16.05.2013 Views

Understanding Statistics in the Behavioral Sciences ... - NelsonBrain

Understanding Statistics in the Behavioral Sciences ... - NelsonBrain

Understanding Statistics in the Behavioral Sciences ... - NelsonBrain

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Licensed to:<br />

6 C H A P T E R 1 <strong>Statistics</strong> and Scientifi c Method<br />

DEFINITIONS<br />

Scientifi c Method<br />

Although <strong>the</strong> scientifi c method uses both reason<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>in</strong>tuition for establish<strong>in</strong>g truth,<br />

its reliance on objective assessment is what differentiates this method from <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs.<br />

At <strong>the</strong> heart of science lies <strong>the</strong> scientifi c experiment. The method of science is ra<strong>the</strong>r<br />

straightforward. By some means, usually by reason<strong>in</strong>g deductively from exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ory<br />

or <strong>in</strong>ductively from exist<strong>in</strong>g facts or through <strong>in</strong>tuition, <strong>the</strong> scientist arrives at a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis<br />

about some feature of reality. He or she <strong>the</strong>n designs an experiment to objectively<br />

test <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. The data from <strong>the</strong> experiment are <strong>the</strong>n analyzed statistically, and<br />

<strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis is ei<strong>the</strong>r supported or rejected. The feature of overrid<strong>in</strong>g importance<br />

<strong>in</strong> this methodology is that no matter what <strong>the</strong> scientist believes is true regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

hypo<strong>the</strong>sis under study, <strong>the</strong> experiment provides <strong>the</strong> basis for an objective evaluation of<br />

<strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. The data from <strong>the</strong> experiment force a conclusion consonant with reality.<br />

Thus, scientifi c methodology has a built-<strong>in</strong> safeguard for ensur<strong>in</strong>g that truth assertions<br />

of any sort about reality must conform to what is demonstrated to be objectively true<br />

about <strong>the</strong> phenomena before <strong>the</strong> assertions are given <strong>the</strong> status of scientifi c truth.<br />

An important aspect of this methodology is that <strong>the</strong> experimenter can hold <strong>in</strong>correct<br />

hunches, and <strong>the</strong> data will expose <strong>the</strong>m. The hunches can <strong>the</strong>n be revised <strong>in</strong> light of <strong>the</strong><br />

data and retested. This methodology, although sometimes pa<strong>in</strong>stak<strong>in</strong>gly slow, has a selfcorrect<strong>in</strong>g<br />

feature that, over <strong>the</strong> long run, has a high probability of yield<strong>in</strong>g truth. S<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong><br />

this textbook we emphasize statistical analysis ra<strong>the</strong>r than experimental design, we cannot<br />

spend a great deal of time discuss<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> design of experiments. Never<strong>the</strong>less, some<br />

experimental design will be covered because it is so <strong>in</strong>tertw<strong>in</strong>ed with statistical analysis.<br />

In discuss<strong>in</strong>g this and o<strong>the</strong>r material throughout <strong>the</strong> book, we shall be us<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong><br />

technical terms. The terms and <strong>the</strong>ir defi nitions follow:<br />

◆ Population A population is <strong>the</strong> complete set of <strong>in</strong>dividuals, objects, or scores<br />

that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigator is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g. In an actual experiment, <strong>the</strong> population<br />

is <strong>the</strong> larger group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals from which <strong>the</strong> subjects run <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> experiment<br />

have been taken.<br />

◆ Sample A sample is a subset of <strong>the</strong> population. In an experiment, for economical<br />

reasons, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigator usually collects data on a smaller group of subjects<br />

than <strong>the</strong> entire population. This smaller group is called <strong>the</strong> sample.<br />

◆ Variable A variable is any property or characteristic of some event, object, or<br />

person that may have different values at different times depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> conditions.<br />

Height, weight, reaction time, and drug dosage are examples of variables.<br />

A variable should be contrasted with a constant, which, of course, does not have<br />

different values at different times. An example is <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>the</strong>matical constant π; it<br />

always has <strong>the</strong> same value (3.14 to two-decimal-place accuracy).<br />

◆ Independent variable (IV) The <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>in</strong> an experiment is <strong>the</strong><br />

variable that is systematically manipulated by <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigator. In most experiments,<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigator is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> effect that one variable,<br />

say, variable A, has on one or more o<strong>the</strong>r variables. To do so, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigator manipulates<br />

<strong>the</strong> levels of variable A and measures <strong>the</strong> effect on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r variables.<br />

Variable A is called <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependent variable because its levels are controlled<br />

by <strong>the</strong> experimenter, <strong>in</strong>dependent of any change <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r variables. To illustrate,<br />

an <strong>in</strong>vestigator might be <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> effect of alcohol on social behavior.<br />

To <strong>in</strong>vestigate this, he or she would probably vary <strong>the</strong> amount of alcohol<br />

Copyright 2011 Cengage Learn<strong>in</strong>g. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, <strong>in</strong> whole or <strong>in</strong> part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from <strong>the</strong> eBook and/or eChapter(s).<br />

Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect <strong>the</strong> overall learn<strong>in</strong>g experience. Cengage Learn<strong>in</strong>g reserves <strong>the</strong> right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!