13.09.2013 Views

Head-Body Temperature Differences in Free-Ranging Rubber Boas

Head-Body Temperature Differences in Free-Ranging Rubber Boas

Head-Body Temperature Differences in Free-Ranging Rubber Boas

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

00530 (paratype of A. komaii); Chiaolip<strong>in</strong>g, Chiai Pref.:<br />

KUZ 32954-955; Tamaopu, Chiai. Pref.: NMNS 01480;<br />

Kuantzul<strong>in</strong>g, Ta<strong>in</strong>an Pref.: CAS 18006 00530 (paratype of A. komaii); Chiaolip<strong>in</strong>g, Chiai Pref.:<br />

KUZ 32954-955; Tamaopu, Chiai. Pref.: NMNS 01480;<br />

Kuantzul<strong>in</strong>g, (holotype of A.<br />

formosensis), KUZ 19386; Chipen Wenchuan, Taitung<br />

Pref.: NMNS 01609; Chufengshan, P<strong>in</strong>gtung Pref.:<br />

NMNS 00685; detailed loc. unknown: TM RS0053,<br />

Ta<strong>in</strong>an Pref.: CAS 18006 (holotype of A.<br />

formosensis), KUZ 19386; Chipen Wenchuan, Taitung<br />

Pref.: NMNS 01609; Chufengshan, P<strong>in</strong>gtung Pref.:<br />

NMNS 00685; detailed loc. unknown: TM RS0053,<br />

Journal of Herpetology, Vol. 31, No. 1, pp. 87-93, 1997<br />

Copyright 1997 Society for the Journal of Herpetology,<br />

Study of Amphibians and Reptiles<br />

Vol. 31, No. 1, pp. 87-93, 1997<br />

Copyright 1997 Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles<br />

SYSTEMATICS OF EAST ASIAN PAREAS<br />

0127. P. iwasakii-Ishigakijima I., Yaeyama Group:<br />

KUZ 28133-134*, 28446, 28448, 28450, 32933-934,<br />

33107, NTNU B-201290, OMNH R2323; Iriomotejima<br />

I., Yaeyama Is. Group: KUZ 21304*, 32932, 0127. P. iwasakii-Ishigakijima I., Yaeyama Group:<br />

KUZ 28133-134*, 28446, 28448, 28450, 32933-934,<br />

33107, NTNU B-201290, OMNH R2323; Iriomotejima<br />

I., Yaeyama Is. Group: MUR 1954-<br />

1-08-1; Yaeyama Group (detailed locality unknown):<br />

OPM one uncatalogued specimen.<br />

KUZ 21304*, 32932, MUR 1954-<br />

1-08-1; Yaeyama Group (detailed locality unknown):<br />

OPM one uncatalogued specimen.<br />

<strong>Head</strong>-body <strong>Temperature</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Head</strong>-body <strong>Temperature</strong> <strong>Free</strong>-rang<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Rubber</strong> <strong>Boas</strong><br />

<strong>Differences</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Free</strong>-rang<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Rubber</strong> <strong>Boas</strong><br />

MICHAEL E. DORCAS1 AND CHARLES R. PETERSON<br />

Department of Biological Sciences Idaho State University, Pocatello, Idaho 83209, USA<br />

ABSTRACT.-Although most studies of reptilian thermal biology have measured body temperature from a<br />

s<strong>in</strong>gle location <strong>in</strong> an animal, the presence of regional temperature differences with<strong>in</strong> the bodies of reptiles<br />

should be considered when conduct<strong>in</strong>g detailed studies of their thermal biology. As part of an extensive<br />

study of rubber boa (Char<strong>in</strong>a bottae) thermal biology, we measured the oral and cloacal temperatures of 45<br />

free-rang<strong>in</strong>g rubber boas from June 1990 to August 1995. We used oral temperature as an <strong>in</strong>dicator of head<br />

temperature and cloacal temperature as an <strong>in</strong>dicator of body temperature. Oral temperatures ranged from<br />

13.8 C to 32.2 C and cloacal temperatures ranged from 11.5 C to 34.5 C. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the daytime, rubber boas gen-<br />

erally exhibited warmer heads at average body temperatures below their thermal preference (thermal pref-<br />

erence = 27.4 C) and cooler heads at average body temperatures above their thermal preference. At night, ac-<br />

tive rubber boas exhibited significantly higher head temperatures than body temperatures (mean differ-<br />

ence = 2.0 C). This study represents the first report of regional body temperature differences exhibited by<br />

a reptile dur<strong>in</strong>g nocturnal activity and supports the generalization that head temperature <strong>in</strong> rep-<br />

tiles is ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed with<strong>in</strong> more narrow limits than body temperature dur<strong>in</strong>g the day. Further studies are<br />

required to fully understand both the causes and consequences of regional temperature differences <strong>in</strong> free-<br />

rang<strong>in</strong>g reptiles.<br />

Studies of the causes and consequences of<br />

body temperature variation <strong>in</strong> reptiles can lead<br />

to a better understand<strong>in</strong>g of their overall ecology<br />

<strong>in</strong> addition to augment<strong>in</strong>g our general<br />

knowledge of thermal biology (Huey, 1982; Lillywhite,<br />

1987; Peterson et al., 1993). Although<br />

most studies of reptilian thermal biology have<br />

measured body temperature from a s<strong>in</strong>gle location<br />

<strong>in</strong> an animal, usually cloacal or core temperature,<br />

the existence of regional temperature<br />

differences with<strong>in</strong> the bodies of reptiles should<br />

be considered when conduct<strong>in</strong>g detailed studies<br />

of their thermal biology (Dill, 1972; Peterson<br />

et al., 1993). Determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the underly<strong>in</strong>g mecha-<br />

Studies of the causes and consequences of<br />

body temperature<br />

nisms responsible for regional temperature differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> reptiles is often difficult. Regional<br />

temperature differences have been attributed to<br />

several nonexclusive factors, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g physical<br />

differences among body regions (e.g., surface<br />

variation <strong>in</strong> reptiles can lead<br />

to a better understand<strong>in</strong>g of their overall ecology<br />

<strong>in</strong> addition to augment<strong>in</strong>g our general<br />

knowledge of thermal biology (Huey, 1982; Lillywhite,<br />

1987; Peterson et al., 1993). Although<br />

most studies of reptilian thermal biology have<br />

measured body temperature from a s<strong>in</strong>gle location<br />

<strong>in</strong> an animal, usually cloacal or core temperature,<br />

the existence of regional temperature<br />

differences with<strong>in</strong> the bodies of reptiles should<br />

be considered when conduct<strong>in</strong>g detailed studies<br />

of their thermal biology (Dill, 1972; Peterson<br />

et al., 1993). Determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the underly<strong>in</strong>g mecha-<br />

nisms responsible for regional temperature differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> reptiles is often difficult. Regional<br />

temperature differences have been attributed to<br />

several nonexclusive factors, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g physical<br />

differences among body regions (e.g., surface<br />

Present Address: Savannah River Ecology Labo-<br />

ratory, Drawer E, Aiken, South Carol<strong>in</strong>a 29802, USA.<br />

87<br />

area to volume ratios; Pough and McFarland,1976),<br />

behavioral thermoregulation (e.g.,<br />

differential exposure to solar radiation; Heath,<br />

1964; Hammerson, 1977; Gregory, 1990), and<br />

physiological mechanisms (e.g., blood shunts or<br />

countercurrent heat exchangers; Heath, 1966;<br />

Webb and Heatwole, 1971).<br />

Several studies of head-body temperature differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> reptiles <strong>in</strong>dicate that head temperature<br />

is ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed with<strong>in</strong> more narrow limits<br />

than body temperature (Heath, 1964; Webb et<br />

al., 1972; Johnson, 1973; Peterson, 1982; 1987).<br />

Ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of head temperature with<strong>in</strong> more<br />

narrow limits than body temperature may be<br />

due to the fact that the thermoregulatory control<br />

center is located <strong>in</strong> the bra<strong>in</strong> of reptiles (Berk<br />

and Heath, 1975). Additionally, precise regulation<br />

of head temperature may help to optimize<br />

central nervous system function (Block and<br />

Carey, 1985). Precise regulation of head temperature<br />

may be related to the propensity of<br />

area to volume ratios; Pough and McFarland,1976),<br />

behavioral thermoregulation (e.g.,<br />

differential exposure to solar radiation; Heath,<br />

1964; Hammerson, 1977; Gregory, 1990), and<br />

physiological<br />

many diurnal reptiles to allow the head to warm<br />

before they fully emerge from nighttime retreats<br />

mechanisms (e.g., blood shunts or<br />

countercurrent heat exchangers; Heath, 1966;<br />

Webb and Heatwole, 1971).<br />

Several studies of head-body temperature differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> reptiles <strong>in</strong>dicate that head temperature<br />

is ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed with<strong>in</strong> more narrow limits<br />

than body temperature (Heath, 1964; Webb et<br />

al., 1972; Johnson, 1973; Peterson, 1982; 1987).<br />

Ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of head temperature with<strong>in</strong> more<br />

narrow limits than body temperature may be<br />

due to the fact that the thermoregulatory control<br />

center is located <strong>in</strong> the bra<strong>in</strong> of reptiles (Berk<br />

and Heath, 1975). Additionally, precise regulation<br />

of head temperature may help to optimize<br />

central nervous system function (Block and<br />

Carey, 1985). Precise regulation of head temperature<br />

may be related to the propensity of<br />

many diurnal reptiles to allow the head to warm<br />

before they fully emerge from nighttime retreats


88<br />

M. E. DORCAS AND C. R. PETERSON<br />

(Fitch, 1960; Heath, 1964; Hammerson, 1977;<br />

Gregory, 1990).<br />

Because of their elongate form, snakes can<br />

exhibit considerable regional differences <strong>in</strong><br />

body temperature (Fitch, 1960; Lillywhite, 1987;<br />

Peterson et al., 1993). Consequently, snakes<br />

offer an excellent opportunity to exam<strong>in</strong>e regional<br />

temperature differences <strong>in</strong> detail. Numerous<br />

reports exist describ<strong>in</strong>g regional temperature<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> snakes under laboratory<br />

or semi-natural conditions (Regal, 1966; Webb<br />

and Heatwole, 1971; Dill, 1972; Johnson, 1973,<br />

1975a, b; Hammerson, 1977) whereas fewer<br />

reports exist for free-rang<strong>in</strong>g snakes (V<strong>in</strong>cent,<br />

1975; Peterson, 1987; Gregory, 1990). No study<br />

has exam<strong>in</strong>ed, <strong>in</strong> detail, regional temperature<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> free-rang<strong>in</strong>g nocturnal snakes.<br />

Studies of regional temperature differences <strong>in</strong><br />

snakes active at night, when they do not have<br />

access to solar radiation, will help us to further<br />

understand both the behavioral and physiological<br />

mechanisms by which snakes ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><br />

regional temperature differences and potentially<br />

shed light on the functional and ecological<br />

effects of regional temperature differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> reptiles.<br />

As part of an extensive study of rubber boa<br />

(Char<strong>in</strong>a bottae) thermal biology, we exam<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> the head and body temperatures<br />

of free-rang<strong>in</strong>g rubber boas <strong>in</strong> southeastern<br />

Idaho. <strong>Rubber</strong> boas are ideal subjects for studies<br />

of regional temperature differences because<br />

they are extremely docile. Thus, measurement of<br />

their temperature is relatively easy and <strong>in</strong>volves<br />

m<strong>in</strong>imal handl<strong>in</strong>g. Additionally, rubber boas are<br />

often nocturnal, frequently at very low body<br />

temperatures (Dorcas, 1995), and thus provide<br />

an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g comparison to studies of regional<br />

temperature differences <strong>in</strong> diurnal species.<br />

In this study, we compare our results with<br />

previous research, propose possible mecha-<br />

nisms to expla<strong>in</strong> the existence of regional temperature<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> rubber boas, and discuss<br />

what is needed to further understand both the<br />

causes and consequences of regional temperature<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> reptiles.<br />

MATERIALS AND METHODS<br />

We determ<strong>in</strong>ed the thermal preference (= selected<br />

temperature; Pough and Gans, 1982) of<br />

rubber boas by measur<strong>in</strong>g the cloacal temperatures<br />

of 19 rubber boas <strong>in</strong> a laboratory thermal<br />

gradient. Snakes were collected from eastern<br />

Wash<strong>in</strong>gton and southeastern Idaho. We housed<br />

snakes <strong>in</strong>dividually <strong>in</strong> 37.8 L aquaria with a substrate<br />

of 3-5 cm of aspen bedd<strong>in</strong>g covered with<br />

two layers of newspaper. All snakes had been<br />

<strong>in</strong> captivity at least one month before thermal<br />

preference experiments began. Us<strong>in</strong>g fluores-<br />

matched that of southeastern Idaho. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

active season (May through October), the ambient<br />

room temperature was kept at approximately<br />

18 C. One end of each cage was placed<br />

on heat tapes to raise the operative temperature<br />

(Bakken, 1992) at that end of the cage to 38 C.<br />

To produce a relatively l<strong>in</strong>ear thermal gradient<br />

between the front and rear of each cage, we<br />

glued<br />

cent light<strong>in</strong>g, we provided a photoperiod that<br />

two layers of t<strong>in</strong> to the cage bottoms to<br />

conduct heat to the cooler end. This arrangement<br />

allowed the snakes to select body temperatures<br />

between 18 C and 38 C. All body temperature<br />

measurements were made dur<strong>in</strong>g the summers<br />

(1 June-31 August) of 1990 and 1991 between<br />

1100 and 1400 h. To make the<br />

measurements, we carefully lifted the tips of the<br />

snakes' tails with one hand and <strong>in</strong>serted a thermocouple<br />

approximately 1 cm <strong>in</strong>to their cloacas,<br />

disturb<strong>in</strong>g the snakes as little as possible. We<br />

used a thermocouple thermometer (Model<br />

HH23, Omega, Stamford, CT) with a type T thermocouple<br />

(36 gauge). To reduce irritation to the<br />

snakes, the tip of the thermocouple was coated<br />

with paraff<strong>in</strong> and petroleum jelly was used as a<br />

lubricant. To determ<strong>in</strong>e the thermal preference<br />

of rubber boas, we used only temperatures<br />

taken from nongravid, nondigest<strong>in</strong>g snakes to<br />

calculate each snake's mean body temperature<br />

and a s<strong>in</strong>gle grand mean for all snakes. We used<br />

the grand mean as the thermal preference.<br />

To <strong>in</strong>vestigate head and body temperature<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> free-rang<strong>in</strong>g rubber boas, we measured<br />

the oral and cloacal temperatures of rubber<br />

boas when encountered <strong>in</strong> the field from<br />

June 1990 to August 1995. Oral temperatures<br />

were taken by grasp<strong>in</strong>g the snake beh<strong>in</strong>d the<br />

head and <strong>in</strong>sert<strong>in</strong>g the bulb of a quick-read<strong>in</strong>g<br />

thermometer (model T-6000, Miller and Weber<br />

Inc., Queens, NY) approximately 2 cm <strong>in</strong>to the<br />

mouth and anterior esophagus. Cloacal temperatures<br />

were taken by carefully lift<strong>in</strong>g the tail<br />

and <strong>in</strong>sert<strong>in</strong>g the bulb of the thermometer approximately<br />

2 cm <strong>in</strong>to the cloaca and rectum. We<br />

wiped the bulb of the thermometer with a dry<br />

cloth between measurements. Both oral and<br />

cloacal temperatures were measured to the nearest<br />

0.1 C with<strong>in</strong> 20 sec of each other and with<strong>in</strong><br />

30 sec of capture. Care was taken to touch the<br />

snakes as little as possible so as to m<strong>in</strong>imize the<br />

effects on their body temperatures. When possible,<br />

snakes were left on the ground while temperatures<br />

were taken. On five of the eleven occasions<br />

on which we measured the temperatures<br />

of snakes at night, we also measured soil surface<br />

and 1 cm air temperatures.<br />

Measurements were not taken <strong>in</strong> a fixed order.<br />

Instead, we arbitrarily took either the cloacal<br />

or oral temperature first (oral temperatures<br />

were taken first for about 1/2 of the measurements).<br />

On a few occasions we remeasured


HEAD AND BODY TEMPERATURES OF RUBBER BOAS<br />

TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics summariz<strong>in</strong>g the measurements of oral and cloacal temperatures of rubber<br />

boas (Char<strong>in</strong>a bottae) dur<strong>in</strong>g the day and at night. The mean difference is presented as the mean absolute value<br />

of the differences between oral and cloacal temperatures.<br />

Oral temperature (C) Cloacal temperature (C) Mean (C)<br />

N M<strong>in</strong>imum Maximum M<strong>in</strong>imum Maximum 1Oral-Cloacall<br />

Day 34 15.2 32.2 13.8 34.5 2.0<br />

Night 11 13.8 28.2 11.5 25.8 2.0<br />

snakes' cloacal temperatures after first tak<strong>in</strong>g<br />

their cloacal and oral temperatures (<strong>in</strong> that order)<br />

and observed no change from the first cloacal<br />

temperature taken.<br />

The oral and cloacal temperatures of 45 different<br />

snakes were measured. Some snakes were<br />

encountered more than once dur<strong>in</strong>g the course<br />

of the study (e.g., telemetered snakes) and we<br />

measured their temperatures each time. To<br />

avoid problems of non<strong>in</strong>dependence, we randomly<br />

selected one pair of measurements to use<br />

<strong>in</strong> the analyses for each snake that had multiple<br />

measurements.<br />

We used oral temperature as an <strong>in</strong>dicator of<br />

head temperature and cloacal temperature as an<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicator of body temperature. We exam<strong>in</strong>ed the<br />

relationship between oral and cloacal temperatures<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>ear regression and <strong>in</strong>terpreted a<br />

slope -71 as an <strong>in</strong>dication of regional temperature<br />

differences. For further statistical analysis,<br />

we considered snakes measured dur<strong>in</strong>g daytime<br />

separately from those measured at night. The<br />

daytime measurements were taken between<br />

0831 and 1952 MST and were analyzed by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

the relationship between 13.8 C to 32.2 C and cloacal temperatures ranged<br />

from 11.5 C to 34.5 C (Table 1). The relationship<br />

between oral and cloacal temperature had a<br />

slope significantly<br />

average body<br />

temperature (calculated as the mean of oral and<br />

cloacal temperature) and the difference between<br />

oral and cloacal temperatures us<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>ear regression.<br />

We considered the mean of oral and<br />

cloacal temperatures to be a better <strong>in</strong>dicator of<br />

overall body temperature than either oral or<br />

cloacal temperature alone. For the 11 snakes<br />

measured at night (taken between 2010 and 0136<br />

MST), we tested for a difference <strong>in</strong> oral and cloacal<br />

temperatures us<strong>in</strong>g a paired t-test. All statistical<br />

tests were conducted us<strong>in</strong>g SYSTAT<br />

(Wilk<strong>in</strong>son, 1990) with a rejection level of a<br />

0.05.<br />

RESULTS<br />

different than one (upper and<br />

lower 95% confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals equal 0.69 and<br />

0.81 respectively), thus <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g the presence<br />

of regional temperature differences (Fig. 1). Dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the day, at average body temperatures be-<br />

low their thermal preference, snakes tended to<br />

have oral temperatures considerably higher<br />

than their cloacal temperatures. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the day,<br />

at average body temperatures above their thermal<br />

preference, snakes tended to have oral temperatures<br />

lower than their cloacal temperatures<br />

(R2 = 0.60, F = 47.6, df = 32, P < 0.001; Fig.<br />

2A). At night, snakes were active at relatively<br />

low body temperatures (Dorcas, 1995) and had<br />

oral temperatures that averaged 2.0 C higher<br />

than their cloacal temperatures (t < 9.8, df = 10,<br />

P < 0.001; Table 1 and Fig. 2A and B). In all five<br />

<strong>in</strong>stances <strong>in</strong> which we measured soil surface and<br />

1 cm air temperatures (<strong>in</strong> conjunction with oral<br />

and cloacal temperatures) at night, the oral and<br />

cloacal temperatures were lower than the soil<br />

35 -<br />

30 -<br />

- 25<br />

Cu<br />

a)<br />

. 20-<br />

I-<br />

2 15 -<br />

0<br />

O Day<br />

.1<br />

* * -<br />

Night<br />

0<br />

.1<br />

I<br />

10<br />

I *<br />

* ?<br />

10 15 20 25 30 35<br />

Cloacal <strong>Temperature</strong> (?C)<br />

The thermal preference of rubber boas was<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>ed from 942 temperature measure-<br />

ments made on 19 snakes <strong>in</strong> the laboratory ther-<br />

mal gradient. Snake temperatures ranged from<br />

20 C to 36 C and the <strong>in</strong>dividual mean tempera-<br />

tures for the snakes varied from 25.6 C to 29.0<br />

C. The grand mean (thermal preference) was<br />

27.4 C.<br />

In the field, oral temperatures ranged from<br />

89<br />

FIG. 1. The relationship between oral and cloacal<br />

body temperatures <strong>in</strong> rubber boas (Char<strong>in</strong>a bottae).<br />

Open symbols represent measurements made dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the daytime and filled symbols represent measurements<br />

made at night. The dashed l<strong>in</strong>e represents a<br />

1:1 relationship between oral and cloacal temperatures<br />

(i.e., no regional temperature differences). The<br />

solid l<strong>in</strong>e is the regression represent<strong>in</strong>g the relationship<br />

between oral and cloacal temperature <strong>in</strong> rubber<br />

boas (v = (0.75)x + 6.86).


90<br />

A<br />

a<br />

-<br />

Q. a)<br />

o E<br />

(U<br />

8<br />

6-<br />

4-<br />

2<br />

B<br />

I0<br />

0<br />

a v<br />

..- -<br />

-2<br />

1<br />

M. E. DORCAS AND C. R. PETERSON<br />

tion from predation (Dorcas, 1995). When males<br />

or nongravid females were found dur<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

daytime, they were usually <strong>in</strong>active (not mov<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

and partially or completely covered warmer<br />

head<br />

by<br />

dead leaves, which allowed them to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><br />

relatively high body temperatures without be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

completely exposed.<br />

-_ _ _,...<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

-Cs- oral = coacal<br />

| '~ ~ | warmer<br />

8 <<br />

II<br />

body<br />

"'"<br />

Thermal<br />

Preference<br />

O Day<br />

* Night<br />

10 15 20 25 30 35<br />

Average <strong>Body</strong> <strong>Temperature</strong> (C)<br />

B<br />

Air <strong>Temperature</strong> = 13.0?C<br />

\ \\\\E ..Substrate . <strong>Temperature</strong> 16.5C<br />

FIG. 2. A) The relationship of average body temperature<br />

(calculated as the mean of the head and cloacal<br />

temperature) and the difference between head<br />

and cloacal temperature <strong>in</strong> rubber boas (Char<strong>in</strong>a bottae).<br />

The data po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> the upper half of part A represent<br />

snakes with warmer heads and data po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong><br />

the lower half of the figure represent snakes with<br />

warmer cloacas. Open symbols represent measurements<br />

made dur<strong>in</strong>g the daytime and filled symbols<br />

represent measurements made at night. The measurements<br />

result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the data po<strong>in</strong>t labeled "B" are illustrated<br />

<strong>in</strong> part B of the figure. The regression l<strong>in</strong>e is<br />

generated from daytime measurements only (y =<br />

(-0.34)x + 9.53, R2 = 0.60, df = 33, P < 0.001). Note<br />

that the po<strong>in</strong>t at which the regression l<strong>in</strong>e crosses the<br />

l<strong>in</strong>e represent<strong>in</strong>g equal head and cloacal temperatures<br />

co<strong>in</strong>cides with the thermal preference of rubber boas.<br />

See text for further discussion. B) A diagram represent<strong>in</strong>g<br />

one example of a set of environmental (soil surface<br />

and 1 cm air) and snake oral and cloacal temperatures<br />

taken at night. Note that substrate temperature<br />

was higher than either oral or cloacal temperature but<br />

both oral and cloacal temperatures were higher than<br />

the air temperature. This snake was an adult female<br />

and was captured and its temperatures measured on<br />

11 July 1991 at 2154 MST (data po<strong>in</strong>t labeled "B" <strong>in</strong><br />

part A).<br />

surface temperature but higher than the air temperature<br />

(Fig. 2B).<br />

Dur<strong>in</strong>g the day, snakes were usually encountered<br />

while bask<strong>in</strong>g, sometimes only partially<br />

exposed under dead leaves or <strong>in</strong> rock crevices<br />

(Dorcas, 1995). Most of the high temperature<br />

read<strong>in</strong>gs (i.e., >30 C) were of gravid females us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a large rock Two ma<strong>in</strong> po<strong>in</strong>ts arise from this study. First,<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g the daytime, rubber boas exhibited<br />

warmer heads at temperatures below their thermal<br />

preference and warmer bodies at temperatures<br />

above their thermal preference. Second,<br />

rubber boas active at night exhibited significantly<br />

higher<br />

outcropp<strong>in</strong>g as a "rookery"<br />

(Klauber, 1972; Graves and Duvall, 1993; Cobb,<br />

1994) which allowed thermoregulation at high,<br />

stable body temperatures and apparent protec-<br />

head temperatures than body tem-<br />

peratures.<br />

The observation that rubber boas appear to<br />

regulate their head temperature more precisely<br />

than their body temperature dur<strong>in</strong>g the daytime<br />

is consistent with the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of previous<br />

researchers (Webb and Heatwole, 1971; Hammerson,<br />

1977; Johnson, 1973; Peterson, 1982,<br />

1987). The differences observed <strong>in</strong> head and<br />

body temperatures may be due to several nonexclusive<br />

factors. Pough and McFarland (1976)<br />

showed that substantial differences <strong>in</strong> head and<br />

body temperatures can be found <strong>in</strong> dead, as well<br />

as live, green iguanas (Iguana iguana), thus <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a passive mechanism for the ma<strong>in</strong>tenance<br />

of regional temperature differences. They attributed<br />

the temperature differences observed to<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> the surface area to volume ratios,<br />

and thus heat<strong>in</strong>g and cool<strong>in</strong>g rates, of the iguanas'<br />

heads and bodies. Because the heads of rubber<br />

boas are not well differentiated from their<br />

bodies, it is unlikely that the differences observed<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g the daytime are due to such passive,<br />

physical mechanisms. We believe that an<br />

active, rather than passive, mechanism is the<br />

most likely cause of regional temperature differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> rubber boas dur<strong>in</strong>g the daytime.<br />

Active ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of regional temperature<br />

differences can <strong>in</strong>volve behavioral mechanisms,<br />

physiological mechanisms, or both. Physiological<br />

mechanisms (blood shunts or countercurrent<br />

heat exchangers) responsible for regional<br />

temperature differences have been described<br />

for several species of reptiles, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g<br />

some pythons (Webb and Heatwole, 1971; Johnson,<br />

1973) and lizards (Heath, 1966; Webb et al.,<br />

1972; Spray and Belk<strong>in</strong>, 1973; Crawford et al.,<br />

1977). However, the role of physiological mechanisms<br />

<strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g regional temperature differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> rubber boas dur<strong>in</strong>g the daytime is<br />

unknown.<br />

It is clear that, dur<strong>in</strong>g the daytime, behavioral<br />

o u 0<br />

m<br />

0<br />

-4-<br />

-6-<br />

-8<br />

I * I I I I<br />

mechanisms play an important role <strong>in</strong> the ma<strong>in</strong>tenance<br />

of regional temperature differences by<br />

rubber boas. Gravid rubber boas frequently


HEAD AND BODY TEMPERATURES OF RUBBER BOAS<br />

warm their develop<strong>in</strong>g embryos by expos<strong>in</strong>g<br />

only the posterior portion of their body to solar<br />

radiation, which likely is important for accelerat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

embryonic developmental rate (Dorcas,<br />

1995). Accelerat<strong>in</strong>g embryonic developmental<br />

rate may be especially important dur<strong>in</strong>g cool<br />

years at northern latitudes and/or high alti-<br />

know if regional temperature differences persist<br />

<strong>in</strong> the early morn<strong>in</strong>g hours.<br />

Whereas the exact mechanism is unknown,<br />

warm<strong>in</strong>g of the head dur<strong>in</strong>g nocturnal activity<br />

likely occurs via heat transfer from the substrate.<br />

Whether physiological (e.g., blood<br />

shunts or counter-current exchangers) or behavioral<br />

activities (e.g., periodically bury<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tudes, if the snakes are to give birth before time<br />

for hibernation (Dorcas, 1995). While warm<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the posterior portion of their body to high temperatures<br />

(maximum recorded = 36 C), the<br />

snakes ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> considerably lower head temperatures<br />

(maximum recorded = 32.5 C). This<br />

is apparently done by shield<strong>in</strong>g their head from<br />

direct solar radiation, although physiological<br />

processes cannot be ruled out. Likewise, the<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of higher head temperatures at<br />

lower average body temperatures (below their<br />

thermal preference) may be the result of behavioral<br />

processes (e.g., heliothermal and/or thigmothermal<br />

activities), physiological processes,<br />

or both. However, dur<strong>in</strong>g this study, we did not<br />

observe head bask<strong>in</strong>g, a behavior apparently<br />

common <strong>in</strong> some diurnal snakes (Hammerson,<br />

1977,1979,1987,1989; Gregory, 1990). Whatever<br />

the mechanism, the ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of head temperatures<br />

with<strong>in</strong> more narrow limits than body<br />

temperatures dur<strong>in</strong>g the daytime supports the<br />

premise that their head <strong>in</strong> loose soil) contribute to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

temperature control of the central<br />

nervous system is a primary factor <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the observed patterns of regional temperature<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> reptiles.<br />

This study is the first to demonstrate the pres-<br />

warmer head temperatures is unknown. A<br />

physiologically-based hypothesis is that, as a<br />

snake crawls along the ground, heat is transferred<br />

from the warmer ground to the snake's<br />

body via its ventral surface. By restrict<strong>in</strong>g blood<br />

flow to the dorsum and allow<strong>in</strong>g warm blood<br />

from the venter to flow anteriorly toward the<br />

head, the snake might ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> head temperatures<br />

a few degrees higher than the rest of its<br />

body. Whatever the mechanism, the presence of<br />

warmer head temperatures dur<strong>in</strong>g activity at<br />

low overall body temperatures further supports<br />

the view that regional temperature differences<br />

may play an important role <strong>in</strong> optimiz<strong>in</strong>g cen-<br />

tral nervous system function (Heath, 1964;<br />

Campbell, 1969; Webb et al., 1972; J. Kauffman<br />

and A. Bennett, pers. comm.). However, the<br />

functional consequences of ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g head<br />

temperature 2-3 C above body temperature<br />

have yet to be determ<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

Further studies are needed to exam<strong>in</strong>e both<br />

the causes and consequences of regional temperature<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> reptiles. Detailed studies<br />

ence of regional temperature differences (i.e.,<br />

higher head temperatures) <strong>in</strong> a reptile while active<br />

at night. All of the nighttime measurements<br />

were made on active snakes, mostly from snakes<br />

found cross<strong>in</strong>g dirt roads (us<strong>in</strong>g rectil<strong>in</strong>ear locomotion)<br />

while we were road cruis<strong>in</strong>g. While<br />

the thermal environment above ground is typically<br />

assumed to be spatially homogeneous at<br />

night, our results <strong>in</strong>dicate that this is not necessarily<br />

the case. On the five occasions on which<br />

we took environmental measurements <strong>in</strong> conjunction<br />

with snake body temperatures at night,<br />

we found a temperature difference of several degrees<br />

between the soil surface and the cooler air<br />

1 cm above it. This temperature difference between<br />

the soil and the air just above it would<br />

allow rubber boas to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> head temperatures<br />

2-3 C higher than their body temperatures<br />

while they are active at night (Fig. 2A). It should<br />

be noted that the gradient between air and soil<br />

temperatures should decrease throughout the<br />

night as the soil temperature cools, thus reduc<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the opportunities for generat<strong>in</strong>g regional<br />

temperature differences. We did not record us<strong>in</strong>g temperature sensitive radiotelemetry and<br />

automated telemetric monitor<strong>in</strong>g (Peterson and<br />

Dorcas, 1992) should improve our ability to describe<br />

regional temperature differences <strong>in</strong> freerang<strong>in</strong>g<br />

any<br />

temperatures of snakes between 0136 and 0831<br />

MST, even though rubber boas are active at<br />

those times (Dorcas, 1995), and thus we do not<br />

animals. Laboratory or field enclosure<br />

studies us<strong>in</strong>g radiotelemetry, temperature sensitive<br />

PIT (passive <strong>in</strong>tegrated transponder) tags,<br />

and <strong>in</strong>frared video-photography coupled with<br />

detailed physiological studies (e.g., blood flow<br />

experiments) should greatly enhance our understand<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of how regional temperature differ-<br />

ences are ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

Whereas exam<strong>in</strong>ation of the causes of regional<br />

body temperature variation <strong>in</strong> reptiles will certa<strong>in</strong>ly<br />

be enlighten<strong>in</strong>g, studies of the consequences<br />

of regional temperature differences are<br />

needed to further understand its adaptive significance<br />

<strong>in</strong> reptiles. Although precise ma<strong>in</strong>tenance<br />

of head temperatures is presumably important<br />

for central nervous system function <strong>in</strong><br />

reptiles, tests of this assumption are required.<br />

Investigations should <strong>in</strong>clude experiments to<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>e the functional consequences of regional<br />

temperature differences for processes<br />

such as crawl<strong>in</strong>g, swimm<strong>in</strong>g, and tongue flick<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and experiments exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the ecological<br />

consequences of regional temperature differ-<br />

ences for behaviors such as prey detection, prey<br />

91


92<br />

M. E. DORCAS AND C. R. PETERSON<br />

capture, and escape from predators. Given the<br />

physical characteristics of snakes and the tractability<br />

of many species for conduct<strong>in</strong>g both<br />

laboratory and field <strong>in</strong>vestigations, it is clear<br />

that studies of snakes can play an important role<br />

<strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g a deeper understand<strong>in</strong>g of the<br />

causes and consequences of regional temperature<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> reptiles.<br />

Acknowledgments.-We thank Jim Strawn and<br />

Patty Strawn for allow<strong>in</strong>g us to conduct much<br />

of this research on their property and for<br />

directly assist<strong>in</strong>g us with many logistic details.<br />

Michael McDonald assisted with mak<strong>in</strong>g temperature<br />

measurements of some snakes. Mark<br />

Gerber allowed us to use his data on three<br />

snakes measured while he was road cruis<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

We thank J. Whitfield Gibbons, Just<strong>in</strong> D. Congdon,<br />

John R. Lee, Jane K. Distler, Julian C. Lee,<br />

and two anonymous reviewers for provid<strong>in</strong>g<br />

useful comments on the manuscript. This research<br />

was supported by awards from Sigma Xi,<br />

the American Museum (Theodore Roosevelt<br />

Memorial Fund), the Gaige Award of the American<br />

Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists,<br />

the Northwest Science Association, the<br />

Chicago Herpetological Society, the Graduate<br />

School and the Department of Biological Sciences<br />

at Idaho State University, and a National<br />

Science Foundation Doctoral Dissertation Grant<br />

IBN-9224230 to M. E. Dorcas (C. R. Peterson,<br />

sponsor). Data analysis and manuscript preparation<br />

were supported by Contract DE-AC09-<br />

76SR00819 between the U.S. Department of Energy<br />

and the University of Georgia's Savannah<br />

River Ecology Laboratory.<br />

LITERATURE CITED<br />

BAKKEN, G. S. 1992. Measurement and application of<br />

operative and standard operative temperatures <strong>in</strong><br />

ecology. Amer. Zool. 32:194-216.<br />

BERK, M. L., AND J. E. HEATH. 1975. Effects of preoptic,<br />

hypothalamic, and telencephalic lesions on<br />

thermoregulation <strong>in</strong> the lizard, Dipsosaurus dorsa-<br />

lis. J. Thermal Biol. 1:65-78.<br />

BLOCK, B. A., AND F G. CAREY. 1985. Warm bra<strong>in</strong> and<br />

eye temperatures <strong>in</strong> sharks. J. Comp. Physiol. B<br />

156:229-236.<br />

CAMPBELL, H. W. 1969. The effects of temperature on<br />

the auditory sensitivity of lizards. Physiol. Zool.<br />

42:183-210.<br />

COBB, V. A. 1994. The thermal ecology of pregnancy<br />

<strong>in</strong> Great Bas<strong>in</strong> Rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis lutosus).<br />

Unpubl. Ph.D. Thesis. Idaho State Univ.,<br />

Pocatello.<br />

CRAWFORD, E. C. JR., J. PALOMEQUE, AND B. J. BARBER.<br />

1977. A physiological basis for head-body temperature<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> a pant<strong>in</strong>g lizard. DORCAS, M. E. 1995. Test<strong>in</strong>g the coadaptation hypothesis:<br />

the thermoregulatory behavior and thermal<br />

physiology of the rubber boa (Char<strong>in</strong>a bottae). Unpubl.<br />

Comp.<br />

Biochem. Physiol. 56A:161-163.<br />

DILL, C. D. 1972. Reptilian core temperatures: variation<br />

with<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals. Copeia 1972:577-579.<br />

Ph.D. Thesis, Idaho State Univ., Pocatello.<br />

FITCH, H. S. 1960. Autecology of the copperhead.<br />

Univ. Kansas Pub. Mus. Nat. Hist. 13:85-288.<br />

GRAVES, B. M., AND D. DUVALL. 1993. Reproduction,<br />

rookery use, and thermoregulation <strong>in</strong> free-rang<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

pregnant Crotalus viridis viridis. J. Herpetol. 27:33-<br />

41.<br />

GREGORY, P. T. 1990. <strong>Temperature</strong> differences between<br />

head and body <strong>in</strong> garter snakes (Thamnophis) at a<br />

den <strong>in</strong> central British Columbia. J. Herpetol.<br />

24:241-245.<br />

HAMMERSON, G. A. 1977. <strong>Head</strong>-body temperature differences<br />

monitored by telemetry <strong>in</strong> the snake Masticophis<br />

flagellum piceus. Comp. Biochem. Physiol.<br />

57:399-402.<br />

. 1979. Thermal ecology of the striped racer,<br />

Masticophis lateralis. Herpetologica 35:267-273.<br />

. 1987. Thermal behavior of the snake Coluber<br />

constrictor <strong>in</strong> west-central California. J. Therm. Biol.<br />

12:195-197.<br />

. 1989. Effects of weather and feed<strong>in</strong>g on body<br />

temperature and activity <strong>in</strong> the snake Masticophis<br />

flagellum. J. Therm. Biol. 14:219-224.<br />

HEATH, J. E. 1964. <strong>Head</strong>-body temperature differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> horned lizards. Physiol. Zool. 37:273-279.<br />

. 1966. Venous shunts <strong>in</strong> the cephalic s<strong>in</strong>uses<br />

of horned lizards. Physiol. Zool. 39:30-35.<br />

HUEY, R. B. 1982. <strong>Temperature</strong>, physiology, and the<br />

ecology of reptiles. In C. Gans and E H. Pough<br />

(eds.), Biology of the Reptilia. Vol. 12, pp. 25-91<br />

Academic Press, New York.<br />

JOHNSON, C. R. 1973. Thermoregulation <strong>in</strong> pythons-<br />

II. <strong>Head</strong>-body temperature differences and thermal<br />

preferenda <strong>in</strong> Australian pythons. Comp. Biochem.<br />

Physiol. 45A:1065-1087.<br />

. 1975a. <strong>Head</strong>-body temperature thermal control,<br />

thermal preferenda, and voluntary maxima <strong>in</strong><br />

the taipan, Oxyuranus scutellatus (Serpentes: Elapidae).<br />

Zool. J. L<strong>in</strong>n. Soc. London 56:1-12.<br />

1975b. Thermoregulation <strong>in</strong> the Papuan-New<br />

Gu<strong>in</strong>ean boid and colubrid snakes, Candoia car<strong>in</strong>ata,<br />

Candoia aspera and Boiga irregulatis. Zool. J.<br />

L<strong>in</strong>n. Soc. London 56:283-290.<br />

KLAUBER, L. M. 1972. Rattlesnakes: Their Habits, Life<br />

Histories, and Influence on Mank<strong>in</strong>d. Vol. 1. University<br />

of California Press, Berkeley.<br />

LILLYWHITE, H. B. 1987. <strong>Temperature</strong>, energetics, and<br />

physiological ecology. In R. A. Seigel, J. T. Coll<strong>in</strong>s,<br />

and S. S. Novak (eds.), Snakes: Ecology and Evolutionary<br />

Biology, pp. 422-477. MacMillan, New<br />

York.<br />

PETERSON, C. R. 1982. <strong>Body</strong> temperature variation <strong>in</strong><br />

free-liv<strong>in</strong>g garter snakes (Thamnophis elegans va-<br />

grans). Unpubl. Ph.D. Thesis. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton State<br />

Univ., Pullman.<br />

. 1987. Daily variation <strong>in</strong> the body temperatures<br />

of free-rang<strong>in</strong>g garter snakes. Ecology<br />

68:160-169.<br />

, AND M. E. DORCAS. 1992. The use of automated<br />

data acquisition techniques <strong>in</strong> monitor<strong>in</strong>g<br />

amphibian and reptile populations. In D. R.<br />

McCullough and R. H. Barrett, (eds.), Wildlife<br />

2001: Populations, pp. 369-378. Elsevier Applied<br />

Science, London.


HEAD AND BODY TEMPERATURES OF RUBBER BOAS<br />

,A. R. GIBSON, AND M. E. DORCAS. 1993. Snake<br />

thermal ecology: the causes and consequences of<br />

body temperature variation. In R. A. Seigel and J.<br />

T. Coll<strong>in</strong>s, (eds.), Snakes: Ecology and Behavior,<br />

pp. 241-314. McGraw-Hill, New York.<br />

POUGH, F H., AND C. GANS. 1982. The vocabulary of<br />

reptilian thermoregulation. In C. Gans and F. H.<br />

Pough (eds.), Biology of the Reptilia. Vol. 12, pp.<br />

17-23 Academic Press, New York.<br />

POUGH, F. H., AND W. N. MCFARLAND. 1976. A physical<br />

basis for head-body temperature differences <strong>in</strong><br />

reptiles. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 53A:301-303.<br />

REGAL, P. J. 1966. Thermophilic responses follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

feed<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> reptiles. Copeia 1966:588-590.<br />

,A. R. GIBSON, AND M. E. DORCAS. 1993. Snake<br />

thermal ecology: the causes and consequences of<br />

body temperature<br />

SPRAY, D. C., AND D. B. BELKIN. 1973. Thermal patterns<br />

<strong>in</strong> the heat<strong>in</strong>g and cool<strong>in</strong>g of Iguana iguana<br />

variation. In R. A. Seigel and J.<br />

T. Coll<strong>in</strong>s, (eds.), Snakes: Ecology and Behavior,<br />

pp. 241-314. McGraw-Hill, New York.<br />

POUGH, F H., AND C. GANS. 1982. The vocabulary of<br />

reptilian thermoregulation. In C. Gans and F. H.<br />

Pough (eds.), Biology of the Reptilia. Vol. 12, pp.<br />

17-23 Academic Press, New York.<br />

POUGH, F. H., AND W. N. MCFARLAND. 1976. A physical<br />

basis for head-body temperature differences <strong>in</strong><br />

reptiles. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 53A:301-303.<br />

REGAL, P. J. 1966. Thermophilic responses follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

feed<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> reptiles. Copeia 1966:588-590.<br />

SPRAY, D. C., AND D. B. BELKIN. 1973. Thermal patterns<br />

<strong>in</strong> the heat<strong>in</strong>g and cool<strong>in</strong>g of Iguana iguana<br />

Journal of Herpetology, Vol. 31, 31, No. 1, 1, pp. 93-98, 93-98, 1997<br />

Copyright 1997 Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles<br />

Postmetamorphic Development of<br />

Supernumerary Thyroid Glands <strong>in</strong><br />

Pleurodeles waltl<br />

STEFANO GOZZO,1'4 ALESSANDRA TAGLIONI,' RITA CASET-<br />

TI,1 CLAUDIO BAGNOLI,2 AND VINCENZO MONACO,3 'IS-<br />

tituto di Medic<strong>in</strong>a Sperimentale, Consiglio Nazionale delle<br />

Ricerche (CNR), 2Laboratorio di Parassitologia, Istituto<br />

Superiore di Sanita, and 3Dipartimento Ambiente, ENEA,<br />

Rome, Italy.<br />

Dur<strong>in</strong>g a study of histological material obta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

from Pleurodeles waltl, our group observed an unexpected<br />

subdivision of thyroid tissue <strong>in</strong>to more masses<br />

than has generally been reported for other species of<br />

amphibians (Gorbman, 1959; Gorbman and Bern,<br />

1962; Gorbman, 1964; Norris, 1985a). In the present<br />

study, a description of these supernumerary thyroid<br />

aggregations of Pleurodeles waltl <strong>in</strong> various developmental<br />

stages is given. Moreover, the histological reaction<br />

of these structures to thiourea, a substance <strong>in</strong>hibit<strong>in</strong>g<br />

thyroid hormones, was <strong>in</strong>vestigated to verify<br />

their reciprocal biochemical aff<strong>in</strong>ity <strong>in</strong> postmetamorphic<br />

animals.<br />

All the animals used <strong>in</strong> this study were collected<br />

from a breed<strong>in</strong>g stock of Pleurodeles waltl ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

at the ENEA-Casaccia Center <strong>in</strong> Rome. The artificial<br />

breed<strong>in</strong>g of this species began with the importation<br />

<strong>in</strong>to Italy of 20 specimens from a locality near Granada<br />

<strong>in</strong> Spa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> 1985.<br />

The larval stages were reared <strong>in</strong> an outdoor artificial<br />

pond (8 x 6 x 1 m). They were extensively ex-<br />

posed to sunlight and fed on Hyla tadpoles, <strong>in</strong>sects,<br />

worms, daphnia, and other crustaceans. After metamorphosis,<br />

four Dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

groups of five animals each were<br />

a study of histological material obta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

from Pleurodeles waltl, our group observed an unexpected<br />

subdivision of thyroid tissue <strong>in</strong>to more masses<br />

than has generally been reported for other species of<br />

amphibians (Gorbman, 1959; Gorbman and Bern,<br />

1962; Gorbman, 1964; Norris, 1985a). In the present<br />

study, a description of these supernumerary thyroid<br />

aggregations of Pleurodeles waltl <strong>in</strong> various developmental<br />

stages is given. Moreover, the histological reaction<br />

of these structures to thiourea, a substance <strong>in</strong>hibit<strong>in</strong>g<br />

thyroid hormones, was <strong>in</strong>vestigated to verify<br />

their reciprocal biochemical aff<strong>in</strong>ity <strong>in</strong> postmetamorphic<br />

animals.<br />

All the animals used <strong>in</strong> this study were collected<br />

from a breed<strong>in</strong>g stock of Pleurodeles waltl ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

at the ENEA-Casaccia Center <strong>in</strong> Rome. The artificial<br />

breed<strong>in</strong>g of this species began with the importation<br />

<strong>in</strong>to Italy of 20 specimens from a locality near Granada<br />

<strong>in</strong> Spa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> 1985.<br />

The larval stages were reared <strong>in</strong> an outdoor artificial<br />

pond (8 x 6 x 1 m). They were extensively ex-<br />

posed to sunlight and fed on Hyla tadpoles, <strong>in</strong>sects,<br />

worms, daphnia, and other crustaceans. After metamorphosis,<br />

four groups of five animals each were<br />

4<br />

Present Address: Istituto di Medic<strong>in</strong>a Sperimentale,<br />

CNR c/o AMB-PRO-TOSS, ENEA Casaccia, S. P.<br />

Anguillarese km 4<br />

Present Address: Istituto di Medic<strong>in</strong>a Sperimentale,<br />

CNR c/o AMB-PRO-TOSS, ENEA Casaccia, S. P.<br />

Anguillarese 1.3, 00060, Rome, Italy.<br />

km 1.3, 00060, Rome, Italy.<br />

and Ctenosaura hemilopha. Comp. Biochem. Physiol.<br />

44A:881-892.<br />

VINCENT, T. 1975. <strong>Body</strong> temperatures of Thamnophis<br />

sirtalis parietalis at the den site. J. Herpetol. 9:252-<br />

and Ctenosaura hemilopha. Comp. Biochem. Physiol.<br />

44A:881-892.<br />

VINCENT, T. 1975. <strong>Body</strong> temperatures of Thamnophis<br />

sirtalis parietalis at the den site. J. Herpetol.<br />

254.<br />

WEBB, G. J. W., AND H. HEATWOLE. 1971. Patterns of<br />

heat distribution with<strong>in</strong> the bodies of some Australian<br />

pythons. Copeia 1971:209-220.<br />

C. R. JOHNSON, AND B. T. FIRTH. 1972. <strong>Head</strong>body<br />

temperature differences <strong>in</strong> lizards. Physiol.<br />

Zool. 45:130-142.<br />

WILKINSON, L. 1990. SYSTAT: the system for statistics.<br />

Systat, Inc., Evanston, Ill<strong>in</strong>ois.<br />

9:252-<br />

254.<br />

WEBB, G. J. W., AND H. HEATWOLE. 1971. Patterns of<br />

heat distribution with<strong>in</strong> the bodies of some Australian<br />

pythons. Copeia 1971:209-220.<br />

C. R. JOHNSON, AND B. T. FIRTH. 1972. <strong>Head</strong>body<br />

temperature differences <strong>in</strong> lizards. Physiol.<br />

Zool. 45:130-142.<br />

WILKINSON, L. 1990. SYSTAT: the system for statistics.<br />

Systat, Inc., Evanston, Ill<strong>in</strong>ois.<br />

Accepted: 30 October 1996.<br />

SHORTER COMMUNICATIONS<br />

93<br />

placed <strong>in</strong> separate aquaria (40 x 30 x 20 cm) conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

10 L of water, renewed weekly, and through which<br />

air bubbled. Two of the aquaria conta<strong>in</strong>ed 0.16% of<br />

thiourea, also renewed weekly; the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g aquaria<br />

were used for the controls. The animals were fed ad<br />

libitum on strips of beef twice weekly. After one year,<br />

these animals were killed and subjected to histological<br />

analysis.<br />

Ten newly hatched larvae, ten larvae aged 30 d, five<br />

specimens at metamorphic climax, ten control postmetamorphic<br />

specimens, and ten thiourea-exposed<br />

postmetamorphic specimens were used for histological<br />

studies. All the animals were anaesthetized <strong>in</strong> a humid,<br />

ether-saturated box and fixed for ten days <strong>in</strong><br />

Dubosq-Brazil conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 37% formal<strong>in</strong> (Beccari and<br />

Mazzi, 1972). <strong>Body</strong> weight, total body length, snout-<br />

vent length, and the widest head size were measured<br />

after fixation. The anterior regions of the body were<br />

then removed, washed for two hours <strong>in</strong> tap water, and<br />

decalcified for two days <strong>in</strong> a solution composed of 85<br />

parts distilled H20, 10 parts 37% formal<strong>in</strong>, and 5 parts<br />

formic acid. After decalcification, the pieces were<br />

aga<strong>in</strong> washed for six hours <strong>in</strong> runn<strong>in</strong>g tap water and<br />

the picric acid was removed us<strong>in</strong>g a solution of 80%<br />

alcohol and 10% ammonium acetate. The samples<br />

were then dehydrated <strong>in</strong> alcohol, embedded <strong>in</strong> a mixture<br />

of 80% paraff<strong>in</strong>, 16% stearic acid, and 4% white<br />

beeswax, and sliced orthogonally to the animal's longitud<strong>in</strong>al<br />

axis <strong>in</strong>to 15 ,um serial sections. All serial sections<br />

were sta<strong>in</strong>ed with hematoxyl<strong>in</strong>-eos<strong>in</strong>.<br />

The control group and the thiourea-exposed animals,<br />

one year after the metamorphosis, were compared<br />

<strong>in</strong> order to identify differences due to the treatment<br />

between body weight, total body length, snoutvent<br />

length, and widest head size. Data were subjected<br />

to one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to determ<strong>in</strong>e<br />

significant differences between group values.<br />

A differentiation of thyroid tissue <strong>in</strong>to follicles was<br />

not observed <strong>in</strong> the newly-hatched larvae of Pleurodeles<br />

waltl. At the age of 30 d the larvae revealed two<br />

large pyriform thyroid organs located <strong>in</strong> the lower jaw,<br />

to the left and the right of the central musculature.<br />

They displayed wide follicles mostly conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g col-<br />

placed<br />

loid with vacuoles, and a high density of red blood<br />

cells was observed with<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terfollicular spaces.<br />

<strong>in</strong> separate aquaria (40 x 30 x 20 cm) conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

10 L of water, renewed weekly, and through which<br />

air bubbled. Two of the aquaria conta<strong>in</strong>ed 0.16% of<br />

thiourea, also renewed weekly; the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g aquaria<br />

were used for the controls. The animals were fed ad<br />

libitum on strips of beef twice weekly. After one year,<br />

these animals were killed and subjected to histological<br />

analysis.<br />

Ten newly hatched larvae, ten larvae aged 30 d, five<br />

specimens at metamorphic climax, ten control postmetamorphic<br />

specimens, and ten thiourea-exposed<br />

postmetamorphic specimens were used for histological<br />

studies. All the animals were anaesthetized <strong>in</strong> a humid,<br />

ether-saturated box and fixed for ten days <strong>in</strong><br />

Dubosq-Brazil conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 37% formal<strong>in</strong> (Beccari and<br />

Mazzi, 1972). <strong>Body</strong> weight, total body length, snout-<br />

vent length, and the widest head size were measured<br />

after fixation. The anterior regions of the body were<br />

then removed, washed for two hours <strong>in</strong> tap water, and<br />

decalcified for two days <strong>in</strong> a solution composed of 85<br />

parts distilled H20, 10 parts 37% formal<strong>in</strong>, and 5 parts<br />

formic acid. After decalcification, the pieces were<br />

aga<strong>in</strong> washed for six hours <strong>in</strong> runn<strong>in</strong>g tap water and<br />

the picric acid was removed us<strong>in</strong>g a solution of 80%<br />

alcohol and 10% ammonium acetate. The samples<br />

were then dehydrated <strong>in</strong> alcohol, embedded <strong>in</strong> a mixture<br />

of 80% paraff<strong>in</strong>, 16% stearic acid, and 4% white<br />

beeswax, and sliced orthogonally to the animal's longitud<strong>in</strong>al<br />

axis <strong>in</strong>to 15 ,um serial sections. All serial sections<br />

were sta<strong>in</strong>ed with hematoxyl<strong>in</strong>-eos<strong>in</strong>.<br />

The control group and the thiourea-exposed animals,<br />

one year after the metamorphosis, were compared<br />

<strong>in</strong> order to identify differences due to the treatment<br />

between body weight, total body length, snoutvent<br />

length, and widest head size. Data were subjected<br />

to one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to determ<strong>in</strong>e<br />

significant differences between group values.<br />

A differentiation of thyroid tissue <strong>in</strong>to follicles was<br />

not observed <strong>in</strong> the newly-hatched larvae of Pleurodeles<br />

waltl. At the age of 30 d the larvae revealed two<br />

large pyriform thyroid organs located <strong>in</strong> the lower jaw,<br />

to the left and the right of the central musculature.<br />

They displayed wide follicles mostly conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g col-<br />

loid with vacuoles, and a high density of red blood<br />

cells was observed with<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terfollicular spaces.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!