13.01.2015 Views

The role of the mother's voice in developing mother's ... - FIU Infant Lab

The role of the mother's voice in developing mother's ... - FIU Infant Lab

The role of the mother's voice in developing mother's ... - FIU Infant Lab

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Infant</strong> and Child Development<br />

Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)<br />

Published onl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> Wiley InterScience (www.<strong>in</strong>terscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/icd.376<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice <strong>in</strong><br />

Develop<strong>in</strong>g Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Face<br />

Preference: Evidence for Intermodal<br />

Perception at Birth<br />

<br />

F. Z. Sai*<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Psychology, U.A.E. University, U.A.E.<br />

Four experiments are described which <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>the</strong> <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> <strong>in</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face at<br />

birth. Experiment 1 replicated our previous f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (Br. J. Dev.<br />

Psychol. 1989; 7: 3–15; <strong>The</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> human face perception by<br />

very young <strong>in</strong>fants. Ph.D. <strong>The</strong>sis, University <strong>of</strong> Glasgow, Scotland,<br />

UK, 1990) <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face<br />

when a control for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> and odours was used only<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g. A second experiment adopted <strong>the</strong> same<br />

procedures, but controlled for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> from birth<br />

through test<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>The</strong> neonates were at no time exposed to <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>. Under <strong>the</strong>se conditions, no preference was found.<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>r, neonates showed only few head turns towards both <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g. Experiment 3 looked<br />

at <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> head turns under conditions where <strong>the</strong> newborn<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants were exposed to both <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> and face from<br />

birth to 5 to 15 m<strong>in</strong> prior to test<strong>in</strong>g. Aga<strong>in</strong>, a strong preference for<br />

<strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face was demonstrated. Such preference, however,<br />

vanished <strong>in</strong> Experiment 4, when neonates had no previous<br />

exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face comb<strong>in</strong>ation. <strong>The</strong> conclusion<br />

drawn is that a prior experience with both <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> and<br />

face is necessary for <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> face recognition, and that<br />

<strong>in</strong>termodal perception is evident at birth. <strong>The</strong> neonates’ ability to<br />

recognize <strong>the</strong> face <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r is most likely to be rooted <strong>in</strong><br />

prenatal learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>. Copyright # 2004 John<br />

Wiley & Sons, Ltd.<br />

Key words: neonates; mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face recognition; <strong>in</strong>termodal perception<br />

at birth<br />

Increas<strong>in</strong>g evidence suggests that newborn <strong>in</strong>fants can rapidly learn about faces.<br />

Sai (1990), Bushnell et al. (1989), Field et al. (1984) and Pascalis et al. (1995) found<br />

statistically reliable preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face, as opposed to a female<br />

*Correspondence to: Dr. F. Z. Sai, Associate Pr<strong>of</strong>essor, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychology, U.A.E.<br />

University, Al-A<strong>in</strong>, P.O. Box 17771, U.A.E. E-mail: sai_fz@hotmail.com, f.zohra@uaeu.ac.ae<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


30<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

stranger’s face from 4, 12, 72 and 78 h, respectively. Us<strong>in</strong>g a high amplitude<br />

suck<strong>in</strong>g (HAS) technique, Walton et al. (1992) reported that <strong>in</strong>fants from 12 h <strong>of</strong><br />

age produced more suck<strong>in</strong>g responses to <strong>the</strong> videotaped image <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

face than that <strong>of</strong> a female stranger.<br />

Not only do neonates prefer to look at a composite <strong>of</strong> previously seen faces<br />

than at a composite <strong>of</strong> previously unseen faces (Walton and Bower, 1993), but can<br />

recognize a learned face over transformations (Walton et al., 1997), and even learn<br />

an image similar to a composite <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> faces <strong>the</strong>y have seen <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first hours <strong>of</strong><br />

birth (Slater et al., 1998). Altoge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>se data suggest a rapid learn<strong>in</strong>g about<br />

faces with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first hours <strong>of</strong> birth, and <strong>the</strong> establishment <strong>of</strong>}at least}a<br />

rudimentary representation <strong>of</strong> faces. None<strong>the</strong>less <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> such learn<strong>in</strong>g and<br />

what is be<strong>in</strong>g learned rema<strong>in</strong>s obscure.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re has been more than one <strong>in</strong>terpretation <strong>of</strong>fered to expla<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> data<br />

suggest<strong>in</strong>g early preference, such that <strong>the</strong>re exists a configurational response <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> neonatal period. Goren et al. (1975) and Dziurawiec (1987) reported that<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants only m<strong>in</strong>utes old oriented <strong>the</strong>ir heads to follow a two-dimensional<br />

schematic face-like pattern than ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> two patterns consist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same<br />

facial features <strong>in</strong> different arrangements. Johnson and Morton (1991) who<br />

replicated Goren et al.’s study proposed <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> ‘Conspec’. Consist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong><br />

three dots, <strong>the</strong> Conspec serves to direct <strong>the</strong> newborn’s visual attention to faces.<br />

While this proposition seems plausible, <strong>the</strong>re are some data that are not easily<br />

expla<strong>in</strong>ed (e.g. Turati et al.’s (2002) results). Preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s live face<br />

vanished with <strong>the</strong> slightest orientation, half-pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> that face (Sai, 1990), and<br />

when hair-face l<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>formation was masked (Pascalis et al., 1995). Recognition <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> half-pr<strong>of</strong>ile pose <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face was, however, possible only at 4–5<br />

weeks from birth (Sai and Bushnell, 1988), and it was only at 10–12 weeks that<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants demonstrated preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face shown <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ile (Sai,<br />

1990). Toge<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>se f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs seem to favour <strong>the</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g hypo<strong>the</strong>sis; it Is<br />

possible that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants had not been sufficiently exposed to <strong>the</strong> half-pr<strong>of</strong>ile and<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>ile poses <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face. If recognition is based solely on facial<br />

configuration, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>in</strong>fants should have been able to show preference for <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face s<strong>in</strong>ce at least some <strong>in</strong>formation about facial configuration was still<br />

available, at least <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> half-pr<strong>of</strong>ile pose.<br />

It is fairly clear <strong>in</strong> retrospect that <strong>the</strong>re were some methodological shortcom<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

with <strong>the</strong> studies that used real faces. For example Field et al.’s (1984)<br />

study failed to take <strong>the</strong> olfactory <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong>to account. Those which<br />

controlled for discrim<strong>in</strong>ation on <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> olfactory <strong>in</strong>formation, implemented<br />

a control over <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s and stranger’s odours only dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g (Sai,<br />

1990; Bushnell et al., 1989) and, so was <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>. Never<strong>the</strong>less, exposure<br />

to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face from birth to <strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g might have facilitated<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s visual features. Evidence suggests learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

<strong>voice</strong> at birth. For example, <strong>in</strong> a study by DeCasper and Fifer (1980), 2–4-day-old<br />

neonates preferred <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>. Moon et al. (1991) demonstrated that<br />

with both Spanish and English speak<strong>in</strong>g women, 2-day-old newborns preferred<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s language.<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore, <strong>the</strong> question <strong>of</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> is, particularly<br />

critical for understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mechanism underly<strong>in</strong>g preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

face at birth. <strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face, like many o<strong>the</strong>r stimuli we experience is<br />

<strong>in</strong>termodal <strong>in</strong> that it provides <strong>in</strong>formation to more than one sensory modality. For<br />

example, dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teraction with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>in</strong>fants are provided with tactile<br />

stimulation, warmth and exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>, odours, taste <strong>of</strong> her<br />

milk and even to <strong>the</strong> rhythms to her heart beats. This wide range <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 31<br />

may not only play a <strong>role</strong> <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face recognition, but is<br />

likely to lay ground to early cross-modal learn<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Research has now clearly revealed many <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>termodal capabilities<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fancy (for review see, Bushnell and Boudreau, 1991, 1993; Lewkowicz<br />

and Lickliter, 1995; Rose and Ruff, 1987). Some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>termodal relations<br />

are amodal and redundant across <strong>the</strong> senses. For example, <strong>the</strong> synchrony<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>voice</strong> and mouth when a person speaks. O<strong>the</strong>r cues are modality-specific and<br />

arbitrary-related across <strong>the</strong> senses, such as <strong>the</strong> pair<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> a face and <strong>the</strong> sound <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>voice</strong>.<br />

From birth, newborn <strong>in</strong>fants perceive a wide range <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>variant amodal<br />

relations. For example, <strong>the</strong>y reliably co-ord<strong>in</strong>ate visual and auditory space, <strong>in</strong><br />

that <strong>the</strong>y turn <strong>the</strong>ir heads and eyes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> direction <strong>of</strong> a sound source, suggest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

that knowledge about spatial location is provided by both visual and auditory<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation (e.g. Wer<strong>the</strong>imer, 1961; Muir and Field, 1979; Clifton et al., 1981;<br />

Butterworth, 1983; Muir and Clifton, 1985). At 5 weeks, <strong>in</strong>fants can recognize a<br />

visual shape (a pacifier) that had been experienced tactually by suck<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

suggest<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong> pacifier is coded both tactually and visually (Meltz<strong>of</strong>f and<br />

Borton, 1979). Twenty week old <strong>in</strong>fants show a sensitivity to temporal synchrony<br />

specified <strong>in</strong>termodally <strong>in</strong> that <strong>the</strong>y detect <strong>the</strong> common rhythm and duration <strong>of</strong><br />

tones and flash<strong>in</strong>g lights (Lewkowicz, 1986). Around this age, <strong>in</strong>fants can also<br />

match appropriately <strong>the</strong> sounds made ei<strong>the</strong>r by a s<strong>in</strong>gle unitary element or by a<br />

cluster <strong>of</strong> smaller elements (Bahrick, 1987, 1988). Toge<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>se f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>dicate<br />

that <strong>in</strong>fants from birth perceive amodal relations.<br />

On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, recent evidence suggests that arbitrary <strong>in</strong>termodal<br />

relations are detected and learned <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fancy too. Two-day-old <strong>in</strong>fants can learn<br />

arbitrary visual–auditory associations when familiarized to two alternat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

visual stimuli differ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> colour and orientation, and when each association<br />

was accompanied by its appropriate sound (Slater et al., 1997). By 14 weeks,<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants were able to learn to associate <strong>the</strong> sound <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong><br />

with <strong>the</strong> sight <strong>of</strong> her face (Spelke and Owsley, 1979), and 24 week olds<br />

learned <strong>the</strong> association between <strong>the</strong> colour <strong>of</strong> a conta<strong>in</strong>er and <strong>the</strong> taste <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> food<br />

it conta<strong>in</strong>ed (Reardon and Bushnell, 1988). Prior to 12 weeks, <strong>in</strong>fants were not<br />

able to detect <strong>the</strong> arbitrary pitch colour/shape relations (Bahrick et al., 1996).<br />

Only <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>of</strong> 28 weeks demonstrated learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> arbitrary relations<br />

(Bahrick, 1994). If young <strong>in</strong>fants are able to learn <strong>the</strong> synchrony <strong>of</strong> <strong>voice</strong> and mouth<br />

when <strong>the</strong> person speaks (amodal <strong>in</strong>formation), and can learn <strong>the</strong> pair<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> a<br />

stimulus and <strong>the</strong> sound that stimulus produces (arbitrary <strong>in</strong>formation), <strong>the</strong>n learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

about faces would be most likely <strong>the</strong> outcome <strong>of</strong> an early <strong>in</strong>termodal process<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

It is, however, possible that <strong>the</strong>re exists a specific mechanism for learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

visual features <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face. If this were <strong>the</strong> case, <strong>the</strong>n newborn <strong>in</strong>fants<br />

would still prefer <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face to that <strong>of</strong> a female stranger even when <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> is not available to <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants from birth through test<strong>in</strong>g. On <strong>the</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r hand, preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face might disappear with <strong>the</strong><br />

unavailability <strong>of</strong> a postnatal auditory experience to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>. In this<br />

case, <strong>the</strong> conclusion that would be drawn is that face preference does not solely<br />

depend on visual process<strong>in</strong>g. Instead, we would assume that face preference is<br />

<strong>the</strong> product <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>termodal learn<strong>in</strong>g, especially s<strong>in</strong>ce 2-day-old newborn <strong>in</strong>fants<br />

are able to learn arbitrary visual–auditory associations (Slater et al., 1997) as<br />

mentioned earlier. However, we do not have evidence concern<strong>in</strong>g whe<strong>the</strong>r<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants are able to perceive <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face–<strong>voice</strong> associations and later use<br />

such <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> recognition test. <strong>The</strong> experiments described here were<br />

designed to determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> <strong>in</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g preference for<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


32<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

<strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face at birth. <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants were, <strong>the</strong>refore, not exposed to <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> from birth through test<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Some clarification <strong>of</strong> our understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants’ learn<strong>in</strong>g about faces, and<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g facial process<strong>in</strong>g is likely to emerge<br />

when newborn <strong>in</strong>fants are tested us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> same procedures as <strong>in</strong> our previous<br />

studies (Sai, 1990; Bushnel et al., 1989), with a visual preference task and paired<br />

ra<strong>the</strong>r than sequential presentations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> comparison faces. This paper<br />

describes four studies which used non-Caucasian participants.<br />

EXPERIMENT 1<br />

<strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> present experiments was to determ<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong> experience<br />

with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> <strong>in</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g face recognition <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> neonatal period.<br />

However, s<strong>in</strong>ce our previous studies (Sai and Bushnell, 1987; Bushnell et al., 1989;<br />

Sai, 1990) demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> neonatal period<br />

tested only Caucasian neonates from <strong>the</strong> area <strong>of</strong> Glasgow, Scotland (UK), and <strong>the</strong><br />

participants <strong>in</strong> this research were Malaysians, it was seen advisable to first<br />

replicate <strong>the</strong>se earlier f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs on Malaysian neonates.<br />

Before present<strong>in</strong>g details about <strong>the</strong> methodology and procedures followed <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>se experiments, it is worth po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g out that <strong>the</strong> newborn <strong>in</strong>fants were<br />

reta<strong>in</strong>ed on <strong>the</strong> ward at all times, <strong>in</strong> bass<strong>in</strong>ets located adjacent to <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

bed. <strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs had cont<strong>in</strong>uous responsibility for <strong>the</strong> care <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>fants.<br />

Although <strong>the</strong> total <strong>in</strong>teractive contact hours <strong>in</strong>volved with<strong>in</strong> each mo<strong>the</strong>r–<strong>in</strong>fant<br />

dyad was not assessed, some provisional observations (Sai, 1990) showed that<br />

<strong>the</strong>re was a fairly substantial period <strong>of</strong> contact with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first 24 h. However,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re was a considerable variability across dyads.<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce our earlier f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs had <strong>in</strong>dicated no significant relationship between age<br />

and extent <strong>of</strong> preference, age was not considered as a potential factor, and was<br />

<strong>the</strong>refore not <strong>in</strong>cluded. Similarly, sex factor was not <strong>in</strong>corporated for <strong>the</strong> same<br />

reason (see Sai, 1990).<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Fourteen newborn <strong>in</strong>fants, seven males and seven females (M ¼ 3:00 h,<br />

range ¼ 224h;M birth weight ¼ 2:75 kg, range ¼ 2:7022:90) participated <strong>in</strong> this<br />

study. <strong>The</strong>y all had normal Apgar scores after 5 m<strong>in</strong> (M ¼ 9, range ¼ 8210), and<br />

throughout test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>y rema<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> behavioural state <strong>of</strong> alert <strong>in</strong>activity<br />

(Ashton, 1973). All <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>in</strong> this and subsequent studies were normal,<br />

healthy and full-term <strong>in</strong>fants (37 weeks gestation) with no complications dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

delivery. <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants were primarily from middle class and diverse ethnic<br />

backgrounds <strong>of</strong> Malaysia (Malay, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese and H<strong>in</strong>dou). <strong>The</strong>y were volunteered<br />

by <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>rs on <strong>the</strong> wards <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> University Hospital, Kuala Lumpur where<br />

<strong>the</strong>y were born. <strong>The</strong> data <strong>of</strong> ten additional <strong>in</strong>fants were discarded from <strong>the</strong> study:<br />

four for side bias <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir look<strong>in</strong>g behaviour (see procedure section for details<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g criteria for side bias), three for excessive fuss<strong>in</strong>ess, two for persistent<br />

sleep<strong>in</strong>g and one because <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r made sounds dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Stimuli and Apparatus<br />

<strong>The</strong> stimulus faces for each participant <strong>in</strong> this experiment were <strong>the</strong> live face <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> participant’s mo<strong>the</strong>r and that <strong>of</strong> a female stranger. This was ano<strong>the</strong>r mo<strong>the</strong>r<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 33<br />

from <strong>the</strong> same maternity ward or, ano<strong>the</strong>r young woman. To ensure that<br />

preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face was not be<strong>in</strong>g forced by an <strong>in</strong>advertent bias <strong>in</strong><br />

terms <strong>of</strong> overall brightness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face or <strong>the</strong> hair colour, a control over brightness<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face and hair colour was <strong>in</strong>corporated. In Field et al.’s study (1984), hair<br />

colour and face brightness cues were randomly varied across participants so that<br />

sometimes <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face should have been brighter and sometimes less<br />

brighter than <strong>the</strong> stranger’s, and sometimes <strong>the</strong> hair colour should have been<br />

lighter and sometimes darker. Yet, it could be argued that <strong>the</strong>re might still have<br />

been a systematic bias along <strong>the</strong>se dimensions <strong>in</strong> favour <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs’ faces<br />

s<strong>in</strong>ce Field et al., used <strong>the</strong> same small set <strong>of</strong> comparison faces throughout <strong>the</strong><br />

study, and this might have resulted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> demonstrated preferences. <strong>The</strong>refore, it<br />

was seen advisable to match each mo<strong>the</strong>r on <strong>the</strong> wards with ano<strong>the</strong>r female<br />

stranger who was judged to be broadly comparable <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> hair colour, hair<br />

shape and facial complexion.<br />

<strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and a volunteer female stranger were asked to sit beh<strong>in</strong>d a large<br />

white screen (2 2.5 m) <strong>in</strong>to which at head height were cut two apertures<br />

(30 25 cm), one at ei<strong>the</strong>r side <strong>of</strong> mid-l<strong>in</strong>e and separated by 12 cm. <strong>The</strong>se allowed<br />

a good view <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hair and face <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> stimuli from <strong>the</strong> neonate’s position, with<br />

light<strong>in</strong>g be<strong>in</strong>g provided by two fluorescent tubes above and <strong>in</strong> front <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> faces.<br />

In order to prevent any contam<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> data due to differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

cloth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> stimulus faces, <strong>the</strong>y each had white sheet draped around <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

necks to effectively mask <strong>the</strong>ir cloth<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>The</strong> visible background, a curta<strong>in</strong><br />

backcloth, was <strong>the</strong> same for both stimulus faces (see diagram).<br />

Through a hole, which was made <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middle <strong>of</strong> this panel, between <strong>the</strong> two<br />

apertures, a video camera recorded <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant’s face dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> whole test<strong>in</strong>g and<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


34<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

served for later calculation <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>terobserver reliability. To successfully control for<br />

olfactory <strong>in</strong>formation, a strong air-freshener was liberally coated onto <strong>the</strong> screen<br />

surround<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> faces before <strong>the</strong> neonate was brought up to <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g room and<br />

immediately before each trial.<br />

Procedure and Design<br />

<strong>The</strong> method and procedures were identical to those <strong>of</strong> our previous studies. A<br />

control over <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> and olfactory <strong>in</strong>formation was implemented only<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g test<strong>in</strong>g. Neonates were exposed from birth to 5 to 15 m<strong>in</strong>, prior test<strong>in</strong>g, to<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face, <strong>voice</strong>s and odours. Dur<strong>in</strong>g this period, <strong>the</strong>y all had a first<br />

contact with <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>rs immediately after birth, and on average once every<br />

2–4 h. <strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs were encouraged to breastfeed and <strong>in</strong>teract with <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>fant,<br />

as <strong>the</strong>y would normally do.<br />

All <strong>in</strong>fants were <strong>in</strong> a state <strong>of</strong> quiet alertness. Each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m was brought from<br />

<strong>the</strong> wards <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> maternity hospital to an adjacent room where two<br />

experimenters bl<strong>in</strong>d as to <strong>the</strong> identity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger had set<br />

up <strong>the</strong> stimulus faces. <strong>The</strong> participant was held by an Experimenter who sat on<br />

a chair fac<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mid-l<strong>in</strong>e separat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> two apertures from where <strong>the</strong><br />

stimulus faces appeared. <strong>The</strong> Experimenter ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed this optimal state<br />

throughout <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g. A tra<strong>in</strong>ed white-coated observer who was unaware <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> identity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger, stood centrally beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> faces,<br />

view<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant’s visual fixations through <strong>the</strong> same aperture as <strong>the</strong> video<br />

camera, and record<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> total duration <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se fixations on a pair <strong>of</strong> buttons<br />

box connected to a lap tope. Whenever <strong>the</strong> participant looked at <strong>the</strong> right-hand<br />

stimulus face, <strong>the</strong> central observer pressed <strong>the</strong> right-hand switch; similarly, <strong>the</strong><br />

left-hand switch was pressed whenever <strong>the</strong> participant fixated <strong>the</strong> left-hand<br />

stimulus face. Off-target visual behaviours <strong>in</strong>cluded clos<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> eyes and turn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

head and eyes away from <strong>the</strong> stimulus figure. <strong>The</strong> computer was programmed<br />

to record visual fixations on l<strong>in</strong>e, and signal to <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> trial end<strong>in</strong>g. A<br />

permanent record <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant’s visual fixations was saved and pr<strong>in</strong>ted for<br />

later analysis.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were two trials for each participant. Trials commenced once both<br />

stimulus figures had been satisfactorily <strong>in</strong>structed to adopt a neutral<br />

expression, to look directly at <strong>the</strong> neonate’s eyes and to nei<strong>the</strong>r move nor<br />

make a sound dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> brief test period. At this time, <strong>the</strong> neonate was<br />

<strong>in</strong>troduced to <strong>the</strong> stimuli by be<strong>in</strong>g brought upright <strong>in</strong>to a seated position at a<br />

po<strong>in</strong>t 30 cm from <strong>the</strong> central po<strong>in</strong>t between <strong>the</strong> two faces; <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant’s head<br />

be<strong>in</strong>g supported but free to move <strong>in</strong> a lateral arc. <strong>The</strong> first trial began with <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>fant’s fixation <strong>of</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>r face, and term<strong>in</strong>ated after a 20 s fixation to ei<strong>the</strong>r or<br />

both faces had been accumulated. After <strong>the</strong> first trial, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant was withdrawn<br />

from <strong>the</strong> apparatus and <strong>the</strong> two stimulus figures changed seats to counteract<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> side bias. <strong>The</strong> procedure was repeated for <strong>the</strong> second trial until<br />

a fur<strong>the</strong>r 20 s fixation had been accumulated. Over <strong>the</strong> two trials, <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

face was shown once on <strong>the</strong> left and once on <strong>the</strong> right, <strong>the</strong> order be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

randomly determ<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

Two criteria were established for <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> participant’s data. First, it was<br />

required that <strong>the</strong> two trials must be completed to have sufficient data for<br />

analyses. Second, to ensure that <strong>the</strong> participant had viewed both faces and had<br />

<strong>the</strong> opportunity to actively choose between <strong>the</strong>m, neonates were required to<br />

fixate both stimulus faces. <strong>The</strong> participant’s data were excluded if <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant<br />

spent less than 5 s view<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> least preferred side over <strong>the</strong> two trials. For each<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 35<br />

<strong>in</strong>fant, <strong>the</strong> total fixation time towards ei<strong>the</strong>r face was based on <strong>the</strong> live record<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> central observer, and was <strong>the</strong>n compared to <strong>the</strong> video record<strong>in</strong>g. For <strong>the</strong><br />

test trials <strong>of</strong> eight <strong>in</strong>fants, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>ter-observer reliability was high (Pearson<br />

r ¼ 0:86).<br />

All Malay <strong>in</strong>fants ðn ¼ 12Þ heard <strong>the</strong> <strong>voice</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir fa<strong>the</strong>r’s call for prayer. It<br />

was delivered far from <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>rs to prevent any association <strong>of</strong> <strong>voice</strong> cues<br />

with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face. In <strong>the</strong> Malay culture, this message is given to <strong>the</strong><br />

newborn baby soon after birth for 2–3 m<strong>in</strong> to ensure that it will always be heard<br />

and answered.<br />

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> data were expressed <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> percentage <strong>of</strong> total look<strong>in</strong>g time (PTLT)<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants spent fixat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face on each trial. Look<strong>in</strong>g percentage for both<br />

trials were averaged for each participant (Table 1). Each newborn <strong>in</strong>fant gave<br />

data for two trials, giv<strong>in</strong>g 28 trials for all <strong>the</strong> 14 participants. From <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

with both trials <strong>in</strong>cluded, 12 <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 14 <strong>in</strong>fants looked longer at <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face.<br />

<strong>The</strong> total visual fixations to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> female stranger accumulated,<br />

respectively, 58.53 and 41.47%. <strong>The</strong> means preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r on trials 1<br />

and 2 were, respectively, 59.21% (S:D: ¼ 13:3) and 57.85% (S:D: ¼ 9:72). <strong>The</strong>se<br />

preferences are given <strong>in</strong> Figure 1.<br />

To address <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>in</strong> research question, whe<strong>the</strong>r neonates showed significant<br />

preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face over <strong>the</strong> stranger’s, s<strong>in</strong>gle-sample t tests were<br />

conducted. <strong>The</strong> results <strong>in</strong>dicated that <strong>the</strong> newborn <strong>in</strong>fants showed significant<br />

preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face on trial 1, ðtð13Þ ¼2:59, p50:02), on trial 2,<br />

(tð13Þ ¼3:02, p50:010) and on <strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed trials (tð13Þ ¼3:83, p50:002).<br />

<strong>The</strong> data replicate and extend <strong>the</strong> earlier f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs that though few hours old,<br />

Malaysian newborn <strong>in</strong>fants are able to recognize <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face from that <strong>of</strong><br />

a female stranger even when matched closely <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> hair colour, hair length<br />

and hair form; and when a control over olfactory and auditory cues was<br />

implemented dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g. However, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> control over <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

Table 1. Experiment 1: <strong>Infant</strong>s’ preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face across trials 1 and 2 and<br />

<strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed trials<br />

<strong>Infant</strong> Sex Trial 1 (T1) Trial 2 (T2) Comb<strong>in</strong>ed trials (COMTs)<br />

1 M 65 50 57.5<br />

2 M 75 70 72.5<br />

3 M 30 48 39<br />

4 M 50 78 64<br />

5 M 65 70 67.5<br />

6 M 55 59 57<br />

7 M 60 50 55<br />

8 F 65 61 63<br />

9 F 85 48 66.5<br />

10 F 45 64 54.5<br />

11 F 60 51 55.5<br />

12 F 65 60 62.5<br />

13 F 50 52 51<br />

14 F 59 49 54<br />

Mean 59.21 57.85 58.53<br />

S.D. 13.3 9.72 8.34<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


36<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

Figure 1. Percentage preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face on trials 1, 2 and on <strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

trials.<br />

<strong>voice</strong> was limited to <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g period, experience with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> prior<br />

test<strong>in</strong>g might have facilitated learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s visual features.<br />

EXPERIMENT 2<br />

<strong>The</strong> second experiment was designed to tackle <strong>the</strong> question <strong>of</strong> auditory cues by<br />

prevent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants from hear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> from birth through test<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

All o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>formation was available to <strong>the</strong> participants from birth to 5 to 15 m<strong>in</strong><br />

prior test<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs were encouraged to visually <strong>in</strong>teract and play with <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants without talk<strong>in</strong>g or mak<strong>in</strong>g any k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> sounds. O<strong>the</strong>rwise a similar design<br />

and test procedures to those used <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first experiment were adopted.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Participants were 14, six males and eight females (M ¼ 7:35 h, range¼2 –12 h;<br />

M birth weight¼2.91 kg, range¼2.61–3.64; with normal Apgar scores at 5 m<strong>in</strong><br />

(M ¼ 9, range 7–10), born at <strong>the</strong> Maternity Department, University Hospital,<br />

Kuala Lumpur, where <strong>the</strong>y were tested. <strong>The</strong> data <strong>of</strong> 16 additional <strong>in</strong>fants (n<strong>in</strong>e<br />

males and seven females) were discarded from <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al sample because <strong>of</strong> side<br />

bias <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir look<strong>in</strong>g behaviour ðn ¼ 8Þ, or <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants fussed or fell asleep before<br />

<strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g was over ðn ¼ 6Þ, or <strong>the</strong>re were technical failures <strong>of</strong> various k<strong>in</strong>ds<br />

ðn ¼ 4Þ. <strong>The</strong> subjects were volunteered by <strong>the</strong>ir parents who agreed to participate<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> control <strong>of</strong> <strong>voice</strong> cues.<br />

Stimuli and Apparatus<br />

<strong>The</strong> live faces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and that <strong>of</strong> a female stranger were used, matched<br />

as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first experiment. <strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs were Malaysians (Malay, H<strong>in</strong>dou, and<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 37<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese). <strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r–stranger pair was judged to be broadly comparable <strong>in</strong><br />

terms <strong>of</strong> hair colour, shape and facial brightness for each <strong>in</strong>fant.<br />

PROCEDURE AND DESIGN<br />

<strong>The</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g procedure was similar to that <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1 except a rigorous<br />

control was implemented over <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> from birth through test<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Meanwhile all <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r cues (visual, olfactory, taste and tactual) were available<br />

to <strong>the</strong> neonates from birth to 5 to 15 m<strong>in</strong> prior test<strong>in</strong>g. To ensure that <strong>the</strong> neonates<br />

were at no po<strong>in</strong>t exposed to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> prior test<strong>in</strong>g, Experimenter 1<br />

attended <strong>the</strong> delivery <strong>of</strong> all <strong>the</strong> neonates and rema<strong>in</strong>ed with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs and<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>fants from birth through test<strong>in</strong>g. To successfully control for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

<strong>voice</strong>, only one pair (mo<strong>the</strong>r–<strong>in</strong>fant) was monitored at a time until <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g was<br />

over.<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants were reta<strong>in</strong>ed on <strong>the</strong> ward at all times <strong>in</strong> bass<strong>in</strong>ets, located adjacent<br />

to <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>rs’ bed. <strong>The</strong>y were positioned <strong>in</strong> a way that <strong>the</strong>y faced each o<strong>the</strong>r,<br />

and could <strong>the</strong>refore see each o<strong>the</strong>r from <strong>the</strong>ir bed. <strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs were <strong>in</strong>structed to<br />

<strong>in</strong>teract with <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>fant, as <strong>the</strong>y would usually do without us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>voice</strong> or<br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g any sound. <strong>The</strong>y were encouraged to establish visual contacts with <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants at birth, and at least once every 2–4 h dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> feed<strong>in</strong>g–sooth<strong>in</strong>g<br />

process. <strong>The</strong>re were cases ðn ¼ 4Þ where <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs made sounds, and <strong>the</strong><br />

participants were discarded from <strong>the</strong> sample before test<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

<strong>The</strong> rout<strong>in</strong>e <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hospital helped <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> success <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>voice</strong> control. <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant<br />

was first kept with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Intensive Care Unit or <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> nurs<strong>in</strong>g room<br />

for <strong>the</strong> first 2 h. Once on <strong>the</strong> wards, <strong>the</strong>re were sometimes sounds and noises at a<br />

lower level, but far from <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant. Aga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>the</strong> Malay babies ðn ¼ 9Þ heard <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

fa<strong>the</strong>r’s call for prayer far from <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r to prevent any association with <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face. <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>terobserver agreement for <strong>the</strong> test trials <strong>of</strong> eight <strong>in</strong>fants,<br />

measured as <strong>the</strong> correlation between <strong>the</strong> observers’ look<strong>in</strong>g times to both sides,<br />

was high, (Pearson r ¼ 0:87).<br />

Table 2.<br />

<strong>Infant</strong>s’ preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face across Trials <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> no <strong>voice</strong> condition<br />

<strong>Infant</strong> Sex Age (h) Trial 1 Trial 2 Comb<strong>in</strong>ed trials<br />

1 M 5 40 60 50<br />

2 M 5.3 80 60 70<br />

3 M 6.55 30 5 17.5<br />

4 M 8 50 55 72.5<br />

5 M 7.3 65 65 65<br />

6 M 6 30 90 60<br />

7 F 5 40 45 42.5<br />

8 F 5.2 60 40 50<br />

9 F 5 60 40 50<br />

10 F 4.27 45 65 55<br />

11 F 7 20 50 35<br />

12 F 3.17 45 65 30<br />

13 F 5.54 10 50 72.5<br />

14 F 12 65 80 72.5<br />

Mean 6.09 45.71 55 53.03<br />

S.D. 2.12 19.3 20.19 17.26<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


38<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> data were expressed <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> PTLT <strong>in</strong>fants spent fixat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

face on each trial. Look<strong>in</strong>g percentages for both trials were also averaged for each<br />

participant (Table 2).<br />

To assess <strong>the</strong> <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong> experience with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> <strong>in</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face, s<strong>in</strong>gle-sample t test were conducted on <strong>the</strong><br />

PTLTs to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face on trials 1, 2 and on <strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed trials (Trials 1 and<br />

2). As a group, <strong>the</strong> newborn <strong>in</strong>fants failed to significantly discrim<strong>in</strong>ate and<br />

recognize <strong>the</strong> face <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r on trial 1 (tð13Þ ¼ 0:83, p ¼ ns), trial 2<br />

(tð13Þ ¼0:92, p ¼ ns), and on <strong>the</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed trials (tð13Þ ¼0:65, p ¼ ns). Figure 2<br />

displays <strong>the</strong> means percentage preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face across trials.<br />

Experience with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> prior test<strong>in</strong>g seems necessary for mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

face recognition. <strong>The</strong> question how much experience with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–<br />

face is needed for such learn<strong>in</strong>g to develop rema<strong>in</strong>s however, unanswered.<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>r analyses were conducted to determ<strong>in</strong>e whe<strong>the</strong>r preference for <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r differed as a function <strong>of</strong> condition and position <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face. <strong>The</strong><br />

performance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1 was compared to that <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>in</strong><br />

Experiment 2. A mixed factorial design with condition (<strong>voice</strong> vs no <strong>voice</strong>) and<br />

side preferences (position <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r on <strong>the</strong> right side vs left side) as ma<strong>in</strong><br />

factors revealed no ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> condition (Fð1; 26Þ ¼3:09, p ¼ ns), side<br />

preferences (Fð1; 26Þ ¼0:001, p ¼ ns) or <strong>in</strong>teraction, condition X side preferences<br />

(Fð1; 26Þ ¼0:099, p ¼ ns). <strong>The</strong> results <strong>in</strong>dicated that although <strong>the</strong> newborn <strong>in</strong>fants<br />

showed no significant differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir preferential behaviour across<br />

conditions, a slight preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r was demonstrated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>voice</strong><br />

condition (Table 3). Fur<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants demonstrated a slight preference for <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face shown on <strong>the</strong> left side <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>voice</strong> experiment. This pattern was<br />

however, reversed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> no-<strong>voice</strong> experiment. (Figure 3). Perhaps when <strong>in</strong>fants<br />

failed to discrim<strong>in</strong>ate between <strong>the</strong> two faces, <strong>the</strong>y tended to look more to <strong>the</strong> right<br />

side. This is consistent with early reports suggest<strong>in</strong>g significant head turns<br />

towards <strong>the</strong> right side <strong>in</strong> 3 month-old <strong>in</strong>fants (Gesell and Ames, 1947; Turkewitz<br />

et al., 1965). In a study by Bahrick et al. (1996) side preferences was however not<br />

significant at 5 or 8 months.<br />

An <strong>in</strong>terview was conducted with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> female stranger at <strong>the</strong><br />

end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> each <strong>in</strong>fant <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> no-<strong>voice</strong> experiment. We asked whe<strong>the</strong>r<br />

Figure 2. Percentage preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face across trials <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> no <strong>voice</strong><br />

condition.<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 39<br />

Table 3. Preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face across<br />

conditions (<strong>voice</strong>–no-<strong>voice</strong> condition)<br />

Voice<br />

No <strong>voice</strong><br />

<strong>Infant</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r Mo<strong>the</strong>r<br />

1 57.5 50<br />

2 72.5 70<br />

3 39 17.5<br />

4 64 72.5<br />

5 67.5 65<br />

6 57 60<br />

7 55 42.5<br />

8 63 50<br />

9 66.5 50<br />

10 54.5 55<br />

11 55.5 35<br />

12 62.5 30<br />

13 51 72.5<br />

14 54 72.5<br />

Mean 58.53 53.03<br />

S.D. 8.34 17.26<br />

Figure 3. Percentage preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face across conditions (<strong>voice</strong>–no <strong>voice</strong>).<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant was attracted to <strong>the</strong> stimulus faces. For <strong>the</strong> test trials <strong>of</strong> eight <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>the</strong><br />

agreement that <strong>in</strong>fants were not attracted to <strong>the</strong> faces was high (Pearson,<br />

r ¼ 0:87).<br />

To conclude, <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from Experiment 2 <strong>in</strong>dicate that newborn <strong>in</strong>fants are<br />

not able to recognize <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face without a previous experience with <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>. <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> face perception <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> neonatal period seems<br />

to be, <strong>the</strong>refore, cont<strong>in</strong>gent on a prior exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>. It is worth<br />

not<strong>in</strong>g that compared to our previous test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>of</strong> Expt 1, neonates<br />

showed only few head turns towards <strong>the</strong> stimulus faces <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> no-<strong>voice</strong><br />

condition. Never<strong>the</strong>less evidence suggests that defense and orientation responses<br />

are present even <strong>in</strong> uterus. Moreover, quiet<strong>in</strong>g characterized by a decreased fetal<br />

heart rate (FHR) and body movements <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g head orientation are signs <strong>of</strong><br />

attention and, perhaps, fetal cognition (Kisilevsky et al., 1998). Thus, s<strong>in</strong>ce<br />

orientation response is present <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> term foetus, <strong>the</strong>n it should be present at<br />

birth. In <strong>the</strong> present study, <strong>the</strong> removal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>, as a familiar cue,<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


40<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

probably did not only h<strong>in</strong>der <strong>the</strong> neonates’ ability to learn <strong>the</strong> amodal relations,<br />

but it also depressed <strong>the</strong>ir attention to stimuli around <strong>the</strong>m. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, a study<br />

designed to <strong>in</strong>vestigate <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants’ head turns across both <strong>the</strong> <strong>voice</strong><br />

and <strong>the</strong> no-<strong>voice</strong> conditions would certa<strong>in</strong>ly clarify <strong>the</strong> <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face at birth.<br />

EXPERIMENT 3<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> <strong>in</strong> facilitat<strong>in</strong>g early mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face preference was<br />

fur<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>vestigated us<strong>in</strong>g head turn as a response measure <strong>in</strong> Experiments 3 and<br />

4. In our previous test<strong>in</strong>g (Sai, 1990; Bushnell et al., 1989; Sai and Bushnell, 1988),<br />

<strong>the</strong> newborn <strong>in</strong>fants showed a number <strong>of</strong> head turns towards <strong>the</strong> stimulus faces.<br />

Such activity disappeared along with <strong>the</strong> control <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> <strong>in</strong> Exp. 2.<br />

It is however well established that sounds trigger head turns on <strong>the</strong> part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

newborn <strong>in</strong>fants. <strong>The</strong>y can shift <strong>the</strong>ir eyes and heads <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> direction <strong>of</strong> a sound<br />

quickly and accurately (Field et al., 1980), and that <strong>the</strong>y will make consistent head<br />

turns towards a sound source even when <strong>the</strong>re is no visible stimulus at <strong>the</strong> sound<br />

locus (Muir and Field, 1979). Moreover, young <strong>in</strong>fants can orient appropriately if<br />

<strong>the</strong>re is a visual pattern at <strong>the</strong> source <strong>of</strong> a sound (Castillo and Butterworth, 1981).<br />

However, we do not know whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>fants who have never heard <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

<strong>voice</strong> will orient towards faces, or whe<strong>the</strong>r neonates will orient to silent faces<br />

even if one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m is <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Ten neonates, five males and five females (Mean age¼7.5 h, range¼4–10; Mean<br />

birth weight¼3.40 kg, range 2.80–4.30), with normal Apgar Scores after 5 m<strong>in</strong><br />

(M ¼ 9:20, range 7–10), born at <strong>the</strong> Maternity Department, Larbaa Hospital,<br />

Algeria. <strong>The</strong> data <strong>of</strong> eight additional <strong>in</strong>fants (five males, three females) were<br />

collected and rejected from <strong>the</strong> study: four for side bias <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir look<strong>in</strong>g<br />

behaviour, three for fall<strong>in</strong>g asleep and one for experimenter error.<br />

Procedure and Design<br />

Stimuli, apparatus and procedures were identical to those <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1,<br />

except that head orientation was used as a dependent variable. Head orientation<br />

research provided reliable results (see Goren et al., 1975; Maurer and Young,<br />

1983). Studies <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g sound localization (Field et al., 1980; Morrongiello<br />

et al., 1990; Muir, 1985) have reported that neonates can shift <strong>the</strong>ir eyes and heads<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> direction <strong>of</strong> a sound quickly and accurately.<br />

Head orientation (head turn) was def<strong>in</strong>ed as when <strong>the</strong> baby oriented both his<br />

eyes and his head towards <strong>the</strong> stimulus. <strong>The</strong>refore, only <strong>in</strong>stances where <strong>in</strong>fants<br />

oriented both <strong>the</strong> head and eyes towards a stimulus face were live recorded.<br />

When newborn <strong>in</strong>fants oriented towards a face by more than 45 degree relative to<br />

mid-l<strong>in</strong>e were not considered as head turns. <strong>The</strong> reason for this selection was that<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants needed to demonstrate a full head turn so that <strong>the</strong> stimulus facial features<br />

would be discrim<strong>in</strong>ated and perceived. For each head turn, a score <strong>of</strong> ‘1’ was<br />

recorded. Alternatively, a zero was written down if <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant did not show head<br />

orientation. Each neonate was given a 2 paired stimulus faces (<strong>the</strong> faces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r and that <strong>of</strong> a female stranger) preferential look<strong>in</strong>g trials, each cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 41<br />

until 20 s <strong>of</strong> head orientation had been accumulated. Stimulus presentations was<br />

<strong>in</strong> a random order, with <strong>the</strong> left–right position<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r/stranger faces<br />

counterbalanced across neonates. O<strong>the</strong>r details <strong>of</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g are as described <strong>in</strong><br />

Experiment 1.<br />

For each <strong>in</strong>fant, <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> head turns towards <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> female<br />

stranger were manually recorded on <strong>the</strong> record<strong>in</strong>g sheet by <strong>the</strong> central observer.<br />

For assessment <strong>of</strong> reliability <strong>the</strong> scor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> videotapes <strong>of</strong> five <strong>in</strong>fants was<br />

performed by two <strong>in</strong>dependent coders who were bl<strong>in</strong>d as to <strong>the</strong> identity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m was <strong>the</strong> primary coder who scored all <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>fants’ head turns. Both coders were tra<strong>in</strong>ed for this task. <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>terobserver<br />

agreement was high, (rð8Þ ¼0:87).<br />

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

A repeated measures Anova was conducted on <strong>the</strong> PTLTs to determ<strong>in</strong>e whe<strong>the</strong>r<br />

<strong>the</strong> neonates showed any ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> trials (T1 and T2) or face (Mo<strong>the</strong>r and<br />

Table 4. Number <strong>of</strong> head turns towards <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger across trials <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>voice</strong> condition<br />

<strong>Infant</strong> Sex Age (h) MT1 MT2 MCTS SCTs<br />

1 M 10 3 2 5 2 1<br />

2 M 7 5 4 9 4 2<br />

3 M 4 2 2 4 5 3<br />

4 M 9 4 7 11 6 2<br />

5 M 7 2 4 6 5 3<br />

6 F 6 4 6 10 9 5<br />

7 F 7 5 4 9 8 4<br />

8 F 8 4 3 7 5 2<br />

9 F 9 6 4 10 6 4<br />

10 F 8 3 5 8 4 2<br />

Mean 7.5 3.8 4.1 7.9 5.4 2.8<br />

S.D. 1.71 1.32 1.59 2.33 2.01 1.23<br />

Figure 4. Number <strong>of</strong> head turns towards <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger across trials <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>voice</strong> condition.<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


42<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

stranger). <strong>Infant</strong>s showed more head turns towards <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face<br />

(M ¼ 3:95) than towards <strong>the</strong> female stranger’s face (M ¼ 2:7) and this difference<br />

was significant (Fðl; 9Þ ¼15:4, p ¼ 0:003). Also, neonates turned <strong>the</strong>ir heads more<br />

towards <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r on trial 2 (M ¼ 4:1) than on Trial 1 (M ¼ 3:8), and this<br />

difference was significant (Fð1; 9Þ ¼0:018, p ¼ ns), (Table 4). <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>of</strong><br />

face x trial failed to reach significance (Fðl; 9Þ ¼0:78, p ¼ ns). Preferences for <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger are given <strong>in</strong> Figure 4.<br />

From <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs with both trials <strong>in</strong>cluded, newborn <strong>in</strong>fants demonstrated<br />

more head turns towards <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face (M ¼ 7:9) than towards <strong>the</strong> female<br />

stranger (M ¼ 5:4). Fur<strong>the</strong>r, head movements were observed but were not<br />

considered as full head turns, as def<strong>in</strong>ed above. Preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face<br />

was fur<strong>the</strong>r replicated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> present study with non-Caucasian faces, and with<br />

head orientation as a response measure. A possible <strong>in</strong>terpretation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

is that s<strong>in</strong>ce both <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> and visual cues were available from birth to<br />

<strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g, neonates might have demonstrated a long-term memory effect<br />

for <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face relations <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> recognition test.<br />

EXPERIMENT 4<br />

<strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> this study was to determ<strong>in</strong>e whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> absence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

<strong>voice</strong>, from birth through test<strong>in</strong>g, would result <strong>in</strong> less head turns towards <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 15 participants (eight males and seven females) successfully tested<br />

<strong>in</strong> this experiment while a fur<strong>the</strong>r 10 <strong>in</strong>fants were elim<strong>in</strong>ated from <strong>the</strong> sample<br />

because <strong>of</strong> side bias <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir look<strong>in</strong>g behaviour ðn ¼ 4Þ or fell asleep ðn ¼ 6Þ. <strong>The</strong><br />

mean age <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sample was 6.31 h (range 2–10), <strong>the</strong> mean birth weight was<br />

3.30 kg (range 2.98–3.60), <strong>the</strong> mean Apgar score after 5 m<strong>in</strong> was 9.50 range 7–10).<br />

<strong>The</strong>y were born and tested at <strong>the</strong> Maternity Department; Larbaa Hospital,<br />

Algeria.<br />

Procedure and Design<br />

<strong>The</strong> stimuli, apparatus and procedures were identical to those <strong>in</strong> Experiment 2,<br />

except head orientation was adopted as a dependent variable as <strong>in</strong> Experiment 3.<br />

Participants were randomly assigned to <strong>the</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g. A tra<strong>in</strong>ed observer, unaware<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subject’s mo<strong>the</strong>r, monitored head orientations us<strong>in</strong>g video tap<strong>in</strong>g. A<br />

second observer monitored head orientations for half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subjects. Interobserver<br />

reliability was calculated and <strong>the</strong> correlation between observations <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> primary and secondary observer was high (Pearson r ¼ 0:87).<br />

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

Head orientations towards <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger were calculated as before,<br />

and this <strong>in</strong>formation is set out <strong>in</strong> Table 5. <strong>The</strong> subjects had almost equal<br />

preferences for <strong>the</strong> faces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r (M ¼ 1:72) and that <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> female stranger<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 43<br />

(M ¼ 1:94). A repeated measure analysis <strong>of</strong> variance was performed with trials<br />

(trial 1 trial 2) and face (mo<strong>the</strong>r, stranger) as with<strong>in</strong> subjects factors. <strong>The</strong> results<br />

revealed no ma<strong>in</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> face (Fðl; 15Þ ¼1:77, p ¼ ns) with newborn <strong>in</strong>fants<br />

demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g no preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face when no previous postnatal<br />

exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> was available to <strong>the</strong>m from birth (Figure 5). No<br />

ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> trials (Fðl; 15Þ ¼0:51, p ¼ ns), or <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>of</strong> trials x face<br />

(Fðl; 15Þ ¼0:69, p ¼ ns) were found.<br />

Additional analyses assessed <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> experience with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong><br />

and side preferences across Experiments 3 and 4. A 2 (condition: <strong>voice</strong> vs no<br />

<strong>voice</strong>) x 2 (position <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r: right vs left) mixed factorial design was<br />

Table 5.<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> head turns towards <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> female stranger across trials<br />

<strong>Infant</strong> Sex Age (h) Mt1 Mt2 MCTs St1 St2 SCTs<br />

1 M 6 1 4 5 2 4 6<br />

2 M 9 3 2 5 3 4 7<br />

3 M 6 3 1 4 2 2 4<br />

4 M 7 1 2 3 1 3 4<br />

5 M 7 1 3 4 3 1 4<br />

6 M 5 2 1 3 1 1 2<br />

7 M 10 1 3 4 1 4 5<br />

8 M 7 2 2 4 1 2 3<br />

9 F 8 2 2 4 3 0 3<br />

10 F 9 0 3 3 2 2 4<br />

11 F 5 2 2 4 3 2 5<br />

12 F 6 2 1 3 2 1 3<br />

13 F 8 0 0 0 2 2 4<br />

14 F 4 1 2 3 3 0 3<br />

15 F 2 2 1 3 1 2 3<br />

16 F 2 1 2 3 1 1 2<br />

Mean 6.31 1.5 1.94 3.43 1.94 1.94 3.87<br />

S.D. 2.33 0.89 0.99 1.15 0.85 1.29 1.36<br />

Figure 5. Number <strong>of</strong> head turns towards <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger across trials <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

no-<strong>voice</strong> condition.<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


44<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

conducted to determ<strong>in</strong>e whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>fants showed any ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> experience<br />

with <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> or side preferences. <strong>The</strong> results revealed a significant<br />

ma<strong>in</strong> condition effect (Fð1; 24Þ ¼40:42, p ¼ 0:05) with neonates who had<br />

experience <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g more head turns toward <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face (M ¼ 3:9, S:D: ¼ 0:26) than those who had not been exposed to<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> (M ¼ 1:72, S:D: ¼ 0:213). <strong>The</strong>re were no ma<strong>in</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> side<br />

preferences (Fðl; 24Þ ¼0:208, p ¼ ns), or <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>of</strong> condition x side preference<br />

Figure 6. Number <strong>of</strong> total head turns to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger across <strong>voice</strong>–no-<strong>voice</strong><br />

conditions.<br />

Table 6. Number <strong>of</strong> total head turns towards <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger across <strong>voice</strong>–<br />

no-<strong>voice</strong> conditions<br />

Voice condition<br />

No-<strong>voice</strong> condition<br />

<strong>Infant</strong> MCTS SCTS <strong>Infant</strong> MCTS SCTS<br />

1 5 2 1 5 6<br />

2 9 4 2 5 7<br />

3 4 5 3 4 4<br />

4 11 6 4 3 4<br />

5 6 5 5 4 4<br />

6 10 9 6 3 2<br />

7 9 8 7 4 5<br />

8 7 5 8 4 3<br />

9 10 6 9 4 3<br />

10 8 4 10 3 4<br />

11 4 5<br />

12 3 3<br />

13 0 4<br />

14 3 3<br />

15 3 3<br />

16 3 2<br />

Mean 7.9 5.4 3.43 3.87<br />

S.D. 2.33 2.01 1.15 1.36<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 45<br />

for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face (Fðl; 24Þ ¼0:208, p ¼ ns). <strong>The</strong> occurrences <strong>of</strong> head turns<br />

toward <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>voice</strong> experiment were more frequent than <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

no-<strong>voice</strong> experiment regardless <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> position <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r (Figure 6). <strong>The</strong><br />

number <strong>of</strong> head turns towards <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and <strong>the</strong> stranger were calculated as<br />

before and are displayed <strong>in</strong> Table 6.<br />

GENERAL DISCUSSION<br />

<strong>The</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from <strong>the</strong>se studies show that neonates can visually recognize <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face only if a postnatal exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face comb<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

was available. A common expressed <strong>in</strong>terpretation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face recognition <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> neonatal period is <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> an <strong>in</strong>nate<br />

perceptual mechanism that detects and responds specifically to faces (Johnson<br />

and Morton, 1991; Meltz<strong>of</strong>f, 1995; Walton et al., 1997). <strong>The</strong> present results do not<br />

support this hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. If <strong>the</strong> CONSPEC mechanism, were <strong>the</strong> only mediator for<br />

face preference at birth, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>in</strong>fants would have been able to recognize <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face even when no postnatal exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> was<br />

available. As well as show<strong>in</strong>g that at birth <strong>the</strong>re is a preference for some spatial<br />

frequencies that are shared by o<strong>the</strong>r stimuli o<strong>the</strong>r than face-like pattern, would be<br />

<strong>in</strong>sufficient to claim that <strong>the</strong>re is an <strong>in</strong>nate mechanism specific for faces. <strong>The</strong><br />

present f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>dicate that preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face is not exclusively<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>ed by <strong>the</strong> unique structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> face. Ra<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>in</strong>termodal structural<br />

properties, which o<strong>the</strong>r stimuli possess, would be crucial <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

newborns’ preferences for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face.<br />

Thus, <strong>the</strong> model may be able to expla<strong>in</strong> face preference <strong>in</strong> older <strong>in</strong>fants,<br />

but it cannot account for <strong>the</strong> data from studies with real faces which<br />

had been previously associated with <strong>voice</strong> cues such as <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

<strong>voice</strong>. Nor can it expla<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> neonates’ failure to recognize <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

half-pr<strong>of</strong>ile face where <strong>in</strong>formation about <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal configuration was still<br />

available (Sai, 1994).<br />

Given that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant is born <strong>in</strong> a multimodal stimulation unit, it is very absurd<br />

to constra<strong>in</strong> preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face with<strong>in</strong> visual parameters.<br />

Alternatively, an explanation, which takes <strong>in</strong>to account both <strong>the</strong> auditory and<br />

visual <strong>in</strong>formation, would be plausible, especially that young <strong>in</strong>fants are adept<br />

perceivers <strong>of</strong> multimodal stimulation (Lewkowicz and Lickliter, 1994). <strong>The</strong>y<br />

rapidly turn <strong>the</strong>ir heads and eyes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> direction <strong>of</strong> a sound source (Wer<strong>the</strong>imer,<br />

1961; Butterworth, 1983; Muir and Clifton, 1985), and <strong>the</strong>y quickly learn arbitrary<br />

visual–auditory associations (Slater et al., 1997). Thus, <strong>the</strong> amodal <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>of</strong><br />

temporal synchrony <strong>of</strong> <strong>voice</strong> and lips seems likely to facilitate learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face comb<strong>in</strong>ation. <strong>The</strong> multimodal stimulation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

face–<strong>voice</strong> could be more powerful than Conspec <strong>in</strong> attract<strong>in</strong>g newborns’ eye and<br />

head turns.<br />

Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face association might be <strong>the</strong>refore, <strong>in</strong>itially<br />

triggered by <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>, a highly familiar stimulus from <strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong><br />

uterus. It is <strong>the</strong>n streng<strong>the</strong>ned with exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> and face<br />

association. Once this latter has been encoded, <strong>the</strong> sight <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face<br />

alone might elicit a visual recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face as demonstrated <strong>in</strong><br />

Exps. 1 and 3.<br />

<strong>The</strong> particular attractiveness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> has been documented by<br />

several authors (see Andre-Thomas, 1966; Brazelton, 1978). <strong>The</strong> argument<br />

account<strong>in</strong>g for this phenomenon could be a postnatal association <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


46<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

<strong>voice</strong> with positive re<strong>in</strong>forcers such as <strong>the</strong> sight <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r. Conversely,<br />

preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face is likely to be rooted <strong>in</strong> foetal exposure to <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s speech (Decasper and Spence, 1991; Turkewitz, 1988; Richards et al.,<br />

1992). A grow<strong>in</strong>g body <strong>of</strong> evidence has demonstrated <strong>the</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> foetal<br />

sensory systems dur<strong>in</strong>g gestation, as well <strong>the</strong> sequential functional development<br />

<strong>of</strong> sensory systems (for review, see Lecanuet and Schaal, 1996). <strong>The</strong> auditory<br />

system develops much earlier than <strong>the</strong> visual system (Gottlieb, 1971), and healthy<br />

full-term human foetuses respond to a wide variety <strong>of</strong> sounds and vibrations<br />

orig<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g from outside <strong>the</strong> womb (Birnholz and Benaceraf, 1983; Kisilevsky and<br />

Muir, 1991; Lecanuet et al., 1986; Seraf<strong>in</strong>i and L<strong>in</strong>dsay, 1984). As well, FHR and<br />

movements to vibroacoustic stimulation were reported <strong>in</strong> foetuses as early as 26<br />

weeks gestation (Kisilevsky et al., 1992). Data from studies on uterus speech<br />

transmission performed with a hydrophone near <strong>the</strong> foetal head dur<strong>in</strong>g delivery,<br />

have revealed a significant better transmission <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> maternal <strong>voice</strong> than <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

external <strong>voice</strong> (Querleu et al., 1988). We also know that <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> is <strong>the</strong><br />

only auditory stimulus that is transmitted <strong>in</strong>ternally via body tissues and bones<br />

(Petitjean, 1989); it also <strong>in</strong>volves diaphragmatic movements generat<strong>in</strong>g pressure<br />

changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> fluid medium <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> uterus. Also, prenates <strong>of</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs who<br />

listened to ‘soap opera’ <strong>the</strong>me music dur<strong>in</strong>g pregnancy showed a preference for<br />

<strong>the</strong> same music dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> postnatal period (Hepper, 1988). <strong>The</strong> longer <strong>the</strong><br />

prenatal exposure <strong>of</strong> human neonates, liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Osaka airport neighbourhood,<br />

to airplane noises, <strong>the</strong> better <strong>the</strong>y slept compared to babies whose mo<strong>the</strong>rs<br />

had lived <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> area <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> airport for shorter times dur<strong>in</strong>g pregnancy (Ando and<br />

Hattori, 1977). <strong>The</strong> work <strong>of</strong> DeCasper and Fifer (1980), DeCasper and Spence<br />

(1986), DeCasper and Prescott (1984) reveals a complexity <strong>of</strong> bond<strong>in</strong>g and<br />

language process<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> prenatal period. Given that <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong><br />

potential sensory stimulation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> foetal milieu as well <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> foetal<br />

sensory sensitivity to acoustic (e.g. Lecanuet et al., 1995) and vibroacoustic (e.g.<br />

Kisilevsky, 1995) stimuli, and <strong>the</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>uity <strong>of</strong> foetal and newborn response to<br />

sound and vibration, it is, <strong>the</strong>refore, possible that neonates are born with a longterm<br />

memory for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>.<br />

However, <strong>the</strong> question whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> neonates rapidly associate <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

<strong>voice</strong>–face at birth, or demonstrate a long-term memory effect for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

<strong>voice</strong> learned while <strong>the</strong> foetus was still <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s womb rema<strong>in</strong>s<br />

unanswered. Never<strong>the</strong>less, data from one <strong>of</strong> our studies <strong>in</strong> which newborn<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants were exposed to both <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> and face, among o<strong>the</strong>rs, show<br />

that mo<strong>the</strong>r preference was stronger with shorter delays than with longer delays<br />

between last exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r and preference test<strong>in</strong>g (Sai, 1990), <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

that learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face is very stable and established as a long-term<br />

memory. <strong>The</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face association seems, <strong>the</strong>refore, to take place at<br />

least shortly after <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fant’s birth.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs replicate and extend our previous research suggest<strong>in</strong>g a rapid<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face at birth. Such learn<strong>in</strong>g, however, vanished when a<br />

previous exposure to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face comb<strong>in</strong>ations (Exps 2 and 4) was<br />

made unavailable to neonates from birth through test<strong>in</strong>g. A prior exposure to <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face comb<strong>in</strong>ation rema<strong>in</strong>s, <strong>the</strong>refore, a significant predictor <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face recognition at least with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first days <strong>of</strong> birth. We still do not<br />

know precisely what visual features, <strong>the</strong> newborns associated to <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s<br />

<strong>voice</strong> <strong>in</strong> order to make <strong>the</strong>ir preferential choices. Never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong> results<br />

strongly suggest an <strong>in</strong>termodal learn<strong>in</strong>g at birth, probably facilitated by a longterm<br />

memory for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong> learned <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> conf<strong>in</strong>es <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> womb. One<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g direction for future research concerns <strong>the</strong> amount <strong>of</strong> exposure to <strong>the</strong><br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 47<br />

mo<strong>the</strong>r’s <strong>voice</strong>–face comb<strong>in</strong>ation that <strong>the</strong> newborn <strong>in</strong>fants need to develop<br />

preference for <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face.<br />

Note: A comment made by an anonymous reviewer concerns <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> related<br />

t-tests <strong>in</strong> present studies and <strong>in</strong> our previous research (Sai and Bushnell, 1988;<br />

Bushnell et al., 1989; Sai, 1990). <strong>The</strong> data have been analysed aga<strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>glesample<br />

t tests as suggested by <strong>the</strong> reviewer, and significant results were obta<strong>in</strong>ed,<br />

as is <strong>the</strong> case <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> present studies.<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

This research was partly supported by IIU) Malaysia. I gratefully acknowledge<br />

<strong>the</strong> assistance <strong>of</strong> Nabila Sai and Sarah Amrani <strong>in</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> neonates.<br />

Appreciation is expressed to <strong>the</strong> parents and <strong>in</strong>fants who participated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

study, as well as to <strong>the</strong> nurses and Staff <strong>of</strong> both Larbaa Hospital, Algeria and <strong>of</strong><br />

University Hospital <strong>of</strong> Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia. I also thank Pr. Darw<strong>in</strong> Muir,<br />

Dr R. Dillon, Dr C. Moon, an anonymous reviewer and Mohammed Defairi for<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>sightful comments on an earlier version <strong>of</strong> this paper.<br />

Portions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se data were presented at Development, 2000: A conference on<br />

Developmental Psychology, University <strong>of</strong> Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada, and at <strong>the</strong><br />

XII Biennial International Conference on <strong>Infant</strong> studies, Brighton, UK. July 2000.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Ando Y, Hattori H. 1977. Effects <strong>of</strong> noise on sleep <strong>of</strong> babies. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Acoustical Society<br />

<strong>of</strong> America 47: 1128–1130.<br />

Andre-Thomas AS. 1966. Locomotion from prenatal life. Spatic Society. He<strong>in</strong>emann:<br />

London.<br />

Ashton R. 1973. <strong>The</strong> state variable <strong>in</strong> neonatal research: A review. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly<br />

19: 3–20.<br />

Asl<strong>in</strong> RN. 1987. Visual and auditory development <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fancy. In Handbook <strong>of</strong> <strong>Infant</strong><br />

Development (2nd edn), Os<strong>of</strong>sky JD (ed.). Wiley: New York.<br />

Bahrick LE. 1987. <strong>Infant</strong>s’ <strong>in</strong>termodal perception <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two levels <strong>of</strong> temporal structure <strong>in</strong><br />

natural events. <strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 10: 387–416.<br />

Bahrick LE. 1988. Intermodal learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fancy: learn<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> two k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>variant relations <strong>in</strong> audible and visible events. Child Development 59: 197–209.<br />

Bahrick LE. 1994. <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants’ sensitivity to arbitrary <strong>in</strong>termodal relations.<br />

Ecological Psychology 6: 111–123.<br />

Bahrick LE, Moss L, Fodil C. 1996. <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> visual self-recognition <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fancy.<br />

Ecological Psychology 8: 189–208.<br />

Birnholz JC, Benaceraf BR. 1983. <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> human fetal hear<strong>in</strong>g. Science 222:<br />

516–518.<br />

Brazelton TB. 1978. <strong>The</strong> remarquebale talents <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> newborn. Birth and <strong>the</strong> Family Journal 5:<br />

4–10.<br />

Bushnell EW, Boudreau JP. 1991. <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> Haptic perception dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>fancy. In<br />

<strong>The</strong> Psychology <strong>of</strong> Touch, Heller MA, Schiff W (eds). Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ.<br />

Bushnell EW, Boudreau JP. 1993. Motor development and <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d: <strong>The</strong> potential <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

motor abilities as a determ<strong>in</strong>ant <strong>of</strong> aspects <strong>of</strong> perceptual development. Child Development<br />

64: 1005–1021.<br />

Bushnell IWR, Sai FZ, Mull<strong>in</strong> J. 1989. Neonatal recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face. British<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Developmental Psychology 7: 3–15.<br />

Butterworth G. 1983. Structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> human <strong>in</strong>fancy. In Advances <strong>in</strong> Infancy<br />

Research, Lipsitt LP, Rovee-Collier CK (eds), vol. 2. Ablex: Norwood, NJ; 1–19.<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


48<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

Caron AJ, Caron RF, MacLean DJ. 1988. <strong>Infant</strong> discrim<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong> naturalistic emotional<br />

expressions: <strong>the</strong> <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong> face and <strong>voice</strong>. Child Development 59: 604–616.<br />

Castillo M, Butterworth GE. 1981. Neonatal localisation <strong>of</strong> a sound <strong>in</strong> visual space.<br />

Perception 10: 331–338.<br />

Clifton RK, Morrongiello BA, Kulig JW, Dowd JM. 1981. Newborns’orientation towards<br />

sound: possible implications for cortical development. Child Development 53: 833–838.<br />

DeCasper AJ, Fifer W. 1980. Of human bond<strong>in</strong>g: newborns prefer <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>rs’ <strong>voice</strong>s.<br />

Science 208: 1174–1176.<br />

DeCasper AJ, Prescott PA. 1984. Human newborns’ perception <strong>of</strong> male <strong>voice</strong>s: preference,<br />

discrim<strong>in</strong>ation, and re<strong>in</strong>forc<strong>in</strong>g value. Developmental Psychobiology 17: 481–491.<br />

DeCasper AJ, Spence MJ. 1986. Prenatal maternal speech <strong>in</strong>fluences newborns’ perception<br />

<strong>of</strong> speech sounds. <strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 9: 133–150.<br />

DeCasper AJ, Spence MJ. 1991. Auditory mediated behavior dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> prenatal period: a<br />

cognitive view. In Newborn Attention Biological Constra<strong>in</strong>ts and <strong>the</strong> Influence <strong>of</strong> Experience,<br />

Weiss MJS, Zealot PR (eds). Ablex: Norwood, NJ; 142–176.<br />

Dziurawiec S. 1987. Neonates’ attention to faces. In Developmental Aspects <strong>of</strong> Face<br />

Recognition, Bruce V, Ellis H, Young A (eds). Unpublished report on ESRC workshop,<br />

Grange-over-sands.<br />

Fantz RL. 1966. Pattern discrim<strong>in</strong>ation and selective attention as determ<strong>in</strong>ants <strong>of</strong><br />

perceptual development from birth. In Perceptual Development <strong>in</strong> Children, Kidd AH,<br />

Rivoire JL (eds). International Universities Press: New York.<br />

Fearon I, Kisilevsky BS. 1994. Fetal responsiveness to vibroacoustic stimulation: <strong>in</strong>fluence<br />

<strong>of</strong> vibrator characteristics. Poster presented at <strong>the</strong> 9th International Conference on <strong>Infant</strong><br />

Studies, Paris.<br />

Field J, Muir D, Pilon R, S<strong>in</strong>clair M, Dodwell P. 1980. <strong>Infant</strong>s’ orientation to lateral sounds<br />

from birth to three months. Child Development 51: 295–298.<br />

Field TM, Cohen D, Garcia R, Greenberg R. 1984. Mo<strong>the</strong>r–stranger face discrim<strong>in</strong>ation by<br />

<strong>the</strong> newborn. <strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 7: 19–25.<br />

Gesell A, Ames LB. 1947. <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> handedness. Genetic Psychology 70: 155–176.<br />

Gibson EJ. 1983. Development <strong>of</strong> knowledge about <strong>in</strong>termodal unity. Two views. In Piaget<br />

and <strong>the</strong> Foundations <strong>of</strong> Knowledge, Liben LS (ed.). Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ; 19–41.<br />

Goren CC, Sarty M, Wu PYK. 1975. Visual follow<strong>in</strong>g and pattern discrim<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong> face-like<br />

stimuli by newborn <strong>in</strong>fants. Pediatrics 56: 544–548.<br />

Gottlieb G. 1971. Development <strong>of</strong> Species Identification <strong>in</strong> Birds. University <strong>of</strong> Chicago Press:<br />

Chicago.<br />

Gottlieb G. 1978. Development <strong>of</strong> species identification <strong>in</strong> duckl<strong>in</strong>gs. IV. Changes <strong>in</strong><br />

specific perception caused by auditory deprivation. Journal <strong>of</strong> Comparative Physiological<br />

Psychology 92: 375–387.<br />

Gottlieb G. 1991. Epigenetic systems view <strong>of</strong> human development. Developmental<br />

Psychology 27: 33–34.<br />

Hepper PG. 1988. Fetal ‘soap’ addiction. <strong>The</strong> Lancet 1: 1147–1148.<br />

Johnson MH, Morton J. 1991. Biology and Cognitive Development. Blackwell: Oxford, UK.<br />

Kisilevsky BS. 1995. <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> stimulus and subject variables on human fetal<br />

responses to sound and vibration. In Fetal Development: A Psychobiological Perspective,<br />

Lecanuet J-P, Fifer WP, Kransnegor NA, Smo<strong>the</strong>rman WP (eds). Lawrence Erlbaum:<br />

Hillsdale, NJ; 263–278.<br />

Kisilevsky BS, Fearon I, Muir DW. 1998. Fetuses differentiate vibroacoustic stimuli. <strong>Infant</strong><br />

Behavior and Development 1(1): 25–46.<br />

Kisilevsky BS, Muir DW. 1991. Human fetal and subsequent newborn responses to sound<br />

and vibration. <strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 14: 1–26.<br />

Kisilevsky BS, Muir DW, Low JA. 1992. Maturation <strong>of</strong> human fetal responses to<br />

vibroacoustic stimulation. Child Development 63: 1497–1508.<br />

Kuhl PK, Meltz<strong>of</strong>f AN. 1982. <strong>The</strong> bimodal perception <strong>of</strong> speech <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fancy. Science 218:<br />

1138–1141.<br />

Kuhl PK, Meltz<strong>of</strong>f AN. 1984. <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>termodal representation <strong>of</strong> speech <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants. <strong>Infant</strong><br />

Behavior and Development 7: 361–381.<br />

Lecanuet J-P, Grannier-Deferre C, Busnel M-C. 1995. Human fetal auditory perception. In<br />

Fetal Development: A Psychological Perspective, Lecanuet J-P, Fifer WP, Krasnegor NA,<br />

Smo<strong>the</strong>rman WP (eds). Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ; 239–262.<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


<strong>The</strong> Role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s Voice 49<br />

Lecanuet J-P, Grannier-Deferre C, Cohen H, LeHouezec R, Busnel M-C. 1986. Fetal<br />

responses to acoustic stimulation depend on heart rate variability pattern, stimulus<br />

<strong>in</strong>tensity, and repletion. Early Human Development 13: 269–283.<br />

Lecanuet J-P, Schaal B. 1996. Fetal sensory competencies. European Journal <strong>of</strong> Obstetrics and<br />

Gynecology and Reproductive Biology 68: 1–23.<br />

Lewkowicz DJ. 1986. Developmental changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants’ bisensory response to<br />

synchronous durations. <strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 9: 335–353.<br />

Lewkowicz DJ, Edmondson D. 1993. <strong>Infant</strong>s’ responsiveness to <strong>the</strong> multimodal properties<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> human face. Presented at <strong>the</strong> society for Research <strong>in</strong> Child Development Meet<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

New Orleans, LA, March.<br />

Lewkowicz DJ, Lickliter R. 1994. <strong>The</strong> Development <strong>of</strong> Intersensory Perception: Comparative<br />

Perspectives. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ.<br />

Lewkowicz DJ, Lickliter R (eds). 1995. <strong>The</strong> Development <strong>of</strong> Intersensory Perception. Erlbaum:<br />

Hillsdale, NJ.<br />

Maurer D, Young R. 1983. Newborns’ follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> natural and distorted and arrangements<br />

<strong>of</strong> facial features. <strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 6: 127–131.<br />

Meltz<strong>of</strong>f AN. 1995. <strong>Infant</strong>s’ understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> people and th<strong>in</strong>gs: from body imitation to<br />

folk psychology. In <strong>The</strong> Body and <strong>The</strong> Self, Bermudez JL, Marcel A, Ellan N (eds). MIT:<br />

Cambridge, MA; 43–69.<br />

Meltz<strong>of</strong>f AN, Borton RW. 1979. Intermodal match<strong>in</strong>g by human neonates. Nature 282:<br />

403–404.<br />

Moon C, Cooper R, Fifer W. 1993. <strong>Infant</strong>s prefer native language. <strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and<br />

Development 16: 495–500.<br />

Moon C, Panneton-Cooper R, Fifer WP. 1991. Two-day-olds prefer <strong>the</strong> maternal language,<br />

I.S.D.P. Meet<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Morrongiello BA, Fenwick KD, Chance G. 1990. Sound localization acuity <strong>in</strong> very young<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants: an observer-based test<strong>in</strong>g procedure. Developmental Psychology 26: 75–84.<br />

Muir DW. 1985. <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>fants’ auditory spatial sensitivity. In Auditory<br />

Development <strong>in</strong> Infancy. Trehub SE, Schneider B (eds). Plenum Press: New York; 51–83.<br />

Muir DW, Clifton R. 1985. <strong>Infant</strong>s’ orientation to localization <strong>of</strong> sound sources. In <strong>The</strong><br />

Measurement <strong>of</strong> Audition and Vision Dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> First Year <strong>of</strong> Life: A Methodological Overview,<br />

Gottlieb G, Krasnegor N (eds). Ablex: Norwood, NJ; 171–194.<br />

Muir DW, Field J. 1979. Newborn <strong>in</strong>fants orient to sounds. Child Development 50: 431–436.<br />

Pascalis O, Dechonen S, Morton J, Duruelle C, Grenet F. 1995. Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face recognition <strong>in</strong><br />

neonates: a replication and an extension. <strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 18: 79–85.<br />

Petitjean C. 1989. Une condition de l’audition foetale: la conduction sonore osseuse.<br />

Consequences cl<strong>in</strong>iques et applications pratiques envisagees. M.D., University <strong>of</strong><br />

Besancon.<br />

Querleu D, Renard X, Versyp F, Paris-Delrue L, Crep<strong>in</strong> G. 1988. Fetal hear<strong>in</strong>g. European<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Obstetrics and Reproductive Biology 29: 191–212.<br />

Reardon P, Bushnell EW. 1988. <strong>Infant</strong>s’ sensitivity to arbitrary pair<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> color and taste.<br />

<strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 11: 245–250.<br />

Richards DS, Frentzen B, Gerhardt KJ, McCann ME, Abrahams RM. 1992. Sound levels <strong>in</strong><br />

human uterus. Obstetrics and Gynecology 80: 186–190.<br />

Rose SA, Ruff HA. 1987. Cross-modal abilities <strong>in</strong> human <strong>in</strong>fants. In Handbook <strong>of</strong> <strong>Infant</strong><br />

Development (2nd edn), Os<strong>of</strong>sky J (ed.). Wiley: New York; 318–362.<br />

Rubel EW. 1985. Auditory system development. In Measurement <strong>of</strong> Audition and Vision <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

First Year <strong>of</strong> Postnatal Life, Gottlieb G, Krasnegor NA (eds). Ablex: Norwood, NJ; 53–90.<br />

Sai FZ. 1990a. <strong>The</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> human face perception by very young <strong>in</strong>fants. Ph.D. <strong>The</strong>sis,<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Glasgow, Scotland, UK, submitted.<br />

Sai FZ. 1990b. Le nouveau- ne reconnait sa mere. El Watan: Algeria.<br />

Sai FZ. 1994. <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> perception <strong>in</strong>variance across <strong>the</strong> first three months <strong>of</strong><br />

life. Paper presented at <strong>the</strong> International Conference <strong>of</strong> <strong>Infant</strong>s Studies, Paris, France.<br />

Organised by <strong>the</strong> Society <strong>of</strong> <strong>Infant</strong> Studies.<br />

Sai FZ. 1996. Perception <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>variance at birth. Paper presented at <strong>the</strong> Medical Convention,<br />

Malaysia.<br />

Sai FZ. 1997. Evidence for <strong>in</strong>termodal perception at birth. Paper presented at <strong>the</strong><br />

International Conference on Psychology, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia.<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)


50<br />

F. Z. Sai<br />

Sai FZ. 2000. Neonatal recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face. Evidence for <strong>in</strong>termodal<br />

perception. Paper presented at <strong>the</strong> XII Biennial International Conference on <strong>Infant</strong><br />

Studies (ICIS), Brighton, UK.<br />

Sai FZ, Bushnell IWR. 1987. Neonatal recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s face. University <strong>of</strong><br />

Glasgow Psychological Report. No. 87/1: Glasgow University.<br />

Sai FZ, Bushnell IWR. 1988. <strong>The</strong> perception <strong>of</strong> faces <strong>in</strong> different poses by 1-month olds.<br />

British Journal <strong>of</strong> Developmental Psychology 6: 35–41.<br />

Scibetta JJ, Rosen MG, Hochbbberg CJ, Chik L. 1971. Human fetal bra<strong>in</strong> response to sound<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g labor. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Obstetrics and Gynecology 109: 82–85.<br />

Seraf<strong>in</strong>i P, L<strong>in</strong>dsay MBJ, Nagey DA, Pupk<strong>in</strong> MJ, Tseng P, Crenshaw C Jr. 1984. Antepartum<br />

fetal heart rate response to sound stimulation: <strong>the</strong> acoustic stimulation test. American<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Obstetrics and Gynecology 148: 41–45.<br />

Slater A, Brown E, Badenoch M. 1997. Intermodal perception at birth: newborn <strong>in</strong>fants’<br />

memory for arbitrary auditory–visual pair<strong>in</strong>gs. Early Development and Parent<strong>in</strong>g 6:<br />

99–104.<br />

Slater A, Butterworth G, Samuels C. 1998. Newborn <strong>in</strong>fants prefer attractive faces. <strong>Infant</strong><br />

Behavior and Development 21(2): 345–354.<br />

Spelke B, Owsley CJ. 1979. Intermodal exploration and knowledge <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>fancy. <strong>Infant</strong><br />

Behaviour and Development 2: 13–24.<br />

Turati C, Simion F, Milani I, Umila C. 2002. Newborns’ preference for faces: what is crucial.<br />

Developmental Psychology 38(6): 875–882.<br />

Turkewitz G. 1988. A prenatal source for <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> hemispheric specialization. In<br />

Bra<strong>in</strong> Lateralization <strong>in</strong> Children, Molfese DL, Segalowitz J (eds). Guilford Press: New York;<br />

73–83.<br />

Turkewitz G, Gordon EW, Birch HG. 1965. Head turn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> human neonate:<br />

spontaneous pattern. <strong>The</strong> Journal <strong>of</strong> Genetic Psychology 107: 143–158.<br />

Walker-Andrews AS. l997. <strong>Infant</strong>s’ perception <strong>of</strong> expressive behaviors: differentiation <strong>of</strong><br />

multimodal <strong>in</strong>formation. <strong>The</strong> Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong> 12(3): 437–456.<br />

Walton GE, Amstrong ES, Bower TGR. 1997. Faces as forms <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> world <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> newborn.<br />

<strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 20: 537–543.<br />

Walton GE, Bower NNJA, Bower TGR. 1992. Recognition <strong>of</strong> familiar faces by newborns.<br />

<strong>Infant</strong> Behavior and Development 15: 265–269.<br />

Walton GE, Bower TGR. 1993. Newborns form ‘prototypes’ <strong>in</strong> less than 1 m<strong>in</strong>ute.<br />

Psychological Science 4: 203–205.<br />

Wer<strong>the</strong>imer M. 1961. Psychomotor coord<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong> auditory and visual space at birth.<br />

Science 134: 1692.<br />

Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child Dev. 14: 29–50 (2005)

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!