01.01.2015 Views

Speech Disruptions in the Narratives of English-Speaking Children ...

Speech Disruptions in the Narratives of English-Speaking Children ...

Speech Disruptions in the Narratives of English-Speaking Children ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Cl<strong>in</strong>ical and Experimental Implications<br />

The current f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs have two implications for cl<strong>in</strong>icians<br />

and researchers who are <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> speech<br />

disruptions <strong>in</strong> special populations. First, <strong>the</strong> measurement<br />

<strong>of</strong> silent pauses shorter than 2,000 ms should be<br />

<strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to language sample analyses. Traditional<br />

transcription conventions (e.g., Miller & Chapman, 2000)<br />

focus only on silent pauses that are equal or greater than<br />

2,000 ms. Our results showed that silent pauses <strong>of</strong> 500 ms<br />

to 1,000 ms, ra<strong>the</strong>r than those greater than 2,000 ms,<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>guished fourth-grade children with SLI from <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

age-matched peers. Second, <strong>the</strong> results from current<br />

and previous studies (Boscolo et al., 2002; Dollaghan &<br />

Campbell, 1990) showed that token frequency <strong>of</strong> disruptions<br />

divided by <strong>the</strong> total number <strong>of</strong> words is a measure<br />

that is sensitive to language impairment. Future<br />

studies exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g speech disruption rates <strong>in</strong> special<br />

populations may need to take <strong>the</strong> total number <strong>of</strong> words<br />

<strong>in</strong>to consideration (cf. Rispoli, 2003).<br />

Issues for Future Research<br />

This study did not <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>the</strong> disruption type,<br />

prolongation, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> analysis. Prolongation <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong><br />

leng<strong>the</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> word segments or syllables and is determ<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

perceptually relative to <strong>the</strong> speech tempo <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> preced<strong>in</strong>g syllables (Liu, 1998). We did not <strong>in</strong>clude<br />

prolongation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> analysis because we could not<br />

reliably identify <strong>the</strong> prolongation <strong>of</strong> syllables, especially<br />

phrase-f<strong>in</strong>al syllables. Prolongation, however, tends to<br />

signal <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> retriev<strong>in</strong>g words or phrases (Liu,<br />

1998). <strong>Children</strong> with SLI appear to have lexical retrieval<br />

difficulties. Exclud<strong>in</strong>g prolongation <strong>in</strong> this study may<br />

have limited <strong>the</strong> opportunity to capture <strong>the</strong> details <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>se difficulties <strong>in</strong> children with SLI. Therefore, we suggest<br />

that future studies <strong>in</strong>clude prolongation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

analysis and exam<strong>in</strong>e its relation to lexical retrieval<br />

deficits.<br />

This study demonstrates that silent pauses are<br />

sensitive to <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g deficits <strong>of</strong> children with SLI.<br />

The difference between fourth-grade children with SLI<br />

and <strong>the</strong>ir age-matched peers is restricted to silent pauses<br />

<strong>of</strong> 500–1,000 ms. There might, however, be developmental<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> this pattern. For <strong>in</strong>stance, younger<br />

children with SLI may differ from <strong>the</strong>ir age-matched<br />

peers <strong>in</strong> silent pauses <strong>of</strong> 1,000–2,000 ms. This f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

might also vary by us<strong>in</strong>g different tasks (e.g., conversation,<br />

expository discourse). In addition, we did not f<strong>in</strong>d<br />

that children with and without SLI differed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong><br />

filled pauses or <strong>in</strong>terjections probably because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

limitation <strong>of</strong> narrative tasks. Future research should<br />

address potential developmental changes <strong>in</strong> speech disruptions<br />

and should <strong>in</strong>clude different tasks to verify<br />

<strong>the</strong>se issues.<br />

Conclusion<br />

<strong>Speech</strong> disruptions reflect <strong>the</strong> cognitive processes<br />

underly<strong>in</strong>g language production. This study exam<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

<strong>the</strong> types, frequencies, and distribution <strong>of</strong> speech disruptions<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> narratives <strong>of</strong> fourth-grade children with<br />

SLI and <strong>the</strong>ir age-matched and language-matched<br />

peers. <strong>Children</strong> with SLI produced speech disruptions<br />

more <strong>of</strong>ten than <strong>the</strong>ir age-matched peers but not <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

language-matched peers, suggest<strong>in</strong>g a relationship<br />

between language ability and speech disruption. The difference<br />

between children with SLI and <strong>the</strong>ir age-matched<br />

peers was conf<strong>in</strong>ed to silent pauses <strong>of</strong> 500–1,000 ms.<br />

<strong>Children</strong> with SLI did not produce more disruptions<br />

across <strong>the</strong> board <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> production <strong>of</strong> narratives; ra<strong>the</strong>r,<br />

<strong>the</strong>y outnumbered <strong>the</strong>ir peers <strong>in</strong> disruptions before<br />

phrases but not before sentences, clauses, or words.<br />

Taken toge<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>se f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that speech disruptions<br />

at phrase boundaries produced by children<br />

with SLI reflect <strong>the</strong>ir underly<strong>in</strong>g lexical and syntactic<br />

immaturity.<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

This study was supported by Grant 1-P50-DC02726 from<br />

<strong>the</strong> National Institute on Deafness and O<strong>the</strong>r Communication<br />

Disorders. We thank Marc Fey for <strong>of</strong>fer<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> transcriptions<br />

<strong>of</strong> narratives; Rick Arenas, Juanita Limas, Marlea O’Brien,<br />

Chun-yi Shen, Yi-li Yang, and Xuyang Zhang for assist<strong>in</strong>g with<br />

preparation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> article; Hs<strong>in</strong>-jen Hsu; Amanda J. Owen; <strong>the</strong><br />

Language Development Group; and students <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> scientific<br />

writ<strong>in</strong>g class (Spr<strong>in</strong>g 2006, University <strong>of</strong> Iowa).<br />

References<br />

Abbot-Smith, K., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Exemplar-learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and schematization <strong>in</strong> a usage-based account <strong>of</strong> syntactic<br />

acquisition. The L<strong>in</strong>guistic Review, 23, 275–290.<br />

Bernste<strong>in</strong>, N. (1981). Are <strong>the</strong>re constra<strong>in</strong>ts on childhood<br />

disfluency Journal <strong>of</strong> Fluency Disorders, 6, 341–350.<br />

Bloodste<strong>in</strong>, O., & Grossman, M. (1981). Early stutter<strong>in</strong>gs:<br />

Some aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir form and distribution. Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Speech</strong> and Hear<strong>in</strong>g Research, 24, 298–302.<br />

Bock, K., & Levelt, W. (1994). Language production: Grammatical<br />

encod<strong>in</strong>g. In M. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook <strong>of</strong><br />

psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics (pp. 945–984). San Diego: Academic Press.<br />

Boomer, D. (1965). Hesitation and grammatical encod<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Language and <strong>Speech</strong>, 15, 103–113.<br />

Boscolo, B., Bernste<strong>in</strong> Ratner, N., & Rescorla, L. (2002).<br />

Fluency <strong>of</strong> school-aged children with a history <strong>of</strong> specific<br />

expressive language impairment: An exploratory study.<br />

American Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Speech</strong>-Language Pathology, 11, 41–49.<br />

Chang, F., Dell, G., & Bock, K. (2006). Becom<strong>in</strong>g syntactic.<br />

Psychological Review, 113, 234–272.<br />

Clark, H. H. (2002). Speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> time. <strong>Speech</strong> Communication,<br />

36, 5–13.<br />

Downloaded from jslhr.asha.org on November 10, 2010<br />

Guo et al.: <strong>Speech</strong> <strong>Disruptions</strong> <strong>in</strong> SLI <strong>Narratives</strong> 735

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!