24.04.2013 Views

Florida manatees, Trichechus manatus latirostris, respond to ...

Florida manatees, Trichechus manatus latirostris, respond to ...

Florida manatees, Trichechus manatus latirostris, respond to ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Abstract<br />

<strong>Florida</strong> <strong>manatees</strong>, <strong>Trichechus</strong> <strong>manatus</strong> <strong>latirostris</strong>, <strong>respond</strong><br />

<strong>to</strong> approaching vessels<br />

Stephanie M. Nowacek a,b , Randall S. Wells a,b , Edward C.G. Owen a ,<br />

Todd R. Speakman a,b , Richard O. Flamm c , Douglas P. Nowacek a,*<br />

a Mote Marine Labora<strong>to</strong>ry, 1600 Ken Thompson Parkway, Sarasota, FL 34236, USA<br />

b Chicago Zoological Society, USA<br />

c <strong>Florida</strong> Marine Research Institute, <strong>Florida</strong> Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission, USA<br />

Received 18 April 2003; received in revised form 28 August 2003; accepted 5 November 2003<br />

<strong>Florida</strong> <strong>manatees</strong> inhabit shallow coastal and estuarine waters of the southeast US, a range that brings them in<strong>to</strong> frequent<br />

contact with vessels. More than 30% of documented annual mortalities are attributed <strong>to</strong> vessel collisions, and most living animals<br />

bear the scars of multiple, non-lethal encounters. To document the behavior of <strong>manatees</strong> in the presence of vessels, we recorded their<br />

movements with an overhead video system. We scored six aspects of behavior during 170 vessel approaches, and compared their<br />

behavior with 187 control segments when no boats were present. Manatees in shallow waters and at the edge of the channel <strong>respond</strong>ed<br />

<strong>to</strong> approaches by orienting <strong>to</strong>wards the nearest deep water, a boat channel, and increasing their swimming speed. Close<br />

boat approaches and shallow water depths exacerbated these responses. Our results indicate that <strong>manatees</strong> detect and <strong>respond</strong> <strong>to</strong><br />

approaching vessels with an apparent flight response, a response which includes movement <strong>to</strong>wards deeper water. If given sufficient<br />

time, i.e., approached or passed slowly, the <strong>manatees</strong> may then be able <strong>to</strong> reach deeper water and safe depths.<br />

Ó 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.<br />

Keywords: Manatee; Behavior; Vessel; Collision; Conservation<br />

Biological Conservation 119 (2004) 517–523<br />

The West Indian manatee, <strong>Trichechus</strong> <strong>manatus</strong>, inhabits<br />

the warm, coastal, marine and fresh water systems<br />

fringing the Caribbean Sea, Gulf of Mexico, and<br />

western Atlantic Ocean (Lefebvre et al., 2001). Within<br />

this range, these slow-moving, shallow-water animals<br />

face a variety of serious threats from humans, including<br />

hunting, habitat loss, entanglement in fishing gear, entrapment<br />

in water control structures, and collisions with<br />

boats. These threats vary in intensity over time and<br />

between locations (OÕShea, 1988). Even where <strong>manatees</strong><br />

are subject <strong>to</strong> seemingly strong protective measures,<br />

mortality and morbidity from human activities can be<br />

exceedingly common (Reynolds, 1999).<br />

The <strong>Florida</strong> subspecies of the West Indian manatee,<br />

<strong>Trichechus</strong> <strong>manatus</strong> <strong>latirostris</strong>, ranges throughout the<br />

* Cor<strong>respond</strong>ing author. Present address: Department of Oceanography,<br />

<strong>Florida</strong> State University, 509 OSB, West Call St., Tallahassee,<br />

FL 32306-4320, USA. Tel.: +1-850-645-1547; fax: +1-850-644-2581.<br />

E-mail address: nowacek@ocean.fsu.edu (D.P. Nowacek).<br />

0006-3207/$ - see front matter. Ó 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.<br />

doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2003.11.020<br />

BIOLOGICAL<br />

CONSERVATION<br />

www.elsevier.com/locate/biocon<br />

waters of <strong>Florida</strong>, sometimes venturing in<strong>to</strong> other states<br />

along the Gulf and Atlantic coasts during warmer<br />

months. The <strong>Florida</strong> manatee, more than any other<br />

subspecies, is faced with frequent encounters with boats,<br />

sometimes leading <strong>to</strong> death or serious injury from collision<br />

impact or cuts from propellers (Ackerman et al.,<br />

1995; Wright et al., 1995). On average, about 30% of<br />

documented manatee deaths each year are due <strong>to</strong> collisions<br />

with vessels (Ackerman et al., 1995; Commission,<br />

2001; Wright et al., 1995). Deaths from boat strikes involve<br />

all age classes and occur year-round over much of<br />

the subspeciesÕ range (Ackerman et al., 1995; OÕShea,<br />

1988; OÕShea, 1995).<br />

The number of documented mortalities is only a<br />

minimum estimate of the direct impact of boats on<br />

<strong>manatees</strong>; it does not include animals struck by vessels<br />

but not killed, or those killed but whose carcasses were<br />

not recovered or found. Many <strong>Florida</strong> <strong>manatees</strong> bear<br />

scars of multiple past collisions with boats; some of<br />

these injuries appear debilitating (OÕShea, 1995). Of the


518 S.M. Nowacek et al. / Biological Conservation 119 (2004) 517–523<br />

1184 living individuals in the identification scar catalog<br />

that bear boat strike scars, 97% have scar patterns indicative<br />

of more than one strike (OÕShea et al., 2001).<br />

The long-term effects, both at the individual and population<br />

levels, of repeated serious injuries on manatee<br />

survivorship, reproduction, and quality of life remain <strong>to</strong><br />

be evaluated.<br />

In spite of the large numbers of injuries and deaths<br />

resulting from watercraft, few observations of collisions<br />

or of manatee behavior in response <strong>to</strong> approaching<br />

boats have been recorded (Wright et al., 1995). In the<br />

absence of systematic observations of manatee-boat interactions,<br />

much speculation exists about whether or<br />

how <strong>manatees</strong> <strong>respond</strong> <strong>to</strong> approaching boats. Detailed<br />

information on the behavioral responses of <strong>manatees</strong> <strong>to</strong><br />

boat approaches would be valuable for devising boattraffic<br />

management plans <strong>to</strong> reduce manatee mortality.<br />

Such information has been difficult <strong>to</strong> obtain, because<br />

they submerge for extended periods of time, very little of<br />

the animal is visible when it surfaces, and water clarity is<br />

poor in much of the speciesÕ range where boats are operating<br />

near them. Observations may be further complicated<br />

by the presence of the observer.<br />

Recent efforts <strong>to</strong> devise techniques for making<br />

continuous observations of marine mammals have resulted<br />

in the development of a remotely-operated<br />

overhead video recording system suspended from a<br />

helium-filled aerostat tethered <strong>to</strong> a 6 m support vessel<br />

(Nowacek D.P. et al., 2001). The video camera can<br />

provide excellent visual penetration in<strong>to</strong> the water<br />

column, allowing observation of behavioral details<br />

that cannot be obtained in any other manner. Because<br />

of concerns raised by reports of boat collisions with<br />

bottlenose dolphins (Wells and Scott, 1997), this<br />

technique was applied <strong>to</strong> experimental studies of behavioral<br />

responses of dolphins <strong>to</strong> approaches by vessels<br />

(Nowacek S.M. et al., 2001). The overhead video<br />

system has also been used in videogrammetry studies<br />

of <strong>manatees</strong> (Flamm et al., 2000). Given the adaptability<br />

of the system, we extended the overhead video<br />

approach <strong>to</strong> studies of the behavioral responses of<br />

<strong>manatees</strong> <strong>to</strong> vessel approaches.<br />

1. Methods<br />

We observed <strong>manatees</strong> in the waters near City Island,<br />

Sarasota, FL, adjacent <strong>to</strong> Mote Marine Labora<strong>to</strong>ry<br />

(Fig. 1). The channel <strong>to</strong> the north side of the City Island<br />

seagrass meadows is a designated ‘‘Water Sports Area’’,<br />

near a boat ramp, and is used heavily by water skiers on<br />

weekends and holidays. The Water Sports Area ranged<br />

in depth from 0.7 <strong>to</strong> 4.8 m, though most of the channel<br />

was less than 3 m deep and the fringing seagrass<br />

meadows were typically less than 2 m deep. Manatees<br />

were found in seagrass meadows and channels, and<br />

along the edges where the shallows slope in<strong>to</strong> the<br />

channels.<br />

We conducted focal animal behavioral observations<br />

on <strong>manatees</strong> under circumstances where continuous<br />

observations were possible (Altmann, 1974). Focal<br />

<strong>manatees</strong> were selected on the basis of their occurrence<br />

in relatively clear water, and the existence of distinctive<br />

features that would facilitate their recognition<br />

Fig. 1. The study area was a small area of the inshore waters near Sarasota, FL composed primarily of seagrass beds with a deep channel nearby.<br />

Observations most often started when <strong>manatees</strong> were in the shallows or at the edge of the channel. We followed animals according <strong>to</strong> the behavioral<br />

pro<strong>to</strong>col explained in the text, including attempts <strong>to</strong> maintain contact even when they moved in<strong>to</strong> the channel.


throughout the course of our observations. We refrained<br />

from approaching manatee mothers with young calves.<br />

During two field seasons, two types of vessel approaches<br />

were used: opportunistic (1999 and 2000) and<br />

experimental (1999 only). Opportunistic approaches<br />

were recordings of vessels operated by individuals independent<br />

of our study that happened <strong>to</strong> pass within the<br />

field of view of our aerial video camera while a focal<br />

manatee was visible in the frame. During experimental<br />

approaches, a 5.5 m long, 150 hp outboard-powered<br />

center console vessel under our control was directed via<br />

VHF radio <strong>to</strong>ward <strong>manatees</strong> under observation via the<br />

aerostat. This kind of boat represents one of the more<br />

common classes of vessels in the study area. As per our<br />

US Fish and Wildlife Service Research Permit, the vessel<br />

was directed along a straight-line course so that it would<br />

pass greater than three manatee body lengths from the<br />

focal manatee. The experimental boat approach began<br />

about 100 m away from the manatee(s), and ended the<br />

same distance past the animal(s). The approach vessel<br />

attempted <strong>to</strong> maintain a course and speed in a channel<br />

as it reached its point of closest approach <strong>to</strong> the focal<br />

manatee. To maximize the safety of the <strong>manatees</strong>, the<br />

experimental approach vessel carried a dedicated observer<br />

<strong>to</strong> watch for <strong>manatees</strong> moving in<strong>to</strong> the path of the<br />

boat and a short-range television system that carried the<br />

transmitted view from the overhead video camera.<br />

The support vessel was anchored during boat approaches,<br />

when possible. This provided a stable view of<br />

the waters surrounding the focal manatee, and eliminated<br />

the support vesselÕs engine as a possible confounding<br />

source of disturbance. Observers onboard the<br />

support vessel collected real-time data on the timing and<br />

circumstances of the experimental and opportunistic<br />

approaches, videotape counter information, weather<br />

and sea surface conditions, and the presence of other<br />

<strong>manatees</strong> and vessels in the area. Upon completion of a<br />

set of experimental trials or a series of opportunistic<br />

approaches on a focal manatee, the approach vessel<br />

crew collected data on water depth and turbidity (via<br />

Secchi disk) at the manateeÕs location.<br />

Video recordings of opportunistic and experimental<br />

approaches were reviewed on a high quality, flat screen<br />

24 in. video moni<strong>to</strong>r (Sony Trinitron) and categorized<br />

based on quality. The ability <strong>to</strong> continuously track be-<br />

S.M. Nowacek et al. / Biological Conservation 119 (2004) 517–523 519<br />

Table 1<br />

Description of behavioral parameters used <strong>to</strong> evaluate responses <strong>to</strong> boat approaches<br />

Parameter Description<br />

Heading Compass orientation if stationary, or swimming direction<br />

Mobility Whether the animal is traveling or stationary<br />

Swimming speed Paddle stroke frequency or creation of white water<br />

Inter-animal distance Distance between the focal manatee and the nearest manatee<br />

Channel Turning <strong>to</strong>ward, or moving <strong>to</strong>ward or in<strong>to</strong> deeper water (>2 m)<br />

Swimming depth Depth of water in which focal animal is swimming<br />

Behaviors were scored during video review and then compared between approach and control segments.<br />

haviors of the focal animal was imperative, so segments<br />

in which the focal animal was not continuously viewable<br />

were considered ‘‘unusable’’ and were not included in<br />

the analyses (for example, turbidity during early experiments<br />

often precluded continuous observations). Two<br />

sets of video segments were analyzed. The first set included<br />

the approach segments, which were typically<br />

about 30 s long. The second set included randomly selected<br />

control segments, also lasting about 30 s. Each<br />

control segment occurred at least 5 min before or after a<br />

vessel approach. Control segments were spaced at least 2<br />

min apart <strong>to</strong> ensure independence between segments.<br />

Changes in behavior, along with distance between the<br />

focal manatee and the approach vessel, were measured<br />

and recorded. Two independent observers watched each<br />

approach segment twice <strong>to</strong> ensure scoring consistency.<br />

Distance estimates were made by direct measure on the<br />

flat screen, using known-length reference objects (approach<br />

vessel and components) in appropriate orientations<br />

<strong>to</strong> minimize problems associated with parallax<br />

(Flamm et al., 2000).<br />

We measured six behavioral parameters (Table 1).<br />

Accurate measures of amount of change (e.g., number<br />

of degrees of heading change) were not possible from the<br />

video image as there was no way <strong>to</strong> calibrate measurements<br />

given differences in zoom and viewing angle. Instead,<br />

each approach segment was scored using either<br />

‘‘change’’ or ‘‘no change’’ for each of the parameters.<br />

When changes in the behavioral parameters were detected,<br />

we recorded the direction of the change, e.g., an<br />

increase in swim speed or a decrease in inter-animal<br />

distance.<br />

The analyses were hierarchical. First, we assessed<br />

whether the frequency of change in any of the behavioral<br />

categories varied between the vessel approach<br />

segment (i.e., experimental treatments) and the control<br />

segments by applying a test for an additive effect (due <strong>to</strong><br />

treatment) at the logistic scale, but allowing for heterogeneity<br />

between individual animals (McCullagh and<br />

Nelder, 1989). Second, if differences were found we then<br />

compared vessel approach segments <strong>to</strong> assess the specific<br />

circumstances where changes occurred by applying a<br />

generalized linear model using a binary distribution and<br />

a logit link function. Data on habitat and boat features<br />

were categorical (Table 2) and tests were conducted with


520 S.M. Nowacek et al. / Biological Conservation 119 (2004) 517–523<br />

Table 2<br />

Description of approach boat characteristics and habitat types encountered during the study<br />

Parameter Description<br />

Approaching boat speed Categorized as idle, plow, or plane<br />

Approaching boat type Categorized by engine type: outboard, inboard, jet or non-mo<strong>to</strong>r<br />

Approach distance Distance between approaching boat and focal manatee at closest approach (m)<br />

Manatee habitat Shallow ( 6 2 m), edge (within 1 m of channel edge), or deep/channel (>2 m)<br />

Approaching boat habitat Shallow ( 6 2 m), edge (within 1 m of channel edge), or deep/channel (>2 m)<br />

Statistica Ò . These methods have been successfully applied<br />

in testing the responses of other marine mammals<br />

<strong>to</strong> vessel approaches (Nowacek S.M. et al., 2001). Significance<br />

was assigned at the 6 0.05 level.<br />

It should be noted that the terms ‘‘approaches’’ and<br />

‘‘passes’’ are considered interchangeable, referring <strong>to</strong><br />

vessels observed moving within the same field of video<br />

view as focal <strong>manatees</strong>. Few ‘‘approaches’’ actually<br />

brought vessels on an interception vec<strong>to</strong>r with the focal<br />

<strong>manatees</strong>, rather the vessels passed by the <strong>manatees</strong> at<br />

distances ranging up <strong>to</strong> 145 m (mean ¼ 19.9 m,<br />

SD ¼ 20.5 m, n ¼ 170).<br />

2. Results<br />

We observed boat approaches on 22 days during 13–<br />

26 May 1999 and 29 April–10 June 2000 (Table 3). La<br />

Ni~na-related conditions of higher than normal winds<br />

and turbid waters hampered our efforts during 2000. Of<br />

a <strong>to</strong>tal of 317 passes recorded during the two years of<br />

the study, 170 involving 30 <strong>manatees</strong> met our criteria for<br />

being considered usable. Additionally, 187 control segments<br />

were scored.<br />

A detectable change in at least one behavioral category<br />

relative <strong>to</strong> boat approaches was noted in 84 (49%) of the<br />

170 usable passes. However, significant changes were<br />

only noted in two behavioral parameters. Significantly<br />

more changes in behavior were found during approach<br />

segments than during control segments for the variables<br />

‘‘channel’’ (test statistic value ¼ 3.353, p < 0:05), and<br />

‘‘swimming speed’’ (test statistic value ¼ 5.097, p < 0:05).<br />

In 42 of 112 cases (37.5%), <strong>manatees</strong> turned <strong>to</strong>ward or<br />

moved <strong>to</strong>ward or in<strong>to</strong> a channel as a boat approached.<br />

Similarly, in 30 of 152 cases (19.7%) <strong>manatees</strong> changed<br />

their swimming speeds as boats approached, 90% of these<br />

cases involved increases in swimming speed. No clear<br />

patterns were found for the remaining behavioral variables<br />

of heading, swimming depth, inter-animal distance,<br />

and mobility as indica<strong>to</strong>rs of response <strong>to</strong> boat<br />

approaches.<br />

We examined these findings with respect <strong>to</strong> approach<br />

boat and habitat parameters, including the distance of<br />

the boat from the manatee at the point of closest approach<br />

or passage. Channel behavior, an apparent<br />

generalized response involving turning <strong>to</strong>ward or moving<br />

<strong>to</strong>ward or in<strong>to</strong> deep water, occurred without specific<br />

regard <strong>to</strong> boat type, boat speed, distance from the<br />

manatee (within the upper limit of our visual field, about<br />

145 m), the kind of habitat in which the boat was operating,<br />

or the kind of habitat occupied by the manatee.<br />

A <strong>to</strong>tal of 152 experimental or opportunistic segments<br />

was suitable for evaluation of changes in swimming<br />

speed and associated fac<strong>to</strong>rs. Changes were<br />

observed in 30 of these segments and 27 of them involved<br />

increases in swimming speed. Significant changes,<br />

however, were detected only during close<br />

approaches; no significant changes in swimming speed<br />

were seen when the boat passed farther than 25 m from<br />

the focal animal. Swimming speed was significantly affected<br />

by the following fac<strong>to</strong>rs during approaches that<br />

occurred less than 25 m from the focal animal: focal<br />

animal (F ¼ 3:18, df ¼ 24, p < 0:001), manatee habitat<br />

(F ¼ 5:08, df ¼ 2, p < 0:01) and approaching boat<br />

habitat (F ¼ 5:86, df ¼ 2, p < 0:01). Additionally, approaching<br />

boat distance was a significant covariate<br />

Table 3<br />

Summary of effort from each field season including number of approaches suitable for analysis<br />

Year No. field days Approach type No. focal <strong>manatees</strong> No. vessel passes No. control segments<br />

1999 9 Opportunistic 9 33 123a 1999 9 Experimental 16 96<br />

2000 8 Opportunistic 9 41 64<br />

Total 22b 30c 170 187<br />

Approach types are explained in the text; briefly, ÔopportunisticÕ approaches consisted of observations of transiting vessels not under our control,<br />

and ÔexperimentalÕ were those conducted with boats under our control.<br />

a<br />

123 control segments for all of 1999 (opportunistic and experimental combined).<br />

b<br />

In 1999, both experimental and opportunistic observations were conducted on the same days.<br />

c<br />

In 1999, some of the focal animals were subject <strong>to</strong> both experimental and opportunistic approaches.


Fig. 2. Percent change in swimming speed in observed manatee habitats.<br />

Distance categories, 0–9 and 10–19 m, refer <strong>to</strong> the distance observed<br />

between an approaching vessel and a manatee at the point of<br />

closest approach. See Table 2 and text for complete descriptions of the<br />

habitats and for statistical comparisons.<br />

(F ¼ 14:79, df ¼ 1, p < 0:001) such that the frequency<br />

of change was correlated with approach distance.<br />

To further investigate the relationship between approach<br />

distance and frequency of change in swimming<br />

speed, we looked at responses in limited distance categories:<br />

0–9 and 10–19 m from the boat. We found that<br />

when a boat approached or passed less than 10 m from<br />

the focal animal, changes in swimming speed were significantly<br />

affected by manatee habitat (F ¼ 8:37, df ¼ 1,<br />

p < 0:05; Fig. 2) and approach distance (covariate,<br />

F ¼ 10:77, df ¼ 1, p < 0:01). When a boat approached<br />

or passed 10–19 m from the focal animal both manatee<br />

habitat and approaching boat habitat were significant<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs affecting the frequency of change in swimming<br />

speed (manatee habitat: F ¼ 4:22, df ¼ 2, p < 0:05,<br />

Fig. 2; approaching boat habitat: F ¼ 5:10, df ¼ 2,<br />

p < 0:05). Within each manatee habitat type, <strong>manatees</strong><br />

were more likely <strong>to</strong> change their swimming speed when<br />

boats moved <strong>to</strong> within 9 m than when they were at 10–<br />

19 m, with the highest occurrence of swimming speed<br />

change (87.5%) when the <strong>manatees</strong> were approached<br />

closely in the shallowest waters (Fig. 2). Of 54 cases in<br />

which approaching boat habitat was recorded, analyses<br />

show that <strong>manatees</strong> were more likely <strong>to</strong> change their<br />

swimming speed if the approaching boat was in the<br />

shallow habitat (25.0%) than in the edge habitat (12.5%)<br />

or the deep habitat (11.9%).<br />

3. Discussion<br />

Our results show that <strong>manatees</strong> do in fact <strong>respond</strong> <strong>to</strong><br />

approaching or passing boats, and it appears that they<br />

often begin <strong>to</strong> <strong>respond</strong> when the vessels are at distances<br />

of 25–50 m, though we can not say at what distance the<br />

<strong>manatees</strong> initially detected the approaching vessels. It is<br />

not surprising that <strong>manatees</strong> can detect boats given the<br />

frequencies of sound boats produce (Richardson et al.,<br />

1995) and the range of frequencies <strong>manatees</strong> can hear<br />

S.M. Nowacek et al. / Biological Conservation 119 (2004) 517–523 521<br />

(Bullock et al., 1980; Bullock et al., 1982; Gerstein et al.,<br />

1999; Ketten et al., 1992). That they actually <strong>respond</strong>,<br />

primarily by moving <strong>to</strong>ward or in<strong>to</strong> deeper water<br />

(channels) and increasing speed, has not, <strong>to</strong> our<br />

knowledge, been reported in the scientific literature.<br />

Unfortunately, this response may take the manatee in<strong>to</strong><br />

the path of the approaching vessel before it can reach<br />

water of sufficient depth <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> move safely beneath<br />

the boat.<br />

While we have demonstrated that <strong>manatees</strong> do <strong>respond</strong><br />

<strong>to</strong> boat approaches, these responses varied with<br />

the individual manatee. General patterns of response do<br />

not emerge until changes in behavior are considered<br />

within very limited approach distance categories. Differential<br />

responses among individuals might be attributed<br />

<strong>to</strong> any of a variety of fac<strong>to</strong>rs, possibly including,<br />

but not limited <strong>to</strong> age, prior exposure <strong>to</strong> boats, reproductive<br />

state, hearing ability, or activity. However, these<br />

potential fac<strong>to</strong>rs diminish in importance for approaches<br />

at close range since at these distances we began <strong>to</strong> see a<br />

uniform response: increased swimming speed and<br />

movement <strong>to</strong>ward or in<strong>to</strong> deeper water.<br />

Our results also show that distance is not the only<br />

significant fac<strong>to</strong>r affecting changes in behavior. Manatee<br />

habitat and approaching boat habitat also significantly<br />

affected frequencies of behavioral change. Sound propagation<br />

is typically poor in shallow water and <strong>manatees</strong><br />

may have no way of clearly localizing the direction from<br />

which a vessel is approaching. Manatees in deep water<br />

have more options for <strong>respond</strong>ing <strong>to</strong> approaching boats.<br />

In channels they can increase their swimming speed,<br />

change their heading, and/or change their swimming<br />

depth. Often <strong>manatees</strong> in channels were observed <strong>to</strong> dive<br />

<strong>to</strong> depths greater than the visibility limit indicated by<br />

our Secchi disk, placing them safely beneath the keels<br />

and propellers of almost all of the vessels operating in<br />

the area.<br />

Our data clearly demonstrate that <strong>manatees</strong> make<br />

changes in their behavior in response <strong>to</strong> boat approaches<br />

at distances of at least 25 m. Responses were<br />

noted <strong>to</strong> occur as close as 1 m and as far as 68 m from<br />

the approaching vessel. Frequencies of behavioral<br />

change vary depending especially on the distance from<br />

the approaching boat and the manateeÕs habitat. Approaching<br />

closely and/or approaching in shallow water<br />

increase risk of collision as it becomes more difficult for<br />

<strong>manatees</strong> <strong>to</strong> move safely out of or below the path of the<br />

approaching vessel.<br />

If the animals are capable of avoiding approaching<br />

boats, which they did successfully with every approach<br />

during our study, it seems fair <strong>to</strong> ask why they continue<br />

<strong>to</strong> be hit. Several possibilities exist, and they fall in<strong>to</strong><br />

two major categories: (i) <strong>manatees</strong> detect approaching<br />

vessels but do not <strong>respond</strong> appropriately or in sufficient<br />

time <strong>to</strong> avoid being hit, or (ii) <strong>manatees</strong> are unable<br />

<strong>to</strong> detect approaching boats due <strong>to</strong> some individual


522 S.M. Nowacek et al. / Biological Conservation 119 (2004) 517–523<br />

problem or environmental fac<strong>to</strong>r. With regard <strong>to</strong> the<br />

first category, many <strong>manatees</strong> live in areas of extremely<br />

dense vessel traffic and so may habituate <strong>to</strong> the sounds<br />

of approaching/passing boats, and/or they may not be<br />

able <strong>to</strong> discriminate between multiple approaching<br />

boats. In these situations they may simply miscalculate<br />

which approaching boat actually poses a threat. Another<br />

possibility is that they are unable <strong>to</strong> localize the<br />

position of the boat accurately enough <strong>to</strong> avoid collision<br />

and therefore move in the wrong direction or fail <strong>to</strong><br />

move soon enough. Given the ana<strong>to</strong>my of the manatee<br />

audi<strong>to</strong>ry system (Ketten et al., 1992), they are unlikely<br />

<strong>to</strong> possess the ability <strong>to</strong> localize the acoustic frequencies<br />

produced by boats as effectively as other marine mammals<br />

that inhabit similar environments, e.g., bottlenose<br />

dolphins (Au, 1993).<br />

With regard <strong>to</strong> the second category, we have demonstrated<br />

that <strong>manatees</strong> are capable of detecting and<br />

<strong>respond</strong>ing <strong>to</strong> approaching boats at relatively long<br />

ranges. Fac<strong>to</strong>rs affecting an individualÕs physical ability<br />

or motivation <strong>to</strong> <strong>respond</strong> were discussed earlier, but<br />

environmental characteristics may also affect their ability<br />

<strong>to</strong> correctly estimate critical pieces of information<br />

about the noise source. Noise from other sources, including<br />

other boats, could mask or partially cover the<br />

sounds of a boat that poses an actual threat. In addition,<br />

transmission of sound in shallow water is affected by<br />

water depth, physiography, substrate type, and vegetative<br />

cover (Kinsler et al., 2000; Urick, 1983), and as<br />

these fac<strong>to</strong>rs vary so might a manateeÕs ability <strong>to</strong> correctly<br />

assess the speed, position, and range of an approaching<br />

boat, all of which could contribute <strong>to</strong> a<br />

collision.<br />

The most prevalent current management strategy <strong>to</strong><br />

reduce manatee morbidity and mortality from boat<br />

strikes is <strong>to</strong> slow vessels in areas of high manatee density.<br />

Our results support this strategy in two ways.<br />

Vessel speed was not a significant fac<strong>to</strong>r in eliciting a<br />

response, i.e., fast boats elicited no more or fewer responses<br />

than slow boats, and collisions at high speed<br />

would obviously cause more trauma. Secondly, we have<br />

demonstrated that <strong>manatees</strong> are capable of appropriately<br />

<strong>respond</strong>ing <strong>to</strong> approaching boats. So, despite<br />

longer exposure time, slower vessel speeds likely afford<br />

the <strong>manatees</strong> and the vessel opera<strong>to</strong>rs extra time that<br />

both may need <strong>to</strong> assess the threat and, if necessary, <strong>to</strong><br />

take appropriate action <strong>to</strong> avoid a collision.<br />

Acknowledgements<br />

Primary support for this project was provided by<br />

the Save-the-Manatee Trust Fund administered by the<br />

<strong>Florida</strong> Marine Research Institute (FMRI) of the<br />

<strong>Florida</strong> Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission.<br />

We especially thank Dr. James Powell of FMRI, now of<br />

Wildlife Trust, for his support and encouragement. We<br />

would also like <strong>to</strong> thank Dr. John Reynolds for his insight<br />

and review of early versions of this manuscript.<br />

Bob BondeÕs comments also helped <strong>to</strong> improve the final<br />

manuscript. Staff time for the project and access <strong>to</strong> the<br />

aerostat, support vessel, and approach vessel were provided<br />

by the Chicago Zoological Society, Woods Hole<br />

Oceanographic Institution, and Dolphin Biology Research<br />

Institute. Many of our statistical analyses are<br />

based on the advice of Andy Solow from the Woods<br />

Hole Oceanographic Institution and Colin Simpfendorfer<br />

from Mote Marine Labora<strong>to</strong>ry. This research<br />

was conducted under Federal Fish and Wildlife Research<br />

Permit No. MA843809-0.<br />

References<br />

Ackerman, B.B., Wright, S.D., Bonde, R.K., Odell, D.K., Banowetz,<br />

D.J., 1995. Trends and patterns in mortality of <strong>manatees</strong> in<br />

<strong>Florida</strong>, 1974–1992, In: T.J. OÕShea, B.B. Ackerman, H.F. Percival<br />

(Eds.), Population Biology of the <strong>Florida</strong> Manatee, National<br />

Biological Service Information Report, Ft. Collins, CO, pp. 223–<br />

258.<br />

Altmann, J., 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods.<br />

Behaviour 49, 227–267.<br />

Au, W.W.L., 1993. The Sonar of Dolphins. Springer-Verlag, New<br />

York.<br />

Bullock, T.H., Domning, D.P., Best, R.C., 1980. Evoked brain<br />

potentials demonstrate hearing in a manatee (<strong>Trichechus</strong> inunguis).<br />

Journal of Mammalogy 61 (1), 130–133.<br />

Bullock, T.H., OÕShea, T.J., McClune, M.C., 1982. Audi<strong>to</strong>ry evoked<br />

potentials in the West Indian manatee (Sirenia: <strong>Trichechus</strong> <strong>manatus</strong>).<br />

Journal Comparative Physiology 148, 547–554.<br />

Commission, Marine Mammal, Annual Report <strong>to</strong> Congress, 2001.<br />

Flamm, R.O., Owen, E.C.G., Weiss, C.F., Wells, R.S., Nowacek, D.P.,<br />

2000. Aerial videogrammetry from a tethered airship <strong>to</strong> assess<br />

manatee life-stage structure. Marine Mammal Science 16 (3), 617–<br />

630.<br />

Gerstein, E.R., Gerstein, L., Forsythe, S.E., Blue, J.E., 1999. The<br />

underwater audiogram of the West Indian manatee (<strong>Trichechus</strong><br />

<strong>manatus</strong>). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 105 (6),<br />

3575–3583.<br />

Ketten, D.R., Odell, D.K., Domning, D.P., 1992. Structure, function,<br />

and adaptation of the manatee ear. In: Thomas, J. (Ed.), Marine<br />

Mammal Sensory Systems. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 77–95.<br />

Kinsler, L.E., Frey, A.R., Coppens, A.B., Sanders, J.V., 2000.<br />

Fundamentals of Acoustics, fourth ed. Wiley, New York.<br />

Lefebvre, L.W., Marmontel, M., Reid, J.P., Rathbun, G.B., Domning,<br />

D.P., 2001. Status and biogeography of the West Indian manatee.<br />

In: Woods, C.A., Sergile, F.E. (Eds.), Biogeography of the West<br />

Indies: Patterns and Perspectives. CRC Press, Boca Ra<strong>to</strong>n, pp.<br />

425–474.<br />

McCullagh, P., Nelder, J., 1989. Generalized Linear Models. Chapman<br />

and Hall, London.<br />

Nowacek, D.P., Wells, R.S., Tyack, P.L., 2001. A platform for<br />

continuous behavioral and acoustic observations of free-ranging<br />

marine mammals: overhead video combined with underwater<br />

audio. Marine Mammal Science 17 (1), 191–199.<br />

Nowacek, S.M., Wells, R.S., Solow, A., 2001. Short-term effects of<br />

boat traffic on bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, in Sarasota<br />

Bay, <strong>Florida</strong>. Marine Mammal Science 17 (4), 673–688.<br />

OÕShea, T.J., 1988. The past, present, and future of <strong>manatees</strong> in the<br />

southeastern United States: realities, misunderstandings, and


enigmas presented at the Southeastern Non-game and Endangered<br />

Wildlife Symposium.<br />

OÕShea, T.J., 1995. Waterborne recreation and the <strong>Florida</strong> manatee.<br />

In: Knight, R.L., Gutzwiller, K.J. (Eds.), Wildlife and Recreationists.<br />

Island Press, Washing<strong>to</strong>n, DC, pp. 297–311.<br />

OÕShea, T.J., Lefebvre, L.W., Beck, C.A., 2001. <strong>Florida</strong> <strong>manatees</strong>:<br />

perspectives on popluations, pain and protection. In: Dierauf, L.A.,<br />

Gulland, F.M.D. (Eds.), CRC Handbook of Marine Mammal<br />

Medicine. CRC Press, Boca Ra<strong>to</strong>n, pp. 31–43.<br />

Reynolds, J.E., 1999. Efforts <strong>to</strong> conserve the <strong>manatees</strong>. In: Twiss, J.R.,<br />

Reeves, R.R. (Eds.), Conservation and Management of Marine<br />

Mammals. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washing<strong>to</strong>n, DC, pp.<br />

267–295.<br />

S.M. Nowacek et al. / Biological Conservation 119 (2004) 517–523 523<br />

Richardson, W. John, Greene, Charles R., Malme, Charles I.,<br />

Thomson, Denis H., 1995. Marine Mammals and Noise. Academic<br />

Press, San Diego.<br />

Urick, R.J., 1983. Principles of Underwater Sound, third ed. McGraw-<br />

Hill, New York.<br />

Wells, R.S., Scott, M.D., 1997. Seasonal incidence of boat strikes on<br />

bottlenose dolphins near Sarasota, Flordia. Marine Mammal<br />

Science 13, 475–480.<br />

Wright, S.D., Ackerman, B.B., Bonde, R.K., Beck, C.A., Banowetz,<br />

D.J., 1995. Analysis of watercraft-related mortality of <strong>manatees</strong> in<br />

<strong>Florida</strong>, 1979–1991 In: T.J. OÕShea, B.B. Ackerman, H.F. Percival<br />

(Eds.), Population Biology of the <strong>Florida</strong> Manatee, National<br />

Biological Service Information Report, Ft. Collins, CO.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!