06.05.2013 Views

Perceived Self-Efficacy in Health Behaviour Change The Construct of

Perceived Self-Efficacy in Health Behaviour Change The Construct of

Perceived Self-Efficacy in Health Behaviour Change The Construct of

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Perceived</strong> <strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Efficacy</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Health</strong> <strong>Behaviour</strong> <strong>Change</strong><br />

Ralf Schwarzer 1 , Aleksandra Luszczynska 2 and Amelie U. Wiedemann 1<br />

1 Freie University, Berl<strong>in</strong><br />

2 Warsaw School <strong>of</strong> Social Psychology, Poland<br />

<strong>Perceived</strong> self-efficacy is a competence-based, prospective, and operative construct that can be<br />

used to predict, expla<strong>in</strong>, and change health behaviours. Numerous studies attest to the fact that<br />

self-efficacy is a substantial factor when it comes to chang<strong>in</strong>g behaviours. Moreover, the nature <strong>of</strong><br />

self-efficacy changes as people progress through a course <strong>of</strong> action, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with some<br />

motivation to change and end<strong>in</strong>g with habitual performance or relapse. Thus, research on phasespecific<br />

self-efficacy is an important agenda <strong>in</strong> psychology.<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>Construct</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Perceived</strong> <strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Efficacy</strong><br />

<strong>Perceived</strong> self-efficacy is concerned with <strong>in</strong>dividuals’ beliefs <strong>in</strong> their capability to exercise<br />

control over challeng<strong>in</strong>g demands and their own function<strong>in</strong>g. In his unify<strong>in</strong>g Social Cognitive<br />

<strong>The</strong>ory (SCT), Bandura (1977, 1997) hypothesized that expectations <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy are selfregulatory<br />

cognitions that determ<strong>in</strong>e whether <strong>in</strong>strumental actions will be <strong>in</strong>itiated, how much<br />

effort will be expended, and how long it will be susta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the face <strong>of</strong> obstacles and failures.<br />

In particular, self-efficacy has an <strong>in</strong>fluence on prepar<strong>in</strong>g for action because self-efficacy<br />

levels can enhance or impede motivation: self-efficacy not only <strong>in</strong>fluences the challenges that<br />

people decide to meet, but also how high they set their goals (e.g., “I <strong>in</strong>tend to reduce my<br />

smok<strong>in</strong>g,” or “I <strong>in</strong>tend to quit smok<strong>in</strong>g altogether”). That is, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with high selfefficacy<br />

select more challeng<strong>in</strong>g and ambitious goals than those low <strong>in</strong> self-efficacy.<br />

Additionally, persons with low self-efficacy harbour pessimistic thoughts about their likely<br />

accomplishments and personal development. However, self-efficacy is also directly related to<br />

behaviour, referr<strong>in</strong>g to the confidence that one can employ the skills necessary to resist<br />

temptation, cope with stress, and mobilize one's resources required to meet the situational<br />

demands. Dur<strong>in</strong>g action, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with high self-efficacy focus on opportunities, rather<br />

than on obstacles (e.g., “At my university there is a smok<strong>in</strong>g ban, anyway,” <strong>in</strong>stead <strong>of</strong> “<strong>The</strong>re<br />

are still a lot <strong>of</strong> ashtrays at my university.”). In sum, self-efficacy beliefs affect the persistence<br />

to cont<strong>in</strong>ue goal striv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the face <strong>of</strong> barriers and setbacks that may underm<strong>in</strong>e motivation.<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-efficacy is based on different sources (Bandura, 1997). First, self-efficacy beliefs can<br />

be enhanced through personal accomplishment or mastery, as far as success is attributed<br />

<strong>in</strong>ternally and can be repeated. A second source is vicarious experience. When a ‘model<br />

person’, that is similar to the <strong>in</strong>dividual, successfully masters a difficult situation, social<br />

comparison processes can enhance self-efficacy beliefs. Third, self-efficacy beliefs can also<br />

be enhanced through verbal persuasion by others (e.g., a health educator reassures a patient<br />

that she will certa<strong>in</strong>ly perform cancer screen<strong>in</strong>g properly due to her competence). <strong>The</strong> last<br />

source <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence is emotional arousal, that is, the person may experience no apprehension<br />

<strong>in</strong> a threaten<strong>in</strong>g situation and, as a result, may feel capable <strong>of</strong> master<strong>in</strong>g the situation. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

four <strong>in</strong>formational sources vary <strong>in</strong> strength and importance <strong>in</strong> the order presented here, with<br />

personal mastery be<strong>in</strong>g the strongest source <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy. However, self-efficacy can also<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluence behaviour change through the emotions that might arise while pursu<strong>in</strong>g the goal:<br />

optimistic self-beliefs about one’s own competence create positive affective states <strong>in</strong>stead <strong>of</strong><br />

negative ones (e.g., anxiety). While positive emotions may positively <strong>in</strong>fluence goal pursuit,<br />

negative emotions may generate cognitive confusion that leads to worse outcomes. <strong>Self</strong>efficacy<br />

may also affect an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s emotional states by the appraisal <strong>of</strong> stressful stimuli


as a threat, harm, or challenge. Thus, optimistic self-beliefs about one’s ability to execute an<br />

action successfully can <strong>in</strong>fluence one’s cognitive, motivational, and affective processes.<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-efficacy can be differentiated from other self-related cognitions. For example, selfefficacy<br />

is not the same as unrealistic optimism, s<strong>in</strong>ce it is based on experience and does not<br />

lead to unreasonable risk tak<strong>in</strong>g. Instead, it leads to venturesome behaviour that is with<strong>in</strong><br />

reach <strong>of</strong> one's capabilities. Compared to other similar constructs such as self-esteem, selfconcept,<br />

and sense <strong>of</strong> control, the essential characteristic <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy lies <strong>in</strong> three aspects:<br />

(a) self-efficacy implies an <strong>in</strong>ternal attribution (the person is the cause <strong>of</strong> the action), (b) it is<br />

prospective, referr<strong>in</strong>g to future behaviours, and (c) it is an operative construct, which means<br />

that this cognition is proximal to the critical behaviour.<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Efficacy</strong> Predicts <strong>Health</strong>-Promot<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Behaviour</strong>s<br />

A large body <strong>of</strong> research supports the predictive value <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy. To illustrate the<br />

association between perceived self-efficacy and health-promot<strong>in</strong>g behaviours, some examples<br />

from the areas <strong>of</strong> physical exercise and dietary behaviours are chosen. What both behaviours<br />

have <strong>in</strong> common is they have to be executed regularly to be effective and that most people do<br />

not meet the recommended criteria. Regard<strong>in</strong>g physical activity, studies employ<strong>in</strong>g a motion<br />

detector to measure physical activity objectively showed that 12.6 m<strong>in</strong>utes daily are spent on<br />

vigorous physical activity (Strauss, Rodzilsky, Burack, & Col<strong>in</strong>, 2001). Besides<br />

demographics, biological factors, physical environment factors, and physical activity<br />

characteristics, psychological determ<strong>in</strong>ants that are open to change, such as self-efficacy,<br />

<strong>in</strong>tention, and perceived barriers play a crucial role <strong>in</strong> the adoption <strong>of</strong> an active lifestyle<br />

(Sallis & Owen, 1999). In this regard, self-efficacy is expected to operate regardless <strong>of</strong> ethnic<br />

background and culture. However, there is some evidence <strong>of</strong> an ethnicity effect: optimistic<br />

self-beliefs are associated with moderate physical activity among White girls, but not among<br />

African-American girls (Bungum et al., 1999).<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-efficacy is related to objective measures (motion detectors) <strong>of</strong> current physical activity<br />

that monitor physical activity over a specified time period, and for example, related to higher<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> physical activity among 10- to 16-year-old people (Strauss et al., 2001). <strong>Self</strong>efficacy<br />

is also a strong predictor <strong>of</strong> future activity: <strong>in</strong> a sample <strong>of</strong> cardiac patients, selfefficacy<br />

was associated with physical activity measured after 4 and 12 months (Scholz,<br />

Sniehotta, & Schwarzer, 2005). Across a set <strong>of</strong> psychosocial and environmental variables,<br />

perceived confidence <strong>in</strong> the ability to be active was the only variable that differentiated<br />

between both, active and low-active African-American boys and girls (Trost, Pate, Ward,<br />

Saunders, & R<strong>in</strong>er, 1999). This result was obta<strong>in</strong>ed after the measurement <strong>of</strong> social cognitive<br />

variables by means <strong>of</strong> an accelerometer attached to an elastic belt and worn over the right hip<br />

for seven days. For boys, <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> sport organizations was another predictor <strong>of</strong> activity<br />

levels, while active girls also perceived more positive outcomes <strong>of</strong> activity.<br />

When assess<strong>in</strong>g relations between self-efficacy and physical activity, the conceptualization<br />

<strong>of</strong> self-efficacy may be more or less specific <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> behaviours or barriers. For example,<br />

optimistic beliefs about one’s competencies for moderate and vigorous physical exercise can<br />

be dist<strong>in</strong>guished. <strong>The</strong>se beliefs might be subdivided further, for example <strong>in</strong>to beliefs about<br />

overcom<strong>in</strong>g external and <strong>in</strong>ternal barriers to vigorous physical activity. Both k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> selfefficacy<br />

were found to be related to physical activity <strong>of</strong> high-school students (Dwyer, Allison,<br />

& Mak<strong>in</strong>, 1998).<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-efficacy can mediate between other cognitions and people’s physical activity.<br />

Employ<strong>in</strong>g self-efficacy <strong>in</strong> conjunction with constructs from the <strong>The</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> Planned<br />

<strong>Behaviour</strong> (TPB; Ajzen, 1991), Motl et al. (2002) searched for predictors <strong>of</strong> moderate and<br />

vigorous physical activity among Black and White adolescent girls. <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy turned out


to be the only significant direct predictor <strong>of</strong> moderate physical activity. For vigorous activity,<br />

two significant direct predictors were found: self-efficacy and behavioural control. Intentions,<br />

attitudes, and subjective norms were <strong>in</strong>terrelated and were moderately or strongly related to<br />

self-efficacy. Intentions, attitudes and subjective norms were only <strong>in</strong>directly related to<br />

physical activity, and they’re effects were mediated by self-efficacy.<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-efficacy might also mediate the relations between physical activity and other<br />

constructs, for example parental support for the child’s physical activity (Trost et al., 2003).<br />

Other studies provide evidence that self-efficacy is the strongest correlate <strong>of</strong> people’ physical<br />

activity, when compared with other cognitions such as perceived benefits, perceived barriers,<br />

and social norms (Wu, Pender, & Nouredd<strong>in</strong>e, 2003).<br />

Regard<strong>in</strong>g behaviour change, <strong>in</strong>tervention studies that aimed at an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy<br />

and healthy nutrition employed computer games to facilitate mastery experience. Based on<br />

SCT, the educational activities <strong>in</strong> the game aimed at <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the preference for healthy<br />

foods (Baranowski et al., 2003). Us<strong>in</strong>g multiple exposures, this approach <strong>in</strong>creased mastery<br />

experience <strong>in</strong> ask<strong>in</strong>g for healthy food at home and when eat<strong>in</strong>g out, as well as the skills to<br />

prepare healthy foods by means <strong>of</strong> virtual recipes and virtual food preparation. Compared to<br />

persons <strong>in</strong> control groups, people participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> such an <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong>creased their<br />

consumption <strong>of</strong> fruits and vegetables significantly (Baranowski et al., 2003).<br />

Interventions aimed at chang<strong>in</strong>g self-efficacy along with other social-cognitive variables<br />

were effective <strong>in</strong> chang<strong>in</strong>g physical activity and nutrition <strong>in</strong> children and adolescents. In a<br />

sample <strong>of</strong> over 6,000 children and adolescents, self-efficacy and <strong>in</strong>tentions determ<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

healthy food choices while self-efficacy and perceived social support determ<strong>in</strong>ed physical<br />

activity, as measured three years later (Edmundson et al., 1996). Treatment based on SCT<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ed with an <strong>in</strong>tervention aimed at <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g motivation affected self-efficacy levels<br />

and fruit and vegetable <strong>in</strong>take among people (Wilson et al., 2002).<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>Perceived</strong> <strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Efficacy</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Health</strong> <strong>Behaviour</strong> <strong>The</strong>ories<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-efficacy <strong>in</strong>stigates the adoption, <strong>in</strong>itiation, and ma<strong>in</strong>tenance <strong>of</strong> health-promot<strong>in</strong>g<br />

behaviours. Prom<strong>in</strong>ent theories <strong>of</strong> behaviour change such as the <strong>The</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> Planned <strong>Behaviour</strong><br />

(Ajzen, 1991), the Social Cognitive <strong>The</strong>ory (Bandura, 1977, 1997), the Transtheoretical<br />

Model (Prochaska, DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992), and the <strong>Health</strong> Action Process Approach<br />

(Schwarzer, 2008), <strong>in</strong>clude a variety <strong>of</strong> cognitions that either directly or <strong>in</strong>directly <strong>in</strong>fluence<br />

health behaviours (Lippke, & Ziegelmann, 2008).<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>The</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> Planned <strong>Behaviour</strong> (TPB; Ajzen, 1991) claims that <strong>in</strong>tention is the most<br />

proximal predictor <strong>of</strong> behaviour. Cognitions that affect a specific <strong>in</strong>tention are (a) attitude<br />

toward the behaviour (evaluation <strong>of</strong> perform<strong>in</strong>g the behaviour), (b) subjective norm, (the<br />

extent to which a person believes that significant others would want them to perform a<br />

behaviour), and (c) perceived behavioural control (perception about be<strong>in</strong>g able to perform a<br />

specific behaviour). <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy and behavioural control may be seen as almost synonymous<br />

constructs.<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Social Cognitive <strong>The</strong>ory (SCT; Bandura, 1997), personal sense <strong>of</strong> control<br />

facilitates a change <strong>of</strong> health behaviour. <strong>Perceived</strong> self-efficacy perta<strong>in</strong>s to a sense <strong>of</strong> control<br />

over one's environment and behaviour. <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy expectations are cognitions that<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>e whether health behaviour change will be <strong>in</strong>itiated, how much effort will be<br />

<strong>in</strong>vested, and how long efforts will be ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the face <strong>of</strong> setbacks (Schwarzer, 2008).<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-efficacy beliefs affect the amount <strong>of</strong> effort to change risk behaviour. <strong>Perceived</strong> selfefficacy<br />

is directly related to health behaviour, but it also affects health behaviours <strong>in</strong>directly,<br />

through the impact on goals. Both, TPB and SCT are not theories developed particularly to


expla<strong>in</strong> and predict health behaviour. However, a large body <strong>of</strong> research underl<strong>in</strong>es their<br />

value regard<strong>in</strong>g health-related behaviour (Conner & Norman, 2005).<br />

<strong>The</strong> Transtheoretical Model (TTM; Prochaska, DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992) proposes<br />

five stages <strong>of</strong> health behaviour change. <strong>The</strong> first one is the precontemplation stage, <strong>in</strong> which<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals do not consider mak<strong>in</strong>g any behaviour change. In the contemplation stage,<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals consider chang<strong>in</strong>g a specific health behaviour, but they have not yet decided to<br />

make any changes. In the preparation stage, they prepare to change the behaviour. In the<br />

action stage, a new goal behaviour is <strong>in</strong>itiated. When the action is performed for a longer time<br />

period, the ma<strong>in</strong>tenance stage is reached (Prochaska et al., 1992). A sixth stage is sometimes<br />

mentioned, the term<strong>in</strong>ation stage, <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>dividuals no longer experience any temptation to<br />

revert to their old habits. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the TTM, self-efficacy and perceived positive (“pros”)<br />

and negative (“cons”) outcomes are seen as the ma<strong>in</strong> social-cognitive variables that change<br />

across the stages. <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy beliefs are typically low <strong>in</strong> early stages and <strong>in</strong>crease when<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals move on to the later stages.<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>Health</strong> Action Process Approach (HAPA; Schwarzer, 2008) suggests a dist<strong>in</strong>ction<br />

between (a) a motivation phase <strong>in</strong> which pre<strong>in</strong>tentional processes may lead to a behavioural<br />

<strong>in</strong>tention, and (b) a volition phase, <strong>in</strong> which post<strong>in</strong>tentional processes may lead to actual<br />

health behaviour.<br />

Different patterns <strong>of</strong> social-cognitive predictors may emerge <strong>in</strong> both phases, and particular<br />

attention is paid to post<strong>in</strong>tentional mechanisms. <strong>The</strong> HAPA applies to all health behaviours.<br />

More precisely, <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial motivation phase, a person develops an <strong>in</strong>tention to act. In this<br />

phase, risk perception (“I have a high risk <strong>of</strong> suffer<strong>in</strong>g from diabetes because <strong>of</strong> my body<br />

weight”) is merely seen as a distal antecedent with<strong>in</strong> the motivation phase. Risk perception <strong>in</strong><br />

itself is not enough to entice a person to form an <strong>in</strong>tention. Rather, it sets the stage for a<br />

contemplation process and further elaboration about consequences and competencies.<br />

Similarly, outcome expectancies (“If I eat healthy foods, I will reduce my weight”) are chiefly<br />

seen as be<strong>in</strong>g important <strong>in</strong> the motivation phase, when a person balances the pros and cons <strong>of</strong><br />

the consequences <strong>of</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> behaviour. Further, one needs to believe <strong>in</strong> one's capability to<br />

perform a desired action (“I am capable <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>itiat<strong>in</strong>g a healthier diet <strong>in</strong> spite <strong>of</strong> temptations”),<br />

otherwise one will fail to <strong>in</strong>itiate that action. Outcome expectancies operate <strong>in</strong> concert with<br />

perceived self-efficacy, both <strong>of</strong> them contribut<strong>in</strong>g substantially to the formation <strong>of</strong> an<br />

<strong>in</strong>tention.<br />

In the subsequent volitional phase, after a person has developed an <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ation toward<br />

adopt<strong>in</strong>g a particular health behaviour, the “good <strong>in</strong>tention” has to be transformed <strong>in</strong>to<br />

detailed <strong>in</strong>structions on how to perform the desired action. <strong>The</strong>se plans, which specify the<br />

when, where, and how <strong>of</strong> a desired action, carry the structure <strong>of</strong> “When situation S arises, I<br />

will perform response R.” Thus, a global <strong>in</strong>tention can be specified by a set <strong>of</strong> subord<strong>in</strong>ate<br />

<strong>in</strong>tentions and action plans that conta<strong>in</strong> algorithms <strong>of</strong> action sequences. <strong>The</strong> volition phase is<br />

also strongly affected by self-efficacy. <strong>The</strong> number and quality <strong>of</strong> action plans depend on<br />

one’s perceived competence and experience, and it has been shown that recovery self-efficacy<br />

predicts both action plans as well as cop<strong>in</strong>g plans, which are plans to overcome anticipated<br />

barriers (Ziegelmann & Lippke, 2007). <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy beliefs <strong>in</strong>fluence the cognitive<br />

construction <strong>of</strong> specific action plans, for example by visualiz<strong>in</strong>g scenarios that may guide goal<br />

atta<strong>in</strong>ment. <strong>The</strong> volition phase <strong>in</strong>cludes the processes <strong>of</strong> tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>itiative, ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

behaviour change, and manag<strong>in</strong>g relapse, and self-efficacy beliefs might be specific for these<br />

processes (see Schwarzer & Luszczynska, 2008; Marlatt, Baer, & Quigley, 1995).<br />

As seen <strong>in</strong> the above-mentioned examples, most prom<strong>in</strong>ent health behaviour theories<br />

<strong>in</strong>clude self-efficacy beliefs (or synonymous constructs). <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy is a proximal and<br />

direct predictor <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tention or behaviour. Its effects on behaviour might be mediated by other<br />

cognitions, such as <strong>in</strong>tentions (see SCT, TPB, HAPA). Across stages <strong>of</strong> change, an <strong>in</strong>crease


<strong>of</strong> self-efficacy is expected (see HAPA, TTM), and self-efficacy can predict movements<br />

between such stages (Wiedemann, Lippke, Reuter, Schüz, Ziegelmann, & Schwarzer, <strong>in</strong><br />

press).<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Efficacy</strong> Reflect<strong>in</strong>g Different Challenges with<strong>in</strong> the <strong>Behaviour</strong> <strong>Change</strong> Process<br />

<strong>Perceived</strong> self-efficacy has been found to be important at all stages <strong>in</strong> the health behaviour<br />

change process (Bandura, 1997), but it does not always constitute exactly the same construct.<br />

Its mean<strong>in</strong>g depends on the particular situation <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals who may be more or less<br />

advanced <strong>in</strong> the change process. Action self-efficacy, cop<strong>in</strong>g self-efficacy, and recovery selfefficacy<br />

have been dist<strong>in</strong>guished by Marlatt, Baer, and Quigley (1995) <strong>in</strong> the doma<strong>in</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

addictive behaviours. <strong>The</strong> rationale for several phase-specific self-efficacy beliefs is that<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g the course <strong>of</strong> health behaviour change, different beliefs are required to master different<br />

tasks. For example, a person might be confident <strong>in</strong> his or her capability to make an attempt to<br />

quit a certa<strong>in</strong> behaviour (i.e., high action self-efficacy), but might not be very confident to<br />

resume abst<strong>in</strong>ence after a lapse (low recovery self-efficacy).<br />

Preaction self-efficacy (also called action self-efficacy or task self-efficacy) refers to the<br />

first phase <strong>of</strong> the process, <strong>in</strong> which an <strong>in</strong>dividual does not yet act, but develops a motivation<br />

to do so. Individuals high <strong>in</strong> preaction self-efficacy imag<strong>in</strong>e success, and anticipate potential<br />

outcomes <strong>of</strong> diverse strategies. While preaction self-efficacy is <strong>in</strong>strumental <strong>in</strong> the motivation<br />

phase, the two follow<strong>in</strong>g constructs are <strong>in</strong>strumental <strong>in</strong> the subsequent volition phase, and<br />

can, therefore, be summarized under the head<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> “volitional self-efficacy.”<br />

Ma<strong>in</strong>tenance self-efficacy (also called cop<strong>in</strong>g self-efficacy) represents optimistic beliefs<br />

about one’s capability to deal with barriers that arise dur<strong>in</strong>g the ma<strong>in</strong>tenance period (the term<br />

“cop<strong>in</strong>g self-efficacy” has also been used <strong>in</strong> a different sense; therefore, we prefer the term<br />

“ma<strong>in</strong>tenance self-efficacy”). A novel health behaviour might turn out to be much more<br />

difficult to adhere to than expected, but a self-efficacious person responds confidently with<br />

better strategies, more effort, and prolonged persistence <strong>in</strong> overcom<strong>in</strong>g such hurdles. Once an<br />

action has been taken, <strong>in</strong>dividuals with high ma<strong>in</strong>tenance self-efficacy <strong>in</strong>vest more effort and<br />

persist longer than those who are less self-efficacious.<br />

Recovery self-efficacy addresses the experience <strong>of</strong> failure, lapses, and setbacks. High selfefficacious<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals are optimistic to get back on track after be<strong>in</strong>g derailed. <strong>The</strong>y trust their<br />

competence to rega<strong>in</strong> control after a setback and to reduce harm (Marlatt et al., 2005).<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is a functional difference between these self-efficacy constructs, whereas their temporal<br />

sequence is less important. Different phase-specific self-efficacy beliefs may be harbored at<br />

the same po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> time. <strong>The</strong> assumption is that they operate <strong>in</strong> a different manner. For<br />

example, recovery self-efficacy is most functional when it comes to resum<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>terrupted<br />

cha<strong>in</strong> <strong>of</strong> action, whereas action self-efficacy is most functional when fac<strong>in</strong>g a novel<br />

challeng<strong>in</strong>g demand (Luszczynska, Mazurkiewicz, Ziegelmann, & Schwarzer, 2007;<br />

Luszczynska & Sutton, 2006). This dist<strong>in</strong>ction between phase-specific self-efficacy beliefs<br />

has proven useful <strong>in</strong> various doma<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> behaviour change. Preaction self-efficacy tends to<br />

predict <strong>in</strong>tentions, whereas ma<strong>in</strong>tenance self-efficacy tends to predict behaviours. Individuals<br />

who had recovered from a setback needed different self-beliefs than those who had<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed their levels <strong>of</strong> activity (Scholz et al., 2005). Rodgers, Hall, Blanchard, McAuley,<br />

and Munroe (2002) have found evidence for phase-specific self-efficacy beliefs <strong>in</strong> the doma<strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong> exercise behaviour (i.e., task self-efficacy, cop<strong>in</strong>g self-efficacy, and schedul<strong>in</strong>g selfefficacy).<br />

In studies apply<strong>in</strong>g the HAPA, phase-specific self-efficacy differed <strong>in</strong> the effects on<br />

various preventive health behaviours such as breast self-exam<strong>in</strong>ation (Luszczynska &<br />

Schwarzer, 2003), dietary behaviours (Renner et al., 2008), and physical exercise (Renner,


Spivak, Kwon & Schwarzer, 2007; Scholz et al., 2005; Schwarzer, Luszczynska, Ziegelmann,<br />

Scholz, & Lippke, 2008).<br />

<strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Efficacy</strong> <strong>in</strong> Dietary <strong>Behaviour</strong> <strong>Change</strong>: Two Studies<br />

<strong>Perceived</strong> self-efficacy is supposed to act as a mediator or as a moderator between<br />

<strong>in</strong>tentions and behaviours. Studies are needed to exam<strong>in</strong>e the mechanisms that might play a<br />

role <strong>in</strong> different contexts for different samples and different health behaviours. Study I aims at<br />

improv<strong>in</strong>g dietary self-efficacy and at compar<strong>in</strong>g such self-efficacy <strong>in</strong>terventions with a<br />

plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention.<br />

Our general assumption is that people need a m<strong>in</strong>imum level <strong>of</strong> perceived self-efficacy for<br />

most processes <strong>of</strong> health behaviour change. Thus, self-efficacy may serve as a moderator <strong>of</strong><br />

various mediat<strong>in</strong>g processes. Study II analyzes whether cop<strong>in</strong>g plann<strong>in</strong>g (mediator variable)<br />

mediates the effect <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tentions (<strong>in</strong>dependent variable) on dietary behaviour (dependent<br />

variable) as a function <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy levels (moderator).<br />

Study I: Improv<strong>in</strong>g Fruit and Vegetable Consumption: A <strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Efficacy</strong> Intervention<br />

Compared to a Comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>Self</strong>-<strong>Efficacy</strong> and Plann<strong>in</strong>g Intervention<br />

Effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>terventions target<strong>in</strong>g self-efficacy alone or comb<strong>in</strong>ed with action plans were<br />

exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the context <strong>of</strong> fruit and vegetable consumption (Luszczynska, Tryburcy, &<br />

Schwarzer, 2007). E-mail messages were sent to a self-efficacy group, a comb<strong>in</strong>ed selfefficacy<br />

and action plann<strong>in</strong>g group, and a control group. At a 6-month follow-up, 200 adults<br />

reported their fruit and vegetable consumption, along with current levels <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy and<br />

plann<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>The</strong> two experimental groups ga<strong>in</strong>ed equally from the <strong>in</strong>terventions, as documented<br />

by changes <strong>in</strong> behaviour (see Figure 1). In both <strong>in</strong>tervention groups, change <strong>in</strong> respective<br />

cognitions predicted change <strong>in</strong> fruit and vegetable consumption.<br />

Figure 1. <strong>Change</strong>s <strong>in</strong> fruit and vegetable consumption <strong>in</strong> two <strong>in</strong>tervention groups and the<br />

control group


<strong>The</strong> results also shed light on the work<strong>in</strong>g mechanism <strong>of</strong> these <strong>in</strong>terventions: self-efficacy<br />

change mediated the effects <strong>of</strong> the self-efficacy <strong>in</strong>tervention on change <strong>in</strong> fruit and vegetable<br />

consumption, whereas changes <strong>in</strong> both self-efficacy and plann<strong>in</strong>g mediated the effects <strong>of</strong> the<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>tervention (address<strong>in</strong>g self-efficacy and plann<strong>in</strong>g). <strong>The</strong> results support Bandura’s<br />

(1997) suggestions, show<strong>in</strong>g that an enhancement <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy by means <strong>of</strong> verbal<br />

persuasion and positive emotion may result <strong>in</strong> behaviour change. <strong>The</strong> results are also <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e<br />

with studies show<strong>in</strong>g that plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terventions may work because they encourage a person<br />

to engage <strong>in</strong> more frequent use <strong>of</strong> strategy plann<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Study II: Moderated Mediation <strong>of</strong> Dietary <strong>Behaviour</strong> <strong>Change</strong> <strong>in</strong> South Korean Women<br />

Previous analyses (Lippke et al., <strong>in</strong> press) have confirmed the partial mediation <strong>of</strong> the<br />

<strong>in</strong>tention-behaviour relationship by plann<strong>in</strong>g, and identified levels <strong>of</strong> perceived self-efficacy<br />

as moderators <strong>of</strong> this mediation. As this study predicted dietary behaviours, and not<br />

behavioural change, the analyses needed to be replicated. This was done <strong>in</strong> a longitud<strong>in</strong>al<br />

sample <strong>of</strong> 358 women that responded to two questionnaires six months apart <strong>in</strong> a study <strong>in</strong><br />

South Korea (Gutierrez et al., 2009; Renner et al., 2008).<br />

Moderated mediation analyses <strong>in</strong>dicated a moderation effect (see Figure 2). <strong>The</strong> path<br />

diagram consists <strong>of</strong> a mediator model (to expla<strong>in</strong> plann<strong>in</strong>g) and a dependent variable model<br />

(to expla<strong>in</strong> behaviour). First, <strong>in</strong>tentions emerged as the best predictor <strong>of</strong> cop<strong>in</strong>g plann<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

followed by the <strong>in</strong>teraction between <strong>in</strong>tentions and self-efficacy, whereas the ma<strong>in</strong> effect <strong>of</strong><br />

self-efficacy was rather negligible, overall account<strong>in</strong>g for 18% <strong>of</strong> the plann<strong>in</strong>g variance.<br />

Second, basel<strong>in</strong>e behaviour was the best predictor <strong>of</strong> Time 2 low-fat diet, followed by<br />

plann<strong>in</strong>g, whereas <strong>in</strong>tentions did not make a contribution, overall account<strong>in</strong>g for 51% <strong>of</strong> the<br />

behaviour variance. Thus, there is a full mediation <strong>of</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tention-behaviour relation via<br />

plann<strong>in</strong>g, moderated by self-efficacy.<br />

Figure 2. Moderated mediation model for South Korean women<br />

This analysis corroborated the hypothesized mediation effect, conditional upon the value <strong>of</strong><br />

self-efficacy, underscor<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that plann<strong>in</strong>g translated <strong>in</strong>tentions <strong>in</strong>to behaviour, but<br />

not with<strong>in</strong> the subgroup <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals who had very low levels <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy.


Discussion: Implications for Interventions<br />

If <strong>in</strong>dividuals are <strong>in</strong> a more advanced stage <strong>in</strong> the health behaviour change process and have<br />

already given it a try, <strong>in</strong>terventions should aim at ma<strong>in</strong>tenance self-efficacy. For example, if<br />

people try to adhere to a healthy nutrition, an <strong>in</strong>tervention should address their ability to deal<br />

with specific barriers that arise dur<strong>in</strong>g the ma<strong>in</strong>tenance phase, such as high-risk situations that<br />

impose temptations and might be a trap for relapses. <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy that refers to the<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>tenance <strong>of</strong> physical activity, however, is a poor predictor <strong>of</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tensity <strong>of</strong> relapse to a<br />

sedentary life style. People who relapsed to their old habits (e.g., not us<strong>in</strong>g condoms) should<br />

be treated <strong>in</strong> a manner that enhances their beliefs about their ability to rega<strong>in</strong> control after a<br />

setback. It may not be sufficient if an <strong>in</strong>tervention raises optimistic beliefs about be<strong>in</strong>g able to<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> condom use. Instead, improved beliefs about the ability to renegotiate condom use<br />

may get people who experienced a relapse back on track (recovery self-efficacy).<br />

Future <strong>in</strong>terventions for chang<strong>in</strong>g health behaviours and health perceptions should dist<strong>in</strong>guish<br />

between three research perspectives: the first perspective, employed most <strong>of</strong>ten so far, focuses<br />

on enhanc<strong>in</strong>g behaviour-specific self-efficacy but does not take <strong>in</strong>to account the stages with<strong>in</strong><br />

the health behaviour change process. <strong>The</strong> second perspective might focus on general selfefficacy<br />

beliefs, under the assumption that if an <strong>in</strong>tervention aims at more general beliefs, it<br />

would affect a wider range <strong>of</strong> behaviours and life skills. <strong>The</strong>se, <strong>in</strong> turn, would then be<br />

transferred to other behaviours. <strong>The</strong> focus <strong>in</strong> the third approach could be on the dist<strong>in</strong>ct<br />

m<strong>in</strong>dsets <strong>of</strong> those people currently <strong>in</strong> the motivation (preaction self-efficacy) or volition<br />

(ma<strong>in</strong>tenance and recovery self-efficacy) stages. Here, the object would be to enhance<br />

optimistic beliefs that are exclusive for a particular stage. Such a process strategy, as part <strong>of</strong> a<br />

more comprehensive health behaviour theory, may <strong>in</strong>crease the likelihood that people would<br />

reduce their risk-tak<strong>in</strong>g and adhere to a healthier lifestyle.<br />

Contact Author:<br />

Ralf Schwarzer<br />

Freie Universität Berl<strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>Health</strong> Psychology Department<br />

Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berl<strong>in</strong>, Germany<br />

FAX +49 30 838 55634<br />

Email: health@zedat.fu-berl<strong>in</strong>.de<br />

References<br />

Ajzen, I. (1991). <strong>The</strong> theory <strong>of</strong> planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human<br />

Decision Processes, 50, 179-211.<br />

Bandura, A. (1977) <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy: Toward a unify<strong>in</strong>g theory <strong>of</strong> behavioral change,<br />

Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.<br />

Bandura, A. (1997). <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy: <strong>The</strong> exercise <strong>of</strong> control. New York: Freeman.


Baranowski, T., Baranowski, J., Cullen, K.W., Marsh, T., Islam, N., Zakerei, I., Honess-<br />

Morreale, L., & deMoor, C. (2003) Squire’s quest: dietary outcome evaluation <strong>of</strong> a<br />

multimedia game, American Journal <strong>of</strong> Preventive Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 24, 52-61.<br />

Bungum, T., Pate, R., Dowda, M., & V<strong>in</strong>cent, M. (1999). Correlates <strong>of</strong> physical activity<br />

among African-American and Caucasian female adolescents. American Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Health</strong> Behavior, 23, 25-31.<br />

Conner, M. T. & Norman, P. (Eds.), Predict<strong>in</strong>g health behaviour: Research and practice with<br />

social cognition models (2nd Ed.). Maidenhead: Open University Press.<br />

Dwyer, J. J., Allison, K. R., & Mak<strong>in</strong>, S. (1998). Internal structure <strong>of</strong> a measure <strong>of</strong> self-<br />

efficacy <strong>in</strong> physical activity among high-school students. Social Science and Medic<strong>in</strong>e,<br />

46, 1175-1182.<br />

Edmundson, E., Parcel, G. S., Feldman, H.A., Elder, J., Perry, C. L., Johnson, C. C, Willston,<br />

B. J., Stone, E. J., Yang, M., Lytle, L., & Webber, L. (1996). <strong>The</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> the child<br />

and adolescent trial for cardiovascular health upon psychosocial determ<strong>in</strong>ants <strong>of</strong> diet<br />

and physical activity behavior. Preventive Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 25, 442-454.<br />

Gutiérrez-Doña, B., Lippke, S., Renner, B., Kwon, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2009). <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy<br />

and plann<strong>in</strong>g predict dietary behaviors <strong>in</strong> Costa Rican and South Korean women: Two<br />

moderated mediation analyses. Applied Psychology: <strong>Health</strong> and Well-Be<strong>in</strong>g, 1, 91-<br />

104.<br />

Lippke, S., Wiedemann, A., Ziegelmann, J. P., Reuter, T., & Schwarzer, R. (<strong>in</strong> press). <strong>Self</strong>-<br />

efficacy moderates the mediation <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tentions <strong>in</strong>to behavior via plans. American<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Health</strong> Promotion.<br />

Lippke, S., & Ziegelmann, J. P. (2008). <strong>The</strong>ory-based health behavior change: Develop<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

test<strong>in</strong>g and apply<strong>in</strong>g theories for evidence-based <strong>in</strong>terventions. Applied Psychology:<br />

International Review, 57, 698-716.


Luszczynska, A., Mazurkiewicz, M., Ziegelman J. P., & Schwarzer, R. (2007). Recovery self-<br />

efficacy and <strong>in</strong>tention as predictors <strong>of</strong> runn<strong>in</strong>g or jogg<strong>in</strong>g behavior: A cross-lagged<br />

panel analysis over a two-year period. Psychology <strong>of</strong> Sport and Exercise, 8, 247-260.<br />

Luszczynska, A., & Schwarzer, R. (2003). Plann<strong>in</strong>g and self-efficacy <strong>in</strong> the adoption and<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>tenance <strong>of</strong> breast self-exam<strong>in</strong>ation: A longitud<strong>in</strong>al study on self-regulatory<br />

cognitions. Psychology and <strong>Health</strong>, 18, 93-108.<br />

Luszczynska, A., Tryburcy, M., & Schwarzer, R. (2007). Improv<strong>in</strong>g fruit and vegetable<br />

consumption: A self-efficacy <strong>in</strong>tervention compared to a comb<strong>in</strong>ed self-efficacy and<br />

plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention. <strong>Health</strong> Education Research, 22, 630-638.<br />

Luszczynska, A., & Sutton, S. (2006). Physical activity after cardiac rehabilitation: Evidence<br />

that different types <strong>of</strong> self-efficacy are important <strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ers and relapsers.<br />

Rehabilitation Psychology, 51(4), 314-321.<br />

Marlatt, G. A., Baer, J. S. and Quigley, L. A. (1995) <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy and addictive behavior, <strong>in</strong><br />

A. Bandura (ed), <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy <strong>in</strong> chang<strong>in</strong>g societies (pp. 289-315). New York:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Motl, R. W., Dishman, R. K., Saunders, R. P., Dowda, M., Felton, G., Ward, D. S., & Pate, R.<br />

R. (2002). Exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g social-cognitive determ<strong>in</strong>ants <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tention and physical activity<br />

among black and white adolescent girls us<strong>in</strong>g structural equation model<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>Health</strong><br />

Psychology, 21, 459-467.<br />

Prochaska, J. O., DiClemente, C. C., & Norcross, J. C. (1992). In search <strong>of</strong> how people<br />

change: Applications to addictive behaviors. American Psychologist, 47, 1102-1114.<br />

Renner, B., Kwon, S., Yang, B.-H., Paik, K-C., Kim, S. H., Roh, S., Song, J., & Schwarzer,<br />

R. (2008). Social-cognitive predictors <strong>of</strong> dietary behaviors <strong>in</strong> South Korean men and<br />

women. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Behavioral Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 15(1), 4-13.


Renner, B., Spivak, Y., Kwon, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2007). Does age make a difference?<br />

Predict<strong>in</strong>g physical activity <strong>of</strong> South Koreans. Psychology and Ag<strong>in</strong>g, 22, 482-493.<br />

Rodgers, W. M., Hall, C. R., Blanchard, C. M., McAuley, E., & Munroe, K. J. (2002). Task<br />

and schedul<strong>in</strong>g self-efficacy as predictors <strong>of</strong> exercise behavior. Psychology & <strong>Health</strong>,<br />

27, 405-416.<br />

Sallis, J. F., & Owen, N. (1999). Physical activity and behavioral medic<strong>in</strong>e. Thousand Oaks,<br />

CA: Sage.<br />

Scholz, U., Sniehotta, F. F., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). Predict<strong>in</strong>g physical exercise <strong>in</strong> cardiac<br />

rehabilitation: <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> phase-specific self-efficacy beliefs. Journal <strong>of</strong> Sport &<br />

Exercise Psychology, 27, 135-151.<br />

Schwarzer, R. (2008). Model<strong>in</strong>g health behavior change: How to predict and modify the<br />

adoption and ma<strong>in</strong>tenance <strong>of</strong> health behaviors. Applied Psychology: An International<br />

Review, 57(1), 1-29.<br />

Schwarzer, R., & Luszczynska, A. (2007). <strong>Self</strong>-efficacy. In M. Gerrard & K. D. McCaul<br />

(Eds.), <strong>Health</strong> behavior constructs: <strong>The</strong>ory, measurement, and research. National<br />

Cancer Institute Website. Retrieved from http://cancercontrol.cancer.gov/constructs<br />

Schwarzer, R., & Luszczynska, A., (2008). How to overcome health-compromis<strong>in</strong>g<br />

behaviors: <strong>The</strong> health action process approach. European Psychologist, 2, 141-151.<br />

Schwarzer, R., Luszczynska, A., Ziegelmann, J. P., Scholz, U., & Lippke, S. (2008). Social-<br />

cognitive predictors <strong>of</strong> physical exercise adherence: Three longitud<strong>in</strong>al studies <strong>in</strong><br />

rehabilitation. <strong>Health</strong> Psychology, 27(1, Suppl.), S54-S63.<br />

Strauss, R. S., Rodzilsky, D., Burack, G., & Col<strong>in</strong>, M. (2001). Psychosocial correlates <strong>of</strong><br />

physical activity <strong>in</strong> healthy children. Archives <strong>of</strong> Paediatrics and Adolescent Medic<strong>in</strong>e,<br />

155, 897-902.


Trost, S. G., Pate, R. R., Ward, D. S., Saunders, R., & R<strong>in</strong>er, W. (1999). Determ<strong>in</strong>ants <strong>of</strong><br />

physical activity <strong>in</strong> active and low-active, sixth-grade African American youth.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> School <strong>Health</strong>, 69, 29-34.<br />

Trost, S. G., Sallis, J. F., Pate, R. R., Freedson, P. S., Taylor, W. C., & Dowda, M. (2003).<br />

Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g a model <strong>of</strong> parental <strong>in</strong>fluence on youth physical activity. American Journal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Preventive Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 25, 277-282.<br />

Wiedemann, A. U., Lippke, S., Reuter, T., Schüz, B., Ziegelmann, J. P., & Schwarzer, R.<br />

(2009). Prediction <strong>of</strong> stage transitions <strong>in</strong> fruit and vegetable <strong>in</strong>take. <strong>Health</strong> Education<br />

Research, 24, 596-607.<br />

Wilson, D. K., Friend, R., Teasley, N., Green, S., Reaves, I. L., & Sica, D.A . (2002).<br />

Motivational versus social cognitive <strong>in</strong>terventions for promot<strong>in</strong>g fruit and vegetable<br />

<strong>in</strong>take and physical activity <strong>in</strong> African American adolescents. Annals <strong>of</strong> Behavioral<br />

Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 24, 310-319.<br />

Wu, S. Y., Pender, N., & Nouredd<strong>in</strong>e, S. (2003). Gender differences <strong>in</strong> the psychosocial<br />

cognitive correlates <strong>of</strong> physical activity among Taiwanese adolescents: A structural<br />

equation model<strong>in</strong>g approach. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Behavioral Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 10, 93-<br />

105.<br />

Ziegelmann, J. P., & Lippke, S. (2007). Plann<strong>in</strong>g and strategy use <strong>in</strong> health behavior change:<br />

A life span view. International Journal <strong>of</strong> Behavioral Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 14, 30-39.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!