13.01.2015 Views

The role of the mother's voice in developing mother's ... - FIU Infant Lab

The role of the mother's voice in developing mother's ... - FIU Infant Lab

The role of the mother's voice in developing mother's ... - FIU Infant Lab

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

30<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

stranger’s face from 4, 12, 72 and 78 h, respectively. Us<strong>in</strong>g a high amplitude<br />

suck<strong>in</strong>g (HAS) technique, Walton et al. (1992) reported that <strong>in</strong>fants from 12 h <strong>of</strong><br />

age produced more suck<strong>in</strong>g responses to <strong>the</strong> videotaped image <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

face than that <strong>of</strong> a female stranger.<br />

Not only do neonates prefer to look at a composite <strong>of</strong> previously seen faces<br />

than at a composite <strong>of</strong> previously unseen faces (Walton and Bower, 1993), but can<br />

recognize a learned face over transformations (Walton et al., 1997), and even learn<br />

an image similar to a composite <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> faces <strong>the</strong>y have seen <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first hours <strong>of</strong><br />

birth (Slater et al., 1998). Altoge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>se data suggest a rapid learn<strong>in</strong>g about<br />

faces with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first hours <strong>of</strong> birth, and <strong>the</strong> establishment <strong>of</strong>}at least}a<br />

rudimentary representation <strong>of</strong> faces. None<strong>the</strong>less <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> such learn<strong>in</strong>g and<br />

what is be<strong>in</strong>g learned rema<strong>in</strong>s obscure.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re has been more than one <strong>in</strong>terpretation <strong>of</strong>fered to expla<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> data<br />

suggest<strong>in</strong>g early preference, such that <strong>the</strong>re exists a configurational response <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> neonatal period. Goren et al. (1975) and Dziurawiec (1987) reported that<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants only m<strong>in</strong>utes old oriented <strong>the</strong>ir heads to follow a two-dimensional<br />

schematic face-like pattern than ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> two patterns consist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same<br />

facial features <strong>in</strong> different arrangements. Johnson and Morton (1991) who<br />

replicated Goren et al.’s study proposed <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> ‘Conspec’. Consist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong><br />

three dots, <strong>the</strong> Conspec serves to direct <strong>the</strong> newborn’s visual attention to faces.<br />

While this proposition seems plausible, <strong>the</strong>re are some data that are not easily<br />

expla<strong>in</strong>ed (e.g. Turati et al.’s (2002) results). Preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s live face<br />

vanished with <strong>the</strong> slightest orientation, half-pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> that face (Sai, 1990), and<br />

when hair-face l<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>formation was masked (Pascalis et al., 1995). Recognition <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> half-pr<strong>of</strong>ile pose <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face was, however, possible only at 4–5<br />

weeks from birth (Sai and Bushnell, 1988), and it was only at 10–12 weeks that<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants demonstrated preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face shown <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile (Sai,<br />

1990). Toge<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>se f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs seem to favour <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g hypo<strong>the</strong>sis; it Is<br />

possible that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants had not been sufficiently exposed to <strong>the</strong> half-pr<strong>of</strong>ile and<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>ile poses <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face. If recognition is based solely on facial<br />

configuration, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>in</strong>fants should have been able to show preference for <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face s<strong>in</strong>ce at least some <strong>in</strong>formation about facial configuration was still<br />

available, at least <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> half-pr<strong>of</strong>ile pose.<br />

It is fairly clear <strong>in</strong> retrospect that <strong>the</strong>re were some methodological shortcom<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

with <strong>the</strong> studies that used real faces. For example Field et al.’s (1984)<br />

study failed to take <strong>the</strong> olfactory <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong>to account. Those which<br />

controlled for discrim<strong>in</strong>ation on <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> olfactory <strong>in</strong>formation, implemented<br />

a control over <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s and stranger’s odours only dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g (Sai,<br />

1990; Bushnell et al., 1989) and, so was <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>. Never<strong>the</strong>less, exposure<br />

to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face from birth to <strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g might have facilitated<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s visual features. Evidence suggests learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

<strong>voice</strong> at birth. For example, <strong>in</strong> a study by DeCasper and Fifer (1980), 2–4-day-old<br />

neonates preferred <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>. Moon et al. (1991) demonstrated that<br />

with both Spanish and English speak<strong>in</strong>g women, 2-day-old newborns preferred<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s language.<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore, <strong>the</strong> question <strong>of</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> is, particularly<br />

critical for understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mechanism underly<strong>in</strong>g preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

face at birth. <strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face, like many o<strong>the</strong>r stimuli we experience is<br />

<strong>in</strong>termodal <strong>in</strong> that it provides <strong>in</strong>formation to more than one sensory modality. For<br />

example, dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teraction with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>in</strong>fants are provided with tactile<br />

stimulation, warmth and exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>, odours, taste <strong>of</strong> her<br />

milk and even to <strong>the</strong> rhythms to her heart beats. This wide range <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!