20.07.2013 Views

duration in the tonal phonology of pingyao chinese - UCLA ...

duration in the tonal phonology of pingyao chinese - UCLA ...

duration in the tonal phonology of pingyao chinese - UCLA ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

DURATION IN THE TONAL PHONOLOGY OF<br />

PINGYAO CHINESE<br />

JIE ZHANG<br />

zhang@ucla.edu<br />

This <strong>the</strong>sis <strong>in</strong>vestigates <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> phonetic <strong>duration</strong> on <strong>tonal</strong> behavior,<br />

especially tone sandhi, <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. Two types <strong>of</strong> phonetic <strong>duration</strong><br />

are under consideration: <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> rime <strong>in</strong> a syllable, and <strong>the</strong> <strong>duration</strong><br />

between <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> a rime and <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g rime across a<br />

syllable boundary. Rime <strong>duration</strong> conditions <strong>the</strong> pitch distance between <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>tonal</strong> targets <strong>of</strong> a contour tone—<strong>the</strong> longer <strong>the</strong> rime, <strong>the</strong> more pronounced<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> transition. The overall <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> a disyllabic or multisyllabic word<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>s <strong>the</strong> pitch movements <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word—<strong>the</strong> pitch can only be allowed<br />

to <strong>in</strong>flect up to a certa<strong>in</strong> complexity <strong>in</strong> a word. Moreover, given <strong>the</strong> limited<br />

<strong>duration</strong> between <strong>the</strong> end pitch <strong>of</strong> a rime and <strong>the</strong> start<strong>in</strong>g pitch <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g rime, <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> grammar prefers a less drastic pitch change across<br />

<strong>the</strong> syllable boundary. A general OT grammar is first constructed based on<br />

<strong>the</strong>se <strong>duration</strong> factors and o<strong>the</strong>r traditional phonological considerations such<br />

as faithfulness and <strong>the</strong> OCP. A language-specific OT grammar for P<strong>in</strong>gyao<br />

is <strong>the</strong>n presented. The grammar accounts for <strong>the</strong> sandhi pattern as well as<br />

lexical realization <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao tones. Its advantages over previouslydocumented<br />

rule-based analyses are also discussed.<br />

1. INTRODUCTION<br />

In this <strong>the</strong>sis, I <strong>in</strong>vestigate <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> phonetic <strong>duration</strong> on <strong>tonal</strong><br />

behavior, especially tone sandhi, <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. The correlation<br />

between phonetic <strong>duration</strong> and <strong>tonal</strong> behavior was first <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> a<br />

series <strong>of</strong> works by Duanmu (Duanmu 1990, 1992, 1993, 1994a,<br />

1994b). This idea is fur<strong>the</strong>r explored <strong>in</strong> this <strong>the</strong>sis. Two types <strong>of</strong><br />

phonetic <strong>duration</strong> are under consideration: <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> rime <strong>in</strong> a<br />

syllable, and <strong>the</strong> <strong>duration</strong> between <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> a rime and <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g rime across a syllable boundary. I argue that <strong>the</strong> rime<br />

<strong>duration</strong> conditions <strong>the</strong> pitch distance between <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> targets <strong>of</strong> a<br />

contour tone that <strong>the</strong> rime realizes—<strong>the</strong> longer <strong>the</strong> rime <strong>duration</strong>, <strong>the</strong><br />

more pronounced a <strong>tonal</strong> transition can be realized on it. In o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

words, a certa<strong>in</strong> rime <strong>duration</strong> only allows <strong>the</strong> pitch to travel up to a<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> distance. As stop-closed syllables are usually shorter than open<br />

syllables <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects (see Kao 1971 on Cantonese, Zee and<br />

Maddieson 1979 on Shanghai, among o<strong>the</strong>rs), <strong>the</strong> widely documented<br />

phenomenon—less pronounced <strong>tonal</strong> contours on stop-closed syllables<br />

than open syllables <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects —is a manifestation <strong>of</strong> this<br />

condition. Similarly, <strong>the</strong> overall <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> a disyllabic or<br />

multisyllabic word constra<strong>in</strong>s <strong>the</strong> pitch movements <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word—<strong>the</strong><br />

pitch can only be allowed to <strong>in</strong>flect up to a certa<strong>in</strong> complexity <strong>in</strong> a<br />

word depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word. I argue that tone sandhi on


2 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

disyllabic or multisyllabic words <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects partly results from<br />

such constra<strong>in</strong>ts. Ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>duration</strong>al factor for tone sandhi is <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>duration</strong> between <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> a rime and <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

rime across a syllable boundary. Given <strong>the</strong> limited <strong>duration</strong> between<br />

<strong>the</strong> end pitch <strong>of</strong> a rime and <strong>the</strong> start<strong>in</strong>g pitch <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g rime, <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>tonal</strong> grammar prefers a less drastic pitch change across <strong>the</strong> syllable<br />

boundary. This preference, <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g with o<strong>the</strong>r phonetic or<br />

phonological factors, might lead to tone sandhi—paradigmatic changes<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> values <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> relevant syllables.<br />

This <strong>the</strong>sis adopts <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical framework <strong>of</strong> Optimality Theory<br />

(Pr<strong>in</strong>ce and Smolensky 1993) and is organized <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g way:<br />

Section 2 discusses <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t families that are relevant to <strong>the</strong><br />

account <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> behavior <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, illustrated by examples from<br />

various dialects. The functional motivations for <strong>the</strong>se constra<strong>in</strong>ts are<br />

also <strong>in</strong>vestigated; Section 3 gives a detailed account for various aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese with special focus on <strong>the</strong><br />

disyllabic tone sandhi, utiliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> families <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts formulated<br />

<strong>in</strong> Section 2; Section 4 is <strong>the</strong> conclusion.<br />

2. THE CONSTRAINT FAMILIES<br />

Under <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical framework <strong>of</strong> Optimality Theory, I propose that<br />

<strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g three families <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts, which I term Duration,<br />

Faithfulness and Register, are responsible for <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong> a<br />

given dialect <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. The Duration constra<strong>in</strong>t family encompasses<br />

<strong>the</strong> restrictions on pitch movements on <strong>the</strong> rime, <strong>the</strong> disyllabic or<br />

multisyllabic word, and at <strong>the</strong> syllable boundary; <strong>the</strong> Faithfulness<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t family is responsible for both <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g-base and basesandhi<br />

<strong>tonal</strong> correspondence; and <strong>the</strong> Register constra<strong>in</strong>t family consists<br />

<strong>of</strong> a series <strong>of</strong> traditional OCP constra<strong>in</strong>ts on <strong>tonal</strong> registers, bann<strong>in</strong>g<br />

two or more adjacent high or adjacent low registers. Sections 2.1—2.3<br />

<strong>in</strong>troduce each constra<strong>in</strong>t family <strong>in</strong> turn.<br />

2.1. Constra<strong>in</strong>t Family I: Duration<br />

As stated above, <strong>the</strong> Duration constra<strong>in</strong>t family consists <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts<br />

on pitch movements on <strong>the</strong> rime, <strong>the</strong> disyllabic or multisyllabic word,<br />

and at <strong>the</strong> syllable boundary. It is thus natural to consider that this<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t family is composed <strong>of</strong> three sub-families: Duration (Rime),<br />

Duration (Word) and Duration (Boundary).


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 3<br />

2.1.1. Sub-Family I: Duration (Rime)<br />

In many Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects, one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> possible syllable structures is<br />

/C 0V 1T/, where /T/ stands for voiceless stops /p/, /t/, /k/, or ///. We<br />

call this type <strong>of</strong> syllables stop-closed syllables. We fur<strong>the</strong>r term<br />

syllables with <strong>the</strong> structure /C 0V 1// checked syllables. The tones that<br />

are realized on stop-closed syllables are called ru tones <strong>in</strong> traditional<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese documentation. It is generally agreed among Ch<strong>in</strong>ese l<strong>in</strong>guists<br />

that <strong>the</strong> checked syllables are characteristically shorter <strong>in</strong> <strong>duration</strong> than<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r types <strong>of</strong> syllables (L<strong>in</strong> and Wang 1992, Wang 1985). Historical<br />

sources such as Kangxi Zidian (Kangxi Dictionary) 1 documented that<br />

<strong>the</strong> checked syllables were characterized by <strong>the</strong>ir short length. Recent<br />

phonetic studies by Kao (1971) and Zee and Maddieson (1979) also<br />

show that <strong>in</strong> modern Cantonese and Shanghai dialects, <strong>the</strong> average<br />

<strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> non-checked syllables is longer than that <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> checked<br />

syllables. Tables (1) and (2) summarize <strong>the</strong> some results <strong>of</strong> Kao (1971)<br />

and Zee and Maddieson (1979).<br />

(1) Cantonese (Kao 1971)<br />

Syll<br />

Type<br />

Syll<br />

Dur<br />

Tonal<br />

Value<br />

/CV/ /CVM/<br />

(M=/j w<br />

m n N/)<br />

/CV * M/<br />

(M=/j w<br />

m n N/)<br />

/CVT/<br />

(T=/p t<br />

k/)<br />

/CV * T/<br />

(T=/p t<br />

k/)<br />

339ms 352ms 294ms 207ms 117ms<br />

/53 2 , 35, 21, 23, 55, 33, 22/ /5, 3, 2/<br />

(2) Shanghai (Zee and Maddieson 1979)<br />

Syll Open Syllable Checked Syllable<br />

Type Tone A Tone B Tone C Tone D Tone E<br />

Syll<br />

Dur<br />

262ms 327ms 332ms 102ms 147ms<br />

Tonal<br />

Value<br />

/42/ /35/ /24/ /5/ /23/<br />

As can be seen from <strong>the</strong> tables, <strong>in</strong> Cantonese, whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> vowel is<br />

long or short, <strong>the</strong> average syllable <strong>duration</strong> for a stop-closed syllable is<br />

1 Kangxi Zidian is a dictionary produced under <strong>the</strong> imperial order <strong>of</strong> Emperor Kangxi<br />

by a committee <strong>of</strong> lexicographers <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> year 1716.<br />

2 I use <strong>the</strong> traditional Chao letters to represent tones. So 1 <strong>in</strong>dicates <strong>the</strong> lowest pitch<br />

and 5 <strong>in</strong>dicates <strong>the</strong> highest pitch. Contour tones are represented by concatenat<strong>in</strong>g two<br />

or more level tone symbols. E.g., 35 represents a high ris<strong>in</strong>g tone.


4 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

considerably shorter than that for a sonorant-closed syllable or an open<br />

syllable (207ms and 117ms vs. 352ms, 294ms and 339ms); <strong>in</strong><br />

Shanghai, <strong>the</strong> average <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two types <strong>of</strong> checked syllables<br />

(102ms and 147ms) is only around one third to one half <strong>of</strong> that <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

three types <strong>of</strong> open syllables (262ms, 327ms and 332ms).<br />

It can also be seen from <strong>the</strong> tables that <strong>the</strong> ru tones—tones realized<br />

on stop-closed syllables—<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se two dialects are ei<strong>the</strong>r level or only<br />

with a weak contour. In Cantonese, <strong>the</strong> three ru tones are all level<br />

tones—high-level /5/, mid-level /3/ and low-level /2/, while <strong>the</strong> tones<br />

realized on o<strong>the</strong>r types <strong>of</strong> syllables can have contours such as /53/, /35/,<br />

/21/ and /23/. In Shanghai, <strong>the</strong> ru tone associated with <strong>the</strong> shortest<br />

type <strong>of</strong> syllables—Tone D, is a high-level tone /5/, and <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r ru<br />

tone—Tone E, has a weaker pitch rise compared to Tone C (/23/ vs.<br />

/24/). In o<strong>the</strong>r words, <strong>the</strong> shorter <strong>the</strong> rime, <strong>the</strong> flatter <strong>the</strong> pitch contour<br />

it permits.<br />

To account for <strong>the</strong>se phenomena regard<strong>in</strong>g ru tone realization, I<br />

propose that <strong>in</strong> a <strong>tonal</strong> grammar, a family <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts—Duration<br />

(Rime), should be postulated. It is formulated as <strong>in</strong> (3).<br />

(3) a. The family consists <strong>of</strong> a series <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts:<br />

Dur(R)f1, Dur(R)f2, ..., Dur(R)fn (n>1)<br />

b. For 1≤k≤n, constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(R)fk is def<strong>in</strong>ed as follows:<br />

a pitch contour x is licensed by a rime whose sonorant<br />

<strong>duration</strong> is at least fk(x).<br />

c. For 1≤k


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 5<br />

The <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic rank<strong>in</strong>g is determ<strong>in</strong>ed by <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> candidates<br />

which satisfy <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(R)fk+1 are a subset <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> candidates<br />

which satisfy <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(R)fk. 3 Thus <strong>the</strong> violation <strong>of</strong><br />

Dur(R)fk implies <strong>the</strong> violation <strong>of</strong> Dur(R)fk+1. Therefore <strong>the</strong> only<br />

sensible rank<strong>in</strong>g between <strong>the</strong> two constra<strong>in</strong>ts is Dur(R)fk ≥≥<br />

Dur(R)fk+1, o<strong>the</strong>rwise <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> Dur(R)fk will never be visible.<br />

For a specific function fk, <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g generalizations can be made:<br />

(5) a. if n (n=1 or 2) pitch targets have to be reached to produce<br />

tone x, and n+1 pitch targets have to be reached to produce<br />

tone y, <strong>the</strong>n f k(x)≤f k(y).<br />

b. if for both tone x and tone y, n (n=2 or 3) pitch targets<br />

have to be reached to produced <strong>the</strong>m, and <strong>the</strong> accumulative<br />

pitch distance between pitch targets is smaller for tone x<br />

than for tone y, <strong>the</strong>n f k(x)≤f k(y).<br />

The implication <strong>of</strong> (5a) is that <strong>the</strong> preferred rime <strong>duration</strong> to license a<br />

contour tone is no shorter than that to license a level tone, and <strong>the</strong><br />

preferred rime <strong>duration</strong> to license a concave or convex contour is no<br />

shorter than that to license a simple ris<strong>in</strong>g or fall<strong>in</strong>g contour; and <strong>the</strong><br />

implication <strong>of</strong> (5b) is that for two <strong>tonal</strong> contours with <strong>the</strong> same number<br />

<strong>of</strong> pitch targets, <strong>the</strong> far<strong>the</strong>r apart <strong>the</strong> pitch targets, <strong>the</strong> longer <strong>the</strong> rime is<br />

preferred to be.<br />

There are articulatory and auditory reasons for <strong>the</strong>se generalizations.<br />

Articulatorily, a pitch change is implemented by laryngeal structure<br />

movements, and <strong>the</strong>se movements are made possible by contraction and<br />

relaxation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> relevant muscles (Borden, Harris and Raphael 1994,<br />

Erickson, Baer and Harris 1983, Kakita and Hiki 1976, L<strong>in</strong>dqvist 1972,<br />

Ohala 1978, Ohala and Ewan 1973, Sundberg 1973). A more<br />

complicated pitch change <strong>in</strong>volves more complicated laryngeal structure<br />

movements and muscle state change, thus prefers to have a longer time<br />

to implement; and a <strong>tonal</strong> contour with far<strong>the</strong>r-apart pitch targets<br />

requires <strong>the</strong> muscles to contract or relax to a greater degree, thus also<br />

prefers a greater <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> its carrier. Auditorily, <strong>the</strong> perceived <strong>tonal</strong><br />

contour depends on <strong>the</strong> <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tone carrier. Black (1970),<br />

Greenberg and Zee (1979) document that given <strong>the</strong> same distance <strong>of</strong><br />

pitch movement, <strong>the</strong> longer <strong>the</strong> <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> vowel, <strong>the</strong> more<br />

“contour-like” <strong>the</strong> tone is perceived by <strong>the</strong> listener. Thus it is<br />

3 This can be proved as follows. Suppose for pitch contour t, a sonorous rime<br />

<strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> d satisfies <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(R) fk+1 , <strong>the</strong>n from <strong>the</strong> def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>of</strong><br />

Dur(R) fk+1 , d≥fk+1(t). S<strong>in</strong>ce fk+1(t)≥fk(t), we have d≥fk(t). Thus tone t on <strong>duration</strong><br />

d satisfies <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(R) fk . Therefore, <strong>the</strong> candidates which satisfy <strong>the</strong><br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(R) fk+1 are a subset <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> candidates which satisfy <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

Dur(R) fk .


6 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

preferable to have a longer rime <strong>duration</strong> to enhance <strong>the</strong> perception <strong>of</strong> a<br />

<strong>tonal</strong> contour with more <strong>in</strong>flections or greater pitch change.<br />

The Dur(R) constra<strong>in</strong>ts that directly <strong>in</strong>teract with o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

phonological or phonetic factors <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

actual <strong>tonal</strong> realization are only a subset <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t family,<br />

because <strong>in</strong> this family, some constra<strong>in</strong>ts rank so high that <strong>the</strong>y are<br />

never violated, and some constra<strong>in</strong>ts rank so low that <strong>the</strong>y are never<br />

satisfied. For example, <strong>the</strong> top-ranked constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(R)f1 can be<br />

considered as a universal requirement on <strong>tonal</strong> realization purely<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>ed by limitations <strong>of</strong> human physiology. It bans contour<br />

realizations that humans cannot physically achieve due to <strong>in</strong>sufficient<br />

<strong>duration</strong>. S<strong>in</strong>ce it h<strong>in</strong>ges on universal biomechanical limitations <strong>of</strong><br />

humans, it is necessarily undom<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>in</strong> every <strong>tonal</strong> grammar. On <strong>the</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r hand, a constra<strong>in</strong>t that requires <strong>the</strong> rime <strong>duration</strong> to be at least 10s<br />

is ranked so low <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar that it is never active, because no<br />

rimes will ever be that long <strong>in</strong> any given language.<br />

Two questions rema<strong>in</strong> unsolved regard<strong>in</strong>g this constra<strong>in</strong>t sub-family:<br />

how high can “n” be counted <strong>in</strong> a grammar and how is <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terval<br />

between f k(x) and f k+1(x) (1≤k


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 7<br />

quantitative phonetic study has been done on this topic, I do not have<br />

specific language data to support ei<strong>the</strong>r hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. My guess would be<br />

that <strong>the</strong>re exist dialects which exploit ei<strong>the</strong>r possibility.<br />

Bao (1990) and Chen (1996), <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir account <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ru tone<br />

realization <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, consider <strong>the</strong> weak pitch transition <strong>of</strong> ru<br />

tones <strong>in</strong> comparison to non-ru tones to be a low-level phonetic<br />

implementation process (also see Yip 1995). But more has to be said<br />

to account for <strong>the</strong> abundance <strong>of</strong> level ru tones <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects. The<br />

phonologically active constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration (Rime) under <strong>the</strong> OT<br />

framework allows us to give a unified account for <strong>the</strong> “levelness” and<br />

“less-abruptness” <strong>of</strong> ru tones with<strong>in</strong> <strong>phonology</strong>. The account for<br />

P<strong>in</strong>gyao ru tone realization is given <strong>in</strong> Section 3.3.3.1.<br />

2.1.2. Sub-Family II: Duration (Word)<br />

Contour dissimilation has been documented as a commonly attested<br />

tone sandhi pattern <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects (Bao 1992, Yue-Hashimoto<br />

1987). Some disyllabic sandhi patterns <strong>in</strong> Zhenjiang (Zhang 1985), a<br />

Mandar<strong>in</strong> dialect spoken <strong>in</strong> sou<strong>the</strong>rn Ch<strong>in</strong>a, are given <strong>in</strong> (6) to illustrate<br />

<strong>the</strong> contour dissimilation process <strong>in</strong> tone sandhi.<br />

(6) Zhenjiang (Zhang 1985)<br />

a. 42-42 —> 35-42 b. 42-31 —> 35-31<br />

c. 31-42 —> 35-42 d. 31-31 —> 35-31<br />

As can be seen <strong>in</strong> (6), <strong>in</strong> Zhenjiang, when two fall<strong>in</strong>g contours occur<br />

adjacent to each o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> base form, <strong>the</strong> first fall<strong>in</strong>g contour<br />

becomes a ris<strong>in</strong>g contour <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form.<br />

A similar pattern can be seen <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao (Hou 1980, 1982a).<br />

Examples <strong>in</strong> (7) illustrate some sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong> disyllabic and<br />

trisyllabic words <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao. Examples (7a) and (7b) are attested <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

sandhi <strong>of</strong> modifier-noun disyllabic words; examples (7c) and (7d) are<br />

attested <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi <strong>of</strong> subject-predicate trisyllabic words with <strong>the</strong><br />

form AAB where A is <strong>the</strong> reduplicated subject morpheme and B is <strong>the</strong><br />

predicate morpheme.<br />

(7) P<strong>in</strong>gyao (Hou 1980, 1982a)<br />

a. 35-13 —> 35-53 b. 35-35 —> 35-53<br />

c. 35-35-13 —> 35-53-13 d. 35-35-35 —> 35-53-13<br />

In <strong>the</strong> disyllabic forms illustrated by (7a) and (7b), when two ris<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tones are juxtaposed, <strong>the</strong> second ris<strong>in</strong>g tone changes to a high fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tone; <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> trisyllabic forms illustrated by (7c) and (7d), <strong>the</strong> second


8 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

ris<strong>in</strong>g tone <strong>in</strong> a three-adjacent-rise <strong>tonal</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation also changes to a<br />

fall<strong>in</strong>g tone.<br />

In order to formally describe <strong>the</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> pitch contour result<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from <strong>the</strong> contour dissimilation process, I def<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t as follows:<br />

(8) Def<strong>in</strong>ition: a <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t is a po<strong>in</strong>t T i <strong>in</strong> time<br />

such that <strong>the</strong> pitch value <strong>of</strong> T i is ei<strong>the</strong>r higher than <strong>the</strong> pitch<br />

value <strong>of</strong> T i-1 and T i+1, or lower than <strong>the</strong> pitch value <strong>of</strong> T i-1 and<br />

T i+1.<br />

The result <strong>of</strong> above-mentioned contour dissimilation process can thus<br />

be generally described as <strong>the</strong> reduction <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts: <strong>in</strong> a<br />

disyllabic word with a <strong>tonal</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation 35-35, <strong>the</strong>re are two <strong>tonal</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts, but <strong>in</strong> 35-53, <strong>the</strong>re is only one; <strong>in</strong> a trisyllabic word<br />

with a <strong>tonal</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation 35-35-13, <strong>the</strong>re are four <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection<br />

po<strong>in</strong>ts, but <strong>in</strong> 35-53-13, <strong>the</strong>re are only two. These are shown<br />

schematically <strong>in</strong> (9) and (10).<br />

(9) a. 35-35 b.35-53<br />

5 5<br />

55<br />

3 3<br />

3 3<br />

(10) a.35-35-13 b.35-53-13<br />

5 5<br />

5 5<br />

3 3<br />

1<br />

3<br />

3 3<br />

Thus <strong>in</strong>stead <strong>of</strong> referr<strong>in</strong>g directly to this type <strong>of</strong> contour<br />

dissimilation, I propose that a <strong>tonal</strong> grammar directly poses restrictions<br />

on <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts that can be realized on a<br />

phonological word or phrase due to its limited <strong>duration</strong>. A family <strong>of</strong><br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts—Duration (Word) which specifies <strong>the</strong> maximum number <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> a phonological word is formulated as follows:<br />

(11) a.The family consists <strong>of</strong> a series <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts:<br />

Dur(W)g1, Dur(W)g2, ..., Dur(W)gn (n>1)<br />

b.For 1≤k≤n, constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(W)gk is def<strong>in</strong>ed as follows:<br />

1<br />

3


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 9<br />

a word with x syllables can carry at most gk(x) <strong>tonal</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts.<br />

c. For 1≤k


10 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

able to license a fairly complicated pitch transition, too-frequent<br />

acceleration and deceleration <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> laryngeal muscles are still<br />

dispreferred. Thus even if <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>ventory <strong>of</strong> one particular dialect<br />

allows <strong>the</strong> possibility <strong>of</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g more <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> a<br />

phonological word, <strong>the</strong> dispreference for too-complex a muscle<br />

movement <strong>in</strong> a short <strong>duration</strong> might put a stricter upper limit for <strong>the</strong><br />

number <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts that can be realized <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> doma<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong><br />

question.<br />

Processes described as contour dissimilation may also result <strong>in</strong> an<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts, as illustrated by examples from<br />

Huojia—a nor<strong>the</strong>rn dialect (He 1979), and P<strong>in</strong>gyao (Hou 1980) <strong>in</strong><br />

(14)—(15).<br />

(14) Huojia (He 1979)<br />

53-31 —> 53-13<br />

(15) P<strong>in</strong>gyao (Hou 1980)<br />

13-35 —> 31-35<br />

In all <strong>the</strong>se cases, <strong>the</strong> tone sandhi causes <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts to change from 0 to 1. The example <strong>in</strong> Huojia is<br />

schematically shown <strong>in</strong> (16).<br />

(16) 53-31 —> 53-13<br />

5<br />

5<br />

3<br />

3<br />

1<br />

—><br />

This phenomenon can be regarded as an effect <strong>of</strong> a constra<strong>in</strong>t which<br />

poses restrictions on <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imum number <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong><br />

a word. The constra<strong>in</strong>t is formulated <strong>in</strong> (17).<br />

(17) Dur(W) M<strong>in</strong>(Inf): a phonological word should have at least<br />

one <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t.<br />

The motivation for this constra<strong>in</strong>t might have perceptual bases. A<br />

parallel to <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t is stress. If we identify a lexical<br />

unit—a word—with one stress maximum, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> culm<strong>in</strong>ative<br />

function <strong>of</strong> stress is to mark <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> lexical units <strong>in</strong> a doma<strong>in</strong><br />

without necessarily identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir boundaries (Trubetzkoy 1939).<br />

The <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t is by def<strong>in</strong>ition culm<strong>in</strong>ative. Its presence<br />

3<br />

1<br />

3


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 11<br />

may serve <strong>the</strong> same function as <strong>the</strong> culm<strong>in</strong>ative property <strong>of</strong> stress,<br />

namely, mark<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> lexical units. Ano<strong>the</strong>r analogy that<br />

can be drawn to <strong>the</strong> preference <strong>of</strong> a <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t is <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>dispensability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nucleus <strong>in</strong> a syllable. The nucleus <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

syllable marks <strong>the</strong> peak <strong>of</strong> richness <strong>in</strong> spectral <strong>in</strong>formation and energy<br />

and helps <strong>the</strong> demarcation <strong>of</strong> syllable doma<strong>in</strong>s. It is conceivable that a<br />

peak <strong>in</strong> fundamental frequency would help <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> demarcation <strong>of</strong> word<br />

doma<strong>in</strong>s.<br />

Clearly <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t on m<strong>in</strong>imum number <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts<br />

is not motivated by <strong>duration</strong>al requirements. But for <strong>the</strong> sake <strong>of</strong><br />

group<strong>in</strong>g all <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t toge<strong>the</strong>r, I<br />

still group it under <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t sub-family Duration (Word).<br />

The rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> Dur (W) M<strong>in</strong>(Inf) and <strong>the</strong> series <strong>of</strong> Dur(W)g<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts is unsettled and can vary across dialects. Thus a dialect <strong>in</strong><br />

favor <strong>of</strong> at least one <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t and a dialect that prefers no<br />

<strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t at all are both conceivable. P<strong>in</strong>gyao is an attested<br />

case <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> former type, and <strong>the</strong> abundance <strong>of</strong> dialects which display a<br />

spread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> contour on <strong>the</strong> first syllable to <strong>the</strong> whole word,<br />

such as Danyang (Lü 1980) and Shanghai (Shen 1981, Xu et.al 1981),<br />

can be taken as examples <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> latter type. Some tone sandhi <strong>of</strong> Old<br />

Shanghai (from Shen 1981) is given <strong>in</strong> (18). Underl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>dicates tones<br />

on checked syllables.<br />

(18) Old Shanghai (Shen 1981)<br />

53-53 —> 55-53 13 -53 —> 11 -13<br />

53-35 —> 55-31 13 -35 —> 11 -13<br />

53-13 —> 55-53 13 -13 —> 11 -13<br />

In conclusion, Duration (Word) consists <strong>of</strong> a group <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts<br />

mandat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> a word or phrase.<br />

The constra<strong>in</strong>ts on <strong>the</strong> maximum number <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts are<br />

ranked <strong>in</strong> a hierarchy as <strong>in</strong> (12). Ano<strong>the</strong>r constra<strong>in</strong>t requires that a word<br />

has at least one <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t. These constra<strong>in</strong>ts are both<br />

functionally motivated and empirically grounded.<br />

2.1.3. Sub-Family III: Duration (Boundary)<br />

Tonal coarticulation is a widely attested phenomenon <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

dialects (Shen 1990, Xu 1994, 1997) as well as o<strong>the</strong>r East Asian<br />

languages such as Thai and Vietnamese (Abramson 1979, Han and Kim<br />

1974). For <strong>in</strong>stance, Shen (1990)’s results show that <strong>in</strong> standard<br />

Mandar<strong>in</strong>, when a high fall<strong>in</strong>g tones 51 occurs before a high level tone<br />

55 or ano<strong>the</strong>r high fall<strong>in</strong>g tone 51, <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fset <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first tone does not


12 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

fall as low as before a ris<strong>in</strong>g tone 35 or dipp<strong>in</strong>g tone 214. This effect<br />

is taken by Shen (1990) and Xu (1994) to be phonetic adaptation and<br />

thus dist<strong>in</strong>guished from tone sandhi which supposedly has phonemic<br />

effects.<br />

Three major dist<strong>in</strong>ctions are made between tone sandhi and <strong>tonal</strong><br />

coarticulation <strong>in</strong> Shen (1990). First, tone sandhi is attributed to<br />

language-specific morphophonemic constra<strong>in</strong>ts, while <strong>tonal</strong><br />

coarticulation is attributed to language-<strong>in</strong>dependent biomechanical<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts. Second, sandhi tones may be considered as a result <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong><br />

dissimilation or assimilation, but <strong>tonal</strong> coarticulation cannot be<br />

described <strong>in</strong> such terms. Third, <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> tone sandhi are phonemic,<br />

but <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> coarticulation are purely phonetic. In o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

words, tone sandhi does not have to preserve <strong>tonal</strong> identity while <strong>tonal</strong><br />

coarticulation does.<br />

I, on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, would like to propose that under <strong>the</strong> OT<br />

framework, <strong>the</strong>se dist<strong>in</strong>ctions between tone sandhi and <strong>tonal</strong><br />

coarticulation can be reconciled <strong>in</strong> a <strong>tonal</strong> grammar which utilizes<br />

articulatorily and auditorily based constra<strong>in</strong>ts.<br />

Let us consider <strong>the</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>ctions discussed <strong>in</strong> Shen (1990) one by one.<br />

First, s<strong>in</strong>ce Optimality Theory accounts for cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

variations by differences <strong>in</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g, not constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

composition (Pr<strong>in</strong>ce and Smolensky 1993), different tone sandhi<br />

behavior <strong>of</strong> various dialects can be accounted for by language-specific<br />

rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> language-<strong>in</strong>dependent constra<strong>in</strong>ts, some <strong>of</strong> which have<br />

universal biomechanical bases. As a matter <strong>of</strong> fact, <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t subfamilies<br />

Duration (Rime) and Duration (Word) proposed <strong>in</strong> previous<br />

sections both have articulatory and auditory bases. I have argued that<br />

<strong>the</strong>y are relevant to <strong>the</strong> sandhi realization and should be encoded <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

grammar. Thus tone sandhi does have access to <strong>the</strong> universal<br />

biomechanical constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> an OT grammar. The “language-specific<br />

morpho-phonemic constra<strong>in</strong>ts” are manifestations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> different<br />

rank<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> language-<strong>in</strong>dependent constra<strong>in</strong>ts.<br />

Second, <strong>tonal</strong> dissimilation and assimilation might be <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong><br />

phonetic constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> an OT grammar. Section 2.1.2, for <strong>in</strong>stance,<br />

examplifies that some <strong>tonal</strong> dissimilation phenomena can be accounted<br />

for by <strong>the</strong> same phonetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciples which supposedly account for <strong>tonal</strong><br />

coarticulation. Hence <strong>the</strong> second dist<strong>in</strong>ction made <strong>in</strong> Shen (1990) does<br />

not necessarily hold <strong>in</strong> an OT grammar.


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 13<br />

Third, <strong>the</strong> phonemic effects <strong>of</strong> tone sandhi and phonetic effects <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>tonal</strong> coarticulation can also be regarded as <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

rank<strong>in</strong>g differences. In a language which only displays <strong>tonal</strong><br />

coarticulation like Cantonese, <strong>the</strong> faithfulness constra<strong>in</strong>ts which require<br />

<strong>the</strong> sandhi tones to have <strong>the</strong> same identity as <strong>the</strong> base tones and <strong>the</strong><br />

articulatorily-based constra<strong>in</strong>ts which prefer tones to coarticulate rank as<br />

undom<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar, while <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts whose satisfaction<br />

would yield paradigmatic <strong>tonal</strong> changes rank low and are suppressed by<br />

<strong>the</strong> more highly ranked faithfulness constra<strong>in</strong>ts; but <strong>the</strong> faithfulness<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts could potentially be violable and <strong>in</strong>teract with conflict<strong>in</strong>g<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> a different grammar. These conflict<strong>in</strong>g constra<strong>in</strong>ts,<br />

toge<strong>the</strong>r with <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts that favor <strong>tonal</strong> coarticulation, may yield a<br />

language which displays paradigmatic <strong>tonal</strong> changes. Moreover,<br />

sometimes <strong>the</strong> phonemic vs. phonetic dist<strong>in</strong>ction is not clear-cut. For<br />

example, <strong>in</strong> J<strong>in</strong>gpho—a Tibeto-Burman language spoken by J<strong>in</strong>gpho<br />

people <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a (Liu 1984), <strong>the</strong>re are three basic lexical tones: 31, 55<br />

and 33. When a 31 tone is preceded by a 55 tone, it becomes a high<br />

fall<strong>in</strong>g tone 51: 55-31 —> 55-51. This can be regarded as a <strong>tonal</strong><br />

coarticulation process. But <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> identity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base tone has been<br />

changed from a low fall<strong>in</strong>g to a high fall<strong>in</strong>g. 4 Also, <strong>in</strong> J<strong>in</strong>gpho, 51<br />

carries some load <strong>of</strong> contrast such that it can occur as a lexical tone <strong>in</strong><br />

k<strong>in</strong>ship terms, fur<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that this <strong>tonal</strong> coarticulation process<br />

might actually <strong>in</strong>volve a phonemic change. Because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se reasons,<br />

<strong>the</strong> phonemic vs. phonetic dist<strong>in</strong>ction between tone sandhi and <strong>tonal</strong><br />

coarticulation does not firmly hold ei<strong>the</strong>r.<br />

As an alternative, I propose that <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> grammar has a sub-family<br />

<strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts: Duration (Boundary). It is formulated as <strong>in</strong> (19).<br />

(19) a. Suppose α 0, α 1, α 2, ..., α n are a series <strong>of</strong> real numbers<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g frequency differences on f 0 dimension, and<br />

0=α 0


14 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

c. For 0≤k≤n, Dur(B)fallαk is def<strong>in</strong>ed as follows:<br />

a pitch fall greater than α k across a syllable boundary is<br />

not allowed.<br />

Given 0=α 0


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 15<br />

between two contrastive tones and it does not play any contrastive role<br />

itself, it is not important for this pitch change per se to be saliently<br />

perceived. Moreover, s<strong>in</strong>ce this <strong>duration</strong> is most likely loaded with<br />

spectral and <strong>in</strong>tensity changes, <strong>the</strong> perceptual sensitivity to <strong>tonal</strong><br />

movement is considerably decreased dur<strong>in</strong>g this period (House 1990).<br />

This makes <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> change <strong>in</strong> this <strong>duration</strong> difficult to resolve.<br />

Therefore, it is well-justified to consider only <strong>the</strong> articulatory factors<br />

<strong>in</strong>stead <strong>of</strong> both <strong>the</strong> articulatory and <strong>the</strong> auditory factors <strong>in</strong> construct<strong>in</strong>g<br />

this family <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts and establish<strong>in</strong>g its <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic rank<strong>in</strong>g. Given<br />

that <strong>the</strong> <strong>duration</strong> between two rimes is generally considerably shorter<br />

than <strong>the</strong> average <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> a syllable (around 50ms vs. around 250ms,<br />

measured from 10 tokens <strong>of</strong> my own speech <strong>in</strong> Mandar<strong>in</strong>), <strong>the</strong> fact that<br />

an abrupt pitch change can sometimes occur across <strong>the</strong> syllable<br />

boundary, while such an abrupt change almost never occurs on a very<br />

short syllable supports <strong>the</strong> view that articulation is <strong>the</strong> only<br />

consideration for pitch changes across <strong>the</strong> syllable boundary, while both<br />

articulation and perception are at stake for pitch changes on a syllable.<br />

Intuitively, <strong>the</strong>re can be ano<strong>the</strong>r series <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts that simply<br />

restrict <strong>the</strong> pitch difference across a syllable boundary regardless <strong>of</strong><br />

ris<strong>in</strong>g or lower<strong>in</strong>g. These constra<strong>in</strong>ts may be regarded as <strong>the</strong><br />

conjunction <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts Dur(B)riseα k and Dur(B)fallαk (Hewitt<br />

and Crowhurst 1995, Kirchner 1996, Smolensky 1995). They are<br />

formulated as follows:<br />

(22) For 0≤k≤n, Dur(B)αk=Dur(B)riseαk ∧ Dur(B)fallαk:<br />

a pitch change greater than αk across a syllable boundary is<br />

not allowed.<br />

Given that a violation <strong>of</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>r Dur(B)riseαk or Dur(B)fallαk<br />

will imply a violation <strong>of</strong> Dur(B)αk, <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic rank<strong>in</strong>g is<br />

rendered:<br />

(23) For 0≤k≤n, Dur(B)riseαk, Dur(B)fallαk ≥≥ Dur(B)αk.<br />

From (20), (21) and (23), <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts and <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

this constra<strong>in</strong>t sub-family can be summarized as <strong>in</strong> (24).


16 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

(24) Dur(B)riseαn≥≥Dur(B)riseαn-1≥≥...≥≥Dur(B)riseα0<br />

|∨ |∨ |∨<br />

|∨ |∨ |∨<br />

Dur(B)fallαn≥≥Dur(B)fallαn-1≥≥...≥≥Dur(B)fallα0<br />

|∨ |∨ |∨<br />

|∨ |∨ |∨<br />

Dur(B)αn ≥≥ Dur(B)αn-1 ≥≥...≥≥ Dur(B)α0<br />

The assimilatory <strong>tonal</strong> coarticulation effect can be captured by such a<br />

family <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts. The ubiquity <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> coarticulation can be taken<br />

as an effect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> high rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se constra<strong>in</strong>ts. And as it is argued<br />

earlier <strong>in</strong> this section, <strong>the</strong> phonemic effects <strong>of</strong> tone sandhi can be<br />

regarded as a result <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se constra<strong>in</strong>ts with <strong>the</strong><br />

faithfulness constra<strong>in</strong>ts as well as o<strong>the</strong>r phonetic or phonological<br />

requirements <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar. The constra<strong>in</strong>t family Faithfulness is<br />

discussed <strong>in</strong> Section 2.2, and <strong>the</strong> detailed analysis <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao tone<br />

sandhi <strong>in</strong> Section 3 gives an example <strong>of</strong> how <strong>the</strong>se constra<strong>in</strong>ts function<br />

<strong>in</strong> a particular grammar.<br />

2.2. Constra<strong>in</strong>t Family II: Faithfulness<br />

In a <strong>tonal</strong> grammar, <strong>the</strong>re must be a family <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts that requires<br />

<strong>the</strong> surface lexical tone to be similar to <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g lexical tone (if<br />

such tone is to be postulated), and <strong>the</strong> sandhi form to be similar to <strong>the</strong><br />

base form. O<strong>the</strong>rwise, <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> form that optimally satisfies <strong>the</strong><br />

“tonotactic” constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar will be <strong>the</strong> realization <strong>of</strong> every<br />

s<strong>in</strong>gle form. The constra<strong>in</strong>t family Faithfulness serves this purpose.<br />

Its basic components are def<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> (25a)—(25b).<br />

(25) a. Pres(R): preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> register <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base form (or<br />

<strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>tonal</strong> representation) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form (or<br />

<strong>the</strong> surface representation).<br />

b. Pres(C): preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> contour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base form (or<br />

<strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>tonal</strong> representation) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form (or<br />

<strong>the</strong> surface representation).<br />

The concepts <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> register and <strong>tonal</strong> contour are collectively taken<br />

from Wang (1967), Yip (1980), Bao (1990), Duamnu (1990), among<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs. Tonal contour refers to <strong>the</strong> change <strong>in</strong> fundamental frequency <strong>of</strong><br />

a tone across <strong>the</strong> time doma<strong>in</strong>. Thus <strong>the</strong> basic values for a <strong>tonal</strong><br />

contour are ris<strong>in</strong>g and fall<strong>in</strong>g. In some <strong>tonal</strong> systems, <strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se two basic values is exploited, render<strong>in</strong>g a convex or concave<br />

<strong>tonal</strong> contour. Moreover, some tone languages divide <strong>the</strong> pitch range<br />

<strong>in</strong>to two—high and low, and use this division to cross-classify with<br />

<strong>tonal</strong> contour to express <strong>tonal</strong> contrast. This is <strong>the</strong> notion <strong>of</strong> register.


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 17<br />

The two pitch ranges are referred to as high register and low register.<br />

For example, <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao, a 13 tone can be characterized as a low ris<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tone, while a 53 tone can be characterized as a high fall<strong>in</strong>g tone.<br />

For syllables <strong>in</strong> different prosodic positions, <strong>the</strong> rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

faithfulness constra<strong>in</strong>ts might differ, because conceivably, it is easier<br />

for a tone on a prosodically weak syllable to sacrifice its identity <strong>in</strong><br />

tone sandhi than a tone on a prosodically strong syllable. Thus (25a)<br />

and (25b) can each be considered as a group <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts ranked<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to a prom<strong>in</strong>ence hierarchy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllables, with <strong>the</strong> syllables<br />

highest <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ence hierarchy rank <strong>the</strong> highest <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> faithfulness<br />

hierarchy. This idea is formalized <strong>in</strong> (26). Pres(σ k, R) means to<br />

preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> register <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable σ k, and Pres(σσ k, C) means<br />

to preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> contour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable σ k.<br />

(26) If σ 1—σ n (n>1) are syllables prosodically ranked from high to<br />

low, <strong>the</strong>n<br />

a. Pres(σ 1, R) ≥≥ Pres(σ 2, R) ≥≥ ... ≥≥ Pres(σ n, R).<br />

b.Pres(σσ 1, C) ≥≥ Pres(σ 2, C) ≥≥ ... ≥≥ Pres(σ n, C).<br />

Yue-Hashimoto (1987) po<strong>in</strong>ts out that <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects, <strong>the</strong><br />

prosodically dom<strong>in</strong>ant syllable usually displays a full range <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong><br />

contrasts after sandhi while <strong>the</strong> prosodically weak syllable tends to<br />

undergo <strong>tonal</strong> neutralization. Many Wu dialects <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese have a leftprom<strong>in</strong>ence<br />

prosodic property; thus tone sandhi <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se dialects usually<br />

neutralizes <strong>the</strong> second syllable <strong>in</strong> a disyllabic word while keeps <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>tonal</strong> contrast on <strong>the</strong> first syllable. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>in</strong> numerous<br />

Nor<strong>the</strong>rn and M<strong>in</strong> dialects, <strong>tonal</strong> contrasts are more faithfully reta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong><br />

word-f<strong>in</strong>al position due to <strong>the</strong>ir right-prom<strong>in</strong>ence prosodic property<br />

(Wright 1983, Yue-Hashimoto 1987). These phenomena can be taken<br />

as results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> (26).<br />

Ano<strong>the</strong>r piece <strong>of</strong> evidence that supports <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> (26)<br />

is from <strong>the</strong> tone sandhi <strong>of</strong> subject-predicate AAB words <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao. In<br />

P<strong>in</strong>gyao as well as many o<strong>the</strong>r Nor<strong>the</strong>rn Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects, a morpheme<br />

can be reduplicated to denote dim<strong>in</strong>utive or endear<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>g. Thus<br />

<strong>the</strong> base form for an AAB word is a subject-predicate disyllabic word<br />

AB, and <strong>the</strong> subject syllable A is reduplicated. Some examples <strong>of</strong><br />

subject-predicate AAB words <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao are given <strong>in</strong> (27a), and <strong>the</strong> tone<br />

sandhi paradigm <strong>of</strong> this type <strong>of</strong> words is given <strong>in</strong> (27b).<br />

(27) a.p´N p´N pø/<br />

book book th<strong>in</strong><br />

“<strong>the</strong> book is th<strong>in</strong>”


18 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

yø/ yø/ kÓu<br />

medic<strong>in</strong>e medic<strong>in</strong>e bitter<br />

“<strong>the</strong> medic<strong>in</strong>e tastes bitter”<br />

sø/ sø/ tsÓ´u<br />

rope rope thick<br />

“<strong>the</strong> rope is thick”<br />

b. σ 1, σ 2\σ 3 13 35 53<br />

13-13 13-13-13 13-31-35 13-35-423<br />

35-35 35-53-13 35-53-13 13-35-423<br />

53-53 53-13-13 53-31-35 53-35-423<br />

As can be seen <strong>in</strong> (27), <strong>in</strong> most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms, <strong>the</strong> word-<strong>in</strong>itial<br />

and word-f<strong>in</strong>al syllables preserve <strong>the</strong> tone <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir base form (let us<br />

assume that <strong>the</strong> high fall<strong>in</strong>g property <strong>of</strong> 53 <strong>in</strong> preserved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first half<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form 423), but <strong>the</strong> tone on <strong>the</strong> word-medial syllable is<br />

drastically different from its base form. This phenomenon becomes<br />

easily understandable if we adopt <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> (26) and <strong>the</strong><br />

fact that word-<strong>in</strong>itial and word-f<strong>in</strong>al syllables are prosodically stronger<br />

than word-medial syllables.<br />

The phonetic correlates <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> prosodic differences between “strong”<br />

and “weak” syllables rema<strong>in</strong> unclear to me, especially <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> disyllabic<br />

forms. Nei<strong>the</strong>r Yue-Hashimoto (1987) nor Wright (1983) gives<br />

quantitative data on <strong>duration</strong> or energy differences between <strong>the</strong><br />

prosodically strong and weak syllables.<br />

The constra<strong>in</strong>ts Pres(σ k, R) and Pres(σ k, C) can be conjo<strong>in</strong>ed to<br />

form a constra<strong>in</strong>t Pres(σ k, T), def<strong>in</strong>ed as <strong>in</strong> (28).<br />

(28) For 1≤k≤n, Pres(σ k, T)=Pres(σ k, R) ∧ Pres(σ k, C):<br />

preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> register and <strong>tonal</strong> contour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base form<br />

(or <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>tonal</strong> representation) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form (or<br />

<strong>the</strong> surface representation).<br />

Aga<strong>in</strong>, s<strong>in</strong>ce a violation <strong>of</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>r Pres(σ k, R) or Pres(σ k, C) will<br />

imply a violation <strong>of</strong> Pres(σ k, T), <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> (29)<br />

follows:<br />

(29) For 1≤k≤n, Pres(σ k, R), Pres(σ k, C) ≥≥ Pres(σ k, T).


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 19<br />

A summary <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts and <strong>the</strong>ir rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

sub-family is given <strong>in</strong> (30).<br />

(30) If σ1—σn (n>1) are syllables prosodically ranked from high to<br />

low, <strong>the</strong>n<br />

Pres(σ 1, R) ≥≥ Pres(σ 2, R) ≥≥ ... ≥≥ Pres(σ n, R)<br />

|∨ |∨ |∨<br />

|∨ |∨ |∨<br />

Pres(σ 1, T) ≥≥ Pres(σ 2, T) ≥≥ ... ≥≥ Pres(σ n, T)<br />

|∧ |∧ |∧<br />

|∧ |∧ |∧<br />

Pres(σσ 1, C) ≥≥ Pres(σ 2, C) ≥≥ ... ≥≥ Pres(σ n, C)<br />

2.3. Constra<strong>in</strong>t Family III: Register<br />

The constra<strong>in</strong>t families Duration and Faithfulness do not suffice <strong>in</strong><br />

account<strong>in</strong>g for all <strong>the</strong> possible sandhi patterns. The follow<strong>in</strong>g sandhi<br />

from a predicate-object disyllabic P<strong>in</strong>gyao word illustrates this po<strong>in</strong>t:<br />

(31) 35-35 —> 31-35<br />

ex: pœ ÇiN<br />

fail spirit<br />

“to disappo<strong>in</strong>t”<br />

kuei tßÓAN<br />

passs<strong>in</strong>g<br />

“performance <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> village after summer harvest”<br />

In (31), <strong>the</strong> first 35 <strong>in</strong> a 35-35 concatenation becomes a low fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tone 31. But whe<strong>the</strong>r from <strong>the</strong> consideration <strong>of</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> pitch<br />

distance across <strong>the</strong> syllable boundary or be<strong>in</strong>g more faithful to <strong>the</strong> base<br />

form, 53-35 will be a better sandhi form than 31-35: <strong>the</strong>re is no pitch<br />

difference across <strong>the</strong> syllable boundary for 53-35, but <strong>the</strong>re is a pitch<br />

change at <strong>the</strong> syllable boundary for 31-35; 53 preserves <strong>the</strong> high <strong>tonal</strong><br />

register <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base tone 35 while 31 fails to do so; and just as 31-35,<br />

53-35 has only one <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t. What makes 31-35 <strong>the</strong><br />

w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g form? The traditional account for this phenomenon is register<br />

dissimilation. Along this l<strong>in</strong>e, I propose that <strong>in</strong> an OT grammar, <strong>the</strong>re<br />

are constra<strong>in</strong>ts which pose restriction on <strong>the</strong> concatenation <strong>of</strong> same<br />

registers. In general terms, for a word with n syllables, <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

family <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts which I term Register is at stake:<br />

(32) a.The constra<strong>in</strong>t family consists <strong>of</strong> a series constra<strong>in</strong>ts:<br />

Reg(n), Reg(n-1), ..., Reg(2) (n≥2)<br />

b.For 2≤k≤n, constra<strong>in</strong>t Reg(k) is def<strong>in</strong>ed as follows:


20 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

same register on k adjacent syllables is not allowed.<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce when j>k, <strong>the</strong> violation <strong>of</strong> Reg(j) implies <strong>the</strong> violation <strong>of</strong><br />

Reg(k), <strong>the</strong>re is an <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic rank<strong>in</strong>g among <strong>the</strong>se constra<strong>in</strong>ts:<br />

(33) Reg(n) ≥≥ Reg(n-1) ≥≥ ... ≥≥ Reg(2).<br />

For <strong>the</strong> above disyllabic words <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao, <strong>the</strong> only valid constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

from this constra<strong>in</strong>t family is Reg(2), and it requires <strong>the</strong> registers <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> two tones to be different. Let us assume for now that <strong>the</strong> predicateobject<br />

P<strong>in</strong>gyao words are right-prom<strong>in</strong>ent and that Pres(σ2) and<br />

Num(Inf)≤≤1 are undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> this particular <strong>tonal</strong><br />

grammar. Then by rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Reg(2) over Dur(B) 0 and<br />

Pres(σ 1, R), <strong>the</strong> correct sandhi form 31-35 is derived from <strong>the</strong><br />

grammar, as illustrated by <strong>the</strong> tableau <strong>in</strong> (34).<br />

(34) 35-35 —> 31-35<br />

35-35 Reg(2) Dur(B) 0 Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 31-35 * *<br />

53-35 *!<br />

Similar to <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t on <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imum number <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection<br />

po<strong>in</strong>t, <strong>the</strong> motivation for this family <strong>of</strong> dissimilation constra<strong>in</strong>ts might<br />

also have perceptual bases. Bladon (1986), after Tyler et.al (1982) and<br />

Delgutte et.al (1982), proposes that a dynamic change <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> speech<br />

signal facilitates auditory nerve fir<strong>in</strong>g. Although Bladon’s discussion<br />

primarily focuses on changes <strong>of</strong> spectral characteristics and acoustic<br />

energy, it is conceivable that a dynamic change <strong>in</strong> fundamental<br />

frequency will have <strong>the</strong> similar effect. If so, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> preference for<br />

register dissimilation <strong>in</strong> a <strong>tonal</strong> grammar directly follows.<br />

In this section, not claim<strong>in</strong>g exhaustiveness, I have laid out three<br />

families <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts which are responsible for <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects: Duration, Faithfulness and Register. The articulatory<br />

and/or auditory motivations for each family <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts were also<br />

discussed. In <strong>the</strong> next section, I give a complete account for <strong>the</strong><br />

P<strong>in</strong>gyao disyllabic tone sandhi utiliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts proposed <strong>in</strong> this<br />

section, thus fur<strong>the</strong>r support<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> claim that phonetic <strong>duration</strong> plays a<br />

decisive role <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong> a tone language.<br />

3. PINGYAO TONE SANDHI<br />

3.1. Introduction to P<strong>in</strong>gyao and <strong>the</strong> J<strong>in</strong> Dialects


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 21<br />

P<strong>in</strong>gyao is a dialect spoken <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao City, Shanxi Prov<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong><br />

central Ch<strong>in</strong>a. It belongs to <strong>the</strong> J<strong>in</strong> dialect group which <strong>in</strong>cludes<br />

dialects spoken <strong>in</strong> Shanxi and its vic<strong>in</strong>ity. The existence <strong>of</strong> checked<br />

syllables (syllables that end <strong>in</strong> a glottal stop) and <strong>the</strong> exploitation <strong>of</strong><br />

structure-sensitive tone sandhi, namely, associat<strong>in</strong>g different tone sandhi<br />

processes with words <strong>of</strong> different syntactic structures, are two <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

ma<strong>in</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se dialects.<br />

My data for P<strong>in</strong>gyao are taken from Hou (1980, 1982a, 1982b) as<br />

well as field notes from work with 2 native speakers <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao—Zhao<br />

Xiuy<strong>in</strong>g and Xue Baoq<strong>in</strong>g—I carried out dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> summer <strong>of</strong> 1996.<br />

Most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms documented <strong>in</strong> Hou (1980) and Hou (1982a)<br />

were recorded and pitch tracks were made on a CSL (Computerized<br />

Speech Laboratory) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Phonetics Laboratory <strong>of</strong> <strong>UCLA</strong>. The<br />

experimental results generally co<strong>in</strong>cided with Hou’s documentation.<br />

Therefore Hou’s transcription was used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonological analysis<br />

unless o<strong>the</strong>rwise noted.<br />

3.2. The Phonetic Inventory <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao<br />

In traditional Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialectology, <strong>the</strong> phonetic <strong>in</strong>ventory <strong>of</strong> a dialect<br />

is usually given <strong>in</strong> two groups: onsets and rimes. The follow<strong>in</strong>g data<br />

from Hou (1980) abide by this tradition. In <strong>the</strong> onset chart, column and<br />

row represent place and manner <strong>of</strong> articulation respectively. The rime<br />

chart is arranged by rime <strong>in</strong>itials accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> tradition <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

documentation. The non-IPA symbols <strong>in</strong> Hou (1980) are changed <strong>in</strong>to<br />

<strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g IPA symbols. The apical vowels follow<strong>in</strong>g /s, z, ts,<br />

tsÓ/ and /ß, Ω, Êß, ÊßÓ/ are represented by /È s / and /È ’ / respetively. Notice<br />

that a possible rime is <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form /V 1/, /V 1N/, or /V 1//. On very rare<br />

occasions <strong>the</strong> rime is <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> form /V 1’/.<br />

(35) P<strong>in</strong>gyao (Hou 1980)<br />

a. Onsets:<br />

p t k<br />

pÓ tÓ kÓ<br />

s ß Ç x<br />

z Ω<br />

ts Êß cÇ<br />

tsÓ ÊßÓ cÇÓ<br />

m n nz =≠ N<br />

l


22 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

b.Rimes:<br />

A iA uA yA<br />

È ’ Ø iØ yØ<br />

u´ y´<br />

œ uœ<br />

O iO<br />

È ’ i u y<br />

È s Ë<br />

´’<br />

ei uei<br />

´u i´u<br />

AN iAN uAN<br />

´N iN uN yN<br />

ø/ iø/ uø/ yø/<br />

P<strong>in</strong>gyao has five lexical tones: 13, 23 , 35, 53, 54 . The underl<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

tones are ru tones which are realized only on checked syllables. Due to<br />

<strong>the</strong> phonetic characteristic <strong>of</strong> checked syllables, <strong>the</strong>y are also called<br />

“short tones” <strong>in</strong> Hou (1980). Examples <strong>in</strong> (36) show lexical items that<br />

carry <strong>the</strong>se tones.<br />

(36) a.13 pu “to hatch”<br />

iN “overcast”<br />

b. 23 pø/ “to push aside”<br />

xuø/ “hair”<br />

c. 35 pu “cloth”<br />

tuN “to move”<br />

d. 53 pu “to mend”<br />

tiN “nap”<br />

e. 54 pø/ “a musical <strong>in</strong>strument”<br />

xuø/ “to live”<br />

In tone sandhi forms, five o<strong>the</strong>r tones might also appear: 31, 32 , 45 ,<br />

423, 423 .<br />

3.3. P<strong>in</strong>gyao Tone Sandhi<br />

Tone sandhi behavior <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao is syntactically conditioned. Words <strong>in</strong><br />

different syntactic configurations have different tone sandhi forms even<br />

if <strong>the</strong>y have <strong>the</strong> same base form. In this section, I first present <strong>the</strong> data<br />

for <strong>the</strong> predicate-object/subject-predicate disyllabic tone sandhi <strong>in</strong><br />

P<strong>in</strong>gyao and discuss Bao (1990) and Chen (1996)’s analyses for it, <strong>the</strong>n<br />

I <strong>of</strong>fer a different account for <strong>the</strong> data <strong>in</strong> an OT framework, utiliz<strong>in</strong>g<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts I proposed <strong>in</strong> Section 2. This account is <strong>the</strong>n extended to<br />

expla<strong>in</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r disyllabic sandhi pattern <strong>of</strong> a different syntactic


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 23<br />

configurations <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao which Bao 1990 and Chen 1996 fail to<br />

account for.<br />

3.3.1. Predicate-Object/Subject-Predicate Disyllabic Word Tone Sandhi<br />

Tone sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong> disyllabic words <strong>of</strong> predicate-object or subjectpredicate<br />

configuration <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao is summarized <strong>in</strong> (37). The leftmost<br />

column and <strong>the</strong> top row show <strong>the</strong> base form <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first and second<br />

syllable respectively. The body <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> table <strong>in</strong>dicates <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> disyllabic words. Some examples are given <strong>in</strong> (38).<br />

(37) σ 1\σ 2 13 23 35 53 54<br />

13 13-13 13- 23 31-35 35-423 35- 423<br />

23 23 -13 23 - 23 32 -35 45 -423 45 - 423<br />

35 13-13 13- 23 31-35 35-423 35- 423<br />

53 53-13 53- 23 53-35 35-423 35- 423<br />

54 54 -13 54 - 23 54 -35 45 -423 45 - 423<br />

(38) Base form Sandhi form Gloss<br />

kuaN m´N 13-13 13-13 “to close <strong>the</strong> door”<br />

close door<br />

tÇiA pœ 13-35 31-35 “<strong>the</strong> family is broken up”<br />

home fail<br />

tÇi mA 13-53 35-423 “to ride a horse”<br />

ride horse<br />

yØ ß´N 35-13 13-13 “<strong>the</strong> yard is deep”<br />

yard deep<br />

pœ ÇiN 35-35 31-35 “to disappo<strong>in</strong>t”<br />

fail spirit<br />

xA y 35-53 35-423 “to ra<strong>in</strong>”<br />

down ra<strong>in</strong><br />

kÓ´u tiØ 53-13 53-13 “honeymou<strong>the</strong>d”<br />

mouth sweet<br />

tsu´ tÇiA 53-35 53-35 “to raise <strong>the</strong> price”<br />

raise price<br />

´’ nzuAN 53-53 35-423 “easy to be persuaded”<br />

ear s<strong>of</strong>t<br />

3.3.2. Bao (1990) and Chen (1996)<br />

Bao (1990) and Chen (1996) give two similar rule-based analyses for<br />

<strong>the</strong> data <strong>in</strong> (37). In both analyses, <strong>the</strong> ru tones 23 and 54 are considered<br />

to be derived from non-ru tones 13 and 53 respectively. The reasons for<br />

<strong>the</strong> assumption are: 13 and 23 , 53 and 54 are not only phonetically


24 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

similar, but <strong>the</strong>y also display <strong>the</strong> same sandhi behavior. Examples that<br />

illustrate <strong>the</strong> latter po<strong>in</strong>t are given <strong>in</strong> (39).<br />

(39) 13-35—>31-35 23 -35—> 32 -35 (contour reveral on σ 1)<br />

35-13—>13-13 35- 23 —>13- 23 (register lower<strong>in</strong>g on σ 1)<br />

53-35—>53-35 54 -35—> 54 -35 (no change)<br />

35-53—>35-423 35- 54 —>35- 423 (f<strong>in</strong>al pitch rise)<br />

In (39), <strong>the</strong> left column shows sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> non-ru tones 13<br />

and 53 and <strong>the</strong> right column shows <strong>the</strong> sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir ru tone<br />

counterpart 23 and 54 , we can clearly see that 13 and 23 , 53 and 54<br />

behave identically <strong>in</strong> tone sandhi except that <strong>the</strong> sandhi form <strong>of</strong> a non-ru<br />

tone is still a non-ru tone while <strong>the</strong> sandhi form <strong>of</strong> a ru tone is still a ru<br />

tone. The sandhi table <strong>in</strong> (37) can thus be simplified to <strong>the</strong> table <strong>in</strong><br />

(40) which only illustrates <strong>the</strong> sandhi <strong>of</strong> non-ru tones. The sandhi <strong>of</strong> ru<br />

tones can be deduced from <strong>the</strong> sandhi <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir non-ru counterparts. The<br />

less-pronounced pitch transition for ru tones is attributed to low-level<br />

phonetic implementation: <strong>the</strong> contour tends to level <strong>of</strong>f when realized<br />

on short syllables (Bao 1990).<br />

(40) σ 1\σ 2 13 35 53<br />

13 13-13 31-35 35-423<br />

35 13-13 31-35 35-423<br />

53 53-13 53-35 35-423<br />

The <strong>tonal</strong> representation that Bao (1990) argues for is shown <strong>in</strong> (41).<br />

(41) T 2<br />

c r<br />

2<br />

t t<br />

The representation <strong>in</strong> (41) can be construed as: tone (T) consists <strong>of</strong><br />

Contour (c) and Register (r). In addition, <strong>the</strong> Contour node is allowed<br />

to branch (Bao 1990). Bao’s representation <strong>of</strong> 13, 35 and 53 are given<br />

<strong>in</strong> (42).


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 25<br />

(42) a. 13 b.35 c. 53<br />

T T T<br />

2 2 2<br />

c r c r c r<br />

2 | 2 | 2 |<br />

t t L t t H t t H<br />

| | | | | |<br />

l h l h h l<br />

Bao (1990)’s account appeals to three rules: Register Spread<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

Contour Meta<strong>the</strong>sis and Register Lower<strong>in</strong>g. The formulation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se<br />

three rules is illustrated <strong>in</strong> (43), (44) and (45).<br />

(43) Register Spread<strong>in</strong>g: spread <strong>the</strong> register <strong>of</strong> σ 2 to σ 1 if σ 1 has a<br />

ris<strong>in</strong>g contour.<br />

l h<br />

T T<br />

c r r<br />

(44) Contour Meta<strong>the</strong>sis: reverse <strong>the</strong> contour <strong>of</strong> σ 1 if σ 1 and σ 2<br />

have <strong>the</strong> same contour and σ 2 has a high register.<br />

T T<br />

| ty<br />

c —> c / __ c r<br />

fh fh fh |<br />

x y y x x y H<br />

(45) Register Lower<strong>in</strong>g: lower <strong>the</strong> register <strong>of</strong> σ 1 from H to L if<br />

both σ 1 and σ 2 have a ris<strong>in</strong>g contour.<br />

T T<br />

fh fh<br />

r—> r/ __ c c r<br />

| | fh fh<br />

H L l h l h<br />

These three rules are strictly ordered: R-Lower<strong>in</strong>g>Meta<strong>the</strong>sis>R-<br />

Spread<strong>in</strong>g. The derivation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms is shown <strong>in</strong> (46). R-


26 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

Lower<strong>in</strong>g>Meta<strong>the</strong>sis guarantees 35-35 becomes 31-35, not 53-35, and<br />

Meta<strong>the</strong>sis>R-Spread<strong>in</strong>g guarantees 13-35 becomes 31-35, not 53-35.<br />

(46) Base Tone R-Lower<strong>in</strong>g Meta<strong>the</strong>sis R-Spread<strong>in</strong>g Surface<br />

13-13 — — — 13-13<br />

13-35 — 31-35 — 31-35<br />

13-53 — — 35-53 35-423<br />

35-13 13-13 — — 13-13<br />

35-35 13-35 31-35 — 31-35<br />

35-53<br />

—<br />

—<br />

— 35-423<br />

53-13 — — — 53-13<br />

53-35 — — — 53-35<br />

53-53 — 35-53 — 35-423<br />

The 423 concave tone is <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> system by a Contour<br />

Formation rule which simply adds an h-node under <strong>the</strong> contour node<br />

after <strong>the</strong> application <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> three rules <strong>in</strong> (43)-(45).<br />

(47) Contour Formation<br />

c ] w<br />

t¥P<br />

h l h<br />

Bao (1990) takes <strong>the</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao case to be an argument for <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong><br />

representation <strong>in</strong> (41) <strong>in</strong> which Register and Contour are <strong>in</strong> a sister<br />

relation s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong> his analysis, <strong>the</strong> Register node has to be able to spread<br />

alone as required by <strong>the</strong> rule <strong>in</strong> (43). Chen (1996), on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand,<br />

<strong>of</strong>fers an alternative analysis by merg<strong>in</strong>g R-Lower<strong>in</strong>g and R-Spread<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong>to one R-Neutralization rule, hence tak<strong>in</strong>g P<strong>in</strong>gyao as an argument<br />

for a more restricted <strong>tonal</strong> representation <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> contour pitch<br />

nodes are dom<strong>in</strong>ated by <strong>the</strong> Register node, as <strong>in</strong> (48). The formulation<br />

<strong>of</strong> Register Neutralization is shown <strong>in</strong> (49).<br />

(48) T (=r)<br />

2<br />

t t (=c)


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 27<br />

(49) Register Neutralization: when σ 1 has a ris<strong>in</strong>g contour, its<br />

register becomes high if σ 2 has a fall<strong>in</strong>g contour and becomes<br />

low if σ 2 has a ris<strong>in</strong>g contour.<br />

T<br />

c r --><br />

l h<br />

H / __<br />

L / __<br />

c<br />

h l<br />

c<br />

l h<br />

The order <strong>of</strong> this rule and <strong>the</strong> Contour Meta<strong>the</strong>sis rule is: R-<br />

Neutralization > Meta<strong>the</strong>sis.<br />

Chen (1996) argues that <strong>the</strong> alternative analysis he <strong>of</strong>fers has several<br />

advantages over Bao’s analysis. Trivially, it employs fewer rules.<br />

More importantly, <strong>the</strong> R-Neutralization rule stated <strong>in</strong> (49) makes<br />

<strong>in</strong>tuitive sense s<strong>in</strong>ce it encodes <strong>the</strong> anticipatory effect <strong>of</strong> pitch<br />

modulation—<strong>the</strong> register rises <strong>in</strong> anticipation <strong>of</strong> a fall, and lowers <strong>in</strong><br />

preparation for a rise. 5 The derivation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms is shown <strong>in</strong><br />

(50).<br />

(50) Base Tone R-Neutralization Meta<strong>the</strong>sis Surface<br />

13-13 — — 13-13<br />

13-35 — 31-35 31-35<br />

13-53 35-53 — 35-423<br />

35-13 13-13 — 13-13<br />

35-35 13-35 31-35 31-35<br />

35-53<br />

—<br />

— 35-423<br />

53-13 — — 53-13<br />

53-35 — — 53-35<br />

53-53 — 35-53 35-423<br />

5 Chen (1996) also claims that <strong>the</strong> rules <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> alternative analysis act like<br />

wellformedness conditions (WFCs) to be simultaneously met by <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms. Thus<br />

<strong>the</strong> extr<strong>in</strong>sic rule order that Bao’s analysis h<strong>in</strong>ges on can be dispensed with. But a<br />

closer scrut<strong>in</strong>y <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> derivation process <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms reveals that even though<br />

<strong>the</strong> sandhi forms satisfy <strong>the</strong> WFCs <strong>in</strong>stantiated by <strong>the</strong> rules simultaneously, <strong>the</strong><br />

derivation <strong>of</strong> 31-35 from 35-53 still h<strong>in</strong>ges on <strong>the</strong> extr<strong>in</strong>sic order R-<br />

Neutralization>Meta<strong>the</strong>sis. If <strong>the</strong> order <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> rules is reversed, 53-35, an unattested<br />

sandhi form for 35-53, will be <strong>the</strong> derived pattern. This is illustrated by <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

process:<br />

Base tone --> Meta<strong>the</strong>sis --> R-Neutralization --> Surface tone<br />

35-35 53-35 n/a 53-35


28 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

To summarize, Bao (1990) and Chen (1996) give two rule-based<br />

analyses <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> predicate-object/subject-predicate disyllabic tone sandhi<br />

<strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao <strong>in</strong> support <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir different <strong>tonal</strong> representations ((41) for<br />

Bao and (48) for Chen). In <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g section, I give an OT analysis<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> data utiliz<strong>in</strong>g articulatorily and/or auditorily based constra<strong>in</strong>ts<br />

motivated <strong>in</strong> Section 2 and fur<strong>the</strong>r argue that <strong>the</strong> crucial issue <strong>in</strong> tone<br />

sandhi is how <strong>the</strong> articulatory/auditory constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar are<br />

satisfied, not <strong>the</strong> geometrical representation <strong>of</strong> tone.<br />

3.3.3. An OT Analysis for Predicate-Object/Subject-Predicate<br />

Disyllabic Word Tone Sandhi<br />

3.3.3.1. Ru Tones vs. Non-Ru Tones <strong>in</strong> Lexical Realization<br />

As mentioned <strong>in</strong> Section 2.1.1, <strong>in</strong> both Bao and Chen’s analyses, <strong>the</strong> ru<br />

tones 23 and 54 are considered to be derived from <strong>the</strong>ir non-ru<br />

counterparts—13 and 53. The less-pronounced pitch transition for <strong>the</strong><br />

ru tones is <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> contours level<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong>f on checked syllables—a<br />

low-level phonetic implementation process (also see Yip 1995). In <strong>the</strong><br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g analysis, I <strong>in</strong>stead propose that <strong>the</strong> abruptness difference <strong>in</strong><br />

pitch transition on syllables <strong>of</strong> various length is <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> a subfamily<br />

<strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts Duration (Rime) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration<br />

<strong>in</strong> a <strong>tonal</strong> grammar.<br />

As discussed <strong>in</strong> Section 2.1.1, checked syllables are characteristically<br />

shorter <strong>in</strong> <strong>duration</strong> than o<strong>the</strong>r types <strong>of</strong> syllables (/CV/ and /CVN/). To<br />

verify this generalization <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao, a phonetic study on <strong>the</strong> <strong>duration</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong> checked syllables vs. non-checked syllables was carried out with one<br />

native speaker <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao.<br />

The speaker read 16 disyllabic P<strong>in</strong>gyao words, each word with 2<br />

repetitions. Of <strong>the</strong> 64 syllables recorded, 32 were checked syllables and<br />

<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r 32 were non-checked syllables. Of <strong>the</strong> 32 checked syllables,<br />

16 occurred <strong>in</strong> word-<strong>in</strong>itial position and 16 <strong>in</strong> word-f<strong>in</strong>al position; and<br />

<strong>the</strong> same for <strong>the</strong> 32 non-checked syllables. Thus <strong>the</strong> prosodic contexts<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> checked syllables and non-checked syllables were evenly matched.<br />

The <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sonorant portion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> rime <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se tokens was<br />

measured. The result showed that <strong>the</strong> average <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sonorant<br />

portion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> rime <strong>in</strong> a checked syllable was 148ms, while <strong>the</strong> same<br />

measurement for non-checked syllables was 260ms—<strong>the</strong> former was<br />

only 57% <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> latter. These results illustrate that <strong>the</strong> generalization<br />

“checked syllables are significantly shorter than non-checked syllables”<br />

holds true <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao.


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 29<br />

Let us assume that Dur(R)fk is <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t at stake from <strong>the</strong><br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration (Rime) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> account <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao ru tone<br />

realization. The def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>of</strong> Dur(R)fk is repeated <strong>in</strong> (51a). Most<br />

importantly, <strong>the</strong> function fk has <strong>the</strong> properties <strong>in</strong> (51b)-(51d): 6<br />

(51) a. Dur(R)fk: a pitch contour x is licensed by a rime whose<br />

sonorant <strong>duration</strong> is at least fk(x).<br />

b. 148ms


30 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

(55) CV/ /13/ —> [23]<br />

Input:<br />

CV/ /13/<br />

Dur(R)fk Pres(σ, C) Pres(σ, R)<br />

CV/ [13] *!<br />

✿ CV/ [23] *<br />

CV/ [33] **!<br />

CV/ [35] *! *<br />

CV/ [45] * *!<br />

When <strong>the</strong> syllable is open or has a nasal coda as <strong>in</strong> (54), s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong><br />

sonorant portion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> rime is around 260ms and<br />

148ms


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 31<br />

to be phonologically active constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> an OT grammar, <strong>in</strong> this case<br />

Duration (Rime) constra<strong>in</strong>t family.<br />

3.3.3.2. An OT Analysis for Predicate-Object/Subject-Predicate<br />

Disyllabic Word Tone Sandhi<br />

The sandhi pattern <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> predicate-object/subject-predicate disyllabic<br />

words <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao is repeated <strong>in</strong> (56). Only <strong>the</strong> non-checked forms are<br />

<strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> table. The sandhi for <strong>the</strong> checked forms is discussed<br />

afterwards.<br />

(56) σ 1\σ 2 13 35 53<br />

13 13-13 31-35 35-423<br />

35 13-13 31-35 35-423<br />

53 53-13 53-35 35-423<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g facts are to be observed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi pattern:<br />

• The tone on σ 2 is always preserved, even when 53 becomes 423 <strong>in</strong><br />

σ 2 position, <strong>the</strong> high fall<strong>in</strong>g property <strong>of</strong> 53 is preserved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first<br />

half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form.<br />

• A pitch rise always surfaces at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word.<br />

• 53 is a more stable tone than 13 and 35—<strong>in</strong> σ 1 position, it is more<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten preserved than 13 and 35; <strong>in</strong> σ 2 position, although <strong>the</strong> sandhi<br />

form is 423, <strong>the</strong> high fall<strong>in</strong>g property <strong>of</strong> 53 is preserved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first<br />

half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form.<br />

Three correspond<strong>in</strong>g constra<strong>in</strong>ts are posited <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar to capture<br />

<strong>the</strong>se facts:<br />

• Pres(σ 2, T): preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> property on <strong>the</strong> second syllable.<br />

• Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise: <strong>the</strong>re must be a pitch rise word f<strong>in</strong>ally.<br />

• Pres(53): preserve <strong>the</strong> property <strong>of</strong> a base high fall<strong>in</strong>g tone <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

sandhi form.<br />

The first constra<strong>in</strong>t Pres(σ 2, T) comes from <strong>the</strong> Faithfulness<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t family. I attribute <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>violability <strong>of</strong> this constra<strong>in</strong>t to <strong>the</strong><br />

right-prom<strong>in</strong>ence prosodic property <strong>of</strong> this type <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao words.<br />

Aga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>the</strong> phonetic correlates <strong>of</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ence are not clear to me. The<br />

phonetic study I carried out with a native speaker <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao did not<br />

reveal significant differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>duration</strong> or energy between <strong>the</strong> first and<br />

second syllables <strong>in</strong> a disyllabic word with predicate-object or subjectpredicate<br />

configuration. One possibility is that <strong>the</strong>re were such<br />

phonetic differences at some stage <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> historical development <strong>of</strong><br />

P<strong>in</strong>gyao, but <strong>the</strong>se differences were lost <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> diachronic changes.


32 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

Thus <strong>the</strong> mystery <strong>of</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ence is to be unveiled by fur<strong>the</strong>r research <strong>in</strong><br />

historical P<strong>in</strong>gyao <strong>phonology</strong> or more advanced phonetic measurement<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> synchronic data.<br />

The second and third constra<strong>in</strong>ts do not belong to <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

families discussed <strong>in</strong> Section 2 and are particular to <strong>the</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao dialect.<br />

The second constra<strong>in</strong>t Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise might also be related to<br />

<strong>the</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> second syllable. Sundberg (1973)’s study shows<br />

that <strong>the</strong> implementation <strong>of</strong> a ris<strong>in</strong>g pitch requires more articulatory<br />

effort and longer <strong>duration</strong> than that <strong>of</strong> a fall<strong>in</strong>g pitch. On one hand,<br />

speakers are will<strong>in</strong>g to exert more articulatory effort <strong>in</strong> a stronger<br />

prosodic position; on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, a metrically prom<strong>in</strong>ent position<br />

might require gestures which need more production effort to mark its<br />

prom<strong>in</strong>ence. E.g., English stress is manifested by longer <strong>duration</strong> and<br />

greater <strong>in</strong>tensity. In P<strong>in</strong>gyao, we speculate that an end pitch rise is one<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mechanisms that embody <strong>the</strong> right-prom<strong>in</strong>ence <strong>of</strong> a phonological<br />

word.<br />

The third constra<strong>in</strong>t Pres(53) seems to be quite arbitrary. But<br />

consider that 53 is <strong>the</strong> only underly<strong>in</strong>g fall<strong>in</strong>g tone <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>ventory <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao, its special status <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao tonology becomes<br />

more understandable. Moreover, it is widely acknowledged, especially<br />

<strong>in</strong> African languages, that <strong>in</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> assimilation processes, a high tone<br />

typically cause surround<strong>in</strong>g non-high tones to raise <strong>the</strong>ir pitch, while a<br />

low tone usually does not lower a high tone to a low tone (Hyman and<br />

Schuh 1974, Stevick 1969). Mbui and Ngizim are languages that<br />

display this asymmetry (Hyman and Schuh 1974). This <strong>in</strong>dicates <strong>the</strong><br />

special status <strong>of</strong> high tones <strong>in</strong> tone languages—<strong>the</strong>y are more <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

preserved than low tones. For <strong>the</strong>sereasons, although Pres(53) is a<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t peculiar to P<strong>in</strong>gyao, it is not arbitrary. It is shown later <strong>in</strong><br />

this section that <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> tone sandhi <strong>of</strong> disyllabic words <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r syntactic<br />

configurations, Pres(53) is also a highly ranked constra<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

grammar.<br />

As for <strong>the</strong> rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se three constra<strong>in</strong>ts, while <strong>the</strong> first two are<br />

undom<strong>in</strong>ated as <strong>the</strong> sandhi pattern suggests, <strong>the</strong> last constra<strong>in</strong>t is<br />

violable s<strong>in</strong>ce a base form 53-53 concatenation becomes 35-423 after<br />

sandhi. This fact suggests that <strong>the</strong>re are o<strong>the</strong>r constra<strong>in</strong>ts that <strong>in</strong>teract<br />

with <strong>the</strong>se constra<strong>in</strong>ts to determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> sandhi <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> system.<br />

Notice that <strong>the</strong> change from 53-423 to 35-423 renders <strong>the</strong> difference<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> word. As illustrated <strong>in</strong><br />

(57), <strong>the</strong>re are 3 <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> base form 53-423, while<br />

<strong>the</strong>re are only 2 <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form 35-423.


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 33<br />

(57) a.53-423 b.35-423<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

3<br />

2<br />

I propose that <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g constra<strong>in</strong>t from <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t family<br />

Duration (Word) is at stake <strong>in</strong> this particular grammar:<br />

(58) Dur(W)gk/gk(2)=2: a word with x syllables can carry at<br />

most gk(x) <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts, and <strong>the</strong> function gk satisfies<br />

<strong>the</strong> condition gk(2)=2.<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce we are only deal<strong>in</strong>g with disyllabic forms here, for clarity and<br />

simplicity reasons, we give <strong>the</strong> above constra<strong>in</strong>t <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g name:<br />

Num(Inf)≤2.<br />

A look at <strong>the</strong> data <strong>in</strong> (56) reveals that this constra<strong>in</strong>t is never violated<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms. The sandhi forms 13-13 and 35-423 have two<br />

<strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts, and <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms 31-35, 53-13 and 53-35<br />

have one. This is shown schematically <strong>in</strong> (59). We thus rank <strong>the</strong><br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t Num(Inf)≤2 as undom<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>in</strong> this grammar.<br />

(59) a.13-13 b.35-423<br />

5<br />

3 3<br />

4<br />

1<br />

1<br />

3<br />

c. 31-35 d. 53-13 e. 53-35<br />

5 5<br />

5 5<br />

3 3 3 3<br />

1<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

2<br />

3<br />

3 3<br />

The schematic <strong>in</strong> (59) also suggests that <strong>the</strong>re is always at least one<br />

<strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> this type <strong>of</strong> disyllabic words. Forms like 13-<br />

35 or 53-31 which do not have <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts do not surface as<br />

sandhi forms. Therefore <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(W) M<strong>in</strong>(Inf) from <strong>the</strong>


34 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration (Word) plays a role <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> tone sandhi <strong>in</strong><br />

question. The def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>of</strong> Dur(W) M<strong>in</strong>(Inf) is repeated <strong>in</strong> (60).<br />

(60) Dur(W) M<strong>in</strong>(Inf): a phonological word should have at least<br />

one <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>t.<br />

Aga<strong>in</strong>, for reasons <strong>of</strong> simplicity and clarity, I use <strong>the</strong> name<br />

Num(Inf)≥1 for fur<strong>the</strong>r reference <strong>of</strong> this constra<strong>in</strong>t. S<strong>in</strong>ce this<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t is never violated <strong>in</strong> this type <strong>of</strong> sandhi, we rank it as<br />

undom<strong>in</strong>ated as well.<br />

So far we have five work<strong>in</strong>g constra<strong>in</strong>ts for <strong>the</strong> predicateobject/subject-predicate<br />

disyllabic word tone sandhi <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao:<br />

Pres(σ 2, T), Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise, Num(Inf)≤2, Num(Inf)≥1 and<br />

Pres(53). While <strong>the</strong> first four constra<strong>in</strong>ts are undom<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

grammar, <strong>the</strong> last constra<strong>in</strong>t is violable and dom<strong>in</strong>ated by <strong>the</strong> first three<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts. The rank<strong>in</strong>g is illustrated <strong>in</strong> (61).<br />

(61) Pres(σ 2, T), Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise, Num(Inf)≤2,<br />

Num(Inf)≥1 >> Pres(53)<br />

Let us see what <strong>the</strong>se constra<strong>in</strong>ts and <strong>the</strong>ir rank<strong>in</strong>g give us so far.<br />

First, <strong>the</strong>y guarantee that when <strong>the</strong> second syllable is 53 <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> base<br />

form, its sandhi form will be 423. This is true because <strong>in</strong> order to<br />

satisfy both Pres(σ 2, T) and Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise, <strong>the</strong> sandhi form<br />

must be 423—<strong>the</strong> first half <strong>of</strong> which preserves <strong>the</strong> high fall<strong>in</strong>g property<br />

<strong>of</strong> 53 and <strong>the</strong> second half <strong>of</strong> which implements a pitch rise. We might<br />

wonder why 534 or 535 is not <strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g sandhi form, because <strong>the</strong>y<br />

more faithfully preserve <strong>the</strong> high fall<strong>in</strong>g property <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base tone 53<br />

and realize a f<strong>in</strong>al pitch rise as well. My belief is that <strong>the</strong> sandhi<br />

realization might well be 534 or 535. S<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>re is only one<br />

contrastive concave tone <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms, we expect <strong>the</strong> speakers to<br />

have greater phonetic variation <strong>in</strong> its realization. 423 might only<br />

reflect one possible realization and why it is transcribed as such could<br />

be purely due to <strong>the</strong> random choice <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>al field worker.<br />

Second, no matter what <strong>the</strong> base tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first syllable is, 31-423<br />

and 53-423 cannot be <strong>the</strong> possible sandhi tone. This is obviously valid<br />

because both 31-423 and 53-423 have three <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts,<br />

violat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>t Num(Inf)≤2.


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 35<br />

But we get a tie between 35-423 and 13-423 for <strong>the</strong> base forms 13-<br />

53, 35-53 and 53-53 (recall that <strong>the</strong> correct sandhi form for all <strong>the</strong>se<br />

three base forms is 35-423). This is illustrated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> tableau <strong>in</strong> (62).<br />

(62) 13-53, 35-53, 53-53 —> ?<br />

13-53<br />

35-53<br />

53-53<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 2, T)<br />

WFR Num<br />

(Inf)≤2<br />

Num<br />

(Inf)≥1<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

✿ 35-423 *<br />

✿ 13-423 *<br />

Notice <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g difference between <strong>the</strong>se two candidates: s<strong>in</strong>ce<br />

<strong>the</strong> first candidate 35-423 has a high ris<strong>in</strong>g tone on σ 1 while <strong>the</strong> second<br />

candidate 13-423 has a low ris<strong>in</strong>g tone at <strong>the</strong> same position, across <strong>the</strong><br />

boundary between σ 1 and σ 2, 35-423 implements a pitch fall while 13-<br />

423 implements a pitch rise. This is shown schematically <strong>in</strong> (63).<br />

(63) a.35-423 b.13-423<br />

5<br />

pitch fall<br />

4<br />

3<br />

3<br />

pitch rise<br />

4<br />

3<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1 2<br />

To derive <strong>the</strong> correct sandhi form 35-423, <strong>the</strong> asymmetry between<br />

pitch rise and pitch fall across <strong>the</strong> syllable boundary has to be taken<br />

<strong>in</strong>to account <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar. The <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic rank<strong>in</strong>g Dur(B)riseαk ≥≥<br />

Dur(B)fallαk <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration (Boundary) just suits<br />

our needs. The relevant constra<strong>in</strong>ts here and <strong>the</strong>ir rank<strong>in</strong>g can be<br />

specified as <strong>in</strong> (64).<br />

(64) Dur(B)rise0: no pitch rise across a syllable boundary.<br />

Dur(B)fall0: no pitch fall across a syllable boundary.<br />

Dur(B)rise0 >> Dur(B)fall0.<br />

As a matter <strong>of</strong> fact, <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(B)fall0 is so lowly ranked <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> hierarchy that it does not play any role <strong>in</strong> select<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> actual<br />

w<strong>in</strong>ners <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms.<br />

To decide <strong>the</strong> rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(B)rise0, we first notice<br />

that 31-35, which has a pitch rise across syllable boundary, is a<br />

legitimate sandhi form (13-35 —> 31-35, 35-35 —> 31-35). Thus this


36 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t is violable. Its rank<strong>in</strong>g with <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r violable constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

Pres(53) is unsettled so far. The tableau <strong>in</strong> (65) illustrates <strong>the</strong><br />

derivation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> correct sandhi form 35-423 from <strong>the</strong> base forms 13-53,<br />

35-53 and 53-53.<br />

(65) 13-53, 35-53, 53-53 —> 35-423<br />

13-53<br />

35-53<br />

53-53<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 2, T) WFR<br />

Num<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤2<br />

Num<br />

(Inf)<br />

≥1<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

rise0<br />

✿ 35-423 *<br />

13-423 * *!<br />

But <strong>the</strong>re are still unresolved problems. The constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> (64) do<br />

not immediately give us <strong>the</strong> correct sandhi form for <strong>the</strong> base form 35-<br />

13. The tableau <strong>in</strong> (66) shows that four candidates are <strong>in</strong> a tie.<br />

(66) 35-13 —> ?<br />

35-13 Pres<br />

(σ 2, T) WFR<br />

✿ 13-13<br />

✿ 53-13<br />

✿ 31-13<br />

✿ 35-13<br />

Num<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤2<br />

Num<br />

(Inf)<br />

≥1<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

rise0<br />

Recall that <strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g candidate for base form 35-13 is 13-13, not<br />

<strong>the</strong> most faithful 35-13. Notice that one crucial difference between 13-<br />

13 and 35-13 is that <strong>the</strong>re is a sharp pitch fall across <strong>the</strong> syllable<br />

boundary for 35-13, but only a moderate pitch fall for 13-13. The<br />

<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic rank<strong>in</strong>g Dur(B)fallαj ≥≥ Dur(B)fallα i (α j>α i) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration (Boundary) can capture this fact. These<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts and <strong>the</strong>ir rank<strong>in</strong>g are specified <strong>in</strong> this grammar as <strong>in</strong> (67).<br />

(67) Dur(B)fall3: no sharp pitch fall across a syllable boundary.<br />

Dur(B)fall1: no small pitch fall across a syllable boundary.<br />

Dur(B)fall3 ≥≥ Dur(B)fall1.<br />

The number 3 and 1 <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> difference <strong>in</strong> pitch <strong>in</strong> Chao letters.<br />

For a specific speaker, α j and α i should be certa<strong>in</strong> frequency values. I<br />

am us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Chao letters for simplicity and uniformity reasons.<br />

The constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(B)fall1, though it outranks (or is equally ranked<br />

with) Dur(B)fall0, is still a very lowly ranked constra<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> this


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 37<br />

particular grammar and plays no role <strong>in</strong> sandhi form selection. Thus<br />

<strong>the</strong> two constra<strong>in</strong>ts from <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration (Boundary) that<br />

play decisive roles <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi grammar here are: Dur(B)rise0 and<br />

Dur(B)fall3. The rank<strong>in</strong>g between <strong>the</strong>se two constra<strong>in</strong>ts is<br />

undeterm<strong>in</strong>ed. We can thus comb<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong>se two constra<strong>in</strong>ts and call <strong>the</strong><br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ed constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(B). The def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>of</strong> Dur(B) is shown <strong>in</strong><br />

(68).<br />

(68) Dur(B) = def Dur(B)rise0 ∧ Dur(B)fall3<br />

This constra<strong>in</strong>t comprises all <strong>the</strong> restrictions on <strong>the</strong> pitch change<br />

across a syllable boundary <strong>in</strong> this sandhi grammar: a pitch rise or a<br />

sharp pitch fall is disallowed. Replac<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Dur(B)rise0 constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

<strong>in</strong> tableau (65) with <strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(B) does not change<br />

<strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tableau, and replac<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Dur(B)rise0 constra<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong><br />

tableau (66) with <strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed constra<strong>in</strong>t rules out <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>correct sandhi<br />

form 35-13.<br />

As for select<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>ner among <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r three candidates—13-13,<br />

53-13 and 31-13 <strong>in</strong> tableau (66), I propose that <strong>the</strong> faithfulness<br />

requirement on <strong>the</strong> tone on σ 1 plays a decisive role. Compare <strong>the</strong> three<br />

candidates with <strong>the</strong> base form 35-13, 13-13 preserves <strong>the</strong> ris<strong>in</strong>g contour<br />

but does keep <strong>the</strong> high register <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base tone on σ 1; 53-13 preserves<br />

<strong>the</strong> high register but loses <strong>the</strong> ris<strong>in</strong>g contour on σ 1; while <strong>the</strong> third<br />

candidate 31-13 preserves nei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> register nor <strong>the</strong> contour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base<br />

form <strong>of</strong> σ 1. The fact that 13-13 w<strong>in</strong>s over 53-13 suggests that it is<br />

more important to preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> contour than to preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong><br />

register. Thus Pres(σ 1, C) and Pres(σ 1, R) should be two separate<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar, and <strong>the</strong> former outranks <strong>the</strong> latter. To<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se two constra<strong>in</strong>ts with respect to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

violable constra<strong>in</strong>ts—Pres(53) and Dur(B), two possibilities, (69a)<br />

and (69b), need to be enterta<strong>in</strong>ed:<br />

(69) a.Pres(53), Dur(B), Pres(σ 1, C) >> Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

b.Pres(σ 1, C) >> Pres(53), Dur(B), Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

The tableau <strong>in</strong> (70) illustrates that <strong>the</strong> rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> (69a) gives us <strong>the</strong><br />

correct w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g candidate 13-13 for <strong>the</strong> base form 35-13. The tableau<br />

<strong>in</strong> (71) shows that <strong>the</strong> rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> (69b) gives us two tied<br />

candidates—13-13 and 35-13. Thus (69a) is <strong>the</strong> correct rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts. The undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>ts Pres(σ 2, T), Word F<strong>in</strong>al<br />

Rise, Num(Inf)≤2 and Num(Inf)≥1 are not <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

tableaux.


38 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

(70) 35-13 —> 13-13<br />

35-13 Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 13-13 *<br />

35-13 *!<br />

53-13 *!<br />

31-13 *! *<br />

(71) 35-13 —> ?<br />

35-13 Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 13-13 *<br />

✿ 35-13 *<br />

53-13 *!<br />

31-13 *! *<br />

Add<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>se two constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> tableau <strong>in</strong> (65) does not alter its<br />

w<strong>in</strong>ner—35-423, as shown <strong>in</strong> (72)—(74). Aga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts are not <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> tableaux. 53-423 and 31-423 violate<br />

<strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>t Num(Inf)≤≤2 and thus are not listed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

tableaux.<br />

(72) 13-53 —> 35-423<br />

13-53 Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 35-423 *<br />

13-423 *!<br />

(73) 35-53 —> 35-423<br />

35-53 Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 35-423<br />

13-423 *! *<br />

(74) 53-53 —> 35-423<br />

53-53 Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 35-423 * *<br />

13-423 * * *! *<br />

A summary <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> constriants and <strong>the</strong>ir rank<strong>in</strong>g so far are given <strong>in</strong><br />

(75). These constra<strong>in</strong>ts and <strong>the</strong>ir rank<strong>in</strong>g can also account for <strong>the</strong><br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g sandhi: 13-13 —> 13-13, 53-13 —> 53-13 and 53-35 —><br />

53-35. Tableaux (76)—(78) illustrate <strong>the</strong> derivation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se sandhi<br />

forms.


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 39<br />

(75) Pres(σ 2, T), WFR, Num(Inf)≤2, Num(Inf)≥1<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(53), Dur(B), Pres(σ 1, C)<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

(76) 13-13 —> 13-13<br />

13-13 Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 13-13<br />

35-13 *! *<br />

53-13 *! *<br />

31-13 *!<br />

(77) 53-13 —> 53-13<br />

53-13 Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 53-13<br />

35-13 *! * *<br />

13-13 *! * *<br />

31-13 *! *<br />

(78) 53-35 —> 53-35<br />

53-35 Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 53-35<br />

35-35 *! *<br />

31-35 *! * *<br />

13-35 * * *<br />

(The last candidate loses because it violates <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t Num(Inf)≥1).<br />

Two sandhi forms are still left unaccounted for: 13-35 —> 31-35 and<br />

35-35 —> 31-35. The tableaux <strong>in</strong> (79) and (80) show that <strong>the</strong> above<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts and rank<strong>in</strong>g give us <strong>in</strong>correct sandhi forms—35-35 <strong>in</strong> both<br />

cases.<br />

(79) 13-35 —> 35-35 (<strong>in</strong>correct w<strong>in</strong>ner)<br />

13-35 Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 35-35 *<br />

53-35 *! *<br />

31-35 *! *<br />

13-35


40 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

(The last candidate loses because it violates <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t Num(Inf)≥1).<br />

(80) 35-35 —> 35-35 (<strong>in</strong>correct w<strong>in</strong>ner)<br />

35-35 Pres(53) Dur(B) Pres(σ 1, C) Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 35-35<br />

53-35 *!<br />

31-35 *! * *<br />

13-35 *<br />

(The last candidate loses because it violates <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t Num(Inf)≥1).<br />

To derive <strong>the</strong> actual w<strong>in</strong>ner 31-35 <strong>in</strong> both cases, we need <strong>the</strong><br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g constra<strong>in</strong>t from <strong>the</strong> Register constra<strong>in</strong>t family:<br />

(81) Reg(2) H: no adjacent high registers.<br />

This is a variation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> general constra<strong>in</strong>t Reg(2) proposed <strong>in</strong><br />

Section 2.3 such that it only bans two adjacent high registers. It is<br />

only natural that we should have a Reg(2) L constra<strong>in</strong>t which bans two<br />

adjacent low registers <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar as well. But <strong>in</strong> this case, this<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t is lowly ranked <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> hierarchy and it does not play any role<br />

<strong>in</strong> select<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> actual w<strong>in</strong>ners <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms.<br />

The constra<strong>in</strong>t Reg(2) H has to be ranked above <strong>the</strong> syllable<br />

boundary constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(B) to guarantee that 31-35 w<strong>in</strong>s over 35-35<br />

and 53-35 for <strong>the</strong> base forms 13-35 and 35-35, and it has to be ranked<br />

below Pres(53) to make sure that <strong>the</strong> correct realization <strong>of</strong> 53-35 is<br />

still 53-35. As for <strong>the</strong> sandhi form 35-423, I do not consider <strong>the</strong> second<br />

syllable to have a strictly high register, thus this constra<strong>in</strong>t is not<br />

relevant.<br />

So far, I have given all <strong>the</strong> relevant constra<strong>in</strong>ts for <strong>the</strong> predicateobject/subject-predicate<br />

disyllabic word tone sandhi <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao. The<br />

rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se constra<strong>in</strong>ts is summarized <strong>in</strong> (82).


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 41<br />

(82) Pres(σ2, T), WFR, Num(Inf)≤2, Num(Inf)≥≥1<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(53)<br />

⇓<br />

Reg(2) H<br />

⇓<br />

Dur(B), Pres(σ 1, C)<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

Tableaux (83)-(84) illustrate how 31-35 is derived from <strong>the</strong> base<br />

forms 13-35 and 35-35. Undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>ts are not listed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

tableaux and only candidates which do not violate <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts are enterta<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

(83) 13-35 —> 31-35<br />

13-35 Pres(53) Reg(2) H Dur(B) Pres<br />

(σ 1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 31-35 * *<br />

35-35 *! *<br />

53-35 *! * *<br />

(84) 35-35 —> 31-35<br />

35-35 Pres(53) Reg(2) H Dur(B) Pres<br />

(σ 1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 31-35 * * *<br />

35-35 *!<br />

53-35 *! *<br />

Add<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> last constra<strong>in</strong>t Reg(2) H does not affect <strong>the</strong> analysis we<br />

give to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r sandhi behavior. The derivation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r seven<br />

sandhi forms is given aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> full-fledged tableaux which <strong>in</strong>clude<br />

all <strong>the</strong> violable constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> (85)-(91). Only <strong>the</strong> candidates which do<br />

not violate <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>ts are considered.<br />

(85) 13-13 —> 13-13<br />

13-13 Pres(53) Reg(2) H Dur(B) Pres<br />

(σ 1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 13-13<br />

35-13 *! *<br />

53-13 *! *<br />

31-13 *!


42 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

(86) 35-13 —> 13-13<br />

35-13 Pres(53) Reg(2) H Dur(B) Pres<br />

(σ 1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 13-13 *<br />

35-13 *!<br />

53-13 *!<br />

31-13 *! *<br />

(87) 53-13 —> 53-13<br />

53-13 Pres(53) Reg(2) H Dur(B) Pres<br />

(σ 1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 53-13<br />

35-13 *! * *<br />

13-13 *! * *<br />

31-13 *! *<br />

(88) 53-35 —> 53-35<br />

53-35 Pres(53) Reg(2) H Dur(B) Pres<br />

(σ 1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 53-35 *<br />

35-35 *! * *<br />

31-35 *! * *<br />

(89) 13-53 —> 35-423<br />

13-53 Pres(53) Reg(2) H Dur(B) Pres<br />

(σ 1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 35-423 *<br />

13-423 *!<br />

(90) 35-53 —> 35-423<br />

35-53 Pres(53) Reg(2) H Dur(B) Pres<br />

(σ 1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 35-423<br />

13-423 *! *<br />

(91) 53-53 —> 35-423<br />

53-53 Pres(53) Reg(2) H Dur(B) Pres<br />

(σ 1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1, R)<br />

✿ 35-423 * *<br />

13-423 * * *! *


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 43<br />

Up to now, <strong>the</strong> non-ru-tone sandhi behavior for <strong>the</strong> predicateobject/subject-predicate<br />

disyllabic words <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>yao has been accounted<br />

for. To account for <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g ru-tone sandhi behavior, we need<br />

to rank <strong>the</strong> Dur(R)fk constra<strong>in</strong>t posited <strong>in</strong> (51a) as undom<strong>in</strong>ated.<br />

Example (51) is repeated as (92).<br />

(92) a. Dur(R)fk: a pitch contour x is licensed by a rime whose<br />

sonorant <strong>duration</strong> is at least fk(x).<br />

b. 148ms


44 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

(93) Pres(Dur): preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>duration</strong> characteristic <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

syllable.<br />

(94) F<strong>in</strong>al rank<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

Dur(R)fk, Pres(σ 2, T), WFR, Num(Inf)≤2, Num(Inf)≥1<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(Dur)<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(53)<br />

⇓<br />

Reg(2) H<br />

⇓<br />

Dur(B), Pres(σ 1, C)<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

Suppose f k(423)148ms (aga<strong>in</strong> recall that <strong>the</strong> average <strong>duration</strong><br />

for <strong>the</strong> sonorant portion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> rime <strong>of</strong> a checked syllable is about<br />

148ms), when 423 has to be realized on a sandhi form due to <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>violability <strong>of</strong> Pres(σ 2, T) (when σ 2=53) and Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise,<br />

Pres(Dur) is sacrificed and <strong>the</strong> syllable is leng<strong>the</strong>ned. But this is <strong>the</strong><br />

only context <strong>in</strong> which Pres(Dur) is sacrificed because it is only<br />

dom<strong>in</strong>ated by <strong>the</strong>se undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>ts.<br />

This section has given an OT analysis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> predicate-object/subjectpredicate<br />

disyllabic word tone sandhi <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao, appeal<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts motivated <strong>in</strong> Section 2 or <strong>the</strong>ir variants. The analysis<br />

seems to be more complicated than <strong>the</strong> rule-based one, but <strong>the</strong><br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts are more clearly motivated than <strong>the</strong> rules Bao and Chen’s<br />

analyses appeal to, both by reference to o<strong>the</strong>r tone sandhi systems and<br />

by reference to articulatory and perceptual pr<strong>in</strong>ciples, and <strong>the</strong> advantages<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> OT mechanism have been generally accepted. Moreover, nei<strong>the</strong>r<br />

Bao and Chen’s analysis can be readily adapted to account for <strong>the</strong> tone<br />

sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r categories <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao words. As a matter <strong>of</strong><br />

fact, no attempt has been made <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir papers. In <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

section, I show that <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r major type <strong>of</strong> disyllabic P<strong>in</strong>gyao tone<br />

sandhi can be accounted for by constra<strong>in</strong>ts similar to <strong>the</strong> ones proposed<br />

here, but with m<strong>in</strong>imally different rank<strong>in</strong>g.


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 45<br />

3.3.4. An OT Analysis for Tone Sandhi <strong>of</strong> Disyllabic Words with<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r Syntactic Configurations<br />

Disyllabic words with syntactic configurations o<strong>the</strong>r than predicateobject<br />

or subject-predicate have different tone sandhi behavior than that<br />

we discussed <strong>in</strong> section 3.3.3. Some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se syntactic configurations<br />

are: modifier-noun, verb-verb or noun-noun concatenation and<br />

predicate-adjunct. Hou (1980) documents this type <strong>of</strong> disyllabic tone<br />

sandhi as Type B sandhi while he refers to <strong>the</strong> sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong><br />

predicate-object or subject-predicate disyllabic words as Type A sandhi.<br />

Due to complexity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> data as well as <strong>the</strong> analysis, <strong>the</strong><br />

organization <strong>of</strong> this section is slightly different from <strong>the</strong> previous<br />

section. After <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> data, I give all <strong>the</strong> relevant constra<strong>in</strong>ts<br />

and <strong>the</strong>ir rank<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>the</strong> OT account. The differences <strong>in</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

composition and rank<strong>in</strong>g between Type A and Type B sandhi are<br />

discussed afterwards. How <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g is established is given<br />

with <strong>the</strong> aid <strong>of</strong> full-fledged tableaux which render <strong>the</strong> correct sandhi<br />

forms.<br />

The table <strong>in</strong> (95) summarizes <strong>the</strong> Type B sandhi pattern <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao.<br />

(95) σ 1\σ 2 13 23 35 53 54<br />

13 (yang) 13-13 13- 23 31-35 35-423 35- 423<br />

13 (y<strong>in</strong>) 31-35 31- 45 13-13 31-53 31- 54<br />

23 32 -35 32 - 45 23 -13 32 -53 32 - 54<br />

35 35-53 35- 54 35-53 35-53 35- 54<br />

53 53-13 53- 23 53-35 53-53 53- 54<br />

54 54 -13 54 - 23 54 -35 45 -53 54 - 54<br />

Several th<strong>in</strong>gs are worth notic<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> table. First, when <strong>in</strong> σ 1<br />

position, <strong>the</strong> sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong> 13 tone is divided <strong>in</strong>to two—y<strong>in</strong> and<br />

yang. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to historical documents <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>phonology</strong>, y<strong>in</strong><br />

and yang are <strong>tonal</strong> registers developed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> languages due to <strong>the</strong> loss<br />

<strong>of</strong> voic<strong>in</strong>g constrasts <strong>in</strong> obstruents <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> onset position. Y<strong>in</strong> register<br />

is associated with <strong>the</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>ally voiceless onset obstruents and usually<br />

is higher <strong>in</strong> pitch, while yang register is associated with <strong>the</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>ally<br />

voiced ones and <strong>of</strong>ten lower <strong>in</strong> pitch. In P<strong>in</strong>gyao, although lexically,<br />

y<strong>in</strong> p<strong>in</strong>g 7 and yang p<strong>in</strong>g are both realized as 13, <strong>in</strong> Type B tone sandhi,<br />

<strong>the</strong>y behave differently, cue<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir different orig<strong>in</strong>s. Second, <strong>in</strong> σ 1<br />

position, 23 is a short tone counterpart <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> y<strong>in</strong> p<strong>in</strong>g tone 13 s<strong>in</strong>ce<br />

<strong>the</strong>y have <strong>the</strong> same sandhi behavior; yang p<strong>in</strong>g 13 has exactly <strong>the</strong> same<br />

7 P<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> category that <strong>the</strong> 13 tone <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao belongs to <strong>in</strong> traditional<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese terms.


46 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

sandhi behavior as <strong>the</strong> 13 tone <strong>in</strong> Type A sandhi (cf. (37)). Third, <strong>the</strong><br />

boxed case is <strong>the</strong> only data <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> ru tone does not have <strong>the</strong> same<br />

sandhi as it non-ru counterpart. I consulted J.-Y. Hou--<strong>the</strong> author <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

orig<strong>in</strong>al documents. He assured me that it was not a mistake <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

documentation and he was puzzled by this case as well. I thus take this<br />

case as an anomaly and leave it unaccounted for.<br />

Tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> fact that yang p<strong>in</strong>g 13 has <strong>the</strong> same sandhi behavior as 13<br />

<strong>in</strong> Type A sandhi to be an idiosyncracy <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao and ignor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

anomaly, <strong>the</strong> table <strong>in</strong> (95) can thus be simplified as (96), which<br />

<strong>in</strong>cludes only <strong>the</strong> sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong> non-ru tones. The sandhi <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

ru tone counterparts can be similarly accounted for as it is done <strong>in</strong> Type<br />

A sandhi.<br />

(96) σ1\σ2 13 35 53<br />

13 (y<strong>in</strong>) 31-35 13-13 31-53<br />

35 35-53 35-53 35-53<br />

53 53-13 53-35 53-53<br />

Observe that <strong>in</strong> (96), <strong>the</strong> sandhi form <strong>of</strong> y<strong>in</strong> p<strong>in</strong>g 13 is realized as a<br />

fall<strong>in</strong>g tone except <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> boxed case (we may also observe <strong>in</strong> (95) that<br />

most sandhi forms <strong>of</strong> yang p<strong>in</strong>g 13 are realized as ris<strong>in</strong>g tones).<br />

Consider<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong> y<strong>in</strong> p<strong>in</strong>g tone resulted from an orig<strong>in</strong>ally voiceless<br />

onset while <strong>the</strong> yang p<strong>in</strong>g tone from an orig<strong>in</strong>ally voiced onset, <strong>the</strong><br />

pattern is only phonetically natural. Hombert (1975, 1978)<br />

experimented on <strong>the</strong> fundamental frequency values follow<strong>in</strong>g American<br />

English and Yoruba stops and found that voiceless stops gave rise to a<br />

higher pitch and a fall<strong>in</strong>g contour to <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g vowel, while a lower<br />

pitch and a ris<strong>in</strong>g contour resulted after voiced stops. The phenomenon<br />

can be attributed to various accounts, e.g., aerodynamic effects, vocal<br />

fold tension, and larynx height. Thus <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao, we can<br />

make <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g assumption regard<strong>in</strong>g its historical changes <strong>in</strong> tone:<br />

<strong>the</strong> historical voiceless onsets caused <strong>the</strong> syllable to have a fall<strong>in</strong>g tone,<br />

and <strong>the</strong> historical voiced onsets caused <strong>the</strong> syllable to have a ris<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tone. A fur<strong>the</strong>r assumption we have to make is that <strong>in</strong> present-day<br />

P<strong>in</strong>gyao, this y<strong>in</strong>/yang dist<strong>in</strong>ction has been preserved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi<br />

tones, but is lost <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> citation tones. Given that <strong>the</strong> fact that sandhi<br />

tones are more conservative <strong>in</strong> preserv<strong>in</strong>g historical lexical contrasts has<br />

been observed <strong>in</strong> many o<strong>the</strong>r Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects (Hou and Wen 1993, Wen<br />

1985, Yue-Hashimoto 1987, Zhang 1997), this assumption is at least<br />

factually well-grounded. To capture this effect, we may posit different<br />

underly<strong>in</strong>g representations for <strong>the</strong> sandhi tones and achieve <strong>the</strong><br />

neutralization effect <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> citation forms by constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g. In <strong>the</strong><br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g analysis, I opt to posit a constra<strong>in</strong>t Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang<br />

Preservation to ensure <strong>the</strong> splitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base 13 <strong>in</strong>to two different


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 47<br />

sets <strong>of</strong> behavior <strong>in</strong> sandhi, simply because we have been operat<strong>in</strong>g with<br />

<strong>the</strong> practice <strong>of</strong> deriv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> sandhi tone from <strong>the</strong> base tone. But I would<br />

like <strong>the</strong> readers to deem this as a notational variant <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> canonical<br />

practice <strong>of</strong> consider<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> contrast to be neutralized <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> citation<br />

form, as this constra<strong>in</strong>t is only posited for <strong>the</strong> reason <strong>of</strong> simplicity and<br />

consistency with <strong>the</strong> rest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi account, and no claim such as<br />

<strong>the</strong> historical facts are directly reflected <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> synchronic grammar has<br />

been made.<br />

• Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang Preservation: <strong>in</strong> sandhi forms, y<strong>in</strong> tones are fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and yang tones are ris<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

If we consider <strong>the</strong> boxed sandhi form <strong>in</strong> (89) to be an anomaly, <strong>the</strong><br />

above constra<strong>in</strong>t should be ranked as an undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>t. The<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r constra<strong>in</strong>ts that are relevant to <strong>the</strong> account <strong>of</strong> Type B disyllabic<br />

sandhi <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao are listed below:<br />

From <strong>the</strong> Duration family:<br />

• Dur(W)gk/gk(2)=2: a word cannot have more than two <strong>tonal</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts. (Num(Inf)≤≤2 hereafter)<br />

• Dur(W)gj/gj(2)=1: a word cannot have more than one <strong>tonal</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts. (Num(Inf)≤1 hereafter)<br />

• Dur(W) M<strong>in</strong>(Inf): a word must have at least one <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection<br />

po<strong>in</strong>ts.<br />

(Num(Inf)≥1 hereafter)<br />

• Dur(B)rise0 ∧ Dur(B)fall3: no pitch rise or pronounced pitch<br />

fall across a syllable boundary. (Dur(B) hereafter)<br />

From <strong>the</strong> Faithfulness family:<br />

• Pres(σ 1, C): preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> contour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first syllable.<br />

• Pres(σ 1, R): preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> register <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first syllable.<br />

• Pres(σ 2, C): preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> contour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> second syllable.<br />

• Pres(σ 2, R): preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> register <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> second syllable.<br />

From <strong>the</strong> Register family:<br />

• Reg(2) L: two adjacent low registers is disallowed.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r constra<strong>in</strong>ts:<br />

• Pres(53): preserve <strong>the</strong> property <strong>of</strong> a base high fall<strong>in</strong>g tone <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

sandhi form.<br />

• *Non-Lexical Tone: a tone not <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> lexical <strong>in</strong>ventory is not<br />

allowed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form.<br />

The rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts is shown <strong>in</strong> (97).


48 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

(97) Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang Preservation, Num(Inf)≤2, Num(Inf)≥1<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(σ 1, C), Pres(σ 1, R), Pres(53)<br />

⇓<br />

Num(Inf)≤1, Reg(2) L<br />

⇓<br />

Dur(B), *Non-Lexical Tone, Pres(σ 2, C)<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(σ 2, R)<br />

There are <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts and rank<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from those <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Type A sandhi.<br />

First, while <strong>in</strong> Type A sandhi, σ 2 is <strong>the</strong> “prom<strong>in</strong>ent” syllable and <strong>the</strong><br />

preservation <strong>of</strong> its <strong>tonal</strong> property is highly ranked <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar, <strong>in</strong><br />

Type B sandhi, σ 1 plays <strong>the</strong> “prom<strong>in</strong>ent” role and its tones are more<br />

faithfully reta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms. The phonetic measurement <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> two syllables <strong>in</strong> a Type B word did not reveal significant differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>duration</strong> and energy. But we might aga<strong>in</strong> hope that <strong>the</strong> mystery<br />

<strong>of</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ence will be unveiled by historical study or more advanced<br />

phonetic measurement. 8 Also notice that Pres(σ 1, C) and Pres(σ 1,<br />

R) are not undom<strong>in</strong>ated as <strong>the</strong>ir counterpart Pres(σ 2) <strong>in</strong> Type A<br />

sandhi. But it is only dom<strong>in</strong>ated by <strong>the</strong> Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang Preservation<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t.<br />

Second, Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise is not a relevant constra<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> Type B<br />

tone sandhi. This might have to do with <strong>the</strong> “non-prom<strong>in</strong>ence” <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

second syllable <strong>in</strong> Type B words.<br />

Third, *Non-Lexical Tone is a constra<strong>in</strong>t which does not play<br />

any role <strong>in</strong> select<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> actual w<strong>in</strong>ners <strong>of</strong> Type A sandhi. But this<br />

does not necessarily mean that it is not <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> grammar <strong>of</strong> Type A<br />

at all. It could be because <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t is covered by <strong>the</strong><br />

8 The difference <strong>in</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ence position with regard to syntactic configuration <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

word is not idiosyncratic to P<strong>in</strong>gyao. Yue-Hashimoto (1987) documents that two<br />

nor<strong>the</strong>rn Wu dialects--Tangxi and Shaox<strong>in</strong>g, display both first-syllable dom<strong>in</strong>ant type<br />

and last-syllable dom<strong>in</strong>ant type tone sandhi. A direct quote from Yue-Hashimoto<br />

(1987) is given below:<br />

“In fact, <strong>in</strong> Tangxi, when <strong>the</strong> doma<strong>in</strong> <strong>of</strong> tone sandhi is a compound or a phrase with<br />

stress on <strong>the</strong> first syllable, <strong>the</strong> first-syllable dom<strong>in</strong>ant type <strong>of</strong> sandhi occurs, ..., but when<br />

<strong>the</strong> doma<strong>in</strong> is Verb-Object type <strong>of</strong> Verb Phrase with stress on <strong>the</strong> last syllale, naturally<br />

<strong>the</strong> last-syllable dom<strong>in</strong>ant type occurs; while <strong>in</strong> Shaox<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> first-syllable dom<strong>in</strong>ant type<br />

is found <strong>in</strong> compounds <strong>of</strong> two or three syllables and <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> reduplication <strong>of</strong> classifiers<br />

and verbs but <strong>the</strong> last-syllable dom<strong>in</strong>ant type is found <strong>in</strong> disyllabic phrases and<br />

trisyllabic numeral comb<strong>in</strong>ation.”


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 49<br />

effects <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r constra<strong>in</strong>ts. In fact, rank<strong>in</strong>g this constra<strong>in</strong>t on <strong>the</strong> same<br />

tier with Dur(B) and Pres(σ 1, C) <strong>in</strong> Type A <strong>tonal</strong> grammar (cf.<br />

(82)), as it is ranked on <strong>the</strong> same tier with Dur(B) and Pres(σσ 2, C )<br />

<strong>in</strong> Type B grammar, does not alter any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> its w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g sandhi forms<br />

(Readers may try to <strong>in</strong>sert this constra<strong>in</strong>t by rank<strong>in</strong>g it on a par with<br />

Dur(B) and Pres(σ 1, C) <strong>in</strong> tableaux (83)-(91), and will see that <strong>the</strong><br />

actual w<strong>in</strong>ners are susta<strong>in</strong>ed upon <strong>in</strong>sertion <strong>of</strong> this constra<strong>in</strong>t). The<br />

motivation for this constra<strong>in</strong>t is <strong>the</strong> simplicity <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>ventory <strong>in</strong><br />

sandhi forms.<br />

Fourth, Num(Inf)≤1 is not discussed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> grammar <strong>of</strong> Type<br />

A words ei<strong>the</strong>r. But if it is, it should be ranked as <strong>in</strong>dicated below:<br />

(98) Dur(B), Pres(σ 1, C)<br />

⇓<br />

Num(Inf)≤≤≤≤1<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(σ 1, R)<br />

Thus <strong>the</strong> difference between Type A and Type B <strong>tonal</strong> grammar with<br />

respect to this particular constra<strong>in</strong>t is that it ranks higher <strong>in</strong> Type B<br />

than <strong>in</strong> Type A. The fact that it ranks lower than Num(Inf)≤2 <strong>in</strong><br />

both grammars reflects <strong>the</strong> generalization made <strong>in</strong> Section 2: if g k>g j,<br />

<strong>the</strong>n Dur(W)gk ≥≥ Dur(W)gj (recall that Num(Inf)≤2 is <strong>the</strong><br />

shorthand name for Dur(W)gk/gk(2)=2 and Num(Inf)≤1 is <strong>the</strong><br />

shorthand name for Dur(W)gj/gj(2)=1).<br />

Lastly, unlike Type A sandhi <strong>in</strong> which Reg(2) H is highly ranked <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> grammar while Reg(2) L does not play any role <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

sandhi behavior, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> Type B sandhi, <strong>the</strong> situation is <strong>the</strong><br />

opposite: Reg(2) L is ranked high and Reg(2) H falls out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

picture.<br />

How <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> (97) is determ<strong>in</strong>ed is a long and dreary<br />

process. But <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>ts can be easily sifted out. In<br />

(96), Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang Preservation, Num(Inf)≤2, and Num(Inf)≥≥1<br />

are simply never violated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms (except <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case boxed<br />

<strong>in</strong> (96), which I consider to be an anomaly). Thus it is reasonable to<br />

rank <strong>the</strong>se constra<strong>in</strong>ts at <strong>the</strong> top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hierarchy. Although Pres(53)<br />

is also never violated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi forms, to be consistent with its<br />

rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar for Type A sandhi, I still consider it to be<br />

dom<strong>in</strong>ated only by constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> top tier <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hierarchy. The base<br />

tone on σ 1 is always preserved except when it is y<strong>in</strong> p<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g


50 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

that <strong>the</strong> faithfulness requirements on <strong>the</strong> first syllable—Pres(σ 1, C )<br />

and Pres(σ 1, R), are only outranked by <strong>the</strong> Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang<br />

Preservation constra<strong>in</strong>t. S<strong>in</strong>ce Num(Inf)≤2, and Num(Inf)≥1<br />

are on a par with Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang Preservation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

hierarchy, Pres(σ 1, C) and Pres(σ 1, R) are dom<strong>in</strong>ated by <strong>the</strong>se<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts too.<br />

How <strong>the</strong> rest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g is determ<strong>in</strong>ed is not nearly as<br />

straight-forward. To spare <strong>the</strong> readers excruciat<strong>in</strong>g details, <strong>the</strong> rank<strong>in</strong>g<br />

is illustrated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g way: tableaux (99)—(106) illustrate how<br />

<strong>the</strong> sandhi forms <strong>in</strong> (96) are derived from <strong>the</strong>ir base tones; after each<br />

tableau, its contribution on <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t composition and rank<strong>in</strong>g (if<br />

any) is discussed. In <strong>the</strong>se tableaux, <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>ts are<br />

not listed, and only <strong>the</strong> candidates which do not violate <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts are enterta<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

(99) 13(y<strong>in</strong>)-13 —> 31-35<br />

13-13 Pres<br />

(σ1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ1, R)<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

No.<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤1<br />

Reg<br />

(2) L<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

*N-<br />

Lex<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

R)<br />

✿ 31-35 * * * *<br />

31-13 * *! *<br />

31-53 * *! * * * *<br />

31-31 * *! * * * *<br />

53-13 * *!<br />

53-35 * *! *<br />

53-53 * *! * * * *<br />

This tableau makes <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g contribution to <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

composition and rank<strong>in</strong>g: The w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> 31-35 over 31-13 illustrates<br />

that Reg(2) L is a relevant constra<strong>in</strong>t and Reg(2) L >> Dur(B),<br />

Pres(σ 2, R). The loss <strong>of</strong> 53-13 <strong>in</strong>dicates that Pres(σ 1, C) and<br />

Pres(σ 1, R) should <strong>in</strong>deed be counted as two different constra<strong>in</strong>ts, and<br />

Pres(σσ 1, R) >> Dur(B), *Non-Lex, Pres(σ 2, R).


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 51<br />

(100) 13(y<strong>in</strong>)-53 —> 31-53<br />

13-53 Pres<br />

(σ1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ1, R)<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

No.<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤1<br />

Reg<br />

(2) L<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

*N-<br />

Lex<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

R)<br />

✿ 31-53 * * * *<br />

31-13 * *! * * * *<br />

31-35 * *! * * *<br />

31-31 * *! * * * *<br />

53-53 * *! * *<br />

53-35 * *! *<br />

53-13 * *! * *<br />

(101) 35-13 —> 35-53<br />

35-13 Pres<br />

(σ1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ1, R)<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

No.<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤1<br />

Reg<br />

(2) L<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

*N-<br />

Lex<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

R)<br />

✿ 35-53 * *<br />

35-31 * *!<br />

35-35 *! *<br />

35-13 *! *<br />

Due to <strong>the</strong> loss <strong>of</strong> 35-31 to 35-53, *Non-Lex is a relevant<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t here and should outrank Pres(σ 2,R). Also, that 35-53 is<br />

more harmonic than 35-35 <strong>in</strong>dicates that Num(Inf)≤1 is active and it<br />

outranks Pres(σ 2,C). The loss <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> most faithful candidate 35-13<br />

illustrates that at least one <strong>of</strong> Num(Inf)≤1 and Dur(B) should<br />

outrank both Pres(σ 2,C) and Pres(σ 2,R).<br />

(102) 35-35 —> 35-53<br />

35-35 Pres<br />

(σ1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ1, R)<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

No.<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤1<br />

Reg<br />

(2) L<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

*N-<br />

Lex<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

R)<br />

✿ 35-53 *<br />

35-31 * *! *<br />

35-35 *!<br />

35-13 *! * *<br />

The loss <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> most faithful form 35-35 to 35-53 <strong>in</strong> this tableau<br />

fur<strong>the</strong>r illustrates that Num(Inf)≤1 outranks Pres(σ 2,C).


52 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

(103) 35-53 —> 35-53<br />

35-53 Pres<br />

(σ1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1,<br />

R)<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

No.<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤1<br />

Reg<br />

(2) L<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

*N-<br />

Lex<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

R)<br />

✿ 35-53<br />

35-31 *! * *<br />

35-35 *! * *<br />

35-13 *! * * * *<br />

(104) 53-13 —> 53-13<br />

53-13 Pres<br />

(σ1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1,<br />

R)<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

No.<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤1<br />

Reg<br />

(2) L<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

*N-<br />

Lex<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

R)<br />

✿ 53-13<br />

53-35 *!<br />

53-53 *! * * *<br />

(105) 53-35 —> 53-35<br />

53-35 Pres<br />

(σ1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ 1,<br />

R)<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

No.<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤1<br />

Reg<br />

(2) L<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

*N-<br />

Lex<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

R)<br />

✿ 53-35<br />

53-13 *!<br />

53-53 *! * *<br />

(106) 53-53 —> 53-53<br />

53-53 Pres<br />

(σ1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ1, R)<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

No.<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤1<br />

Reg<br />

(2) L<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

*N-<br />

Lex<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

R)<br />

✿ 53-53 * *<br />

53-13 *! * *<br />

53-35 *! *<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce from tableaux (101) and (102), we know that Num(Inf)≤≤1<br />

>> Pres(σσ 2,C), <strong>the</strong> w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> 53-53 over 53-35 <strong>in</strong> this tableau<br />

illustrates that Pres(53) >> Num(Inf)≤≤1.<br />

The def<strong>in</strong>ite constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g that can be derived through candidate<br />

comparison can thus be summarized as follows:<br />

(107) • Pres(σ 1, R) >> Dur(B), *Non-Lex, Pres(σ 2, R)


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 53<br />

• Pres(53) >> Num(Inf)≤1<br />

• Num(Inf)≤1 >> Pres(σ 2,C)<br />

• Reg(2) L >> Dur(B), Pres(σ 2, R)<br />

• *Non-Lex >> Pres(σ 2,R)<br />

Aside from <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>gs revealed by candidate comparison<br />

as done <strong>in</strong> tableaux (99)—(106), some o<strong>the</strong>r rank<strong>in</strong>g relations are<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>ed by <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciple that <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> grammars for Type A sandhi<br />

and Type B sandhi should be m<strong>in</strong>imally different. Thus <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Type A sandhi grammar, if not driven by contradictory data,<br />

should not be changed <strong>in</strong> Type B sandhi grammar. Consider Pres(σ 1,<br />

T) <strong>in</strong> Type B sandhi corresponds with Pres(σσ 2, T) <strong>in</strong> Type A sandhi<br />

as <strong>the</strong> requirement to preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> property <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ent<br />

syllable, and consider Pres(σ 2, T) <strong>in</strong> Type B sandhi corresponds with<br />

Pres(σ 1, T) <strong>in</strong> Type A sandhi as <strong>the</strong> requirement to preserve <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong><br />

property <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> non-prom<strong>in</strong>ent syllable, <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g rank<strong>in</strong>g relations<br />

for Type B sandhi can be added (cf. (82)):<br />

(108) • Dur(B) >> Pres(σ 2, R)<br />

• Pres(σ 2, C) >> Pres(σ 2, R)<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g directly results from (107), (108)<br />

and <strong>the</strong> discussion before (99).<br />

(109) Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang Preservation, Num(Inf)≤2, Num(Inf)≥1<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(σ 1, C), Pres(σ 1, R), Pres(53)<br />

⇓<br />

Num(Inf)≤1<br />

⇓<br />

Dur(B), *Non-Lexical Tone, Pres(σ 2, C)<br />

⇓<br />

Pres(σ 2, R)<br />

The rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> *Non-Lex and Reg(2) L <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> hierarchy has some<br />

degree <strong>of</strong> freedom. As discussed before, s<strong>in</strong>ce rank<strong>in</strong>g *Non-Lex on a<br />

par with Dur(B) and Pres(σ 1, C) gives us <strong>the</strong> correct sandhi pattern<br />

<strong>of</strong> Type A words, rank<strong>in</strong>g this constra<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g place <strong>in</strong><br />

Type B sandhi, namely, on <strong>the</strong> same tier with Dur(B) and Pres(σ 1,<br />

C), seems to be a reasonable choice. S<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> register dissimilation<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t Reg(2) H <strong>in</strong> Type A sandhi grammar is dom<strong>in</strong>ated by


54 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

Pres(53), it is natural to consider that <strong>the</strong> register dissimilation<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t Reg(2) L <strong>in</strong> Type B sandhi grammar is also dom<strong>in</strong>ated by<br />

this constra<strong>in</strong>t. Then s<strong>in</strong>ce Reg(2) L outranks Dur(B), it should be<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> same tier with Num(Inf)≤1. Thus <strong>the</strong> complete constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

rank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> (97) is established.<br />

The boxed form <strong>in</strong> (96) cannot be derived from this analysis. The<br />

sandhi output <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar for <strong>the</strong> base form 13(y<strong>in</strong>)-35 is 31-35,<br />

and <strong>the</strong> attested form 13-13 loses because it violates <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang Preservation. We consider this case to be an<br />

anomaly. The tableau is shown <strong>in</strong> (110).<br />

(110) 13(y<strong>in</strong>)-35 —> ?<br />

13-35 Pres<br />

(σ1, C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ1, R)<br />

Pres<br />

(53)<br />

No.<br />

(Inf)<br />

≤1<br />

Reg<br />

(2) L<br />

Dur<br />

(B)<br />

*N-<br />

Lex<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

C)<br />

Pres<br />

(σ2,<br />

R)<br />

✿ 31-35 * * *<br />

31-13 * *! * *<br />

31-53 * *! * * *<br />

31-31 * *! * * * * *<br />

53-13 * *! *<br />

53-35 * *!<br />

53-53 * *! * * *<br />

As for <strong>the</strong> sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong> ru tones for this type <strong>of</strong> words, we still<br />

consider <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t from <strong>the</strong> family Duration (Rime)—Dur(R)fk to<br />

be undom<strong>in</strong>ated and <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Pres(Dur) to be dom<strong>in</strong>ated only by<br />

<strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>ts. S<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> undom<strong>in</strong>ated constra<strong>in</strong>ts<br />

Y<strong>in</strong>/Yang Preservation, Num(Inf)≤≤2 and Num(Inf)≥1 do not<br />

require any obligatory concave tone to be realized on ei<strong>the</strong>r one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

syllables <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form, Pres(Dur) is never violated. Thus <strong>the</strong><br />

same tableaux as <strong>in</strong> (99)—(106) will give us <strong>the</strong> correct results <strong>of</strong> rutone<br />

sandhi for Type B disyllabic P<strong>in</strong>gyao words.<br />

The lack <strong>of</strong> 423 (or 423 ) tone on <strong>the</strong> Type B sandhi forms is <strong>the</strong><br />

direct result <strong>of</strong> not hav<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

grammar. In Type A words, <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise is<br />

motivated by <strong>the</strong> “right prom<strong>in</strong>ence” <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> words. Type B words,<br />

characterized by <strong>the</strong> “left prom<strong>in</strong>ence” property, lacks this motivation.<br />

On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, Word Initial Rise is not a natural constra<strong>in</strong>t, or<br />

is a constra<strong>in</strong>t that ranks very low <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar. The reasons are<br />

two-fold. First, s<strong>in</strong>ce a ris<strong>in</strong>g tone takes a longer time to implement<br />

than a fall<strong>in</strong>g tone (Sundberg 1973), and <strong>the</strong> word-<strong>in</strong>itial syllable is<br />

very likely to be shorter than <strong>the</strong> word-f<strong>in</strong>al syllable due to lack <strong>of</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 55<br />

leng<strong>the</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g, Word Initial Rise is somewhat unnatural compared to<br />

Word F<strong>in</strong>al Rise. Second, if <strong>the</strong> ris<strong>in</strong>g pitch is used to facilitate <strong>the</strong><br />

mark<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> prom<strong>in</strong>ence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable, a ris<strong>in</strong>g pitch on <strong>the</strong> word<strong>in</strong>itial<br />

syllable will not be nearly as salient a marker as a ris<strong>in</strong>g pitch<br />

on <strong>the</strong> word-f<strong>in</strong>al syllable because it could be coarticulated or masked by<br />

<strong>the</strong> pitch on <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g syllable. Therefore, <strong>the</strong> lack on concave<br />

tones on <strong>the</strong> Type B sandhi forms is not an accident <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> grammar,<br />

but has articulatory and auditory reasons. These reasons are reflected <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t organization <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> grammar.<br />

This section gives an analysis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tone sandhi behavior <strong>of</strong> Type B<br />

disyllabic P<strong>in</strong>gyao words—modifier-noun compound, verb-verb or<br />

noun-noun concatenation and predicate-adjunct compound. The analysis<br />

is achieved by appeal<strong>in</strong>g to similar constra<strong>in</strong>ts to Type A sandhi and<br />

m<strong>in</strong>imally different constra<strong>in</strong>t rank<strong>in</strong>g. It shows that <strong>the</strong> framework <strong>of</strong><br />

analysis for Type A sandhi <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao can be extended to o<strong>the</strong>r types <strong>of</strong><br />

sandhi <strong>in</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao <strong>in</strong> a clear fashion. This has not been done by <strong>the</strong><br />

previous rule-based analyses proposed by Bao (1990) and Chen (1996).<br />

As we can see from <strong>the</strong> analyses <strong>of</strong> ru tone realization and both Type A<br />

and Type B sandhi <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao, <strong>the</strong> crucial component to an OT account<br />

for <strong>tonal</strong> behavior is <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration, which <strong>in</strong>cludes<br />

three sub-families <strong>of</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ts—Duration (Rime), Duration (Word)<br />

and Duration (Boundary). The account for ru tone realization appeals to<br />

<strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t Dur(R)fk—a member <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration<br />

(Rime); both <strong>the</strong> analyses <strong>of</strong> Type A and Type B sandhi appeal to <strong>the</strong><br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ts Num(Inf)≤2, Num(Inf)≥1, Dur(B)rise0 and<br />

Dur(B)fall3, <strong>the</strong> first two <strong>of</strong> which are members <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

family Duration (Word), and <strong>the</strong> last two <strong>of</strong> which are members <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

constra<strong>in</strong>t family Duration (Boundary). Toge<strong>the</strong>r with <strong>the</strong> faithfulness<br />

requirements <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sandhi form (or surface representation <strong>of</strong> lexical<br />

tone) to <strong>the</strong> base form (or underly<strong>in</strong>g representation <strong>of</strong> lexical tone) and<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r traditional phonological considerations like <strong>the</strong> OCP effect <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>tonal</strong> register, a <strong>tonal</strong> grammar can be achieved to expla<strong>in</strong> complicated<br />

sandhi patterns as well as lexical realization <strong>of</strong> tones.<br />

4. CONCLUSION<br />

In this <strong>the</strong>sis, I have discussed <strong>the</strong> possible composition <strong>of</strong> a <strong>tonal</strong><br />

grammar which aims at account<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>the</strong> tone sandhi behavior as well<br />

as lexical realization <strong>of</strong> tones <strong>in</strong> an OT framework. P<strong>in</strong>gyao—a<br />

nor<strong>the</strong>rn Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialect belong<strong>in</strong>g to J<strong>in</strong> dialect group—was taken as<br />

an example to illustrate <strong>the</strong> construction <strong>of</strong> such a grammar. I have<br />

argued that phonetic <strong>duration</strong> plays a decisive role <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> behavior<br />

<strong>of</strong> a Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialect. The less pronounced pitch contour on a stopclosed<br />

syllable is due to <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>sufficient sonorous rime <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>


56 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

syllable; <strong>the</strong> limited number <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong> <strong>in</strong>flection po<strong>in</strong>ts is partially <strong>the</strong><br />

effect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> limited <strong>duration</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word; and <strong>the</strong> ubiquity <strong>of</strong> <strong>tonal</strong><br />

coarticulation phenomenon is <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> limited <strong>duration</strong> between <strong>the</strong><br />

end pitch <strong>of</strong> a syllable and <strong>the</strong> start<strong>in</strong>g pitch <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> next syllable. These<br />

<strong>duration</strong> considerations are all encoded as constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> family<br />

Duration <strong>in</strong> an OT grammar. Toge<strong>the</strong>r with o<strong>the</strong>r traditional<br />

phonological considerations like faithfulness and OCP, a <strong>tonal</strong> grammar<br />

can be achieved to expla<strong>in</strong> complicated sandhi patterns as well as lexical<br />

realization <strong>of</strong> tones.


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 57<br />

REFERENCES<br />

ABRAMSON, ARTHUR. S. 1979. The Coarticulation <strong>of</strong> Tones: An<br />

Acoustic Study <strong>of</strong> Thai. In T. L. Thongkum, P. Kullavanijaya, V.<br />

Panupong, and T. L. T<strong>in</strong>gsabadh (eds.), Studies <strong>in</strong> Thai and Mon-<br />

Khmer Phonetics and Phonology <strong>in</strong> Honour <strong>of</strong> Eugénie J. A .<br />

Henderson: 2-9. Chulalong-Korn University Press, Bankok.<br />

BAO, ZHIMING. 1990. On <strong>the</strong> Nature <strong>of</strong> Tone. Ph. D. dissertation,<br />

MIT.<br />

___. 1992. Toward a Typology <strong>of</strong> Tone Sandhi. Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

18th Annual Meet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Berkeley L<strong>in</strong>guistics Society, Special<br />

Session on <strong>the</strong> Typology <strong>of</strong> Tone Languages: 1-12.<br />

BLACK, JOHN W. 1970. The Magnitude <strong>of</strong> Pitch Inflection.<br />

Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 6th International Congress <strong>of</strong> Phonetic<br />

Sciences: 177-181. Prague.<br />

BLADON, ANTHONY. 1986. Phonetics for Hearers. In G. McGregor<br />

(ed.), Language for Hearers: 1-24. Oxford: Pergamon Press.<br />

BORDEN, GLORIA J., KATHERINE S. HARRIS and LAWRENCE J.<br />

RAPHAEL. 1994. Speech Science Primer: Physiology, Acoustics<br />

and Perception <strong>of</strong> Speech. Third Edition. Williams and Wilk<strong>in</strong>s,<br />

Baltimore.<br />

CHEN, MATTHEW. 1996. Tonal Geometry—A Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Perspective.<br />

In C.-T. J. Huang and Y.-H. A. Li (ed.), New Horizons <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistics: 49-95. Kluwer Academic Publishers.<br />

COHN, ABIGAIL. 1990. Phonetic and Phonological Rules <strong>of</strong><br />

Nasalization. <strong>UCLA</strong> Ph.D. dissertation, distributed as <strong>UCLA</strong><br />

Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> Phonetics 76.<br />

DELGUTTE, BERTRAND. 1982. Some Correlates <strong>of</strong> Phonetic<br />

Dist<strong>in</strong>ctions at <strong>the</strong> Level <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Auditory Nerve. <strong>in</strong> R. Carlson and<br />

B. Granström (eds.), The Representation <strong>of</strong> Speech <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Peripheral Auditory System: 131-150. Elsevier Biomedical,<br />

Amsterdam.<br />

DUANMU, SAN. 1990. A Formal Study <strong>of</strong> Syllable, Tone, Stress and<br />

Doma<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Languages. Ph. D. dissertation, MIT.<br />

___. 1992. Re-Exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Contour Tone Units <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Languages. Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 18th Annual Meet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Berkeley L<strong>in</strong>guistics Society, Special Session on <strong>the</strong> Typology <strong>of</strong><br />

Tone Languages: 80-91.<br />

___. 1993. Rime Length, Stress, and Association Doma<strong>in</strong>s. Journal<br />

<strong>of</strong> East Asian L<strong>in</strong>guistics 2: 1-44.<br />

___. 1994a. Syllabic Weight and Syllabic Duration: A Correlation<br />

between Phonology and Phonetics. Phonology 11: 1-24.<br />

___. 1994b. Aga<strong>in</strong>st Contour Tone Units. L<strong>in</strong>guistic Inquiry 25:<br />

555-608.


58 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

ERICKSON, DONNA, THOMAS BAER and KATHERINE S. HARRIS.<br />

1983. The Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Strap Muscles <strong>in</strong> Pitch Lower<strong>in</strong>g. In D.<br />

M. Bless and J. H. Abbs (Ed.), Vocal Fold Physiology:<br />

Contemporary Research and Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Issue: 279-285. College-Hill<br />

Press, San Diego.<br />

GREENBERG, STEVEN AND ERIC ZEE. 1979. On <strong>the</strong> Perception <strong>of</strong><br />

Contour Tones. <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> Phonetics 45: 150-<br />

164.<br />

HAN, MIEKO S. and KONG-ON KIM. 1974. Phonetic Variation <strong>of</strong><br />

Vietnamese Tone <strong>in</strong> Disyllabic Utterances. Journal <strong>of</strong> Phonetics 2:<br />

223-232.<br />

HE, WEI. 1979. Huojia Fangyan de Liandu Biandiao (Tone Sandhi <strong>of</strong><br />

Huojia Dialect). Fangyan (Dialects) 1979: 122-136.<br />

HEWITT, MARK S. and MEGAN J. CROWHURST. 1995. Conjunctive<br />

Constra<strong>in</strong>ts and Templates <strong>in</strong> Optimality Theory. Ms., University<br />

<strong>of</strong> British Columbia, University <strong>of</strong> North Carol<strong>in</strong>a.<br />

HOMBERT, JEAN-MARIE 1975. Towards a Theory <strong>of</strong> Tonogenesis:<br />

An Empirical, Physiologically and Perceptually-Based Account <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Development <strong>of</strong> Tonal Contrasts <strong>in</strong> Languages. Ph.D.<br />

dissertation, UC Berkeley.<br />

___. 1978. Consonant Types, Vowel Quality, and Tone. In V. A.<br />

Fromk<strong>in</strong> (Ed.), Tone: A L<strong>in</strong>guistic Survey: 77-111. Academic<br />

Press, New York.<br />

HOU, JINGYI. 1980. P<strong>in</strong>gyao Fangyan de Liandu Biandiao (Tone<br />

Sandhi <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Dialect). Fangyan (Dialects) 1980: 1-14.<br />

___. 1982a. P<strong>in</strong>gyao Fangyan Sanzizu de Liandu Biandiao (Trisyllabic<br />

Word Tone Sandhi <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Dialect). Fangyan (Dialects) 1982:<br />

7-14.<br />

___. 1982b. P<strong>in</strong>gyao Fangyan Guangyongshi Sanzizu de Liandu<br />

Biandiao (Broad-Sensed Trisyllabic Word Tone Sandhi <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao<br />

Dialect). Fangyan (Dialects) 1982: 85-99.<br />

___ and DUANZHENG WEN. 1993. Shanxi Fangyan Diaocha Yanjiu<br />

Baogao (Research Reports on Shanxi Dialects). Shanxi Gaoxiao<br />

Lianhe Chubanshe (The Jo<strong>in</strong>t Publish<strong>in</strong>g House <strong>of</strong> Shanxi<br />

Universities), Taiyuan, Ch<strong>in</strong>a.<br />

HOUSE, DAVID. 1990. Tonal Perception <strong>in</strong> Speech. Lund University<br />

Press, Sweden.<br />

HYMAN, LARRY M. and RUSSELL G. SCHUH. 1974. Universal <strong>of</strong><br />

Tone Rules: Evidence from West Africa. L<strong>in</strong>guistic Inquiry 5:<br />

81-115.<br />

KAKITA, YUKI and SHIZUO HIKI. 1976. Investigation <strong>of</strong> Laryngeal<br />

Control <strong>in</strong> Speech by Use <strong>of</strong> Thyrometer. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Acoustical Society <strong>of</strong> America 59: 669-674.<br />

KAO, DIANA L. (1971). Structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Syllable <strong>in</strong> Cantonese. The<br />

Hague, Mouton.


Zhang—Duration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> P<strong>in</strong>gyao Ch<strong>in</strong>ese 59<br />

KEATING, PATRICIA A. 1990. The W<strong>in</strong>dow Model <strong>of</strong> Coarticulation:<br />

Articulatory Evidence. In J. K<strong>in</strong>gston and M. Beckman (Ed.),<br />

Papers <strong>in</strong> Laboratory Phonology I: 451-475. Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

___. 1996. The Phonology-Phonetics Interface. In U. Kle<strong>in</strong>henz<br />

(Ed.), Interfaces <strong>in</strong> Phonology: 262-278. Studia Grammatica 41.<br />

Akademie Verlag, Berl<strong>in</strong>.<br />

KIRCHNER, ROBERT. 1996. Synchronic Cha<strong>in</strong> Shifts <strong>in</strong> Optimality<br />

Theory. L<strong>in</strong>guistic Inquiry 27: 341-350.<br />

LIN, TAO. and LIJIA WANG. 1992. Yuy<strong>in</strong>xue Jiaocheng (A Course <strong>in</strong><br />

Phonetics). Beij<strong>in</strong>g Daxue Chubanshe (Beij<strong>in</strong>g University Press).<br />

LINDQVIST, JAN. 1972. Laryngeal Articulation Studied on Swedish<br />

Subjects. Speech Transmission Laboratory Quarterly Progress and<br />

Status Report 2-3: 10-27. Dept. <strong>of</strong> Speech Communication,<br />

Speech Transmission Laboratory, Royal Institute <strong>of</strong> Technology,<br />

Stockholm, Sweden.<br />

LIU, LU. 1984. J<strong>in</strong>gphozu Yuyan Jianzhi (J<strong>in</strong>gphoyu) (A Brief Report<br />

on J<strong>in</strong>gpho Language). M<strong>in</strong>zu Chubanshe (National M<strong>in</strong>ority<br />

Publish<strong>in</strong>g House), Beij<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

LÜ, SHUXIANG. 1980. Danyang Fangyan de Shengdiao Xitong (The<br />

Tonal System <strong>of</strong> Danyang Dialect). Fangyan (Dialects) 1980: 85-<br />

122.<br />

OHALA, JOHN. J. 1978. Production <strong>of</strong> tone. In V. A. Fromk<strong>in</strong> (Ed.),<br />

Tone: A L<strong>in</strong>guistic Survey: 5-39. Academic Press, New York.<br />

___ and W. G. EWAN. 1973. Speed <strong>of</strong> Pitch Change. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Acoustical Society <strong>of</strong> America 53: 345.<br />

PRINCE, ALAN and PAUL SMOLENSKY. 1993. Optimality Theory:<br />

Constra<strong>in</strong>t Interaction <strong>in</strong> Generative Grammar. Ms., Rutgers<br />

University, University <strong>of</strong> Colorado, Boulder.<br />

SHEN, TONG. 1981. Laopai Shanghai Fangyan de Liandu Biandiao<br />

(Tone Sandhi <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Old Shanghai Dialect). Fangyan (Dialects)<br />

1981: 131-144.<br />

SHEN, XIAONAN S. 1990. Tonal Coarticulation <strong>in</strong> Mandar<strong>in</strong>. Journal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Phonetics 18: 281-295.<br />

SMOLENSKY, PAUL. 1995. On <strong>the</strong> Structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Constra<strong>in</strong>t<br />

Component <strong>of</strong> Con <strong>of</strong> UG. Handout <strong>of</strong> talk at <strong>UCLA</strong>, 4/7/95.<br />

STEVICK, EARL W. 1969. Tone <strong>in</strong> Bantu. Internation Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

American L<strong>in</strong>guistics 35: 330-341.<br />

SUNDBERG, JOHAN. 1973. Data on Maximum Speed <strong>of</strong> Pitch<br />

Changes. Speech Transmission Laboratory Quarterly Progress and<br />

Status Report 4: 39-47. Dept. <strong>of</strong> Speech Communication, Speech<br />

Transmission Laboratory, Royal Institute <strong>of</strong> Technology,<br />

Stockholm, Sweden.


60 <strong>UCLA</strong> Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 3<br />

TRUBETZKOY, NIKOLAI S. 1939. Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples <strong>of</strong> Phonology.<br />

Translated by C. A. M. Baltaxe. University <strong>of</strong> California Press,<br />

Berkeley, California, 1969.<br />

TYLER, RICHARD S., QUENTINE SUMMERFIELD, ELIZABETH J.<br />

WOOD, and MARIANO A. FERNANDEZ. 1982. Psychoacoustic<br />

and Phonetic Temporal Process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Normal and Hear<strong>in</strong>g-Impaired<br />

Listeners. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Acoustical Society <strong>of</strong> America 72: 740-<br />

752.<br />

WANG, WILLIAM S.-Y. 1967. Phonological Features <strong>of</strong> Tone.<br />

International Journal <strong>of</strong> American L<strong>in</strong>guistics 33.2: 93-105.<br />

WANG, LI. 1985. Haiyu Yuy<strong>in</strong> Shi (A History <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Phonetics). Zhongguo Shehui Kexueyuan Chubanshe (Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Academy <strong>of</strong> Social Sciences Publish<strong>in</strong>g House).<br />

WEN, DUANZHENG. 1985. X<strong>in</strong>zhou Fanyanzhi (X<strong>in</strong>zhou Dialect).<br />

Beij<strong>in</strong>g: Yuwen Chubanshe (The Philology Publish<strong>in</strong>g House).<br />

WRIGHT, MARTHA S. 1983. A Metrical Approach to Tone Sandhi <strong>in</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Dialects. Ph. D. dissertation, University <strong>of</strong><br />

Massachusetts, Amherst.<br />

XU, BAOHUA, ZHENZHU TANG and NAIRONG QIAN. 1981. X<strong>in</strong>pai<br />

Shanghai Fangyan de Liandu Biandiao (Tone Sandhi <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> New<br />

Shanghai Dialect). Fangyan (Dialects) 1981: 145-155.<br />

XU, YI. 1994. Production and Perception <strong>of</strong> Coarticulated Tones.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Acoustical Society <strong>of</strong> America 95: 2240-2253.<br />

___. 1997. Contextual Tonal Variations <strong>in</strong> Mandar<strong>in</strong>. Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Phonetics 25: 61-83.<br />

YIP, MOIRA. 1980. The Tonal Phonology <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. Ph. D.<br />

dissertation, MIT.<br />

___. 1995. Tone <strong>in</strong> East Asian Languages. In J. Goldsmith (Ed.),<br />

Handbook <strong>of</strong> Theoretical Phonology: 476-494. Oxford: Blackwell.<br />

YUE-HASHIMOTO, ANNE O.-K. 1987. Tone Sandhi across Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Dialects. In The Wang Li Memorial Volumes, English volume:<br />

445-474.<br />

ZEE, ERIC and IAN MADDIESON (1979). Tones and Tone Sandhi <strong>in</strong><br />

Shanghai: Phonetic Evidence and Phonological Analysis. <strong>UCLA</strong><br />

Work<strong>in</strong>g Papers <strong>in</strong> Phonetics 45: 93-129.<br />

ZHANG, HONGMING. 1997. Regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Property <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Enter<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Tone <strong>in</strong> Zhongyuan Y<strong>in</strong>yun. Paper presented at <strong>the</strong> N<strong>in</strong>eth North<br />

American Conference on Ch<strong>in</strong>ese L<strong>in</strong>guistics (NACCL-9).<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Victoria, British Columbia.<br />

ZHANG, HONGNIAN. 1985. Zhenjiang Fangyan de Liandu Biandiao<br />

(Tone Sandhi <strong>of</strong> Zhenjiang Dialect). Fangyan (Dialects) 1985:<br />

191-204.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!