64, p< .001. Membership<br />

<strong>in</strong> the low-accepted group significantly depressed the odds of<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g a friend, whereas membership <strong>in</strong> the high-accepted<br />

group significantly augmented these odds. Specifically, 45.3%<br />

of low-accepted, 82.3% of average-accepted, <strong>and</strong> 93.8% of highaccepted<br />

children had at least one friend. A ma<strong>in</strong> effect of<br />

gender was found, likelihood ratio x 2 (L N = 881) = 7.09, p <<br />

.01: Girls (81.6%) were more likely than boys (74.2%) to have a<br />

friend. The <strong>in</strong>teraction of gender <strong>and</strong> level of acceptance did<br />

not significantly alter the odds of hav<strong>in</strong>g versus not hav<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

friend.<br />

The second logit analysis, us<strong>in</strong>g the str<strong>in</strong>gent, very best<br />

friendship criterion, revealed a highly significant effect for children's<br />

level of acceptance, likelihood ratio x 2 (2, N = 881) =<br />

53.10, p < .001, <strong>and</strong> a nearly significant Gender X Level of<br />

Acceptance <strong>in</strong>teraction, likelihood ratio x 2 (2, N= 881) = 5.26,<br />

p < .07. Membership <strong>in</strong> the low-accepted group significantly<br />

depressed the odds of hav<strong>in</strong>g a very best friend, whereas membership<br />

<strong>in</strong> the high-accepted group significantly augmented<br />

these odds. Whereas 58.27o of average-accepted children <strong>and</strong><br />

69.5% of high-accepted children had very best friends, only<br />

29.3% of low-accepted children had very best friends. Furthermore,<br />

whereas the proportion of boys with very best friends<br />

<strong>and</strong> the proportion of girls with very best friends were roughly<br />

equivalent <strong>in</strong> both the high- <strong>and</strong> average-accepted groups<br />

(72.3% vs. 66.7% <strong>and</strong> 56.2% vs. 60.7%, respectively), low-accepted<br />

girls (39.5%) were twice as likely as low-accepted boys<br />

(18.9%) to have a mutual very best friend. A comparison of<br />

these results with the correspond<strong>in</strong>g results from the earlier<br />

analyses suggests that tighten<strong>in</strong>g the friendship criterion is particularly<br />

likely to affect the estimate of the number of low-accepted<br />

boys with friends.<br />

Next, us<strong>in</strong>g the more <strong>in</strong>clusive def<strong>in</strong>ition, we exam<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

whether there were differences <strong>in</strong> the number of best friends<br />

children had as a function of gender <strong>and</strong> level of acceptance. A<br />

2 (gender) X 3 (level of acceptance) analysis of variance (AN-<br />

OVA) yielded a highly significant effect for level of acceptance,<br />

F(2,875) = 81.02, p < .0001; a ma<strong>in</strong> effect for gender, F(l ,875)<br />

= 10.39, p < .001; <strong>and</strong> no Gender X Level of Acceptance <strong>in</strong>teraction,<br />

F(2,875) = 0.79, ns. Post hoc (Tukey) comparisons <strong>in</strong>dicated<br />

that low-accepted children had significantly fewer friends<br />

(M = 0.65, SD = 0.82) than average-accepted children (M =<br />

1.50, SD = 0.98), who <strong>in</strong> turn had significantly fewer friendships<br />

than high-accepted children (M= 2.03, SD = 0.89). Girls<br />

(M = 1.54, SD = 1.01) had significantly more friends than boys<br />

(M= 1.35, SD= 1.02).<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, we studied the relationship between the acceptance<br />

levels of very best friends. To accomplish this, we identified all<br />

dyads that met our sociometric criteria for very best friendship<br />

(A r = 351). We then classified each dyad accord<strong>in</strong>g to the level of<br />

acceptance of the partners. Of the 351 friendships, there were 9<br />

dyads <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g 2 low-accepted children, 36 dyads <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

low-accepted <strong>and</strong> an average-accepted child, 3 dyads <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a low-accepted <strong>and</strong> a high-accepted child, 179 dyads <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g 2<br />

average-accepted children, 104 dyads <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g an average-accepted<br />

<strong>and</strong> a high-accepted child, <strong>and</strong> 20 dyads <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g 2<br />

high-accepted children. A one-sample chi-square test revealed<br />

that the observed distribution of configurations departed significantly<br />

from its expected distribution, x 2 (5, N= 351)= 79.23, p<br />

< .0001, given r<strong>and</strong>om pair<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the relative frequency of<br />

children <strong>in</strong> the three acceptance groups. Inspection of the st<strong>and</strong>ardized<br />

residuals from this analysis <strong>in</strong>dicated that the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal<br />

reason for this disparity rests with the pattern of low-accepted<br />

children's <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> friendships. Whereas the number<br />

of low-low friendship dyads was consistent with<br />

expectations (9 observed vs. 10 expected), the number of dyads<br />

<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a low-accepted child with either an average-accepted<br />

or a high-accepted partner was markedly less than the number<br />

expected through r<strong>and</strong>om pair<strong>in</strong>g (36 observed vs. 82 expected<br />

<strong>and</strong> 3 observed vs. 17 expected, respectively). Thus, low-accepted<br />

children's relative lack of <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> friendships can<br />

be understood as a tendency for lower participation <strong>in</strong> friendships<br />

with better-accepted children, specifically, <strong>and</strong> not as an<br />

<strong>in</strong>discrim<strong>in</strong>ant tendency toward less <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> friendship.<br />

At the same time, it is important to note that low-accepted<br />

children's <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> friendship is not restricted to <strong>in</strong>volvement<br />

<strong>in</strong> dyads with other low-accepted partners. Indeed, the<br />

majority of dyads <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g low-accepted children were dyads<br />

<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a low-accepted child <strong>and</strong> an average-accepted partner<br />

(36 out of 48 dyads, or 75%).<br />

<strong>Friendship</strong> <strong>Quality</strong><br />

Identification of subscales. To identify subscales on the<br />

FQQ, we performed a pr<strong>in</strong>cipal-components analysis (oblique<br />

rotation) on the responses of the 484 children with very best<br />

friends to the 40 primary items <strong>in</strong> the FQQ. This analysis resulted<br />

<strong>in</strong> six factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. The six<br />

factors, which were <strong>in</strong> close agreement with the predicted underly<strong>in</strong>g<br />

structure of the measure based on pilot test<strong>in</strong>g, were<br />

labeled Intimate Exchange, Conflict Resolution, Companionship<br />

<strong>and</strong> Recreation, Help <strong>and</strong> Guidance, Validation <strong>and</strong> Car<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Conflict <strong>and</strong> Betrayal. The rightmost column of Table<br />

1 shows the structure coefficient from the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal-components<br />

analysis for each item <strong>in</strong> relation to the factor on which it<br />

showed the highest load<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The pr<strong>in</strong>cipal-components analysis was used to guide the<br />

cluster<strong>in</strong>g of items <strong>in</strong>to subscales. Specifically, we formed one<br />

subscale for each factor <strong>in</strong> the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal-components analysis<br />

by comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the items that loaded highly on each factor. Table<br />

1 displays the Cronbach alpha for these six subscales. As is<br />

evident, the <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency of each subscale was satisfactory.<br />

A child's score for each subscale was the average of his or<br />

her rat<strong>in</strong>gs for the relevant items (unit weight<strong>in</strong>g), after appropriate<br />

reverse scor<strong>in</strong>g if necessary. The six subscales were mod-


erately to highly <strong>in</strong>tercorrelated, with rs rang<strong>in</strong>g from. 16 to .75<br />

<strong>in</strong> absolute magnitude. 6 Conflict <strong>and</strong> Betrayal correlated negatively<br />

with all other subscales. All other subscales were positively<br />

<strong>in</strong>tercorrelated.<br />

Acceptance <strong>and</strong> friendship quality. The focus of this analysis<br />

was on whether low-accepted children who have mutual very<br />

best friendships have friendships that are of comparable quality<br />

to the friendships of other children. This issue was exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong><br />

a 2 (gender) X 3 (level of acceptance) multivariate analysis of<br />

variance, with the six subscale scores from the FQQ serv<strong>in</strong>g as<br />

dependent variables. 7<br />

The results of this analysis yielded a significant multivariate<br />

ma<strong>in</strong> effect for gender, F(6, 454) = 4.87, p < .001; a significant<br />

multivariate ma<strong>in</strong> effect for level of acceptance, F(l 2, 910) =<br />

3.17, p < .001; <strong>and</strong> a nonsignificant multivariate Gender X<br />

Level of Acceptance <strong>in</strong>teraction, F(\2, 910) = 1.32. Follow-up<br />

univariate analyses revealed gender differences for four of the<br />

six friendship quality variables. Table 2 gives the mean, st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

deviation, <strong>and</strong> sample size for each gender on each of the<br />

six friendship quality variables <strong>and</strong> the correspond<strong>in</strong>g F value<br />

for the gender comparison. Boys <strong>and</strong> girls did not differ <strong>in</strong> their<br />

characterizations of their very best friendship with respect to<br />

either conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal or companionship <strong>and</strong> recreation.<br />

However, boys did report less <strong>in</strong>timate exchange, more difficulty<br />

resolv<strong>in</strong>g conflict, less validation <strong>and</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> less<br />

help <strong>and</strong> guidance <strong>in</strong> their friendships than did girls.<br />

Follow-up univariate analyses <strong>in</strong>dicated acceptance-level differences<br />

for five of the six friendship quality variables. Table 3<br />

shows these results. The mean, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, <strong>and</strong> sample<br />

size for each acceptance group for each friendship quality variable<br />

are also shown <strong>in</strong> Table 3, along with the ^statistic for each<br />

correspond<strong>in</strong>g comparison <strong>and</strong> the results of post hoc pairwise<br />

comparisons of means (Tukey).<br />

As shown <strong>in</strong> Table 3, children of differ<strong>in</strong>g levels of acceptance<br />

did not differ significantly with respect to how much<br />

companionship <strong>and</strong> recreation they saw their friendships as<br />

provid<strong>in</strong>g. However, by their reports, low-accepted children's<br />

friendships were less adequate than the friendships of other<br />

children <strong>in</strong> every other respect. Low-accepted children reported<br />

significantly less validation <strong>and</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g, more difficulty<br />

resolv<strong>in</strong>g conflict, <strong>and</strong> less help <strong>and</strong> guidance than both highaccepted<br />

<strong>and</strong> average-accepted children, who did not differ<br />

from one another <strong>in</strong> these respects. Low-accepted children also<br />

reported significantly less <strong>in</strong>timate disclosure <strong>in</strong> their friendships<br />

than high-accepted children. Low-accepted children reported<br />

more conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal <strong>in</strong> their friendships than did<br />

average-accepted children but not more conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal<br />

than high-accepted children, although the means were <strong>in</strong> that<br />

direction. High-accepted children reported somewhat more<br />

conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal than average-accepted children, but this<br />

difference was not significant. F<strong>in</strong>ally, it is of <strong>in</strong>terest that lowaccepted<br />

children showed the greatest variability of any group<br />

on every friendship quality, except conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal (see the<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations <strong>in</strong> Table 3).<br />

Acceptance, <strong>Friendship</strong> <strong>Quality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Friendship</strong><br />

Satisfaction<br />

In general, children with very best friends expressed relatively<br />

high satisfaction with these relationships. Low-accepted<br />

FRIENDSHIP AND FRIENDSHIP QUALITY<br />

617<br />

children (M= 11.95, SD = 4.28) reported slightly less friendship<br />

satisfaction than did either average-accepted (M = 13.04, SD =<br />

3.07) or high-accepted (M= 13.19, SD = 3.04) children. However,<br />

these differences were not significant, F(2, 458) = 1.89.<br />

Boys tended to express more satisfaction (M = 13.28, SD = 2.82)<br />

with their very best friendships than girls (M = 12.65, SD =<br />

3.52), but this difference did not reach significance, F(l, 458) =<br />

3.35, p < .07. The Gender X Acceptance Level <strong>in</strong>teraction was<br />

not significant, F(2, 458) = 2.04.<br />

Correlations computed between each of the friendship quality<br />

subscales <strong>and</strong> the satisfaction composite <strong>in</strong>dicated that positive<br />

friendship qualities (i.e., validation <strong>and</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g, companionship<br />

<strong>and</strong> recreation, help <strong>and</strong> guidance, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>timate exchange)<br />

were associated with higher satisfaction with the relationship,<br />

whereas perceptions of conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal were associated<br />

with lower satisfaction. These correlations ranged <strong>in</strong> absolute<br />

magnitude from .35 to .52 <strong>and</strong> were highly similar across the<br />

three acceptance groups. 8 All correlations were statistically significant<br />

(p < .01). A multiple regression analysis with satisfaction<br />

regressed simultaneously on the six friendship quality subscales<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicated that the six friendship quality variables accounted<br />

for 46% of the variance <strong>in</strong> satisfaction, R = .68, F(6,<br />

452) = 65.01,/?< .0001. 9<br />

<strong>Friendship</strong> Adjustment, Acceptance, <strong>and</strong> Lonel<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

Sociometric rat<strong>in</strong>gs of acceptance were negatively correlated<br />

with lonel<strong>in</strong>ess, r = -.39, p < .001, for the entire sample. This<br />

correlation is consistent with past research (see Asher et al.,<br />

1990). We performed a <strong>Friendship</strong> Status (friended vs. friend-<br />

6<br />

A table of these correlations is available on request from Jeffrey G.<br />

Parker.<br />

7<br />

In these <strong>and</strong> subsequent analyses of friendship quality, all children's<br />

responses were <strong>in</strong>cluded as long as the child was a participant <strong>in</strong> a very<br />

best friendship. We assume that the perceptions of the <strong>in</strong>dividual children,<br />

<strong>and</strong> not the dyad itself, are the appropriate unit of analysis <strong>in</strong> this<br />

context. Thus, rat<strong>in</strong>gs by 2 children of the same friendship are considered<br />

separate data po<strong>in</strong>ts. As a precaution aga<strong>in</strong>st violations of the<br />

<strong>in</strong>dependence of the data, however, we repeated all analyses <strong>in</strong> the<br />

present study <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g friendship quality after elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g any possibility<br />

of dependencies across subjects. Specifically, there were 133<br />

friendships (<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g 266 children) <strong>in</strong> which potential dependencies<br />

<strong>in</strong> the data existed (i.e., where both partners contributed data on<br />

friendship quality). For these pairs, the data of one member of the dyad<br />

were r<strong>and</strong>omly dropped from statistical analyses, with the qualification<br />

that <strong>in</strong> mixed-acceptance group pairs <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g an average-accepted<br />

child with either a high-accepted child or a low-accepted child,<br />

the high-accepted or low-accepted child was preferentially reta<strong>in</strong>ed to<br />

maximize the sample sizes of these extreme groups. Analyses were<br />

then rerun on the new, reduced data set (A' = 484 - 133 = 351). These<br />

reanalyses resulted <strong>in</strong> no qualifications concern<strong>in</strong>g significant versus<br />

nonsignificant effects.<br />

8<br />

A table of these correlations is available on request from Jeffrey G.<br />

Parker.<br />

9<br />

Although the regression analysis provided <strong>in</strong>formation about the<br />

beta weights for each of the friendship quality variables, comparisons<br />

among these betas would be mislead<strong>in</strong>g because the existence of substantial<br />

correlations between these variables resulted <strong>in</strong> multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity<br />

<strong>and</strong> mutual suppression among the predictors <strong>in</strong> the multivariate<br />

context.


618 JEFFREY G. PARKER AND STEVEN R. ASHER<br />

Table 2<br />

Comparisons of Boys <strong>and</strong> Girls on <strong>Friendship</strong> <strong>Quality</strong> Variables<br />

Variable<br />

Validation <strong>and</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal<br />

Companionship <strong>and</strong><br />

recreation<br />

Help <strong>and</strong> guidance<br />

Intimate exchange<br />

Conflict resolution<br />

n<br />

230<br />

232<br />

231<br />

232<br />

231<br />

231<br />

Girls<br />

M<br />

2.85<br />

1.04<br />

2.71<br />

2.54<br />

2.47<br />

2.95<br />

*p


Table 4<br />

Summary of Hierarchical Regressions of<br />

<strong>Friendship</strong> Qualities on "Pure" Lonel<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

<strong>Quality</strong><br />

Validation <strong>and</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal<br />

Companionship <strong>and</strong><br />

recreation<br />

Help <strong>and</strong> guidance<br />

Intimate exchange<br />

Conflict resolution<br />

r<br />

-.29<br />

.18<br />

-.26<br />

-.26<br />

-.19<br />

-.29<br />

-.262<br />

.160<br />

-.250<br />

-.229<br />

-.175<br />

-.276<br />

FRIENDSHIP AND FRIENDSHIP QUALITY<br />

•^change<br />

.07<br />

.03<br />

.06<br />

.05<br />

.03<br />

.07<br />

F( 1,455)<br />

34.26**<br />

12.84*<br />

32.57**<br />

26.21**<br />

14.03*<br />

38.77**<br />

* Controll<strong>in</strong>g for gender, sociometric rat<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> Gender X Sociometric<br />

Rat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teraction.<br />

*p -.222; all ps < .0001). Aga<strong>in</strong>,<br />

neither gender nor the <strong>in</strong>teraction terms <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g gender, acceptance<br />

by peers, or friendship quality significantly predicted<br />

lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>in</strong> any regression.<br />

Discussion<br />

The present data suggest that the dist<strong>in</strong>ction between acceptance<br />

<strong>and</strong> friendship adjustment is a mean<strong>in</strong>gful one <strong>and</strong><br />

should be preserved. With respect to the prevalence of friendships,<br />

for example, we found that not all highly accepted children<br />

had friends, even when the focus was on best as opposed to<br />

very best friendship. Indeed, just under a third of high-accepted<br />

children did not have the child they named as their very<br />

best friend <strong>in</strong>clude them among his or her list of three friends.<br />

Conversely, many, although certa<strong>in</strong>ly not most, low-accepted<br />

children did have friends. Relax<strong>in</strong>g the criteria for identify<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

dyad as friends led to many more children at all levels of acceptance<br />

identified as hav<strong>in</strong>g friends, especially low-accepted<br />

boys. Thus, it would be <strong>in</strong>correct to characterize all low-accepted<br />

children as children without friends.<br />

Similarly, the analyses <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g children's reports of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

<strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction <strong>in</strong>dicated that children's friendship<br />

adjustment had an <strong>in</strong>fluence on children's feel<strong>in</strong>gs of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

above <strong>and</strong> beyond the <strong>in</strong>fluence of peer group acceptance,<br />

further support<strong>in</strong>g the dist<strong>in</strong>ction between friendship<br />

adjustment <strong>and</strong> group acceptance. Two different sets of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

support this po<strong>in</strong>t. First, children without best friends<br />

were more lonely than children with best friends, <strong>and</strong> this was<br />

true regardless of how well accepted they were. Thus, an addi-<br />

619<br />

tive relationship between acceptance <strong>and</strong> friendship obta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

with respect to lonel<strong>in</strong>ess. Second, the regression analyses that<br />

focused on the qualities of children's best friendships <strong>in</strong>dicated<br />

that friendship quality <strong>and</strong> acceptance contributed separately<br />

<strong>and</strong> about equally to the prediction of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess. Together,<br />

these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that children's feel<strong>in</strong>gs of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess can<br />

arise from several sources that, <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation, can seriously<br />

underm<strong>in</strong>e children's feel<strong>in</strong>gs of well-be<strong>in</strong>g. Receiv<strong>in</strong>g poor acceptance<br />

by peers, lack<strong>in</strong>g a friend, or hav<strong>in</strong>g a friendship that<br />

fails to meet important relationship needs each contribute. (See<br />

Weiss, 1973, for a related discussion <strong>in</strong> the literature on lonel<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

<strong>in</strong> adulthood.)<br />

Although results of the present study support the dist<strong>in</strong>ction<br />

between group acceptance <strong>and</strong> friendship, the extent of the<br />

connection between children's adjustment <strong>in</strong> the two doma<strong>in</strong>s<br />

should not be m<strong>in</strong>imized. Better-accepted children were more<br />

likely to be <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> specific dyadic friendships than were<br />

less-accepted children. Indeed, high-accepted <strong>and</strong> average-accepted<br />

children were about twice as likely to have a very best<br />

friend as low-accepted children. This disparity was large for<br />

girls but was especially large for boys; fewer than 1 <strong>in</strong> 5 low-accepted<br />

boys had very best friends.<br />

Likewise, analyses <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the new measure of friendship<br />

quality po<strong>in</strong>ted to important differences between low-accepted,<br />

average-accepted, <strong>and</strong> high-accepted children's friendships<br />

with respect to validation <strong>and</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g, help <strong>and</strong> guidance,<br />

conflict resolution, <strong>in</strong>timate exchange, <strong>and</strong> conflict <strong>and</strong><br />

betrayal. Low-accepted children's very best friendships were<br />

more problematic <strong>in</strong> each of these respects than the friendships<br />

of other children.<br />

The development of a self-report <strong>in</strong>strument for assess<strong>in</strong>g<br />

children's perceptions is another contribution of this research.<br />

The friendship quality measure conta<strong>in</strong>s subscales with good<br />

<strong>in</strong>ternal consistency <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that children can reliably describe<br />

features of their close peer relationships. In addition to<br />

the several differences noted for low-accepted versus better-accepted<br />

children, results from the friendship quality measure<br />

also yielded <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation about gender <strong>in</strong> relation to<br />

friendship quality. Whereas two dimensions of friendship quality<br />

did not dist<strong>in</strong>guish boys' <strong>and</strong> girls' friendships from one<br />

another (i.e., conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal, companionship <strong>and</strong> recreation),<br />

girls reported higher levels than boys of validation <strong>and</strong><br />

support, help <strong>and</strong> guidance, conflict resolution, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>timate<br />

exchange. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are consistent with the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

most (e.g., Buhrmester & Furman, 1987; Furman & Buhrmester,<br />

1985; Sharabany et al., 1981) but not all (e.g., Berndt & Perry,<br />

1986) past studies that have compared boys' <strong>and</strong> girls' friendships.<br />

Research is needed on the factors that give rise to these<br />

gender differences. One suggestion has been that differences <strong>in</strong><br />

the play preferences <strong>and</strong> group structures of boys <strong>and</strong> girls (e.g.,<br />

the greater tendency for boys to play <strong>in</strong> large groups <strong>and</strong> for<br />

their groups to be hierarchically organized) give rise to differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> the qualities of their close personal relationships (see<br />

Maccoby, 1990). To date, however, this hypothesis has not received<br />

empirical scrut<strong>in</strong>y or support.<br />

A further po<strong>in</strong>t about friendship quality concerns the heterogeneity<br />

with<strong>in</strong> the low-accepted group. This group showed the<br />

largest with<strong>in</strong>-group variability of all groups on five of the six<br />

qualities. An important task for future research will be to docu-


620 JEFFREY G. PARKER AND STEVEN R. ASHER<br />

merit <strong>and</strong> account for cases <strong>in</strong> which low-accepted children<br />

have friendships that are not dissimilar from those of other<br />

children. Recent research suggests the existence of aggressive<br />

versus withdrawn subgroups of low-accepted children, with the<br />

withdrawn subgroup report<strong>in</strong>g more lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> social dissatisfaction<br />

(Parkhurst&Asher, 1992; Williams &Asher, 1987).<br />

Attention to these subgroups might also reveal differences <strong>in</strong><br />

the quality of their friendships.<br />

<strong>Friendship</strong> quality was assessed <strong>in</strong> this research through the<br />

reports of the children themselves. There is much to recommend<br />

this way of assess<strong>in</strong>g the properties of friendship (see<br />

Furman, 1984b). It is particularly useful when the qualities<br />

under study are <strong>in</strong>frequent <strong>and</strong> difficult to observe, as is the<br />

case for many of the qualities of friendship under study here<br />

(e.g., conflict <strong>and</strong> betrayal, <strong>in</strong>timacy), or when it is the subjective<br />

impact of behavior (such as a personal feel<strong>in</strong>g of validation) that<br />

is of most <strong>in</strong>terest. At the same time, rely<strong>in</strong>g on children's reports<br />

also poses certa<strong>in</strong> important <strong>in</strong>terpretative challenges to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g children's friendship experiences. To beg<strong>in</strong><br />

with, because there are two parties to the relationship, friendships<br />

have two subjective realities rather than one (Furman,<br />

1984b), <strong>and</strong> these may not always co<strong>in</strong>cide. To illustrate, as<br />

noted earlier (see Footnote 7), for a subset (n = 133) of the reciprocal<br />

very best friendships <strong>in</strong> our sample, data on friendship<br />

quality were available from both partners. For these pairs,<br />

correlations between each partner's rat<strong>in</strong>gs ranged from .64 for<br />

companionship <strong>and</strong> recreation to .21 for conflict resolution,<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g considerable differences <strong>in</strong> the op<strong>in</strong>ions of partners.<br />

Such differences may be due to real differences <strong>in</strong> the partners'<br />

experiences <strong>in</strong> the relationship. But, just as important, they<br />

may signal that the dyad has not progressed far toward a shared<br />

underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the mean<strong>in</strong>g of each other's behavior. In future<br />

research, the concordance of the partners' perspectives on<br />

the relationship might be studied more systematically as a w<strong>in</strong>dow<br />

on the quality of children's friendships.<br />

Studies that rely on children's reports about their friendships<br />

also must allow that children's construals of their relationships<br />

do not represent objective accounts of their actual social <strong>in</strong>teraction.<br />

It would be desirable <strong>in</strong> future research to supplement<br />

children's reports of their friendship's qualities with observational<br />

data on how qualities such as <strong>in</strong>timate disclosure, conflict<br />

resolution, help <strong>and</strong> guidance, <strong>and</strong> so on are expressed <strong>in</strong><br />

the context of accepted <strong>and</strong> unaccepted children's <strong>in</strong>teractions<br />

with their very best friends. Doubtless, further differences <strong>in</strong><br />

the friendships of highly accepted <strong>and</strong> poorly accepted children<br />

will be observed.<br />

The emphasis given to children's feel<strong>in</strong>gs of lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong><br />

social dissatisfaction <strong>in</strong> relation to friendship adjustment <strong>and</strong><br />

peer acceptance <strong>in</strong> the present study is <strong>in</strong> keep<strong>in</strong>g with the<br />

<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terest with<strong>in</strong> developmental psychology <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tersection<br />

of affect <strong>and</strong> social adjustment (Thompson, 1990).<br />

Moreover, because lonel<strong>in</strong>ess is already a well-established concommitant<br />

of problems of group acceptance, it represents a<br />

particularly good <strong>in</strong>itial criterion aga<strong>in</strong>st which to judge the<br />

magnitude of the predictive validity of measures of friendship<br />

<strong>and</strong> friendship quality <strong>in</strong> children. However, future research<br />

should consider the divergent as well as the convergent correlates<br />

of friendship adjustment <strong>and</strong> peer acceptance. For the<br />

most part, scholars who have stressed the importance of posi-<br />

tive peer experiences <strong>in</strong> childhood have not sharply differentiated<br />

between the benefits associated with friendship as opposed<br />

to general peer acceptance. Thus, considerable conceptual<br />

groundwork rema<strong>in</strong>s to be established. However, as<br />

Furman <strong>and</strong> Robb<strong>in</strong>s (1985) have compell<strong>in</strong>gly argued, it seems<br />

reasonable that friendship <strong>and</strong> group acceptance would contribute<br />

<strong>in</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>ctive ways to children's socialization. The close,<br />

accept<strong>in</strong>g context of friendship, for example, may permit children<br />

greater latitude <strong>in</strong> the behavior that they explore <strong>and</strong> the<br />

attitudes that they express. In addition, the voluntary nature of<br />

friendship may make it an extremely important context for underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

the skills <strong>and</strong> requirements of commitment, personal<br />

responsibility, <strong>and</strong> loyalty. And friendships may meet specific<br />

needs of children, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g needs for <strong>in</strong>timacy, social support,<br />

<strong>in</strong>strumental aid, <strong>and</strong> a reliable ally. On the other h<strong>and</strong>,<br />

peer acceptance may be an important prerequisite for <strong>and</strong> outgrowth<br />

of children's leadership <strong>and</strong> assertive skills <strong>and</strong> may<br />

meet children's needs to feel that they are a part of a larger<br />

community, someth<strong>in</strong>g that a particular friendship relationship<br />

is less likely to provide.<br />

References<br />

Asher, S. R., & Coie, J. D. (Eds.). (1990). Peer rejection <strong>in</strong> childhood.<br />

Cambridge, Engl<strong>and</strong>: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Asher, S. R., & Hymel, S. (1981). Children's social competence <strong>in</strong> peer<br />

relations: Sociometric <strong>and</strong> behavioral assessment. In J. D. W<strong>in</strong>e &<br />

M. D. Smye (Eds.), Social competence (pp. 125-157). New York:<br />

Guilford Press.<br />

Asher, S. R., Hymel, S., & Renshaw, R D. (1984). Lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>in</strong> children.<br />

Child Development, 55,1456-1464.<br />

Asher, S. R., & Parker, J. G. (1989). The significance of peer relationship<br />

problems <strong>in</strong> childhood. In B. H.Schneider.G. Attili, J. Nadel,&<br />

R. P. Weissberg (Eds.), Social competence <strong>in</strong> developmental perspective<br />

(pp. 5-23). Amsterdam: Kluwer Academic.<br />

Asher, S. R., Parkhurst. J. T, Hymel, S., & Williams, G. A. (1990). Peer<br />

rejection <strong>and</strong> lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>in</strong> childhood. In S. R. Asher & J. D. Coie<br />

(Eds.), Peer rejection <strong>in</strong> childhood (pp. 253-273). Cambridge, Engl<strong>and</strong>:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Asher, S. R., S<strong>in</strong>gleton, L. C, & Taylor, A. R. (1982, April). Acceptance<br />

versus friendship: A longitud<strong>in</strong>al study of racial <strong>in</strong>tegration. Paper<br />

presented at the annual meet<strong>in</strong>gs of the American Educational Research<br />

Association, New York.<br />

Asher, S. R., & Wheeler, V A. (1985). Children's lonel<strong>in</strong>ess: A comparison<br />

of rejected <strong>and</strong> neglected peer status. Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><br />

Cl<strong>in</strong>ical <strong>Psychology</strong>, 53. 500-505.<br />

Berghout-Aust<strong>in</strong>, A. M. (1985). Young children's attention to dyadic<br />

conversation as modified by sociometric status. Genetic, Social, <strong>and</strong><br />

General <strong>Psychology</strong> Monographs, 111, 151-165.<br />

Berndt, T. J. (1984). Sociometric, social-cognitive, <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

measures for the study of friendship <strong>and</strong> popularity. In T. Field, J. L.<br />

Roopnar<strong>in</strong>e, & M. Segal (Eds.), <strong>Friendship</strong>s <strong>in</strong> normal <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>icapped<br />

children (pp. 31-52). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.<br />

Berndt, T. J., & Perry, T. B. (1986). Children's perceptions of friendships<br />

as supportive relationships. Developmental <strong>Psychology</strong>, 22, 640-648.<br />

Bichard, S. L., Alden, L., Walker, L. J., & McMahon, R. J. (1988).<br />

<strong>Friendship</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> socially accepted, rejected, <strong>and</strong> neglected<br />

children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 34, 33-46.<br />

Blyth, D. A. (1983). Surviv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> thriv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the social world: A commentary<br />

on six new studies of popular, rejected, <strong>and</strong> neglected children.<br />

Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 449-458.


FRIENDSHIP AND FRIENDSHIP QUALITY 621<br />

Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W (1987). The development of compan- H<strong>in</strong>de, R. A. (1979). Towards underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g relations. San Diego, CA:<br />

ionship <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>timacy. Child Development, 58,1101-1113.<br />

Academic Press.<br />

Bukowski, W M., & Hoza, B. (1989). Popularity <strong>and</strong> friendship: Issues Howes, C. (1988). Peer <strong>in</strong>teraction of young children. Monographs of<br />

<strong>in</strong> theory, measurement, <strong>and</strong> outcome. In T. J. Berndt & G. W Ladd the Society for Research <strong>in</strong> Child Development, 53(1, Serial No. 217).<br />

(Eds.), Peer relationships <strong>in</strong> child development (pp. 15-45). New \brk: Kurdek, L. A., & Krile, D. (1982). A developmental analyses of the<br />

Wiley.<br />

relation between peer acceptance <strong>and</strong> both <strong>in</strong>terpersonal under-<br />

Bukowski, W M., Hoza, B., & Newcomb, A. F. (1987). <strong>Friendship</strong>, popust<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> perceived social self-competence. Child Development,<br />

larity, <strong>and</strong> the "self dur<strong>in</strong>g adolescence. Unpublished manuscript, 53, 1485-1491.<br />

University of Ma<strong>in</strong>e, Department of <strong>Psychology</strong>.<br />

Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender <strong>and</strong> relationships: A developmental<br />

Buzzelli, C. A. (1988). The development of trust <strong>in</strong> children's relations perspective. American Psychologist, 45, 513-520.<br />

with peers. Child Study Journal, 18, 33-41.<br />

Mannar<strong>in</strong>o, A. P. (1976). <strong>Friendship</strong> patterns <strong>and</strong> altruistic behavior <strong>in</strong><br />

Carlson-Jones, D. (1985). Persuasive appeals <strong>and</strong> responses to appeals preadolescent males. Developmental <strong>Psychology</strong>, 12, 555-556.<br />

among friends <strong>and</strong> acqua<strong>in</strong>tances. Child Development, 56, 757-763. McGuire, K. D, & Weisz, J. R. (1982). Social cognition <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

Carlson-Jones, D., & Bowl<strong>in</strong>g, B. (1988, March). Preschool friends <strong>and</strong> correlates of preadolescent chumship. Child Development, 53,1478affective<br />

knowledge: A comparison on mutual <strong>and</strong> unilateral friends. 1484.<br />

Paper presented at the Southwest Conference on Human Develop- Nicholls, J. G., Licht, B. G., & Pearl, R. A. (1982). Some dangers of<br />

ment, Charleston, SC.<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g personality questionnaires to study personality. Psychological<br />

Cassidy, J., & Asher, S. R. (1992). Lonel<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>and</strong> peer relations <strong>in</strong> Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 92, 572-580.<br />

young children. Child Development, 63, 350-365.<br />

Oden, S., & Asher, S. R. (1977). Coach<strong>in</strong>g children <strong>in</strong> social skills for<br />

Crick, N. R, & Ladd, G. W. (1993). Children's perceptions of their peer friendship mak<strong>in</strong>g. Child Development, 48, 495-506.<br />

experiences: Attributions, lonel<strong>in</strong>ess, social anxiety, <strong>and</strong> social avoid- Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1989, April). Peer relations <strong>and</strong> social<br />

ance. Developmental <strong>Psychology</strong>, 29, 244-254.<br />

adjustment: Are friendship <strong>and</strong> group acceptance dist<strong>in</strong>ct doma<strong>in</strong>s?<br />

Drewry, D. L., & Clark, M. L. (1984). Factors important <strong>in</strong> the forma- In W M. Bukowski (Chair), Properties, processes, <strong>and</strong> effects of<br />

tion of preschooler's friendships. Journal of Genetic <strong>Psychology</strong>, 146, friendship relations dur<strong>in</strong>g childhood <strong>and</strong> adolescence. Symposium<br />

37-44.<br />

conducted at the biennial meet<strong>in</strong>g of the Society for Research <strong>in</strong><br />

Epste<strong>in</strong>, J. L. (1986). <strong>Friendship</strong> selection: Developmental <strong>and</strong> environ- Child Development, Kansas City, MO.<br />

mental <strong>in</strong>fluences. In E. C. Mueller & C. R. Cooper (Eds.), Process Parkhurst, J. T, & Asher, S. R. (1992). Peer rejection <strong>in</strong> middle school:<br />

<strong>and</strong> outcome <strong>in</strong> peer relationships (pp. 129-160). San Diego, CA: Subgroup differences <strong>in</strong> behavior, lonel<strong>in</strong>ess, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

Academic Press.<br />

concerns. Developmental <strong>Psychology</strong>, 28, 231-241.<br />

Feltman, R. E, Doyle, A. B., Schwartzman, A. E., Serb<strong>in</strong>, L. A., & Rizzo, T. A. (1988). The relationship between friendship <strong>and</strong> sociome-<br />

Led<strong>in</strong>gham, J. E. (1985). <strong>Friendship</strong> <strong>in</strong> normal <strong>and</strong> deviant children. tric judgments of peer acceptance <strong>and</strong> rejection. Child Study Jour-<br />

Journal of Early Adolescence, 5, 371-382.<br />

nal, 18, 161-191.<br />

Furman, W. (1984a). Issues <strong>in</strong> the assessment of social skills of normal Roopnar<strong>in</strong>e, J. L., & Field, T. M. (1984). Play <strong>in</strong>teractions of friends <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>icapped children. In T. Field, J. L. Roopnar<strong>in</strong>e, & M. Segal acqua<strong>in</strong>tances <strong>in</strong> nursery school. In T. Field, J. L. Roopnar<strong>in</strong>e, & M.<br />

(Eds.), <strong>Friendship</strong>s <strong>in</strong> normal <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>icapped children (pp. 3-30). Segal (Eds.), <strong>Friendship</strong>s <strong>in</strong> normal <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>icapped children (pp.<br />

Norwood, NJ: Ablex.<br />

89-98). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.<br />

Furman, W (1984b). Some observations on the study of personal rela- Sharabany, R., Gershoni, R., & Hofman, J. (1981). Girlfriend, boytionships.<br />

In J. C. Masters & K. Yark<strong>in</strong>-Lev<strong>in</strong> (Eds.), Boundary areas friend: Age <strong>and</strong> sex differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>timate friendship. Developmen-<br />

<strong>in</strong> social <strong>and</strong> developmental psychology (pp. 16-42). San Diego, CA: tal <strong>Psychology</strong>, 17, 800-808.<br />

Academic Press.<br />

S<strong>in</strong>gleton, L. C, & Asher, S. R. (1977). Peer preferences <strong>and</strong> social<br />

Furman, W, & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children's perceptions of the <strong>in</strong>teraction among third-grade children <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>tegrated school dis-<br />

personal relationships <strong>in</strong> their social networks. Developmental Psytrict. Journal of Educational <strong>Psychology</strong>, 69, 330-336.<br />

chology, 21, 1016-1024.<br />

Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment <strong>and</strong> the construction of<br />

Furman, W, & Robb<strong>in</strong>s, P. (1985). What's the po<strong>in</strong>t?: Selection of treat- relationships. In W W Hartup & Z. Rub<strong>in</strong> (Eds.), Relationships <strong>and</strong><br />

ment objectives. In B. Schneider, K. H. Rub<strong>in</strong>, & J. E. Led<strong>in</strong>gham development (pp. 51-72). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

(Eds.), Children's peer relations: Issues <strong>in</strong> assessment <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terventionSullivan,<br />

H. S. (1953). The <strong>in</strong>terpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York:<br />

(pp. 41-54). New York: Spr<strong>in</strong>ger-Verlag.<br />

Norton.<br />

Gottman, J. M. (1983). How children become friends. Monographs of Thompson, R. A. (Ed.). (1990). Socioemotionaldevelopment:Nebraska<br />

the Society for Research <strong>in</strong> Child Development, 48{i, Serial No. 201). symposium on motivation, 1988 (Vol. 36). L<strong>in</strong>coln: University of Ne-<br />

Gottman, J. M., Gonso, J., & Rasmussen, B. (1975). Social <strong>in</strong>teraction, braska Press.<br />

social competence, <strong>and</strong> friendship <strong>in</strong> children. Child Development, Vespo, J. E., & Caplan, M. Z. (1988, March). Preschoolers' differential<br />

46,709-718.<br />

conflict behaviors with friends <strong>and</strong> acqua<strong>in</strong>tances. Poster presented at<br />

Gottman, J. M., & Parkhurst, J. (1980). A developmental theory of the Southeast Conference on Human Development, Charleston, SC.<br />

friendship <strong>and</strong> acqua<strong>in</strong>tanceship processes. In W A. Coll<strong>in</strong>s (Eds.), Williams, G. A., & Asher, S. R. (1987, April). Peer <strong>and</strong> self perceptions of<br />

M<strong>in</strong>nesota symposia on child development: Development of cognition, peer rejected children: Issues <strong>in</strong> classification <strong>and</strong> subgroup<strong>in</strong>g. Paper<br />

affect, <strong>and</strong> social relations (Vol. 13, pp. 197-253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- presented at the biennial meet<strong>in</strong>gs of the Society for Research <strong>in</strong><br />

baum.<br />

Child Development, Baltimore.<br />

Hartup, W W (1986). On relationships <strong>and</strong> development. In W W<br />

Hartup & Z. Rub<strong>in</strong> (Eds.), Relationships <strong>and</strong> development (pp. 1 -26).<br />

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Weiss, R. S. (1973). Lonel<strong>in</strong>ess: The experience of emotional <strong>and</strong> social<br />

isolation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<br />

Hartup, W W, & Laursen, B. (1989, April). Contextual constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>and</strong><br />

children's friendship relations. Paper presented at the biennial meet<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

of the Society for Research <strong>in</strong> Child Development, Kansas City,<br />

MO.<br />

Received May 8,1991<br />

Revision received June 10,1992<br />

Accepted August 19,1992 •

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!