24.10.2012 Views

The Role of Male Sexual Arousal in Rape: Six Models

The Role of Male Sexual Arousal in Rape: Six Models

The Role of Male Sexual Arousal in Rape: Six Models

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

might go on to postulate that sexually aggressive men differ<br />

from nonaggressive men <strong>in</strong> that their ability to suppress sexual<br />

arousal is less than normal. This hypothesis has not been tested<br />

to any extent, but Wydra, Marshall, Earls, and Barbaree (1983)<br />

compared rapists and nonrapists <strong>in</strong> their ability to suppress<br />

arousal and found no differences.<br />

At the very least, because <strong>of</strong> the advantages <strong>of</strong> parsimony and<br />

the simplicity <strong>of</strong> Hall's model, the "ability to suppress" model<br />

will force the rest <strong>of</strong> us who opt for more complex stimulus<br />

control models to exam<strong>in</strong>e and elim<strong>in</strong>ate the possibility that<br />

our results are not due to this straightforward ability to sup-<br />

press.<br />

Response Compatibifity<br />

Blader and Marshall (1989) argue directly aga<strong>in</strong>st the sexual<br />

preference hypothesis that posits that rapists "prefer" noncon-<br />

sent<strong>in</strong>g sexual activity. <strong>The</strong>y argue that there is no evidence for<br />

sexual arousal to force and violence as an antecedent response<br />

to rape, as the view <strong>of</strong> rape as a paraphilia would have us be-<br />

lieve, because the cues <strong>of</strong>nonconsent and violence cannot stim-<br />

ulate a man to rape when they are produced by the very behav-<br />

ior that is <strong>in</strong> question. Furthermore, Blader and Marshall re-<br />

view the research literature and conclude that rapists are not<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>guished from nonrapists by their erectile responses to<br />

sexual violence <strong>in</strong> the laboratory.<br />

Blader and Marshall (1989) po<strong>in</strong>t to the fact that when a man<br />

rapes, his sexual arousal accompanies his assaultive behavior.<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore, what dist<strong>in</strong>guishes rapists from nonrapists is an abil-<br />

ity to perform the responses <strong>of</strong> hostile aggression and sexual<br />

arousal at the same time. <strong>The</strong>se authors argue that, <strong>in</strong> the nor-<br />

mal man, the two responses are mutually <strong>in</strong>hibitory: When the<br />

aggressive response has been emitted it precludes sexual<br />

arousal, and when sexually aroused, a man is <strong>in</strong>capable <strong>of</strong> an<br />

aggressive response. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to these authors, "response com-<br />

patibility" <strong>of</strong> aggressive and sexual behaviors is the psychologi-<br />

cal hallmark <strong>of</strong> the rapist. This model is presented as a diagram<br />

<strong>in</strong> Figure 1.<br />

Blader and Marshall (1989) referred to an unpublished man-<br />

uscript (Blader, Marshall, & Barbaree, 1988) that described an<br />

experimental paradigm designed to study response compatibil-<br />

ity. <strong>The</strong> paradigm <strong>in</strong>volved a comb<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong> penile plethys-<br />

mography methodology to study the stimulus control <strong>of</strong> sexual<br />

arousal with analog laboratory procedures that have been used<br />

to study aggressive responses. Subjects who were be<strong>in</strong>g moni-<br />

tored for erectile responses to various sexual stimuli were made<br />

to believe that they were also deliver<strong>in</strong>g a noxious stimulus to<br />

another subject (actually a female confederate <strong>of</strong> the experi-<br />

menter). When subjects were required to present a noxious stim-<br />

ulus to a woman, sexual arousal was greatly reduced. However,<br />

when subjects were provoked to anger by the woman <strong>in</strong> a nega-<br />

tive evaluation manipulation, subject's sexual arousal was<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed while the subject presented noxious stimuli to the<br />

confederate. In this circumstance, the aggressive respond<strong>in</strong>g<br />

did not <strong>in</strong>hibit the sexual response, and the two responses were<br />

therefore seen to be compatible.<br />

It is too early to tell how productive this model will be <strong>in</strong><br />

generat<strong>in</strong>g research ideas or how helpful it will be <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong><br />

our understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> sexual aggression. <strong>The</strong> results <strong>of</strong> the re-<br />

SPECIAL SECTION: MALE SEXUAL AROUSAL IN RAPE 623<br />

S<br />

Aggression I <strong>Arousal</strong><br />

j (')<br />

S<br />

Normal - Responses Incompatible<br />

Aggression <strong>Arousal</strong><br />

j (0)<br />

Pathological - Responses Compatible<br />

Figure 1. <strong>The</strong> response compatibility model.<br />

search paradigm may be just as well accommodated by the<br />

stimulus dis<strong>in</strong>hibition model described below. Upon some re-<br />

flection, we have come to believe the argument at the heart <strong>of</strong><br />

the response compatibility model may be tautological. When a<br />

man rapes, by def<strong>in</strong>ition, he is committ<strong>in</strong>g an aggressive and a<br />

sexual act at the same time. <strong>The</strong> co-occurrence, and therefore<br />

the compatibility <strong>of</strong> these two responses, is the def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g feature<br />

<strong>of</strong> rape behavior. <strong>The</strong>refore, when response compatibility is<br />

used as an explanatory concept for rape, the argument these<br />

authors present reduces to the circular statement "a man rapes<br />

because he rapes:' Nevertheless, the response compatibility<br />

model and the experimental paradigm described above may<br />

ultimately lead to useful assessment methodologies and re-<br />

search f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

Stimulus Control <strong>Models</strong><br />

For the purposes <strong>of</strong> this exposition, the follow<strong>in</strong>g def<strong>in</strong>itions,<br />

presented <strong>in</strong> advance, will assist <strong>in</strong> the clear articulation <strong>of</strong> the<br />

models presented. A sexual stimulus is one that has the effect <strong>of</strong><br />

evok<strong>in</strong>g sexual arousal <strong>in</strong> the average or typical male. It usually<br />

<strong>in</strong>volves the description <strong>of</strong> sexual activity or the description <strong>of</strong><br />

the physical attributes <strong>of</strong> a woman <strong>in</strong> various forms <strong>of</strong> undress.<br />

A rape stimulus is that part <strong>of</strong> the verbal description <strong>of</strong> rape<br />

that describes elements <strong>of</strong> force, violence, victim harm, pa<strong>in</strong>,<br />

humiliation, or fear. Excitation is a process whereby a stimulus<br />

evokes or <strong>in</strong>creases sexual arousal, and <strong>in</strong>hibition is a process<br />

whereby a stimulus reduces or limits the strength <strong>of</strong> sexual<br />

arousal to a sexual stimulus (Barbaree, 1990).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!