21.09.2013 Views

Syntactic Structures

Syntactic Structures

Syntactic Structures

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

SYNTACTIC STRUCTURES<br />

transformation: in some cases reassignment of constituent structure<br />

is preferable. We have thus been following the course outlined in<br />

§ 6. Making use of phrase structure and transformations, we are<br />

trying to construct a grammar of English that will be simpler than<br />

any proposed alternative; and we are giving no thought to the<br />

question of how one might actually arrive at this grammar in some<br />

mechanical way from an English corpus, no matter how extensive.<br />

Our weaker goal of evaluation instead of discovery eliminates any<br />

fear of vicious circularity in the cases discussed above. The intuitive<br />

correspondences and explanations of apparent irregularities seem<br />

to me to offer important evidence for the correctness of the approach<br />

we have been following. Cf. § 8.<br />

8<br />

THE EXPLANATORY POWER OF LINGUISTIC<br />

THEORY<br />

8.1 So far we have considered the linguist's task to be that of<br />

producing a device of some sort (called a grammar) for generating<br />

all and only the sentences of a language, which we have assumed<br />

were somehow given in advance. We have seen that this conception<br />

of the linguist's activities leads us naturally to describe languages in<br />

terms of a set of levels of representation, some of which are quite<br />

abstract and non-trivial. In particular, it leads us to establish phrase<br />

structure and transformational structure as distinct levels of<br />

representation for grammatical sentences. We shall now proceed to<br />

formulate the linguist's goals in quite different and independent<br />

terms which, however, lead to very similar notions of linguistic<br />

structure.<br />

There are many facts about language and linguistic behavior that<br />

require explanation beyond the fact that such and such a string<br />

(which no one may ever have produced) is or is not a sentence. It is<br />

reasonable to expect grammars to provide explanations for some<br />

of these facts. For example, for many English speakers the phoneme<br />

sequence /əneym/ can be understood ambiguously as either "a<br />

name" or "an aim". If our grammar were a one-level system dealing<br />

only with phonemes, we would have no explanation for this fact.<br />

But when we develop the level of morphological representation, we<br />

find that, for quite independent reasons, we are forced to set up<br />

morphemes "a", "an", "aim" and "name", associated with the<br />

phonemic shapes /əə/, /ən/, /eym/ and /neym/. Hence, as an automatic<br />

consequence of the attempt to set up the morphology in the<br />

simplest possible way we find that the phoneme sequence /əneym/ is<br />

ambiguously represented on the morphological level. In general,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!