21.11.2014 Views

Use of Personal Documents - Le Moyne - Le Moyne College

Use of Personal Documents - Le Moyne - Le Moyne College

Use of Personal Documents - Le Moyne - Le Moyne College

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong>:<br />

A Contemporary Reappraisal<br />

Vincent W. Hevern, Ph.D.<br />

<strong>Le</strong> <strong>Moyne</strong> <strong>College</strong><br />

Paper presented at the Annual Meeting <strong>of</strong> the American Psychological Association,<br />

Boston, MA, August, 1999<br />

Allport’s advocacy <strong>of</strong> proper research methodology for a scientific psychology <strong>of</strong> the individual<br />

person received crucial and comprehensive expression in a 1942 monograph, published by the<br />

Social Science Research Council (SSRC), which examined a broad class <strong>of</strong> non-experimental<br />

data sources. Recent historiographical studies <strong>of</strong> the SSRC and <strong>of</strong> Allport’s scholarly program in<br />

the 1930s and further archival sources are deployed to understand (1) theoretical concerns<br />

and socio-cultural factors reflected in Allport’s analysis, (2) the functions served by its<br />

publication for psychology and the wider social science community, and (3) an appraisal <strong>of</strong> its<br />

continuing status and relevance for researchers focusing upon personal documentary sources.<br />

After completing his doctoral degree at Harvard in 1922, Gordon W. Allport traveled to<br />

Europe to explore the state <strong>of</strong> Continental (particularly German) and British psychology during<br />

a two-year Sheldon postgraduate fellowship. Writing about this experience near the end <strong>of</strong> his<br />

life, he described “the powerful impact <strong>of</strong> [his] German teachers” in both Berlin and, later,<br />

Hamburg, upon his subsequent pr<strong>of</strong>essional development. 1 He recognized a special debt to<br />

William Stern in whose Hamburg home he lived for six months and who taught him the<br />

difference between “the common variety <strong>of</strong> differential psychology…and a truly personalistic<br />

psychology that focuses upon the organization, and not the mere pr<strong>of</strong>iling, <strong>of</strong> an individual’s<br />

traits.” 2 Indeed, in a 1924 paper Allport contended that contemporary methodologies <strong>of</strong><br />

personality study were flawed by an analytic reduction <strong>of</strong> the person to a set <strong>of</strong> unconnected<br />

and separate traits. 3 What was lost, he argued, was that particular constellation or “form<br />

quality” <strong>of</strong> personal traits which comprise actual individuals in their unique integration. Though<br />

he moved beyond the Gestalt-inspired language <strong>of</strong> this early formulation, Allport maintained a<br />

dogged interest in both the “undivided” individual personality and scientific methods <strong>of</strong><br />

studying such individuals throughout four subsequent decades <strong>of</strong> research and teaching. He<br />

returned repeatedly to a consideration <strong>of</strong> the pivotal role played by research methodology visà-vis<br />

a scientific understanding <strong>of</strong> the individual personality. 4 His approach to proper<br />

methodology, however, <strong>of</strong>ten conflicted with the prevailing experimental operationism <strong>of</strong> his<br />

colleagues. Reflecting on the impact <strong>of</strong> his German postgraduate experience, Allport noted that<br />

“by the time I returned to America I was already a little bit out <strong>of</strong> touch and out <strong>of</strong> step with<br />

the Anglo-American traditions <strong>of</strong> positivism, statistics, and objectivism. Ever since then I’ve<br />

been somewhat <strong>of</strong> a maverick.” 5 As Mischel remarks, Allport’s was “the lone voice speaking out<br />

critically against [the] dimensionalization <strong>of</strong> personality with numbers” during the 1930s and<br />

beyond. 6<br />

The centrality ascribed by Allport to proper research method and the monograph which<br />

is the subject <strong>of</strong> this paper can be judged from the initial pages <strong>of</strong> the autobiographical essay<br />

he wrote the year before his death. There he posed the query -- “how shall a psychological life<br />

I am grateful to <strong>Le</strong> <strong>Moyne</strong> <strong>College</strong> and its Faculty Research and Development Committee for a sabbatical<br />

leave in 1998-1999 which permitted me freedom from other academic responsibilities so that I might work on this<br />

and other projects. The staff <strong>of</strong> the Orradre Library at Santa Clara University were particularly helpful in securing<br />

many materials used in this study. Citations from the Gordon W. Allport Papers are used with permission <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Harvard University Archives. Materials from the records <strong>of</strong> the Social Science Research Council (SSRC) are cited<br />

with permission <strong>of</strong> the Rockefeller Archive Center (RAC), Sleepy Hollow, NY, and I am particularly grateful to Monica<br />

S. Blank, Managing Archivist at the RAC, for her help in identifying and locating materials related to the Committee<br />

on Appraisal <strong>of</strong> Research.<br />

Correspondence and comments regarding this paper may be directed to the author at Department <strong>of</strong><br />

Psychology, <strong>Le</strong> <strong>Moyne</strong> <strong>College</strong>, Syracuse, NY 13214. Electronic mail may be sent to hevern@maple.lemoyne.edu.<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

2<br />

history be written?” -- and suggested that pursuing an answer to this question was, perhaps,<br />

the unifying theme <strong>of</strong> his pr<strong>of</strong>essional career. 7 He then pointed to his 1942 work, The <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong> in Psychological Science (UPD), which summarized “important published<br />

researches” about this question. 8 Yet, the work to which he refers has been out <strong>of</strong> print for<br />

more than four decades and is now available to most students and researchers only as a single<br />

volume within the collection <strong>of</strong> older college or university libraries. Nonetheless, the text holds<br />

a prominent place within Allportian scholarship in his own estimation and this essay seeks to<br />

detail reasons for this judgment.<br />

Prepared for the Committee on Appraisal <strong>of</strong> Research (CAR) <strong>of</strong> the Social Science<br />

Research Council (SSRC), the UPD is a 210-page reflection upon scientific knowledge in<br />

psychology and appropriate research methodologies employing a broad class <strong>of</strong> nonexperimental<br />

data sources: “any self-revealing record that intentionally or unintentionally<br />

yields information regarding the structure, dynamics, and functioning <strong>of</strong> the author’s mental<br />

life” 9 In a three-part structure, Allport charts the use, the form, and the value <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

documents within the science <strong>of</strong> psychology. He provides a comprehensive survey <strong>of</strong> the<br />

literature derived from these data with citations <strong>of</strong> almost 200 individual works from 1881 until<br />

1941. The references he examined tend to be contemporary with more than half published in<br />

the previous decade and 84% appearing since 1922. 10 He analyzes the uncritical, critical, and<br />

experimental uses <strong>of</strong> documents from an historical perspective. After explaining 13 different<br />

motivations for authoring personal documents (special pleading, exhibitionism, desire for<br />

order, etc.) he groups them into four basic categories (autobiographies, questionnaires and<br />

interview transcripts, diaries and letters, and artistic and projective documents). He argues the<br />

case against personal documents (their lack <strong>of</strong> objectivity, poor validity, etc.) as well as the<br />

rationale for their use. He insists that researchers apply the traditional criteria <strong>of</strong> science -- “an<br />

understanding, a power <strong>of</strong> prediction, and a power <strong>of</strong> control, beyond that which [one] can<br />

achieve through … unaided common sense” -- in judging their utility. 11 He concludes that their<br />

use is not only justified but that<br />

Properly used, such documents anchor a discipline in the bedrock <strong>of</strong> human experience, make<br />

the most <strong>of</strong> the predilictive value <strong>of</strong> the single case in the normal process <strong>of</strong> human thought,<br />

exploit the idiographic principles <strong>of</strong> reasoning, and aid in meeting (more adequately than can<br />

unaided actuarial methods <strong>of</strong> work) the three critical tests <strong>of</strong> science: understanding, prediction,<br />

and control. 12<br />

Some recent commentators have lauded the UPD as the “classic text,” the “standard<br />

reference” or the “beacon” for investigators interested in human documents as raw materials<br />

suitable for the study <strong>of</strong> personality and behavior. 13 Nonetheless, the same work has been<br />

frequently overlooked by psychologists and other social scientists in the decades following its<br />

publication and more than ten years have passed since the last analyses specifically examined<br />

this monograph. 14<br />

The neglect afforded the UPD is unfortunate, but understandable. For at least two<br />

decades following the Second World War, both the laboratory-based experimental paradigm<br />

and its allies in the fields <strong>of</strong> clinical testing and applied quantitative measurement tended to<br />

define American psychology completely. Little room remained for the viewpoint Allport<br />

advocated. 15 Only in the last two decades has psychology’s understanding <strong>of</strong> itself historically<br />

and conceptually been opened to the kinds <strong>of</strong> research sources and critiques which might<br />

appreciate the UPD as more than a curiosity swept aside by the juggernaut <strong>of</strong> experimental<br />

positivism.<br />

From the perspective <strong>of</strong> the historian, consider three recent historical advances which<br />

address issues congenial to Allport’s own stance: (1) Danziger’s seminal analysis <strong>of</strong> the social<br />

structuring <strong>of</strong> psychological experimentation and the changeable, even narrowed, role <strong>of</strong><br />

“subjects” in early 20 th century research projects; (2) his subsequent study <strong>of</strong> the sociocultural<br />

construction <strong>of</strong> now commonplace linguistic categories such as intelligence, personality and<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

3<br />

motivation which emerged in the psychological research <strong>of</strong> the pre-World War II era, and (3)<br />

the growing appreciation <strong>of</strong> multiple interacting social and cultural forces and goals which<br />

shaped the direction <strong>of</strong> American psychological science during the discipline’s first seventy-five<br />

years, e.g., the impact <strong>of</strong> the both world wars or the needs <strong>of</strong> industry and government for<br />

analytic tools and social knowledge by which to manage social masses. 16 These findings open<br />

an arena in which Allport’s methodological arguments assume a more central importance than<br />

was evident to many peers in the decades immediately after 1942. Of course, some<br />

contemporaries seem to have intuited the value <strong>of</strong> Allport’s stance. In early 1940, Princeton<br />

psychologist, Carroll Pratt, reported to him the rebuke <strong>of</strong> John Gilbert Beebe-Center when he<br />

questioned Allport’s logic about individual cases: “’Listen Pratt,’ said Beebe-Center, ‘what<br />

Gordon in doing is so goddam much more important than anything you or I have ever done or<br />

ever will do that it isn’t even funny’.” 17<br />

In parallel to the work <strong>of</strong> recent historians <strong>of</strong> the behavioral sciences have been two<br />

complimentary theoretical phenomena which extend or, at least, have an affinity for certain<br />

Allportian themes: (1) a wide-ranging critique <strong>of</strong> positivism in social science which seeks, at<br />

least, to raise the question <strong>of</strong> different conceptions <strong>of</strong> the nature <strong>of</strong> the social scientific<br />

enterprise, and (2) the development during the past 15 years <strong>of</strong> alternative perspectives in<br />

psychology in response to these critiques. These perspectives have been variously termed<br />

interpretivist, narrative, hermeneutic, discursive and cultural. 18 The vantage provided both by<br />

these historical and theoretical understandings and the data I examine in the bulk <strong>of</strong> this paper<br />

lead me to suggest that Allport’s UPD serves as an important mid-point <strong>of</strong> demarcation: on one<br />

side lie unrealized 19 th century aspirations for a cultural-historical science <strong>of</strong> the person<br />

(Geisteswissenschaft) or Wundt’s “second psychology” which Allport glimpsed in his work with<br />

Stern and others in Germany while on the other stands an expanding contemporary<br />

engagement by psychologists in biographical data and the life story as research foci. 19<br />

I will revisit Allport’s UPD in three stages. First, I explore the context within which the<br />

monograph was written by examining the institutional concerns <strong>of</strong> its sponsor, the SSRC, and<br />

the author’s own preoccupations in the years before he wrote the UPD. I will note ways in<br />

which these concerns and preoccupations found representation within the manuscript he<br />

fashioned. Secondly, I <strong>of</strong>fer a brief narrative on the process by which he composed the actual<br />

text <strong>of</strong> the monograph. Previous descriptions have failed to make clear why it was Allport who<br />

undertook this work and how he approached its completion. Finally, I survey responses <strong>of</strong> his<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>essional colleagues both privately and in print as they provided an estimate <strong>of</strong> his<br />

achievement in the UPD.<br />

A. The SSRC as Sponsor<br />

I. Background to the UPD<br />

The composition <strong>of</strong> the UPD was not the result <strong>of</strong> an independent decision by Allport to<br />

undertake this arduous task on the basis <strong>of</strong> his own scholarly judgment. Rather, it arose at an<br />

historical intersection <strong>of</strong> pressing institutional concerns by its commissioning sponsor, the<br />

SSRC, and an amalgam <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>essional and personal beliefs, goals, and responsibilities by its<br />

author. Before considering how Allport’s situation is reflected the style and content <strong>of</strong> the UPD,<br />

I propose to examine first how the SSRC originally came to ask Allport to prepare this<br />

appraisal and the role it was expected to serve in the organization’s understanding <strong>of</strong> its own<br />

mission.<br />

We should recognize that the importance <strong>of</strong> the SSRC and similar research organizations<br />

has begun to receive increasing scholarly attention over the past generation. As historian<br />

Olivier Zunz argues, from the second half <strong>of</strong> the 19th through the middle <strong>of</strong> the 20 th century,<br />

the development <strong>of</strong> world-wide dominance by the U.S. economically and politically rested upon<br />

a unique “institutional matrix <strong>of</strong> business corporations, research universities and institutes,<br />

government agencies, and foundations” which deployed a “reorganization <strong>of</strong> knowledge, [and]<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

4<br />

not merely the power <strong>of</strong> capital accumulation” in order to achieve the American hegemony so<br />

clearly in view by the 1950s. 20 Crucial to reorganizing knowledge was the role <strong>of</strong> “social<br />

intelligence” –- a term <strong>of</strong> economist Edmund Day <strong>of</strong> the Rockefeller Foundation and, later,<br />

President <strong>of</strong> Cornell University -- by which social scientists sought “the understanding and<br />

control <strong>of</strong> social institutions and social processes in the solution <strong>of</strong> pressing social problems.” 21<br />

In this century, private philanthropic foundations, notably the Laura Spelman Rockefeller<br />

Memorial (LSRM), <strong>of</strong>fered extensive funding for both university- and private institute-based<br />

research projects in pursuit <strong>of</strong> higher levels <strong>of</strong> social intelligence. 22 The commission to Allport<br />

which resulted in the UPD is an example <strong>of</strong> that “institutional matrix” in action and reflected<br />

SSRC expectations that significant fundamental improvements could be achieved in the<br />

generation <strong>of</strong> social knowledge.<br />

Organized initially in 1923 by Wesley C. Mitchell and Charles E. Merriam <strong>of</strong> the American<br />

Political Science Association and incorporated the following year, the Social Science Research<br />

Council served as the predominant coordinating body for cross-disciplinary research across<br />

seven academic fields (anthropology, economics, history, political science, psychology,<br />

sociology, and statistics) during the years leading up to the Second World War and beyond. 23<br />

Headquartered in New York City, the SSRC was funded predominantly over its first two<br />

decades <strong>of</strong> existence by grants from the LSRM and other Rockefeller-connected sources.<br />

During those years the Council influenced the direction <strong>of</strong> American social science via multiple<br />

routes: its postgraduate fellowship and grant-in-aid programs, its sponsorship <strong>of</strong><br />

interdisciplinary conferences, technical consultation to governmental commissions and<br />

bureaus, and the commissioning <strong>of</strong> a broad set <strong>of</strong> focused research projects. 24 .<br />

In the years preceding the Great Depression, the SSRC initiated research projects which<br />

sought to elucidate both fundamental issues in social science theory and practical means by<br />

which these fields might contribute to the national commonweal. 25 One early effort at crossdisciplinary<br />

theoretical work beginning in 1923 involved the Committee on Scientific Method in<br />

the Social Sciences (CSMSS). Kenneth Prewett, currently head <strong>of</strong> the SSRC, judges that this<br />

committee “labored long and hard but not entirely successfully.” 26 Under the leadership <strong>of</strong><br />

Stuart A. Rice, it produced equivocal results during six years <strong>of</strong> effort. Its final report, Methods<br />

in Social Science: A Case Book (published in 1931), advanced the demand for a rigorously<br />

scientific methodology in the social sciences but failed to achieve any overall synthesis <strong>of</strong><br />

approach by which such a l<strong>of</strong>ty goal might be reached. As Fisher notes, “the difficulties in<br />

establishing one perspective on social research, which meant a common universe <strong>of</strong> discourse,<br />

had proved insurmountable.” 27 With the onset <strong>of</strong> severe dislocation in the American economic<br />

and social system in the early 1930s, the SSRC shifted its focus increasingly from foundational<br />

or theoretical issues to practical pursuits in which social science might alleviate effects <strong>of</strong> the<br />

economic downturn. 28<br />

Beginning its second decade <strong>of</strong> operation, troubling questions regarding its purpose,<br />

direction, and perhaps, funding began to confront the SSRC. Donald Fisher concludes that the<br />

Council faced a choice by the second half the 1930s: Would it primarily advance plans <strong>of</strong> social<br />

engineering and enlightened intervention in the public sphere, or would it recover a more<br />

formative role by promoting research to provide social science with more secure theoretical<br />

foundations? 29 He claims that the SSRC was under pressure to make such a choice by<br />

representatives <strong>of</strong> Rockefeller philanthropy who were concerned by a lack <strong>of</strong> focus in the<br />

Council’s work. 30 To address its future direction, a Committee on Review <strong>of</strong> Council Policy<br />

(CRCP) was set up in 1936. The CRCP commissioned University <strong>of</strong> Chicago sociologist, Louis<br />

Wirth, to evaluate the Council and its functioning. Accepting most <strong>of</strong> Wirth’s findings, the CRCP<br />

submitted its report to the full Council in September <strong>of</strong> 1937. The committee supported the<br />

view that the SSRC could “render its greatest service to society by exploring the fundamentals<br />

<strong>of</strong> social science.” 31 In practical terms they advised that the Council emphasize “research<br />

promotion, appraisal, and planning” as a overriding priority. 32<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

5<br />

These recommendations led directly to the establishment <strong>of</strong> the Committee on Appraisal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Research (with Edward Day as its first chairman) by the Council’s central Policies and<br />

Procedures Committee (PPC) in September, 1937. This action was “one means <strong>of</strong> carrying into<br />

effect the principle that intellectual leadership in the social sciences is the primary function <strong>of</strong><br />

the Council.” 33 Quoting Edwin G. Nourse, chair <strong>of</strong> the PPC, Fisher notes that Day had made a<br />

critical intervention before the Policies Committee. Day challenged whether the PPC “had any<br />

clearly formulated idea as to what social science really is or whether it was doing anything<br />

serious and systematic in the direction <strong>of</strong> finding out.” 34 The specific role <strong>of</strong> the newly-formed<br />

committee was debated at great length by the PPC on December 12, 1937 when Day further<br />

questioned the meaning <strong>of</strong> the word “appraisal” in its title. He preferred the phrase<br />

“identification <strong>of</strong> scientific accomplishment.” 35 The PPC generally agreed with Day and, even<br />

after he left the CAR as Chair in 1939, his understanding <strong>of</strong> appraisal—an emphasis on the<br />

cumulative progress <strong>of</strong> shared fundamentals across the disciplines--guided its efforts. 36<br />

Following internal debate by the PPC regarding how successfully the Council had responded to<br />

the CRCP’s original critique, the scope <strong>of</strong> the CAR’s mandate was expanded even further in<br />

1940 to include “planning undertakings designed specifically for general improvement <strong>of</strong> the<br />

quality <strong>of</strong> work in the social sciences” 37<br />

During its nine years <strong>of</strong> operation (1937-1946), three broadly overlapping phases<br />

characterized the work <strong>of</strong> the CAR: (1) appraisal <strong>of</strong> significant individual research projects<br />

across each discipline (1938-1940), (2) evaluation <strong>of</strong> the role <strong>of</strong> personal documents as data<br />

sources in social science (1940-1943), and (3) replication <strong>of</strong> previous research with personal<br />

documents via additional analytic approaches (1940-1946). The initial strategy adopted by the<br />

CAR involved the selection <strong>of</strong> “the most significant productions in [each disciplinary area] <strong>of</strong><br />

specialization in America” since the First World War. In supporting this tack, Day believed that<br />

the committee would be able to document “the progressive development <strong>of</strong> the social sciences<br />

through increased emphasis on the verification <strong>of</strong> research accomplishments and the need for<br />

integrated, cumulative results.” 38 Notably, the CAR never authorized an appraisal in<br />

psychology since psychologists could never recommend a single text to appraise. Asked to<br />

choose between Thurstone’s Vectors <strong>of</strong> the Mind and Allport’s <strong>Personal</strong>ity: A Psychological<br />

Interpretation psychologists were too divided to justify the CAR undertaking an appraisal <strong>of</strong><br />

any specific text in the discipline. 39<br />

From early 1938 until the middle <strong>of</strong> 1940, the CAR focused upon the specific<br />

reappraisals <strong>of</strong> individual publications. The most influential <strong>of</strong> these was an assessment <strong>of</strong> the<br />

widely-regarded study by Thomas and Znaniecki, The Polish Peasant in Europe and America,<br />

which was carried out by Herbert Blumer to great acclaim. 40 Blumer’s success led to a crucial<br />

conference in New York City on December 10, 1938 at which its author, Thomas, Allport, and<br />

15 other social scientists met to review this appraisal. 41 In wide-ranging discussions,<br />

participants were struck particularly by Blumer’s assertion that, despite their broad citations <strong>of</strong><br />

personal documents (letters, diaries, transcripts <strong>of</strong> interviews), Thomas and Znaniecki’s<br />

conclusions were not directly and inductively derived from the documents themselves. Rather,<br />

the first-person data sources they used only confirmed their judgments originally generated<br />

via other means and evidence.<br />

The impact <strong>of</strong> Blumer’s conclusions was sufficiently strong that the CAR found it now<br />

faced a new issue. At its July 28, 1940 meeting Ernest Burgess suggested to the CAR that it<br />

undertake an examination <strong>of</strong> the “recent development in the use <strong>of</strong> human document material<br />

in social science research.” 42 Burgess, Robert Redfield, and Donald Young prepared a<br />

memorandum for the committee outlining how this suggestion might be implemented. In<br />

September, 1940 CAR Chairman Redfield reported to the full Council this plan <strong>of</strong> action which<br />

linked the expanded responsibilities <strong>of</strong> the committee to investigate new methods <strong>of</strong> research<br />

in the social science with Blumer’s powerful critique. More specifically, Redfield suggested that<br />

the CAR undertake four experimental projects. Several <strong>of</strong> these would replicate via alternative<br />

methods a well-regarded study <strong>of</strong> Robert Angell on the impact <strong>of</strong> the Depression upon the<br />

family for which personal documents had served as principal data sources. He also tentatively<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

6<br />

suggested that “we re-employ Blumer to do the general overall critical review <strong>of</strong> the literature<br />

upon the human document in sociology.” 43<br />

At its October, 20, 1940 meeting, the CAR endorsed the two-prong strategy <strong>of</strong> Burgess<br />

and his colleagues which Redfield discussed the month before. First, Robert Angell rather than<br />

Herbert Blumer would be asked to prepare a literature review <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> the personal<br />

document in sociology. And, secondly, a “repeat study <strong>of</strong> the data in Angell’s ‘The Family<br />

Encounters the Depression’” would employ alternative analytic methods while parallel work<br />

employing Angell’s methodology would be used with a different data set. 44 The committee<br />

hoped that these studies would demonstrate the repeatability <strong>of</strong> scientific methods <strong>of</strong><br />

investigation using documentary data in one social science, a goal reached regularly by the<br />

natural sciences. At the next CAR meeting on November 10, 1940, the committee’s discussion<br />

questioned more generally how personal documents were used in fields other than sociology.<br />

The committee then reviewed the names <strong>of</strong> candidates to appraise document-based research<br />

in each <strong>of</strong> the five other disciplines (excepting sociology and statistics). P<strong>of</strong>fenberger didn’t<br />

suggest any name but asked to confer with Robert M. Elliott about a possible candidate in<br />

psychology. 45 In sum, the work <strong>of</strong> Blumer had brought the CAR to a theoretical impasse which<br />

again confronted the SSRC with the need to review fundamental questions about the<br />

generation <strong>of</strong> knowledge in the social sciences–a methodological conundrum left unsolved by<br />

the work <strong>of</strong> the CSMSS a decade earlier. The stage was set, too, for the role Allport was to<br />

play in assessing the utility <strong>of</strong> personal documents in psychological science.<br />

B. Gordon W. Allport as Author<br />

Recently published studies <strong>of</strong> Allport and his colleagues during the 1920s through 1940s<br />

have begun to fill in the gap created by the absence <strong>of</strong> any comprehensive biography <strong>of</strong><br />

Allport. They provide extremely helpful contexts and insights by which to evaluate the author’s<br />

concerns in the pre-War era. <strong>Le</strong>t me review briefly three studies appearing since 1997 and link<br />

them to more general themes which influenced Allport’s efforts in the UPD: Ian Nicholson’s<br />

work on Allport’s ethical and value system in its relationship to the development <strong>of</strong> personality<br />

psychology, Katherine Pandora’s pivotal history <strong>of</strong> Allport and the Murphys, Gardner and Lois<br />

Barclay, during the 1930s, and Nicole Barenbaum’s investigation <strong>of</strong> Allport’s use <strong>of</strong> cases. I will<br />

also briefly look at Allport’s relationship with German refugee psychologists before World War<br />

II because <strong>of</strong> its influence on his willingness to work on the UPD.<br />

<strong>Personal</strong>ity Psychology and Enduring Ethical Concerns. Ian Nicholson has ably<br />

documented the impact <strong>of</strong> Allport’s upbringing and personal belief system upon the new<br />

“personality psychology” which he championed in the 1920s and 1930s. 46 Allport embraced the<br />

new language <strong>of</strong> “personality,” Nicholson argues, as a psychological concept capable <strong>of</strong><br />

empirical and objective study in a way that the earlier notion <strong>of</strong> “character,” so laden with<br />

moralistic and religious overtones, could never be. Simultaneously, though, Allport’s study <strong>of</strong><br />

enduring personality traits presupposes an essential unity <strong>of</strong> the individual which behaviorallyoriented<br />

peers never endorsed. Further, study <strong>of</strong> personality tended to restore to prominence<br />

in psychological discourse older Victorian ideals <strong>of</strong> the productive, conscientious, and morallyupright<br />

citizen. 47 Nicholson points to descriptors <strong>of</strong> the mature personality which Allport<br />

detailed at length in his groundbreaking text, <strong>Personal</strong>ity: A Psychological Interpretation. Many<br />

<strong>of</strong> these qualities embody a 19 th century ethical sensibility. 48 As Jerome Bruner who<br />

collaborated closely with Allport as a graduate student suggested recently “there was, in a<br />

word, an enormous concern with individuality in Allport’s work. I sometimes think that it grew<br />

not so much from political convictions, but from religious ones stemming from Protestant<br />

theology. Each soul, as it were, had a uniqueness for him…” 49<br />

A clear sensitivity to ethical issues and values, particularly the sacredness <strong>of</strong> the<br />

individual as an ethical value beyond par, is evident throughout the UPD. In arguing the case<br />

for personal documents, Allport held that they have the potential to align science on the side <strong>of</strong><br />

a democratic value system: “According to the ethics <strong>of</strong> democracy, respect and value are to be<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

7<br />

ascribed to each individual person. But mass methods <strong>of</strong> research do little to engender<br />

understanding or respect for the individual person. A shift <strong>of</strong> interest to personal<br />

documents…cannot fail to bring psychological science closer in life with the ethics <strong>of</strong><br />

democracy.” 50 Later, he argues: “In a democratic society the therapeutist is required to<br />

advance the welfare <strong>of</strong> the person: personal documents tell him wherein this welfare<br />

consists.” 51 In countering a objection that personal documents can be self-deceiving, Allport<br />

argued that they express “one person’s view <strong>of</strong> life. A person is a self-regarding focus <strong>of</strong><br />

value…Every self regards itself as sacred, and a document produced from precisely this point<br />

<strong>of</strong> view is exactly what we desire.” 52 Similarly, he pointed out that “inviolable self-regard<br />

seems to be the eternal thread found in all personal documents. But this expression <strong>of</strong> egovalue<br />

is precisely what we want in a document.” 53<br />

Creating a Psychological Science for a Democratic Society. In the absence <strong>of</strong> a definitive<br />

biography, the compelling study <strong>of</strong> Katherine Pandora, Rebels within the Ranks (1997), stands<br />

as the best available study <strong>of</strong> Allport in the years before the Second World War. 54 Her analysis<br />

is closely allied with the general thrust <strong>of</strong> Nicholson’s findings. She traces the socio-political<br />

goals <strong>of</strong> Allport and the Murphys, Gardner and Lois Barlcay, during the 1930s as they<br />

challenged the conventional experimental ethos <strong>of</strong> American psychology. These “rebels”<br />

<strong>of</strong>fered an expanded alternative vision <strong>of</strong> psychological science more supportive <strong>of</strong> democratic<br />

ideals and practices. Pandora builds upon Nicholson’s review by examining Allport’s ethical<br />

concerns as they relate primarily to the public sphere. In Allport’s view, a science aligned with<br />

matters <strong>of</strong> urgent civic moment was imperative if totalitarian and fascist tendencies arising<br />

from the political and economic disorder <strong>of</strong> the era were to be defeated and democratic<br />

principles advanced. 55 These scholarly nonconformists, Pandora argues, “rejected the image <strong>of</strong><br />

the laboratory as an ivory tower, contested the canons <strong>of</strong> objectivity that characterized current<br />

research practice, and argued against reducing the natural and unnatural worlds to the lowest<br />

possible terms.” 56<br />

Allport’s dissent took a variety <strong>of</strong> forms including his insistent demand that psychology<br />

account scientifically for individuals, and not just groups, at the levels <strong>of</strong> description,<br />

prediction, and control. He inveighed repeatedly against orthodox empiricists who believed<br />

that knowledge <strong>of</strong> a population’s average was equivalent to knowing about specific individuals<br />

within that population. 57 For example, coincidental to writing the UPD, Allport completed a<br />

lengthy critique <strong>of</strong> a manuscript on the prediction <strong>of</strong> personal adjustment by Horst, Wallin, and<br />

Guttman which he submitted to SSRC Director, Robert T. Crane. Allport broadly castigated<br />

their text for its methodological one-sidedness and bias. He observed that “as we approach the<br />

‘sub-class’ <strong>of</strong> one, the whole process [<strong>of</strong> prediction] seems to change and we predict from<br />

intimate knowledge <strong>of</strong> the single case (from its own laws <strong>of</strong> behavior), and not on the grounds<br />

<strong>of</strong> the behavior <strong>of</strong> whole populations.” 58 He would later describe the UPD to Crane as<br />

“essentially an elaboration” <strong>of</strong> this critique and included an explicit review <strong>of</strong> his arguments<br />

about prediction <strong>of</strong> individual lives in the UPD itself. 59<br />

Allport feared that an unacceptable price--one destructive <strong>of</strong> democracy--would be<br />

exacted eventually for the narrowed scientific focus <strong>of</strong> social science knowledge gathered in<br />

this period. As Olivier Zunz comments, “by resorting to all sorts <strong>of</strong> averaging and using only<br />

what they thought were neutral categories <strong>of</strong> knowledge in a multicultural society, Americans<br />

also learned how to sanitize facts by avoiding the complexities embedded in class, gender,<br />

age, race, or ethnicity.” 60 Allport gave a particularly explicit expression <strong>of</strong> his fears in an<br />

unpublished keynote address presented at the 75 th anniversary meeting <strong>of</strong> the New England<br />

Intercollegiate Conference on Psychology (NEICP) on April 4, 1941 at the University <strong>of</strong> New<br />

Hampshire in Durham. 61 . His audience consisted primarily <strong>of</strong> undergraduate students and his<br />

talk sought to chart the near future <strong>of</strong> the discipline they were beginning to study. Allport<br />

depicted the assault on humanity’s sense <strong>of</strong> self-worth and value which some types <strong>of</strong> science<br />

seemed to encourage. He claimed that “especially in modern psychology man does not<br />

recognize himself, at least not the best that is in him. Disillusioned and disgusted with science<br />

man turns to political movements to recover his sense <strong>of</strong> self-importance. Totalitarianism with<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

8<br />

all its arrogant stupidity may win his allegiance.” 62 Allport had just begun to work on the UPD<br />

at the time he gave this talk. It is no wonder, then, that Allport reminds his readers in the UPD<br />

that “until recently psychologists have been inclined to detach themselves from politics and<br />

from ethics, but social catastrophe is now breaking down this isolation.” 63<br />

Throughout the late 1930s Allport engaged in an escalating rhetoric <strong>of</strong> disagreement<br />

over scientific methodology with his experimentally-inclined colleagues. 64 His motivation<br />

embraced the vision <strong>of</strong> a more broadly realistic psychology, one grounded in daily life. He was<br />

convinced <strong>of</strong> its potential to address fundamental human concerns. Hence, he exploited<br />

venues, public and private, to argue for this heterodox scientific program. For example,<br />

consider his presidency <strong>of</strong> the APA during 1938-1939. A longstanding prerogative afforded its<br />

presidents was the opportunity to present a major lecture on a self-chosen topic at the<br />

organization’s annual convention. The significance <strong>of</strong> these talks was magnified by their<br />

widespread distribution; they were automatically printed in either the Psychological Review or<br />

Psychological Bulletin. 65 Contemplating his own remarks, Allport was aware that the addresses<br />

<strong>of</strong> his immediate predecessors hailed what they believed to be the growing success <strong>of</strong><br />

experimental methods and statistical analyses to solve theoretical and practical problems <strong>of</strong><br />

importance to psychologists. In 1933 Louis Thurstone heralded the power <strong>of</strong> multiple factor<br />

analysis to describe human mental abilities. In 1934 Joseph Peterson detailed technical<br />

advances in behavioral explanations for perceptual and motor learning in organisms. In 1936,<br />

Clark Hull <strong>of</strong>fered an extremely detailed rationale for a hypothetico-deductive system which<br />

would secure psychology’s identity as a (quasi-natural) science. And, the following year<br />

Edward C. Tolman discussed “the complex <strong>of</strong> causal determinants” <strong>of</strong> the behavior <strong>of</strong> rats in a<br />

laboratory maze and found important motivational principles <strong>of</strong> significance for organisms<br />

generally. Finally, in 1938, John Frederick Dashiell portrayed the relations between psychology<br />

and other fields as increasingly interdisciplinary and, perhaps with some naïve expectancy,<br />

claimed to discern signs <strong>of</strong> a growing rapprochement between the experimental and “clinical”<br />

viewpoints within psychology itself. 66<br />

In Karl Scheibe’s estimate, “this was a Dark Age for the human being in psychology, for<br />

the force <strong>of</strong> the scientific theology swept all before it,” -- a time which reflected “the conceit<br />

that everything <strong>of</strong> psychological importance could in principle be demonstrated by the rat at<br />

the choice point in the maze.” 67 In response, Allport <strong>of</strong>fered a powerful dissent to the claims <strong>of</strong><br />

his laboratory-based peers in his presidential talk delivered just six days after Hitler had<br />

invaded Poland in 1939. 68 Entitled “The Psychologist’s Frame <strong>of</strong> Reference” Allport first<br />

presented a solidly empirical survey <strong>of</strong> trends across fifty years <strong>of</strong> scientific publication. He<br />

highlighted the recent growth in animal and nonverbal human research topics which avoided<br />

altogether higher levels <strong>of</strong> human cognition. He pointed to the growth <strong>of</strong> sophisticated<br />

statistical analyses and methodological demands for operationalism, the “watchword <strong>of</strong> an<br />

austere empiricism.” 69 He, then, engaged in a sharply rhetorical reflection upon the sterility<br />

and dangers resulting from the methodological directions revealed in that review. 70 His<br />

language was, at turns, slyly ironic, broadly jocular, and pointedly scornful.<br />

In the second half <strong>of</strong> his address, Allport explicitly singled out articles and opinions <strong>of</strong><br />

experimental purists—Lashley, Tolman, Bills, Stevens, and Boring among them—and<br />

challenged their claim to an exclusive understanding <strong>of</strong> research methods required by<br />

psychological science. He characterized his positivist peers as divorced from the urgent<br />

concerns <strong>of</strong> a world in chaos: “though [the experimentalist] generally repudiates a dualism <strong>of</strong><br />

mind and body, he welcomes the equally stultifying dualism <strong>of</strong> laboratory and life.” 71 In an<br />

audacious coda, Allport focused upon historical rigidities in political thought (autocracy in<br />

ancient Greece, the medieval Church) and included the example <strong>of</strong> E. R. Jaensch, head <strong>of</strong> the<br />

German psychological society and Nazi-ideologue, who advocated a psychology tied closely to<br />

the racist aims <strong>of</strong> his government. 72 In arguing from these potentially explosive examples,<br />

Allport sought to uphold the advantage <strong>of</strong> a pluralism in psychology’s scientific methods: “the<br />

desirability <strong>of</strong> keeping alive diversified investigations and a diversified sense <strong>of</strong> importance is<br />

the generous lesson that democracy teaches us.” 73 As he would argue before the students at<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

9<br />

the University <strong>of</strong> New Hampshire 18 months later, he feared the outcome <strong>of</strong> a cultist disregard<br />

for real-life issues and the embrace <strong>of</strong> trivial research projects by many experimentalists who<br />

demanded “slavish subservience” to the operational propositions famously advanced by A. G.<br />

Bills. Thus, Allport called for a psychology which would also be “rational, teleological,<br />

qualitative, idiographic, synoptic, and even non-operational”. 74 In the years following this talk,<br />

the UPD served as a major new platform in which he would advocate these goals in detail.<br />

The <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> Case Studies. Nicole Barenbaum has taken exception to the observation<br />

made by some critics that, for all <strong>of</strong> his advocacy <strong>of</strong> case studies and personal documents in<br />

the understanding <strong>of</strong> the individual, Allport only undertook a single case study himself, that <strong>of</strong><br />

“Jenny” and her letters. 75 In a finely documented account, she demonstrates that by 1938<br />

Allport was working upon a second but ultimately unpublished case, that <strong>of</strong> “Marion Taylor” (a<br />

pseudonym), and he did so most probably with his wife, Ada, as a close collaborator.<br />

<strong>Le</strong>t me expand upon Barenbaum’s analysis. The criticism that Allport had very limited<br />

experience with actual case studies rests upon the criterion <strong>of</strong> formal publication. In <strong>Le</strong>tters<br />

from Jenny, his work became available to the public while, in the example <strong>of</strong> “Marion Taylor,”<br />

his (and Ada Allport’s) analysis never reached print. Commentators, though, seem to overlook<br />

the pedagogical use <strong>of</strong> personal documents and case studies for which Allport had a far<br />

broader experience and commitment beginning early in his teaching career and continuing<br />

until his retirement. Indeed, in a recent paper, Barenbaum points to Allport’s graduate classes<br />

in the late 1930s in which he sought to expand the scientific grounding <strong>of</strong> case study methods;<br />

the research findings <strong>of</strong> several students in these seminars did reach print. 76 Jerome Bruner, a<br />

participant in Allport’s seminar, recently recalled that “[Allport] was very interested somehow<br />

in specifying a set <strong>of</strong> rules that would, in the course <strong>of</strong> autobiography, describe some sort <strong>of</strong><br />

inherent organization <strong>of</strong> the person that was responsive to, yet autonomous from, the person<br />

as socially or culturally defined by her position in the world.” 77 (Barenbaum also identifies<br />

Allport’s editorship <strong>of</strong> the Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal and Social Psychology as the prompt for a<br />

renewed appearance <strong>of</strong> published case studies in that journal from 1943 through 1949.)<br />

An early example <strong>of</strong> case-based pedagogy comes in Allport’s use <strong>of</strong> the autobiography <strong>of</strong><br />

University <strong>of</strong> Wisconsin English pr<strong>of</strong>essor, William Ellery Channing <strong>Le</strong>onard, entitled The<br />

Locomotive God. 78 Writing to its author in 1928, Allport described that he and his wife<br />

originally came upon <strong>Le</strong>onard’s poem, Two Lives, in the autumn <strong>of</strong> 1927 and subsequently<br />

read the autobiography which “fitted almost miraculously with my own interests and plans.” 79<br />

Allport explained that he had been “not entirely satisfied with my methods” <strong>of</strong> teaching a<br />

course in personality at both Harvard and Dartmouth. So, he created an “experimental course”<br />

which employed “the case-method” and the “intuitive or verstehende method” with 16 Junior<br />

and Senior Dartmouth students. Allport devised a detailed series <strong>of</strong> “<strong>Le</strong>itfragen” or focused<br />

questions tied to the content <strong>of</strong> <strong>Le</strong>onard’s text which would provoke student thought and<br />

reflection. 80 He sent a copy <strong>of</strong> these questions to <strong>Le</strong>onard for his review and asked for his frank<br />

opinion about a possible article which Allport would write for “other psychologists [who] might<br />

be interested in my use <strong>of</strong> your book for instruction.” 81 A decade later, in 1938, Allport again<br />

praised the pedagogical utility <strong>of</strong> The Locomotive God which he regularly employed in class at<br />

Harvard from 1930 onward. “This morning,” Allport told <strong>Le</strong>onard, “I lectured on points raised<br />

by the first fifty pages <strong>of</strong> the book to a class <strong>of</strong> 250 men. Their absorption was complete and I<br />

can easily tell that their reading <strong>of</strong> the book is making its usual deep and beneficent<br />

impression.” 82<br />

The <strong>Le</strong>onard case was one <strong>of</strong> many which Allport employed in the undergraduate<br />

classroom. Among his papers in the Harvard University Archives, there are multiple case study<br />

materials including the “Case <strong>of</strong> MB (Regression or Shell Shock),” H. G. Well’s Experiment in<br />

Autobiography, and two cases developed by Jean Evans (“Miller” and “Johnny Rocco”) 83<br />

Allport’s use <strong>of</strong> “Johnny Rocco” came in his post-war Social Relations 1A class at Harvard<br />

where it served as the basis <strong>of</strong> class discussions about mechanisms <strong>of</strong> personality and the<br />

research problem <strong>of</strong> predicting individual development. 84 Other cases are also found among his<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

10<br />

teaching notes. Allport’s commitment to the utility <strong>of</strong> personal documents in case studies<br />

extended well beyond the one published text usually cited.<br />

Allport’s long experience using case studies in teaching is reflected in the UPD in several<br />

ways. <strong>Le</strong>onard and Wells’ autobiographies are cited as important examples <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

documents with a potential to engage researchers: “the psychologist is still challenged to<br />

account for <strong>Le</strong>onard the individual: for the valid portions <strong>of</strong> his narrative and for his selfdeception<br />

as well.” 85 He also admits the use <strong>of</strong> documents such as these for illustrative and<br />

didactic purposes. Classroom textbooks are filled with personal documents that exemplify<br />

psychological principles though such materials “possess no distinctive scientific merit” unless<br />

they are “used as tools <strong>of</strong> discovery.” 86 He himself had derived “an empirical-intuitive theory <strong>of</strong><br />

understanding from the reactions <strong>of</strong> students to reading <strong>Le</strong>onard’s Locomotive God.” 87 Finally,<br />

Allport displays a depth understanding <strong>of</strong> the processes by which authors <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

documents craft them throughout the UPD. Years <strong>of</strong> classroom review gave Allport a pr<strong>of</strong>ound<br />

familiarity with these cases and he astutely employed that knowledge to explain personal<br />

documents in convincing fashion.<br />

The Situation <strong>of</strong> European Émigré Psychologists. Allport’s relationship to refugee<br />

psychologists in the immediate pre-war period deserves comment because <strong>of</strong> its influence on<br />

the UPD. In the late summer <strong>of</strong> 1940 the APA received a report from its Committee on<br />

Displaced Foreign Psychologists (CDFP) warning that “it is not too calamitous to say that the<br />

position <strong>of</strong> scientific psychology throughout the world, excepting only in the Americas is in<br />

extremis. Virtually the only way now open to us to fight for our pr<strong>of</strong>essional integrity is to<br />

strengthen our efforts in behalf <strong>of</strong> psychologists who have managed to take refuge here from<br />

tyranny abroad.” 88 Allport had initiated this committee at the beginning <strong>of</strong> his APA presidency<br />

in 1938 and served on it personally and continuously until it ceased functioning in 1943, well<br />

after the United States entered the war. 89 The CDFP was charged initially with the task <strong>of</strong><br />

“surveying the problem <strong>of</strong> psychologists displaced from their positions and livelihood in other<br />

countries and seeking asylum and pr<strong>of</strong>essional opportunities elsewhere.” But, it was forced<br />

almost immediately to expand its task to include finding “non-competitive” positions for the<br />

hundreds <strong>of</strong> refugee scholars and clinicians who found themselves, jobless and without<br />

resources, in America. 90<br />

Allport’s close familiarity with German academic psychologists over the years since his<br />

sojourn in Europe in 1922-1924 resulted in his serving as a crucial resource in the efforts <strong>of</strong><br />

several psychologists to escape fascist governments even prior to the establishment <strong>of</strong> the<br />

CDFP. Allport witnessed the malignant aftereffects <strong>of</strong> Hitler’s assumption <strong>of</strong> power in 1933<br />

upon the lives <strong>of</strong> friends and colleagues: the suicides <strong>of</strong> psychologists Martha Muchow and<br />

Otto Lipmann, the destruction <strong>of</strong> William Stern’s Hamburg laboratory and many <strong>of</strong> his<br />

research files, and the inevitable emigration <strong>of</strong> scholars such as Karl and Charlotte Bühler and<br />

Stern himself. 91 Their desperation is vividly conveyed by Charlotte Bühler writing to Allport<br />

from Vienna in September, 1937: “I feel now however more and more the one wish to leave<br />

Europe for good and to settle in America…Financial and political conditions make scientific<br />

work in Europe increasingly difficult. We lack literature and exchange, money and security.<br />

Psychology has died out in Germany and was never much in other European countries. In<br />

child psychology, Piaget and I are fighting alone ...” 92 In the case <strong>of</strong> both his old mentor,<br />

William Stern as well as the Bühlers, Allport was instrumental in arranging their eventual<br />

settlement in the United States and securing positions for Stern and Charlotte Bühler. 93<br />

The sad example <strong>of</strong> German psychology, Allport’s intellectual home, weighed heavily in<br />

his critique <strong>of</strong> American research methods. 94 There was a visceral, passionate quality to his<br />

anger and dismay when he considered what had transpired on the Continent in the previous<br />

decade. In his NEIPC talk <strong>of</strong> April, 1941, one <strong>of</strong> Allport’s closing images was frightening:<br />

It is not at all beyond the range <strong>of</strong> our imagination that all <strong>of</strong> us psychologists should be<br />

regimented and goose-steppe (sic) from morning to night, inventing meretricious race theories,<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

11<br />

testing ability (sic) to drive Panzar (sic) tanks, or poisoning the mind <strong>of</strong> friends and enemies alike<br />

with crude propaganda. Every single psychologist in Germany is doing this slave labor for an<br />

enslaved science. And Germany was once our model <strong>of</strong> freedom in the psychological world. 95<br />

In authoring the UPD, it fell to Allport–there was no one else to do the job–to communicate<br />

what his early colleagues, especially Stern, had taught him lest the stimulating intellectual<br />

world into which he entered in 1922 would, like Stern’s research notes, be lost forever. He<br />

had been sure to highlight the work <strong>of</strong> Spranger, Stern, Bühler, and others in his earlier<br />

<strong>Personal</strong>ity: A Psychological Interpretation. 96 In the UPD, he again assayed the contributions<br />

<strong>of</strong> Stern and Bühler, both <strong>of</strong> whom had done considerable work with life documents. Indeed,<br />

he expanded his analysis <strong>of</strong> the idiographic uses <strong>of</strong> documentary data which they inspired<br />

considerably beyond his 1937 textbook.<br />

II. The Monograph: Preparation and Scope<br />

On December 17, 1940 P<strong>of</strong>fenberger wrote Allport introducing him to a “comprehensive<br />

survey <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> the personal document in Social Science” which the CAR had begun to<br />

conduct. He solicited Allport’s advice about who might carry out a survey in psychology as well<br />

as how long and costly such a project might be. 97 Responding within a week, Allport nominated<br />

himself for such an undertaking: “no psychologists excepting myself and the Hamburg<br />

Psychological Institute have been interested in the systematic study <strong>of</strong> this problem from the<br />

methodological perspective. And now, <strong>of</strong> course, the Hamburg Institute is destroyed and the<br />

two leading figures in this methodological work (Stern and Muchow) are dead.” 98 Here the<br />

experiences <strong>of</strong> Germany under fascism served as a motivation in Allport’s decision to volunteer<br />

for this effort: his continuing debt to Stern was clear.<br />

On January 10, 1941 P<strong>of</strong>fenberger was enthusiastic in accepting Allport’s selfnomination:<br />

“I am grateful, too, that you modestly suggest yourself as the man to do the job,<br />

for you certainly are that!” He further defined the task to include “a thorough and critical<br />

examination <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Personal</strong> Document in psychology in order that its good points<br />

may be applied in other divisions <strong>of</strong> Social Science and its bad points avoided.” In somewhat<br />

confusing fashion, P<strong>of</strong>fenberger indicated that third-party case studies including whole<br />

biographies would not be subject to review although “personal materials included within a<br />

biography would be”. Allport was asked to prepare a more detailed description <strong>of</strong> the project<br />

along with a cost estimate for presentation to the CAR. 99 Allport immediately responded by<br />

proposing (1) the use <strong>of</strong> a research assistant (for a stipend <strong>of</strong> $200) throughout the spring<br />

semester to explore the literature and (2) his own involvement to “digest the material and put<br />

it in proper form for use <strong>of</strong> the SSRC” throughout the summer <strong>of</strong> 1941 (Allport asked for an<br />

honorarium <strong>of</strong> $500 since he would forego other “summer school work”). He suggested a<br />

completion date <strong>of</strong> September 1, 1941 for the project, but cautioned P<strong>of</strong>fenberger that<br />

“national defense work <strong>of</strong> some kind or other might claim my time and take necessary<br />

precedence.” 100<br />

Allport’s plan was accepted by the CAR and he was formally assigned the task by<br />

P<strong>of</strong>fenberger on February 11, 1941. 101 It was not completely clear to Allport what the CAR<br />

expected would be included in his review under the rubric <strong>of</strong> psychology: would it extend to<br />

the “closely connected” fields <strong>of</strong> anthropology, psychiatry, psychoanalysis or literature? 102<br />

P<strong>of</strong>fenberger clarified its scope when he informed Allport that “the Committee did not discuss<br />

just what the limits <strong>of</strong> psychology were to be. It is my impression, however, that we are<br />

justified in limiting [your study] so as to exclude the obviously psychoanalytic and<br />

psychiatric…Literature as such should not be included, although when literary works as<br />

biographies are used by psychologists for interpretive purposes, then they would be<br />

included.” 103<br />

With these guidelines in hand, Allport immediately began to work on the project. It is<br />

not clear whom he hired to assist in the research for the Spring, 1941 semester. He had asked<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

12<br />

for a stipend for a single assistant but spoke about “research assistants [who] have been<br />

working along” in a note to P<strong>of</strong>fenberger at the end <strong>of</strong> May. In this same message he<br />

acknowledged receipt <strong>of</strong> Angell’s appraisal <strong>of</strong> personal documents in sociology in manuscript<br />

form which would help guide his own study 104 Whether one or more assistants were hired,<br />

Allport did devise a comprehensive five-page questionnaire by which to extract the most<br />

salient characteristics <strong>of</strong> each study under review. Since the finished UPD contains almost 200<br />

separate items, it is no wonder that later he would note: “this schedule was found to be too<br />

searching.” 105 The actual writing <strong>of</strong> the monograph did not begin until June, 1941. 106 By that<br />

time, Allport had already secured a later completion deadline <strong>of</strong> October 1. The concurrent<br />

press <strong>of</strong> work that spring which required “some interruptions for defense work” as well as the<br />

project “turn[ing] out to be larger than I thought” were reasons Allport advanced to justify his<br />

need for additional time. 107<br />

Throughout the spring and summer <strong>of</strong> 1941 competing tasks clamored for Allport’s<br />

attention. Along with Robert Yerkes, he led a standing committee on psychological factors in<br />

morale, formed by the Emergency Committee on Psychology. This group worked intensively<br />

during the winter and spring <strong>of</strong> 1941 on a survey and bibliography on German Psychological<br />

Welfare which was published that June. 108 As James Capshew notes, Allport’s Harvard <strong>of</strong>fice<br />

from this period onward served as “an un<strong>of</strong>ficial clearinghouse for morale studies as well as a<br />

communications center for social psychologists seeking national service.” 109 I’ve previously<br />

mentioned the keynote speech Allport presented on April 4, 1941 to the NEICP at the<br />

University <strong>of</strong> New Hampshire. 110 The extensive critique which Allport <strong>of</strong>fered to SSRC Executive<br />

Director Crane regarding the monograph <strong>of</strong> Horst et al. on the prediction <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

adjustment was submitted on June 2, 1941. Later in August Ernest Burgess sent Allport an<br />

additional set <strong>of</strong> papers – by himself, Cottrell and Stouffer – on the question <strong>of</strong> case study and<br />

asked for their immediate review prior to a Chicago sociological meeting in September. 111<br />

Allport completed an analysis <strong>of</strong> these works as Burgess requested: an archival copy <strong>of</strong> a<br />

heavily-corrected 3-page analysis <strong>of</strong> Stouffer’s paper attests to Allport’s conscientious<br />

review. 112 Allport also commented on a fourth paper by George A. Lundberg presented at the<br />

same Chicago symposium. Lundberg thanked Allport for his comments in a letter <strong>of</strong> October 2,<br />

1941. 113 Lundberg also stated that he would like to obtain a copy <strong>of</strong> Allport’s remarks to<br />

Burgess, presumably dealing with Burgess’ own paper. Throughout the preparation <strong>of</strong> the UPD<br />

Allport’s attention seems to have been divided among a variety <strong>of</strong> pressing tasks. Nonetheless,<br />

he submitted multiple copies <strong>of</strong> his completed manuscript to Crane on October 1. In doing so<br />

he acknowledged his viewpoint on the role <strong>of</strong> personal documents would provoke criticism by<br />

other social scientists, but he could “only hope that my present report which is a continuation<br />

<strong>of</strong> the discussion will result in still further clarifications. I fully realize that I am in the minority.<br />

But, I believe that minority voices have their place.” 114<br />

III. Response to the UPD<br />

Allport’s manuscript circulated among members <strong>of</strong> the CAR directly upon its receipt in<br />

New York. And, in response its readers were enthusiastic and uniformly appreciative <strong>of</strong> the<br />

quality and scope <strong>of</strong> his analysis. P<strong>of</strong>fenberger sent Allport a handwritten note as soon as he<br />

completed reading the UPD: “It is just such a masterly job as I knew you would turn out. I<br />

want to express my personal appreciation over and above anything that the Council may<br />

formally say…My recommendation is to publish it as a special [SSRC] bulletin…” 115 Similarly,<br />

Edwin Nourse, Director <strong>of</strong> The Brookings Institution and CAR member, praised the study to<br />

Allport in a letter on October 27: “I want to express the very great enjoyment and intellectual<br />

stimulus that I derived from reading this document.” He went on to report that enough<br />

members <strong>of</strong> the CAR were present at a PPC meeting two days earlier that they had “come to a<br />

unanimous and enthusiastic decision to print this report promptly as a Council bulletin.” 116<br />

Their motivation to move ahead quickly with publication rested upon both “the high quality <strong>of</strong><br />

the review and its methodological value for others preparing similar appraisals.” 117 It is<br />

noteworthy that Allport’s work would appear in print prior to the already completed report <strong>of</strong><br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

13<br />

Angell regarding documents in sociology. The <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong> in Psychological<br />

Science was published in March, 1942 and reissued in 1951. 118<br />

Judged by its sales, Allport’s text was one <strong>of</strong> the CAR’s more successful volumes. In its<br />

first month <strong>of</strong> distribution, the SSRC sold an average <strong>of</strong> 10 copies per day, a rate higher than<br />

any other bulletin in the past. 119 Three years following the UPD’s publication, Ernest Burgess<br />

reported that 1100 copies had been distributed compared with 450 copies <strong>of</strong> the text by<br />

Gottschalk, Kluckhohn and Angell during its first two years in press. 120 Among the earlier CAR<br />

appraisal texts, Blumer’s well-received review <strong>of</strong> The Polish Peasant had sold just 465 copies<br />

in its first two years <strong>of</strong> distribution (September, 1939-September, 1941). 121<br />

Published reviews <strong>of</strong> the UPD were not extensive and those which appeared tended to<br />

reflect judgments congenial to the reviewers’ overall methodological allegiances. The text was<br />

ignored by most psychology journals: no mention <strong>of</strong> it is made in Psychological Bulletin, which<br />

published many reviews annually. Only two psychologists, Ralph K. White <strong>of</strong> Cornell University<br />

and Peter Hampton <strong>of</strong> Western <strong>College</strong> (Oxford, Ohio), seem to have commented on it in print<br />

while at least three notices by sociologists were forthcoming (from Robert Park, then at Fisk<br />

University; George A. Lundberg <strong>of</strong> Bennington <strong>College</strong>; and Emory S. Bogardus, University <strong>of</strong><br />

Southern California). 122<br />

Peter Hampton’s 1944 review, the longest given the UPD, strongly sided with Allport’s<br />

attention to the “vertical dimension <strong>of</strong> behavior in a single individual” and questioned the<br />

ability <strong>of</strong> positivist statistical methods ever to appreciate the unique personality. “The real<br />

failing <strong>of</strong> the psychologist today,” Hampton argued, “is his inability or rather hesitancy in<br />

using his own mind in interpreting personality.” 123 In a stance foreshadowing contemporary<br />

themes <strong>of</strong> qualitative research validation and narrative psychological perspectives, Hampton<br />

saw the issue <strong>of</strong> “congruence” as central to the validity <strong>of</strong> documentary investigative methods<br />

and held that Allport’s balanced view <strong>of</strong> the matter was persuasive. 124 White concluded that<br />

Allport’s appraisal “<strong>of</strong>fers a wholly satisfying synthesis.” 125 Allport’s allegiance to the familiar<br />

tripartite test <strong>of</strong> science--understanding, prediction, and control--in the UPD’s chapter 11 (The<br />

Case for <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong>, “the most original in the book”) was justified: idiographic<br />

analyses <strong>of</strong> personal documents could meet these criteria.<br />

Among Allport’s sociological colleagues, both Bogardus and Park were, on balance,<br />

favorably disposed to the text while Lundberg <strong>of</strong>fered a qualified dissent. Bogardus’ reserved<br />

enthusiasm judged that ”the author has maintained a reasonable degree <strong>of</strong> objectivity,<br />

although evidently favorable from the start toward personal documents as scientific materials”<br />

and finds Allport’s canvass <strong>of</strong> the topic extensive and thorough. 126 Park was satisfied with the<br />

UPD as it provided “a particularly lucid survey, based on a wide knowledge <strong>of</strong> the literature at<br />

home and abroad, <strong>of</strong> the logical problems involved in the use <strong>of</strong> personal documents…for the<br />

understanding <strong>of</strong> human nature and society.” Yet he believed that Allport had failed “to<br />

indicate how these same documents could be used by idiographic sciences—for example,<br />

cultural anthropology and psychoanalysis—which are not sciences at all in the more austere<br />

sense <strong>of</strong> statistical and behavioristic psychology.” 127 Lundberg’s evaluation was judicious, if<br />

ultimately negative. He praised its survey <strong>of</strong> the history and use <strong>of</strong> personal documents, but<br />

immediately challenged Allport’s analytic method: Part III (The Value <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong>)<br />

<strong>of</strong> the text “[repeats] the principal fallacies that have characterized discussions <strong>of</strong> this subject<br />

in the past…In this connection arises the old question as to whether one may generalize from<br />

a singe case. The author’s affirmative conclusion on this point and its attendant reasoning is<br />

in the opinion <strong>of</strong> this reviewer, simply fallacious.” He then cited his critique <strong>of</strong> case<br />

methodology published in Sociometry and invited his readers to review his argumentation<br />

there. 128<br />

What was Allport’s own evaluation <strong>of</strong> the UPD at the time he completed it? Comments in<br />

his correspondence indicate that he was quite apprehensive about this work and felt he had<br />

not achieved what he hoped he might. In response to P<strong>of</strong>fenberger’s handwritten note <strong>of</strong><br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

14<br />

appreciation, Allport immediately poured out sentiments which, in their frankness, had not<br />

appeared previously in their exchanges:<br />

You have no idea what a relief it was to receive your unsolicited note expressing satisfaction with<br />

my report. The later was written with so much distraction and pressure <strong>of</strong> defense work that I<br />

finally sent it <strong>of</strong>f with feelings <strong>of</strong> distress and failure. As a review I felt that it was adequate, but<br />

the burning central problem was left unsolved, and I did not even feel that I had pushed the<br />

solution very far forward. 129<br />

His next comments suggest the ambition which weighed upon his efforts:<br />

To render the logic <strong>of</strong> the case method acceptable to hard-headed American empiricists is a long<br />

and difficult job…I know I shall be soundly abused for my own failures to speak the last and<br />

convincing words. But it is simply too big a job, and one that requires more time and effort than<br />

the three months at my disposal” to complete the task. 130<br />

Earlier he had confided to Crane: “I do not feel entirely satisfied with the product because I<br />

think it is a full year’s job to write a good book on the subject.” 131 He also expressed his sense<br />

that the UPD was not quite sufficient a response to positivist claims in responding to Nourse’s<br />

positive comments: “I am only too aware <strong>of</strong> my failure to solve the riddle <strong>of</strong> the single case in<br />

a statistical conception <strong>of</strong> science, but shall feel satisfied if I have at least stated the riddle<br />

clearly enough to encourage others to work with me toward the solution.” 132<br />

IV. Reappraising the UPD<br />

What conclusions might we draw from these data to better understand Allport and his<br />

monograph almost sixty years after it was first issued? <strong>Le</strong>t me suggest a number <strong>of</strong><br />

suppositions which appear justified by the evidence I’ve gathered and presented here.<br />

First, the UPD stands as a more methodologically-focused though highly rhetorical<br />

successor to specific earlier appeals by Allport (e.g., in 1937 and 1940) for psychology to alter<br />

its embrace <strong>of</strong> what he believed to be a hopelessly narrow (and, therefore, fatally-flawed)<br />

experimentalism. The urgency <strong>of</strong> his expression lay in a pr<strong>of</strong>ound conviction that contemporary<br />

psychology’s quantitative and operationalized methods, without the counterbalance <strong>of</strong><br />

idiographic approaches, would fail to generate the social intelligence required to root out<br />

totalitarian instincts and advance democratic ideals then under severe assault. Further, he<br />

doubted whether quantitative results would ever permit psychology to claim title to itself as a<br />

science <strong>of</strong> the individual.<br />

Secondly, Allport’s apprehension that he had not achieved a convincing settlement <strong>of</strong><br />

the problem <strong>of</strong> the “case method” proved prophetic among psychologists during the decades<br />

following the UPD’s publication. As his reviewers’ comments suggest, his arguments were not<br />

persuasive to those whose investigative toolkit required quantification, operationalization, and<br />

direct manipulation to authenticate the scientific quality <strong>of</strong> their work. As Danziger argues,<br />

some psychologists in his audience were committed to an early “stimulus-response” framework<br />

while others had begun to adopt the metalanguage <strong>of</strong> “variables” by the beginning <strong>of</strong> the<br />

1950s. 133 In either case, Allport’s stance was rejected and it would take over three decades<br />

before similar arguments on method could be heard once more in psychology. The abiding<br />

attitude in the generation following the UPD’s appearance was expressed in Robert Holt’s<br />

stinging 1962 description <strong>of</strong> Allport’s stance as a hardy “perennial weed in psychology’s<br />

conceptual garden”. 134<br />

Thirdly, the circle <strong>of</strong> researchers to whom the SSRC represented a vital component <strong>of</strong><br />

their pr<strong>of</strong>essional lives was narrow and tended to involve many <strong>of</strong> the same individuals in<br />

multiple capacities. The names <strong>of</strong> Allport, P<strong>of</strong>fenberger, Burgess, Nourse, Angell, Wirth,<br />

Blumer, and others appear repeatedly in the records <strong>of</strong> the PPC and the Council itself<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

15<br />

throughout the 1930s and early 1940s. The difficulty experienced by the CAR when it failed to<br />

commission an appraisal <strong>of</strong> a psychological research text may have been symptomatic <strong>of</strong> a<br />

deeper disjunction: while some psychologists were among the SSRC’s major supporters,<br />

important experimental voices in psychology had little to do with the organization. Indeed, it is<br />

hard to escape a sense that the Committee on Appraisal <strong>of</strong> Research never fully engaged the<br />

mainstream positivist tradition in its work and was, therefore, never as influential as it had<br />

hoped it would be.<br />

Fourthly, this review should not be construed as a Whig history. In light <strong>of</strong><br />

contemporary standards, it is clear that Allport was not interested or sensitive to some factors<br />

which today might be obvious: culture, ethnicity, language, gender, or the very format <strong>of</strong> the<br />

experimental situation as a social variable. 135 As Bruner states, “how little we considered the<br />

extent to which the dialectic <strong>of</strong> the culture could itself give shape to how one chose to<br />

construct a version <strong>of</strong> the self extended over a lifetime.” 136 Wittgensteinian understandings <strong>of</strong><br />

language as a game and the role <strong>of</strong> such game playing when persons created documents are<br />

missing from the kinds <strong>of</strong> analyses which Allport <strong>of</strong>fered in the UPD. 137 The absence <strong>of</strong> these<br />

concerns however does not vitiate the UPD’s distinctive voice in its own day.<br />

Fifthly, I cannot avoid mention <strong>of</strong> Allport’s frequently-noted distaste for psychoanalytic<br />

approaches to personality. His antipathy may have influenced the UPD’s earnest quest for<br />

methodological alternatives to a pervasive Freudian influence in the interpretation <strong>of</strong> personal<br />

documentary materials from the 1920s onward. 138 Surely his classification <strong>of</strong> Freud as an<br />

example <strong>of</strong> an “uncritical” user <strong>of</strong> personal documents demonstrated Allport’s continuing<br />

refusal to regard psychoanalysis as scientific.<br />

Finally, we may be approaching a time in which it is possible to reassess Allport in a<br />

truly comprehensive fashion. Perhaps someone may begin work in the near future on that full<br />

biography <strong>of</strong> this central actor which the history <strong>of</strong> psychology lacks to its obvious detriment.<br />

In pursuing this research, I became aware <strong>of</strong> a growing sense <strong>of</strong> admiration for Allport. His<br />

writing demonstrates a felicity <strong>of</strong> expression which I envy. His archived papers en masse detail<br />

an individual who committed extraordinary energies at an exhausting pace on behalf <strong>of</strong> so<br />

many who sought his help. His rhetoric still retains a punch after more than half a century.<br />

And, his defense <strong>of</strong> the individual as inviolable continues to caution psychologists who mistake<br />

their data for the individual person whom they are studying. Researchers could do far worse<br />

than to begin to read again or for the first time the pr<strong>of</strong>essional and personal documents <strong>of</strong><br />

Allport’s own life.<br />

1 Gordon W. Allport, “Gordon W. Allport,“ in A History <strong>of</strong> Psychology in Autobiography, Vol. 5, eds.<br />

Edwin G. Boring and Gardner Lindzey (New York: Appleton-Century-Cr<strong>of</strong>ts, 1967), pp. 3-25. This essay<br />

also is reprinted in Gordon W. Allport, The Person in Psychology: Selected Essays by Gordon W. Allport<br />

(Boston, MA: Beacon Press, 1968), pp. 376-409. Given his deep interest in the development <strong>of</strong> the<br />

individual person across the lifespan, it is ironic that no substantial biography <strong>of</strong> Gordon W. Allport<br />

(henceforth, GWA) has appeared in years since his death. Thomas F. Pettigrew’s obituary <strong>of</strong> GWA [first<br />

published as “Gordon Willard Allport. 1897-1967,” Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity and Social Psychology 12<br />

(1969): 1-5] is reprinted with longer interviews <strong>of</strong> GWA in Richard I. Evans, Gordon Allport: The Man<br />

and His Ideas (New York: E. P. Dutton, 1970). Calvin S. Hall and Gardner Lindzey provide an excellent<br />

older introduction in their textbook, Theories <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity (2 nd ed.) (New York: John Wiley and Sons:<br />

1970), pp. 258-297. Alvin H. Smith <strong>of</strong>fers a helpful illustrated biographical essay, “Gordon W. Allport: A<br />

Becoming <strong>Personal</strong>ity,” pp. 356-363, in A Pictorial History <strong>of</strong> Psychology ed. Wolfgang G. Bringmann,<br />

Helmut E. Lück, Rudolf Miller, and Charles E. Early (Chicago: Quintessence Publishing Co., 1997).<br />

Shorter biographical references include “Allport, Gordon Willard,” in Biographical Dictionary <strong>of</strong><br />

Psychology eds. Noel P. Sheehy, Antony J. Chapman, and Wendy A. Conroy (London/New York:<br />

Routledge, 1997), pp. 9-11 and Phillip E. Vernon, “Allport, Gordon Willard (1897-1967)” in Encyclopedia<br />

<strong>of</strong> Psychology, Vol. 4, ed. Raymond J. Corsini (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1994, 2 nd ed.), p. 4.<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

16<br />

Colleagues, students, and researchers <strong>of</strong>fer helpful biographical glimpses <strong>of</strong> GWA among the essays in<br />

Kenneth H. Craik, Robert Hogan, and Raymond N. Wolfe (eds.), Fifty Years <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity Psychology<br />

(New York: Plenum Press, 1993). Biographical details <strong>of</strong> Allport’s earlier years are scattered throughout<br />

Ian A. M. Nicholson, “Gordon Allport, Character and ‘the Culture <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity,’ 1897-1937,” History <strong>of</strong><br />

Psychology 1 (1998): 52-68.<br />

2 Allport, 1967, p. 10. See, too, an appreciation <strong>of</strong> Stern in Gordon W. Allport, “The <strong>Personal</strong>istic<br />

Psychology <strong>of</strong> William Stern,” in Historical Roots <strong>of</strong> Contemporary Psychology ed. Benjamin B. Wolman<br />

(New York: Harper & Row, 1968), pp. 321-337.<br />

3 Gordon W. Allport, “The Study <strong>of</strong> the Undivided <strong>Personal</strong>ity,” Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal and Social<br />

Psychology 19 (1924): 132-142.<br />

4 See Gordon W. Allport, “The Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity by the Intuitive Method. An Experiment in Teaching<br />

from The Locomotive God,” Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal & Social Psychology 24 (1929): 14-27; “The<br />

Psychologist’s Frame <strong>of</strong> Reference,” Psychological Bulletin 37 (1940): 1-28; and “The General and the<br />

Unique in Psychological Science,” Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity 30 (1962): 405-422.<br />

5 Allport in Evans, 1967, p. 18.<br />

6 Walter Mischel, “Looking for <strong>Personal</strong>ity” in A Century <strong>of</strong> Psychology as Science, eds. Sigmund Koch<br />

and Dennis E. <strong>Le</strong>ary (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1985, reprinted 1992), 515-526.<br />

7 Allport, 1967, p. 3.<br />

8 Gordon W. Allport, The <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong> in Psychological Science: Prepared for the<br />

Committee on Appraisal <strong>of</strong> Research. Bulletin #49 (New York: Social Science Research Council, 1942).<br />

This monograph was subsequently reprinted in 1951. Henceforth, cited as UPD.<br />

9 UPD, p. xii.<br />

10 A frequency count by decade <strong>of</strong> the 198 items in the UPD bibliography suggests the recency <strong>of</strong> the<br />

literature reviewed. Item counts (percentages) show 1932-1941: 106 items (53.5%); 1922-1931: 60<br />

(30.3%); 1912-1921: 11 (5.6%); 1902-1911: 11 (5.6%); prior to 1902: 8 (4%); no date: 1 (0.5%);<br />

and, in press: 1 (0.5%)<br />

11 UPD, p. 148.<br />

12 UPD, p. 191<br />

13 Ken Plummer, <strong>Documents</strong> <strong>of</strong> Life: An Introduction to the Problems and Literature <strong>of</strong> a Humanistic<br />

Method (London: George Allen & Unwin, 1983), p. 35; Jennifer Platt, A History <strong>of</strong> Sociological Research<br />

Methods in America, 1920-1960 (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1996), p. 146; Lawrence S.<br />

Wrightsman, “Allport’s <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong>: Then and Now,” pp. 165-166 in Craik et al., 1993.<br />

14 Lawrence S. Wrightsman, “<strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong> as Data in Conceptualizing Adult <strong>Personal</strong>ity<br />

Development,” <strong>Personal</strong>ity and Social Psychology Bulletin 7 (1991): 367-385; Wrightsman, 1993. Note<br />

that the papers in Craik et al., 1993, were originally presented at a conference in 1987.<br />

15 See James H Capshew, Psychologists on the March: Science, Practice, and Pr<strong>of</strong>essional Identity in<br />

America, 1929-1969 (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1999) and Ellen Herman, The Romance <strong>of</strong><br />

American Psychology: Political Culture in the Age <strong>of</strong> Experts (Berkeley, CA: University <strong>of</strong> California<br />

Press, 1995).<br />

16 See Kurt Danziger, Constructing the Subject: Historical Origins <strong>of</strong> Psychological Research (Cambridge,<br />

UK: Cambridge University Press, 1990) and his Naming the Mind: How Psychology Found Its Language<br />

(London: Sage Publications, 1997). An understanding <strong>of</strong> the forces shaping American psychology<br />

receives sophisticated and integrated treatment in Roger Smith, The Norton History <strong>of</strong> the Human<br />

Sciences (New York: W. W. Norton & Company, 1997). Dorothy Ross links the development <strong>of</strong> the<br />

social sciences in the U.S. from the early 19 th century up to 1929 to their involvement in “the classical<br />

ideology <strong>of</strong> liberal individualism” and “the national identity <strong>of</strong> American exceptionalism” in The Origins <strong>of</strong><br />

American Social Science (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1991), pp. xiii, xiv. Other analyses <strong>of</strong><br />

these forces are found in James H. Capshew, “Psychologists on Site: A Reconnaissance <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Historiography <strong>of</strong> the Laboratory,” American Psychologist 47 (1992): 132-142; Jill G. Morawski (ed.),<br />

The Rise <strong>of</strong> Experimentation in American Psychology (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1988); Jill<br />

G. Morawski, “Educating the Emotions: Academic Psychology, Textbooks, and the Psychology Industry,<br />

1890-1940,” pp. 217-244 in Inventing the Psychological: Toward a Cultural History <strong>of</strong> Emotional Life in<br />

America eds. Joel Pfister and Nancy Schnog (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1997); and Michael<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

17<br />

M. Sokal (ed.), Psychological Testing and American Society, 1890-1930 (New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers<br />

University Press, 1987). Capshew, 1999, provides a detailed appreciation <strong>of</strong> the background and role <strong>of</strong><br />

psychologists during the Second World War in shaping the post-war discipline. Analyses <strong>of</strong> post-war<br />

trends are also found in Herman, 1995, and Phillip Cushman, Constructing the Self, Constructing<br />

America: A Cultural History <strong>of</strong> Psychotherapy (Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1995).<br />

17 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> January 25, 1940 from Carroll T. Pratt to Gordon W. Allport (henceforth, GWA). Papers <strong>of</strong><br />

Gordon W. Allport. HUG 4118.50, Box 4, Folder 135 “Presidential Speeches.” Harvard University<br />

Archives (henceforth, HUA). John Gilbert Beebe-Center, a Harvard colleague <strong>of</strong> Allport, was himself an<br />

experimental psychologist who published several papers with S. S. Stevens, one <strong>of</strong> Allport’s fiercest<br />

methodological critics.<br />

18 Contemporary critiques <strong>of</strong> positivism and the theoretical foundations <strong>of</strong> the social sciences (especially<br />

psychology) can be found in works such as Sacha Bem and Huib Looren de Jong, Theoretical Issues in<br />

Psychology: An Introduction (London, Sage Publications, 1997); Brent D. Slife and Richard N. Williams,<br />

What’s Behind the Research? Discovering Hidden Assumptions in the Behavioral Sciences (Thousand<br />

Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995); M. Brewster Smith, “Psychology and the Decline <strong>of</strong> Positivism: The Case for a<br />

Human Science,” pp. 53-69 in Perspectives on Behavioral Science: The Colorado <strong>Le</strong>ctures ed. Richard<br />

Jessor (Boulder, CO: Westview Press, 1991); and Jonathan A. Smith, Rom Harré, and Luk Van<br />

Langenhove (eds.), Rethinking Psychology (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995). References to alternative<br />

perspectives in psychology (including narrative, discursive, and cultural psychology) include Jerome S.<br />

Bruner, Acts <strong>of</strong> Meaning (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1990) and his “The Narrative<br />

Construction <strong>of</strong> Reality,” Critical Inquiry 18 (1991): 1-21; Michael Cole, Cultural Psychology: A Once and<br />

Future Discipline (Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press <strong>of</strong> the Harvard University Press, 1996); Rom Harré and<br />

Grant Gillett, The Discursive Mind (London, Sage Publications, 1994); Vincent W. Hevern, Resources for<br />

Narrative Psychology: Guide and Annotated Bibliography (Online; Syracuse, NY; Available on the WWW<br />

at http://maple.lemoyne.edu/~hevern/narpsych.html); George S. Howard, “Culture Tales: A Narrative<br />

Approach to Thinking, Cross-Cultural Psychology and Psychotherapy,” American Psychologist 46 (1991):<br />

187-197; Dan P. McAdams, The Stories We Live By: <strong>Personal</strong> Myths and the Making <strong>of</strong> the Self (New<br />

York: William Morrow, 1993); Theodore R. Sarbin (ed.), Narrative Psychology: The Storied Nature <strong>of</strong><br />

Human Conduct (New York: Praeger, 1986); and James W. Stigler, Richard A. Shweder, and Gilbert<br />

Herdt (eds.), Cultural Psychology: Essays on Human Development (New York: Cambridge University<br />

Press, 1990).<br />

19 The cultural aspirations <strong>of</strong> psychological science in the 19 th century and earlier receives fine<br />

explication in Gustav Jahoda, Crossroads Between Culture and Mind: Continuities and Change in<br />

Theories <strong>of</strong> Human Nature (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1993). Wundt’s “second<br />

psychology” and its modern status is described by Emily D. Cahan and Sheldon H. White, “Proposals for<br />

a Second Psychology,” American Psychologist 47 (1992): 224-235. Contemporary concerns with<br />

biographical and life history data have been detailed by Amia Lieblich, Rivka Tuval-Mashiach, and Tamar<br />

Zilber, Narrative Research: Reading, Analysis, and Interpretation (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage<br />

Publications, 1998); Louis M. Smith, “Biographical Methods,” pp. 286-305, and D. Jean Clandinin and F.<br />

Michael Connelly, “<strong>Personal</strong> Experience Methods,” pp. 413-427 in Handbook <strong>of</strong> Qualitative Research,<br />

eds. Normal K. Denzin and Yvonna Y. Lincoln (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, 1994); Plummer,<br />

1983; and William McK. Runyan, Life Histories and Psychobiography: Explorations in Theory and<br />

Development (New York: Oxford University Press, 1984).<br />

20 Olivier Zunz, Why the American Century? (Chicago, IL: University <strong>of</strong> Chicago Press, 1998), xi-xii. See,<br />

too, Theodore M. Porter, The Rise <strong>of</strong> Statistical Thinking, 1820-1900 (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University<br />

Press, 1986) and his subsequent study, Trust in Numbers: The Pursuit <strong>of</strong> Objectivity in Science and<br />

Public Life (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1995) for an understanding <strong>of</strong> the emerging<br />

scientific production <strong>of</strong> knowledge in modern Western culture.<br />

21 Quoted by Zunz, p. 36. Day was the original chair <strong>of</strong> the Committee on Appraisal <strong>of</strong> Research (CAR)<br />

which sponsored Allport’s monograph. Following work at the Rockefeller Foundation, Day was<br />

successively Dean <strong>of</strong> Social Sciences at the University <strong>of</strong> Chicago and President <strong>of</strong> Cornell University.<br />

22 Zunz, pp. 36-39. The history <strong>of</strong> the LSRM can be found in Martin Bulmer and Joan Bulmer,<br />

“Philanthropy and Social Science in the 1920s: Beardsley Ruml and the Laura Spelman Rockefeller<br />

Memorial, 1922-29,” Minerva 19 (1981), 347-407. A helpful overview <strong>of</strong> the role <strong>of</strong> philanthropic and<br />

other sources <strong>of</strong> funding in the social sciences, particularly sociology, is found in Chapter 5 (“Funding<br />

and Research Methods,” pp. 142-199) <strong>of</strong> Platt, 1996. Other research includes David C. Hammack and<br />

Stanton Wheeler, Social Science in the Making: Essays on the Russell Sage Foundation, 1907-1972<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

18<br />

(New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1994); Barry D. Karl and Stanley N. Katz, “The American Private<br />

Philanthropic Foundation and the Public Sphere, 1890-1930,” Minerva 19 (1981): 251-257; and Ross,<br />

1991, pp. 400-407. Ross, 1991, points out that in just one seven-year period (1922-1929), the LSRM<br />

and SSRC “dispensed about forty-one million dollars to American social science, social work, and their<br />

institutions” (p. 402). A contended thesis linking the Rockefeller Foundation to specific programmatic<br />

decisions <strong>of</strong> the SSRC is advanced by Donald Fisher, “The Role <strong>of</strong> Philanthropic Foundations in the<br />

Reproduction and Production <strong>of</strong> Hegemony: The Rockefeller Foundations and the Social Sciences,”<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> the British Sociological Association 17 (May, 1983): 206-233 His analysis is challenged by<br />

Martin Bulmer, “Philanthropic Foundations and the Development <strong>of</strong> the Social Sciences in the Early<br />

Twentieth Century: A Reply to Donald Fisher,” Sociology, 18 (November 1984): 572-578 and Salma<br />

Ahmad, “American Foundations and the Development <strong>of</strong> the Social Sciences between the Wars:<br />

Comment on the Debate between Martin Bulmer and Donald Fisher,” Sociology 25 (1991): 511-520.<br />

23 Donald Fisher, Fundamental Development <strong>of</strong> the Social Sciences: Rockefeller Philanthropy and the<br />

United States Social Science Research Council (Ann Arbor, MI: The University <strong>of</strong> Michigan Press, 1993).<br />

Fisher’s is the first major reexamination <strong>of</strong> the SSRC since the opening <strong>of</strong> the organization’s archives to<br />

outsider researchers. See, too, Kenneth Prewett, “A Brief History <strong>of</strong> the SSRC,” (on-line, no date,<br />

downloaded July 30, 1999, available on the WWW at http://www.ssrc.org/histbri.htm). Also, “Social<br />

Science Research Council Archives, 1924-1990” (on-line at the Rockefeller Archives Center [RAC], no<br />

date; downloaded July 1, 1999, available on the WWW at<br />

http://www.rockefeller.edu/archive.ctr/ssrc.html). Prewett observes that “the Council would deal only<br />

with such problems as involve two or more disciplines” in order to achieve a “cross-fertilization <strong>of</strong> the<br />

social disciplines.” The constituent societies <strong>of</strong> the SSRC in the late 1930s were the American<br />

Anthropological Association, American Economic Association, American Historical Association, American<br />

Political Science Association, American Psychological Association, American Sociological Society, and<br />

American Statistical Association.<br />

24 See especially Fisher, 1993, chapter 6 (pp. 199-227) for an overall view <strong>of</strong> the SSRC’s impact. In<br />

pursuing research, the central Policies and Procedures Committee (PPC) <strong>of</strong> the SSRC normally appointed<br />

freestanding committees <strong>of</strong> academics; in turn, these committees initiated, supervised, and evaluated<br />

specific research studies according to the mandate (and funding) given by the PPC and reported their<br />

results back to the both the PPC and the Council itself<br />

25 Ibid.<br />

26 Prewett, loc. cit.<br />

27 Fisher, 1993, p. 206. Stuart A. Rice (ed.) Methods in Social Science: A Case Book (Chicago, IL:<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Chicago Press, 1931). See Ross, 1991, pp. 401-402 for a discussion <strong>of</strong> SSRC work on<br />

methodology in the 1920s. Prewett, loc. cit., points out that the singular “Method” <strong>of</strong> the committee’s<br />

title became the plural “Methods” <strong>of</strong> their book’s title.<br />

28 Fisher, 1993, pp. 204-205.<br />

29 Fisher, 1993, pp. 167-172. Fisher sets this debate within the context <strong>of</strong> both personality and<br />

management struggles within the Council, especially regarding SSRC Executive Director Robert T. Crane<br />

and the imminent end <strong>of</strong> Rockefeller financial support in 1940.<br />

30 Fisher, 1993, p. 171. Siding with Bulmer and Bulmer, 1981, Platt, 1996, strenuously disagrees with<br />

Fisher’s 1993 thesis that Rockefeller philanthropy directly influenced the specific topics and strategies<br />

adopted by the SSRC.<br />

31 Fisher, 1993, pp. 172-178, reviews the origins, content, and impact <strong>of</strong> Wirth’s report. Quotation from<br />

the CRCP’s report to the Council is given by Fisher, p. 175.<br />

32 Fisher, 1993, p. 176.<br />

33 PPC Minutes, September 15, 1937. In Record Group: Committee on Appraisal, 1937-46; Series I.<br />

Committee Reports. Box 118, Folder 638, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> CAR, SSRC Archives (RAC), p. 6. The<br />

primary archival record <strong>of</strong> the CAR is found in this folder, #638, in Box 118. It consists <strong>of</strong> a single bound<br />

volume in which both complete and excerpted agenda, reports, and Minutes <strong>of</strong> meetings <strong>of</strong> the CAR,<br />

PPC, and the Council itself – all as they relate to the CAR – have been grouped together chronologically<br />

to form a sequential narrative <strong>of</strong> the CAR’s functioning; this volume will be cited henceforth as Various<br />

Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR.<br />

34 Nourse is quoted by Fisher, 1993, p. 177.<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

19<br />

35 PPC Minutes, December 12, 1937, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, pp. 7-10. This discussion generated<br />

one <strong>of</strong> the longest set <strong>of</strong> minutes found among the records <strong>of</strong> the CAR. Fears resurfaced at this meeting<br />

which had been voiced initially in the debate founding the CAR and would be echoed in later years,<br />

namely, that appraisals <strong>of</strong> significant social science research would result in nothing more than “glorified<br />

book reviews” (ibid., p. 8). One respondent countered that such an outcome might be salutary, given<br />

the sorry state <strong>of</strong> book reviewing in the social sciences generally.<br />

36 Day was succeeded by Robert Redfield as CAR Chairman in 1939. PPC Minutes, April 14, 1939,<br />

Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 20.<br />

37 PPC Minutes, January 28, 1940, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 32. The formal expansion <strong>of</strong> the CAR’s<br />

mandate came two months later (PPC Minutes, March 15, 1940, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 33).<br />

38 Council Minutes, April 2, 1938, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 11.<br />

39 The decision to seek a review is documented in the CAR Meeting Minutes, April 3, 1938, Various<br />

Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 81. The two proposed psychology texts were Louis L. Thurstone, The Vectors <strong>of</strong><br />

Mind: Multiple-Factor Analysis for the Isolation <strong>of</strong> Primary Traits (Chicago, IL: University <strong>of</strong> Chicago<br />

Press, 1935) and Gordon W. Allport, <strong>Personal</strong>ity: A Psychological Interpretation (New York: Henry Holt<br />

and Co., 1937). The inability to reach a decision was announced by P<strong>of</strong>fenberger to the Council at its<br />

Fall meeting (Council Minutes, September 13-15, 1938, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 14).<br />

40 William I. Thomas and Florian Znaniecki, The Polish Peasant in Europe and America. (Boston, MA:<br />

Gorham Press, Vols. I and II, 1918; Vol. III, 1919; Vols. IV and V, 1920; New York: Knopf, two volume<br />

ed., 1927). Herbert Blumer, An Appraisal <strong>of</strong> Thomas and Znaniecki’s The Polish Peasant in Europe and<br />

America. Bulletin 44. (New York: Social Science Council, 1939).<br />

41 A transcript <strong>of</strong> deliberations at this conference was incorporated along with Blumer’s appraisal and<br />

issued as a single SSRC bulletin (Blumer, 1939).<br />

42 CAR Meeting Minutes, July 28, 1940, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 89. In the UPD, Allport is quite<br />

sensitive to Blumer’s analysis and responds to that critique at several points, e.g., pp. 147, 151 and the<br />

entire Chapter 12, “The Problem <strong>of</strong> Conceptualization.” He also cites his own experience in his justcompleted<br />

analyses <strong>of</strong> the autobiographies <strong>of</strong> German refugees: “none <strong>of</strong> the major conclusions <strong>of</strong> the<br />

study were anticipated in the pre-arranged schedule for analysis—pro<strong>of</strong> that it is possible for the<br />

investigator to take from personal documents more than he puts in.” (UPD, p. 82). He is referring to<br />

Gordon W. Allport, Jerome S. Bruner, and E. M. Jandorf, “<strong>Personal</strong>ity under Social Catastrophe: Ninety<br />

Life-histories <strong>of</strong> the Nazi Revolution,” Character and <strong>Personal</strong>ity 10 (1941): 1-22.<br />

43 Council Minutes, September 10-12, 1940, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, pp. 37-41. The quote is found<br />

on p. 40. The undated memorandum <strong>of</strong> Burgess to which Redfield refers in his statement to the Council<br />

(p. 39) is “The <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Personal</strong> Document in Sociology” in Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, pp. 93-100,<br />

where it is appended to the CAR meeting Minutes <strong>of</strong> October 20, 1940. The research under review was<br />

Robert C. Angell, The Family Encounters the Depression (New York: C. Scribner’s Sons, 1936).<br />

44 CAR Meeting Minutes, October 20, 1940, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, pp. 90-92.<br />

45 CAR Meeting Minutes, November 10, 1940, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, pp. 101-103.<br />

46 Nicholson, 1998. Ian Nicholson, “Moral Projects and Disciplinary Practices: Gordon Allport and the<br />

Development <strong>of</strong> American <strong>Personal</strong>ity Psychology (Ph.D. Dissertation, York University, 1996). For an<br />

more focused analysis <strong>of</strong> the Allportian value system’s impact upon his earliest course in personality<br />

theory, see Ian A. M. Nicholson, “To ‘Correlate Psychology and Social Ethics’: Gordon Allport and the<br />

First Course in American <strong>Personal</strong>ity Psychology,” Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity 65 (1997): 733-742.<br />

47 Nicholson, 1998, see especially pp. 60-63.<br />

48 Allport, 1937, especially Chapter 8 “The Mature <strong>Personal</strong>ity” (pp. 213-231).<br />

49 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> Jerome S. Bruner to author, dated 4 March 1999.<br />

50 UPD, p. 148.<br />

51 UPD, p. 161.<br />

52 UPD, p. 132. Emphasis in the original.<br />

53 UPD, p. 182.<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

20<br />

54 Katherine Pandora, Rebels within the Ranks: Psychologists’ Critique <strong>of</strong> Scientific Authority and<br />

Democratic Realities in New Deal America (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1997).<br />

55 Ibid., esp. pp. 111-118.<br />

56 Ibid., p. 3.<br />

57 Ibid., esp. pp. 61-75; 84-89.<br />

58 “Critical Report on S. S. R. C. Monograph Predictions <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> Adjustment,” submitted to Dr.<br />

Robert T. Crane, June 2, 1941 in Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA, HUG4118.50, Box 3, Folder 94. HUA. Quote from p. 4<br />

<strong>of</strong> manuscript. Emphasis in the original. The monograph was published as Paul Horts, Paul Walling, and<br />

Louis Guttman (eds.), The Prediction <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> Adjustment: A Survey <strong>of</strong> Logical Problems and<br />

Research Techniques. Bulletin 48. (New York: Social Science Research Council, 1941)<br />

59 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> October 1, 1941 by GWA to Robert T. Crane. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA in Box 3, Folder 94 (Committee<br />

on Appraisal 1941). HUG4118.50. HUA; UPD, p. 156.<br />

60 Zunz, 1999, p. 68.<br />

61 “Psychology in the Near Future.” HUG4118.60. Box 3, Folder 87. HUA. Pandora, 1997, is uncertain<br />

about the dating <strong>of</strong> his speech and indicated it was either 1940 or 1941 (see her endnote #52, p. 188).<br />

While the manuscript itself is undated, a copy <strong>of</strong> the dated program (“Program <strong>of</strong> the 75 th Anniversary<br />

New England Intercollegiate Conference on Psychology, University <strong>of</strong> New Hampshire.”) is located in the<br />

same folder as the text in the HUA and secures 1941 as the year <strong>of</strong> its presentation.<br />

62 “Psychology in the Near Future,” p. 11a. In line with the practice <strong>of</strong> his generation, Allport almost<br />

always employs the masculine pronoun in reference to the anonymous other. I’ve not altered these uses<br />

in most cases though I recognize that they do sound jarring to present day ears.<br />

63 UPD, p. 149.<br />

64 Important expressions <strong>of</strong> experimental operationism in psychology to which Allport responded<br />

included Arthur G. Bills, “Changing Views <strong>of</strong> Psychology as Science,” Psychological Review 45 (1938):<br />

377-394; Clark L. Hull, “Mind, Mechanism, and Adaptive Behavior,” Psychological Review 44 (1937): 1-<br />

32; Stanley S. Stevens, “Psychology and the Science <strong>of</strong> Science,” Psychological Bulletin 36 (1939): 221-<br />

263; and Edward C. Tolman, “The Determination <strong>of</strong> Behavior by Stimuli-Past and To Come,”<br />

Psychological Review 45 (1938): 1-41. Bills’ 1938 paper challenged Allport directly in its famous<br />

definition <strong>of</strong> psychology as science: “At present the following criteria are widely accepted: that<br />

psychology as science is empirical, mechanistic, quantitative, nomothetic, analytic, and operational” (p.<br />

377). Allport’s direct response is found in his presidential address (see Note #74 below).<br />

65 Until 1937, APA presidential addresses appeared in the Psychological Review. Subsequently they were<br />

published in the Psychological Bulletin. Both journals, though, printed them as the first article <strong>of</strong> a new<br />

volume.<br />

66 Louis L. Thurstone, “The Vectors <strong>of</strong> Mind,” Psychological Review 41 (1934): 1-32; Joseph Peterson,<br />

“Aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>Le</strong>arning,” Psychological Review 42 (1935): 1-27; Hull, 1937; Tolman, 1938; and John<br />

Frederick Dashiell, “Some Rapproachement in Contemporary Psychology,” Psychological Bulletin 36<br />

(1939): 1-24.<br />

67 Karl E. Scheibe, “Metamorphoses in the Psychologist’s Advantage,” in The Rise <strong>of</strong> Experimentation in<br />

American Psychology, ed. Jill G. Morawski (New Haven: Yale University Press), p. 63.<br />

68 Allport, 1940. He delivered this address on September 7, 1939.<br />

69 Allport, 1940, p. 12.<br />

70 The research underlying Allport’s empirical analysis in his 1939 presidential address is detailed in<br />

Jerome S. Bruner and Gordon W. Allport, “Fifty Years <strong>of</strong> Change in American Psychology,” Psychological<br />

Bulletin 37 (1940): 757-776.<br />

71 Ibid., p. 22.<br />

72 Allport’s decision to cite Jaensch may have been a purposely satiric one grounded in a knowledge that<br />

the German’s experimental animal research work was used to justify Nazi racial policies. For example,<br />

see E. R. Jaensch, Der Huehnerh<strong>of</strong> als Forschungs- und Aufklaerungsmittel in menschlichen<br />

Rassenfragen. [The Chicken Yard as a Medium for Research and Explanation <strong>of</strong> Human Race Problems].<br />

(Berlin: Verlag Paul Parey, 1939).<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

21<br />

73 Allport, 1940, p. 26.<br />

74 Ibid. Emphases are in the original. See Bills, 1938, above.<br />

75 Nicole B. Barenbaum, “The Case(s) <strong>of</strong> Gordon Allport,” Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity 65 (1997): 743-755.<br />

Barenbaum cites several critics who take Allport to task; these include D. Capps, “An Allportian Analysis<br />

<strong>of</strong> Augustine,” International Journal for the Psychology <strong>of</strong> Religion 4 (1994): 205-228; Robert R. Holt,<br />

Methods in Clinical Psychology (New York: Plenum, 1978); and Christopher Peterson, <strong>Personal</strong>ity (San<br />

Diego: Harcourt, Brace, 1988). The published case study by Allport is found in his <strong>Le</strong>tters from Jenny<br />

(New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1965). David G. Winter <strong>of</strong>fers a crucial analysis <strong>of</strong> this work and its<br />

origins in “Gordon Allport and ‘<strong>Le</strong>tters from Jenny’,” in Craik et al., 1993, pp. 147-163.<br />

76 Nicole Barenbaum, “ ‘The Most Revealing Method <strong>of</strong> All’: Gordon Allport and Case Studies.” Paper<br />

presented at the annual meeting <strong>of</strong> Cheiron, Richmond, VA, June 19, 1997. She cites several published<br />

studies: A. L. Baldwin, “The Statistical Analysis <strong>of</strong> the Structure <strong>of</strong> a Single <strong>Personal</strong>ity,” Psychological<br />

Bulletin 37 (1940): 518-519; A. L. Baldwin, “<strong>Personal</strong> Structure Analysis: A Statistical Method for<br />

Investigating the Single <strong>Personal</strong>ity,” Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal and Social Psychology 37 (1942); D. Cartright<br />

and J. R. P. French, “The Reliability <strong>of</strong> Life-History Studies,” Character and <strong>Personal</strong>ity, 8, 110-119; and,<br />

Norman A. Polansky, “How Shall A Life-History Be Written?,” Character and <strong>Personal</strong>ity 9 (1941): 188-<br />

207.<br />

77 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> March 4, 1999 by Jerome S. Bruner to author. He also remembered “…that seminar…was<br />

made up <strong>of</strong> a bunch <strong>of</strong> strong-minded graduate students, much as he describes them in his (1967)<br />

autobiography.”<br />

78 William Ellery <strong>Le</strong>onard, The Locomotive-God (New York: Century Co., 1927). <strong>Le</strong>onard died on May 2,<br />

1944 at age 68. He was a poet and widely published scholar <strong>of</strong> English and Latin literature. His obituary<br />

appeared in the New York Herald Tribune, May 3, 1944, where he is cited for his Locomotive God and<br />

Two Lives: A Poem (privately printed in 1923; New York: B.W. Huebsch, 1925).<br />

79 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> April 5, 1928 by G.W.A. to Pr<strong>of</strong>essor W. E. <strong>Le</strong>onard. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. HUG4118.50. Box 2,<br />

Folder 56. W. E. <strong>Le</strong>onard: Correspondence and Misc. Material. HUA.<br />

80 Ibid. The compendium <strong>of</strong> <strong>Le</strong>itfragen included 128 items geared to the 19 chapters <strong>of</strong> the Locomotive<br />

God. A mimeographed copy <strong>of</strong> this 17-page listing, entitled “Case Study #1. The Locomotive God by<br />

William Ellery <strong>Le</strong>onard. Questions for Consideration,” is found in Papers <strong>of</strong> G.W.A. HUG4118.50. Box 2,<br />

Folder 57. W. E. <strong>Le</strong>onard: Locomotive God. HUA.<br />

81 The article appeared a year later as Allport, 1929.<br />

82 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> January 6, 1938 by GWA to Pr<strong>of</strong>essor W. E. <strong>Le</strong>onard. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. HUG4118.50. Box 2,<br />

Folder 56. W. E. <strong>Le</strong>onard: Correspondence and Misc. Material. HUA. In a close reading <strong>of</strong> archival and<br />

published sources, Nicole Barenbaum argues that Allport’s changing understanding <strong>of</strong> <strong>Le</strong>onard’s case in<br />

the late 1920s and 1930s reveals a transition from the behavioral orientation <strong>of</strong> his doctoral thesis to<br />

the concept <strong>of</strong> “functional autonomy” <strong>of</strong> his classic personality text (Nichole Barenbaum, “From Habits<br />

To Functional Autonomy: Gordon Allport And The Locomotive-God.” Paper presented at the symposium<br />

“The Allport Brothers” at the meeting <strong>of</strong> the Canadian Psychological Association, Toronto, June 12,<br />

1997.)<br />

83 Case <strong>of</strong> M.B. “Repression or Shell Shock.” Folder 50; Case <strong>of</strong> “Miller.” Folder 52.; Case <strong>of</strong> “Johnny<br />

Rocco.” Folder 54; H. G. Wells: Experiment in Autobiography. “Questions for Discussion.” Psychology<br />

16b 1937-1938. Folder 55. These are all found in Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA HUG4118.50. Box 2. HUA. Jean Evans<br />

originally published two separate case studies which Allport used in class: Jean Evans, “Johnny Rocco,”<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal and Social Psychology 43 (1948): 357-383 and “Miller: A Case Report,” Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Abnormal and Social Psychology 45 (1950): 359-479. She later published these and another case jointly<br />

in Jean Evans, Three Men: An Experiment in the Biography <strong>of</strong> Emotion (New York: Knopf, 1954) for<br />

which Allport wrote an introduction. Wells’ text appeared as H. G. (Herbert George) Wells, Experiment in<br />

Autobiography: Discoveries and Conclusions <strong>of</strong> a Very Ordinary Brain (New York: The Macmillan<br />

Company, 1934).<br />

84 Allport’s pedagogy is detailed by one <strong>of</strong> his Radcliffe <strong>College</strong> students, Pauline B. Hahn, “Johnny<br />

Rocco—Teaching Material for Elementary Students,” Journal <strong>of</strong> Abnormal and Social Psychology 43<br />

(1948): 384-390.<br />

85 UPD, p. 63, emphasis in original. A contemporary exemplum <strong>of</strong> Allport’s claim is illustrated in two<br />

highly acclaimed works <strong>of</strong> Louis A. Sass which employ the personal writings <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics: Madness<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

22<br />

and Modernism: Insanity in the Light <strong>of</strong> Modern Art, Literature, and Thought (Cambridge, MA: Harvard<br />

University Press, 1994) and The Paradoxes <strong>of</strong> Delusion: Wittgenstein, Schreber and the Schizophrenic<br />

Mind (Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 1995).<br />

86 UPD, p. 48.<br />

87 UPD, p. 51.<br />

88 Report <strong>of</strong> the Committee on Displaced Foreign Psychologists (CDFP), p. 718, in “Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Forty-Eighth Annual Meeting <strong>of</strong> the American Psychological Association, Inc., Pennsylvania State<br />

<strong>College</strong>, September 4, 5, 6, 7, 1940,” Psychological Bulletin 37 (1940): 699-741.<br />

89 An annual report <strong>of</strong> the CDFP was submitted to the APA between 1939-1942: Psychological Bulletin 36<br />

(1939), pp. 761-765; 37 (1940), pp. 715-718; 38 (1941), pp. 843-844; 39 (1942), pp. 733-734.<br />

90 Report <strong>of</strong> the CDFP in Psychological Bulletin 36 (1939), p. 761.<br />

91 James T. Lamiell, “William Stern: More Than ‘The IQ Guy’,” in Portraits <strong>of</strong> Pioneers in Psychology,<br />

Volume II, eds. Gregory A. Kimble, C. Alan Boneau, and Michael Wertheimer (Washington, DC:<br />

American Psychological Association and Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1996), pp. 73-85.<br />

92 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> September 17, 1937 by Charlotte Bühler to GWA. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. HUG4118.10.<br />

Miscellaneous Correspondence 1930-1945. Box 1. Folder: Bühler, Karl & Charlotte. HUA.<br />

93 Detailed correspondence exists among the GWA papers in which Allport took a lead in sponsoring<br />

Stern’s entrance to the U.S. and in coping with an escalating set <strong>of</strong> requests by Charlotte Bühler who<br />

had some difficulty coping with American cultural norms vis-à-vis the pr<strong>of</strong>essorate. See Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA.<br />

HUG4118.10. Miscellaneous Correspondence 1930-1945. Box 10. Folder: Stern, William, Gunther, and<br />

Clara and the Bühler folder previously cited. HUA.<br />

94 Among notes Allport used for a March 30, 1961 talk before the Harvard Historians on the “Origins <strong>of</strong><br />

Contemporary American Psychology,” he described his text in personality as “half German, half<br />

American” and joked it “belongs in the middle <strong>of</strong> the Atlantic Ocean”. He also noted that the highly<br />

European study <strong>of</strong> “Characterology” might not be a “major school” but it was “<strong>of</strong> chief interest to me.”<br />

HUG4118.60. Box 3. Folder 89, p. 6. HUA.<br />

95 “Psychology in the Near Future,” p. 14.<br />

96 Allport, 1937, esp. pp. 227-231, 395-398.<br />

97 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> December 17, 1940 by A. T. P<strong>of</strong>fenberger (henceforth, ATP) to GWA. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA, in Box<br />

3, Folder 94 (Committee on Appraisal 1941) HUG4118.50. HUA.<br />

98 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> December 23, 1940 by GWA to ATP. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA in Box 3, Folder 94 (Committee on<br />

Appraisal 1941). HUG4118.50. HUA. (Underlining in the original.)<br />

99 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> January 10, 1941 by ATP to GWA. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA in Box 3, Folder 94 (Committee on<br />

Appraisal 1941). HUG4118.50. HUA.<br />

100 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> January 13, 1941 by GWA to ATP. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA in Box 3, Folder 94 (Committee on<br />

Appraisal 1941). HUG4118.50. HUA.<br />

101 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> February 11, 1941 by ATP to GWA. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA in Box 3, Folder 94 (Committee on<br />

Appraisal 1941). HUG4118.50. HUA.<br />

102 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> March 6, 1941 by GWA to ATP. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA in Box 3, Folder 94 (Committee on Appraisal<br />

1941). HUG4118.50. HUA.<br />

103 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> March 11, 1951 by ATP to GWA. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA in Box 3, Folder 94 (Committee on<br />

Appraisal 1941). HUG4118.50. HUA.<br />

104 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> May 28, 1941 by GWA to ATP. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA in Box 8. HUG4118.10. HUA.<br />

105 “Guide for Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong>,” found in Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA, Box 3, Folder 94. HUG4118.50.<br />

HUA. Quote is from Allport, 1941, p. xv.<br />

106 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> May 28, 1941 by GWA to ATP. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA in Box 8. HUG4118.10. HUA.<br />

107 Undated draft letter by GWA to ATP. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA, Box 3, Folder 94, HUG4118.50. HUA. The dating<br />

<strong>of</strong> this letter is probably early in April, 1941 since P<strong>of</strong>fenberger replied to GWA on April 11, 1941<br />

acknowledging Allport’s progress report: “I am not surprised that the job turned out to be bigger than<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

23<br />

you anticipated” and assuring him that an October 1 submission would be okay. This letter resides<br />

immediately following the undated draft <strong>of</strong> GWA in the HUA.<br />

108 Karl M. Dallbach, “The Emergency Committee in Psychology, National Research Council,” American<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology 59 (1946): 496-582. “Report <strong>of</strong> the Division <strong>of</strong> Anthropology and Psychology <strong>of</strong><br />

the National Research Council,” Psychological Bulletin 30 (1941): 871-872. Capshew, 1999, discusses<br />

the mobilization <strong>of</strong> psychology for war work at length. Ladislas Farago and L. F. Gittler, German<br />

Psychological Warfare: Survey and Bibliography (New York: Committee for National Morale, June 1941).<br />

Farago alone edited the revised second edition <strong>of</strong> this volume which appeared soon afterward in<br />

September 1941.<br />

109 Capshew, 1999, p. 117.<br />

110 “Psychology in the Near Future.”<br />

111 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> August 8, 1941 by Ernest W. Burgess to GWA. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. Box 3, Folder 94.<br />

HUG4118.50. HUA.<br />

112 An undated 3-page manuscript beginning “Stauffer’s (sic) paper is the most lucid…” and covered with<br />

multiple corrections in Allport’s hand is found in the Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA, Box 3, Folder 94, HUG4118.50.<br />

HUA. This manuscript clearly refers to the text <strong>of</strong> Samuel A. Stouffer, “Notes on the Case-Study and the<br />

Unique Case,” Sociometry 4 (1941): 349-357. That same journal number contains the two other papers:<br />

Ernest W. Burgess, “An Experiment in the Standardization <strong>of</strong> the Case-Study Method,” pp. 329-348, and<br />

<strong>Le</strong>onard S. Cottrell, Jr., “The Case-Study Method in Prediction,” pp. 358-370.<br />

113 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> October 2, 1941 by George A. Lundberg to GWA. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. Box 3, Folder 94.<br />

HUG4118.50. HUA. Lundberg’s paper appeared as “Case-Studies vs. Statistical Methods—An Issue<br />

Based on Misunderstanding,” Sociometry 4 (1941): 379-383.<br />

114 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> October 1, 1941 by GWA to Crane. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. Box 3, Folder 94. HUG4118.50. HUA.<br />

115 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> October 17, 1941 by ATP to GWA. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. Box 8. Folder P. HUG4118.10. HUA.<br />

116 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> October 27, 1941 by Edwin G. Nourse to GWA. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. Box 8, Folder N.<br />

HUG4118.10. Correspondence, 1930-1945. HUA.<br />

117 Minutes <strong>of</strong> PPC Meeting, October 25, 1941, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 46.<br />

118 Council Agenda, March 28-29, 1942. Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 47. Louis R. Gottschalk, Clyde<br />

Kluckhohn, and Robert C. Angell, The <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong> in History, Anthropology, and<br />

Sociology. Bulletin No. 53 (New York: Social Science Research Council, 1945).<br />

119 Council Minutes, March 28-29, 1942, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 48.<br />

120 Council Agenda for September 11-13, 1945, Appendix 11, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p. 73.<br />

121 Council Agenda for September 9-11, 1941, Various Minutes <strong>of</strong> the CAR, p., 45<br />

122 The reviews included those by Emory S. Bogardus in Sociology and Social Research 26 (1942): 482;<br />

Peter Hampton in the Journal <strong>of</strong> General Psychology 30 (1944): 277-283; George A. Lundberg in<br />

Sociometry 5 (1942): 317; Robert E. Park in the American Sociological Review 7 (1942): 435-437; and<br />

Ralph K. White in the American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychology 57 (1944): 590-593.<br />

123 Hampton, 1944, p. 277.<br />

124 The presentiment in Hampton’s 1944 review <strong>of</strong> Bruner’s 1990 argument for the importance <strong>of</strong><br />

verisimilitude in narrative qualitative research projects is striking.<br />

125 White, 1944, p. 590.<br />

126 Bogardus, 1942.<br />

127 Park, 1942, p. 436.<br />

128 Lundberg, 1942. The case method critique is found in Lundberg, 1941.<br />

129 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> October 18, 1941 by GWA to ATP. Box 8. Folder P. HUG4118.10. HUA.<br />

130 Ibid.<br />

131 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> October 1, 1941 <strong>of</strong> GWA to Robert T. Crane. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. Box 3, Folder 94. HUG4118.50.<br />

HUA.<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern


Hevern: Allport’s (1942) <strong>Use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong> <strong>Documents</strong><br />

24<br />

132 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> November 3, 1941 by GWA to Edwin G. Nourse. Papers <strong>of</strong> GWA. Box 8, Folder N.<br />

HUG4118.10. Correspondence 1930-1945. HUA.<br />

133 See Danziger, 1997, esp. pp. 158-180.<br />

134 Robert R. Holt, “Individuality and Generalization in the Psychology <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity,” Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Personal</strong>ity 30 (1962): 377-404. Allport’s “reply” came in “The General and the Unique in Psychological<br />

Science,” pp. 405-422. The “debate” was the product <strong>of</strong> juxtaposing two independent submissions<br />

which, the journal’s editor noted, “[did] not represent a knowing dialogue between the authors.” (p.<br />

376).<br />

135 Missing from the UPD’s list <strong>of</strong> references is the work by Saul Rosenzweig, “The Experimental<br />

Situation as a Psychological Problem,” Psychological Review, 40 (1933): 337-354. Allport had cited this<br />

study in his 1937 <strong>Personal</strong>ity text but missed the larger point that experiments are themselves social<br />

situations inviting analysis and not simply higher degrees <strong>of</strong> control.<br />

136 <strong>Le</strong>tter <strong>of</strong> Jerome Bruner to author, March 4, 1999.<br />

137 Bem & Looren de Jong (1997); Pauline M. Rosenau, Post-modernism and the Social Sciences:<br />

Insights, Inroads, and Intrusions (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1991).<br />

138 Allport’s attitudes toward Freud are documented in Allport’s “Autobiography,” 1967, esp. p. 8; Nevitt<br />

Sanford, “What Have We <strong>Le</strong>arned About <strong>Personal</strong>ity,” in Koch and <strong>Le</strong>ary (1985), pp. 490-514; and David<br />

G. Winter, “Allport’s Life and Allport’s Psychology,” Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Personal</strong>ity 65 (1997): 723-731. The<br />

pervasive influence <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic theorizing in the study <strong>of</strong> biographical data after the First World<br />

War is well documented in John A. Garraty, “The Interrelations <strong>of</strong> Psychology and Biography,”<br />

Psychological Bulletin 51 (1954): 569-582. See, too, Ann Douglas’ persuasive intellectual history,<br />

Terrible Honesty: Mongrel Manhattan in the 1920s (New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 1995), for an<br />

appreciation <strong>of</strong> the pr<strong>of</strong>ound cultural impact <strong>of</strong> Freudian thought on the 1920s and beyond.<br />

© 1999 Vincent Hevern

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!