04.04.2013 Views

Sex and Sensibility: Margaret Mead's Descriptive ... - Trent University

Sex and Sensibility: Margaret Mead's Descriptive ... - Trent University

Sex and Sensibility: Margaret Mead's Descriptive ... - Trent University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Reviews in Anthropology, Vol. 33, pp. 111–130<br />

Copyright # 2004 Taylor & Francis Inc.<br />

ISSN: 0093-8157 print<br />

DOI: 10.1080=00938150490447439<br />

<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong>: <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead’s<br />

<strong>Descriptive</strong> <strong>and</strong> Rhetorical Ethnography<br />

Roger Ivar Lohmann<br />

Mead, <strong>Margaret</strong>. The Mountain Arapesh, with a new introduction by<br />

Paul B. Roscoe. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers, 2002 [1938,<br />

1940]. Volume I: xxxi þ 339 pp., <strong>and</strong> Volume II: xiii þ 358 pp.<br />

$69.95, paper.<br />

Mead, <strong>Margaret</strong>. <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> Temperament in Three Primitive Societies, with<br />

new introductions by Helen Fisher <strong>and</strong> Mary Catherine Bateson. New York:<br />

Perennial, 2001 [1935]. xxiv þ 311 pp. including index. $15.00, paper.<br />

Mead, <strong>Margaret</strong>. Male <strong>and</strong> Female, with new introductions by Helen Fisher<br />

<strong>and</strong> Mary Catherine Bateson. New York: Perennial, 2001 [1949]. xi þ<br />

448 pp. including index. $15.00, paper.<br />

As always one can find that <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead had done it.<br />

Theodore Schwartz (1981, p. 10)<br />

If Franz Boas is the father of American anthropology, it is tempting to designate<br />

<strong>Margaret</strong> Mead as the mother. While a generational equivalence<br />

between the two is belied by the fact that Mead was Boas’s student<br />

<strong>and</strong> therefore his cultural <strong>and</strong> intellectual ‘‘child,’’ to us, their scholarly<br />

descendants, they st<strong>and</strong> as godlike parents in all their brilliance <strong>and</strong><br />

ROGER IVAR LOHMANN is Assistant Professor of Anthropology at <strong>Trent</strong> <strong>University</strong>, Oshawa,<br />

Ontario, Canada. A Melanesianist, his interests include cultural change, religion, <strong>and</strong> dreaming.<br />

He is the editor of Dream Travelers: Sleep Experiences <strong>and</strong> Culture in the Western Pacific<br />

(Palgrave Macmillan, 2003) <strong>and</strong> ‘‘Perspectives on the Category ‘Supernatural,’’’ a special issue<br />

of Anthropological Forum, 13(2), 2003.<br />

I offer my sincere thanks to Lise Dobrin, Lourdes Giordani, Gerald Sullivan, <strong>and</strong> the anonymous<br />

reviewers for reading drafts of this article <strong>and</strong> offering helpful suggestions.<br />

Address correspondence to Roger I. Lohmann, Department of Anthropology, <strong>Trent</strong><br />

<strong>University</strong>, 2000 Simcoe Street North, Oshawa, Ontario L1H 7L7, Canada. E-mail: rogerlohmann@<br />

trentu.ca<br />

111


112 R. I. Lohmann<br />

imperfection, to be both embraced <strong>and</strong> rejected. The vehemence of Mead’s<br />

defense by a wide swath of American cultural anthropologists following<br />

Derek Freeman’s (1983) critique of her Samoan work (e.g., Ember 1985),<br />

liberal values, <strong>and</strong> the powerful role she attributed to culture in<br />

determining human action <strong>and</strong> potentials, points to a deep but ambivalent<br />

affection <strong>and</strong> identification with her that is evocative of children’s relationships<br />

with their mother. Moreover, the relative lack of direct engagement<br />

with her work or citations of her (<strong>and</strong> Boas’s) writings in current anthropology<br />

could be interpreted as revealing adult children’s implicit <strong>and</strong><br />

ashamed movement from under their parents’ thumb. Adding to this<br />

Mead’s emphasis on exploring gender <strong>and</strong> sexuality in both her personal<br />

<strong>and</strong> scholarly life, Freud could have had a field day analyzing the image of<br />

<strong>Margaret</strong> Mead in the anthropological subconscious. In returning to a<br />

respected ancestor’s legacy for us, one must, therefore, be vigilant lest<br />

either identity with or rebellious wish for independence from the predecessor<br />

impede balanced openness in critical reappraisal. With so much<br />

at stake, rereading Mead for what she has to contribute to current anthropology<br />

is a rewarding challenge.<br />

The centennial of <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead’s birth in 2001 provided the impetus<br />

to reissue many of her books. I give four volumes a fresh look as<br />

statements on her methods, theories, <strong>and</strong> rhetorical depictions of culture,<br />

<strong>and</strong> particularly gender. The Mountain Arapesh is a two-volume, detailed<br />

ethnography <strong>and</strong> analysis of a single group in what is now Papua New<br />

Guinea. It was written for anthropologists, with the goal of providing as<br />

full a description as possible from several perspectives. <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> Temperament<br />

<strong>and</strong> Male <strong>and</strong> Female represent Mead’s fullest statements on gender,<br />

written for mixed audiences. In them, Mead presents her vision of how<br />

various cultures make sense of sex <strong>and</strong> how Americans should act sensibly<br />

with regard to it. These books show the range of Mead’s career as scholar,<br />

popular writer, social commentator, <strong>and</strong> advisor, who held the ears of two<br />

generations while producing rich ethnography <strong>and</strong> theory for anthropology.<br />

I critique elements of Mead’s work, including her rhetorical tendency<br />

to use idealized individuals <strong>and</strong> situations to illustrate the cultural configurations<br />

that Mead abstracted, making these ‘‘patterns of culture’’ (Benedict,<br />

1953 [1934]) appear to be data rather than interpretations of<br />

data. This practice contributes to Mead’s well-known penchant for excessive<br />

generalization. From the perspective of early twenty first century<br />

anthropology, a striking characteristic of all of the books reviewed here<br />

is their paucity of citations <strong>and</strong> references. In spite of these shortcomings,<br />

I praise Mead’s exhaustive description in words <strong>and</strong> images of local ideas<br />

<strong>and</strong> artifacts—a practice that has suffered in more recent ethnography—


<strong>and</strong> her innovative field <strong>and</strong> writing methods, some of which anticipated<br />

later developments in anthropology, <strong>and</strong> some of which have been<br />

largely forgotten, to the poverty of anthropology. I discuss each work<br />

in turn.<br />

THE MOUNTAIN ARAPESH<br />

<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong> 113<br />

Several lengthy papers originally published by the American Museum of<br />

Natural History appear in this two-volume presentation, with a new introduction<br />

by Paul Roscoe. They are the result of Mead’s fieldwork with Reo<br />

Fortune in 1931–1932 among the Mountain Arapesh of New Guinea. From<br />

the perspective of 2004, both are marvels to behold in that they present<br />

so much ethnographic detail <strong>and</strong> illustrations of material as well as ideational<br />

culture. Reading them is evocative of fieldwork—Mead lays out the<br />

complexity <strong>and</strong> detail of a people’s life, conveying the immensity of even<br />

a small society, <strong>and</strong> the impossibility of recording ‘‘everything’’ in an<br />

ethnography.<br />

Paul (‘‘Jim’’) Roscoe’s valuable introduction fills in some details of political<br />

structure, which Mead does not emphasize <strong>and</strong> tells what has happened<br />

to the people of Alitoa village since Mead <strong>and</strong> Fortune’s departure, including<br />

the Japanese occupation <strong>and</strong> Allied bombardment during World War II,<br />

followed by dispersal to other areas. When Roscoe visited in 1997, some<br />

people had begun reoccupying the site. This new edition’s usefulness could<br />

have been greatly enhanced had the publisher allowed the addition of an<br />

index, making Mead’s rich descriptive materials, buried in a text with a<br />

sometimes-unclear stratigraphy, accessible for comparative <strong>and</strong> theoretical<br />

work.<br />

Volume I includes two parts, the first entitled ‘‘An Importing Culture’’<br />

<strong>and</strong> the second ‘‘Supernaturalism.’’ In the first, Mead documents the movement<br />

of ideas <strong>and</strong> materials among the Mountain Arapesh <strong>and</strong> their neighbors.<br />

Mead describes the Mountain Arapesh as attracted to importation<br />

rather than invention out of insecurity in their creative abilities. Techniques<br />

<strong>and</strong> rights for performance, as well as material objects, are imported.<br />

Foreign technologies, dances, <strong>and</strong> spells have greater value when they<br />

are exotic imports <strong>and</strong> less as they become familiar (p. 22).<br />

Mead’s sophisticated attention to indigenous diffusion is valuable for<br />

those who study religious conversion, language shift, <strong>and</strong> other forms of<br />

cultural contact <strong>and</strong> change, since it shows the fundamental similarity between<br />

the importation of material objects <strong>and</strong> ideas, whose ‘‘reception or<br />

rejection may depend upon structural, functional, eth[n]ological, or ideological<br />

similarity or non-similarity’’ (p. 21). Moreover, it serves as a reminder


114 R. I. Lohmann<br />

that competing hegemonies <strong>and</strong> counter-hegemonies complicated human<br />

life long before the world system extended its discursive tentacles to all corners<br />

of the postcolonial world. Perhaps still recovering from the excesses of<br />

extreme diffusionists (summarized in Barnard, 2000, pp. 49–54), whose proponents<br />

emphasized the uninventiveness of people <strong>and</strong> attributed even<br />

very widespread ideas to diffusion, cultural anthropology still regards the<br />

term ‘‘diffusion’’ with an unreasonable distaste. It remains relevant, however,<br />

to current interest in various peoples’ attraction for <strong>and</strong> pursuit of<br />

styles <strong>and</strong> technologies that they associate with an abstracted notion of<br />

‘‘modernity,’’ an idea that has become cultural anthropology’s current darling.<br />

Older work on diffusion, including Mead’s, deserves renewed critical<br />

but open-minded attention to put our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of modernity into a<br />

broader context of the transmission <strong>and</strong> reception of culture, which have<br />

been going on since before the genus Homo evolved.<br />

Like much of Mead’s writing, ‘‘An Importing Culture’’ is written in a<br />

rather flow-of-consciousness style, with too few headings. However, it<br />

exemplifies the best detail from the Boasian tradition, covering cooking,<br />

drugs, sleeping habits, graphical communication systems, recipes, house<br />

building, <strong>and</strong> types of art. Anthropologists interested in material culture will<br />

marvel at this book’s detail on how things are made <strong>and</strong> used, with plentiful<br />

drawings <strong>and</strong> photographs—something that experimental archaeologists<br />

<strong>and</strong> ethnoarchaeologists do well but that has unfortunately become rare<br />

in ethnographies (for an excellent descriptive ethnography focused<br />

on New Guinea net bags, including how to make them, see Mackenzie,<br />

1991).<br />

While Mead emphasizes cultures as configurations or patterns, she<br />

recognizes that ideas can move between cultures <strong>and</strong> change configurations.<br />

An applied anthropologist seeking to change American culture, she<br />

clearly did not consider cultures to be discrete <strong>and</strong> timeless as past anthropologists<br />

have sometimes been accused of doing. Nor did Mead consider<br />

‘‘primitive’’ Arapesh culture to be bounded <strong>and</strong> unchanging, but rather<br />

she saw it as continually borrowing from others (pp. 9–11). Maria Lepowsky<br />

(2000, p. 135) makes a similar observation regarding the Sicilian peasant<br />

ethnography of Mead’s forgotten contemporary, Charlotte Gower. As<br />

Herbert Lewis (1998) argues, such stereotypes of early anthropology must<br />

be recognized as inaccurate <strong>and</strong> no longer perpetuated.<br />

In the second portion of Volume One, titled ‘‘Supernaturalism,’’ Mead<br />

discusses the more purely ideational elements of culture, presented as long<br />

lists of myths <strong>and</strong> food taboos. ‘‘The fundamental premises of Arapesh culture,’’<br />

she holds, ‘‘are organized on an affective, rather than upon a cognitive<br />

basis’’ (p. 227). Therefore, Mead interprets Arapesh mythology as expressing<br />

emotional conundrums in the culture, such as mutual fear of the sexes,


<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong> 115<br />

rather than as explaining phenomena (p. 234). While it can be argued that<br />

E. B. Tylor (1877 [1871]) overemphasized the rationality of religion in his effort<br />

to show that even primitives are capable of intellectual thought, Mead<br />

may err on the other side in blanketing the whole of Mountain Arapesh religiosity<br />

as cognitively unengaged. Mead provides several myth texts that<br />

she collected from a single informant <strong>and</strong> describes the informant’s personality,<br />

but provides no analysis of the myths (p. 247). They are nevertheless<br />

extremely valuable, particularly when combined with the large collection of<br />

texts published by Fortune (1942) in his Arapesh grammar. Mead’s versions<br />

of the cassowary wife tale (p. 264) are particularly interesting, given Donald<br />

Tuzin’s (1997) vivid analysis of how this myth subsequently changed following<br />

Christianization, reflecting threats to Ilahita Arapesh masculinity (see<br />

also Stephen Leavitt’s (1998) discussion of adolescent masculinity among<br />

Bumbita Arapesh).<br />

Volume Two also provides a mass of data, but its sections are a<br />

series of ethnographic experiments. In many ways Mead here anticipates<br />

debates that occurred in the anthropology of the 1980 s <strong>and</strong> 1990 s on experimental<br />

ethnography, representing others, <strong>and</strong> reflexivity (e.g., Clifford &<br />

Marcus, 1986; see Lutkehaus, 1995). As Mead says in the first paragraph,<br />

These papers represent an experiment in method of presentation, an attempt to solve a number<br />

of presentation problems: how to present the material so that students wishing to use it at different<br />

levels of abstraction <strong>and</strong> generalization may do so without impediment; how to satisfy<br />

this first dem<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> yet keep the illustrative detail close enough to the discussion so that its<br />

particular relevance to any point is not lost; what orders of materials should be presented<br />

together; what type of presentation best represents the peculiar form <strong>and</strong> emphases of Arapesh<br />

culture. (p. 5)<br />

This sentence (just one in a very lengthy paragraph) also gives a sense<br />

of what it is like to be curled up with these two volumes. While I admire<br />

Mead’s detail <strong>and</strong> thoughtfulness, it unfortunately comes across as very<br />

long-winded <strong>and</strong> tends, like rice noodles forgotten on the stove, to become<br />

a glutinous mass in one’s mind. This is unfortunate, for there is much of use<br />

here if one has the patience to separate the noodles.<br />

Volume Two has three sections: one on Arapesh socioeconomic life, a<br />

diary of events in Alitoa village, <strong>and</strong> a focus on a key informant. In the<br />

first section of Volume Two, Mead deftly shows the emotional <strong>and</strong> cognitive<br />

peculiarities of Arapesh economic relationships, including people’s<br />

anxiety over balancing obligations to others with the need to complete<br />

their own tasks (p. 47). People see themselves as temporary stewards of<br />

possessions, to be passed on to succeeding generations, giving a definite<br />

sense of descent group possession rather than individual ownership per se<br />

(p. 51).


116 R. I. Lohmann<br />

Anthropologists who associate the emergence of political complexity<br />

with the control of a surplus will be interested in the fact that while the<br />

Arapesh were malnourished, <strong>and</strong> from that perspective had no surplus at<br />

all, they nevertheless perceived that they did have a surplus. They scraped<br />

together food to host feasts that gave prestige in spite of the fact that<br />

everyone went home hungry (pp. 55–56). This shows that the surpluses<br />

redistributed by big men can be imagined as well as real (cf. Bloch, 1989<br />

[1979], who shows how exchange of real goods for imaginary blessings<br />

supported leaders in Madagascar, even though those goods derived from<br />

the people rather than the leaders).<br />

The second section in Volume Two comprises the diary’s introduction,<br />

which is much more readable than the rest of the volume. Here Mead clarifies<br />

her own <strong>and</strong> Fortune’s relationship with the people. Following this,<br />

Mead usefully provides a cast of characters, which unfortunately is dense<br />

<strong>and</strong> unevenly detailed. In this section, Mead’s awareness of personality variation<br />

is crystal clear, yet she nevertheless abstracts an ideal personality,<br />

exemplifying the primary contradiction in the old culture-<strong>and</strong>-personality<br />

school (p. 75). The value of the diary is that it gives a glimpse into the daily<br />

flow of events in the village, as noticed by the ethnographer. It has a raw<br />

data feel to it, giving extraordinary detail, but by being labeled only according<br />

to major events <strong>and</strong> dates one cannot quickly access other points of<br />

interest in relation to times of occurrence. Mead concludes the diary with<br />

an entire census of people, hamlets, spirit-places, owners of coconut palms,<br />

gens (clan) ownership of omens <strong>and</strong> individual ownership of charms, a list<br />

<strong>and</strong> description of all marriages, <strong>and</strong> various other data lists.<br />

The final section of the second volume, which consists of everything<br />

from her fieldnotes on one informant, might be considered a prototype of<br />

person-centered ethnography. She seeks to document how a culture is seen<br />

from the perspective of one of its participants, a man named Unabelin. A<br />

second goal is to document the value of furthering interaction between psychologists<br />

<strong>and</strong> anthropologists by presenting the full data of a Rorschach<br />

(inkblot) projective test administered to this informant along with the interpretations<br />

of various scholars. Unabelin identifies the inkblots as being like<br />

dreams, ghosts, rotten things, <strong>and</strong> what one sees when one is insane<br />

(p. 343). In other words, he sees the blots for what they are—unclear<br />

images. By contrast, the comments on Unabelin’s test results by several psychologists<br />

seemed to me impressionistic <strong>and</strong> idiosyncratic, but Mead considers<br />

them helpful.<br />

In order to avoid anthropologists’ biases, Mead suggests having ‘‘routine<br />

field-workers’’ collect <strong>and</strong> publish ‘‘unanalyzed materials to use as controls’’<br />

upon which more highly trained fieldworkers can build (p. 262). ‘‘Let<br />

... the subsequent investigator have full access to this material, uncolored


<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong> 117<br />

as it is by any speculative hypotheses, <strong>and</strong> then let him go in <strong>and</strong> make a<br />

systematic attempt to underst<strong>and</strong> the culture’’ (p. 262). Yet Mead recognizes<br />

that ‘‘There is no such thing as an unbiased report’’ (p. 265), so this<br />

suggestion appears unrealistic even in her own account of it. She<br />

declares, before this became anthropological common sense, that the<br />

best way to reduce bias is to make it explicit through reflexivity, allowing<br />

readers to correct for tendencies on the part of the researcher. ‘‘This<br />

type of self-conscious allowance for bias may be, although I almost<br />

doubt it, somewhat better than a complete lack of self-consciousness<br />

about one’s own temperament <strong>and</strong> character. It may, on the other h<strong>and</strong>,<br />

only serve to fill the pages of the account with a series of ‘buts’ <strong>and</strong> ‘ifs’’’<br />

(p. 266).<br />

Far less problematic, she asserts, is finding someone in the culture<br />

whose temperament is like one’s own to interview intensively, seeing the<br />

culture through his or her eyes in a maximally congruent way (p. 266).<br />

She chooses Unabelin, whom she admires <strong>and</strong> considers, like herself, to<br />

be an intellectual who values intensity. Generalizing from this, she remarks<br />

that her depiction of the Arapesh as a group is more faithful than her depiction<br />

of the Munduggumor, because she can relate to <strong>and</strong> admire the former<br />

more than the latter (p. 269).<br />

Acknowledging the role of personal congruence between ethnographer<br />

<strong>and</strong> informants in allowing greater empathetic communication is a useful<br />

methodological insight. However, both sympathy <strong>and</strong> dislike of the<br />

anthropologist for a particular informant—or for an entire culture—also<br />

introduce biases that hinder underst<strong>and</strong>ing, as I discuss below. Liking<br />

allows one to empathize, which is a primary means of relating to others,<br />

but it can also harm one’s ability to see from other perspectives as much<br />

as disliking can.<br />

As Ira Bashkow <strong>and</strong> Lise Dobrin (forthcoming) note, however,<br />

Mead’s field correspondence reveals that she was in fact ambivalent about<br />

the Mountain Arapesh because of their ostensible lack of lavish display.<br />

Moreover, they argue that Mead’s generalizations about the Mountain<br />

Arapesh <strong>and</strong> the peoples that she subsequently studied in developing her<br />

thesis for <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> Temperament reflect the flip-flopping gender <strong>and</strong> power<br />

relations Mead was experiencing in her developing love triangle with<br />

Fortune <strong>and</strong> her future husb<strong>and</strong>, Gregory Bateson. Indeed, psychohistorical<br />

studies of anthropologists like Bashkow <strong>and</strong> Dobrin’s affirm Mead’s point<br />

that anthropologists’ affections influence <strong>and</strong> even provide a kind of<br />

undocumented field data for their intellectual quests.<br />

The remainder of the final section is the full text of her interviews with<br />

Unabelin <strong>and</strong> recounted events from the diary that refer to him. She reiterates<br />

some materials in different contexts as a way of experimenting with


118 R. I. Lohmann<br />

ways of presenting (cf. Wolf, 1992, who presents accounts of a single event<br />

from three different perspectives, altering the event’s impression).<br />

The Mountain Arapesh consists of many flowing observations <strong>and</strong> lacks<br />

a conclusion. One does, however, get a sense of how messy lived reality is.<br />

Mead admits, ‘‘I realize that it is tedious <strong>and</strong> difficult to follow such a mass of<br />

remote detail...but so unformalized, so unsystematic, is Arapesh behavior<br />

that in any attempt to state the rules which govern their economic behavior<br />

one is immediately driven to concrete cases’’ (vol. II, p. 48). I think Mead lets<br />

herself off too easily here. Perhaps it was her very desire to find an abstract<br />

ideal system, what she meant by ‘‘culture,’’ thwarted by her own intense<br />

engagement with her field notes, that led her to throw up her h<strong>and</strong>s in this<br />

way. A second weakness is that when making cross-cultural comparisons<br />

she only does so with societies that she herself has studied. There is remarkably<br />

little direct engagement with other ethnographic literature (e.g., see<br />

vol. II, p. 43). Even where she refers directly to points in the literature,<br />

she rarely cites these, as when she mentions a controversy about primitive<br />

economics without saying what it is or who its proponents are (vol. II, p. 57).<br />

Finally, while Mead’s tendency to exaggerate is found less in The Mountain<br />

Arapesh than in her popular works, even here we read ‘‘Theft was<br />

unknown,’’ with the footnote to this very sentence mentioning that there<br />

were thefts, though she sees these as ‘‘secondary to the expression of some<br />

form of emotion’’ (vol. II, p. 50). Indeed, Fortune (1939) shows that theft of<br />

others’ wives was a major cause of interlocality warfare. But then, Mead also<br />

virtually denies that the Mountain Arapesh had war.<br />

SEX AND TEMPERAMENT IN THREE PRIMITIVE SOCIETIES<br />

In this famous work, originally published in 1935, Mead compares ideals of<br />

masculinity <strong>and</strong> femininity among three groups she <strong>and</strong> Fortune studied in<br />

New Guinea, <strong>and</strong> she finds that they vary to the point of opposition.<br />

Arapesh ideals hold that both women <strong>and</strong> men should be nurturant, nonviolent,<br />

<strong>and</strong> caring; Mundugumor ideals hold that both women <strong>and</strong> men<br />

should be independent, selfish, <strong>and</strong> aggressive; <strong>and</strong> Tchambuli ideals hold<br />

that men should be artistic <strong>and</strong> vain while women should be pragmatic <strong>and</strong><br />

plain. Mead spends 146 pages of the book describing the Arapesh, 66 pages<br />

of it on the Mundugumor, <strong>and</strong> 37 pages of it on the Tchambuli, with the<br />

remainder devoted to a theoretical discussion. This imbalance does not<br />

serve to demonstrate the thesis of the book.<br />

Mead provides ethnographic evidence that cultural traditions may or<br />

may not link particular elements of temperament to sex, <strong>and</strong> when they<br />

do, it is arbitrary which are considered masculine or feminine. Mead’s


<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong> 119<br />

general thesis st<strong>and</strong>s, as subsequent work documenting the malleability of<br />

gender roles <strong>and</strong> ideals shows (e.g., Lepowsky, 1993, who describes Vanatinai<br />

society, in which men <strong>and</strong> women are engaged in many of the same<br />

tasks <strong>and</strong> are virtually egalitarian). It is now anthropological common<br />

sense that personality traits may or may not be linked to gender, <strong>and</strong><br />

when a trait is linked with gender, there are consequences for the psychological<br />

adaptation of individuals depending on whether they match the<br />

ideal.<br />

However, the configurations with which Mead sometimes stereotypes<br />

these particular groups are open to greater dispute. As noted above, for<br />

example, in her effort to emphasize the generalization that Mountain<br />

Arapesh men <strong>and</strong> women are gentle <strong>and</strong> maternal, she describes their<br />

society as unwarlike. Reo Fortune (1939), following his divorce from Mead,<br />

responded in print with evidence to the contrary. Ethnographers remain<br />

impressed with this evidence, <strong>and</strong> explain the difference between Mead’s<br />

<strong>and</strong> Fortune’s interpretations in terms of different ethnographic values <strong>and</strong><br />

experiences (Dobrin & Bashkow, forthcoming), <strong>and</strong> that configurationist<br />

theory shaped Mead’s generalizations even when she provided contradicting<br />

data (Roscoe, 2003). Sullivan (2004) points out, however, that Mead’s<br />

configurationism differs from Benedict’s in that Mead sought causal mechanisms<br />

for personality development, such as childrearing practices <strong>and</strong> inherited<br />

temperaments. While Mead is widely known to have generalized to a fault,<br />

that same boldness is what allowed her to pursue <strong>and</strong> tackle big questions.<br />

Mead’s question in this book is a vast <strong>and</strong> tangled one: what is the relationship<br />

among physical sex, cultural surroundings, <strong>and</strong> personality? This<br />

question was an obvious next step following her first work, Coming of Age<br />

in Samoa, on the relationship between physical adolescence <strong>and</strong> emotional<br />

tenor (Mead, 2001 [1928]). Mead was grappling with the problem of gender,<br />

though without benefit of the term.<br />

Mead begins by observing that ‘‘man has taken a few hints <strong>and</strong> woven<br />

them into the beautiful imaginative social fabrics that we call civilisations’’ (p.<br />

xxxi). For Mead, then, culture is like connective threads woven into cloth by<br />

the imagination. Temperament, in her usage, is more or less innate personality<br />

that culture can only caress, not chisel. In discussing the psychological<br />

consequences for individuals who are born with characteristics considered<br />

typical of the opposite sex, Mead brilliantly illustrates Ruth Benedict’s thesis<br />

(1953 [1934]) that the same personality will excel in one cultural environment<br />

but be a misfit in another. To the eyes of an anthropologist reading in 2004,<br />

however, Mead’s willingness to occasionally characterize people she knew<br />

using such globally condemnatory language as ‘‘stupid’’ <strong>and</strong> ‘‘dull-witted’’<br />

is remarkable: ‘‘Now Menala was stupid—easy-going, good-natured, but<br />

stupid’’ (pp. 118–119). This reflects both a greater confidence in her ability


120 R. I. Lohmann<br />

to objectively assess other people’s intelligence than current cultural anthropologists<br />

possess, <strong>and</strong> a greater willingness to honestly express in print<br />

negative feelings about her subjects than we are now at liberty to exercise.<br />

Her extremely confident attitude seems to betray an egocentrism, ironically<br />

in the midst of her compassionate relating to others, that also appears in the<br />

way she interacted with her friends <strong>and</strong> colleagues. It is tempting to regard<br />

the very few citations in the text as yet another symptom of this attitude;<br />

however, Mead was writing in a time—a cultural situation—in which she<br />

was free to present her ideas without the laborious effort of tying them into<br />

the work of others, as anthropologists are expected to do today.<br />

A feature of Mead’s ethnographic writing, already present in Coming of<br />

Age in Samoa in the form of her description of a ‘‘typical day,’’ is her use of<br />

evocative, idealized scenarios. In these she describes what she has taken to<br />

be routine as though it were a single event, but in idealized form, rather than<br />

in the form of an example of a particular occurrence that she observed. For<br />

example, she writes ‘‘A party of visitors from another locality first asks for<br />

fire, which their hosts immediately give them; then a low-voiced excited<br />

conversation begins’’ (p. 6). This tendency is also reflected in her descriptions<br />

of idealized people in certain positions or situations in their societies.<br />

This suite of ethnographic writing methods has value in turning generalized,<br />

passively constructed descriptions of the sociocultural system into stories<br />

about individuals, which, one must confess, are much easier to follow with<br />

sustained interest. However, one must doubt just how typical these situations<br />

are. Moreover, when Mead describes the feelings of her hypothetical<br />

individuals, psychological anthropologists must wonder how she thinks<br />

she knows what people feel, <strong>and</strong> what field experiences with real events<br />

<strong>and</strong> people led her to reach these generalizations (e.g., p. 232). The Mountain<br />

Arapesh, of course, provides some answers to these questions.<br />

A related device is using exceptional cases to prove the rule. For<br />

example, in support of Mead’s depiction of Arapesh culture as valuing<br />

cooperation <strong>and</strong> sharing of resources, she mentions the case of an individual<br />

who in a séance determined that the sorcerer was motivated by desire to<br />

prevent his victim’s children from sharing l<strong>and</strong> (p. 17). Ironically, it is these<br />

‘‘deviants’’ that seem to interest Mead the most as individuals. Whereas<br />

traditional norms <strong>and</strong> successful psychological adaptations are illustrated<br />

with hypothetical people <strong>and</strong> situations, conflicts that arise between certain<br />

personalities <strong>and</strong> the sociocultural order in which they find themselves are<br />

described with loving care <strong>and</strong> actual data. Certainly this tendency to make<br />

sweeping statements is behind much of the criticism Mead has endured.<br />

Nevertheless, Mead is cognizant of some dangers of generalizing conventions;<br />

for example, she notes the potential misunderst<strong>and</strong>ings that might<br />

arise from her use of the ethnographic present tense (pp. 5, 157).


<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong> 121<br />

As Martin Orans (1999) observes, Freeman exaggerated the cultural<br />

determinism of both Mead <strong>and</strong> Franz Boas in his attacks on Mead’s Samoan<br />

work. However, among Freeman’s (1983, p. 255) legitimate critiques is his<br />

point that while Mead claims that Samoan adolescence was free of stress,<br />

she contradicts her own point by citing many examples of girls who did<br />

not adjust well, but hides this by rhetorically depicting them as atypical<br />

<strong>and</strong> relegating them to a separate section of the ethnography. This same<br />

tendency can be seen in defining certain people as deviants in <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> Temperament—it<br />

is as if she is bringing ‘‘normal’’ Arapesh, Mudugumor, <strong>and</strong><br />

Tchambuli values into existence by contrasting them with people who do<br />

not fit her ideal characterizations of ‘‘the culture.’’<br />

In Mead’s writing, culture appears to be an external force that people<br />

react to; yet the culture is internally borne by the people themselves.<br />

Where is the locus of culture for Mead? The pattern ultimately derives<br />

from individuals. ‘‘This spectrum [of culture] is the range of individual<br />

differences which lie back of the so much more conspicuous cultural<br />

emphases, <strong>and</strong> it is to this that we must turn to find the explanation of<br />

cultural inspiration’’ (p. 265). Yet the relationship between individual<br />

<strong>and</strong> culture remains unclear, not only in this work but also in much of<br />

current anthropology (for a useful approach to this issue, see Strauss &<br />

Quinn, 1998).<br />

In Nancy McDowell’s (1991) ethnography of the Mundugumor—whose<br />

descendants call themselves the Biwat—based on Mead’s <strong>and</strong> Fortune’s<br />

fieldnotes, she expresses a similar impression of Mead’s notion of culture:<br />

‘‘She seemed to operate with a concept of culture as something that existed<br />

outside individuals, separate <strong>and</strong> apart from them. In doing this, she saw<br />

culture as a concrete body of abstractions that existed in one relatively simple<br />

<strong>and</strong> coherent form, something that individuals learned more or less of,<br />

that they learned well or not so well’’ (McDowell, 1991, p. 17).<br />

Mead relied heavily on one or a few informants to obtain a description<br />

of the culture but did not see this as a worry. As McDowell (1991, p. 18) puts<br />

it,<br />

Working with one ‘‘good’’ informant posed no significant problem for Mead because if that<br />

informant knew the culture well, there was no problem of sampling or perspective. ...The<br />

more sophisticated notion of a distributive model of culture [see Keesing 1981, pp. 71–72]<br />

did not exist at this time, <strong>and</strong> Mead never explicitly recognized the theoretical <strong>and</strong> methodological<br />

problems resulting when individuals <strong>and</strong> segments of a population differ.<br />

Nevertheless, McDowell also observes that Mead noted differences<br />

between men <strong>and</strong> women in the cultures she studied <strong>and</strong> was an astounding<br />

fieldworker whose accomplishments far outstripped the theoretical<br />

limitations of her time.


122 R. I. Lohmann<br />

Mead refers to Mundugumor culture as ‘‘malfunctioning’’ (p. 167). By<br />

this she means that the system produces frequent conflicts (p. 168). The<br />

impracticality of their cultural rules, such as the one against intergenerational<br />

marriage, means that people have to break them constantly (p.<br />

170). Because Mundugumor ideals are unreachable, people always feel they<br />

are doing wrong. Yet the Arapesh, with their equally unrealistic ideals of<br />

niceness, must also feel guilt when their anger boils over; however, Mead<br />

does not treat this as a systematic weakness of the culture (pp. 172–173).<br />

This is one symptom of Turnbull’s (1972, 1987 [1962]) ‘‘mountain people<br />

versus forest people’’ syndrome—depicting a culture of which one disapproves<br />

in a disparaging tone, while cooingly describing the one that one<br />

admires. To give another example, Mead describes a husb<strong>and</strong>’s many<br />

taboos during his wife’s pregnancy benignly for the Arapesh, but as disturbing<br />

for the Mundugumor (pp. 178–179). This appears again in her discussion<br />

of Tchambuli: both Mundugumor <strong>and</strong> Tchambuli held that each boy<br />

should have the opportunity to kill a captive. Yet in describing this same custom,<br />

Mead uses decidedly calm terms in describing the Tchambuli practice,<br />

<strong>and</strong> menacing, disapproving terms when describing the Mundugumor one<br />

(p. 226). This is in part due to her wish to depict the different ethos surrounding<br />

the same act in each society, <strong>and</strong> of course to fulfill her rhetorical<br />

purpose. Ethnographers convey many subtle messages by our choice of<br />

terms, a fact of which both authors <strong>and</strong> readers are wise to remain cognizant<br />

(Lohmann, 2003a).<br />

How accurate are these shorth<strong>and</strong> characterizations of cultures? In<br />

Deborah Gewertz’s (1983, p. 11) study of the Tchambuli, now spelled<br />

Chambri, she found that the female dominance Mead reported was not<br />

some essential quality of Chambri culture, but was rather ‘‘the temporary result<br />

...of certain specific historical circumstances.’’ All anthropologists must<br />

walk the line between describing the functioning of systems, abstracted as<br />

models of social structure, <strong>and</strong> the historical particulars of individuals<br />

through time. Mead faced these problems sometimes squarely, sometimes<br />

obliquely.<br />

‘‘To the extent that a culture is integrated <strong>and</strong>...uncompromising in<br />

its moral <strong>and</strong> spiritual preferences,’’ Mead writes, ‘‘it condemns some of<br />

its members ...to live alien to it’’ (p. 272). Living in a fantasy world<br />

can, she writes, help deviants escape enough from their cultural environments<br />

to live effectively within them (p. 274). However, when one’s personality<br />

is not only unsuited to one’s culture, but is also linked to the<br />

opposite sex, this adds another blow at healthy identity (p. 274). Mead<br />

does not suggest that an <strong>and</strong>rogynous society is any better than a gender-differentiated<br />

one; in fact, the former inhibits the building of a rich<br />

<strong>and</strong> colorful culture. Rather, she calls for what would now be called a


multicultural society, in which movement between available subcultures<br />

makes it possible for more people to find a satisfying home (p. 296;<br />

see Banner, 2003, p. 359).<br />

Mead held E. B. Tylor’s view that anthropology is a reformer’s science.<br />

In this regard her work is refreshing for early twenty first century anthropology,<br />

as it recovers from the negativity of postmodernism, because it is<br />

not only critical, but also positive, hopeful, <strong>and</strong> thought-provokingly<br />

practical. These are traits too often missing from some recent critical work,<br />

particularly in subaltern studies, as Alan Beals (2002, p. 214) observes.<br />

MALE AND FEMALE<br />

<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong> 123<br />

In Male <strong>and</strong> Female, originally published in 1949, Mead exp<strong>and</strong>s on the<br />

conclusions she reached about gender, though she still does not use this<br />

term. It would acquire its present meaning—the cultural elaboration of biological<br />

sex—<strong>and</strong> enter anthropology from sexology later (see Banner, 2003,<br />

pp. 351, 512 n9). She draws on her Pacific fieldwork <strong>and</strong> her knowledge of<br />

American culture as a participant. Mead is speaking to the American public<br />

(<strong>and</strong> indeed to the American pubic!), to educate them about the value of<br />

cross-cultural comparison in underst<strong>and</strong>ing their own changing gender systems<br />

(p. 12). Mead writes that we should focus on our potentials as gendered<br />

beings rather than our limits, <strong>and</strong> identifies herself as an applied<br />

anthropologist (p. 37).<br />

While Mead tends to pontificate, she does explicitly acknowledge her<br />

positionality <strong>and</strong> perspective before this became st<strong>and</strong>ard practice in<br />

anthropology. ‘‘This book is being written from the st<strong>and</strong>point of a woman<br />

of middle age, of an American, <strong>and</strong> of an anthropologist’’ (p. 21). Mead again<br />

presents <strong>and</strong> cites her own work almost exclusively. She compares the seven<br />

Pacific societies in which she had worked (Arapesh, Mundugumor,<br />

Tchambuli, Iatmül, Manus, Bali, <strong>and</strong> Samoa) with American society, describing<br />

how child-rearing practices influence sex identity. And Mead here<br />

extends her rhetorical practice of creating hypothetical, generalized individuals<br />

to sometimes creating imaginary (or at least unnamed) societies. She<br />

frequently uses imaginary scenes in place of data to support her conclusions<br />

(e.g., p. 78).<br />

In all seven of the Pacific societies Mead studied, girls are clothed earlier<br />

than boys. She explains this as ‘‘an expression that they are waiting<br />

women, while the boys have manhood still to achieve’’ (p. 97). In New<br />

Guinea societies, she asserts, girls are understood to automatically become<br />

women <strong>and</strong> produce children, while boys must actively be made through<br />

cultural intervention (this generalization is borne out in many cases; see


124 R. I. Lohmann<br />

Herdt, 1987 [1981]). Male superiority relies on ritual secrets that in some<br />

myths were stolen from women, the true creative force in the world.<br />

‘‘Men owe their manhood to a theft <strong>and</strong> a theatrical mime, which would fall<br />

to the ground in a moment as mere dust <strong>and</strong> ashes if its true constituents<br />

were known’’ (p. 95). This very occurrence, predicted by Mead, is documented<br />

among the Arapesh in Tuzin’s (1997) description of a Christian ‘‘revival’’<br />

movement that swept away the men’s secrecy-derived religious power.<br />

Mead identifies the physical differences between males <strong>and</strong> females<br />

with which cultures must contend. Females appear to automatically become<br />

parents. Males have no such obvious role in physical creation, <strong>and</strong> therefore<br />

tend to strive for creative power in other arenas. Females have sharp divisions<br />

in their life cycles, which ease identity formation: ‘‘menarche, defloration,<br />

pregnancy, birth, lactation, <strong>and</strong> menopause’’ (pp. 151–152). Males, on<br />

the other h<strong>and</strong>, have no discrete boundaries in their lifecycles to mark sexual<br />

maturity, <strong>and</strong> fatherhood, because it occurs via the body of another, cannot<br />

be experienced so directly as motherhood, <strong>and</strong> is highly subject to<br />

cultural elaboration. Mead goes so far as to call fatherhood a ‘‘social invention’’<br />

(p. 170). This too, encourages men to develop other forms of<br />

manmade creativity to build a male identity, often with women being the<br />

‘‘natural’’ objects upon which they build their ‘‘cultural’’ masculinity—this<br />

is a precursor to Sherry Ortner’s (1974), ‘‘Is Female to Male as Nature Is to<br />

Culture?’’:<br />

Stage after stage in women’s life-histories thus st<strong>and</strong>, irrevocable, indisputable, accomplished.<br />

This gives a natural basis for the little girl’s emphasis on being rather than on doing. The little<br />

boy learns that he must act like a boy, do things, prove that he is a boy, <strong>and</strong> prove it over <strong>and</strong><br />

over again, while the little girl learns that she is a girl, <strong>and</strong> all she has to do is to refrain from<br />

acting like a boy...There is no exact moment at which the boy can say, ‘‘Now I am a man,’’<br />

unless society steps in <strong>and</strong> gives a definition (p. 162).<br />

Two pages later, in making another point, she provides evidence to<br />

contradict her own generalization. Among the Arapesh, a girl’s menarche<br />

is marked by ‘‘Older women of her family [who] attend her <strong>and</strong> instruct<br />

her in rolling stinging nettle-leaves <strong>and</strong> inserting them into her vulva to<br />

make her breasts grow’’ (p. 164). So girls, too, can be at least enhanced if<br />

not absolutely made by cultural means. Nevertheless, Mead links female<br />

to nature <strong>and</strong> male to culture—the imagined ideal—which must be imposed<br />

upon the pre-existing, natural world. And this natural femininity is a barrier<br />

to the imaginative type of creativity in which men tend to specialize (p. 168).<br />

In effect, Mead argues that female ties to nature limit their imaginative or<br />

cultural development at which men excel because of being forced to do<br />

rather than merely be. Women, it would seem, are awake in the physical<br />

world, while men are dreaming in an imagined one, <strong>and</strong> projecting their


<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong> 125<br />

dreamscape onto the l<strong>and</strong>scape. An interesting suggestion, but contradicted<br />

by women’s often greater engagement with spirit possession (e.g., Boddy,<br />

1988; Lohmann, 2003b, pp. 200–201) <strong>and</strong> dominance in some religions<br />

(Sered, 1994; for a comparison of projected dreamscapes of female <strong>and</strong><br />

male initiates in a New Guinea society, see Kempf & Hermann, 2003). In<br />

Male <strong>and</strong> Female we see ideas that would later be brought together in<br />

ecofeminism, in which woman is identified with Earth <strong>and</strong> the natural<br />

environment, which man’s culture shapes, dominates, <strong>and</strong> abuses (see,<br />

e.g., Warren, 1987).<br />

Mead puts contemporary American views of what is naturally female or<br />

male to the test by comparing them to views found among the peoples she<br />

has studied, revealing that much of what Americans assume to be natural is<br />

in fact cultural. ‘‘Natural,’’ in this context, refers to reality as it really is, while<br />

‘‘cultural’’ refers to peoples’ idealized models of reality, implying that these<br />

models are more or less inaccurate, or at least stylized perceptions of physical<br />

reality. This approach continues to be followed by those in the discipline<br />

who question anthropological models of what is natural, though Mead is<br />

seldom cited in such work (e.g., Yanagisako & Delaney, 1995). Mead dispatches,<br />

for example, the view that the female orgasm is ‘‘natural’’: ‘‘Comparative<br />

cultural material gives no grounds for assuming that an orgasm is<br />

an integral <strong>and</strong> unlearned part of women’s sexual response, as it is of men’s<br />

sexual response, <strong>and</strong> strongly suggests that a greater part of women’s copulatory<br />

behaviour is learned’’ (p. 206).<br />

According to Mead, social fatherhood <strong>and</strong> sexual partnership for males,<br />

on the one h<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> orgasm <strong>and</strong> sexual focus for females, on the other, are<br />

both potentials that must be learned. Unlearned, in women, is a desire for<br />

children, <strong>and</strong> in men, is uncommitted promiscuity:<br />

Male sexuality seems originally focused to no goal beyond immediate discharge; it is society<br />

that provides the male with a desire for children, for patterned interpersonal relationships that<br />

order, control, <strong>and</strong> elaborate his original impulses. (p. 212)<br />

Girls can certainly learn not to want children, but such learning seems always to be<br />

socially imposed. Every delicate detail of the female body may of course be reinterpreted by<br />

the culture, <strong>and</strong> no longer be recognised as the doorway to new life. The breasts may be<br />

labeled as erotic zones, to be trained <strong>and</strong> cherished only because they are valuable supplements<br />

to love-making, not because they will one day feed children (p. 214; on this last<br />

point, see Dettwyler, 1995, whose cross-cultural study indicates that breasts are not universally<br />

eroticized).<br />

Mead finds a commonality in all the diversity of American culture: that<br />

ideals of personal happiness <strong>and</strong> fulfillment fill American consciousness.<br />

They influence perception, causing Americans either to see what is not ideal<br />

as though it were, or else to look upon the nonideal with extreme dissatisfaction<br />

(pp. 238–242). In support of this assertion, she wryly points out that


126 R. I. Lohmann<br />

Americans consider love more genuine if one falls in love spontaneously,<br />

without the benefit of caution or learning about the beloved first (p. 320).<br />

Mead identifies Americans as less sensual than Europeans, <strong>and</strong> accounts<br />

for this in terms of ‘‘the gap that all Americans face when the actual<br />

sensuous experience must be adjusted to the visual ideal that is held up<br />

before them. No sensuous actuality fits the dream, each must be to a degree<br />

denied, blurred, or critically rejected, so that one may continue to live’’<br />

(p. 242). To this lack of fit between ideal <strong>and</strong> real, Mead attributes the focus<br />

on looks in American love culture. Dubious here is Mead’s assumption that<br />

appearance can be ideal, but feel cannot. However, her indication of the<br />

dissatisfied idealism as a driving force of American culture strikes me as<br />

valuable.<br />

Continuing the theme of sensuality, Mead criticizes 1940s American<br />

bottle-feeding practices for its impact on people’s sensual sensibilities:<br />

‘‘For the primary learning experience that is the physical prototype of the<br />

sex relationship—a complimentary relation between the body of the mother<br />

<strong>and</strong> the body of the child—is substituted a relationship between the child<br />

<strong>and</strong> an object, an object that imitates the breast, but which is not h<strong>and</strong>led<br />

as either part of the mother or part of the baby’’ (p. 249).<br />

There is an assumption here that the memory of suckling will influence<br />

adult life profoundly. Will men <strong>and</strong> women who were bottle-fed as infants<br />

treat their lovers as objects rather than people? This is an empirical question,<br />

<strong>and</strong> one very difficult to answer, yet Mead treats it as demonstrated.<br />

Moving from infancy to adolescence, Mead characterizes the rules of<br />

dating in 1940s America: boys are to ask, dates should be in public for display<br />

purposes, <strong>and</strong> dating is done for prestige, not out of sexual attraction<br />

(pp. 264–267). Rather than being a people preoccupied with sex, appearance,<br />

<strong>and</strong> love for itself, Mead asserts that ‘‘this continuous emphasis on<br />

the sexually relevant physical appearance is an outcome of using a heterosexual<br />

game [dating] as the prototype for success <strong>and</strong> popularity in adolescence’’<br />

(p. 268). Mead points out the contradictions in this system that<br />

the cultural participant must navigate. ‘‘We actually place our young people<br />

in a virtually intolerable situation, giving them the entire setting for [sexual]<br />

behaviour for which we then punish them whenever it occurs’’ (p. 268).<br />

Mead emphasized the significance of cultural contradictions for individual<br />

lives, but she also makes a point similar to Karl Marx’s, that is, that they<br />

are causal of cultural change. ‘‘[T]o the degree that a style of beauty that [is]<br />

unobtainable by most people [is in force], or a style of bravery or initiative[,]<br />

... then both men <strong>and</strong> women suffer ...[T]his suffering, this discrepancy,<br />

this sense of failure in an enjoined role, is the point of leverage for social<br />

change’’ (p. 276). Finding <strong>and</strong> commenting on hypothetical consequences<br />

of such contradictions make up a major, <strong>and</strong> interesting, portion of this


work. Mead describes the difficulties of boys being told not to ‘‘be<br />

women’’—it gives them a fear of losing what is inalienable, their sex. American<br />

women’s new freedom to take ‘‘male’’ jobs has ironically created ‘‘a<br />

society that appears to throw its doors wide open to women, but translates<br />

her every step towards success as having been damaging—to her own<br />

chances of marriage, <strong>and</strong> to the men whom she passes on the road’’(p. 291).<br />

Mead struggles with two incompatible positions of freedom versus tradition:<br />

individuals should be free to create their own gender ideals, yet they<br />

should also embrace the beauty of their traditional gender roles. That is, on<br />

the one h<strong>and</strong>, people should not be limited by gender ideals (p. 346), but<br />

on the other h<strong>and</strong>, male <strong>and</strong> female are marvelously different, <strong>and</strong> these differences<br />

should be celebrated, as the cliché now has it, to allow satisfying<br />

identity formation (p. 349; cf. Banner, 2003, pp. 358–359). Her ultimate<br />

choice appears to be more freedom, <strong>and</strong> she calls in her 1962 introduction<br />

for the ‘‘provision for many different styles of self-realization <strong>and</strong> sex behavior’’<br />

(p. xxxv), <strong>and</strong> sees access to birth control pills as making this more<br />

possible.<br />

These editions of <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> Temperament <strong>and</strong> Male <strong>and</strong> Female are<br />

unfortunately printed on low-quality paper. They share the same two introductions,<br />

written by Helen Fisher <strong>and</strong> Mary Catherine Bateson, rather than<br />

each having its own. Fisher places these works in the historical nature–<br />

nurture debate <strong>and</strong> adds that the brain-mind has come to be recognized<br />

as a third influence that creates behavior patterns. Indeed, agency,<br />

consciousness, cognition, <strong>and</strong> neurobiology are pressing concerns for<br />

twenty first century anthropology. In Bateson’s introduction, she recounts<br />

that Mead was asked by her first editor to write about the relevance of<br />

her observations for ordinary Americans. This is certainly one of the reasons<br />

why some of Mead’s work still makes interesting <strong>and</strong> useful reading for<br />

beginning students.<br />

CONCLUSION<br />

<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong> 127<br />

A revival of interest in <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead is apparent in a number of<br />

recent <strong>and</strong> forthcoming works addressing her life <strong>and</strong> work <strong>and</strong> their relevance<br />

for current anthropology. Among these, two recent biographies by<br />

Lois Banner (2003) <strong>and</strong> Hilary Lapsley (1999) provide enormous insight into<br />

Mead’s intellectual history <strong>and</strong> drive, <strong>and</strong> how these were tied up with her<br />

relationships, particularly with Ruth Benedict. Gerald Sullivan (2004) offers<br />

a valuable descriptive analysis of Mead’s unpublished ‘‘squares’’ system of<br />

human psychological types that developed in discussions between herself,<br />

Reo Fortune, <strong>and</strong> Gregory Bateson in the Sepik, <strong>and</strong> that lies behind much


128 R. I. Lohmann<br />

of her thinking. And David Lipset’s (2003, p. 712) rereading of <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Temperament <strong>and</strong> the writings of its critics defends Mead’s basic findings<br />

<strong>and</strong> declares her work prescient of subsequent developments in feminist<br />

anthropology.<br />

Reading older work critically, we are reminded of how far we have<br />

come as a discipline, but we also come to appreciate the accomplishments<br />

of our intellectual ancestors, including their insights that have been forgotten<br />

or falsely stereotyped but which have relevance to current work. No<br />

longer could some of Mead’s problematic rhetorical techniques pass muster<br />

in twenty first century anthropology, but reading Mead’s work with an open<br />

mind can allow us to rediscover some of her valuable goals, methods, <strong>and</strong><br />

insights that we have lost along the way. Like the writings of the best ethnographers<br />

to follow her, <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead’s ethnography at its best describes<br />

cultures—<strong>and</strong> the people who bear <strong>and</strong> participate in them—in minute<br />

detail. Unlike most current work, however, it also encourages <strong>and</strong> shows<br />

us as individuals how to reproduce <strong>and</strong> use unfamiliar cultural elements<br />

in the same way that through learning a foreign language one learns how<br />

to think <strong>and</strong> express oneself in a new <strong>and</strong> useful way. And learning about<br />

alternative ways of thinking, feeling, <strong>and</strong> being so that we can actively<br />

enrich our lives is surely one of the most important reasons for doing<br />

anthropology. <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead, perhaps more than any other anthropologist,<br />

wisely put anthropology in the service of this goal, <strong>and</strong> her writings remain<br />

to urge the rest of us to do the same.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Banner, L. W. (2003). Intertwined lives: <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead, Ruth Benedict, <strong>and</strong> their circle.<br />

New York: Alfred A. Knopf.<br />

Barnard, A. (2000). History <strong>and</strong> theory in anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge <strong>University</strong><br />

Press.<br />

Bashkow, I. & Dobrin, L. M. (forthcoming). The great arc of human possibilities <strong>and</strong> a small<br />

circle of friends: The social microcosm of Mead’s <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> Temperament. In<br />

R. H<strong>and</strong>ler (Ed.), Centers <strong>and</strong> peripheries. History of anthropology 11. Madison, WI:<br />

<strong>University</strong> of Wisconsin Press.<br />

Beals, A. R. (2002). Anthropology made new: Writing against <strong>and</strong> without culture. Reviews in<br />

Anthropology 31(3), 213–229.<br />

Benedict, R. (1953 [1934]). Patterns of culture. New York: Mentor.<br />

Bloch, M. (1989 [1979]). The disconnection between power <strong>and</strong> rank as a process: An outline of<br />

the development of kingdoms in central Madagascar. In M. Bloch (Ed.), Ritual, history <strong>and</strong><br />

power: Selected papers in anthropology (pp. 44–88). London: The Athlone Press.<br />

Boddy, J. (1988). Spirits <strong>and</strong> selves in northern Sudan: The cultural therapeutics of possession<br />

<strong>and</strong> trance. American Ethnologist 15(1), 4–27.<br />

Clifford, J. & Marcus, G. (Eds.). (1986). Writing culture: The poetics <strong>and</strong> politics of ethnography.<br />

Berkeley, CA: <strong>University</strong> of California Press.


<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sensibility</strong> 129<br />

Dettwyler, K. A. (1995). Beauty <strong>and</strong> the breast: The cultural context of breastfeeding in the<br />

United States. In P. Stuart-Macadam & K. A. Dettwyler (Eds.), Breastfeeding: Biocultural<br />

perspectives (pp. 167–215). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.<br />

Dobrin, L. M. & Bashkow, I. (forthcoming). Arapesh warfare: Mead <strong>and</strong> Fortune’s clash of<br />

ethnographic temperament . In R. H<strong>and</strong>ler (Ed.), Centers <strong>and</strong> peripheries. History of<br />

anthropology 11. Madison, WI: <strong>University</strong> of Wisconsin Press.<br />

Ember, M. (1985). Evidence <strong>and</strong> science in ethnography: Reflections on the Freeman-Mead<br />

controversy. American Anthropologist 87(4), 906–910.<br />

Fortune, R. F. (1939). Arapesh warfare. American Anthropologist 41, 22–41.<br />

Fortune, R. F. (1942). Arapesh. Publications of the American Ethnological Society 19. New York:<br />

J. Augustin.<br />

Freeman, D. (1983). <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead <strong>and</strong> Samoa: The making <strong>and</strong> unmaking of an anthropological<br />

myth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Gewertz, D. B. (1983). Sepik River societies: A historical ethnography of the Chambri <strong>and</strong> their<br />

neighbors. New Haven, CT: Yale <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Herdt, G. H. (1987 [1981]). Guardians of the flutes: Idioms of masculinity. New York: Columbia<br />

<strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Keesing, R. (1981). Cultural anthropology: A contemporary perspective. New York: Holt,<br />

Rinehart <strong>and</strong> Winston.<br />

Kempf, W. & Hermann, E. (2003). Dreamscapes: Transcending the local in initiation rites among<br />

the Ngaing of Papua New Guinea. In R. I. Lohmann (Ed.), Dream travelers: Sleep experiences<br />

<strong>and</strong> culture in the Western Pacific (pp. 60–85). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.<br />

Lapsley, H. (1999). <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead <strong>and</strong> Ruth Benedict: The kinship of women. Amherst, MA:<br />

<strong>University</strong> of Massachusetts Press.<br />

Leavitt, S. C. (1998). The bikhet mystique: Masculine identity <strong>and</strong> patterns of rebellion among<br />

Bumbita adolescent males. In G. Herdt & S. C. Leavitt (Eds.), Adolescence in Pacific Isl<strong>and</strong><br />

Societies (pp. 173–194). Pittsburgh, PA: <strong>University</strong> of Pittsburgh Press.<br />

Lepowsky, M. (1993). Fruit of the motherl<strong>and</strong>: Gender in an egalitarian society. New York:<br />

Columbia <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Lepowsky, M. (2000). Charlotte Gower <strong>and</strong> the subterranean history of anthropology. In<br />

R. H<strong>and</strong>ler (Ed.), Excluded ancestors, inventible traditions: Essays toward a more inclusive<br />

history of anthropology 9, 123–170. Madison, WI: <strong>University</strong> of Wisconsin Press.<br />

Lewis, H. S. (1998). The misrepresentation of anthropology <strong>and</strong> its consequences. American<br />

Anthropologist 100(3), 716–731.<br />

Lipset, D. (2003). Rereading <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> temperament: <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead’s Sepik triptych <strong>and</strong> its ethnographic<br />

critics. Anthropological Quarterly 76(4), 693–714.<br />

Lohmann, R. I. (2003a). Introduction: Naming the ineffable. In R. I. Lohmann (Ed.) Perspectives<br />

on the category ‘‘supernatural,’’ special issue, Anthropological Forum 13(2),<br />

116–124.<br />

Lohmann, R. I. (2003b). Supernatural encounters of the Asabano in two traditions <strong>and</strong> three<br />

states of consciousness. In R. I. Lohmann (Ed.), Dream travelers: Sleep experiences <strong>and</strong> culture<br />

in the Western Pacific (pp. 188–210). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.<br />

Lutkenhaus, N. C. (1995). <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead <strong>and</strong> the ‘‘rustling-of-the-wind-in-the-palm-trees<br />

school’’ of ethnographic writing. In R. Behar & D. A. Gordon (Eds.) Women writing culture<br />

(pp. 186–206) Berkeley: <strong>University</strong> of California Press.<br />

MacKenzie, M. A. (1991). Androgynous objects: String bags <strong>and</strong> gender in central New Guinea.<br />

Chur, Switzerl<strong>and</strong>: Harwood Academic Publishers.<br />

McDowell, N. (1991). The Mundugumor: From the field notes of <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead <strong>and</strong> Reo<br />

Fortune. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.<br />

Mead, M. (2001 [1928]). Coming of age in Samoa. New York: Perennial=HarperCollins.


130 R. I. Lohmann<br />

Orans, M. (1999). Mead misrepresented. Science 283(5408), 1649–1650.<br />

Ortner, S. (1974). Is female to male as nature is to culture? In M. Z. Rosaldo & L. Lamphere<br />

(Eds.), Woman, culture, <strong>and</strong> society (pp. 67–87). Stanford, CA: Stanford <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Roscoe, P. (2003). <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead, Reo Fortune, <strong>and</strong> Mountain Arapesh warfare. American<br />

Anthropologist 105(3), 581–591.<br />

Schwartz, T. (1981). The acquisition of culture. Ethos 9(1), 4–17.<br />

Sered, S. S. (1994). Priestess, mother, sacred sister: Religions dominated by women. Oxford:<br />

Oxford <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Strauss, C. & Quinn, N. (1998). A cognitive theory of cultural meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge<br />

<strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Sullivan, G. (2004). A four-fold humanity: <strong>Margaret</strong> Mead <strong>and</strong> psychological types. Journal of<br />

the history of the behavioral sciences 40(2), in press.<br />

Turnbull, C. (1972). The mountain people. New York: Simon <strong>and</strong> Schuster.<br />

Turnbull, C. (1987 [1962]). The forest people. New York: Simon <strong>and</strong> Schuster.<br />

Tuzin, D. (1997). The cassowary’s revenge: The life <strong>and</strong> death of masculinity in a New Guinea<br />

society. Chicago: <strong>University</strong> of Chicago Press.<br />

Tylor, E. B. (1877 [1871]). Primitive culture: Researches into the development of mythology,<br />

philosophy, religion, language, art, <strong>and</strong> custom. New York: Henry Holt.<br />

Warren, K. J. (1987). Feminism <strong>and</strong> ecology: Making connections. Environmental Ethics 9(1),<br />

3–20.<br />

Wolf, M. (1992). A thrice-told tale: Feminism, postmodernism <strong>and</strong> ethnographic responsibility.<br />

Stanford, CA: Stanford <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Yanagisako, S. & Delaney, C. (1995). Naturalizing power. In S. Yanagisako & C. Delaney (Eds.),<br />

Naturalizing power: Essays in feminist cultural analysis (pp. 1–22). New York: Routledge.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!