29.06.2013 Views

Social context and cognition in SLA: a variationist perspective elaine ...

Social context and cognition in SLA: a variationist perspective elaine ...

Social context and cognition in SLA: a variationist perspective elaine ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

A <strong>variationist</strong> <strong>perspective</strong> 61<br />

such as availability of attention, but also on factors of social <strong>context</strong> such as<br />

the ‘accuracy dem<strong>and</strong> of the situation’ <strong>and</strong> ‘various listener-based discourse<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts’(p. 324). A similar review of research studies on L2 learners’<br />

awareness of negative feedback also concludes that such awareness is affected<br />

differently by different social <strong>context</strong>s:<br />

there are differences between the fi nd<strong>in</strong>gs of laboratory <strong>and</strong> classroom studies,<br />

differences between primarily structure-focused <strong>and</strong> primarily content-focused<br />

classrooms, <strong>and</strong> differences between observational studies of naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>g<br />

feedback patterns <strong>in</strong> classrooms <strong>and</strong> experimental studies that focus on specifi c<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic features <strong>and</strong> feedback types.<br />

(Nicholas, Lightbown, <strong>and</strong> Spada 2001: 751)<br />

For example, Lyster <strong>and</strong> Ranta (1997) showed that learners’ notic<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

implicit corrective feedback was different depend<strong>in</strong>g on whether that feedback<br />

occurred <strong>in</strong> a classroom focused on mean<strong>in</strong>g (e.g. an immersion classroom)<br />

as opposed to a classroom focused on form (e.g. a grammar-focused class). If<br />

it is true that learners must notice the difference between their own <strong>and</strong> new<br />

language forms <strong>in</strong> order to acquire a new L2 form, <strong>and</strong> social sett<strong>in</strong>g affects<br />

this notic<strong>in</strong>g, then social sett<strong>in</strong>g must also affect acquisition.<br />

Of course, notic<strong>in</strong>g may not always result <strong>in</strong> uptake; social <strong>context</strong> affects<br />

learners’ will<strong>in</strong>gness to accept the corrective feedback that they notice <strong>and</strong> use<br />

it <strong>in</strong> their own speech. Here we must come to terms with an obvious fact: all<br />

corrective feedback on l<strong>in</strong>guistic form is <strong>in</strong>herently value-laden <strong>and</strong> social, by<br />

its very nature. Correction is about gett<strong>in</strong>g someone else to align more closely<br />

to one’s own st<strong>and</strong>ard; do<strong>in</strong>g this <strong>in</strong>volves the corrector’s assumption that he<br />

or she has the right <strong>and</strong> the power to direct the correction. Acceptance of a<br />

correction is also acceptance of the corrector’s right to correct. It is hard to<br />

imag<strong>in</strong>e corrective feedback provided by a human that does not <strong>in</strong>volve this<br />

k<strong>in</strong>d of assertion <strong>and</strong> negotiation of social power <strong>in</strong> a social relationship.<br />

Corrective feedback on form provided by a computer program (e.g. spellchecks)<br />

could be argued to lack that k<strong>in</strong>d of social message, but not feedback<br />

provided by humans. 6<br />

Rider (2005) documents <strong>in</strong> a university Italian class the complexity of the<br />

social negotiation <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> giv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> receiv<strong>in</strong>g corrective feedback when<br />

learners are asked to provide this to one another <strong>in</strong> pairs as they review the<br />

essays they are writ<strong>in</strong>g. Learner R <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g example is somewhat<br />

overbear<strong>in</strong>g; she enjoys prestige <strong>in</strong> this class, be<strong>in</strong>g generally recognized as the<br />

most profi cient <strong>in</strong> Italian, but learner M also is recognized as hav<strong>in</strong>g considerable<br />

ability <strong>in</strong> the language. In the follow<strong>in</strong>g exchange, R requests feedback<br />

from M on her (<strong>in</strong> fact <strong>in</strong>correct) use of passato prossimo tense (ha lavorato),<br />

but M fails to provide feedback.<br />

(5) r: Il primo . . . È un uomo vecchio . . . Faceva bel tempo, um (7)<br />

Uomo vecchio, ah, ha lavorato? Nel giard<strong>in</strong>o, sì?<br />

(The fi rst [frame] . . . It’s an old man . . . It was nice weather,<br />

um (7) Old man, ah, worked? In the garden, yes?)<br />

04_Batstone_Ch04.<strong>in</strong>dd 61 1/27/2010 10:40:49 PM

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!