20.01.2015 Views

Situation Awareness: Review of Mica Endsley's 1995 Articles on ...

Situation Awareness: Review of Mica Endsley's 1995 Articles on ...

Situation Awareness: Review of Mica Endsley's 1995 Articles on ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

398 June 2008 – Human Factors<br />

so, provide the less familiar reader with a sense<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the importance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the c<strong>on</strong>struct. I then describe<br />

a few important subsequent developments in the<br />

field, forecast in Endsley (<str<strong>on</strong>g>1995</str<strong>on</strong>g>b).<br />

DEFINITIONS AND FUNDAMENTALS<br />

Endsley (<str<strong>on</strong>g>1995</str<strong>on</strong>g>b) defines situati<strong>on</strong> awareness informally<br />

and intuitively as “knowing what’s going<br />

<strong>on</strong>” and, more formally, as “the percepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

elements in the envir<strong>on</strong>ment within a volume <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

time and space, the comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their meaning<br />

and the projecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their status in the near<br />

future” (p. 36). The latter definiti<strong>on</strong> appears to have<br />

stood the test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> time reas<strong>on</strong>ably well and is expressed<br />

in highly similar terms by other authors<br />

today (e.g., Durso, Raws<strong>on</strong>, & Girotto, 2007; Tenney<br />

& Pew, 2007). Importantly, then, her treatment<br />

both “drills down” to the comp<strong>on</strong>ents <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

and draws back to c<strong>on</strong>sider it in a larger c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> human performance and cogniti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In drilling down, Endsley draws the careful<br />

distincti<strong>on</strong> between the three levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> situati<strong>on</strong><br />

awareness (SA) defined by percepti<strong>on</strong> (including<br />

“noticing”), comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, and projecti<strong>on</strong>. Clearly,<br />

higher levels depend <strong>on</strong> the success <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> lower<br />

levels. Thus, the air traffic c<strong>on</strong>troller may first notice<br />

a change in trajectory (or <strong>on</strong>set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a c<strong>on</strong>flict<br />

alert), then comprehend that this means aircraft are<br />

now <strong>on</strong> a c<strong>on</strong>verging trajectory (laterally or vertically),<br />

and finally understand when, in the future,<br />

a c<strong>on</strong>flict may take place and how serious it will<br />

be. The diagnostic distincti<strong>on</strong> between these three<br />

levels is important, not <strong>on</strong>ly because they point to<br />

different perceptual/cognitive operati<strong>on</strong>s but also<br />

because breakdowns in each may have very different<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequences for addressing them, through<br />

training or system design. For example, a breakdown<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Level 1 SA would lead to the design <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

better alerts. A breakdown <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Level 3 SA would<br />

lead to incorporati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> predictive displays.<br />

Drawing back, Endsley c<strong>on</strong>siders the definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

in the c<strong>on</strong>text <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> other aspects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> human-system<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong> and, in so doing, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fers the important<br />

service <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> explicitly characterizing what SAis not<br />

(exclusi<strong>on</strong>s), hence avoiding the pitfalls <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> merely<br />

characterizing SA as “good performance” and<br />

thereby also avoiding the creati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a somewhat<br />

vacuous c<strong>on</strong>cept. These distincti<strong>on</strong>s are important<br />

and real both in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> models <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> human informati<strong>on</strong><br />

processing and characterizati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> system<br />

behavior. In particular, her writing emphasizes<br />

three things that SA is not.<br />

First, SA is not acti<strong>on</strong> or performance. That<br />

is, the understanding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a situati<strong>on</strong> is quite distinct<br />

from the manual or vocal acti<strong>on</strong> taken in resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

to that situati<strong>on</strong> (even if that acti<strong>on</strong> is an<br />

informati<strong>on</strong>-seeking <strong>on</strong>e designed to improve<br />

SA). In this regard, the distincti<strong>on</strong> between SA<br />

and acti<strong>on</strong> is analogous to the classic distincti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in decisi<strong>on</strong> theory between state-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-the-world estimati<strong>on</strong><br />

and choice (Edwards, 1961) and, in medicine,<br />

between diagnosis and treatment (Garg<br />

et al., 2005). Thus, <strong>on</strong>e might say that good SA is<br />

generally necessary but not sufficient for good<br />

performance. An operator with excellent SA <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<br />

failing system may not possess the knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

procedures to remedy the failure or may not have<br />

the motor executi<strong>on</strong> skills to implement that remedy.<br />

In c<strong>on</strong>trast, when automati<strong>on</strong> can support effective<br />

performance, is it generally possible to<br />

have good system performance manifest in the absence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> good SA. As an example here, a pilot who<br />

is flying a flight director may be accurately nulling<br />

the error symbol (and hence performing well by<br />

flying the plane al<strong>on</strong>g the desired path) but have<br />

very little awareness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> where the plane is located<br />

over the ground.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, SAis not the same as l<strong>on</strong>g-term memory<br />

knowledge. Here it is important that the c<strong>on</strong>struct<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> SAis primarily applicable <strong>on</strong>ly in dynamic<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s where variables are changing, typically<br />

over the course <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>ds or minutes. The definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fered by Durso, Raws<strong>on</strong>, and Girotto (2007),<br />

“comprehensi<strong>on</strong> or understanding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a dynamic<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ment” (p. 164), is appropriate here. Hence,<br />

static (declarative or procedural) knowledge about<br />

the state <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the system, characterizing l<strong>on</strong>g-term<br />

memory, is less directly relevant to the c<strong>on</strong>tents <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

SA, particularly at Level 2. Stated in other terms,<br />

the “time c<strong>on</strong>stant” <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> l<strong>on</strong>g-term memory (and its<br />

associated c<strong>on</strong>structs such as scripts, schemas, and<br />

mental models) is in the order <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> hours, days, and<br />

years, whereas the time c<strong>on</strong>stant <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dynamic system<br />

changes, in which SAis the most relevant c<strong>on</strong>struct,<br />

is in the order <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>ds, minutes, or, at<br />

most, a few hours. As such, the memory system<br />

more closely associated with SAappears to be that<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> l<strong>on</strong>g-term working memory (Durso & Gr<strong>on</strong>lund,<br />

1999; Ericss<strong>on</strong> & Kintch, <str<strong>on</strong>g>1995</str<strong>on</strong>g>; Wickens,<br />

2000).<br />

Third, the product <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> SA is not the same as the<br />

process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> updating situati<strong>on</strong> awareness. This is<br />

a fuzzier exclusi<strong>on</strong>, paralleling the distincti<strong>on</strong><br />

that Adams, Tenney, and Pew (<str<strong>on</strong>g>1995</str<strong>on</strong>g>) make in the

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!