12.11.2014 Views

Spatial hemineglect in humans - Cisi

Spatial hemineglect in humans - Cisi

Spatial hemineglect in humans - Cisi

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

G. Kerkho€ / Progress <strong>in</strong> Neurobiology 63 (2001) 1±27 3<br />

Table 1<br />

Summary of sensory, motor and representational neglect phenomena occurr<strong>in</strong>g after unilateral lesions <strong>in</strong> <strong>humans</strong><br />

Type of neglect<br />

Visual<br />

Auditory<br />

Somatosensory<br />

Olfactory<br />

Motor<br />

Representational<br />

De®nition and typical behaviour<br />

Patient searches for stimuli with eye- and head-movements preferentially <strong>in</strong> the ipsilesional hemispace. Omission of<br />

contralesional stimuli dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g, writ<strong>in</strong>g, draw<strong>in</strong>g of geometric stimuli, bisect<strong>in</strong>g horizontal l<strong>in</strong>es, or eat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from a plate. Ipsilesional deviation of the perceived subjective straight ahead.<br />

Patient does not react to sound/speech stimuli from the contralateral hemispace. He/she may turn to the<br />

ipsilesional side when addressed from the contralesional. When several speakers are present the patient responds<br />

preferentially to the most ipsilesional one, irrespective of who has spoken. Ipsilesional deviation of the perceived<br />

auditory midl<strong>in</strong>e position <strong>in</strong> front space.<br />

Ignor<strong>in</strong>g of tactile stimulation (i.e. touch) or pa<strong>in</strong>ful stimuli (cold/hot stimuli, jammed ®ngers <strong>in</strong> wheel or spokes of<br />

the wheelchair) on the contralesional body half. Mislocalization of tactile stimuli <strong>in</strong> this part. Subjective shift of<br />

own body-midl<strong>in</strong>e (i.e. position of the sp<strong>in</strong>e) to the ipsilesional side.<br />

Ignor<strong>in</strong>g smells delivered to one nostril. Rarely observed <strong>in</strong> daily life s<strong>in</strong>ce stimuli are easily detected with the other<br />

nostril.<br />

Reduced use of contralesional arm/leg which is not completely attributable to a sensorimotor loss. Reduced arm<br />

sway dur<strong>in</strong>g walk<strong>in</strong>g; reduced use of contralesional arm dur<strong>in</strong>g bimanual activities (i.e. eat<strong>in</strong>g, carry<strong>in</strong>g loads);<br />

dragg<strong>in</strong>g beh<strong>in</strong>d the contralesional leg/foot dur<strong>in</strong>g walk<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Patient describes few items located <strong>in</strong> the contralesional part of an imag<strong>in</strong>ed scene (i.e. a famous city place, the<br />

own liv<strong>in</strong>g room or house), but describes much more items when describ<strong>in</strong>g the same scene from a di€erent<br />

perspective (1808 rotated).<br />

lesioned patients 1 (Stone et al., 1991) when tested immediately<br />

(with<strong>in</strong> 7 days) after lesion onset. The absolute<br />

percentage of neglect depends critically on the<br />

criterion or test used but the asymmetry <strong>in</strong> the occurrence<br />

of contralesional neglect has been found across<br />

di€erent samples and methods (Schenkenberg et al.,<br />

1980). Recovery is considerably quicker and more<br />

complete <strong>in</strong> neglect after left-hemisphere lesions as<br />

opposed to right-hemisphere lesions (Stone et al.,<br />

1991). Thus, there is a clear hemispheric asymmetry<br />

show<strong>in</strong>g that neglect is more frequent, more severe and<br />

more permanent follow<strong>in</strong>g right-hemispheric lesions.<br />

However, transient rightsided neglect after left unilateral<br />

lesions occurs <strong>in</strong> some cases (Welman, 1969; Peru<br />

and P<strong>in</strong>na, 1997; Kerkho€ and Zoelch, 1998). More<br />

long-last<strong>in</strong>g rightsided neglect occurs <strong>in</strong> patients with<br />

bilateral cerebral lesions (We<strong>in</strong>traub et al., 1996).<br />

2.2. Mechanisms of recovery<br />

1 S<strong>in</strong>ce leftsided neglect after right cerebral lesions is the most frequent<br />

type of neglect the term ``neglect'' <strong>in</strong> this review refers always<br />

to leftsided neglect <strong>in</strong> <strong>humans</strong> if not <strong>in</strong>dicated otherwise. Of course,<br />

this does not exclude the fact that rightsided neglect after uni- or bilateral<br />

lesions may occur (more rarely) as well.<br />

Recovery from the most obvious signs of neglect<br />

(i.e. the tendency to orient to the ipsilesional side and<br />

the lack of visual exploration <strong>in</strong> the contralesional<br />

hemispace) has been noted <strong>in</strong> the majority of patients<br />

with<strong>in</strong> the ®rst 6 months (Lawson, 1962; Hier et al.,<br />

1983). In the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 25% neglect may persist for<br />

up to 12 years (Zarit and Kahn, 1974) and performance<br />

<strong>in</strong> tasks which are sensitive to neglect may decl<strong>in</strong>e<br />

aga<strong>in</strong> after cessation of apparently successful rehabilitation<br />

treatment <strong>in</strong> the cl<strong>in</strong>ic (Paolucci et al., 1998;<br />

Hier et al., 1983). Recovery from neglect is more prom<strong>in</strong>ent<br />

after left compared with right-sided cerebral<br />

lesions (Stone et al., 1991). Furthermore, recovery<br />

from ``frontal'' neglect is more rapid and more complete<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>humans</strong> than from the classical ``parietal''<br />

neglect syndrome (Matt<strong>in</strong>gley et al., 1994a). Substantial<br />

recovery is less likely after large lesions and <strong>in</strong><br />

those patients with di€use bra<strong>in</strong> atrophy <strong>in</strong> addition to<br />

the focal right hemispheric lesion (Lev<strong>in</strong>e et al., 1986)<br />

Little is known about the mechanisms guid<strong>in</strong>g spontaneous<br />

recovery and/or those enabl<strong>in</strong>g treatmentguided<br />

improvements dur<strong>in</strong>g rehabilitation. Pantano et<br />

al. (1992) reported a concomitant <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> regional<br />

cerebral blood ¯ow (rCBF) <strong>in</strong> the posterior areas of<br />

the damaged right hemisphere and the anterior areas<br />

of the <strong>in</strong>tact, left hemisphere probably <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

frontal eye ®elds after a speci®c neglect treatment <strong>in</strong><br />

their patients. Only the left frontal activation, <strong>in</strong> the<br />

region of the frontal eye ®elds, covaried with improved<br />

visual scann<strong>in</strong>g behavior after treatment. The authors<br />

concluded that the left (<strong>in</strong>tact) frontal eye ®elds are<br />

crucial for recovery of visual scann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> neglect. In a<br />

later PET study with three neglect patients, this ®nd<strong>in</strong>g<br />

was not uniformly replicated (Pizzamiglio et al., 1998).<br />

In this study, behavioral recovery seemed to covary<br />

with improved blood ¯ow <strong>in</strong> surviv<strong>in</strong>g areas of the<br />

lesioned hemisphere.<br />

Similar results have been obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> primate studies<br />

where neglect was <strong>in</strong>duced by lesion<strong>in</strong>g the frontal<br />

polysensory association cortex. Metabolic mapp<strong>in</strong>g for<br />

local glucose utilization (2-DG) showed a widespread<br />

reduction of glucose <strong>in</strong> ipsilesional striatal and partially<br />

thalamic nuclei connected with the frontal lobe<br />

whereas no reductions were found <strong>in</strong> cortical areas

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!