03.06.2014 Views

Personality theories of successful aging - University of Florida ...

Personality theories of successful aging - University of Florida ...

Personality theories of successful aging - University of Florida ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Flandbook <strong>of</strong><br />

Gerontology<br />

Evidence-Based Approaches<br />

to Theory, Practice, and Policy<br />

Edited by<br />

fames A. Blackburn/ PhD<br />

Catherine N. Dulmus, PhD<br />

john Wiley & Sons, Inc.<br />

_^'-


This book is printed on acid-free paper. @<br />

Copyright @ 2007 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved.<br />

by<br />

l"Plirlr"9 iohn Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey.<br />

Published simultaneously in Canada.<br />

Wiley Bicentennial Logo: Richard J. Pacifico<br />

No part <strong>of</strong> this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any<br />

form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise,<br />

except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 <strong>of</strong> the 7976 United States Copyrighi Act, without<br />

either the.prior written permission <strong>of</strong> the Publisher, or authorization throu"gh"payment <strong>of</strong> the<br />

fee<br />

3gf.gp]11t".p_".-1opy<br />

to_the Copyright Clearance Center, rnc.,222 Rosew:ood Drive, Danvers,<br />

MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to<br />

the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions liepaitment, John'Wiley &<br />

sons, Inc., 111 River street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-601,1, f ax (201V48-6008, or online it<br />

http: //www.wiley.com/golpermissions.<br />

To out<br />

Limit <strong>of</strong> Liability/Disclaimer <strong>of</strong> Warranty: Whiie the publisher and author have used their best<br />

efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the<br />

accuracy or completeness <strong>of</strong> the contents <strong>of</strong> this book and specifically disclaim anyimplied<br />

warranties <strong>of</strong> merchantability or fitness for a particular puipose. No-warranty may be created or<br />

extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and itrategies contained<br />

herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a pr<strong>of</strong>essionil where<br />

appropriate. Neither the publisher nor author shall be liable for any Ioss bf pr<strong>of</strong>it or any other<br />

commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidenial, consequential, oi other<br />

damages.<br />

This publication is designed to_ provide accurate and authoritative information in regard to the<br />

subject matter covered. It is sold with the understanding that the publisher is not enigaged in<br />

rendering pr<strong>of</strong>essional services. If legal, accounting, medical, psyihological or any oihlr expert<br />

assistance is required, the services <strong>of</strong> a competent pr<strong>of</strong>essionai person ihould be iought.<br />

Designations used by companies to distinguish their products are <strong>of</strong>ten claimed as trademarks.<br />

In all instances where John.Wiley & Sons,lnc. is aware <strong>of</strong> a claim, the product names appear in<br />

initial capital or all capital letters. Readers, however, should contact the appropriate companies<br />

for more complete information regarding trademarks and registration.<br />

For general information on our other products and services please contact our Customer Care<br />

Department within the United states at (800) 762-2974. outside the United states at<br />

(377) 572-3993 or fax (317) 572-4002.<br />

Wiley also publishes its books.in a variety <strong>of</strong> electronic formats. Some content that appears in<br />

print may not be available in electronic books. For more information about Wiley products, visit<br />

our web site at www.wiley.com.<br />

Library <strong>of</strong> Congress Cataloging-in-Publication<br />

Data:<br />

Handbook <strong>of</strong> gerontology : evidence-based approaches to iheory,<br />

practice, and policy / edited by James A. Blackburn, Catherine N.<br />

Dulmus.<br />

P;cm<br />

Includes bibliographical references.<br />

ISBN-13: 978-0-477-77170-8 (cloth : alk. paper)<br />

1. Ceriatrics-Handbooks, manuais, etc. j. Gerontolosv-Hand_<br />

books, manuais, etc. 3. Evidence-based medicine-Hanltooks,<br />

manuals, etc. L Blackburn, James A., ph. D. II. Dulmus.<br />

Catherine N.<br />

1. Aging-physiology.<br />

^[DNLM:<br />

Aging-psychology.<br />

3. Ceriatrics-methods. 4. Health Policy. 5.'Health-services for<br />

the Aged. WT 104 H2343 20071<br />

RC9s2.55.H395 2007<br />

678.97-dc22<br />

2006027674<br />

Printed in the United States <strong>of</strong> America<br />

10987654321


nomic and methodological<br />

anisms ol euerudaV cotnition<br />

'dnmentnls <strong>of</strong> assessment nnd<br />

rehavioral treatments: An<br />

t guide to psrlcltothern1ty and<br />

I Association.<br />

CHAPTER 4<br />

<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong><br />

Successful Aging<br />

NATALIE C. EBNER and ALEXANDRA M, FITEUND<br />

N rHrs cHAprER, we combine an action-theoretical view with a life spandevelopmental<br />

perspective on personality and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> to examine the<br />

role <strong>of</strong> goals and goal-related processes for adaptive development. One <strong>of</strong> the<br />

basic assumptions <strong>of</strong> this integration is that goals and goal-related processes motivate,<br />

organize, and direct behavior and development over time. By setting, pursuing,<br />

and maintaining goals, persons can actively shape their development in<br />

interaction with their given physical, cultural-historical, and social living context.<br />

This renders goals and goal-related processes central to personality and <strong>successful</strong><br />

development across the life span as they constitute a link between the<br />

person and his or her life contexts over time.<br />

We start this chapter by defining the concept <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> deuelopment and<br />

<strong>aging</strong>.Then we outline the concept <strong>of</strong> personality underlying our approach using<br />

McAdams's (1990,1995) model <strong>of</strong> levels <strong>of</strong> personality. This model integrates dispositional<br />

traits, personal concerns and goals, and life narratives as three levels<br />

<strong>of</strong> personality. We discuss the notion <strong>of</strong> goals as personality-in-context by describing<br />

goals as dynamic aspects <strong>of</strong> personality reflecting the interaction <strong>of</strong> a<br />

person with his or her environment over time. Finally, to highlight the role <strong>of</strong><br />

personal goals for <strong>successful</strong> development, we present three prominent conceptual<br />

frameworks <strong>of</strong> developmental regulation: the model <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization,<br />

and compensation (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 7990), the dual-process model <strong>of</strong><br />

coping (BrandtstZidter& Renner, 1990), and the model <strong>of</strong> primary and secondary<br />

control (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995).<br />

WHAT IS SUCCESSFUL DEVELOPMENT?<br />

On a very broad level, current life span-developmental psychology <strong>of</strong>ten describes<br />

adaptive (or <strong>successful</strong>) development as a lifelong process <strong>of</strong> generating<br />

new resources as well as adjusting to physical, social, and psychological changes<br />

with the overall aim to simultaneously maximize developmental gains and


88 EvrpENce-BRSt:Il TuEol


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 89<br />

ensen/ 1996; P. B. Baltes,<br />

Labouvie-Vief, 1981). At<br />

repted set <strong>of</strong> criteria that<br />

laborated by Freund and<br />

lstions regard i ng cri teria<br />

<strong>aging</strong> in particular. P. B.<br />

:cessful <strong>aging</strong> longevity,<br />

:ial competence, procluc-<br />

Similarly, Lawton (1983)<br />

>jective satisfaction with<br />

tppiness or goal achievenotor<br />

behavior, and cogt<br />

as financial or living<br />

ortcomings <strong>of</strong> such lists,<br />

ld be combined, whether<br />

other, and whether the<br />

:ollection to which criteted.<br />

res to defining <strong>successful</strong><br />

characteristics constitut-<br />

; the guiding question <strong>of</strong><br />

(2) process-oriented ap-<br />

:he question What are the<br />

r time? These approaches<br />

sly adapting to changing<br />

rments to fit their needs,<br />

Busch-Rossnagel, 1981).<br />

reeds and capabilities on<br />

:onstraints on the other<br />

Lracterization <strong>of</strong> successspecific,<br />

single criterion.<br />

:-criterion approach that<br />

re only criterion for suc-<br />

'herefore, to understand<br />

in ernpirical studies it is<br />

there is no single def ining<br />

is seen as indicating<br />

d ages well (Neugarten,<br />

rishes between "broad"<br />

mber <strong>of</strong> defining facets.<br />

ng (e.9., Lawton, 7975).<br />

esses the importance <strong>of</strong><br />

tl functioninS;. Her contce,<br />

environmental maspersonal<br />

growth, and<br />

nguish between two dirn<br />

(life satisfaction) and<br />

an emotional dimension (encompassing positive and negative affect; Bradburn,<br />

1969; Campbell, Converse, & Rodgers, 1976; Diener, 1984; Veenhoven, 1991).<br />

These dimensions can be either used in a domain-general way or applied to specific<br />

life domains.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the critiques <strong>of</strong> subjective well-being as the main or even sole criterion<br />

for <strong>successful</strong> development is that it does not explicitly take environmental conditions<br />

into consideration that support <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> or the <strong>successful</strong> interaction<br />

between a person and his or her life context (Havighurst, 1963; Lawton,<br />

1989). A multiple-criteria approach, in contrast, aims at an integration <strong>of</strong> subjective<br />

indicators (e.g., personal life satisfaction and happiness) and more objective<br />

indicators (e.g., everyday instrumental competence). These criteria can be either<br />

short term (e.g., last week's happiness) or long term (e.9., meaning and purpose<br />

in life), domain-specific (e.g., satisfaction with financial situation) or general<br />

(e.g., overall life satisfaction), as well as static and with a definite end point (e.g.,<br />

life satisfaction at a given point in time) or dynamic (e.g., change in life satisfaction<br />

over time; Freund & Riediger, 2003).<br />

Taken together, we distinguish four main approaches to conceptualizing <strong>successful</strong><br />

development and <strong>aging</strong>: criteria-oriented, process-oriented, single-criterion, and<br />

multi-criteria approaches. Table 4.1 summarizes these four outlined approaches.<br />

As we pointed out, there is currently no agreement as to which single criterion<br />

or combination <strong>of</strong> criteria best defines <strong>successful</strong> development. One <strong>of</strong> the reasons<br />

for this lack <strong>of</strong> consensus is that there is no generally agreed-upon model <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong> that could serve as the basis for such a definition.<br />

Instead, there are numerous theoretical approaches to <strong>successful</strong> development<br />

and even more empirical findings pertaining to more or less specific factors<br />

contributing to adaptive development. Integration <strong>of</strong> these findings is difficult<br />

because studies use different indicators for <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>.<br />

It is beyond the scope <strong>of</strong> this chapter to <strong>of</strong>fer a comprehensive, integrative<br />

model <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development. Instead, we aim at approaching the topic<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> on different levels <strong>of</strong> personality, focusing on the level <strong>of</strong><br />

personal goals and taking different criteria and dimensions into account.<br />

Approach<br />

Table 4.1<br />

Approaches to Conceptualizing Successf ul Development and Aging<br />

Aim <strong>of</strong> Approach (Leading Question)<br />

Criteria-oriented Description <strong>of</strong> characteristics constituting success as an outcome<br />

<strong>of</strong> development ("What ls successf ul development?")<br />

Process-oriented Persoective on individuals as in continuous interaction with their<br />

environment ("What are the processes and conditions that foster<br />

developmental success over time?")<br />

Multiple criteria<br />

Single criterion<br />

Integration <strong>of</strong> subjective and objective indicators ("How can<br />

subjective and objective indicators be integrated into one index <strong>of</strong><br />

successf u I development?")<br />

Assessment <strong>of</strong> a person's global or domain-specific subjective wellbeing<br />

("How can subjective well-being as one indicator <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong><br />

development be assessed [general or domain-specif ic, once or<br />

repeated lyl?")


90 EvrppNcE-BesEp Trrsc-rr


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging<br />

9I<br />

,1987,\997) that (a) deind<br />

that developmental<br />

r gains and losses), and<br />

;es and outcomes serve<br />

rproach. This approach<br />

:r adaptive ways (M. M.<br />

re detail, we posit that<br />

ng the processes <strong>of</strong> suc-<br />

)06). Before elaboratino -a)<br />

bed the concept <strong>of</strong> per-<br />

:y in terms <strong>of</strong> three lev<strong>aging</strong>?<br />

To address this<br />

zli ty. F ollow ing Freund<br />

rn personality and sucs<br />

(1995,1996) compristiongl<br />

trnits. Traits are<br />

nonconditional disposhow<br />

considerable sta-<br />

;. Personal goals are a<br />

:rsonality descriptions<br />

ranisms, copi ng stra tertal,<br />

and strategic conre,<br />

place, and/or social<br />

they integrate and inrconstructed<br />

past, per-<br />

IcAdams (2001), most<br />

lives with unity, purolving<br />

life narratives,<br />

lemes. Level lll <strong>of</strong> perll,<br />

political, economic,<br />

oes the life story <strong>of</strong> a<br />

:vident in the notion<br />

n different contexts"<br />

all <strong>of</strong> these life narra-<br />

, points in life, chance<br />

llowing sections elab-<br />

:sonality traits, that is,<br />

and stable across time<br />

n <strong>of</strong> the stability argufor<br />

a s<strong>of</strong>ter version <strong>of</strong><br />

on personality traits<br />

refers to the identification <strong>of</strong> a universal structure <strong>of</strong> personality, individual differences,<br />

and the extent <strong>of</strong> longitudinal stability (Costa& McCrae, 7994,7995).<br />

There is consensus that personality can be well described by the Big Five personality<br />

traits: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and<br />

openness to new experiences. These five factors have been identified by means <strong>of</strong><br />

factor analysis across various instruments and samples. Cross-sectional as well as<br />

longitudinal evidence on the structural invariance <strong>of</strong> the Big Five exists for adulthood<br />

and into old age (Costa& McCrae, 1994; Small , Hertzog, Hultsch, & Dixon,<br />

2003). Moreover, according to a meta-analysis by Roberts and DelVecchio (2000),<br />

the rank-order stability <strong>of</strong> the Big Five increases across the life span. These results<br />

were recently supplemented with regard to old and very old age by longitudinal<br />

<strong>aging</strong> studies (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003; Small et al., 2003).<br />

Contrary to some trait personality theorists' view that personality is "set like<br />

plaster" after the age <strong>of</strong> 30 (Costa& McCrae, 7994), cross-sectional as well as longitudinal<br />

evidence on mean levels <strong>of</strong> personality show changes in some <strong>of</strong> the Big<br />

Five characteristics across adulthood. Neuroticism, for example, decreases across<br />

adulthood (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003) and shows some increase again in very old<br />

age (Small et al., 2003). Moreover, in addition to becoming more neurotic, elderly<br />

people tend to become less open to new experiences and less extraverted (Costa,<br />

Herbst, McCrae, & Siegler, 2000), whereas they tend to become slightly more<br />

agreeable and conscientious (Helson & Kwan, 2000).<br />

Summarizing this empirical evidence based on trait as well as growth models <strong>of</strong><br />

personality clearly suggests that personality development in adulthood and old<br />

age is characterized by both stability and change. Regarding the relationship <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Big Five to subjective indicators <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> well, neuroticism is typically found to be<br />

negatively associated with measures <strong>of</strong> subjective well-being, whereas extraversion<br />

shows a positive relation to well-being. For instance, Isaacowitz and Smith (2003)<br />

demonstrated that in old and very old age (70 to 102 years), neuroticism is linked to<br />

higher negative and lower positive affect. The opposite pattern was found for extraversion<br />

(negative correlation with negative affect, positive correlation with positive<br />

affect). Note, however, that this pattern also holds for younger age groups (e.g., Diener.<br />

1984) and thus does not seem to constitute a deaelopmenfal phenomenon.<br />

Jurrrruc ro LEVEL III: PEnsoNal IpeNtttv<br />

In contrast to dispositional traits, life narratives are heavily influenced by specificities<br />

<strong>of</strong> the context and are subject to change across situations and time.<br />

McAdams (1990) argues that a unified or integrated life story, however, provides<br />

individuals with an identity and allows them to maintain a coherent self-concept<br />

across the entire life span (Cohen, 1998). Life stories are told to different audiences<br />

with different purposes: A life story told to a future spouse might look very<br />

different from the life story told to a future employer, not because the narrator<br />

becomes a different person in between the two events but because different aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> a biography seem important and merit elaboration in these two contexts.<br />

According to McAdams, identity is not derived from one (veridical) life story but<br />

from the very act <strong>of</strong> meaning-making and integration <strong>of</strong> different aspects <strong>of</strong> a life<br />

into one coherent story (albeit possibly a different one each time it is told).<br />

The concept <strong>of</strong> autobiographical memories is closely related to the concept <strong>of</strong><br />

life stories (see also Habermas & Bluck, 2000). Some <strong>theories</strong> on autobiographical


92 EvrpsNcr-BasEpTueoRv<br />

memory (e.g., Pillemer, 7992) hold that it can serve three broad functions: directive<br />

(planning for present and future behaviors), self (self-continuity, psychodynamic<br />

integrity), and social (social bonding, communication; Bluck & Alea, 2002).<br />

The directive function involves using the past during conversations to guide present<br />

and future thought and behavior such as problem solving, developing opinions<br />

and attitudes, or providing flexibility in the construction and updating <strong>of</strong><br />

rules. The self-function provides the ability to support and promote continuity<br />

and development <strong>of</strong> the self and preserves a sense <strong>of</strong> being a coherent person over<br />

time. This function serves to explain oneself to others (and to oneself ). The social<br />

(intimacy or bonding) function, f inally, is important for developing, maintaining,<br />

and nurturing social relationships. This makes us a more believable and persuasive<br />

partner in conversations, provides the listener with information about ourselves,<br />

and allows us to better understand and empathize with others.<br />

Life stories can be related to <strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong> in multiple<br />

ways. As pointed out by Erikson (1968),life review becomes a major developmental<br />

task in old age. This life stage, according to Erikson, is characterized by<br />

the two opposite poles <strong>of</strong> "integrity" and "despair." Following Erikson, wisdom<br />

emerges when people are able to accept both the positive and negative events <strong>of</strong><br />

their lives and do not simply gloss over disappointments and failures but are able<br />

to integrate them in a coherent life story together with positive aspects <strong>of</strong> their<br />

past lives. If one understands <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> as reaching this point, one might<br />

argue that positivity <strong>of</strong> the life story should not be a criterion <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> well. Interestingly,<br />

already at midage, it is the theme <strong>of</strong> redemption (i.e., turning a negative<br />

event into a positive one), not a smooth life path filled with positive events, that<br />

is, for instance, related to generativity (i.e., giving to the next generation in some<br />

way; McAdams,2001) as a possible indicator <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development.<br />

Another argument could be made for continuity <strong>of</strong> the life story as a milestone <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong> (Bluck & Alea, 2002). As it turns out, however,<br />

continuity <strong>of</strong> a life story over time is not necessarily related to positive functioning<br />

in old age. ln a study by Coleman, Ivani-Chalian, and Robinson (1998), a substantial<br />

percentage <strong>of</strong> their sample <strong>of</strong> older adults (> 80 years) did not conceive <strong>of</strong> their life<br />

story as a coherent one, but were not necessarily dissatisfied with their present life.<br />

The specific autobiographical memories that are most accessible at a given time<br />

may largely depend on a person's goals. As goal-relevant information is more important<br />

than information not related to personal goals, it is also more likely to be<br />

activated and accessible at a certain time point (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000).<br />

As we will see, personal goals can change over the life span. As a consequence, a<br />

set <strong>of</strong> memories that was once goal-related and self-defining may no longer be so<br />

at a later point in life (Singer & Salovey, 1993). Abandoned earlier goals, however,<br />

can define one's "past self" and thereby provide a psychological history <strong>of</strong><br />

changes in the self. In this way, changes in goals can contribute to life narratives<br />

("What did I use to want in the past, and how did it change over time?").<br />

People construe their self and their identity partially around their personal<br />

goals because goals can <strong>of</strong>fer a sense <strong>of</strong> purpose and meaning in life (Little, 1989)<br />

and provide the possibility for integration <strong>of</strong> otherwise unconnected behaviors<br />

into one unifying frame (Freund, 2006b). As will be elaborated more in the next<br />

sections, personal goals can therefore be seen as part <strong>of</strong> the binding thread that<br />

links the person with his or her context (Freund, 2006b; Little, 1989; McAdams,<br />

1996). This will then clarify how personal concerns and personal goals as Level II<br />

constructs link Level I and Level III.<br />

PERSC<br />

From early on, I<br />

blocks <strong>of</strong> persona<br />

ality, Allport (193<br />

motives as perso<br />

be divorced fron<br />

conduct employt<br />

have the capacit.<br />

tiate responses a<br />

characteristic <strong>of</strong>,<br />

Personal goals ,<br />

tions <strong>of</strong> states a 1<br />

mons/ 1996). They<br />

(1990) suggests lo<br />

tween "being" (i.r<br />

ioral responses ir<br />

That is, goals are<br />

they may be influr<br />

they motivate, str<br />

ingful action unit<br />

Klinger, 7977) an<br />

(cf. Boesch, 7991;<br />

tions in a given r<br />

witzer & Moskow<br />

reduce situationa<br />

As goals are it<br />

Level II <strong>of</strong> personi<br />

<strong>of</strong> personality-inand<br />

his or her eru<br />

the person's basi<br />

or she lives. Spec<br />

processes that sp<br />

crucial in this re1<br />

in the present con<br />

ger , 2006, for a re<<br />

and underlie goal<br />

DrprNrNc CoNrr><br />

Context can be t<br />

person, such as c<br />

environment. It dr<br />

personality, but al<br />

or geographic con<br />

nities in that they<br />

can express his or<br />

siblings may rend


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 93<br />

broad functions: direclf-continuity,<br />

psychodyon;<br />

Bluck & Alea, 2002).<br />

ersations to guide preslving,<br />

developing opin-<br />

Ltction and updating <strong>of</strong><br />

nd promote continuity<br />

; a coherent person over<br />

I to oneself). The social<br />

:veloping, maintaining,<br />

believable and persua-<br />

.nformation about ourwith<br />

others.<br />

and <strong>aging</strong> in multiple<br />

ts a major developmenn,<br />

is characterized by<br />

wing Erikson, wisdom<br />

and negative events <strong>of</strong><br />

rd failures but are able<br />

rsitive aspects <strong>of</strong> their<br />

;this point, one might<br />

on <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> well. Intert.e.,<br />

turning a negative<br />

h positive events, that<br />

:xt generation in some<br />

levelopment.<br />

story as a milestone <strong>of</strong><br />

it turns out, however,<br />

:o positive functioning<br />

ln (1998), a substantial<br />

rt conceive <strong>of</strong> their life<br />

with their present life.<br />

essible at a given time<br />

formation is more imalso<br />

more likely to be<br />

)leydell-Pearce,<br />

2000).<br />

.. As a consequence/ a<br />

I may no longer be so<br />

rarlier goals, however,<br />

:hological history <strong>of</strong><br />

rute to life narratives<br />

>ver time?").<br />

round their personal<br />

g in life (Little, 1989)<br />

connected behaviors<br />

ted more in the next<br />

rbinding thread that<br />

Itle, 7989; McAdams,<br />

onal goals as Level II<br />

PERSONAL GOALS: PERSONALITY_IN.CONTEXT<br />

From early on, personality psychology has conceptualized goals as building<br />

blocks <strong>of</strong> personality (Freund& Riediger, 2006).In his dynamic theory <strong>of</strong> personality,<br />

Allport (7937, pp.320-321,) regards<br />

motives as personalized systems <strong>of</strong> tensions, in which the core <strong>of</strong> impulse is not tcr<br />

be divorced from the images, ideas <strong>of</strong> goal, past experience, capacities, and style <strong>of</strong><br />

conduct employed in obtaining the goal. Only individualized patterns <strong>of</strong> motives<br />

have the capacity to select stimuli, to control and direct segmental tensions, to initiate<br />

responses and to render them equivalent, in ways that are consistent with, and<br />

characteristic <strong>of</strong>, the person himself.<br />

Personal goals are <strong>of</strong>ten defined as consciously accessible cognitive representations<br />

<strong>of</strong> states a person wants to achieve, maintain, or avoid in the future (Emmons,<br />

1996). They are relatively consistent across situations and over time. Cantor<br />

(1990) suggests locating goals at an intermediate level <strong>of</strong> personality, the level between<br />

"being" G.e., personality traits, basic dispositions) and "doing" (i.e., behavioral<br />

responses in a given situation), a distinction introduced by Allport (7937).<br />

That is, goals are not as broad and comprehensive as personality traits, although<br />

they may be inf luenced by them. They are also not as specific as behaviors. Rather,<br />

they motivate, structure, and direct behavior over time and situations into meaningful<br />

action units (Baumeister, 1991; Emmons, 1986; Ford,7987; Gollwitzer, 1990;<br />

Klinger, 1977) and favor the acquisition and use <strong>of</strong> resources to obtain the goals<br />

(cf. Boesch, 1991; Freund, 2003). Coals can focus attention on those stimuli and actions<br />

in a given situation that are goal-relevant (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Gollwitzer<br />

& Moskowit 2,7996; Kruglanski ,7996; Pervin, 1989). As a consequence, they<br />

reduce situational complexity and facilitate interaction with the environment.<br />

As goals are inherently content- or domain-specific, Little (1989) called this<br />

Level II <strong>of</strong> personality the "personality-in-context." According to Little, the notion<br />

<strong>of</strong> personality-in-context is based on the idea <strong>of</strong> an interaction between the person<br />

and his or her environment over time. Personal goals ref lect the interface between<br />

the person's basic personality traits and the specific environment in which he<br />

or she lives. Specifically, the content a goal refers to as well as the goal-related<br />

processes that specify how people select, pursue, and disengage from goals are<br />

crucial in this regard. Addressing the issue <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong><br />

in the present context, we do not focus on the content <strong>of</strong> goals (see Freund & Riediger,2006,<br />

for a recent overview) but on the motivational processes that accompany<br />

and underlie goal-related processes and their relations to <strong>successful</strong> development.<br />

DpprrvrNc CoNrExr<br />

Context can be broadly defined as the set <strong>of</strong> circumstances that surrounds a<br />

person, such as culture, historical time, family, social relations, and geographical<br />

environment. It does not only serve as a background for the behavioral expression <strong>of</strong><br />

personality, but also plays an important role in shaping personality. Cultural, social,<br />

or geographic conditions may represent limitations as well as chances and opportunities<br />

in that they exclude or allow certain possibilities <strong>of</strong> whether and how a person<br />

can express his or her basic personality traits. For example, growing up with older<br />

siblings may render a person more agreeable because only by being more agreeable


94 EvrpENce-BasepTuEoRv<br />

might the younger child, constantly lagging behind the older siblings regarding verbal,<br />

cognitive, and motor skills, be able to achieve his or her goals. Contextual constraints<br />

fulfill an important function in that they specify the boundaries without<br />

which personality development would be impossible, as the space <strong>of</strong> possible developmental<br />

trajectories would be too vast and unstructured otherwise.<br />

Life span-developmental psychology (P. g. Baltes, 1987, 7997) provides a useful<br />

framework for understanding how person and context interact over time. This theoretical<br />

framework regards development as a lifelong process <strong>of</strong> flexible adaptation<br />

to changes in opportunities and constraints on biological, social, and<br />

psychological levels (P. B. Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, 2006; P. B. Baltes,<br />

Reese, & Lipsitt,1980; Brandtstddter & Lerner, 7999;Bronfenbrenner, l9S8; Lerner<br />

& Busch-Rossnagel, 1981). This assumption has two implications: (1) Individuals<br />

exist in environments that create and define possibilities and limiting bclundaries<br />

for individual pathways; (2) individuals select and create their own living contexts<br />

and thus can play an active part in shaping their lives.<br />

How ro INrrnncr wtrn ONr's CoNlrxr: Goels aNp<br />

Coa l-Rp lerep Pnocnsses<br />

Goals and the basic regulatory processes <strong>of</strong> goal selection, pursuit, and disengagement<br />

are central aspects that help to explain an individual's interaction with his or<br />

her environment (Brandtstddter, 1998; Chapman & Skinner,1985; Emmons, 1986; Freund,<br />

2003, 2006b; Freund & Baltes, 2000; Gollwitzer & Bargh, 1996; Heckhausen/<br />

1999). Goals emerge from and determine the nature <strong>of</strong> people's transactions with the<br />

surrounding world. Experiencing the effects but also the limitations <strong>of</strong> goal-directed<br />

action can feed back to the individual's view <strong>of</strong> the world, the self, personal goals, beliefs<br />

and wishes, and further goal-directed actions. Thus, there is an interplay among<br />

goals, goal-directed actions, and developmental contexts (Brandtstiidter, 1998).<br />

Addressing the contextual nature <strong>of</strong> goals, Freund (2003) distinguishes two interacting<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> representation: social expectations and personal goals. Social<br />

expectations are ref lected in social norms that inform about age-graded opportunity<br />

structures. Age-related social norms can define opportunities and limitations<br />

for developmental trajectories <strong>of</strong> a person's life course. For example, they<br />

indicate the appropriate time to marry and to have children. Thereby, age-related<br />

social expectations or norms can also give an indication about the availability <strong>of</strong><br />

resources in certain life domains for specific age groups (Freund, 2003; Heckhausen,<br />

1999). Setting personal goals in accordance with social norms and expectations<br />

may help to take advantage <strong>of</strong> these available resources. Through social<br />

sanctions as well as approval or disapproval by the society, social expectations<br />

can serve as an orientation or standard for the development, selection, pursuit,<br />

maintenance, and disengagement <strong>of</strong> personal goals (Cantor,7994;Freund,7997;<br />

Nurmi, 1992). Social expectations are also reflected in personal beliefs about the<br />

appropriate timing and sequencing <strong>of</strong> goals, and personal beliefs incorporate individual<br />

values and experiences. Together, social and personal expectations directly<br />

influence behavior and also the personal goals an individual selects and<br />

pursues. This influence can be conscious or unconscious, as, in addition to consciously<br />

represented goals, automatized goals and unconscious motives impact<br />

on behavior and development (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000).<br />

In sum, goals can be viewed as the building blocks <strong>of</strong> personality, person-context<br />

interactions, and lifelong development. The concept <strong>of</strong> personal goals seems particularly<br />

well suited fo<br />

ful <strong>aging</strong> as it allows<br />

furthers our unders<br />

vidual differences ir<br />

particular imPortan<<br />

alternatives and the<br />

goals, also in the fa<br />

prove individual con<br />

for further goal atta.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the basic<br />

developmental Pers<br />

interaction with a 1<br />

actively shaPe their<br />

Ford, 1987; Freund<br />

the person to his or<br />

opment across the I<br />

consider goal selec<br />

goal-related resour<br />

TrtE Rolp oF GoALI<br />

FOR SUCCESSPUT- DE<br />

By nature, human<br />

around the Pursuit<br />

competence, relate<br />

ideal outcomes aga<br />

their progress in tl<br />

ness <strong>of</strong> their goal-r<br />

has shown that Per<br />

Omodei & Wearinl<br />

the mechanisms ul<br />

Processes influenc'<br />

There are differt<br />

<strong>successful</strong> develoP<br />

their goals or reac<br />

1,954; McClelland,<br />

well-being dePend<br />

when these are in<br />

Rogers, 1963; RYan<br />

authors argue tha<br />

(Brunstein, Schult<br />

goals themselves t<br />

related to life sa<br />

Rothermund (2002<br />

argue that neither<br />

<strong>successful</strong> develoP<br />

depression if theY<br />

Thus, setting at<br />

important self-reg<br />

& Renner,7990;C


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 95<br />

:r siblings regarding verrr<br />

goals. Contextual conthe<br />

boundaries without<br />

l space <strong>of</strong> possible develrtherwise.<br />

1997) provides a useful<br />

'ract over time. This thercess<br />

<strong>of</strong> flexible adaptabiological,<br />

social, and<br />

nger,2006; P. B. Baltes,<br />

enbrenner, 1988; Lerner<br />

cations: (1) Individuals<br />

Lnd limiting boundaries<br />

heir own living contexts<br />

pursuit, and disengage-<br />

; interaction with his or<br />

985; Emmons , 1986; Frergh,<br />

7996; Heckhausen,<br />

l's transactions with the<br />

itations <strong>of</strong> goal-directed<br />

r self, personal goals, bere<br />

is an interplay among<br />

ndtstiidter, 1998).<br />

;) distinguishes two inpersonal<br />

goals. Social<br />

tt age-graded opportu-<br />

,ortunities and limitarse.<br />

For example, they<br />

n. Thereby, age-related<br />

bout the availability <strong>of</strong><br />

(Freund, 2003; Hcckrcial<br />

norms and expcc-<br />

)urces. Through social<br />

ty, social expectations<br />

:nt, selection, pursuit,<br />

>r,7994; Freund, 1997;<br />

sonal beliefs about the<br />

beliefs incorporate insonal<br />

expectations dindividual<br />

selects and<br />

as, in addition to conicious<br />

motives impact<br />

rnality, person-context<br />

nalgoalseems partic-<br />

ularly well suited for a developmental approach to personality <strong>theories</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong><br />

<strong>aging</strong> as it allows integrating motivational processes into a life span context and<br />

furthers our understanding <strong>of</strong> both the direction <strong>of</strong> development and <strong>of</strong> interindividual<br />

differences in the leuel <strong>of</strong> functioning in various life domains over time. Of<br />

particular importance in this regard is the selection <strong>of</strong> goals from a pool <strong>of</strong> possible<br />

alternatives and the investment <strong>of</strong> resources into the pursuit and maintenance <strong>of</strong><br />

goals, also in the face <strong>of</strong> setbacks and losses. Goal selection and pursuit can improve<br />

individual competences and generate new resources, which can then be used<br />

for further goal attainment.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the basic assumptions <strong>of</strong> joining an action-theoretical with a life spandevelopmental<br />

perspective in personality <strong>theories</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> is that, in<br />

interaction with a given physical, cultural-historical, and social context, people<br />

actively shape their own developmental context (Brandtstiidter & Lerner, 7999;<br />

Ford, 1987; Freund & Baltes, 2000; Lerner & Busch-Rossnagel, 1981). Goals link<br />

the person to his or her life contexts and thus are central to personality and development<br />

across the life span. In the following, we introduce three approaches that<br />

consider goal selection, initiation <strong>of</strong> goal-related actions, and the investment <strong>of</strong><br />

goal-reiated resources as crucial for achieving desired developmental outcomes.<br />

TuE Rolr or Conr.s aNp GonI-RELATED PRocessgs<br />

roR Succsssr-'u r. Duv pI-oPMENT<br />

By nature, human beings are goal-oriented organisms. They organize their lives<br />

around the pursuit <strong>of</strong> goals that reflect their fundamental needs (e.9., autonomy,<br />

competence, relatedness; Ryan, 1995). Goals provide persons with standards and<br />

ideal outcomes against which they can evaluate their actual level <strong>of</strong> functioning,<br />

their progress in the direction <strong>of</strong> higher levels <strong>of</strong> functioning, and the effectiveness<br />

<strong>of</strong> their goal-related behaviors (Carver & Scheier, 1990). Empirical evidence<br />

has shown that personal goals are positively related to well-being (Emmons,7986;<br />

Omodei & Wearing, 1990; Palys & Little, 1983; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). What are<br />

the mechanisms underlying this relation? That is, how do goals and goal-related<br />

processes inf luence well-being and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>?<br />

There are different views on hor,v goals affect well-being. Tt is <strong>of</strong>ten argued that<br />

<strong>successful</strong> development implies that individuals succeed in progressing toward<br />

their goals or reaching desired states (M. M. Baltes & Carstensen, 1996; Maslow,<br />

1954; McClelland, 1987). Humanistic psychology proposes that a person's sense <strong>of</strong><br />

well-being depends on the person's progress toward his or her goals, especially<br />

when these are in accord with "organismic" or "innate" needs (Maslow, 1954;<br />

Rogers, 1963; Ryan, 1995; see also Brunstein, Schultheiss, & Crdssman, 1998). Other<br />

authors argue that having goals can in itself be a predictor <strong>of</strong> life satisfaction<br />

(Brunstein, Schultheiss, & Maier, 1999; Emmons, 7996). Still others posit that not<br />

goals themselves but rather the al'ailability <strong>of</strong> goal-related resources is positively<br />

related to life satisfaction (Diener & Fujita, 1995). Finally, Brandtstddter and<br />

Rothermund (2002) maintain that goals can ambivalently affect well-being. They<br />

argue that neither goals nor resources themselves bring about goal attainment or<br />

<strong>successful</strong> development. In fact, unattainable goals can lead to dissatisfaction and<br />

depression if they are not abandoned or readjusted.<br />

Thus, setting and pursuing challenging but attainable goals and standards are<br />

important self-regulatory processes conducive to <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> (Brandtsttidter<br />

& Renner, 1990; Carver & Scheier, 7981,7982; Freund & Baltes,2000; Heckhausen,


96 EvrppNcr:-BesppTueorrv<br />

1999). In addition, diseng<strong>aging</strong> from goals that are no longer attainable, restructuring<br />

goals, and reallocating goal-related resources into alternative goals and<br />

goal domains are facets <strong>of</strong> adaptive functioning (Klinger, 1975; Wrosch & Heckhausen,1999).<br />

To elaborate further on the idea that personal goals and goal-related processes<br />

are central for <strong>successful</strong> development and to understand better the underlying<br />

mechanisms <strong>of</strong> the relationship between goals and well-being, we introduce three<br />

current self-regulatory <strong>theories</strong> <strong>of</strong> adaptive development in the following part <strong>of</strong><br />

this chapter. All three <strong>theories</strong> view development as multidirectional, comprising<br />

gains and losses across the entire life span. They refer to personal goals and basic<br />

goal-related processes such as setting, pursuing, reformulating, and diseng<strong>aging</strong><br />

from goals as means to <strong>successful</strong>ly shape individual development. All three<br />

frameworks converge in their assumption that resources are Iimited throughout<br />

life, and increasingly so in old age, and that <strong>successful</strong> developmental regulation<br />

requires suitable mechanisms for allocation <strong>of</strong> these limited resources. The three<br />

<strong>theories</strong> vary, however, in their particular focus (e.g., on life span strategies and<br />

personal goals, coping processes, and control processes), in their particular characteristics<br />

<strong>of</strong> the proposed mechanism <strong>of</strong> resource allocation, as well as in their<br />

postulated generality. Table 4.2 summarizes the three theoretical frameworks.<br />

Table 4.2<br />

Goal-Related Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging<br />

Theoretical Framework Central Strateoies Specif ic Examples<br />

Model <strong>of</strong> Selection,<br />

Optimization, and<br />

Compensation<br />

Dual-Process Model <strong>of</strong><br />

Assimilative and<br />

Accommodative Coping<br />

Model <strong>of</strong> Optimization in<br />

Primary and Secondary<br />

Control<br />

Selection<br />

Optimization<br />

Compensation<br />

Assimilative coping<br />

Accommodative coping<br />

Selective primary<br />

control<br />

Compensatory primary<br />

control<br />

Selective secondary<br />

control<br />

Compensatory<br />

secondary control<br />

Focusing on Spanish as a foreign<br />

ranguage<br />

Investing more time to improve<br />

Spanish-language skills<br />

Hiring a translator when traveling<br />

in Spain<br />

Altering dietary habits to attain<br />

the desired bodily appearance<br />

Accepting your physical state and<br />

giving up the goal to run the<br />

marathon after knee surgery<br />

Seeing each other more f requently<br />

to improve the relationship<br />

Seeing a marriage guidance counselor<br />

to improve the relationship<br />

Regarding the current partner as<br />

the "only love <strong>of</strong> life"<br />

Regarding the former partner as<br />

a "wrong choice" af ter the<br />

relationship ends<br />

TrrsoRrrtcel<br />

THE }<br />

A<br />

MI<br />

UNDE<br />

Assu<br />

The model <strong>of</strong> selec<br />

eral, metatheoretir<br />

P. B. Baltes,7997;P<br />

It assumes a dYna<br />

stages <strong>of</strong> life (chik<br />

different levels <strong>of</strong> I<br />

ferent domains <strong>of</strong><br />

regulation). In the<br />

source generation<br />

processes bY addr<br />

tioning. As such, S<br />

that helP to manal<br />

ory proPoses thre<br />

ment: selection, opt<br />

have various Poss<br />

(e.g., cognition, sc<br />

vary along the <<br />

unconscious (Fret<br />

At the most ger<br />

ization or canaliz<br />

Selection includes<br />

ternative develoP<br />

limited resource<br />

through this conc<br />

evolve. Thus, sel<br />

sources (P. B. Bal<br />

losses in resource<br />

Optimizatron rc<br />

nance <strong>of</strong> internal<br />

els <strong>of</strong> functionir<br />

achieving higher<br />

pertise. ExPertis<br />

achieved onlY tt<br />

Krampe, & Tesch<br />

ComPensatrcnr<br />

functions in Prer<br />

gains nnd losses i<br />

ful <strong>aging</strong> needs<br />

model does this I<br />

encountering a l<<br />

in the face <strong>of</strong> lost<br />

The SOC thec<br />

tives, such as soc


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 97<br />

rger attainable, restruc,<br />

o alternative goals and<br />

1975; Wrosch & Heck-<br />

I goal-related processes<br />

I better the underlying<br />

ing, we introduce three<br />

n the following part <strong>of</strong><br />

Cirectional, comprising<br />

ersonal goals and basic<br />

ating, and diseng<strong>aging</strong><br />

levelopment. All three<br />

rre limited throughout<br />

relopmental regulation<br />

ld resources. The three<br />

ife span strategies and<br />

r their particular charion,<br />

as well as in their<br />

retical frameworks.<br />

'lg<br />

lecific Examples<br />

on Spanish as a foreign<br />

Trore time to improve<br />

rnguage skills<br />

anslator when traveling<br />

etary habits to attain<br />

1 bodily appearance<br />

your physical state and<br />

he goal to run the<br />

rfter knee surgery<br />

;h other more f requently<br />

the relationship<br />

larriage guidance coun-<br />

)rove the relationship<br />

the current partner as<br />

rve <strong>of</strong> life"<br />

the former partner as<br />

hoice" af ter the<br />

) ends<br />

THE MODEL OF SELECTION, OPTIMIZATION,<br />

AND COMPENSATION: A GENERAL,<br />

METATHEORETICAL FRAMEWORK FOR<br />

UNDERSTANDING ADAPTIVE DEVELOPMENT<br />

TutonprrceL Assu M pt'toNS<br />

The model <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization, and compensation (SOC) constitutes a general,<br />

metatheoretical framework for understanding human development (e.9.,<br />

P. B. Baltes, 1997;P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 7980,7990; P. B. Baltes, Freund, & Li,2005).<br />

It assumes a dynamic between developmental gains and losses across various<br />

stages <strong>of</strong> life (childhood, adolescence, adulthood, old age) that is represented at<br />

different levels <strong>of</strong> analysis (e.g., neuronal, behavioral) and within and across different<br />

domains <strong>of</strong> functioning (e.g., physical or cognitive development, emotion<br />

regulation). In the context <strong>of</strong> SOC theory, life span development is a process <strong>of</strong> resource<br />

generation and regulation. This theory describes general developmental<br />

processes by addressing both the direction <strong>of</strong> development and the leael <strong>of</strong> functioning.<br />

As such, SOC is a model <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development, specifying processes<br />

that help to manage changing resources over the life span. In particular, the theory<br />

proposes three fundamental and universal processes <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development:<br />

selection, optirnization, and compensatiotr. All three processes are proposed to<br />

have various possible phenotypic realizations depending on functional domains<br />

(e.g., cognition, social), sociocultural contexts, and person-specific features that<br />

vary along the dimensions active-passive, internal-external, and consciousunconscious<br />

(Freund, Li, & Baltes,7999).<br />

At the most general level <strong>of</strong> definition, selection refers to the process <strong>of</strong> specialization<br />

or canalization <strong>of</strong> a particular pathway or set <strong>of</strong> pathways <strong>of</strong> development.<br />

Selection includes the delineating and narrowing down <strong>of</strong> a range <strong>of</strong> possible alternative<br />

developmental trajectories. Selection also serves the management <strong>of</strong><br />

limited resources by concentrating resources on delineated domains. Only<br />

through this concentration can specification occur and certain skills and abilities<br />

evolve. Thus, selection is a general-purpose mechanism to generate new resources<br />

(P. B. Baltes et al., 2005). Selection can occur electiaely or as a response to<br />

losses in resources (i.e., Ioss-based).<br />

Optinizatiot'L refers to the acquisition, application, coordination, and maintenance<br />

<strong>of</strong> internal and external resources (means) involved in attaining higher levels<br />

<strong>of</strong> functioning (Freund & Baltes, 2000). Optimization is important for<br />

achieving higher levels <strong>of</strong> functioning, as has been shown in the literature on expertise.<br />

Expertise, a high skill level in a selected functional domain, can be<br />

achieved only through a substantial amount <strong>of</strong> deliberate practice (Ericsson,<br />

Krampe, & Tesch-Romer,7993; Krampe & Baltes,2003).<br />

Compensntion refers to the investment <strong>of</strong> means to avoid or counteract losses in<br />

functions in previously available resources. Given that development entails both<br />

gains and losses across the entire life span (P. B. Baltes , 7987), a model <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong><br />

<strong>aging</strong> needs to take processes <strong>of</strong> man<strong>aging</strong> losses into account. The SOC<br />

model does this by including loss-based selection (referring to reorientation after<br />

encountering a loss) and compensation (aimed at the maintenance <strong>of</strong> functioning<br />

in the face <strong>of</strong> losses).<br />

The SOC theory can be approached from many different theoretical perspectives,<br />

such as social, behavioral-learning, cognitive, and neuropsychological (M. M.


98 EvropNcE-BesppTHectnv<br />

Baltes & Carstensen,1996; P. B. Baltes et a1.,2005; P. B. Baltes & Singer,2001). Freund<br />

and Baltes (2000) propose an action-theoretical conceptualization <strong>of</strong> SOC. This<br />

approach embeds SOC in the context <strong>of</strong> active life-management and refers to developmental<br />

regulation through the selection, pursuit, and maintenance <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

goals.<br />

From an action-theoretical perspective, selectiotr denotes processes related to<br />

goal setting. Electiae selection refers to developing and choosing a subset <strong>of</strong> goals<br />

out <strong>of</strong> multiple options. Given the finite nature <strong>of</strong> resources, selection needs to<br />

take place to realize developmental potentials. Loss-based selection refers to adapting<br />

existing or developing new goals to fit losses in resources. Restructuring<br />

one's goal hierarchy by exclusively focusing on the most important goal, Iowering<br />

one's aspiration level, or finding a substitution for unattainable goals are possible<br />

manifestations. From an action-theoretical perspective, optimization and compensation<br />

refer to processes <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit, that is, the investment <strong>of</strong> resources<br />

into obtaining and refining goal-relevant means. Optimization refers to means for<br />

achieving desired outcomes and aims at coordination, acquisition, and generation<br />

<strong>of</strong> new resources or skills, serving the growth aspect <strong>of</strong> development. Compensation,<br />

from an action-theoretical perspective, addresses the regulation <strong>of</strong> loss or<br />

decline in means and goal-relevant resources that threatens the maintenance <strong>of</strong> a<br />

given level <strong>of</strong> functioning. When previous goal-relevant means are no longer<br />

available, compensation can involve substitution <strong>of</strong> lost means. It thus denotes a<br />

strategy for man<strong>aging</strong> and counteracting losses by changing goal-relevant means<br />

to attain a certain goal instead <strong>of</strong> choosing another goal (as in loss-based selection).<br />

The use <strong>of</strong> hearing aids to counteract hearing losses is one example. Phenotypically,<br />

the strategies used for optimization and compensation, such as<br />

acquisition and practice <strong>of</strong> skills, energy and time investment, or activation <strong>of</strong><br />

new or unused resources, can overlap.<br />

The theory states that the use <strong>of</strong> SOC strategies changes over the life span as a<br />

function <strong>of</strong> available resources. The expected peak <strong>of</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> all three regulatory<br />

principles is in adulthood, with elective selection and compensation increasing<br />

in importance with advancing age when resource losses become more<br />

salient (Freund & Baltes, 2002b).<br />

Aoaplrve DEvplopvgNr as ENpoRSEMENT oF THE Mopal ol Snr-ecrroN.<br />

OptrHarzarroN, ANI) Col,rrnxsa'noN Tr{Roucuout LrrE<br />

Several studies provide evidence for the role <strong>of</strong> SOC in the description, explanation,<br />

and modification <strong>of</strong> development and support their impact on adaptive lifemanagement<br />

(see P. B. Baltes et aL.,2005, for an overview). Evidence suggests that<br />

the relative prevalence <strong>of</strong> using and coordinating SOC changes with age and that<br />

people who engage in SOC behaviors show more positive outcomes. The existing<br />

research comprises different empirical specifications referring to global levels<br />

and specific life contexts. It encompasses different assessment levels such as selfreport<br />

measures (P. B. Baltes, Baltes, Freund, & Lang, 1999), behavior observation<br />

(".9., M. M. Baltes, 7996), and experimental studies (e.g., Li, Lindenberger, Freund,<br />

& Baltes, 2001). Some approaches simultaneously investigate all three components<br />

<strong>of</strong> SOC as well as their orchestration (e.g., Riediger, Li, & Lindenberger,<br />

2006), whereas others focus on some <strong>of</strong> the processes only (e.g., Ebner, Freund, &<br />

Baltes, 2006; Freund, 2006a).<br />

Addressing the c<br />

edge <strong>of</strong> SOC as stri<br />

identified a bodY o<br />

are represented in<br />

human life as exprr<br />

proverb "You can't<br />

proverbs were use(<br />

tions <strong>of</strong>, or preferet<br />

SOC model. In a <<br />

SOC-related and a<br />

life-management si<br />

more frequently ar<br />

theory, these PrefeI<br />

in tasks referring tr<br />

leisure. This sugge<br />

SOC-related prove<br />

and that their judgt<br />

in line with what tl<br />

To investigate tl<br />

adaptive life-manal<br />

the SOC questionr<br />

(2002b) investigate<br />

adults and found I<br />

haviors reported t<br />

being. The associ<br />

variables such as tt<br />

teristics such as ne<br />

expected, SOC-rel<br />

hood; that is, mit<br />

younger and older<br />

positive age trend.<br />

less use <strong>of</strong> oPtimiz<br />

mization and com<br />

strategies are effor<br />

sources available il<br />

ple suffering from<br />

SOC questionnairr<br />

(1998) showed thal<br />

adults betweenT2<br />

strategies also shc<br />

tive affect, satisfac<br />

Wiese and collt<br />

model to goal pu<br />

questionnaire stu<br />

haviors <strong>of</strong> younge<br />

specifically. High<br />

strategies was Pot<br />

nal data replicate<br />

ment in SOC beh;


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successfttl Aging 99<br />

Ites & Singer,2001). Frertualization<br />

<strong>of</strong> SOC. Th is<br />

ment and refers to develmaintenance<br />

<strong>of</strong> personal<br />

tes processes related to<br />

losing a subset <strong>of</strong> goals<br />

rces, selection needs rcr<br />

selection refers to adapt-<br />

)sources. Restructuring<br />

nportant goal, lowering<br />

nable goals are possible<br />

optimization and comnvestment<br />

<strong>of</strong> resources<br />

tion refers to means for<br />

risition, and generation<br />

levelopment. Compensa-<br />

Le regulation <strong>of</strong> loss or<br />

ts the maintenance <strong>of</strong> a<br />

: means are no longer<br />

eans. It thus denotes a<br />

rg goal-relevant means<br />

fas in loss-based selecis<br />

one example. Phenormpensation,<br />

such as<br />

:ment, or activation <strong>of</strong><br />

i over the life span as a<br />

ession <strong>of</strong> all three regand<br />

compensation ine<br />

losses become more<br />

,F SELECTION,<br />

description, explanarpact<br />

on adaptive lifelvidence<br />

suggests that<br />

Lges with age and that<br />

utcomes. The existing<br />

rring to global levels<br />

:nt levels such as selfbehavior<br />

observation<br />

,i, Lindenberger, Frestigate<br />

all three com-<br />

', Li, & Lindenberger,<br />

).9., Ebner, Freund, &<br />

Addressing the question whether there exists cultural and individual knowledge<br />

<strong>of</strong> SOC as strategies <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> development, Freund and Baltes (2002a)<br />

identified a body <strong>of</strong> proverbs reflecting SOC. This suggests that SOC strategies<br />

are represented in cultural knowledge about pragmatic aspects <strong>of</strong> everyday<br />

human life as expressed in proverbs. Selection, for instance, is expressed in the<br />

oroverb "You can't have the cake and eat it too." In a series <strong>of</strong> studies, these<br />

proverbs were used to examine whether younger and older adults' lay conceptions<br />

<strong>of</strong>, or preferences for, life-management behaviors were consistent with the<br />

SOC model. In a choice-reaction time task, participants wele asked to match<br />

SOC-related and alternative proverbs to sentence stems indicative <strong>of</strong> everyday<br />

life-management situations. Both age groups chose SOC-related proverbs both<br />

more frequently and faster than alternative proverbs. Consistent with the SOC<br />

theory, these preferences for SOC-related proverbs over alternatives existed only<br />

in tasks referring to goals and success, but not in contexts involving relaxation or<br />

leisure. This suggests that both younger and older adults have a preference for<br />

SOC-related proverbs over alternatives in everyday life-management situations<br />

and that their judgment <strong>of</strong> the adaptive use <strong>of</strong> these life-management strategies is<br />

in line with what the SOC model proPoses.<br />

To investigate the impact <strong>of</strong> a person's self-reported engagement in SOC on<br />

adaptive life-management and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>, several studies used versions <strong>of</strong><br />

the SOC questionnaire developed by P. B. Baltes et al. (1999). Freund and Baltes<br />

(2002b) investigated a heterogeneous sample <strong>of</strong> younger, middle-aged, and older<br />

adults and found that participants who indicated high engagement in SOC behaviors<br />

reported more positive emotions and higher levels <strong>of</strong> subjective wellbeing.<br />

The associations remained stable after controlling for self-regulation<br />

variables such as tenacious and flexible goal pursuit as well as for person characteristiCs<br />

such as neuroticiSm, extraversion, and Openness tO new experiences. As<br />

expected, SOC-related strategies were most strongly expressed in middle adulthood;<br />

that is, middle-aged adults showed higher endorsement <strong>of</strong> SOC than<br />

younger and older adults. One exception was elective selection, which showed a<br />

positive age trend. This suggests that older adults are more selective but report<br />

less use <strong>of</strong> optimization and compensation. This weakening <strong>of</strong> self-reported optimization<br />

and compensation in older ages could be due to the fact that these two<br />

strategies are effortful and resource demanding. Both strategies may exceed resources<br />

available in late life. This is especially true for the oldest old and for people<br />

suffering from severe illness (P. B. Baltes & Smith, 2003;Jopp,2002)' Using the<br />

SOC questionnaire in the context <strong>of</strong> the Berlin Aging Study, Freund and Baltes<br />

(1998) showed that SOC is effective well into very old age. They found that older<br />

adults between 72 and 102 years who reported more engagement in SOC-related<br />

strategies also showed higher expressions <strong>of</strong> subl'ective well-being such as positive<br />

affect, satisfaction with <strong>aging</strong>, lack <strong>of</strong> agitation, and absence <strong>of</strong> loneliness.<br />

Wiese and colleagues (Wiese, Freund, & Baltes, 2000,2002) applied the SOC<br />

model to goal pursuit in the work and family domains. In a cross-sectional<br />

questionnaire study, Wiese et al. (2000) measured self-reported use <strong>of</strong> SOC behaviors<br />

<strong>of</strong> younger to middle-aged pr<strong>of</strong>essionals in general as well as domainspecifically.<br />

Higher endorsement <strong>of</strong> general and work- and family-related SOC<br />

strategies was positively related to various indicators <strong>of</strong> well-being. Longitudinal<br />

data replicated these positive associations between self-reported engagement<br />

in SOC behaviors and global as well as work-related subjective well-being


100 EvrppNcr-Bnse o THnonv<br />

(Wiese et aL.,2002). Moreover, participants with primarily work-oriented goals<br />

reported higher levels <strong>of</strong> general and work-related well-being than those who<br />

engaged in both work and family goals. These findings underscore the importance<br />

<strong>of</strong> selecting goals and setting priorities as adaptive regulatory mechanisms<br />

in personal goal involvement and development.<br />

Taken together, the summarized self-report studies support a positive association<br />

between the endorsement <strong>of</strong> SOC-related strategies and indicators <strong>of</strong> global<br />

and domain-specific well-being. Similar findings supporting the adaptive use <strong>of</strong><br />

SOC are reported by Lang, Rieckmann, and Baltes (2002; see also M. M. Baltes &<br />

Lang,7997). Thus, SOC seems to contribute to <strong>successful</strong> development and <strong>aging</strong><br />

throughout the adult years and into old and very old age.<br />

Taking a more process-oriented approach, a number <strong>of</strong> studies addressed the<br />

question <strong>of</strong> ruhst ospects <strong>of</strong> and horu selection, optimization, and compensation are<br />

related to positive functioning across life. A life span-developmental perspective<br />

ar5iues that the overall dynamic between optimization or approach goals (i.e.,<br />

goals directed at approaching positive outcomes) and compensation or avoidance<br />

goals (i.e., goals directed at preventing negative outcomes) should change with<br />

age. Civen the increase <strong>of</strong> losses and the decrease <strong>of</strong> gains over the life span, goals<br />

are hypothesized to shif t from a gain orientation (i.e., optimi zation, approach) in<br />

young adulthood to an orientation toward maintenance or loss avoidance (i.e.,<br />

compensation, avoidance) in later life (Ebner, 2005; Freund & Baltes, 2000; Freund<br />

& Ebner, 2005; Staudinger, Marsiske, & Baltes, 1995). If such an age-related shift<br />

in goal orientation is adaptive for man<strong>aging</strong> <strong>successful</strong>ly the changing balance <strong>of</strong><br />

gains and losses, younger adults should pr<strong>of</strong>it more from adopting goals that aim<br />

at optimizing their performance and older adults more from working on goals<br />

that are geared toward compensating losses in previously available means. Using<br />

a behavioral measure <strong>of</strong> persistence, namely, time, on a simple sensorimotor task<br />

that was framed in terms <strong>of</strong> either (a) optimizing performance or (b) compensating<br />

for losses, Freund (2006a) found evidence for age-differential motivational effects<br />

clf optimization versus compensation. Compared to younger adults, older<br />

adults were more persistent when pursuing the goal to compensate for a loss and<br />

Iess persistent when pursuing the goal to optimize their performance.<br />

In a similar vein, a series <strong>of</strong> studies by Ebner et al. (2006) propose that goals can<br />

be directed toward the improvement <strong>of</strong> functioning, toward the maintenance <strong>of</strong><br />

functional levels, and toward avoidance or regulation <strong>of</strong> loss. In a multimethod<br />

approach (self-report, preference-choice behavior), as well as different life contexts<br />

(cognitive and physical functioning), Ebner et al. found age-group differences<br />

in goai orientation and in the associations between goal orientation and<br />

subjective well-being. As expected and in line with the results by Freund (2006a),<br />

younger adults showed a primary goal orientation toward growth, whereas goal<br />

orientation toward maintenance and loss prevention became more important in<br />

old age. This age-related difference was shown to be related to perceived availability<br />

<strong>of</strong> resources. Moreover, loss prevention was negatively related to general<br />

well-being in younger but not older adults. Interestingly, in old age, orienting<br />

goals toward maintaining functions was positively associated with general subjective<br />

well-being. Together, the studies by Freund and Ebner et al. suggest that<br />

shifting one's goal orientation from the promotion <strong>of</strong> gains toward maintenance<br />

and loss prevention from early to late adulthood constitutes one mechanism <strong>of</strong><br />

adaptive goal selection.<br />

A study bY Ried<br />

goal relations as P<br />

adaptive aspects oI<br />

tion whether the rt<br />

in everyday life an<br />

was that older adu<br />

flict than younger<br />

competence to sele<br />

nonconflicting goi<br />

found that intergo<br />

well-being in ever<br />

ciated with highe<br />

goal attainment ol<br />

tiple goals into acc<br />

subjective well-be<br />

Several studies<br />

strategies on the I<br />

vided evidence fo<br />

outcomes in the c<br />

and Schmitz founr<br />

time studying the<br />

students low in se<br />

their exams.<br />

Several exPerin<br />

related differenct<br />

(Kemper, Hermar<br />

2001; Lindenbergt<br />

ferent from self-s<br />

allow individuals<br />

acterized by the<br />

menter) at the sa<br />

SOC in coordinat<br />

formance <strong>of</strong> mem<br />

performing both I<br />

primary domain I<br />

lected walking o<br />

Moreover, older<br />

handrail) to keel<br />

whereas younger<br />

thinking (i.e., slo<br />

bered). These agt<br />

over cognitive tas<br />

limits by increasi<br />

Taken together<br />

ioral level. Older<br />

ing) and comPe<br />

performance whr<br />

sources Primaril'<br />

to solving a men


Personalittl Theories o,f Successful Aging 101<br />

ily work-oriented goals<br />

t-being than those who<br />

underscore the importive<br />

regulatory mecharport<br />

a positive associatnd<br />

indicators <strong>of</strong> global<br />

ting the adaptive use <strong>of</strong><br />

see also M. M. Baltes &<br />

development and <strong>aging</strong><br />

I studies addressed the<br />

, and compensation are<br />

elopmental perspective<br />

:r approach goals (i.e.,<br />

pensation or avoidance<br />

rs) should change with<br />

cver the life span, goals<br />

mization, approach) in<br />

or loss avoidance (i.e.,<br />

& Baltes, 2000; Freund<br />

ch an age-related shif t<br />

he changing balance <strong>of</strong><br />

dopting goals that aim<br />

rom working on goals<br />

lvailable means. Using<br />

rple sensorimotor task<br />

lnce or (b) compensat-<br />

'ential motiva tiona I efyounger<br />

adults, older<br />

tpensate for a loss and<br />

lrformance.<br />

propose that goals can<br />

:d the maintenance <strong>of</strong><br />

)ss. In a multimethod<br />

as different life conrnd<br />

age-group differgoal<br />

orientation and<br />

Llts by Freund (2006a),<br />

growth, whereas goal<br />

ne more rmportant in<br />

:d to perceived availely<br />

related to general<br />

in old age, orienting<br />

:ed with general subrer<br />

et al. suggest that<br />

toward maintenance<br />

es one mechanism <strong>of</strong><br />

A study by Riediger, Freund, and Baltes (2005) addressed the nature <strong>of</strong> intergoal<br />

relations as possible characteristics differentiating between more and less<br />

adaptive aspects <strong>of</strong> goal selection. Specifically, Riediger et a1. examined the question<br />

whether the relationship among multiple personal goals affects goal pursuit<br />

in everyday life and subl'ective well-being. The first notable finding <strong>of</strong> their study<br />

was that older adults reported more facilitation between their goals and less conflict<br />

than younger adults. This finding can be interpreted as indicating that the<br />

competence to select personal goals so that they form a coherent, congruent, and<br />

nonconflicting goal system increases across adulthood. Moreover, the authors<br />

found that intergoal conf lict was negatively related to indicators <strong>of</strong> psychological<br />

well-being in everyday life over time. Intergoal facilitation, in contrast, was associated<br />

with higher involvement in goal pursuit and an increased likelihood <strong>of</strong><br />

goal attainment ol.er time. This study underscores the importance <strong>of</strong> taking multiple<br />

goals into account when studying the complex way goals affect behavior and<br />

subjective well-bei ng.<br />

Several studies demonstrate the relevance <strong>of</strong> SOC as adaptive life-management<br />

strategies on the behavioral level. For instance, Wiese and Schmitz (2002) provided<br />

evidence for the relationship between self-reported SOC and behavioral<br />

outcomes in the context <strong>of</strong> study success. In a sample <strong>of</strong> college students, Wiese<br />

and Schmitz found that students high in self-reported SOC behaviors spent more<br />

time studying than students low in self-reported SOC behaviors. Additionally,<br />

students low in self-reported selection and optimization were more likely to skip<br />

their exams.<br />

Several experimental studies identified SOC processes and demonstrated agerelated<br />

differences in these processes in the context <strong>of</strong> dual-task paradigms<br />

(Kemper, Herman, & Lian, 2003; Krampe, Rapp, Bondar, & Baltes, 2003; Li et al.,<br />

2001; Lindenberger, Marsiske, & Baltes,2000; Rapp, Krampe, & Baltes, 2006). Different<br />

from self-selected goals, as assessed in most studies reported earlier, that<br />

allow individuals to pursue one goal af ter the other, a dual-task situation is characterized<br />

by the necessity to mana€le two tasks (provided for by the experimenter)<br />

at the same time. Li et al., for example, conducted work on the role <strong>of</strong><br />

SOC in coordinating cogritive and sensorimotor behavior (i.e., simultaneous performance<br />

<strong>of</strong> memorizing and walking). Older adults showed greater costs when<br />

performing both tasks concurrently. As expected on the basis that the body is the<br />

primary domain where <strong>aging</strong> losses occur and serious risks exist, older adults selected<br />

walking over memorizing, as the primary target <strong>of</strong> resource allocation.<br />

Moreover, older adults showed more compensatory strategies (i.e., using a<br />

handrail) to keep their balance and to maintain higher levels <strong>of</strong> performance,<br />

whereas younger adults showed more compensatory strategies in the domain <strong>of</strong><br />

thinking (i.e., slowing down the speed <strong>of</strong> presentation for words to be remembered).<br />

These age-associated effects <strong>of</strong> differential use <strong>of</strong> SOC in favor <strong>of</strong> motor<br />

over cognitive tasks were stronger when the behavioral system was tested at its<br />

limits by increasing task diff iculty.<br />

Taken together, these findings supplement the relevance <strong>of</strong> SOC on the behavioral<br />

level. Older adults engage in selection (i.e., prioritizing walking over thinking)<br />

and compensation (i.e., use <strong>of</strong> a handrail) to preserve their walking<br />

performance when at risk. This preference <strong>of</strong> older adults to allocate their resources<br />

primarily to motor behavior (such as keeping one's balance) rather than<br />

to solving a memory problem is an example <strong>of</strong> highly automatized and largely


702 EvroeNct-Baspp THsc-rr


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Agirtg 103<br />

milar results rvith cogrpeting<br />

tasks. Bondar,<br />

er adults could be in-<br />

Lents. In the case <strong>of</strong> a<br />

however, older adults<br />

ntial allocation system<br />

lms to be adaptive to<br />

;ts that there exists cules<br />

and that SOC is reaged,<br />

and older adults.<br />

eas younger adults are<br />

rr adults show a higher<br />

racting losses. Studies<br />

heir resources to those<br />

ival.<br />

I research strengthens<br />

aptive developmental<br />

el can be regarded as a<br />

: span development. lt<br />

r to master the general<br />

ranges in goal-relevant<br />

mmodative coping by<br />

r, 1990), also places a<br />

ccessful development.<br />

ATIVE AND<br />

rework to understand<br />

tion and real-location<br />

l accommodative coptiidter<br />

& Greve,7994;<br />

2002; Brandtsteidter &<br />

;elf-regulatory stratecourses<br />

<strong>of</strong> individual<br />

I (e.g., tenacious goal<br />

adjustment). Specifi-<br />

I efforts to change Iife<br />

ls to personal preferental,<br />

self-corrective,<br />

tentionally changing<br />

rdy appearance to the<br />

justment <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

es to situational cong,<br />

or disengagement<br />

riration levels, rescal-<br />

ing criteria <strong>of</strong> success, or even selecting new, feasible goals. Accommodation includes<br />

emotional and cognitive reappraisal; it cannot be initiated intentionally, but<br />

shapes the selection <strong>of</strong> goals, and thus constitutes a basic mechanism <strong>of</strong> action regulation<br />

in the face <strong>of</strong> loss.<br />

The model specifies differential situational conditions that selectively activate<br />

or inhibit the two modes <strong>of</strong> coping. Assimilative tendencies dominate as long as<br />

people feel able to actively change a given situation. When experiences <strong>of</strong> loss increase<br />

or assimilative attempts to change the situation are ineffective and actionoutcome<br />

expectancies become low, accommodative tendencies are activated. This<br />

is also the case when assimilative modes are inefficient from the outset. Thus, people<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten first employ assimilative coping efforts to actively overcome obstacles<br />

that block their goals. If these attempts are un<strong>successful</strong>, a gradual shift to accommodative<br />

processes <strong>of</strong> goal adjustment occurs, which is modulated by personal<br />

and situational factors (e.g., goal importance; Brandtstiidter & Wentura,7995).<br />

According to the model, goal attainment leads to positive affect. Negative emotions<br />

arise when losses <strong>of</strong> goal-relevant resources lead to unattainability <strong>of</strong> goals<br />

and one falls short <strong>of</strong> one's ambitions and personal standards. Discrepancies between<br />

actual states and desired outcomes become especially salient in later phases<br />

<strong>of</strong> life when irrevocable Iosses and uncontrollable events cumulate. Brandtstddter<br />

and colleagues (Brandtstadter,1,999; Brandtstiidter & Greve, 7994;Brandtstiidter &<br />

Renner, 1990) maintain that in old age this growing impact <strong>of</strong> negative events on<br />

personal development favors an age-related shift from an active, assimilative to a<br />

more accepting, accommodative coping. As the aim <strong>of</strong> accommodative coping is to<br />

make the given situation appear less negative and more acceptable, this strategy<br />

may help individuals to maintain positive developmental perspectives into old age.<br />

With declining resources, as is especially the case in old age, it may not be possible<br />

to maintain the highest standard in multiple domains (Brandtstiidter & Wentura,<br />

1995). Accommodative coping therefore becomes particularly effective to protect<br />

against decline in well-being and favors adaptive development in later life (see also<br />

Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995). Thus, the dual-process model <strong>of</strong> coping characterizes<br />

<strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> primarily as a shift from actively counteracting the onset <strong>of</strong> agerelated<br />

losses to cognitively restructuring personal goals and standards.<br />

Aoerrrvu Drvrlc-rt'vENr es Srttr-r'INC FRoM Asstl,rtlantvn ro<br />

AccoHavopnrtve CctptNC THRouGHour LIFE<br />

Empirical evidence supports the expectation <strong>of</strong> age-related differences in the empioyment<br />

<strong>of</strong> assimilative and accommodative coping strategies. In a self-report<br />

study with younger, middle-aged, and older adults, Brandtstiidter and Renner<br />

(1990) found that self-reported tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment<br />

were complementarily associated with age. There was an age-graded gradual shift<br />

from active, tenacious goal pursuit to accepting, flexible goal adjustment. In addition,<br />

the study demonstrated a buffering effect <strong>of</strong> flexible goal adjustment with<br />

regard to the impact <strong>of</strong> perceived deficits on dissatisfaction with personal development.<br />

This finding suggests that flexibly adjusting one's goals helps to maintain a<br />

sense <strong>of</strong> well-being and satisfaction despite the actual experience <strong>of</strong> loss'<br />

In a large sample <strong>of</strong> older adults, Rothermund and Brandtst2idter (2003) found<br />

that accommodative strategies to overcome functional impairments had a curvilinear,<br />

inverted U-shaped relationship to age. The strategies increased up to the


104 EvrpeNcr-Baseo Tupor


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 105<br />

cy <strong>of</strong> action-related rercreased<br />

again. Rotherng<br />

that the increasing<br />

rits the usage and effistructured<br />

interviews<br />

ts are expressed in biord<br />

age and <strong>aging</strong>. They<br />

commodative thoughts<br />

rsitive attitude toward<br />

n their life.<br />

rift from assimilative tcr<br />

he latter in later adu [tnecessary<br />

for tenacious<br />

i Renner, 1990; Brandt-<br />

,trategies become more<br />

ing losses in temporal,<br />

oals and priorities. Acvith<br />

irreversible losses<br />

outcomes that diverge<br />

rcess model <strong>of</strong> coping<br />

ts a function <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong>constitutes<br />

one aspect<br />

zation in primary and<br />

explaining how people<br />

es in adulthood.<br />

RIMARY<br />

ntrol (OPS) by Hecklaptive<br />

developmental<br />

khausen, 1997,7999):<br />

lnt by selecting goals,<br />

r developmental ecolrol<br />

over their lives. ln<br />

1 and compensation as<br />

rlation (Heckhausen,<br />

from the SOC model,<br />

e focused investment<br />

B. Baltes and Baltes,<br />

the definition in the<br />

rn implies the use <strong>of</strong><br />

urces are insufficient<br />

L is the higher order<br />

tion (Heckhausen &<br />

Similar to the dual-process model <strong>of</strong> assimilative and accommodative coping,<br />

the OPS model particularly emphasizes the role <strong>of</strong> perceived personal control for<br />

developmental regulation. It assumes that humans have a basic need for control,<br />

that is, to protect themselves in the face <strong>of</strong> difficulties and setbacks. The primary<br />

way to achieve control is by modifying the environment according to one's goals<br />

by making instrumental efforts (i.e., primary control). If such primary control efforts<br />

are not available or fail, secondary control takes place, that is, modifying one's<br />

goals and standards or eng<strong>aging</strong> in self-protective attributions and favorable social<br />

comparisons. Secondary control aims at focusing and protecting motivational<br />

resources for primary control and buffers negative effects <strong>of</strong> failing in primary<br />

control strivings. The theory posits a primacy <strong>of</strong> primary over secondary control<br />

strategies. According to the model, people aim at maximizing primary control.<br />

Secondary control strivings serve to bolster primary control. They are the secondbest<br />

strategy af ter primary control strivings fail or are not available.<br />

Two forms <strong>of</strong> primary and secondary control strategies are distinguished: se/cctiae<br />

and compensatory. Selective primary control comprises actions that are directly<br />

aimed at goal achievement. It denotes the focused investment <strong>of</strong> resources<br />

into the pursuit <strong>of</strong> a chosen goal. Examples are time investment or investment <strong>of</strong><br />

effort and skills into pursuinp; one's goals. Compensatory primary control refers<br />

to the investment <strong>of</strong> external resources. It involves the recruitment <strong>of</strong> external<br />

help or technical aids for the attainment <strong>of</strong> a chosen goal. lt comes into play when<br />

internal resources for goal achievement are insufficient or depleted. Examples are<br />

employing the help <strong>of</strong> others or applying new, alternative resources. Selective secondary<br />

control is directed at the internal world to promote the volitional commitment<br />

to a chosen goal. It subsumes metavolitional strategies to keep oneself<br />

focused on the pursuit <strong>of</strong> selected goals, for example, by avoiding distractions.<br />

Examples are boosting the value <strong>of</strong> a chosen goal or the perception <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

control. Finally, compensatory secondary control refers to cognitive reframing <strong>of</strong><br />

goals such as down-regulating the desirability <strong>of</strong> a blocked goal or diseng<strong>aging</strong><br />

from it. It serves as a buffer for negative effects <strong>of</strong> failure. It involves strategies<br />

such as disengagement from unattainable goals, downward social comparison,<br />

and external causal attributions.<br />

According to the authors, none <strong>of</strong> these four strategies is functional per se.<br />

Rather, a higher order optimization process is postulated that coordinates control<br />

strivings such that the potential for primary control is maximized across life.<br />

Consistent with this proposition, empirical evidence indicates that self-protective<br />

compensatory strategies (compensatory secondary control) become more prevalent<br />

and more adaptive in later adulthood and when opportunity structures for<br />

goal attainment are unfavorable (Wrosch & Heckhausen, 1999), whereas continued<br />

involvement in primary control efforts is maladaptive in such situations<br />

(Chipperfield, Perry, & Menec, 1999; Heckhausen, Wrosch, & Fleeson,2001).<br />

According to the OPS model, maximization <strong>of</strong> primary control potential characterizes<br />

adaptive functioning. This, however, becomes particularly difficult in<br />

older ages when losses become more prominent and less under personal control<br />

and when declines in goal-relevant resources make the achievement <strong>of</strong> resourceintensive<br />

goals increasingly difficult (Schulz, 1986). As a consequence, with advancing<br />

age, compensatory primary and secondary control strategies gain<br />

increasing importance for <strong>successful</strong> life management. The use <strong>of</strong> secondary<br />

control strategies is theorized to contribute to a long term increase in primary


706 Evrt>r:Ncr--BesEp Tnrorv<br />

control in that it buffers negative motivational effects <strong>of</strong> failure and maintains<br />

self-efficacy and well-being. Therefore, these strategies should be employed<br />

more frequent in old age (cf. Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995). However, OPS theory<br />

arEiues that it is not the four control strategies separately that bring about adaptive<br />

development but rather their orchestration in line with developmental opportunities<br />

and constraints. The following section provides empirical support<br />

for these assumptions.<br />

ApnrrrrvE DrvElorvrNT AS Sl rrFTtNcl rRol,r PnruaRy ro SECoNDAny<br />

CoNrRol Srnnrlclss rl tRC)ucHOUr LrtE<br />

In a self-report study, Heckhausen,Schulz, and Wrosch (1998) observed that the<br />

use <strong>of</strong> control strategies increased from early to late adulthood. Unexpectedly,<br />

however, compensatory secondary control did not show a positive age trend. Positive<br />

associations between all four types <strong>of</strong> control strategies and self-esteem supported<br />

the role <strong>of</strong> primary and secondary control strategies for adaptive<br />

functioning. Wrosch and Heckhausen (1999) demonstrated that diseng<strong>aging</strong> from<br />

unobtainable goals and reallocating resources to alternative domains (i.e., compensatory<br />

secondary control) is functional when goal-relevant resources decline<br />

and opportunity structures for goal attainment become less favorable. The authors<br />

examined goals in the partnership domain in recently committed or separated<br />

adults in early adulthood and late midlife. Younger separated adults<br />

mentioned more partnership goals and reported more primary control strivings<br />

for attaining their goals. ln contrast, older separated adults reported more secondary<br />

control actions such as goal deactivation. They disengaged from partnership<br />

goals and invested their resources into alternative social domains. Moreover,<br />

younger separated adults reported a decline in positive affect over a time period<br />

<strong>of</strong> 15 months when they disengaged from their partnership goals, whereas for<br />

older separated adults goal deactivation proved beneficial. One explanation is<br />

that the objective chances to remarry are lower for people in late middle age compared<br />

to young adulthood. The age-related change in opportunities for attaining<br />

the partnership goal, then, is ref lected in a shift in importance and functional impact<br />

<strong>of</strong> this goal. These findings can be viewed as evidence supporting the notion<br />

that individuals activate and deactivate developmental goals in accordance with<br />

age-graded changes in opportunities for goal achievement.<br />

Similarly, Heckhausen et al. (2001) investigated developmental regulation before<br />

and after the developmental deadline for childbearing. Younger and middleaged<br />

women with or without children were compared with regard to various<br />

indicators <strong>of</strong> primary and secondary control striving for goal attainment versus<br />

goal disengagement and self-protection. Women approaching the developmental<br />

deadline for childbearing were actively engaged in the goal <strong>of</strong> bearing a child. In<br />

contrast, women who had passed the deadline disengaged from the goal. These<br />

differences were evident in the number and type <strong>of</strong> goals nominated as well as<br />

the salience <strong>of</strong> information related to children. Moreover, women who had passed<br />

the deadline but for whom child-related information was still more salient were<br />

more prone to negative affect. Thus, a selective responsiveness to information relevant<br />

for an obsolete developmental goal appears to have negative implications<br />

for well-being.<br />

Taken together,<br />

the endorsement c<br />

ance developmenti<br />

trol strategies incr<br />

adaptive developn<br />

come increasingly<br />

the SOC model ar<br />

the fruitfulness o<br />

in-context in termr<br />

final section, we a<br />

S<br />

This chapter com<br />

perspective on pet<br />

derstanding <strong>of</strong> go<br />

environment intet<br />

in this chapter (th<br />

model) argue that<br />

motivating, orgar<br />

related processes<br />

related processes<br />

management <strong>of</strong> r<br />

losses is a key to s<br />

cific processes pl<br />

adulthood. There<br />

yet, no integratior<br />

(Freund & Riedig<br />

positive associati<<br />

believe that it will<br />

to close this gap.<br />

Another future<br />

practical relevanct<br />

related goals are t<br />

sues become mos<br />

1992), our practici<br />

related processesI<br />

Research has d<br />

and losses has a d<br />

suit when implen<br />

framed messages<br />

through a regular<br />

the avoidance <strong>of</strong><br />

physical activity;<br />

themselves as hig<br />

old age, loss-fran<br />

sages become mot


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Strccessful Aging 107<br />

'failure and maintains<br />

s should be employed<br />

l. However, OPS theory<br />

that bring about adap-<br />

,ith developmental opides<br />

empirical support<br />

:ONDARY<br />

998) observed that the<br />

Llthood. Unexpectedly,<br />

'ositive age trend. Posi-<br />

:s and self-esteem sup-<br />

:rategies for adaptive<br />

that diseng<strong>aging</strong> from<br />

ve domains (i.e., comvant<br />

resources decline<br />

ess favorable. The auly<br />

committed or sepalger<br />

separated adults<br />

mary control strivings<br />

ts reported more seclngaged<br />

from partneraldomains.<br />

Moreover,<br />

ect over a time period<br />

Lip goals, whereas for<br />

l. One explanation is<br />

t late middle age comrtunities<br />

for attaining<br />

tce and functional imsupporting<br />

the notion<br />

ls in accordance with<br />

mental regulation be-<br />

Younger and middleith<br />

regard to various<br />

ral attainment versus<br />

ng the developmental<br />

<strong>of</strong> bearing a child. In<br />

from the goal. These<br />

nominated as well as<br />

rmen who had passed<br />

:ill more salient were<br />

ss to information relregative<br />

implications<br />

Taken together, the f indings in the context <strong>of</strong> OPS theory provide evidence that<br />

the endorsement <strong>of</strong> control strategies favors <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> as they help to balance<br />

developmental gains and losses. Specifically, the use <strong>of</strong> compensatory control<br />

strategies increases in importance with positive effects on well-being and<br />

adaptive developmental regulation when goal-related resources are lost or become<br />

increasingly threatened, as is <strong>of</strong>ten the case in late adulthood. As is true for<br />

the SOC model and the dual-process model <strong>of</strong> coping, the OPS model supports<br />

the fruitfulness <strong>of</strong> approaching <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> by investigating personalityin-context<br />

in terms <strong>of</strong> personal goals and goal-related processes. In the next and<br />

final section, we address this brief Iy.<br />

SUMMARY, FUTURE OUTLOOK, AND<br />

PRACTICAL RELEVANCE<br />

This chapter combined an action-theoretical with a life span-developmental<br />

perspective on personality and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. This integration allows an understanding<br />

<strong>of</strong> goals as dynamic aspects <strong>of</strong> personality that reflect the personenvironment<br />

interaction over time. All three theoretical frameworks presented<br />

in this chapter (the SOC model, the dual-process model <strong>of</strong> coping, and the OPS<br />

model) argue that individuals can actively shape their development through the<br />

motivating, organizinp;, and directing functions <strong>of</strong> personal goals and goalrelated<br />

processes, and they all empirically support the role <strong>of</strong> goals and goalrelated<br />

processes for adaptive development. Moreover, they all stress that the<br />

management <strong>of</strong> resource changes from a dominance <strong>of</strong> gains to increasing<br />

losses is a key to <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. They differ, however, with regard to the specific<br />

processes proposed to best serve the management <strong>of</strong> resources across<br />

adulthood. There exists good evidence supporting each <strong>of</strong> these models. As <strong>of</strong><br />

yet, no integ;ration or systematic comparison <strong>of</strong> the models has been attempted<br />

(Freund & Riediger, 2003; for exceptions, see Freund & Baltes, 2002a,2002b, for<br />

positive associations <strong>of</strong> SOC and assimilative and accommodative coping). We<br />

believe that it will be a fruitful next step in future research on <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong><br />

to close this gap.<br />

Another future research direction in this field should be to investigate the<br />

oractical relevance <strong>of</strong> the three models. As various studies have shown that workielated<br />

goals are most central for younger adults and goals related to health issues<br />

become most prominent in older adults (Nurmi, 1992; Rapkin & Fischer,<br />

1992), our practical examples and speculations about the role <strong>of</strong> goals and goalrelated<br />

processes for <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> refer to the domains <strong>of</strong> health and work.<br />

Research has demonstrated that framing health messages in terms <strong>of</strong> gains<br />

and losses has a differential effect on health-related goal adoption and goal pursuit<br />

when implementing health programs in younger and older adults. Cainframed<br />

messages emphasize benefits gained (e.g., becoming more attractive<br />

through a regular physical workout), whereas loss-framed messages emphasize<br />

the avoidance <strong>of</strong> benefits lost (e.g., preventing weight gain through regular<br />

physical activity; Brendl, Higgins, & Lemm, 7995). When individuals perceive<br />

themselves as highly vulnerable to certain health risks, as is <strong>of</strong>ten the case in<br />

old age, loss-framed messages are more effective, whereas gain-framed messages<br />

become more effective when perceived vulnerability is low, as is typically


108 Evrpr-:Ncp-Besgt> TusoRY<br />

the case at younger ages (Lee & Aaker, 2004). Framing a health program with an<br />

emphasis on benefits lost, as opposed to benefits gained, might therefore be<br />

more persuasive for older adults. As a consequence, elderly people might be<br />

more persistent in pursuing the program and might manage to improve their<br />

physical and cognitive functioning and, in this sense, age more <strong>successful</strong>ly.<br />

The opposite should be true for younger adults. But not only the direction <strong>of</strong><br />

goals might have practical implications but also the underlying motivation <strong>of</strong><br />

goals. Riediger (2001) showed, for instance, that goals related to attractiveness<br />

and fitness are central for younger adults, whereas health in itself becomes the<br />

main motivation for regular physical activity in older adults. Taking this into<br />

consideration when setting up <strong>successful</strong> training concePts for younger and<br />

older adults appears to be <strong>of</strong> great importance.<br />

The second example <strong>of</strong> the possible applied consequences <strong>of</strong> this research is<br />

motivation at the workplace. With demographic changes (a combination <strong>of</strong><br />

lower birth rates and more people becoming older in much better cognitive and<br />

physical conditions; e.g., P. B. Baltes, 1997), it is likely that retirement age will<br />

increase to keep well-educated and highly experienced people in the workforce.<br />

Here, it will become an important task <strong>of</strong> the future to take into account how<br />

goals and goal-related processes in the work domain might change over adulthood.<br />

In contrast to younger people, older adults might simply not be primarily<br />

motivated to achieve new and higher outcomes, but instead focus on maintenance<br />

and prevention <strong>of</strong> losses. Providing younger as well as older adults with<br />

work tasks that meet their underlying motivation and that are framed in ageappropriate<br />

ways (i.e., gain frames for younger adults and loss frames for older<br />

adults) may increase their work-related performance as well as their personal<br />

satisfaction with work in young but also in olci age. Moreover, encour<strong>aging</strong> accommodative<br />

coping styles and providing opportunities for primary control<br />

even and especially when resources decline will likely help older adults to derive<br />

meaning and satisfaction from their workplace.<br />

In sum, we believe that conceptualizing <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong> using a framework <strong>of</strong><br />

person-in-context fosters our understanding <strong>of</strong> the processes underlying <strong>aging</strong><br />

well and will ultimately also help shape environments in a more age-friendly way<br />

to support the strengths <strong>of</strong> old and very old age and to maintain positive functioning<br />

even in the face <strong>of</strong> decreasing resources.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Allport, G. W. (1937). PersLtnality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Hogrefe.<br />

Baltes, M. M. (1996) . The mnny .faces <strong>of</strong> dependencv in old nge. New York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong><br />

Irress.<br />

Baltes, M. M., & Carstensen,L.L. (7996). The process <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. A.qeirtg nnd Societr1,<br />

16,397-422.<br />

Baltes, M. M., & Lang, F. R. (1997). Everyday functioning and <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>: The impact<br />

<strong>of</strong> resources. Psychology and Aging,12, 433-443.<br />

Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions <strong>of</strong> life-span developmental psychology: On<br />

the clynamics between growth and decline. Dettektpmentnl Psychoktgy,23,611-626.<br />

Baltes, P. B. (7997). On the incomplete architecture <strong>of</strong> human ontogeny: Selection, optimization,<br />

and compensation as foundation <strong>of</strong> developmental theory. Atncrican Pstlchologist,<br />

52,366-380.<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Baltes<br />

Methodological and<br />

<strong>aging</strong> on the central n<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Baltes<br />

model <strong>of</strong> selective<br />

(Eds.), Successful ag<br />

Cambridge Univers<br />

Baltes, P. B., Baltes, M<br />

tion, optimization,a<br />

Germany: Max Plar<br />

Baltes, P. B., Freund, I<br />

In M. Johnson, V. L<br />

book <strong>of</strong> age and <strong>aging</strong><br />

Baltes, P. B., Lindenbe<br />

tal psychology. In V<br />

Theoretical models <strong>of</strong><br />

Baltes, P. 8., Reese, H.<br />

nual Reoieut <strong>of</strong> PsYc<br />

Baltes, P. 8., & Singer<br />

cultural orchestra<br />

<strong>of</strong> the Academia Eurt<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Smith<br />

<strong>aging</strong> <strong>of</strong> the young<br />

Bargh, I. A., & Fergus<br />

mental processes. I<br />

Baumeister, R. F. (199<br />

Bluck, S., & Alea, N. (l<br />

I remember the aut<br />

iniscence work (pp. I<br />

Boesch, E. E. (1991<br />

Springer.<br />

Bondar, A., Krampe, l<br />

young and older adt<br />

Max Planck Institt<br />

Bradburn, N. M. (196<br />

Brandtstiidter, J. $98<br />

action: Considerat<br />

fiir Entzoicklungsp<br />

Brandtstadter, I. $99<br />

R. M. Lerner (Eds.<br />

opment (5th ed., pp<br />

Brandtstiidter, l. (799<br />

togenetic bases <strong>of</strong><br />

(Eds.), Action and t<br />

Thousand Oaks, C<br />

Brandtstiidter, J., & G<br />

Deuelopmental Reo<br />

Brandtst2idter, J., & L<br />

search through the It


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Sttccessful Aging 109<br />

)alth program with an<br />

J, might therefore be<br />

erly people might be<br />

nage to improve their<br />

ge more <strong>successful</strong>ly.<br />

only the direction <strong>of</strong><br />

erlying motivation <strong>of</strong><br />

ated to attractiveness<br />

in itself becomes the<br />

ults. Taking this into<br />

:pts for younger and<br />

:es <strong>of</strong> this research is<br />

:s (a combination <strong>of</strong><br />

r better cognitive and<br />

tt retirement age will<br />

rple in the workforce.<br />

lke into account how<br />

ht change over adultnply<br />

not be primarily<br />

:ad focus on mainte-<br />

I as older adults with<br />

at are framed in ageloss<br />

frames for older<br />

vell as their personal<br />

)ver/ encour<strong>aging</strong> acfor<br />

primary control<br />

Ip older adults to deusing<br />

a framework <strong>of</strong><br />

ses underlying <strong>aging</strong><br />

rore age-friendly way<br />

ain positive functionv<br />

York: Hogrefe.<br />

>rk: Cambridge Univer-<br />

I <strong>aging</strong>. Ageing and Soci-<br />

:cessful <strong>aging</strong>: The imnental<br />

psychologv: On<br />

ology, 23, 611-626.<br />

:ogeny: Selection, optieory.<br />

Arne r ic nn P sy chol -<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1980). Plasticity and variability in psychological <strong>aging</strong>:<br />

Methodological and theoretical issues. In G. E. Gurski (Ed.), Determining the effects <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>aging</strong> on the central neraousystem (pp. 41,-66). Berlin, Germany: Schering.<br />

Baltes, P. 8., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>: The<br />

model <strong>of</strong> selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes<br />

(Eds.), Successftrl <strong>aging</strong>: Perspectiues from the behaaioral sciences (pp. 1-3a). New York:<br />

Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Baltes, P. B., Baltes, M. M., Freund, A. M., & Lang, F. R. (1999). The measurement <strong>of</strong> selection,<br />

optimization, and compensation (SOC) by self-report: Technical report 1999. Berlin,<br />

Germany: Max Planck Institute for Human Development.<br />

Baltes, P. B., Freund, A. M., & Li, S.-C. (2005). The psychological science <strong>of</strong> human <strong>aging</strong>.<br />

In M. Johnson, V. L. Bengtson, P. Coleman, & T. Kirkwood (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook<br />

<strong>of</strong> age and <strong>aging</strong> (pp. a7-71). New York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Life-span theory in developmental<br />

psychology. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> child psychology: Vol. 1.<br />

Theoretical models <strong>of</strong> human deztelopment (6th ed., pp. 569-664). New York: Wiley.<br />

Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Life-span developmental psychology. Annual<br />

Reaiew <strong>of</strong> Psychology, 31, 65-110.<br />

Baltes, P. B., & Singer, T. (2001). Plasticity and the ageing mind: An exemplar <strong>of</strong> the biocultural<br />

orchestration <strong>of</strong> brain and behaviou r. European Reaiezu: Interdisciplinary lournal<br />

<strong>of</strong> the Academia Europaen, 9,59-76.<br />

Baltes, P. 8., & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong>: From <strong>successful</strong><br />

<strong>aging</strong> <strong>of</strong> the young old to the dilemmas <strong>of</strong> the fourth age. Gerontology, 49,123-135.<br />

Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity <strong>of</strong> higher<br />

mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 925-945.<br />

Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings <strong>of</strong> life. New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Bluck, S., & Alea, N. (2002). Exploring the functions <strong>of</strong> autobiographical memory: Why do<br />

I remember the autumn? In f . D. Webster & B. K. Haight (Eds.), Critical adoances in rentiniscence<br />

work (pp.61-75). New York: Springer.<br />

Boesch, E. E. (1991). Symbolic action theory and cultural psychology. Berlin, Germany:<br />

Springer.<br />

Bondar, A., Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Balance takes priority oaer cognition: Can<br />

young and older adults deliberately control resource allocation? Unpublished manuscript,<br />

Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin.<br />

Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure <strong>of</strong> psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldine.<br />

Brandtstlidter, J. (1986). Personal control over development and development-regulating<br />

action: Considerations and results concerning a neglected research topic. Zeitschrift<br />

fiir Ent w i ck lu ngsp s y ch ol o gi e un d P ii d ago g isch e P sy c holo g ie, 18, 376 -334.<br />

Brandtstddter, f. (1998). Action perspectives on human development. In W. Damon &<br />

R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> child psychology: VoL 1. Theoretical models <strong>of</strong> human deuelopment<br />

(5th ed., pp. 807-863). New York: Wiley.<br />

Brandtstiidter, J. Q999). The self in action and development: Cultural, biosocial, and ontogenetic<br />

bases <strong>of</strong> intentional self-development. In J. Brandtstiidter & R. M. Lerner<br />

(Eds.), Action and self-deaelopment: Theory and research through the life span (pp.37-65).<br />

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Brandtstiidter, f., & Greve , W. (1994). The <strong>aging</strong> self: Stabiiizing and protective processes.<br />

Deael opm e ntal Reai eu, 14, 52-80.<br />

Brandtstddter,J., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.).(1999).<br />

Action and self-deztelopment: Theory and research<br />

throttgh the life span. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.


110 Evrptrucn-BRspu Tnsonv<br />

Brandtstiidter,I., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment:<br />

Explication and age-related analysis <strong>of</strong> assimilative and accommodative strategies<br />

<strong>of</strong> coping. Psychology and Aging, 5,58-67.<br />

Brandtsti,idter, f .,& Rothermund, K. (2002). The life-course dynamics <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit and<br />

goal adjustment: A two-process framework. Deaelopmental Reaiezu, 22,777-1'50.<br />

Brandtstiidter,J., & Rothermund, K., & Schmitz, U. (1997). Coping resources in later life.<br />

European Journal <strong>of</strong> Applied Psychology, 47,707-114.<br />

Brandtstbdter, J., & Wentura, D. (1995). Adjustment to shifting possibility frontiers in<br />

later life: Complementary adaptive modes. In R. A. Dixon & L. Bbckman (Eds.), Compensnting<br />

for psychological deficits and declines: Man<strong>aging</strong> losses nnd promoting gains<br />

(pp. 83-106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Brandtstiidter, J., Wentura, D., & Greve, W. (1993). Adaptive resources <strong>of</strong> the <strong>aging</strong> self:<br />

Outlines <strong>of</strong> an emergent perspective. Internationnl lournal <strong>of</strong> Behauioral Deaelopment,'16,<br />

323-349.<br />

Brendl, C. M., Higgins, E. T., & Lemm, K. M. (i995). Sensitivity to varying gains and<br />

losses: The role <strong>of</strong> self-discrepancies and event framing. lournnl <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social<br />

Psychologv, 69, 7028-1051.<br />

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1988). Inieracting systems in human development: Research paradigms-Present<br />

and future. In N. Bolger & A. Caspi (Eds.), Person in context: Deaelopmental<br />

processes-Human deuelopment in cultural and historicsl contexts (pp. 25-49). New<br />

York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Griissman, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional<br />

well-being: The moderating role <strong>of</strong> motive dispositions. lournal <strong>of</strong> Personnlity and Social<br />

Psychology, 75, 494-508.<br />

Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Maier, G. W. (1999). The pursuit <strong>of</strong> personal goals: A<br />

motivational approach to well-being and life adjustment. In J. Brandtstiidter & R. M.<br />

Lerner (Eds.), Action and self-deoelopment: Theory and research through the life-span<br />

(pp.169*196). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W.L. (7976)'The quality <strong>of</strong> American life: Perceptions,<br />

eaaluations, and satisfactions. New York: Russell-Sage.<br />

Cantor, N. (1990). From thought to behavior: "Having" and "doing" in the study <strong>of</strong> personali<br />

ty and cogni tion. Amer i can P sy cholo gi st, 45, 735<br />

-750.<br />

Cantor, N. (1994). Life task problem solving: Situational affordances and personal needs.<br />

<strong>Personality</strong> nnd Socisl Psychology Bulletin, 20,235-243.<br />

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control theory approach<br />

to htrman behattior. New York: Springer.<br />

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual framework for<br />

personality, social, clinical, and health psychology. Psychological Bullctitt,92,111-735.<br />

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions <strong>of</strong> positive and negative affect:<br />

A controi-process view. Psychological Reztiew, 97, 79-35.<br />

Chapman, M., & Skinner, E. A. (1985). Action in development: Development in action. In<br />

M. Frese & J. Sabini (Eds.), GoaI directed behaaior: The concept <strong>of</strong> action in psychology<br />

(pp. 200-213). Hillsdale, Nf : Erlbaum.<br />

Chipperfield,J. G., Perry, R. P., & Menec, V. H. (1999). Primary and secondary controlenhancing<br />

strategies: Implications for health in later hfe. lotrrnal <strong>of</strong> Aging and Hcalth,<br />

11, 517-539.<br />

Cohen, G. (199S). The effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> on autobiographical memory. In C. P. Thompson,<br />

D. J. Herrmann, D. Bruce, D. J. Read, D. G. Payne, & M. P. Toglia (Eds.) , Autobiographi'<br />

cttl me nLory: Theoretical and applied perspectioes (pp. 105-123). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Coleman, P. C., Ivani<strong>of</strong><br />

life themes in oL<br />

Conway, M. A., & P1<br />

memories in the se<br />

Costa, P. T., Herbst,<br />

life: Stability, int<br />

365-378.<br />

Costa, P. T., & McCra<br />

sonality. In T. F.<br />

(pp. 21-a0). Wash<br />

Costa, P. T., & McCra<br />

ment using the Re<br />

64,21.-50.<br />

Diener, E. (1984). Sub<br />

Diener, E., & Fulita, F<br />

nomothetic and id<br />

926-935.<br />

Ebner, N. C. (2005). S<br />

ferences itr persona<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Berl<br />

Ebner, N. C., Freund<br />

goal orientation frr<br />

and prevention <strong>of</strong><br />

Emmons, R. A. (1986<br />

being. lournal <strong>of</strong> Pe<br />

Emmons, R. A. (1996<br />

P. M. Gollwitzer &<br />

tiaation to behaaio<br />

Ericsson, K. A., & Kri<br />

tice in the acquisi<br />

Erikson, E. H. (1968)<br />

Ford, D. H. (1987). Hr<br />

behaaior and perso<br />

Freund, A. M. (199<br />

salience <strong>of</strong> age in t<br />

Freund, A. M. (2003<br />

233-242.<br />

Freund, A. M. (2006<br />

and older adults. I<br />

Freund, A. M. (200<br />

adulthood. In B. R<br />

suit: Goals, action a<br />

Freund, A. M., & Bal<br />

gies <strong>of</strong> life manag<br />

Psychology and Agi<br />

Freund, A. M., & Ba<br />

compensation: An<br />

regulation. In W. l<br />

and consciousnes (


<strong>Personality</strong> Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 777<br />

and flexible goal adjustnd<br />

accommodative strateamics<br />

<strong>of</strong> goal pursuit and<br />

aiezo, 22, 117-750.<br />

ing resources in later life.<br />

rg possibility frontiers in<br />

L. Biickman (Eds.), Comtsses<br />

and promoting gains<br />

sources <strong>of</strong> the <strong>aging</strong> self:<br />

lehaaioral Deoebpment, 1 6,<br />

ity to varying gains and<br />

nl <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social<br />

tlopment: Research para-<br />

Person in context: Deaelop-<br />

:ontexts(pp. 25-49). New<br />

onal goals and emotional<br />

nl <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> nnd Social<br />

rsuit <strong>of</strong>personal goals: A<br />

J. Brandtstiidter & R. M.<br />

uch through the life-span<br />

ty <strong>of</strong> American life: Percep-<br />

,ing" in the study <strong>of</strong> pernces<br />

and personal needs.<br />

: A control theory approach<br />

onceptual framework for<br />

'cal Bulletin, 92, 771-135.<br />

positive and negative af-<br />

)evelopmeni in action. In<br />

pt <strong>of</strong> action in psychology<br />

I and secondary controlrnal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Aging and Health,<br />

rory. In C. P. Thompson,<br />

3lia (Eds.), Autobiogrnphiillsdale,<br />

NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Coleman, P. G., Ivani-Chalian, C., & Robinson, M. (1998). The story continues: Persistence<br />

<strong>of</strong> life themes in old age. Ageing nnd Society, 18,389-479.<br />

Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction <strong>of</strong> autobiographicarl<br />

memories in the self-memorv system. Psychological Rcuiezu, 107,26I-288.<br />

Costa, P. T., Herbst, J. H., McCrae, R. R., & Siegler, I. C. (2000). <strong>Personality</strong> at micllife:<br />

Stability, intrinsic maturation, and response to life events. Assessment, 7,<br />

365-378.<br />

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence for stability <strong>of</strong> adult personality.<br />

In T. F. Heatherton & J. L. Weinberp;er (Eds.), Can parsonality change?<br />

(pp. 21-40). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.<br />

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierarchical pcrsonality assessment<br />

using the Revised NEO <strong>Personality</strong> Inventory. lournnl <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> Assessment,<br />

64,21-50.<br />

Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bullctirt,95,542-575.<br />

Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjective well-being: A<br />

nomothetic and idiographic approach.lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> nnd Social Psychologr1,68,<br />

926-935.<br />

Ebner, N. C. (2005). Striaing for gains and preaenting losscs: Multi-nrcthod eoidence on the dit'-<br />

ferences in pcrsonal goal orientntion in enrly nnd latc ndulthood. Doctoral dissertation, Free<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Berlin.<br />

Ebner, N. C., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Developmental changes in personal<br />

goal orientation from young to late adulthood: From striving for gains to maintenance<br />

and prevention <strong>of</strong> losses. Psychologrl nnd Agirtg, 21,661-678.<br />

Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective wellberng,.<br />

lourrml <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> ortd Socinl Psychology,5T, 1058-1068.<br />

Emmons, If. A. (1996). Striving and feeling: Personal goals and subjective well-being. In<br />

P. M. Gollwitzer 8xJ. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology <strong>of</strong> oction: Linkirrg cognition nnd nro<br />

tiaotion to behnttiLtr (pp. 313-337). New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Ericsson, K. A., & Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role <strong>of</strong> deliberative practice<br />

in the acquisition <strong>of</strong> expert performance. Psr1chology Rcaicu,, 100,363-406.<br />

Erikson, E. H. (1968). ldetttity: Youth nnd crlsis. New York: Norton.<br />

Ford, D. H. (1987). Humans ns self-corrstructing liuing sqstents: A dettalopmental perspecti-oe on<br />

behaaior anLl Stcrsonnlity. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Freund, A. M. (1997). lndividuating age-salience: A psychological perspective on the<br />

salience <strong>of</strong> age in thc life course. Human Deaelo1tment, 40,287*292.<br />

Freund, A. M. (2003). The role <strong>of</strong> goals for development. Psycltologische I?.undschnu,54,<br />

L.).) - L+ Z.<br />

Freund, A. M. (2006a). Differential motivational consequences <strong>of</strong> goal focus in younger<br />

and older adults. Psychologtl nntl Agirrg, 21 ,240-252.<br />

Freund, A. M. (2006b). Levels <strong>of</strong> goals-Understanding motivational processes across<br />

adulthood. In B. R. Little, K. Salmela-Aro, & S. D. Phillips (Eds.), Pcrsonal project pursuit:<br />

Conls, nctiort nnd human f lourislrirg. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Selection, optimization, and compensation as strategies<br />

<strong>of</strong> life management: Correlations with subjective indicators <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>.<br />

Psychologrl ond Aging, 13, 531-543.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). The orchestration <strong>of</strong> seiection, optimization and<br />

compensation: An action-theoretical conceptualization <strong>of</strong> a theory <strong>of</strong> developmental<br />

regulation. In W. J. Perrig & A. Grob (Eds.), Control <strong>of</strong> humatt bchaztior, mental processes,<br />

and consciottsricss (pp. 35-58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.


112 EvrpeNcE-Bnsr:o TuroRY<br />

Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002a). The adaptiveness <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization, and<br />

compensation as strategies <strong>of</strong> life management: Evidence from a preference study on<br />

proverbs. J ou r n al <strong>of</strong> C e ront ology : P sy cltologic nl S c i e nce s, 57 B, 426 - 434.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002b). Life management strategies <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization<br />

and compensation: Measurement by self-report and construct validity. lournal <strong>of</strong><br />

Personnlity and Social Pstlchttlogtl, 82, 642-662.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Ebner, N. C. (2005). The <strong>aging</strong> self: Shifting from promoting gains to<br />

balancing losses. ln W. Greve, K. Rothermund, & D. Wentura (Eds.), The adaptitte self:<br />

Personal continuittl nrd intentional sel.f-deaebpme nt (pp.185-202). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe<br />

& Huber.<br />

Freund, A. M., Li, K. Z. H., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). Successful development and <strong>aging</strong>: The<br />

role <strong>of</strong> selectiorr, optimization, and compensation in <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. In J. Brandtstddter<br />

& R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Action nntl self-deztelopment: Theory and research through the<br />

Iife spnrt (pp. a01-43a). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2003). Successful <strong>aging</strong>. In R. M Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks,<br />

& J. Mistry (Eds.), Contprehensiae handbook <strong>of</strong> psychology: Vol. 6. Deaelopmental<br />

psychoktgv (pp. 601-628). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.<br />

Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2006). Coals as building blocks <strong>of</strong> personality and development<br />

in adulthood. ln D. K. Mroczek & T. D. Little (Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> personality deoelopntent<br />

(pp. 353-372). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino<br />

(Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> motiuation and cogrtition: VLtl.2. Foundations <strong>of</strong> social behaaior<br />

(pp. 53-92). New York: Cuiiford Press.<br />

Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, j. A. (Eds.). (1996). The psychology <strong>of</strong> action: Linking cognition<br />

ard ntotiuntion to behaaior. New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, C. B. (1996). Goal effects on action and cogr:rition. In E. T.<br />

Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook <strong>of</strong> bnsic principles<br />

(pp.361-399). New York: Gr-riiford Press.<br />

Habermas, T., & Btuck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The emergence <strong>of</strong> the life story in adolescence.<br />

Psycltrlttgical Bullctin, 126, 748-769.<br />

Havighurst, I{. J. (1963). Successful <strong>aging</strong>. In R. H. Wiliams, C. Tibbitts, & W. Donahue<br />

(Eds.), The process <strong>of</strong> nging: Vol.1. Social and psychLtlogical perspectiues (pp.299-320). New<br />

York: Atherton Prcss.<br />

Heckhausen,J. $997). Developmental regulation across adulthood: Primary and secondary<br />

control <strong>of</strong> age-related changes. Deuelopmental Psychology, 33,776-187.<br />

Heckhausen, J. 0999). DettclLtpnrcntal ragulatiott in adulthood: Age-norrnatitte and sociostructural<br />

constraints ns ndaTttiachallenges. New York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory <strong>of</strong> control. Psychological Reaiew,<br />

102,284-304.<br />

Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1998). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Selection<br />

and compensation via primary and secondary control. In J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck<br />

(Eds.), Motiuntion nnd selt'-rcgulation across the life span (pp. 50-77). New York: Cambridge<br />

<strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Heckhausen,J.,Schulz,I{., & Wrosch, C. (1998). Deuelopmental regulation in ndulthood: Optintization<br />

in primttry nnd sccondary control. Unpublished manuscript, Max Planck Institute<br />

for Human Development, Berlin.<br />

Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Fleeson, W. (2001). Developmental regulation before and<br />

after a developmental deadline: The sample case <strong>of</strong> "biological clock" for childbearing.<br />

Psychology nnd Aging,16, 400-473.<br />

Helson, R., & Kwan, V.<br />

ture and Processe I<br />

sonality psychology (\<br />

Isaacowitz, D. M., & Sr<br />

<strong>of</strong> GerontologY, 588,I<br />

Iopp, D. (2002). Succ<br />

and adaptizte life-m<br />

BerIin.<br />

Kemper, S., Herman, F<br />

for young and old<br />

speech or noise. Ps<br />

Klinger, E. (1975). Con<br />

Psychoktgicnl Reuieu<br />

Klinger, E. (7977). Mea<br />

neapolis: Universii<br />

Krampe, R. T., & Balt<br />

resource allocatio<br />

Sternberg & E' L. C<br />

cies, and ex7ertise \P<br />

Krampe, R. T., RaPP,l<br />

sources during the<br />

Der Nerztenarzt, 74,<br />

"<br />

Kruglanski, A. W. (19<br />

(Eds.), The PsYcho<br />

New York: SPringe<br />

Kunzmann, U. (1999)<br />

being. Berlin. Cerm<br />

Labouvie-Vief, G. (19<br />

<strong>aging</strong> in life-sPan P<br />

uals as Producers <strong>of</strong> t<br />

demic Press.<br />

Lang, F. R., Rieckman<br />

facilitate strategie<br />

tioning? lournal <strong>of</strong> r<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1975)<br />

<strong>of</strong> GerontologY, 30, E<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1983)<br />

Gerontologist, 23, 34<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1989<br />

K. W. Schaie (Eds.<br />

Springer.<br />

Lawton, M. P., & Nat<br />

M. P. Lawton (Eds<br />

ington, DC: Ameri<br />

Lee, A. Y., & Aaker, J<br />

tory fit on Proces<br />

ogy, 86,205-21'8.<br />

Lerner, R. M., & Bus<br />

life-span Per sPe c tirle


Personulity Theorie s <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 113<br />

lction, optimization, and<br />

rm a preference study on<br />

6-434.<br />

es <strong>of</strong> selection, optimizarstruct<br />

validity. lotrnal <strong>of</strong><br />

from promoting gains to<br />

a (Eds.), The adaptizte self:<br />

l). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe<br />

elopment and <strong>aging</strong>: The<br />

;sful <strong>aging</strong>. In J. Brandtry<br />

and research through the<br />

M Lerner, M. A. Easterogy:<br />

Vol. 6. Deaelopmental<br />

rf personality and devel-<br />

-Iandbook<br />

<strong>of</strong> personality det.<br />

Higgins & R. M. Sor-<br />

'ndations <strong>of</strong> social behaaior<br />

f action: Linking cognition<br />

rn and cognition. In E. T.<br />

rdbook <strong>of</strong> basic principles<br />

f the life story in adoles-<br />

Tibbitts, & W. Donahue<br />

:tiaes (pp. 299-320). Nen'<br />

ld: Primary and secondt,176-787.<br />

normatiae and sociost r ucriversity<br />

Press.<br />

:ol. Psychological Reuiezu,<br />

in adulthood: Selection<br />

ckhausen & C. S. Dweck<br />

C-77). New York: Cam-<br />

,ulation in adulthood: Opcript,<br />

Max Planck Insti-<br />

I regulation before and<br />

al clock" for childbear-<br />

Helson, R., & Kwan, V. S. Y. (2000). <strong>Personality</strong> development in adulthood: The broad picture<br />

and processes in one longitudinal sample. In S. Hampson (Ed.), Adoances in personality<br />

psLtcholo;qy (Vol. 1, pp. 77-106). London: Routledge.<br />

Isaacowitz, D. M., & Smith, l. (2003). Positive and negative affect in very old age. lournals<br />

<strong>of</strong> CerontolLtgy, 588, P743-P152.<br />

Jopp, D. (2002). Successful <strong>aging</strong>: On the ftrnctional interplatl between personal resources<br />

and adaptiae life-managenrent strategies. Doctoral dissertation, Free <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

Berlin.<br />

Kemper, S., Herman, It. E., & Lian, C. H. T. (2003). The costs <strong>of</strong> doing two things at once<br />

for young and older adults: Talking while walking, finger tapping, and ignoring<br />

speech or noise. Psychology nnd Aging, 18,181-192.<br />

Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences <strong>of</strong> commitment to and disengagement from incentives.<br />

Psychological Reaieitt, 82, 1-25.<br />

Klinger, E. (1977). Menning and uoid: Inner experience nnd the incentiues in people<br />

's /lzres. Minneapolis:<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Minnesota Press.<br />

Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). lntelligence as adaptive resource development and<br />

resource allocation: A new look through the lenses <strong>of</strong> SOC and expertise. In R. J.<br />

Sternberg & E. L. Crigorenko (Eds.), Perspectiaes on tlte psychology <strong>of</strong> abilities, cornpetencies,<br />

nnd expertise (pp. 31-69). New Ycrrk: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Krampe, R. T., Rapp, M. A., Bondar, A., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Allocation <strong>of</strong> cognitive resources<br />

during the simultaneous performance <strong>of</strong> cognitive and sensorimotor tasks.<br />

Der Ne rue nnrzt, 7 4, 271-278.<br />

Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Coals as knowledge structures. ln P. M. Collwitzer & J. A. Bargh<br />

(Eds.), The pstlchologq <strong>of</strong> action: Linking cogttition and ntotiantion to behaaior (pp. 599-618).<br />

New York: Springer.<br />

Kunzmann, U. (1999). Being nnd feeling in control: Trtto sources <strong>of</strong> older people's emotional uellbeing.Berlin,<br />

Cermany: Max Planck Institute for Human Development.<br />

Labouvie-Vief, C. (1981). Proactive and reactive aspects <strong>of</strong> constructivism: Crowth and<br />

<strong>aging</strong> in Iife-span perspective. In R. M. Lerner & N. A. Busch-Rossnagel (Eds.), Indioidttals<br />

as producers <strong>of</strong> their deoelopment: A life-spnn perspectizte (pp.I97-230). New York: Academic<br />

Press.<br />

Lang, F. R., Rieckmann, N., & Baltes, M. M. (2002). Adapting to <strong>aging</strong> losses: Do resources<br />

facilitate strategies <strong>of</strong> selection, compensation, and optimization in everyday functioning?<br />

lournal <strong>of</strong> Gertmtttlogy: Psychological Sciences, 578, 507-509.<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1975). The Philadeiphia Geriatric Center Morale Scale: A revision. lournal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Gerontology , 30 , 85 -89 .<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1983). Environment and other determinants <strong>of</strong> well-being in older people.<br />

Gerontolog ist, 23, 349-357.<br />

Lawton, M. P. (1989). Environmental proactivity in older people. In V. L. Bengston &<br />

K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Tlrc corrrse <strong>of</strong> lnter life: Resenrch and rtflections (pp. 15-23). New York:<br />

Springer.<br />

Lawton, M. P., & Nahemow, L. (7973). Ecology and the <strong>aging</strong> process. In C. Eisdorfer &<br />

M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology <strong>of</strong> adult deoeloTtntent nnd <strong>aging</strong> (pp. 619-674). Washington,<br />

DC: American Psychological Associatron.<br />

Lee, A. Y., & Aaker, J. L. (2004). Bringing the frame into focus: The influence <strong>of</strong> regulatory<br />

fit on processing fluency and persuasion. lottrnal <strong>of</strong> Persttnalittl nnd Social Psychology,<br />

86,205-278.<br />

Lerner, R. M., & Busch-Rossnagel, A. (1981). Indiaidunls ns producers <strong>of</strong> their deuelopment: A<br />

life-span perspcctiue. New York: Academic Press.


774 EvrprNcr-BasnuTsEoRv<br />

Li,K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Walking while memorizing:<br />

Age-related differences in compensatory behavior. Psychological Science, 12,<br />

230-237.<br />

Lindenberger, U., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Memorizing while walking: Increase<br />

in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. Psychology and Agirtg,3,<br />

477-436.<br />

Little, B. R. (1989). Personal project analysis: Trivial pursuits, magnificent obsessions, and<br />

the search for coherence. ln D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), <strong>Personality</strong> psychology: Recent<br />

trends and emerging directions (pp. 15-31). New York: Springer.<br />

Maslow, A. (1954). Motiantion and personality. New York: Harper & Row.<br />

McAdams, D. P. (1990). Unity and purpose in human lives: The emergence <strong>of</strong> identity as<br />

a life story. In A. I. Rabin, R. A. Zucker, R. A. Emmons, & S. J. Frank (Eds.), Studying<br />

persotls nnd littcs (pp. 148-190). New York: Springer.<br />

McAdams, D. P. (1995). What do we know when we know a person? lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>,<br />

63,365-396.<br />

McAdams, D. P. (1996). <strong>Personality</strong>, modernity, and the sioried self: A contemporary<br />

framework for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7,295-327.<br />

McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology <strong>of</strong> life stories. Reaiew <strong>of</strong> Ceneral Psychology, 5,<br />

700-122.<br />

McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motioation. New York: Cambridge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, R. A., III. (2003). Modeling intraindividual change in personality<br />

traits: Findings from the Normative Aging Study. lournals <strong>of</strong> Cerontologv, 588,<br />

P153-P165.<br />

Neugarten, B. L., Havighurst, R. J., & Tobin, S. S. (1961). The measurement <strong>of</strong> life satisfaction.<br />

lournol <strong>of</strong> Cerontology, 16,134-143.<br />

Nurmi, I.-E. (1992). Age differences in adult life goals, concerns, and their temporal extension:<br />

A life-course approach to future-oriented motivation. Internstionnl Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Behnuiornl Deuelopment, 15, 187-508.<br />

Omodei, M. M., & Wearing, A. J. (1990). Need satisfaction and involvement in personal<br />

projects: Toward an integratir.'e model <strong>of</strong> subjective well-being. lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><br />

and Social Psychology, 59, 762-769.<br />

Palys, T. S., & Little, B. If. (1983). Perceived life satisfaction and the organization <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

project systems. lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social Psychology, 44,7227-7230.<br />

Pervin, L. A. (1989). Goal concepts: Themes, issues, and questions. In L. A. Pervin<br />

(Ed.), Goal concepts in personality nnd social psychology (pp.473-479). Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Pillemer, D. B. (1992). Remembering personal circumstances: A functional analysis. In E.<br />

Winograd & U. Neisser (Eds.), Emory Syntposia in Cognition: Affect lnd accuracy in recall-Stttdics<br />

<strong>of</strong> "f lnshbulb" memories (4th ed., pp.236-26\. New York: Cambridge Unir.ersity<br />

Press.<br />

Rapkin, B. D., & Fischer, K. (1992). Framing the construct <strong>of</strong> life satisfaction in terms <strong>of</strong><br />

older adults' personal goals. Psyclrologtl and Aging, 7, 138*149.<br />

Rapp, M. A., Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Adaptive task prioritization in <strong>aging</strong>:<br />

Selective resource allocation to postural control is preserved in Alzheimer disease.<br />

American lournal <strong>of</strong> Ceriatr ic Psychiatry, 14, 52-61.<br />

Riediger, M. (2001). On the dynamic relations among multiple gLtals: Intergoal conf lict and intergoal<br />

facilitation in younger and older adttlthood. Doctoral dissertation, Free <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

Berlin.<br />

Riediger, M., Freund, I<br />

Intergoal facilitatio<br />

lournals <strong>of</strong> Gerontolo<br />

Riediger, M., Li, S.-C.,{<br />

tion (SOC) as devel<br />

preview. In J. E. Bir<br />

ed., pp. 289-313). AI<br />

Roberts, B. W., & Del<br />

traits from childhoo<br />

Iogical Bulletin, 126,i<br />

Rogers, C. (1963). The<br />

ness. In M. R. Jonest<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Nebra<br />

Rothermund, K., & Br;<br />

From compensator<br />

Ryan, R. M. (1995). PsY<br />

nal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>, 63<br />

Ryff, C. D. (1989). In t<br />

middle-aged and ok<br />

Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psyc<br />

Science, 4,99-104.<br />

Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C<br />

lournal <strong>of</strong> Personali<br />

Schulz, R. (1986). Succ<br />

opment and Aging Nt<br />

Schulz, R., & Heckhau<br />

chologist, 51,702-77<br />

Sheldon, K. M., & Elli<br />

well-being: The sel<br />

482-497.<br />

Singer, I. A., & Salove<br />

Small, B. J., Hertzog,<br />

adult personalitY or<br />

<strong>of</strong> Gerontology, 588,<br />

Srivastava, S., John, O.<br />

early and middle ac<br />

and Social Psycholo<br />

Staudinger, U. M., Ma<br />

Iater adulthood: Po<br />

chetti & D. Cohen (<br />

tion (pp.801-847)I<br />

Thomae, H.E. (1'976)<br />

Veenhoven, R. (1991)<br />

spots. In F. Strack,I<br />

disciplinar y perspec<br />

Wiese, B. S., Freund, I<br />

tion: An action-rel<br />

ior,57,273-300.


Personnlity Theories <strong>of</strong> Successful Aging 115<br />

01). Walking while memo-<br />

. Psychological Science, 72,<br />

rizing while walking: lnt.<br />

Psychology and Aging, 3,<br />

agnif icent obsessions, and<br />

<strong>Personality</strong> psy chology : Reinger.<br />

& Row.<br />

) emergence <strong>of</strong> identity as<br />

i. J. Frank (Eds.), Studying<br />

son? lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>,<br />

ied self: A contemporary<br />

-321.<br />

t <strong>of</strong> Cenernl Psychology, 5,<br />

lge <strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

idual change in personalrnnls<br />

<strong>of</strong> Gerontology, 588,<br />

asurement <strong>of</strong> life satisfacs,<br />

and their temporal ex-<br />

>n. lnternational lournal <strong>of</strong><br />

involvement i n personal<br />

ing. lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><br />

I the organization <strong>of</strong> pergy,<br />

44, 1227-7230.<br />

estions. In L. A. Pervin<br />

473-479). Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

functional analysis. In E.<br />

Affect and accuracy irt rerw<br />

York: Cambridge Uni-<br />

: satisfaction in terms <strong>of</strong><br />

< prioritization in <strong>aging</strong>:<br />

d in Alzheimer disease.<br />

'ntergoal<br />

rcnf lict and intcration,<br />

Free <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

Riediger, M., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). Man<strong>aging</strong> life through personal goals:<br />

Intergoal facilitation and intensity <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit in younger and older adulthood.<br />

lottrnals <strong>of</strong> Gerontology, 608, P84-P91.<br />

Riediger, M., Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U. (2006). Seleciion, optimization, and compensation<br />

(SOC) as developmental mechanisms <strong>of</strong> adaptive resource allocation: Review and<br />

preview. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> the psychology <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong> (6th<br />

ed., pp. 289-313). Amsterdam: Elsevier.<br />

Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency <strong>of</strong> personality<br />

traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review <strong>of</strong> longitudinal studies. Psychological<br />

Bulletin, 126, 3*25.<br />

Rogers, C. (1963). The actualisation tendency in relation to "motives" and to consciousness.<br />

In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motiaation (Vol. 11, pp.1.-2Q. Lincoln:<br />

<strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Nebraska Press.<br />

Rothermund, K., & Brandtstiidter, J. (2003). Coping with deficits and losses in later life:<br />

From compensatory action to accommodation. Psycllology and Aging,18,896-905.<br />

Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation <strong>of</strong> integrative processes. /or''-<br />

nal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong>, 63, 397-127 .<br />

Ryff, C. D. (1989). In the eye <strong>of</strong> the beholder: Views <strong>of</strong> psychological well-being among<br />

middle-aged and oider adults. Psychology and Aging, 4,795-270.<br />

Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Current Directions in Psychological<br />

Science. 1.99-104.<br />

Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure <strong>of</strong> psychological well-being revisiied.<br />

lotrrnal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social Psychology, 69,71.9-727.<br />

Schulz, R. (1986). Successful <strong>aging</strong>: Balancing primary and secondary control. Adult Deuelopment<br />

and Aging Nerus, 13,2-4.<br />

Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. 0996). A life-span model <strong>of</strong> <strong>successful</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. American Psychologist,<br />

51, 702-774.<br />

Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. I. 0999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal<br />

well-being: The self-concordance model. lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social Psychology, 76,<br />

482-497.<br />

Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1993). The remembered self. New York: Free Press.<br />

Small, B. J., Hertzog,, C., Hultsch, D. F., & Dixon, R. A. (2003). Stability and change in<br />

adult personality over 6 years: Findings from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. lournals<br />

<strong>of</strong> Gerontology, 588, P166-P176.<br />

Srivastava, S., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, l. (2003). Development <strong>of</strong> personality in<br />

early and middle adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent change? lournal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong><br />

nnd Social Psychology, 84, 7041-1053.<br />

Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Resilience and reserve capacity in<br />

later adulthood: Potentials and limits <strong>of</strong> development across the life span. In D. Cicchetti<br />

& D. Cohen (Eds.), Deaelopmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, and adaptation<br />

(pp.801.-847). New York: Wiley.<br />

Thomae, H. E. (1976). Patterns <strong>of</strong> <strong>aging</strong>. Basel: Karger.<br />

Veenhoven, R. (1991). Questions on happiness: Classical topics, modern answers, blind<br />

spots. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjectiae zuell-being: Vol.2. An interd<br />

i sc ipl i n a r y p e r sp e c t iae (pp. 7 -26). Oxf ord : Pergamon Press.<br />

Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Selection, opiimization, and compensation:<br />

An action-related approach to work and partnership. lournal <strong>of</strong> Vocational Behaaior.<br />

57,273-300.


776 EvrupNcn-Besro TrrsoRv<br />

Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002). Subjective career success and emotional<br />

well-being: Longitudinal predictive power <strong>of</strong> selection, optimization and compensation.<br />

lottrnal Ltf Vocationnl Behaaior, 60,321-335.<br />

Wiese, B. S., & Schmitz, B. (2002). Action regulaiion at university: Application <strong>of</strong> a developmental<br />

meta-model. Zeitschrift fiir Entwicklungspsychologie und Piidngogischc Psychologie,<br />

34,80-94.<br />

Wrosch, C., & Heckhausen, I.0999). Control processes before and after passing a developmental<br />

deadline: Activation and deactivation <strong>of</strong> intimate relationship goals. lournal<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>Personality</strong> and Social Psychology, 77, 475-427.<br />

Wi<br />

SC<br />

o<br />

SUSANNI<br />

Jts<br />

rurs cHAPTER\<br />

I<br />

novel constructs r<br />

Ilongings). Both d<br />

utopias. Our work or<br />

opmental constructs<br />

tion and conduct <strong>of</strong><br />

the past, the present<br />

capturing the comp<br />

nents <strong>of</strong> the structur<br />

berger, & Staudinge<br />

In the search for s<br />

formed not only by r<br />

discourse in everyda<br />

(G eis t e sw is s e nsch af t e n<br />

much for making psy<br />

sumed that in these t<br />

search had shied awe<br />

In the selection <strong>of</strong> th<br />

unchartered territor<br />

The theoreticai and empi<br />

and directed by Paul B. I<br />

many thanks especially tr<br />

the various phases <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Jacqui Smith, Ursula M. S

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!