19.11.2014 Views

Levels of Processing Effects on Implicit and Explicit Memory Tasks ...

Levels of Processing Effects on Implicit and Explicit Memory Tasks ...

Levels of Processing Effects on Implicit and Explicit Memory Tasks ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Levels</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Processing</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>Implicit</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Explicit</strong> <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>Tasks</strong>:<br />

Using Questi<strong>on</strong> Positi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

Investigate the Lexical-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Processing</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Hypothesis<br />

Ben R. Newell 1 <strong>and</strong> Sally Andrews 2<br />

1 University College L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>, UK <strong>and</strong> 2 University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sydney, Australia<br />

Abstract. One interpretati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing effects (LOP) <strong>on</strong> priming in implicit tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> memory is in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

deficits in lexical processing during shallow study tasks. In two experiments the extent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing engaged in<br />

during st<strong>and</strong>ard shallow encoding tasks was manipulated by placing the encoding questi<strong>on</strong> either before or after the target<br />

stimulus. Clear evidence was found in explicit memory tasks that placing the questi<strong>on</strong> after the target stimulus increased<br />

the depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words presented in shallow encoding tasks. In c<strong>on</strong>trast, there was no evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> such an effect<br />

<strong>on</strong> the priming observed in implicit memory tasks. The results suggest that the role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing in LOP effects<br />

<strong>on</strong> priming requires further specificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Key words: implicit <strong>and</strong> explicit memory, levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing, lexical processing hypothesis<br />

Dissociati<strong>on</strong>s have played an important role in the<br />

vast body <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> recent research <strong>on</strong> implicit <strong>and</strong> explicit<br />

memory tasks (see Foster & Jelicic, 1999, for reviews)<br />

because they are argued to provide <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

primary sources <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> evidence for the independence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the mechanisms underlying these two forms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

memory (e.g. Squire, 1994; but see Buchner & Wippich,<br />

2000, <strong>and</strong> Dunn & Kirsner, 1988).<br />

One <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the variables most frequently investigated<br />

in the search for dissociati<strong>on</strong>s between implicit <strong>and</strong><br />

explicit memory is the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing required<br />

during stimulus encoding (Roediger & McDermott,<br />

1993). The basic levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing (LOP) effect is<br />

that “deep” semantic study processing leads to better<br />

The support <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Ec<strong>on</strong>omic <strong>and</strong> Social Research<br />

Council the Comm<strong>on</strong>wealth Scholarship <strong>and</strong> Fellowship<br />

Fund <strong>and</strong> the Australian Research Council are gratefully<br />

acknowledged. We thank Caleb Owens <strong>and</strong> Nicola West<strong>on</strong><br />

for their help in collecting the data.<br />

subsequent memory performance than “shallow”<br />

perceptual or physical study processing (Craik &<br />

Lockhart, 1972). This is <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the most robust findings<br />

in st<strong>and</strong>ard explicit tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> memory, such as<br />

recall <strong>and</strong> recogniti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Early investigati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance in tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicit<br />

memory found no effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP (Graf &<br />

M<strong>and</strong>ler, 1984; Jacoby & Dallas, 1981). Given the<br />

centrality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the noti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> deep semantic processing<br />

to theories <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> memory encoding, this apparent dissociati<strong>on</strong><br />

seemed to provide str<strong>on</strong>g evidence for the<br />

existence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> either different memory systems (Tulving,<br />

1983) or different retrieval processes (Jacoby &<br />

Dallas, 1981). LOP effects therefore were seen as a<br />

useful vehicle for precisely specifying these crucial<br />

theoretical distincti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> were used to index memory<br />

processes thought to be crucial to explicit memory<br />

such as elaborati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., M<strong>and</strong>ler, Hams<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Dorfman, 1990) <strong>and</strong> retrieval intenti<strong>on</strong>ality (Schacter,<br />

Bowers, & Booker, 1989). More recent data dem-<br />

DOI: 10.1027//1618-3169.51.2.1<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

2 Ben R. Newell & Sally Andrews<br />

<strong>on</strong>strating LOP effects <strong>on</strong> implicit memory tests<br />

have, however, challenged the assumpti<strong>on</strong> that LOP<br />

effects are restricted to explicit memory (see<br />

Brown & Mitchell, 1994; Challis & Brodbeck, 1992;<br />

Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn & Gardiner, 1998, for reviews).<br />

These findings also have implicati<strong>on</strong>s for accounts<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processes resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the LOP<br />

effect itself.<br />

Three major mechanisms have been proposed to<br />

underlie the LOP effects observed in implicit memory<br />

tasks. One source <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the effects could be the influence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> voluntary retrieval strategies. Despite experimenters’<br />

attempts to disguise the nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

indirect memory task, participants may intenti<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

retrieve items from the previous study episode (e.g.,<br />

Bowers & Schacter, 1990; Toth, Reingold, & Jacoby,<br />

1994). <strong>Implicit</strong> memory test performance clearly can<br />

be influenced by explicit retrieval strategies <strong>and</strong> subjects<br />

who are instructed about the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between<br />

the study <strong>and</strong> test tasks, or become sp<strong>on</strong>taneously<br />

aware <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this relati<strong>on</strong>ship, show str<strong>on</strong>ger LOP<br />

effects in perceptual implicit memory tests (Bowers<br />

& Schacter, 1990). However, small LOP effects<br />

have also been found in implicit memory tasks under<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s designed to minimize c<strong>on</strong>taminati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

for participants who are unaware <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the study-test<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ship (Challis & Brodbeck, 1992; Richards<strong>on</strong>-<br />

Klavehn & Gardiner, 1998).<br />

A sec<strong>on</strong>d c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to LOP effects <strong>on</strong> implicit<br />

memory performance appears to arise from the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>ceptually driven retrieval processes.<br />

Support for this view derives from differences between<br />

the LOP effects observed in perceptual implicit<br />

memory tests such as perceptual identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

or stem/fragment completi<strong>on</strong> tasks <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

tests such as generati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> category exemplars or answering<br />

general knowledge questi<strong>on</strong>s. Early comparis<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the two types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> tasks suggested that LOP<br />

effects occurred in c<strong>on</strong>ceptual tests but not in perceptual<br />

tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicit memory (e.g., Blaxt<strong>on</strong>, 1989;<br />

Hamman, 1990; Rajaram & Roediger, 1993; Roediger,<br />

Srinivas, & Weld<strong>on</strong>, 1989). This c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

has since been qualified by the results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a number<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> more recent experiments <strong>and</strong> reviews showing that<br />

attenuated LOP effects do occur in perceptual implicit<br />

memory tests. Although these effects are not<br />

always significant, the numerical advantage for deep<br />

over shallow study processing is c<strong>on</strong>sistent across the<br />

majority <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studies (see Brown & Mitchell, 1994;<br />

Challis & Brodbeck, 1992 for meta-analyses). Thus,<br />

although the LOP effect <strong>on</strong> implicit memory tests is<br />

enhanced under c<strong>on</strong>ceptually driven retrieval c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

it is thought to remain in at least attenuated<br />

form in many perceptual tests.<br />

Both <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the accounts discussed so far essentially<br />

attribute LOP effects in perceptual implicit tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

memory to the same processes that give rise to LOP<br />

Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13<br />

effects <strong>on</strong> explicit memory performance because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

“c<strong>on</strong>taminati<strong>on</strong>” from processes that were supposed<br />

to be eliminated from the measure. In this sense,<br />

these accounts imply that the task employed to assess<br />

implicit memory has failed to meet the goal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> operati<strong>on</strong>alizing<br />

“involuntary retrieval <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> perceptual informati<strong>on</strong>”<br />

(Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn & Gardiner, 1998,<br />

p. 605), because they are sensitive to either voluntary<br />

retrieval mechanisms or to influences from n<strong>on</strong>perceptual<br />

informati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

A third account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP effects <strong>on</strong> implicit memory<br />

tests attributes them to processes that are inherent<br />

rather than epiphenomenal to the intended task.<br />

This lexical processing hypothesis assumes that stimuli<br />

must engage lexical processing at encoding in order<br />

to yield priming effects in implicit memory tasks.<br />

Challis <strong>and</strong> Brodbeck (1992) first suggested this hypothesis<br />

<strong>on</strong> the basis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a review <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the literature <strong>on</strong><br />

LOP effects <strong>on</strong> perceptual implicit memory tests.<br />

They noted that, in a typical LOP experiment, participants<br />

know before they see a target item what task<br />

they are required to perform <strong>on</strong> it (e.g., Does it have<br />

a “p” in it? Can it be used as a noun?). This means<br />

that shallow processing tasks may allow “an attenuati<strong>on</strong><br />

in the perceptual processing necessary to derive<br />

the lexical form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the word” (p. 605). For example,<br />

participants who are simply required to check for the<br />

presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a letter or count the number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ants<br />

or vowels may not complete processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

stimulus to a lexical level. If lexical processing is<br />

necessary for priming in perceptual implicit memory<br />

tests, the impoverished processing in the shallow relative<br />

to the deep study c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s would be reflected<br />

in reduced priming for words encoded in shallow<br />

versus deep encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s; that is, there would<br />

be an LOP effect <strong>on</strong> priming.<br />

Two studies provide supportive, but indirect, evidence<br />

for the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing to<br />

priming. Weld<strong>on</strong> (1991) used anagram stimuli at<br />

study, which were unrecognizable as words unless<br />

they were rearranged according to a rule <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> swapping<br />

the vowels (e.g., tripocs). Priming <strong>on</strong> a subsequent<br />

word fragment completi<strong>on</strong> task for the re-arranged<br />

words (e.g., tropics) was <strong>on</strong>ly observed when participants<br />

were told to use the rule to rearrange the anagrams<br />

at encoding. Exposure to identical study items<br />

<strong>and</strong> test fragments without this instructi<strong>on</strong> yielded<br />

no priming suggesting that priming depended <strong>on</strong> processing<br />

the stimulus as a lexical unit <strong>and</strong> not <strong>on</strong> perceptual<br />

processing al<strong>on</strong>e. Lexical c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s to<br />

priming are also implicated by an experiment c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

by Hayman <strong>and</strong> Jacoby (1989) that c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> either a precued or a postcued letter-identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

task followed by a word-identificati<strong>on</strong> task. Words<br />

presented in the precued letter-identificati<strong>on</strong> task did<br />

not yield any priming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subsequent word identificati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

but words in the postcued letter identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

LOP <strong>and</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong> Positi<strong>on</strong><br />

3<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> produced str<strong>on</strong>g priming (Hayman & Jacoby,<br />

1989). This suggests that maintaining the word<br />

in working memory prior to letter naming, as required<br />

in the post-cued c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, was necessary to<br />

produce subsequent whole word priming.<br />

The most direct test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the lexical processing hypothesis<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP effects in implicit memory tests is<br />

provided by comparing performance in a shallow<br />

study c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> which promotes whole word processing<br />

with that in another shallow study c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> that<br />

discourages it (Brown & Mitchell, 1994). Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn<br />

<strong>and</strong> Gardiner (1998) presented data<br />

from two experiments that employed such manipulati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Their first experiment compared the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

graphemic, ph<strong>on</strong>emic, <strong>and</strong> semantic study tasks <strong>on</strong><br />

stem completi<strong>on</strong> tests that varied <strong>on</strong>ly in the instructi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

given to subjects. The direct or “intenti<strong>on</strong>al”<br />

memory test, in which participants were told to complete<br />

stems with words from the study lists, showed<br />

a st<strong>and</strong>ard LOP effect Ð memory following semantic<br />

study was greater than that following ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

study which, in turn, yielded str<strong>on</strong>ger memory than<br />

a graphemic task. In the “incidental” or indirect test,<br />

in which participants were told to complete stems<br />

with the first word they thought <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>, priming<br />

following semantic <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic study was equivalent,<br />

<strong>and</strong> greater than that following graphemic<br />

study. Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner argued that<br />

these data supported the lexical processing hypothesis<br />

because their graphemic task (counting the<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> enclosed spaces within a word) was less<br />

likely to engage lexical processing than the ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

task (counting syllables), accounting for the reduced<br />

priming following graphemic compared to<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>emic encoding. This hypothesis was supported<br />

by the results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a sec<strong>on</strong>d experiment in which a<br />

lexical decisi<strong>on</strong> judgment was required following<br />

each encoding task. This c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> eliminated the<br />

deficit in priming following the graphemic task <strong>and</strong><br />

yielded equivalent priming in all encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Using Questi<strong>on</strong> Positi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

Investigate the Locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP <str<strong>on</strong>g>Effects</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

The present experiments investigated the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

another manipulati<strong>on</strong> that seems likely to influence<br />

the extent to which items are subject to lexical processing<br />

at encoding <strong>and</strong> that follows directly from<br />

Challis <strong>and</strong> Brodbeck’s (1992) original formulati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the lexical processing hypothesis. The experiments<br />

compared performance <strong>on</strong> explicit <strong>and</strong> implicit<br />

tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> memory as a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whether the<br />

encoding questi<strong>on</strong> that participants were required to<br />

answer for each item was presented before or after<br />

the item itself. This allowed us to test directly<br />

whether foreknowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the encoding task influences<br />

the LOP effects observed in implicit <strong>and</strong> explicit<br />

memory tests (Challis & Brodbeck, 1992; Roediger<br />

& McDermott, 1993).<br />

The use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the before/after manipulati<strong>on</strong> was<br />

based <strong>on</strong> earlier work by Craik (1977) <strong>and</strong> Moeser<br />

(1983) in experiments involving <strong>on</strong>ly explicit tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

memory. Those studies presented findings c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with the view that participants in shallow encoding<br />

tasks truncate their processing when they know what<br />

encoding operati<strong>on</strong> they are required to perform before<br />

the item appears, but that truncati<strong>on</strong> is prevented<br />

when participants must maintain the item until they<br />

are presented with the encoding questi<strong>on</strong> (Roediger<br />

& McDermott, 1993). For example, Moeser<br />

(1983) dem<strong>on</strong>strated that when the questi<strong>on</strong> appeared<br />

before the word, participants in both a case<br />

judgment (upper/lower) <strong>and</strong> a letter identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> performed at a similarly low level, whereas<br />

participants in the semantic c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> exhibited a<br />

higher level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> recogniti<strong>on</strong> performance Ð the classic<br />

LOP effect. However, when the encoding questi<strong>on</strong><br />

was presented after the word, the advantage for semantic<br />

over letter encoding tasks disappeared, although<br />

both led to better memory performance than<br />

the case task. Presumably, the case judgment task<br />

was performed by retaining the case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the target<br />

rather than the individual item <strong>and</strong> therefore showed<br />

no memory benefit in the questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Craik (1977) reported similar improvements for shallowly<br />

encoded items in a questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

although there was still an overall advantage for semantically<br />

encoded items.<br />

If the recogniti<strong>on</strong> benefit that Moeser (1983) <strong>and</strong><br />

Craik (1977) observed in the questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the shallow encoding tasks reflects lexical processing<br />

that is normally truncated when the encoding<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> is known prior to target presentati<strong>on</strong>, then<br />

the questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> manipulati<strong>on</strong> provides a means<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> evaluating the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing<br />

to LOP effects in both explicit <strong>and</strong> implicit memory<br />

tasks. In particular, if priming effects in tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicit<br />

memory depend <strong>on</strong> lexical processing, then<br />

they should be modulated by questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>. Presenting<br />

the questi<strong>on</strong> after the word forces participants<br />

to maintain the item in memory. Assuming that<br />

memory maintenance relies <strong>on</strong> lexical codes (Hayman<br />

& Jacoby, 1989), the questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

should ensure that all items are processed to a lexical<br />

level regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding questi<strong>on</strong>. In turn, this<br />

should eliminate any comp<strong>on</strong>ents <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the LOP effect<br />

due to differential lexical involvement in the same<br />

way that requiring a lexical decisi<strong>on</strong> judgment eliminated<br />

the differential priming effects across encoding<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s in Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner’s<br />

(1998) sec<strong>on</strong>d experiment.<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

4 Ben R. Newell & Sally Andrews<br />

Experiments 1A <strong>and</strong> 1B<br />

Experiment 1A examined the reliability <strong>and</strong> generalizability<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> manipulati<strong>on</strong> in tests<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicit memory by attempting to replicate Moeser’s<br />

(1983) <strong>and</strong> Craik’s (1977) results using novel<br />

encoding tasks. Experiment 1B extended the questi<strong>on</strong><br />

positi<strong>on</strong> manipulati<strong>on</strong> to an implicit stem completi<strong>on</strong><br />

memory task to provide evidence relevant to<br />

the lexical processing hypothesis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> priming effects.<br />

Following Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner’s<br />

(1998) recommendati<strong>on</strong>, we included two relatively<br />

shallow encoding tasks that differ in the degree to<br />

which they encourage lexical processing. Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn<br />

<strong>and</strong> Gardiner found reduced priming effects<br />

<strong>on</strong> implicit memory <strong>on</strong>ly for items subjected to<br />

a graphemic encoding task; ph<strong>on</strong>ologically encoded<br />

items yielded priming equivalent to that following<br />

semantic encoding. This is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the view<br />

that lexical retrieval is the primary determinant <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

priming effects because retrieving the ph<strong>on</strong>ological<br />

form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a printed word usually relies <strong>on</strong> lexical retrieval<br />

(e.g., Andrews, 1982). By c<strong>on</strong>trast, semantic<br />

encoding resulted in better explicit memory than the<br />

two other encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

This highlights a further implicati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the lexical<br />

processing account Ð if LOP effects <strong>on</strong> priming in<br />

implicit memory tasks reflect lexical retrieval, but<br />

are insensitive to the deeper c<strong>on</strong>ceptual processing<br />

that c<strong>on</strong>tributes to LOP effects <strong>on</strong> explicit memory<br />

performance, explicit <strong>and</strong> implicit memory tests will<br />

show a different pattern across encoding task levels<br />

reflecting reliance <strong>on</strong> at least partially distinct mechanisms.<br />

To provide a sensitive test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these different<br />

patterns, Experiments 1A <strong>and</strong> 1B compared graphemic,<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ological, <strong>and</strong> semantic encoding tasks.<br />

Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner’s (1998) graphemic<br />

task involved counting the number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> enclosed<br />

spaces in words Ð an extremely shallow form<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding. This particular task was employed in<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e (Bowers & Schacter, 1990) <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the 38 studies<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP effects <strong>on</strong> implicit memory performance reviewed<br />

by Brown <strong>and</strong> Mitchell (1994), while a further<br />

5 studies (Challis & Brodbeck, 1992; Roediger,<br />

Weld<strong>on</strong>, Stadler, & Riegler, 1992) required judgments<br />

about ascending <strong>and</strong> descending letters that<br />

also require attenti<strong>on</strong> to letter features. Most <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

shallow encoding tasks included in their meta-analysis<br />

required attenti<strong>on</strong> to the letter-level or higher<br />

(e.g., letter detecti<strong>on</strong>, vowel comparis<strong>on</strong>s). If the attenuated<br />

lexical processing account applies <strong>on</strong>ly to<br />

very shallow encoding tasks, then it provides a limited<br />

account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP effects <strong>on</strong> tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicit memory.<br />

To evaluate the generality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn<br />

<strong>and</strong> Gardiner’s (1998) findings about the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing to priming, Experiments<br />

1A <strong>and</strong> 1B used letter detecti<strong>on</strong> as the graphemic<br />

Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13<br />

encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. This is less shallow than counting<br />

the number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> enclosed spaces, but still appears<br />

to permit truncati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing if participants<br />

know in advance which letter they are searching for<br />

(Hayman & Jacoby, 1989). Experiments 1A <strong>and</strong> 1B<br />

compared the letter encoding task with a rhyme task<br />

designed to encourage ph<strong>on</strong>ological coding <strong>and</strong> a semantic<br />

task requiring judgments <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning relati<strong>on</strong>ships.<br />

In each encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, participants received<br />

a different specific questi<strong>on</strong> for each word c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

an item-specific cue letter or word that provided<br />

the basis for a binary letter, rhyme, or semantic judgment<br />

(e.g., the word horse might be paired with the<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s: Does it c<strong>on</strong>tain the letter r? Does it rhyme<br />

with course?, orIs it semantically related to rider?).<br />

The particular questi<strong>on</strong> for each target item was presented<br />

either before or after the target word. In the<br />

after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the word disappeared before the<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> was presented to force participants to encode<br />

the word in short-term working memory.<br />

In both experiments, the LOP manipulati<strong>on</strong> was<br />

implemented as a within-participants factor. Between-participants<br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong>s are thought to<br />

magnify LOP effects, presumably because they enhance<br />

the likelihood that participants will differentiate<br />

their encoding processes according to the particular<br />

dem<strong>and</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the encoding instructi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Thapar<br />

& Greene, 1994, though see also Brown &<br />

Mitchell, 1994). 1 However, such designs also allow<br />

participants to use different retrieval strategies re-<br />

1 In a previous experiment we used a between-participants<br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP. The design was equivalent to<br />

Experiments 1A <strong>and</strong> 1B combined, that is, three types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

encoding task (letter, rhyme, semantic) followed by a recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

or stem completi<strong>on</strong> task. Both test task <strong>and</strong> encoding<br />

task were manipulated between-participants, with<br />

over 40 participants in each encoding task c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Despite<br />

using this design, the size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the LOP effects we observed<br />

were very similar in over-all magnitude to those in<br />

Experiments 1A <strong>and</strong> 1B. In the recogniti<strong>on</strong> task <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this<br />

previous experiment, the difference in the mean proporti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words recognized following the letter task <strong>and</strong> the<br />

rhyme task was .20 <strong>and</strong> .01 in the Before <strong>and</strong> After c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

respectively. These differences are identical to those<br />

observed in Experiment 1A (see Table 1). In the stem completi<strong>on</strong><br />

task, the difference in the mean proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stems<br />

completed following the letter <strong>and</strong> the rhyme task was .04<br />

<strong>and</strong> .03 in the Before <strong>and</strong> After c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s respectively.<br />

Again, these values are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the differences <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

.02 <strong>and</strong> .01 reported for the stem-completi<strong>on</strong> task used in<br />

Experiment 1B. Given the similarity in the pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> data<br />

for both within <strong>and</strong> between-participant designs, we opted<br />

for a within-participant design because it does not appear<br />

to affect the likelihood <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> obtaining LOP effects with our<br />

materials (cf. Brown & Mitchell, 1994, <strong>and</strong> see General<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong>), but does eliminate potential c<strong>on</strong>taminati<strong>on</strong><br />

from differential retrieval strategies (cf. Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn<br />

& Gardiner, 1998).<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

LOP <strong>and</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong> Positi<strong>on</strong><br />

5<br />

flecting their different encoding experiences. These<br />

different strategies, rather than incidental or intenti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

memory test instructi<strong>on</strong>s, might be resp<strong>on</strong>sible<br />

for any differences in the patterns <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP effects<br />

obtained (Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn & Gardiner, 1998).<br />

To eliminate these differential retrieval strategies,<br />

<strong>and</strong> provide a str<strong>on</strong>ger test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong><br />

positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> its interacti<strong>on</strong> with LOP c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

LOP encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were r<strong>and</strong>omly intermixed<br />

across the study list.<br />

Method<br />

Overview<br />

Both experiments c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a study phase in<br />

which level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding task was manipulated within<br />

subjects, <strong>and</strong> a test phase in which whole studied<br />

items (Experiment 1A) or stems <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studied items<br />

(Experiments 1B) were presented with distracter<br />

items. The experiments were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in different<br />

locati<strong>on</strong>s, but used the same stimuli <strong>and</strong> experimental<br />

program to present the encoding tasks. The <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

difference between the experiments was the form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

memory test: Experiment 1A used a recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

memory task following Moeser (1983) <strong>and</strong> Craik<br />

(1977), while Experiment 1B used a stem completi<strong>on</strong><br />

task, <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the most widely used perceptual implicit<br />

memory tasks.<br />

Participants<br />

The participants in Experiment 1A were 28 members<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the University College L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> research community<br />

who participated in the experiment in return<br />

for £ 2.50 remunerati<strong>on</strong>. Participants were r<strong>and</strong>omly<br />

allocated to either the before or after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, resulting<br />

in 14 in each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The participants in Experiment 1B were 42<br />

undergraduate students from the University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> New<br />

South Wales, Sydney, who received course credit for<br />

participati<strong>on</strong>. Half were r<strong>and</strong>omly allocated to the<br />

before <strong>and</strong> half to the after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Materials<br />

Four sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 12 target words were generated that were<br />

matched <strong>on</strong> word frequency <strong>and</strong> number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> possible<br />

completi<strong>on</strong>s (mean word frequencies in the CELEX<br />

database: List A = 54.7, List B = 54.5, List C = 55.1,<br />

List D = 58.4). Participants were each presented with<br />

<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four possible lists that rotated the word sets<br />

across encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Target items were fully<br />

rotated across the distracter c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each memory<br />

task to obtain baseline false alarm <strong>and</strong> stem completi<strong>on</strong><br />

rates for target words. Thus, there were 36<br />

target items in each study list <strong>and</strong> 12 distracter items<br />

that appeared <strong>on</strong>ly in the memory test. The first three<br />

letters <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each word (the word stem in Experiment<br />

1B) in all lists was the beginning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> at least 10 comm<strong>on</strong><br />

words.<br />

Regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding list, all participants received<br />

identical test lists c<strong>on</strong>taining whole items (Experiment<br />

1A) or stems (Experiment 1B) <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> all four<br />

sub-lists. The test sheets were single A4 piece <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> paper<br />

c<strong>on</strong>taining the 48 words or stems.<br />

Procedure<br />

The encoding phase <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the experiment was run <strong>on</strong><br />

IBM PC computers using the Inquisit experiment<br />

generator program (Draine, 1996). All instructi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for carrying out the tasks were presented <strong>on</strong> the computer.<br />

Participants were presented with two “worked<br />

examples” <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding questi<strong>on</strong>s that<br />

they would encounter. Progressi<strong>on</strong> through these examples<br />

was subject paced.<br />

The letter task simply required participants to indicate<br />

with a butt<strong>on</strong>-press whether or not a particular<br />

letter was present in the target word. The rhyme task<br />

required participants to indicate whether or not the<br />

target rhymed with a cue word presented in the questi<strong>on</strong><br />

(e.g., Does it rhyme with course? for the target<br />

word horse). Questi<strong>on</strong>s in the semantic task asked<br />

participants to make judgments <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantic relati<strong>on</strong>ship<br />

(e.g., Is it semantically related to vegetable? for<br />

the word carrot). Rhyme cue words were selected<br />

to ensure that participants could not rely solely <strong>on</strong><br />

orthographic informati<strong>on</strong> to make their judgment <strong>and</strong><br />

semantic cues were selected from associati<strong>on</strong> norms<br />

as str<strong>on</strong>g producti<strong>on</strong> associates <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the target word.<br />

Equal numbers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> “yes” <strong>and</strong> “no” resp<strong>on</strong>ses were required<br />

in each encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. In both experiments,<br />

participants resp<strong>on</strong>ded to a r<strong>and</strong>omized ordering<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> three encoding tasks. Following the examples,<br />

ten practice trials were presented. Before trials c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the words “NEXT TRIAL” in red lettering<br />

in the center <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the screen for 500ms, a white screen<br />

for 500ms, the encoding questi<strong>on</strong> for 1500ms, a<br />

white screen for 500ms, the item for 1500ms <strong>and</strong> a<br />

blank screen for 1000ms. In the after trials the order<br />

for the questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the item were reversed. Participants<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ded “yes” or “no” to the processing<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> by pressing assigned keys. On completi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the practice trials, participants initiated the target<br />

trials by pressing the space bar. With the excepti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two initial buffer items, the order <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> items within blocks c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an individually<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

6 Ben R. Newell & Sally Andrews<br />

r<strong>and</strong>omized ordering <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> three encoding tasks (letter,<br />

rhyme, <strong>and</strong> semantic).<br />

Participants were then given the appropriate test<br />

list. For Experiment 1A this c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whole<br />

words (targets <strong>and</strong> distracters). Participants were instructed<br />

to circle words they had seen in the encoding<br />

task. In Experiment 1B, the test list c<strong>on</strong>tained 3-<br />

letter stems <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> targets <strong>and</strong> distracter words. To disguise<br />

the implicit memory dem<strong>and</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the stem completi<strong>on</strong><br />

task, participants completed a 5 minute filler<br />

stem-completi<strong>on</strong> task immediately after encoding<br />

which c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> three letter stems <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the names <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

48 cities in the world (e.g., SYD___) that participants<br />

were told to complete with as many city names<br />

as possible. The test list was then distributed <strong>and</strong><br />

participants were instructed to complete the word<br />

stems with the first word that came to mind (Experiment<br />

1B).<br />

Results <strong>and</strong> Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Table 1 (upper panel) displays the mean proporti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words recognized for all c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experiment<br />

1A. Unless otherwise indicated, the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> significance<br />

for all tests reported was set at the .05 level.<br />

A 3(Encoding Task: letter, rhyme, semantic) ¥<br />

2(Questi<strong>on</strong> Positi<strong>on</strong>: before, after) mixed ANOVA<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> the corrected recogniti<strong>on</strong> scores (HitsÐ<br />

False Alarms), revealed a main effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding<br />

task dem<strong>on</strong>strating the st<strong>and</strong>ard LOP effect, F(2,<br />

52) = 39.52). There was no main effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong><br />

positi<strong>on</strong>, F(1, 26) = 1.82), but the effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding<br />

task interacted with questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>, F(2, 52) =<br />

3.84, because the memory for letter encoded items<br />

improved in the questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> relative to<br />

the questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. In the before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

performance following the rhyme task was better<br />

than in the letter task, t(13) = 2.97), whereas in<br />

the after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> there was no difference between<br />

performance following letter or rhyme encoding,<br />

t(13) = .168. This pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> data replicates Moeser’s<br />

(1983) <strong>and</strong> Craik’s (1977) findings regarding the ef-<br />

fects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the<br />

claim that participants in questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

truncate their processing in letter identificati<strong>on</strong> encoding<br />

tasks.<br />

The mean proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stems completed with the<br />

target word in each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experiment 1B is<br />

presented in the bottom panel <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Table 1. The completi<strong>on</strong><br />

rates for all encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were significantly<br />

higher than when the same items appeared<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly as distracter stems in the test task (before<br />

targets: t(20) = 4.39, 3.80, 4.87 for the letter, rhyme,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Semantic tasks respectively; after targets: t(20) =<br />

4.22, 4.81, 3.83) dem<strong>on</strong>strating that significant priming<br />

was observed following all encoding tasks.<br />

An ANOVA analysis directly paralleling that c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

<strong>on</strong> the data for Experiment 1A revealed no<br />

main effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> either encoding task, F(2, 80) = .118,<br />

or questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>, F(1, 40) = .046). Further, c<strong>on</strong>trary<br />

to the pattern predicted by the lexical processing<br />

hypothesis, the interacti<strong>on</strong> between questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> encoding task was not significant, F(2,<br />

80) = .621. There was no suggesti<strong>on</strong> that LOP effects<br />

were eliminated in the after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> results for the recogniti<strong>on</strong> memory<br />

task <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experiment 1A is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the view<br />

that lexical processing is truncated in the before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

(Craik, 1977; Moeser, 1983). The rhyme <strong>and</strong><br />

semantic judgment tasks both required processing to<br />

at least the lexical level because rhyming pairs were<br />

orthographically different (e.g., horse, course) but<br />

letter detecti<strong>on</strong> can, in principle, be performed without<br />

retrieving the lexical code for the target item.<br />

The memory improvement for letter encoded items<br />

in the after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> is compatible with the view that<br />

maintaining the word in memory encourages lexical<br />

processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a depth similar to that required for the<br />

rhyme judgment task.<br />

However, the results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the stem completi<strong>on</strong> task<br />

did not show the interacti<strong>on</strong> between LOP <strong>and</strong> questi<strong>on</strong><br />

positi<strong>on</strong> that was predicted from the lexical processing<br />

hypothesis. Despite significant priming for<br />

words from all encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, there was no<br />

evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an LOP effect in the st<strong>and</strong>ard questi<strong>on</strong><br />

before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> no modulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> priming by<br />

Table 1. Mean Proporti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Words Recognized in Experiment 1A <strong>and</strong> Mean Proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Stems Completed<br />

in Experiment 1B as a Functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Encoding Task <strong>and</strong> Test Type<br />

Experiment 1A Letter Rhyme Semantic Unstudied<br />

Recogniti<strong>on</strong> Before .35 .55 .75 .05<br />

After .51 .50 .77 .03<br />

Experiment 1B<br />

Stem Before .36 .34 .36 .18<br />

Completi<strong>on</strong> After .35 .36 .33 .17<br />

Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

LOP <strong>and</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong> Positi<strong>on</strong><br />

7<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>. This failure to find a LOP effect<br />

<strong>on</strong> implicit memory performance is, in itself, not unusual.<br />

Brown <strong>and</strong> Mitchell’s (1994) meta-analysis<br />

showed that semantic encoding tasks yielded numerically<br />

larger priming effects in a majority <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the 38<br />

studies investigating LOP effects <strong>on</strong> perceptual implicit<br />

memory tasks, but most were small in absolute<br />

magnitude <strong>and</strong> they were based <strong>on</strong> a range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>semantic<br />

encoding tasks. Notably, at least 15 explicitly<br />

required ph<strong>on</strong>ological judgments (e.g., vowel comparis<strong>on</strong>,<br />

syllable judgment) <strong>and</strong> others, such as c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant-vowel<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong>, might also have encouraged<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ological encoding. Both the present data<br />

<strong>and</strong> those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner<br />

(1998) show that ph<strong>on</strong>ological encoding tasks yield<br />

deeper processing than graphemic or letter-based<br />

shallow tasks. The five studies in Brown <strong>and</strong> Mitchell’s<br />

(1994) review that used the letter detecti<strong>on</strong> encoding<br />

task <strong>and</strong> stem completi<strong>on</strong> memory test used<br />

in Experiment 1B showed n<strong>on</strong>significant LOP effects<br />

ranging from .02 to .04 (Jacoby & Dallas, 1981;<br />

Java & Gardiner, 1991; Naito, 1990; Park & Shaw,<br />

1992), c<strong>on</strong>sistent with our results. What is surprising,<br />

however, is the lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an effect <strong>on</strong> priming<br />

given the apparent increase in the extent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical<br />

processing observed in explicit memory performance<br />

for the questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Experiment 2<br />

Experiment 2 was designed to provide more refined<br />

evidence about whether questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> modulates<br />

LOP effects in explicit <strong>and</strong> implicit tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> memory.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to increasing the number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> participants<br />

(to increase power) <strong>and</strong> testing all participants in the<br />

same locati<strong>on</strong> under identical encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

three major modificati<strong>on</strong>s were made to the design<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experiment 1.<br />

First, a cued stem-recall test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicit memory<br />

was substituted for the recogniti<strong>on</strong> memory test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Experiment 1A to achieve greater equivalence between<br />

the procedures used to assess explicit <strong>and</strong> implicit<br />

memory performance. A direct comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

this test with the implicit stem completi<strong>on</strong> test allows<br />

the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> identical test protocols for both memory<br />

measures so that the <strong>on</strong>ly difference between the two<br />

tests is the instructi<strong>on</strong>s given to participants: in the<br />

implicit stem completi<strong>on</strong> task they were instructed to<br />

complete the 3-letter stems with the first word they<br />

thought <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> while in the cued stem recall test they<br />

were told to complete the stems with words from the<br />

study task. Using identical physical cues in the two<br />

tests has been argued to equate the informati<strong>on</strong> available<br />

for voluntary retrieval strategies to operate <strong>on</strong><br />

across the two types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> memory test (Schacter et al.<br />

1989).<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>dly, we introduced an additi<strong>on</strong>al test c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

an exclusi<strong>on</strong> stem completi<strong>on</strong> task, to provide<br />

more fine-grained evidence about the basis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> any<br />

observed effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>. Exclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

tasks instruct participants to complete stems with<br />

words that were not part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the study list in order to<br />

attempt to distinguish familiarity from recollecti<strong>on</strong>based<br />

memories (Jacoby, 1991). Under such instructi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> higher completi<strong>on</strong> rates for studied<br />

than unstudied words, implies that the studied items<br />

are familiar but not able to be explicitly recollected.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>versely, completi<strong>on</strong> rates for studied items that<br />

are significantly lower than those for unstudied items<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strate successful exclusi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studied items<br />

<strong>and</strong> imply that participants could explicitly recollect<br />

the study episode. The data for this task will provide<br />

insight into the locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> effect.<br />

If the truncated processing that appears to characterize<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard questi<strong>on</strong> before LOP paradigms reduces<br />

the degree to which items accrue familiarity as a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> study encoding, then exclusi<strong>on</strong> stemcompleti<strong>on</strong><br />

rates for items from the before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

should be lower for items processed in shallow encoding<br />

tasks <strong>and</strong> will increase with the depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing.<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the exclusi<strong>on</strong> stem-completi<strong>on</strong><br />

data for before <strong>and</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s will shed<br />

light <strong>on</strong> the nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the additi<strong>on</strong>al processing that<br />

participants engage when they must maintain the<br />

word in memory until the questi<strong>on</strong> is presented. If<br />

these processes increase the recollectability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> items,<br />

exclusi<strong>on</strong> stem-completi<strong>on</strong> rates will be lower<br />

following questi<strong>on</strong> after than questi<strong>on</strong> before encoding<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

The third modificati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cerned the graphemic<br />

encoding task. A possible explanati<strong>on</strong> for the absence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP effects in the st<strong>and</strong>ard before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the stem-completi<strong>on</strong> data in Experiment 1B is that<br />

our shallow task (letter detecti<strong>on</strong>) was not shallow<br />

enough to be sensitive to the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> truncated lexical<br />

processing <strong>on</strong> stem completi<strong>on</strong> performance. Experiment<br />

2 used a low-level feature task similar to<br />

Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner’s graphemic task.<br />

The task required participants to make judgments<br />

about the numbers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> enclosed spaces, or numbers<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ascending <strong>and</strong> descending letters in the target<br />

words. If feature tasks encourage greater truncati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing than letter tasks, there should<br />

be a LOP effect <strong>on</strong> implicit priming in the st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. On the assumpti<strong>on</strong> that<br />

maintaining the word in memory requires that it is<br />

lexically processed, the LOP effect should be eliminated<br />

in the questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

8 Ben R. Newell & Sally Andrews<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Two hundred <strong>and</strong> eight psychology students from the<br />

University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sydney participated in the experiment<br />

as part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a practical class. Seventy-two participants<br />

performed a cued stem-recall task <strong>and</strong> 72 performed<br />

a stem-completi<strong>on</strong> task. A further 64 participants<br />

performed the exclusi<strong>on</strong> stem-completi<strong>on</strong> task. For<br />

all tasks participants were evenly divided across the<br />

two questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Materials <strong>and</strong> Procedure<br />

The materials <strong>and</strong> procedure for the encoding task<br />

were identical to those used in Experiment 1A <strong>and</strong><br />

1B, except that the ph<strong>on</strong>ological task was replaced<br />

by a feature encoding task. This latter task required<br />

subjects to make YES/NO resp<strong>on</strong>ses to questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

such as “Does the word c<strong>on</strong>tain letters with enclosed<br />

spaces?” or “Does the word c<strong>on</strong>tain ascending letters?”<br />

Participants in all test c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s completed the<br />

filler city stem completi<strong>on</strong> task used in Experiment<br />

1B for 5 minutes between encoding <strong>and</strong> test. The test<br />

sheets were identical in all 3 c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, c<strong>on</strong>sisting<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the stems <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 36 target words <strong>and</strong> 12 distracter<br />

words, but the stems falling into each encoding <strong>and</strong><br />

distracter c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> varied according to the counterbalanced<br />

encoding list participants had been presented<br />

with. Instructi<strong>on</strong>s appropriate to the cued<br />

stem recall, stem completi<strong>on</strong> or exclusi<strong>on</strong> stem completi<strong>on</strong><br />

test were printed <strong>on</strong> the top <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the test sheet.<br />

Results <strong>and</strong> Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Table 2 displays the mean proporti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stems completed<br />

with target words for all c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experiment<br />

2. Significant priming was observed in all c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the stem completi<strong>on</strong> task (before targets:<br />

t(35) = 4.31, 3.81, 3.63 for the feature, letter, <strong>and</strong><br />

semantic tasks respectively; after targets: t(35) =<br />

2.99, 2.42, 3.64). Significant differences between<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> rates for studied <strong>and</strong> unstudied words<br />

were also observed in all but the letter/before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the exclusi<strong>on</strong> task (before targets: t(31) =<br />

3.36, 1.25, Ð4.17 for the feature, letter, <strong>and</strong> semantic<br />

tasks respectively; after targets, ts (31) = -2.92, Ð<br />

2.52, Ð4.56). (A positive t value indicates that the<br />

proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studied words completed was higher<br />

than the proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> unstudied words, <strong>and</strong> a negative<br />

t value indicates that the proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studied<br />

words completed was lower.) The mean proporti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studied words completed was significantly higher<br />

than the mean proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> unstudied words completed<br />

in all c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the cued stem recall task.<br />

Analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the cued stem-recall data showed the<br />

expected main effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding task, F(2, 70) =<br />

22.21, with performance improving as the depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the task increased; <strong>and</strong> a main effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

F(1, 70) = 5.76), indicating improved performance<br />

when the target appeared after the questi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

This main effect was modified by a significant interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

between qesti<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> encoding<br />

task, F(2, 70) = 5.07, indicating that an improvement<br />

was <strong>on</strong>ly observed in the two shallow encoding tasks.<br />

Similar analyses <strong>on</strong> the priming effects for the<br />

stem completi<strong>on</strong> task showed a pattern identical with<br />

that found in Experiment 1B Ð no main effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

encoding task, F(2, 70) = .503, or <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

F(1, 70) = .273, <strong>and</strong> no significant interacti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

F(2, 70) = .394. This identical pattern suggests that<br />

it is not a difference in the depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> our shallow task<br />

that is resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the discrepancy between our<br />

results <strong>and</strong> those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner<br />

(1998).<br />

Analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the exclusi<strong>on</strong> task data showed a<br />

pattern identical with that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the cued stem recall<br />

task Ð main effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding task, F(2, 62) =<br />

15.98, <strong>and</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>, F(1, 62) = 10.32, <strong>and</strong><br />

a significant interacti<strong>on</strong> between the two variables,<br />

F(2, 62) = 7.44. In this case, the interacti<strong>on</strong> occurred<br />

because participants were significantly less likely to<br />

complete stems with target words when they had<br />

been studied than when they had not, suggesting that<br />

they could explicitly recollect, <strong>and</strong> therefore exclude<br />

Table 2. Mean Proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Stems Completed as a Functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Encoding Task <strong>and</strong> Test Type in Experiment 2<br />

Feature Letter Semantic Unstudied<br />

Cued Stem Recall<br />

Stem Completi<strong>on</strong><br />

Exclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

Before .22 .23 .50 .10<br />

After .35 .39 .52 .10<br />

Before .35 .34 .35 .24<br />

After .30 .28 .32 .21<br />

Before .17 .13 .04 .10<br />

After .06 .07 .04 .11<br />

Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

LOP <strong>and</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong> Positi<strong>on</strong><br />

9<br />

studied items. This was also true for semantically<br />

processed items from the questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

but not for items from shallow encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Instead, feature encoding dem<strong>on</strong>strated the significantly<br />

higher completi<strong>on</strong> rates for studied items that<br />

indicate reliance <strong>on</strong> familiarity in the absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicit<br />

recollecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Thus, the data for Experiment 2 parallel those for<br />

Experiments 1A <strong>and</strong> 1B. The questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> manipulati<strong>on</strong><br />

had clear effects <strong>on</strong> both cued stem recall<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong> performance in the exclusi<strong>on</strong> task. The<br />

pattern also suggests that a lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> power was not<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>sible for our findings in Experiment 1B. Experiment<br />

2 used almost twice the number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> participants<br />

(36 compared to 21) in each level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the questi<strong>on</strong><br />

positi<strong>on</strong> manipulati<strong>on</strong>, but the pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> results<br />

was the same. The fact that significant LOP <strong>and</strong><br />

questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> effects were observed in the exclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

task, in which the magnitude <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the overall<br />

priming effect was <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the same magnitude as that<br />

observed in stem completi<strong>on</strong>, provides further evidence<br />

that the absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these effects for the latter<br />

task is not due to a lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> power.<br />

The data from Experiment 2 are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with<br />

the view that shallow LOP tasks lead to truncati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing, <strong>and</strong> that such truncati<strong>on</strong> is prevented<br />

when participants must maintain the word in<br />

memory until the questi<strong>on</strong> is presented. In the explicit<br />

cued-recall task, this was dem<strong>on</strong>strated by better<br />

memory for feature <strong>and</strong> letter <strong>and</strong> encoded words<br />

in the questi<strong>on</strong> after than the questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> did not influence performance<br />

for semantically encoded items c<strong>on</strong>sistent with<br />

the fact that semantic judgments require processing<br />

to at least the lexical level even when the questi<strong>on</strong> is<br />

known before the item is presented (Craik, 1977).<br />

The exclusi<strong>on</strong> data c<strong>on</strong>firm that feature <strong>and</strong> letter<br />

encoded items were processed more deeply in the<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> because participants successfully<br />

avoided completing their stems with target<br />

words (i.e., completi<strong>on</strong> rates were significantly lower<br />

for studied than n<strong>on</strong>studied words) suggesting that<br />

they could recollect the target words from the study<br />

phase. By c<strong>on</strong>trast, items from the shallow encoding<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were not successfully excluded<br />

implying that they had accrued familiarity<br />

during encoding but could not be recollected. Paralleling<br />

the cued-recall data, semantically encoded<br />

items were completely insensitive to the questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong><br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>firming that questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong>ly influences performance for encoding tasks<br />

that can be performed without full lexical processing<br />

when the encoding questi<strong>on</strong> is known by the time the<br />

target is presented.<br />

Despite these clear indicati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the influence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP <strong>on</strong> explicit<br />

memory performance, there is no evidence that this<br />

truncati<strong>on</strong> influenced priming effects in the stem<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> task. Significant priming was observed<br />

following all encoding tasks but the magnitude <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

priming effect was not influenced by either the type<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding task or questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

General Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Three possible sources <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP effects <strong>on</strong> implicit<br />

memory tasks have been proposed. Two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these are<br />

epiphenomenal to the intended task Ð the influence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> voluntary retrieval strategies (e.g., Bowers &<br />

Schacter, 1990) <strong>and</strong> the influence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>ceptually<br />

driven retrieval processes (e.g., Mulligan, Guyer, &<br />

Bel<strong>and</strong>, 1999). The third is inherent to the task Ð the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing. Carefully c<strong>on</strong>trolled<br />

studies dem<strong>on</strong>strating LOP effects in the absence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> influences from the first <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these two<br />

sources has led some researchers to favor the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical-processing as an explanati<strong>on</strong> for the<br />

effects because some shallow processing tasks allow<br />

participants to truncate processing below the lexical<br />

level. Most explicitly, Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner’s<br />

(1998) finding that LOP effects <strong>on</strong> stem completi<strong>on</strong><br />

performance were eliminated when participants<br />

were required to make a lexical classificati<strong>on</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se following graphemic encoding led them to<br />

c<strong>on</strong>clude that “a lexical processing deficit during<br />

shallow study processing is <strong>on</strong>e factor that must be<br />

seriously c<strong>on</strong>sidered when interpreting depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing<br />

effects” (p.605). The present experiments<br />

sought further evidence for this lexical-processing<br />

hypothesis by using another manipulati<strong>on</strong> hypothesized<br />

to influence the extent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing<br />

engaged in during shallow study processing Ð<br />

namely the relative positi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the encoding questi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> the target word (Challis & Brodbeck, 1992).<br />

The pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> data for the explicit tasks in Experiments<br />

1A <strong>and</strong> 2 is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the assumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

that shallow encoding tasks like letter detecti<strong>on</strong> or<br />

feature-identificati<strong>on</strong> allow truncati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing,<br />

but that such truncati<strong>on</strong> is prevented when<br />

items must be maintained in memory until the encoding<br />

operati<strong>on</strong> is known (Challis & Brodbeck,<br />

1992; Moeser, 1983; Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn & Gardiner,<br />

1998). However, in c<strong>on</strong>tradicti<strong>on</strong> to the predicti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the lexical processing hypothesis, there was<br />

no evidence that the differential lexical processing<br />

associated with the questi<strong>on</strong> after manipulati<strong>on</strong> affected<br />

priming in implicit stem completi<strong>on</strong> tasks.<br />

Significant priming was, however, observed for all<br />

encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s indicating that the task was sufficiently<br />

sensitive to detect implicit memory effects.<br />

A possible explanati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this apparent c<strong>on</strong>tradicti<strong>on</strong><br />

is that <strong>on</strong>ly certain types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> shallow processing<br />

allow or encourage truncati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing.<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

10 Ben R. Newell & Sally Andrews<br />

Thus, the absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> significant effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LOP or<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the stem completi<strong>on</strong> data could<br />

be interpreted as dem<strong>on</strong>strating that all encoding<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s led to lexical processing even in the st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, so there was no truncati<strong>on</strong><br />

to be eliminated by maintaining the item in<br />

memory. The exclusi<strong>on</strong> data from Experiment 2 appear<br />

to c<strong>on</strong>tradict this interpretati<strong>on</strong>. These data<br />

clearly c<strong>on</strong>firm that questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> influenced the<br />

depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the item as reflected in participants’<br />

ability to explicitly recollect <strong>and</strong> exclude<br />

studied items. Further, they show that the impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

this differential processing was greatest for shallower<br />

processing tasks Ð the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exclusi<strong>on</strong> performance<br />

for semantically encoded items was completely<br />

unaffected by questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> suggesting<br />

that the additi<strong>on</strong>al processing c<strong>on</strong>ducted in the questi<strong>on</strong><br />

after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> overlaps with that required in<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard deep encoding tasks.<br />

More importantly, the data for the exclusi<strong>on</strong> task<br />

c<strong>on</strong>firm that shallow processing in the st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> is associated with poor<br />

recollecti<strong>on</strong> but enhanced familiarity, particularly<br />

following feature-based encoding tasks. This is c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with truncati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> some form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing<br />

that is important for effective recollecti<strong>on</strong> but irrelevant<br />

to the increment in familiarity that occurs for<br />

studied items. Identifying the high completi<strong>on</strong> rates<br />

observed in these c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s with familiarity might<br />

be seen as incompatible with the widely accepted<br />

view that perceptual implicit memory is due to familiarity<br />

(e.g., Jacoby & Dallas, 1981): if the words presented<br />

in shallow questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s are<br />

more familiar than unstudied words, as dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

by the high completi<strong>on</strong> rates in the exclusi<strong>on</strong> task,<br />

why d<strong>on</strong>’t these items show differential priming in<br />

the implicit stem completi<strong>on</strong> task? The answer to this<br />

apparent c<strong>on</strong>tradicti<strong>on</strong> lies in the fact that exclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

performance is a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> both familiarity <strong>and</strong> recollecti<strong>on</strong><br />

(Jacoby, 1991) Ð participants are told that,<br />

if they can recollect seeing a word in the encoding<br />

task, they should not provide it as a completi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Thus, familiarity influences the probability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

item being activated by the stem, but higher levels<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> recollecti<strong>on</strong> allow participants to exclude familiar<br />

words. The differential exclusi<strong>on</strong> rates as a functi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> are therefore perfectly compatible<br />

with the assumpti<strong>on</strong> that all items accrue<br />

equivalent familiarity during encoding, but that both<br />

LOP <strong>and</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> influence the recollectability<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the words <strong>and</strong> therefore the probability<br />

that target words activated by the stem are successfully<br />

excluded.<br />

Thus, in combinati<strong>on</strong>, the data from the cued<br />

stem recall <strong>and</strong> exclusi<strong>on</strong> tasks show clear <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

evidence that presenting the questi<strong>on</strong> after the<br />

word led participants to process the item more<br />

Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13<br />

deeply <strong>and</strong> eliminated the truncati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing<br />

that apparently characterizes shallow processing<br />

tasks in the st<strong>and</strong>ard questi<strong>on</strong> before LOP paradigm.<br />

The exclusi<strong>on</strong> data also show that st<strong>and</strong>ard questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

before shallow encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s led to an increment<br />

in familiarity, suggesting that the processes<br />

that were truncated in these c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s do not c<strong>on</strong>tribute<br />

to familiarity-based effects <strong>on</strong> stem completi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

This c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the absence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> effects <strong>on</strong> implicit memory performance<br />

<strong>and</strong> implies that completi<strong>on</strong> rates in this<br />

task were primarily a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiarity <strong>and</strong><br />

were unaffected by the differences in recollecti<strong>on</strong><br />

that clearly influenced both explicit memory <strong>and</strong> exclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

performance. Thus, presenting the questi<strong>on</strong><br />

before the target word does appear to lead to a truncati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing, but whatever is truncated is not<br />

relevant to priming effects in stem completi<strong>on</strong> performance.<br />

The complete absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> any LOP effect <strong>on</strong> implicit<br />

stem completi<strong>on</strong> performance in both experiments<br />

is both an advantage <strong>and</strong> a disadvantage. On<br />

the <strong>on</strong>e h<strong>and</strong>, the lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an LOP effect implies that<br />

we have successfully disguised the memory dem<strong>and</strong>s<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the task to avoid c<strong>on</strong>taminati<strong>on</strong> from voluntary<br />

retrieval. This is most clearly dem<strong>on</strong>strated by the<br />

absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> any advantage for semantically encoded<br />

items in the stem-completi<strong>on</strong> data <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> either experiment.<br />

This form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding had very robust effects<br />

<strong>on</strong> explicit stem completi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> also increased<br />

participants’ success in excluding studied words c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with enhanced recollecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these items presumably<br />

reflecting their deeper c<strong>on</strong>ceptual processing.<br />

There was, however, no hint <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an advantage for<br />

the semantic encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> in the stem completi<strong>on</strong><br />

task indicating that it was unc<strong>on</strong>taminated by<br />

voluntary recollecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> insensitive to c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

processing at encoding.<br />

The disadvantage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> finding no LOP effect <strong>on</strong><br />

implicit memory performance is that we have been<br />

unable to directly test whether manipulating questi<strong>on</strong><br />

positi<strong>on</strong> can eliminate an LOP effect <strong>on</strong> implicit<br />

memory Ð as the lexical processing hypothesis implies<br />

Ð because we did not find any LOP effect in<br />

the st<strong>and</strong>ard questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

It is possible that something about the procedure<br />

we used is resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the null-pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> results<br />

in the stem-completi<strong>on</strong> tasks. For example, perhaps<br />

the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a within-participants manipulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding<br />

task was insufficient for inducing a “taskmindset”<br />

that might develop from using a betweenparticipants<br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong>, which in turn might accentuate<br />

the likelihood <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> obtaining effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Although such an effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> between<br />

versus within participant designs has been suggested<br />

(cf., Roediger & McDermott, 1993), the evidence is<br />

equivocal. Brown <strong>and</strong> Mitchell (1994) compared the<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

LOP <strong>and</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong> Positi<strong>on</strong><br />

11<br />

proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> significant LOP effects in studies using<br />

both within <strong>and</strong> between participant designs <strong>and</strong><br />

found a numerically (but not significantly) higher<br />

proporti<strong>on</strong> (84 %) in within-participant designs than<br />

in between-participant designs (72 %). They c<strong>on</strong>cluded<br />

that the type <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> design did not have a significant<br />

impact <strong>on</strong> the likelihood <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> obtaining LOP effects.<br />

Furthermore, as previously noted (in Footnote 1),<br />

we have c<strong>on</strong>ducted a number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> other experiments<br />

using the same items <strong>and</strong> encoding c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. These<br />

experiment have employed both within <strong>and</strong> betweenparticipant<br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding task <strong>and</strong> questi<strong>on</strong><br />

positi<strong>on</strong>, have used fragment completi<strong>on</strong> as well<br />

as stem-completi<strong>on</strong> tasks, <strong>and</strong> have all used large<br />

sample sizes <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the order used in Experiment 2. In<br />

all these experiments we repeatedly found the same<br />

pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> results Ð effects c<strong>on</strong>sistent with truncati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> explicit measures, <strong>and</strong> no effect <strong>on</strong> implicit measures.<br />

It therefore seems unlikely to us that procedural<br />

issues are driving the null pattern. It is also<br />

important to emphasize that the absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an LOP<br />

effect <strong>on</strong> stem completi<strong>on</strong> performance is not, in<br />

itself, surprising. Most <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the effects summarized in<br />

Brown <strong>and</strong> Mitchell’s (1994) meta-analysis were<br />

numerically small <strong>and</strong> many were not individually<br />

significant.<br />

Given the absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an LOP effect <strong>on</strong> implicit<br />

memory in the questi<strong>on</strong> before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, we obviously<br />

cannot directly evaluate whether this effect is<br />

modulated by questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> as predicted by the<br />

lexical processing hypothesis. Nevertheless, the results<br />

do present a challenge that needs to be addressed<br />

by prop<strong>on</strong>ents <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the basis<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicit priming because they show that priming<br />

effects in implicit stem completi<strong>on</strong> are insensitive to<br />

a manipulati<strong>on</strong> that does clearly prevent truncati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

some form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> deeper processing. Lexical processing<br />

would seem an obvious prerequisite to such processing,<br />

but implicit memory performance was not sensitive<br />

to questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong>. This implies either that the<br />

truncati<strong>on</strong> effects dem<strong>on</strong>strated in the explicit memory<br />

<strong>and</strong> exclusi<strong>on</strong> tasks occurred after lexical processing<br />

or that the form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing required<br />

to yield implicit memory is different to that<br />

required to maintain a word in memory.<br />

Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner (1998) were<br />

careful to qualify their claims about exactly what<br />

form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing mediates priming. They<br />

distinguished between lexical access, that operates<br />

automatically even in shallow study tasks, <strong>and</strong> lexical<br />

processing that occurs in tasks that require “attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

to the study stimuli as individual lexical<br />

units”. They argued that the latter is necessary to<br />

support priming, but acknowledged that it would be<br />

“extremely difficult to resolve whether it is lexical<br />

access or lexical processing that fails during graphe-<br />

mic study tasks” (p. 606). If this distincti<strong>on</strong> is accepted,<br />

the present results might be seen as evidence<br />

that priming depends <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> lexical access <strong>and</strong> not<br />

<strong>on</strong> lexical processing: the significant priming observed<br />

following all encoding tasks could be attributed<br />

to lexical access <strong>and</strong> the deeper processing dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

in the questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s to lexical<br />

processing. However, this is a descripti<strong>on</strong> rather than<br />

an explanati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the differential effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong><br />

positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> explicit <strong>and</strong> implicit memory performance.<br />

It also fails to explain why the featural encoding<br />

task <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experiment 2, which was modeled<br />

<strong>on</strong> that used by Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner<br />

(1998), apparently induced the form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical processing<br />

required to yield both priming <strong>and</strong> familiarity-based<br />

completi<strong>on</strong>s in the exclusi<strong>on</strong> task Ð while<br />

Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner’s feature encoding<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> did not.<br />

It is possible that our graphemic task was somewhat<br />

easier than that used by Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn<br />

<strong>and</strong> Gardiner (1998). To equate the binary classificati<strong>on</strong><br />

requirements across the three LOP c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> to allow different questi<strong>on</strong>s to be asked for each<br />

item so that participants in the questi<strong>on</strong> after c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

could not predict the encoding operati<strong>on</strong>, we required<br />

a decisi<strong>on</strong> as to whether a particular feature<br />

(enclosed space, ascender letter etc) was present or<br />

absent in the stimulus, while their task required participants<br />

to count the number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> enclosed spaces in<br />

each item. Perhaps the latter more difficult task requires<br />

participants to suppress lexical codes in order<br />

to allow attenti<strong>on</strong> to the featural level.<br />

Support for this possibility derives from recent<br />

evidence that words presented in a color-naming<br />

Stroop task yield smaller priming effects <strong>on</strong> subsequent<br />

word fragment <strong>and</strong> stem completi<strong>on</strong> tests than<br />

when the same words are presented in a word naming<br />

task (Rajaram, Srinivas, & Travers, 2001). Interference<br />

effects <strong>on</strong> color naming dem<strong>on</strong>strated that lexical<br />

access had occurred in the Stroop c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, so<br />

Rajaram et al. (2001) argued that the results showed<br />

that “perceptual implicit memory depends <strong>on</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

requirements bey<strong>on</strong>d those needed for simple<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong>” (p. 926). <strong>Tasks</strong> that simply divert attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

away from the target word without preventing<br />

its identificati<strong>on</strong> do not reduce priming (e.g., Mulligan<br />

& Hartman, 1996; MacD<strong>on</strong>ald & MacLeod,<br />

1998), so Rajaram et al. attributed the reduced priming<br />

following Stroop color naming to the inhibiti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the word representati<strong>on</strong> that is required to select<br />

the ink color. That is, word identificati<strong>on</strong> is sufficient<br />

to yield priming regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whether the word is<br />

the focus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong>, but task dem<strong>and</strong>s that require<br />

inhibiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the word representati<strong>on</strong> reduce priming.<br />

Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner (1998) do not<br />

specify whether the deficit in lexical processing that<br />

they argue to be resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the reduced priming<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

12 Ben R. Newell & Sally Andrews<br />

in their graphemic c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> is a result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> truncati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing or an inhibiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the formati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<br />

lexical representati<strong>on</strong>. An explanati<strong>on</strong> framed in<br />

terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inhibiti<strong>on</strong> or suppressi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical codes,<br />

rather than truncated processing, may provide a way<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> rec<strong>on</strong>ciling the results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the present experiments<br />

with those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner<br />

(1998) while maintaining the proposal that lexical<br />

processing is necessary for priming <strong>on</strong> perceptual<br />

tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicit memory. The automatic lexical retrieval<br />

that Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn <strong>and</strong> Gardiner label<br />

as lexical access may occur even in very shallow<br />

feature-based tasks like that we used in Experiment<br />

2 Ð <strong>and</strong> support implicit priming effects. Deficits in<br />

lexical processing that give rise to reduced priming<br />

for shallow tasks, <strong>and</strong> therefore an LOP effect <strong>on</strong><br />

implicit memory, may <strong>on</strong>ly occur when the encoding<br />

task requires suppressi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these automatically activated<br />

lexical codes.<br />

In c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>, we believe that the questi<strong>on</strong> positi<strong>on</strong><br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong> is an interesting <strong>and</strong> novel procedure<br />

for investigating LOP effects <strong>on</strong> implicit<br />

memory tests. The results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the current experiments<br />

are not completely c<strong>on</strong>clusive regarding the role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

lexical processing in producing LOP effects, but they<br />

do raise important questi<strong>on</strong>s about the generality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the lexical processing hypothesis. Further evaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the questi<strong>on</strong>-positi<strong>on</strong> manipulati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

perhaps in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with other potentially more<br />

sensitive <strong>and</strong> reliable implicit memory measures<br />

(e.g., word <strong>and</strong> picture identificati<strong>on</strong>, see Buchner &<br />

Wippich, 2000), will lead to a greater underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the processes underlying the involuntary retrieval<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> perceptual informati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

References<br />

Andrews, S. (1982). Ph<strong>on</strong>ological encoding: Is the regularity<br />

effect c<strong>on</strong>sistent? <strong>Memory</strong> & Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 10, 565Ð<br />

575.<br />

Blaxt<strong>on</strong>, T. A. (1989). Investigating dissociati<strong>on</strong>s am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

memory measures: Support for a transfer-appropriate<br />

processing framework. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental<br />

Psychology: Learning, <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>and</strong> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 15,<br />

657Ð668.<br />

Bowers, J., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). <strong>Implicit</strong> memory <strong>and</strong><br />

test awareness. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology:<br />

Learning, <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>and</strong> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 16, 404Ð416.<br />

Brown, A. S., & Mitchell, D. B. (1994). A reevaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantic versus n<strong>on</strong>semantic processing in implicit<br />

memory. <strong>Memory</strong> & Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 22, 533Ð541.<br />

Buchner, A., & Wippich, W. (2000). On the reliability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

implicit <strong>and</strong> explicit measures. Cognitive Psychology,<br />

40, 227Ð259.<br />

Challis, B. H., & Brodbeck, D. R. (1992). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Levels</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing<br />

affects priming in word fragment completi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Learning, <strong>Memory</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 18, 595Ð607.<br />

Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13<br />

Craik, F. I. M (1977). Depth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing in recall <strong>and</strong><br />

recogniti<strong>on</strong>. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Performance<br />

VI. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Levels</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing:<br />

A framework for memory research. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Verbal learning <strong>and</strong> Verbal Behaviour, 11, 671Ð684.<br />

Draine, S. (1996). Inquisit (Versi<strong>on</strong> Build 1.28): Millisec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

S<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>tware.<br />

Dunn, J. C., & Kirsner, K. (1988). Discovering functi<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

independent mental processes: The principle <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reversed<br />

associati<strong>on</strong>. Psychological Review, 95, 91Ð101.<br />

Foster, J. K., & Jelicic, M. (Eds.) (1999). <strong>Memory</strong>: Systems,<br />

process or functi<strong>on</strong>. Oxford: Oxford University<br />

Press.<br />

Graf, P., & M<strong>and</strong>ler, G. (1984). Activati<strong>on</strong> makes words<br />

more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Verbal Learning <strong>and</strong> Verbal Behaviour, 23,<br />

553Ð568.<br />

Hamman, S. B. (1990). Level-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing effects in c<strong>on</strong>ceptually<br />

driven implicit tasks. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental<br />

Psychology: Learning, memory <strong>and</strong> cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 16,<br />

970Ð977.<br />

Hayman, C. G., & Jacoby, L. L. (1989). Specific word<br />

transfer as a measure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing in the word-superiority<br />

paradigm. <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>and</strong> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 17(2), 125Ð<br />

133.<br />

Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociati<strong>on</strong> framework:<br />

Separating automatic from intenti<strong>on</strong>al uses <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> memory.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>and</strong> Language, 30, 513Ð541.<br />

Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relati<strong>on</strong>ship<br />

between autobiographical memory <strong>and</strong> perceptual<br />

learning. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology General,<br />

110, 306Ð340.<br />

Java, R. I., & Gardiner, J. M. (1991). Priming <strong>and</strong> aging:<br />

Further evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> preserved memory functi<strong>on</strong>. American<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 104, 89Ð100.<br />

MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, P. A., & MacLeod, C. M. (1998). The influence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> at encoding <strong>on</strong> direct <strong>and</strong> indirect remembering.<br />

Acta Psychologica, 98, 291Ð310.<br />

M<strong>and</strong>ler, G., Hams<strong>on</strong>, C. O., & Dorfman, J. (1990). Tests<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dual process theory: word priming <strong>and</strong> recogniti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The Quarterly Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology:<br />

Secti<strong>on</strong> A, 42A, 713Ð739.<br />

Moeser, D. (1983). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Levels</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing: Qualitative differences<br />

or task-dem<strong>and</strong> differences? <strong>Memory</strong> & Cogniti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

11, 316Ð323.<br />

Mulligan, N. W., Guyer, P. S., & Bel<strong>and</strong>, A. (1999). The<br />

effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing <strong>and</strong> organizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

implicit memory in the category exemplar producti<strong>on</strong><br />

test. <strong>Memory</strong> & Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 27, 633Ð647.<br />

Mulligan, N. W., & Hartman, M. (1996). Divided attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> indirect memory tests. <strong>Memory</strong> & Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 24,<br />

453Ð465.<br />

Naito, M. (1990). Repetiti<strong>on</strong> priming in children <strong>and</strong><br />

adults: Age related dissociati<strong>on</strong> between implicit <strong>and</strong><br />

explicit memory. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Child<br />

Psychology, 50, 462Ð484.<br />

Park, D. C., & Shaw, R. J. (1992). Effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

support <strong>on</strong> implicit <strong>and</strong> explicit memory in younger<br />

<strong>and</strong> older adults. Psychology & Aging, 7, 632Ð642.<br />

Rajaram, S. & Roediger, H. L., III, (1993). Direct comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four implicit memory tests. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental<br />

Psychology: Learning, <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>and</strong> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

19, 765Ð776.<br />

Rajaram, S., Srinivas, K. & Travers, S. (2001). The effects<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> perceptual implicit memory. <strong>Memory</strong> &<br />

Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 29, 920Ð930.<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers


42059$ $$$4 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Ð EXPPSY 51/02/2004 Ð 1. Bel. Ð 17-10-03 19:34:52 Ð Rev 16.04x<br />

LOP <strong>and</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong> Positi<strong>on</strong><br />

13<br />

Richards<strong>on</strong>-Klavehn, A., & Gardiner, J. M. (1998). Depth<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing<br />

effects <strong>on</strong> priming in stem completi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the voluntary-c<strong>on</strong>taminati<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>ceptual-processing,<br />

<strong>and</strong> lexical-processing hypotheses. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Experimental Psychology: Learning, <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 24, 593Ð609.<br />

Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1993). <strong>Implicit</strong><br />

memory in normal human participants. In F. Boller &<br />

J. Grafman (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Neuropsychology, Vol.<br />

8 (pp. 63Ð131). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.<br />

Roediger, H. L., III, Srinivas, K., & Weld<strong>on</strong>, M. S. (1989).<br />

Dissociati<strong>on</strong>s between implicit measures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> retenti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In S. Lew<strong>and</strong>owsky, J. C. Dunn, & K. Kirsner (Eds.),<br />

<strong>Implicit</strong> memory:Theoretical issues (pp. 67Ð84). Hillsdale,<br />

NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Roediger, H. L., Weld<strong>on</strong>, M. S., Stadler, M. A., & Riegler,<br />

G. L. (1992). Direct comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two implicit memory<br />

tests: Word fragment <strong>and</strong> word stem completi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Learning, <strong>Memory</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 18, 1251Ð1269.<br />

Schacter, D. L., Bowers, J., & Booker J. (1989). Intenti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

awareness <strong>and</strong> implicit memory: the retrieval intenti<strong>on</strong>ality<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong>. In S. Lew<strong>and</strong>owsky, J. C. Dunn, & K.<br />

Kirsner (Eds.), <strong>Implicit</strong> memory: Theoretical issues<br />

(pp. 47Ð65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Squire, L. R. (1994). Declarative <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>declarative memory:<br />

Multiple brain systems supporting learning <strong>and</strong><br />

memory. In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), <strong>Memory</strong><br />

Systems 1994 (pp. 203ºÐ31). MIT Press, Cambridge,<br />

MA.<br />

Thapar, A., & Greene, R. L. (1994). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

processing <strong>on</strong> implicit <strong>and</strong> explicit tasks. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Experimental Psychology: Learning, <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 20, 671Ð679.<br />

Toth, J. P., Reingold, E. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Toward<br />

a redefiniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicit memory: Process dissociati<strong>on</strong><br />

following elaborative processing <strong>and</strong> self-generati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Learning, <strong>Memory</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 20, 290Ð303.<br />

Tulving, E. (1983). Elements <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> episodic memory. Oxford<br />

University Press, New York.<br />

Weld<strong>on</strong>, M. S. (1991). Mechanisms underlying priming <strong>on</strong><br />

perceptual tests. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology:<br />

Learning, <strong>Memory</strong> <strong>and</strong> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 17, 526Ð541.<br />

Ben R. Newell<br />

Department <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology<br />

University College L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong><br />

L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> WC1E 6BT<br />

UK<br />

Tel.: +442076795324<br />

Fax: +442074364276<br />

E-mail: b.newell@ucl.ac.uk<br />

Received March 25, 2003<br />

Revisi<strong>on</strong> received June 16, 2003<br />

Accepted June 18, 2003<br />

” 2004 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2004; Vol. 51(2): 1Ð13

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!