08.03.2014 Views

Introduction To Qualities Of Romantic Relationships In - International ...

Introduction To Qualities Of Romantic Relationships In - International ...

Introduction To Qualities Of Romantic Relationships In - International ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

<strong><strong>In</strong>troduction</strong> to <strong>Qualities</strong> of<br />

<strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong> in<br />

Adolescence and Adulthood<br />

Karina Weichold<br />

Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Jena<br />

Jena, Germany<br />

E-mail: karina.weichold@uni-jena.de<br />

and<br />

Bonnie L. Barber<br />

School of Psychology, Murdoch University<br />

Perth, Western Australia, Australia<br />

E-mail: b.barber@murdoch.edu.au<br />

<strong>Romantic</strong> relationships offer opportunities for some of the most joyful and fulfilling<br />

moments in life, as well as some of the most miserable. Research is growing on both of these<br />

aspects of partnerships, and their roles in development. What is perhaps more neglected is<br />

the consideration of the role of culture in the study of how romantic relationships facilitate<br />

or hinder healthy development. This issue of the newsletter brings together scholars from<br />

all corners of the globe in our search to understand diverse qualities of romantic relationships<br />

in adolescence and adulthood, and how those are linked to development.<br />

We lead off with a North American feature focused on the<br />

developmental contributions of romantic relationships to<br />

key psychosocial tasks in adolescence. The second feature<br />

highlights some of the darker sides of romantic relationships<br />

for young adults in <strong>In</strong>dia. The third article underscores<br />

the challenges and opportunities in romantic<br />

relationships for sexual minority youth. The final feature<br />

considers the spousal experiences and meaning of marriage<br />

in Africa. Our two commentaries, from distinguished<br />

scholars in Israel and Germany, expand on these themes,<br />

and suggest important new considerations for future<br />

research. All of the authors highlight the centrality of<br />

romantic bonds to our social experiences, offering insights<br />

into the growing breadth of research on developmentally<br />

crucial features of relationships.<br />

<strong>Of</strong>fering fresh approaches to research in this area are<br />

two “Reports from the Lab.” Articles in this section report<br />

on scholars’ everyday working conditions or collaborations<br />

within a research setting that may be unusual or challenging.<br />

<strong>In</strong> this case, we have one paper from a research group<br />

in Finland investigating spousal relationships. The data<br />

were collected using a diary method via mobile phones to<br />

capture daily family dynamics. A second report focuses on<br />

the initiation of romantic relationships and the establishment<br />

of committed relationships in American young<br />

adults. Data here were collected through online assessments.<br />

Both papers introduce modern ways of collecting<br />

prospective longitudinal data in the field of exploring<br />

romantic relationships and they summarize advantages<br />

and difficulties experienced by applying such modes of<br />

data collection.<br />

The contributors to the Special Section features,<br />

commentary, and lab stories include scholars who are<br />

international leaders in research focused on romantic<br />

relationships. We are grateful for the contributions of<br />

these scholars who have reminded us of the diversity of<br />

experiences in romantic partnerships.<br />

Adolescent <strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong>:<br />

Beginnings, Endings, and Psychosocial<br />

Challenges<br />

Jennifer Connolly and Caroline McIsaac<br />

Department of Psychology, York University,<br />

<strong>To</strong>ronto, Canada<br />

E-mail: connolly@yorku.ca<br />

<strong>Romantic</strong> relationships first develop in the adolescent years<br />

and their emergence forever changes the nature of young<br />

people’s interpersonal and intrapersonal functioning.<br />

<strong>Relationships</strong> with a romantic partner are fundamentally<br />

different from any other relationship that an adolescent<br />

may have had up to that point because of their potential to<br />

meet sexual needs as well as social and personal ones.<br />

These relationships have also been posited to make important<br />

contributions to the development of intimacy, sexual<br />

identity and autonomy throughout adolescence. Yet it has<br />

only been in recent years that these relationships have<br />

been recognized by developmental psychologists (Collins,<br />

2003) as “dating” was previously viewed as an activity not<br />

1


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

worthy of serious theoretical or empirical concern. This<br />

state of affairs has changed dramatically in the last decade<br />

or so and there is now a substantial body of theory and<br />

research on adolescent romantic relationships and their<br />

developmental significance.<br />

Our interest in the study of romantic relationships grew<br />

out of a research agenda focused on adolescents’ peer<br />

relationships. Studying adolescents’ peer networks and<br />

their interactions with friends showed that there were<br />

important relationships which were not being fully<br />

accounted for. <strong>In</strong> particular, there were significant relationships<br />

with other-sex peers, some of whom were boyfriends<br />

or girlfriends, embedded in their social networks which<br />

were not being modeled (Connolly & Johnson, 1996). These<br />

initial findings inspired a compelling curiosity to better<br />

understand these relationships and created a research<br />

agenda broadly focused on their development. <strong>To</strong>gether<br />

with my graduate students, we have been pursuing three<br />

central research themes over the past decade. These topics<br />

can be summarized in the following way: 1) understanding<br />

how and why adolescents’ romantic relationships begin<br />

and end; 2) examining intimacy and autonomy in romantic<br />

relationships, and finally; 3) exploring the developmental<br />

and contextual predictors of precocious entry into romantic<br />

relationships. We have approached this research from a<br />

developmental-contextual perspective in which we understand<br />

romantic relationships as unfolding within a broader<br />

social context made of up peers, family and ecological<br />

systems. Within this approach we are cognizant that<br />

romantic relationships are themselves a context for the<br />

navigation of salient developmental tasks (Connolly &<br />

Goldberg, 1999). <strong>In</strong> this article, we summarize some of our<br />

findings, focusing on how they advance our understanding<br />

of adolescents’ romantic relationships and also noting areas<br />

for future study.<br />

First, it is important to situate our research geographically<br />

and methodologically. Our research is largely<br />

conducted in the “field” that is, in middle schools and<br />

high schools in the south-central region of Canada. This<br />

is a typically North American context, closer in values<br />

and structures to America than to Europe or other parts<br />

of the world. Canada, and especially <strong>To</strong>ronto, is very<br />

diverse culturally and a wide range of ethno-cultural<br />

groups are represented in our samples. This diversity has<br />

allowed us to explore facets of romantic relationships<br />

across cultural lines, which we believe is critical to understanding<br />

romantic relationships and the contributing role<br />

of context in their development. Turning to our methods,<br />

we typically use self-report surveys completed by<br />

students in school. Conscious of the limitations associated<br />

with these samples of convenience, we have devised<br />

numerous strategies to maximize students’ participation<br />

in our research, and our samples typically include<br />

upwards of 70 percent of the eligible students (McIsaac,<br />

McKenney, & Connolly, 2007). Our research is most often<br />

collected longitudinally over periods ranging from one<br />

year to over eight years. <strong>In</strong> addition, we have used observational<br />

methods to study the internal dynamics of<br />

adolescents’ relationships with their romantic partners,<br />

especially in the study of conflict and negotiation. As with<br />

our school-based research, our observational studies are<br />

also collected “in situ”, through visits to adolescents’<br />

homes. These multiple methods allow us to gain a more<br />

nuanced understanding of youth relationships in diverse<br />

settings.<br />

Beginnings of <strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong><br />

One of our primary concerns has been to understand how<br />

and when romantic relationships begin. This undertaking<br />

is more complex than it might appear at first glance as there<br />

is considerable fluidity in the boundaries that define<br />

adolescents’ romantic relationships. At any one point in<br />

time, roughly half of adolescents report that they are<br />

currently in a romantic relationship. But it would be incorrect<br />

to assume that the other half of adolescents have no<br />

romantic involvements.<br />

Findings from our lab suggest that they are most likely<br />

involved in romantic activities that are part of a continuum<br />

leading from mixed-gender involvements to actual<br />

romantic relationships (Connolly, Craig, Goldberg, &<br />

Pepler, 2004). <strong>In</strong> a study of 1284 adolescents in grades 5<br />

through 8, we tabulated the kinds of romantic involvements<br />

students reported over the course of a school year.<br />

Our results document three types of romantic activities:<br />

mixed-gender affiliations, dating in groups, and dyadic<br />

romantic relationships. We also found that these romantic<br />

activities were sequentially organized in stages that led<br />

adolescents progressively away from associating only with<br />

same-gender friends to having a boyfriend or girlfriend.<br />

Dating in groups appeared to be a key activity along this<br />

progression because it bridged lower and higher levels of<br />

romantic involvement, functioning as the activity that most<br />

often preceded the formation of a romantic couple as well<br />

as the most frequent romantic activity following the termination<br />

of a relationship. We also found that dating in groups<br />

was more commonly reported than dyadic relationships,<br />

consistent with the emergent and fluid nature of romantic<br />

relationships in adolescence.<br />

The importance of group dating was also highlighted<br />

in a study of 180 high school students whom we followed<br />

from grade 9 to 11 (Connolly, Furman, & Konarski, 2000).<br />

We found that their mixed-gender peer groups comprised<br />

the most proximal social context for forming romantic<br />

relationships. This continuity can be attributed to their<br />

shared base in meeting affiliative needs. As such, within the<br />

North American context in which adolescent peer connections<br />

are encouraged, group dating is the point of intersection<br />

for romantic interests and the world of peers.<br />

Endings of <strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong><br />

While developmentally important and phenomenologically<br />

salient, romantic relationships tend to be shortlived,<br />

often lasting less than six months to a year (Connolly<br />

& Johnson, 1996). It is almost inevitable then that most<br />

young people will experience a romantic break-up at some<br />

point during their adolescence (Shulman & Kipnis, 2001).<br />

Despite being a normative aspect of romantic experience,<br />

very little is known about these dissolutions or why they<br />

occur.<br />

<strong>In</strong> our lab we have embarked on some initial investigations<br />

of romantic dissolutions, focusing on mapping<br />

these experiences and their normative functions. <strong>In</strong> two<br />

studies of young people ranging in age from 11 to 25<br />

(total N = 1357) we have obtained information on their<br />

2


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

Table 1. <strong>Romantic</strong> Relationship Dissolution Experiences Across<br />

Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood<br />

Age range<br />

Experience with dissolutions 11–13 14–16 17–19 20–25<br />

Had a break-up<br />

No 54% 30% 23% 6%<br />

Yes 46% 70% 77% 94%<br />

Number of break-ups<br />

1–2 65% 71% 56% 38%<br />

3–5 17% 21% 28% 33%<br />

>5 18% 8% 16% 23%<br />

Break-up in past 12 months<br />

Yes 78% 72% 66% 48%<br />

No 22% 28% 34% 52%<br />

Break-up initiator<br />

Self 40% 41% 42% 45%<br />

Mutual 44% 40% 38% 35%<br />

Partner 16% 19% 20% 20%<br />

Note. For 11–13 n = 533, 14–16 n = 409, 17–19 n = 223, 20–25<br />

n = 192.<br />

relationship turnovers over the course of a year, (Connolly<br />

& McIsaac, 2003; 2004). These results, shown in Table 1,<br />

support the view that relationship terminations are<br />

common throughout adolescence. Moreover, the number of<br />

youths who have had a termination increases steadily with<br />

age, as well as the total number of terminations that they<br />

have had. Also varying with age is the likelihood of having<br />

had a recent breakup, with younger adolescents being more<br />

likely to report a breakup in the last 12 months, compared<br />

to late adolescents and emerging adults. This is most<br />

probably due to the increased capacity for older youths to<br />

sustain more enduring relationships which are less vulnerable<br />

to rapid dissolution. Constant across age, adolescents<br />

told us that their relationships more often ended because of<br />

their own or shared dissatisfaction rather than being<br />

rejected by their partner.<br />

Although the retrospective nature of our data do not<br />

allow us to draw firm conclusions about what actually transpired<br />

at the time of the break-up, our findings nonetheless<br />

suggest that adolescents continue to think about their failed<br />

relationships. Studying their dissolution accounts, even<br />

after the event, may provide a window into the psychosocial<br />

needs which were not met in that relationship. From<br />

a developmental perspective, the unmet needs that lead to<br />

break-ups may be very similar to the social needs which<br />

motivate adolescent involvement in relationships in the<br />

first place. One direction that we are pursuing in our<br />

research is to employ prospective data to explore whether<br />

these needs can be framed in terms of adolescents’<br />

struggles with key psychosocial tasks.<br />

<strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong> and Psychosocial<br />

Tasks<br />

Based on our research, as well as that of others, it is our<br />

view that romantic relationships in adolescence provide an<br />

important opportunity for learning about how to connect<br />

with a significant other while simultaneously maintaining<br />

some degree of autonomy in their thoughts, behaviors and<br />

self-perceptions (Connolly & Goldberg, 1999). Several<br />

theorists (Blatt & Blass, 1996; Shulman & Knafo, 1997) have<br />

conceptualized the balance between connectedness and<br />

individuality as a life span challenge of close relationships<br />

and we have found this view helpful in conceptualizing<br />

adolescent romantic relationships. <strong>In</strong> our view, the ways<br />

in which connectedness and autonomy are expressed<br />

in a romantic relationship emerges progressively, as<br />

adolescents gain experience with romantic partnerships<br />

and also as their relationships deepen emotionally. <strong>In</strong>itially,<br />

romantic relationships are motivated by a need for connectedness<br />

which is cumulatively expressed through passionate<br />

attraction, affiliative bonding, and emotional intimacy<br />

(Connolly & Goldberg, 1999). These interwoven forms of<br />

connecting with a partner are apparent in adolescents’<br />

concepts of the motivational appeal of romantic relationships.<br />

<strong>In</strong> a study of 1755 young adolescents, perceptions of<br />

the unique features of romantic relationships were evaluated<br />

with an open-ended response format. Using content<br />

analysis to code their responses, we found a clustering of<br />

connectedness functions which included affiliation, sexuality<br />

and emotional intimacy. Affiliation was the most<br />

commonly reported connectedness feature, a finding which<br />

supports our work on the importance of companionship<br />

and shared activities in romantic development for<br />

adolescents. A multi-level understanding of connectedness<br />

was evident, despite the young age of the participants and<br />

their relative lack of experience with a romantic partner.<br />

Consistent with this, intimacy functions continued to be<br />

fundamental to romantic relationships throughout adolescence.<br />

At the same time there is increasing capacity to actualize<br />

these forms of intimacy with age. Older adolescents,<br />

by virtue of their expanding experiences with romance, are<br />

able to sustain longer-term relationships within which<br />

intimacy can flourish (Connolly & Johnson, 1996; Seiffge-<br />

Krenke, 2003).<br />

<strong>In</strong> contrast, we know less about the expression of<br />

autonomy with a romantic partner. This may be due to the<br />

traditional empirical focus on the parent-child relationship<br />

as the central context for the resolution of autonomy tasks<br />

and the difficulty of generalizing these findings to the<br />

romantic domain (Collins, Laursen, Mortensen, Leubker, &<br />

Ferreria, 1997). Further complicating matters, autonomy<br />

issues are not always apparent in adolescents’ romantic<br />

relationships because the need to differentiate the self from<br />

the partner only arises when high levels of intimacy<br />

threaten personal boundaries. These issues typically come<br />

to the forefront in the latter stages of romantic development<br />

when age and experience combine to lead to more<br />

committed relationships. Consistent with this developmental<br />

view, our study of the romantic concepts of young<br />

adolescents did not reveal any recognition of the potential<br />

autonomy components of romantic relationships<br />

(Connolly, Craig, Goldberg, & Pepler, 1999). Similarly, in a<br />

study of 230 mid- and late adolescents, we found that<br />

comfort with independent decision-making was associated<br />

with intimacy only in longer-term romantic relationships<br />

(Taradash, Connolly, Pepler, Craig, & Costa, 2001). Girls in<br />

this study, in contrast to the boys, showed more ease with<br />

asserting their own ideas and decisions in their romantic<br />

3


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

relationships, a finding that echoes girls’ more advanced<br />

fluency in dyadic relationships. We have continued to look<br />

at how boys and girls negotiate autonomy in their relationships<br />

with each other in an observational study of conflict<br />

management in romantic couples at age 16 (McIsaac,<br />

Connolly, McKenney, Pepler, & Craig, manuscript under<br />

review). Conflict is an interpersonal situation which<br />

demands self-expression and so is ideally suited to the<br />

study of autonomy processes. <strong>In</strong> analyzing the dynamics<br />

between boyfriends and girlfriends, we found that couples<br />

who managed conflict successfully showed both a capacity<br />

for the respectful self-expressions of their differences and<br />

an acceptance of the independence of their partners. The<br />

reciprocity was largely dependent on the girl’s actions in<br />

the relationship as her facility with conflict negotiation set<br />

the tone for the autonomous expression of both herself and<br />

her partner. <strong>Of</strong> particular interest, we also found that girls<br />

who were restrictive in their management of conflict also<br />

influenced their boyfriends, but in a negative direction. A<br />

future direction of our research program is to better understand<br />

gendered styles of interaction and how they may lead<br />

to differential success in the balancing of autonomy with<br />

intimacy.<br />

Early Timing of <strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong><br />

Our studies of romantic development reveal typical<br />

patterns of romantic activities and romantic relationships<br />

across early, middle and late adolescence. Yet viewed<br />

through a different lens, these findings can reveal patterns<br />

of romantic activity that are atypical. We have been especially<br />

interested in early entry into romantic relationships<br />

because precocity in the heterosexual domain is a risk factor<br />

for depression, school problems, and externalizing behaviors<br />

(Bingham & Crockett, 1996; Neeman, Hubbard, &<br />

Masten, 1995). <strong>In</strong> our longitudinal study of young<br />

adolescents, we found that having a boyfriend or girlfriend<br />

before the age of 13 was linked to specific peer and family<br />

dynamics (Friedlander, Connolly, Pepler, & Craig, in press).<br />

<strong>In</strong> particular we found that atypical romantic patterns were<br />

associated with involvement in peer groups with high<br />

levels of dating and delinquent activities, as well as low<br />

supervision in the family. We have also studied how early<br />

adolescent romantic trajectories may be influenced by problematic<br />

peer behaviors. During the middle-school years,<br />

peer harassment expands to opposite-sex targets<br />

(McMaster, Connolly, Pepler, & Craig, 2002) and this may<br />

create non-optimal conditions for emerging romantic<br />

relationships. Our study of middle-school youth found that<br />

early adolescents who bullied their peers were more likely<br />

than non-bullies to report high levels of conflict and<br />

physical aggression with their partner (Connolly, Pepler,<br />

Craig, & Taradash, 2000).<br />

At first glance these studies of young adolescents may<br />

lead to the conclusion that romantic involvement at this age<br />

is harmful. We caution against this interpretation and<br />

suggest that when studying potential risks, one must be<br />

attentive to the level of involvement and the type of<br />

activity. <strong>In</strong> fact, our staging studies show that casual affiliative<br />

attractions are highly normative for early adolescents<br />

and other researchers have shown that these activities are<br />

linked to positive perceptions of the self (Darling, Dowdy,<br />

Van Horn, & Caldwell, 1999).<br />

Conclusions<br />

Our research on the stages, tasks and timing of romantic<br />

relationships paints a holistic picture of these interactions,<br />

highlighting their developmental underpinnings as well as<br />

their responsiveness to contextual variation. Disconfirming<br />

earlier views that romantic relationships are unknowable<br />

and unimportant, our research suggests that they unfold<br />

systematically and serve multiple psychosocial functions.<br />

This view reinforces the notion that romantic relationships<br />

are a critical social context for preparing youth for successful<br />

relationships in later life.<br />

References<br />

Bingham, C.R., & Crockett, L.J. (1996). Longitudinal adjustment<br />

patterns of boys and girls experiencing early,<br />

middle, and late sexual intercourse. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 32, 647–658.<br />

Blatt, S.J., & Blass, R.B. (1996). Relatedness and selfdefinition:<br />

A dialectic model of personality development.<br />

<strong>In</strong> G. Noam & K. Fischer (Eds.), Development and<br />

vulnerability in close relationships (pp. 309–338). NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Collins, W.A. (2003). More than myth: The developmental<br />

significance of romantic relationships during adolescence.<br />

Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 1–24.<br />

Collins, W.A., Laursen, B., Mortensen, N., Luebker, C., &<br />

Ferreria, M. (1997). Conflict processes and transitions in<br />

parent and peer relationships: Implications for<br />

autonomy and regulation. Journal of Adolescent Research,<br />

12, 178–198.<br />

Connolly, J., Craig, W., Goldberg, A., & Pepler, D. (1999).<br />

Conceptions of cross-sex friendships and romantic<br />

relationships in early adolescence. Journal of Youth and<br />

Adolescence, 28, 481–493.<br />

Connolly, J., Craig, W., Goldberg, A., & Pepler, D. (2004).<br />

Mixed-gender groups, dating and romantic relationships<br />

in early adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence,<br />

14, 185–207.<br />

Connolly, J., Furman, W., & Konarski, R. (2000). The role of<br />

peers in the emergence of heterosexual romantic<br />

relationships in adolescence. Child Development, 71,<br />

1395–1408.<br />

Connolly, J., & Goldberg, A. (1999). <strong>Romantic</strong> relationships<br />

in adolescence: The role of friends and peers in their<br />

emergence and development. <strong>In</strong> W. Furman, B. Brown,<br />

& C. Feiring (Eds.), The development of romantic relationships<br />

in adolescence (pp. 266–290). NY: Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Connolly, J., & Johnson, A. (1996). Adolescents’ romantic<br />

relationships and the structure and quality of their close<br />

interpersonal ties. Personal <strong>Relationships</strong>, 3, 185–195.<br />

Connolly, J., & McIsaac, C. (2003). Developmental-contextual<br />

analysis of adolescents’ attributions for romantic<br />

relationship dissolution. <strong>In</strong> J. Connolly & W. Furman<br />

(Chairs), Understanding romantic relationship break-ups in<br />

adolescence. Paper symposium presented at the biennial<br />

meeting for the Society for Research in Child Development,<br />

Tampa, FL.<br />

Connolly, J., & McIsaac, C. (2004). <strong>Romantic</strong> dissolutions in<br />

adolescence: Negative affect, attributions, and romantic<br />

confidence. <strong>In</strong> J. Connolly & C. Feiring (Chairs), Positive<br />

4


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

and negative processes within romantic relationships. Poster<br />

symposium presented at the biennial meeting of the<br />

Society for Research on Adolescence, Baltimore, MD.<br />

Connolly, J., Pepler, D., Craig, W., & Taradash, A. (2000).<br />

Dating experiences of bullies in early adolescence. Child<br />

Maltreatment, 5, 299–310.<br />

Darling, N., Dowdy, B.B., Van Horn, M.L., & Caldwell, L.L.<br />

(1999). Mixed-sex settings and the perception of<br />

competence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28,<br />

461–481.<br />

Friedlander, L., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (in<br />

press). Biological, familial, and peer influences on<br />

dating in early adolescence. Archives of Sexual Behavior.<br />

McIsaac, C., Connolly, J., McKenney, K.S., Pepler, D., &<br />

Craig, W. (manuscript under review). Conflict negotiation<br />

and autonomy processes in adolescent romantic<br />

relationships: An observational study of interdependency<br />

in boyfriend and girlfriend effects. Journal of<br />

Adolescence.<br />

McIsaac, C., McKenney, K.S., & Connolly, J. (2007). “<strong>To</strong><br />

Participate or Not Participate?”: Adolescents’ Response to<br />

Strategies Targeting <strong>In</strong>creased Participation. Poster<br />

presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for<br />

Research on Child Development, Boston, MA.<br />

McMaster, L., Connolly, J.A., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (2002).<br />

Peer to peer sexual harassment in early adolescence.<br />

Development and Psychopathology, 10, 25–56.<br />

Neemann, J., Hubbard, J., & Masten, A.S. (1995). The<br />

changing importance of romantic relationship involvement<br />

to competence from late childhood to late adolescence.<br />

Development and Psychopathology, 7, 727–750.<br />

Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2003). Testing theories of romantic<br />

development from adolescence to young adulthood:<br />

Evidence of a developmental sequence. <strong>In</strong>ternational<br />

Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 519–531.<br />

Shulman, S., & Kipnis, O. (2001). Adolescent romantic<br />

relationships: A look from the future. Journal of Adolescence,<br />

24, 337–351.<br />

Shulman, S., & Knafo, D. (1997). Balancing closeness and<br />

individuality in adolescent close relationships. <strong>In</strong>ternational<br />

Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 687–702.<br />

Taradash, A., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., Craig, W., & Costa, M.<br />

(2001). The interpersonal context of romantic autonomy<br />

in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 365–377.<br />

<strong>Romantic</strong> Relational Aggression and<br />

Psychosocial Adjustment among<br />

Adolescents<br />

Sunil Saini<br />

Department of Applied Psychology, Guru<br />

Jambheshwar University of Science and Technology,<br />

Hisar, Haryana, <strong>In</strong>dia<br />

E-mail: ssaini.psy@gmail.com<br />

The early adolescent years are a critical transitional period<br />

because it is during this time that boys and girls progress<br />

from same-sex to other-sex relationships, including<br />

involvement with romantic partners (Connolly &<br />

Goldberg, 1999). <strong>Romantic</strong> relationships are an integral<br />

feature of adolescent development. These relationships<br />

typically begin around 14 to 15 years of age, initially as an<br />

extension of involvement in mixed gender peer groups<br />

(Feiring, 1996). The romantic relationship is associated with<br />

both positive mental health such as the provision of social<br />

support, the enhancement of self-esteem, preparation for<br />

adult relationships, and the development of intimacy<br />

(Connolly & Goldberg, 1999) and negative mental health<br />

such as feelings of depression, especially among adolescent<br />

girls (Davila, Steinberg, Kachadourian, Cobb, & Fincham,<br />

2004). Not much literature is available on how romantic<br />

relational aggression and victimization affects psychosocial<br />

functioning in adolescence. Linder, Crick, and Collins<br />

(2002) found that college students who reported using<br />

relational aggression within their romantic relationships<br />

were less trusting of their current or most recent partner,<br />

and more frustrated, jealous, and clingy in their romantic<br />

relationships. Bagner, Storch, and Preston (2007) also found<br />

similar results indicating that romantic relational<br />

aggression was related to poor psychosocial functioning.<br />

Aggression in <strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong><br />

Aggression in romantic relationship can be broadly defined<br />

across a range of behaviours, including physical assault,<br />

kidnapping, acid throwing, rape and even murder.<br />

However, it is also important to consider the less physical,<br />

but still hurtful, relational aggression in romantic relationships.<br />

Examples include flirting with others to make a<br />

romantic partner jealous, threatening to break up with a<br />

partner if the partner will not comply, or giving a partner<br />

the silent treatment when angry. Media has highlighted<br />

many such cases in the capital of <strong>In</strong>dia and other metro<br />

cities. Although research has been conducted on romantic<br />

relational aggression in Western cultures, little research has<br />

examined romantic relational problems in <strong>In</strong>dia. Therefore,<br />

the present research is focused on romantic relational<br />

aggression and psychosocial functioning among <strong>In</strong>dian<br />

youths.<br />

Relational aggression in romantic relationships is of<br />

great concern due to its negative individual and societal<br />

impact. It has been found that adolescents who date,<br />

particularly those who experience stress in their romantic<br />

relationships, report higher levels of depressive symptoms<br />

than their non-dating peers (Davila et al., 2004). Specifically<br />

with regard to romantic relational aggression, evidence<br />

suggests that men and women experience romantic<br />

relational aggression at similar rates and that romantic<br />

relational aggression predicts a negative perception of<br />

relationship quality and depression (Bagner et al, 2007;<br />

Linder et al., 2002; Morales & Cullerton-Sen, 2000). <strong>In</strong> view<br />

of such findings, it is especially critical to identify relational<br />

aggression in romantic relationships and associated<br />

personal and social risks.<br />

There is a strong relation between romantic relational<br />

aggression and relational victimization. Some of the roots<br />

of these issues may manifest in earlier peer relationships.<br />

Relational aggression toward peers and being the target of<br />

relational aggression in the peer group are associated with<br />

loneliness, depression, and peer rejection (Crick & Grotpeter,<br />

1995). Friendships of relationally aggressive children<br />

have higher levels of exclusivity and jealousy than the<br />

friendships of non-relationally aggressive children (Crick &<br />

Grotpeter, 1995). Further, relationally victimized children<br />

5


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

report lower levels of friendship quality and satisfaction<br />

than non-relationally victimized children (Grotpeter,<br />

Geiger, Nukulkij, & Crick, 2001). Such early links of<br />

relational aggression to relationship quality highlight the<br />

need for the study of relational aggression in romantic<br />

relationships, particularly as it may be linked to the<br />

psychosocial adjustment areas of social anxiety, loneliness,<br />

depression, and alcohol use.<br />

<strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong> and Psychosocial<br />

Adjustment<br />

Social anxiety means the fear of being negatively evaluated<br />

by others. These evaluative concerns lead to unwanted<br />

anxious feelings, thoughts, and sensations in (real or anticipated)<br />

situations. <strong>To</strong> limit contact with these unwanted<br />

experiences, socially anxious people exert a great deal of<br />

effort to avoid and control anxiety and the situations that<br />

might induce it (Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, &<br />

Strosahl, 1996). Socially anxious people are more socially<br />

isolated, less likely to be married or in romantic relationships,<br />

and have unsatisfactory social relationships<br />

(Wittchen, Fuetsch, Sonntag, Muller, & Liebowitz, 2000).<br />

There is minimal work on romantic relational aggression<br />

and social anxiety. <strong>In</strong> a recent study, Bagner et al. (2007)<br />

found that romantic relational aggression was positively<br />

related with social anxiety in females.<br />

Loneliness is a state of emotional distress arising from a<br />

discrepancy between one’s desired and achieved level of<br />

social interaction (Peplau & Perlman, 1982). It is distressing<br />

particularly in romantic relationships. Previous research has<br />

asked participants whether or not they are lonely, calculated<br />

percentages, and computed correlations with personality or<br />

other demographic variables. Loneliness as a maladjustment<br />

within romantic relationships has been mostly overlooked<br />

in romantic relationship research literature. The<br />

present study examined loneliness as the consequence of<br />

aggression in romantic relationship in <strong>In</strong>dian youths.<br />

Studying sources of depression in romantic relationships<br />

is of much interest because depression is the most<br />

prevalent problem during adolescence (particularly for<br />

girls) and is associated with romantic dysfunction at later<br />

ages (Whisman, 2001). Symptoms of depression and social<br />

anxiety are common characteristics of adolescence<br />

(Birmaher et al., 1996; La Greca & Lopez, 1998) and may be<br />

risk factors for impairment in adulthood (Aalto-Setaelae,<br />

Marttunen, Tuulio-Henriksson, Poikolainen, & Loennqvist,<br />

2002). Symptoms of depression and social anxiety may also<br />

be precursors to more severe psychopathology, including<br />

major depressive disorder and social anxiety disorder,<br />

which are chronic disorders that often originate in adolescence<br />

and continue into adulthood (Birmaher et al., 1996).<br />

It is likely that relational aggression is related to levels of<br />

depression experiences within relationships.<br />

Studying the impact of romantic relationship aggression<br />

on psychosocial adjustment is very important because<br />

youths may be more vulnerable to negative outcomes due<br />

to their inexperience and lack of close supervision. Studies<br />

have shown that more than 25 % of adolescents are victims<br />

of dating aggression (Wolfe & Feiring, 2000) resulting in<br />

romantic break-ups which in turn, lead to drinking, suicidal<br />

ideation, attempts and completion (Brendt et al., 1993;<br />

Joyner & Udry, 2000; Monroe, Rhode, Seeley, & Lewinsohn,<br />

1999). Peer pressure further facilitates the perpetration of<br />

aggressive acts in romantic relationships. Thus, by understanding<br />

the relationship between aggression in romantic<br />

relationships and adjustment difficulties, more effective<br />

interventions for these individuals can be implemented.<br />

The Present Research<br />

The following three hypotheses guided this study: (i) there<br />

would be a significant gender difference in aggression in<br />

romantic relationship and experience of romantic relational<br />

victimization among youths (ii) romantic relational<br />

aggression would be significantly associated with romantic<br />

relational victimization (iii) romantic relational aggression<br />

and victimization would be positively correlated with<br />

social anxiety, loneliness, depression and alcohol use.<br />

Adolescents from the age group of 16–22 years from<br />

Guru Jambheswar University of Science and Technology,<br />

Hisar were surveyed. <strong>Romantic</strong> relational aggression was<br />

assessed by using scale developed by Morales, Ruh, and<br />

Werner (2002). Social anxiety was measure by using La<br />

Greca and Lopez (1998). Loneliness was measured by using<br />

Jha’s (1997) loneliness scale. Similarly, depression was<br />

measure by The Beck Depression <strong>In</strong>ventory Second Edition<br />

developed by Beck, Steer, and Brow (1996). Alcohol<br />

problems were assessed by seven items related with<br />

alcohol-related behaviors and consequences associated<br />

with alcohol use and dependence. Results are summarized<br />

in Table 1 and Table 2.<br />

Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of <strong>Romantic</strong> relational aggression, <strong>Romantic</strong> relational victimization and Social Anxiety, Loneliness,<br />

and Depression<br />

<strong>To</strong>tal sample<br />

Gender<br />

(N = 183) Male (N = 73) Female (110)<br />

Dependent variable Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) t<br />

<strong>Romantic</strong> Relational Aggression 10.50 (7.1) 12.8 (7.13) 8.90 (6.6) 3.69**<br />

<strong>Romantic</strong> Relational Victimization 9.24 (7.2) 11.8 (6.68) 7.50 (7.0) 4.11**<br />

Social Anxiety 75.37 (14.6) 76.95 (10.08) 74.32 (16.8) 1.19**<br />

Loneliness 95.48 (14.7) 91.39 (14.7)0 98.19 (14.1) 3.13**<br />

Depression 12.70 (10.9) 15.67 (12.0)0 10.73 (9.0) 3.16**<br />

*p < .05; **p < .01.<br />

6


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

Table 2. Pearson product-moment correlations among <strong>Romantic</strong> Relational Aggression and Social Anxiety,<br />

Loneliness, and Depression (N = 183)<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

1. <strong>Romantic</strong> Relational Aggression 1.00**<br />

2. <strong>Romantic</strong> Relational victimization .79** 1.00**<br />

3. Social Anxiety .21** .25** 1.00*<br />

4. Loneliness .25** .16** .16* 1.00**<br />

5. Depression .39** .31** .08* .24** 1.00<br />

*p < .05; **p < .01 (2-tailed).<br />

Findings reveal a significant gender difference in<br />

romantic relational aggression. This is inconsistent with<br />

past research (Bagner et al., 2007; Linder et al., 2002) that<br />

has not found gender differences in romantic relational<br />

aggression. The gender differences indicate that males<br />

report both more relational aggression and victimization.<br />

There are different possible explanations for this. First,<br />

males may have higher self-esteem, and larger egos than<br />

females, and are perhaps therefore more sensitive to provocation<br />

and loss of respect by others (Fischer, Rodreiguez, &<br />

Mosquera, 2001). Further, males get more angry when their<br />

partner does not pay attention to them. This might be due<br />

to cultural differences and developmental issues. For<br />

example, girls in the <strong>In</strong>dian socio-cultural context, are<br />

“supposed to” be quiet, calm and show more refined<br />

behavior in comparison to males (Saini & Trama, in press).<br />

Therefore, it is possible that socialization practices and<br />

modelling may inhibit the emergence of relational<br />

aggression among girls when compared to the western<br />

cultures studied in previous findings.<br />

My second hypothesis was to examine the association<br />

between romantic relational aggression and romantic<br />

relational victimization. There is a high relation between<br />

romantic relational aggression and victimization, suggesting<br />

that relationships characterized by these behaviours<br />

have some reciprocal aggression. However, it is also<br />

important to note that use of self-report may not accurately<br />

reflect levels of victimization. Males simply may be more<br />

likely than females to report being victims of aggression,<br />

including romantic relational aggression. Therefore, it is<br />

suggested to use other methods such as having both<br />

partners in the relationship report to confirm this result.<br />

The final aim of the study was to examine correlations<br />

between romantic relational aggression, victimization, and<br />

psychosocial functioning. The findings show that romantic<br />

relational aggression was significantly and positively<br />

related to social anxiety, loneliness and depressive<br />

symptoms. <strong>In</strong>dividuals who reported using romantic<br />

relational aggression reported being more socially anxious,<br />

lonely and depressive. Results are consistent with previous<br />

findings on romantic relational aggression and depressive<br />

symptoms (Davila et al., 2004; Kashdan, Jeffrey, Volkmann,<br />

Breen, & Han, 2007). La Greca and Harrison (2005) also<br />

found that negative interactions with a best friend (e.g.<br />

conflict, exclusion) predicted adolescents’ depressive<br />

symptoms, even after controlling for other aspects of social<br />

functioning (rejection, victimization).<br />

<strong>In</strong>dividuals who reported being victimized in romantic<br />

relationship were similarly more socially anxious, lonely,<br />

and depressed. Results are consistent with previous<br />

findings (Graham, & Juvonen, 1998; Linder et al., 2002).<br />

Graham and Juvonen (1998) found that peer victimization<br />

in adolescents was associated with internal distress, including<br />

feelings of loneliness, social anxiety, and low self-worth.<br />

Both overt and relational victimization have been related to<br />

adolescents’ reports of depression, loneliness, and low selfesteem<br />

(Prinstein, Boergers, & Vernberg, 2001). Prospero<br />

(2007) in a study of mental health symptoms of partner<br />

violence also found significant relationships between<br />

partner violence victimization and anxiety, depression,<br />

hostility and somatization.<br />

Conclusion<br />

<strong>In</strong> sum, the associations between romantic relational<br />

aggression, victimization and psychosocial functioning are<br />

consistent with previous findings, i.e. higher levels of<br />

romantic relational aggression and victimization are<br />

associated with maladjustment and poor psychosocial<br />

functioning. These relationships are independent of gender<br />

and play an important role in development of social<br />

anxiety, loneliness and depressive symptoms. No association<br />

was found between romantic relational aggression<br />

and alcohol use. Only ten boys and two girls reported<br />

alcohol use. It might be due to the participants’ socioeconomic<br />

background.<br />

Limitations of the study should also be noted. First,<br />

the participants were university students in <strong>In</strong>dia,<br />

limiting generalizability and therefore conclusions cannot<br />

be made about romantic relational aggression in other<br />

populations. Second, there was a big gender difference in<br />

the sample. Third, the present findings yielded evidence<br />

of concurrent associations between romantic relational<br />

aggression and victimization and depressive symptoms,<br />

but they are unable to allow us to infer which, if either,<br />

preceded the other. Fourth, students did not report any<br />

alcohol use. It might be that they have underreported<br />

these behaviors.<br />

Despite these limitations, the results of the present<br />

findings have several practical and clinical implications.<br />

Most importantly, the present findings have important<br />

implications in school/college/university setting where<br />

such relationships are very common and health care<br />

providers can be aware of risk factors involved with<br />

romantic relational aggression and victimization so that<br />

students at risk could be identified and necessary services<br />

could be provided. Additionally these findings demonstrate<br />

that knowing about an individual’s use of relational<br />

7


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

aggression against one’s romantic partner is also an indicator<br />

of victimization in the relationship and his/her<br />

psychosocial functioning.<br />

References<br />

Aalto-Setaelae, T., Marttunen, M., Tuulio-Henriksson, A.,<br />

Poikolainen, K., & Loennqvist, J. (2002). Depressive<br />

symptoms in adolescence as predictors of early adulthood<br />

depressive disorders and maladjustment.<br />

American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1235–1237.<br />

Bagner, D.M., Storch, E.A., & Preston, A.S. (2007). <strong>Romantic</strong><br />

Relational Aggression: What about Gender? Journal of<br />

Family Violence, 22, 19–24.<br />

Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., & Brown, G.K. (1996). Beck Depression<br />

<strong>In</strong>ventory manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological<br />

Corporation.<br />

Birmaher, B., Ryan, N.D., Douglas, E.W., Brent, D.A.,<br />

Kaufman, J., Dahl, R.E., Perel, J., & Nelson, B. (1996).<br />

Childhood and adolescent depression: A review of the<br />

past 10 years. Part I. Journal of the American Academy of<br />

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1427–1439.<br />

Brendt, D.A., Perper, J.A., Moritz, G., Baugher, M., Roth, C.,<br />

Balach, L. et al. (1993). Stressful life events, psychopathology,<br />

and adolescent suicide: A case control study.<br />

Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 23, 179–187.<br />

Carver, K., Joyner, K., & Udry, J.R. (2003). National<br />

estimates of adolescent romantic relationships. <strong>In</strong><br />

P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adolescent romantic relationships and<br />

sexual behavior: Theory, research, and practical implications<br />

(pp. 291–329). New York: Cambridge University.<br />

Connolly, J., & Goldberg, A. (1999). <strong>Romantic</strong> relationships<br />

in adolescence: The role of friends and peers in their<br />

emergence and development. <strong>In</strong> W. Furman, B.B. Brown,<br />

& C. Feiring (Eds.), The development of romantic relationships<br />

in adolescence (pp. 266–290). New York: Cambridge<br />

Press.<br />

Crick, N.R., & Grotpeter, J.K. (1995). Relational aggression,<br />

gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child<br />

Development, 66, 710–722.<br />

Davila, J., Steinberg, S.J., Kachadourian, L., Cobb, R., &<br />

Fincham, F. (2004). <strong>Romantic</strong> involvement and depressive<br />

symptoms in early and late adolescence: The role of<br />

preoccupied relational style. Personal <strong>Relationships</strong>, 11,<br />

161–178.<br />

Fischer, A.H., Rodreiguez, & Mosquera, P.M. (2001). What<br />

concerns men? Women or other men. Psychology, Evolution<br />

and Gender, 3, 5–25.<br />

Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (1998). Self-blame and peer<br />

victimization in middle school: An attributional<br />

analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34, 587–538.<br />

Grotpeter, J.K., Geiger, T.C., Nukulkij, P., & Crick, N.R.<br />

(2001). Friendships of relationally and physically victimized<br />

children: With friends like these, who needs enemies?<br />

Unpublished manuscript.<br />

Hayes, S.C., Wilson, K.G., Gifford, E.V., Follette, V.M., &<br />

Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral<br />

disorders: A functional dimensional approach to<br />

diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and<br />

Clinical Psychology, 64, 1152–1168.<br />

Jha, P.K. (1997). Loneliness scale manual. Agra, <strong>In</strong>dia:<br />

National Psychological Cooperation.<br />

Joyner, K., & Udry, J.R. (2000). You don’t bring me anything<br />

but down: Adolescent romance and depression. Journal<br />

of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 369–391.<br />

Kashdan, T.B., Volkmann, J.R., Breen, W.E., & Han, S.<br />

(2007). Social anxiety and romantic relationships: The<br />

costs and benefits of negative emotion expression are<br />

context-dependent. Journal of Anxiety disorder, 21,<br />

475–492.<br />

La Greca, A.M., & Harrison, H.M. (2005). Adolescent peer<br />

relations, friendships, and romantic relationships: Do<br />

they predict social anxiety and depression? Journal of<br />

Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 49–61.<br />

La Greca, A.M., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among<br />

adolescents: Linkages with peer relations and friendships.<br />

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 83–94.<br />

Linder, J.R., Crick, N.R., & Collins, W.A. (2002). Relational<br />

aggression and victimization in young adults’ romantic<br />

relationships: Associations with perceptions of parent,<br />

peer, and romantic relationship quality. Social Development,<br />

11, 69–86.<br />

Magdol, L., Moffitt, T.E., Caspi, A., & Silva, P.A. (1998).<br />

Developmental antecedents of partner abuse: A<br />

prospective-longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal<br />

Psychology, 107, 375–389.<br />

Monroe, S.M., Rohde, P., Seeley, J.R., & Lewinsohn, P.M.<br />

(1999). Life events and depression in adolescence:<br />

Relationship loss as a prospective risk factor for first<br />

onset of major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal<br />

Psychology, 108, 606–614.<br />

Morales, J.R., & Cullerton-Sen, C. (2000, March). Relational<br />

and physical aggression and psychological adjustment in<br />

adolescent peer and romantic relationships. Poster<br />

presented at the Biennial Meetings of the Society for<br />

Research on Adolescence, Chicago, IL.<br />

Morales, J.R., Ruh, J., & Werner, N. (2002). Adult<br />

aggression/victimization measure. Unpublished manuscript.<br />

Peplau, L.A., & Perlman, D. (1982). Perspectives on loneliness.<br />

<strong>In</strong> L.A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness. A<br />

sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy<br />

(pp. 1–18). New York: Wiley.<br />

Prinstein, M.J., Boergers, J., & Vernberg, E.M. (2001). Overt and<br />

relational aggression in athletes: Social-psychological<br />

adjustment of aggressors and victims. Journal of Clinical<br />

Child Psychology, 30, 479–491.<br />

Prospero, M. (2007). Mental health symptoms among male<br />

victims of partner violence. American Journal of Men’s<br />

Health, 1, 269–277.<br />

Saini, S., & Trama, S. (in press). Gender difference in anger,<br />

hostility and aggression. Asian Journal of Social Psychology.<br />

Wittchen, H.U., Fuetsch, M., Sonntag, H., Mueller, N., &<br />

Liebowitz, M. (2000). Disability and quality of life in<br />

pure and comorbid social phobia: Findings from a<br />

controlled study. European Psychiatry, 15, 46–58.<br />

Wolfe, D.A., & Feiring, C. (2000). Dating violence through<br />

the lens of adolescent romantic relationships. Child<br />

Maltreatment, 5, 360–363.<br />

8


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

Same-Sex <strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong> in<br />

Adolescence and Adulthood<br />

Lisa M. Diamond<br />

Department of Psychology, University of Utah<br />

Salt Lake City, Utah, USA<br />

E-mail: diamond@psych.utah.edu<br />

The 1983 publication of Blumstein and Schwartz’s American<br />

Couples marked a turning point in research on same-sex<br />

relationships. During the 10 years prior to the publication<br />

of this volume, which reported the most detailed and<br />

thoroughgoing comparisons to date between heterosexual<br />

and same-sex couples, fewer than 50 books, chapters, or<br />

articles in the psychological literature had focused specifically<br />

on same-sex couples. <strong>In</strong> the 10 years after the book’s<br />

publication, the number of publications increased 5-fold,<br />

and nearly doubled again during the next 10 years. These<br />

are welcome changes: Rigorous investigations of same-sex<br />

romantic relationships from adolescence to adulthood<br />

make a number of important contributions to relationship<br />

research more generally.<br />

First, such investigations diversify our fundamental<br />

knowledge base about romantic relationships. Historically,<br />

most academic scholarship on romantic relationships has<br />

focused exclusively on heterosexual couples. Hence,<br />

comparing the relationship processes and experiences of<br />

same-sex and heterosexual couples allows researchers to<br />

explore a broader range of process-oriented theories about<br />

romantic relationship functioning and the factors that<br />

promote relationship stability and satisfaction across<br />

different contexts and within different populations. Second,<br />

examining same-sex relationships allows for more complex<br />

investigations of the role of gender in relationship functioning.<br />

Most romantic relationship research takes for granted<br />

that every romantic couple includes one male and one<br />

female. Yet by directly comparing male-female couples to<br />

female-female and male-male couples, researchers can<br />

begin to disentangle how different combinations of genderlinked<br />

attitudes, skills, and expectations influence relationship<br />

functioning. Finally, investigating same-sex romantic<br />

relationships reveals critically important information about<br />

sexual-minority (i.e., non-heterosexual) individuals and the<br />

ways in which their social and developmental experiences<br />

are unique.<br />

The Social Context of Same-Sex <strong>Romantic</strong><br />

<strong>Relationships</strong><br />

Typically, same-sex romantic relationships are described as<br />

lesbian/gay/bisexual relationships, but this is not entirely<br />

accurate. Not all individuals involved in same-sex relationships<br />

consider themselves lesbian, gay, or bisexual, and<br />

furthermore, openly-identified lesbian/gay/bisexual individuals<br />

often have histories of both same-sex and other-sex<br />

relationships. Hence it is important not to presume that<br />

identity labels and relationship experiences (past, present,<br />

or future) correspond. This is particularly true across<br />

different cultural environments. Presently in the United<br />

States, there are large and visible lesbian-gay-bisexual<br />

communities in many large urban centers, where individuals<br />

pursuing same-sex relationships can receive social<br />

support and validation. Yet this is not uniformly true crossculturally.<br />

Hence, researchers interested in pursuing international<br />

investigations of same-sex romantic relationships<br />

must take particular care not to presume that participants<br />

in such relationships will openly claim lesbian-gaybisexual<br />

identities.<br />

My own longitudinal research on sexual identity, for<br />

example, has found surprisingly high variability in sexual<br />

identification and patterns of sexual relationships in young<br />

women over time. <strong>In</strong> 1995 I began interviewing 89 lesbian,<br />

bisexual, “questioning,” and “unlabeled” women about<br />

their attractions, identities, and behaviors, and have reinterviewed<br />

these women every two years since then, with<br />

a retention rate of approximately 90 % (Diamond, 1998;<br />

2000b; 2003; 2005; 2008; in press). Since the beginning of the<br />

study, two thirds of the participants have changed their<br />

identity labels, and the majority have histories of romantic<br />

or sexual relationships that flatly contradict their current<br />

identity labels (i.e., lesbian-identified women getting<br />

involved with men, heterosexually-identified women<br />

getting involved with women). Hence, an important challenge<br />

for research in this area is to adopt broad strategies<br />

of sample recuitment and data collection that capture the<br />

full range of individuals’ same-sex and other-sex experiences—and<br />

their psychosocial implications—regardless of<br />

how individuals personally conceptualize and label their<br />

sexuality.<br />

<strong>In</strong> growing acknowledgment of these complexities,<br />

researchers increasingly use the term sexual minorities to<br />

refer to all men and women with same-sex attractions and/<br />

or relationships, either currently or in the past. This term<br />

acknowledges the fact that regardless of one’s identity<br />

label, same-sex attractions and relationships—in both the<br />

U.S. and around the globe—place individuals firmly outside<br />

conventional societal norms.<br />

This emphasis on minority status is relevant and<br />

important because although tolerance and acceptance of<br />

same-sex sexuality have been gradually increasing (Loftus,<br />

2001), considerable prejudice and sometimes outright<br />

condemnation of same-sex sexuality continues to exist. This<br />

creates a range of unique social and psychological challenges<br />

for same-sex couples, such as the threat of physical<br />

violence (i.e., Brenner, 1995), disapproval or denial of one’s<br />

relationship from either partner’s family-of-origin (Caron<br />

& Ulin, 1997; LaSala, 2000; Oswald, 2002; Patterson, 2000),<br />

and also low-level stressors such as difficulty making hotel<br />

room reservations (Jones, 1996), receiving poor service and<br />

rude treatment during routine shopping (Walters &<br />

Curran, 1996), or uncertainty about bringing one’s partner<br />

to family functions (Caron & Ulin, 1997; Oswald, 2002).<br />

<strong>Of</strong> course, such factors are likely to vary dramatically<br />

as a function of different cultures’ attitudes toward samesex<br />

sexuality. Given that the bulk of research on same-sex<br />

couples is conducted in the United States, it is important to<br />

keep in mind that, among Western nations, Americans are<br />

particularly conservative in this regard. Widmer, Treas, and<br />

Newcomb’s (1998) analysis of 24 industrialized countries<br />

participating in the <strong>In</strong>ternational Social Survey Program<br />

found that 70% of Americans believe that homosexual sex<br />

is “always wrong,” compared with 39% of Canadians, 58%<br />

of British, 45% of Spaniards, and 42% of West Germans. The<br />

most conservative attitudes were found in Northern<br />

Ireland (80% reporting “always wrong”), Hungary (83%)<br />

9


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

and the Philippines (84%), whereas the most accepting attitudes<br />

were found in the Netherlands (19%). Such variation<br />

must be taken into account when drawing inferences about<br />

the relevance of social stigma for same-sex couples across<br />

diverse cultural contexts. It also bears noting that no<br />

research has systematically tested whether same-sex<br />

couples living in more tolerant communities, cultures, or<br />

nations have substantially different relationship dynamics<br />

or outcomes than those living in more stigmatizing<br />

environments. This is an important area for future research.<br />

The Role of Gender<br />

Gender differences in interpersonal attitudes, cognitions,<br />

and behaviors, and their implications for couple functioning,<br />

have long been topics of vigorous research and debate,<br />

and studies of same-sex couples have provided unique<br />

opportunities to examine how broadly gender-related<br />

effects operate. One of the most common research questions<br />

is whether sexual-minority individuals are “genderinverted”<br />

in their interpersonal functioning, such that gay<br />

men resemble heterosexual women and lesbians resemble<br />

heterosexual men. Research findings suggest that when it<br />

comes to relationship behavior, this is not the case; rather,<br />

studies commonly find that combining two men or two<br />

women in a couple relationship actually tends to magnify<br />

gender-specific patterns.<br />

For example, consistent with the notion that women are<br />

socialized to develop greater interest in interpersonal<br />

intimacy and to be more “relationally-focused,” studies<br />

have detected a tendency for lesbian couples to report<br />

greater intimacy with their partners, assessed by selfreported<br />

factors such as shared time together and the<br />

degree to which partners maintained a “couple” identity<br />

(Diamond, 1998), as well as heightened levels of cohesion,<br />

adaptability, and satisfaction (Zacks, Green, & Marrow,<br />

1988). Yet interestingly lesbian couples have not been found<br />

to show greater relationship stability than either gay male<br />

or heterosexual couples. Rather, studies (such as Kurdek,<br />

1998) suggest that having barriers to dissolving a relationship<br />

is more important for keeping it together than having a<br />

“double-dose” of female-typed relationship skills and<br />

maintenance strategies.<br />

Another topic of interest with regard to gender magnification<br />

in same-sex couples concerns power and equality<br />

in domains ranging from decision-making to influence<br />

strategies, household labor, and problem solving. Although<br />

stereotypes have historically presumed that same-sex<br />

couples implicitly designate one partner to take the classically<br />

“female” role and one partner to take the “male” role<br />

in these domains, research does not bear out this view.<br />

Rather, gay and lesbian couples place a high value on<br />

equity in their relationships, and lesbians in particular<br />

report particular success in achieving equitable arrangements<br />

(Peplau & Cochran, 1980, September). Strategies for<br />

achieving equity follow a number of different patterns.<br />

With respect to household responsibilities, research indicates<br />

that same-sex couples develop largely idiosyncratic<br />

arrangements, allowing their respective interests and<br />

desires to shape daily practice (Huston & Schwartz, 2002).<br />

Accordingly, it is not uncommon for same-sex partners to<br />

mix and match female-typed and male-typed tasks and<br />

roles (i.e., Amy handles auto maintenance and most of the<br />

cooking, while Deb takes care of social arrangements and<br />

financial planning). This is not to suggest, of course, that<br />

same-sex couples are uniformly successful in avoiding<br />

power differentials. For example, research has found that<br />

among both gay male and lesbian couples, income discrepancies<br />

tend to be associated with power differentials<br />

(Caldwell & Peplau, 1984; Harry, 1984; Harry & DeVall,<br />

1978; Reilly & Lynch, 1990), more so for for gay men than<br />

for women (Blumstein & Schwartz, 1983).<br />

Some of the most interesting directions for future<br />

research concern gender-related patterns of conflict resolution.<br />

Given the increasing evidence that (1) gender-related<br />

conflict dynamics have clear-cut implications for both<br />

mental and physical health, and that (2) these health effects<br />

are themselves differentiated by gender, one intriguing<br />

new direction for future research involves moving beyond<br />

the global self-report measures that have characterized<br />

previous research on this topic and collecting coordinated<br />

physiological and observational data on conflict resolution<br />

in same-sex couples. This is a direction my own laboratory<br />

is currently pursuing. Specifically, following a methodology<br />

that has long been used with married heterosexual<br />

couples, we are bringing same-sex couples into our<br />

psychophysiological laboratory and videotaping them as<br />

they discuss a common topic of disagreement. Meanwhile,<br />

we are measuring each partner’s cardiovascular and<br />

endocrinological reactivity to the conflict discussion.<br />

Previous research has suggested that the classic “female<br />

demand, male withdrawal” pattern is particularly aversive<br />

to both men and women (but especially women) and is<br />

associated with detrimental patterns of high and sustained<br />

physiological reactivity. But will same-sex couples show<br />

the same pattern? Is a “double dose” of withdrawal as bad<br />

for female-female couples as for male-female couples, or is<br />

withdrawal less aversive for women when it comes from<br />

another woman? By directly comparing male-male, femalefemale,<br />

and male-female couples’ conflict patterns and<br />

their physiological reactivity, we hope to introduce a new<br />

level of specificity into the traditional investigations of<br />

gender-related dynamics in same-sex couples, and to<br />

explore their important implications for physical health.<br />

Special Issues for Adolescents<br />

It took some time for researchers studying sexual-minority<br />

youths to devote significant attention to their romantic<br />

relationships. Up until 10–15 years ago the vast majority of<br />

extant research on sexual-minority youths focused on their<br />

sexual practices, identity development, stigmatization/<br />

victimization, and suicidality. Although these are important<br />

issues deserving of attention, researchers generally<br />

overlooked the more mundane features of adolescent life,<br />

such as routine relationship formation, maintenance, and<br />

dissolution.<br />

This neglect hampered our understanding of the<br />

normative socio-emotional developmental trajectories of<br />

sexual-minority youths. <strong>Of</strong> the multiple transitions that<br />

take place during the adolescent and young adult years,<br />

those involving interpersonal relationships are among the<br />

most salient and important. Adolescents undergo notable<br />

increases in their desires and capacities for emotional<br />

intimacy with peers, and these changes can profoundly<br />

reshape their close relationships and the sense of self they<br />

10


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

derive from these relationships. These transformations<br />

might be particularly meaningful for sexual-minority<br />

youths, given that same-sex sexuality is a fundamentally<br />

interpersonal as well as intrapsychic phenomenon.<br />

Because of their stigmatized minority status, sexualminority<br />

youths sometimes have to settle for partners and<br />

relationships they consider less than ideal. Others adapt to<br />

these hurdles by seeking out a diverse variety of sexual and<br />

affectionate same-sex bonds, many of which violate<br />

traditional conventional definitions of friendship and<br />

romance (Diamond, 2000a). It has been difficult to systematically<br />

chart and interpret such diverse relationship<br />

patterns because research on adolescent relationships has<br />

traditionally relied on vague, theoretically impoverished<br />

distinctions between different types of intimate relationships<br />

(Diamond, Savin-Williams, & Dubé, 1999). Rough<br />

lines are drawn between platonic and romantic relationships,<br />

and between casual and serious romances, but rarely<br />

do researchers probe the full range of affectional and sexual<br />

ties that adolescents from diverse backgrounds might<br />

consider.<br />

For example, one pattern of same-sex romantic<br />

intimacy that has been observed primarily among young<br />

women is passionate friendships (Diamond, 2000a). These<br />

bonds typically contain the affective and behavioral<br />

features of romantic relationships—such as emotional<br />

passion, possessiveness, exclusivity, and frequent physical<br />

affection—without explicit sexual desire or activity<br />

(although in some cases, the bonds may eventually become<br />

physically intimate). Thus, passionate friendships may<br />

provide sexual-minority youths with all of the benefits of a<br />

same-sex romantic relationship (most notably, social<br />

support and emotional intimacy) while avoiding many of<br />

the risks and complications. Notably, a similar degree of<br />

fluidity and ambiguity between friendships and romantic<br />

relationships is also observed among adult sexual-minority<br />

women (Nardi & Sherrod, 1994; Rose, Zand, & Cimi, 1993;<br />

Weinstock, 1997).<br />

Thus, many sexual-minority youths’ most extensive<br />

experience with same-sex intimacy will occur in relationships<br />

that look nothing like typical dating or romantic<br />

relationships. These diverse relationship types might be<br />

conceptualized as different solutions to the problems all<br />

youths—but especially sexual minority youths—face in<br />

balancing their needs and desires for sexual and affectional<br />

intimacy with the risks posed by different types of intimate<br />

relationships, as well as the constraints they face in finding<br />

the partners and relationships they want.<br />

It is important to note, however, that these constraints<br />

take a discernible toll on sexual-minority youths’ psychological<br />

well-being. <strong>In</strong> one recent study comparing the<br />

relationship experiences and expectations of heterosexual<br />

and sexual-minority youths, my research team used quantitative<br />

surveys and qualitative interviews to investigate<br />

the mental health implications of sexual-minority and<br />

heterosexual youths’ experiences and expectations regarding<br />

friendships and romantic relationships (Diamond &<br />

Lucas, 2004). Consistent with prior research, we found that<br />

sexual-minority youths had higher levels of anxiety, loneliness,<br />

depression, and physical symptomology than their<br />

heterosexual counterparts. Yet importantly, these effects<br />

did not appear to be attributable to sexual-minority youths’<br />

overall perceptions of stress and stigmatization, as is<br />

commonly assumed. Rather, they were mediated by differences<br />

between heterosexual and sexual-minority youths’<br />

romantic relationship expectations. Specifically, sexualminority<br />

youths reported significantly greater fears that<br />

they would never be able to find “the kind of romantic<br />

relationship I want” in the future, even though they had<br />

social networks of comparable size and rates of romantic<br />

involvement that were similar to their heterosexual counterparts<br />

(this particular sample of sexual-minority youths<br />

were fairly “out” and active in the local lesbian-gaybisexual<br />

community, which increased their opportunities<br />

for same-sex romance). Mediational analyses demonstrated<br />

that these relationship fears were responsible for these<br />

youths’ disproportionately low well-being. Such findings<br />

provide a stark demonstration of the importance of<br />

intimate relationship experiences for adolescent mental<br />

health.<br />

Considerations for Future Research<br />

<strong>In</strong> considering the history and future of psychological<br />

research on sexual-minority relationships, the underlying<br />

cultural assumptions and unavoidable political dimensions<br />

that shape the asking and answering of questions about<br />

same-sex relationships warrant continual scrutiny. <strong>In</strong> an<br />

influential critique of early research on lesbian and gay<br />

individuals, Kitzinger (1987) pointed out that the longstanding<br />

emphasis on documenting the lack of significant<br />

mental health differences between gay/lesbian and heterosexual<br />

individuals might have appeared to represent the<br />

triumph of scientific objectivity over social prejudice, but in<br />

fact functioned to reinforce the social disenfranchisement of<br />

sexual-minority individuals by implicitly predicating their<br />

social acceptability on patterns of thought, feeling, and<br />

behavior that were judged “normal” and “healthy” by<br />

mainstream society. Her analysis demonstrates the importance<br />

of continually monitoring our own explicit and<br />

implicit theories of sexuality and relationships in order to<br />

appropriately represent how these phenomena develop,<br />

unfold, and interact within the life courses of diverse<br />

sexual-minority individuals.<br />

These aims, of course, introduce notable challenges for<br />

researchers. As noted above, perhaps chief among these is<br />

the issue of sampling appropriately diverse populations of<br />

sexual minorities. <strong>In</strong> my own work, I have continually<br />

struggled to reach out to “hidden pockets” within the<br />

sexual-minority population, striving to include as broad a<br />

range of men and women as possible. Given the stigmatization<br />

faced by sexual minorities, this continues to be an<br />

uphill battle. One hopeful advance is the rise of the internet,<br />

which provides a means for reaching individuals who<br />

remain highly secretive about their same-sex sexuality, and<br />

who might never attend a Pride parade, read an advertisement<br />

in a lesbian-gay-bisexual publication, or see a flyer<br />

posted at a lesbian-gay-bisexual community center.<br />

Researchers have yet to comprehensively assess how<br />

internet samples resemble and differ from samples<br />

collected in conventional ways, and clearly such analyses<br />

are critically important if we are to make appropriate<br />

generalizations from the findings of future research.<br />

Another important obstacle, particularly for developmentally-oriented<br />

research on same-sex relationships<br />

among adolescents, is the prevailing conservativism of<br />

11


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

contemporary American culture regarding adolescent<br />

sexuality. Even in just the past 10 years, researchers have<br />

noted a rising tide of opposition to studies seeking to ask<br />

adolescents direct questions about their sexual and<br />

romantic feelings and experiences, despite the fact that the<br />

lack of a thoroughgoing understanding of youths’ experiences<br />

will clearly hamper our efforts to provide them with<br />

appropriate psychological and informational resources to<br />

make healthy decisions about their relationships. This<br />

increasing conservativism is particularly pernicious when<br />

it comes to same-sex sexuality. Researchers such as myself<br />

have had to become active advocates for the intellectual<br />

and social value of our research, arming ourselves with the<br />

wealth of evidence showing that adolescents are, in fact,<br />

well-served by the collection and dissemination of more and<br />

better data about the diversity of their intimate experiences<br />

and their developmental implications.<br />

Despite these challenges, I remain optimistic. The scope<br />

of research on same-sex relationships over the lifespan has<br />

broadened and diversified to a degree that I could scarcely<br />

have imagined when I first began studying this topic, and<br />

no doubt these changes will continue to produce a deeper<br />

understanding of intimate relationships in the context of<br />

same-sex sexuality and a deeper understanding of same-sex<br />

sexuality in the context of intimate relationships.<br />

References<br />

Blumstein, P., & Schwartz, P. (1983). American couples:<br />

Money, work, sex. New York: Morrow.<br />

Brenner, C. (1995). Eight bullets: One woman’s story of surviving<br />

anti-gay violence. Ithaca, NY: Firebrand Books.<br />

Caldwell, M.A., & Peplau, L.A. (1984). The balance of<br />

power in lesbian relationships. Sex Roles, 10, 587–599.<br />

Caron, S.L., & Ulin, M. (1997). Closeting and the quality of<br />

lesbian relationships. Families in Society, 78, 413–419.<br />

Diamond, L.M. (1998). Development of sexual orientation<br />

among adolescent and young adult women. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 34, 1085–1095.<br />

Diamond, L. M. (2000a). Passionate friendships among<br />

adolescent sexual-minority women. Journal of Research<br />

on Adolescence, 10, 191–209.<br />

Diamond, L.M. (2000b). Sexual identity, attractions, and<br />

behavior among young sexual-minority women over a<br />

two-year period. Developmental Psychology, 36, 241–250.<br />

Diamond, L.M. (2003). Was it a phase? Young women’s<br />

relinquishment of lesbian/bisexual identities over a 5-<br />

year period. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,<br />

84, 352–364.<br />

Diamond, L.M. (2005). A new view of lesbian subtypes:<br />

Stable vs. fluid identity trajectories over an 8-year<br />

period. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 119–128.<br />

Diamond, L.M. (2008). Sexual fluidity: Understanding<br />

women’s love and desire. Cambridge, MA: Harvard<br />

University Press.<br />

Diamond, L.M. (in press). Female bisexuality from adolescence<br />

to adulthood: Results from a 10 year longitudinal<br />

study. Developmental Psychology.<br />

Diamond, L.M., & Lucas, S. (2004). Sexual-minority and<br />

heterosexual youths’ peer and family relationships:<br />

Experiences, expectations, and implications for wellbeing.<br />

Journal of Research on Adolescence, 14, 313–340.<br />

Diamond, L.M., Savin-Williams, R.C., & Dubé, E.M. (1999).<br />

Sex, dating, passionate friendships, and romance:<br />

<strong>In</strong>timate peer relations among lesbian, gay, and<br />

bisexual adolescents. <strong>In</strong> W. Furman & B. B. Brown<br />

(Eds.), Development of romantic relationships in adolescence<br />

(pp. 175–210): Cambridge University Press.<br />

Harry, J. (1984). Gay couples. New York: Praeger.<br />

Harry, J., & DeVall, W.B. (1978). The social organization of gay<br />

males. New York: Praeger.<br />

Huston, M., & Schwartz, P. (2002). Gendered dynamics in<br />

the romantic relationships of lesbians and gay men. <strong>In</strong><br />

A. E. Hunter (Ed.), Readings in the psychology of gender:<br />

Exploring our differences and commonalities (pp. 167–178).<br />

Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.<br />

Jones, D.A. (1996). Discrimination against same-sex couples<br />

in hotel reservation policies. Journal of Homosexuality,<br />

31, 153–159.<br />

Kitzinger, C. (1987). The social construction of lesbianism.<br />

London: Sage.<br />

Kurdek, L.A. (1998). Relationship outcomes and their<br />

predictors: Longitudinal evidence from heterosexual<br />

married, gay cohabiting, and lesbian cohabiting<br />

couples. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 553–568.<br />

LaSala, M.C. (2000). Gay male couples: The importance of<br />

coming out and being out to parents. Journal of Homosexuality,<br />

39, 47–71.<br />

Loftus, J. (2001). America’s liberalization in attitudes<br />

toward homosexuality. American Sociological Review, 66,<br />

762–782.<br />

Nardi, P.M., & Sherrod, D. (1994). Friendship in the lives of<br />

gay men and lesbians. Journal of Social and Personal<br />

<strong>Relationships</strong>, 11, 185–199.<br />

Oswald, R.F. (2002). <strong>In</strong>clusion and belonging in the family<br />

rituals of gay and lesbian people. Journal of Family<br />

Psychology, 16, 428–436.<br />

Patterson, C. J. (2000). Family relationships of lesbians and<br />

gay men. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62,<br />

1052–1069.<br />

Peplau, L.A., & Cochran, S.D. (1980, September). Sex differences<br />

in values concerning love relationships. Paper<br />

presented at the American Psychological Association,<br />

Montreal, Canada.<br />

Reilly, M.E., & Lynch, J.M. (1990). Power-sharing in lesbian<br />

partnerships. Journal of Homosexuality, 19, 1–30.<br />

Rose, S., Zand, D., & Cimi, M.A. (1993). Lesbian courtship<br />

scripts. <strong>In</strong> E.D. Rothblum & K.A. Brehony (Eds.), Boston<br />

marriages (pp. 70–85). Amherst: University of Massachusetts<br />

Press.<br />

Walters, A.S., & Curran, M.C. (1996). “Excuse me, sir? May<br />

I help you and your boyfriend?”: Salespersons’ differential<br />

treatment of homosexual and straight customers.<br />

Journal of Homosexuality, 31, 135–152.<br />

Weinstock, J.S. (1997). Lesbian friendships: Simply an alternative<br />

or is this a revolution? <strong>In</strong> J.S. Weinstock (Chair),<br />

Lesbian friendships and social change. Symposium<br />

conducted at the annual meetings of the Association for<br />

Women in Psychology, Pittsburgh, PA.<br />

Widmer, E.D., Treas, J., & Newcomb, R. (1998). Attitudes<br />

toward nonmarital sex in 24 countries. Journal of Sex<br />

Research, 35, 349–358.<br />

Zacks, E., Green, R.-J., & Marrow, J. (1988). Comparing<br />

lesbian and heterosexual couples on the Circumplex<br />

Model: An initial investigation. Family Process, 27,<br />

471–484.<br />

12


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

Coping Strategies of Marital<br />

<strong>Relationships</strong> in Bamenda Cameroon<br />

Gladys Ngoran<br />

Human Development Resource Centre<br />

Bamenda Cameroon<br />

E-mail: gladngov@yahoo.com or<br />

ngopmbiy@yahoo.com<br />

Meaning of Marriage in Africa<br />

Sub-Saharan Africans hold marriage in high esteem as a<br />

fundamental union that gives rise to a family which is the<br />

basic unit of society and the springboard of life. With this<br />

in mind, families endeavour to inculcate the expectation of<br />

marriage in their children at an early age. As the children<br />

grow up, they know that they will marry in future.<br />

Anything a child does is appreciated in relation to<br />

marriage.<br />

<strong>In</strong> grassland Cameroon, as in most parts of sub-Saharan<br />

Africa, it is common to hear parents say; “That was well<br />

done son/daughter. Your wife/husband will always be<br />

happy if you can keep it up.” “Are you sure your husband<br />

will eat this type of poorly prepared food? Always take<br />

time to cook well.” “No woman will like a coward for a<br />

husband. Be a man.”<br />

Preparing children for marriage is one of a constellation<br />

of cultural values which are jealously guarded. These<br />

values as Nsamenang (1992) explains are established in the<br />

personal preferences and inner regulations of individuals.<br />

These values are passes down through the generations thus<br />

using the family as a medium of continuity of the lineage<br />

and preservation of values. When children come of age or<br />

become mature, parents expect them to be settled in<br />

marriage.<br />

Marriage is seen as a legitimate way to procreate and<br />

have socially integrated children (Nsamenang, 2000). These<br />

children are considered as insurance for parents in their old<br />

age. The children will cater for the elders and give them the<br />

necessary comfort in their later lives.<br />

Marriages are also a contract between the families of<br />

the two spouses thus creating a relationship that Filani<br />

(1984) describes as extensive, with social network ramifications.<br />

Choosing a marriage partner takes on varied<br />

forms, with parents playing a vital role. <strong>In</strong> some communities<br />

within Africa, parents indicate the wish to have a wife<br />

or husband for their children from a particular family<br />

background (Mbiti, 1969). Serious investigation is thus<br />

made into the family background of the chosen partner<br />

and into his/her way of life before accepting that person<br />

for their child.<br />

One key factor in African marriages is that marriage<br />

between close relations is not allowed; in most of grassland<br />

Cameroon it is believed that if close relatives marry<br />

they could die as punishment for violating this traditional<br />

rule. Parents in particular and society in general therefore<br />

strive to prevent marriages between close relatives,<br />

especially because the living dead (ancestors) do not<br />

feel happy about such relationships given that incest is<br />

taboo.<br />

<strong>In</strong>vestigating Problems and Coping<br />

Strategies in Cameroon Couples<br />

Based on this complex and cherished traditional background<br />

of African marriage, we designed the current study<br />

to investigate coping strategies in marital relationships. The<br />

specific objectives were to identify sources of marital difficulties<br />

and to determine what sustains marriage or keeps<br />

spouses together despite these problems. Above all, this<br />

research work was meant to provide an insight into marital<br />

life as background information for instituting marital and<br />

family counselling.<br />

We conducted our study in Bamenda, a town in the<br />

North West Province (in an urban setting) in the English<br />

speaking part of Cameroon. This town has a population of<br />

329600 inhabitants (MINPAT, 2000) coming from diverse<br />

ethnic origins of Cameroon. The research was carried out<br />

using a questionnaire survey of 31 items adapted from<br />

those of Filani (1984), Bakare (1970) and Kersten (1990),<br />

enriched by additional relevant literature. It comprised a<br />

demographic section and three sub scales, which were<br />

designed to measure personal data, communication and<br />

attitude to marital life, marital cooperation and disaffection<br />

and finally, marital adjustments. The construct, face and<br />

content validity of this instrument were ascertained with<br />

the assistance of an expert psychologist.<br />

Data was collected from a literate sample of 160 volunteers<br />

(34.4% male and 65.6% female) drawn from Christian<br />

groups and social groups in the town of Bamenda. The age<br />

range of this sample was 61 years with the youngest spouse<br />

being 21years and the oldest 82 years old. Their mean age<br />

was 41 years while their mean duration in the same marital<br />

relationship was 21 years. <strong>Of</strong> the 160 respondents, 86.2% of<br />

them are still in their marital relationship, 6.8% are widows<br />

and 5.4% are separated, while 1.6% are divorced.<br />

The data underscored the existence of marital<br />

problems among couples. The most frequently reported<br />

problems revolved first on disagreement over the use of<br />

finances (58.8%). <strong>In</strong>fidelity (involvement in extra marital<br />

sexual relationship) was second with a percentage rating<br />

of 34.34% followed closely by the intrusion of in-laws<br />

rated at 32.2%. Despite these marital problems, couples<br />

persisted in the union to uphold the traditional precepts<br />

that see marriage as a lifelong contract. Tradition therefore<br />

rejects divorce.<br />

Respondents also acknowledged that they seldom<br />

discussed family issues and would hardly listen to each<br />

other’s point of view, except when it concerned children.<br />

They perceived children as a binding force in marriage and<br />

the main reason for persistence in marital unions and the<br />

continuation of interaction between separated or divorced<br />

partners.<br />

Given the incidence of problems of finance, infidelity,<br />

intrusion of in-laws and poor communication reported in<br />

this study, we can conclude that happiness was limited<br />

amongst respondents. Partners’ disagreement over<br />

finances revealed a spirit of greed and selfishness amongst<br />

them. This could lead to the disintegration of the home.<br />

These problems were probably exacerbated by<br />

traditional Cameroonian belief that the man is the head of<br />

the family and has control over everything within his<br />

family, including the wife’s money. <strong>In</strong>fidelity may have<br />

arisen in part because most partners are not true to<br />

13


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

themselves when it concerns their sexual life. Sex is<br />

considered a taboo topic for discussion in most of our<br />

communities (Nsamenang, 2004) yet there are problems<br />

among spouses that make their sex lives unenjoyable<br />

thereby leading to extramarital relationships. If partners<br />

could feel free towards each other and express their<br />

feelings frankly, a solution could be sought. <strong>In</strong> this way,<br />

infidelity could be checked thus letting out ill feeling and<br />

distress that comes with it. Tradition holds that marriage<br />

is polygamous, but under the influence of Christianity<br />

many people commit to monogamous marriage and then<br />

indulge in secret extramarital relationships. Society sometimes<br />

looks at men in monogamous relationships as being<br />

weak, so some men indulge in extra marital relationships<br />

to prove their manhood and wealth. When people who<br />

live together for life become a nuisance to one another,<br />

their marriage becomes permeated by ill feeling and frustration.<br />

<strong>In</strong> Cameroon, as in the rest of sub-Saharan Africa,<br />

in-laws can help a young couple to establish a loving<br />

marriage or break it down by initiating conflicts based on<br />

jealousy, greed, or other negative traits.. As tradition<br />

demands, parents-in-law should accept their children-inlaw<br />

as members of their family. This will enable the<br />

family union to remain amicable and keep the extended<br />

family strong.<br />

Our results also revealed that communication was poor,<br />

primarily because spouses did not listen to each other.<br />

Communication without listening could be viewed as a<br />

waste of time and expression of the mind. The above<br />

explanation implies that there is good and bad communication<br />

as Wright (1981) says it. Good communication brings<br />

joy and bad communication reduces happiness. When<br />

communication is poor, cooperation within the family will<br />

be lacking. Cooperation shows a spirit of joint effort with<br />

common goals to be achieved. If we could go by the adage<br />

“united we stand, divided we fall” and the marriage principle<br />

of one plus one being one, then we can see that<br />

cooperation is a great component of marriage and family<br />

life. When marital partners are ignorant or neglect the<br />

importance of functioning as one, they will likely not have<br />

time for in-depth family discussion.<br />

African Marriage—A Contract Between<br />

Spousal Families<br />

Respondents in this study cope with marital life and the<br />

above discussed problems because of their respect for and<br />

attachment to traditional norms and the pro-natalist value<br />

of marriage as a lifelong union. During marriage solemnisation<br />

over a calabash of palm wine in the Northwest<br />

Province of Cameroon, the young couple are made to<br />

understand that marriage is permanent before they share a<br />

cup of palm wine. This sharing thus opens the way for all<br />

other family members present to drink the wine, after<br />

emphasising on the permanent nature of the relationship<br />

being created by that rite.<br />

Upholding these traditional precepts is imperative<br />

because it is a deep-seated indigenous norm upon which<br />

the stability of the society and the pride and dignity of its<br />

members depend. By respecting these traditional beliefs<br />

and upholding the sanctity of marriage, one’s personality<br />

and self-esteem are affirmed. Maintaining a marital<br />

relationship is seen as a developmental marker of maturity,<br />

which is indeed fulfilled with the birth and proper care of<br />

one’s children. Those who fail to fulfill their duties after<br />

marriage, or do not marry, bring shame and disgrace to<br />

themselves and their families. Society looks down on them<br />

and many people do not allow their children to marry into<br />

such families because of fears of contamination. When the<br />

marriage of children is threatened by problems of any kind,<br />

parents get in forcibly to bring peace because when those<br />

marriages are shaken it is an indication that their role in<br />

child upbringing and transmission of traditional cultural<br />

values was not efficiently done, indicating a failure on their<br />

part.<br />

Persistence in marriage in spite of problems signifies<br />

people’s ability to endure pain and hardship in order to<br />

uphold the sanctity of marriage, which is seen as the only<br />

acceptable cultural institution for child bearing and child<br />

rearing. By showing an enduring spirit, people further<br />

reveal that they are attached to the principle of moderation<br />

and functioning within one’s circumstances.<br />

The High Value of Children<br />

Children are seen as the major reason for the institution of<br />

marriage. Thus, partners bear all the troubles and difficulties<br />

of marriage for the sake of the children born into that<br />

relationship. It is believed that children brought up in the<br />

absence of one parent as a consequence of divorce will be<br />

maladjusted in one way or another. Children are the fruits<br />

of marriage, and when they cannot be brought up jointly<br />

because of marital problems, the affected parents become a<br />

Figure 1. Spousal Union Figure 2. The value of children<br />

14


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

Figure 3.<br />

disgrace to society. If people fail in child upbringing,<br />

society sees them as failures.<br />

Children are insurance for parents in old age because<br />

they will give aged parents the necessary assistance in<br />

later life: abandoning parents in old age would violate a<br />

fundamental duty. Parents who fail to give their children<br />

the best of education that can enable them earn a living<br />

will have nobody to take proper care of them with love<br />

and concern. Children who marry and have a family<br />

become a source of great pride to their parents; infertility<br />

and childlessness are a loathsome prospect. The children<br />

are said to be a source of continuation of the family lineage<br />

and insurance against permanent death, (Nsamenang,<br />

2004). You are alive even after death because your whole<br />

image and behaviour are reflected in the children you gave<br />

birth to. Since children in this sense will become one’s<br />

history, everybody protects them and will consequently<br />

face any odds to have the children grow within a marital<br />

union. There are, however, single parents who do manage<br />

to bring up their children with utmost care so that they<br />

grow to live by the expectations of society and equally get<br />

to enjoy marital life.<br />

Parts of these findings tie in with those of Soetan (1982)<br />

which revealed that in-laws have an effect on the marital<br />

life of men and women, and those of Filani (1984) which<br />

recommended training skills to foster marital adjustments.<br />

Over and above all, the findings are consistent with the<br />

image of the African family being in a crisis of change<br />

(Weisner, Kilbride, & Bradely, 1997)<br />

Conclusion<br />

Marriage contract<br />

Our study is subjected to some limitations. Firstly, some of<br />

the social groups used for this research work meet in a<br />

common venue once a month, and others on a weekly basis.<br />

This slowed down the data collection process. It was timeconsuming<br />

and financially demanding to get to these<br />

groups several times to get the questionnaire copies from<br />

the respondents. Secondly, only Christians were used for<br />

this study because Moslems, who constitute part of the<br />

population of Cameroon and this research area, received<br />

the questionnaire copies but did not return them, the reason<br />

being that they cannot discuss their marital issues with a<br />

non Moslem. It must be noted that this group of people give<br />

their children in marriage as early as 12 years (girls in<br />

particular).<br />

Finally, only the literate were used for this study in a<br />

region that has illiterates. The use of an interview might<br />

have provided some details that the questionnaire could<br />

not provide, but because we needed to maintain confidentiality<br />

this could not be applied.<br />

Nevertheless, our study revealed a picture of couples in<br />

Cameroon persisting despite marital problems because of<br />

respect for age-old traditions. Though the study did not<br />

explore the effects of marital problems (and related frustration<br />

and stress) on respondents’ psycho-emotional well<br />

being, an association between the two is very likely<br />

(Johnson & Booth, 1990). The nature of the marital<br />

problems identified suggests the need for premarital and<br />

marital counselling. This would enable couples to identify<br />

their areas of weakness in relation to each other, and in the<br />

relationship as a whole. Consequently, adjustments could<br />

be made that may help to create an amicable relationship,<br />

even in times of socio-cultural changes that have led some<br />

Cameroonian couples into divorce and its challenges<br />

within a community that rejects it.<br />

References<br />

Bakare, C.G.M. (1970). Marital Adjustment Scale. Behavioural<br />

Science Research Unit Ibadan Nigeria.<br />

Filani, T.O. (1984). An Experimental Study of Communication<br />

Training and Cognitive Restructuring on Marital Adjustment<br />

of some Couples. Ibadan Nigeria.<br />

Johnson, D.R., & Booth, A. (1990). Rural Economic Decline<br />

and Marital Qualit. A panel study of farm marriage.<br />

Family Relations Journal of Applied Family and Studies,<br />

39(2).<br />

Kersten, K.K. (1990). Family <strong>In</strong>terventions. Family Relations<br />

Journal of Applied Family Studies, 39(3).<br />

Mbiti, J.S. (1969). African Philosophy. Chaucer Press.<br />

MINPAT (2000). Cameroon Census Report by the Ministry of<br />

Plan and Regional Development.<br />

Nsamenang, A.B. (1992). Human Development in Cultural<br />

Context. A Third World Perspective.<br />

Nsamenang, A.B. (2000). Family and Marital Life Counselling.<br />

Lecture Notes: B.U.S.T Bamenda Cameroon.<br />

Nsamenang, A.B. (2004). Family and Marital Life Counselling.<br />

Lecture Notes: B.U.S.T Bamenda Cameroon.<br />

Soetan A. (1982). The Effects <strong>Of</strong> <strong>In</strong>—Laws on The Marital<br />

Adjustment of Couples in Nigeria. University Ibadon<br />

Nigeria.<br />

Weisner, Bradley, & Kilbride (1997). African Families And The<br />

Crisis <strong>Of</strong> Social Change. London.<br />

Wright, H.N. (1981). The Family that Listens. Victor Books.<br />

15


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

COMMENTARY: Adolescent and Young Adult<br />

<strong>Romantic</strong> <strong>Relationships</strong>—The Need for a<br />

Broader Conceptual Perspective<br />

Shmuel Shulman<br />

Department of Psychology, Bar Ilan University<br />

Ramat Gan, Israel<br />

E-mail: Shulman@mail.biu.ac.il<br />

<strong>In</strong> the last decade or so the study of adolescent romantic<br />

relationships has evolved dramatically, shedding light on the<br />

centrality and importance of romance in the lives of adolescents<br />

(Collins, 2003). The main body of research on adolescent<br />

romantic relationships has been conceptualized within a<br />

developmental framework and draws from the study of peer<br />

relationships, which has affected the nature of sampling and<br />

research methodology. The four papers in this issue which focus<br />

on different aspects of romantic relationships and may seem<br />

unrelated to each other point, in my opinion, to the broader<br />

universal features and principles of romantic relationships and<br />

their function for the individual and society.<br />

<strong>In</strong> their paper, Connolly and McIsaac describe how their<br />

interest in the study of romantic relationships grew out of a<br />

research agenda focused on adolescents’ peer relationships<br />

(Furman, Brown, & Feiring, 1999). Within this understanding,<br />

earlier experiences with friends are carried over into, enacted<br />

and operate the emergent romantic relationships. For example,<br />

the capacity to balance the needs of self and other is associated<br />

with intimacy in romantic relationships (Friedlander,<br />

Connolly, Pepler, & Craig, in press). <strong>In</strong> a similar vein, a history of<br />

having been bullied by one’s peer group may create later problematic<br />

conditions for young people’s romantic relationships<br />

(Williams, Connolly, & Cribbie, in press).<br />

However, despite the similarities to friendships, romantic<br />

relationships represent a highly distinctive, novel feature of<br />

adolescent social experience (Giordano, Manning, & Longmore,<br />

2006). For example, in contrast to close friendships that are<br />

more settled, romantic relationships are characterized by<br />

heightened emotionality: love, attraction, sexual desires, and<br />

jealousy (Larson, Clore, & Wood, 1999). Though romantic<br />

relationships are the main relational context of sexual behavior<br />

(Furman & Shaffer-Hand, 2006), the fields of sexuality and<br />

romantic relationships among adolescents have been described<br />

as separate domains (Furman, 2002; Florsheim, 2003). The<br />

separation between romantic relationships and sexual behavior<br />

does not allow for a full understanding of the content and<br />

meaning of romantic relationships; understanding romantic<br />

relationships in a more integrative mode may lead to some new<br />

insights.<br />

<strong>In</strong> her seminal work, Fisher (Fisher, 1998, 2006; Fisher, Aron,<br />

Mashek, Li, & Brown, 2002) differentiated between three types<br />

of love experiences: lust, romantic attraction and romantic<br />

attachment. Lust is characterized by the craving for sexual gratification;<br />

attraction is characterized by elation, heightened energy,<br />

and craving for union with a beloved; and attachment is characterized<br />

by maintenance of proximity, affiliation, and a sense of<br />

security and mutual commitment.<br />

Fisher’s work, conceptualized within an evolutionary framework<br />

(Fisher, 1998, 2006; Fisher et al., 2002) suggests that<br />

humans are programmed to build and maintain mating relationships<br />

for survival and procreation. Contingent upon the biological<br />

network for reproduction, adolescents and young adults,<br />

just like more mature adults, are susceptible to the search for<br />

a partner, the capacity to be emotionally swept away by<br />

romantic love and the desire to invest oneself in maintaining a<br />

relationship (Fisher, 2006, p. 9).<br />

Attraction has evolved to enable individuals to select<br />

preferable potential mating partners and to motivate them to<br />

focus their courtship attention on these individuals, thereby<br />

conserving courtship time and energy (Fisher, 1998; Fisher et<br />

al., 2002). Attraction is characterized by elation, heightened<br />

energy, mood swings, focused attention, obsessive thinking<br />

about a partner and craving for emotional union with him or<br />

her (Aron, Fisher, Mashek, Strong, Li, & Brown, 2005; Fisher et<br />

al., 2002). <strong>In</strong>dividuals reporting feelings of attraction experience<br />

a host of labile psycho-physiological responses such as exhilaration<br />

and euphoria. They often think about the beloved obsessively,<br />

to the extent of “intrusive thinking,” including exclusively<br />

focused attention on the beloved and his/her well-being (Fisher,<br />

1998).<br />

Attachment or companionate love (Fisher, 2006) is characterized<br />

by maintenance of proximity, affiliative gestures, and<br />

expressions of calm and contentedness when in contact with<br />

the long-term mating partner. Similarly, within the framework of<br />

marital relationships and social psychology, adult as well as<br />

adolescent romantic relationships have been conceptualized in<br />

terms of affiliation, intimacy, attachment, and commitment<br />

(Furman, Brown, & Feiring, 1999; Shaver & Hazan, 1988).<br />

<strong>In</strong>timacy leads to a deeper and more committed form of<br />

relationship (Connolly & Goldberg, 1999). <strong>In</strong> turn, the presence<br />

of commitment leads to a long-term relationship, enhancing the<br />

bonding between partners, enabling them to express deeper<br />

levels of caring for each other (Brown, 1999) and forming the<br />

basis for the optimal development of offspring (Fisher, 2006).<br />

Adopting an ethological perspective suggests that romantic<br />

relationships among adolescents are more than simply a different<br />

form of peer relationship. While on the one hand, becoming<br />

attracted romantically, sexually involved, and learning to stay in<br />

a steady relationship are markers of adolescent development<br />

(Brown, 1999; Carver, Joyner, & Udry, 2003; Shulman & Scharf,<br />

2000), these milestones are also central for an adaptive transition<br />

to adulthood aimed at procreation and the well-being of<br />

the next generation. Understanding romantic relationships as<br />

expressions of an ethologically driven process can explain their<br />

intensive dynamics and the importance culture attributes to<br />

them.<br />

A reading of the four papers shows that they did not<br />

emerge solely from a developmental perspective on adolescent<br />

romantic relationships, but also incorporate, in my opinion,<br />

ethological understandings despite the fact that this is not<br />

discussed explicitly. As they outline, Connolly and McIsaac’s<br />

study grew out from a peer relationships agenda and their<br />

paper discusses components such as group dating and the<br />

balancing of connectedness and autonomy in romantic relationships.<br />

Connolly and McIsaac also examine romantic break-ups<br />

and tie them to psychosocial adolescent issues of intimacy and<br />

autonomy. However, despite its association with age related<br />

tasks, relationship termination is a major issue in the study of<br />

romantic relationships, and yet it is less examined in connection<br />

to adolescent friendships.<br />

Negotiating the needs of self and other is an important<br />

adolescent task in relations with parents, friends, and romantic<br />

partners (Shulman & Knafo, 1997). Difficulties in this task may<br />

lead to relationship difficulty or relationship termination. Saini’s<br />

16


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

paper describes another form of relational difficulty—romantic<br />

aggression—and shows the extent to which aggression and<br />

victimization between romantic partners affects individual<br />

mental health. Adolescent and young adult college students<br />

who reported being victimized by their romantic partner were<br />

more likely to report higher levels of anxiety and depression.<br />

From an ethological perspective these two papers focus on<br />

conditions differentiating between adaptive and less adaptive<br />

romantic relationships. During adolescence, individuals are<br />

supposed to learn to cope with the inevitable strains of keeping<br />

a romantic relationship intact (Connolly and McIsaac) and<br />

managing conflicts and aggression (Saini). From an ethological<br />

perspective, acquiring the capability to become involved in a<br />

stable and adaptive romantic relationship is crucial for procreation<br />

and the well-being of the next generation as it is related<br />

to individual functioning within a relationship (Saini—low<br />

depression and anxiety) and to withstanding difficulties that may<br />

lead to a break-up of the relationship (Connolly and McIsaac).<br />

The importance of establishing a long-lasting marital bond<br />

for the sake of reproduction and the well-being of the next<br />

generation is further embodied in Ngoran’s study on marital<br />

relationships in Bamenda Cameroon. It was interesting to learn<br />

how cultural transmission of rules, norms and values teaches<br />

and leads the younger generation starting from early childhood<br />

to adopt the role of husband/wife and of parent. Moreover,<br />

parents are supposed to bear marital and personal difficulties<br />

for the sake of the children born into the marital bond.<br />

Diamond’s paper, though originating in the United States,<br />

does not examine typically researched adolescent romantic<br />

relationships. Yet through the examination of young women<br />

who are highly variable in their sexual identification, Diamond<br />

offers new insights that are relevant for the understanding of<br />

romance across different populations. Diamond describes<br />

patterns of bonds that contain the affective and behavioral<br />

features of romantic relationships at large, such as emotional<br />

passion, exclusivity, physical affection, and sexual behavior. Her<br />

original contribution is the description of passionate friendships<br />

(Diamond, 2000) that resemble the behavior and emotional<br />

intensity of romantic relationships but lack explicit sexual activity<br />

or desire.<br />

Given the existing prejudices against sexual minority<br />

persons and personal struggles with sexual identity, it can be<br />

understood why the intimate relationships of some of these<br />

young women do not include explicit sexual activity or desire.<br />

Returning to Fisher’s ethological model (Fisher, 1998, 2006;<br />

Fisher, Aron, Mashek, Strong, Li, & Brown, 2002) can be helpful<br />

in better understanding the meaning of passionate friendships.<br />

According to Fisher, love experiences consist of lust, romantic<br />

attraction and romantic attachment. Refraining from explicit<br />

sexual activity does not necessarily exclude the additional forms<br />

of love experiences: attraction and attachment. The existence<br />

of the romantic attraction component is what differentiates<br />

between common friendships and passionate friendships<br />

(which are closer to and resemble full romantic relationships).<br />

<strong>In</strong>terestingly, as outlined above, the major body of research<br />

on adolescent romantic relationships was mainly generated<br />

within a developmental and peer relationship perspective.<br />

However, though the majority of adolescents’ sexual behaviors<br />

occur in the context of their romantic relationships (Giordano,<br />

Manning, & Longmore, 2006), the fields of adolescent romantic<br />

relationships and sexual behavior have not been integrated<br />

(Furman, 2002) and authors in one field rarely reference the<br />

body of knowledge in the other field. The research on relationships<br />

treats relationships almost as if they were platonic. <strong>In</strong><br />

contrast, the study of sexual behavior has been conducted<br />

within a health-risk framework and from a deficit model and<br />

rarely considers the relational context in which the sexual<br />

behavior is occurring (Welsh, Rostosky, & Kawaguchi, 2000).<br />

<strong>In</strong>tegration of the developmental and sexual perspectives<br />

on relationships is conducive to a comprehensive understanding<br />

of adolescent romantic relationships and their development.<br />

<strong>In</strong>tegration of the two fields can be helpful for differentiating<br />

between adolescents whose sexual behaviors are expressions<br />

of normal, healthy exploration and those whose expressions of<br />

sexuality are symptomatic of psychological turmoil (Diamond<br />

& Savin-Williams, 2003; Florsheim, 2003; Lefkowitz & Gillen,<br />

2006). A normative perspective examines the meaning of sexual<br />

behaviors, diversity of sexual behaviors (e.g., the importance of<br />

kissing; Welsh, Haugen, Widman, Darling, & Grello, 2005), the<br />

contexts in which they occur, and the individual characteristics<br />

of adolescents that can change the meaning of the sexual<br />

behaviors (Welsh et al., 2000).<br />

<strong>In</strong> two separate studies in the United States and in Israel,<br />

Welsh and her colleagues (Grello, Welsh, Dickson, & Harper,<br />

2003; Grello, Welsh, & Harper, 2006), and we, ourselves,<br />

(Shulman, Weisman, & Schleyer, 2008) examined the interplay<br />

between romantic development and sexual behavior and its<br />

association with depression or problem behavior. By distinguishing<br />

between the varied relational contexts of the sexual<br />

behavior, our findings revealed that sexual behavior within a<br />

steady romantic relationship, representing mastering the capability<br />

to become involved in stable relationships, was not associated<br />

with increased depressive symptoms. However, sexual<br />

behavior within transient romantic encounters, representing a<br />

possible a lag in romantic development, was associated with<br />

increased depressive symptoms.<br />

While Giordano and Furman (Giordano, et al.,, 2006;<br />

Furman & Shaffer-Hand, 2006) called attention to the fact that<br />

adolescent romantic relationships and sexual behavior are not<br />

integrated, little attention has been paid to the role of attraction<br />

in romantic relationships. Though studies within a social<br />

psychology perspective have dealt with and examined romantic<br />

attraction (see for example Hatfield & Sprecher, 1986; Sternberg,<br />

1998), this attraction has, however, been conceptualized<br />

as one form of a romantic experience but has not been seen<br />

as one of the three basic types of romantic relationships. <strong>In</strong> a<br />

forthcoming paper, we (Shulman, Mayes, Cohen, Swain, &<br />

Leckman, in press) examined the interplay between the intensity<br />

of romantic attraction among couples at the early stage of<br />

a relationship and their ability to cope with and resolve<br />

disagreements. Findings suggested that higher levels of romantic<br />

attraction are related to partners’ inclination to downplay their<br />

disagreements and to negotiate their differences less successfully.<br />

A second assessment of these couples after a period of six<br />

weeks showed that where levels of romantic attraction among<br />

romantic partners stayed stable, the partners showed an<br />

increasing ability to recognize and face their disagreements and<br />

that a tendency to negotiate disagreements more effectively<br />

started to emerge. Conceptually, these findings suggest that two<br />

processes operate within a bond between romantic partners.<br />

The first process refers to the attraction or preoccupation<br />

between partners as outlined by Fisher and the second refers<br />

to the quality of conflict negotiation that evolves over time,<br />

representing the movement toward a stable and balanced<br />

relationship. Whereas at the initial stage of a romantic bond the<br />

attraction process overshadows partners’ behavior as theorized<br />

17


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

by Fisher (Fisher, 1998, 2006; Fisher et al., 2002), over time<br />

attachment between couples tends to become more prominent<br />

and is expressed through their increased competence in<br />

negotiating differences that is crucial for relationship stability.<br />

I approached the reading of the four papers from within<br />

the common romantic relationships approach that is embedded<br />

within a developmental and peer relationship framework.<br />

However, after reading them and realizing the unique contribution<br />

of each paper, I realized that a broader framework is<br />

required to put the four papers in context. Rereading them<br />

within Fisher’s ethological perspective points to the behavioral<br />

and emotional intensity of romantic relationships which are part<br />

of a powerful network for reproduction (Fisher, 2006, p. 9) and<br />

for this reason it is evidenced across cultures, adolescence and<br />

young adulthood, and finds unique expression in sexual<br />

minority populations.<br />

References<br />

Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H., & Brown, L.L.<br />

(2005). Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated<br />

with early-stage intense romantic love. Journal of<br />

Neurophysiology, 94, 327–337.<br />

Brown, B.B. (1999). “You’re going out with who?” Peer group<br />

influences on adolescent romantic relationships. <strong>In</strong>: W.<br />

Furman, B. B. Brown & C. Feiring (Eds.), The development of<br />

romantic relationships in adolescence (pp. 291–329). New<br />

York: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Carver, K., Joyner, K., & Udry, J.R. (2003). National estimates of<br />

adolescent romantic relationships. <strong>In</strong> P. Florsheim (Ed.),<br />

Adolescent romantic relations and sexual behavior: Theory,<br />

research, and practical implications (pp. 23–56). Mahawah, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Collins, W.A. (2003). More than myth: The developmental<br />

significance of romantic relationships during adolescence.<br />

Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 1–24.<br />

Connolly, J., & Goldberg, A. (1999). <strong>Romantic</strong> relationships in<br />

adolescence: the role of friends and peers in their emergence<br />

and development. <strong>In</strong> W. Furman, B. B. Brown, &<br />

C. Feiring (Eds.), The development of romantic relationships in<br />

adolescence (pp. 266–290). New York: Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Diamond, L.M. (2000). Passionate friendships among adolescent<br />

sexual-minority women. Journal of Research on Adolescence,<br />

10, 191–209.<br />

Diamond, L.M., & Savin-Williams, R.C. (2003). The intimate<br />

relationships of sexual minority youths. <strong>In</strong> G. Adams &<br />

M. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence<br />

(pp. 393–412). Malden, MA: Blackwell.<br />

Fisher, H.E. (1998). Lust, attraction, and attachment in<br />

mammalian reproduction. Human Nature, 9, 23–52.<br />

Fisher, H.E., Aron, A., Mashek, D., Li, H., & Brown, L.L. (2002).<br />

Defining the brain systems of lust, romantic attraction, and<br />

attachment. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 413–9.<br />

Fisher, H.E. (2006). Broken hearts: The nature and risks of<br />

romantic rejection. <strong>In</strong> A.C. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.),<br />

Romance and sex in adolescence and emerging adulthood:<br />

Risks and opportunities (pp. 3–28). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Florsheim, P. (2003). Adolescent romantic and sexual behavior:<br />

What we know and where we go from here. <strong>In</strong> P. Florsheim<br />

(Ed.) Adolescent romantic relations and sexual behavior:<br />

Theory, research, and practical implications (pp. 371–385).<br />

Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Friedlander, L., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (in press).<br />

Biological, Familial and peer influences on dating in early<br />

adolescence. Archives of Sexual Behavior.<br />

Furman, W., Brown, B.B., & Feiring, C. (Eds.) (1999). The development<br />

of romantic relationships in adolescence. New York:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Furman, W. (2002) The emerging field of adolescent romantic<br />

relationships. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11,<br />

177–180.<br />

Furman, W., & Shaffer-Hand, L. (2006). The slippery nature of<br />

romantic relationships: Issues in definition and differentiation.<br />

<strong>In</strong> A. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds), Romance and sex in<br />

adolescence and emerging adulthood: Risks and opportunities<br />

(pp. 171–178). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Giordano, P.C., Manning, W.D., & Longmore, M.A. (2006).<br />

Adolescent romantic relationships: An emerging portrait of<br />

their nature and developmental significance. <strong>In</strong> A.C. Crouter<br />

& A. Booth (Eds.), Romance and sex in adolescence and<br />

emerging adulthood (pp. 127–150). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Grello, C.M., Welsh, D.P., Dickson, J.W., & Harper, M.S. (2003).<br />

Dating and sexual relationship trajectories and adolescent<br />

functioning. Adolescent & Family Health, 3, 103–112.<br />

Grello, C.M., Welsh, D.P., & Harper, M.S. (2006). No strings<br />

attached: The nature of casual sex in late adolescents. The<br />

Journal of Sex Research, 43, 255–268.<br />

Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1986). Measuring passionate love in<br />

intimate relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 9, 383–410.<br />

Larson, R.W., Clore, G.L., & Wood, G.A. (1999). The emotions<br />

of romantic relationships: Do they wreak havoc on<br />

adolescents? <strong>In</strong> C. Feiring, W. Furman, & B.B. Brown (Eds.)<br />

The development of romantic relationships in adolescence<br />

(pp. 19–49). NY, US: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Lefkowitz, E.S., & Gillen, M.M. (2006). “Sex is just a normal part<br />

of life”: Sexuality in emerging adulthood. <strong>In</strong> J.J. Arnett &<br />

J.L. Tanner (Eds.), Emerging adults in America: Coming of age<br />

in the 21 st century (pp. 235–256). Washington, DC: APA.<br />

Shaver, P.R., & Hazan, C. (1988). A biased overview of the study<br />

of love. Journal of Social and Personal <strong>Relationships</strong>, 5,<br />

473–501.<br />

Shulman, S., & Knafo, D. (1997). Balancing closeness and individuality<br />

in adolescent close relationship. <strong>In</strong>ternational Journal of<br />

Behavioral Development, 21, 687–702.<br />

Shulman, S., Mayes, L.C., Cohen, T., Swain, J.E., & Leckman, J.F. (in<br />

press). <strong>Romantic</strong> attraction and conflict negotiation among<br />

late adolescent and early adult romantic couples. Journal of<br />

Adolescence.<br />

Shulman, S., & Scharf, M. (2000). Adolescent romantic behaviors<br />

and perceptions: Age-related differences and links with<br />

family and peer relationships. Journal of Research on Adolescence,<br />

10, 99–118.<br />

Shulman, S., Waisman, O., & Schleyer, M. (2008). Sexual behavior<br />

and depression: The role of adolescent casual and stable<br />

relationships. <strong>In</strong> S. Shulman (Chair), Adolescent sexual<br />

behavior and depression. Symposium accepted for presentation<br />

at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research<br />

on Adolescence, Chicago, IL.<br />

Sternberg, R.J. (1998). Cupid’s arrow, the course of love through<br />

time. New York: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Welsh, D.P., Rostosky, S.S., & Kawaguchi, M.C. (2000). A normative<br />

perspective of adolescent girls’ developing sexuality. <strong>In</strong><br />

C.B. Travis & J.S. White (Eds.), Sexuality, society, and feminism:<br />

Psychological perspectives on women (pp. 111–140). Washington,<br />

DC: APA.<br />

18


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

Welsh, D.P., Haugen, P.T., Widman, L., Darling, N., & Grello, C.M.<br />

(2005). Kissing is good: A developmental investigation of<br />

sexuality in adolescent romantic couples. Sexuality Research<br />

and Social Policy, 2, 32–41.<br />

Williams, T., Connolly, J., & Cribbie, R. (in press). Light and heavy<br />

heterosexual activities of young Canadian adolescents:<br />

Normative patterns and differential predictors. Journal of<br />

Research on Adolescence.<br />

COMMENTARY: Why is a Cross-Cultural<br />

Perspective on <strong>Romantic</strong> Experiences in<br />

Adolescence Clearly Needed?<br />

<strong>In</strong>ge Seiffge-Krenke<br />

Department of Psychology, University of Mainz<br />

Mainz, Germany<br />

E-mail: seiffge@uni-mainz.de<br />

The scientific study of romantic relations during adolescence<br />

has only recently begun to grow in developmental psychology.<br />

It is not an easy study, as relationships with romantic partners<br />

during the early stages of adolescence are mostly casual, less<br />

intense, and short-lived. Even at later stages, romantic experiences<br />

may assume a variety of forms, ranging from those which<br />

parallel “close friendships” to those typical for “casual dating” or<br />

“exclusive dating” relationships. Sometimes the adolescents<br />

themselves are not sure whether they are involved in a crossgender<br />

friendship or a romantic relationship (Leaper &<br />

Anderson, 1997).<br />

<strong>In</strong> recent years, models of romantic development have been<br />

proposed (Brown, 1999; Connolly & Goldberg, 1999), according<br />

to which relationships progress from a more casual initial<br />

stage towards a more committed affection phase. <strong>In</strong> addition,<br />

studies have supported the idea that adolescent romance<br />

develops in terms of phases that follow a specific sequence with<br />

a different focus on self, and on the impact of both peers and<br />

the romantic partner (Seiffge-Krenke, 2003). Further research<br />

has analyzed the beginnings and endings of romance, nicely<br />

summarized by Jennifer Connolly and Caroline McIsaac in this<br />

newsletter, and informed us that unmet needs that lead to a<br />

break-up may be very similar to the needs that motivate young<br />

people to start a relationship.<br />

Studies placing romantic relationships of adolescents in the<br />

context of other significant relationships at this age, such as<br />

relationships with parents, peers, siblings and close same-sex<br />

friends, are on the increase. However, as Lisa Diamond showed<br />

in this newsletter, it took some time until researchers turned<br />

their attention to romantic development in sexual minority<br />

youth; her contribution underscores the variety of different<br />

types and functions of romantic relationships in this population.<br />

That romantic experiences are linked with different indices<br />

of adaptation is a substantial finding in several studies. <strong>In</strong>creases<br />

in depression, and in externalizing behaviors were found among<br />

youth who dated regularly. It is noteworthy that, in recent<br />

studies, the frequency and impact of aggression in romance and<br />

couple relationship has come into focus. Two contributions in<br />

this newsletter address this topic. <strong>In</strong> research based in <strong>In</strong>dia,<br />

Sunil Saini shows that relationship aggression is linked to a<br />

more negative perception of the relationships and, that males<br />

and females experience equally high levels of relationship<br />

aggression, highlighting the idea that aggression is reciprocal.<br />

Gladys Ngoran’s contribution further elaborates on the “darker<br />

side” of partnerships. It became apparent in her research in<br />

Cameroon that the lack of communication is common for<br />

couples, and that separation is perceived as personal shame and<br />

a problem for the larger community. Her contribution, like that<br />

of Connolly and McIsaac from Canada, shows that it is necessary<br />

to include the cultural background in order to advance our<br />

understanding of romantic relations.<br />

Clearly, we need more studies on adolescent couples from<br />

different ethnic backgrounds. But why is it urgently necessary<br />

to adopt a cultural perspective? <strong>In</strong> Western societies,<br />

adolescents make up a decreasing proportion of the overall<br />

population, whereas an increasing proportion of such youth are<br />

characterized by diversity in ethnic background and greater<br />

poverty. <strong>In</strong> general, culture teaches adolescents about romance,<br />

based on accepted traditions, mores and practices. Culture and<br />

tradition determine the meaning and significance of romantic<br />

experiences, and as a result determine how adolescents behave<br />

in romantic exchanges (Brown et al., 2002). These cultural<br />

differences can be traced into marriage, as the contribution of<br />

Ngoran in this newsletter shows.<br />

There are significant variations in dating and marriage rituals<br />

across different ethnic groups (Kagitcibasi, 1996). There is also<br />

quite high variation in the values and goals of a romance. For<br />

example, the Western and North American worldview places<br />

great value on individual expression, freedom and choice. <strong>In</strong><br />

European countries, the idealization of romance has a long<br />

tradition since medieval times. For the Eastern worldview, what<br />

is best for the social group to which one belongs is more<br />

important than individual choice and freedom. For the African<br />

worldview, spirituality is important, and the focus is on connectedness<br />

to the extended family, as the contribution of Ngoran<br />

underscores. There are different cultural scripts stipulating the<br />

extent to which positive or negative emotions (such as<br />

aggression) are openly expressed. Furthermore, the impact of<br />

close friends and peers on the choice of romantic partner<br />

among teens is more pronounced in Western and North<br />

American cultures, while acceptance and expectations from<br />

parents are more important in other cultures. And there is of<br />

course a high variation in religiosity, parenting practices, and<br />

styles of intimacy in diverse nations which impacts romantic<br />

relationships. Conceptions of romance, behavior and scripts in<br />

romantic encounters are all influenced by the cultural background<br />

of the adolescents.<br />

Because the developmental context of many adolescents in<br />

Western industrialized countries is now characterized by<br />

increasing ethnic diversity, romantic partners not infrequently<br />

come from different cultural backgrounds. Migrant adolescents<br />

may attempt to assimilate into the new culture by having a<br />

romance; the romantic liaison may serve as a way of being<br />

acculturated into the others partner’s group. On the one hand,<br />

cross-ethnic relationships offer a rich opportunity for broadening<br />

relationship experiences. On the other hand, tensions and<br />

conflicts may arise between developing an ethnic identity and<br />

trying to fit into the broader majority culture.<br />

<strong>In</strong>deed, conflicts are an integral part of any relationship, and<br />

it is important to balance both negative, i.e. conflict-related, and<br />

positive relationship qualities in romantic partnerships. Not<br />

much research to date has dealt with specific stressors in<br />

romantic experiences. Longitudinal research on conflict<br />

management (Nieder & Seiffge-Krenke, 2001) revealed a significant<br />

decrease in romantic stress over time; concurrently, the<br />

19


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

quality of romantic relationships changed towards greater<br />

intimacy and affection.<br />

Adolescents from different cultures, for example individualistic<br />

and collectivistic cultures, may experience quite different<br />

types of stressors when attempting to gain emotional<br />

autonomy from parents. <strong>In</strong> addition, the ramifications a<br />

romantic relationship has on close friendships may differ across<br />

cultures. Further, issues of commitment and closeness in the<br />

romantic partnership, as compared to individuation, may be of<br />

different concern for adolescents stemming from different<br />

countries. Finally, the sexual aspect of romantic relations may<br />

cause stress, depending on cultural norms and prescriptions.<br />

<strong>In</strong> a 20-nation study (N=15347, age 12 to 18 years), typical<br />

stressors adolescents experienced in romantic relationships<br />

depending on their cultural background were assessed (Seiffge-<br />

Krenke, 2006). Adolescents filled in questionnaires assessing<br />

recent romantic stressors. According to Brown et al. (2002), the<br />

adolescents from the 17 nations were grouped together<br />

according to regions, including Middle Europe (for example<br />

Germany, Switzerland, the Netherlands, England), Southern<br />

Europe (for example Greece, Italy, France, Spain) Northern<br />

Europe (for example Finland, Norway) Eastern Europe (for<br />

example, Poland, Russia, Estonia, Lithuania) Asia and the far East<br />

(for example, Pakistan, Hong Kong; Korea ) and South America<br />

(for example, Peru, Mexico).<br />

The findings showed that romantic stress was highest<br />

among adolescents from Middle Europe, followed by Southern<br />

Europe, and that adolescents from the Far East, South America<br />

and Eastern Europe named considerably fewer romantic stressors.<br />

Feelings of jealousy and the impact of romance on close<br />

friendships were of great concern for adolescents from Middle<br />

and Southern Europe. Adolescents from South America and<br />

the Far East were mainly stressed by feeling unsure about the<br />

romantic partners’ feelings and by the fear of hurting their<br />

respective partner. Age differences emerged, too, illustrating<br />

that the early phases of romantic encounters were perceived<br />

as more stressful then later stages. Noteworthy, too, were<br />

gender differences, with females worrying more about the<br />

impact of the romance on their close friendships with other<br />

girls, while males were more concerned about not having a<br />

partner. <strong>In</strong>terestingly, differences depending on the marital status<br />

of the parents were also found. Adolescents from two-parent<br />

families were much more concerned about the romantic<br />

relationship than adolescents from single parent families.<br />

Considering the effect size of the findings, the impact of the<br />

culture or region was by far the most important, followed by<br />

the impact of the family status of the adolescent. Age and<br />

gender effects were of minor importance. Taken together, the<br />

study shows that it is important to consider diversity in<br />

romantic relationships, depending on the cultural background<br />

of the adolescents.<br />

Gray and Steinberg (1999) have suggested that the stress<br />

perceived in romantic relationships is only partly related to the<br />

romantic affair as such and that its origin lies in the diverse functions<br />

romantic relationships serve for adolescents. They<br />

proposed that the development of romantic interests is inherently<br />

linked to the task of separation and individuation from the<br />

family. The emergence of romantic activity can be seen as part<br />

of a more general process in the development of emotional<br />

autonomy first taking place in the family context and later on,<br />

as the quality of romances changes from casual to more<br />

committed, in the peer context. Apparently, as the abovementioned<br />

findings illustrate, adolescents from different cultures<br />

showed a different focus on the self, the impact of peers, and<br />

concern for the partner. However, a lot of research is still<br />

necessary to clarify the culture-dependent conceptions of<br />

romance, the different behavioral scripts and the cultural framework<br />

of romantic experience. Also, the developmental<br />

sequence following diverse phases which has been established<br />

for Western industrialized cultures, needs to be validated in<br />

different cross-cultural settings. It is not clear whether the typical<br />

Western sequence from more casual, short-term encounters<br />

to exclusive, long-term and bonded relationships is characteristic<br />

for adolescents living in other cultures. Thus, romantic stress<br />

may not only differ, depending on the phases of romantic<br />

development, but is influenced by the cultural context.<br />

Linking adolescent romantic experiences to the context of<br />

their rapidly changing social world may clarify the outstanding<br />

functions of romance in adolescence as well as the specific<br />

strains a romantic relationship places on those same<br />

adolescents.<br />

References<br />

Brown, B.B. (1999). You’re going out with who? Peer group influences<br />

on adolescent romantic relationships. <strong>In</strong> W. Furman,<br />

B.B. Brown, & C. Feiring, (Eds.), The development of romantic<br />

relationships in adolescence (pp. 291–329). New York:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Brown, B.B., Larson, & Saraswathi, T.S. (2002). The world’s youth.<br />

The adolescence in eight regions of the globe. Cambridge:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Connolly, J.A., & Goldberg, A. (1999). <strong>Romantic</strong> relationships in<br />

adolescence: The role of friends and peers in their emergence.<br />

<strong>In</strong> W. Furman, B.B. Brown, & C. Feiring, (Eds.), The<br />

development of romantic relationships in adolescence<br />

(pp. 266–290). New York: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Kağıtc‚ıbas‚ı, C. (1996). Family and human development across<br />

cultures. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.<br />

Leaper, C., & Anderson, K.J. (1997). Gender development and<br />

heterosexual romantic relationships during adolescence. <strong>In</strong><br />

S. Shulman & W.A. Collins (Eds.), <strong>Romantic</strong> relationships in<br />

adolescence: Developmental perspectives (pp. 85–104). San<br />

Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Nieder, T., & Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2001). Coping with stress in<br />

different phases of romantic development. Journal of<br />

Adolescence, 24, 297–311.<br />

Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2003). Testing theories of romantic development<br />

from adolescence to young adulthood: Evidence of a<br />

developmental sequence. <strong>In</strong>ternational Journal of Behavioral<br />

Development, 27, 519–531.<br />

Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2006). Nach Pisa. Stress in der Schule und mit<br />

den Eltern. Bewältigungskompetenz deutscher Jugendlicher<br />

im internationalen Vergleich. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck &<br />

Ruprecht.<br />

20


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

Reports from the Lab<br />

Studying the Formation of <strong>Romantic</strong><br />

<strong>Relationships</strong>: A Case for Prospective<br />

Online Studies<br />

<strong>In</strong>es Schindler*<br />

Department of Psychology, University of Utah<br />

Salt Lake City, USA<br />

E-mail: ines.schindler@uni-jena.de<br />

A lot of research has focused on the initiation, development,<br />

and dynamics of romantic relationships (cf. Cate &<br />

Lloyd, 1992; Gottman, 1994; Surra, Gray, Cottle, &<br />

Boettcher, 2004). However, one gap in this literature has<br />

remained: We know little about what happens between the<br />

first date and the establishment of a committed relationship<br />

(or failure to do so); and what we do know is mostly based<br />

on retrospective accounts. <strong>In</strong> the following, I will report on<br />

our experiences in the longitudinal Partner and Study<br />

Selection (PASS) Study (conducted by <strong>In</strong>es Schindler,<br />

Cynthia A. Berg, and Christopher P. Fagundes at the<br />

University of Utah, and funded through a research fellowship<br />

and research grant of the German Research Foundation<br />

(DFG) awarded to <strong>In</strong>es Schindler, as part of which<br />

we ventured into this largely uncharted territory of early<br />

relationship formation. With this project we had to face<br />

three major issues that complicate the study of how people<br />

select romantic partners and that may be responsible for the<br />

described dearth of longitudinal research in this area: (1) It<br />

is impossible to know when an individual will start dating<br />

a potential partner; (2) It is imperative to gather data on a<br />

fledgling relationship right after the first few dates, that is,<br />

when the dating partners have not yet decided on the fate<br />

of their relationship; (3) <strong>In</strong>complete data as a result of<br />

missed assessments or participant dropout are unavoidable.<br />

Our way of dealing with these three issues was to<br />

conduct a prospective longitudinal study on partner selection<br />

where we mainly employed online assessments. I will<br />

now share some insights that we gained during the PASS<br />

Study as well as suggestions for future research.<br />

The PASS Study: Design, Procedure, and<br />

Research Questions<br />

Our central aim in conducting the PASS Study was to<br />

investigate the process of selecting a romantic partner<br />

and/or college major longitudinally in a sample of undergraduate<br />

students (University of Utah and Salt Lake<br />

Community College). We recruited 150 heterosexual<br />

participants (53% women) who were not in a committed<br />

relationship (n =125) and/or undecided on their major (n =<br />

55) and followed up on their decision-making process for<br />

the next 5–12 months (depending on when they were<br />

recruited). Participants completed five-day diaries, weekly<br />

*<strong>In</strong>es Schindler is now at the Center for Applied Developmental Science,<br />

Friedrich Schiller University of Jena, Germany.<br />

assessments (partner choice only), and monthly assessments<br />

online, which asked about their perceptions of the<br />

decision to be made (e.g., importance, irreversibility),<br />

decision criteria (e.g., matching interests with partner,<br />

others’ opinion), and ways of making a decision (e.g.,<br />

relying on intuition, rational thought). Whereas the aspect<br />

of the study focused on choice of major started once participants<br />

had been recruited, we had to wait for our participants<br />

to start dating before we could begin with the partner<br />

choice part. As soon as participants indicated that they had<br />

gone on a first or second date and intended to date this<br />

partner again, we had them complete a five-day partner<br />

diary, followed by weekly partner assessments for the next<br />

eight weeks or until they either stopped dating this partner<br />

or had entered into a committed relationship (i.e., dating<br />

seriously and exclusively). If no decision on whether to<br />

commit to this partner or not had been reached within eight<br />

weeks (which only happened in four cases), we followed<br />

up on the dating relationship with monthly assessments.<br />

Once participants had started a committed relationship, we<br />

followed up on this relationship with monthly assessments<br />

(asking about continuation of the relationship, relationship<br />

satisfaction, and perceptions of the partner) until the end of<br />

the study.<br />

The final PASS Study assessments were conducted in<br />

October 2007 and we are currently in the process of analyzing<br />

the longitudinal data. Right now, our analyses focus on<br />

two research questions. First, we investigate how dispositional<br />

attachment to romantic partners influences the<br />

partner choice process and how normative attachment<br />

functions (cf. Hazan & Zeifman, 1994) develop in a new<br />

relationship between the first few dates and up to two years<br />

into the relationship. We found that people who reported a<br />

disposition toward avoidant partner attachment at the<br />

intake assessment were less likely to commit to a romantic<br />

partner during the study (after controlling for age, gender,<br />

time of study participation, desire to form a committed<br />

relationship, number of previous relationships, and selfrated<br />

physical attractiveness; Murdock, Schindler,<br />

Fagundes, & Diamond, 2008). With regard to normative<br />

attachment functions, our preliminary findings clearly indicated<br />

that partner attachment starts developing even before<br />

a relationship is considered as committed.<br />

A second research question focuses on the decisionmaking<br />

process during partner choice. I have conducted<br />

initial analyses of how the perceived attractiveness (ratings<br />

of importance of decision criteria ratings of dating<br />

partner on criteria) of chosen and nonchosen partners<br />

changes before the decision to either commit to this partner<br />

or stop dating is reached. These analyses show that the<br />

experimentally well-established bolstering of preferred<br />

alternatives and denigration of rejected alternatives (for a<br />

review see Brownstein, 2003) also occurs during partner<br />

choice in real life: People who form a committed relationship<br />

increase the perceived attractiveness of their dating<br />

partner prior to commitment and people who stop dating<br />

report decreasing attractiveness of their partner. Future<br />

21


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

analyses will focus on interindividual differences in the<br />

amount of change in perceived attractiveness, how they can<br />

be predicted, and how they relate to satisfaction with one’s<br />

decision.<br />

A Watched Pot Never Boils? How Many<br />

Participants Does It Take to Study<br />

Relationship Formation?<br />

A challenge that arises with the described prospective<br />

approach to studying partner choice is that participants<br />

will not necessarily date or start a committed relationship<br />

within 5–12 months of recruitment. Therefore, it would be<br />

helpful to have some information on the percentage of a<br />

given population who will form a romantic relationship<br />

within a defined time frame. Unfortunately, when we<br />

started the PASS Study, we had no such information available.<br />

Our strategy therefore was to recruit undergraduates<br />

between 18–27 years who indicated that they were looking<br />

for a romantic partner. We assumed that undergraduates of<br />

this age group (who also were about 50% Mormon as is<br />

representative of the Salt Lake City area) would be highly<br />

motivated to date and, as part of the undergraduate experience,<br />

would have plenty of opportunities to meet new<br />

people of the opposite sex. <strong>In</strong> spite of these favorable<br />

conditions, we found that the majority of our participants<br />

were unsuccessful in forming a committed relationship<br />

during the study—and what complicates things even more,<br />

those who entered into a committed relationship did not<br />

necessarily stay in that relationship.<br />

The changes in relationship status that we observed in<br />

the PASS Study between August 2006 and August 2007 are<br />

illustrated in Figure 1. As can be seen in panel A, out of<br />

those 61 women and 64 men who indicated that they were<br />

looking for a romantic partner at the intake assessment,<br />

30% dropped out of the study or did not provide any information<br />

on their dating life. Although we saw a fair amount<br />

of dating activity among those who remained in the study,<br />

34% did not date at all or at least never made it past a first<br />

date (or series of first dates) and 10% dated casually but<br />

never entered into a committed relationship. Finally, 26%<br />

committed to partner at some point in the study. It should<br />

A: Out of those 125 participants who were<br />

initially not committed to a partner . . .<br />

be noted, however, that we studied up to four different<br />

dating partners per participant and that out of those 33<br />

participants who committed to a romantic partner eight<br />

only did so after they had dated other partners casually. It<br />

was also interesting that 12% of the committed relationships<br />

in the PASS Study did not survive for more than six<br />

months (Figure 1, panel B). <strong>In</strong> sum, based on our experience<br />

we would suggest that researchers interested in the<br />

prospective study of romantic relationship development<br />

during young adulthood consider that only about a quarter<br />

of a given sample may actually form a longer-term<br />

committed relationship during the period of observation.<br />

<strong>In</strong> addition to recruiting large samples, researchers may<br />

want to consider other ways of increasing the chances of<br />

observing the formation of relationships. Speed-dating was<br />

recently discovered as a valuable method for studying<br />

romantic attraction (Finkel, Eastwick, & Matthews, 2007)<br />

and should also be a useful tool in studying relationship<br />

development prospectively. <strong>In</strong> our future research, we plan<br />

to include a subsample of participants in a speed-dating<br />

study and follow them up longitudinally for a year to<br />

increase the chances of observing partner selection<br />

processes. This will further increase the likelihood of<br />

having both dating partners in the sample. While we would<br />

have been interested in the perspective of both dating<br />

partners, we decided that trying to recruit their dating<br />

partners through our PASS Study participants would have<br />

created too much of an interference in these fledgling<br />

relationships.<br />

It’s Now or Never! Online Assessments as a<br />

Way to Ease Data Collection<br />

A second challenge that we faced was to ensure that participants<br />

contact us when they start dating and are able to<br />

complete partner assessments in close succession to going<br />

out on a date. We dealt with this problem by using online<br />

data collection methods. We e-mailed our participants once<br />

a month and asked them to fill out college major assessments.<br />

As part of these e-mails, we included a monthly<br />

reminder for participants to please contact us as soon as<br />

they start dating a potential partner. On the day following<br />

B: Out of those 58 participants who already were<br />

in a committed relationship or had started one . . .<br />

2% unknown<br />

26% committed<br />

to a partner<br />

10% dated<br />

casually<br />

30% unknown<br />

34% did not date/<br />

get past 1st date<br />

71% were still<br />

committed to their<br />

partner at the final<br />

assessment<br />

12% broke<br />

up within<br />

6 months<br />

15% broke<br />

up after over<br />

6 months<br />

Figure 1. Changes in relationship status observed during the PASS Study: Formation (panel A) and termination (panel B) of<br />

relationships.<br />

22


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

the date, participants were instructed to go to our webpage<br />

and log on with an individual user-id and password to<br />

complete a partner diary on five consecutive days. The<br />

diary was followed by weekly online assessments. <strong>In</strong><br />

addition to allowing participants easy access to the questionnaires<br />

at their preferred time (some actually did fill out<br />

assessments at 2 am), the online questionnaires enabled us<br />

to realize an individualized schedule for every participant,<br />

include person-specific information (such as the name of<br />

their dating partners), and include a branching structure in<br />

the assessments so that only questions applicable for this<br />

participant were asked. For instance, our study included<br />

four different partner assessments (diary, weekly and<br />

monthly dating assessments, assessment for committed<br />

relationships) and an online database that contained the<br />

information about which assessment is to be displayed for<br />

each participant at his or her next login. The partner questionnaires<br />

always started by asking whether the participant<br />

is still dating a specific partner. <strong>In</strong> case the participant indicated<br />

the he or she is not dating any longer, the assessment<br />

asked about who terminated the relationship and reasons<br />

for its termination instead of how much the participant<br />

would like to commit to this partner.<br />

We found the online assessments to be a great asset to<br />

our study as they were a fast and convenient method for<br />

participants to provide responses and also saved a lot of<br />

time for data entry and correcting inevitable errors in data<br />

entries. By employing user-ids and passwords, we also<br />

made sure that only our participants had access to questionnaires.<br />

However, the downside to employing online<br />

assessments with this level of sophistication is that their<br />

development requires programming skills that researchers<br />

in psychology usually do not have. We greatly benefited<br />

from having a computer professional available in the<br />

psychology department of the University of Utah who<br />

devoted countless hours to programming our online<br />

assessments. Researchers who do not know how to<br />

program in HTML or use java script (or do not have the<br />

time to do so) would be well advised to recruit the help of<br />

someone with these programming skills before attempting<br />

to conduct a study like the PASS Study. While there are<br />

solutions for conducting online research for people without<br />

programming skills (cf. Reynolds, Woods, & Baker, 2007 for<br />

different ways of conducting online research), we doubt<br />

that it would have been possible to create a questionnaire<br />

and database structure like the one we used with these<br />

applications.<br />

Overall, we highly recommend online assessments for<br />

researchers interested in relationship development, especially<br />

when conducting research with an undergraduate<br />

population who usually have access to a computer at home<br />

and/or at school. While this makes completing assessments<br />

more convenient, however, it is still possible to “miss the<br />

action” when waiting for participants to contact the<br />

research team. Even though we sent monthly e-mail<br />

reminders, we still had a few participants who failed to<br />

notify us of a dating partner before the relationship was<br />

committed. <strong>In</strong> our future research, we plan to include<br />

monthly online assessments asking about all dates of the<br />

past month (even if they did not progress past a first date)<br />

to further reduce the likelihood of participants not reporting<br />

on dating relationships.<br />

You Can’t Have It All: Dealing with<br />

<strong>In</strong>complete Data<br />

Even if data collection is conducted online and puts<br />

minimal demands on participants, it is impossible to avoid<br />

some missed assessments or sample attrition—especially if<br />

measurements are conducted once a month or even more<br />

often throughout a year. Most participants in our study had<br />

missed assessments even if they did not drop out of the<br />

study altogether. People got busy with coursework, were<br />

temporarily out of town, or had trouble with a broken<br />

computer. As a result of missed assessments, the different<br />

timing of participants dating, and different durations of<br />

study participation, the number of partner assessments<br />

available for each participant varies widely (from none to<br />

several diaries, weekly assessments, and monthly assessments<br />

for different partners). This would have created a<br />

problem when analyzing these data before full information<br />

maximum likelihood estimation was implemented in<br />

statistical software (e.g., Mplus, HLM) to analyze incomplete<br />

data without either imputing or dropping missing<br />

observations. Currently it is easy to analyze longitudinal<br />

data with different numbers of observations per participant<br />

and model the exact timing and spacing of repeated observations<br />

(for more details see Collins & Sayer, 2001; McArdle<br />

& Nesselroade, 2003; Singer & Willett, 2003). <strong>In</strong> the PASS<br />

Study, we organize our data so as to represent time before<br />

and after making a decision. We model weekly change until<br />

the decision to either commit to this partner or stop dating<br />

is reached. For committed relationships, we model monthly<br />

change across months since the beginning of the relationship.<br />

Thus, owing to current data analysis options, there is<br />

no need to shy away from designs that will most likely lead<br />

to incomplete data and irregular intervals..<br />

References<br />

Brownstein, A.L. (2003). Biased predecision processing.<br />

Psychological Bulletin, 129, 545–568.<br />

Cate, R.M., & Lloyd, S.A. (1992). Courtship. Newbury Park,<br />

CA: Sage.<br />

Collins, L.M., & Sayer, A.G. (Eds.) (2001). New methods for<br />

the analysis of change. Washington, DC: American<br />

Psychological Association.<br />

Finkel, E.J., Eastwick, P.W., & Matthews, J. (2007). Speeddating<br />

as an invaluable tool for studying romantic<br />

attraction: A methodological primer. Personal <strong>Relationships</strong>,<br />

14, 149–166.<br />

Gottman, J.M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship<br />

between marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale,<br />

NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1994). Sex and the psychological<br />

tether. <strong>In</strong> K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.),<br />

Advances in personal relationships: Vol. 5 Attachment<br />

processes in adulthood (pp. 151–177). London: Kingsley.<br />

McArdle, J.J., & Nesselroade, J.R. (2003). Growth curve<br />

analysis in contemporary psychological research. <strong>In</strong><br />

J.A. Schinka & W.F. Velicer (Eds.), Handbook of psychology:<br />

Vol. 2: Research methods in psychology (pp. 447–480).<br />

Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.<br />

Murdock, K.W., Schindler, I., Fagundes, C.P., & Diamond,<br />

L.M. (2008). Maybe it is me? How attachment style and<br />

depressive symptoms predict failed relationship initiation<br />

23


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

and commitment. Poster presented at the 9th Annual<br />

Meeting of the Society for Personality and Social<br />

Psychology, Albuquerque, February 7–9, 2008.<br />

Reynolds, R.A., Woods, R., & Baker, J.D. (Eds.) (2007).<br />

Handbook of research on electronic surveys and measurements.<br />

Hershey, PA: Idea Group Reference/IGI Global.<br />

Singer, J.D., & Willett, J.B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data<br />

analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York:<br />

Oxford University Press.<br />

Surra, C.A., Gray, C.R., Cottle, N., & Boettcher, T.M.J.<br />

(2004). Research on mate selection and premarital<br />

relationships: What do we really know? <strong>In</strong> A.L. Vangelisti<br />

(Ed.), Handbook of family communication (pp. 53–82).<br />

Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Studying the Daily Life of Couples by<br />

Using Mobile Diaries<br />

Kaisa Malinen and Anna Rönkä<br />

Family Research Centre, University of Jyväskylä,<br />

Finland<br />

E-mail: kaisa.malinen@jyu.fi and<br />

anna.ronka@edu.jyu.fi<br />

The Palette study, located in the Family Research Centre at<br />

the University of Jyväskylä, is a research project concentrating<br />

on the everyday life of families with young children. As<br />

an important aspect of family life, the spousal relationship<br />

is studied in the Palette project using methods suitable for<br />

a daily approach. One aim of the project is to develop diary<br />

methods that capture daily family dynamics. Technology<br />

has developed enormously during recent years, offering<br />

researchers several new possibilities for data collection. <strong>In</strong><br />

this article we will demonstrate how we have used mobile<br />

phones as a diary data collection tool in studying daily<br />

family life among Finnish couples.<br />

Daily life of couples<br />

Daily life is one of the new areas of interest in marital and<br />

family research (e.g., Laurenceau & Bolger, 2005). A daily<br />

life approach was put together and named as the Emotion<br />

Transmission Paradigm in Family Research at the end of<br />

the last millennium by Larson and Almeida (1999). <strong>In</strong><br />

marital studies, rather than asking “What are the determinants<br />

of spousal satisfaction?” or “Who will be divorced?”<br />

this new research paradigm sets the following types of<br />

research questions: “What kinds of emotions are triggered<br />

in daily situations between spouses?”, “What are the ‘good,<br />

bad and ugly’ moments for spouses in their everyday life?”<br />

and “Why are there better and worse days and weeks in the<br />

marital relationship?”.<br />

Emotions occupy an important role in research on daily<br />

spousal relationships because they are considered barometers<br />

of daily wellbeing and because of their central role in<br />

families today. Other issues that are commonly studied by<br />

taking a daily approach include the quantity and the<br />

quality of spousal interaction, daily and weekly rhythms,<br />

and daily stressors and coping behaviors. <strong>In</strong> families with<br />

young children, parents divide their time and energy<br />

between many life spheres: children, work, homework,<br />

friends, relatives, hobbies—and the spousal relationship.<br />

Therefore, studies on daily life have focused on time use,<br />

reconciliation in the spousal relationship and working life,<br />

and emotional transmission between different life spheres.<br />

The spousal relationship is a central moderator in this life<br />

palette as it can either facilitate or hinder the quality of<br />

daily life.<br />

A large part of the existing knowledge of the spousal<br />

relationship has been gained through observations and<br />

questionnaires, and time frames of seconds and minutes or<br />

of months and even years have been utilized. As stated by<br />

Larson and Almeida (1999), the study of daily life links<br />

these research traditions together by filling in the missing<br />

time frame of hours and days. Observational studies may<br />

capture the micro aspects of spousal interaction, but the<br />

question of the generalizability of these results to the<br />

natural contexts of couples’ daily lives remains open. On<br />

the other hand, longitudinal studies (or questionnaire<br />

studies in general) may answer questions about the effects<br />

of work stress on spousal satisfaction, but leave open the<br />

question of mechanisms through which these life spheres<br />

interact with each other. Against this background, in the<br />

Palette study diaries were chosen as a tool to examine the<br />

spousal relationship from the viewpoint of everyday life. <strong>In</strong><br />

contrast to observations and questionnaires, diaries permit<br />

the examination of day-to-day events and experiences in<br />

their natural context and enable the effects of retrospection<br />

to be minimized.<br />

Diary method<br />

<strong>In</strong> diary studies, the participants make frequent reports on<br />

the events and experiences of their daily lives. The idea is<br />

to study the phenomenon in question for a limited time<br />

intensively, meaning, for example, several times a day over<br />

the course of a week. The main benefit is to obtain memoryunbiased,<br />

situation-specific information on daily emotions,<br />

interactions and use of time. This kind of approach offers<br />

researchers better possibilities to study causal relations and<br />

rapid changes. The diary method is well suited to marital<br />

research as it enables both members of a relationship to be<br />

studied at the same time, thus allowing their different<br />

voices to be heard.<br />

The wide variety of diary methods available differ in<br />

a number of methodological aspects, such as frequency of<br />

measurement points (ESM, daily diary approach); type of<br />

design (interval-, signal- and event-based); form of diary<br />

questions (e.g., structured, open-ended, daily check-list)<br />

and use of reporting tools (e.g., paper-and-pencil, electronic).<br />

Recently, the diary approach has been intensively<br />

developed (see reviews by Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, 2003;<br />

Christensen, Barrett, Bliss-Moreau, Lebo, & Kaschub, 2003;<br />

Scollon, Kim-Pieto, & Diener, 2003). One of the main areas<br />

of development concerns the tools used in diary reporting.<br />

The traditional paper-and-pencil method has been<br />

replaced or at least complemented by a range of new electronic<br />

tools and innovations such as pagers, telephones<br />

and palmtops (for more on the ongoing debate regarding<br />

“paper or plastic” see Green, Rafaeli, Bolger, Shrout, &<br />

Reis, 2006).<br />

24


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

How did we do it? The mobile diary design<br />

in the Palette study<br />

<strong>In</strong> the Family Research Centre we have developed a diary<br />

method for the study of daily family dynamics in which<br />

traditional paper-and-pencil and a new technology, i.e.<br />

mobile phones, are used side by side. We have used and<br />

tested this format in two family studies: the Pilot study in<br />

2003 and the Palette study in 2006.<br />

The purpose of Palette study, funded by the Academy<br />

of Finland, was to shed light on everyday family life in<br />

working families with young children. <strong>In</strong>tensive diary data<br />

covering a one-week time span was collected at the end of<br />

the year 2006. <strong>In</strong> addition, a larger number of parents (n =<br />

204 mothers and 161 fathers in 208 families) filled in questionnaires<br />

concerning, for example, work, parenting styles<br />

and stress, the marital relationship, child well-being and<br />

temperament and day-care. The diary design involved altogether<br />

107 families, each family consisting of parents (or<br />

stepparents) and one target child. The children were all<br />

drawn from municipal day care centres. Diary data were<br />

collected by using three types of diary: a mobile diary, a<br />

paper-and-pencil diary and a child diary. <strong>In</strong> the case of the<br />

paper-and-pencil diary, parents answered in the traditional<br />

manner the diary questions every evening, which were<br />

given in a separate booklet containing a daily check-list,<br />

questions about use of time, and four open-ended questions<br />

concerning both good and challenging situations<br />

within the family. The mobile diary study involved 42<br />

families and included both parents (or stepparents) from<br />

the family.<br />

The design of our mobile diary was a combination of<br />

the interval- and event-based design—the reporting<br />

occurred at specific time periods in accordance with the<br />

participants’ working schedule (more about the mobile<br />

diary method in Rönkä, Malinen, Lämsä, Kinnunen, &<br />

<strong>To</strong>lvanen, 2007). <strong>In</strong> order to be able to study work-family<br />

interaction (e.g., spillover and crossover) in a relatively<br />

small sample we included in our mobile study only the<br />

families in which both parents worked in regular day shifts.<br />

The one-week diary phase began on Monday morning and<br />

ended on the following Sunday evening. The mobile diary<br />

consisted of ten questions concerning mood and interactions<br />

to be answered three times a day in the form of<br />

short text messages (SMSs) using a mobile phone. As both<br />

parents from 42 families kept mobile diaries for seven days<br />

the mobile diary data comprise a total of over 1600 SMSs<br />

(missing answers excluded). The reporting times and<br />

instructions together with the structured diary questions<br />

were given in a diary booklet. The booklet was designed to<br />

be compact in size so that it could be easily carried by the<br />

participants. The participants sent their answers, a list of 10<br />

numbers, by SMSs. On workdays the reporting time was in<br />

the morning before going to work, in the afternoon and in<br />

the evening before going to bed.<br />

As we were using a technically based data collection<br />

tool and the answers were sent directly to the database, we<br />

were able to react immediately to the responses. The<br />

participants were told that the program would not take<br />

delivery of answers that were not sent within the agreed<br />

times. Moreover, there was no possibility to change the<br />

answers afterwards. The diary program involved several<br />

activities that gave feedback about the transmission of<br />

answers and helped the participants to remember to<br />

answer at the fixed times. They got a reminder SMSs every<br />

morning and a feedback message after a successful transmission<br />

or in the case of a mistake (for example, too many<br />

or too few numbers). The costs of using the mobile diary as<br />

a data collection tool consisted of the fee paid to the information<br />

technology firm and the costs of sending text<br />

messages (approximately 21 SMSs per person; max. 10<br />

cents per SMS). As the participants were carrying their own<br />

mobile phones we did not have to invest in any equipment.<br />

Moreover, owing to the fact that the data went directly to<br />

the database, there were no coding costs, and coding errors<br />

were minimized.<br />

What can a mobile diary study reveal about<br />

spousal relationships?<br />

Given that the participants answered the mobile diary three<br />

times a day for one week and that both spouses rated the<br />

aspects of their relationship, there are several alternative<br />

routes by which to analyze the data. <strong>In</strong> this article we will<br />

demonstrate some of the questions that can be answered on<br />

the basis of diary data, focusing in particular on intimacy<br />

in the spousal relationship. The question, “Please rate how<br />

much (mental and physical) intimacy you felt regarding<br />

your spousal relationship during this morning/evening?”<br />

was asked in the mobile diary every morning and evening<br />

during the week.<br />

How do women and men evaluate intimacy<br />

in their relationship? Do spouses share the<br />

same feelings?<br />

Women and men rated intimacy in their spousal relationship<br />

at the same level. The average of all ratings was just<br />

above the middle of the scale from 1 to 7. Feelings of<br />

intimacy were highly reciprocal between the spouses, as the<br />

high between-spouses correlations showed. <strong>In</strong> Larson’s<br />

and Richards’s (1994) words, the realities of spouses were<br />

shared in this aspect.<br />

Does the time of day or the day of the<br />

week make a difference?<br />

Feelings of intimacy were higher during the weekend than<br />

on weekdays. <strong>In</strong> contrast, intimacy did not vary according<br />

to the time of day. The quality of spousal relationships<br />

seemed to rise on the weekend, a time when couples<br />

usually spend more time together and have more free time.<br />

Is there any between-individuals variation?<br />

Do individuals always feel the same way?<br />

Considerable mean level differences were found between<br />

individuals. For some participants the spousal relationship<br />

seemed to offer more feelings of intimacy than it did for<br />

others. On the other hand, feelings of intimacy seemed to<br />

be highly susceptible to changing circumstances, as there<br />

was considerable fluctuation in the momentary intimacy<br />

scores around each individual’s mean level. That is to say,<br />

intimacy in the spousal relationship is not a stable characteristic<br />

of the relationship; instead, feelings of intimacy<br />

25


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

Women<br />

7 =<br />

Very<br />

much<br />

Men<br />

Mon Tues Wed Thu Fri Sat Sun<br />

Morning<br />

Evening<br />

1 =<br />

Not<br />

at all<br />

Mon Tues Wed Thu Fri Sat Sun<br />

Morning<br />

Evening<br />

Figure 1.<br />

Spousal intimacy during the week<br />

within a relationship are in constant flux. Our preliminary<br />

results show that feelings of intimacy as evaluated in a<br />

mobile diary every morning and evening were related, for<br />

example, to the evaluations of daily satisfaction in the<br />

spousal relationship and success in work-family reconciliation<br />

given during the same evening in the paper-andpencil<br />

diary.<br />

<strong>In</strong> addition to spousal relationships, we are currently<br />

analyzing the daily transmission of mood between work<br />

and family using the mobile. We have found, for example,<br />

that among daily work experiences and moods (e.g., happiness,<br />

efficacy, anger) stress is felt at home in a consistent<br />

way among both women and men. Even though parents<br />

were generally relatively happy and relaxed during the<br />

week, tiredness was common, especially in the evening<br />

time.<br />

Mobile diary—A low threshold tool for<br />

participants and researchers<br />

On the basis of our experiences, the diary method is well<br />

suited to gathering information about the spousal relationship,<br />

which is characterized by constant variation in<br />

emotions and tensions in response to the flow of daily life.<br />

As our results show, the feelings of spouses towards each<br />

other vary widely and thus one challenge of the research is<br />

to find the important factors behind this daily fluctuation<br />

and a basis for more permanent evaluations of the relationship.<br />

As both spouses keep diaries at the same time, the<br />

dynamic nature of the spousal relationships can be<br />

captured in a way which is unreachable with many other<br />

methods. The differences in partners’ reports should not be<br />

seen as a sign of unreliability; on the contrary, these distinctions<br />

mirror the multiple voices in families and in spousal<br />

relationships in particular. A further advantage of the diary<br />

method is that the role of memory bias in reporting can be<br />

minimized since reporting occurs close to actual events and<br />

experiences.<br />

We find many extra benefits to be gained from using a<br />

mobile diary to study the spousal relationship. Sending<br />

answers about intimate feelings as SMSs increases the<br />

perception of privacy and anonymity, as it minimizes the<br />

possibility that others will see them. The mobile diary is a<br />

low threshold tool for both participants and researchers<br />

because of its familiarity and inexpensiveness. <strong>In</strong> comparison<br />

to PDAs—which are commonly used as an electronic<br />

diary tool—the mobile phone is easy to handle and no technical<br />

assistance is usually needed. Like other electronic<br />

tools, the mobile diary meets the criterion of preventing<br />

backfilling. Moreover, researchers are able to check whether<br />

the participants have given their answers during the time<br />

range set (for more on backfilling see Piaseccki, Hufford,<br />

Solhan, & Trull, 2007). Reminders can be given either<br />

beforehand or afterwards (if the participant has not given<br />

his/her answer in time).<br />

During the diary week we had only few minor technical<br />

problems, which did not impinge on the research in a<br />

remarkable way. We think that the main reason for the<br />

absence of technical problems is the familiarity of the<br />

mobile phone to the participants. Moreover, because our<br />

diary program was rarely incompatible with the participants’<br />

mobile phones, few families had to be excluded<br />

from our sample. According to our experiences, the main<br />

limitation of the mobile diary method is the fact that owing<br />

to the SMS format the space for answers is limited. Therefore,<br />

the questions used have to be carefully designed. If<br />

more detailed information is needed—as is usually the<br />

case—the mobile diary should be complemented with<br />

other methods, such as the paper-and-pencil diary and the<br />

questionnaire.<br />

References<br />

Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods:<br />

Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology,<br />

54, 579–616.<br />

26


2008 NEWSLETTER Number 1 Serial No. 53<br />

Christensen, T.C., Barrett, L.F., Bliss-Moreau, E., Lebo, K., &<br />

Kaschub, C. (2003). A Practical guide to experiencesampling<br />

procedures. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4,<br />

53–78.<br />

Green, A.S., Rafaeli, E., Bolger, N., Shrout, P.E., & Reis, H.T.<br />

(2006). Paper or plastic? Data equivalence in paper and<br />

electronic diaries. Psychological Methods, 11, 87–105.<br />

Larson, R.W., & Almeida, D.M. (1999). Emotional transmission<br />

in the daily lives of families: A new paradigm<br />

for studying family process. Journal of Marriage and the<br />

Family, 61, 5–20.<br />

Larson, R., & Richards, M.H. (1994). Divergent realities. The<br />

emotional lives of mothers, fathers, and adolescents. Basic-<br />

Books. A Division of HaperCollinsPublishers.<br />

Laurenceau, J., & Bolger, N. (2005). Using diary methods to<br />

study marital and family processes. Journal of Family<br />

Psychology, 19, 86–97.<br />

Piaseccki, T., Hufford, M.R., Solhan, M., & Trull, T. (2007).<br />

Assessing clients in their natural environments with<br />

electronic diaries: Rationale, benefits, limitations, and<br />

barriers. Psychological Assessment, 19, 25–43.<br />

Rönkä, A., Malinen, K., Lämsä, T., Kinnunen, U., &<br />

<strong>To</strong>lvanen, A. (2007). Capturing daily family dynamics<br />

via text messages: A development of a mobile diary.<br />

Manuscript submitted for publication.<br />

Scollon, C.N., Kim-Prieto, C., & Diener, E. (2003). Experience<br />

sampling: Promises and pitfalls, strengths and<br />

weaknesses. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 5–34.<br />

The Family Research Centre<br />

The Family Research Centre is a multidisciplinary research<br />

centre which promotes and coordinates basic and applied<br />

family research and researcher training nationally and<br />

internationally. Currently the FRC is occupied in organizing<br />

the 4 th international congress of the European Society on<br />

Family Relations (ESFR), which will take place 24–27<br />

September 2008 in Jyväskylä, Finland. More information<br />

about the scientific program of the congress, submission of<br />

abstracts and registration are available at: http://www.jyu.fi/<br />

esfr2008<br />

Editorial<br />

Editor Editor Copy Editing: Typesetting:<br />

Karina Weichold Bonnie L. Barber Lucy Hahn Allset Journals & Books<br />

Correspondence address: ISSBD Newsletter Murdoch University Scarborough, UK<br />

ISSBD Newsletter<br />

School of Psychology<br />

Department of Developmental Murdoch University Production: Printing:<br />

Psychology Perth, Western Australia SAGE Publications Ltd Page Brothers Ltd<br />

CADS—Center for Applied 6150 Australia 1 Oliver’s Yard Norwich, UK<br />

Developmental Science Email: b.barber@murdoch.edu.au 55 City Road<br />

University of Jena<br />

London EC1Y 1SP<br />

Am Steiger 3/Haus 1<br />

D-07743 Jena, Germany<br />

Email: karina.weichold@uni-jena.de<br />

27


<strong>In</strong>ternational Society for the Study of Behavioural Development<br />

Notes from The PresidentP<br />

2008—another terrific biennial ISSBD meeting coming up in<br />

Wuerzburg, Germany! Program Chair Wolfgang Schneider and<br />

his colleagues have done an outstanding job of recruiting distinguished<br />

invited speakers and symposia. The submitted program<br />

promises to be equally interesting and important. The results of<br />

the ISSBD election will be announced at the Business Meeting,<br />

along with other significant plans. View the meeting plans<br />

on the conference website: http://www.issbd2008.de/.<br />

The winning 2010 conference bidders will unveil their plans<br />

for the 2010 meeting at the end of the 2008 meeting. Robert<br />

Serpell and his team from Africa submitted a programmatically<br />

and logistically rigorous proposal for the 2010 meeting in<br />

Lusaka, Zambia that impressed the 2010 selection committee.<br />

This will be a new continent for an ISSBD meeting, providing<br />

new opportunities for ISSBD learning and collaborations. Do<br />

not miss this one!<br />

<strong>In</strong>ternational organizations like ISSBD have become more<br />

important than ever. With globalization and many emergent<br />

trends as background, understanding of human development<br />

worldwide is essential for everything from commerce and trade<br />

to war and human suffering to knowing how best to support<br />

positive development for individuals, their families, and<br />

communities. We have a significant opportunity in ISSBD to<br />

surface—through our meetings and our publications—the most<br />

outstanding and important research to inform these applications<br />

as well as to advance developmental science.<br />

For example, for their 2007 Development Report, the World<br />

Bank focused on youth (This report may be accessed at<br />

http://go.worldbank.org/N17EU24T31.). The annual World<br />

Bank development report highlights the most significant issue<br />

for attention in the global development community. This is an<br />

impressive report, whose approach to programs was based on<br />

understanding of transitions in development. While any of us<br />

might have changed an aspect or two of the report, I was<br />

amazed and pleased by the focus on human development and<br />

the overall soundness of the analysis and recommendations.<br />

Although it is challenging to try to influence policy outcomes,<br />

the impact can be enormous.<br />

From the scientific perspective, an example of an important<br />

emergent trend is the cumulative body of findings coming from<br />

brain research, and specifically the more recent hybrids like<br />

social neuroscience (the topic of one of our 2008 ISSBD preconference<br />

workshops.) <strong>In</strong>creasingly findings are identifying the<br />

wholeness of human functioning, supporting the hypotheses<br />

and previous research of some ISSBD members. Social and<br />

environmental factors influence genes and the brain, and the<br />

brain is the grand integrator of effects on human development.<br />

We live in exciting times. We humans are still seriously<br />

flawed in how we capitalize—or not—on our opportunities. We<br />

can only hope that we will improve our capacity to learn from<br />

science and from our history. I look forward to our exchanges in<br />

Wuerzburg!<br />

Anne C. Petersen, President<br />

MAJOR CONFERENCES AND WORKSHOPS OF INTEREST<br />

2008 July 3–6<br />

2 nd <strong>In</strong>ternational Congress on <strong>In</strong>terpersonal Acceptance<br />

and Rejection<br />

Location: Rethymno, Island of Crete, Greece<br />

Website: www.isipar08.org<br />

2009 April 2–4<br />

Society for Research on Child Development Biennial<br />

Meeting (SRCD)<br />

Location: Denver, Colorado, USA<br />

Website: www.srcd.org<br />

2008 July 13–17<br />

20 th Biennial Meeting of the <strong>In</strong>ternational Society for<br />

the Study of Behavioural Development (ISSBD)<br />

Location: Wuerzburg, Germany<br />

Website: www.issbd2008.de<br />

2008 July 20–25<br />

XXIX <strong>In</strong>ternational Congress of Psychology (ICP)<br />

Location: Berlin, Germany<br />

Website: www.icp2008.de<br />

2009 July 7–10<br />

11 th European Congress of Psychology (ECP)<br />

Location: Oslo, Norway<br />

Website: www.ecp2009.no<br />

2009 August 18–22<br />

European Conference on Developmental Psychology<br />

Location: Vilnius, Lithuania<br />

Website: www.esdp2009.com<br />

2008 July 27–31<br />

XIX <strong>In</strong>ternational Congress of the <strong>In</strong>ternational<br />

Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology (IACCP)<br />

Location: Bremen, Germany<br />

Website: www.iaccp.org<br />

28

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!