19.01.2013 Views

Emotional modulation of the postauricular reflex

Emotional modulation of the postauricular reflex

Emotional modulation of the postauricular reflex

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Psychophysiology, 41 (2004), 426–432. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in <strong>the</strong> USA.<br />

Copyright r 2004 Society for Psychophysiological Research<br />

DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.00160.x<br />

BRIEF REPORT<br />

<strong>Emotional</strong> <strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong><br />

STEPHEN D. BENNING, a CHRISTOPHER J. PATRICK, a and ALAN R. LANG b<br />

a Department <strong>of</strong> Psychology, University <strong>of</strong> Minnesota–Twin Cities, Minneapolis, Minnesota, USA<br />

b Department <strong>of</strong> Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida 32306, USA<br />

Abstract<br />

A large literature now exists on emotional <strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> startle blink <strong>reflex</strong>. The current study examined affective<br />

<strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong>, which can be measured in relation to <strong>the</strong> same noise probe used to evoke <strong>the</strong><br />

startle <strong>reflex</strong>. We recorded <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> during viewing <strong>of</strong> pictures that varied systematically in emotional<br />

valence, content, and intensity. A significant linear valence <strong>modulation</strong> effect was found, with pleasant pictures<br />

potentiating and aversive pictures inhibiting <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> in comparison with neutral pictures. This<br />

modulatory effect did not vary as a function <strong>of</strong> picture content, but it was most robust for highly intense emotional<br />

pictures. Implications for <strong>the</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> basic emotional action tendencies are discussed.<br />

Descriptors: Post-auricular, Emotion, Appetitive, Startle, Reflex, Post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong><br />

It is well known that <strong>the</strong> startle blink <strong>reflex</strong> to an abrupt acoustic<br />

probe is modulated by <strong>the</strong> emotional valence <strong>of</strong> foreground<br />

stimulation. Startle magnitude is enhanced (potentiated) during<br />

viewing <strong>of</strong> unpleasant pictures compared with neutral pictures,<br />

and inhibited during viewing <strong>of</strong> pleasant pictures (cf. Lang,<br />

Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1990; Vrana, Spence, & Lang, 1988). A<br />

different <strong>reflex</strong> evoked by <strong>the</strong> same noise probe used to elicit<br />

startle is <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong>. Although several studies have<br />

investigated attentional <strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> this <strong>reflex</strong> (e.g., Hackley,<br />

Woldorff, & Hillyard, 1987; Patuzzi & O’Beirne, 1999; Sollers &<br />

Hackley, 1997), <strong>the</strong> emotional <strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> has not been similarly investigated. The current study<br />

examined post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> <strong>modulation</strong> as a function <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

valence, <strong>the</strong>matic content, and affective intensity <strong>of</strong> picture<br />

stimuli to determine whe<strong>the</strong>r this <strong>reflex</strong> might provide unique<br />

information about on-line affective processing.<br />

Emotion and Reflex Modulation<br />

Reflexes are valuable to measure in studies <strong>of</strong> emotion because<br />

<strong>the</strong>y have <strong>the</strong> potential to tap <strong>the</strong> basic action dispositionsFappetitive-approach<br />

and defensive-withdrawalFthat underlie posi-<br />

This study was supported by grants MH17069, MH52384, and<br />

MH65137 from <strong>the</strong> National Institute <strong>of</strong> Mental Health, Grant<br />

AA12164 from NIAAA, and by funds from <strong>the</strong> Hathaway endowment<br />

at <strong>the</strong> University <strong>of</strong> Minnesota. We are grateful to Daniel Blonigen and<br />

Brian Hicks for <strong>the</strong>ir assistance with stimulus selection, counterbalancing,<br />

and collection <strong>of</strong> data, and to Edward Bernat for his scholarly<br />

comments on drafts <strong>of</strong> this manuscript.<br />

Address reprint requests to: Christopher J. Patrick, Department <strong>of</strong><br />

Psychology, University <strong>of</strong> Minnesota, Elliott Hall, 75 East River Road,<br />

Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA. E-mail: cpatrick@tc.umn.edu.<br />

426<br />

tive and negative emotional states. In this regard, Lang et al.<br />

(1990) proposed a response-matching explanation for <strong>the</strong> linear<br />

valence <strong>modulation</strong> effect observed for <strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong>: A<br />

defensive <strong>reflex</strong>, such as startle, is enhanced during aversive picture<br />

viewing because <strong>the</strong> motivational state induced by <strong>the</strong> picture is<br />

defensive, leading to a synergistic facilitation. Conversely, startle is<br />

inhibited during pleasant pictures because here <strong>the</strong> ongoing<br />

approach disposition opposes <strong>the</strong> defensive startle reaction. From<br />

this perspective, a reverse pattern <strong>of</strong> <strong>modulation</strong> would be<br />

expected for an appetitive <strong>reflex</strong>Fthat is, potentiation during<br />

pleasant pictures and inhibition during aversive pictures.<br />

If this response-matching explanation <strong>of</strong> startle <strong>modulation</strong> in<br />

humans is accurate, <strong>the</strong>n intense emotional pictures that evoke<br />

strong appetitive or defensive motivational states should produce<br />

maximal modulatory effects on startle. Consistent with this,<br />

Cuthbert, Bradley, and Lang (1996) reported that blink potentiation<br />

was greater for high- versus low-arousal aversive pictures, and<br />

that blink inhibition was greater for high- versus low-arousal<br />

pleasant pictures. There is also evidence that modulatory effects<br />

are stronger for picture contents that are tied to primary motives:<br />

Blink inhibition is maximal for pleasant pictures that are sexual in<br />

content, and potentiation is maximal for directly threatening<br />

aversive pictures (Bradley, Codispoti, Cuthbert, & Lang, 2001;<br />

Levenston, Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 2000). The idea that startle<br />

potentiation during aversive picture viewing reflects defensive<br />

mobilization also fits with neuroscience research indicating that<br />

fear-potentiated startle in rats is mediated by input from <strong>the</strong><br />

subcortical amygdala to <strong>the</strong> midbrain component <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> startle<br />

circuit, <strong>the</strong> nucleus reticularis pontis caudalis (Davis, Gendelman,<br />

Tischler, & Gendelman, 1982; Hitchcock & Davis, 1986).<br />

From this response-matching perspective, o<strong>the</strong>r defensive<br />

<strong>reflex</strong>es should show modulatory patterns akin to startle.


Emotion and <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> 427<br />

Moulder, Bradley, Requin, and Lang (1995) reported a parallel<br />

linear valence <strong>modulation</strong> pattern (i.e., aversive4pleasant) for <strong>the</strong><br />

H<strong>of</strong>fman <strong>reflex</strong>, a spinal flexion <strong>reflex</strong> elicited by aversive electric<br />

shock. In this study, <strong>modulation</strong> effects were stronger for<br />

emotional pictures <strong>of</strong> high intensity, and more pronounced for<br />

participants who rated <strong>the</strong> shock as aversive. In contrast to this,<br />

<strong>the</strong> magnitude <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> spinal T <strong>reflex</strong>, elicited by nonaversive<br />

tapping or vibratory stimulation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Achilles tendon, is<br />

comparably enhanced during both pleasant and unpleasant<br />

pictures in relation to neutral (Bonnet, Bradley, Lang, & Requin,<br />

1995)Fimplying facilitation by arousal (i.e., nonspecific activation;<br />

cf. Lang et al., 1990). An opposing quadratic pattern,<br />

involving inhibited probe response during both pleasant and<br />

aversive pictures compared with neutral, has been reported for<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r measures (e.g., noise-evoked P300 brain potential; Schupp,<br />

Cuthbert, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, 1997). This pattern<br />

indicates a cross-modal attentional effect (cf. Anthony & Graham,<br />

1985) arising from <strong>the</strong> fact that visual affective stimuli draw more<br />

attention away from <strong>the</strong> acoustic modality (Lang et al., 1990).<br />

The Post-Auricular Reflex<br />

The post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> is a vestigial muscle response in humans<br />

that acts to pull <strong>the</strong> ear backward (Gray, 1901/1995). It is<br />

recorded by positioning electrodes over <strong>the</strong> post-auricular muscle<br />

behind <strong>the</strong> ear (O’Beirne & Patuzzi, 1999). Because this <strong>reflex</strong> is<br />

evoked by an acoustic probe stimulus, it can be assessed<br />

concurrently with <strong>the</strong> startle blink (cf. Hackley, 1993). The<br />

post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> has a much faster latency than <strong>the</strong> blink<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> (i.e., 9–11 ms vs. 45–50 ms; Hackley et al., 1987), implying<br />

a simpler brain stem circuitry. Following Cassella and Davis<br />

(1986), Hackley (1993) <strong>the</strong>orized that <strong>the</strong> circuit for <strong>the</strong> <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

<strong>reflex</strong> parallels <strong>the</strong> startle circuit in terms <strong>of</strong> input (i.e.,<br />

cochlear nucleus) and output (facial motor nucleus) components,<br />

but that it does not include <strong>the</strong> nucleus reticularis pontis<br />

caudalisF<strong>the</strong> juncture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> startle circuit at which input is<br />

received from <strong>the</strong> amygdala, accounting for <strong>the</strong> fear-potentiated<br />

startle effect (Davis et al., 1982). If this model <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

circuit is correct, one might expect a different pattern <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>modulation</strong> for <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> compared with <strong>the</strong><br />

startle <strong>reflex</strong>.<br />

The post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> has been assessed in a small number<br />

<strong>of</strong> attention studies in humans. The magnitude <strong>of</strong> this <strong>reflex</strong>, like<br />

that <strong>of</strong> startle, is attenuated if <strong>the</strong> <strong>reflex</strong>-eliciting noise is preceded<br />

by <strong>the</strong> occurrence <strong>of</strong> a transient acoustic stimulus (prepulse)Fan<br />

effect known as ‘‘prepulse inhibition’’ (Hackley, 1993; Hackley et<br />

al., 1987). Prepulse inhibition is thought to reflect a low-level<br />

sensory gating mechanism that affords ‘‘protection’’ at early<br />

stages <strong>of</strong> perceptual processing (Braff, Geyer, & Swerdlow, 2001;<br />

Graham, 1975). On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong><br />

appears less subject to controlled attentional influences than<br />

startle. Hackley et al. (1987) reported that prepulse inhibition<br />

was enhanced for <strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong>, but not <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong>, when participants were instructed to attend to <strong>the</strong> prepulse<br />

stimulus. This dissociation in effects was interpreted in terms <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> faster, simpler circuitry <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> (see also<br />

Hackley, 1993).<br />

However, when instructed to attend to one ear or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r in<br />

an auditory task, participants showed generally enhanced <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

<strong>reflex</strong> magnitudes, and enhanced prepulse inhibition <strong>of</strong><br />

this <strong>reflex</strong>, on <strong>the</strong> side to which attention was directed (Hackley,<br />

1993; Hackley et al., 1987). Hackley et al. (1987) attributed this<br />

effect to motor priming (i.e., a tensing <strong>of</strong> muscles on <strong>the</strong><br />

instructed side) ra<strong>the</strong>r than attention per se. Consistent with this,<br />

Patuzzi and O’Beirne (1999) reported a unilateral increase in<br />

baseline activity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular muscle, and an affiliated<br />

increase in <strong>the</strong> magnitude <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> to noise<br />

probes, when participants were instructed to rotate <strong>the</strong>ir eyes<br />

toward one side <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> head or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand,<br />

it has been shown that activity in <strong>the</strong> post-auricular muscle region<br />

decreases during processing <strong>of</strong> near-threshold auditory<br />

stimulation compared with more intense auditory stimulationFconsistent<br />

with <strong>the</strong> idea that somatic activation decreases<br />

to enhance perceptual sensitivity during orienting (Stekelenburg<br />

& van Boxtel, 2001, 2002). However, <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong><br />

was not assessed in <strong>the</strong>se latter studies, so it is unclear what<br />

impact <strong>the</strong>se changes in baseline EMG activity would have on its<br />

magnitude.<br />

Current Study<br />

The current study was conducted to examine modulatory effects<br />

<strong>of</strong> emotion on <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong>. To do this, we delivered<br />

unwarned noise probes to participants during viewing <strong>of</strong><br />

pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant pictures and recorded<br />

responses <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular muscle behind each ear. To gain<br />

additional information about modulatory effects, we manipulated<br />

both <strong>the</strong> intensity and <strong>the</strong>matic content <strong>of</strong> pleasant and<br />

unpleasant pictures.<br />

The status <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> as a defensive or<br />

appetitive <strong>reflex</strong> is unclear. However, <strong>the</strong> response-matching<br />

hypo<strong>the</strong>sis (Lang et al., 1990) makes clear predictions in ei<strong>the</strong>r<br />

case. If <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> is defensive in nature, it should<br />

be potentiated during aversive pictures and inhibited during<br />

pleasant pictures. If it is appetitive, it should be inhibited during<br />

unpleasant pictures and potentiated during pleasant pictures.<br />

Based on findings with <strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong>, it was fur<strong>the</strong>r predicted<br />

that modulatory effects in ei<strong>the</strong>r case would be maximal for<br />

pleasant and aversive pictures <strong>of</strong> high intensity.<br />

The literature on affective <strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> startle and o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

<strong>reflex</strong>es provided a basis for alternative hypo<strong>the</strong>ses as well. One is<br />

that <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> might be facilitated by nonspecific<br />

arousal ra<strong>the</strong>r than valence, with magnitude greater during both<br />

pleasant and aversive pictures compared to neutral (cf. spinal T<br />

<strong>reflex</strong>; Bonnet et al., 1995). Ano<strong>the</strong>r is that <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> would be inhibited as a function <strong>of</strong> cross-modal attention,<br />

with <strong>reflex</strong> magnitude diminished during both pleasant and<br />

unpleasant pictures relative to neutral (cf. probe-elicited P300;<br />

Schupp et al., 1997).<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Participants were 24 male undergraduates (M age 5 19.2 years,<br />

SD 5 1.25) recruited from psychology classes at <strong>the</strong> University <strong>of</strong><br />

Minnesota. The sample included only men because <strong>the</strong> current<br />

data were collected as part <strong>of</strong> a pilot for a project involving male<br />

prisoners. Individuals with visual or hearing impairments, as<br />

assessed via a screening questionnaire, were excluded from <strong>the</strong><br />

study. Students received course credit for <strong>the</strong>ir participation.<br />

Experimental Stimuli and Design<br />

Participants viewed digitized color photographs (22 pleasant, 22<br />

neutral, and 22 unpleasant) selected from <strong>the</strong> International<br />

Affective Picture System (IAPS; Center for <strong>the</strong> Study <strong>of</strong> Emotion


428 S.D. Benning, C.J. Patrick, and A.R. Lang<br />

and Attention, 1999). Fifty-four <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pictures were selected to<br />

represent specific <strong>the</strong>matic contents, as follows: 1<br />

Pleasant contents: erotic scenes (n 5 9; e.g., nude females,<br />

intimate couples); adventure scenes (n 5 9; e.g., cliff diving,<br />

motorcycle racing).<br />

Neutral contents: inactive people or neutral human faces<br />

(n 5 9); household objects or kitchen utensils (n 5 9).<br />

Unpleasant contents: scenes <strong>of</strong> victimization <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r people<br />

(n 5 9; e.g., aggression, physical brutality, and combat);<br />

threatening figures or weapons directed at <strong>the</strong> viewer (n 5 9;<br />

e.g., pointed guns, menacing attackers).<br />

Subsets <strong>of</strong> low and high intensity pictures, as defined by IAPS<br />

normative ratings, were included in each emotional content<br />

category (erotic, adventure, victim, threat). High-intensity pleasant<br />

pictures were higher in rated valence and arousal, Ms(SDs) 5 7.33<br />

(0.31) and 6.49 (0.53), respectively, than low-intensity pleasant<br />

pictures, Ms (SDs) 5 5.92 (0.47) and 4.66 (0.63). High-intensity<br />

unpleasant pictures were lower in normative valence and higher in<br />

arousal, Ms(SDs) 5 2.79 (0.25) and 6.49 (0.52), respectively, than<br />

low-intensity unpleasant pictures, Ms(SDs) 5 3.91 (0.36) and 4.64<br />

(0.63). Low-intensity pleasant and unpleasant pictures were<br />

equidistant from neutral in terms <strong>of</strong> rated valence and arousal,<br />

as were high-intensity pleasant and unpleasant pictures.<br />

Between 3 s and 5 s after <strong>the</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se 54 pictures,<br />

a brief binaural white noise probe (50 ms, 105 dB,o10 ms rise<br />

time) was presented that elicited <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong>. To<br />

diminish <strong>the</strong> predictability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> noises, nine no-probe picture<br />

trials were interspersed with <strong>the</strong> probe trials. Additionally, three<br />

probed picture trials (IAPS numbers 4650, 7080, and 9252) were<br />

included at <strong>the</strong> start <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> series to familiarize participants with<br />

<strong>the</strong> stimuli and to habituate large initial startle reactions.<br />

Habituation trials and no-probe picture trials were excluded<br />

from <strong>the</strong> analyses.<br />

Noise probes were generated using a Coulbourn S81-02 white<br />

noise module and S82-24 audio amplifier. The probe was<br />

presented binaurally through Telephonics headphones (first half<br />

<strong>of</strong> participant sample) or Etymotic insert earphones (second<br />

half). Statistical tests revealed no significant effects <strong>of</strong> method <strong>of</strong><br />

noise delivery, so <strong>the</strong> data were collapsed across this factor in all<br />

analyses reported. The noise probes occurred 3, 4, or 5 s after<br />

picture onset. A total <strong>of</strong> nine probes were also delivered at<br />

varying points during intertrial intervals (ITIs) to reduce <strong>the</strong><br />

predictability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> noise stimulus.<br />

Six stimulus orders were used to balance <strong>the</strong> presentation <strong>of</strong><br />

pictures and startle probes across participants. As noted, each<br />

order included 3 habituation trials followed by 54 probed picture<br />

trials, interspersed with 9 no-probe picture trials. The 54 probed<br />

picture trials were organized into three blocks, each consisting <strong>of</strong><br />

three pleasant, three neutral, and three unpleasant pictures.<br />

Within and between orders, <strong>the</strong> positioning <strong>of</strong> pictures and<br />

1 The 54 probed pictures, listed by <strong>the</strong>ir IAPS identification numbers,<br />

were as follows: eroticF2381, 4000, 4233 (4617), 4274, 4230, 4653<br />

(4750), 4690, 4687, 4290 (4651); adventureF4533 (8032), 8041, 8033,<br />

5622 (8250), 5626, 5623, 8370 (8180), 8080, 8042; neutralF2190, 2210,<br />

2214, 2372, 2480, 2495, 2850, 2890, 9700, 7002, 7030, 7034, 7040, 7050,<br />

7150, 7205, 7705, 7710; victimF6010, 2520, 9594 (4621), 6571, 9400,<br />

6530 (3550), 9250, 3400, 6350 (3500); threatF2100 (6241), 2682, 2130,<br />

6242 (6244), 6370, 6243, 6510 (6250), 6260, 6230. The pictures in<br />

paren<strong>the</strong>ses are alternate exemplars from <strong>the</strong> same content category that<br />

were substituted within some stimulus orders to achieve counterbalancing<br />

<strong>of</strong> conditions (valence, content, intensity) across run orders.<br />

startle probes was counterbalanced such that all valence, content,<br />

and intensity conditions were represented equally across orders<br />

at each serial position. Not more than two pictures <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same<br />

valence occurred consecutively within any stimulus order, and<br />

pictures <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same content never appeared consecutively.<br />

During <strong>the</strong> experiment, participants sat in a padded recliner at<br />

a distance <strong>of</strong> 120 cm from a 21-in. computer monitor positioned<br />

directly in front <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m. Physiological responses were recorded<br />

using a PC computer running VPM data acquisition s<strong>of</strong>tware<br />

(version 11.2; Cook, Atkinson, & Lang, 1987). A second<br />

computer controlled picture presentation. The post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> data for <strong>the</strong> current sample were collected as part <strong>of</strong> a<br />

larger sample investigation <strong>of</strong> emotion and picture viewing in<br />

which a variety <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r response measures (i.e., heart rate, skin<br />

conductance, facial muscle activity, startle blink, EEG, and<br />

affective ratings) were recorded. 2 The current report is limited to<br />

<strong>the</strong> post-auricular data because this response measure was<br />

available for only a subset <strong>of</strong> participants and we wished to<br />

highlight findings for this novel measure here.<br />

Physiological Measures<br />

The post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> was measured as described by Sollers<br />

and Hackley (1997). The pinna (outer ear) was pulled forward,<br />

and behind each ear, a pair <strong>of</strong> Med Associates 0.25 cm Ag-AgCl<br />

electrodes was positioned around <strong>the</strong> tendon <strong>of</strong> insertion for <strong>the</strong><br />

post-auricular muscleFreadily identifiable in most cases as a<br />

fibrous strip connecting <strong>the</strong> pinna and <strong>the</strong> scalp midway up <strong>the</strong><br />

pinna. One electrode was placed directly adjacent to <strong>the</strong> tendon<br />

on <strong>the</strong> surface <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pinna, and <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r electrode was placed on<br />

<strong>the</strong> scalp over <strong>the</strong> post-auricular muscle. Prior to placement, sites<br />

were scrubbed with conductive gel to reduce impedances below 5<br />

kO. Raw electromyographic (EMG) signals for each ear were<br />

recorded for 50 ms before noise probe onset until 250 ms after<br />

probe onset using a Coulbourn S75-01 high gain bioamplifier.<br />

The sampling rate was 1,000 Hz.<br />

The data were filtered on-line with a bandpass <strong>of</strong> 8–1,000 Hz,<br />

and rectified <strong>of</strong>f-line using Matlab s<strong>of</strong>tware (MathWorks, 2000).<br />

Post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong>es were scored after averaging <strong>the</strong> rectified<br />

waveformsacrosstrialswithinconditions(Hackleyetal.,1987;<br />

Sollers & Hackley, 1997). The magnitude <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> was scored from <strong>the</strong> aggregate waveform as a baseline-topeak<br />

measure. The peak was calculated as <strong>the</strong> maximum EMG<br />

activity within a window <strong>of</strong> 8–30 ms after noise probe onset, 3<br />

whereas <strong>the</strong> baseline was calculated as <strong>the</strong> average rectified <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

EMG activity during <strong>the</strong> 50 ms before <strong>the</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

noise probe (Sollers & Hackley, 1997). Although all participants<br />

showed substantial increases in post-auricular EMG activity<br />

during <strong>the</strong> 8–30 ms post-probe-onset window, several participants<br />

had <strong>reflex</strong> magnitudes that were difficult to differentiate from<br />

background EMG activity. Therefore, <strong>the</strong> peak <strong>of</strong> each aggregate<br />

waveform was manually scored to ensure that <strong>the</strong> algorithm<br />

detected responses appropriately (cf. Sollers & Hackley, 1997); in<br />

all but three aggregate waveforms, <strong>the</strong> manually assigned peak<br />

was identical to <strong>the</strong> algorithmically picked peak.<br />

2 Startle blink data for this subset <strong>of</strong> participants followed <strong>the</strong> usual<br />

valence <strong>modulation</strong> pattern, with blink magnitudes during pleasant<br />

pictures inhibited and those during unpleasant pictures potentiated<br />

compared to those during neutral pictures (cf. Benning, Patrick, Hicks,<br />

Blonigen, & Lang, 2001).<br />

3 Analyses conducted with <strong>the</strong> peak calculated as <strong>the</strong> mean <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

activity within <strong>the</strong> 8–30 ms post-probe window and <strong>the</strong> baseline<br />

calculated as above yielded identical patterns <strong>of</strong> results.


Emotion and <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> 429<br />

Procedure<br />

Participants provided informed written consent and <strong>the</strong>n<br />

completed a biographical form that screened for physical<br />

ailments, medication use, and visual and auditory impairments.<br />

Following electrode attachment, participants were advised <strong>the</strong>y<br />

would be viewing a series <strong>of</strong> pictures and rating <strong>the</strong>ir reactions to<br />

each. They were instructed to watch each picture <strong>the</strong> entire time it<br />

was on <strong>the</strong> screen and disregard occasional noises occurring<br />

through <strong>the</strong> headphones or earphones. Before <strong>the</strong> main picture<br />

series began, participants were given a demonstration <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Self-<br />

Assessment Manikin (SAM; Lang, 1980) rating procedure, in<br />

which <strong>the</strong>y characterized <strong>the</strong>ir reactions to pictures on dimensions<br />

<strong>of</strong> valence, arousal, and dominance (cf. Lang, 1980), as well<br />

as interest (cf. Levenston et al., 2000). Each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 66 pictures was<br />

preceded by a blank screen for 3 s and was <strong>the</strong>n presented for 6 s.<br />

Seven seconds after picture <strong>of</strong>fset, <strong>the</strong> ratings display appeared<br />

and <strong>the</strong> participant completed <strong>the</strong> four ratings. The interval<br />

between completion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ratings and <strong>the</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> next<br />

picture ranged between 8 s and 14 s, averaging 11 s.<br />

Data Processing and Analysis<br />

Because <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> is a ‘‘micro<strong>reflex</strong>’’ with a low<br />

signal-to-noise ratio (cf. Hackley et al., 1987), averaging <strong>of</strong> trials<br />

within condition was required to score <strong>the</strong> <strong>reflex</strong>. For one set <strong>of</strong><br />

analyses, we aggregated <strong>the</strong> rectified post-auricular waveforms<br />

by <strong>the</strong>matic content within picture valence category. Because<br />

<strong>the</strong>re were equivalent numbers <strong>of</strong> human and object pictures<br />

within <strong>the</strong> neutral category, we aggregated across trials within<br />

each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se neutral contents, and we also aggregated across<br />

trials for erotic and adventure contents within <strong>the</strong> pleasant<br />

category, and victim and threat contents within <strong>the</strong> unpleasant<br />

category. Each aggregate waveform thus incorporated data for<br />

nine picture trials. A nine-trial aggregate waveform incorporating<br />

data for all ITI probes was also formed. Post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong><br />

peak magnitude was scored from <strong>the</strong>se aggregate waveforms,<br />

along with mean post-auricular muscle activity during <strong>the</strong> preprobe<br />

baseline. Effects <strong>of</strong> picture valence were examined by<br />

collapsing magnitude scores across contents within valence<br />

category. A subsidiary set <strong>of</strong> analyses was performed to examine<br />

effects <strong>of</strong> affective stimulus intensity on post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong><br />

magnitude. For <strong>the</strong>se analyses, we aggregated post-auricular<br />

waveforms by picture intensity level (low, high) within pleasant<br />

and unpleasant valence categories, again yielding aggregate<br />

waveforms consisting <strong>of</strong> nine trials per condition, and extracted<br />

peak magnitude scores from <strong>the</strong>se aggregates.<br />

Using <strong>the</strong> resultant scores, <strong>the</strong> following analyses were<br />

performed:<br />

1. To examine <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> visual foreground engagement on<br />

post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> magnitude, we conducted a one-way<br />

repeated measures ANOVA in which <strong>the</strong> neutral picture<br />

condition was compared against <strong>the</strong> ITI (no-picture) condition.<br />

A parallel one-way ANOVA was conducted to examine<br />

<strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> foreground engagement on post-auricular<br />

baseline activity.<br />

2. To examine effects <strong>of</strong> picture valence (pleasant, neutral,<br />

unpleasant) and ear recording site (left, right) on <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

magnitude, a 3 2 multivariate ANOVA was<br />

performed with <strong>the</strong>se variables included as within-subjects<br />

factors. Significant main effects were subsequently examined<br />

in terms <strong>of</strong> orthogonal linear (e.g., pleasant vs. unpleasant)<br />

and quadratic (e.g., pleasant/unpleasant vs. neutral) contrasts<br />

(cf. Bradley, Cuthbert, & Lang, 1993; Bonnet et al., 1995;<br />

Moulder et al., 1995). A supplemental one-way MANOVA<br />

was performed to examine effects <strong>of</strong> picture valence on<br />

baseline post-auricular activity.<br />

3. With regard to picture content and intensity, we were<br />

interested in whe<strong>the</strong>r affect-<strong>modulation</strong> effects for <strong>the</strong> <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

<strong>reflex</strong> would be tied to specific contents or to<br />

pictures <strong>of</strong> high intensity. We addressed <strong>the</strong>se questions by<br />

performing two sets <strong>of</strong> planned contrasts using one-way<br />

ANOVAs. One involved comparisons <strong>of</strong> affective contents<br />

within each valence against each o<strong>the</strong>r, and <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r entailed<br />

contrasts <strong>of</strong> pleasant and unpleasant pictures <strong>of</strong> each intensity<br />

(low, high) against each o<strong>the</strong>r.<br />

An a level <strong>of</strong> .05 was used as <strong>the</strong> criterion for significance in all<br />

statistical tests.<br />

Results<br />

Picture Foreground versus ITI<br />

Post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> magnitudes were smaller during neutral<br />

pictures compared with ITIs, Ms(SDs) 5 6.64 (6.18) mVand 7.98<br />

(5.71) mV, respectively, F(1,23) 5 4.65, p 5 .04, Z 2 5 .17. There was<br />

also a trend for baseline post-auricular EMG activity during neutral<br />

pictures to be smaller than during ITIs, Ms(SDs) 5 0.74 (0.32) mV<br />

and 0.84 (0.36) mV, respectively, F(1,23) 5 2.92, p 5 .10, Z 2 5 .11.<br />

Picture Valence<br />

Figure 1 displays grand average waveforms for <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> by picture valence, with average <strong>reflex</strong> magnitudes depicted<br />

in <strong>the</strong> inset bar graph. A significant main effect <strong>of</strong> picture valence<br />

was found for post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> magnitude, F(2,22) 5 4.16,<br />

p 5 .03, Z 2 5 .27. 4 As shown in Figure 1, <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong><br />

was larger during pleasant pictures than unpleasant pictures,<br />

linear Valence F(1,23) 5 8.50, p 5 .01, Z 2 5 .27. Follow-up<br />

paired t tests showed that post-auricular magnitudes tended to<br />

be greater during pleasant than neutral pictures, t(23) 5 1.94,<br />

p 5 .07, whereas post-auricular magnitudes during neutral and<br />

unpleasant pictures did not differ, t(23) 5 1.11, p 5 .28. No main<br />

effect <strong>of</strong> ear recording site (right vs. left) was found,<br />

F(1,23) 5 1.00, p 5 .33, Z 2 5 .04, and <strong>the</strong> Ear Valence interaction<br />

was not significant, F(2,22) 5 0.31, p 5 .74, Z 2 5 .01.<br />

As fur<strong>the</strong>r evidence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> consistency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

valence <strong>modulation</strong> effect, 75% (18/24; 95% confidence interval<br />

5 55%–88%) <strong>of</strong> participants showed numerically greater<br />

post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> magnitudes during pleasant compared with<br />

unpleasant pictures, and 71% (17/24; 95% confidence interval<br />

5 51%–85%) showed numerically greater post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> magnitudes during pleasant versus neutral pictures. Both<br />

proportions significantly exceeded chance, according to formulae<br />

provided by Newcombe (1998).<br />

The MANOVA with picture valence as <strong>the</strong> within-subjects<br />

factor and mean pre-probe baseline post-auricular EMG activity<br />

4 A supplementary analysis was performed in which picture trials were<br />

subdivided into three sequential blocks, and post-auricular waveforms<br />

were aggregated by valence (pleasant, neutral, unpleasant) within each<br />

block. As in <strong>the</strong> primary MANOVA, this analysis yielded a significant<br />

valence main effect, and also a significant main effect for block,<br />

F(2,22) 5 6.77, p 5 .011, Z 2 5 .338, but no Block Valence interaction,<br />

F(4,20) 5 0.95, p 5 .441, Z 2 5 .040. Thus, while <strong>the</strong> magnitude <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> declined over <strong>the</strong> session, <strong>the</strong> emotional <strong>modulation</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> remained constantFconsistent with <strong>the</strong><br />

findings <strong>of</strong> Bradley et al. (1993) for <strong>the</strong> startle blink <strong>reflex</strong>.


430 S.D. Benning, C.J. Patrick, and A.R. Lang<br />

PA EMG Activity (µV)<br />

6<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

-25 0 25 50 75 100 125<br />

as <strong>the</strong> dependent variable revealed no effect <strong>of</strong> picture valence,<br />

F(2,22) 5 0.30, p 5 .74, Z 2 5 .03. Post-auricular baseline Ms<br />

(SDs) for pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant pictures were as<br />

follows: 0.74 (0.29) mV, 0.75 (0.32) mV, and 0.72 (0.34) mV,<br />

respectively.<br />

Affective Content and Intensity<br />

Within pleasant contents, <strong>the</strong> magnitude <strong>of</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong>es<br />

during erotic pictures did not differ from those during adventure<br />

pictures, Ms 5 7.23 and 7.21 mV (SDs 5 5.86 and 6.12), respectively,<br />

F(1,23) 5 0.00, p 5 .96, Z 2 5 .00. Likewise, within unpleasant<br />

contents, post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> magnitudes did not differ<br />

between victim and threat pictures, Ms 5 6.24 and 6.38 mV(SDs 5<br />

5.20 and 5.58), respectively, F(1,23) 5 0.11, p 5 .75, Z 2 5 .01.<br />

Post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> magnitudes were significantly greater during<br />

high-intensity pleasant pictures than during high-intensity<br />

unpleasant pictures, Ms 5 8.11 and 6.81 mV (SDs 5 6.87 and<br />

5.59), respectively, F(1,23) 5 7.78, p 5 .01, Z 2 5 .25, but <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

<strong>reflex</strong> magnitudes during low-intensity pleasant and<br />

unpleasant pictures did not differ significantly from each o<strong>the</strong>r,<br />

Ms 5 7.77 and 7.18 mV (SDs 5 6.48 and 6.18), respectively,<br />

F(1,23) 5 2.31, p 5 .14, Z 2 5 .09.<br />

Discussion<br />

Pleasant<br />

Neutral<br />

Unpleasant<br />

PA Peak Magnitude (µV)<br />

8<br />

7<br />

6<br />

5<br />

Time Relative to Probe Onset (ms)<br />

Pleasant Neutral Unpleasant<br />

Picture Valence<br />

Figure 1. Grand average post-auricular (PA) <strong>reflex</strong> waveforms (main<br />

panel) and magnitudes SE (inset) by picture valence.<br />

Our findings indicate that <strong>the</strong> magnitude <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> is reliably modulated by emotional valence. Specifically, we<br />

found that <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> tended to be potentiated<br />

during viewing <strong>of</strong> pleasant pictures compared with neutral, and<br />

inhibited during viewing <strong>of</strong> aversive pictures, particularly during<br />

high-intensity pictures. 5 This linear <strong>modulation</strong> pattern is<br />

5 In a previous investigation <strong>of</strong> affect and attention during picture<br />

viewing that employed a wider range <strong>of</strong> probe times (Bradley, Drobes, &<br />

Lang, 1996), measurement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> was included along<br />

with assessment <strong>of</strong> blink startle and tone probe responses (M. M.<br />

Bradley, pers. comm., May 7, 2003). Although post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong><br />

magnitude in this study was somewhat higher during pleasant versus<br />

unpleasant pictures, <strong>the</strong> difference was not significant. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand,<br />

more recently, a significant linear valence effect on post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong><br />

magnitude, mirroring <strong>the</strong> finding reported here, was obtained by investigators<br />

at Indiana University (Ames, Merritt, Stout, & Hetrick, 2003).<br />

opposite to that observed for <strong>the</strong> defensive startle blink <strong>reflex</strong><br />

(Lang et al., 1990). Analyses <strong>of</strong> post-auricular muscle activity<br />

during <strong>the</strong> pre-probe baseline period indicated that this modulatory<br />

effect was not attributable to differences in baseline EMG<br />

(as would be expected if <strong>the</strong>se results were due to head motion or<br />

eye gaze; Patuzzi & O’Beirne, 1999; Stekelenburg & van Boxtel,<br />

2001, 2002), because baseline post-auricular activity did not differ<br />

across picture valence categories. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, baseline<br />

post-auricular activity was reduced in connection with attenuated<br />

post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> response during viewing <strong>of</strong> neutral foregrounds<br />

in relation to ITIs. This finding is consistent with <strong>the</strong> idea<br />

that tonic activity in various somatic systems, including <strong>the</strong> <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

musculature, decreases to enhance perceptual sensitivity<br />

during orienting (Stekelenburg & van Boxtel, 2001, 2002).<br />

Lang et al. (1990) proposed a motivational priming hypo<strong>the</strong>sis<br />

to account for modulatory effects <strong>of</strong> emotional valence on<br />

<strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong>. Citing Konorski (1967), <strong>the</strong>y postulated that<br />

defensive <strong>reflex</strong>es like <strong>the</strong> startle blink are primed during negative<br />

emotional states and inhibited during positive emotional states.<br />

The potentiation effects observed for <strong>the</strong> neck (Cassella, Harty,<br />

& Davis, 1986), blink (Lang et al., 1990), and whole-body (Davis<br />

et al., 1982) reactions to noise probes under conditions <strong>of</strong> threat<br />

are consistent with <strong>the</strong> idea that <strong>the</strong>se are components <strong>of</strong> a<br />

common defensive startle response. Although <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> is likewise elicited by a sudden, loud noise, our data argue<br />

against <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> being a short latency component<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> defensive startle <strong>reflex</strong> (Hackley, 1993). Indeed, <strong>the</strong>re is<br />

evidence that <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> may have a different<br />

circuitry than <strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong> (Cassella & Davis, 1986; Hackley,<br />

1993), though more recent research with specific excitotoxic<br />

lesions in <strong>the</strong> circuitry <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pinna <strong>reflex</strong> in rats (ra<strong>the</strong>r than with<br />

general fiber-destroying electrolytic lesions; Cassella & Davis,<br />

1986) suggests that <strong>the</strong> circuitry for <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> may<br />

also include <strong>the</strong> nucleus reticularis pontis caudalis (Davis,<br />

Walker, & Yee, 1999; Li & Frost, 1996).<br />

Lang et al.’s (1990) motivational priming hypo<strong>the</strong>sis postulates<br />

that appetitive <strong>reflex</strong>es (salivation) would be facilitated<br />

during positive emotional states and inhibited during negative<br />

emotional states. In this regard, <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> behaved<br />

as an appetitive <strong>reflex</strong> in <strong>the</strong> current study, as it was potentiated<br />

during viewing <strong>of</strong> pleasant pictures and attenuated during<br />

viewing <strong>of</strong> unpleasant pictures. The fact that <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> increased during pleasant picture viewing indicates that<br />

some o<strong>the</strong>r mechanism (appetitive motor priming) operated<br />

against <strong>the</strong> influence <strong>of</strong> cross-modal sensory engagementFanalogous<br />

to <strong>the</strong> impact <strong>of</strong> defense system activation on <strong>the</strong> startle<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> during unpleasant pictures, which opposes <strong>the</strong> inhibitory<br />

influence <strong>of</strong> cross-modal attention in picture viewing (Cuthbert et<br />

al., 1996; Lang et al., 1997).<br />

In <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong>, a specific neural mechanism<br />

has been identified to account for <strong>the</strong> facilitatory effect <strong>of</strong><br />

aversive foreground stimulationFnamely, an input from <strong>the</strong><br />

amygdala to <strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong> circuit (Davis et al., 1982). A<br />

corresponding input from <strong>the</strong> brain’s appetitive system to <strong>the</strong><br />

post-auricular circuit could conceivably account for potentiation<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> during pleasant foreground stimulation.<br />

Research on <strong>the</strong> neural circuitry <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pinna <strong>reflex</strong>, <strong>the</strong><br />

animal analogue <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong>, indicates that <strong>the</strong><br />

motoneurons <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> facial nerve that innervates <strong>the</strong> pinna receive<br />

additional input from <strong>the</strong> retrorubral nucleus (Li & Frost, 1996),<br />

a midbrain dopaminergic structure that has been associated with<br />

sensitivity to reward stimuli in rats (Waraczynski & Perkins,


Emotion and <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> 431<br />

2000) and with motor deficits arising from Parkinson’s disease in<br />

humans (Heim et al., 2002).<br />

An alternative possibility is that increased post-auricular<br />

reactivity reflects enhanced orienting to <strong>the</strong> noise probe stimulus.<br />

Indeed, <strong>the</strong>re is evidence that unexpected noises can evoke<br />

observable movement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular (pinna) muscle in<br />

primates in whom this muscle serves more than a vestigial<br />

function. Naturalistic observations <strong>of</strong> Hanuman langurs, a lower<br />

primate species, have revealed that in adult males, loud and<br />

surprising sounds yield brief (i.e., 0.2–0.3 s) movements <strong>of</strong> both<br />

<strong>the</strong> pinna and <strong>the</strong> head in <strong>the</strong> direction <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> noise (Trivedi &<br />

Mohnot, 2002), suggesting that <strong>the</strong> pinna <strong>reflex</strong> serves an<br />

orienting function in this primate species. It may be that states <strong>of</strong><br />

attentional engagement, such as those associated with viewing <strong>of</strong><br />

pleasurable pictures, facilitate this phasic <strong>reflex</strong>ive orienting<br />

reaction. However, because pleasant and unpleasant pictures<br />

engage more attention than neutral (Lang et al., 1990, 1997), <strong>the</strong><br />

predicted attentional pattern would have been one <strong>of</strong> <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

<strong>reflex</strong> inhibition for both affective categories compared<br />

to neutral. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, juveniles and females <strong>of</strong> this species<br />

exhibit greater short-duration pinna movements primarily<br />

during play, feeding, and foraging for food (Trivedi & Mohnot,<br />

2002), suggesting heightened responsiveness <strong>of</strong> this <strong>reflex</strong>ive<br />

system specifically during pleasurable activities.<br />

Never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong>re is evidence that unpleasant pictures also<br />

engage attention (Lang et al., 1997), so it is possible that some<br />

types <strong>of</strong> aversive pictures might likewise facilitate <strong>the</strong> <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

<strong>reflex</strong>. The current study focused on threat and physical<br />

attack pictures that tend to be perceived as fearful. Modulatory<br />

Ames, K. A., Merritt, N. P., Stout, K., & Hetrick, W. P. (2003).<br />

Differential effects <strong>of</strong> affective <strong>modulation</strong> on orbicularis and <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

indices <strong>of</strong> startle. Psychophysiology, 40, S22.<br />

Anthony, B. J., & Graham, F. K. (1985). Blink <strong>reflex</strong> modification by<br />

selective attention: Evidence for <strong>the</strong> <strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> automatic<br />

processing. Biological Psychology, 21, 43–59.<br />

Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Hicks, B., Blonigen, D., & Lang, A. R.<br />

(2001). Affective <strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> blink startle and post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong>es to noise probes. Psychophysiology, 38, S24.<br />

Bonnet, M., Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., & Requin, J. (1995).<br />

Modulation <strong>of</strong> spinal <strong>reflex</strong>es: Arousal, pleasure, action. Psychophysiology,<br />

32, 367–372.<br />

Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001).<br />

Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in<br />

picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298.<br />

Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion, novelty,<br />

and <strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong>: Habituation in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience,<br />

107, 970–980.<br />

Bradley, M. M., Drobes, D., & Lang, P. J. (1996). A probe for all<br />

reasons: Reflex and RTmeasures in perception. Psychophysiology, 33,<br />

S25.<br />

Braff, D. L., Geyer, M. A., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2001). Human studies <strong>of</strong><br />

prepulse inhibition <strong>of</strong> startle: Normal subjects, patient groups, and<br />

pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology, 156, 234–258.<br />

Cassella, J. V., & Davis, M. (1986). Habituation, prepulse inhibition, fear<br />

conditioning, and drug <strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> acoustically elicited pinna<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 39–44.<br />

Cassella, J. V., Harty, P. T., & Davis, M. (1986). Fear conditioning, prepulse<br />

inhibition and drug <strong>modulation</strong> <strong>of</strong> a short latency startle<br />

response measured electromyographically from neck muscles in <strong>the</strong><br />

rat. Physiology and Behavior, 36, 1187–1191.<br />

Center for <strong>the</strong> Study <strong>of</strong> Emotion and Attention [CSEA-NIMH]. (1999).<br />

The international affective picture system: Digitized photographs.<br />

Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology,<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Florida.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

effects may differ for o<strong>the</strong>r aversive contents, such as sadness,<br />

disgust, or mutilation pictures. It will also be important in future<br />

research to examine effects for o<strong>the</strong>r pleasurable picture contents<br />

(e.g., babies, animals, food). In addition, <strong>the</strong>re are o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

limitations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> current study that need to be addressed before<br />

firm conclusions can be advanced regarding <strong>the</strong> post-auricular<br />

<strong>reflex</strong> as an index <strong>of</strong> emotion. The current sample included only<br />

men; thus, it will be important to replicate <strong>the</strong>se findings in<br />

women. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, it will be important to assess <strong>modulation</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> in emotional processing tasks o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

than picture viewing. A key question is whe<strong>the</strong>r potentiation <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> would be observed in appetitive<br />

conditioning or reward anticipation contexts, as is true for<br />

startle in corresponding aversive paradigms (e.g., Hamm,<br />

Greenwald, Bradley, & Lang, 1993; Patrick & Berthot, 1995).<br />

Affirmative findings would lend support to <strong>the</strong> idea that <strong>the</strong> <strong>postauricular</strong><br />

<strong>reflex</strong> is primed during appetitive states. Finally, in light<br />

<strong>of</strong> recent data indicating that hedonic valence and approach–<br />

withdrawal aspects <strong>of</strong> emotion are dissociable, it would be<br />

informative to assess whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong> is<br />

enhanced or inhibited during a negatively valent approach state<br />

(e.g., anger; Harmon-Jones & Sigelman, 2001).<br />

Notwithstanding <strong>the</strong>se limitations, <strong>the</strong> findings <strong>of</strong> our study<br />

are intriguing. They suggest that <strong>the</strong> post-auricular <strong>reflex</strong>, which<br />

is evoked by <strong>the</strong> same acoustic probe as <strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong>, may<br />

provide an index <strong>of</strong> appetitive system activation. If so, this<br />

<strong>reflex</strong>ive measure could provide a valuable tool for studying basic<br />

appetitive processes and investigating deficiencies in positive<br />

affect associated with psychopathology.<br />

Cook, E. W. III, Atkinson, L. S., & Lang, K. G. (1987). Stimulus control<br />

and data acquisition for IBM PCs and compatibles. Psychophysiology,<br />

2, 726–727.<br />

Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1996). Probing picture<br />

perception: Activation and emotion. Psychophysiology, 33, 103–111.<br />

Davis, M., Gendelman, D. S., Tischler, M. D., & Gendelman, P. M.<br />

(1982). A primary acoustic startle circuit: Lesion and stimulation<br />

studies. Journal <strong>of</strong> Neuroscience, 2, 791–805.<br />

Davis, M., Walker, D. L., & Lee, Y. (1999). Neurophysiology and<br />

neuropharmacology <strong>of</strong> startle and its affective <strong>modulation</strong>. In M. E.<br />

Dawson, M. E. Schell, & A. H. Bohmelt (Eds.), Startle modification:<br />

Implications for neuroscience, cognitive science, and clinical science (pp.<br />

95–117). New York: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Graham, F. K. (1975). The more or less startling effects <strong>of</strong> weak<br />

prestimulation. Psychophysiology, 2, 238–247.<br />

Gray, H. (1901/1995). Anatomy: Descriptive and surgical (15th edn). New<br />

York: Barnes and Noble Books, Inc.<br />

Hackley, S. A. (1993). An evaluation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> automaticity <strong>of</strong> sensory<br />

processing using event-related potentials and brain-stem <strong>reflex</strong>es.<br />

Psychophysiology, 30, 415–428.<br />

Hackley, S. A., Woldorff, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1987). Combined use <strong>of</strong><br />

micro<strong>reflex</strong>es and event-related brain potentials as measures <strong>of</strong><br />

auditory selective attention. Psychophysiology, 24, 632–647.<br />

Hamm, A. O., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993).<br />

<strong>Emotional</strong> learning, hedonic change, and <strong>the</strong> startle probe. Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Abnormal Psychology, 102, 453–465.<br />

Harmon-Jones, E., & Sigelman, J. (2001). State anger and prefrontal<br />

brain activity: Evidence that insult-related relative left-prefrontal<br />

activation is associated with experienced anger and aggression.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 797–803.<br />

Heim, C., Sova, L., Kurz, T., Kolasiewicz, W., Schwegler, H., & Sontag,<br />

K.-H. (2002). Partial loss <strong>of</strong> dopaminergic neurons in <strong>the</strong> substantia<br />

nigra, ventrotegmental area and <strong>the</strong> retrorubral area – Model <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

early beginning <strong>of</strong> Parkinson’s symptomatology? Journal <strong>of</strong> Neural<br />

Transmission, 109, 691–709.


432 S.D. Benning, C.J. Patrick, and A.R. Lang<br />

Hitchcock, J., & Davis, M. (1986). Lesions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> amygdala, but not <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> cerebellum or red nucleus, block conditioned fear as measured<br />

with <strong>the</strong> potentiated startle paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100,<br />

11–22.<br />

Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain: An interdisciplinary<br />

approach. Chicago: University <strong>of</strong> Chicago Press.<br />

Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment:<br />

Computer applications. In J. B. Sidowski, J. H. Johnson, & T. A.<br />

Williams (Eds.), Technology in mental health care delivery systems (pp.<br />

119–137). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.<br />

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). Emotion,<br />

attention, and <strong>the</strong> startle <strong>reflex</strong>. Psychological Review, 97, 377–395.<br />

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated<br />

attention: Affect, activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons,<br />

& M. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational<br />

processes (pp. 97–135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Levenston, G. K., Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000).<br />

The psychopath as observer: Emotion and attention in picture<br />

processing. Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal Psychology, 109, 373–385.<br />

Li, L., & Frost, B. J. (1996). Azimuthal sensitivity <strong>of</strong> rat pinna <strong>reflex</strong>:<br />

EMG recordings from cervicoauricular muscles. Hearing Research,<br />

100, 192–200.<br />

MathWorks, Inc. (2000). Matlab (Version 6.0.0.88). Natick, MA:<br />

Author.<br />

Moulder, B., Bradley, M., Requin, J., & Lang, P. (1995). Tales <strong>of</strong><br />

H<strong>of</strong>fman: Spinal <strong>reflex</strong>es and emotion. Psychophysiology, 32, S54.<br />

Newcombe, R. G. (1998). Two-sided confidence intervals for <strong>the</strong> single<br />

proportion: Comparison <strong>of</strong> seven methods. Statistics in Medicine, 17,<br />

857–872.<br />

O’Beirne, G. A., & Patuzzi, R. B. (1999). Basic properties <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> soundevoked<br />

post-auricular muscle response (PAMR). Hearing Research,<br />

138, 115–132.<br />

Patrick, C. J., & Berthot, B. D. (1995). Startle potentiation during<br />

anticipation <strong>of</strong> a noxious stimulus: Active versus passive response<br />

sets. Psychophysiology, 32, 72–80.<br />

Patuzzi, R. B., & O’Beirne, G. A. (1999). Effects <strong>of</strong> eye rotation on <strong>the</strong><br />

sound-evoked post-auricular muscle response (PAMR). Hearing<br />

Research, 138, 133–146.<br />

Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M., Birbaumber, N., & Lang,<br />

P. J. (1997). Probe P3 and blinks: Two measures <strong>of</strong> affective startle<br />

<strong>modulation</strong>. Psychophysiology, 34, 1–6.<br />

Sollers, J. J., & Hackley, S. A. (1997). Effects <strong>of</strong> foreperiod duration on<br />

<strong>reflex</strong>ive and voluntary responses to intense noise bursts. Psychophysiology,<br />

34, 518–526.<br />

Stekelenburg, J. J., & van Boxtel, A. (2001). Inhibition <strong>of</strong> pericranial<br />

muscle activity, respiration, and heart rate enhances auditory<br />

sensitivity. Psychophysiology, 38, 629–641.<br />

Stekelenburg, J. J., & van Boxtel, A. (2002). Pericranial muscular,<br />

respiratory, and heart rate components <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> orienting response.<br />

Psychophysiology, 39, 707–722.<br />

Trivedi, S., & Mohnot, S. M. (2002). Functional status <strong>of</strong> pinna<br />

muscles in Hanuman langurs, Semnopi<strong>the</strong>cus entellus: Speculation<br />

on functional loss <strong>of</strong> pinna muscles in humans. Primate Report, 63,<br />

41–47.<br />

Vrana, S. R., Spence, E. L., & Lang, P. J. (1988). The startle probe<br />

response: A new measure <strong>of</strong> emotion? Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal<br />

Psychology, 97, 487–491.<br />

Waraczynski, M., & Perkins, M. (2000). Temporary inactivation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

retrorubral fields decreases <strong>the</strong> rewarding effect <strong>of</strong> medial forebrain<br />

bundle stimulation. Brain Research, 885, 154–165.<br />

(Received March 4, 2003; Accepted October 13, 2003)

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!