14.08.2013 Views

JDM in adolescents 1 Judgment and decision making in ...

JDM in adolescents 1 Judgment and decision making in ...

JDM in adolescents 1 Judgment and decision making in ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Judgment</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong><br />

Wändi Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong><br />

Carnegie Mellon University<br />

To appear <strong>in</strong>:<br />

<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 1<br />

M.K. Dhami, A. Schlottmann, <strong>and</strong> M. Waldmann (Eds.), <strong>Judgment</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g as a<br />

skill: Learn<strong>in</strong>g, development, <strong>and</strong> evolution.<br />

Please address correspondence to Wändi Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, Carnegie Mellon University,<br />

Department of Social <strong>and</strong> Decision Sciences, 5000 Forbes Avenue, Porter Hall 208,<br />

Pittsburgh PA 15213; w<strong>and</strong>i@cmu.edu (email); 412-268-3237 (phone); 412-268-6938 (fax)<br />

This chapter benefited from comments by Sonya Brady, M<strong>and</strong>eep Dhami, Baruch Fischhoff,<br />

Anne Schlottmann, <strong>and</strong> one anonymous reviewer, as well as from research assistance by<br />

M<strong>and</strong>y Holbrook.


Introduction<br />

<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 2<br />

Adolescence is a stage <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>dividuals transition from childhood to adulthood,<br />

typically def<strong>in</strong>ed as spann<strong>in</strong>g the teenage years. Adolescents face many important <strong>decision</strong>s,<br />

often for the first time <strong>in</strong> their lives. These <strong>decision</strong>s may <strong>in</strong>volve dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g alcohol, smok<strong>in</strong>g<br />

cigarettes, us<strong>in</strong>g other substances, <strong>and</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g sex (Eaton et al., 2006; Faden, 2006; Kle<strong>in</strong>,<br />

2006), as well as engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> aggressive <strong>and</strong> violent behaviors (Loeber <strong>and</strong> Hay, 1997). As<br />

newly licensed drivers, <strong>adolescents</strong> need to make <strong>decision</strong>s about driv<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>in</strong>fluence their<br />

risk of experienc<strong>in</strong>g motor-vehicle accidents (Lourens, Vissers, <strong>and</strong> Jessurun; 1999; Turner<br />

<strong>and</strong> McClure, 2003). Adolescents may legally decide to seek part-time employment, with<br />

potential consequences for their performance <strong>in</strong> school (Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, Fegley, <strong>and</strong> Dornbusch,<br />

1993).<br />

Policy makers, parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong> other adults responsible for the welfare of<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> need to decide whether <strong>adolescents</strong> have the competence to make <strong>decision</strong>s on<br />

their own. If adolescent <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g competence is overestimated, then <strong>adolescents</strong> may<br />

feel overwhelmed with <strong>decision</strong>s that are too difficult for them. If it is underestimated,<br />

however, then <strong>adolescents</strong> may needlessly be kept from the autonomy they seem to desire,<br />

<strong>and</strong> may be ill-prepared for mak<strong>in</strong>g their own <strong>decision</strong>s later <strong>in</strong> life.<br />

The question of whether <strong>adolescents</strong> have sufficient <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence also<br />

has legal implications. If <strong>adolescents</strong> have the <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence of adults, one<br />

could argue that they should legally be treated as adults. That is, competent <strong>adolescents</strong><br />

should not need parental consent to seek care for their sexual health, substance use problems,<br />

or mental health. However, it could also be argued that competent <strong>adolescents</strong> should be<br />

tried as adults when they are accused as a crime (see Halpern-Felsher <strong>and</strong> Cauffman, 2001).<br />

Behavioral <strong>decision</strong> research offers a framework for evaluat<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong>, if needed,<br />

improv<strong>in</strong>g adolescent <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g competence, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>cludes the follow<strong>in</strong>g three l<strong>in</strong>es of


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 3<br />

research (Fischhoff, 2008). First, research on <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volves normative analyses<br />

that identify the pr<strong>in</strong>ciples to be followed <strong>in</strong> order to make <strong>decision</strong>s that are most likely to<br />

meet the <strong>decision</strong>-makers goals. Second, descriptive analyses exam<strong>in</strong>e how <strong>adolescents</strong><br />

make <strong>decision</strong>s, with judgments about the quality of their <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g processes be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

made on the basis of these normative pr<strong>in</strong>ciples. Third, prescriptive analyses identify ways to<br />

improve <strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g processes, focus<strong>in</strong>g on fix<strong>in</strong>g the gaps between the<br />

normative <strong>and</strong> the descriptive results. The follow<strong>in</strong>g sections discuss each of these three<br />

l<strong>in</strong>es of research, as it has focused on adolescent <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence. The studies<br />

discussed <strong>in</strong> each section are drawn from research <strong>in</strong> judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g as well<br />

as developmental psychology <strong>and</strong> public health.<br />

Normative Analysis: How Should Adolescents Make Decisions?<br />

Studies of how people make <strong>decision</strong>s typically <strong>in</strong>clude normative judgments about<br />

the quality of these <strong>decision</strong>s, <strong>in</strong> terms of how these <strong>decision</strong>s should have been made. In<br />

general, these normative theories posit that a good <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g process should <strong>in</strong>volve<br />

(a) assess<strong>in</strong>g beliefs about the likelihood that different possible outcomes will occur, for each<br />

of the available options (b) assess<strong>in</strong>g the value of each possible outcome related to each<br />

available option, <strong>in</strong> terms of how well it helps the <strong>decision</strong> maker to achieve his or her goals,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (c) comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g probabilistic beliefs <strong>and</strong> values to choose the option that is most likely to<br />

lead to the best possible outcome for the <strong>decision</strong> maker. The sections below provide more<br />

detail about how normative theories propose to judge the quality of these three <strong>decision</strong>-<br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g processes.<br />

Assess<strong>in</strong>g beliefs<br />

A <strong>decision</strong>-maker’s assessed probability beliefs may be evaluated for correspondence,<br />

<strong>in</strong> terms of comparison to an external objective criterion, <strong>and</strong> coherence, <strong>in</strong> terms of be<strong>in</strong>g


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 4<br />

consistent with other beliefs expressed by the <strong>decision</strong> maker. For example, judged<br />

probabilities about specific events (e.g., gett<strong>in</strong>g a high school diploma by age 20) should be<br />

an accurate reflection of statistical population estimates (e.g., the percent of 20-year olds who<br />

have a high school diploma). Because such statistical estimates are often not available for<br />

every event, probability judgments may also be evaluated by their coherence. That is,<br />

multiple probability judgments made by one <strong>in</strong>dividual should be consistent with each other,<br />

adher<strong>in</strong>g to the rules of probability theory (Fischhoff, 2008; Hammond, 1990). For example,<br />

the judged probability of two events happen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> conjunction (e.g., be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> school next year<br />

<strong>and</strong> gett<strong>in</strong>g a high school diploma by age 20) should be smaller than or equal to the judged<br />

probability of each <strong>in</strong>dividual event happen<strong>in</strong>g on its own. Similarly, the judged probabilities<br />

of mutually exclusive events (e.g., gett<strong>in</strong>g a high-school diploma by age 20 versus not)<br />

should add up to 100%. Such coherence is a necessary, but not a sufficient, condition for<br />

accuracy.<br />

Assess<strong>in</strong>g values<br />

A <strong>decision</strong>-maker’s values may also be evaluated <strong>in</strong> terms of correspondence, <strong>in</strong><br />

terms of adherence to normative st<strong>and</strong>ards, <strong>and</strong> coherence, <strong>in</strong> terms of show<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternal<br />

consistency. Correspondence norms that apply to assessed values <strong>in</strong>clude the sunk cost rule,<br />

which posits that good <strong>decision</strong>s should be made on the basis of expected future outcomes,<br />

without consider<strong>in</strong>g unrecoverable past expenditures. The latter are also referred to as “sunk<br />

costs.” Thus, it is normatively <strong>in</strong>correct to cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>in</strong>vestments <strong>in</strong> a course of action that is<br />

no longer profitable. For example, a good <strong>decision</strong> maker should be will<strong>in</strong>g to stop watch<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a movie that he or she no longer enjoys, even if he or she paid to watch it (see Arkes <strong>and</strong><br />

Blumer, 1985).<br />

Judg<strong>in</strong>g assessed values by their coherence <strong>in</strong>volves compar<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s<br />

choices for <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency, or the extent to which they reflect stable preferences. While


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 5<br />

values may vary across different <strong>decision</strong> makers, an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s values should be unaffected<br />

by normatively irrelevant variations <strong>in</strong> the way <strong>in</strong>formation is presented. For example,<br />

judgments of condom quality should be <strong>in</strong>dependent of whether a type of condom is<br />

described as 95% effective or 5% <strong>in</strong>effective (L<strong>in</strong>ville, Fisher, <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, 1993), <strong>and</strong><br />

evaluations of a medical treatment should be <strong>in</strong>dependent of whether it is described as hav<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a 75% survival rate or as hav<strong>in</strong>g a 25% mortality rate (Lev<strong>in</strong>, Schnittjer, <strong>and</strong> Thee, 1988).<br />

Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g beliefs <strong>and</strong> values to make choices<br />

Normative theories pose that <strong>decision</strong> makers should choose the course of action that<br />

is most likely to lead to an outcome that will best meet their goals, given their values <strong>and</strong><br />

their beliefs. Mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>s typically <strong>in</strong>volves mak<strong>in</strong>g trade-offs, compar<strong>in</strong>g all of the<br />

available options <strong>in</strong> terms of their expected risks <strong>and</strong> benefits. Good <strong>decision</strong> makers may<br />

defensibly choose to take risks to <strong>in</strong>crease the likelihood of ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the benefits they desire<br />

(Goldberg <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, 2000; Goldberg, Halpern-Felsher, <strong>and</strong> Millste<strong>in</strong>, 2002). For<br />

example, <strong>adolescents</strong> may choose to have sex to foster closeness <strong>in</strong> their romantic<br />

relationships, thus accept<strong>in</strong>g the risk of unwanted pregnancy <strong>and</strong> sexually transmitted<br />

<strong>in</strong>fections (Brady <strong>and</strong> Halpern-Felsher, 2008; Furman <strong>and</strong> Shaffer, 2003). Adolescents may<br />

also choose to experiment with smok<strong>in</strong>g cigarettes <strong>and</strong> dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g alcohol to look more “cool”<br />

<strong>and</strong> feel more relaxed, thus accept<strong>in</strong>g the potential damag<strong>in</strong>g health effects (Brady, Song, <strong>and</strong><br />

Halpern-Felsher, 2008). Decisions to engage <strong>in</strong> some potentially risky behaviors, such as<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g sex <strong>and</strong> dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g alcohol, may actually be considered part of a normal adolescence<br />

(Baumr<strong>in</strong>d, 1987), <strong>and</strong>, ultimately, a normal adulthood.<br />

Furthermore, <strong>in</strong>dividuals may need to make different <strong>decision</strong>s <strong>in</strong> order to optimize<br />

their goals, as a result of hav<strong>in</strong>g different beliefs <strong>and</strong> values. A specific option may be likely<br />

to meet one <strong>in</strong>dividual’s goals, but not another’s. If <strong>adolescents</strong> <strong>and</strong> adults show systematic<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> their beliefs <strong>and</strong> values, then there may be <strong>in</strong>stances <strong>in</strong> which they should not


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 6<br />

be mak<strong>in</strong>g the same <strong>decision</strong>s. Moreover, their differences <strong>in</strong> beliefs <strong>and</strong> values may make it<br />

difficult for them to judge the quality of one another’s <strong>decision</strong>s (Keren <strong>and</strong> Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>,<br />

2003).<br />

Potential shortcom<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

Because <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g outcomes are easier to observe than <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g<br />

processes, it may be tempt<strong>in</strong>g to evaluate the quality of <strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong>s by the<br />

outcomes they experience (Baron <strong>and</strong> Hershey, 1988). People who do so may fail to<br />

recognize that <strong>decision</strong>s tend to be made under uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty, with chance play<strong>in</strong>g a role <strong>in</strong><br />

determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the obta<strong>in</strong>ed outcomes. As a result of chance, good <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g processes<br />

may sometimes result <strong>in</strong> a poor outcome, <strong>and</strong> bad <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g processes may sometimes<br />

result <strong>in</strong> a good outcome. Normative theories therefore posit that <strong>decision</strong>s should be<br />

evaluated on the basis of their processes, rather than their outcomes (e.g., Edwards, Kiss,<br />

Majone, <strong>and</strong> Toda, 1984; Keren <strong>and</strong> Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, 2003; Raiffa, 1968; von W<strong>in</strong>terfeldt<br />

<strong>and</strong> Edwards, 1986). In the long run, however, better <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g processes should result<br />

<strong>in</strong> better <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g outcomes – <strong>in</strong> terms of fulfill<strong>in</strong>g the goals of the <strong>decision</strong> maker<br />

(Frisch <strong>and</strong> Clemen, 1994; Frisch <strong>and</strong> Jones, 1993; Hershey <strong>and</strong> Baron, 1992, 1995).<br />

If <strong>adolescents</strong> are judged by the outcomes they experience, they may appear to be<br />

poor <strong>decision</strong> makers. Indeed, <strong>adolescents</strong> may be more likely than adults to experience<br />

specific negative life outcomes. For example, <strong>adolescents</strong> are more likely than adults to be<br />

<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> motor vehicle accidents, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those related to dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> driv<strong>in</strong>g (Lourens<br />

et al., 1999; National Highway Traffic Safety Adm<strong>in</strong>istration, 2003; Turner <strong>and</strong> McClure,<br />

2003). Sexually active <strong>adolescents</strong> are more likely than sexually active adults to experience<br />

condom failures (Crosby, S<strong>and</strong>ers, Yarber, <strong>and</strong> Graham, 2003; Macaluso et al., 1999), <strong>and</strong> to<br />

acquire sexually transmitted <strong>in</strong>fections such as HIV (Centers for Disease Control <strong>and</strong><br />

Prevention, 2004). Compared to adult women, adolescent females are more likely to


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 7<br />

experience sexual assault (Humphrey <strong>and</strong> White, 2000; Smith, White, <strong>and</strong> Holl<strong>and</strong>, 2003),<br />

<strong>and</strong> have higher abortion rates (Jones, Darroch, <strong>and</strong> Henshaw, 2002). Although the<br />

experience of negative life outcomes may be affected by uncontrollable factors such as<br />

disadvantaged socio-economic status (Brady <strong>and</strong> Mathhews, 2002) <strong>and</strong> exposure to violence<br />

(Brady, Tschann, Pasch, Flores, <strong>and</strong> Ozer, 2008), the choices <strong>in</strong>dividuals make presumably<br />

also affect the outcomes they experience <strong>in</strong> their lives.<br />

Yet, some have argued that it is appropriate for adults to judge the quality of<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong>s, because they may have <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to how <strong>adolescents</strong> will feel about<br />

their <strong>decision</strong>s <strong>in</strong> their adult future (Reyna <strong>and</strong> Farley, 2006). That argument may be<br />

especially relevant for adolescent <strong>decision</strong>s that may have consequences persist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to<br />

adulthood. Studies <strong>in</strong> a variety of doma<strong>in</strong>s suggest that <strong>adolescents</strong>’ risky choices are <strong>in</strong>deed<br />

related to experienc<strong>in</strong>g negative outcomes later <strong>in</strong> life. For example, <strong>in</strong>itiat<strong>in</strong>g cigarette<br />

smok<strong>in</strong>g at a younger age is related to smok<strong>in</strong>g more <strong>and</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g nicot<strong>in</strong>e dependence <strong>in</strong><br />

subsequent years (Gervais, O’Loughl<strong>in</strong>, Meshefedjian, Bancej, <strong>and</strong> Tremblay, 2006) <strong>and</strong><br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g a higher risk of gett<strong>in</strong>g lung cancer as an adult (Hegmann et al., 1993). Start<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

dr<strong>in</strong>k at a younger age is related to develop<strong>in</strong>g alcohol dependence <strong>in</strong> adulthood, as well as to<br />

academic difficulties, employment problems, substance use, <strong>and</strong> violent behaviors – all of<br />

which may affect life outcomes experienced <strong>in</strong> adulthood (Ellickson, Tucker, <strong>and</strong> Kle<strong>in</strong>,<br />

2003; Grant <strong>and</strong> Dawson, 1997; Gruber, DiClemente, Anderson, <strong>and</strong> Lodico, 1996).<br />

Adolescent obesity, presumably reflect<strong>in</strong>g unhealthy choices <strong>in</strong> diet <strong>and</strong> exercise, is likely to<br />

persist <strong>in</strong>to adulthood, <strong>and</strong> is related to <strong>in</strong>creased risk of heart disease, other health problems,<br />

<strong>and</strong> premature death (Must, 1996). Adolescent females are physiologically more susceptible<br />

than adult women to acquir<strong>in</strong>g sexually transmitted <strong>in</strong>fections (Critchlow et al., 1995), whose<br />

long-term consequences may <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong>fertility, <strong>and</strong> cervical cancer (Dillner et al., 1997;<br />

Grodste<strong>in</strong>, Goldman, <strong>and</strong> Cramer, 1993; Wall<strong>in</strong> et al., 2002). Because the reported results


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 8<br />

tend to be correlational, it is unclear whether, for these specific topics, <strong>decision</strong>s made as an<br />

adolescent did <strong>in</strong>deed affect the outcomes experienced as an adult. However, logically, it<br />

makes sense that some of <strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong>s will have long-term consequences,<br />

potentially caus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>al regret <strong>in</strong> adulthood.<br />

Us<strong>in</strong>g adults’ feel<strong>in</strong>gs of regret as a st<strong>and</strong>ard for judg<strong>in</strong>g the quality of life <strong>decision</strong>s<br />

may also mean question<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>s made <strong>in</strong> adulthood. When asked to report their life<br />

regrets, adults <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>decision</strong>s that are likely made <strong>in</strong> adulthood, such as not work<strong>in</strong>g harder<br />

<strong>in</strong> one’s career, or not gett<strong>in</strong>g a divorce (Gilovich <strong>and</strong> Medvec, 1995; Lecci, Ocun, <strong>and</strong><br />

Karoly, 1994; Wrosch <strong>and</strong> Heckhausen, 2002).<br />

Nevertheless, regret may be used as a st<strong>and</strong>ard for evaluat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>s. Researchers<br />

<strong>in</strong> judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g have developed regret theory, def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g regret as the<br />

difference between the experienced outcome <strong>and</strong> the best outcome that would have been<br />

experienced if another course of action had been chosen (Loomes <strong>and</strong> Sugden, 1982). Taken<br />

as a normative theory of <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g, regret theory posits that good <strong>decision</strong>s are ones<br />

that m<strong>in</strong>imize regret, given the <strong>decision</strong> maker’s beliefs <strong>and</strong> values. Do<strong>in</strong>g so would entail<br />

the avoidance of choos<strong>in</strong>g options that have any chance of lead<strong>in</strong>g to a catastrophic outcome<br />

– even if, overall, it has the best chance of fulfill<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>decision</strong>-maker’s goals. In that<br />

sense, regret theory deviates from traditional normative theories of <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The next section presents descriptive studies of how <strong>adolescents</strong> actually make<br />

judgments <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong>s, as compared to normative st<strong>and</strong>ards. It exam<strong>in</strong>es whether<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> have the ability to adhere to the various normative pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

<strong>and</strong> are able to apply these skills to <strong>decision</strong>s when faced with the pressures of real-world<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s. It provides possible explanations for why <strong>adolescents</strong> may be more likely than<br />

adults to take seem<strong>in</strong>gly unwise risks, <strong>in</strong> some real-world contexts.


Descriptive Analysis: How Do Adolescents Make Decisions?<br />

<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 9<br />

Descriptive behavioural research exam<strong>in</strong>es how people actually make judgments <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>decision</strong>s, typically compar<strong>in</strong>g elements of their <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g performance to specific<br />

normative st<strong>and</strong>ards. For example, research participants may be given questions that ask<br />

them to assess the probability of various events happen<strong>in</strong>g to them, to exam<strong>in</strong>e whether their<br />

assessed beliefs are <strong>in</strong>ternally consistent. Similarly, research participants may be given<br />

vignettes describ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g scenarios <strong>in</strong> which costs have been sunk, to exam<strong>in</strong>e<br />

whether their values follow the sunk-cost rule. Most of these studies hone <strong>in</strong> on specific<br />

elements of the <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g process, systematically vary<strong>in</strong>g elements of hypothetical<br />

<strong>decision</strong> scenarios. Such experiments have contributed to researchers’ underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of<br />

when <strong>and</strong> why people’s <strong>decision</strong>s violate pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of coherence <strong>and</strong> correspondence (Dawes<br />

<strong>and</strong> Hastie, 2001; Kahneman, Slovic, <strong>and</strong> Tversky, 1982; Yates, 1990).<br />

Assum<strong>in</strong>g that the conditions under which <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g errors occur generalize to<br />

the broader population, judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g researchers have typically recruited<br />

undergraduate college students for participation <strong>in</strong> their studies. Participants under the age of<br />

18 are more difficult to recruit, requir<strong>in</strong>g approval from officials at the schools or other<br />

organizations through which they are contacted, as well as written parental consent<br />

(Esbensen, Melde, Taylor, <strong>and</strong> Peterson, 2008). Perhaps as a result, the descriptive studies<br />

that have been conducted <strong>in</strong> the field of judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g have largely ignored<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence, as well as the developmental<br />

progression of <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence across the lifespan. However, there is a small<br />

but grow<strong>in</strong>g body of research on <strong>in</strong>dividual differences as well as adolescent <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(see Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, Parker <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, 2007a; Jacobs <strong>and</strong> Klaczynski, 2002; Lopes,<br />

1987; Parker <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, 2005; Stanovich <strong>and</strong> West, 1998, 2000, 2008).


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 10<br />

Traditionally, developmental research of cognitive development has assumed a l<strong>in</strong>ear<br />

progression toward greater logic <strong>and</strong> efficiency <strong>in</strong> adulthood (Piaget <strong>and</strong> Inhelder, 1978). By<br />

contrast, descriptive research <strong>in</strong> judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g seems to suggest that adults<br />

may not always follow normative pr<strong>in</strong>ciples when mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>s (Dawes <strong>and</strong> Hastie,<br />

2001; Kahneman, Slovic, <strong>and</strong> Tversky, 1982; Yates, 1990). Therefore, the quality of<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong>s should not just be evaluated <strong>in</strong> terms of normative st<strong>and</strong>ards, but also<br />

<strong>in</strong> comparison to adult <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Whether or not they follow normative pr<strong>in</strong>ciples,<br />

adults are considered competent <strong>in</strong> the eyes of the law. It could therefore be argued that<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> should earn the same rights as adults when their <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g skills become<br />

similar to those of adults (Halpern-Felsher <strong>and</strong> Cauffman, 2001).<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g section discusses descriptive research on adolescent <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

as compared to normative st<strong>and</strong>ards, as well as adult <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. It is based on recent<br />

reviews of the literature that describe how <strong>adolescents</strong> perform on the judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong><br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g tasks that have traditionally been studied us<strong>in</strong>g adults (Fischhoff, 2008; Jacobs <strong>and</strong><br />

Klaczynski, 2001; Reyna <strong>and</strong> Farley, 2006). Like the previous section on normative <strong>decision</strong><br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g, the present section is divided <strong>in</strong> the ability (a) to assess beliefs, (b) to assess values,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (c) to comb<strong>in</strong>e beliefs <strong>and</strong> values to choose between options.<br />

Assess<strong>in</strong>g beliefs<br />

Studies on <strong>adolescents</strong>’ ability to assess probability beliefs have used st<strong>and</strong>ards of<br />

correspondence as well as st<strong>and</strong>ards of coherence. Few studies have evaluated <strong>adolescents</strong>’<br />

probability judgments by the st<strong>and</strong>ards of coherence. Those that have seem to suggest that<br />

children <strong>and</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> may be better able than adults to judge probabilities that are<br />

<strong>in</strong>ternally consistent. For example, as children transition <strong>in</strong>to adolescence <strong>and</strong> young<br />

adulthood, they become more, rather than less, likely to commit the conjunction fallacy -- by<br />

judg<strong>in</strong>g higher probabilities for the comb<strong>in</strong>ation of two events happen<strong>in</strong>g than each


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 11<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual event happen<strong>in</strong>g (Davidson, 1995; Klaczynksi, 2001). However, they are also<br />

more likely to judge probabilities on the basis of base rates rather than stereotypes, thus be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

able to avoid the representativeness heuristic (Jacobs <strong>and</strong> Potenza, 1991).<br />

Studies that use st<strong>and</strong>ards of correspondence suggest that <strong>adolescents</strong>’ judged<br />

probabilities of different events happen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> their lives are related to their actual life<br />

experiences. In the 1997 wave of the National Longitud<strong>in</strong>al Study of Youth, a nationally<br />

representative sample of U.S. <strong>adolescents</strong> was asked to judge their probability of<br />

experienc<strong>in</strong>g various significant life events, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g gett<strong>in</strong>g a high school diploma by age<br />

20. Their mean judged probability for this event was significantly related to self-reports of<br />

whether or not these adolescent respondents were <strong>in</strong> school, as given on the survey (Fischhoff<br />

et al., 2000). The National Longitud<strong>in</strong>al Study of Youth has been able to follow these<br />

adolescent respondents through early adulthood, ask<strong>in</strong>g them every subsequent year whether<br />

or not various events have happened <strong>in</strong> their lives. Longitud<strong>in</strong>al analyses showed that the<br />

probability judgments of hav<strong>in</strong>g a high school diploma by age 20, as given by participat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> <strong>in</strong> 1997 were relatively accurate, with participants’ mean probability judgment<br />

reflect<strong>in</strong>g the proportion of these participants who reported hav<strong>in</strong>g a high school diploma by<br />

the time they were surveyed at age 20, <strong>and</strong> valid, as seen <strong>in</strong> significant correlations between<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals’ probability judgments <strong>and</strong> self-reports of whether or not they had a high school<br />

diplma at age 20 (Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, Parker, <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, 2007b).<br />

Additional research suggested that <strong>adolescents</strong>’ judged probabilities may even have<br />

predictive validity <strong>in</strong> contexts that are emotionally charged. Female <strong>adolescents</strong>’ judged<br />

probabilities of whether they may be <strong>in</strong>fected with Chlamydia have been shown to predict<br />

whether or not they subsequently test positive on a cl<strong>in</strong>ical Chlamydia test, even after<br />

controll<strong>in</strong>g for demographic variables <strong>and</strong> sexual history (Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, Downs, Murray,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, <strong>in</strong> press). Thus, these probability judgments may be simpler, less obtrusive,


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 12<br />

<strong>and</strong> conta<strong>in</strong> more <strong>in</strong>formation than self-reports of demographic background <strong>and</strong> sexual<br />

activity that typically <strong>in</strong>form physicians’ <strong>decision</strong>s about whether or not to give patients a<br />

cl<strong>in</strong>ical Chlamydia test.<br />

However, st<strong>and</strong>ards of correspondence prescribe that it is not sufficient for <strong>decision</strong><br />

makers to judge probabilities that have external validity, <strong>in</strong> terms of be<strong>in</strong>g related to their<br />

actual life experiences. To <strong>in</strong>form effective <strong>decision</strong>s, probability judgments should also be<br />

accurate, <strong>in</strong> terms of reflect<strong>in</strong>g the objective percent chance that the predicted event will<br />

happen. Significant correlations between judged probabilities <strong>and</strong> outcomes may be observed<br />

even when <strong>decision</strong> makers misjudge the absolute magnitude of probabilities. Indeed,<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong>’ judged probabilities of dy<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the next year <strong>and</strong> by age 20 are significantly<br />

correlated to the threats they perceive <strong>in</strong> their lives – yet, their absolute probability judgments<br />

show that they largely overestimate their actual probability of dy<strong>in</strong>g (Fischhoff, Bru<strong>in</strong>e de<br />

Bru<strong>in</strong>, Parker, Millste<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Halpern-Felsher, <strong>in</strong> press). By comparison, adults seem better<br />

capable than <strong>adolescents</strong>, at judg<strong>in</strong>g their probability of dy<strong>in</strong>g by a certa<strong>in</strong> age (Hurd <strong>and</strong><br />

McGarry, 1995, 2002).<br />

Compared to adults, <strong>adolescents</strong> also overestimate their likelihood of experienc<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

variety of health-related risks, such as becom<strong>in</strong>g addicted to alcohol, cigarettes or other<br />

various substances, gett<strong>in</strong>g a sexually transmitted disease, <strong>and</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g an unwanted pregnancy<br />

(Cohn, Macfarlane, Yanez, <strong>and</strong> Imai, 1995; Millste<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> Halpern-Felsher, 2002; Quadrel,<br />

Fischhoff, <strong>and</strong> Davis, 1993). As <strong>adolescents</strong> age <strong>and</strong> ga<strong>in</strong> experience with the related risky<br />

behaviors (e.g., dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g alcohol smok<strong>in</strong>g cigarettes, us<strong>in</strong>g substances, hav<strong>in</strong>g sex), they may<br />

learn that negative outcomes are not actually as likely as they perceived them to be. For<br />

example, sexually active <strong>adolescents</strong> tend to have lower <strong>and</strong> more realistic judgments of the<br />

risks of gett<strong>in</strong>g pregnant or contract<strong>in</strong>g HIV (Gerrard, Gibbons, Benth<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Hessl<strong>in</strong>g, 1996;<br />

Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001).


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 13<br />

Adolescents’ tendency to overestimate these probabilities can only partially be<br />

expla<strong>in</strong>ed by their confusion about how to accurately use probabilities. Adolescents are more<br />

likely than adults to use “50%” when they do not know what number to use <strong>in</strong> response to a<br />

probability question, lead<strong>in</strong>g to overestimations of small risks (Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, Fischhoff,<br />

Millste<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Halpern-Felsher, 2000). However, even when 50% responses are removed<br />

from the analyses, <strong>adolescents</strong>’ judged probabilities may still be higher than adults’ judged<br />

probabilities.<br />

Assess<strong>in</strong>g values<br />

Few descriptive studies have exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>adolescents</strong>’ ability to assess values. Those<br />

that have used the st<strong>and</strong>ards of coherence <strong>and</strong> correspondence, <strong>and</strong> show mixed results.<br />

Some studies suggest support for traditional theories of l<strong>in</strong>ear developmental progression.<br />

That evidence comes from studies about correspondence to normative rules. Compared to<br />

early-<strong>adolescents</strong>, mid-<strong>adolescents</strong> are less likely to commit a variety of reason<strong>in</strong>g errors<br />

(Klaczynski, 2001), <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the sunk-cost fallacy (Klaczynski <strong>and</strong> Cottrell, 2004).<br />

However, despite the possible age-related improvement, adults still honor sunk costs when<br />

normative rules hold that they should not (Arkes <strong>and</strong> Blumer, 1985).<br />

Other evidence questions the traditional theories of l<strong>in</strong>ear developmental progression.<br />

Compared to older <strong>decision</strong> makers, young children aged 4-5 are less likely to make fram<strong>in</strong>g<br />

errors, such that their preferences are less likely to be affected by normatively irrelevant<br />

variations <strong>in</strong> the description of <strong>decision</strong> options (Reyna <strong>and</strong> Ellis, 1994). Thus, <strong>adolescents</strong>’<br />

values may show less coherence than that of children, <strong>in</strong> the sense of show<strong>in</strong>g consistency<br />

across different frames. Adults’ preferences are also <strong>in</strong>consistent, <strong>in</strong> terms of show<strong>in</strong>g this<br />

fram<strong>in</strong>g effect (Tversky <strong>and</strong> Kahneman, 1981).


Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g beliefs <strong>and</strong> values<br />

<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 14<br />

Few studies have exam<strong>in</strong>ed how <strong>adolescents</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>e beliefs <strong>and</strong> values, when<br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g their <strong>decision</strong>s. Those that have been conducted suggest that <strong>adolescents</strong> are<br />

outperformed by adults. Compared to adults, <strong>adolescents</strong> generate fewer risks <strong>and</strong> benefits of<br />

<strong>decision</strong> options (Beyth-Marom, Aust<strong>in</strong>, Fischhoff, Palmgren, <strong>and</strong> Jacobs-Quadrel, 1993;<br />

Halpern-Felsher <strong>and</strong> Cauffman, 2001) <strong>and</strong> are less likely to report seek<strong>in</strong>g advice to support<br />

their <strong>decision</strong>s (Halpern-Felsher <strong>and</strong> Cauffman, 2001). This f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g may be due to<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g less likely than adults to recognize the limitations of their <strong>decision</strong>-relevant<br />

knowledge. Indeed, some studies suggest that such overconfidence <strong>in</strong> <strong>decision</strong>-relevant<br />

knowledge decreases with age (Crawford <strong>and</strong> Stankov, 1996; Klaczynski, 2001).<br />

Potential shortcom<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

It should be noted that the developmental studies that have been described <strong>in</strong> this<br />

section used a cross-sectional design compar<strong>in</strong>g different groups of adults <strong>and</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong>,<br />

rather than a longitud<strong>in</strong>al design follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>adolescents</strong> as they grow older over time. One<br />

potential draw-back of cross-sectional designs is that differences between <strong>adolescents</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

adults may reflect variations <strong>in</strong> the experiences of the <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> these cohorts rather than<br />

developmental differences. For example, <strong>adolescents</strong>’ exposure to exaggerated warn<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

about the risks of hav<strong>in</strong>g sex may lead them to judge higher risks of gett<strong>in</strong>g pregnant or<br />

sexually transmitted <strong>in</strong>fections compared to adults who were not subject to such education<br />

(Cohn et al., 1995; Millste<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> Halpern-Felsher, 2002; Quadrel et al., 1993). Such cohort<br />

effects may vary as the nature of sexuality education changes over time.<br />

To date, descriptive studies exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g how <strong>adolescents</strong> perform on various traditional<br />

judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g tasks have suggested a mixed pattern of results. Adolescents’<br />

assessments of probabilities may be coherent, <strong>in</strong> terms of show<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency,<br />

without necessarily show<strong>in</strong>g correspondence to external st<strong>and</strong>ards. Possibly, coherence


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 15<br />

reflects a cognitive skill that <strong>adolescents</strong> have mastered, while correspondence improves with<br />

experience that <strong>adolescents</strong> still lack. The latter is supported by the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>adolescents</strong>’<br />

probability judgments become more accurate as they ga<strong>in</strong> experience with risky behaviours<br />

<strong>and</strong> related outcomes (Gerrard et al., 1996; Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001).<br />

Fewer studies have exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>adolescents</strong>’ ability to assess values, or to comb<strong>in</strong>e<br />

beliefs <strong>and</strong> values to make <strong>decision</strong>s. However, here, too, a mixed pattern of results emerged,<br />

across studies us<strong>in</strong>g different measures, <strong>in</strong> terms of the development of these abilities with<br />

age. Some studies showed more <strong>and</strong> others showed less normatively appropriate behaviour<br />

with age. When adults are shown to deviate from normative rules <strong>in</strong> their judgments <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>decision</strong>s, their presumed errors seem to show a systematic rather than a r<strong>and</strong>om pattern.<br />

These results suggest that adults are not confused about how to answer, but, rather, have<br />

learned to systematically violate specific normative rules as they age. Possibly, they are<br />

rely<strong>in</strong>g on simplify<strong>in</strong>g heuristics, or rules of thumb, to make their judgments <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong>s.<br />

It has been argued that us<strong>in</strong>g these heuristics may be adaptive, because they reduce the<br />

cognitive capacity needed to make effective judgments <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong>s (Gigerenzer, Todd, <strong>and</strong><br />

the ABC Group, 1999). Moreover, these heuristics may even be appropriate, if real-world<br />

judgments <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong>s are structured <strong>in</strong> a way that is different from those used <strong>in</strong> these<br />

judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g studies. For example, it has been argued that recipients<br />

should be sensitive to whether real-world messages are framed <strong>in</strong> positive or negative terms,<br />

because it presents relevant <strong>in</strong>formation held by the speaker (McKenzie, 2004; Sher <strong>and</strong><br />

McKenzie, 2005; McKenzie <strong>and</strong> Nelson, 2003).<br />

Recent theories of adolescent <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g have adopted a dual-process approach<br />

to expla<strong>in</strong> the mixed pattern of results described above. Although dual-process approaches<br />

are still <strong>in</strong> need of development (for critiques, see Keren <strong>and</strong> Schul, <strong>in</strong> press), they generally<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>guish between analytical <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tuitive abilities (e.g., Evans <strong>and</strong> Over, 1996; Sloman,


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 16<br />

1996; Stanovich <strong>and</strong> West, 2000). Analytical abilities may be needed to figure out how to<br />

adhere to normative rules of judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g, while <strong>in</strong>tuitive ability may<br />

additionally be needed to make <strong>decision</strong>s <strong>in</strong> the real world. Developmental dual-process<br />

theories suggest that these two sets of abilities may mature at different rates, such that<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> may have the analytical ability needed to follow complex normative pr<strong>in</strong>ciples,<br />

but lack the <strong>in</strong>tuitive ability to make real-world <strong>decision</strong>s. For example, it has been<br />

suggested that, compared to adults, <strong>adolescents</strong> are still lack<strong>in</strong>g the emotional/<strong>in</strong>tuitive ability<br />

needed to recognize when heuristics may benefit <strong>decision</strong>s (Jacobs <strong>and</strong> Klaczynski, 2001;<br />

Klaczynksi <strong>and</strong> Cottrell, 2004); quickly comprehend the <strong>in</strong>tuitive gist of a risky <strong>decision</strong><br />

(Reyna, 2004; Reyna <strong>and</strong> Farley, 2006); resist “hot” impulses sway<strong>in</strong>g them from the<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s they had planned to make <strong>in</strong> a “cold” state (Gerrard, Gibbons, Houlihan, Stock, <strong>and</strong><br />

Pomery, 2008); have sufficient self-regulation to <strong>in</strong>hibit their drive to seek rewards<br />

(Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, 2008); <strong>and</strong> to resist tak<strong>in</strong>g risks <strong>in</strong> the presence of their peers that they would not<br />

have taken if they were on their own (Gardner <strong>and</strong> Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, 2005).<br />

If <strong>adolescents</strong>’ ability to make hypothetical <strong>decision</strong>s <strong>in</strong> the psychological laboratory<br />

does not translate to the <strong>decision</strong>s they make <strong>in</strong> the real world, then traditional judgment <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g studies may have limited external validity (Dhami, Hertwig, <strong>and</strong> Hoffrage,<br />

2004; Gigerenzer et al., 1999; Kle<strong>in</strong>, 1999). Fortunately, two recent studies have suggested<br />

that performance on traditionally studied hypothetical judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g tasks is<br />

significantly correlated to real-world <strong>decision</strong> outcomes, thus suggest<strong>in</strong>g some level of<br />

external validity (Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong> et al., 2007a; Parker <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, 2005). Both studies<br />

used a comprehensive measure of Decision-Mak<strong>in</strong>g Competence, one developed for<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> (Youth-DMC; Parker <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, 2005) <strong>and</strong> one developed for adults (Adult-<br />

DMC; Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong> et al., 2007a), cover<strong>in</strong>g hypothetical <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g tasks that have<br />

traditionally been used to exam<strong>in</strong>e adherence to normative rules relevant to belief assessment


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 17<br />

(e.g. consistency <strong>in</strong> risk perception), value assessment (e.g., resistance to fram<strong>in</strong>g, resistance<br />

to sunk costs), <strong>and</strong> the comb<strong>in</strong>ation of beliefs <strong>and</strong> values when mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>s (e.g.,<br />

apply<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong> rules). Adolescents who are more likely to follow these norms are less<br />

likely to engage <strong>in</strong> risky <strong>and</strong> del<strong>in</strong>quent behaviours (Parker <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, 2005). Adults<br />

who are better able to follow these norms obta<strong>in</strong> better outcomes <strong>in</strong> their lives, as self-<br />

reported <strong>in</strong> the Decision Outcome Inventory (Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong> et al. 2007a). Both of these<br />

results hold after controll<strong>in</strong>g for performance on exist<strong>in</strong>g measures of general cognitive<br />

ability, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence measures are more proximal to real-<br />

world <strong>decision</strong> outcomes.<br />

These validation studies, however, do not show whether there is a causal l<strong>in</strong>k between<br />

<strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence <strong>and</strong> life outcomes. R<strong>and</strong>omized controlled studies would be<br />

needed to exam<strong>in</strong>e whether people who are taught to follow normative rules of judgment <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g will improve their life outcomes, compared to a control group. Participants<br />

<strong>in</strong> such studies would be r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned to receiv<strong>in</strong>g education <strong>in</strong> <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g or a<br />

control group, <strong>and</strong> followed over time to exam<strong>in</strong>e the effect on their performance on paper-<br />

<strong>and</strong>-pencil measures of <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence as well as their experienced life<br />

<strong>decision</strong> outcomes. In the next section, we discuss prescriptive analyses aim<strong>in</strong>g to improve<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence <strong>and</strong> outcomes.<br />

Prescriptive Analysis: How Can We Improve Adolescents’ Decisions?<br />

Prescriptive studies exam<strong>in</strong>e the effectiveness of strategies for improv<strong>in</strong>g adolescent<br />

<strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Below, we discuss programs that have targeted <strong>adolescents</strong>’ health<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s <strong>and</strong> programs that have targeted <strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g.


Programs target<strong>in</strong>g adolescent health behaviors<br />

<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 18<br />

Adolescents have been targeted by many <strong>in</strong>terventions designed to <strong>in</strong>form their<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s <strong>in</strong> a wide variety of doma<strong>in</strong>s, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g, for example, sexual behavior, eat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

disorders, obesity, physical activity, smok<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> drug use (for reviews, see Kirby, 2002a,<br />

2002b; Stice, Shaw, <strong>and</strong> Marti, 2006; Skara <strong>and</strong> Sussman, 2003; Wiehe, Garrison, Christakis,<br />

Ebel, <strong>and</strong> Rivara, 2005). Many of these programs have little to no effect on <strong>adolescents</strong>’ real-<br />

world <strong>decision</strong>s, possibly because they are not based on normative analyses of how<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> should make <strong>decision</strong>s, or descriptive analyses of how <strong>adolescents</strong> are mak<strong>in</strong>g<br />

these <strong>decision</strong>s. Reviews of sexuality education have suggested that programs that are<br />

grounded <strong>in</strong> normative <strong>and</strong> descriptive approaches may be more likely to affect <strong>adolescents</strong>’<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> outcomes (e.g., DiCenso, Guyatt, Willan, <strong>and</strong> Griffith, 2002; Kim, Stanton, Li,<br />

Dickerson, <strong>and</strong> Galbraith, 1997; McKay, 2000; Michie <strong>and</strong> Abraham, 2004).<br />

If programs have a normative basis, they tend to rely on the health belief model<br />

(Becker <strong>and</strong> Rosenstock, 1987), the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991), or protection-<br />

motivation theory (Rogers, 1983). These theories propose l<strong>in</strong>ear statistical models to predict<br />

<strong>in</strong>tended health behavior from variables such as perceived vulnerability to risk, perceived<br />

social norms, perceived barriers to implement<strong>in</strong>g risk reduc<strong>in</strong>g behaviors, <strong>and</strong> perceived self-<br />

efficacy to overcome those barriers. Indeed, these general concepts are loosely based on the<br />

normative approach to <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g that is described above. However, these l<strong>in</strong>ear<br />

models have been criticized for, among other th<strong>in</strong>gs, be<strong>in</strong>g statistically <strong>in</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>guishable,<br />

generat<strong>in</strong>g few testable hypotheses, <strong>and</strong> fail<strong>in</strong>g to provide specific suggestions on what<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> need to know to improve their <strong>decision</strong>s (Brewer <strong>and</strong> Rimer, 2008; Dawes <strong>and</strong><br />

Corrigan, 1974; Ogden, 2003; Reyna <strong>and</strong> Farley, 2006). Moreover, the <strong>in</strong>tentions they aim to<br />

predict may not be related to actual behaviors, especially among <strong>adolescents</strong>, who may be<br />

less able than adults to behave as they <strong>in</strong>tended, when faced with real-world pressures on


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 19<br />

<strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g (Gardner <strong>and</strong> Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, 2005; Gerrard et al., 2008; Reyna <strong>and</strong> Farley,<br />

2006).<br />

Although these models of health behavior have been adapted from normative theories<br />

of <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g, they differ <strong>in</strong> important respects. While health theories of behavior<br />

change evaluate a <strong>decision</strong> by whether a risk reduction behavior is chosen, normative theories<br />

of <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g evaluate a <strong>decision</strong> by the quality of <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g processes. As a<br />

result, one ma<strong>in</strong> difference between the two approaches perta<strong>in</strong>s to their treatment of judged<br />

risks. Health theories of behavior change predict that better <strong>decision</strong>s (as shown <strong>in</strong> choos<strong>in</strong>g<br />

risk reduction behaviors) will be made by <strong>decision</strong> makers who perceive themselves to be at<br />

higher risk, <strong>and</strong> even overestimate their risk. Normative theories of <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g predict<br />

that better <strong>decision</strong>s (as shown <strong>in</strong> better processes) will be made by <strong>decision</strong> makers who can<br />

accurately judge absolute <strong>and</strong> relative risks of different behaviors.<br />

Thus, models of health behavior may <strong>in</strong>spire communications that aim to <strong>in</strong>crease<br />

perceived risk without consider<strong>in</strong>g accuracy. Perhaps as a result of such warn<strong>in</strong>g messages,<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> are more likely than adults to overestimate the risks of negative health outcomes<br />

related to risky behaviors, such as hav<strong>in</strong>g sex <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g various substances (Millste<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Halpern-Felsher, 2002). However, when <strong>adolescents</strong> start engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> those risky behaviors,<br />

they may f<strong>in</strong>d out that these negative outcomes are not actually that likely. For example,<br />

sexually active <strong>adolescents</strong> judge the risks of gett<strong>in</strong>g pregnant or contract<strong>in</strong>g HIV as lower<br />

than do absta<strong>in</strong>ers, <strong>and</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> who had been passengers <strong>in</strong> a car with a drunk driver<br />

judged the risk of gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to a drunk-driv<strong>in</strong>g accident as lower than those who did not have<br />

that experience (Gerrard et al., 1996; Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001). Perhaps as a result,<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> may become frustrated with educators’ use of scare tactics (DiCenso, Borthwick,<br />

Busca, Creatura, Holmes, Kalagian, <strong>and</strong> Part<strong>in</strong>gon, 2001).


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 20<br />

These results counter the folk wisdom that <strong>adolescents</strong> perceive themselves as<br />

<strong>in</strong>vulnerable, <strong>and</strong> have implications beyond the psychological laboratory. Adolescents may<br />

experience serious consequences from overestimat<strong>in</strong>g risks. Female <strong>adolescents</strong> who do not<br />

get pregnant after an episode of unprotected sex may conclude that they are <strong>in</strong>fertile, <strong>and</strong><br />

cont<strong>in</strong>ue to engage <strong>in</strong> more unprotected sex (Downs, Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, Murray, <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff,<br />

2004). That conclusion is likely to be unwarranted, <strong>and</strong> can have serious consequences: those<br />

who believe that they are <strong>in</strong>fertile are more likely to have unprotected sex, which puts them at<br />

risk for pregnancy (Downs, Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, et al., 2004). Thus, programs may be more<br />

effective <strong>in</strong> the long run if they teach people to have realistic expectations (Rothman, 2000).<br />

Programs target<strong>in</strong>g adolescent <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Few <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g researchers have been <strong>in</strong>volved with the development <strong>and</strong><br />

evaluation of programs target<strong>in</strong>g adolescent <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence (for exceptions, see<br />

Baron <strong>and</strong> Brown, 1991). One notable exception is the GOFER program, which aims to<br />

teach <strong>adolescents</strong> general <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g skills, <strong>in</strong> the hope of ultimately improv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong>’ real-world <strong>decision</strong>s <strong>and</strong> outcomes (Mann, Harmoni, Power, Beswick, <strong>and</strong><br />

Ormond, 1988). The program teaches <strong>adolescents</strong> the normative approach to mak<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s described above, emphasiz<strong>in</strong>g the relevance of assess<strong>in</strong>g valid <strong>and</strong> consistent<br />

beliefs <strong>and</strong> values, <strong>and</strong> comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g beliefs <strong>and</strong> values to choose the option that is most likely<br />

to achieve the <strong>decision</strong>-maker’s goals. In addition, it also borrows from descriptive research,<br />

teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>adolescents</strong> how to cope with the social, motivational, <strong>and</strong> emotional pressures that<br />

may sway their <strong>decision</strong>s (Janis <strong>and</strong> Mann, 1977). The program has been shown to improve<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong>’ self-reports of their <strong>decision</strong> habits, as well as their knowledge of <strong>decision</strong>-<br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g processes, compared to a no-treatment control group. However, it has not been<br />

evaluated <strong>in</strong> terms of effects on real-world <strong>decision</strong>s <strong>and</strong> outcomes.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 21<br />

Another <strong>in</strong>tervention developed by <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g researchers specifically targets<br />

female <strong>adolescents</strong>’ sexual <strong>decision</strong>s (Downs, Murray, et al., 2004; see also Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>,<br />

Downs, <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff, 2007). An <strong>in</strong>teractive DVD addressed deliberative skills <strong>and</strong><br />

emotional/experiential skills that <strong>adolescents</strong> seemed to need to make better <strong>decision</strong>s, as<br />

suggested <strong>in</strong> descriptive research discussed above. For example, the program explicitly<br />

addressed female <strong>adolescents</strong>’ tendency to overestimate sex-related risks, show<strong>in</strong>g how they<br />

vary for different behaviors <strong>and</strong> accumulate with repeated exposures. It also addressed<br />

female <strong>adolescents</strong>’ perceptions of hav<strong>in</strong>g little control over their <strong>in</strong>teractions with their<br />

partners, teach<strong>in</strong>g them the skills needed to negotiate safer sexual behaviors with their sexual<br />

partners. Specifically, the DVD showed adolescent females <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g with adolescent<br />

males, allow<strong>in</strong>g viewers to choose how the female character would respond to pressure from<br />

the male character. Viewers were asked to perform cognitive rehearsal, imag<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g what they<br />

would say or do <strong>in</strong> such a situation (B<strong>and</strong>ura, 2000; Maibach <strong>and</strong> Flora, 1993), that <strong>in</strong>creases<br />

the perceived feasibility of implement<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>s which may otherwise seem difficult or<br />

daunt<strong>in</strong>g (Driskell, Copper, <strong>and</strong> Moran, 1994). A longitud<strong>in</strong>al trial follow<strong>in</strong>g 300 sexually<br />

active female <strong>adolescents</strong> over the course of six months showed that, compared to controls,<br />

those view<strong>in</strong>g the DVD were significantly more likely to become abst<strong>in</strong>ent, less likely to<br />

experience condom failures if they did choose to have sex, <strong>and</strong> less likely to be diagnosed<br />

with a sexually transmitted <strong>in</strong>fection (Downs, Murray, et al., 2004).<br />

Potential shortcom<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

Unfortunately, policy makers <strong>and</strong> educators do not always use evidence provided by<br />

exist<strong>in</strong>g prescriptive research to select programs for <strong>adolescents</strong>. For example, many U.S.<br />

schools present <strong>adolescents</strong> with abst<strong>in</strong>ence-only education that leaves out <strong>in</strong>formation about<br />

condom use, because it is feared that present<strong>in</strong>g such <strong>in</strong>formation will encourage <strong>adolescents</strong><br />

to become sexually active – even though there is no evidence for that claim (Kirby, 2002a).


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 22<br />

Similar arguments have been made to prevent <strong>adolescents</strong> from access<strong>in</strong>g free condoms or<br />

over-the-counter emergency contraception (also referred to as the morn<strong>in</strong>g after pill), without<br />

there be<strong>in</strong>g any evidence that these risk reduction strategies <strong>in</strong>crease sexual activity (Gold,<br />

Wolford, Smith, <strong>and</strong> Parker, 2004; Kirby, 2002b). Needle exchange programs have also been<br />

opposed, even though they have been shown to reduce needle shar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> related HIV-risk,<br />

without <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g drug use <strong>in</strong> the community (Drucker, Lurie, Wodak, <strong>and</strong> Alcabes, 1998).<br />

Present<strong>in</strong>g <strong>adolescents</strong> with <strong>in</strong>effective programs creates opportunity costs, <strong>in</strong> the<br />

sense of us<strong>in</strong>g time that could have been spent on effective programs. It may also potentially<br />

harm <strong>adolescents</strong>, if they are taught strategies that make them worse off. For example,<br />

sexuality education promot<strong>in</strong>g “abst<strong>in</strong>ence-only” may be less effective than those that<br />

promote both abst<strong>in</strong>ence <strong>and</strong> condom use (McKay, 2000; Kirby, 2002). Adolescents who<br />

choose to be abst<strong>in</strong>ent may be less likely to use contraception when they eventually decide to<br />

have sex (Bearman <strong>and</strong> Brueckner, 2001). Even <strong>adolescents</strong> who believe that they are<br />

“abst<strong>in</strong>ent” may be putt<strong>in</strong>g themselves at risk for acquir<strong>in</strong>g sexually transmitted <strong>in</strong>fections,<br />

because they may <strong>in</strong>terpret it as <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g oral sex (Remez, 2000) <strong>and</strong> even anal sex<br />

(Schuster, Bell, <strong>and</strong> Kanouse, 1996).<br />

Even when educators present <strong>adolescents</strong> with programs that have been shown to be<br />

effective, they may make changes that threaten that effectiveness – either <strong>in</strong>advertently or on<br />

the basis on their own <strong>in</strong>tuitions. Teachers have also been shown to leave out taboo topics<br />

that are relevant to <strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong>s (Halper<strong>in</strong>, 1999). Do<strong>in</strong>g so may threaten the<br />

quality of the program, <strong>and</strong> reduce its previously reported effect on adolescent <strong>decision</strong><br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g. Indeed, programs that effectively reduced <strong>adolescents</strong>’ risk tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> r<strong>and</strong>omized<br />

controlled trials have not had a good record <strong>in</strong> community sett<strong>in</strong>gs, due to reduced fidelity to<br />

content (Maher, Peterman, Osewe, Odusanya, <strong>and</strong> Scerba, 2003; Rob<strong>in</strong>son et al., 2002;<br />

Valdiserri, Ogden, <strong>and</strong> McCray, 2003). In the context of obesity prevention, tra<strong>in</strong>ed


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 23<br />

<strong>in</strong>terventionists were better than regular classroom teachers <strong>in</strong> effectively convey<strong>in</strong>g program<br />

content (Stice, Shaw, <strong>and</strong> Marti, 2006). Moreover, video-based programs about condom use<br />

are more effective than facilitator-led programs <strong>in</strong> terms of reduc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>adolescents</strong>’ probability<br />

of acquir<strong>in</strong>g sexually transmitted <strong>in</strong>fections (O’Donnell, O’Donnell, San Doval, Duran, &<br />

Labes, 1995; O’Donnell, San Doval, Duran, & O’Donnell, 1998). These results suggest the<br />

importance of consistently present<strong>in</strong>g effective program content teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>adolescents</strong> how to<br />

improve their real-world <strong>decision</strong>s <strong>and</strong> their experienced outcomes.<br />

Summary <strong>and</strong> Conclusions<br />

Behavioral <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g offers three <strong>in</strong>terrelated l<strong>in</strong>es of research study<strong>in</strong>g<br />

adolescent <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Normative approaches state how <strong>adolescents</strong> should be mak<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> posit that beliefs <strong>and</strong> values should show correspondence to external objective<br />

criteria as well as coherence or <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency. Because chance plays a role <strong>in</strong><br />

determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g outcomes, even the best possible <strong>decision</strong> may lead to an undesired outcome.<br />

Therefore, normative approaches hold that <strong>decision</strong>s should be evaluated by their processes<br />

<strong>and</strong> not by their outcomes. Across multiple <strong>decision</strong>s, however, good processes should be<br />

more likely than bad processes to lead to good outcomes. With risk tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> related<br />

negative life outcomes seem<strong>in</strong>g more common for <strong>adolescents</strong> than for adults, it has been<br />

argued that <strong>adolescents</strong> are relatively poor <strong>decision</strong> makers – especially if they come to regret<br />

their <strong>decision</strong>s later <strong>in</strong> life. However, if later experiences of regret are used as a st<strong>and</strong>ard for<br />

evaluat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>s, then adults may also be judged as bad <strong>decision</strong> makers. Many life<br />

regrets reported by adults <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>decision</strong>s that are likely made <strong>in</strong> adulthood, such as not<br />

work<strong>in</strong>g harder <strong>in</strong> one’s career, or not gett<strong>in</strong>g a divorce (Gilovich <strong>and</strong> Medvec, 1995; Lecci,<br />

et al., 1994; Wrosch <strong>and</strong> Heckhausen, 2002). Yet, good <strong>decision</strong>s may be ones that m<strong>in</strong>imize


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 24<br />

expected regret, by avoid<strong>in</strong>g options that may produce a catastrophic outcome (Loomes <strong>and</strong><br />

Sugden, 1982).<br />

Descriptive approaches exam<strong>in</strong>e how <strong>adolescents</strong> actually make <strong>decision</strong>s, compar<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the processes they use to those prescribed by normative st<strong>and</strong>ards. Adolescents may<br />

generally be similar to adults <strong>in</strong> terms of their deliberative ability to adhere to various<br />

normative pr<strong>in</strong>ciples, as measured with hypothetical <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g problems. However,<br />

they may lack <strong>in</strong>tuitive skills, mak<strong>in</strong>g them less likely than adults to recognize when<br />

heuristics may benefit <strong>decision</strong>s (Jacobs <strong>and</strong> Klaczynski, 2001;Klaczynksi <strong>and</strong> Cottrell,<br />

2004), quickly comprehend the <strong>in</strong>tuitive gist of a risky <strong>decision</strong> (Reyna, 2004; Reyna <strong>and</strong><br />

Farley, 2006), resist impulses towards unplanned risk tak<strong>in</strong>g (Gerrard et al., 2008), have<br />

sufficient self-regulation to <strong>in</strong>hibit their drive to seek rewards (Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, 2008), or counteract<br />

the <strong>in</strong>fluences of their peers (Gardner & Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, 2005). As a result, <strong>adolescents</strong> may be<br />

more likely than adults to sway from their <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>decision</strong>s when faced with real-world<br />

pressures on <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. However, more research is needed to underst<strong>and</strong> the specific<br />

<strong>in</strong>tuitive skills <strong>adolescents</strong> would need to make better real-world <strong>decision</strong>s even when faced<br />

with peers <strong>and</strong> impulsivity, how these skills develop as they grow <strong>in</strong>to adults, <strong>and</strong> whether<br />

their effective implementation can be taught before adulthood.<br />

Prescriptive approaches aim to improve <strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong>s. Most programs<br />

target<strong>in</strong>g adolescent <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g have been developed by researchers <strong>in</strong> public health,<br />

<strong>and</strong> focus on health <strong>decision</strong>s about sexual behavior, eat<strong>in</strong>g disorders, obesity, <strong>and</strong> other<br />

topics relevant to <strong>adolescents</strong>. Many of these public health programs show mixed results at<br />

best, possibly because <strong>decision</strong>s about what to teach <strong>adolescents</strong> are often not based on<br />

evidence of what works (Michie <strong>and</strong> Abraham, 2004; Reyna <strong>and</strong> Farley, 2006). Effective<br />

programs tend to be grounded <strong>in</strong> normative <strong>and</strong> descriptive analyses, teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>adolescents</strong><br />

how to overcome any difficulties that they may have <strong>in</strong> apply<strong>in</strong>g normative pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 25<br />

<strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g, as identified <strong>in</strong> descriptive studies (DiCenso et al., 2002; Kim et al., 1997;<br />

Kirby, 2002a, 2002b; McKay, 2000; Michie <strong>and</strong> Abraham, 2004). If programs have a<br />

normative basis, they tend to rely on traditional models of health behavior change. Some of<br />

these models suggest that <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g perceptions of risk will make it more likely that<br />

recipients will avoid risky behaviors. However, as <strong>adolescents</strong> mature, they may ga<strong>in</strong><br />

experience with risky behaviors, <strong>and</strong> may learn that negative outcomes may not be as likely<br />

as they had thought (Gerrard et al., 1996; Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001). Therefore, long-term<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of healthy behaviors may be more likely with programs that foster realistic<br />

expectations (Rothman, 2000).<br />

Researchers <strong>in</strong> judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g tend to be at home <strong>in</strong> normative <strong>and</strong><br />

descriptive approaches. Yet, to date, they have conducted relatively few prescriptive studies<br />

to develop <strong>and</strong> evaluate programs teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g skills to <strong>adolescents</strong> (Baron <strong>and</strong><br />

Brown, 1991). One exception is the GOFER program, which showed improvements <strong>in</strong> self-<br />

reports of <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g, but was not evaluated <strong>in</strong> terms of effects on actual real-world<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s (Mann et al., 1988). Another exception is a DVD-<strong>in</strong>tervention that successfully<br />

taught female <strong>adolescents</strong> to avoid sexually transmitted <strong>in</strong>fections, as measured <strong>in</strong> self-reports<br />

<strong>and</strong> on a cl<strong>in</strong>ical test (Downs, Murray et al., 2004). These results suggest that <strong>adolescents</strong><br />

may be taught how to make better real-world <strong>decision</strong>s, even ones that may be made <strong>in</strong> “hot”<br />

emotional states rather than “cold” emotional states, <strong>and</strong> under the pressures of real-world<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s.<br />

Traditionally, researchers <strong>in</strong> judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g have aimed to underst<strong>and</strong><br />

general psychological processes that underlie <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g, by study<strong>in</strong>g hypothetical<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s made <strong>in</strong> the lab. The development of effective prescriptive programs requires a<br />

better underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of how <strong>adolescents</strong> make real-world <strong>decision</strong>s, as well as the<br />

deliberative <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tuitive <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g skills they need to improve those <strong>decision</strong>s –


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 26<br />

especially when they may be swayed by peers or their own impulsivity. It also requires<br />

validated measures of these skills, to follow their developmental progression, identify needs<br />

for improvement, <strong>and</strong> to evaluate prescriptive programs that aim to establish that<br />

improvement. Recently validated measures of <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence (Parker <strong>and</strong><br />

Fischhoff, 2005; Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong> et al., 2007a) may help to <strong>in</strong>form such descriptive <strong>and</strong><br />

prescriptive l<strong>in</strong>es of research on adolescent <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Thus, behavioral <strong>decision</strong><br />

research may provide the theoretical approach <strong>and</strong> the methodological tools needed to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> how <strong>adolescents</strong> should make their <strong>decision</strong>s, how they actually make their<br />

<strong>decision</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> how they can improve their <strong>decision</strong>s, if they are provided with programs.<br />

References<br />

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior <strong>and</strong> Human<br />

Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.<br />

Arkes, H. R., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> Human Decision Processes, 35, 124–140.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (2000). Self-efficacy: The foundation of agency. In W. J. Perrig, & A. Grob<br />

(Eds.), Control of human behavior, mental processes, <strong>and</strong> consciousness: Essays <strong>in</strong> honor<br />

of the 60th birthday of August Flammer (pp. 17–33). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum<br />

Associates.<br />

Baron, J., & Brown, R.V. (1991). Teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>adolescents</strong>. Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Baron, J., & Hershey, J. (1988). Outcome bias <strong>in</strong> <strong>decision</strong> evaluation. Journal of Personality<br />

<strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 54, 569-579.


Baumr<strong>in</strong>d, D. (1987). A developmental perspective on adolescent risk tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 27<br />

contemporary America. New Directions for Child <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Development, 37, 93-<br />

125.<br />

Becker, M. H., & Rosenstock, J. M. (1987). Compar<strong>in</strong>g social learn<strong>in</strong>g theory <strong>and</strong> the health<br />

belief model. In W. B. Ward (Ed.), Advances <strong>in</strong> health education <strong>and</strong> promotion (pp.<br />

245–249). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.<br />

Beyth-Marom, R., Aust<strong>in</strong>, L., Fischhoff, B., Palmgren, C., & Jacobs-Quadrel, M. J. (1993).<br />

Perceived consequences of risky behaviors: Adults <strong>and</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong>. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 29, 549–563.<br />

Brady, S. S., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2008). Social <strong>and</strong> emotional consequences of<br />

refra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g from sexual activity among sexually experienced <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>experienced youths <strong>in</strong><br />

California. American Journal of Public Health, 98, 162-168.<br />

Brady, S. S., & Matthews, K. A. (2002). The <strong>in</strong>fluence of socioeconomic status <strong>and</strong> ethnicity<br />

on <strong>adolescents</strong>’ exposure to stressful life events. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27,<br />

575–583.<br />

Brady, S. S., Song, A. V., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2008). Adolescents report both positive<br />

<strong>and</strong> negative consequences of experimentation with cigarette use. Preventive Medic<strong>in</strong>e,<br />

46, 585-590.<br />

Brady, S. S., Tschann, J. M., Pasch, L. A., Flores, E., & Ozer, E. J. (2008). Violence<br />

<strong>in</strong>volvement, substance use, <strong>and</strong> sexual activity among Mexican-American <strong>and</strong> European-<br />

American <strong>adolescents</strong>. Journal of Adolescent Health, 43, 285-295.<br />

Brewer, N. T., & Rimer, B. K. (2008). Perspectives on health behavior theories that focus on<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals. In: K. Glanz, B.K. Rimer, & K. Viswanath. Health behavior <strong>and</strong> health<br />

education: Theory, research, <strong>and</strong> practice. 4 th Edition. San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 28<br />

Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., Downs, J. S., & Fischhoff, B. (2007). Adolescents’ th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about the<br />

risks <strong>and</strong> benefits of sexual behavior. In M. C. Lovett, & P. Shah (Eds.) Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g with<br />

data (pp. 421-439). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum,.<br />

Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., Downs, J. S., Murray, P. M., & Fischhoff, B. (<strong>in</strong> press). Can female<br />

<strong>adolescents</strong> tell whether they have a Chlamydia <strong>in</strong>fection? Medical Decision Mak<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., & Fischhoff, B. (2000). The effect of question format on measured<br />

HIV/AIDS knowledge <strong>in</strong> detention center teens, high school students, <strong>and</strong> adults. AIDS<br />

Education <strong>and</strong> Prevention, 12, 187-198.<br />

Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., Fischhoff, B., Millste<strong>in</strong>, S. G., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2000). Verbal<br />

<strong>and</strong> numerical expressions of probability: “It’s a fifty-fifty chance.” Organizational<br />

Behavior <strong>and</strong> Human Decision Processes, 81, 115-131.<br />

Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2007a). Individual differences <strong>in</strong> Adult<br />

Decision-Mak<strong>in</strong>g Competence. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 92, 938-<br />

956.<br />

Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2007b). Can teens predict significant<br />

life events? Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, 208-210.<br />

Cates, W. Jr., & Wasserheit, J. N. (1991). Genital chlamydial <strong>in</strong>fections: Epidemiology <strong>and</strong><br />

reproductive sequelae. American Journal of Obstetrics <strong>and</strong> Gynecology, 164, 1771-1781.<br />

Centers for Disease Control <strong>and</strong> Prevention (2004). Youth risk behavior surveillance –<br />

United States, 2003. Morbidity <strong>and</strong> Mortality Weekly Report, 53, 1-96.<br />

Cohn, L., Macfarlane, S., Yanez, C., & Imai, W. (1995). Risk-perception: Differences<br />

between <strong>adolescents</strong> <strong>and</strong> adults. Health Psychology, 14, 217-222.<br />

Critchlow, C. W., Wölner-Hanssen, P., Eschenbach, D. A., Kiviat, N.B., Koutsky, L. A.,<br />

Stevens, C. E., & Holmes, K. K. (1995). Determ<strong>in</strong>ents of cervical ectopia <strong>and</strong> of


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 29<br />

cervicitis – age, oral contraception, specific cervical <strong>in</strong>fection, smok<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> douch<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

American Journal of Obstetrics <strong>and</strong> Gynecology, 173, 534-543.<br />

Crosby, R. A., S<strong>and</strong>ers, S., Yarber, W. L., & Graham, C. A. (2003). Condom-use errors <strong>and</strong><br />

problems. A neglected aspect of studies assess<strong>in</strong>g condom effectiveness. American<br />

Journal of Preventive Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 24, 367-370.<br />

Davidson, D. (1995). The representativeness heuristic <strong>and</strong> the conjunction fallacy effect <strong>in</strong><br />

children’s <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 328-346.<br />

Dawes, R. M., & Corrigan, B. (1974). L<strong>in</strong>ear models <strong>in</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Psychological<br />

Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 81, 95–106.<br />

Dawes, R. M., & Hastie, R. (2001). Rational choice <strong>in</strong> an uncerta<strong>in</strong> world: The psychology<br />

of judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

DiCenso, A., Borthwick, V. W., Busca, C. A., Creatura, C., Holmes, J. A., Kalagian, W. F.,<br />

& Part<strong>in</strong>gton, B. M. (2001). Complet<strong>in</strong>g the picture: Adolscents talk about what’s<br />

miss<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> sexual health services. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 92, 35-38.<br />

DiCenso, A., Guyatt, G., Willan, A., & Griffith, L. (2002). Interventions to reduce<br />

un<strong>in</strong>tended pregnancies among <strong>adolescents</strong>: Systematic review of r<strong>and</strong>omized controlled<br />

trials. British Medical Journal, 324, 1426-1435.<br />

Dillner, J., Leht<strong>in</strong>en, M., Björge, T., Luostar<strong>in</strong>en, T., Youngman, L., Jellum, E., Koskela, P.,<br />

Gislefoss, R.E., Hallmans, G., Paavonen, J., Sapp, M., Schiller, J.T., Hakul<strong>in</strong>en, T.,<br />

Thoresen, S., & Hakama, M. (1997). Prospective seroepidemiologic study of human<br />

papilomavirus <strong>in</strong>fection as a risk factor for <strong>in</strong>vasive cervical cancer. Journal of the<br />

National Cancer Institute, 89, 1293-1299.<br />

Downs, J. S., Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., Murray, P. J., & Fischhoff, B. (2004). When “it only takes<br />

once” fails: Perceived <strong>in</strong>fertility after unsafe sex predicts condom use <strong>and</strong> STI<br />

acquisition. Journal of Pediatric <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Gynecology, 17, 224.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 30<br />

Downs, J. S., Murray, P. J., Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., White, J. P., Palmgren, C., & Fischhoff, B.<br />

(2004). Interactive video behavioral <strong>in</strong>tervention to reduce adolescent females' STD risk:<br />

A r<strong>and</strong>omized controlled trial. Social Science & Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 59, 1561-1572.<br />

Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance performance?<br />

Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 481–492.<br />

Drucker, E., Lurie, P., Wodak, A., <strong>and</strong> Alcabes, P. (1998). Measur<strong>in</strong>g harm reduction: the<br />

effects of needle <strong>and</strong> syr<strong>in</strong>ge exchange programs <strong>and</strong> methadone ma<strong>in</strong>tenance on the<br />

ecology of HIV. AIDS 12, S217-S230.<br />

Eaton, D. K., Kann, L., K<strong>in</strong>chen, S., Ross, J., Hawk<strong>in</strong>s, J., Harris, W. A., Lowry, R.,<br />

McManus, T., Chyen, D., Shankl<strong>in</strong>, S., Lim, C., Grunbaum, J. A., & Wechsler, H. (2006).<br />

Youth risk behavior surveillance – United States, 2005. Morbidity <strong>and</strong> Mortality Weekly<br />

Report, 55, 1-108.<br />

Edwards, W., Kiss, I., Majone, G., & Toda, M. (1984). What constitutes “a good <strong>decision</strong>”?<br />

Acta Psychologica, 56, 5-27.<br />

Ellickson, P.L., Tucker, J.S., & Kle<strong>in</strong>, D.J. (2003). Ten-year prospective study of public<br />

health problems associated with early dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. Pediatrics, 111, 949-955.<br />

Esbensen, F. A., Melde, C., Taylor, T. J., & Peterson, D. (2008). Active parental consent <strong>in</strong><br />

school-based research: How much is enough <strong>and</strong> how do we get it? Educational Review,<br />

32, 335-362.<br />

Evans, J. B. St. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Reason<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> rationality. Hove, UK: Psychology<br />

Press.<br />

Faden, V.B. (2006). Trends <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>itiation of alcohol use <strong>in</strong> the United States. Alcoholism:<br />

Cl<strong>in</strong>ical <strong>and</strong> Experimental Research, 30, 1011-1022.<br />

Fischhoff, B. (2008). Assess<strong>in</strong>g adolescent <strong>decision</strong>-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence. Developmental<br />

Review, 28, 12-28.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 31<br />

Fischhoff, B., Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., Parker, A. M., Millste<strong>in</strong>, S. G., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L.<br />

(<strong>in</strong> press). Adolescents’ Perceived Risk of Dy<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of Adolescent Health.<br />

Fischhoff, B., Parker, A. M., Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W., Downs, J. S., Palmgren, C., Dawes, R. &<br />

Manski, C. (2000). Teen expectations for significant life events. Public Op<strong>in</strong>ion<br />

Quarterly, 64, 189-205.<br />

Frisch, D., & Clemen, R. T. (1994). Beyond expected utility: Reth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g behavioral <strong>decision</strong><br />

research. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 116, 46-54.<br />

Frisch, D., & Jones, S. K. (1993). Assess<strong>in</strong>g the accuracy of <strong>decision</strong>s. Theory <strong>and</strong><br />

Psychology, 3, 115-135.<br />

Furman, W., & Shaffer, L. (2003). The role of romantic relationships <strong>in</strong> adolescent<br />

development. In P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adolescent romantic relations <strong>and</strong> sexual behavior:<br />

Theory, research, <strong>and</strong> practical implications (pp. 3–22). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum<br />

Associates.<br />

Gardner, M., & Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, L. (2005). Peer <strong>in</strong>fluence on risk tak<strong>in</strong>g, risk preference, <strong>and</strong> risky<br />

<strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> adolescence <strong>and</strong> adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 41, 625–635.<br />

Gerrard, M. Gibbons, F. X., Benth<strong>in</strong>, A. C., & Hessl<strong>in</strong>g, R. M. (1996). A longitud<strong>in</strong>al study<br />

of the reciprocal nature of risk behaviors <strong>and</strong> cognitions <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong>: What you do<br />

shapes what you th<strong>in</strong>k, <strong>and</strong> vice versa. Health Psychology, 15, 344-354.<br />

Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F. X., Houlihan, A. E., Stock, M. L., & Pomery, E. A. (2008). A dual-<br />

process approach to health risk <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g: The prototype will<strong>in</strong>gness model.<br />

Developmental Review, 28, 29-61.<br />

Gervais, A., O’Loughl<strong>in</strong>, J., Meshefedjian, G., Bancej, C., & Tremblay, M. (2006).<br />

Milestones <strong>in</strong> the natural course of onset of cigarette use among <strong>adolescents</strong>. Canadian<br />

Medical Association Journal, 175, 255-261.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 32<br />

Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Group (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart.<br />

New York, NY: Oxford University Press.<br />

Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1995). The experience of regret: What, when, <strong>and</strong> why.<br />

Psychological Review, 102, 379-395.<br />

Gold, M. A., Wolford, J. E., Smith, K. A., & Parker, A. M. (2004). The effects of advance<br />

provision of emergency contraception on adolescent women’s sexual <strong>and</strong> contraceptive<br />

behaviors. Journal of Pediatric <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Gynecology, 17, 87-96.<br />

Goldberg, J. H., & Fischhoff, B. (2000). The long-term risks <strong>and</strong> the short-term benefits:<br />

Perceptions of potentially addictive activities. Health Psychology, 19, 299–303.<br />

Goldberg, J. H., Halpern-Felsher, B. L., & Millste<strong>in</strong>, S. G. (2002). Beyond <strong>in</strong>vulnerability:<br />

The importance of benefits <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong>’ <strong>decision</strong>s to dr<strong>in</strong>k alcohol. Health Psychology,<br />

21, 477–484.<br />

Grant, B.F., & Dawson, D.A. (1997). Age at dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g onset <strong>and</strong> its association with DSM-IV<br />

alcohol use <strong>and</strong> problem behavior <strong>in</strong> late adolescence: Results from the National<br />

Longitud<strong>in</strong>al Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. Journal of Substance Abuse, 9, 103110.<br />

Grodste<strong>in</strong>, F., Goldman, M.B., Cramer, D.W. (1993). Relation of tubal <strong>in</strong>fertility to history<br />

of sexually transmitted diseases. American Journal of Epidemiology, 137, 577-584.<br />

Gruber, E., DiClemente, R.J., Anderson, M.M.,, & Lodico, M. (1996). Early dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g onset<br />

<strong>and</strong> its association with alcohol use <strong>and</strong> problem behavior <strong>in</strong> late adolescence. Preventive<br />

Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 25, 293–300.<br />

Hammond, K. R. (1990). Functionalism <strong>and</strong> illusionism: Can <strong>in</strong>tegration be usefully<br />

achieved? In R. M. Hogarth (Ed.). Insights <strong>in</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g: A tribute to Hillel J.<br />

E<strong>in</strong>horn (pp. 227-261). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.<br />

Halper<strong>in</strong>, D. T. (1999). Heterosexual anal <strong>in</strong>tercourse: Prevalence, cultural factors, <strong>and</strong> HIV<br />

<strong>in</strong>fection <strong>and</strong> other health risks, Part I. AIDS Patient Care <strong>and</strong> STDs, 13, 717–30.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 33<br />

Halpern-Felsher, B. L., Millste<strong>in</strong>, S. G., Ellen, J. M., Adler, N. E., Tschann, J. M., & Biehl,<br />

M. (2001). The role of behavioral experience <strong>in</strong> judg<strong>in</strong>g risks. Health Psychology, 20,<br />

120-126.<br />

Halpern-Felsher, B. L., & Cauffman, E. (2001). Costs <strong>and</strong> benefits of a <strong>decision</strong>: Decision-<br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g competence <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> <strong>and</strong> adults. Journal of Applied Developmental<br />

Psychology, 22, 257-273.<br />

Hegmann, K. T., Fraser, A. M., Keaney, R. P., Moser, S. E., Nilasena, D. S., Sedlars, M.,<br />

Higham-Gren, L., & Lyon, J. L. (1993). The effect of age at smok<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>itiation on lung<br />

cancer risk. Epidemiology, 4, 444-448.<br />

Hershey, J. C., & Baron, J. (1992). <strong>Judgment</strong> by outcomes: When is it justified?<br />

Organizational Behavior <strong>and</strong> Human Decision Processes, 53, 89-93.<br />

Hershey, J. C., & Baron, J. (1995). <strong>Judgment</strong> by outcomes: When is it warranted?<br />

Organizational Behavior <strong>and</strong> Human Decision Processes, 62, 127.<br />

Humphrey, J. A., & White, J. W. (2000). Women’s vulnerability to sexual assault from<br />

adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27, 419-424.<br />

Hurd, M. D., & McGarry, K. (1995). Evaluation of the subjective probabilities of survival <strong>in</strong><br />

the Health <strong>and</strong> Retirement Study. The Journal of Human Resources, 30, S268-S292.<br />

Hurd, M. D., & McGarry, K. (2002). The predictive validity of subjective probabilities of<br />

survival. The Economic Journal, 112, 966-985.<br />

Jacobs, J. E., & Klaczynski, P. A. (2002). The development of judgment <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong><br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g childhood <strong>and</strong> adolescence. Current Directions <strong>in</strong> Psychological Science,<br />

44, 304-316.<br />

Jacobs, J. E., & Potenza, M. (1991). The use of judgment heuristics to make social <strong>and</strong><br />

object <strong>decision</strong>s: A developmental perspective. Child Development, 62, 166-178.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 34<br />

Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision mak<strong>in</strong>g: A psychological analysis of conflict,<br />

choice, <strong>and</strong> commitment. New York, NY: Free Press.<br />

Jones, R. K., Darroch, J. E., & Henshaw, S. K. (2002). Patterns <strong>in</strong> the socioeconomic<br />

characteristics of women obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g abortions <strong>in</strong> 2000-2001. Perspectives on Sexual <strong>and</strong><br />

Reproductive Health, 34, 226-235.<br />

Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). <strong>Judgment</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g under<br />

uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty: Heuristics <strong>and</strong> biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Keren, G., & Bru<strong>in</strong>e de Bru<strong>in</strong>, W. (2003). On the assessment of <strong>decision</strong> quality:<br />

Considerations regard<strong>in</strong>g utility, conflict, <strong>and</strong> accountability. In D. Hardman & L.<br />

Macchi (Eds.), Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. Psychological perspectives on reason<strong>in</strong>g, judgment <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g (pp. 347-363). Hoboken NJ: Wiley..<br />

Keren, G., & Schul, Y. (<strong>in</strong> press). Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of<br />

two-system theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science.<br />

Kim, N., Stanton, B., Li, X., Dickers<strong>in</strong>, K., & Galbraith, J. (1997). Effectiveness of the 40<br />

adolescent AIDS risk reduction <strong>in</strong>terventions: A quantitative review. Journal of<br />

Adolescent Health, 20, 204-215.<br />

Kirby, D. (2002a). Effective approaches to reduc<strong>in</strong>g adolescent unprotected sex, pregnancy,<br />

<strong>and</strong> childbear<strong>in</strong>g. The Journal of Sex Research, 39, 51–57.<br />

Kirby, D. (2002b). The impact of schools <strong>and</strong> school programs upon adolescent sexual<br />

behavior. The Journal of Sex Research, 39, 27-33.<br />

Klaczynski, P. A. (2001). Analytic <strong>and</strong> heuristic processes <strong>in</strong>fluences on adolescent<br />

reason<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Child Development, 72, 844-861.<br />

Klaczynski, P. A., & Cottrell, J. M. (2004). A dual-process approach to cognitive<br />

development: The case of children’s underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of sunk cost <strong>decision</strong>s. Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><br />

Reason<strong>in</strong>g, 10, 147-174.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 35<br />

Kle<strong>in</strong>, G. (1999). Sources of power: How people make <strong>decision</strong>s. Cambridge, MA: MIT<br />

Press.<br />

Kle<strong>in</strong>, J. D. (2006). Adolescents <strong>and</strong> smok<strong>in</strong>g: The first puff may be the worst. Canadian<br />

Medical Association Journal, 175, 262-263.<br />

Lecci, L., Okun, M. A., Karoly, P. (1994). Life regrets <strong>and</strong> current goals as predictors of<br />

psychological adjustment. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 66, 731-741.<br />

L<strong>in</strong>ville, P. W., Fisher, G. W., & Fischhoff, B. (1993). AIDS risk perceptions <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong><br />

biases. In J. B. Pryor & G. D. Reeder (Eds.), The social psychology of HIV <strong>in</strong>fection (pp.<br />

5–38). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1982). Regret theory: An alternative theory of rational choice<br />

under uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty. The Economic Journal, 92, 805-824.<br />

Lourens, P. F., Vissers, J. A. M. M., & Jessurun, M. (1999). Annual mileage, driv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

violations, <strong>and</strong> accident <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> relation to drivers' sex, age, <strong>and</strong> level of<br />

education. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 31, 593-597.<br />

Macaluso, M., Kelaghan, J., Artz, L., Aust<strong>in</strong>, H., Fleenor, M., Hook III, E.W., & Valappil, T.<br />

(1999). Mechanical failure of the latex condom <strong>in</strong> a cohort of women at high STD<br />

risk. Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 26, 450-458.<br />

Maher, J.E., Peterman, T.A., Osewe, P.L., Odusanya, S., & Scerba, J.R. (2003). Evaluation<br />

of a community-based organization’s <strong>in</strong>tervention to reduce the <strong>in</strong>cidence of sexually<br />

transmitted diseases: A r<strong>and</strong>omized, controlled trial. Southern Medical Journal, 96,<br />

248-253.<br />

Maibach, E., & Flora, J. A. (1993). Symbolic model<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> cognitive rehearsal: Us<strong>in</strong>g video<br />

to promote AIDS prevention self-efficacy. Communication Research, 20, 517–545.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 36<br />

Mann, L., Harmoni, R., Power, C., Beswick, G., & Ormond, C. (1988). Effectiveness of the<br />

GOFER course <strong>in</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g for high school students. Journal of Behavioral<br />

Decision Mak<strong>in</strong>g, 1, 159-168.<br />

McKay, A. (2000). Prevention of sexually transmitted <strong>in</strong>fections <strong>in</strong> different populations: A<br />

review of behaviorally effective <strong>and</strong> cost-effective <strong>in</strong>terventions. The Canadian Journal<br />

of Human Sexuality, 9, 95–120.<br />

McKenzie, C.R.M. (2004). Fram<strong>in</strong>g effects <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>ference tasks – <strong>and</strong> why they are<br />

normatively defensible. Memory & Cognition, 32, 874-885.<br />

McKenzie, C.R.M., & Nelson, J.D. (2003). What a speaker's choice of frame reveals:<br />

Reference po<strong>in</strong>ts, frame selection, <strong>and</strong> fram<strong>in</strong>g effects. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong> & Review,<br />

10, 596-602.<br />

Michie, S. & Abraham, C. (2004). Interventions to change health behaviors: Evidence-based<br />

or evidence-<strong>in</strong>spired? Psychology & Health, 19, 29–49.<br />

Millste<strong>in</strong>, S. G., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2002). <strong>Judgment</strong>s about risk <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>vulnerability<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> <strong>and</strong> young adults. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12, 399-422.<br />

Must, A. (1996). Morbidity <strong>and</strong> mortality associated with elevated body weight <strong>in</strong> children<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong>. American Journal of Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Nutrition, 63, 445S-447S.<br />

O’Donnell, C.R., O’Donell, L., San Doval, A., Duran, R., & Labes, K. (1998). Reductions <strong>in</strong><br />

STD <strong>in</strong>fections subsequent to an STD cl<strong>in</strong>ic visit. Us<strong>in</strong>g video-based patient education to<br />

supplement provider <strong>in</strong>teractions. Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 25, 161-168.<br />

O’Donnell, L.N., San Doval, A., Duran, R., & O’Donnell, C. (1995). Video-based sexually<br />

transmitted disease patient education: Its impact on condom acquisition. American<br />

Journal of Public Health, 85, 817-822.<br />

Ogden, J. (2003). Some problems with social cognition models: A pragmatic <strong>and</strong> conceptual<br />

analysis. Health Psychology, 22, 424–428.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 37<br />

Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2005). Decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g competence: External validation<br />

through an <strong>in</strong>dividual-differences approach. Journal of Behavioral Decision Mak<strong>in</strong>g, 18,<br />

1–27.<br />

Quadrel, M. J., Fischhoff, B., & Davis, W. (1993). Adolescent (<strong>in</strong>)vulnerability. American<br />

Psychologist, 48, 102-116.<br />

Raiffa, H. (1968). Decision analysis. Read<strong>in</strong>g, MA: Addison-Wesley.<br />

Reyna, V. F. (2004). How people make <strong>decision</strong>s that <strong>in</strong>volve risk: A dual-process<br />

approach. Currrent Directions <strong>in</strong> Psychological Science, 13, 60-66.<br />

Reyna, V. F., & Ellis, S. C. (1994). Fuzzy-trace theory <strong>and</strong> fram<strong>in</strong>g effects <strong>in</strong> children’s<br />

risky <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Psychological Science, 5, 275-279.<br />

Reyna, V. F., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk <strong>and</strong> rationality <strong>in</strong> adolescent <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Psychological Perspectives <strong>in</strong> the Public Interest, 7, 1-44.<br />

Rob<strong>in</strong>son, B.E., Uhl, G., M<strong>in</strong>er, M., Bockt<strong>in</strong>g, W.O., Scheltema, K.E., Rosser, B.R., &<br />

Westover, B. (2002). Evaluation of a sexual health approach to prevent HIV among low<br />

<strong>in</strong>come, urban, primarily African American women: Results of a r<strong>and</strong>omized controlled<br />

trial. AIDS Education & Prevention, 14, 81-96.<br />

Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive <strong>and</strong> physiological processes <strong>in</strong> fear appeals <strong>and</strong> attitude<br />

change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, R. E. Petty, & D.<br />

Shapiro (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: A sourcebook (pp. 153–176). New York:<br />

Guilford.<br />

Rothman, A. J. (2000). Toward a theory-based analysis of behavioral ma<strong>in</strong>tenance. Health<br />

Psychology, 19, 64-69.<br />

Schuster, M. A., Bell, R. M., & Kanouse, D. E. (1996). The sexual practices of adolescent<br />

virg<strong>in</strong>s: Genital sexual activities of high school students who have never had vag<strong>in</strong>al<br />

<strong>in</strong>tercourse. American Journal of Public Health, 86, 1570–1576.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 38<br />

Sher, S., & McKenzie, C. R. M. (2005). Information leakage from logically equivalent<br />

frames. Cognition, 101, 467-494.<br />

Skara, S., & Sussman, S. (2003). A review of 25 long-term adolescent tobacco <strong>and</strong> other<br />

drug use prevention program evaluations. Preventive Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 37, 451-477.<br />

Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reason<strong>in</strong>g. Psychological<br />

Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 119, 3–22.<br />

Smith, P. H., White, J. W., & Holl<strong>and</strong>, L. J. (2003). A longitud<strong>in</strong>al perspective on dat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

violence among adolescent <strong>and</strong> college-age women. American Journal of Public Health,<br />

93, 1104-1109.<br />

Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences <strong>in</strong> rational thought. Journal of<br />

Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 161–188.<br />

Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences <strong>in</strong> reason<strong>in</strong>g: Implications for<br />

the rationality debate? Behavioral <strong>and</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong> Sciences, 23, 645–726.<br />

Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008). On the relative <strong>in</strong>dependence of th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g biases <strong>and</strong><br />

cognitive ability. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 94, 672-695.<br />

Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-tak<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Developmental Review, 28, 78-106.<br />

Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, L., Fegley, S., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1993). Negative impact of part-time work on<br />

adolescent adjustment: Evidence from a longitud<strong>in</strong>al study. Developmental Psychology,<br />

29, 171-180.<br />

Stice, E., Shaw, H., & Marti, C.N. (2006). A meta-analytic review of obesity prevention<br />

programs for children <strong>and</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong>: The sk<strong>in</strong>ny on <strong>in</strong>terventions that work.<br />

Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 132, 667-691.


<strong>JDM</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>adolescents</strong> 39<br />

Turner, C. & McClure, R. (2003). Age <strong>and</strong> gender differences <strong>in</strong> risk tak<strong>in</strong>g behaviour as an<br />

explanation for high <strong>in</strong>cidence of motor vehicle crashes as a driver <strong>in</strong> young males.<br />

Injury Control <strong>and</strong> Safety Promotion, 10, 123-130.<br />

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The fram<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>decision</strong>s <strong>and</strong> the psychology of<br />

choice. Science, 211, 453-458.<br />

Valdeserri, R.O., Ogden, L.L., & McCray, E. (2003). Accomplishments <strong>in</strong> HIV prevention<br />

science: Implications for stemm<strong>in</strong>g the epidemic. Nature Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 9, 881-886.<br />

Wall<strong>in</strong>, K. L., Wiklund, F., Luostar<strong>in</strong>en, T., Ǻngström, T., Antilla, T., Bergman, F.,<br />

Hallmans, G., Ikaheimo, I., Koskela, P., Leht<strong>in</strong>en, M., Stendahl, U., Paavonen, J., &<br />

Dillner, J. (2002). A population-based prospective study of Chlamydia Trachomatis<br />

<strong>in</strong>fection <strong>and</strong> cervical carc<strong>in</strong>oma. International Journal of Cancer, 101, 371-374.<br />

Wiehe, S. E., Garrison, M. M., Christakis, D. A., Ebel, B. E., & Rivara, F. P. (2005). A<br />

systematic review of school-based smok<strong>in</strong>g prevention trials with long-term follow-up.<br />

Journal of Adolescent Health, 36, 162-169.<br />

von W<strong>in</strong>terfeldt, D., & Edwards, W. (1986). Decision analysis <strong>and</strong> behavioral research.<br />

New York: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Perceived control of life regrets: Good for young <strong>and</strong><br />

bad for old adults. Psychology <strong>and</strong> Ag<strong>in</strong>g, 17, 340-350.<br />

Yates, J. F. (1990). <strong>Judgment</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>decision</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!