26.04.2014 Views

Health Psychology - Cardiff University

Health Psychology - Cardiff University

Health Psychology - Cardiff University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

6 PAYAPROM, BENNETT, ALABASTER, AND TANTIPONG<br />

difference was not significant (89.90% vs. 84.31%, respectively;<br />

2 1.39, df 1, p .23).<br />

Predicting Intentions to Obtain the Flu Vaccine<br />

In order to evaluate the ability of the HAPA model to predict the<br />

end-point of the motivational processes (intentions), change scores<br />

between T1 and T2 were created for each theoretical predictor<br />

variable (risk perception, outcome expectancy, self-efficacy), for<br />

all participants. Pearson correlations were then used to identify the<br />

association between change in each of the independent variables<br />

(and age) and changes in intentions. Changes in risk perceptions,<br />

outcome expectancies, and self-efficacy were significantly associated<br />

with changes in intentions (see Table 2). To examine whether<br />

planning was associated with changes in these mediator variables,<br />

a series of t tests was conducted comparing change scores on each<br />

variable according to whether or not participants had engaged in<br />

planning. These showed planning to be associated with significantly<br />

greater changes over time on all the variables (see Table 2).<br />

A linear regression analysis was then conducted including these<br />

variables. Together they explained 46% of the variance in intentions<br />

(adjusted R 2 .46, F 27.823, p .001). However, only<br />

planning ( 0.17, p .003), outcome expectancy change ( <br />

0.40, p .001), and self-efficacy change in relation to arranging<br />

time and transportation, 0.31, p .001) made significant<br />

contributions to the final equation (see Table 3). Planning may<br />

have contributed to changes in intentions both directly and through<br />

its influence on other variables, and in particular self-efficacy in<br />

arranging time and transport to vaccination. To test this hypothesis,<br />

a test of mediation was conducted using the bootstrap procedure,<br />

with intentions as the dependent variable, planning as the<br />

independent variable, and self-efficacy in arranging time and transport<br />

to vaccination as the mediating variable. As predicted, the<br />

direct effect of planning on intentions remained significant ( <br />

0.483, SE .176, t 2.75, p .006). However, a significant<br />

Table 3<br />

Prediction of Change in Intentions (n 201)<br />

Variable B SE Beta t p<br />

Planning .52 .17 .17 2.96 .003<br />

Change in:<br />

Perceived severity of influenza .00 .02 .01 .15 .878<br />

Outcome expectancy .15 .02 .40 6.45 .001<br />

Self-efficacy in coping with vaccine<br />

side-effects .06 .06 .08 1.03 .304<br />

Self-efficacy for arranging time and<br />

transportation .16 .04 .31 3.995 .001<br />

Note. Adj. R 2 .46, F 27.823, p .001.<br />

mediation effect was found (point estimate .280, 95% bias<br />

corrected CI 0.126–0.510).<br />

Predicting Influenza Vaccination Behavior<br />

A second (logistic) regression, involving all HAPA variables<br />

measured at T2 (perceived risk, perceived severity of influenza,<br />

outcome expectancies, self-efficacy in coping with vaccine side<br />

effects, self-efficacy for arranging time and transportation, intention,<br />

and action planning) explored the ability of the HAPA to<br />

predict behavioral outcomes. Results summarized in Table 4<br />

showed that overall, the model had an adequate fit to the data; the<br />

model yielded a Nagelkerke R 2 of 0.52 and the Hosmer and<br />

Lemeshow was not significant ( 2 15.12, df 8, p .07). The<br />

results revealed a strong association between vaccination outcome<br />

and intention (odds ratio 3.89, p .001) and a smaller, but still<br />

significant, association with self-efficacy for arranging time and<br />

transportation (odds ratio 1.70, p .016). No independent<br />

association between planning and vaccination was found. Mediation<br />

analysis indicated that self-efficacy for arranging time and<br />

Table 2<br />

Pearson’s Correlation Matrix of Change Scores of Social-Cognitive Variables and Between Group T-Tests of Mean Differences<br />

According to Use of Planning<br />

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7<br />

Age 1.00 .03 .11 .04 .07 .10 .13<br />

Changes in:<br />

Perceived risk of developing influenza .03 1.00 .49 .34 .23 .16 .29 <br />

Perceived severity of influenza .11 .49 1.00 .35 .21 .27 .32 <br />

Outcome expectancies .04 .34 .35 1.00 .43 .33 .54 <br />

Self-efficacy in coping with vaccine side-effects .07 .23 .21 .43 1.00 .66 .49 <br />

Self-efficacy for arranging time and transportation .100 .16 .27 .33 .66 1.00 .54 <br />

Intention .13 .29 .32 .54 .49 .54 1.00<br />

Mean (SD) change T1-T2<br />

No plan<br />

Plan<br />

t<br />

p<br />

Perceived risk of developing influenza 1.06 (1.45) 2.05 (1.37) 4.98 0.001<br />

Perceived severity of influenza 4.48 (4.9) 6.37 (4.35) 2.88 0.01<br />

Outcome expectancies 2.03 (3.99) 3.41 (3.83) 2.48 0.014<br />

Self-efficacy in coping with vaccine side-effects 1.10 (2.31) 2.24 (1.76) 3.896 0.001<br />

Self-efficacy for arranging time and transportation 1.00 (2.96) 2.81 (2.45) 4.69 0.001<br />

Intention 1.06 (1.45) 2.05 (1.37) 4.98 0.001<br />

p .05 (two-tailed).<br />

p .01 (two-tailed).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!