20.01.2015 Views

Scavenging Behaviors of Cottonmouth Snakes at ... - ResearchGate

Scavenging Behaviors of Cottonmouth Snakes at ... - ResearchGate

Scavenging Behaviors of Cottonmouth Snakes at ... - ResearchGate

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Herpetological Review, 2002, 33(x), xx–xx.<br />

© 2002 by Society for the Study <strong>of</strong> Amphibians and Reptiles<br />

<strong>Scavenging</strong> <strong>Behaviors</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Cottonmouth</strong> <strong>Snakes</strong> <strong>at</strong><br />

Island Bird Rookeries<br />

HARVEY B. LILLYWHITE *<br />

COLEMAN M. SHEEHY III<br />

and<br />

MARSHALL D. MCCUE<br />

Seahorse Key Marine Labor<strong>at</strong>ory, Department <strong>of</strong> Zoology<br />

The University <strong>of</strong> Florida, Gainesville, Florida 32611-8525, USA<br />

e-mail [HBL]: hbl@zoo.ufl.edu<br />

*<br />

To whom correspondence should be sent concerning this manuscript.<br />

<strong>Cottonmouth</strong> snakes, Agkistrodon piscivorus, are well known<br />

as generalist and opportunistic feeders consuming fish, amphibians,<br />

rodents, birds, various reptiles including snakes and conspecifics,<br />

and carrion (Gloyd and Conant 1990; Savitzky 1992). The<br />

Florida cottonmouth, A. p. conanti, is an abundant snake throughout<br />

much <strong>of</strong> Florida, with well-established popul<strong>at</strong>ions on some<br />

<strong>of</strong> the <strong>of</strong>fshore islands. The success <strong>of</strong> this species in varied habit<strong>at</strong>s<br />

is rel<strong>at</strong>ed, in part, to the wide range <strong>of</strong> prey items th<strong>at</strong> provide<br />

a potential energy base for popul<strong>at</strong>ions (see also Fritts 1988).<br />

Insular popul<strong>at</strong>ions <strong>of</strong> cottonmouths inhabiting the Cedar Keys<br />

<strong>of</strong> Florida have been studied by Wharton (1966, 1969) who focused<br />

his investig<strong>at</strong>ions on an unusually dense popul<strong>at</strong>ion <strong>of</strong> snakes<br />

inhabiting Seahorse Key. This island is approxim<strong>at</strong>ely 64 ha in<br />

size and was estim<strong>at</strong>ed to support 600 cottonmouths during the<br />

period when Wharton conducted his studies. The island has no<br />

permanent fresh w<strong>at</strong>er, and the cottonmouths inhabit a mixed hardwood<br />

forest where they are entirely terrestrial. These snakes subsist<br />

largely or entirely on fish th<strong>at</strong> are dropped or regurgit<strong>at</strong>ed by<br />

colonial wading birds th<strong>at</strong> nest on the island in large numbers from<br />

March through September or October. At present, the colonial<br />

nesting birds are largely represented by Brown Pelicans (Pelecanus<br />

occidentaliscarolinensis), Double-crested Cormorants<br />

(Phalacrocorax auritus), White Ibis (Eudocimus albus), and several<br />

species <strong>of</strong> herons and egrets.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> us (HL) has confirmed and extended the observ<strong>at</strong>ions <strong>of</strong><br />

cottonmouth behaviors recorded by Wharton. At Seahorse Key<br />

these snakes characteristically forage <strong>at</strong> night on substr<strong>at</strong>e directly<br />

bene<strong>at</strong>h bird rookeries where avian excreta alter the veget<strong>at</strong>ion<br />

and humus layers <strong>of</strong> soil. Foraging snakes methodically investig<strong>at</strong>e<br />

these environments where numerous objects become spl<strong>at</strong>tered<br />

with fluids th<strong>at</strong> are regurgit<strong>at</strong>ed or excreted by birds and<br />

presumably contain fish odors. The snakes largely consume fish,<br />

although occasional young birds fallen from nests might also be<br />

ingested. It is not clear, however, whether birds are e<strong>at</strong>en for their<br />

own sake or because they bear fish odors. Dead birds th<strong>at</strong> are ignored<br />

by snakes are frequently found in or near the rookeries. Both<br />

their shape and size appear to render them too difficult to swallow.<br />

Foraging snakes are generally quite placid and can be approached<br />

and observed <strong>at</strong> close distances. They readily e<strong>at</strong> fish<br />

when these are presented to, or thrown near, a foraging snake.<br />

<strong>Snakes</strong> are also observed to investig<strong>at</strong>e and grasp objects such as<br />

plant m<strong>at</strong>erials, bones, or bird fe<strong>at</strong>hers th<strong>at</strong> have contacted avian<br />

excreta or fish fluids. These are usually released following contact<br />

with the internal tissues <strong>of</strong> the mouth.<br />

Recently we observed some unusual scavenging behaviors in a<br />

foraging female cottonmouth estim<strong>at</strong>ed to be about 110 cm in total<br />

body length. The snake was first seen <strong>at</strong> 1810 h EDST and was<br />

observed for 20 min while it foraged in an open area bene<strong>at</strong>h a<br />

Brown Pelican rookery <strong>at</strong> Seahorse Key, 2 June 2001. The substr<strong>at</strong>e<br />

was comprised <strong>of</strong> sandy soil covered with many fallen twigs,<br />

dried leaves, and other sc<strong>at</strong>tered debris. Most <strong>of</strong> these objects were<br />

white or mottled due to ur<strong>at</strong>es th<strong>at</strong> were excreted by the pelicans<br />

nesting overhead. The area was in full sun, and the ambient temper<strong>at</strong>ure<br />

and humidity <strong>at</strong> the time <strong>of</strong> observ<strong>at</strong>ion were 31.4°C and<br />

61%, respectively. The snake appeared to be in good health and<br />

was somewh<strong>at</strong> distended posteriorad, suggesting possible pregnancy.<br />

The snake crawled very slowly and deliber<strong>at</strong>ely over the substr<strong>at</strong>e<br />

while investig<strong>at</strong>ing various objects with frequent tongue<br />

flicks. We first observed the animal through binoculars from a<br />

distance <strong>of</strong> about 8 m. Eventually, we advanced closer to the snake,<br />

which continued to forage and eventually approached us within<br />

0.5 m distance. Although the snake appeared to be aware <strong>of</strong> our<br />

presence <strong>at</strong> this point, it continued to forage as described.<br />

Soon after we first saw the snake, it investig<strong>at</strong>ed the ground<br />

intensely <strong>at</strong> one loc<strong>at</strong>ion and positioned its head nearly vertically<br />

with the mouth directed downward as it grasped a dark object.<br />

When the snake lifted its head, the grasped item was seen to be<br />

soil as half <strong>of</strong> it broke away from th<strong>at</strong> being held in the mouth and<br />

fell to the ground. The snake then lifted its head upward <strong>at</strong> an<br />

angle <strong>of</strong> about 30° and swallowed the soil, which was seen to be<br />

damp and loosely held together with moisture. L<strong>at</strong>er inspection <strong>of</strong><br />

the ground on which the snake foraged revealed several loc<strong>at</strong>ions<br />

where the soil was wetted, due either to excreta or regurgit<strong>at</strong>ed<br />

fluids from birds overhead.<br />

Following this act, the snake continued to investig<strong>at</strong>e objects<br />

th<strong>at</strong> were on the ground. Next it investig<strong>at</strong>ed the blunt end <strong>of</strong> a<br />

wooden branch about 1 cm in diameter and bearing several stains<br />

<strong>of</strong> bird excreta. The snake grasped this branch and <strong>at</strong>tempted to<br />

advance the jaws over it, but the object was soon dropped and the<br />

effort abandoned. The snake continued to crawl slowly over the<br />

substr<strong>at</strong>e using rectilinear locomotion while investig<strong>at</strong>ing objects<br />

with searching movements <strong>of</strong> the head and tongue.<br />

Finally, the snake stopped to investig<strong>at</strong>e an object roughly 4 x 4<br />

x 1.5 cm in overall size and comprised <strong>of</strong> a partial fish skull with<br />

other bones, debris and several dry leaves adhering to it. The snake<br />

grasped this object and advanced its jaws to hold the bulk <strong>of</strong> the<br />

item firmly within its mouth (Fig. 1). Then it elev<strong>at</strong>ed its head<br />

further <strong>of</strong>f the ground, crawled forward about 3 m and stopped<br />

with the head elev<strong>at</strong>ed. We believe the item would have been swallowed,<br />

but we disturbed the snake while advancing to take photographs.<br />

The snake crawled forward and disappeared within the<br />

forest edge th<strong>at</strong> bordered the rookery site.<br />

These and other observ<strong>at</strong>ions <strong>of</strong> cottonmouth foraging behaviors<br />

<strong>at</strong> Seahorse Key suggest th<strong>at</strong> numerous, diverse objects <strong>of</strong><br />

appropri<strong>at</strong>e size, shape, and odor might be ingested by foraging<br />

snakes. Thus, feces th<strong>at</strong> are defec<strong>at</strong>ed by wild-caught snakes sometimes<br />

contain leaves th<strong>at</strong> are ingested inadvertently as adhering<br />

parts <strong>of</strong> carrion. In general, Agkistrodon species appear to rely<br />

more on chemosensory inform<strong>at</strong>ion during foraging than do other<br />

crotalines (Chiszar et al. 1979; Cock Buning et al. 1981). It has<br />

been suggested th<strong>at</strong> evolutionary reliance on an envenom<strong>at</strong>ion


Wikelski and Trillmich 1997).<br />

Acknowledgments.—We owe special thanks to Kenneth Litzenberger<br />

<strong>of</strong> the Lower Suwannee and Cedar Keys N<strong>at</strong>ional Wildlife Refuge management<br />

<strong>of</strong>fice for a special use permit to study cottonmouth snakes <strong>at</strong><br />

Seahorse Key. Logistical support was provided by the University <strong>of</strong> Florida<br />

Seahorse Key Marine Labor<strong>at</strong>ory.<br />

LITERATURE CITED<br />

FIG. 1. A female Florida cottonmouth, Agkistrodon piscivorus conanti,<br />

ingesting a dry mass <strong>of</strong> fish skull with adhering bones, dry leaves, sticks,<br />

and soil debris <strong>at</strong> a pelican rookery on Seahorse Key. Note the generally<br />

white appearance <strong>of</strong> the ground and numerous white spots on the leaves<br />

and other objects, including the snake. These are dried ur<strong>at</strong>es excreted by<br />

pelicans from the overhead tree canopy.<br />

feeding system and use <strong>of</strong> a strongly proteolytic venom, coupled<br />

with predominance <strong>of</strong> chemosensory inform<strong>at</strong>ion during foraging,<br />

have likely conferred a predilection for carrion in the cottonmouth<br />

(Savitzky 1992). However, the significance <strong>of</strong> such behavior<br />

in energetic contexts has not previously been emphasized.<br />

At Seahorse Key, fish dropped by nesting birds can be a “boom<br />

or bust” situ<strong>at</strong>ion. In the 1970s, as many as 200,000 colonial birds<br />

occupied the Cedar Keys, and 95% <strong>of</strong> all nesting occurred on<br />

Seahorse Key (U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service, unpublished records).<br />

Such numbers produce abundant and concentr<strong>at</strong>ed energy resources<br />

for cottonmouths, which literally gorge themselves during periods<br />

lasting from April/May to October/November (l<strong>at</strong>e fall nesting<br />

<strong>at</strong>tributable to pelicans; HBL, unpublished observ<strong>at</strong>ions). In<br />

recent years, however, drought has severely impacted wetlands<br />

on which many <strong>of</strong> the nesting birds depend for feeding their chicks.<br />

Consequently, the number <strong>of</strong> nesting birds has diminished gre<strong>at</strong>ly.<br />

Recent counts have been as low as 10,000 or fewer nesting birds.<br />

During the severe 2000 drought in Florida (especially <strong>at</strong> Seahorse<br />

Key), avian nesting was gre<strong>at</strong>ly curtailed <strong>at</strong> Seahorse Key, and the<br />

snake foraging period was shortened to July–October. Moreover,<br />

cottonmouths do not forage during drought, even though dropped<br />

fish might be available <strong>at</strong> the rookeries (unpublished observ<strong>at</strong>ions).<br />

Consequently, foraging success can vary gre<strong>at</strong>ly from year to year.<br />

It seems reasonable to infer th<strong>at</strong> carrion degraded by age or adherence<br />

<strong>of</strong> indigestible m<strong>at</strong>erials might be significant resources when<br />

snakes employ foraging behaviors th<strong>at</strong> maximize the intake <strong>of</strong><br />

available energy. <strong>Snakes</strong> <strong>at</strong> Seahorse Key remain bene<strong>at</strong>h or very<br />

near the rookeries, and they do not usually move extensive distances<br />

unless the loc<strong>at</strong>ions <strong>of</strong> food resources change (Wharton<br />

1969). Thus, the scavenging <strong>of</strong> diverse objects th<strong>at</strong> yield potentially<br />

useful energy during foraging might have significant selective<br />

advantages in island environments where altern<strong>at</strong>ive prey items<br />

are scarce or limited. The intensive scavenging behaviors <strong>of</strong> cottonmouth<br />

snakes <strong>at</strong> Seahorse Key perhaps have become incorpor<strong>at</strong>ed<br />

to be part <strong>of</strong> an “island syndrome” <strong>of</strong> behavioral and physiological<br />

modific<strong>at</strong>ions th<strong>at</strong> have evolved in response to resource<br />

limit<strong>at</strong>ions on islands (Adler and Levins 1994; McNab 1994;<br />

ADLER, G. H., AND R. LEVINS.1994. The island syndrome in rodent popul<strong>at</strong>ions.<br />

Quart. Rev. Biol. 69:473–487.<br />

CHISZAR, D., L. SIMONSEN, C. W. RADCLIFFE, AND H. M. SMITH.1979. R<strong>at</strong>e<br />

<strong>of</strong> tongue flicking by cottonmouths (Agkistrodon piscivorus) during<br />

prolonged exposure to various food odors, and strike-induced<br />

chemosensory searching by the cantil (Agkistrodon biline<strong>at</strong>us). Trans.<br />

Kansas Acad. Sci. 82:49–54.<br />

COCK BUNING, T. DE, R. C. GORIS, AND S. TERASHIMA.1981.The role <strong>of</strong><br />

thermosensitivity in the feeding behavior <strong>of</strong> the pit vipers Agkistrodon<br />

blomh<strong>of</strong>fii brevicaud<strong>at</strong>us. Jap. J. Herpetol. 9:7–27.<br />

FRITTS, T. H. 1988. The brown tree snake, Boiga irregularis, a thre<strong>at</strong> to<br />

Pacific islands. U.S. Fish Wildl. Serv., Biol. Rep. 88(31). 36 pp.<br />

GLOYD, H. K., AND R. CONANT. 1990. <strong>Snakes</strong> <strong>of</strong> the Agkistrodon complex.<br />

A Monographic Review. Society for the Study <strong>of</strong> Amphibians and Reptiles.<br />

MCNAB, B. K.1994. Resource use and the survival <strong>of</strong> land and freshw<strong>at</strong>er<br />

vertebr<strong>at</strong>es on oceanic islands. Amer. N<strong>at</strong>. 144:643–660.<br />

SAVITZKY, B. A. C. 1992. Labor<strong>at</strong>ory studies on piscivory in an opportunistic<br />

pit viper, the cottonmouth, Agkistrodon piscivorus. In J. A.<br />

Campbell, and E.D. Brodie, Jr. (eds.), Biology <strong>of</strong> the Pitvipers, pp.<br />

347–368. Selva, Tyler, Texas.<br />

WHARTON, C. H. 1966. Reproduction and growth in the cottonmouth<br />

Agkistrodon piscivorus Lacépède, <strong>of</strong> Cedar Keys, Florida. Copeia<br />

1966:149-161.<br />

______. 1969. The cottonmouth moccasin on Sea Horse Key, Florida.<br />

Bull. Florida St<strong>at</strong>e Mus., Biol. Sci. 14:227–272.<br />

WIKELSKI, M., AND F. TRILLMICH. 1997. Body size and sexual size dimorphism<br />

in marine iguanas fluctu<strong>at</strong>e as a result <strong>of</strong> opposing n<strong>at</strong>ural and<br />

sexual selection: an island comparison. Evolution 51:922–936.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!