03.06.2014 Views

Personality theories of successful aging - University of Florida ...

Personality theories of successful aging - University of Florida ...

Personality theories of successful aging - University of Florida ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Flandbook <strong>of</strong><br />

Gerontology<br />

Evidence-Based Approaches<br />

to Theory, Practice, and Policy<br />

Edited by<br />

fames A. Blackburn/ PhD<br />

Catherine N. Dulmus, PhD<br />

john Wiley & Sons, Inc.<br />

_^'-


This book is printed on acid-free paper. @<br />

Copyright @ 2007 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved.<br />

by<br />

l"Plirlr"9 iohn Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey.<br />

Published simultaneously in Canada.<br />

Wiley Bicentennial Logo: Richard J. Pacifico<br />

No part <strong>of</strong> this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any<br />

form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise,<br />

except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 <strong>of</strong> the 7976 United States Copyrighi Act, without<br />

either the.prior written permission <strong>of</strong> the Publisher, or authorization throu"gh"payment <strong>of</strong> the<br />

fee<br />

3gf.gp]11t".p_".-1opy<br />

to_the Copyright Clearance Center, rnc.,222 Rosew:ood Drive, Danvers,<br />

MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to<br />

the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions liepaitment, John'Wiley &<br />

sons, Inc., 111 River street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-601,1, f ax (201V48-6008, or online it<br />

http: //www.wiley.com/golpermissions.<br />

To out<br />

Limit <strong>of</strong> Liability/Disclaimer <strong>of</strong> Warranty: Whiie the publisher and author have used their best<br />

efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the<br />

accuracy or completeness <strong>of</strong> the contents <strong>of</strong> this book and specifically disclaim anyimplied<br />

warranties <strong>of</strong> merchantability or fitness for a particular puipose. No-warranty may be created or<br />

extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and itrategies contained<br />

herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a pr<strong>of</strong>essionil where<br />

appropriate. Neither the publisher nor author shall be liable for any Ioss bf pr<strong>of</strong>it or any other<br />

commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidenial, consequential, oi other<br />

damages.<br />

This publication is designed to_ provide accurate and authoritative information in regard to the<br />

subject matter covered. It is sold with the understanding that the publisher is not enigaged in<br />

rendering pr<strong>of</strong>essional services. If legal, accounting, medical, psyihological or any oihlr expert<br />

assistance is required, the services <strong>of</strong> a competent pr<strong>of</strong>essionai person ihould be iought.<br />

Designations used by companies to distinguish their products are <strong>of</strong>ten claimed as trademarks.<br />

In all instances where John.Wiley & Sons,lnc. is aware <strong>of</strong> a claim, the product names appear in<br />

initial capital or all capital letters. Readers, however, should contact the appropriate companies<br />

for more complete information regarding trademarks and registration.<br />

For general information on our other products and services please contact our Customer Care<br />

Department within the United states at (800) 762-2974. outside the United states at<br />

(377) 572-3993 or fax (317) 572-4002.<br />

Wiley also publishes its books.in a variety <strong>of</strong> electronic formats. Some content that appears in<br />

print may not be available in electronic books. For more information about Wiley products, visit<br />

our web site at www.wiley.com.<br />

Library <strong>of</strong> Congress Cataloging-in-Publication<br />

Data:<br />

Handbook <strong>of</strong> gerontology : evidence-based approaches to iheory,<br />

practice, and policy / edited by James A. Blackburn, Catherine N.<br />

Dulmus.<br />

P;cm<br />

Includes bibliographical references.<br />

ISBN-13: 978-0-477-77170-8 (cloth : alk. paper)<br />

1. Ceriatrics-Handbooks, manuais, etc. j. Gerontolosv-Hand_<br />

books, manuais, etc. 3. Evidence-based medicine-Hanltooks,<br />

manuals, etc. L Blackburn, James A., ph. D. II. Dulmus.<br />

Catherine N.<br />

1. Aging-physiology.<br />

^[DNLM:<br />

Aging-psychology.<br />

3. Ceriatrics-methods. 4. Health Policy. 5.'Health-services for<br />

the Aged. WT 104 H2343 20071<br />

RC9s2.55.H395 2007<br />

678.97-dc22<br />

2006027674<br />

Printed in the United States <strong>of</strong> America<br />

10987654321


nomic and methodological<br />

anisms ol euerudaV cotnition<br />

'dnmentnls <strong>of</strong> assessment nnd<br />

rehavioral treatments: An<br />

t guide to psrlcltothern1ty and<br />

I Association.<br />

CHAPTER 4<br />

<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong><br />

Successful Aging<br />

NATALIE C. EBNER and ALEXANDRA M, FITEUND<br />

N rHrs cHAprER, we combine an action-theoretical view with a life spandevelopmental<br />

perspective on personality and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> to examine the<br />

role <strong>of</strong> goals and goal-related processes for adaptive development. One <strong>of</strong> the<br />

basic assumptions <strong>of</strong> this integration is that goals and goal-related processes motivate,<br />

organize, and direct behavior and development over time. By setting, pursuing,<br />

and maintaining goals, persons can actively shape their development in<br />

interaction with their given physical, cultural-historical, and social living context.<br />

This renders goals and goal-related processes central to personality and <strong>successful</strong><br />

development across the life span as they constitute a link between the<br />

person and his or her life contexts over time.<br />

We start this chapter by defining the concept <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> deuelopment and<br />

<strong>aging</strong>.Then we outline the concept <strong>of</strong> personality underlying our approach using<br />

McAdams's (1990,1995) model <strong>of</strong> levels <strong>of</strong> personality. This model integrates dispositional<br />

traits, personal concerns and goals, and life narratives as three levels<br />

<strong>of</strong> personality. We discuss the notion <strong>of</strong> goals as personality-in-context by describing<br />

goals as dynamic aspects <strong>of</strong> personality reflecting the interaction <strong>of</strong> a<br />

person with his or her environment over time. Finally, to highlight the role <strong>of</strong><br />

personal goals for <strong>successful</strong> development, we present three prominent conceptual<br />

frameworks <strong>of</strong> developmental regulation: the model <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization,<br />

and compensation (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 7990), the dual-process model <strong>of</strong><br />

coping (BrandtstZidter& Renner, 1990), and the model <strong>of</strong> primary and secondary<br />

control (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995).<br />

WHAT IS SUCCESSFUL DEVELOPMENT?<br />

On a very broad level, current life span-developmental psychology <strong>of</strong>ten describes<br />

adaptive (or <strong>successful</strong>) development as a lifelong process <strong>of</strong> generating<br />

new resources as well as adjusting to physical, social, and psychological changes<br />

with the overall aim to simultaneously maximize developmental gains and


88 EvrpENce-BRSt:Il TuEol


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 89<br />

ensen/ 1996; P. B. Baltes,<br />

Labouvie-Vief, 1981). At<br />

repted set <strong>of</strong> criteria that<br />

laborated by Freund and<br />

lstions regard i ng cri teria<br />

<strong>aging</strong> in particular. P. B.<br />

:cessful <strong>aging</strong> longevity,<br />

:ial competence, procluc-<br />

Similarly, Lawton (1983)<br />

>jective satisfaction with<br />

tppiness or goal achievenotor<br />

behavior, and cogt<br />

as financial or living<br />

ortcomings <strong>of</strong> such lists,<br />

ld be combined, whether<br />

other, and whether the<br />

:ollection to which criteted.<br />

res to defining <strong>successful</strong><br />

characteristics constitut-<br />

; the guiding question <strong>of</strong><br />

(2) process-oriented ap-<br />

:he question What are the<br />

r time? These approaches<br />

sly adapting to changing<br />

rments to fit their needs,<br />

Busch-Rossnagel, 1981).<br />

reeds and capabilities on<br />

:onstraints on the other<br />

Lracterization <strong>of</strong> successspecific,<br />

single criterion.<br />

:-criterion approach that<br />

re only criterion for suc-<br />

'herefore, to understand<br />

in ernpirical studies it is<br />

there is no single def ining<br />

is seen as indicating<br />

d ages well (Neugarten,<br />

rishes between "broad"<br />

mber <strong>of</strong> defining facets.<br />

ng (e.9., Lawton, 7975).<br />

esses the importance <strong>of</strong><br />

tl functioninS;. Her contce,<br />

environmental maspersonal<br />

growth, and<br />

nguish between two dirn<br />

(life satisfaction) and<br />

an emotional dimension (encompassing positive and negative affect; Bradburn,<br />

1969; Campbell, Converse, & Rodgers, 1976; Diener, 1984; Veenhoven, 1991).<br />

These dimensions can be either used in a domain-general way or applied to specific<br />

life domains.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the critiques <strong>of</strong> subjective well-being as the main or even sole criterion<br />

for <strong>successful</strong> development is that it does not explicitly take environmental conditions<br />

into consideration that support <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> or the <strong>successful</strong> interaction<br />

between a person and his or her life context (Havighurst, 1963; Lawton,<br />

1989). A multiple-criteria approach, in contrast, aims at an integration <strong>of</strong> subjective<br />

indicators (e.g., personal life satisfaction and happiness) and more objective<br />

indicators (e.g., everyday instrumental competence). These criteria can be either<br />

short term (e.g., last week's happiness) or long term (e.9., meaning and purpose<br />

in life), domain-specific (e.g., satisfaction with financial situation) or general<br />

(e.g., overall life satisfaction), as well as static and with a definite end point (e.g.,<br />

life satisfaction at a given point in time) or dynamic (e.g., change in life satisfaction<br />

over time; Freund & Riediger, 2003).<br />

Taken together, we distinguish four main approaches to conceptualizing <strong>successful</strong><br />

development and <strong>aging</strong>: criteria-oriented, process-oriented, single-criterion, and<br />

multi-criteria approaches. Table 4.1 summarizes these four outlined approaches.<br />

As we pointed out, there is currently no agreement as to which single criterion<br />

or combination <strong>of</strong> criteria best defines <strong>successful</strong> development. One <strong>of</strong> the reasons<br />

for this lack <strong>of</strong> consensus is that there is no generally agreed-upon model <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong> that could serve as the basis for such a definition.<br />

Instead, there are numerous theoretical approaches to <strong>successful</strong> development<br />

and even more empirical findings pertaining to more or less specific factors<br />

contributing to adaptive development. Integration <strong>of</strong> these findings is difficult<br />

because studies use different indicators for <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>.<br />

It is beyond the scope <strong>of</strong> this chapter to <strong>of</strong>fer a comprehensive, integrative<br />

model <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development. Instead, we aim at approaching the topic<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> on different levels <strong>of</strong> personality, focusing on the level <strong>of</strong><br />

personal goals and taking different criteria and dimensions into account.<br />

Approach<br />

Table 4.1<br />

Approaches to Conceptualizing Successf ul Development and Aging<br />

Aim <strong>of</strong> Approach (Leading Question)<br />

Criteria-oriented Description <strong>of</strong> characteristics constituting success as an outcome<br />

<strong>of</strong> development ("What ls successf ul development?")<br />

Process-oriented Persoective on individuals as in continuous interaction with their<br />

environment ("What are the processes and conditions that foster<br />

developmental success over time?")<br />

Multiple criteria<br />

Single criterion<br />

Integration <strong>of</strong> subjective and objective indicators ("How can<br />

subjective and objective indicators be integrated into one index <strong>of</strong><br />

successf u I development?")<br />

Assessment <strong>of</strong> a person's global or domain-specific subjective wellbeing<br />

("How can subjective well-being as one indicator <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong><br />

development be assessed [general or domain-specif ic, once or<br />

repeated lyl?")


90 EvrppNcE-BesEp Trrsc-rr


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging<br />

9I<br />

,1987,\997) that (a) deind<br />

that developmental<br />

r gains and losses), and<br />

;es and outcomes serve<br />

rproach. This approach<br />

:r adaptive ways (M. M.<br />

re detail, we posit that<br />

ng the processes <strong>of</strong> suc-<br />

)06). Before elaboratino -a)<br />

bed the concept <strong>of</strong> per-<br />

:y in terms <strong>of</strong> three lev<strong>aging</strong>?<br />

To address this<br />

zli ty. F ollow ing Freund<br />

rn personality and sucs<br />

(1995,1996) compristiongl<br />

trnits. Traits are<br />

nonconditional disposhow<br />

considerable sta-<br />

;. Personal goals are a<br />

:rsonality descriptions<br />

ranisms, copi ng stra tertal,<br />

and strategic conre,<br />

place, and/or social<br />

they integrate and inrconstructed<br />

past, per-<br />

IcAdams (2001), most<br />

lives with unity, purolving<br />

life narratives,<br />

lemes. Level lll <strong>of</strong> perll,<br />

political, economic,<br />

oes the life story <strong>of</strong> a<br />

:vident in the notion<br />

n different contexts"<br />

all <strong>of</strong> these life narra-<br />

, points in life, chance<br />

llowing sections elab-<br />

:sonality traits, that is,<br />

and stable across time<br />

n <strong>of</strong> the stability argufor<br />

a s<strong>of</strong>ter version <strong>of</strong><br />

on personality traits<br />

refers to the identification <strong>of</strong> a universal structure <strong>of</strong> personality, individual differences,<br />

and the extent <strong>of</strong> longitudinal stability (Costa& McCrae, 7994,7995).<br />

There is consensus that personality can be well described by the Big Five personality<br />

traits: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and<br />

openness to new experiences. These five factors have been identified by means <strong>of</strong><br />

factor analysis across various instruments and samples. Cross-sectional as well as<br />

longitudinal evidence on the structural invariance <strong>of</strong> the Big Five exists for adulthood<br />

and into old age (Costa& McCrae, 1994; Small , Hertzog, Hultsch, & Dixon,<br />

2003). Moreover, according to a meta-analysis by Roberts and DelVecchio (2000),<br />

the rank-order stability <strong>of</strong> the Big Five increases across the life span. These results<br />

were recently supplemented with regard to old and very old age by longitudinal<br />

<strong>aging</strong> studies (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003; Small et al., 2003).<br />

Contrary to some trait personality theorists' view that personality is "set like<br />

plaster" after the age <strong>of</strong> 30 (Costa& McCrae, 7994), cross-sectional as well as longitudinal<br />

evidence on mean levels <strong>of</strong> personality show changes in some <strong>of</strong> the Big<br />

Five characteristics across adulthood. Neuroticism, for example, decreases across<br />

adulthood (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003) and shows some increase again in very old<br />

age (Small et al., 2003). Moreover, in addition to becoming more neurotic, elderly<br />

people tend to become less open to new experiences and less extraverted (Costa,<br />

Herbst, McCrae, & Siegler, 2000), whereas they tend to become slightly more<br />

agreeable and conscientious (Helson & Kwan, 2000).<br />

Summarizing this empirical evidence based on trait as well as growth models <strong>of</strong><br />

personality clearly suggests that personality development in adulthood and old<br />

age is characterized by both stability and change. Regarding the relationship <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Big Five to subjective indicators <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> well, neuroticism is typically found to be<br />

negatively associated with measures <strong>of</strong> subjective well-being, whereas extraversion<br />

shows a positive relation to well-being. For instance, Isaacowitz and Smith (2003)<br />

demonstrated that in old and very old age (70 to 102 years), neuroticism is linked to<br />

higher negative and lower positive affect. The opposite pattern was found for extraversion<br />

(negative correlation with negative affect, positive correlation with positive<br />

affect). Note, however, that this pattern also holds for younger age groups (e.g., Diener.<br />

1984) and thus does not seem to constitute a deaelopmenfal phenomenon.<br />

Jurrrruc ro LEVEL III: PEnsoNal IpeNtttv<br />

In contrast to dispositional traits, life narratives are heavily influenced by specificities<br />

<strong>of</strong> the context and are subject to change across situations and time.<br />

McAdams (1990) argues that a unified or integrated life story, however, provides<br />

individuals with an identity and allows them to maintain a coherent self-concept<br />

across the entire life span (Cohen, 1998). Life stories are told to different audiences<br />

with different purposes: A life story told to a future spouse might look very<br />

different from the life story told to a future employer, not because the narrator<br />

becomes a different person in between the two events but because different aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> a biography seem important and merit elaboration in these two contexts.<br />

According to McAdams, identity is not derived from one (veridical) life story but<br />

from the very act <strong>of</strong> meaning-making and integration <strong>of</strong> different aspects <strong>of</strong> a life<br />

into one coherent story (albeit possibly a different one each time it is told).<br />

The concept <strong>of</strong> autobiographical memories is closely related to the concept <strong>of</strong><br />

life stories (see also Habermas & Bluck, 2000). Some <strong>theories</strong> on autobiographical


92 EvrpsNcr-BasEpTueoRv<br />

memory (e.g., Pillemer, 7992) hold that it can serve three broad functions: directive<br />

(planning for present and future behaviors), self (self-continuity, psychodynamic<br />

integrity), and social (social bonding, communication; Bluck & Alea, 2002).<br />

The directive function involves using the past during conversations to guide present<br />

and future thought and behavior such as problem solving, developing opinions<br />

and attitudes, or providing flexibility in the construction and updating <strong>of</strong><br />

rules. The self-function provides the ability to support and promote continuity<br />

and development <strong>of</strong> the self and preserves a sense <strong>of</strong> being a coherent person over<br />

time. This function serves to explain oneself to others (and to oneself ). The social<br />

(intimacy or bonding) function, f inally, is important for developing, maintaining,<br />

and nurturing social relationships. This makes us a more believable and persuasive<br />

partner in conversations, provides the listener with information about ourselves,<br />

and allows us to better understand and empathize with others.<br />

Life stories can be related to <strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong> in multiple<br />

ways. As pointed out by Erikson (1968),life review becomes a major developmental<br />

task in old age. This life stage, according to Erikson, is characterized by<br />

the two opposite poles <strong>of</strong> "integrity" and "despair." Following Erikson, wisdom<br />

emerges when people are able to accept both the positive and negative events <strong>of</strong><br />

their lives and do not simply gloss over disappointments and failures but are able<br />

to integrate them in a coherent life story together with positive aspects <strong>of</strong> their<br />

past lives. If one understands <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> as reaching this point, one might<br />

argue that positivity <strong>of</strong> the life story should not be a criterion <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> well. Interestingly,<br />

already at midage, it is the theme <strong>of</strong> redemption (i.e., turning a negative<br />

event into a positive one), not a smooth life path filled with positive events, that<br />

is, for instance, related to generativity (i.e., giving to the next generation in some<br />

way; McAdams,2001) as a possible indicator <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development.<br />

Another argument could be made for continuity <strong>of</strong> the life story as a milestone <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong> (Bluck & Alea, 2002). As it turns out, however,<br />

continuity <strong>of</strong> a life story over time is not necessarily related to positive functioning<br />

in old age. ln a study by Coleman, Ivani-Chalian, and Robinson (1998), a substantial<br />

percentage <strong>of</strong> their sample <strong>of</strong> older adults (> 80 years) did not conceive <strong>of</strong> their life<br />

story as a coherent one, but were not necessarily dissatisfied with their present life.<br />

The specific autobiographical memories that are most accessible at a given time<br />

may largely depend on a person's goals. As goal-relevant information is more important<br />

than information not related to personal goals, it is also more likely to be<br />

activated and accessible at a certain time point (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000).<br />

As we will see, personal goals can change over the life span. As a consequence, a<br />

set <strong>of</strong> memories that was once goal-related and self-defining may no longer be so<br />

at a later point in life (Singer & Salovey, 1993). Abandoned earlier goals, however,<br />

can define one's "past self" and thereby provide a psychological history <strong>of</strong><br />

changes in the self. In this way, changes in goals can contribute to life narratives<br />

("What did I use to want in the past, and how did it change over time?").<br />

People construe their self and their identity partially around their personal<br />

goals because goals can <strong>of</strong>fer a sense <strong>of</strong> purpose and meaning in life (Little, 1989)<br />

and provide the possibility for integration <strong>of</strong> otherwise unconnected behaviors<br />

into one unifying frame (Freund, 2006b). As will be elaborated more in the next<br />

sections, personal goals can therefore be seen as part <strong>of</strong> the binding thread that<br />

links the person with his or her context (Freund, 2006b; Little, 1989; McAdams,<br />

1996). This will then clarify how personal concerns and personal goals as Level II<br />

constructs link Level I and Level III.<br />

PERSC<br />

From early on, I<br />

blocks <strong>of</strong> persona<br />

ality, Allport (193<br />

motives as perso<br />

be divorced fron<br />

conduct employt<br />

have the capacit.<br />

tiate responses a<br />

characteristic <strong>of</strong>,<br />

Personal goals ,<br />

tions <strong>of</strong> states a 1<br />

mons/ 1996). They<br />

(1990) suggests lo<br />

tween "being" (i.r<br />

ioral responses ir<br />

That is, goals are<br />

they may be influr<br />

they motivate, str<br />

ingful action unit<br />

Klinger, 7977) an<br />

(cf. Boesch, 7991;<br />

tions in a given r<br />

witzer & Moskow<br />

reduce situationa<br />

As goals are it<br />

Level II <strong>of</strong> personi<br />

<strong>of</strong> personality-inand<br />

his or her eru<br />

the person's basi<br />

or she lives. Spec<br />

processes that sp<br />

crucial in this re1<br />

in the present con<br />

ger , 2006, for a re<<br />

and underlie goal<br />

DrprNrNc CoNrr><br />

Context can be t<br />

person, such as c<br />

environment. It dr<br />

personality, but al<br />

or geographic con<br />

nities in that they<br />

can express his or<br />

siblings may rend


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 93<br />

broad functions: direclf-continuity,<br />

psychodyon;<br />

Bluck & Alea, 2002).<br />

ersations to guide preslving,<br />

developing opin-<br />

Ltction and updating <strong>of</strong><br />

nd promote continuity<br />

; a coherent person over<br />

I to oneself). The social<br />

:veloping, maintaining,<br />

believable and persua-<br />

.nformation about ourwith<br />

others.<br />

and <strong>aging</strong> in multiple<br />

ts a major developmenn,<br />

is characterized by<br />

wing Erikson, wisdom<br />

and negative events <strong>of</strong><br />

rd failures but are able<br />

rsitive aspects <strong>of</strong> their<br />

;this point, one might<br />

on <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> well. Intert.e.,<br />

turning a negative<br />

h positive events, that<br />

:xt generation in some<br />

levelopment.<br />

story as a milestone <strong>of</strong><br />

it turns out, however,<br />

:o positive functioning<br />

ln (1998), a substantial<br />

rt conceive <strong>of</strong> their life<br />

with their present life.<br />

essible at a given time<br />

formation is more imalso<br />

more likely to be<br />

)leydell-Pearce,<br />

2000).<br />

.. As a consequence/ a<br />

I may no longer be so<br />

rarlier goals, however,<br />

:hological history <strong>of</strong><br />

rute to life narratives<br />

>ver time?").<br />

round their personal<br />

g in life (Little, 1989)<br />

connected behaviors<br />

ted more in the next<br />

rbinding thread that<br />

Itle, 7989; McAdams,<br />

onal goals as Level II<br />

PERSONAL GOALS: PERSONALITY_IN.CONTEXT<br />

From early on, personality psychology has conceptualized goals as building<br />

blocks <strong>of</strong> personality (Freund& Riediger, 2006).In his dynamic theory <strong>of</strong> personality,<br />

Allport (7937, pp.320-321,) regards<br />

motives as personalized systems <strong>of</strong> tensions, in which the core <strong>of</strong> impulse is not tcr<br />

be divorced from the images, ideas <strong>of</strong> goal, past experience, capacities, and style <strong>of</strong><br />

conduct employed in obtaining the goal. Only individualized patterns <strong>of</strong> motives<br />

have the capacity to select stimuli, to control and direct segmental tensions, to initiate<br />

responses and to render them equivalent, in ways that are consistent with, and<br />

characteristic <strong>of</strong>, the person himself.<br />

Personal goals are <strong>of</strong>ten defined as consciously accessible cognitive representations<br />

<strong>of</strong> states a person wants to achieve, maintain, or avoid in the future (Emmons,<br />

1996). They are relatively consistent across situations and over time. Cantor<br />

(1990) suggests locating goals at an intermediate level <strong>of</strong> personality, the level between<br />

"being" G.e., personality traits, basic dispositions) and "doing" (i.e., behavioral<br />

responses in a given situation), a distinction introduced by Allport (7937).<br />

That is, goals are not as broad and comprehensive as personality traits, although<br />

they may be inf luenced by them. They are also not as specific as behaviors. Rather,<br />

they motivate, structure, and direct behavior over time and situations into meaningful<br />

action units (Baumeister, 1991; Emmons, 1986; Ford,7987; Gollwitzer, 1990;<br />

Klinger, 1977) and favor the acquisition and use <strong>of</strong> resources to obtain the goals<br />

(cf. Boesch, 1991; Freund, 2003). Coals can focus attention on those stimuli and actions<br />

in a given situation that are goal-relevant (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Gollwitzer<br />

& Moskowit 2,7996; Kruglanski ,7996; Pervin, 1989). As a consequence, they<br />

reduce situational complexity and facilitate interaction with the environment.<br />

As goals are inherently content- or domain-specific, Little (1989) called this<br />

Level II <strong>of</strong> personality the "personality-in-context." According to Little, the notion<br />

<strong>of</strong> personality-in-context is based on the idea <strong>of</strong> an interaction between the person<br />

and his or her environment over time. Personal goals ref lect the interface between<br />

the person's basic personality traits and the specific environment in which he<br />

or she lives. Specifically, the content a goal refers to as well as the goal-related<br />

processes that specify how people select, pursue, and disengage from goals are<br />

crucial in this regard. Addressing the issue <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong><br />

in the present context, we do not focus on the content <strong>of</strong> goals (see Freund & Riediger,2006,<br />

for a recent overview) but on the motivational processes that accompany<br />

and underlie goal-related processes and their relations to <strong>successful</strong> development.<br />

DpprrvrNc CoNrExr<br />

Context can be broadly defined as the set <strong>of</strong> circumstances that surrounds a<br />

person, such as culture, historical time, family, social relations, and geographical<br />

environment. It does not only serve as a background for the behavioral expression <strong>of</strong><br />

personality, but also plays an important role in shaping personality. Cultural, social,<br />

or geographic conditions may represent limitations as well as chances and opportunities<br />

in that they exclude or allow certain possibilities <strong>of</strong> whether and how a person<br />

can express his or her basic personality traits. For example, growing up with older<br />

siblings may render a person more agreeable because only by being more agreeable


94 EvrpENce-BasepTuEoRv<br />

might the younger child, constantly lagging behind the older siblings regarding verbal,<br />

cognitive, and motor skills, be able to achieve his or her goals. Contextual constraints<br />

fulfill an important function in that they specify the boundaries without<br />

which personality development would be impossible, as the space <strong>of</strong> possible developmental<br />

trajectories would be too vast and unstructured otherwise.<br />

Life span-developmental psychology (P. g. Baltes, 1987, 7997) provides a useful<br />

framework for understanding how person and context interact over time. This theoretical<br />

framework regards development as a lifelong process <strong>of</strong> flexible adaptation<br />

to changes in opportunities and constraints on biological, social, and<br />

psychological levels (P. B. Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, 2006; P. B. Baltes,<br />

Reese, & Lipsitt,1980; Brandtstddter & Lerner, 7999;Bronfenbrenner, l9S8; Lerner<br />

& Busch-Rossnagel, 1981). This assumption has two implications: (1) Individuals<br />

exist in environments that create and define possibilities and limiting bclundaries<br />

for individual pathways; (2) individuals select and create their own living contexts<br />

and thus can play an active part in shaping their lives.<br />

How ro INrrnncr wtrn ONr's CoNlrxr: Goels aNp<br />

Coa l-Rp lerep Pnocnsses<br />

Goals and the basic regulatory processes <strong>of</strong> goal selection, pursuit, and disengagement<br />

are central aspects that help to explain an individual's interaction with his or<br />

her environment (Brandtstddter, 1998; Chapman & Skinner,1985; Emmons, 1986; Freund,<br />

2003, 2006b; Freund & Baltes, 2000; Gollwitzer & Bargh, 1996; Heckhausen/<br />

1999). Goals emerge from and determine the nature <strong>of</strong> people's transactions with the<br />

surrounding world. Experiencing the effects but also the limitations <strong>of</strong> goal-directed<br />

action can feed back to the individual's view <strong>of</strong> the world, the self, personal goals, beliefs<br />

and wishes, and further goal-directed actions. Thus, there is an interplay among<br />

goals, goal-directed actions, and developmental contexts (Brandtstiidter, 1998).<br />

Addressing the contextual nature <strong>of</strong> goals, Freund (2003) distinguishes two interacting<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> representation: social expectations and personal goals. Social<br />

expectations are ref lected in social norms that inform about age-graded opportunity<br />

structures. Age-related social norms can define opportunities and limitations<br />

for developmental trajectories <strong>of</strong> a person's life course. For example, they<br />

indicate the appropriate time to marry and to have children. Thereby, age-related<br />

social expectations or norms can also give an indication about the availability <strong>of</strong><br />

resources in certain life domains for specific age groups (Freund, 2003; Heckhausen,<br />

1999). Setting personal goals in accordance with social norms and expectations<br />

may help to take advantage <strong>of</strong> these available resources. Through social<br />

sanctions as well as approval or disapproval by the society, social expectations<br />

can serve as an orientation or standard for the development, selection, pursuit,<br />

maintenance, and disengagement <strong>of</strong> personal goals (Cantor,7994;Freund,7997;<br />

Nurmi, 1992). Social expectations are also reflected in personal beliefs about the<br />

appropriate timing and sequencing <strong>of</strong> goals, and personal beliefs incorporate individual<br />

values and experiences. Together, social and personal expectations directly<br />

influence behavior and also the personal goals an individual selects and<br />

pursues. This influence can be conscious or unconscious, as, in addition to consciously<br />

represented goals, automatized goals and unconscious motives impact<br />

on behavior and development (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000).<br />

In sum, goals can be viewed as the building blocks <strong>of</strong> personality, person-context<br />

interactions, and lifelong development. The concept <strong>of</strong> personal goals seems particularly<br />

well suited fo<br />

ful <strong>aging</strong> as it allows<br />

furthers our unders<br />

vidual differences ir<br />

particular imPortan<<br />

alternatives and the<br />

goals, also in the fa<br />

prove individual con<br />

for further goal atta.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the basic<br />

developmental Pers<br />

interaction with a 1<br />

actively shaPe their<br />

Ford, 1987; Freund<br />

the person to his or<br />

opment across the I<br />

consider goal selec<br />

goal-related resour<br />

TrtE Rolp oF GoALI<br />

FOR SUCCESSPUT- DE<br />

By nature, human<br />

around the Pursuit<br />

competence, relate<br />

ideal outcomes aga<br />

their progress in tl<br />

ness <strong>of</strong> their goal-r<br />

has shown that Per<br />

Omodei & Wearinl<br />

the mechanisms ul<br />

Processes influenc'<br />

There are differt<br />

<strong>successful</strong> develoP<br />

their goals or reac<br />

1,954; McClelland,<br />

well-being dePend<br />

when these are in<br />

Rogers, 1963; RYan<br />

authors argue tha<br />

(Brunstein, Schult<br />

goals themselves t<br />

related to life sa<br />

Rothermund (2002<br />

argue that neither<br />

<strong>successful</strong> develoP<br />

depression if theY<br />

Thus, setting at<br />

important self-reg<br />

& Renner,7990;C


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 95<br />

:r siblings regarding verrr<br />

goals. Contextual conthe<br />

boundaries without<br />

l space <strong>of</strong> possible develrtherwise.<br />

1997) provides a useful<br />

'ract over time. This thercess<br />

<strong>of</strong> flexible adaptabiological,<br />

social, and<br />

nger,2006; P. B. Baltes,<br />

enbrenner, 1988; Lerner<br />

cations: (1) Individuals<br />

Lnd limiting boundaries<br />

heir own living contexts<br />

pursuit, and disengage-<br />

; interaction with his or<br />

985; Emmons , 1986; Frergh,<br />

7996; Heckhausen,<br />

l's transactions with the<br />

itations <strong>of</strong> goal-directed<br />

r self, personal goals, bere<br />

is an interplay among<br />

ndtstiidter, 1998).<br />

;) distinguishes two inpersonal<br />

goals. Social<br />

tt age-graded opportu-<br />

,ortunities and limitarse.<br />

For example, they<br />

n. Thereby, age-related<br />

bout the availability <strong>of</strong><br />

(Freund, 2003; Hcckrcial<br />

norms and expcc-<br />

)urces. Through social<br />

ty, social expectations<br />

:nt, selection, pursuit,<br />

>r,7994; Freund, 1997;<br />

sonal beliefs about the<br />

beliefs incorporate insonal<br />

expectations dindividual<br />

selects and<br />

as, in addition to conicious<br />

motives impact<br />

rnality, person-context<br />

nalgoalseems partic-<br />

ularly well suited for a developmental approach to personality <strong>theories</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong><br />

<strong>aging</strong> as it allows integrating motivational processes into a life span context and<br />

furthers our understanding <strong>of</strong> both the direction <strong>of</strong> development and <strong>of</strong> interindividual<br />

differences in the leuel <strong>of</strong> functioning in various life domains over time. Of<br />

particular importance in this regard is the selection <strong>of</strong> goals from a pool <strong>of</strong> possible<br />

alternatives and the investment <strong>of</strong> resources into the pursuit and maintenance <strong>of</strong><br />

goals, also in the face <strong>of</strong> setbacks and losses. Goal selection and pursuit can improve<br />

individual competences and generate new resources, which can then be used<br />

for further goal attainment.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the basic assumptions <strong>of</strong> joining an action-theoretical with a life spandevelopmental<br />

perspective in personality <strong>theories</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> is that, in<br />

interaction with a given physical, cultural-historical, and social context, people<br />

actively shape their own developmental context (Brandtstiidter & Lerner, 7999;<br />

Ford, 1987; Freund & Baltes, 2000; Lerner & Busch-Rossnagel, 1981). Goals link<br />

the person to his or her life contexts and thus are central to personality and development<br />

across the life span. In the following, we introduce three approaches that<br />

consider goal selection, initiation <strong>of</strong> goal-related actions, and the investment <strong>of</strong><br />

goal-reiated resources as crucial for achieving desired developmental outcomes.<br />

TuE Rolr or Conr.s aNp GonI-RELATED PRocessgs<br />

roR Succsssr-'u r. Duv pI-oPMENT<br />

By nature, human beings are goal-oriented organisms. They organize their lives<br />

around the pursuit <strong>of</strong> goals that reflect their fundamental needs (e.9., autonomy,<br />

competence, relatedness; Ryan, 1995). Goals provide persons with standards and<br />

ideal outcomes against which they can evaluate their actual level <strong>of</strong> functioning,<br />

their progress in the direction <strong>of</strong> higher levels <strong>of</strong> functioning, and the effectiveness<br />

<strong>of</strong> their goal-related behaviors (Carver & Scheier, 1990). Empirical evidence<br />

has shown that personal goals are positively related to well-being (Emmons,7986;<br />

Omodei & Wearing, 1990; Palys & Little, 1983; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). What are<br />

the mechanisms underlying this relation? That is, how do goals and goal-related<br />

processes inf luence well-being and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>?<br />

There are different views on hor,v goals affect well-being. Tt is <strong>of</strong>ten argued that<br />

<strong>successful</strong> development implies that individuals succeed in progressing toward<br />

their goals or reaching desired states (M. M. Baltes & Carstensen, 1996; Maslow,<br />

1954; McClelland, 1987). Humanistic psychology proposes that a person's sense <strong>of</strong><br />

well-being depends on the person's progress toward his or her goals, especially<br />

when these are in accord with "organismic" or "innate" needs (Maslow, 1954;<br />

Rogers, 1963; Ryan, 1995; see also Brunstein, Schultheiss, & Crdssman, 1998). Other<br />

authors argue that having goals can in itself be a predictor <strong>of</strong> life satisfaction<br />

(Brunstein, Schultheiss, & Maier, 1999; Emmons, 7996). Still others posit that not<br />

goals themselves but rather the al'ailability <strong>of</strong> goal-related resources is positively<br />

related to life satisfaction (Diener & Fujita, 1995). Finally, Brandtstddter and<br />

Rothermund (2002) maintain that goals can ambivalently affect well-being. They<br />

argue that neither goals nor resources themselves bring about goal attainment or<br />

<strong>successful</strong> development. In fact, unattainable goals can lead to dissatisfaction and<br />

depression if they are not abandoned or readjusted.<br />

Thus, setting and pursuing challenging but attainable goals and standards are<br />

important self-regulatory processes conducive to <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> (Brandtsttidter<br />

& Renner, 1990; Carver & Scheier, 7981,7982; Freund & Baltes,2000; Heckhausen,


96 EvrppNcr:-BesppTueorrv<br />

1999). In addition, diseng<strong>aging</strong> from goals that are no longer attainable, restructuring<br />

goals, and reallocating goal-related resources into alternative goals and<br />

goal domains are facets <strong>of</strong> adaptive functioning (Klinger, 1975; Wrosch & Heckhausen,1999).<br />

To elaborate further on the idea that personal goals and goal-related processes<br />

are central for <strong>successful</strong> development and to understand better the underlying<br />

mechanisms <strong>of</strong> the relationship between goals and well-being, we introduce three<br />

current self-regulatory <strong>theories</strong> <strong>of</strong> adaptive development in the following part <strong>of</strong><br />

this chapter. All three <strong>theories</strong> view development as multidirectional, comprising<br />

gains and losses across the entire life span. They refer to personal goals and basic<br />

goal-related processes such as setting, pursuing, reformulating, and diseng<strong>aging</strong><br />

from goals as means to <strong>successful</strong>ly shape individual development. All three<br />

frameworks converge in their assumption that resources are Iimited throughout<br />

life, and increasingly so in old age, and that <strong>successful</strong> developmental regulation<br />

requires suitable mechanisms for allocation <strong>of</strong> these limited resources. The three<br />

<strong>theories</strong> vary, however, in their particular focus (e.g., on life span strategies and<br />

personal goals, coping processes, and control processes), in their particular characteristics<br />

<strong>of</strong> the proposed mechanism <strong>of</strong> resource allocation, as well as in their<br />

postulated generality. Table 4.2 summarizes the three theoretical frameworks.<br />

Table 4.2<br />

Goal-Related Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging<br />

Theoretical Framework Central Strateoies Specif ic Examples<br />

Model <strong>of</strong> Selection,<br />

Optimization, and<br />

Compensation<br />

Dual-Process Model <strong>of</strong><br />

Assimilative and<br />

Accommodative Coping<br />

Model <strong>of</strong> Optimization in<br />

Primary and Secondary<br />

Control<br />

Selection<br />

Optimization<br />

Compensation<br />

Assimilative coping<br />

Accommodative coping<br />

Selective primary<br />

control<br />

Compensatory primary<br />

control<br />

Selective secondary<br />

control<br />

Compensatory<br />

secondary control<br />

Focusing on Spanish as a foreign<br />

ranguage<br />

Investing more time to improve<br />

Spanish-language skills<br />

Hiring a translator when traveling<br />

in Spain<br />

Altering dietary habits to attain<br />

the desired bodily appearance<br />

Accepting your physical state and<br />

giving up the goal to run the<br />

marathon after knee surgery<br />

Seeing each other more f requently<br />

to improve the relationship<br />

Seeing a marriage guidance counselor<br />

to improve the relationship<br />

Regarding the current partner as<br />

the "only love <strong>of</strong> life"<br />

Regarding the former partner as<br />

a "wrong choice" af ter the<br />

relationship ends<br />

TrrsoRrrtcel<br />

THE }<br />

A<br />

MI<br />

UNDE<br />

Assu<br />

The model <strong>of</strong> selec<br />

eral, metatheoretir<br />

P. B. Baltes,7997;P<br />

It assumes a dYna<br />

stages <strong>of</strong> life (chik<br />

different levels <strong>of</strong> I<br />

ferent domains <strong>of</strong><br />

regulation). In the<br />

source generation<br />

processes bY addr<br />

tioning. As such, S<br />

that helP to manal<br />

ory proPoses thre<br />

ment: selection, opt<br />

have various Poss<br />

(e.g., cognition, sc<br />

vary along the <<br />

unconscious (Fret<br />

At the most ger<br />

ization or canaliz<br />

Selection includes<br />

ternative develoP<br />

limited resource<br />

through this conc<br />

evolve. Thus, sel<br />

sources (P. B. Bal<br />

losses in resource<br />

Optimizatron rc<br />

nance <strong>of</strong> internal<br />

els <strong>of</strong> functionir<br />

achieving higher<br />

pertise. ExPertis<br />

achieved onlY tt<br />

Krampe, & Tesch<br />

ComPensatrcnr<br />

functions in Prer<br />

gains nnd losses i<br />

ful <strong>aging</strong> needs<br />

model does this I<br />

encountering a l<<br />

in the face <strong>of</strong> lost<br />

The SOC thec<br />

tives, such as soc


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 97<br />

rger attainable, restruc,<br />

o alternative goals and<br />

1975; Wrosch & Heck-<br />

I goal-related processes<br />

I better the underlying<br />

ing, we introduce three<br />

n the following part <strong>of</strong><br />

Cirectional, comprising<br />

ersonal goals and basic<br />

ating, and diseng<strong>aging</strong><br />

levelopment. All three<br />

rre limited throughout<br />

relopmental regulation<br />

ld resources. The three<br />

ife span strategies and<br />

r their particular charion,<br />

as well as in their<br />

retical frameworks.<br />

'lg<br />

lecific Examples<br />

on Spanish as a foreign<br />

Trore time to improve<br />

rnguage skills<br />

anslator when traveling<br />

etary habits to attain<br />

1 bodily appearance<br />

your physical state and<br />

he goal to run the<br />

rfter knee surgery<br />

;h other more f requently<br />

the relationship<br />

larriage guidance coun-<br />

)rove the relationship<br />

the current partner as<br />

rve <strong>of</strong> life"<br />

the former partner as<br />

hoice" af ter the<br />

) ends<br />

THE MODEL OF SELECTION, OPTIMIZATION,<br />

AND COMPENSATION: A GENERAL,<br />

METATHEORETICAL FRAMEWORK FOR<br />

UNDERSTANDING ADAPTIVE DEVELOPMENT<br />

TutonprrceL Assu M pt'toNS<br />

The model <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization, and compensation (SOC) constitutes a general,<br />

metatheoretical framework for understanding human development (e.9.,<br />

P. B. Baltes, 1997;P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 7980,7990; P. B. Baltes, Freund, & Li,2005).<br />

It assumes a dynamic between developmental gains and losses across various<br />

stages <strong>of</strong> life (childhood, adolescence, adulthood, old age) that is represented at<br />

different levels <strong>of</strong> analysis (e.g., neuronal, behavioral) and within and across different<br />

domains <strong>of</strong> functioning (e.g., physical or cognitive development, emotion<br />

regulation). In the context <strong>of</strong> SOC theory, life span development is a process <strong>of</strong> resource<br />

generation and regulation. This theory describes general developmental<br />

processes by addressing both the direction <strong>of</strong> development and the leael <strong>of</strong> functioning.<br />

As such, SOC is a model <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development, specifying processes<br />

that help to manage changing resources over the life span. In particular, the theory<br />

proposes three fundamental and universal processes <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development:<br />

selection, optirnization, and compensatiotr. All three processes are proposed to<br />

have various possible phenotypic realizations depending on functional domains<br />

(e.g., cognition, social), sociocultural contexts, and person-specific features that<br />

vary along the dimensions active-passive, internal-external, and consciousunconscious<br />

(Freund, Li, & Baltes,7999).<br />

At the most general level <strong>of</strong> definition, selection refers to the process <strong>of</strong> specialization<br />

or canalization <strong>of</strong> a particular pathway or set <strong>of</strong> pathways <strong>of</strong> development.<br />

Selection includes the delineating and narrowing down <strong>of</strong> a range <strong>of</strong> possible alternative<br />

developmental trajectories. Selection also serves the management <strong>of</strong><br />

limited resources by concentrating resources on delineated domains. Only<br />

through this concentration can specification occur and certain skills and abilities<br />

evolve. Thus, selection is a general-purpose mechanism to generate new resources<br />

(P. B. Baltes et al., 2005). Selection can occur electiaely or as a response to<br />

losses in resources (i.e., Ioss-based).<br />

Optinizatiot'L refers to the acquisition, application, coordination, and maintenance<br />

<strong>of</strong> internal and external resources (means) involved in attaining higher levels<br />

<strong>of</strong> functioning (Freund & Baltes, 2000). Optimization is important for<br />

achieving higher levels <strong>of</strong> functioning, as has been shown in the literature on expertise.<br />

Expertise, a high skill level in a selected functional domain, can be<br />

achieved only through a substantial amount <strong>of</strong> deliberate practice (Ericsson,<br />

Krampe, & Tesch-Romer,7993; Krampe & Baltes,2003).<br />

Compensntion refers to the investment <strong>of</strong> means to avoid or counteract losses in<br />

functions in previously available resources. Given that development entails both<br />

gains and losses across the entire life span (P. B. Baltes , 7987), a model <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong><br />

<strong>aging</strong> needs to take processes <strong>of</strong> man<strong>aging</strong> losses into account. The SOC<br />

model does this by including loss-based selection (referring to reorientation after<br />

encountering a loss) and compensation (aimed at the maintenance <strong>of</strong> functioning<br />

in the face <strong>of</strong> losses).<br />

The SOC theory can be approached from many different theoretical perspectives,<br />

such as social, behavioral-learning, cognitive, and neuropsychological (M. M.


98 EvropNcE-BesppTHectnv<br />

Baltes & Carstensen,1996; P. B. Baltes et a1.,2005; P. B. Baltes & Singer,2001). Freund<br />

and Baltes (2000) propose an action-theoretical conceptualization <strong>of</strong> SOC. This<br />

approach embeds SOC in the context <strong>of</strong> active life-management and refers to developmental<br />

regulation through the selection, pursuit, and maintenance <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

goals.<br />

From an action-theoretical perspective, selectiotr denotes processes related to<br />

goal setting. Electiae selection refers to developing and choosing a subset <strong>of</strong> goals<br />

out <strong>of</strong> multiple options. Given the finite nature <strong>of</strong> resources, selection needs to<br />

take place to realize developmental potentials. Loss-based selection refers to adapting<br />

existing or developing new goals to fit losses in resources. Restructuring<br />

one's goal hierarchy by exclusively focusing on the most important goal, Iowering<br />

one's aspiration level, or finding a substitution for unattainable goals are possible<br />

manifestations. From an action-theoretical perspective, optimization and compensation<br />

refer to processes <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit, that is, the investment <strong>of</strong> resources<br />

into obtaining and refining goal-relevant means. Optimization refers to means for<br />

achieving desired outcomes and aims at coordination, acquisition, and generation<br />

<strong>of</strong> new resources or skills, serving the growth aspect <strong>of</strong> development. Compensation,<br />

from an action-theoretical perspective, addresses the regulation <strong>of</strong> loss or<br />

decline in means and goal-relevant resources that threatens the maintenance <strong>of</strong> a<br />

given level <strong>of</strong> functioning. When previous goal-relevant means are no longer<br />

available, compensation can involve substitution <strong>of</strong> lost means. It thus denotes a<br />

strategy for man<strong>aging</strong> and counteracting losses by changing goal-relevant means<br />

to attain a certain goal instead <strong>of</strong> choosing another goal (as in loss-based selection).<br />

The use <strong>of</strong> hearing aids to counteract hearing losses is one example. Phenotypically,<br />

the strategies used for optimization and compensation, such as<br />

acquisition and practice <strong>of</strong> skills, energy and time investment, or activation <strong>of</strong><br />

new or unused resources, can overlap.<br />

The theory states that the use <strong>of</strong> SOC strategies changes over the life span as a<br />

function <strong>of</strong> available resources. The expected peak <strong>of</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> all three regulatory<br />

principles is in adulthood, with elective selection and compensation increasing<br />

in importance with advancing age when resource losses become more<br />

salient (Freund & Baltes, 2002b).<br />

Aoaplrve DEvplopvgNr as ENpoRSEMENT oF THE Mopal ol Snr-ecrroN.<br />

OptrHarzarroN, ANI) Col,rrnxsa'noN Tr{Roucuout LrrE<br />

Several studies provide evidence for the role <strong>of</strong> SOC in the description, explanation,<br />

and modification <strong>of</strong> development and support their impact on adaptive lifemanagement<br />

(see P. B. Baltes et aL.,2005, for an overview). Evidence suggests that<br />

the relative prevalence <strong>of</strong> using and coordinating SOC changes with age and that<br />

people who engage in SOC behaviors show more positive outcomes. The existing<br />

research comprises different empirical specifications referring to global levels<br />

and specific life contexts. It encompasses different assessment levels such as selfreport<br />

measures (P. B. Baltes, Baltes, Freund, & Lang, 1999), behavior observation<br />

(".9., M. M. Baltes, 7996), and experimental studies (e.g., Li, Lindenberger, Freund,<br />

& Baltes, 2001). Some approaches simultaneously investigate all three components<br />

<strong>of</strong> SOC as well as their orchestration (e.g., Riediger, Li, & Lindenberger,<br />

2006), whereas others focus on some <strong>of</strong> the processes only (e.g., Ebner, Freund, &<br />

Baltes, 2006; Freund, 2006a).<br />

Addressing the c<br />

edge <strong>of</strong> SOC as stri<br />

identified a bodY o<br />

are represented in<br />

human life as exprr<br />

proverb "You can't<br />

proverbs were use(<br />

tions <strong>of</strong>, or preferet<br />

SOC model. In a <<br />

SOC-related and a<br />

life-management si<br />

more frequently ar<br />

theory, these PrefeI<br />

in tasks referring tr<br />

leisure. This sugge<br />

SOC-related prove<br />

and that their judgt<br />

in line with what tl<br />

To investigate tl<br />

adaptive life-manal<br />

the SOC questionr<br />

(2002b) investigate<br />

adults and found I<br />

haviors reported t<br />

being. The associ<br />

variables such as tt<br />

teristics such as ne<br />

expected, SOC-rel<br />

hood; that is, mit<br />

younger and older<br />

positive age trend.<br />

less use <strong>of</strong> oPtimiz<br />

mization and com<br />

strategies are effor<br />

sources available il<br />

ple suffering from<br />

SOC questionnairr<br />

(1998) showed thal<br />

adults betweenT2<br />

strategies also shc<br />

tive affect, satisfac<br />

Wiese and collt<br />

model to goal pu<br />

questionnaire stu<br />

haviors <strong>of</strong> younge<br />

specifically. High<br />

strategies was Pot<br />

nal data replicate<br />

ment in SOC beh;


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successfttl Aging 99<br />

Ites & Singer,2001). Frertualization<br />

<strong>of</strong> SOC. Th is<br />

ment and refers to develmaintenance<br />

<strong>of</strong> personal<br />

tes processes related to<br />

losing a subset <strong>of</strong> goals<br />

rces, selection needs rcr<br />

selection refers to adapt-<br />

)sources. Restructuring<br />

nportant goal, lowering<br />

nable goals are possible<br />

optimization and comnvestment<br />

<strong>of</strong> resources<br />

tion refers to means for<br />

risition, and generation<br />

levelopment. Compensa-<br />

Le regulation <strong>of</strong> loss or<br />

ts the maintenance <strong>of</strong> a<br />

: means are no longer<br />

eans. It thus denotes a<br />

rg goal-relevant means<br />

fas in loss-based selecis<br />

one example. Phenormpensation,<br />

such as<br />

:ment, or activation <strong>of</strong><br />

i over the life span as a<br />

ession <strong>of</strong> all three regand<br />

compensation ine<br />

losses become more<br />

,F SELECTION,<br />

description, explanarpact<br />

on adaptive lifelvidence<br />

suggests that<br />

Lges with age and that<br />

utcomes. The existing<br />

rring to global levels<br />

:nt levels such as selfbehavior<br />

observation<br />

,i, Lindenberger, Frestigate<br />

all three com-<br />

', Li, & Lindenberger,<br />

).9., Ebner, Freund, &<br />

Addressing the question whether there exists cultural and individual knowledge<br />

<strong>of</strong> SOC as strategies <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development, Freund and Baltes (2002a)<br />

identified a body <strong>of</strong> proverbs reflecting SOC. This suggests that SOC strategies<br />

are represented in cultural knowledge about pragmatic aspects <strong>of</strong> everyday<br />

human life as expressed in proverbs. Selection, for instance, is expressed in the<br />

oroverb "You can't have the cake and eat it too." In a series <strong>of</strong> studies, these<br />

proverbs were used to examine whether younger and older adults' lay conceptions<br />

<strong>of</strong>, or preferences for, life-management behaviors were consistent with the<br />

SOC model. In a choice-reaction time task, participants wele asked to match<br />

SOC-related and alternative proverbs to sentence stems indicative <strong>of</strong> everyday<br />

life-management situations. Both age groups chose SOC-related proverbs both<br />

more frequently and faster than alternative proverbs. Consistent with the SOC<br />

theory, these preferences for SOC-related proverbs over alternatives existed only<br />

in tasks referring to goals and success, but not in contexts involving relaxation or<br />

leisure. This suggests that both younger and older adults have a preference for<br />

SOC-related proverbs over alternatives in everyday life-management situations<br />

and that their judgment <strong>of</strong> the adaptive use <strong>of</strong> these life-management strategies is<br />

in line with what the SOC model proPoses.<br />

To investigate the impact <strong>of</strong> a person's self-reported engagement in SOC on<br />

adaptive life-management and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>, several studies used versions <strong>of</strong><br />

the SOC questionnaire developed by P. B. Baltes et al. (1999). Freund and Baltes<br />

(2002b) investigated a heterogeneous sample <strong>of</strong> younger, middle-aged, and older<br />

adults and found that participants who indicated high engagement in SOC behaviors<br />

reported more positive emotions and higher levels <strong>of</strong> subjective wellbeing.<br />

The associations remained stable after controlling for self-regulation<br />

variables such as tenacious and flexible goal pursuit as well as for person characteristiCs<br />

such as neuroticiSm, extraversion, and Openness tO new experiences. As<br />

expected, SOC-related strategies were most strongly expressed in middle adulthood;<br />

that is, middle-aged adults showed higher endorsement <strong>of</strong> SOC than<br />

younger and older adults. One exception was elective selection, which showed a<br />

positive age trend. This suggests that older adults are more selective but report<br />

less use <strong>of</strong> optimization and compensation. This weakening <strong>of</strong> self-reported optimization<br />

and compensation in older ages could be due to the fact that these two<br />

strategies are effortful and resource demanding. Both strategies may exceed resources<br />

available in late life. This is especially true for the oldest old and for people<br />

suffering from severe illness (P. B. Baltes & Smith, 2003;Jopp,2002)' Using the<br />

SOC questionnaire in the context <strong>of</strong> the Berlin Aging Study, Freund and Baltes<br />

(1998) showed that SOC is effective well into very old age. They found that older<br />

adults between 72 and 102 years who reported more engagement in SOC-related<br />

strategies also showed higher expressions <strong>of</strong> subl'ective well-being such as positive<br />

affect, satisfaction with <strong>aging</strong>, lack <strong>of</strong> agitation, and absence <strong>of</strong> loneliness.<br />

Wiese and colleagues (Wiese, Freund, & Baltes, 2000,2002) applied the SOC<br />

model to goal pursuit in the work and family domains. In a cross-sectional<br />

questionnaire study, Wiese et al. (2000) measured self-reported use <strong>of</strong> SOC behaviors<br />

<strong>of</strong> younger to middle-aged pr<strong>of</strong>essionals in general as well as domainspecifically.<br />

Higher endorsement <strong>of</strong> general and work- and family-related SOC<br />

strategies was positively related to various indicators <strong>of</strong> well-being. Longitudinal<br />

data replicated these positive associations between self-reported engagement<br />

in SOC behaviors and global as well as work-related subjective well-being


100 EvrppNcr-Bnse o THnonv<br />

(Wiese et aL.,2002). Moreover, participants with primarily work-oriented goals<br />

reported higher levels <strong>of</strong> general and work-related well-being than those who<br />

engaged in both work and family goals. These findings underscore the importance<br />

<strong>of</strong> selecting goals and setting priorities as adaptive regulatory mechanisms<br />

in personal goal involvement and development.<br />

Taken together, the summarized self-report studies support a positive association<br />

between the endorsement <strong>of</strong> SOC-related strategies and indicators <strong>of</strong> global<br />

and domain-specific well-being. Similar findings supporting the adaptive use <strong>of</strong><br />

SOC are reported by Lang, Rieckmann, and Baltes (2002; see also M. M. Baltes &<br />

Lang,7997). Thus, SOC seems to contribute to <strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong><br />

throughout the adult years and into old and very old age.<br />

Taking a more process-oriented approach, a number <strong>of</strong> studies addressed the<br />

question <strong>of</strong> ruhst ospects <strong>of</strong> and horu selection, optimization, and compensation are<br />

related to positive functioning across life. A life span-developmental perspective<br />

ar5iues that the overall dynamic between optimization or approach goals (i.e.,<br />

goals directed at approaching positive outcomes) and compensation or avoidance<br />

goals (i.e., goals directed at preventing negative outcomes) should change with<br />

age. Civen the increase <strong>of</strong> losses and the decrease <strong>of</strong> gains over the life span, goals<br />

are hypothesized to shif t from a gain orientation (i.e., optimi zation, approach) in<br />

young adulthood to an orientation toward maintenance or loss avoidance (i.e.,<br />

compensation, avoidance) in later life (Ebner, 2005; Freund & Baltes, 2000; Freund<br />

& Ebner, 2005; Staudinger, Marsiske, & Baltes, 1995). If such an age-related shift<br />

in goal orientation is adaptive for man<strong>aging</strong> <strong>successful</strong>ly the changing balance <strong>of</strong><br />

gains and losses, younger adults should pr<strong>of</strong>it more from adopting goals that aim<br />

at optimizing their performance and older adults more from working on goals<br />

that are geared toward compensating losses in previously available means. Using<br />

a behavioral measure <strong>of</strong> persistence, namely, time, on a simple sensorimotor task<br />

that was framed in terms <strong>of</strong> either (a) optimizing performance or (b) compensating<br />

for losses, Freund (2006a) found evidence for age-differential motivational effects<br />

clf optimization versus compensation. Compared to younger adults, older<br />

adults were more persistent when pursuing the goal to compensate for a loss and<br />

Iess persistent when pursuing the goal to optimize their performance.<br />

In a similar vein, a series <strong>of</strong> studies by Ebner et al. (2006) propose that goals can<br />

be directed toward the improvement <strong>of</strong> functioning, toward the maintenance <strong>of</strong><br />

functional levels, and toward avoidance or regulation <strong>of</strong> loss. In a multimethod<br />

approach (self-report, preference-choice behavior), as well as different life contexts<br />

(cognitive and physical functioning), Ebner et al. found age-group differences<br />

in goai orientation and in the associations between goal orientation and<br />

subjective well-being. As expected and in line with the results by Freund (2006a),<br />

younger adults showed a primary goal orientation toward growth, whereas goal<br />

orientation toward maintenance and loss prevention became more important in<br />

old age. This age-related difference was shown to be related to perceived availability<br />

<strong>of</strong> resources. Moreover, loss prevention was negatively related to general<br />

well-being in younger but not older adults. Interestingly, in old age, orienting<br />

goals toward maintaining functions was positively associated with general subjective<br />

well-being. Together, the studies by Freund and Ebner et al. suggest that<br />

shifting one's goal orientation from the promotion <strong>of</strong> gains toward maintenance<br />

and loss prevention from early to late adulthood constitutes one mechanism <strong>of</strong><br />

adaptive goal selection.<br />

A study bY Ried<br />

goal relations as P<br />

adaptive aspects oI<br />

tion whether the rt<br />

in everyday life an<br />

was that older adu<br />

flict than younger<br />

competence to sele<br />

nonconflicting goi<br />

found that intergo<br />

well-being in ever<br />

ciated with highe<br />

goal attainment ol<br />

tiple goals into acc<br />

subjective well-be<br />

Several studies<br />

strategies on the I<br />

vided evidence fo<br />

outcomes in the c<br />

and Schmitz founr<br />

time studying the<br />

students low in se<br />

their exams.<br />

Several exPerin<br />

related differenct<br />

(Kemper, Hermar<br />

2001; Lindenbergt<br />

ferent from self-s<br />

allow individuals<br />

acterized by the<br />

menter) at the sa<br />

SOC in coordinat<br />

formance <strong>of</strong> mem<br />

performing both I<br />

primary domain I<br />

lected walking o<br />

Moreover, older<br />

handrail) to keel<br />

whereas younger<br />

thinking (i.e., slo<br />

bered). These agt<br />

over cognitive tas<br />

limits by increasi<br />

Taken together<br />

ioral level. Older<br />

ing) and comPe<br />

performance whr<br />

sources Primaril'<br />

to solving a men


Personalittl Theories o,f Successful Aging 101<br />

ily work-oriented goals<br />

t-being than those who<br />

underscore the importive<br />

regulatory mecharport<br />

a positive associatnd<br />

indicators <strong>of</strong> global<br />

ting the adaptive use <strong>of</strong><br />

see also M. M. Baltes &<br />

development and <strong>aging</strong><br />

I studies addressed the<br />

, and compensation are<br />

elopmental perspective<br />

:r approach goals (i.e.,<br />

pensation or avoidance<br />

rs) should change with<br />

cver the life span, goals<br />

mization, approach) in<br />

or loss avoidance (i.e.,<br />

& Baltes, 2000; Freund<br />

ch an age-related shif t<br />

he changing balance <strong>of</strong><br />

dopting goals that aim<br />

rom working on goals<br />

lvailable means. Using<br />

rple sensorimotor task<br />

lnce or (b) compensat-<br />

'ential motiva tiona I efyounger<br />

adults, older<br />

tpensate for a loss and<br />

lrformance.<br />

propose that goals can<br />

:d the maintenance <strong>of</strong><br />

)ss. In a multimethod<br />

as different life conrnd<br />

age-group differgoal<br />

orientation and<br />

Llts by Freund (2006a),<br />

growth, whereas goal<br />

ne more rmportant in<br />

:d to perceived availely<br />

related to general<br />

in old age, orienting<br />

:ed with general subrer<br />

et al. suggest that<br />

toward maintenance<br />

es one mechanism <strong>of</strong><br />

A study by Riediger, Freund, and Baltes (2005) addressed the nature <strong>of</strong> intergoal<br />

relations as possible characteristics differentiating between more and less<br />

adaptive aspects <strong>of</strong> goal selection. Specifically, Riediger et a1. examined the question<br />

whether the relationship among multiple personal goals affects goal pursuit<br />

in everyday life and subl'ective well-being. The first notable finding <strong>of</strong> their study<br />

was that older adults reported more facilitation between their goals and less conflict<br />

than younger adults. This finding can be interpreted as indicating that the<br />

competence to select personal goals so that they form a coherent, congruent, and<br />

nonconflicting goal system increases across adulthood. Moreover, the authors<br />

found that intergoal conf lict was negatively related to indicators <strong>of</strong> psychological<br />

well-being in everyday life over time. Intergoal facilitation, in contrast, was associated<br />

with higher involvement in goal pursuit and an increased likelihood <strong>of</strong><br />

goal attainment ol.er time. This study underscores the importance <strong>of</strong> taking multiple<br />

goals into account when studying the complex way goals affect behavior and<br />

subjective well-bei ng.<br />

Several studies demonstrate the relevance <strong>of</strong> SOC as adaptive life-management<br />

strategies on the behavioral level. For instance, Wiese and Schmitz (2002) provided<br />

evidence for the relationship between self-reported SOC and behavioral<br />

outcomes in the context <strong>of</strong> study success. In a sample <strong>of</strong> college students, Wiese<br />

and Schmitz found that students high in self-reported SOC behaviors spent more<br />

time studying than students low in self-reported SOC behaviors. Additionally,<br />

students low in self-reported selection and optimization were more likely to skip<br />

their exams.<br />

Several experimental studies identified SOC processes and demonstrated agerelated<br />

differences in these processes in the context <strong>of</strong> dual-task paradigms<br />

(Kemper, Herman, & Lian, 2003; Krampe, Rapp, Bondar, & Baltes, 2003; Li et al.,<br />

2001; Lindenberger, Marsiske, & Baltes,2000; Rapp, Krampe, & Baltes, 2006). Different<br />

from self-selected goals, as assessed in most studies reported earlier, that<br />

allow individuals to pursue one goal af ter the other, a dual-task situation is characterized<br />

by the necessity to mana€le two tasks (provided for by the experimenter)<br />

at the same time. Li et al., for example, conducted work on the role <strong>of</strong><br />

SOC in coordinating cogritive and sensorimotor behavior (i.e., simultaneous performance<br />

<strong>of</strong> memorizing and walking). Older adults showed greater costs when<br />

performing both tasks concurrently. As expected on the basis that the body is the<br />

primary domain where <strong>aging</strong> losses occur and serious risks exist, older adults selected<br />

walking over memorizing, as the primary target <strong>of</strong> resource allocation.<br />

Moreover, older adults showed more compensatory strategies (i.e., using a<br />

handrail) to keep their balance and to maintain higher levels <strong>of</strong> performance,<br />

whereas younger adults showed more compensatory strategies in the domain <strong>of</strong><br />

thinking (i.e., slowing down the speed <strong>of</strong> presentation for words to be remembered).<br />

These age-associated effects <strong>of</strong> differential use <strong>of</strong> SOC in favor <strong>of</strong> motor<br />

over cognitive tasks were stronger when the behavioral system was tested at its<br />

limits by increasing task diff iculty.<br />

Taken together, these findings supplement the relevance <strong>of</strong> SOC on the behavioral<br />

level. Older adults engage in selection (i.e., prioritizing walking over thinking)<br />

and compensation (i.e., use <strong>of</strong> a handrail) to preserve their walking<br />

performance when at risk. This preference <strong>of</strong> older adults to allocate their resources<br />

primarily to motor behavior (such as keeping one's balance) rather than<br />

to solving a memory problem is an example <strong>of</strong> highly automatized and largely


702 EvroeNct-Baspp THsc-rr


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Agirtg 103<br />

milar results rvith cogrpeting<br />

tasks. Bondar,<br />

er adults could be in-<br />

Lents. In the case <strong>of</strong> a<br />

however, older adults<br />

ntial allocation system<br />

lms to be adaptive to<br />

;ts that there exists cules<br />

and that SOC is reaged,<br />

and older adults.<br />

eas younger adults are<br />

rr adults show a higher<br />

racting losses. Studies<br />

heir resources to those<br />

ival.<br />

I research strengthens<br />

aptive developmental<br />

el can be regarded as a<br />

: span development. lt<br />

r to master the general<br />

ranges in goal-relevant<br />

mmodative coping by<br />

r, 1990), also places a<br />

ccessful development.<br />

ATIVE AND<br />

rework to understand<br />

tion and real-location<br />

l accommodative coptiidter<br />

& Greve,7994;<br />

2002; Brandtsteidter &<br />

;elf-regulatory stratecourses<br />

<strong>of</strong> individual<br />

I (e.g., tenacious goal<br />

adjustment). Specifi-<br />

I efforts to change Iife<br />

ls to personal preferental,<br />

self-corrective,<br />

tentionally changing<br />

rdy appearance to the<br />

justment <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

es to situational cong,<br />

or disengagement<br />

riration levels, rescal-<br />

ing criteria <strong>of</strong> success, or even selecting new, feasible goals. Accommodation includes<br />

emotional and cognitive reappraisal; it cannot be initiated intentionally, but<br />

shapes the selection <strong>of</strong> goals, and thus constitutes a basic mechanism <strong>of</strong> action regulation<br />

in the face <strong>of</strong> loss.<br />

The model specifies differential situational conditions that selectively activate<br />

or inhibit the two modes <strong>of</strong> coping. Assimilative tendencies dominate as long as<br />

people feel able to actively change a given situation. When experiences <strong>of</strong> loss increase<br />

or assimilative attempts to change the situation are ineffective and actionoutcome<br />

expectancies become low, accommodative tendencies are activated. This<br />

is also the case when assimilative modes are inefficient from the outset. Thus, people<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten first employ assimilative coping efforts to actively overcome obstacles<br />

that block their goals. If these attempts are un<strong>successful</strong>, a gradual shift to accommodative<br />

processes <strong>of</strong> goal adjustment occurs, which is modulated by personal<br />

and situational factors (e.g., goal importance; Brandtstiidter & Wentura,7995).<br />

According to the model, goal attainment leads to positive affect. Negative emotions<br />

arise when losses <strong>of</strong> goal-relevant resources lead to unattainability <strong>of</strong> goals<br />

and one falls short <strong>of</strong> one's ambitions and personal standards. Discrepancies between<br />

actual states and desired outcomes become especially salient in later phases<br />

<strong>of</strong> life when irrevocable Iosses and uncontrollable events cumulate. Brandtstddter<br />

and colleagues (Brandtstadter,1,999; Brandtstiidter & Greve, 7994;Brandtstiidter &<br />

Renner, 1990) maintain that in old age this growing impact <strong>of</strong> negative events on<br />

personal development favors an age-related shift from an active, assimilative to a<br />

more accepting, accommodative coping. As the aim <strong>of</strong> accommodative coping is to<br />

make the given situation appear less negative and more acceptable, this strategy<br />

may help individuals to maintain positive developmental perspectives into old age.<br />

With declining resources, as is especially the case in old age, it may not be possible<br />

to maintain the highest standard in multiple domains (Brandtstiidter & Wentura,<br />

1995). Accommodative coping therefore becomes particularly effective to protect<br />

against decline in well-being and favors adaptive development in later life (see also<br />

Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995). Thus, the dual-process model <strong>of</strong> coping characterizes<br />

<strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> primarily as a shift from actively counteracting the onset <strong>of</strong> agerelated<br />

losses to cognitively restructuring personal goals and standards.<br />

Aoerrrvu Drvrlc-rt'vENr es Srttr-r'INC FRoM Asstl,rtlantvn ro<br />

AccoHavopnrtve CctptNC THRouGHour LIFE<br />

Empirical evidence supports the expectation <strong>of</strong> age-related differences in the empioyment<br />

<strong>of</strong> assimilative and accommodative coping strategies. In a self-report<br />

study with younger, middle-aged, and older adults, Brandtstiidter and Renner<br />

(1990) found that self-reported tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment<br />

were complementarily associated with age. There was an age-graded gradual shift<br />

from active, tenacious goal pursuit to accepting, flexible goal adjustment. In addition,<br />

the study demonstrated a buffering effect <strong>of</strong> flexible goal adjustment with<br />

regard to the impact <strong>of</strong> perceived deficits on dissatisfaction with personal development.<br />

This finding suggests that flexibly adjusting one's goals helps to maintain a<br />

sense <strong>of</strong> well-being and satisfaction despite the actual experience <strong>of</strong> loss'<br />

In a large sample <strong>of</strong> older adults, Rothermund and Brandtst2idter (2003) found<br />

that accommodative strategies to overcome functional impairments had a curvilinear,<br />

inverted U-shaped relationship to age. The strategies increased up to the


104 EvrpeNcr-Baseo Tupor


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 105<br />

cy <strong>of</strong> action-related rercreased<br />

again. Rotherng<br />

that the increasing<br />

rits the usage and effistructured<br />

interviews<br />

ts are expressed in biord<br />

age and <strong>aging</strong>. They<br />

commodative thoughts<br />

rsitive attitude toward<br />

n their life.<br />

rift from assimilative tcr<br />

he latter in later adu [tnecessary<br />

for tenacious<br />

i Renner, 1990; Brandt-<br />

,trategies become more<br />

ing losses in temporal,<br />

oals and priorities. Acvith<br />

irreversible losses<br />

outcomes that diverge<br />

rcess model <strong>of</strong> coping<br />

ts a function <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong>constitutes<br />

one aspect<br />

zation in primary and<br />

explaining how people<br />

es in adulthood.<br />

RIMARY<br />

ntrol (OPS) by Hecklaptive<br />

developmental<br />

khausen, 1997,7999):<br />

lnt by selecting goals,<br />

r developmental ecolrol<br />

over their lives. ln<br />

1 and compensation as<br />

rlation (Heckhausen,<br />

from the SOC model,<br />

e focused investment<br />

B. Baltes and Baltes,<br />

the definition in the<br />

rn implies the use <strong>of</strong><br />

urces are insufficient<br />

L is the higher order<br />

tion (Heckhausen &<br />

Similar to the dual-process model <strong>of</strong> assimilative and accommodative coping,<br />

the OPS model particularly emphasizes the role <strong>of</strong> perceived personal control for<br />

developmental regulation. It assumes that humans have a basic need for control,<br />

that is, to protect themselves in the face <strong>of</strong> difficulties and setbacks. The primary<br />

way to achieve control is by modifying the environment according to one's goals<br />

by making instrumental efforts (i.e., primary control). If such primary control efforts<br />

are not available or fail, secondary control takes place, that is, modifying one's<br />

goals and standards or eng<strong>aging</strong> in self-protective attributions and favorable social<br />

comparisons. Secondary control aims at focusing and protecting motivational<br />

resources for primary control and buffers negative effects <strong>of</strong> failing in primary<br />

control strivings. The theory posits a primacy <strong>of</strong> primary over secondary control<br />

strategies. According to the model, people aim at maximizing primary control.<br />

Secondary control strivings serve to bolster primary control. They are the secondbest<br />

strategy af ter primary control strivings fail or are not available.<br />

Two forms <strong>of</strong> primary and secondary control strategies are distinguished: se/cctiae<br />

and compensatory. Selective primary control comprises actions that are directly<br />

aimed at goal achievement. It denotes the focused investment <strong>of</strong> resources<br />

into the pursuit <strong>of</strong> a chosen goal. Examples are time investment or investment <strong>of</strong><br />

effort and skills into pursuinp; one's goals. Compensatory primary control refers<br />

to the investment <strong>of</strong> external resources. It involves the recruitment <strong>of</strong> external<br />

help or technical aids for the attainment <strong>of</strong> a chosen goal. lt comes into play when<br />

internal resources for goal achievement are insufficient or depleted. Examples are<br />

employing the help <strong>of</strong> others or applying new, alternative resources. Selective secondary<br />

control is directed at the internal world to promote the volitional commitment<br />

to a chosen goal. It subsumes metavolitional strategies to keep oneself<br />

focused on the pursuit <strong>of</strong> selected goals, for example, by avoiding distractions.<br />

Examples are boosting the value <strong>of</strong> a chosen goal or the perception <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

control. Finally, compensatory secondary control refers to cognitive reframing <strong>of</strong><br />

goals such as down-regulating the desirability <strong>of</strong> a blocked goal or diseng<strong>aging</strong><br />

from it. It serves as a buffer for negative effects <strong>of</strong> failure. It involves strategies<br />

such as disengagement from unattainable goals, downward social comparison,<br />

and external causal attributions.<br />

According to the authors, none <strong>of</strong> these four strategies is functional per se.<br />

Rather, a higher order optimization process is postulated that coordinates control<br />

strivings such that the potential for primary control is maximized across life.<br />

Consistent with this proposition, empirical evidence indicates that self-protective<br />

compensatory strategies (compensatory secondary control) become more prevalent<br />

and more adaptive in later adulthood and when opportunity structures for<br />

goal attainment are unfavorable (Wrosch & Heckhausen, 1999), whereas continued<br />

involvement in primary control efforts is maladaptive in such situations<br />

(Chipperfield, Perry, & Menec, 1999; Heckhausen, Wrosch, & Fleeson,2001).<br />

According to the OPS model, maximization <strong>of</strong> primary control potential characterizes<br />

adaptive functioning. This, however, becomes particularly difficult in<br />

older ages when losses become more prominent and less under personal control<br />

and when declines in goal-relevant resources make the achievement <strong>of</strong> resourceintensive<br />

goals increasingly difficult (Schulz, 1986). As a consequence, with advancing<br />

age, compensatory primary and secondary control strategies gain<br />

increasing importance for <strong>successful</strong> life management. The use <strong>of</strong> secondary<br />

control strategies is theorized to contribute to a long term increase in primary


706 Evrt>r:Ncr--BesEp Tnrorv<br />

control in that it buffers negative motivational effects <strong>of</strong> failure and maintains<br />

self-efficacy and well-being. Therefore, these strategies should be employed<br />

more frequent in old age (cf. Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995). However, OPS theory<br />

arEiues that it is not the four control strategies separately that bring about adaptive<br />

development but rather their orchestration in line with developmental opportunities<br />

and constraints. The following section provides empirical support<br />

for these assumptions.<br />

ApnrrrrvE DrvElorvrNT AS Sl rrFTtNcl rRol,r PnruaRy ro SECoNDAny<br />

CoNrRol Srnnrlclss rl tRC)ucHOUr LrtE<br />

In a self-report study, Heckhausen,Schulz, and Wrosch (1998) observed that the<br />

use <strong>of</strong> control strategies increased from early to late adulthood. Unexpectedly,<br />

however, compensatory secondary control did not show a positive age trend. Positive<br />

associations between all four types <strong>of</strong> control strategies and self-esteem supported<br />

the role <strong>of</strong> primary and secondary control strategies for adaptive<br />

functioning. Wrosch and Heckhausen (1999) demonstrated that diseng<strong>aging</strong> from<br />

unobtainable goals and reallocating resources to alternative domains (i.e., compensatory<br />

secondary control) is functional when goal-relevant resources decline<br />

and opportunity structures for goal attainment become less favorable. The authors<br />

examined goals in the partnership domain in recently committed or separated<br />

adults in early adulthood and late midlife. Younger separated adults<br />

mentioned more partnership goals and reported more primary control strivings<br />

for attaining their goals. ln contrast, older separated adults reported more secondary<br />

control actions such as goal deactivation. They disengaged from partnership<br />

goals and invested their resources into alternative social domains. Moreover,<br />

younger separated adults reported a decline in positive affect over a time period<br />

<strong>of</strong> 15 months when they disengaged from their partnership goals, whereas for<br />

older separated adults goal deactivation proved beneficial. One explanation is<br />

that the objective chances to remarry are lower for people in late middle age compared<br />

to young adulthood. The age-related change in opportunities for attaining<br />

the partnership goal, then, is ref lected in a shift in importance and functional impact<br />

<strong>of</strong> this goal. These findings can be viewed as evidence supporting the notion<br />

that individuals activate and deactivate developmental goals in accordance with<br />

age-graded changes in opportunities for goal achievement.<br />

Similarly, Heckhausen et al. (2001) investigated developmental regulation before<br />

and after the developmental deadline for childbearing. Younger and middleaged<br />

women with or without children were compared with regard to various<br />

indicators <strong>of</strong> primary and secondary control striving for goal attainment versus<br />

goal disengagement and self-protection. Women approaching the developmental<br />

deadline for childbearing were actively engaged in the goal <strong>of</strong> bearing a child. In<br />

contrast, women who had passed the deadline disengaged from the goal. These<br />

differences were evident in the number and type <strong>of</strong> goals nominated as well as<br />

the salience <strong>of</strong> information related to children. Moreover, women who had passed<br />

the deadline but for whom child-related information was still more salient were<br />

more prone to negative affect. Thus, a selective responsiveness to information relevant<br />

for an obsolete developmental goal appears to have negative implications<br />

for well-being.<br />

Taken together,<br />

the endorsement c<br />

ance developmenti<br />

trol strategies incr<br />

adaptive developn<br />

come increasingly<br />

the SOC model ar<br />

the fruitfulness o<br />

in-context in termr<br />

final section, we a<br />

S<br />

This chapter com<br />

perspective on pet<br />

derstanding <strong>of</strong> go<br />

environment intet<br />

in this chapter (th<br />

model) argue that<br />

motivating, orgar<br />

related processes<br />

related processes<br />

management <strong>of</strong> r<br />

losses is a key to s<br />

cific processes pl<br />

adulthood. There<br />

yet, no integratior<br />

(Freund & Riedig<br />

positive associati<<br />

believe that it will<br />

to close this gap.<br />

Another future<br />

practical relevanct<br />

related goals are t<br />

sues become mos<br />

1992), our practici<br />

related processesI<br />

Research has d<br />

and losses has a d<br />

suit when implen<br />

framed messages<br />

through a regular<br />

the avoidance <strong>of</strong><br />

physical activity;<br />

themselves as hig<br />

old age, loss-fran<br />

sages become mot


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Strccessful Aging 107<br />

'failure and maintains<br />

s should be employed<br />

l. However, OPS theory<br />

that bring about adap-<br />

,ith developmental opides<br />

empirical support<br />

:ONDARY<br />

998) observed that the<br />

Llthood. Unexpectedly,<br />

'ositive age trend. Posi-<br />

:s and self-esteem sup-<br />

:rategies for adaptive<br />

that diseng<strong>aging</strong> from<br />

ve domains (i.e., comvant<br />

resources decline<br />

ess favorable. The auly<br />

committed or sepalger<br />

separated adults<br />

mary control strivings<br />

ts reported more seclngaged<br />

from partneraldomains.<br />

Moreover,<br />

ect over a time period<br />

Lip goals, whereas for<br />

l. One explanation is<br />

t late middle age comrtunities<br />

for attaining<br />

tce and functional imsupporting<br />

the notion<br />

ls in accordance with<br />

mental regulation be-<br />

Younger and middleith<br />

regard to various<br />

ral attainment versus<br />

ng the developmental<br />

<strong>of</strong> bearing a child. In<br />

from the goal. These<br />

nominated as well as<br />

rmen who had passed<br />

:ill more salient were<br />

ss to information relregative<br />

implications<br />

Taken together, the f indings in the context <strong>of</strong> OPS theory provide evidence that<br />

the endorsement <strong>of</strong> control strategies favors <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> as they help to balance<br />

developmental gains and losses. Specifically, the use <strong>of</strong> compensatory control<br />

strategies increases in importance with positive effects on well-being and<br />

adaptive developmental regulation when goal-related resources are lost or become<br />

increasingly threatened, as is <strong>of</strong>ten the case in late adulthood. As is true for<br />

the SOC model and the dual-process model <strong>of</strong> coping, the OPS model supports<br />

the fruitfulness <strong>of</strong> approaching <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> by investigating personalityin-context<br />

in terms <strong>of</strong> personal goals and goal-related processes. In the next and<br />

final section, we address this brief Iy.<br />

SUMMARY, FUTURE OUTLOOK, AND<br />

PRACTICAL RELEVANCE<br />

This chapter combined an action-theoretical with a life span-developmental<br />

perspective on personality and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. This integration allows an understanding<br />

<strong>of</strong> goals as dynamic aspects <strong>of</strong> personality that reflect the personenvironment<br />

interaction over time. All three theoretical frameworks presented<br />

in this chapter (the SOC model, the dual-process model <strong>of</strong> coping, and the OPS<br />

model) argue that individuals can actively shape their development through the<br />

motivating, organizinp;, and directing functions <strong>of</strong> personal goals and goalrelated<br />

processes, and they all empirically support the role <strong>of</strong> goals and goalrelated<br />

processes for adaptive development. Moreover, they all stress that the<br />

management <strong>of</strong> resource changes from a dominance <strong>of</strong> gains to increasing<br />

losses is a key to <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. They differ, however, with regard to the specific<br />

processes proposed to best serve the management <strong>of</strong> resources across<br />

adulthood. There exists good evidence supporting each <strong>of</strong> these models. As <strong>of</strong><br />

yet, no integ;ration or systematic comparison <strong>of</strong> the models has been attempted<br />

(Freund & Riediger, 2003; for exceptions, see Freund & Baltes, 2002a,2002b, for<br />

positive associations <strong>of</strong> SOC and assimilative and accommodative coping). We<br />

believe that it will be a fruitful next step in future research on <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong><br />

to close this gap.<br />

Another future research direction in this field should be to investigate the<br />

oractical relevance <strong>of</strong> the three models. As various studies have shown that workielated<br />

goals are most central for younger adults and goals related to health issues<br />

become most prominent in older adults (Nurmi, 1992; Rapkin & Fischer,<br />

1992), our practical examples and speculations about the role <strong>of</strong> goals and goalrelated<br />

processes for <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> refer to the domains <strong>of</strong> health and work.<br />

Research has demonstrated that framing health messages in terms <strong>of</strong> gains<br />

and losses has a differential effect on health-related goal adoption and goal pursuit<br />

when implementing health programs in younger and older adults. Cainframed<br />

messages emphasize benefits gained (e.g., becoming more attractive<br />

through a regular physical workout), whereas loss-framed messages emphasize<br />

the avoidance <strong>of</strong> benefits lost (e.g., preventing weight gain through regular<br />

physical activity; Brendl, Higgins, & Lemm, 7995). When individuals perceive<br />

themselves as highly vulnerable to certain health risks, as is <strong>of</strong>ten the case in<br />

old age, loss-framed messages are more effective, whereas gain-framed messages<br />

become more effective when perceived vulnerability is low, as is typically


108 Evrpr-:Ncp-Besgt> TusoRY<br />

the case at younger ages (Lee & Aaker, 2004). Framing a health program with an<br />

emphasis on benefits lost, as opposed to benefits gained, might therefore be<br />

more persuasive for older adults. As a consequence, elderly people might be<br />

more persistent in pursuing the program and might manage to improve their<br />

physical and cognitive functioning and, in this sense, age more <strong>successful</strong>ly.<br />

The opposite should be true for younger adults. But not only the direction <strong>of</strong><br />

goals might have practical implications but also the underlying motivation <strong>of</strong><br />

goals. Riediger (2001) showed, for instance, that goals related to attractiveness<br />

and fitness are central for younger adults, whereas health in itself becomes the<br />

main motivation for regular physical activity in older adults. Taking this into<br />

consideration when setting up <strong>successful</strong> training concePts for younger and<br />

older adults appears to be <strong>of</strong> great importance.<br />

The second example <strong>of</strong> the possible applied consequences <strong>of</strong> this research is<br />

motivation at the workplace. With demographic changes (a combination <strong>of</strong><br />

lower birth rates and more people becoming older in much better cognitive and<br />

physical conditions; e.g., P. B. Baltes, 1997), it is likely that retirement age will<br />

increase to keep well-educated and highly experienced people in the workforce.<br />

Here, it will become an important task <strong>of</strong> the future to take into account how<br />

goals and goal-related processes in the work domain might change over adulthood.<br />

In contrast to younger people, older adults might simply not be primarily<br />

motivated to achieve new and higher outcomes, but instead focus on maintenance<br />

and prevention <strong>of</strong> losses. Providing younger as well as older adults with<br />

work tasks that meet their underlying motivation and that are framed in ageappropriate<br />

ways (i.e., gain frames for younger adults and loss frames for older<br />

adults) may increase their work-related performance as well as their personal<br />

satisfaction with work in young but also in olci age. Moreover, encour<strong>aging</strong> accommodative<br />

coping styles and providing opportunities for primary control<br />

even and especially when resources decline will likely help older adults to derive<br />

meaning and satisfaction from their workplace.<br />

In sum, we believe that conceptualizing <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> using a framework <strong>of</strong><br />

person-in-context fosters our understanding <strong>of</strong> the processes underlying <strong>aging</strong><br />

well and will ultimately also help shape environments in a more age-friendly way<br />

to support the strengths <strong>of</strong> old and very old age and to maintain positive functioning<br />

even in the face <strong>of</strong> decreasing resources.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Allport, G. W. (1937). PersLtnality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Hogrefe.<br />

Baltes, M. M. (1996) . The mnny .faces <strong>of</strong> dependencv in old nge. New York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong><br />

Irress.<br />

Baltes, M. M., & Carstensen,L.L. (7996). The process <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. A.qeirtg nnd Societr1,<br />

16,397-422.<br />

Baltes, M. M., & Lang, F. R. (1997). Everyday functioning and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>: The impact<br />

<strong>of</strong> resources. Psychology and Aging,12, 433-443.<br />

Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions <strong>of</strong> life-span developmental psychology: On<br />

the clynamics between growth and decline. Dettektpmentnl Psychoktgy,23,611-626.<br />

Baltes, P. B. (7997). On the incomplete architecture <strong>of</strong> human ontogeny: Selection, optimization,<br />

and compensation as foundation <strong>of</strong> developmental theory. Atncrican Pstlchologist,<br />

52,366-380.<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Baltes<br />

Methodological and<br />

<strong>aging</strong> on the central n<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Baltes<br />

model <strong>of</strong> selective<br />

(Eds.), Successful ag<br />

Cambridge Univers<br />

Baltes, P. B., Baltes, M<br />

tion, optimization,a<br />

Germany: Max Plar<br />

Baltes, P. B., Freund, I<br />

In M. Johnson, V. L<br />

book <strong>of</strong> age and <strong>aging</strong><br />

Baltes, P. B., Lindenbe<br />

tal psychology. In V<br />

Theoretical models <strong>of</strong><br />

Baltes, P. 8., Reese, H.<br />

nual Reoieut <strong>of</strong> PsYc<br />

Baltes, P. 8., & Singer<br />

cultural orchestra<br />

<strong>of</strong> the Academia Eurt<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Smith<br />

<strong>aging</strong> <strong>of</strong> the young<br />

Bargh, I. A., & Fergus<br />

mental processes. I<br />

Baumeister, R. F. (199<br />

Bluck, S., & Alea, N. (l<br />

I remember the aut<br />

iniscence work (pp. I<br />

Boesch, E. E. (1991<br />

Springer.<br />

Bondar, A., Krampe, l<br />

young and older adt<br />

Max Planck Institt<br />

Bradburn, N. M. (196<br />

Brandtstiidter, J. $98<br />

action: Considerat<br />

fiir Entzoicklungsp<br />

Brandtstadter, I. $99<br />

R. M. Lerner (Eds.<br />

opment (5th ed., pp<br />

Brandtstiidter, l. (799<br />

togenetic bases <strong>of</strong><br />

(Eds.), Action and t<br />

Thousand Oaks, C<br />

Brandtstiidter, J., & G<br />

Deuelopmental Reo<br />

Brandtst2idter, J., & L<br />

search through the It


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Sttccessful Aging 109<br />

)alth program with an<br />

J, might therefore be<br />

erly people might be<br />

nage to improve their<br />

ge more <strong>successful</strong>ly.<br />

only the direction <strong>of</strong><br />

erlying motivation <strong>of</strong><br />

ated to attractiveness<br />

in itself becomes the<br />

ults. Taking this into<br />

:pts for younger and<br />

:es <strong>of</strong> this research is<br />

:s (a combination <strong>of</strong><br />

r better cognitive and<br />

tt retirement age will<br />

rple in the workforce.<br />

lke into account how<br />

ht change over adultnply<br />

not be primarily<br />

:ad focus on mainte-<br />

I as older adults with<br />

at are framed in ageloss<br />

frames for older<br />

vell as their personal<br />

)ver/ encour<strong>aging</strong> acfor<br />

primary control<br />

Ip older adults to deusing<br />

a framework <strong>of</strong><br />

ses underlying <strong>aging</strong><br />

rore age-friendly way<br />

ain positive functionv<br />

York: Hogrefe.<br />

>rk: Cambridge Univer-<br />

I <strong>aging</strong>. Ageing and Soci-<br />

:cessful <strong>aging</strong>: The imnental<br />

psychologv: On<br />

ology, 23, 611-626.<br />

:ogeny: Selection, optieory.<br />

Arne r ic nn P sy chol -<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1980). Plasticity and variability in psychological <strong>aging</strong>:<br />

Methodological and theoretical issues. In G. E. Gurski (Ed.), Determining the effects <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>aging</strong> on the central neraousystem (pp. 41,-66). Berlin, Germany: Schering.<br />

Baltes, P. 8., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>: The<br />

model <strong>of</strong> selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes<br />

(Eds.), Successftrl <strong>aging</strong>: Perspectiues from the behaaioral sciences (pp. 1-3a). New York:<br />

Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Baltes, P. B., Baltes, M. M., Freund, A. M., & Lang, F. R. (1999). The measurement <strong>of</strong> selection,<br />

optimization, and compensation (SOC) by self-report: Technical report 1999. Berlin,<br />

Germany: Max Planck Institute for Human Development.<br />

Baltes, P. B., Freund, A. M., & Li, S.-C. (2005). The psychological science <strong>of</strong> human <strong>aging</strong>.<br />

In M. Johnson, V. L. Bengtson, P. Coleman, & T. Kirkwood (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook<br />

<strong>of</strong> age and <strong>aging</strong> (pp. a7-71). New York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Life-span theory in developmental<br />

psychology. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> child psychology: Vol. 1.<br />

Theoretical models <strong>of</strong> human deztelopment (6th ed., pp. 569-664). New York: Wiley.<br />

Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Life-span developmental psychology. Annual<br />

Reaiew <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 31, 65-110.<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Singer, T. (2001). Plasticity and the ageing mind: An exemplar <strong>of</strong> the biocultural<br />

orchestration <strong>of</strong> brain and behaviou r. European Reaiezu: Interdisciplinary lournal<br />

<strong>of</strong> the Academia Europaen, 9,59-76.<br />

Baltes, P. 8., & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong>: From <strong>successful</strong><br />

<strong>aging</strong> <strong>of</strong> the young old to the dilemmas <strong>of</strong> the fourth age. Gerontology, 49,123-135.<br />

Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity <strong>of</strong> higher<br />

mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 925-945.<br />

Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings <strong>of</strong> life. New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Bluck, S., & Alea, N. (2002). Exploring the functions <strong>of</strong> autobiographical memory: Why do<br />

I remember the autumn? In f . D. Webster & B. K. Haight (Eds.), Critical adoances in rentiniscence<br />

work (pp.61-75). New York: Springer.<br />

Boesch, E. E. (1991). Symbolic action theory and cultural psychology. Berlin, Germany:<br />

Springer.<br />

Bondar, A., Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Balance takes priority oaer cognition: Can<br />

young and older adults deliberately control resource allocation? Unpublished manuscript,<br />

Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin.<br />

Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure <strong>of</strong> psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldine.<br />

Brandtstlidter, J. (1986). Personal control over development and development-regulating<br />

action: Considerations and results concerning a neglected research topic. Zeitschrift<br />

fiir Ent w i ck lu ngsp s y ch ol o gi e un d P ii d ago g isch e P sy c holo g ie, 18, 376 -334.<br />

Brandtstddter, f. (1998). Action perspectives on human development. In W. Damon &<br />

R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> child psychology: VoL 1. Theoretical models <strong>of</strong> human deuelopment<br />

(5th ed., pp. 807-863). New York: Wiley.<br />

Brandtstiidter, J. Q999). The self in action and development: Cultural, biosocial, and ontogenetic<br />

bases <strong>of</strong> intentional self-development. In J. Brandtstiidter & R. M. Lerner<br />

(Eds.), Action and self-deaelopment: Theory and research through the life span (pp.37-65).<br />

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Brandtstiidter, f., & Greve , W. (1994). The <strong>aging</strong> self: Stabiiizing and protective processes.<br />

Deael opm e ntal Reai eu, 14, 52-80.<br />

Brandtstddter,J., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.).(1999).<br />

Action and self-deztelopment: Theory and research<br />

throttgh the life span. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.


110 Evrptrucn-BRspu Tnsonv<br />

Brandtstiidter,I., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment:<br />

Explication and age-related analysis <strong>of</strong> assimilative and accommodative strategies<br />

<strong>of</strong> coping. Psychology and Aging, 5,58-67.<br />

Brandtsti,idter, f .,& Rothermund, K. (2002). The life-course dynamics <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit and<br />

goal adjustment: A two-process framework. Deaelopmental Reaiezu, 22,777-1'50.<br />

Brandtstiidter,J., & Rothermund, K., & Schmitz, U. (1997). Coping resources in later life.<br />

European Journal <strong>of</strong> Applied Psychology, 47,707-114.<br />

Brandtstbdter, J., & Wentura, D. (1995). Adjustment to shifting possibility frontiers in<br />

later life: Complementary adaptive modes. In R. A. Dixon & L. Bbckman (Eds.), Compensnting<br />

for psychological deficits and declines: Man<strong>aging</strong> losses nnd promoting gains<br />

(pp. 83-106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Brandtstiidter, J., Wentura, D., & Greve, W. (1993). Adaptive resources <strong>of</strong> the <strong>aging</strong> self:<br />

Outlines <strong>of</strong> an emergent perspective. Internationnl lournal <strong>of</strong> Behauioral Deaelopment,'16,<br />

323-349.<br />

Brendl, C. M., Higgins, E. T., & Lemm, K. M. (i995). Sensitivity to varying gains and<br />

losses: The role <strong>of</strong> self-discrepancies and event framing. lournnl <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social<br />

Psychologv, 69, 7028-1051.<br />

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1988). Inieracting systems in human development: Research paradigms-Present<br />

and future. In N. Bolger & A. Caspi (Eds.), Person in context: Deaelopmental<br />

processes-Human deuelopment in cultural and historicsl contexts (pp. 25-49). New<br />

York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Griissman, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional<br />

well-being: The moderating role <strong>of</strong> motive dispositions. lournal <strong>of</strong> Personnlity and Social<br />

Psychology, 75, 494-508.<br />

Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Maier, G. W. (1999). The pursuit <strong>of</strong> personal goals: A<br />

motivational approach to well-being and life adjustment. In J. Brandtstiidter & R. M.<br />

Lerner (Eds.), Action and self-deoelopment: Theory and research through the life-span<br />

(pp.169*196). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W.L. (7976)'The quality <strong>of</strong> American life: Perceptions,<br />

eaaluations, and satisfactions. New York: Russell-Sage.<br />

Cantor, N. (1990). From thought to behavior: "Having" and "doing" in the study <strong>of</strong> personali<br />

ty and cogni tion. Amer i can P sy cholo gi st, 45, 735<br />

-750.<br />

Cantor, N. (1994). Life task problem solving: Situational affordances and personal needs.<br />

<strong>Personality</strong> nnd Socisl Psychology Bulletin, 20,235-243.<br />

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control theory approach<br />

to htrman behattior. New York: Springer.<br />

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual framework for<br />

personality, social, clinical, and health psychology. Psychological Bullctitt,92,111-735.<br />

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions <strong>of</strong> positive and negative affect:<br />

A controi-process view. Psychological Reztiew, 97, 79-35.<br />

Chapman, M., & Skinner, E. A. (1985). Action in development: Development in action. In<br />

M. Frese & J. Sabini (Eds.), GoaI directed behaaior: The concept <strong>of</strong> action in psychology<br />

(pp. 200-213). Hillsdale, Nf : Erlbaum.<br />

Chipperfield,J. G., Perry, R. P., & Menec, V. H. (1999). Primary and secondary controlenhancing<br />

strategies: Implications for health in later hfe. lotrrnal <strong>of</strong> Aging and Hcalth,<br />

11, 517-539.<br />

Cohen, G. (199S). The effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> on autobiographical memory. In C. P. Thompson,<br />

D. J. Herrmann, D. Bruce, D. J. Read, D. G. Payne, & M. P. Toglia (Eds.) , Autobiographi'<br />

cttl me nLory: Theoretical and applied perspectioes (pp. 105-123). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Coleman, P. C., Ivani<strong>of</strong><br />

life themes in oL<br />

Conway, M. A., & P1<br />

memories in the se<br />

Costa, P. T., Herbst,<br />

life: Stability, int<br />

365-378.<br />

Costa, P. T., & McCra<br />

sonality. In T. F.<br />

(pp. 21-a0). Wash<br />

Costa, P. T., & McCra<br />

ment using the Re<br />

64,21.-50.<br />

Diener, E. (1984). Sub<br />

Diener, E., & Fulita, F<br />

nomothetic and id<br />

926-935.<br />

Ebner, N. C. (2005). S<br />

ferences itr persona<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Berl<br />

Ebner, N. C., Freund<br />

goal orientation frr<br />

and prevention <strong>of</strong><br />

Emmons, R. A. (1986<br />

being. lournal <strong>of</strong> Pe<br />

Emmons, R. A. (1996<br />

P. M. Gollwitzer &<br />

tiaation to behaaio<br />

Ericsson, K. A., & Kri<br />

tice in the acquisi<br />

Erikson, E. H. (1968)<br />

Ford, D. H. (1987). Hr<br />

behaaior and perso<br />

Freund, A. M. (199<br />

salience <strong>of</strong> age in t<br />

Freund, A. M. (2003<br />

233-242.<br />

Freund, A. M. (2006<br />

and older adults. I<br />

Freund, A. M. (200<br />

adulthood. In B. R<br />

suit: Goals, action a<br />

Freund, A. M., & Bal<br />

gies <strong>of</strong> life manag<br />

Psychology and Agi<br />

Freund, A. M., & Ba<br />

compensation: An<br />

regulation. In W. l<br />

and consciousnes (


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 777<br />

and flexible goal adjustnd<br />

accommodative strateamics<br />

<strong>of</strong> goal pursuit and<br />

aiezo, 22, 117-750.<br />

ing resources in later life.<br />

rg possibility frontiers in<br />

L. Biickman (Eds.), Comtsses<br />

and promoting gains<br />

sources <strong>of</strong> the <strong>aging</strong> self:<br />

lehaaioral Deoebpment, 1 6,<br />

ity to varying gains and<br />

nl <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social<br />

tlopment: Research para-<br />

Person in context: Deaelop-<br />

:ontexts(pp. 25-49). New<br />

onal goals and emotional<br />

nl <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> nnd Social<br />

rsuit <strong>of</strong>personal goals: A<br />

J. Brandtstiidter & R. M.<br />

uch through the life-span<br />

ty <strong>of</strong> American life: Percep-<br />

,ing" in the study <strong>of</strong> pernces<br />

and personal needs.<br />

: A control theory approach<br />

onceptual framework for<br />

'cal Bulletin, 92, 771-135.<br />

positive and negative af-<br />

)evelopmeni in action. In<br />

pt <strong>of</strong> action in psychology<br />

I and secondary controlrnal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Aging and Health,<br />

rory. In C. P. Thompson,<br />

3lia (Eds.), Autobiogrnphiillsdale,<br />

NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Coleman, P. G., Ivani-Chalian, C., & Robinson, M. (1998). The story continues: Persistence<br />

<strong>of</strong> life themes in old age. Ageing nnd Society, 18,389-479.<br />

Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction <strong>of</strong> autobiographicarl<br />

memories in the self-memorv system. Psychological Rcuiezu, 107,26I-288.<br />

Costa, P. T., Herbst, J. H., McCrae, R. R., & Siegler, I. C. (2000). <strong>Personality</strong> at micllife:<br />

Stability, intrinsic maturation, and response to life events. Assessment, 7,<br />

365-378.<br />

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence for stability <strong>of</strong> adult personality.<br />

In T. F. Heatherton & J. L. Weinberp;er (Eds.), Can parsonality change?<br />

(pp. 21-40). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.<br />

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierarchical pcrsonality assessment<br />

using the Revised NEO <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory. lournnl <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> Assessment,<br />

64,21-50.<br />

Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bullctirt,95,542-575.<br />

Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjective well-being: A<br />

nomothetic and idiographic approach.lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> nnd Social Psychologr1,68,<br />

926-935.<br />

Ebner, N. C. (2005). Striaing for gains and preaenting losscs: Multi-nrcthod eoidence on the dit'-<br />

ferences in pcrsonal goal orientntion in enrly nnd latc ndulthood. Doctoral dissertation, Free<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Berlin.<br />

Ebner, N. C., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Developmental changes in personal<br />

goal orientation from young to late adulthood: From striving for gains to maintenance<br />

and prevention <strong>of</strong> losses. Psychologrl nnd Agirtg, 21,661-678.<br />

Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective wellberng,.<br />

lourrml <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> ortd Socinl Psychology,5T, 1058-1068.<br />

Emmons, If. A. (1996). Striving and feeling: Personal goals and subjective well-being. In<br />

P. M. Gollwitzer 8xJ. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology <strong>of</strong> oction: Linkirrg cognition nnd nro<br />

tiaotion to behnttiLtr (pp. 313-337). New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Ericsson, K. A., & Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role <strong>of</strong> deliberative practice<br />

in the acquisition <strong>of</strong> expert performance. Psr1chology Rcaicu,, 100,363-406.<br />

Erikson, E. H. (1968). ldetttity: Youth nnd crlsis. New York: Norton.<br />

Ford, D. H. (1987). Humans ns self-corrstructing liuing sqstents: A dettalopmental perspecti-oe on<br />

behaaior anLl Stcrsonnlity. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Freund, A. M. (1997). lndividuating age-salience: A psychological perspective on the<br />

salience <strong>of</strong> age in thc life course. Human Deaelo1tment, 40,287*292.<br />

Freund, A. M. (2003). The role <strong>of</strong> goals for development. Psycltologische I?.undschnu,54,<br />

L.).) - L+ Z.<br />

Freund, A. M. (2006a). Differential motivational consequences <strong>of</strong> goal focus in younger<br />

and older adults. Psychologtl nntl Agirrg, 21 ,240-252.<br />

Freund, A. M. (2006b). Levels <strong>of</strong> goals-Understanding motivational processes across<br />

adulthood. In B. R. Little, K. Salmela-Aro, & S. D. Phillips (Eds.), Pcrsonal project pursuit:<br />

Conls, nctiort nnd human f lourislrirg. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Selection, optimization, and compensation as strategies<br />

<strong>of</strong> life management: Correlations with subjective indicators <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>.<br />

Psychologrl ond Aging, 13, 531-543.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). The orchestration <strong>of</strong> seiection, optimization and<br />

compensation: An action-theoretical conceptualization <strong>of</strong> a theory <strong>of</strong> developmental<br />

regulation. In W. J. Perrig & A. Grob (Eds.), Control <strong>of</strong> humatt bchaztior, mental processes,<br />

and consciottsricss (pp. 35-58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.


112 EvrpeNcE-Bnsr:o TuroRY<br />

Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002a). The adaptiveness <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization, and<br />

compensation as strategies <strong>of</strong> life management: Evidence from a preference study on<br />

proverbs. J ou r n al <strong>of</strong> C e ront ology : P sy cltologic nl S c i e nce s, 57 B, 426 - 434.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002b). Life management strategies <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization<br />

and compensation: Measurement by self-report and construct validity. lournal <strong>of</strong><br />

Personnlity and Social Pstlchttlogtl, 82, 642-662.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Ebner, N. C. (2005). The <strong>aging</strong> self: Shifting from promoting gains to<br />

balancing losses. ln W. Greve, K. Rothermund, & D. Wentura (Eds.), The adaptitte self:<br />

Personal continuittl nrd intentional sel.f-deaebpme nt (pp.185-202). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe<br />

& Huber.<br />

Freund, A. M., Li, K. Z. H., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). Successful development and <strong>aging</strong>: The<br />

role <strong>of</strong> selectiorr, optimization, and compensation in <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. In J. Brandtstddter<br />

& R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Action nntl self-deztelopment: Theory and research through the<br />

Iife spnrt (pp. a01-43a). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2003). Successful <strong>aging</strong>. In R. M Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks,<br />

& J. Mistry (Eds.), Contprehensiae handbook <strong>of</strong> psychology: Vol. 6. Deaelopmental<br />

psychoktgv (pp. 601-628). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2006). Coals as building blocks <strong>of</strong> personality and development<br />

in adulthood. ln D. K. Mroczek & T. D. Little (Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> personality deoelopntent<br />

(pp. 353-372). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino<br />

(Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> motiuation and cogrtition: VLtl.2. Foundations <strong>of</strong> social behaaior<br />

(pp. 53-92). New York: Cuiiford Press.<br />

Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, j. A. (Eds.). (1996). The psychology <strong>of</strong> action: Linking cognition<br />

ard ntotiuntion to behaaior. New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, C. B. (1996). Goal effects on action and cogr:rition. In E. T.<br />

Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook <strong>of</strong> bnsic principles<br />

(pp.361-399). New York: Gr-riiford Press.<br />

Habermas, T., & Btuck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The emergence <strong>of</strong> the life story in adolescence.<br />

Psycltrlttgical Bullctin, 126, 748-769.<br />

Havighurst, I{. J. (1963). Successful <strong>aging</strong>. In R. H. Wiliams, C. Tibbitts, & W. Donahue<br />

(Eds.), The process <strong>of</strong> nging: Vol.1. Social and psychLtlogical perspectiues (pp.299-320). New<br />

York: Atherton Prcss.<br />

Heckhausen,J. $997). Developmental regulation across adulthood: Primary and secondary<br />

control <strong>of</strong> age-related changes. Deuelopmental Psychology, 33,776-187.<br />

Heckhausen, J. 0999). DettclLtpnrcntal ragulatiott in adulthood: Age-norrnatitte and sociostructural<br />

constraints ns ndaTttiachallenges. New York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory <strong>of</strong> control. Psychological Reaiew,<br />

102,284-304.<br />

Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1998). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Selection<br />

and compensation via primary and secondary control. In J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck<br />

(Eds.), Motiuntion nnd selt'-rcgulation across the life span (pp. 50-77). New York: Cambridge<br />

<strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Heckhausen,J.,Schulz,I{., & Wrosch, C. (1998). Deuelopmental regulation in ndulthood: Optintization<br />

in primttry nnd sccondary control. Unpublished manuscript, Max Planck Institute<br />

for Human Development, Berlin.<br />

Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Fleeson, W. (2001). Developmental regulation before and<br />

after a developmental deadline: The sample case <strong>of</strong> "biological clock" for childbearing.<br />

Psychology nnd Aging,16, 400-473.<br />

Helson, R., & Kwan, V.<br />

ture and Processe I<br />

sonality psychology (\<br />

Isaacowitz, D. M., & Sr<br />

<strong>of</strong> GerontologY, 588,I<br />

Iopp, D. (2002). Succ<br />

and adaptizte life-m<br />

BerIin.<br />

Kemper, S., Herman, F<br />

for young and old<br />

speech or noise. Ps<br />

Klinger, E. (1975). Con<br />

Psychoktgicnl Reuieu<br />

Klinger, E. (7977). Mea<br />

neapolis: Universii<br />

Krampe, R. T., & Balt<br />

resource allocatio<br />

Sternberg & E' L. C<br />

cies, and ex7ertise \P<br />

Krampe, R. T., RaPP,l<br />

sources during the<br />

Der Nerztenarzt, 74,<br />

"<br />

Kruglanski, A. W. (19<br />

(Eds.), The PsYcho<br />

New York: SPringe<br />

Kunzmann, U. (1999)<br />

being. Berlin. Cerm<br />

Labouvie-Vief, G. (19<br />

<strong>aging</strong> in life-sPan P<br />

uals as Producers <strong>of</strong> t<br />

demic Press.<br />

Lang, F. R., Rieckman<br />

facilitate strategie<br />

tioning? lournal <strong>of</strong> r<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1975)<br />

<strong>of</strong> GerontologY, 30, E<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1983)<br />

Gerontologist, 23, 34<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1989<br />

K. W. Schaie (Eds.<br />

Springer.<br />

Lawton, M. P., & Nat<br />

M. P. Lawton (Eds<br />

ington, DC: Ameri<br />

Lee, A. Y., & Aaker, J<br />

tory fit on Proces<br />

ogy, 86,205-21'8.<br />

Lerner, R. M., & Bus<br />

life-span Per sPe c tirle


Personulity Theorie s <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 113<br />

lction, optimization, and<br />

rm a preference study on<br />

6-434.<br />

es <strong>of</strong> selection, optimizarstruct<br />

validity. lotrnal <strong>of</strong><br />

from promoting gains to<br />

a (Eds.), The adaptizte self:<br />

l). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe<br />

elopment and <strong>aging</strong>: The<br />

;sful <strong>aging</strong>. In J. Brandtry<br />

and research through the<br />

M Lerner, M. A. Easterogy:<br />

Vol. 6. Deaelopmental<br />

rf personality and devel-<br />

-Iandbook<br />

<strong>of</strong> personality det.<br />

Higgins & R. M. Sor-<br />

'ndations <strong>of</strong> social behaaior<br />

f action: Linking cognition<br />

rn and cognition. In E. T.<br />

rdbook <strong>of</strong> basic principles<br />

f the life story in adoles-<br />

Tibbitts, & W. Donahue<br />

:tiaes (pp. 299-320). Nen'<br />

ld: Primary and secondt,176-787.<br />

normatiae and sociost r ucriversity<br />

Press.<br />

:ol. Psychological Reuiezu,<br />

in adulthood: Selection<br />

ckhausen & C. S. Dweck<br />

C-77). New York: Cam-<br />

,ulation in adulthood: Opcript,<br />

Max Planck Insti-<br />

I regulation before and<br />

al clock" for childbear-<br />

Helson, R., & Kwan, V. S. Y. (2000). <strong>Personality</strong> development in adulthood: The broad picture<br />

and processes in one longitudinal sample. In S. Hampson (Ed.), Adoances in personality<br />

psLtcholo;qy (Vol. 1, pp. 77-106). London: Routledge.<br />

Isaacowitz, D. M., & Smith, l. (2003). Positive and negative affect in very old age. lournals<br />

<strong>of</strong> CerontolLtgy, 588, P743-P152.<br />

Jopp, D. (2002). Successful <strong>aging</strong>: On the ftrnctional interplatl between personal resources<br />

and adaptiae life-managenrent strategies. Doctoral dissertation, Free <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

Berlin.<br />

Kemper, S., Herman, It. E., & Lian, C. H. T. (2003). The costs <strong>of</strong> doing two things at once<br />

for young and older adults: Talking while walking, finger tapping, and ignoring<br />

speech or noise. Psychology nnd Aging, 18,181-192.<br />

Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences <strong>of</strong> commitment to and disengagement from incentives.<br />

Psychological Reaieitt, 82, 1-25.<br />

Klinger, E. (1977). Menning and uoid: Inner experience nnd the incentiues in people<br />

's /lzres. Minneapolis:<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Minnesota Press.<br />

Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). lntelligence as adaptive resource development and<br />

resource allocation: A new look through the lenses <strong>of</strong> SOC and expertise. In R. J.<br />

Sternberg & E. L. Crigorenko (Eds.), Perspectiaes on tlte psychology <strong>of</strong> abilities, cornpetencies,<br />

nnd expertise (pp. 31-69). New Ycrrk: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Krampe, R. T., Rapp, M. A., Bondar, A., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Allocation <strong>of</strong> cognitive resources<br />

during the simultaneous performance <strong>of</strong> cognitive and sensorimotor tasks.<br />

Der Ne rue nnrzt, 7 4, 271-278.<br />

Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Coals as knowledge structures. ln P. M. Collwitzer & J. A. Bargh<br />

(Eds.), The pstlchologq <strong>of</strong> action: Linking cogttition and ntotiantion to behaaior (pp. 599-618).<br />

New York: Springer.<br />

Kunzmann, U. (1999). Being nnd feeling in control: Trtto sources <strong>of</strong> older people's emotional uellbeing.Berlin,<br />

Cermany: Max Planck Institute for Human Development.<br />

Labouvie-Vief, C. (1981). Proactive and reactive aspects <strong>of</strong> constructivism: Crowth and<br />

<strong>aging</strong> in Iife-span perspective. In R. M. Lerner & N. A. Busch-Rossnagel (Eds.), Indioidttals<br />

as producers <strong>of</strong> their deoelopment: A life-spnn perspectizte (pp.I97-230). New York: Academic<br />

Press.<br />

Lang, F. R., Rieckmann, N., & Baltes, M. M. (2002). Adapting to <strong>aging</strong> losses: Do resources<br />

facilitate strategies <strong>of</strong> selection, compensation, and optimization in everyday functioning?<br />

lournal <strong>of</strong> Gertmtttlogy: Psychological Sciences, 578, 507-509.<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1975). The Philadeiphia Geriatric Center Morale Scale: A revision. lournal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Gerontology , 30 , 85 -89 .<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1983). Environment and other determinants <strong>of</strong> well-being in older people.<br />

Gerontolog ist, 23, 349-357.<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1989). Environmental proactivity in older people. In V. L. Bengston &<br />

K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Tlrc corrrse <strong>of</strong> lnter life: Resenrch and rtflections (pp. 15-23). New York:<br />

Springer.<br />

Lawton, M. P., & Nahemow, L. (7973). Ecology and the <strong>aging</strong> process. In C. Eisdorfer &<br />

M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology <strong>of</strong> adult deoeloTtntent nnd <strong>aging</strong> (pp. 619-674). Washington,<br />

DC: American Psychological Associatron.<br />

Lee, A. Y., & Aaker, J. L. (2004). Bringing the frame into focus: The influence <strong>of</strong> regulatory<br />

fit on processing fluency and persuasion. lottrnal <strong>of</strong> Persttnalittl nnd Social Psychology,<br />

86,205-278.<br />

Lerner, R. M., & Busch-Rossnagel, A. (1981). Indiaidunls ns producers <strong>of</strong> their deuelopment: A<br />

life-span perspcctiue. New York: Academic Press.


774 EvrprNcr-BasnuTsEoRv<br />

Li,K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Walking while memorizing:<br />

Age-related differences in compensatory behavior. Psychological Science, 12,<br />

230-237.<br />

Lindenberger, U., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Memorizing while walking: Increase<br />

in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. Psychology and Agirtg,3,<br />

477-436.<br />

Little, B. R. (1989). Personal project analysis: Trivial pursuits, magnificent obsessions, and<br />

the search for coherence. ln D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), <strong>Personality</strong> psychology: Recent<br />

trends and emerging directions (pp. 15-31). New York: Springer.<br />

Maslow, A. (1954). Motiantion and personality. New York: Harper & Row.<br />

McAdams, D. P. (1990). Unity and purpose in human lives: The emergence <strong>of</strong> identity as<br />

a life story. In A. I. Rabin, R. A. Zucker, R. A. Emmons, & S. J. Frank (Eds.), Studying<br />

persotls nnd littcs (pp. 148-190). New York: Springer.<br />

McAdams, D. P. (1995). What do we know when we know a person? lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>,<br />

63,365-396.<br />

McAdams, D. P. (1996). <strong>Personality</strong>, modernity, and the sioried self: A contemporary<br />

framework for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7,295-327.<br />

McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology <strong>of</strong> life stories. Reaiew <strong>of</strong> Ceneral Psychology, 5,<br />

700-122.<br />

McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motioation. New York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, R. A., III. (2003). Modeling intraindividual change in personality<br />

traits: Findings from the Normative Aging Study. lournals <strong>of</strong> Cerontologv, 588,<br />

P153-P165.<br />

Neugarten, B. L., Havighurst, R. J., & Tobin, S. S. (1961). The measurement <strong>of</strong> life satisfaction.<br />

lournol <strong>of</strong> Cerontology, 16,134-143.<br />

Nurmi, I.-E. (1992). Age differences in adult life goals, concerns, and their temporal extension:<br />

A life-course approach to future-oriented motivation. Internstionnl Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Behnuiornl Deuelopment, 15, 187-508.<br />

Omodei, M. M., & Wearing, A. J. (1990). Need satisfaction and involvement in personal<br />

projects: Toward an integratir.'e model <strong>of</strong> subjective well-being. lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><br />

and Social Psychology, 59, 762-769.<br />

Palys, T. S., & Little, B. If. (1983). Perceived life satisfaction and the organization <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

project systems. lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social Psychology, 44,7227-7230.<br />

Pervin, L. A. (1989). Goal concepts: Themes, issues, and questions. In L. A. Pervin<br />

(Ed.), Goal concepts in personality nnd social psychology (pp.473-479). Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Pillemer, D. B. (1992). Remembering personal circumstances: A functional analysis. In E.<br />

Winograd & U. Neisser (Eds.), Emory Syntposia in Cognition: Affect lnd accuracy in recall-Stttdics<br />

<strong>of</strong> "f lnshbulb" memories (4th ed., pp.236-26\. New York: Cambridge Unir.ersity<br />

Press.<br />

Rapkin, B. D., & Fischer, K. (1992). Framing the construct <strong>of</strong> life satisfaction in terms <strong>of</strong><br />

older adults' personal goals. Psyclrologtl and Aging, 7, 138*149.<br />

Rapp, M. A., Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Adaptive task prioritization in <strong>aging</strong>:<br />

Selective resource allocation to postural control is preserved in Alzheimer disease.<br />

American lournal <strong>of</strong> Ceriatr ic Psychiatry, 14, 52-61.<br />

Riediger, M. (2001). On the dynamic relations among multiple gLtals: Intergoal conf lict and intergoal<br />

facilitation in younger and older adttlthood. Doctoral dissertation, Free <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

Berlin.<br />

Riediger, M., Freund, I<br />

Intergoal facilitatio<br />

lournals <strong>of</strong> Gerontolo<br />

Riediger, M., Li, S.-C.,{<br />

tion (SOC) as devel<br />

preview. In J. E. Bir<br />

ed., pp. 289-313). AI<br />

Roberts, B. W., & Del<br />

traits from childhoo<br />

Iogical Bulletin, 126,i<br />

Rogers, C. (1963). The<br />

ness. In M. R. Jonest<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Nebra<br />

Rothermund, K., & Br;<br />

From compensator<br />

Ryan, R. M. (1995). PsY<br />

nal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>, 63<br />

Ryff, C. D. (1989). In t<br />

middle-aged and ok<br />

Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psyc<br />

Science, 4,99-104.<br />

Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C<br />

lournal <strong>of</strong> Personali<br />

Schulz, R. (1986). Succ<br />

opment and Aging Nt<br />

Schulz, R., & Heckhau<br />

chologist, 51,702-77<br />

Sheldon, K. M., & Elli<br />

well-being: The sel<br />

482-497.<br />

Singer, I. A., & Salove<br />

Small, B. J., Hertzog,<br />

adult personalitY or<br />

<strong>of</strong> Gerontology, 588,<br />

Srivastava, S., John, O.<br />

early and middle ac<br />

and Social Psycholo<br />

Staudinger, U. M., Ma<br />

Iater adulthood: Po<br />

chetti & D. Cohen (<br />

tion (pp.801-847)I<br />

Thomae, H.E. (1'976)<br />

Veenhoven, R. (1991)<br />

spots. In F. Strack,I<br />

disciplinar y perspec<br />

Wiese, B. S., Freund, I<br />

tion: An action-rel<br />

ior,57,273-300.


Personnlity Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 115<br />

01). Walking while memo-<br />

. Psychological Science, 72,<br />

rizing while walking: lnt.<br />

Psychology and Aging, 3,<br />

agnif icent obsessions, and<br />

<strong>Personality</strong> psy chology : Reinger.<br />

& Row.<br />

) emergence <strong>of</strong> identity as<br />

i. J. Frank (Eds.), Studying<br />

son? lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>,<br />

ied self: A contemporary<br />

-321.<br />

t <strong>of</strong> Cenernl Psychology, 5,<br />

lge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

idual change in personalrnnls<br />

<strong>of</strong> Gerontology, 588,<br />

asurement <strong>of</strong> life satisfacs,<br />

and their temporal ex-<br />

>n. lnternational lournal <strong>of</strong><br />

involvement i n personal<br />

ing. lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><br />

I the organization <strong>of</strong> pergy,<br />

44, 1227-7230.<br />

estions. In L. A. Pervin<br />

473-479). Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

functional analysis. In E.<br />

Affect and accuracy irt rerw<br />

York: Cambridge Uni-<br />

: satisfaction in terms <strong>of</strong><br />

< prioritization in <strong>aging</strong>:<br />

d in Alzheimer disease.<br />

'ntergoal<br />

rcnf lict and intcration,<br />

Free <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

Riediger, M., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). Man<strong>aging</strong> life through personal goals:<br />

Intergoal facilitation and intensity <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit in younger and older adulthood.<br />

lottrnals <strong>of</strong> Gerontology, 608, P84-P91.<br />

Riediger, M., Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U. (2006). Seleciion, optimization, and compensation<br />

(SOC) as developmental mechanisms <strong>of</strong> adaptive resource allocation: Review and<br />

preview. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> the psychology <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> (6th<br />

ed., pp. 289-313). Amsterdam: Elsevier.<br />

Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency <strong>of</strong> personality<br />

traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review <strong>of</strong> longitudinal studies. Psychological<br />

Bulletin, 126, 3*25.<br />

Rogers, C. (1963). The actualisation tendency in relation to "motives" and to consciousness.<br />

In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motiaation (Vol. 11, pp.1.-2Q. Lincoln:<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Nebraska Press.<br />

Rothermund, K., & Brandtstiidter, J. (2003). Coping with deficits and losses in later life:<br />

From compensatory action to accommodation. Psycllology and Aging,18,896-905.<br />

Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation <strong>of</strong> integrative processes. /or''-<br />

nal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>, 63, 397-127 .<br />

Ryff, C. D. (1989). In the eye <strong>of</strong> the beholder: Views <strong>of</strong> psychological well-being among<br />

middle-aged and oider adults. Psychology and Aging, 4,795-270.<br />

Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Current Directions in Psychological<br />

Science. 1.99-104.<br />

Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure <strong>of</strong> psychological well-being revisiied.<br />

lotrrnal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social Psychology, 69,71.9-727.<br />

Schulz, R. (1986). Successful <strong>aging</strong>: Balancing primary and secondary control. Adult Deuelopment<br />

and Aging Nerus, 13,2-4.<br />

Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. 0996). A life-span model <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. American Psychologist,<br />

51, 702-774.<br />

Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. I. 0999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal<br />

well-being: The self-concordance model. lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social Psychology, 76,<br />

482-497.<br />

Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1993). The remembered self. New York: Free Press.<br />

Small, B. J., Hertzog,, C., Hultsch, D. F., & Dixon, R. A. (2003). Stability and change in<br />

adult personality over 6 years: Findings from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. lournals<br />

<strong>of</strong> Gerontology, 588, P166-P176.<br />

Srivastava, S., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, l. (2003). Development <strong>of</strong> personality in<br />

early and middle adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent change? lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><br />

nnd Social Psychology, 84, 7041-1053.<br />

Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Resilience and reserve capacity in<br />

later adulthood: Potentials and limits <strong>of</strong> development across the life span. In D. Cicchetti<br />

& D. Cohen (Eds.), Deaelopmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, and adaptation<br />

(pp.801.-847). New York: Wiley.<br />

Thomae, H. E. (1976). Patterns <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. Basel: Karger.<br />

Veenhoven, R. (1991). Questions on happiness: Classical topics, modern answers, blind<br />

spots. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjectiae zuell-being: Vol.2. An interd<br />

i sc ipl i n a r y p e r sp e c t iae (pp. 7 -26). Oxf ord : Pergamon Press.<br />

Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Selection, opiimization, and compensation:<br />

An action-related approach to work and partnership. lournal <strong>of</strong> Vocational Behaaior.<br />

57,273-300.


776 EvrupNcn-Besro TrrsoRv<br />

Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002). Subjective career success and emotional<br />

well-being: Longitudinal predictive power <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization and compensation.<br />

lottrnal Ltf Vocationnl Behaaior, 60,321-335.<br />

Wiese, B. S., & Schmitz, B. (2002). Action regulaiion at university: Application <strong>of</strong> a developmental<br />

meta-model. Zeitschrift fiir Entwicklungspsychologie und Piidngogischc Psychologie,<br />

34,80-94.<br />

Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, I.0999). Control processes before and after passing a developmental<br />

deadline: Activation and deactivation <strong>of</strong> intimate relationship goals. lournal<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social Psychology, 77, 475-427.<br />

Wi<br />

SC<br />

o<br />

SUSANNI<br />

Jts<br />

rurs cHAPTER\<br />

I<br />

novel constructs r<br />

Ilongings). Both d<br />

utopias. Our work or<br />

opmental constructs<br />

tion and conduct <strong>of</strong><br />

the past, the present<br />

capturing the comp<br />

nents <strong>of</strong> the structur<br />

berger, & Staudinge<br />

In the search for s<br />

formed not only by r<br />

discourse in everyda<br />

(G eis t e sw is s e nsch af t e n<br />

much for making psy<br />

sumed that in these t<br />

search had shied awe<br />

In the selection <strong>of</strong> th<br />

unchartered territor<br />

The theoreticai and empi<br />

and directed by Paul B. I<br />

many thanks especially tr<br />

the various phases <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Jacqui Smith, Ursula M. S

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!