20.02.2015 Views

Family background and child development up to age 7

Family background and child development up to age 7

Family background and child development up to age 7

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

RESEARCH<br />

Up <strong>to</strong> Age 7:<br />

<strong>Family</strong> Background <strong>and</strong> Child Development<br />

Up <strong>to</strong> Age 7 in the Avon Longitudinal Survey<br />

of Parents <strong>and</strong> Children (ALSPAC)<br />

CPMO Research Team<br />

University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l<br />

Research Report RR808A


Research Report<br />

No 808A<br />

Up <strong>to</strong> Age 7:<br />

<strong>Family</strong> Background <strong>and</strong> Child Development<br />

Up <strong>to</strong> Age 7 in the Avon Longitudinal Survey<br />

of Parents <strong>and</strong> Children (ALSPAC)<br />

CPMO Research Team<br />

University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l<br />

The views expressed in this report are the authors’ <strong>and</strong> do not necessarily reflect those of the Department for Education<br />

<strong>and</strong> Skills.<br />

© University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l 2006<br />

ISBN 978 1 84478 840 8


Up To 7:<br />

<strong>Family</strong> Background <strong>and</strong> Child Development Up <strong>to</strong><br />

Age 7 in the Avon Longitudinal Survey of Parents<br />

<strong>and</strong> Children (ALSPAC).<br />

Prepared by CMPO Research Team, University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l<br />

We would like <strong>to</strong> thank the Department for Education <strong>and</strong> Skills for financial s<strong>up</strong>port on this project.<br />

We are extremely grateful <strong>to</strong> all the mothers who <strong>to</strong>ok part <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> the midwives for their cooperation<br />

<strong>and</strong> help in recruitment. The whole ALSPAC Study Team comprises interviewers, computer<br />

technicians, labora<strong>to</strong>ry technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers <strong>and</strong> man<strong>age</strong>rs who<br />

continue <strong>to</strong> make the study possible. The ALSPAC could not have been undertaken without the<br />

financial s<strong>up</strong>port of the Medical Research Council, the Welcome Trust, UK government departments,<br />

medical charities <strong>and</strong> others. The ALSPAC study is part of the WHO initiated European Longitudinal<br />

Study of Pregnancy & Childhood. The CMPO research team on this project includes Simon Burgess,<br />

Paul Gregg, Emma Hall, Sara Meadows, Stephen Proud, Carol Propper <strong>and</strong> Liz Washbrook. Usual<br />

disclaimers apply.<br />

Leverhulme Centre for Market <strong>and</strong> Public Organisation<br />

University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l<br />

8 Woodl<strong>and</strong> Rd.<br />

Bris<strong>to</strong>l BS8 1TN<br />

e-mail – P.Gregg@bris<strong>to</strong>l.ac.uk<br />

Tel: 0117 928 9061<br />

2


Executive Summary <strong>and</strong> Conclusions<br />

The Research Methodology<br />

We use the rich information in the ALSPAC data set <strong>to</strong> identify <strong>child</strong> outcomes at <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>age</strong> 7, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> explore how these are influenced by measures of family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

proximal, mediating, fac<strong>to</strong>rs such as parenting behaviours <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>care arrangements. Details<br />

of our precise methodology are given in section 3.2 Part 1 of this report. In summary, we begin<br />

by examining the relationship between each of the gro<strong>up</strong>s of mediating variables <strong>and</strong> the two<br />

<strong>child</strong> outcomes (cognitive <strong>and</strong> behavioural) at <strong>age</strong> 5. This allows us <strong>to</strong> exploit the rich data <strong>to</strong><br />

examine the effect of measures of each set of proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs in some depth, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> determine<br />

which fac<strong>to</strong>rs are significantly associated with outcomes. We then examine the<br />

interrelationship between all seven sets of proximal variables <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> outcomes, using the<br />

measures of each set of proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs that have been shown <strong>to</strong> be significantly associated<br />

with the outcomes of interest. In this examination, our focus is on the pathways by which<br />

parental education <strong>and</strong> income affect <strong>child</strong> outcomes at school entry. Causality in this area is<br />

obviously difficult <strong>to</strong> infer with complete confidence owing <strong>to</strong> the complexities of such<br />

interrelationships. Nevertheless, we explore plausible routes (the proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs) through<br />

which the distal fac<strong>to</strong>rs (family <strong>background</strong> measures) might affect <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>.<br />

We focus on income <strong>and</strong> parental education but also explore other family characteristics <strong>and</strong><br />

their relationship with our proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs. These are <strong>age</strong> of mother at birth, numbers of<br />

siblings, ethnicity <strong>and</strong> lone parenthood.<br />

Having established the patterns of association at <strong>age</strong> 5 for cognitive <strong>and</strong> behavioural outcomes,<br />

we then examine (in Part 2 of the report) the persistence of the picture through the first two<br />

years of schooling. In this part, we examine only cognitive outcomes. We aim <strong>to</strong> assess<br />

whether the first two years of education in school changes the picture of which are the key<br />

influences on <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> prior <strong>to</strong> school entry. In particular, we assess whether the<br />

early learning deficits based on family <strong>background</strong> persist <strong>and</strong> the role played in these deficits<br />

by the fac<strong>to</strong>rs identified in Part 1 in this persistence. In addition <strong>to</strong> these fac<strong>to</strong>rs, we also<br />

examine the effect of the school attended by the <strong>child</strong>.<br />

4


The Outcome Measures Used<br />

The data contains two school-based measures of cognitive <strong>development</strong>. These are the Entry<br />

Assessment (EA) test taken at <strong>age</strong> 4 or 5 <strong>and</strong> the Key St<strong>age</strong> 1 (KS1) assessment, administered<br />

in Year 2 at <strong>age</strong> 6 or 7. Each test is composed of four sub-scores that capture ability in reading,<br />

writing, mathematics <strong>and</strong> langu<strong>age</strong> skills (EA only) or spelling (KS1 only). The data also<br />

contains an assessment of cognitive ability administered by the ALSPAC team <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren at<br />

the <strong>age</strong> of 7. Fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis was used <strong>to</strong> combine the sub-scores for each measure in<strong>to</strong> one<br />

overall score. Attrition <strong>and</strong> the fact that not all permissions <strong>to</strong> use tests taken in school have<br />

been given <strong>to</strong> ALSPAC mean the sample size for the EA test score is around 5000. At <strong>age</strong> 7<br />

the availability of the two available scores allows us <strong>to</strong> construct a composite score, which will<br />

be more accurate than a single measure alone. This composite score covers some 7,500<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

The measure of behavioural problems is derived from mother-reported data at <strong>age</strong> 4. As the<br />

results are mother reported there is some question over the accuracy of reporting <strong>and</strong> whether it<br />

may be influenced by the mothers mental (e.g. stress) <strong>and</strong> physical health. The measure<br />

contains five components – scores relating <strong>to</strong> hyperactivity, emotional symp<strong>to</strong>ms, conduct<br />

problems, peer problems <strong>and</strong> a pro-social score (which is reversed as unlike for the other<br />

measures a higher score is a positive outcome). The sample size for this measure is around<br />

9500.<br />

Each measure is st<strong>and</strong>ardised <strong>to</strong> have mean 100 <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10. This enables easy<br />

comparisons across the measures used. A score of plus or minus 1 on each measure therefore<br />

represents a move of plus or minus one tenth of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. This equates <strong>to</strong> roughly<br />

moving plus or minus four percent<strong>age</strong> points from the median. On the EA test score we can<br />

also translate this in<strong>to</strong> months of <strong>development</strong> within a school year. Here then plus or minus<br />

one point of the score equates <strong>to</strong> plus or minus 1 ½ months of <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong>. On the<br />

behaviour score there is little <strong>age</strong> variation <strong>and</strong> so such a comparison between <strong>age</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

behavioural <strong>development</strong> makes little sense. Full details of the ALSPAC cohort <strong>and</strong> the<br />

information in the data set are given in Part 1 of this report. Part 2 gives further information on<br />

the measures of <strong>child</strong> attainment at <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

5


Underst<strong>and</strong>ing the Findings<br />

The approach of this research is <strong>to</strong> explore how the association between distal fac<strong>to</strong>rs such as<br />

family income <strong>and</strong> parental education impact on <strong>child</strong>ren’s early <strong>development</strong>. This is<br />

undertaken in two steps, first we condition on other distal fac<strong>to</strong>rs such as mothers <strong>age</strong>, family<br />

structure. This enables us <strong>to</strong> suggest that the association of <strong>child</strong> outcomes <strong>and</strong> family income<br />

does not stem from income per se but because it is also associated with teen pregnancy, larger<br />

families etc. In the second step we introduce gro<strong>up</strong>ings of proximal influences; Parenting<br />

Behaviour <strong>and</strong> the Home Environment, Maternal Employment, Childcare, Maternal<br />

Health, Social S<strong>up</strong>port <strong>and</strong> Background, Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> Peer Gro<strong>up</strong>s. It is possible<br />

through this approach <strong>to</strong> also look at other <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs, other than income or parental<br />

educations <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> explore how important the proximal influences are <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> (not<br />

just <strong>to</strong> the achievement gaps across income or parental education gro<strong>up</strong>s). In the summary that<br />

follows we define a modest effect equates <strong>to</strong> a n effect of <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> 1/10 of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, a<br />

moderate effect <strong>to</strong> between 1/10 <strong>and</strong> 2/10 of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation <strong>and</strong> a Large effect <strong>to</strong> above<br />

2/10 of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. 1/10 of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation would move a <strong>child</strong> from the middle<br />

of the distribution of attainment (the median) by just under 4 percent<strong>age</strong> points.<br />

Summary of Findings <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 5<br />

Educational Development Up <strong>to</strong> Age 5<br />

• Even by <strong>age</strong> 5 substantial variations in educational attainment are apparent across<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren from different family <strong>background</strong>s. Large early learning variations are<br />

associated with parental education, family income, <strong>age</strong> of mother <strong>and</strong> numbers of<br />

siblings <strong>and</strong> gender of the <strong>child</strong>. Children from the poorest fifth of families have<br />

lower cognitive attainment than <strong>child</strong>ren from richer by nearly 7 percent<strong>age</strong> points<br />

in the raw data (around 10 months behind) <strong>and</strong> less educated (no A-C grades<br />

GCSEs or higher level qualifications) are 9 points or a year behind those from more<br />

educated families. However, these raw results role many aspects of family<br />

<strong>background</strong> <strong>to</strong>gether, for instance the lower income families tend also <strong>to</strong> be less<br />

educated. Once we draw out the separate effects of education <strong>and</strong> income, those<br />

from less educated families (no A-C grades or higher level qualifications) are 5<br />

points behind those with degree educated mothers <strong>and</strong> those from the poorest fifth<br />

of homes are just over 3 points behind those from the most affluent fifth. Boys also<br />

6


have lower attainment than girls, those with younger mothers are behind those with<br />

older mothers <strong>and</strong> those with more older siblings are also behind the first born<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren compared <strong>to</strong> when they were <strong>age</strong>d 5.<br />

• Parents’ teaching (e.g. teaching <strong>child</strong>ren a number of items such as shapes, colours<br />

<strong>and</strong> numbers) <strong>and</strong> reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren during the pre-school period are the largest<br />

single influence on <strong>child</strong>ren’s early learning. After controlling for other influences<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren who are rarely read <strong>to</strong> before school entry are 1.6 points behind <strong>and</strong> those<br />

where the mothers rarely actively teach their <strong>child</strong>ren are 3.6 points behind.<br />

However, differences in teaching <strong>and</strong> reading do not explain very much of the early<br />

learning deficits of <strong>child</strong>ren from poorer <strong>and</strong> less educated families. For instance,<br />

the gap between <strong>child</strong>ren with the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest educated gro<strong>up</strong>s of mothers<br />

falls from 4.9 points <strong>to</strong> 4.4 points once parenting behaviour is included. That is,<br />

parenting behaviours are the mediating influence for just 10% of the gap between<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren with the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest educated gro<strong>up</strong>s of mothers. There is also no<br />

evidence that better educated parents teach <strong>and</strong> read <strong>to</strong> young <strong>child</strong>ren more<br />

effectively; they just do these things somewhat more often. There is evidence,<br />

however, that teen<strong>age</strong> mothers eng<strong>age</strong> in reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> teaching their <strong>child</strong>ren less<br />

often <strong>and</strong> these are key reasons why <strong>child</strong>ren of teen<strong>age</strong> mothers are underachieving<br />

at <strong>age</strong> 5.<br />

• Exposure <strong>to</strong> pre-school <strong>child</strong>care is modestly beneficial <strong>to</strong> early learning but it is a<br />

minor fac<strong>to</strong>r in explaining the early learning deficits observed. Full-time <strong>child</strong>care<br />

prior <strong>to</strong> school entry is no more beneficial than part-time but attending a pre-school<br />

containing high achieving <strong>child</strong>ren is beneficial.<br />

• Centre based care at or before <strong>age</strong> 2 is as beneficial <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s learning as is other<br />

paid care (e.g. <strong>child</strong> minders). Long hours of care (20 or more hours a week) <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong><br />

the <strong>age</strong> of 2 by unpaid carers, such as friends <strong>and</strong> relatives, is associated with lower<br />

attainment, whilst father’s involvement in <strong>child</strong> care before <strong>age</strong> 2 is modestly<br />

beneficial.<br />

• The home environment - in terms of books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong> the attainment of the peer<br />

gro<strong>up</strong> attending the same pre-school setting – make a substantial contribution <strong>to</strong> the<br />

early learning deficits of <strong>child</strong>ren from poorer <strong>and</strong> less educated families. These<br />

proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs are strongly related <strong>to</strong> family income levels.<br />

7


• Mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health <strong>and</strong> her own <strong>child</strong>hood experiences are not<br />

major influences on <strong>child</strong>ren’s early leaning. Despite this, they explain around 10%<br />

of the lower test scores of <strong>child</strong>ren from poorer <strong>and</strong> less well educated households.<br />

This is because they are strongly related <strong>to</strong> income <strong>and</strong> parental education levels.<br />

Behaviour at <strong>age</strong> 5<br />

• Differences in raw behaviour scores across <strong>child</strong>ren from families with different<br />

levels of income <strong>and</strong> education are relatively small compared <strong>to</strong> those observed in<br />

cognitive <strong>development</strong>.<br />

• Mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health, especially stress, anxiety <strong>and</strong> self-esteem, <strong>and</strong><br />

low quality relationships between parents are strongly related <strong>to</strong> poor behavioural<br />

outcomes. In turn these indica<strong>to</strong>rs of poor mental health <strong>and</strong> lack of belief that there<br />

own actions make a difference, are strongly correlated with low income <strong>and</strong> low<br />

educational attainment.<br />

• Parental teaching <strong>and</strong> reading also have marked influences on later behavioural<br />

outcomes (as for learning outcomes). But, in addition, parental behaviours<br />

including weak early maternal bonding, smoking in pregnancy, talking <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

whilst eng<strong>age</strong>d in other activities <strong>and</strong> the <strong>child</strong> watching 6+ hours of TV a week at<br />

18 months are also associated with worse behaviour.<br />

• Centre based care at or before <strong>age</strong> 2 is associated with worse behaviours relative <strong>to</strong><br />

parental carers or <strong>child</strong> minders. Long hours of care (20 or more hours a week) by<br />

unpaid carers, such as friends <strong>and</strong> relatives are also associated with worse<br />

behaviour.<br />

• The differences in raw behaviour scores between the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest education<br />

<strong>and</strong> income gro<strong>up</strong>s are modest in size <strong>and</strong> explained mainly by the correlations<br />

between parenting <strong>and</strong> mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health <strong>and</strong> income or<br />

education. Parenting patterns are more important in driving the differences in<br />

behavioural outcomes, between the most <strong>and</strong> least affluent <strong>child</strong>ren, than they are<br />

for early educational attainment<br />

8


Summary of findings <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 7<br />

Persistence of Early Learning from Age 5 <strong>to</strong> 7<br />

• <strong>Family</strong> Background. In general most dimensions of family <strong>background</strong> have slightly<br />

weaker influences at <strong>age</strong> 7, after two years of schooling, than at <strong>age</strong> 5. This is most<br />

marked for <strong>age</strong> of mother, low birth weight <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> boys (as the gender gap<br />

diminishes but far from disappears). It is fairly marginal, however, for income, parental<br />

education <strong>and</strong> sibling numbers. This pattern of effects is not influenced by the<br />

inclusion or exclusion of our mediating influences.<br />

• Parenting patterns. Most effects (reading, activities, teaching <strong>and</strong> breast feeding)<br />

remain highly persistent, that is they are still important at <strong>age</strong> 7. There is a weakening<br />

of the impact early TV watching on a <strong>child</strong>’s cognitive <strong>development</strong>.<br />

• The home learning environment <strong>and</strong> outings. There is evidence of a minor reduction<br />

in the impact of the home learning environment on <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment between <strong>age</strong>s<br />

5 <strong>and</strong> 7. The <strong>to</strong>y score, having few <strong>to</strong>ys in the home, becomes indistinguishable from<br />

having no effect by <strong>age</strong> 7, whereas there was a significant relationship at <strong>age</strong> 5.<br />

• Child care. The report covers us<strong>age</strong>, types <strong>and</strong> extent of care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 24 months plus<br />

a more detailed study of <strong>child</strong> care exposure <strong>and</strong> limited measures of quality prior <strong>to</strong><br />

school entry at <strong>age</strong> 4. The reliance on friends <strong>and</strong> relatives for early care has<br />

persistently adverse effects on attainment. All other early care arrangements appear<br />

broadly indistinguishable from each other <strong>and</strong> have no significant impact at <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

Variations in the impact of exposure <strong>to</strong> different early learning environments<br />

immediately prior <strong>to</strong> school entry at <strong>age</strong> 4, all disappear after two years of schooling.<br />

Children who had attended only parent-organised play gro<strong>up</strong>s were behind <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

who had attended nursery schools at <strong>age</strong> 5, but show no substantive difference in<br />

attainment by <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

• Maternal physical <strong>and</strong> mental health <strong>and</strong> social s<strong>up</strong>port networks. These fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

had little impact on <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive <strong>development</strong> at <strong>age</strong> 5, but the importance of<br />

the mother’s locus of control score, the extent <strong>to</strong> which the mother feels her choices <strong>and</strong><br />

actions can affect the family’s circumstances, remains persistent through the early<br />

school years.<br />

9


Implications for future research<br />

This research makes a substantive contribution <strong>to</strong> the underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the origins of early<br />

learning differences in <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> their persistence on entry in<strong>to</strong> school. As these <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

<strong>age</strong>, ALSPAC will continue <strong>to</strong> offer a unique method of tracking how early learning <strong>and</strong><br />

behaviour are transmitted through <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> in<strong>to</strong> adulthood <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> assess how other<br />

influences come forward <strong>to</strong> mitigate or accentuate early differences. The data will also be able<br />

<strong>to</strong> track whether recent government initiatives can alter this process, for instance, the Literacy<br />

Hour programme which these <strong>child</strong>ren will have experienced after <strong>age</strong> 7 or programmes such<br />

as the Community Trust Fund of anti-social behaviour.<br />

<strong>Family</strong> <strong>background</strong> is observed <strong>to</strong> be associated with major differences in attainment <strong>and</strong><br />

behaviour on school entry. These family <strong>background</strong> influences cross the dimensions of<br />

parental education, family income, <strong>age</strong> of mother at birth <strong>and</strong> numbers of siblings within the<br />

family. The early learning deficits of those from poorer <strong>and</strong> less educated families remain<br />

largely intact through the first two years of schooling but they are not continuing <strong>to</strong> worsen.<br />

These early learning deficits substantially stem from the association between the measures of<br />

family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> a set of proximal influences drawn from the data. The key proximal<br />

influences differ between behaviour <strong>and</strong> early learning outcomes. For both income <strong>and</strong> parental<br />

education, the mediating influence which is most important for early behaviour outcomes is the<br />

mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health. This includes anxiety, stress <strong>and</strong> a weak locus of control.<br />

Disciplining behaviour is also important. For early learning, the physical home leaning<br />

environment (books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys), parental teaching <strong>and</strong> reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> the make <strong>up</strong> of<br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong>s in pre-school settings are key proximal influences.<br />

Through the first two years of schooling, the pattern of early learning across <strong>child</strong>ren remains<br />

broadly intact, in that most of the drivers of early learning differences before entry <strong>to</strong> school<br />

remain undiminished after two years of school. The deficits associated with having a teen<strong>age</strong><br />

mother weaken <strong>and</strong> those from ethnic minorities are doing better than Whites on aver<strong>age</strong> by<br />

<strong>age</strong> 7 (we do not have large enough samples <strong>to</strong> examine specific ethnic gro<strong>up</strong>s). The evidence<br />

that pre-school setting influences <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> on school entry, including evidence that<br />

the pre-school peer gro<strong>up</strong> matters (which other <strong>child</strong>ren attend the same pre-school) show clear<br />

10


signs of diminishing through the first two years of schooling. A caveat is that this study does<br />

not have the evidence of quality in the pre-school setting available in other studies.<br />

11


Part 1<br />

Early Development Variations <strong>up</strong>on entry in<strong>to</strong> school<br />

1. Introduction 13<br />

2. Measures of Child Attainment <strong>and</strong> Correlations with<br />

Background Characteristics 15<br />

3. Parenting Behaviour <strong>and</strong> the Home Environment 23<br />

4. Maternal Employment 51<br />

5. Childcare 63<br />

6. Maternal Health, Social S<strong>up</strong>port <strong>and</strong> Background 73<br />

7. Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> Peer Gro<strong>up</strong>s 89<br />

8. Combined Analysis 97<br />

9. Conclusions 129<br />

Part 2<br />

The Persistence of Early Learning Variations Through the<br />

First Two Years of Schooling<br />

10. Introduction 135<br />

11. Data & Measures of Child Outcomes 137<br />

12


12. Background Characteristics 145<br />

13. Parenting Behaviours & Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7 149<br />

14. Outings & the Home Learning Environment &<br />

Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7 151<br />

15. Pre-school Childcare & Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7 153<br />

16. Mother’s Childhood & <strong>Family</strong> Background &<br />

Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7 155<br />

17. Mothers Health, Social Networks <strong>and</strong> <strong>Family</strong> Conflicts<br />

& Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7 157<br />

18. Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> Peer Gro<strong>up</strong>s & Attainment Up <strong>to</strong><br />

7 158<br />

19. Overview of <strong>Family</strong> Background & Child<br />

Development on Entry <strong>to</strong> School 161<br />

20. Conclusions 169<br />

References 177<br />

Appendix 1. Variable Definitions 183<br />

Appendix 2. Parenting Figures 219<br />

Appendix 3. Parenting Interaction Tables 223<br />

Appendix 4. Detailed tables of Results for Part II<br />

including School Fixed Effects 232<br />

13


1. Introduction<br />

This report draws <strong>to</strong>gether the research undertaken at the Leverhulme Centre for Market <strong>and</strong><br />

Public Organisation at the University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l for the Department for Education <strong>and</strong> Skills on<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive <strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong> behaviour <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> entry in<strong>to</strong> school. This report has two<br />

parts – Part 1 examines two measures of attainment <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> school entry at <strong>age</strong> 4 <strong>to</strong> 5, one<br />

cognitive <strong>and</strong> one behavioural. Part 2 presents a complementary analysis of the persistence of<br />

variations in early learning during the first two years of school. The study uses the rich data<br />

from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents <strong>and</strong> Children (ALSPAC).<br />

Part 1 examines two measures of attainment, one cognitive <strong>and</strong> one behavioural. It focuses on<br />

seven main sets of proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs, which may mediate between family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> these<br />

outcomes. These are:<br />

• Parenting patterns - covering reading, activities, teaching, etc.;<br />

• The home learning environment <strong>and</strong> outings;<br />

• Maternal employment from the birth <strong>to</strong> when the <strong>child</strong> is <strong>age</strong>d 47 months;<br />

• Child Care us<strong>age</strong> covering types <strong>and</strong> extent of care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> 30 months <strong>and</strong> more detailed<br />

study of <strong>child</strong> care exposure <strong>and</strong> limited measures of quality prior <strong>to</strong> school entry at <strong>age</strong> 4;<br />

• Maternal physical <strong>and</strong> mental health, especially depression, domestic violence <strong>and</strong> intrafamily<br />

conflict;<br />

• Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood experiences; <strong>and</strong><br />

• The neighbourhood of residence <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>s.<br />

We also focus on the role of family income <strong>and</strong> the relationship between income <strong>and</strong><br />

expenditure on <strong>child</strong>ren’s activities, <strong>child</strong>care etc. in the transmission of family <strong>background</strong><br />

in<strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong> attainment. We allow this, in turn, <strong>to</strong> operate through the mechanisms described<br />

above. Likewise we explore the role of parental education on <strong>child</strong>ren’s outcomes <strong>and</strong> the roles<br />

of the fac<strong>to</strong>rs above as mediating influences.<br />

14


2. Measures of Child Attainment <strong>and</strong> Correlations with<br />

Background Characteristics.<br />

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents <strong>and</strong> Children (ALSPAC) is a cohort study of around<br />

13 000 <strong>child</strong>ren born in the Avon area of the UK in 1991 <strong>and</strong> 1992. Hence the survey is after<br />

the bulk of the major expansion of early return <strong>to</strong> work by mothers that occurred in the midlate<br />

1980s (see Gregg et al. 2003) but before the guaranteed half day places at pre-school for<br />

four <strong>and</strong> most recently 3 year olds. Mothers complete <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> three surveys a year, one relating <strong>to</strong><br />

the characteristics of herself <strong>and</strong> the household in general <strong>and</strong> two relating <strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong> 1 . In<br />

addition, mothers answered four questionnaires during their pregnancies. The ALSPAC survey<br />

also contains data from sources other than self-completion questionnaires. The ALSPAC team<br />

have run a number of clinics for <strong>child</strong>ren from the <strong>age</strong> of seven. Here the <strong>child</strong> attends an<br />

assessment centre (with a parent or guardian) run by ALSPAC at which tests are undertaken <strong>to</strong><br />

assess various aspects of the <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>. Records from schools can also be<br />

matched, with a parent’s permission, <strong>to</strong> the individual <strong>child</strong>ren, so data is available on schoolbased<br />

assessments at <strong>age</strong>s 4 <strong>to</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong> again for <strong>age</strong>s 6 <strong>to</strong> 7.<br />

2.1 Cognitive attainment<br />

The two school-based measures of cognitive <strong>development</strong> available in ALSPAC are the Entry<br />

Assessment (EA) test taken at <strong>age</strong> 4 or 5 <strong>and</strong> the Key St<strong>age</strong> 1 (KS1) assessment, which is<br />

administered in Year 2 at <strong>age</strong> 6 or 7. Each test is composed of four subscores that capture<br />

ability in reading, writing, mathematics <strong>and</strong> langu<strong>age</strong> skills (EA only) or spelling (KS1 only).<br />

Our third assessment of cognitive ability was administered, by the ALSPAC team, <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

at the <strong>age</strong> of 7. This ALSPAC literacy score is again composed of a number of subscores, in<br />

this case capturing skills in reading, spelling <strong>and</strong> the manipulation of words. This ALSPAC test<br />

was administered with other <strong>child</strong> assessment under a heading of Focus at 7. From here on we<br />

describe the ALSPAC test as the Focus at 7 test.<br />

To combine the subscores for each measure in<strong>to</strong> one overall score we used fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis. This<br />

method allows the data <strong>to</strong> dictate the relative weights attached <strong>to</strong> each component <strong>and</strong> so <strong>to</strong><br />

distil the maximum possible information in<strong>to</strong> a single measure. Each of the three resulting<br />

1 The mother’s partner also received annual questionnaires but the response here is patchy.<br />

16


scores were then normalised <strong>to</strong> have a mean of 100 <strong>and</strong> a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of 10. As is<br />

inevitable in a survey of the scale of ALSPAC, attrition results in smaller sample sizes for the<br />

later assessments of <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong>. So by <strong>age</strong> 5 there were around 9500 usable cases. In<br />

addition <strong>to</strong> this problem, parents were required <strong>to</strong> give written permission for the release of the<br />

school-based test results. The sample sizes for the Entry Assessment <strong>and</strong> Key St<strong>age</strong> 1 scores<br />

are therefore substantially smaller than for the other measures. Some 6030 <strong>child</strong>ren (just under<br />

two thirds) have permissions for matching of school results. Here we focus on the attainment<br />

measures at 4 or 5. The EA score was not then a national test <strong>and</strong> so those schools not<br />

operating this test, mainly outside the Avon area, do not have these scores. These restrictions<br />

on data mean that our working sample on the EA test scores is just 4,607. The data available at<br />

<strong>age</strong> 7 is far less restrictive.<br />

The small sample sizes raise the question of whether there is major variation in the<br />

characteristics of the population reporting an EA score <strong>and</strong> those that don’t as a result of<br />

attrition <strong>and</strong> giving of permission <strong>to</strong> match <strong>to</strong> the ALSPAC data. We investigated this issue for<br />

a range of observable characteristics such as household income, mother’s <strong>age</strong> <strong>and</strong> educational<br />

attainment, etc <strong>and</strong> concluded that there is little variation in the composition of each sample.<br />

The only clear sample composition difference is an under-representation of lone parents in the<br />

ALSPAC sample. This is substantive among never partnered lone parents <strong>and</strong> those becoming<br />

lone parents within the first year of so of the <strong>child</strong>’s life. Hence there appears <strong>to</strong> be no<br />

substantive attrition bias in the sample composition as measured on observable characteristics<br />

for co<strong>up</strong>le families but there may well be unobservable differences. However, there are clearly<br />

<strong>to</strong>o few lone parents <strong>and</strong> especially never married lone parents <strong>and</strong> so our results for lone<br />

parents need <strong>to</strong> interpreted with caution. We have chosen not <strong>to</strong> re-weight the data <strong>to</strong> raise the<br />

size of lone parent population, as those remaining may be unrepresentative.<br />

As a guide <strong>to</strong> interpreting the size of our estimates, assuming a normal distribution an<br />

increment of 1 point (i.e. a tenth of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation) results in a shift from the median <strong>to</strong><br />

the 54 th percentile, while an increment of 5 points (or half a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation) results in a<br />

shift from the median <strong>to</strong> the 69 th percentile. Table 2.1 shows the pairwise correlations between<br />

the EA score, <strong>and</strong> two other measures of cognitive attainment – the KS1 score, <strong>and</strong> the Focus<br />

at 7 test, an ALSPAC administered literacy score taken at 7 years.<br />

17


Table 2.1: Sample sizes <strong>and</strong> correlations between <strong>child</strong> outcome scores<br />

Entry<br />

Assessment<br />

Key St<strong>age</strong><br />

One<br />

Focus at 7<br />

Behaviour at<br />

<strong>age</strong> 4<br />

Behaviour at<br />

<strong>age</strong> 7<br />

Entry<br />

Assessment<br />

1.0000<br />

Key St<strong>age</strong><br />

One<br />

0.5515 1.0000<br />

Focus at 7<br />

0.4395 0.8381 1.0000<br />

Behaviour at<br />

<strong>age</strong> 4<br />

-0.1906 -0.1906 -0.1323 1.0000<br />

Behaviour at <strong>age</strong><br />

7<br />

-0.2805 -0.3510 -0.2674 0.2535 1.0000<br />

The EA test is moderately strongly correlated with test scores at <strong>age</strong> 6 or 7. In particular there<br />

is a very high correlation with the KS1 score, which is also a school-based assessment. The<br />

school based KS1 <strong>and</strong> the Focus at 7 literacy test have a very high correlation of 0.84, which<br />

suggests these can be considered as two alternative estimates of cognitive attainment with<br />

some measurement error. Hence the EA test is a reasonably strong predic<strong>to</strong>r of later attainment<br />

but there are clearly substantial differences between EA <strong>and</strong> the ALSPAC literacy <strong>and</strong> KS1<br />

tests unlikely <strong>to</strong> be accounted for by the time gap of the tests, which is only around two years.<br />

This suggests that the EA test seems <strong>to</strong> contain far more measurement error than the later tests.<br />

This may reflect far greater teacher discretion in scoring <strong>child</strong>ren than in the national KS1 tests<br />

<strong>and</strong> the clinic based assessment, Focus at 7. There is evidence in the data of schools scoring<br />

p<strong>up</strong>ils consistently higher or lower in the EA tests than in the KS1 tests. As the KS1 <strong>and</strong> Focus<br />

at 7 scores match well, this suggests strongly that there are systematic reporting differences at<br />

school level in the EA scores. Such reporting errors will not bias the results presented unless<br />

they are systematically related <strong>to</strong> p<strong>up</strong>il characteristics. This is explored more in the second<br />

companion report that compares results from the <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong> <strong>age</strong> 7 tests.<br />

2.2 Behavioural attainment at 5<br />

Our measure of behavioural problems is derived from mother-reported data at <strong>age</strong> 4. This<br />

behaviour measure was undertaken through postal questionnaire sent <strong>to</strong> the mother <strong>and</strong> has a<br />

much larger sample size of 9416. As the reports are mother reported there is some question<br />

over the accuracy of reporting <strong>and</strong> whether it may be influenced by the mothers mental (e.g.<br />

stress) <strong>and</strong> physical health. The measure contains five components – scores relating <strong>to</strong><br />

hyperactivity, emotional symp<strong>to</strong>ms, conduct problems, peer problems <strong>and</strong> a pro-social score<br />

(which is reversed as a score is positive unlike for the other behavioural measures). The<br />

behavioural problems measure is the opposite sign <strong>to</strong> the cognitive measures as higher scores<br />

18


indicate more behavioural problems. That this early measure of behaviour is mother reported<br />

might lead <strong>to</strong> biases or inaccuracies in the information reported. There is also a later measure<br />

of behaviour reported by teachers at <strong>age</strong> 7. This is available for a much restricted sub-sample<br />

of around 3,300 cases. As shown in Table 2.1 the correlation between these two measures is<br />

quite low <strong>and</strong> also it is less well correlated with the attainment measures. So it would appear<br />

that there is probably a lot of reporting error. We explore whether this leads <strong>to</strong> systematic<br />

biases by exploring the relationships between the <strong>age</strong> 5 measure <strong>and</strong> behaviour measured at 7<br />

by teachers, on a common sample. The results are fairly similar for most of the variables<br />

considered in our analysis (although the smaller sample often leads <strong>to</strong> statistically insignificant<br />

estimates). However, the results for the mother’s self-esteem score differ significantly <strong>and</strong> so<br />

some caution about this particular measure needs <strong>to</strong> be exercised.<br />

2.3 The effect of <strong>background</strong> controls<br />

Table 2.2 reports a basic regression showing the partially conditional relationship between a set<br />

of <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> the EA test scores. These <strong>background</strong> controls are our base equation<br />

<strong>to</strong> which the other sets of fac<strong>to</strong>rs examined in this report are added sequentially. The<br />

<strong>background</strong> controls are parental education, lone parent status, mother’s <strong>age</strong>, ethnicity <strong>and</strong><br />

gender of the <strong>child</strong>, birth weight <strong>and</strong> numbers of siblings, month <strong>and</strong> year of the birth (the<br />

coefficients on the last 2 are not reported in the table). In this Table <strong>and</strong> through out the report<br />

we will use ***, **, <strong>and</strong> * <strong>to</strong> indicate significance at the 1%, 5% <strong>and</strong> 10% levels respectively.<br />

Table 2.2: Estimates of the effect of household demographic characteristics on Entry<br />

Assessment scores<br />

Without Income<br />

With Income<br />

Log of household income - 2.149***<br />

Financial difficulties before birth (base = no)<br />

Yes<br />

Mother’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

A-level<br />

Degree<br />

Partner’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

A-level<br />

Degree<br />

(0.376)<br />

- -0.247<br />

(0.418)<br />

-2.150*** -1.966***<br />

(0.398) (0.398)<br />

1.278*** 1.057***<br />

(0.320) (0.321)<br />

3.314*** 2.928***<br />

(0.484) (0.487)<br />

-1.657*** -1.460***<br />

(0.373) (0.374)<br />

0.560* 0.459<br />

(0.331) (0.331)<br />

2.546*** 2.176***<br />

(0.449) (0.452)<br />

19


Table 2.2: Estimates of the effect of household demographic characteristics on Entry<br />

Assessment scores (continued)<br />

Lone parent status (base = never lone parent)<br />

Intermittently, with partner at 47 months<br />

Intermittently, no partner at 47 months<br />

Since birth<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth (base = 25-29)<br />

1<br />

Older siblings (base = none)<br />

1<br />

2<br />

-0.021 0.740<br />

(0.829) (0.837)<br />

-1.003 0.425<br />

(0.637) (0.681)<br />

-1.507 0.111<br />

(1.273) (1.299)<br />

-4.071*** -3.536***<br />

(0.978) (0.979)<br />

-0.989** -0.761*<br />

(1) (2)<br />

(0.388) (0.389)<br />

0.638** 0.548*<br />

(0.304) (0.304)<br />

1.083** 0.994**<br />

(0.441) (0.440)<br />

-2.085*** -1.966***<br />

(0.684) (0.683)<br />

3.523*** 3.488***<br />

(0.249) (0.249)<br />

-2.749*** -2.610***<br />

(0.669) (0.667)<br />

-0.826*** -0.771***<br />

(0.261) (0.260)<br />

-0.373 -0.369<br />

(0.552) (0.551)<br />

-0.181 -0.119<br />

(0.313) (0.313)<br />

-2.153*** -2.045***<br />

(0.759) (0.756)<br />

-1.477*** -1.396***<br />

(0.328) (0.328)<br />

-3.063*** -2.974***<br />

(0.438) (0.437)<br />

3+<br />

-4.827*** -4.660***<br />

(0.648) (0.648)<br />

Partner’s employment status at 47 months (base = in work)<br />

Not in work -1.132** -0.385<br />

(0.533) (0.546)<br />

Adj R2 0.2381 0.2429<br />

1. The regression also includes controls for the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> in months at assessment <strong>and</strong> cohort year.<br />

2. Each measure of cognitive <strong>development</strong> is normalised <strong>to</strong> mean 100, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10.<br />

20


The impact of teen mothers is perhaps the most striking result in this table, along with the<br />

impact of large numbers of siblings. To assess the magnitude of the coefficients, the dependent<br />

variable has mean 100 <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10. So the coefficient on teen motherhood in<br />

Table 2.2 is –4.07, so this represents 0.41 of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. The aver<strong>age</strong> position of<br />

those <strong>child</strong>ren with teen mothers, conditional on other fac<strong>to</strong>rs, is the approximately the 34 th<br />

percentile (16 percentile points below the sample mean). This is comparing a teen mother <strong>to</strong><br />

one <strong>age</strong>d 25-29 at birth. Compared with an older mother, 35+, the gap is larger at 5.2 points or<br />

over half a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. As well as teen mothers, there are large differences in<br />

attainment associated with mother’s education <strong>and</strong> having a large number (3+) of older<br />

siblings. Very low birth weight (in lowest 5% of the weight distribution), being a boy <strong>and</strong><br />

having a less well educated father (relative <strong>to</strong> degree educated) are all associated with<br />

substantial attainment deficits. Lone parenthood is not associated with negative outcomes at<br />

<strong>age</strong> 4/5 once other <strong>background</strong> characteristics are conditioned on.<br />

The second column introduces family income as an additional regressor. Despite the richness<br />

of the ALSPAC data in some areas, information on the finances of the household is quite<br />

limited. We have no measure of household income prior <strong>to</strong> 33 months <strong>and</strong> no data on<br />

individual earnings whatsoever. We cannot, therefore, directly assess the contribution of the<br />

mother’s earnings <strong>to</strong> the household on <strong>child</strong> outcomes. Our best available measure for<br />

capturing the financial circumstances of the household is the aver<strong>age</strong> of net household income<br />

at 33 <strong>and</strong> 47 months, expressed in June 1995 prices 2 . Adding in income already gives some<br />

sense of how the other <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs are jointly correlated with attainment <strong>and</strong> family<br />

income. Most of the other <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs are less powerful predic<strong>to</strong>rs of attainment once<br />

family income is conditioned on but the largest shifts are for lone parents, father not working<br />

<strong>and</strong> teen motherhood, although the latter is still a very strong predic<strong>to</strong>r.<br />

This process is now repeated for the behaviour measure. We predict the behaviour variation<br />

less well (the R 2 are lower), in part because there is no <strong>age</strong>-related variation in the behavioural<br />

measure. Note that the scale of the behaviour score is in the reverse direction <strong>to</strong> the EA score<br />

so that a higher score represents more behavioural problems. Hence we would expect most<br />

coefficients <strong>to</strong> have the opposite sign than for the cognitive measure.<br />

2 Income data from the ALSPAC data is b<strong>and</strong>ed. We imputed a median value for each b<strong>and</strong> using data from the<br />

<strong>Family</strong> Expenditure Survey. These values were then expressed in real terms <strong>and</strong> aver<strong>age</strong>d <strong>to</strong> give a <strong>to</strong>tal income<br />

figure.<br />

21


Table 2.3 then examines the impact of these <strong>background</strong> controls on behaviour. Parental<br />

education <strong>and</strong> large families are much less strongly correlated with behavioural outcomes but<br />

teen mothers <strong>and</strong> boys are still associated with substantial deficits. Children of lone parents are<br />

portraying behaviour consistent with higher scores. Overall the behaviour data is much less<br />

predicted by these <strong>background</strong> demographic characteristics. The adjusted R 2 is just 5%<br />

whereas that for educational attainment, excluding for the contribution from <strong>age</strong> adjustment, is<br />

around 14%.<br />

Table 2.3: Estimates of the effect of household demographic characteristics on<br />

Behavioural scores<br />

(1) (2)<br />

Log of household income - -1.447***<br />

Financial difficulties pre birth (base = no)<br />

Yes<br />

Mother’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

A-level<br />

Degree<br />

Partner’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

A-level<br />

Degree<br />

Lone parent status (base = never lone parent)<br />

Intermittently, with partner at 47 months<br />

Intermittently, no partner at 47 months<br />

Since birth<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth (base = 25-29)<br />


Table 2.3: Estimates of the effect of household demographic characteristics on<br />

Behavioural scores (continued)<br />

Ethnicity (base = white)<br />

Non-white<br />

0.694 0.454<br />

Gender (base = male)<br />

Female<br />

Birth weight (base = above median)<br />

Very low<br />

Low<br />

Special care unit at birth (base = no)<br />

Yes<br />

Younger siblings by 47 months (base = none)<br />

1<br />

> 1<br />

Older siblings (base = none)<br />

1<br />

2<br />

(0.542) (0.540)<br />

-2.510*** -2.498***<br />

(0.202) (0.201)<br />

0.870 0.733<br />

(0.571) (0.569)<br />

0.534** 0.510**<br />

(1) (2)<br />

(0.212) (0.211)<br />

0.533 0.537<br />

(0.473) (0.471)<br />

0.672*** 0.661***<br />

(0.240) (0.238)<br />

0.798 0.635<br />

(0.563) (0.561)<br />

1.275*** 1.111***<br />

(0.253) (0.252)<br />

0.795** 0.618*<br />

(0.333) (0.332)<br />

3+<br />

1.065** 0.763<br />

(0.490) (0.488)<br />

Partner’s employment status at 47 months (base = in work)<br />

Not in work 1.904*** 1.151***<br />

(0.377) (0.392)<br />

Adj R2 0.0525 0.0618<br />

Notes<br />

1. Each measure of cognitive <strong>development</strong> is normalised <strong>to</strong> mean 100, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10.<br />

2. ***, **, <strong>and</strong> * indicate significance at the 1%, 5% <strong>and</strong> 10% levels respectively.<br />

The strongest effects observed are associated with teen mothers <strong>and</strong> gender of the <strong>child</strong>.<br />

Mother’s education <strong>and</strong> lone parenthood are also reasonably strongly correlated with mother<br />

reported behavioural problems of the <strong>child</strong>. <strong>Family</strong> income, introduced in column 2, <strong>and</strong><br />

whether the mother reported financial difficulties pre-birth are both strongly related <strong>to</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s reported behavioural problems. Again the estimated relationship between teen<br />

motherhood <strong>and</strong> lone parenthood <strong>and</strong> the <strong>child</strong> outcome is substantially reduced once family<br />

income is conditioned on, although teen motherhood retains a strong conditional correlation.<br />

23


3. Parenting Behaviour <strong>and</strong> Home Environment<br />

This first substantive section looks at the first two gro<strong>up</strong>ings of proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs under<br />

consideration. We start with differences in parenting behaviour across families <strong>and</strong> variations<br />

in the home environment as these are closely related. We correlate available measures with<br />

<strong>child</strong> outcomes at <strong>age</strong> 5, conditioning on the <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs presented above. The<br />

variables cover two of our seven gro<strong>up</strong>ings; those variables which represent a direct parenting<br />

behaviour, such as teaching, reading or how often the mother talks <strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong> whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied<br />

with other things, <strong>and</strong> those characteristics of the <strong>child</strong>’s home learning environment which<br />

reflect more than just parental time or attention. In this latter gro<strong>up</strong> we include <strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong> book<br />

acquisition, trips out <strong>and</strong> housing conditions.<br />

3.1 Relevant Literature<br />

From very early in their lives, <strong>child</strong>ren differ in their cognitive <strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong> their<br />

behaviour. These differences may be highly stable throughout their <strong>child</strong>hoods <strong>and</strong> contribute<br />

<strong>to</strong> the different outcomes of their formal education <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> their risk of social exclusion (e.g.<br />

Jencks et al 1972, Hills, Le Gr<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> Piachaud 2002). Psychological <strong>and</strong> social theories<br />

about the sources of these differences appeal <strong>to</strong> multiple different influences, some located in<br />

the <strong>child</strong>, some in the immediate physical environment, some in the immediate social<br />

experience, some in the social <strong>and</strong> cultural institutions of <strong>child</strong>ren's wider worlds<br />

(Bronfenbrenner 1979, Bronfenbrenner <strong>and</strong> Morris 1998). The possible distal fac<strong>to</strong>rs that have<br />

been suggested range from genes <strong>to</strong> environmental pollution <strong>to</strong> public policy; the suggested<br />

proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs for preschool <strong>development</strong> centre on <strong>child</strong>ren's experiences in their families<br />

<strong>and</strong> in family-like settings. There is extensive evidence (Bloom 1998, Campbell 1995, David<br />

et al 2003, Luthar 1999, Meadows 1993, 1996) from observation-based studies of small<br />

samples (e.g. Dunn, Gallaway <strong>and</strong> Richards 1994, Wells 1985), from larger samples in the US<br />

(e.g. Bradley et al 1989, NICHD 2000, 2002) <strong>and</strong> from both large <strong>and</strong> small intervention<br />

studies (e.g. Ramey <strong>and</strong> Ramey 1998) of correlations between parent behaviour <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

characteristics which suggest that proximal variables such as quantity <strong>and</strong> quality of parent<strong>child</strong><br />

interaction in the preschool years may be causally associated with better cognitive<br />

<strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong> fewer behaviour problems at entry <strong>to</strong> school. Furthermore they suggest that<br />

this causal link may be one of the mechanisms for the differences in <strong>child</strong>ren's attainment <strong>and</strong><br />

24


adjustment associated with distal variables such as social class, family structure, <strong>and</strong> parental<br />

employment, or with genetic/biological influences.<br />

However, the evidence is not sufficient <strong>to</strong> confirm this inference. Many of the studies that<br />

make <strong>up</strong> the evidence base have mostly focused on simple analyses of one input variable <strong>and</strong><br />

one output measure, consigning other variables <strong>to</strong> error. Other studies have used composite<br />

independent variables such as attendance at a Head Start programme <strong>and</strong> have not been able <strong>to</strong><br />

provide information on which parts of the experience have been crucial. Additionally, some<br />

studies have used small <strong>and</strong> possibly unrepresentative samples. Furthermore, it is not<br />

unproblematic <strong>to</strong> extrapolate sensibly from one society <strong>to</strong> another (Rogoff et al 2003).<br />

Correlations <strong>and</strong> effect sizes are typically of only moderate size, thus although it was<br />

appropriate initially <strong>to</strong> use such strategies <strong>to</strong> simplify otherwise intractable problems, <strong>and</strong> they<br />

have led <strong>to</strong> interesting results, the studies have been open <strong>to</strong> criticism (e.g. Scarr 1992) for<br />

failure <strong>to</strong> separate confounded genetic, environmental <strong>and</strong> interaction-based effects; failure <strong>to</strong><br />

compare the influence of parents <strong>and</strong> of other carers, including out-of-home care, or <strong>to</strong><br />

acknowledge the effect of <strong>child</strong>ren on parents; failure <strong>to</strong> identify exactly which parenting<br />

behaviours are affecting which <strong>child</strong> behaviours; <strong>and</strong> failure <strong>to</strong> compare different causes within<br />

the same sample.<br />

The literature on the role of <strong>child</strong>ren’s experience in their families on their cognitive<br />

<strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong> their behaviour involves two main conceptual levels. The first involves more<br />

or less permanent characteristics of family <strong>background</strong>, such as adults’ social class, education<br />

level or intelligence, or family structure, which are associated with different outcomes for the<br />

<strong>child</strong>. The second looks at activities (<strong>and</strong> attitudes) of parents (<strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s other carers),<br />

which make them, moment by moment, more or less effective s<strong>up</strong>porters or inducers of their<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s learning.<br />

<strong>Family</strong> <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive <strong>development</strong><br />

There has been evidence for at least the last century that cognition or, more specifically,<br />

educational achievement, is associated with family <strong>background</strong>. From at least Gal<strong>to</strong>n’s<br />

‘hereditary genius’ onwards, <strong>child</strong>ren with more middle-class <strong>background</strong>s or more educated<br />

parents do better throughout their educational careers, staying in education longer <strong>and</strong> having<br />

higher achievement at most st<strong>age</strong>s. They also tend <strong>to</strong> do somewhat better on IQ tests (though<br />

not during infancy (Slater 1995)), in particular contributing relatively few cases <strong>to</strong> the<br />

25


population of marginally subnormal scorers, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> a lesser degree they also tend <strong>to</strong> do better<br />

on Pi<strong>age</strong>tian tests. The socio-economic status (SES) of the family is still a fairly good predic<strong>to</strong>r<br />

of <strong>child</strong>ren’s academic achievement (Blake 1989, Bornstein <strong>and</strong> Bradley 2003, Bradley <strong>and</strong><br />

Corwyn 2002, Duncan, Brooks-Gunn <strong>and</strong> Klebanov 1994, Duyme, Dumaret <strong>and</strong> Tomkiewicz<br />

1999, Hart <strong>and</strong> Risley 1995, Hoff 2003). Social class differences like these have persisted<br />

despite the existence of a general rise in achievement <strong>and</strong> intelligence <strong>and</strong> of mobility between<br />

social classes between generations, <strong>and</strong> despite the belief in meri<strong>to</strong>cracy <strong>and</strong> egalitarianism<br />

which characterizes countries like Britain <strong>and</strong> the United States.<br />

But social class, or socioeconomic status (SES), is not of itself a causal variable. It is an index<br />

based on the occ<strong>up</strong>ation of the head of the family, <strong>and</strong> thus a guide <strong>to</strong> the family’s income, the<br />

parents’ education, <strong>and</strong>, less directly, <strong>to</strong> a wider set of social circumstances. The same point<br />

applies <strong>to</strong> research on family structure. Social class or family structure research sometimes<br />

focuses on comparing people from different categories (with different ‘social addresses’<br />

(Bronfenbrenner 1979, 1986, Bronfenbrenner et al 1996, 1998, 2000) rather than on the<br />

intervening processes which might explain how the address label comes <strong>to</strong> be associated with<br />

different <strong>development</strong>al outcomes. The results of such research are often open <strong>to</strong> several<br />

interpretations as they do little <strong>to</strong> clarify causal pathways. It is not entirely clear how a variable<br />

like socio-economic status affects <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive achievement, what sort of explana<strong>to</strong>ry<br />

model is appropriate. The correlation between SES <strong>and</strong> intelligence or educational<br />

achievement might reflect differences in innate ‘intelligence’ (Eysenck 1971): however<br />

adoption studies show that the aver<strong>age</strong> level of attainment of adopted <strong>child</strong>ren resembles that<br />

of their adoptive family rather than that of their biological parents (Meadows 2005), studies of<br />

the different SES risk of severe <strong>and</strong> mild mental retardation (e.g. Broman et al 1987) show<br />

different causes <strong>and</strong> different involvement of family <strong>background</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> what promotes high<br />

cognitive attainment in <strong>child</strong>ren from low SES <strong>background</strong>s appears <strong>to</strong> be family interaction<br />

not genes (Kim-Cohen, Moffitt, Caspi <strong>and</strong> Taylor 2004). There might be SES differences in<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s behaviour which differentially facilitates good performance on IQ tests <strong>and</strong> in<br />

school; there might be different reactions by schools <strong>and</strong> other tests <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren differing in<br />

SES. There might be different opportunities available <strong>to</strong> the different social classes (for<br />

example better-funded schools in wealthier areas), even in a social system which believes it is<br />

meri<strong>to</strong>cratic <strong>and</strong> open <strong>to</strong> all those who are talented. There might be differences in health <strong>and</strong><br />

nutrition. These possibilities are by no means mutually exclusive: indeed the different<br />

disadvant<strong>age</strong>s of poverty tend <strong>to</strong> co-occur (Evans 2004.) Whichever cause may apply, we<br />

26


surely need <strong>to</strong> look closely at the moment by moment way the effect is brought about. Work<br />

which looks at more specific variables than family <strong>background</strong> is needed if we are <strong>to</strong> address<br />

the question of why there is variation within classes <strong>and</strong> other overtly similar <strong>background</strong>s, <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>to</strong> elucidate causal chains.<br />

If we are <strong>to</strong> look for explanations of differences in <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive <strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

behaviour in their family interaction rather than their family <strong>background</strong>, the key variables will<br />

include behaviour that is formally or informally educational <strong>and</strong> also behaviour which s<strong>up</strong>ports<br />

positive emotional <strong>and</strong> social <strong>development</strong> (Meadows 1996, 2005). Among the important<br />

variables will be<br />

• the family’s parent-<strong>child</strong> langu<strong>age</strong> use <strong>and</strong> parents’ efforts <strong>to</strong> s<strong>up</strong>port their <strong>child</strong>ren’s<br />

cognition (e.g. Tizard <strong>and</strong> Hughes 1984, Wells 1985, Rice 1989, Snow et al. 1991,<br />

Crain-Thoreson <strong>and</strong> Dale 1992, Lieven, Pine <strong>and</strong> Dresner Barnes 1992, Bates, Dale <strong>and</strong><br />

Thal 1995, Hart <strong>and</strong> Risley 1995, Ochs <strong>and</strong> Schieffelin 1995, Snow 1995, Meadows<br />

1996 <strong>and</strong> 2005, Bloom 1998, Huttenlocher 1998, Evans, Maxwell & Hart 1999, Hoff<br />

<strong>and</strong> Naigles 2002, Senechal <strong>and</strong> LeFevre 2002, Williams <strong>and</strong> Rask 2003, Haney <strong>and</strong><br />

Hill 2004, Johnson-Glenberg <strong>and</strong> Chapman 2004).<br />

• family cohesiveness, communication <strong>and</strong> shared meaning, which may be making<br />

important contributions <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren doing better than expected cognitively (e.g.<br />

Eisenberg <strong>and</strong> Mussen 1989, Heath 1983, Kaye 1984, Meins 1997, 1998, Hubbs-Tait et<br />

al 2002, Howe <strong>and</strong> Rinaldi 2004, Petrill <strong>and</strong> Deater-Deckard 2004,Thompson 2004)<br />

• emotional risk <strong>and</strong> malaise, especially in so far as it impedes facilitative parent-<strong>child</strong><br />

interaction (Cicchetti 2002, Hammen 2003, Ackerman, Brown <strong>and</strong> Izard (2004a,<br />

2004b, Cummings, Keller <strong>and</strong> Davies 2005)<br />

After decades of controversy, there is now strong evidence of the effectiveness of special<br />

educational programmes for <strong>child</strong>ren from disadvant<strong>age</strong>d families (e.g. Barnett 1995,<br />

Burchinal et al 1997, Campbell et al 2001, Campbell <strong>and</strong> Ramey 1995, Campbell et al 2002,<br />

Fan <strong>and</strong> Chen 2001, Mosteller 1995, Ramey <strong>and</strong> Ramey 1998, Reynolds 1994, Reynolds et al<br />

1996, Reynolds <strong>and</strong> Temple 1998, Yoshikawa 1995, Reynolds, Ou <strong>and</strong> Topitzee 2004). Two<br />

particularly well-documented projects, the Abecedarian project (Ramey <strong>and</strong> Ramey 1998) <strong>and</strong><br />

the Chicago Child-Parent Centers (Reynolds, Ou <strong>and</strong> Topitzes 2004) show what the causal<br />

path between intervention <strong>and</strong> outcome may be. There are three main possibilities: that the<br />

27


intervention improves <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive abilities as measured in st<strong>and</strong>ardised tests <strong>and</strong> these<br />

improvements initiate a sequence of improved performance on school tasks <strong>and</strong> tests which<br />

culminate in better school achievement; that the intervention makes families more likely <strong>to</strong><br />

s<strong>up</strong>port the <strong>child</strong>’s cognitive <strong>development</strong> more effectively; that participation in the<br />

programme makes the families seek out better schools for their <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> use the schools<br />

more effectively. The evidence from the major studies is that all these things happen.<br />

Children’s cognition was boosted by their preschool experience <strong>and</strong> this plus continuing family<br />

s<strong>up</strong>port <strong>and</strong> school s<strong>up</strong>port through the later part of their education lead <strong>to</strong> higher rates of<br />

school completion. Program participation enhanced these little <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognition <strong>and</strong><br />

langu<strong>age</strong> <strong>and</strong> their readiness <strong>to</strong> cope with the dem<strong>and</strong>s of formal schooling; this led <strong>to</strong> higher<br />

ratings from teachers, less likelihood of being retained in grade <strong>to</strong> repeat a year, <strong>and</strong> more<br />

completion of high school. Programme involvement of mothers had a positive effect on both<br />

<strong>child</strong> outcomes <strong>and</strong> mothers’ well-being. School s<strong>up</strong>port <strong>and</strong> family s<strong>up</strong>port as they grew older<br />

were the major predic<strong>to</strong>rs of <strong>child</strong>ren avoiding involvement in crime <strong>and</strong> delinquency. The<br />

quality of schooling after the intervention was crucial for the eventual outcome; the early<br />

intervention is not so much an ‘inoculation’ that can prevent failure all by itself so much as an<br />

early advant<strong>age</strong> which can be built on <strong>to</strong> lead, eventually, <strong>to</strong> success, if school <strong>and</strong> family<br />

continue <strong>to</strong> s<strong>up</strong>port the <strong>child</strong>.<br />

Parent activities which ‘encour<strong>age</strong> <strong>development</strong>’, in joint play or through providing<br />

appropriate <strong>and</strong> varied materials <strong>and</strong> experiences, are relevant <strong>to</strong> cognitive achievement. The<br />

core is warm participation in socially <strong>and</strong> intellectually stimulating interactions, with adults<br />

showing reciprocity with <strong>child</strong>ren, being responsive <strong>to</strong> them, <strong>and</strong> providing emotional s<strong>up</strong>port<br />

but also providing some structured, directed experiences with encour<strong>age</strong>ment <strong>and</strong> praise<br />

(Meins 1997, 1998, Hubbs-Tait et al 2002, Petrill <strong>and</strong> Deater-Deckard 2004, Meadows 2005).<br />

Possibly the <strong>child</strong> participant in such interaction derives an enhanced sense of being competent<br />

<strong>and</strong> effective as well as receiving good cognitive opportunities <strong>and</strong> helpful interpretations <strong>and</strong><br />

s<strong>up</strong>port from the adult; it is worth noting that maternal intrusiveness, being very directive <strong>and</strong><br />

controlling, is associated with the <strong>child</strong> doing less well. There are quite consistent positive<br />

correlations between the amount of adult–<strong>child</strong> interaction of this sort <strong>and</strong> the <strong>child</strong>’s cognitive<br />

<strong>development</strong>, which remain even when maternal IQ <strong>and</strong> educational level are partialled out in<br />

an attempt <strong>to</strong> control for passive genotype–environment interaction effects (Gottfried 1984,<br />

Luster <strong>and</strong> Dubow 1992) <strong>and</strong> when other demographic variables are controlled (Gregg,<br />

Washbrook <strong>and</strong> Meadows 2004). It is not entirely clear which components of the parent’s<br />

28


ehaviour have direct effects, <strong>and</strong> which are mediated by other fac<strong>to</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong> the best balance of<br />

behaviour may very from task <strong>to</strong> task <strong>and</strong> <strong>age</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> (Hubbs-Tait et al 2002), but it does seem<br />

<strong>to</strong> be the case that if the parent-<strong>child</strong> interaction is characterized by positive emotional s<strong>up</strong>port,<br />

high cognitive stimulation <strong>and</strong> low parental intrusiveness the <strong>child</strong> is likely <strong>to</strong> do well in terms<br />

of both cognition <strong>and</strong> confidence, while the reverse of this pattern is associated with the <strong>child</strong><br />

doing badly. Thompson (2004) sees parenting <strong>and</strong> the environment of the home as the basis of<br />

individuals’ approaches <strong>to</strong> learning challenges <strong>and</strong> achievement. The relevant literature for key<br />

proximal influences is discussed in more detail in their relevant sections.<br />

The ALSPAC database offers an opportunity <strong>to</strong> look at the relationship between parenting<br />

behaviours <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> outcomes beyond the parent input-<strong>child</strong> output model. Its extensive data<br />

on household demographic characteristics <strong>and</strong> parent characteristics allow us <strong>to</strong> examine the<br />

effect of parenting behaviour in the context of other more distal variables that would be<br />

expected <strong>to</strong> affect the <strong>child</strong>'s <strong>development</strong>. For example, parents' educational level has long<br />

been known <strong>to</strong> be a predic<strong>to</strong>r of <strong>child</strong>ren's educational outcomes but, if the former is causing<br />

the latter, the mechanism could be through genetic transmission of aptitudes essential for<br />

educational achievement, or through the provision of better or worse educational opportunities<br />

in the school years, or through differences in parents' behaviour with <strong>child</strong>ren in the home.<br />

The ALSPAC data offers a similar advant<strong>age</strong> relating <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong> characteristics, which may have<br />

a direct effect on <strong>development</strong> or an indirect one through their evoking of different behaviour<br />

from the parent. For example, the gender difference in educational achievement in the early<br />

st<strong>age</strong>s of schooling could reflect some inherent earlier maturity of girls or differences in their<br />

earlier interaction with significant adults. The presence in the database of multiple measures of<br />

<strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> allows for examination of simultaneous effects; for example whether the<br />

effects of a particular parenting behaviour are as positive for behaviour as for cognition, <strong>and</strong><br />

whether the timing of an experience is important. These issues are theoretically important, but<br />

also are especially important for policy <strong>and</strong> interventions as effects may be bigger for some<br />

subgro<strong>up</strong>s of <strong>child</strong>ren than for others, or may have costs as well as benefits, or may need <strong>to</strong> be<br />

carefully timed.<br />

3.2 Methodology<br />

Our aim in this section is <strong>to</strong> explore the relationship between inherent parenting behaviours <strong>and</strong><br />

the home environment <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> outcomes, both cognitive <strong>and</strong> behavioural, at <strong>age</strong> 4 <strong>to</strong> 5. Our<br />

primary focus here is <strong>to</strong> ask the questions:<br />

29


• What are the observed relationships between different types of parenting behaviours<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> outcomes, conditional on just the limited range of <strong>background</strong> controls<br />

introduced above?<br />

• Which parenting behaviours matter most for which <strong>child</strong> outcomes?<br />

• Are certain sub-populations more effective when employing specific types of<br />

parenting behaviours?<br />

The aim is <strong>to</strong> identify a parsimonious number of parenting variables, all of which have<br />

independent <strong>and</strong> significant effects on <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>. This gro<strong>up</strong> of variables can<br />

then be used in the analysis of the effects of other fac<strong>to</strong>rs as an effective control for the <strong>child</strong>’s<br />

‘home environment’. For example, we can then explore the relationships between family<br />

income, <strong>child</strong>care arrangements <strong>and</strong> parenting <strong>and</strong> the relative importance of each.<br />

Our empirical strategy is <strong>to</strong> first estimate K regressions of the form:<br />

C = β +<br />

i<br />

k<br />

α1 + α<br />

2<br />

X<br />

i<br />

+<br />

k<br />

Pi<br />

ui<br />

k = 1,…, K (1)<br />

where C i is the outcome measure for <strong>child</strong> i, X i is a vec<strong>to</strong>r of controls capturing the basic<br />

k<br />

demographics of the household, <strong>and</strong> P i is a measure of the kth parenting behaviour<br />

experienced by <strong>child</strong> i. Hence our first step is <strong>to</strong> explore the conditional correlations of each<br />

parenting measure with the <strong>child</strong> outcome separately. Although not raw correlations, the β<br />

coefficients show the relationships between parenting <strong>and</strong> outcomes without taking account of<br />

many other fac<strong>to</strong>rs that may have a causal influence on both – for example the mother’s mental<br />

health <strong>and</strong> household income. The main purpose of this is <strong>to</strong> explore whether there is any<br />

evidence at all that the behaviour captured by P k has any impact on <strong>child</strong> outcomes. If not, then<br />

that measure can be dropped from all further analyses.<br />

Rather than using continuous measures of parenting in our estimation, we divide each measure<br />

in<strong>to</strong> discrete categories <strong>and</strong> create dummy variables <strong>to</strong> indicate in<strong>to</strong> which category the parent<br />

falls. This is because<br />

• It allows us <strong>to</strong> create a separate category for missing observations, rather than imputing<br />

values<br />

30


• It allows for non-linearities in the relationship between parenting behaviours <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

outcomes. For instance, not reading <strong>to</strong> a <strong>child</strong> at all may be far more important than the<br />

difference between reading 4 <strong>and</strong> 5 times a week.<br />

• The raw values of the parenting scores vary across measures <strong>and</strong> are difficult <strong>to</strong> interpret.<br />

The use of discrete categories allows comparisons based only on the relative positions of<br />

households in the distribution (for example, we can examine the effect of a move from the<br />

lowest <strong>to</strong> the highest quartile).<br />

In determining the category boundaries, the aim is <strong>to</strong> minimise the number of categories<br />

without losing explana<strong>to</strong>ry power, <strong>and</strong> also <strong>to</strong> maintain the same boundaries for use in both the<br />

EA <strong>and</strong> Behavioural regressions. To do this, we first introduce the variable in<strong>to</strong> the regressions<br />

in a highly disaggregated form (for example in deciles). We then observe the coefficients <strong>and</strong><br />

test which categories can be combined using joint F-tests. This method allows us <strong>to</strong> overcome<br />

the fact that the distribution of some parenting measures is highly skewed <strong>and</strong> so do not lend<br />

themselves <strong>to</strong> division in<strong>to</strong> equal quantiles. (For example, the observed range of the maternal<br />

teaching score is from 10 <strong>to</strong> 30 points. However, 22% of observations score the maximum of<br />

30 points <strong>and</strong> 40% score 29 or above).<br />

The next step is <strong>to</strong> introduce all K parenting measures in<strong>to</strong> the regression simultaneously:<br />

n<br />

1<br />

+ α<br />

2<br />

X<br />

i<br />

+ β<br />

k<br />

k=1<br />

k<br />

C<br />

i<br />

= α ∑ Pi<br />

+ ei<br />

(2)<br />

As many of the measures are likely <strong>to</strong> be highly correlated, this step allows us <strong>to</strong> observe how<br />

the estimated effect of each behaviour changes when all the other behaviours are controlled for.<br />

Measures that give no additional explana<strong>to</strong>ry power (i.e. that are insignificant) can be tested<br />

jointly <strong>and</strong> dropped from the model. The results of the final specification then show us the<br />

magnitude <strong>and</strong> relative importance of different types of parenting activity on <strong>child</strong> outcomes<br />

<strong>and</strong>, when taken <strong>to</strong>gether, provide a concise measure of parental behaviour that can be used as<br />

a control in further analyses. The addition of such household characteristics as the ownership<br />

of a car <strong>and</strong> the prevalence of damp in the home allows the analysis of parenting behaviour <strong>to</strong><br />

be exp<strong>and</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> parenting <strong>and</strong> home environment.<br />

3.2.1 General notes on the construction of the parenting measures<br />

As our <strong>child</strong> outcomes are measured at <strong>age</strong> 4 or 5, in general we have attempted <strong>to</strong> construct<br />

variables that capture the <strong>child</strong>’s experience of a particular behaviour throughout the entire<br />

31


preschool period. To test robustness we do explore a number of alternative specifications that<br />

distinguish between behaviours at different points in time (at 1 ½ <strong>and</strong> 3 ½ years, for example).<br />

Thus we do allow <strong>to</strong> some extent for the fact that parental behaviours might have different<br />

effects at different <strong>age</strong>s of the <strong>child</strong>. However, given the almost over-rich nature of the<br />

ALSPAC data, it is necessary <strong>to</strong> largely abstract from the issue of exact timing in favour of<br />

tractability.<br />

When constructing composite parenting measures that aggregate a large number of individual<br />

questions, it is inevitable that many observations will have a certain number of missing<br />

responses. Rather than imputing many of these missing values, in general we choose <strong>to</strong> focus<br />

only on complete observations. Incomplete observations are thus coded as ‘missing’ <strong>and</strong><br />

comprise a separate category within a given discrete variable, denoted by a specific dummy<br />

variable. This ensures that we can be certain of the composition of the gro<strong>up</strong>s used in intergro<strong>up</strong><br />

comparisons <strong>and</strong> our estimates will not be biased by inappropriate assumptions on the<br />

way that missing values are imputed. The large sample size of the ALSPAC dataset means that,<br />

even using this method, ample observations remain for estimation.<br />

3.2.2 The measures<br />

The following gives a brief description of each of the parenting measures used in our analysis.<br />

The measures used are gro<strong>up</strong>ed in<strong>to</strong> those directly involving parental choices <strong>and</strong> those where<br />

other dimensions of the family <strong>background</strong> may constrain the choices available. These<br />

constraints may reflect lack of income or housing quality. The divide is not always clear-cut<br />

<strong>and</strong> this is, in part, why we discuss <strong>and</strong> analyse these parts simultaneously. Further details are<br />

given in Appendices on the scoring system used <strong>to</strong> construct each variable (Appendix A1), the<br />

distribution of the measures <strong>and</strong> the ways the scores were gro<strong>up</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> construct discrete<br />

variables (Appendix A2).<br />

Parenting Behaviour<br />

Parental reading scores<br />

The aim was <strong>to</strong> create some measure of how frequently the <strong>child</strong> was read <strong>to</strong> by a parent during<br />

the pre-school period as a whole. The mother was asked at regular intervals how frequently<br />

she, <strong>and</strong> also her partner, read <strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong> or showed him/her pictures in books. Table A1 3.1a<br />

gives details of the scoring for each of the mother’s questions <strong>and</strong> of our derived maternal<br />

reading variable. At each <strong>age</strong> observed an intensity of reading score is created ranging from<br />

32


zero <strong>to</strong> four. Then a <strong>to</strong>tal maternal reading score is derived by summing the points for each<br />

response across the four questionnaires. This <strong>to</strong>tal score ranges from zero <strong>to</strong> fifteen. If any<br />

elements are missing then the <strong>to</strong>tal maternal reading score is missing. Then we explored which<br />

parts of the distribution of this score were associated with clear differences in attainment <strong>and</strong><br />

chose four categories that gave the best fit. The very low <strong>and</strong> low intensity of reading scores<br />

each cover a little under a tenth of the observed population, the medium category around a<br />

quarter, leaving the high covering over half of the population. This was because of large<br />

bunching of results with scores at 13 or above where no material difference in the correlation<br />

with outcomes could be distinguished.<br />

A problem arises in deriving the paternal reading score as, in this case, we do not want the<br />

effects of differential reading behaviours <strong>to</strong> be confounded with family structure. Low paternal<br />

reading scores will capture paternal absence as well as low reading involvement. To overcome<br />

this problem, the paternal reading score is only defined for households in which a partner was<br />

continuously present throughout the pre-school period (i.e. the mother responded that she had a<br />

current partner in all of the 8, 21, 33 <strong>and</strong> 47 month questionnaires). Households with any<br />

record of lone parenthood (or missing information) are placed in a category captured by a<br />

separate dummy. Hence our results relate <strong>to</strong> the effect of differences in partners’ reading<br />

behaviour within intact families only <strong>and</strong> do not reflect the effects of paternal absence (which<br />

are captured elsewhere). Given this different interpretation of paternal reading behaviour<br />

relative <strong>to</strong> maternal reading behaviour, we adopt a simplified measure of paternal reading<br />

involvement based on the number of times the partner is observed as being highly involved in<br />

reading <strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong>. Details are given in Table A1 3.2a. Table A1 3.1b shows how the raw<br />

maternal reading score variable was gro<strong>up</strong>ed first in<strong>to</strong> disaggregated categories <strong>and</strong> then, after<br />

a series of specification tests, in<strong>to</strong> final categories. For the paternal measure each score from 0<br />

<strong>to</strong> 4 represents a different discrete category (Table A1 3.2b). The distributions of the two<br />

reading score variables are shown in Figures A2.1 <strong>and</strong> A2.2.<br />

Maternal teaching score<br />

Mothers were asked on three separate occasions whether they tried <strong>to</strong> teach their <strong>child</strong>ren a<br />

number of items (such as colours, numbers, etc). Our derived measure is designed <strong>to</strong> capture<br />

something about both the range of items taught <strong>and</strong> the timing of the teaching activities. Table<br />

A1 3.3a gives details of the points allocated <strong>to</strong> each response. Item scores are coded as missing<br />

if it is not possible <strong>to</strong> identify the <strong>age</strong> at which the mother started teaching the <strong>child</strong> a<br />

33


particular item (for example if there is a missing response <strong>to</strong> the teaching of colours at 18<br />

months <strong>and</strong> a response of yes at 30 months). The <strong>to</strong>tal teaching score is the sum of the<br />

individual item scores <strong>and</strong> if any item score is missing, the <strong>to</strong>tal teaching score is set <strong>to</strong><br />

missing.<br />

The gro<strong>up</strong>ing of the teaching score in<strong>to</strong> discrete categories is shown in Table A1 3.3b <strong>and</strong><br />

graphically in Figure A2.3.<br />

Talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied score<br />

This score is designed <strong>to</strong> capture the degree of stimulation that the <strong>child</strong> receives when the<br />

mother is primarily eng<strong>age</strong>d in activities other than direct <strong>child</strong>care. Mothers were asked at<br />

regular intervals (6, 18, 30 <strong>and</strong> 42 months) ‘How often do you talk <strong>to</strong> your <strong>child</strong> while you do<br />

housework or are occ<strong>up</strong>ied in some other way?’ Responses were scored as shown in Table A1<br />

3.4a <strong>and</strong> then summed <strong>to</strong> create an overall talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied score. If any of the four<br />

responses were missing then the talking score was coded as missing.<br />

Table A1 3.4b <strong>and</strong> Figure A2.4 show the distribution <strong>and</strong> category boundaries of the talking<br />

whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied score.<br />

Television scores<br />

There are questions regarding the <strong>child</strong>’s television watching habits at regular intervals in<br />

ALSPAC but the questions asked are not consistent across questionnaires. For this reason we<br />

are not able <strong>to</strong> construct a composite television score for the whole preschool period. Rather,<br />

we look at two different measures, one early (18 months) <strong>and</strong> one later (38 months). The early<br />

measure relates <strong>to</strong> how often the mother has the television on at different times of the day. The<br />

later measure relates <strong>to</strong> how many hours a day the <strong>child</strong> typically watches television on<br />

weekdays <strong>and</strong> at weekends.<br />

Details of the scoring of the two measures are given in Tables A1 3.5a <strong>and</strong> A1 3.6a. Tables A1<br />

3.5b <strong>and</strong> A1 3.6b <strong>and</strong> Figures A2.5a <strong>and</strong> A2.5b show the distributions <strong>and</strong> category boundaries<br />

for the two television scores.<br />

Breastfed baby<br />

Mother’s were asked both when the <strong>child</strong> was 6 months <strong>and</strong> again when they were 15 months<br />

about the duration of the mother’s breastfeeding. The results have been gro<strong>up</strong>ed in<strong>to</strong> never,<br />

34


less than 6 months <strong>and</strong> more than 6 months. This variable is specified in this way as the mere<br />

act of initiating breastfeeding could have a positive effect on <strong>child</strong>’s <strong>development</strong>, whereas 6<br />

months could be seen as a threshold signifying a lengthy period of breastfeeding.<br />

Smoked in pregnancy<br />

Questions were asked about the mother smoking during her pregnancy at various points of the<br />

pregnancy <strong>and</strong> soon afterwards. This information was used <strong>to</strong> derive a simple dummy variable<br />

of whether the mother did smoke or not.<br />

Maternal bonding score<br />

A number of questions were asked <strong>to</strong> the mother when the <strong>child</strong> was 8 months old, such as ‘I<br />

really enjoy my baby’ <strong>and</strong> ‘it is a great pleasure <strong>to</strong> watch my baby develop’. A scale was<br />

derived from the answers <strong>to</strong> these questions <strong>to</strong> reflect how much the mother feels she has<br />

bonded with her baby, <strong>and</strong> divided in<strong>to</strong> quintiles. As the mother had a choice of four answers<br />

(feel exactly, feel often, feel sometimes, feel never), <strong>and</strong> these answers were recoded from<br />

nought <strong>to</strong> three <strong>and</strong> then all the answers summed, dividing the scale in<strong>to</strong> quintiles seems more<br />

appropriate than using the continuous measure as the quintiles reflect the relative position of<br />

the mother.<br />

Details of the distribution <strong>and</strong> gro<strong>up</strong>ing of the results are found in Table A1 3.9a <strong>and</strong> A1 3.9b<br />

Types of discipline<br />

When the <strong>child</strong> was 42 months old, ALSPAC asked the mother questions on various forms of<br />

discipline. In order <strong>to</strong> use the answers <strong>to</strong> these questions for our analysis, the forms of<br />

discipline are divided in<strong>to</strong> whether they were used at least once a week or less than once a<br />

week. By keeping the forms of discipline separate rather than trying <strong>to</strong> aggregate them in some<br />

way, it is then possible <strong>to</strong> observe whether some forms of discipline are more detrimental than<br />

others. For instance, whether smacking a naughty <strong>child</strong> is more harmful in respect <strong>to</strong> their<br />

<strong>development</strong> than simply ignoring them.<br />

Home Environment <strong>and</strong> Outings<br />

Outings scores<br />

ALSPAC contains data at various intervals (18, 30 <strong>and</strong> 42 months) on the frequency with<br />

which the <strong>child</strong> is taken <strong>to</strong> visit a range of eight different places (for example, <strong>to</strong> the local<br />

35


shops, the park <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> visit friends or family). We experimented with two alternative methods<br />

of specifying the relationship between outings <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> outcomes. Firstly, we adopt a method<br />

that allows the effect on <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong> of outings <strong>to</strong> different places <strong>to</strong> vary. Hence<br />

we can investigate whether trips <strong>to</strong> certain places are more beneficial <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s<br />

<strong>development</strong> than others. Table A1 4.1a gives details of how a score was constructed for visits<br />

<strong>to</strong> each of the eight places over the period from 18 months <strong>to</strong> 42 months. If any of the elements<br />

of a place-specific outings score are missing then that score is coded as missing.<br />

Our second method is <strong>to</strong> construct a composite score of <strong>to</strong>tal outings over the two-year period.<br />

This method treats all type of outings identically <strong>and</strong> makes no distinction between the<br />

different places the <strong>child</strong> might visit. This composite score is simply the sum of the eight<br />

individual place-specific scores. If any of the place-specific outings scores are missing then the<br />

<strong>to</strong>tal outings score is coded as missing.<br />

In order <strong>to</strong> incorporate the place-specific outings scores in<strong>to</strong> our analysis, each one was split<br />

in<strong>to</strong> two categories – high (above the mean) <strong>and</strong> low (below the mean). Table A1 4.1b <strong>and</strong><br />

Figures A2.6a <strong>to</strong> A2.6h show the distributions of each of the scores <strong>and</strong> their category<br />

boundaries. The <strong>to</strong>tal outings score was treated in a similar way <strong>to</strong> the other parenting<br />

variables, entering the regressions first in a disaggregated form <strong>and</strong> then in a more concise<br />

form. Table A1 4.1c <strong>and</strong> Figure A2.7 show the distribution.<br />

Gro<strong>up</strong>ed <strong>to</strong>gether with the place-specific outings scores, due <strong>to</strong> its obvious association with<br />

outings, is whether the household owns a car. This was asked when the <strong>child</strong> was 33 months<br />

<strong>and</strong> is included in the analysis as a dummy variable. Missing results are recorded separately as<br />

missing.<br />

Book score<br />

This score is designed <strong>to</strong> capture how many books the <strong>child</strong> has throughout the preschool<br />

period. Questions on the number of books the <strong>child</strong> owns are asked at 6, 18, 24, 30 <strong>and</strong> 42<br />

months. Table A1 4.2a gives details of how points are allocated <strong>to</strong> each response. The <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

book score is simply the sum of these point scores. If any individual point score is missing the<br />

<strong>to</strong>tal book score is coded as missing.<br />

36


Table A1 4.2b <strong>and</strong> Figure A2.8 show the distribution <strong>and</strong> category boundaries of the book<br />

score.<br />

Toy score (24 months)<br />

This score is designed <strong>to</strong> capture the number <strong>and</strong> range of <strong>to</strong>ys available <strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong> at 2 years<br />

of <strong>age</strong>. Table A1 4.3a shows the scoring for questions on the number of each of eleven types of<br />

<strong>to</strong>ys <strong>to</strong> which the <strong>child</strong> has access. The <strong>to</strong>tal <strong>to</strong>y score is the sum of these scored responses <strong>and</strong><br />

is missing if any of the responses are missing.<br />

Table A1 4.3b <strong>and</strong> Figure A2.9 show the distribution <strong>and</strong> category boundaries of the <strong>to</strong>y score.<br />

Crowding index<br />

This index is created by dividing the number of people in the home by the number of rooms in<br />

the house. Questions regarding the number of people <strong>and</strong> the number of rooms were asked in<br />

the same questionnaire, given when the <strong>child</strong> was 42 months old.<br />

Damp/condensation/mould<br />

Mothers were asked during the 33 month questionnaire how much a problem damp,<br />

condensation <strong>and</strong> mould was in the house.<br />

3.2.3 Simple Correlation analysis<br />

Table 3.1 gives the pairwise (unconditional) correlations between all our parenting measures<br />

(represented continuously where possible), plus the correlations with a number of broad<br />

demographic measures. We can see that most parenting behaviours are positively correlated,<br />

even for those done separately by the mother <strong>and</strong> her partner. For example, the correlation<br />

between the mothers <strong>and</strong> partners’ reading scores is 0.43. There is little evidence on the basis<br />

of the raw data, then, that households substitute learning activities by one parent with the other.<br />

Children in households with relatively high measures of cognitive stimulation tend <strong>to</strong><br />

experience a number of parenting behaviours simultaneously. So, mothers who read <strong>to</strong> their<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren frequently also teach them a wider range of material <strong>and</strong> talk <strong>to</strong> them more frequently<br />

when occ<strong>up</strong>ied with another task. Likewise the <strong>child</strong>ren of mothers with a high teaching score<br />

also have more trips <strong>to</strong> the park <strong>and</strong> places of interest <strong>and</strong> a higher book score but watch less<br />

TV.<br />

37


Turning <strong>to</strong> the correlations with other household characteristics, we see that there are differing<br />

relationships between types of parenting <strong>and</strong> the resources of the household. Outings <strong>to</strong> places<br />

of interest <strong>and</strong> the book score are relatively strongly correlated with household income, while<br />

outings <strong>to</strong> the local shops <strong>and</strong> watching television are negatively correlated. Other measures,<br />

most notably the teaching <strong>and</strong> talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied scores, reveal no relationship with<br />

household income <strong>and</strong> very little relationship with parental education. Table 3.1 also shows<br />

that lone parents do not differ greatly in the parenting behaviours from mothers in co<strong>up</strong>les. The<br />

greatest differences are along dimensions like outings <strong>to</strong> the local shops <strong>and</strong> department s<strong>to</strong>res,<br />

outings <strong>to</strong> places of interest <strong>and</strong> the number of books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys, all of which are relatively<br />

strongly associated with household income. There may be time constraints on mothers which<br />

mean trips <strong>to</strong> shops are more common but trips <strong>to</strong> the zoo or similar are less commonly<br />

undertaken by lone parents. Many of these activities are also correlated with being a first <strong>child</strong><br />

suggesting parents with more <strong>child</strong>ren find it harder <strong>to</strong> find time for reading <strong>and</strong> teaching <strong>to</strong> a<br />

specific <strong>child</strong>. We will explore these relationships more closely later in the report.<br />

3.3 The impact of the parenting variables on outcomes at 5<br />

The first step in our estimation process was <strong>to</strong> estimate equation (1), including each of the K<br />

parenting measures separately in a highly disaggregated form. These narrowly defined<br />

categories were then combined as far as allowed by a test of restrictions. The results presented<br />

here relate only <strong>to</strong> the more parsimonious, second-st<strong>age</strong> specifications.<br />

3.3.1 Single Measure Estimates for Cognitive Development<br />

Table 3.2 gives the conditional ( β ) coefficients from equation (1) with EA as the dependent<br />

k<br />

variable. These are the coefficients on each of the parenting measures when they are added <strong>to</strong><br />

the basic specification in separate gro<strong>up</strong>s. So these estimates take no account of the<br />

correlations between the parenting measures, which are strongly correlated. The aim here is <strong>to</strong><br />

move from observing basic correlations <strong>to</strong> showing which parenting measures are the core<br />

drivers of attainment <strong>and</strong> which are probably just correlated with more important behaviours. It<br />

is clear from Table 3.3 that many of our parenting measures are significantly associated with<br />

the EA scores. The largest coefficients effects are found for those with the lowest scores for the<br />

maternal reading, maternal teaching <strong>and</strong> book scores, all large <strong>and</strong> negative. To give an idea of<br />

the magnitude of these effects, a maternal qualification of CSE/none is associated with a score<br />

of between 2 <strong>and</strong> 3 points lower, on aver<strong>age</strong>, than a qualification of an A-level or higher (the<br />

38


coefficients on education are discussed further below). A move from the <strong>to</strong>p 40 percent b<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong><br />

the lowest 8 per cent b<strong>and</strong> of the teaching score, however, is associated with a drop of 4.6<br />

points, or nearly half a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. Aspects of the home environment are also strongly<br />

correlated with attainment such as the book score (there is over 4 points gap between the <strong>to</strong>p<br />

<strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m categories).<br />

The effects of different parenting behaviours are not solely concentrated in the extremes of the<br />

distributions. Many of the scores (e.g. the maternal reading, teaching, breastfeeding scores)<br />

show a clear gradient in the effect as the score decreases. Smoking in pregnancy, positive<br />

feelings <strong>to</strong>wards motherhood <strong>and</strong> types of discipline do not appear significant for the EA test,<br />

whereas attending an antenatal class <strong>and</strong> breastfeeding are. It is perhaps also worth pointing out<br />

that all the outing destinations do not have the same sign. Whilst trips <strong>to</strong> the library <strong>and</strong> places<br />

of interest are positively associated with <strong>child</strong> outcomes, trips <strong>to</strong> department s<strong>to</strong>res are negative<br />

<strong>and</strong> significant.<br />

A number of measures were found <strong>to</strong> have no correlation even at this early st<strong>age</strong> of<br />

investigation. For instance, car ownership, maternal bonding <strong>and</strong> indica<strong>to</strong>rs of housing<br />

conditions (crowding <strong>and</strong> damp) are not correlated with attainment when only <strong>background</strong><br />

family characteristics are conditioned on.<br />

39


Table 3.1: Pairwise correlations between parenting measures <strong>and</strong> selected household characteristics<br />

Mother's<br />

reading<br />

CORRELATIONS<br />

score<br />

Mother's reading score 1.00<br />

Partner’s<br />

reading<br />

score 1<br />

Partner’s reading score 1 0.43 1.00<br />

Teaching<br />

score<br />

Teaching score 0.40 0.25 1.00<br />

Talking<br />

score<br />

Talking score 0.30 0.19 0.25 1.00<br />

TV at 18<br />

months<br />

TV at 18 months -0.16 -0.15 -0.05 -0.03 1.00<br />

TV at 38<br />

months<br />

TV at 38 months -0.13 -0.10 -0.02 -0.05 0.38 1.00<br />

Never<br />

breastfed<br />

baby<br />

Never breastfed baby 3 -0.13 -0.10 -0.06 0.01 0.19 0.12 1.00<br />

Smoked in<br />

pregnancy<br />

Smoked in pregnancy 1 -0.10 -0.08 0.01 0.02 0.12 0.10 0.14 1.00<br />

Maternal bonding score 0.10 0.05 0.06 0.21 0.01 -0.00 0.03 -0.00 1.00<br />

Maternal<br />

bonding<br />

score<br />

Household owns a car 2 -0.04 -0.05 -0.01 0.00 0.11 0.10 0.12 0.20 0.01 1.00<br />

Household<br />

owns a car 2 Book score Toy score<br />

Book score 0.45 0.31 0.32 0.14 -0.21 -0.13 -0.20 -0.14 0.03 -0.12 1.00<br />

Toy score 0.18 0.13 0.13 0.12 -0.12 -0.10 -0.08 -0.10 0.09 -0.12 0.27 1.00<br />

Crowding index -0.15 -0.14 -0.09 -0.03 0.16 0.11 0.12 0.12 0.05 0.10 -0.22 -0.08 1.00<br />

No damp/condensation/mould problem 0.03 0.02 0.02 0.08 0.01 0.02 0.06 -0.01 0.07 -0.05 0.01 0.03 -0.12 1.00<br />

Ignores <strong>child</strong> when naughty -0.01 -0.00 -0.01 -0.07 0.03 0.03 -0.00 0.04 -0.11 0.02 -0.01 -0.03 0.00 -0.02<br />

Smacks <strong>child</strong> when naughty -0.10 -0.08 -0.04 -0.07 0.15 0.12 0.04 0.03 -0.04 0.02 -0.07 -0.08 0.05 -0.03<br />

Shouts at <strong>child</strong> when naughty -0.10 -0.05 -0.05 -0.09 0.13 0.12 0.00 0.03 -0.13 0.00 -0.04 -0.06 0.04 -0.07<br />

Sends <strong>child</strong> <strong>to</strong> room when naughty 0.01 0.03 0.05 -0.04 0.00 -0.01 -0.02 0.01 -0.06 0.04 0.06 -0.01 -0.00 -0.04<br />

Takes away treats when <strong>child</strong> naughty 0.01 0.02 0.08 0.00 0.07 0.04 0.02 0.06 -0.02 0.04 0.02 -0.02 0.01 -0.01<br />

Tells off <strong>child</strong> when naughty -0.02 -0.00 -0.02 -0.07 0.04 0.04 -0.03 0.01 -0.10 -0.01 0.02 -0.04 0.02 -0.08<br />

Bribes <strong>child</strong> when naughty -0.07 -0.05 -0.03 -0.12 0.07 0.09 -0.01 0.01 -0.11 -0.01 -0.04 -0.02 -0.02 -0.03<br />

Local shops 0.06 0.03 0.03 0.15 0.10 0.08 0.12 0.16 0.07 0.16 -0.09 -0.00 0.12 0.00<br />

Dpt s<strong>to</strong>re 0.07 0.05 0.10 0.08 0.06 0.02 0.08 0.00 0.01 0.08 0.03 0.03 -0.05 0.08<br />

S<strong>up</strong>ermarket 0.03 -0.01 0.04 0.08 0.11 0.09 0.07 0.03 0.05 0.06 -0.04 -0.01 0.06 0.02<br />

Park 0.25 0.21 0.16 0.12 -0.17 -0.11 -0.07 0.01 0.02 0.03 0.16 0.10 -0.07 0.04<br />

Friends 0.10 0.08 0.09 0.12 0.01 -0.01 0.05 0.03 0.09 -0.02 0.11 0.09 -0.04 0.04<br />

Library 0.24 0.15 0.07 0.04 -0.24 -0.17 -0.15 -0.09 -0.03 -0.01 0.21 0.19 -0.05 -0.05<br />

Places interest 0.27 0.23 0.15 0.08 -0.28 -0.22 -0.15 -0.12 -0.01 -0.12 0.29 0.22 -0.20 0.02<br />

Places entertainment 0.12 0.08 0.12 0.06 -0.06 -0.07 -0.02 -0.02 -0.00 -0.06 0.12 0.15 -0.09 0.05<br />

Household income 0.13 0.14 0.04 -0.00 -0.24 -0.16 -0.21 -0.27 -0.03 -0.36 0.24 0.19 -0.28 -0.06<br />

Ever lone parent 2 -0.04 - -0.00 -0.02 0.07 0.05 0.09 0.24 -0.03 0.40 -0.07 -0.09 -0.03 -0.01<br />

Maternal education 3 0.21 0.21 0.06 -0.04 -0.33 -0.23 -0.30 -0.22 -0.08 -0.18 0.26 0.14 -0.23 -0.08<br />

Firstborn <strong>child</strong> 2 0.23 0.26 0.26 0.10 -0.04 -0.01 -0.07 -0.02 -0.04 0.03 -0.28 -0.22 -0.21 0.02<br />

Crowding<br />

index<br />

No damp/<br />

mould<br />

40


Table 3.2: Entry Assessment – coefficients on parenting <strong>and</strong> home environment variables<br />

when included individually in basic specification (N = 5010)<br />

Adj R squared 0.2429 0.2527 0.2579 0.2467 0.2453 0.2510 0.2540 0.2429<br />

Maternal reading score: base = high<br />

Very low - -3.18 *** - - - - - -<br />

Low - -1.99 *** - - - - - -<br />

Medium - -0.95 *** - - - - - -<br />

Number of times partner highly involved in reading (intact families only): base = 2<br />

0 - -1.02 ** - - - - - -<br />

1 - -0.51 - - - - - -<br />

3 - 0.17 - - - - - -<br />

4 - 0.31 - - - - - -<br />

Maternal teaching score: base = high<br />

Very low - - -4.58 *** - - - - -<br />

Low - - -2.61 *** - - - - -<br />

Medium - - -1.30 *** - - - - -<br />

Talking score: base = high<br />

18 month TV score: base = 0-2<br />

Insig<br />

3-5 - - - -0.93 *** - - - -<br />

6 - - - -1.95 *** - - - -<br />

38 month TV score: base = < 10 hrs pwk<br />

10+ hrs pwk - - - -0.77 ** - - - -<br />

Breastfed baby: base = never<br />

0- 6 months - - - - 0.66 ** - -<br />

6 months + - - - - 1.45 *** - -<br />

Smoked in pregnancy<br />

Maternal bonding score<br />

Types of discipline (used at least once a week)<br />

Place –specific outings scores: base = low (for each score)<br />

Local shops - - - - - Insig - -<br />

Dpt s<strong>to</strong>re - - - - - -0.65 ** - -<br />

S<strong>up</strong>ermarket - - - - - Insig - -<br />

Park - - - - - Insig - -<br />

Friends - - - - - Insig - -<br />

Library - - - - - 1.33 *** - -<br />

Places interest - - - - - 1.05 *** - -<br />

Places entertainment - - - - - Insig - -<br />

Household owns a car: base = yes<br />

Book score: base = high<br />

Very low - - - - - - -3.54 *** -<br />

Low - - - - - - -1.58 *** -<br />

Medium - - - - - - -0.90 ** -<br />

Toy score: base = high<br />

Very low - - - - - - -1.88 *** -<br />

Low - - - - - - -0.98 ** -<br />

Medium - - - - - - 0.05 -<br />

Crowding index (people per room): base = < 0.5<br />

Damp/condensation/mould: base = none<br />

Notes<br />

1. All regressions include controls for the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> in months at assessment, cohort year, number of older siblings, number of<br />

younger siblings by 42 months, gender, birth weight, special care unit at birth, ethnicity, mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth <strong>and</strong> highest<br />

educational attainment, lone parent status at 47 months, the partner’s highest educational attainment <strong>and</strong> employment status at 47<br />

months.<br />

2. Each measure of cognitive <strong>development</strong> is normalised <strong>to</strong> mean 100, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10. 3. ***, **, <strong>and</strong> * indicate significance at<br />

the 1%, 5% <strong>and</strong> 10% levels respectively.<br />

Insig<br />

Insig<br />

Insig<br />

Insig<br />

Insig<br />

Insig<br />

41


3.3.2 Estimation Conditional on All Other Parenting Measures<br />

Here we explore which measures still matter when they are all included <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>and</strong> assess the<br />

explana<strong>to</strong>ry power of the parenting variables. Column 1 of Table 3.3 includes all parenting <strong>and</strong><br />

home environment measures simultaneously <strong>and</strong> as before includes a set of basic <strong>background</strong><br />

controls for the demographics of the household. A number of features st<strong>and</strong> out in particular.<br />

Mother’s reading score becomes only half the size of that when other parenting influences are<br />

excluded. Book <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>y scores are also substantially smaller. Some variables are not significant<br />

once other measures are included, trips <strong>to</strong> places of interest <strong>and</strong> crowding, for example.<br />

Column 2 repeats this exercise but gives a more parsimonious representation with insignificant<br />

terms being dropped. The pattern of results is largely unaffected by this process. But partner’s<br />

reading, which was very marginal in column 1, becomes insignificant once other insignificant<br />

terms are excluded <strong>and</strong> it is excluded from further analysis. These results suggest that a<br />

surprisingly large set of parental influences is found <strong>to</strong> have independent effects. Mother’s<br />

teaching score remains the single most powerful predic<strong>to</strong>r of <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> at school entry<br />

but the number of books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys in the house, the amount of TV watched, especially at 18<br />

months, <strong>and</strong> trips <strong>to</strong> the department s<strong>to</strong>res <strong>and</strong> library have independent identifiable effects on<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren. Breastfeeding for 6 months or more also remains significant.<br />

42


Table 3.3: Entry Assessment – coefficients on parenting variables when included<br />

simultaneously in basic specification (N = 5010)<br />

Adj R2 0.2714 0.2709<br />

Coef<br />

Coef<br />

Maternal reading score: base = high<br />

Very low -1.34** -1.69***<br />

Low -0.87* -1.05**<br />

Medium -0.32 -0.44<br />

Number of times partner highly involved in reading (intact families only): base = 2<br />

0 -0.89** -<br />

1 -0.28 -<br />

3 -0.03 -<br />

4 -0.02 -<br />

Maternal teaching score: base = high<br />

Very low -3.47*** -3.55***<br />

Low -1.94*** -1.98***<br />

Medium -1.03*** -1.03***<br />

18 month TV score: base = 0-2<br />

3-5 -0.58* -0.69**<br />

6 -1.30*** -1.53***<br />

38 month TV score: base = < 10 hrs pwk<br />

10+ hrs pwk -0.55* -<br />

Breastfed baby: base = never<br />

0- 6 months 0.47 0.45<br />

6 months + 0.91** 0.89**<br />

Place –specific outings scores: base = low (for each score)<br />

Dpt s<strong>to</strong>re -0.71*** -0.65**<br />

Library 0.83*** 0.91***<br />

Places interest 0.35 -<br />

Book score: base = high<br />

Very low -1.99*** -2.07***<br />

Low -0.58 -0.67*<br />

Medium -0.48 -0.51<br />

Toy score: base = high<br />

Very low -1.06* -1.18*<br />

Low -0.37 -0.45<br />

Medium 0.41 0.37<br />

Notes<br />

1. All regressions include controls for the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> in months at assessment, cohort year, number of older siblings, number of<br />

younger siblings by 42 months, gender, birth weight, special care unit at birth, ethnicity, mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth <strong>and</strong> highest educational<br />

attainment, lone parent status at 47 months, the partner’s highest educational attainment <strong>and</strong> employment status at 47 months.<br />

2. Each measure of cognitive <strong>development</strong> is normalised <strong>to</strong> mean 100, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10.<br />

3. ***, **, <strong>and</strong> * indicate significance at the 1%, 5% <strong>and</strong> 10% levels respectively.<br />

43


3.3.3 Behaviour<br />

Tables 3.4 <strong>and</strong> 3.5 repeat the above analysis for our behaviour measure <strong>and</strong> shows regressions<br />

for a basic set of controls plus each set of parenting <strong>and</strong> home environment measure in turn.<br />

Again these parenting measures are extremely strong predic<strong>to</strong>rs of behavioural problems,<br />

although the magnitudes differ in their relative importance. Reading, teaching <strong>and</strong> the book<br />

score are still strong influences on behaviour but the <strong>to</strong>y score is more important, as is talking<br />

whilst eng<strong>age</strong>d in housework.<br />

There are a wider set of parenting behaviours <strong>and</strong> home environment indica<strong>to</strong>rs that are<br />

individually significant for behavioural outcomes (smoking during pregnancy, positive feelings<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards motherhood <strong>and</strong> types of discipline used) whereas they were insignificant for the<br />

cognitive measure. Feelings about motherhood are extremely strongly correlated with <strong>child</strong><br />

behaviour. Even though the measure of mother’s feelings is assessed at just 8 months <strong>and</strong> the<br />

behaviour measured at around 50-60 months, it is still possible there is some reverse causality<br />

here. Children with persistent behaviour problems may affect mothers’ attitudes at 8 months<br />

<strong>and</strong> still be reflected in behaviour at 60 months <strong>and</strong> as both are mother reported they could both<br />

reflect a mother’s specific feelings <strong>to</strong>ward their <strong>child</strong>ren. Even so the lagging of mother’s<br />

feelings may reflect a vicious cycle of poor <strong>child</strong> behaviour <strong>and</strong> negative feelings by the<br />

mother. The same is perhaps true of forms of discipline used. No one disciplining device st<strong>and</strong>s<br />

out as having adverse effects but rather the use of a lot of different techniques would appear <strong>to</strong><br />

be damaging. This could simply reflect poor behaviour leading <strong>to</strong> more techniques being used<br />

but diverse or inconsistent disciplining may be less effective. More place-specific outings<br />

scores are significant for behaviour <strong>and</strong> housing conditions also.<br />

44


Table 3.4: Behaviour – coefficients on parenting variables when included individually in<br />

basic specification (N = 9416)<br />

Adj R squared 0.0872 0.0922 0.0688 0.1678<br />

Maternal reading score: base = high<br />

Very low 4.26 *** - - -<br />

Low 3.15 *** - - -<br />

Medium 1.22 *** - - -<br />

Number of times partner highly involved in reading (intact families only): base = 2<br />

0 1.15 *** - - -<br />

1 0.51 - - -<br />

3 -0.76 ** - - -<br />

4 -1.75 *** - - -<br />

Maternal teaching score: base = high<br />

Very low - 2.64 *** - -<br />

Low - 2.02 *** - -<br />

Medium - 0.99 *** - -<br />

Talking score: base = high<br />

Low - 4.81 *** - -<br />

Medium - 2.25 *** - -<br />

18 month TV score: base = 0-2<br />

3-5 - - 0.95 *** -<br />

6 - - 1.90 *** -<br />

38 month TV score: base = < 10 hrs pwk<br />

10+ hrs pwk - - 1.45 *** -<br />

Breastfed baby: base = never<br />

0-6 months -0.46 *<br />

6 months + -0.58 **<br />

Smoked in pregnancy: base = no<br />

Yes - - - 1.01 ***<br />

Maternal bonding score: base = highest quintile<br />

Lowest quintile - - - 6.41 ***<br />

2 nd quintile - - - 3.99 ***<br />

3 rd quintile - - - 3.01 ***<br />

4 th quintile - - - 2.01 ***<br />

Types of discipline (used at least once a week)<br />

Ignores <strong>child</strong> when naughty - - - 1.73 ***<br />

Smacks <strong>child</strong> when naughty - - - 1.64 ***<br />

Shouts at <strong>child</strong> when naughty - - - 1.87 ***<br />

Sends <strong>child</strong> <strong>to</strong> room when naughty - - - 1.37 ***<br />

Takes away treats when <strong>child</strong> naughty - - - 0.54 **<br />

Tells off <strong>child</strong> when naughty - - - 1.52 ***<br />

Bribes <strong>child</strong> when naughty - - - 1.81 ***<br />

45


Table 3.5: Behaviour – coefficients on parenting variables when included individually in<br />

basic specification (N = 9416)<br />

Adj R Squared 0.0769 0.0776 0.0671<br />

Place –specific outings scores: base = low (for each score)<br />

Local shops<br />

Insig<br />

Dpt s<strong>to</strong>re -0.46 ** - -<br />

S<strong>up</strong>ermarket -0.46 * - -<br />

Park -0.97 *** - -<br />

Friends -0.88 *** - -<br />

Library -0.79 *** - -<br />

Places interest -1.29 *** - -<br />

Places entertainment -0.44 * - -<br />

Household owns a car:<br />

base = yes<br />

Insig<br />

Book score: base = high<br />

Very low - 2.46 *** -<br />

Low - 1.91 *** -<br />

Medium - 0.80 *** -<br />

Toy score: base = high<br />

Very low - 3.73 *** -<br />

Low - 2.44 *** -<br />

Medium - 1.41 *** -<br />

Crowding index (people per room): base = < 0.5<br />

0.5 <strong>to</strong> < 0.75 - - -0.21<br />

0.75 <strong>to</strong> < 1.00 - - 0.08<br />

1.00+ - - 0.55<br />

Damp/condensation/mould: base = none<br />

Minor problem - - 1.23 ***<br />

Serious Problem - - 2.25 ***<br />

Notes<br />

1. All regressions include controls for the number of older siblings, number of younger siblings by 42 months, gender, birth weight, special<br />

care unit at birth, ethnicity, mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth <strong>and</strong> highest educational attainment, lone parent status at 47 months, the partner’s highest<br />

educational attainment <strong>and</strong> employment status at 47 months.<br />

2. Each measure of cognitive <strong>development</strong> is normalised <strong>to</strong> mean 100, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10.<br />

Table 3.6 controls for all the measures simultaneously. The impact of the teaching <strong>and</strong> reading<br />

scores <strong>and</strong> even TV watching are rather modest for behavioural measures of parenting<br />

behaviours. But the maternal bonding score <strong>and</strong> the large range of discipline measures are<br />

extremely powerful indica<strong>to</strong>rs of behaviour problems. Mother’s talking <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren during<br />

housework is also a robust predic<strong>to</strong>r. Among the indica<strong>to</strong>rs of outings <strong>and</strong> the home<br />

environment the <strong>to</strong>y score is most important but also outings <strong>and</strong> book scores <strong>and</strong> damp,<br />

condensation or mould also seems <strong>to</strong> have an influence on <strong>child</strong>’s behaviour scores.<br />

46


Table 3.6: Behaviour – coefficients on parenting variables when included simultaneously<br />

in basic specification (N = 9416)<br />

Adj R2 0.1995 0.1992<br />

Maternal reading score: base = high<br />

Very low<br />

Low<br />

Medium<br />

1.47<br />

1.24<br />

0.08<br />

***<br />

***<br />

1.56<br />

1.29<br />

0.11<br />

***<br />

***<br />

Number of times partner highly involved in reading (intact families only): base = 2<br />

0<br />

1<br />

3<br />

4<br />

0.80<br />

0.30<br />

-0.40<br />

-0.60<br />

** 0.82<br />

0.30<br />

-0.41<br />

-0.63<br />

Maternal teaching score: base = high<br />

Very low<br />

Low<br />

Medium<br />

Talking score: base = high<br />

Low<br />

Medium<br />

18 month TV score: base = 0-2<br />

3-5<br />

6<br />

1.03<br />

0.80<br />

0.32<br />

2.24<br />

0.92<br />

**<br />

**<br />

***<br />

***<br />

0.31<br />

0.71 **<br />

1.03<br />

0.80<br />

0.32<br />

2.26<br />

0.91<br />

**<br />

**<br />

**<br />

***<br />

***<br />

38 month TV score: base = < 10 hrs pwk<br />

10+ hrs pwk 0.73 *** 0.83 ***<br />

Breastfed baby: base = never<br />

0- 6 months<br />

6 months +<br />

-0.35<br />

-0.26<br />

Smoked in pregnancy: base = no<br />

Yes 0.94 *** 0.97 ***<br />

Maternal bonding score: base = highest quintile<br />

Lowest quintile<br />

2 nd quintile<br />

3 rd quintile<br />

4 th quintile<br />

Types of discipline (used at least once a week)<br />

Ignores <strong>child</strong> when naughty<br />

Smacks <strong>child</strong> when naughty<br />

Shouts at <strong>child</strong> when naughty<br />

Sends <strong>child</strong> <strong>to</strong> room when naughty<br />

Takes away treats when <strong>child</strong> naughty<br />

Tells off <strong>child</strong> when naughty<br />

Bribes <strong>child</strong> when naughty<br />

5.40<br />

3.38<br />

2.48<br />

1.68<br />

1.75<br />

1.31<br />

1.54<br />

1.46<br />

0.80<br />

1.45<br />

1.54<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

Place –specific outings scores: base = low (for each score)<br />

Dpt s<strong>to</strong>re<br />

S<strong>up</strong>ermarket<br />

Park<br />

Friends<br />

Library<br />

Places interest<br />

Places entertainment<br />

-0.30<br />

-0.26<br />

-0.48<br />

-0.53<br />

-0.21<br />

-0.71<br />

-0.46<br />

**<br />

**<br />

***<br />

*<br />

Book score: base = high<br />

Very low<br />

Low<br />

Medium<br />

Toy score: base = high<br />

Very low<br />

Low<br />

Medium<br />

Crowding index (people per room): base = < 0.5<br />

0.5 <strong>to</strong> < 0.75<br />

0.75 <strong>to</strong> < 1.00<br />

1.00+<br />

Damp/condensation/mould: base =<br />

none<br />

Minor problem<br />

Serious Problem<br />

0.34<br />

0.79<br />

0.27<br />

1.65<br />

1.00<br />

0.60<br />

-0.36<br />

-0.26<br />

0.04<br />

0.63<br />

1.21<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

**<br />

***<br />

***<br />

5.40<br />

3.39<br />

2.48<br />

1.69<br />

1.75<br />

1.34<br />

1.55<br />

1.46<br />

0.79<br />

1.46<br />

1.55<br />

-<br />

-<br />

-0.55<br />

-0.52<br />

-<br />

-0.80<br />

-0.49<br />

-<br />

-<br />

-<br />

0.44<br />

0.84<br />

0.29<br />

1.69<br />

1.01<br />

0.60<br />

0.65<br />

1.27<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

**<br />

**<br />

***<br />

**<br />

***<br />

***<br />

***<br />

**<br />

***<br />

***<br />

Notes<br />

1. All regressions include controls for the number of older siblings, number of younger siblings by 42 months, gender, birth<br />

weight, special care unit at birth, ethnicity, mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth <strong>and</strong> highest educational attainment, lone parent status at 47<br />

months, the partner’s highest educational attainment <strong>and</strong> employment status at 47 months.<br />

2. Each measure of cognitive <strong>development</strong> is normalised <strong>to</strong> mean 100, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10.<br />

47


Among the outing types, trips <strong>to</strong> places of interest, places of entertainment, friends <strong>and</strong> the<br />

park are all associated with better behaviour but the library or shops are uncorrelated. Causal<br />

interpretation remains problematic, especially for the discipline measure, as these effects could<br />

reflect family traits rather than the activity per se. However, these parenting <strong>and</strong> home<br />

environment measures are making a huge difference <strong>to</strong> the amount of the variation in<br />

behaviour explained by the model.<br />

3.3.4 Are Better Educated Parents More Effective?<br />

So far we have examined what are the more beneficial parenting activities, in terms of whether<br />

these different behaviours influence a <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>. It is our intention <strong>to</strong> explore<br />

whether these measures of parenting behaviour substantially explain the implied role of<br />

parental education or family income. However, it may also be the case that better educated<br />

parents are doing not just more in terms of outings, books, etc., but also that they are doing it<br />

better by using more educationally orientated activities or because they are more skilled at<br />

parenting. To test this important hypothesis we explore interactions between the key parenting<br />

behaviours <strong>and</strong> parental education. To simplify the number of potential interactions we use a<br />

single parental education interaction term, where the categories are mother’s education if the<br />

question relates <strong>to</strong> the mother only, <strong>and</strong> likewise father only, but a combined parental<br />

education gro<strong>up</strong>ing if the question refers <strong>to</strong> the family more generally. The detailed results of<br />

these interactions are explored in Appendix Tables A3.1 <strong>to</strong> A3.12 but the summary of these<br />

tests is presented in Table 3.7. Each coefficient reported is that for a (F-) test that the<br />

interaction terms between the variable of focus (mothers reading etc.) <strong>and</strong> each of parental<br />

education categories can be accepted as jointly significant. A value of less than 0.1 would<br />

suggest that there is significant variation in the impact of the parenting behaviour variable<br />

across parental education categories. The table suggests that in general there is a high<br />

probability that these interactions are insignificant <strong>and</strong> that the impact of greater pro-learning<br />

behaviour is the same across all parental education gro<strong>up</strong>s.<br />

48


Table 3.7: Summary of F-test for significance of interaction terms (parenting variables *<br />

parental education)<br />

Parenting measure Prob effect of parenting does not vary with<br />

educational gro<strong>up</strong> – joint test of no<br />

variation within all parenting categories 1<br />

Entry Assessment Behaviour<br />

Mother’s reading score 0.5625 0.6341<br />

Partner’s reading score 0.6554 0.5276<br />

Teaching score 0.9124 0.2824<br />

Talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied score 0.5337 0.9422<br />

18 month TV score 0.8567 0.2158<br />

38 month TV score 0.4961 0.5203<br />

Breastfeeding score 0.0295 0.2553<br />

Maternal bonding score 0.8238 0.6620<br />

Mother ignores <strong>child</strong> when naughty 0.6707 0.3192<br />

Mother smacks <strong>child</strong> when naughty 0.5441 0.3194<br />

Mother shouts at <strong>child</strong> when<br />

naughty<br />

0.3705 0.0398<br />

Mother sends <strong>child</strong> <strong>to</strong> room when<br />

naughty<br />

0.4928 0.3209<br />

Mother takes away treats when<br />

<strong>child</strong> is naughty<br />

0.0782 0.5322<br />

Mother tells off <strong>child</strong> when naughty 0.4978 0.0870<br />

Mother bribes <strong>child</strong> when naughty 0.6848 0.0731<br />

Department s<strong>to</strong>res outings score 0.7365 0.4622<br />

Park/playground outings score 0.4592 0.2441<br />

Visits <strong>to</strong> friends/relatives outings<br />

score<br />

0.0745 0.4605<br />

Library outings score 0.0249 0.0228<br />

Places of interest outings score 0.0458 0.4553<br />

Places of entertainment outings<br />

score<br />

0.2594 0.1456<br />

Book score 0.3579 0.6631<br />

Toy score 0.8504 0.9095<br />

Damp/mould/condensation score 0.0802 0.6707<br />

Notes<br />

1. i.e. the probability that the interaction terms can be dropped <strong>and</strong> the original specification of levels only<br />

retained.<br />

2. Each regression includes basic demographic controls.<br />

This needs clear interpretation; parental education remains an important influence after<br />

conditioning on parental behaviours. This residual component may reflect the, as yet, omitted<br />

transmission mechanisms in our study, others not available <strong>to</strong> us <strong>to</strong> explore (e.g. genetic<br />

influences) or a generic capability for the better educated <strong>to</strong> undertake all the discussed<br />

activities more effectively. However, the result is important because it suggests that better<br />

educated parents may read <strong>and</strong> teach their <strong>child</strong>ren more intensively but they do not do it more<br />

49


effectively for any specific elements among our measured parental inputs. The exceptions <strong>to</strong><br />

this general rule occur mainly for outings for the cognitive measures <strong>and</strong> the disciplining<br />

variables for behaviour. These do appear <strong>to</strong> have a variable impact across education gro<strong>up</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

closer investigation suggests that low education parents that undertake more outings have a<br />

beneficial effect on their <strong>child</strong>’s <strong>development</strong>.<br />

In summary, reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> teaching <strong>child</strong>ren are very important <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive<br />

outcomes at <strong>age</strong> 4/5. The home environment (<strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong> books) <strong>and</strong> outings are also substantial<br />

predic<strong>to</strong>rs of attainment. For behaviour mothers bonding with the <strong>child</strong> <strong>and</strong> discipline<br />

strategies are also important, as are <strong>to</strong>y scores <strong>and</strong> outings. Further there is no evidence that<br />

more highly educated parents teach <strong>and</strong> read <strong>to</strong> their <strong>child</strong>ren more effectively than the less<br />

well educated. Teaching <strong>and</strong> reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren, trips out <strong>and</strong> books in the home appear <strong>to</strong> be<br />

equally important <strong>to</strong> a <strong>child</strong>’s pre-school <strong>development</strong>, irrespective of the educational<br />

attainment of the family. This s<strong>up</strong>ports the notion that it is the behaviours captured by these<br />

variables that stimulate <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>, <strong>and</strong> not that the variables simply act as proxies<br />

for other parental characteristics. The implication is that if a family’s reading could be raised<br />

then it would make a direct, positive contribution <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive <strong>development</strong><br />

regardless of parental <strong>background</strong>. Note though that parental education is still an important<br />

direct predic<strong>to</strong>r of <strong>child</strong> attainment suggesting there are still important unexplained routes by<br />

which attainment is raised by better-educated parents.<br />

50


4. Maternal Employment<br />

This section looks at whether mother’s return <strong>to</strong> work timing decision makes any difference <strong>to</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive <strong>and</strong> behavioural <strong>development</strong>. This <strong>to</strong>pic has been the subject of more<br />

substantive analysis (Gregg et al. 2005). Here we aim <strong>to</strong> broadly replicate this research within<br />

the structure of the present report.<br />

4.1 Existing Evidence<br />

The motivation for looking at maternal employment stems from an awareness that patterns of<br />

maternal employment have changed dramatically over the last twenty years or so (see Gregg et<br />

al. 2003) <strong>and</strong> the mainly US evidence suggesting that early maternal employment can hinder<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>. Virtually all of the recent evidence on the effects of early maternal<br />

employment on <strong>child</strong> outcomes is US-based <strong>and</strong> most studies use data from a single source –<br />

the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY). Despite the use of this common dataset,<br />

these studies frequently provide conflicting results as <strong>to</strong> the nature of the impact of mothers’<br />

employment. It is clear that their conclusions are sensitive <strong>to</strong> methodological differences with<br />

regard <strong>to</strong> sample selection, the measures of maternal employment <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> used<br />

<strong>and</strong> the choice of control variables. Comprehensive surveys of this literature exist elsewhere<br />

(see, for example, Waldfogel et. al., 2002, <strong>and</strong> Ruhm, in press). However, because this paper<br />

explores a number of different hypotheses surrounding maternal employment <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

<strong>development</strong>, it is instructive <strong>to</strong> review the evidence on each issue separately. Although much<br />

work was carried out on this <strong>to</strong>pic in the early 1990s, we concentrate here on more recent<br />

studies 3 . These tend <strong>to</strong> address at least partially some of the methodological deficiencies that<br />

have been highlighted in the earlier papers (see Harvey, 1999, <strong>and</strong> Ruhm, 2004, on this issue).<br />

4.1. Is early maternal employment harmful <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong> does its effect vary<br />

with the timing <strong>and</strong> intensity of work?<br />

Although it is by no means universal, a substantial number of the recent US studies report the<br />

result that maternal employment in the first year after the birth has small negative effects on<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive outcomes (relative <strong>to</strong> not working at all in the first three years).<br />

3 For examples of this first wave of studies, see Desai et. al. (1989), Bayder <strong>and</strong> Brooks-Gunn (1991), Belsky <strong>and</strong><br />

Eggebeen (1991), V<strong>and</strong>ell <strong>and</strong> Ramanan (1992), Parcel <strong>and</strong> Meneghan (1994), <strong>and</strong> Greenstein (1995).<br />

52


However, this may be at least partially offset by positive effects of working in the second <strong>and</strong><br />

third years of the <strong>child</strong>’s life 4 . Many studies suggest that these effects are larger for full time<br />

working than part-time or that higher intensity of working in the first year is more damaging.<br />

In general, behavioural problems in <strong>child</strong>ren seem <strong>to</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> maternal employment in the<br />

same directions as cognitive outcomes although here the relation is weak <strong>and</strong> estimates tend <strong>to</strong><br />

be insignificant.<br />

Using five measures of cognitive <strong>development</strong> 5 as the dependent variables in OLS regressions,<br />

Waldfogel et. al. (2002) find significantly poorer outcomes on all five measures for the<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren of mothers who worked at all in the first year of life. The effects of employment in<br />

years 2 <strong>and</strong> 3 are positive when compared <strong>to</strong> not returning <strong>to</strong> work by year 3, although<br />

generally smaller in absolute magnitude <strong>and</strong> less well determined than the early negative<br />

effects. They test whether working in all three years has more harmful effects than employment<br />

in years 2 <strong>and</strong> 3 alone <strong>and</strong> find that delaying employment until after the first year is associated<br />

with significant improvements in the <strong>child</strong>’s performance over working all 3 years. The net<br />

effect of working in all three years compared <strong>to</strong> not returning by <strong>age</strong> 3 was insignificantly<br />

negative. With regard <strong>to</strong> intensity of employment, they find that the negative effects of first<br />

year employment are driven by the effect of full time working (more than 20 hours per week).<br />

The coefficients on first year part time work, whilst generally negative, are small <strong>and</strong><br />

insignificant.<br />

Using a smaller sample but a very similar methodology, Han et. al. (2001) again find negative<br />

effects of first year employment <strong>and</strong> weaker positive effects of later work for white <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

only. In this case, they find no significant differences between part time <strong>and</strong> full time working<br />

in the first year. They also test whether the negative impact of work in the first year is greater<br />

for mothers who returned <strong>to</strong> work more quickly after the birth. They find that the negative<br />

effects of return before the <strong>child</strong> is 10 months old are stronger than for return in the last quarter<br />

of the first year.<br />

Rather than using regression analysis like the other work on the NLSY, Hill et. al. (2001) use<br />

the technique of propensity matching in order <strong>to</strong> identify the effects of early maternal<br />

4 See Section 5.1 for a discussion of the magnitudes of these estimates.<br />

5 The measures of cognitive <strong>development</strong> available in the NLSY are the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-R<br />

(PPVT-R) at <strong>age</strong> 3 or 4 <strong>and</strong> the Peabody Individual Achievement Tests on maths <strong>and</strong> reading recognition (PIAT-<br />

M <strong>and</strong> PIAT-R respectively) at <strong>age</strong> 5 or 6. Some studies also analyse the PIAT scores for <strong>child</strong>ren at later <strong>age</strong>s,<br />

most commonly for 7 or 8 year olds.<br />

53


employment. This technique is discussed further below but we note here that Hill et. al. find<br />

that <strong>child</strong>ren of mothers who worked part-time in the first year would have experienced<br />

adverse effects if their mothers had instead worked full time. They also find that <strong>child</strong>ren of<br />

mothers who worked full time apparently could have benefited if their mothers had not worked<br />

in the first year. The effects of second <strong>and</strong> third year employment on performance tend <strong>to</strong> be<br />

positive but insignificant.<br />

Ruhm (in press) again analyses NLSY data for <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>age</strong>d 3 or 4 <strong>and</strong> 5 or 6. His inclusion in<br />

the regressions of a far more extensive set of <strong>background</strong> controls, however, leads <strong>to</strong> somewhat<br />

different results. He again finds a negative effect of the amount of employment in the first year<br />

but it is only significant for 3 <strong>to</strong> 4 year olds. In contrast with other work, he also finds<br />

significantly negative effects for second <strong>and</strong> third year employment. Ruhm also investigates<br />

whether the timing of return <strong>to</strong> work matters. He finds no significant difference between return<br />

prior <strong>to</strong> 6 months <strong>and</strong> return between 7 <strong>and</strong> 12 months, although his results suggest that<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren gain if mothers delay their return until after the first year. In general, Ruhm’s<br />

specifications impose a linear relationship on the number of hours worked <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> outcomes.<br />

He notes, however, that when he explored potential non-linearities he found some evidence the<br />

employment exceeding 20 hours per week had particularly negative effects.<br />

Harvey (1999) provides a further challenge <strong>to</strong> the finding of damaging effects of early<br />

employment offset by later positive effects in the NLSY. Harvey explores the effects of a<br />

variety of measures of maternal employment such as <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> in weeks at return <strong>and</strong><br />

dummies for any employment in the first year or in the first three years. Relative <strong>to</strong> not<br />

working in the first three years, she finds no evidence of any adverse of effects of early<br />

employment. However, amongst women who do work in the first three years, Harvey finds that<br />

working more hours is associated with a small but significant decrease in test scores.<br />

Turning <strong>to</strong> studies based on data other than the NLSY, Brooks-Gunn et. al. (2002) analyse data<br />

from the richly detailed NICHD Study of Early Childcare. Unlike the NLSY, data are only<br />

available on the cognitive outcomes of infants (<strong>age</strong>d 15, 24 <strong>and</strong> 36 months). Brooks-Gunn et.<br />

al. find that the <strong>child</strong>ren of mothers who are employed by the ninth month score significantly<br />

lower on the assessment at <strong>age</strong> 3, compared <strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong>ren of mothers who do not return<br />

before that date. When they explore whether this effect varies with the intensity of<br />

employment, they find that the adverse effect of full time work in the first nine months is<br />

54


double the effect of part time work. However, they find no evidence of negative effects on<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s scores at younger <strong>age</strong>s.<br />

Gregg et al. (2003) use the ALSPAC data being used here <strong>and</strong> suggest that full-time<br />

employment in the first 18 months after birth has small adverse effects on attainment measured<br />

by tests at <strong>age</strong> 7 but were unable <strong>to</strong> detect these effects in the EA data being used in this study<br />

or in the behaviour measure. The authors suggest that these adverse effects are restricted <strong>to</strong><br />

where informal care by friends <strong>and</strong> relatives is used rather than <strong>child</strong>minders or nursery<br />

centres. Part-time working is never found <strong>to</strong> have adverse consequences. Such modest effects<br />

restricted <strong>to</strong> a small minority of mothers are also found by Joshi <strong>and</strong> Verropoulu (2000). They<br />

analysed data from the British NCDS <strong>and</strong> BCS70 they suggest that first year maternal<br />

employment may lead <strong>to</strong> slightly poorer achievement in reading, but not maths, amongst<br />

school <strong>age</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> that employment between years 1 <strong>and</strong> 4 is associated with a small<br />

improvement in emotional well being. Ermisch <strong>and</strong> Francesconi (2000) use retrospective<br />

information from the British Household Panel Survey <strong>and</strong>, as a result, the measures of<br />

employment <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> are a good deal cruder than those employed in the other<br />

studies. Ermisch <strong>and</strong> Francesconi analyse the effect of maternal employment in the first five<br />

years of life on the probability of achieving at least an A-level as a young adult. They find that<br />

increasing the amount of full time maternal employment before <strong>age</strong> 5 by one year reduces the<br />

probability of achieving at least an A-level by 7 <strong>to</strong> 9 percent<strong>age</strong> points. A similar increase in<br />

part time work lowers the probability but by a smaller amount – 3 <strong>to</strong> 6 percent<strong>age</strong> points.<br />

These results rely on their use of a sibling difference estima<strong>to</strong>r, as their cross-sectional logit<br />

estimates do not identify any significant effects of early maternal employment on later<br />

achievement.<br />

4.2 Measures of maternal employment<br />

Our measure of early maternal employment is based on the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> in months when the<br />

mother returned <strong>to</strong> work. Our analysis focuses on maternal employment prior <strong>to</strong> 34 months<br />

because our data on return times is censored at this point <strong>and</strong> also because this cut-off<br />

corresponds well <strong>to</strong> the three-year break used in many previous studies. However, we also have<br />

an observation of whether the mother is working or not at 47 months <strong>and</strong> hence can construct a<br />

gro<strong>up</strong> for those reported as not returning before 34 months but observed in work at 47 months.<br />

We divide the 0 <strong>to</strong> 34 month period in<strong>to</strong> sub-periods <strong>and</strong> create dummies <strong>to</strong> indicate whether<br />

the mother returned <strong>to</strong> work in each one. Throughout most of our analysis we distinguish<br />

between return prior <strong>to</strong> 18 months <strong>and</strong> return in the 19 <strong>to</strong> 34 month period. Ideally, we would<br />

55


like <strong>to</strong> allow our results <strong>to</strong> vary with the number of hours that the mother worked in a typical<br />

week in the first three years if the <strong>child</strong>’s life. Unfortunately, data on the mother’s hours of<br />

work are only available at the discrete points of 21 <strong>and</strong> 33 months. We therefore use this<br />

information <strong>to</strong> assign mothers <strong>to</strong> either a part time category (less than 30 hours per week) of a<br />

full time category (30 or more hours per week), giving priority <strong>to</strong> the 21-month data.<br />

The proportions of mothers falling in<strong>to</strong> each employment category are shown in Table 4.1.<br />

Some 73 percent of mothers have returned <strong>to</strong> work by 47 months, approximately just prior <strong>to</strong><br />

school entry. The bulk of returns (55% of all mothers) are in the first 18 months with a small<br />

dribble of returners after this date, when virtually all returns are part-time. In the first 18<br />

months return is concentrated between 4 <strong>and</strong> 7 months when paid <strong>and</strong> then unpaid maternity<br />

leave periods, which applied at the time of the ALSPAC cohort, expire (see Burgess et al. 2002<br />

for a detailed analysis of mothers return dates). Virtually all full-time returns are in this<br />

window. However, there are substantial numbers of part-time returners between 7 <strong>and</strong> 18<br />

months. Very few mothers who had not returned <strong>to</strong> work by 34 months were observed in work<br />

at 47 months, just before school entry. The data therefore refers <strong>to</strong> the earliest return date <strong>and</strong><br />

doesn’t imply continuous working thereafter.<br />

Table 4.1 The distribution of return times<br />

Maternal employment % of <strong>to</strong>tal sample 1<br />

0 – 18 months PT 42.4<br />

Of which:<br />

0 – 6 months PT 24.2<br />

7 – 12 months PT 11.3<br />

13 – 18 months PT 6.9<br />

0 – 18 months FT 13.8<br />

Of which:<br />

0 – 6 months FT 10.5<br />

7 – 12 months FT 2.4<br />

13 – 18 months FT 0.9<br />

19 – 34 months 10.5<br />

Of which:<br />

19 – 34 months PT 9.5<br />

19 – 34 months FT 1.0<br />

35 – 47 months 5.9<br />

Not by 47 months 27.5<br />

Total 100.0<br />

Notes:<br />

1.Sample with at least one outcome measure (Entry Assessment or Behaviour), excluding observations where maternal employment data is<br />

missing: N = 7881.<br />

In what follows we distinguish returners pre-18 months according <strong>to</strong> full <strong>and</strong> part-time status,<br />

<strong>and</strong> returned 19-34 months <strong>and</strong> 35-47. We do not distinguish between part <strong>and</strong> full time work<br />

56


for mothers who returned between 19 months or later because so few of these mothers fall in<strong>to</strong><br />

the full time category (only 1.9 per cent of the <strong>to</strong>tal sample). For some regressions this leads <strong>to</strong><br />

cell sizes that are simply <strong>to</strong>o small <strong>to</strong> be of analytical value.<br />

4.3 Results<br />

4.3.1 Children’s cognitive <strong>development</strong><br />

Column 1, Table 4.2 shows that, without conditioning on mother’s education, <strong>age</strong> etc, <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

of mothers who return full-time before 18 months are doing better on aver<strong>age</strong> than other<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren. However, in column 2 we condition on mothers <strong>age</strong>, mothers <strong>and</strong> partners education<br />

<strong>and</strong> other basic controls <strong>and</strong> this result disappears. There is no evidence that <strong>child</strong>ren of women<br />

who return <strong>to</strong> work in the first 18 months are doing less well than others on the EA measure.<br />

Column 3 confirms that there are no sub-periods, especially early return periods, which are<br />

associated with negative effects in our <strong>age</strong> 4/5 measure of educational <strong>development</strong>. Note<br />

Gregg et al. (2003) find later measures of cognitive ability also tend <strong>to</strong> be significantly<br />

negative with a specification similar <strong>to</strong> that reported in column 2, but they find that this adverse<br />

outcome is limited <strong>to</strong> those working full-time <strong>and</strong> use unpaid informal care by friends or<br />

relatives but this was not also true for the EA measure at 4/5. We explore <strong>child</strong>care later.<br />

We next move <strong>to</strong> looking at whether there is any variation in the size of the coefficients on<br />

mothers return date across mothers’ education <strong>and</strong> lone parent status. These interactions are<br />

reported in Table 4.3. The coefficients reported are then the impact of returning by the date<br />

indicated for each panel broken out by each education gro<strong>up</strong>. The comparison is then between<br />

returning by that date relative <strong>to</strong> not working before 47 months for each education gro<strong>up</strong>. The<br />

results suggest that the lone parent interaction terms are never individually statistically<br />

important nor do F-tests suggests any variations across education gro<strong>up</strong>s for each return date.<br />

In other words, there are no differences between lone parents <strong>and</strong> mothers in co<strong>up</strong>les on how<br />

their employment is related the <strong>child</strong>’s cognitive <strong>development</strong>.<br />

Table 4.2: The effects of early maternal employment on Entry Assessment scores<br />

57


(N = 5010)<br />

(1) (2) (3)<br />

Return date Coef. (s.e.) Coef. (s.e.) Coef. (s.e.)<br />

0 –18 months PT 0.558 -0.444 -<br />

(0.385) (0.367)<br />

0 – 6 months PT - - -0.568<br />

(0.409)<br />

7 – 12 months PT - - -0.252<br />

(0.502)<br />

13 – 18 months PT - - -0.323<br />

(0.598)<br />

0 –18 months FT 1.593*** -0.493 -<br />

(0.511) (0.499)<br />

0 – 6 months FT - - -0.367<br />

(0.542)<br />

7 – 12 months FT - - -0.329<br />

(0.951)<br />

13 – 18 months FT - - -2.458<br />

(1.540)<br />

19 – 34 months -0.091 0.081 0.078<br />

(0.556) (0.520) (0.520)<br />

After 34 but in work at 47<br />

months -0.598 -0.394 -0.398<br />

(0.729) (0.676) (0.676)<br />

Test of restrictions 5 : 0.6795<br />

Adj R2 0.0970 0.2363 0.2361<br />

Notes<br />

1. Regression (1) includes controls for the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> at assessment only.<br />

2. Regressions (2) <strong>and</strong> (3) include controls for the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> in months at assessment, cohort year, number of older siblings, number<br />

of younger siblings by 47 months, gender, birth weight, special care unit at birth, ethnicity, mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth <strong>and</strong> highest educational<br />

attainment, lone parent status at 47 months, the partner’s highest educational attainment <strong>and</strong> employment status at 47 months, log of<br />

weekly household income <strong>and</strong> an indica<strong>to</strong>r of pre-birth financial deprivation.<br />

3. Each measure of cognitive <strong>development</strong> is normalised <strong>to</strong> mean 100, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10.<br />

4. ***, **, <strong>and</strong> * indicate significance at the 1%, 5% <strong>and</strong> 10% levels respectively.<br />

5. The F-test of restrictions tests the hypothesis that the effects of part time work before 18 months do not vary with the date of return <strong>and</strong><br />

that simultaneously the effects of full time work do not vary with return date.<br />

For mother’s education there is some evidence the impact on <strong>child</strong>ren of the mother working<br />

before 34 months is more negative where the mother is better educated, with part-time working<br />

before 18 months for degree holders <strong>and</strong> again for 19-34 months showing individual<br />

coefficients that are statistically significant. This suggests that the mother’s foregone parenting<br />

input maybe of greater value for better educated mothers. However, the F-tests suggest that<br />

only for the part-time before 18 months are there significant variations in coefficients across<br />

education gro<strong>up</strong>s. Together with the parenting results, where we found better educated mothers<br />

were eng<strong>age</strong>d in more reading <strong>and</strong> teaching but where there was no evidence this was done<br />

more effectively, this might suggest reduced levels of parenting input where mothers are<br />

working before 18 months. There seems <strong>to</strong> be the reverse relationship for those returning<br />

between 34 <strong>and</strong> 47 months where lesser educated mothers have a large negative coefficient but<br />

the sample sizes here are very small <strong>and</strong> perhaps not <strong>to</strong>o much emphasis should be placed on<br />

this result.<br />

58


Table 4.3: Differential effects of early maternal employment by maternal education <strong>and</strong><br />

lone parent status: Entry Assessment (N = 5010)<br />

Maternal F- test 1 F- test 1<br />

Return Date Coef. (s.e.) P > F = Coef. (s.e.) P > F =<br />

0 – 18 months PT<br />

* CSE/none -0.711<br />

(0.813)<br />

* Vocational/ O-level -0.118 * Never lone parent -0.534<br />

(0.473) (0.391)<br />

0.0302<br />

* A-level 0.180 * Ever lone parent -0.658 0.9089<br />

(0.624) (1.055)<br />

* Degree -2.753***<br />

(0.889)<br />

0 – 18 months FT<br />

* CSE/none 0.961<br />

(1.618)<br />

* Vocational/ O-level -1.360* * Never lone parent -0.810<br />

(0.726) (0.543)<br />

0.2775<br />

* A-level 0.288 * Ever lone parent -0.032 0.6166<br />

(0.833) (1.502)<br />

* Degree -1.226<br />

(1.014)<br />

19 – 34 months<br />

* CSE/none 1.212<br />

(1.210)<br />

* Vocational/ O-level -0.121 * Never lone parent -0.059<br />

(0.676) (0.575)<br />

0.1099<br />

* A-level 1.521 * Ever lone parent 1.707 0.2670<br />

(1.106) (1.510)<br />

* Degree -2.650*<br />

(1.519)<br />

After 34 months but in<br />

work at 47 months<br />

* CSE/none -4.526***<br />

(1.534)<br />

* Vocational/ O-level 0.610 * Never lone parent -0.185<br />

(0.942) (0.725)<br />

0.0095<br />

* A-level 1.572 * Ever lone parent -2.175 0.3651<br />

(1.332) (2.089)<br />

* Degree -2.149<br />

(2.230)<br />

Simultaneous test of<br />

equality of 0.0075 0.6138<br />

interacted terms:<br />

Adj R2 0.2385 0.2361<br />

Notes<br />

1. The F-test of restrictions tests the hypothesis that the interaction terms for each employment gro<strong>up</strong> are equal, i.e. that there is no variation<br />

in the effect of each type of employment with the mothers’ characteristics.<br />

All regressions include controls for the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> in months at assessment, cohort year, number of older siblings, number of younger<br />

siblings by 47 months, gender, birth weight, special care unit at birth, ethnicity, mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth <strong>and</strong> highest educational attainment, lone<br />

parent status at 47 months, the partner’s highest educational attainment <strong>and</strong> employment status at 47 months, log of weekly household income<br />

<strong>and</strong> an indica<strong>to</strong>r of pre-birth financial deprivation<br />

59


Table 4.4: The effects of early maternal employment on Behaviour scores (N = 9416)<br />

(1) (2) (3)<br />

Return date Coef. (s.e.) Coef. (s.e.) Coef. (s.e.)<br />

0 –18 months PT -1.194*** -0.261 -<br />

(0.278) (0.279)<br />

0 – 6 months PT - - -0.302<br />

(0.316)<br />

7 – 12 months PT - - 0.008<br />

(0.400)<br />

13 – 18 months PT - - -0.545<br />

(0.479)<br />

0 –18 months FT -1.321*** 0.353 -<br />

(0.377) (0.390)<br />

0 – 6 months FT - - 0.300<br />

(0.425)<br />

7 – 12 months FT - - 0.390<br />

(0.769)<br />

13 – 18 months FT - - 0.994<br />

(1.244)<br />

19 – 34 months -0.569 -0.257 -0.258<br />

(0.415) (0.406) (0.406)<br />

34 – 47 months -0.134 -0.101 -0.101<br />

(0.513) (0.501) (0.501)<br />

Test of restrictions 4 : 0.8394<br />

Adj R2 0.0023 0.0617 0.0614<br />

Notes:<br />

1. Higher behavioural scores indicate more adverse outcomes.<br />

2. Regression (1) includes no additional controls.<br />

3. Regressions (2) <strong>and</strong> (3) include controls for number of older siblings, number of younger siblings by 47 months, gender, birth<br />

weight, special care unit at birth, ethnicity, mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth <strong>and</strong> highest educational attainment, lone parent status at 47<br />

months, the partner’s highest educational attainment <strong>and</strong> employment status at 47 months, log of weekly household income <strong>and</strong> an<br />

indica<strong>to</strong>r of pre-birth financial deprivation.<br />

4. The F-test of restrictions tests the hypothesis that the effects of part time work before 18 months do not vary with the date of return<br />

<strong>and</strong> that simultaneously the effects of full time work do not vary with return date.<br />

60


Table 4.5: Differential effects of early maternal employment by maternal education <strong>and</strong><br />

lone parent status: Behaviour (N = 9416)<br />

Maternal F- test 1 F- test 1<br />

Employment Coef. (s.e.) P > F = Coef. (s.e.) P > F =<br />

0 – 18 months PT<br />

* CSE/none -0.326<br />

(0.634)<br />

* Vocational/ O-level -0.265 * Never lone parent -0.317<br />

(0.380) (0.298)<br />

0.4295<br />

* A-level -0.697 * Ever lone parent -0.723 0.6192<br />

(0.500) (0.786)<br />

* Degree 0.626<br />

(0.655)<br />

0 – 18 months FT<br />

* CSE/none 0.513<br />

(1.243)<br />

* Vocational/ O-level 0.117 * Never lone parent 0.328<br />

(0.607) (0.421)<br />

0.8125<br />

* A-level 0.175 * Ever lone parent 0.353 0.9830<br />

(0.665) (1.111)<br />

* Degree 0.964<br />

(0.751)<br />

19 – 34 months<br />

* CSE/none -0.407<br />

(0.934)<br />

* Vocational/ O-level -0.131 * Never lone parent 0.264<br />

(0.558) (0.447)<br />

0.4365<br />

* A-level 0.341 * Ever lone parent -2.881** 0.0093<br />

(0.861) (1.139)<br />

* Degree -1.959<br />

(1.170)<br />

34 – 47 months<br />

* CSE/none 0.565<br />

(1.108)<br />

* Vocational/ O-level -0.109 * Never lone parent -0.216<br />

(0.711) (0.543)<br />

0.4997<br />

* A-level -0.926 * Ever lone parent -2.097 0.2328<br />

(1.006) (1.491)<br />

* Degree 1.903<br />

(1.736)<br />

Simultaneous test of<br />

equality of 0.6211 0.0968<br />

interacted terms:<br />

Adj R2 0.0613 0.0630<br />

Notes<br />

1. Higher behavioural scores indicate more adverse outcomes.<br />

2. All regressions include controls for the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> in months at assessment, cohort year, number of older siblings, number of<br />

younger siblings by 47 months, gender, birth weight, special care unit at birth, ethnicity, mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth <strong>and</strong> highest educational<br />

attainment, lone parent status at 47 months, the partner’s highest educational attainment <strong>and</strong> employment status at 47 months, log of<br />

weekly household income <strong>and</strong> an indica<strong>to</strong>r of pre-birth financial difficulties.<br />

61


4.3.2 Behavioural Outcomes<br />

Tables 4.4-4.5 repeat this analysis for behavioural outcomes. As for cognitive <strong>development</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>ren whose mothers return <strong>to</strong> work full-time have better than aver<strong>age</strong> scores (the measure<br />

is inverted relative <strong>to</strong> cognitive <strong>development</strong>) when no other information is conditioned on.<br />

However, this result disappears once parental education <strong>and</strong> other basic <strong>background</strong> controls<br />

are introduced in column 1, as mothers working full-time are better educated <strong>and</strong> older. Unlike<br />

for cognitive <strong>development</strong>, further conditioning on the mothers’ pre-birth employment <strong>and</strong><br />

financial situation <strong>and</strong> occ<strong>up</strong>ation make little further difference. Hence there is no evidence of<br />

any difference in behavioural outcomes according <strong>to</strong> the mothers return <strong>to</strong> work decision. This<br />

is also true across all mother’s education gro<strong>up</strong>s, as none of the interaction terms show any<br />

significant or consistent pattern.<br />

Summary<br />

Maternal employment in the early months <strong>and</strong> years after a birth is not found <strong>to</strong> be a predic<strong>to</strong>r<br />

of the EA test scores on arriving at school at <strong>age</strong> 4 <strong>to</strong> 5 once other <strong>background</strong> characteristics<br />

are conditioned. Nor is there any effect on behaviour scores. We thus do not pursue this line of<br />

study further. Early work contained in Gregg et al. (2003) suggests that there is evidence of<br />

adverse effects observable at <strong>age</strong> 7 in the KS1 <strong>and</strong> the Focus at 7 ALSPAC administered tests<br />

<strong>and</strong> this will be investigated in follow <strong>up</strong> research looking at later attainment.<br />

62


5. Childcare<br />

This section describes the pattern of <strong>child</strong>care us<strong>age</strong> amongst the ALSPAC sample, <strong>and</strong><br />

examines the impact of <strong>child</strong>care, controlling only for our st<strong>and</strong>ard set of controls, on cognitive<br />

<strong>and</strong> behavioural outcomes at <strong>age</strong>s 4 <strong>to</strong> 5. There are two sources of <strong>child</strong>care information<br />

available, the first is taken from mothers’ questionnaires at 8 weeks, 8 months, 15 months <strong>and</strong><br />

24 months. This asked the mother if a particular type of <strong>child</strong>care was used <strong>and</strong> for the number<br />

of hours it was used for. From this we construct measures of whether each type of care was<br />

used more that 20 hours a week at any date <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> just beyond the <strong>child</strong>’s second birthday<br />

(see Appendix 1 <strong>child</strong>care section for more details). The survey at 24 months falls just after<br />

the second birthday, <strong>and</strong> as mothers will answer the questions with a delay of 1 <strong>to</strong> 2 months,<br />

the data covers the two <strong>to</strong> three month period after the second birthday.<br />

As part of the Focus at 7 interviews the mother reported retrospectively the names <strong>and</strong> broad<br />

type of all centre based pre-school providers used. This is the second source of information on<br />

pre-school <strong>child</strong>care. Periods of use <strong>and</strong> number of ½ day sessions attended were also detailed.<br />

From this we can identify the type of the last pre-school attended prior <strong>to</strong> school entry <strong>and</strong> how<br />

many sessions attended. In some cases more than one was attended simultaneously in which<br />

case both are recorded. Hence the gro<strong>up</strong>s are not mutually exclusive. As the individual<br />

provider is identified <strong>and</strong> we have a large proportion of <strong>child</strong>ren within this <strong>age</strong> range in the<br />

Avon study, many <strong>child</strong>ren will attend the same provider. This allows us <strong>to</strong> observe members<br />

of the <strong>child</strong>’s peer gro<strong>up</strong> at the provider <strong>and</strong> this is explored in Section 7 on neighbourhoods<br />

<strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>s later.<br />

5.1 Patterns in the use of <strong>child</strong>care<br />

Table 5.1 presents <strong>child</strong>care use <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> just beyond the ALSPAC <strong>child</strong>’s second birthday.<br />

The categories are not mutually exclusive, as a mother may use several sources of <strong>child</strong>care.<br />

The question refers <strong>to</strong> any care, which replaces that of the mother, not just care for when the<br />

mother is at work.<br />

64


Table 5.1: Childcare us<strong>age</strong> <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Type<br />

Ages 8 weeks, 8,<br />

15 <strong>and</strong> 24 months<br />

All Mothers<br />

At 24 Months<br />

Mother<br />

working<br />

Mother<br />

working<br />

Full-Time<br />

All Mothers<br />

Partner<br />

5-19 hours per week 35.5 29.1 37.8 26.2 41.7<br />

20+ hours per week 40.4 13.5 18.2 25.7 15.5<br />

Total 75.9 42.6 56.0 51.9 57.3<br />

Friend/relative<br />

5-19 hours per week 26.2 17.3 23.1 14.4 26.1<br />

20+ hours per week 22.0 8.7 12.5 17.9 10.6<br />

Total 48.2 26.1 35.6 32.2 36.7<br />

Paid person<br />

5-19 hours per week 8.4 6.9 10.6 7.3 11.6<br />

20+ hours per week 13.5 9.0 16.1 35.3 9.4<br />

Total 21.9 15.9 26.7 42.6 21.0<br />

Centre care<br />

5-19 hours per week 5.3 4.6 5.6 5.6 5.6<br />

20+ hours per week 3.2 2.3 4.0 9.0 2.3<br />

Total 8.5 6.8 9.7 14.6 7.9<br />

Mother<br />

working<br />

Part-time<br />

Figures given are the percent<strong>age</strong> of the <strong>to</strong>tal working sample in each cell (i.e., the sample with at least one outcome measure – Entry<br />

Assessment or Behaviour – excluding observations where data on us<strong>age</strong> of that type of care is missing).<br />

The most common source of <strong>child</strong>care is the partner (around ¾ of the sample have partner’s<br />

s<strong>up</strong>plying care), followed by friends <strong>and</strong> relatives (around half the sample). Apart from very<br />

short hours around three quarters of this care is provided by gr<strong>and</strong>parents. Paid care is used by<br />

around 30% of the sample, either in the form of a person (<strong>child</strong> minder or nanny – 80% of this<br />

gro<strong>up</strong> have <strong>child</strong> minders for above 5 hours of care) or, less commonly, centre based (only<br />

8.5% of the sample). Note that this reflects answers at four <strong>age</strong>s <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> around 26 months <strong>and</strong> so<br />

the use of centre based care at any point in time is very low, especially for more than 20 hours<br />

per week. So pre-2, centre based care is very rarely used for long hours, probably because the<br />

cost is very high. The table also reports <strong>child</strong>care use from the 24 month survey broken down<br />

by employment status of the mother at 21 months. This gives a clearer picture of variations in<br />

us<strong>age</strong> for working mothers <strong>and</strong> across hours of work. It shows that partners do more when the<br />

mother works with higher involvement of at least 5 hours but where the mother works full-time<br />

partner’s involvement for over 20 hours is far more extensive. Increased use of<br />

<strong>child</strong>minders/nannies for 20+ hours (35% as opposed <strong>to</strong> just 9% on aver<strong>age</strong>) is also much more<br />

marked where the mother works full-time. Whilst long hours of care in centres or by friends<br />

<strong>and</strong> relatives also rise with full-time working the steps are much less marked.<br />

65


Post <strong>age</strong>-3 the use of centre-based care rises considerably with virtually all parents using some<br />

form of centre based care between the <strong>age</strong>s of 3 <strong>and</strong> 5. Table 5.2 shows the types of pre-school<br />

providers used most recently prior <strong>to</strong> entry in school. The categories used are reported by the<br />

mothers <strong>and</strong> are thus subject <strong>to</strong> some interpretation, but Nursery class is thought <strong>to</strong> refer <strong>to</strong> a<br />

class attached <strong>to</strong> a school, whilst the LEA nursery probably covers both day nurseries<br />

his<strong>to</strong>rically provided by social services <strong>and</strong> LEA provided nursery schools. We are unable <strong>to</strong><br />

distinguish between these two very different alternatives with any certainty. As well as private<br />

day nurseries there is an unspecified residual nursery category where the parent could not be<br />

more specific. The small “other” gro<strong>up</strong> covers, we think, special needs centres <strong>and</strong> any other<br />

circumstance where mother was unable <strong>to</strong> match the available categories.<br />

Nurseries are widely used but a sizeable proportion of mothers were unable <strong>to</strong> describe the type<br />

of nursery used (whether it was a dedicated public or private nursery). Over 45% of <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

were using a dedicated Nursery (including the unspecified gro<strong>up</strong>) <strong>and</strong> this rises <strong>to</strong> around 60%<br />

if Nursery classes within schools are added 6 . Playgro<strong>up</strong>s were also widely used with 63% using<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong>s at all but only 39% used Playgro<strong>up</strong>s without also using Nursery care as well (see<br />

Table 5.3).<br />

Nursery classes are mainly used for 3-5 sessions per week, especially school-based nurseries.<br />

The use made of playgro<strong>up</strong>s is more limited than that of nurseries. Very few families use more<br />

than 3-5 sessions per week at a playgro<strong>up</strong>, but of the mothers who use the alternative types of<br />

nurseries around 20% use over 5 sessions per week. Mothers also combine the use of different<br />

centre based care.<br />

6 This will have increased markedly more recently as the government now offers a free ½ day place for all three<br />

<strong>and</strong> four year olds.<br />

66


Table 5.2: Centre-based care prior <strong>to</strong> school entry<br />

% of sample using Number of sessions used per week, % of those using<br />

type (after type<br />

36 months) 1-2 sessions 3-5 sessions 6-10 sessions<br />

Nursery class 15.5 1.7 85.5 12.8<br />

LEA nursery 9.3 0.6 74.0 25.3<br />

Private day nursery 13.6 35.4 40.2 24.4<br />

Nursery (undefined) 25.1 20.6 62.9 16.5<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> 63.5 38.8 59.9 1.3<br />

Other 1.7 6.2 29.9 63.9<br />

Figures given are the percent<strong>age</strong> of the <strong>to</strong>tal working sample in each cell (i.e., the sample with at least one outcome measure – Entry<br />

Assessment or Behaviour – excluding observations where data on us<strong>age</strong> of that type of care is missing).<br />

Table 5.3: Combinations of nursery <strong>and</strong> playgro<strong>up</strong> care prior <strong>to</strong> school entry<br />

Type of nursery<br />

used<br />

Used alone<br />

Used in<br />

conjunction with<br />

playgro<strong>up</strong> only<br />

Total<br />

None 0.6 38.6 39.2<br />

Nursery class 8.8 5.2 14.0<br />

LEA nursery school 5.6 2.8 8.4<br />

Private day nursery 6.3 5.0 11.3<br />

Nursery (undefined) 12.1 10.6 22.7<br />

Other 0.8 0.4 1.2<br />

Total 34.3 62.6 96.9*<br />

Table sample is households that used a maximum of one type of nursery (excluding playgro<strong>up</strong>s) post-<strong>age</strong> 3. The<br />

remaining 3.1% of the sample used combinations of two or more types of nursery.<br />

5.2 Patterns of association of <strong>child</strong>care <strong>and</strong> cognitive <strong>and</strong> behavioural outcomes<br />

Column 1 of Table 5.4 examines the relationship between type of <strong>child</strong>care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong><br />

outcomes at <strong>age</strong> 5 with no controls for later <strong>child</strong>care use. Each type of care is split in<strong>to</strong> 2<br />

mutually exclusive categories <strong>and</strong> a test made of the equality of impact of less compared <strong>to</strong><br />

more use of each type of care. The regression analysis controls for our st<strong>and</strong>ard set of controls<br />

(see Table 2.2) <strong>and</strong> the magnitude of the coefficients are comparable within column (<strong>and</strong> with<br />

other sections) as the outcome measures are st<strong>and</strong>ardised <strong>to</strong> mean 100 <strong>and</strong> s.d. 10. Since we do<br />

not control for the correlates of the care patterns, in this preliminary analysis, we are essentially<br />

examining whether any direct or indirect relationship exists between the outcomes <strong>and</strong> the type<br />

of care.<br />

Column 1 indicates that EA scores are positively associated with care from a partner (provided<br />

it is for less than 20 hours a week) <strong>and</strong> use of centre care, particularly of a duration of more<br />

67


than 20 hours per week. Children attending centre care for over 20 hours per week before the<br />

<strong>age</strong> of 2 score, on aver<strong>age</strong>, 1.7 points better than their peers. However, the proportion of<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren in this gro<strong>up</strong> is very small, being just 3 percent of the sample. In contrast, having care<br />

from a friend or relative for over 20 hours per week has a significant negative impact on<br />

outcome. As we are not yet conditioning on other <strong>child</strong>care us<strong>age</strong> <strong>and</strong> other related fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

these are not necessarily causal relationships.<br />

Column 2 of Table 5.4 examines the impact of the last used pre-school provider on EA test<br />

scores, on <strong>to</strong>p of care pre <strong>age</strong> 2, allowing for a differential impact of the type of centre-based<br />

care. Additionally, column 3 differentiates between the number of sessions of each type of<br />

centre based care used each week. Note the use of partner care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong> the negative effect<br />

of unpaid friends or relatives remain but the centre based care result diminishes. Already there<br />

is no strong evidence that early use of centre based care is beneficial <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive<br />

<strong>development</strong>.<br />

Columns 2 <strong>and</strong> 3 indicate that not all types of centre care are associated with positive<br />

outcomes. Column 2 shows attendance at what mothers report as LEA nurseries has a negative<br />

effect on outcomes as does attendance (particularly long attendance) at the undefined other<br />

category of centre care. The LEA nursery effect may reflect that many <strong>child</strong>ren in these<br />

nurseries will be there at the recommendation of social services. The other category includes<br />

special referral units <strong>and</strong> the coefficients will reflect that. When we also explore the<br />

information contained in peer gro<strong>up</strong>s attending the same providers these possible explanations<br />

will be more clearly addressed. Attendance at nurseries (private or other), in contrast, is<br />

associated with positive outcomes. Column 3 shows that the negative impact of LEA nursery<br />

school attendance is greatest for those who attend only a few times, but the sample size here is<br />

small. Of the more commonly used forms of care there is little evidence that longer exposure<br />

within the week is better in terms of <strong>development</strong>. Only for private nurseries do outcomes rise<br />

mono<strong>to</strong>nically with number of sessions <strong>and</strong> even here a restriction <strong>to</strong> just a single term, used or<br />

not used, is not rejected by the data. This suggests that for nursery settings positive attainment<br />

outcomes are as pronounced for part-time as full-time attendance.<br />

68


Table 5.4: Controlling for centre-based <strong>child</strong> care us<strong>age</strong> post-<strong>age</strong> 3 (Entry Assessment)<br />

(1) (2) (3)<br />

Coef S.E. Coef S.E. Coef S.E. F-test 1<br />

Pre-<strong>age</strong> 2 <strong>child</strong>care us<strong>age</strong><br />

P>F =<br />

Partner<br />

5-19 hours per week 0.581* (0.346) 0.624* (0.345) 0.581* (0.346) -<br />

20+ hours per week -0.079 (0.345) 0.005 (0.344) -0.022 (0.345)<br />

Friend/relative<br />

5-19 hours per week -0.269 (0.317) -0.255 (0.316) -0.263 (0.316) -<br />

20+ hours per week -0.906*** (0.342) -0.897*** (0.340) -0.911*** (0.341)<br />

Paid person<br />

5-19 hours per week -0.244 (0.474) -0.297 (0.474) -0.225 (0.474) -<br />

20+ hours per week 0.320 (0.405) 0.258 (0.404) 0.287 (0.406)<br />

Centre care<br />

5-19 hours per week 0.380 (0.604) 0.100 (0.608) 0.122 (0.611) -<br />

20+ hours per week 1.655** (0.830) 1.320 (0.836) 1.425* (0.848)<br />

Post-<strong>age</strong> 3 centre-based care<br />

Nursery class - 0.775* (0.399) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - 0.671 (3.306)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - 0.795* (0.420) 0.8600<br />

6-10 sessions per week - 0.224 (1.049)<br />

LEA nursery - -1.106** (0.485) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - -11.709* (6.132)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - -0.952* (0.543) 0.1845<br />

6-10 sessions per week - -1.553* (0.906)<br />

Private day nursery - 1.400*** (0.430) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - 0.837 (0.597)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - 1.482** (0.616) 0.3791<br />

6-10 sessions per week - 2.140*** (0.826)<br />

Nursery (type undefined) - 1.130*** (0.358) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - 1.557*** (0.604)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - 1.350*** (0.428) 0.0188<br />

6-10 sessions per week - -0.891 (0.824)<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> - 0.377 (0.340) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - 0.221 (0.381)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - 0.430 (0.399) 0.6382<br />

6-10 sessions per week - 1.459 (1.604)<br />

Other - -3.529*** (1.109) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - -4.429 (6.135)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - -2.372 (2.080) 0.3903<br />

6-10 sessions per week - -5.897*** (1.576)<br />

Joint test of restrictions - - - 0.1586<br />

Adj R2 0.2445 0.2519 0.2531<br />

Notes<br />

1. The F-test of restrictions tests the null hypothesis that the coefficients on the number of sessions per week<br />

categories are equal, i.e. that the effect of a particular type of centre-based care does not depend on the<br />

intensity of its use.<br />

2. ***, **, <strong>and</strong> * indicate significance at the 1%, 5% <strong>and</strong> 10% levels respectively.<br />

69


Table 5.5: Controlling for centre-based <strong>child</strong> care us<strong>age</strong> prior <strong>to</strong> school entry (Behaviour)<br />

(1) (2) (3)<br />

Pre-<strong>age</strong> 2 <strong>child</strong>care us<strong>age</strong> Coef S.E. Coef S.E. Coef S.E.<br />

Partner<br />

5-19 hours per week -0.238 (0.277) -0.236 (0.277) -0.211 (0.278) -<br />

20+ hours per week -0.495* (0.275) -0.504* (0.275) -0.495* (0.275)<br />

Friend/relative<br />

5-19 hours per week 0.555** (0.257) 0.573** (0.257) 0.571** (0.257) -<br />

20+ hours per week 0.855*** (0.277) 0.881*** (0.277) 0.855*** (0.277)<br />

Paid person<br />

5-19 hours per week 0.339 (0.384) 0.331 (0.385) 0.334 (0.386) -<br />

20+ hours per week 0.243 (0.325) 0.276 (0.326) 0.343 (0.328)<br />

Centre care<br />

5-19 hours per week 1.669*** (0.470) 1.638*** (0.474) 1.715*** (0.475) -<br />

20+ hours per week 0.103 (0.601) 0.114 (0.606) 0.276 (0.614)<br />

Post-<strong>age</strong> 3 centre-based care<br />

F-test 1<br />

P>F =<br />

Nursery class - -0.020 (0.389) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - 3.559 (2.297)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - 0.026 (0.417) 0.1201<br />

6-10 sessions per week - -1.204 (0.931)<br />

LEA nursery - 1.062** (0.466) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - -7.531 (4.850)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - 0.707 (0.529) 0.0653<br />

6-10 sessions per week - 2.101** (0.840)<br />

Private day nursery - 0.171 (0.397) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - 1.181** (0.576)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - -0.163 (0.576) 0.0257<br />

6-10 sessions per week - -1.137 (0.737)<br />

Nursery (type undefined) - 0.335 (0.330) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - 0.054 (0.559)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - 0.283 (0.396) 0.7595<br />

6-10 sessions per week - 0.663 (0.682)<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> - 0.295 (0.308) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - 0.051 (0.354)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - 0.282 (0.365) 0.4377<br />

6-10 sessions per week - -1.146 (1.297)<br />

Other - 2.044** (0.988) -<br />

1-2 sessions per week - 15.045*** (5.594)<br />

3-5 sessions per week - 1.650 (2.041) 0.0771<br />

6-10 sessions per week - 2.645* (1.389)<br />

Joint test of restrictions - - - 0.0166<br />

Adj R2 0.0634 0.0634 0.0657<br />

Notes<br />

1. The F-test of restrictions tests the null hypothesis that the coefficients on the number of sessions per week categories are equal, i.e. that<br />

the effect of a particular type of centre-based care does not depend on the intensity of its use.<br />

2. All regressions include controls for the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> in months at assessment, cohort year, number of older siblings, number of<br />

younger siblings by 42 months, gender, birth weight, special care unit at birth, ethnicity, mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth <strong>and</strong> highest educational<br />

attainment, lone parent status at 47 months, the partner’s highest educational attainment <strong>and</strong> employment status at 47 months.<br />

3. Each measure of cognitive <strong>development</strong> is normalised <strong>to</strong> mean 100, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 10.<br />

4. ***, **, <strong>and</strong> * indicate significance at the 1%, 5% <strong>and</strong> 10% levels respectively.<br />

Table 5.5 repeats the same analysis for behavioural outcomes. Column 1 suggests that the<br />

association of care pre-<strong>age</strong> 2 with behavioural outcomes is somewhat different (note a negative<br />

score indicates a better outcome). The impact of care by partners is still beneficial <strong>and</strong> the<br />

effect of care by a friend or relative still on balance adverse. The effect of centre care is now<br />

70


that short hours of centre care are associated with worse outcomes. This is not the case for long<br />

hours but note the small sample that has long hours of centre based care. The explana<strong>to</strong>ry<br />

power of the behavioural regression is very low (in contrast <strong>to</strong> the regression for EA). All these<br />

results remain when later care arrangements are conditioned on. So there is a case that early<br />

centre based care is associated <strong>to</strong> poorer later behaviour outcomes.<br />

Column 2 shows that the negative effects of LEA nursery <strong>and</strong> other care on cognitive outcomes<br />

are also present for behavioural outcomes, <strong>and</strong> for LEA nurseries the negative effect is<br />

increasing in us<strong>age</strong>. On aver<strong>age</strong>, other forms of centre-based care have no effect on behaviour<br />

(column 2). In contrast with care at pre-2, it appears that private nurseries are associated with<br />

no harmful effects on behavioural outcomes <strong>and</strong> preliminary evidence of beneficial cognitive<br />

effects, whilst LEA nursery attendance is associated with negative outcomes on both measures.<br />

We will explore <strong>to</strong> what extent these reflect selection of client gro<strong>up</strong>s later.<br />

5.3 Do Patterns of association of <strong>child</strong>care <strong>and</strong> cognitive <strong>and</strong> behavioural outcomes vary<br />

by parental characteristics?<br />

We now examine whether parental education interacts with the type of <strong>child</strong>care used: in other<br />

words, do better-educated parents translate a given type of care in<strong>to</strong> better outcomes?<br />

There are a number of studies suggesting that high quality <strong>child</strong>care is especially beneficial <strong>to</strong><br />

those from deprived <strong>background</strong>s (see Ramey <strong>and</strong> Ramey, 1998, for example). Table 5.6<br />

examines this for cognitive outcomes <strong>and</strong> Table 5.7 for behavioural outcomes. Reading across<br />

rows in Table 5.6 shows no clear pattern as the educational gro<strong>up</strong> of the parent increases. This<br />

is confirmed from the F-tests: the impact of type of care on EA scores is the same across all<br />

categories of parental educational attainment. Table 5.7 shows a very similar pattern for<br />

behaviour. So parental education determines which kind of <strong>child</strong>care is chosen <strong>and</strong> this has an<br />

impact on <strong>child</strong> outcomes. But, conditional on a particular type of care being used <strong>and</strong> a<br />

general term reflecting parental education, better-educated parents do not get better outcomes<br />

through <strong>child</strong>care for their <strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

71


Table 5.6: Interactions of parental education with <strong>child</strong>care us<strong>age</strong> (Entry Assessment)<br />

Parental education 1<br />

CSE/none<br />

Vocational/<br />

O-level<br />

A-level Degree<br />

Partner<br />

5-19 hours per week 0.887 0.975* -0.254 0.188<br />

(0.752) (0.587) (0.629) (0.805)<br />

20+ hours per week -0.689 0.029 0.432 -0.249<br />

(0.713) (0.579) (0.629) (0.817)<br />

Friend/relative<br />

5-19 hours per week 0.381 -0.492 -0.450 1.287<br />

(0.802) (0.440) (0.594) (1.031)<br />

20+ hours per week -0.782 -1.172** -0.598 -0.815<br />

(0.839) (0.474) (0.632) (1.155)<br />

Paid person<br />

5-19 hours per week -3.306** 0.694 -0.623 -0.110<br />

(1.465) (0.791) (0.784) (1.088)<br />

20+ hours per week -1.514 1.136* 0.219 -0.723<br />

(1.691) (0.676) (0.668) (0.791)<br />

Centre care<br />

5-19 hours per week 2.311 -0.328 1.021 -1.894<br />

(2.212) (0.963) (1.058) (1.299)<br />

20+ hours per week 6.739 1.309 1.144 0.898<br />

(5.038) (1.371) (1.447) (1.526)<br />

Prob<br />

(row coefs equal)<br />

P > F =<br />

0.4615<br />

0.6710<br />

0.3503<br />

0.8951<br />

0.1088<br />

0.2205<br />

0.2313<br />

0.7417<br />

Joint test of<br />

restrictions<br />

P > F =<br />

0.1917<br />

0.6891<br />

0.1127<br />

0.4848<br />

Nursery class<br />

1.408* 1.141** 0.236 0.584<br />

(0.821) (0.538) (0.744) (1.044)<br />

0.6584<br />

-<br />

LEA nursery school<br />

-1.495 -1.650** -0.049 -1.306<br />

(1.205) (0.671) (0.845) (1.242)<br />

0.4627<br />

-<br />

Private day nursery<br />

1.052 2.091*** 0.412 0.862<br />

(1.402) (0.637) (0.712) (0.842)<br />

0.2895<br />

-<br />

Nursery (type<br />

undefined) 1.183 0.687 1.615*** 1.495*<br />

(0.843) (0.497) (0.610) (0.853)<br />

0.6146<br />

-<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong><br />

0.306 0.478 0.203 0.106<br />

(0.711) (0.451) (0.567) (0.757)<br />

0.9607<br />

-<br />

Other<br />

-5.117*** -4.455** -0.747 -5.745<br />

(1.949) (1.798) (2.638) (3.584)<br />

0.5432<br />

-<br />

Notes<br />

1. All types of <strong>child</strong>care are interacted with maternal educational attainment, with the exception of care by the partner pre-<strong>age</strong> 2 which is<br />

interacted with paternal education.<br />

2. The interaction terms for each type of care relate <strong>to</strong> separate regressions in which all other types of care (both pre-<strong>age</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong> post-<strong>age</strong> 3)<br />

are included as level controls, i.e. only one set of interaction terms is included per regression.<br />

3. The F-test of restrictions tests the null hypothesis that the coefficients on each of the educational categories are equal, i.e. that the effect<br />

of a particular type of centre-based care does not vary with parental education.<br />

72


Table 5.7: Interactions of parental education with <strong>child</strong>care us<strong>age</strong> (Behaviour)<br />

Parental education 1<br />

CSE/none<br />

Vocational/<br />

O-level<br />

A-level Degree<br />

Partner<br />

5-19 hours per week 0.321 -0.236 -0.636 -0.263<br />

(0.587) (0.502) (0.526) (0.603)<br />

20+ hours per week 0.334 -1.215** -0.418 -0.458<br />

(0.561) (0.489) (0.522) (0.598)<br />

Friend/relative<br />

5-19 hours per week 1.571** 0.671* 0.119 -0.485<br />

(0.635) (0.369) (0.495) (0.772)<br />

20+ hours per week 1.246* 0.896** 0.706 0.395<br />

(0.641) (0.398) (0.541) (0.850)<br />

Paid person<br />

5-19 hours per week -0.633 1.037 0.177 0.203<br />

(1.259) (0.666) (0.640) (0.812)<br />

20+ hours per week 1.216 -0.356 0.833 0.274<br />

(1.364) (0.593) (0.547) (0.588)<br />

Centre care<br />

5-19 hours per week 0.972 1.881** 1.659** 1.336<br />

(1.715) (0.779) (0.822) (0.995)<br />

20+ hours per week -1.993 -1.085 -0.253 1.598<br />

(2.526) (1.180) (1.082) (0.978)<br />

Prob<br />

(row coefs equal)<br />

P > F =<br />

0.6760<br />

0.2029<br />

0.1523<br />

0.8593<br />

0.6178<br />

0.4411<br />

0.9520<br />

0.2418<br />

Joint test of<br />

restrictions<br />

P > F =<br />

0.3155<br />

0.4938<br />

0.5659<br />

0.5905<br />

Nursery class<br />

0.222 -0.348 0.658 -0.559<br />

(0.842) (0.535) (0.709) (0.889)<br />

0.5912<br />

-<br />

LEA nursery school<br />

1.333 0.755 1.142 1.160<br />

(1.139) (0.650) (0.817) (1.201)<br />

0.9597<br />

-<br />

Private day nursery<br />

-1.132 0.187 0.255 0.459<br />

(1.442) (0.630) (0.642) (0.689)<br />

0.7938<br />

-<br />

Nursery (type<br />

undefined) -0.099 0.030 0.437 0.788<br />

(0.830) (0.471) (0.534) (0.635)<br />

0.7182<br />

-<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong><br />

0.151 0.559 -0.216 0.574<br />

(0.618) (0.395) (0.461) (0.561)<br />

0.4485<br />

-<br />

Other<br />

1.503 2.293 4.063* -2.240<br />

(1.871) (1.550) (2.184) (2.825)<br />

0.3534<br />

-<br />

Notes<br />

1. All types of <strong>child</strong>care are interacted with maternal educational attainment, with the exception of care by the partner pre-<strong>age</strong> 2 which is<br />

interacted with paternal education.<br />

2. The interaction terms for each type of care relate <strong>to</strong> separate regressions in which all other types of care (both pre-<strong>age</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong> post-<strong>age</strong> 3)<br />

are included as level controls, i.e. only one set of interaction terms is included per regression.<br />

3. The F-test of restrictions tests the null hypothesis that the coefficients on each of the educational categories are equal, i.e. that the effect<br />

of a particular type of centre-based care does not vary with parental education.<br />

73


6. Maternal Health, Social S<strong>up</strong>port <strong>and</strong> Background.<br />

6.1 The measures<br />

The ALSPAC data contain information on the financial resources of the mother when a <strong>child</strong>, a<br />

(negative) life events score for occurrences that happened <strong>to</strong> her before adulthood, measures of<br />

her mental <strong>and</strong> physical health from pregnancy onwards, measures of her social networks, <strong>and</strong><br />

family conflict. Together, they provide a rich picture of the mother’s health resources,<br />

particularly mental <strong>and</strong> emotional. We gro<strong>up</strong> these in<strong>to</strong> 2 gro<strong>up</strong>s.<br />

1) Mothers <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong>: measures of education, financial resources, <strong>and</strong><br />

life events<br />

These measures are:<br />

• The tenure of mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

• The mother’s mother <strong>and</strong> father’s highest educational qualification<br />

• Whether the father was present (or not) in the mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

• Whether the mother was frequently away from school before or after <strong>age</strong> 11<br />

• Whether the mother feels school was valuable for her (not at all versus all others)<br />

• The mother’s relationship with her own mother<br />

• Home stability<br />

• The weighted life events score.<br />

The last three of these are described in a little more detail here.<br />

Mother’s relationship with her own mother<br />

Mothers were given 12 statements regarding their feelings <strong>to</strong>wards their own mother (or<br />

mother figure) in the first 16 years of their life. Examples are:<br />

• My mother was affectionate <strong>to</strong> me<br />

• My mother appeared <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> my problems <strong>and</strong> worries<br />

Responses were coded 0,1 or 2 with higher scores indicating more positive memories. These<br />

were summed <strong>to</strong> give an overall score with a range of 0 <strong>to</strong> 24. This score was divided in<strong>to</strong><br />

quartiles for use in the regressions.<br />

Home stability during <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

Mothers were asked if the behaviour of their mother (figure) <strong>and</strong> father (figure) <strong>to</strong> them as a<br />

<strong>child</strong> was stable <strong>and</strong> predictable. The home stability variable was coded depending on the most<br />

74


negative response given <strong>to</strong> either of the questions. If both responses were ‘Always stable <strong>and</strong><br />

predictable’ the home stability code is ‘Very stable’. If either parent were at worst ‘Mostly<br />

stable <strong>and</strong> predictable’ then the code ‘Fairly stable’ was assigned. If either parent were at worst<br />

‘Rarely stable <strong>and</strong> predictable’ then the code ‘Unstable’ was assigned. And if either parent<br />

were at worst ‘Never stable <strong>and</strong> predictable’ then the code ‘Very unstable’ was assigned. In our<br />

analysis we distinguished only between a very stable <strong>child</strong>hood home <strong>and</strong> not.<br />

Childhood weighted life events score<br />

Mothers were asked whether a list of 31 events had happened <strong>to</strong> them before the <strong>age</strong> of 17.<br />

Examples of events are experience of sexual abuse, a parent being mentally ill, separation of<br />

parents, illness of siblings. If the event occurred they were asked how much it affected them,<br />

with responses ranging from ‘A lot’ (scored as 4) <strong>to</strong> ‘It did not affect me’ (scored as 1). If the<br />

event did not happen it was scored as zero. These weighted responses were then summed <strong>to</strong><br />

give a <strong>to</strong>tal score. The continuous score was divided in<strong>to</strong> quintiles but, after testing, only the 20<br />

percent with the lowest score (i.e. experiencing the least events) were distinguished from the<br />

rest.<br />

2) Mothers Physical <strong>and</strong> mental health, family conflict <strong>and</strong> social s<strong>up</strong>port<br />

Mother’s physical health<br />

Mothers were asked for a general assessment of their fitness in the 8 week, 8 months, 21<br />

months, 33 months <strong>and</strong> 47 months questionnaires. The responses were scored from 0 <strong>to</strong> 3 with<br />

higher scores indicating better physical health. A continuous <strong>to</strong>tal physical health score was<br />

generated by summing these scores across the five questionnaires, giving a variable with a<br />

range of 0 <strong>to</strong> 15. This continuous score was divided in<strong>to</strong> four categories based roughly on<br />

quartiles.<br />

Mother’s social networks<br />

There are two measures; the social networks score <strong>and</strong> a social s<strong>up</strong>port score. The social<br />

networks score measures the extent of the mother’s social network of friends <strong>and</strong> family. It was<br />

administered in pregnancy <strong>and</strong> is composed of the responses <strong>to</strong> 10 items. Mothers were asked<br />

<strong>to</strong> what extent they agreed with statements such as<br />

• How many people are there that you can talk <strong>to</strong> about personal problems?<br />

• During the last month, how many times did you get <strong>to</strong>gether with one or more of your<br />

relatives or your partner's relatives?<br />

75


Possible responses ranged from ‘This is exactly how I feel’ <strong>to</strong> ‘I never feel this way’. They<br />

were scored from 0 <strong>to</strong> 3 with higher scores indicating wider social networks. The <strong>to</strong>tal score is<br />

the sum of the 10 item scores, which is then divided in<strong>to</strong> 3 quantiles for use in the regressions.<br />

The social s<strong>up</strong>port score measures the perceived level of social s<strong>up</strong>port from family, friends<br />

<strong>and</strong> official <strong>age</strong>ncies. It was administered in pregnancy <strong>and</strong> is composed of the responses <strong>to</strong> 10<br />

items. Mothers were asked <strong>to</strong> what extent they agreed with statements such as<br />

• There is always someone with whom I can share my happiness <strong>and</strong> excitement about<br />

my pregnancy.<br />

• If I feel tired I can rely on my partner <strong>to</strong> take over.<br />

Possible responses ranged from ‘This is exactly how I feel’ <strong>to</strong> ‘I never feel this way’. They<br />

were scored from 0 <strong>to</strong> 3 with higher scores indicating a greater degree of perceived s<strong>up</strong>port.<br />

The <strong>to</strong>tal score is the sum of the 10 item scores, which is then divided in<strong>to</strong> 3 quantiles for use<br />

in the regressions.<br />

Mother’s mental health<br />

The Crown-Crisp Experimental Index (CCEI)<br />

The CCEI is a measure of psycho-neurotic pathology. It is comprised of 23 questions that go <strong>to</strong><br />

make <strong>up</strong> 3 subscores. Each question has the possible responses: Very often, Often, Not very<br />

often, Never. These are coded as 0, 1 or 2 depending on the sense of the question <strong>and</strong> summed.<br />

The CCEI score is the sum of all 23 responses <strong>and</strong> has a range of 0 <strong>to</strong> 46. Higher scores<br />

indicate greater mental distress. Within this there is an anxiety subscore (comprised of 8<br />

questions), a somatic subscore (comprised of 7 questions) <strong>and</strong> a depression subscore<br />

(comprised of 8 questions). The CCEI is measured at 18 weeks gestation, 32 weeks gestation, 8<br />

weeks post-natal, <strong>and</strong> at 8 months. It is also available at 21 months <strong>and</strong> 33 months but these are<br />

not used because they may be subject <strong>to</strong> influence by the <strong>child</strong>’s behaviour. The measure used<br />

in most of our analysis is the mean of the 4 early scores. 7 To allow for non-linearities all the<br />

CCEI measures enter the regressions as discrete variables relating <strong>to</strong> quintiles.<br />

7.<br />

We also explore specifications that relate <strong>to</strong> the CCEI in pregnancy (the aver<strong>age</strong> of the 18 <strong>and</strong> 32 week gestation<br />

scores), in the first year of the <strong>child</strong>’s life (the aver<strong>age</strong> of the 8 week <strong>and</strong> 8 month scores) <strong>and</strong> in years 2 <strong>and</strong> 3 (the<br />

aver<strong>age</strong> of the 21 <strong>and</strong> 33 month scores). Another specification we explore takes the mean of each of the subscores<br />

across all 6 questionnaires.<br />

76


Self-esteem measure<br />

The self-esteem score is derived from 11 questions asked at 33 months. Mothers were asked <strong>to</strong><br />

rate whether 11 statements were true: ‘Almost always’,’ Often’,’ Sometimes’, ‘Seldom’ or<br />

‘Never’. Examples are:<br />

• I feel I am able <strong>to</strong> do things as well as most other people<br />

• I feel I do not have much <strong>to</strong> be proud of<br />

Responses were scored from 0 <strong>to</strong> 4 depending on the sense of the question <strong>and</strong> then summed <strong>to</strong><br />

give an overall score with a range of 0 <strong>to</strong> 44. Higher scores indicate greater self-esteem. Again,<br />

the self-esteem score enters the regressions in quintiles.<br />

Locus of control<br />

The locus of control score is derived from responses <strong>to</strong> a questionnaire administered during<br />

pregnancy. Locus of control of reinforcement has been defined as the perception of a<br />

connection between one’s actions <strong>and</strong> their consequences. People who believe that an outcome<br />

is largely contingent <strong>up</strong>on their own behaviour are seen as having a more internal locus of<br />

control, whereas those who believe that luck, fate, chance or powerful others largely determine<br />

an outcome are considered <strong>to</strong> be more external 8 . The score is based on the responses <strong>to</strong> 12<br />

questions scored as 0 or 1 then summed, leading <strong>to</strong> an overall score with a range of 0 <strong>to</strong> 12.<br />

Higher scores indicate a more external locus of control, i.e. a greater belief in the importance of<br />

luck, fate or chance. Hence we label higher scores as reflecting an “external” sense of where<br />

the locus of control lies. Given the small range of this variable, it was split in<strong>to</strong> low-mediumhigh<br />

categories (in the proportions 35 – 50 – 15) on the basis of observation of the distribution.<br />

<strong>Family</strong> conflict<br />

There are two measures of family conflict:<br />

Cruelty from partner<br />

Mothers were asked whether they had experienced physical or emotional cruelty from their<br />

partner at 8, 21, 33 <strong>and</strong> 47 months. If a mother answered yes <strong>to</strong> either of these items on any of<br />

the four questionnaires, she is classed as having experienced cruelty from her partner.<br />

8 Measures of internality <strong>and</strong> externality have been shown <strong>to</strong> be associated with a number of different fac<strong>to</strong>rs,<br />

including academic achievement, psychological well-being <strong>and</strong> beliefs.<br />

77


Mother’s satisfaction with relationship<br />

The mother’s satisfaction with relationship score is derived from 7 questions in the 33-month<br />

questionnaire. Mothers were asked how satisfied they were with aspects of their relationship,<br />

for example<br />

• Demonstrations of affection<br />

• Amount of time spent <strong>to</strong>gether<br />

• H<strong>and</strong>ling family finances<br />

There are four possible responses ranging from ‘Very satisfied’ <strong>to</strong> ‘Very dissatisfied’ <strong>and</strong> these<br />

are scored from 0 <strong>to</strong> 3 with higher scores indicating greater satisfaction. The <strong>to</strong>tal score is the<br />

sum of the 7 item scores. The continuous score is divided in<strong>to</strong> quintiles for use in the<br />

regressions.<br />

6.2 The correlations between the measures<br />

Table 6.1 presents the correlations between these measures. The correlations indicate that these<br />

measures fall in<strong>to</strong> gro<strong>up</strong>s. Higher risks of <strong>child</strong>hood deprivation (a LA home during <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

<strong>and</strong> low education of parents) are associated with each other (<strong>and</strong> with mother’s own<br />

education) but are not particularly closely associated with the measures of mother’s health. The<br />

various dimensions of the mother’s health are associated, both during the post pregnancy<br />

period <strong>and</strong> from the mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>to</strong> her adulthood. The experience of adverse events<br />

during <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> finding them difficult <strong>to</strong> cope with (the weighted life events score) is<br />

linked <strong>to</strong> later experience of poor mental health (the CCEI score). Post pregnancy poor mental<br />

health, feelings of lack of control <strong>and</strong> low self-esteem are closely associated with each other,<br />

<strong>and</strong> in turn, this set of variables are closely associated with poor social networks. Whilst poor<br />

physical health is not closely associated with low financial resources during <strong>child</strong>hood, it is<br />

reasonably closely associated with having poorer social s<strong>up</strong>port <strong>and</strong> is strongly associated with<br />

poorer mental health. Experiencing cruelty from a partner is associated with experiencing<br />

adverse events during <strong>child</strong>hood, having poorer physical health, lower social s<strong>up</strong>port <strong>and</strong><br />

poorer mental health.<br />

78


Table 6.1: Correlations between selected maternal <strong>background</strong> characteristics<br />

Childhood<br />

home council<br />

rented 1<br />

Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong> Physical health Mother’s social networks<br />

Mother’s father<br />

Mother’s<br />

absent from Mother’s Weighted life relationship<br />

Social<br />

<strong>child</strong>-hood mother had no events in with own Mother’s networks Social<br />

home 1 qual-ifications 1 <strong>child</strong>hood score mother score physical health score s<strong>up</strong>port score<br />

Childhood home council rented 1 1.00<br />

Mother’s father absent from<br />

1<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

0.10 1.00<br />

Mother’s mother had no<br />

1<br />

qualifications<br />

0.32 0.06 1.00<br />

Weighted life events in<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood score<br />

0.06 0.07 -0.06 1.00<br />

Mother’s relationship with own<br />

mother score<br />

-0.10 -0.07 -0.09 -0.25 1.00<br />

Mother’s physical health -0.06 -0.01 -0.04 -0.19 0.19 1.00<br />

Social networks score -0.15 -0.09 -0.17 -0.08 0.26 0.12 1.00<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score -0.09 -0.07 -0.09 -0.14 0.28 0.22 0.49 1.00<br />

Total CCEI score 0.07 0.06 0.05 0.29 -0.27 -0.55 -0.23 -0.36 1.00<br />

Mental health <strong>and</strong> psychological<br />

measures<br />

Total CCEI<br />

score<br />

Locus of<br />

control score<br />

Locus of control score 0.19 0.12 0.22 0.07 -0.17 -0.12 -0.26 -0.25 0.22 1.00<br />

Self-esteem<br />

score<br />

Experienced<br />

cruelty from<br />

partner 1<br />

<strong>Family</strong> conflict<br />

Self-esteem score -0.08 -0.05 -0.07 -0.13 0.22 0.37 0.23 0.34 -0.54 -0.23 1.00<br />

Experienced cruelty from<br />

1<br />

partner<br />

0.03 0.02 0.00 0.19 -0.17 -0.19 -0.13 -0.24 0.31 0.10 -0.19 1.00<br />

Mother’s satisfaction with<br />

relationship<br />

0.00 0.01 0.02 -0.13 0.19 0.27 0.14 0.33 -0.36 -0.07 0.35 -0.33 1.00<br />

Log household income -0.21 -0.10 -0.20 -0.11 0.08 0.12 0.23 0.22 -0.21 -0.38 0.19 -0.21 0.09<br />

prebirth financial difficulties 1 0.07 0.03 0.05 0.12 -0.14 -0.14 -0.18 -0.20 0.26 0.17 -0.17 0.15 -0.15<br />

Ever lone parent 1 0.09 0.11 0.10 0.13 -0.07 -0.07 -0.15 -0.22 0.20 0.21 -0.13 0.33 -0.08<br />

Maternal education -0.28 -0.10 -0.37 0.01 0.03 0.04 0.21 0.11 -0.09 -0.44 0.11 -0.03 -0.03<br />

Paternal education -0.22 -0.09 -0.32 0.00 0.02 0.03 0.17 0.13 -0.08 -0.34 0.08 -0.07 0.02<br />

Firstborn <strong>child</strong> 1 -0.07 0.01 -0.08 -0.01 0.08 0.06 0.08 0.06 -0.04 -0.06 0.06 -0.04 0.03<br />

Notes<br />

1. Dummy variables<br />

2. Bold indicates significance at 1% level, Underlining significance at the 5% level<br />

Mother’s<br />

satisfaction<br />

with the<br />

relationship<br />

79


In sum, the data shows a picture in which individuals who experience poor post pregnancy<br />

mental health are more likely <strong>to</strong> have experienced adverse events during their <strong>child</strong>hood, have<br />

poorer social networks, have poorer physical health <strong>and</strong> are more likely <strong>to</strong> experience cruelty<br />

from their partners. These events are also, as the last part of table 6.1 shows, associated with<br />

both a lower household income <strong>and</strong> single parenthood during the study <strong>child</strong>’s life.<br />

6.3 The effect of maternal health <strong>and</strong> <strong>background</strong> on EA score<br />

Table 6.2 presents the results of equation (1), which estimates the independent impact of each<br />

of the maternal health <strong>and</strong> <strong>background</strong> measures on the EA scores. The results show that<br />

individually most of these measures are correlated with EA <strong>and</strong> that the direction of<br />

correlations is in the expected direction. Those mothers from poorer financial <strong>background</strong>s,<br />

who experience poorer health <strong>and</strong> less social s<strong>up</strong>port whilst their <strong>child</strong> is under 5 have <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

with lower EA scores. However, there appears <strong>to</strong> be little enduring impact of the mental health<br />

related events experienced by the mother during her own <strong>child</strong>hood. In addition, the<br />

magnitudes reported in this table are modest in comparison with those seen in the parenting<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>care sections. This suggests that facets of the mother’s physical <strong>and</strong> mental health, the<br />

mother’s <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> family conflict are not substantive drivers of early <strong>child</strong><br />

<strong>development</strong>.<br />

Table 6.3 explores the joint association between these fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> the EA score. The first<br />

column of coefficients presents a full specification, while the second column drops those<br />

variables that are not significantly different from zero. The results show that a low EA score is<br />

associated with the mother coming from a poorer <strong>background</strong> (having low financial resources<br />

during <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> low educational attainment of the maternal gr<strong>and</strong>mother), with the<br />

mother having poor social networks <strong>and</strong> with the mother feeling she has little control over her<br />

life. However, there is no association of either physical or mental health (as measured by the<br />

CCEI score) with EA test scores once we have allowed for the joint correlations between these<br />

maternal health measures <strong>and</strong> other measures of the mother’s health. Again, the correlations of<br />

these measures with EA are lower than those in the parenting <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>care sections above.<br />

80


Table 6.2: Entry Assessment – coefficients on maternal <strong>background</strong> variables when<br />

included individually in basic specification (N = 5010)<br />

Adj R2 0.2473 0.2429 0.2450 0.2458 0.2429<br />

Mother's <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong><br />

Tenure of mother's <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = mortg<strong>age</strong>/owned))<br />

Council housing -0.89 *** - - - -<br />

Other rented -0.30 - - - -<br />

Other 0.80 - - - -<br />

Mother's father present in <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = not at all)<br />

Throughout 1.73 ** - - - -<br />

Partly 1.54 ** - - - -<br />

Mother's mother's educational attainment (base = CSE/none)<br />

Vocational/O-level 0.721 ** - - - -<br />

A-level 1.30 *** - - - -<br />

Degree 0.18 - - - -<br />

Mother's father's educational attainment Insig - - - -<br />

Childhood events score Insig - - - -<br />

Mother's relationship with mother Insig - - - -<br />

Home stability during <strong>child</strong>hood Insig - - - -<br />

Mother has his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy (base = yes)<br />

No 1.32 ** - - - -<br />

Mother feels school was a valuable experience (base = not sure/generally not/no)<br />

Yes very 0.37 - - - -<br />

Yes generally -0.40 - - - -<br />

Mother's physical health<br />

Mother's physical health score<br />

Insig<br />

Mother's social networks<br />

Social networks score (base = highest third)<br />

Middle third - - 0.64 ** - -<br />

Highest third - - 0.69 ** - -<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score (base = highest third)<br />

Middle third - - 0.44 - -<br />

Highest third - - 1.00 *** - -<br />

Mental health & psychological measures<br />

Total CCEI score (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2nd quintile - - - -0.56 -<br />

3rd quintile - - - -0.63 -<br />

4th quintile - - - -0.51 -<br />

Highest quintile - - - -1.20 *** -<br />

Locus of control score (base = Internal) -<br />

Medium - - - -0.82 *** -<br />

External - - - -1.47 *** -<br />

Self-esteem score - - - Insig -<br />

Interpersonal sensitivity measure subscore - - - Insig -<br />

<strong>Family</strong> conflict<br />

Experience of cruelty from partner - - - - Insig<br />

Mother’s satisfaction with relationship<br />

Insig<br />

81


Table 6.3: Entry Assessment – coefficients on maternal <strong>background</strong> variables when<br />

included simultaneously in basic specification, full <strong>and</strong> parsimonious specifications<br />

(N = 5010)<br />

Adj R2 0.2502 0.2498<br />

Mother's <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong><br />

Tenure of mother's <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = mortg<strong>age</strong>/owned)<br />

Council housing -0.81 ** -0.84 **<br />

Other rented -0.24 -0.25<br />

Other 0.81 0.75<br />

Mother's father present in <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = not at all)<br />

Throughout 1.56 ** 1.66 **<br />

Partly 1.44 * 1.54 **<br />

Mother's mother's educational attainment (base = CSE/none)<br />

Vocational/O-level 0.66 * 0.68 **<br />

A-level 1.24 *** 1.27 ***<br />

Degree 0.05 0.10<br />

Mother has his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy (base = yes)<br />

No 1.12 ** 1.12**<br />

Mother feels school was a valuable experience (base = not sure/generally not/no)<br />

Yes very -0.00 0.11<br />

Yes generally -0.61 * -0.54<br />

Mother's physical health<br />

Mother's social networks<br />

Social networks score (base = lowest third)<br />

Middle third 0.52 * Dropped<br />

Highest third 0.52 -<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score (base = lowest third)<br />

Middle third 0.21 0.43<br />

Highest third 0.72 * 1.06 ***<br />

Mental health & psychological measures<br />

Total CCEI score (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2nd quintile -0.45 Dropped<br />

3rd quintile -0.52 -<br />

4th quintile -0.28 -<br />

Highest quintile -0.93 ** -<br />

Locus of control score (base = Internal)<br />

Medium -0.70 ** -0.72 **<br />

External -1.13 ** -1.23 **<br />

6.4 The effect of maternal health measures on <strong>child</strong> behaviour<br />

Table 6.4 investigates the relationship between individual maternal health <strong>and</strong> her <strong>child</strong>’s<br />

behaviour. The lower the score the fewer the behavioural problems. The table shows strong<br />

association between mothers health <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> behaviour. Mothers with more settled <strong>child</strong>hoods<br />

– with fewer adverse events in their own <strong>child</strong>hood, with better relationships with their<br />

mothers, from more stable household, with little experience of truancy <strong>and</strong> who felt school was<br />

valuable are more likely <strong>to</strong> have <strong>child</strong>ren with fewer behavioural problems. Similarly, mothers<br />

in better mental <strong>and</strong> physical health have <strong>child</strong>ren with fewer behavioural problems. These<br />

correlations are very substantial. Mothers who have better social networks have <strong>child</strong>ren with<br />

82


fewer behavioural problems. Finally, mothers who do not experience cruelty from their<br />

partners <strong>and</strong> who are more satisfied with their relationship have <strong>child</strong>ren with fewer<br />

behavioural problems.<br />

Table 6.5 examines whether these strong patterns are robust <strong>to</strong> the addition of the basic<br />

controls <strong>and</strong> allowing for the correlation between the maternal health, <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> s<strong>up</strong>port<br />

measures. The second column shows that this is predominantly the case. With the exception of<br />

adverse events in <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> the experience of cruelty from a partner, all other measures of<br />

maternal health remain associated with <strong>child</strong> behaviour. Within each measure the relationship<br />

is mono<strong>to</strong>nic, so that within <strong>and</strong> across measures, poorer behaviour is associated with poorer<br />

maternal health. The size of the association between poor behaviour <strong>and</strong> mental health, very<br />

poor physical health, <strong>and</strong> very low self-esteem are large. A <strong>child</strong> with a mother in very poor<br />

health, in the highest quintile of the CCEI score or in the highest quintile of low self esteem is<br />

on aver<strong>age</strong> likely <strong>to</strong> have a behavioural score around 3 points higher than a <strong>child</strong> whose mother<br />

is in very good health, has the lowest CCEI score <strong>and</strong> with the highest self esteem. A<br />

combination of all three would move the <strong>child</strong> almost a whole st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. These are<br />

very substantial effects. However, as noted when discussing the measures this is mother<br />

reported data <strong>and</strong> whilst we looked at teacher reported data for a sub-sample at <strong>age</strong> 7 <strong>and</strong> found<br />

similar patterns there is evidence of a lot of measurement error in these reports. In addition the<br />

mothers self-esteem score did not appear as an important predic<strong>to</strong>r of <strong>age</strong> 7 teacher reported<br />

scores, this raises the prospect that the mother is reporting with bias where she has low selfesteem.<br />

83


Table 6.4: Behaviour – coefficients on <strong>background</strong> variables when included individually<br />

in basic specification (N = 9416)<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Adj R2 0.0914 0.1026 0.0898 0.1439 0.0948<br />

Mother's <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong><br />

Tenure of mother's <strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

Insig<br />

Mother's father present in <strong>child</strong>hood home Insig - - - -<br />

Mother's mother's educational attainment Insig - - - -<br />

Mother's father's educational attainment Insig - - - -<br />

Childhood events score (base = 20 th <strong>to</strong> 100 th percentiles - - - -<br />

Lowest quintile -0.82 ***<br />

Mother's relationship with mother (base = lowest quartile) - - - -<br />

Second quartile -0.43<br />

Third quartile -1.74 *** - - - -<br />

Highest quartile -2.86 *** - - - -<br />

Home stability during <strong>child</strong>hood (base = fairly stable/unstable/very unstable) - - -<br />

Very -0.80 *** - - -<br />

Mother has his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy (base = yes) - - -<br />

No -1.30*** - - -<br />

Mother feels school was a valuable experience (base = not sure/generally not/no) - -<br />

Yes very -2.55***<br />

Yes generally -1.46 *** - - - -<br />

Mother's physical health<br />

Mother's physical health score (base = v high)<br />

V low - 6.27 *** - - -<br />

Low - 3.46 *** - - -<br />

High - 1.72 *** - - -<br />

Mother's social networks<br />

Social networks score (base = highest third)<br />

Middle third - - -0.94 *** - -<br />

Highest third - - -1.66 *** - -<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score (base = highest third)<br />

Middle third - - -1.86 *** - -<br />

Highest third - - -3.68 *** - -<br />

Mental health & psychological measures<br />

Total CCEI score (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2nd quintile - - - 1.94 *** -<br />

3rd quintile - - - 3.00 *** -<br />

4th quintile - - - 3.53 *** -<br />

Highest quintile - - - 5.73 *** -<br />

Locus of control score (base = Internal -<br />

Medium - - - 0.71 *** -<br />

External - - - 2.05 *** -<br />

Self-esteem score (base=highest quintile)<br />

Lowest quintile - - - 4.76 ***<br />

2 nd quintile - - - 2.96 ***<br />

3 rd quintile - - - 2.26 ***<br />

4 th quintile - - - 1.37 ***<br />

Interpersonal sensitivity measure subscore - - - Insig -<br />

<strong>Family</strong> conflict<br />

Experience of cruelty from partner - - - - 1.56 ***<br />

Mother’s satisfaction with relationship (base = highest quintile)<br />

Lowest quintile - - - - 5.19***<br />

2 nd quintile - - - - 3.42***<br />

3 rd quintile - - - - 3.10***<br />

4 th quintile - - - - 1.59***<br />

84


Table 6.5: Behaviour – coefficients on maternal <strong>background</strong> variables when included<br />

simultaneously in basic specification, full <strong>and</strong> parsimonious specifications (N = 9416)<br />

1 2<br />

Adj R2 0.1654 0.1653<br />

Mother's <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong><br />

Childhood events score (base = 20 th <strong>to</strong> 100 th percentiles<br />

Lowest quintile -0.25 Dropped<br />

Mother's relationship with mother (base = lowest quartile)<br />

Second quartile 0.24 0.24<br />

Third quartile -0.70 ** -0.69**<br />

Highest quartile -1.16 *** -1.15***<br />

Home stability during <strong>child</strong>hood (base = fairly stable/unstable/very unstable)<br />

Very 0.15 Dropped<br />

Mother has his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy (base = yes)<br />

No -0.88 ** -0.90**<br />

Mother feels school was a valuable experience (base = not sure/generally not/no)<br />

Yes very -0.82 *** -0.79 **<br />

Yes generally -0.52 ** -0.51 **<br />

Mother's physical health<br />

Mother's physical health score (base = v high)<br />

V low 2.68 *** 2.68 ***<br />

Low 1.16 *** 1.17 ***<br />

High 0.67 ** 0.67 **<br />

Mother's social networks<br />

Social networks score (base = highest third)<br />

Middle third -0.73 *** -0.73 ***<br />

Highest third -1.16 *** -1.17 ***<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score (base = highest third)<br />

Middle third -0.39 -0.40<br />

Highest third -1.11 *** -1.12 ***<br />

Mental health & psychological measures<br />

Total CCEI score (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2nd quintile 1.29 *** 1.30 ***<br />

3rd quintile 1.97 *** 1.99 ***<br />

4th quintile 2.01 *** 2.05 ***<br />

Highest quintile 3.48 *** 3.58 ***<br />

Locus of control score (base = Internal)<br />

Medium 0.52 ** 0.51 **<br />

External 1.36 *** 1.35 ***<br />

Self-esteem score (base = highest quintile)<br />

Lowest quintile 3.45 *** 3.43 ***<br />

2 nd quintile 2.04 *** 2.02 ***<br />

3 rd quintile 1.58 *** 1.57 ***<br />

4 th quintile 0.99 *** 0.98 ***<br />

<strong>Family</strong> conflict<br />

Experienced cruelty from partner 0.30 Dropped<br />

Mother’s satisfaction with relationship (base = highest quintile)<br />

Lowest quintile 2.10 *** 2.17 ***<br />

2 nd quintile 1.27 *** 1.29 ***<br />

3 rd quintile 1.53 *** 1.55 ***<br />

4 th quintile 0.75 ** 0.76 **<br />

85


6.5 Robustness of Mental Health Measures<br />

Tables 6.6 <strong>and</strong> 6.7 examine the robustness of the CCEI measure, which has been shown <strong>to</strong> be<br />

an important determinant of both cognitive <strong>and</strong> behavioural outcomes. The behavioural score,<br />

in particular, is strongly related <strong>to</strong> the CCEI score of the mother.<br />

Table 6.6 shows that the association with the variable measured at any one period, is only well<br />

defined if we slice the distribution in<strong>to</strong> two – those in the <strong>to</strong>p quintile <strong>and</strong> those not. But when<br />

the whole period experience of low mental health is assessed, there is a clearer gradient: the<br />

better health, on aver<strong>age</strong>, the higher the cognitive <strong>development</strong> of the <strong>child</strong>.<br />

Table 6.7, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, shows that no matter when mental health is measured, there is a<br />

very strong association between poorer mental health <strong>and</strong> poorer <strong>child</strong> behaviour. There is also<br />

indication that the closer in time the mother experiences mental health <strong>to</strong> her <strong>child</strong> being <strong>age</strong> 5,<br />

the stronger the association with poor behaviour. This is not true however of each of the<br />

separate subscales. In summary, it appears that there is a very strong association between poor<br />

mental health of the mother – whenever experienced during the first three years of the <strong>child</strong>’s<br />

life - <strong>and</strong> behaviour.<br />

6.6 Interactions with Mother’s Education<br />

As in previous sections we explore whether there are interactions between mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood,<br />

mental <strong>and</strong> physical health <strong>and</strong> relationship measures <strong>and</strong> her education. These F-test results<br />

are reported in Table 6.8 <strong>and</strong> they test whether the additive representation of these measures<br />

with mother’s education is valid or whether <strong>child</strong>ren of better educated mothers suffer more or<br />

less when their mother’s health etc. is below the norm. The results, as before, tend <strong>to</strong> suggest<br />

that the additive representation is broadly accepted. This suggests that there are no major<br />

variations in how mother’s poor mental health, for example, impacts on <strong>child</strong>ren across<br />

parental education gro<strong>up</strong>s. This, however, is not found for mothers’ physical health in the<br />

behaviour equation. Here the test strongly rejects that the impact of poor maternal physical<br />

health on <strong>child</strong>ren is the same for all education gro<strong>up</strong>s. Closer examination of the interaction<br />

terms shows that lower educated mothers have substantially better (more negative scores) for a<br />

given health level. So poor maternal physical health has a much larger adverse effect on<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s behaviour scores among better educated mothers.<br />

86


Table 6.6: Entry Assessment – Tests of robustness for CCEI measure, timing <strong>and</strong> composition of measure<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

CCEI<br />

Pregnancy<br />

period First year<br />

Second <strong>and</strong><br />

third year<br />

Anxiety<br />

subscore<br />

Depression<br />

subscore<br />

Somatic<br />

subscore<br />

Adj R2 0.2442 0.2443 0.2432 0.2434 0.2444 0.2441 0.2456<br />

Score (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2nd quintile -0.56 -0.46 0.51 -0.36 -0.02 -0.18 0.65*<br />

3rd quintile -0.69* -0.26 -0.09 -0.33 -0.38 -0.20 -0.19<br />

4th quintile -0.61 -0.92** -0.28 -0.50 -0.26 -0.78** -0.31<br />

Highest quintile -1.38*** -1.40*** -0.42 -0.95** -0.63 -1.09*** -1.08***<br />

Table 6.7: Behaviour – Tests of robustness for CCEI measure, timing <strong>and</strong> composition of measure<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

CCEI<br />

Pregnancy<br />

period First year<br />

Second <strong>and</strong><br />

third year<br />

Anxiety<br />

subscore<br />

Depression<br />

subscore<br />

Somatic<br />

subscore<br />

Adj R2 0.1232 0.0914 0.1125 0.1250 0.1099 0.1213 0.0982<br />

Score (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2nd quintile 2.47*** 1.94*** 1.94*** 2.58 *** 2.11*** 2.66*** 1.71***<br />

3rd quintile 4.00*** 2.81*** 3.62*** 4.12 *** 3.01*** 3.78*** 2.93***<br />

4th quintile 5.02*** 3.61*** 4.71*** 5.29 *** 4.47*** 4.95*** 4.12***<br />

Highest quintile 7.89*** 5.51*** 6.86*** 7.77 *** 6.81*** 7.76*** 5.96***<br />

87


Table 6.8: Summary of significance of interaction terms (family <strong>background</strong> variables *<br />

maternal education)<br />

Prob effect of family <strong>background</strong> measures<br />

<strong>Family</strong> <strong>background</strong> measure<br />

does not vary with educational gro<strong>up</strong> –<br />

joint test of no variation within all family<br />

<strong>background</strong> categories 1<br />

Entry Assessment Behaviour<br />

Tenure of mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood home 0.8516 -<br />

Mother’s father present in <strong>child</strong>hood home 0.5185 -<br />

Mother’s mother’s educational attainment 0.3586 -<br />

Mother’s relationship with mother - 0.3135<br />

Mother feels school was a valuable<br />

0.5125 0.8089<br />

experience<br />

Mother’s physical health score - 0.0004<br />

Social networks score - 0.3655<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score 0.8652 0.8198<br />

Total CCEI score - 0.3533<br />

Locus of control score 0.9656 0.4918<br />

Self esteem score - 0.5832<br />

Mother’s satisfaction with relationship with<br />

partner<br />

- 0.9138<br />

88


7. Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> Peer Gro<strong>up</strong>s<br />

In this brief section we explore how <strong>to</strong> best represent information on the neighbourhood <strong>and</strong><br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong> of the <strong>child</strong>. In the next section we bring <strong>to</strong>gether the gro<strong>up</strong>s of indica<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong><br />

explore how they correlate with each other <strong>and</strong> with measures of family income <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>background</strong>.<br />

Children do not grow <strong>up</strong> in isolation within the home or school but interact with other people<br />

<strong>and</strong> especially with the <strong>child</strong>ren they share facilities with. This is particularly likely <strong>to</strong> be<br />

important when the <strong>child</strong> is in extended contact with <strong>child</strong>ren whilst at a pre-school. The other<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren a <strong>child</strong> interacts with are often described as their peer gro<strong>up</strong>. It is hard <strong>to</strong> distinguish the<br />

extent <strong>to</strong> which a <strong>child</strong> is influenced by their neighbourhood or peer gro<strong>up</strong> from whether it<br />

reflects some (unobserved) aspect of the parents who have been sorted in<strong>to</strong> neighbourhoods or<br />

chosen a particular pre-school provider. However, important information has come <strong>to</strong> light from<br />

the Moving <strong>to</strong> Opportunity experiments in the US (see Katz, Kling <strong>and</strong> Leibman 2001, Ludwig,<br />

Duncan <strong>and</strong> Hirschfield, 2001 <strong>and</strong> Ludwig, Ladd <strong>and</strong> Duncan, 2001). Here families from poor<br />

neighbourhoods are r<strong>and</strong>omly selected in<strong>to</strong> one of three populations. First, there are those given<br />

financial assistance with rents <strong>to</strong> move <strong>to</strong> a rental unit in a more affluent neighbourhood. 9 If<br />

they did not move they received nothing. A second gro<strong>up</strong> got rent s<strong>up</strong>port but could move <strong>to</strong><br />

any neighbourhood. The third gro<strong>up</strong> received no help in moving from the deprived<br />

neighbourhood. So the treatment is that families receive financial s<strong>up</strong>port <strong>to</strong> meet higher<br />

housing costs associated with moving <strong>to</strong> more affluent (<strong>and</strong> high rent) areas, provided they<br />

make the move.<br />

These studies provide crucial evidence of how neighbourhoods <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>s might influence<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s educational outcomes as moving <strong>to</strong> a better neighbourhood occurs for reasons that are<br />

unrelated <strong>to</strong> the characteristics of the family. Importantly, these moves were not associated with<br />

increases in employment or earnings among adults, so the effects observed are operating purely<br />

through neighbourhood change. The results suggest that moving neighbourhood (which goes<br />

h<strong>and</strong>-in-h<strong>and</strong> with changing school <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong> for most <strong>child</strong>ren) are associated with<br />

marked improvements in behavioural problems <strong>and</strong> school test scores <strong>and</strong>, for school <strong>age</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>ren, a reduction in the number of arrests for violent crime. The <strong>child</strong>ren being assessed<br />

under these experiments are <strong>age</strong>d from 5 <strong>to</strong> 15 <strong>and</strong> hence may not reflect the effects of<br />

9 The rent assistance was in the form of Section 8 housing vouchers which is rent assistance programme in the US<br />

which has some parallels with Housing Benefit in the UK but is more restricted in its availability.<br />

90


neighbourhood on pre-school <strong>child</strong>ren but they do provide powerful evidence of such effects for<br />

older <strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

As ALSPAC is drawn from the former area of Avon, most <strong>child</strong>ren are growing <strong>up</strong> in the same<br />

neighbourhoods within that area <strong>and</strong> indeed are attending the same schools <strong>and</strong> pre-schools. We<br />

can thus merge information on the neighbourhoods the <strong>child</strong>ren live in <strong>and</strong> look at the<br />

characteristics of many of the <strong>child</strong>ren the <strong>child</strong> shares facilities with.<br />

7.1 Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> Peer Gro<strong>up</strong> Measures<br />

First, we have matched in<strong>to</strong> ALSPAC information at ward level from the Index of Multiple<br />

Deprivation IMD (administered by the Office of the Deputy Prime Minister). The overall index<br />

is constructed from six component measures covering employment, health, education, income,<br />

housing <strong>and</strong> access <strong>to</strong> services. There is also a measure of <strong>child</strong> poverty at the ward level that is<br />

not incorporated in<strong>to</strong> the index itself. We explore which of these dimensions is associated with<br />

early <strong>child</strong>hood learning deficits. The measures are very strongly related, as shown in Table 7.1.<br />

Hence they may be best represented as a single composite index or one measure may clearly<br />

dominate in its predictive power. We will therefore explore which dimensions of deprivation are<br />

most strongly correlated with pre-school <strong>age</strong> <strong>child</strong> attainment. It is hard <strong>to</strong> say more about<br />

exactly what drives any observed relationship.<br />

Table 7.1 Correlations between Domains of the Index of Multiple Deprivation in Avon<br />

area<br />

Total index of<br />

multiple deprivation<br />

Education domain<br />

score<br />

Access domain<br />

score<br />

Income domain<br />

score<br />

Employment<br />

domain score<br />

Health domain<br />

score<br />

Housing domain<br />

score<br />

Poverty domain<br />

score<br />

Total index<br />

of multiple<br />

deprivation<br />

1.0000<br />

Education<br />

domain<br />

score<br />

0.8327 1.0000<br />

Access<br />

domain<br />

score<br />

-0.4234 -0.3655 1.0000<br />

Income<br />

domain<br />

score<br />

0.9784 0.7761 -0.4663 1.0000<br />

Employment<br />

domain score<br />

0.9510 0.6762 -0.4985 0.9508 1.0000<br />

Health<br />

domain<br />

score<br />

0.8744 0.7012 -0.4866 0.8705 0.8905 1.0000<br />

Housing<br />

domain<br />

score<br />

0.8030 0.7131 -0.4884 0.7750 0.7277 0.6697 1.0000<br />

Poverty<br />

domain<br />

score<br />

0.9600 0.8070 -0.4408 0.9768 0.9202 0.8452 0.7735 1.0000<br />

Second, as part of the ‘Focus at 7’ face <strong>to</strong> face interviews with mother <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>, the mother was<br />

asked <strong>to</strong> document <strong>child</strong>care providers used from the <strong>age</strong> of 3. Importantly this included the<br />

91


name of the provider as well as the broad type of care <strong>and</strong> number of sessions attended. In many<br />

cases therefore we can observe many <strong>child</strong>ren attending the same pre-school provider<br />

immediately prior <strong>to</strong> school entry. From this then we can get a measure of attainment among the<br />

<strong>child</strong>’s pre-school peer gro<strong>up</strong>. We were concerned that many <strong>child</strong>ren would go on <strong>to</strong> be<br />

assessed by the same teacher for the EA score <strong>and</strong> that any idiosyncratic teacher effects in<br />

scoring would muddy this measure of the pre-school peer gro<strong>up</strong>. Such teacher effects should be<br />

just r<strong>and</strong>om measurement error over the whole population. But within the same school this<br />

practice would give the impression that the peer gro<strong>up</strong> was very influential whereas in reality it<br />

was just teacher bias (a teacher scoring the <strong>child</strong>ren higher or lower than aver<strong>age</strong>). So we<br />

construct our measure of the peer gro<strong>up</strong> from those who attended the same pre-school but did<br />

not attend the same school. These observations are thus independent of the <strong>child</strong>. Using these<br />

observations we, in the first instance, simply aver<strong>age</strong> the EA score. Where a <strong>child</strong> attends more<br />

than one provider we aver<strong>age</strong> across the different providers weighting by the number of sessions<br />

attended. This peer gro<strong>up</strong> measure will capture a range of influences from the natural attainment<br />

of the other <strong>child</strong>ren, their family <strong>background</strong> etc. <strong>and</strong> any common influence of the pre-school.<br />

It thus includes any effects of the quality of the pre-school provider.<br />

We can go further, though, in separating influences. We predict the other <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment<br />

using the general model containing all family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> mediating influences provided in<br />

the data, except the nature of the pre-school provider. This is then a measure of the extent <strong>to</strong><br />

which the <strong>child</strong>ren forming the peer gro<strong>up</strong> are from more affluent, better educated families<br />

which read <strong>to</strong> their <strong>child</strong>ren etc., reflecting how these fac<strong>to</strong>rs influence attainment in our model.<br />

This measure therefore does not contain any element of the quality of pre-school provider as it<br />

is based only on family characteristics of the <strong>child</strong>ren attending it. The unexplained residual<br />

from our estimates then reflects the aver<strong>age</strong> of the variation in attainment across <strong>child</strong>ren in the<br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong> that is not explained by the <strong>child</strong>’s <strong>background</strong>. This will cover the idiosyncratic<br />

component for each individual <strong>child</strong> aver<strong>age</strong>d across the peer gro<strong>up</strong>, which will be influenced<br />

by the quality of the common attendance of the same pre-school provider. It will also reflect<br />

any measurement error in the data. Large amounts of measurement error will make it hard <strong>to</strong><br />

observe <strong>child</strong>care quality for small samples attending each provider.<br />

Table 7.2 gives a flavour of the information this provides showing how the aver<strong>age</strong> EA scores<br />

vary across types of pre-school provider. So, for example, <strong>child</strong>ren attending private nurseries<br />

score well above the mean of 100 on aver<strong>age</strong> (102.77) but this is fully predicted by the<br />

<strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> mediating behaviours of the families <strong>and</strong> the residual term is close <strong>to</strong> 0. This<br />

92


suggests that <strong>child</strong>ren leaving these nurseries may be performing well (as shown in the section<br />

on <strong>child</strong>care types) due <strong>to</strong> the common high quality of their family <strong>background</strong>, not because of<br />

the type of nursery attended. The reverse s<strong>to</strong>ry applies <strong>to</strong> LEA nurseries. In addition, though, the<br />

table shows that there is considerable variation across providers in this residual unexplained<br />

component which may suggest that there are good <strong>and</strong> bad providers within each sec<strong>to</strong>r.<br />

Table 7.2 Peer Gro<strong>up</strong> EA Scores by Pre-school Provider Type<br />

Variable<br />

No. of<br />

observations<br />

Mean<br />

St<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

Deviation<br />

10 th<br />

percentile<br />

90 th percentile<br />

Nursery Class<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers 567 100.44 4.32 94.51 105.88<br />

Predicted part 567 100.73 2.59 97.37 103.75<br />

Residual part 567 -0.21 4.00 -4.31 5.54<br />

LEA nursery<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers 391 99.16 3.60 94.64 103.52<br />

Predicted part 391 98.96 1.89 96.50 101.35<br />

Residual part 391 0.26 3.06 -3.23 3.55<br />

Private nursery<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers 516 102.77 3.69 97.92 105.35<br />

Predicted part 516 102.67 2.55 99.22 105.89<br />

Residual part 516 -0.07 2.75 -3.50 2.95<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong><br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers 1935 100.68 4.44 94.80 106.06<br />

Predicted part 1935 100.73 2.58 97.47 104.15<br />

Residual part 1935 -0.24 3.65 -4.84 3.58<br />

Nursery (type undefined)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers 667 101.54 4.91 93.57 107.22<br />

Predicted part 667 101.66 2.87 97.57 106.06<br />

Residual part 667 -0.46 3.90 -5.84 4.30<br />

Other<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers 38 97.50 4.29 90.52 101.18<br />

Predicted part 38 97.25 2.46 92.62 98.74<br />

Residual part 38 0.32 2.75 -4.14 2.15<br />

When looking at behavioural outcomes we employ the same technique but use the behaviour<br />

scores of the peer gro<strong>up</strong> rather than their EA test results.<br />

7.2 Correlations Between Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> Peer Gro<strong>up</strong> Variables <strong>and</strong> Child Outcomes<br />

Table 7.3 shows how these peer gro<strong>up</strong> measures correlate with cognitive attainment <strong>and</strong> Table<br />

7.4 shows how they correlate with behaviour. Column 1 of Table 7.3 shows the effect of the raw<br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong> measure of aver<strong>age</strong> EA attainment of other <strong>child</strong>ren at the same provider. The results<br />

suggest that the <strong>child</strong> is doing slightly better, given their <strong>background</strong> family fac<strong>to</strong>rs, if attending<br />

a provider with a very high attaining peer gro<strong>up</strong>. But among lower attaining gro<strong>up</strong>s there is no<br />

variation.<br />

93


Column 2 introduces the IMD as a single combined index. This shows a strong correlation with<br />

attainment, such that a higher multiple deprivation score is associated with somewhat lower<br />

attainment. Columns 3 <strong>and</strong> 4 show the elements of the index separately <strong>and</strong> also include the<br />

additional ward level measure of <strong>child</strong> poverty (Column 3 includes the <strong>to</strong>tal index as well).<br />

What is striking is that the ward level indica<strong>to</strong>r with the strongest correlation with attainment is<br />

the Education Domain. This domain consists of 6 indica<strong>to</strong>rs:<br />

• Proportion of Working Age adults with no qualifications (LFS)<br />

• Children <strong>age</strong>d 16+ not in full-time education<br />

• Proportion of 17-19 year olds not applying <strong>to</strong> HE<br />

• KS2 primary school performance<br />

• Primary School Children with English as an additional langu<strong>age</strong><br />

• Primary School Absenteeism<br />

Hence the indica<strong>to</strong>r is derived from current school <strong>age</strong> performance, post-school staying on in<br />

education <strong>and</strong> adult education levels. Whilst primary school <strong>age</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren could be older siblings<br />

of ALSPAC <strong>child</strong>ren, the rest of the measure reflects the wider educational attainment in the<br />

population in the ward. This appears strongly correlated with pre-school attainment of <strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

If Educational Domain is excluded the Employment Domain becomes marginally significant.<br />

Column 5 of Table 7.1 reports the parsimonious specification using only the education domain.<br />

Moving from the 10 th percentile of education deprivation domain <strong>to</strong> the 90 th results in a<br />

predicted decrease in attainment of 2.49 points. This is a reasonably large effect compared <strong>to</strong><br />

others we have seen in this study.<br />

Columns 6 <strong>to</strong> 8 of Table 7.3 explore the implications of decomposing the peer gro<strong>up</strong> effect in<strong>to</strong><br />

that part which reflects the family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> mediating influences of the peers, predicted<br />

by our model, <strong>and</strong> the unexplained component, which will include any influence of the preschool.<br />

Note that the predicted element is large <strong>and</strong> positive. Attending a pre-school that<br />

contains p<strong>up</strong>ils from family <strong>background</strong>s that produces high attainment (as defined by our<br />

model) is highly beneficial <strong>to</strong> a <strong>child</strong>’s outcomes. However, the unexplained part - the r<strong>and</strong>om<br />

variation in <strong>child</strong>ren or characteristics of the provider - is not strongly correlated with the <strong>child</strong>’s<br />

attainment. Indeed, the point estimates are negative. Hence we find strong evidence for the<br />

importance of attending pre-schools that are attended by <strong>child</strong>ren from family <strong>background</strong>s<br />

associated with high attainment. We cannot find any clear evidence of a pre-school performance<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>r that is common <strong>to</strong> all <strong>child</strong>ren attending. Note the peer gro<strong>up</strong> effects reduce the size of the<br />

94


neighbourhood effect somewhat, suggesting that peer gro<strong>up</strong> influences drive part of the<br />

observed neighbourhood effects.<br />

Table 7.3: The effects of neighbourhood on Entry Assessment<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Total index of - -0.054*** 0.101 - - - - -<br />

multiple deprivation (0.010) (0.096)<br />

Education domain - - -1.482*** -1.098*** -1.006*** -0.865*** -1.043*** -0.859***<br />

score (0.446) (0.253) (0.145) (0.147) (0.143) (0.147)<br />

Access domain - - -0.534 -0.319 - - - -<br />

score (0.347) (0.280)<br />

Income domain - - -0.129 -0.056 - - - -<br />

score (0.111) (0.085)<br />

Employment<br />

- - -0.073 0.066 - - - -<br />

domain score (0.172) (0.108)<br />

Health domain - - 0.125 0.043 - - - -<br />

score (0.413) (0.405)<br />

Housing domain - - 0.005 0.122 - - - -<br />

score (0.307) (0.286)<br />

Child poverty index - - 0.022 0.009 - - - -<br />

score (0.048) (0.047)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile -0.323 -0.384 -0.355 -0.359 -0.349 - - -<br />

(0.500) (0.499) (0.499) (0.499) (0.498)<br />

3 rd quintile -0.091 -0.313 -0.310 -0.326 -0.324 - - -<br />

(0.497) (0.497) (0.497) (0.497) (0.496)<br />

4 th quintile 0.034 -0.275 -0.410 -0.396 -0.419 - - -<br />

(0.507) (0.509) (0.512) (0.512) (0.509)<br />

Highest quintile 1.831*** 1.582*** 1.342*** 1.359*** 1.402*** - - -<br />

(0.511) (0.512) (0.515) (0.515) (0.513)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers – predicted value (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile - - - - - 0.146 - 0.347<br />

(0.499) (0.509)<br />

3 rd quintile - - - - - 1.178** - 1.329***<br />

(0.504) (0.509)<br />

4 th quintile - - - - - 2.166*** - 2.333***<br />

(0.509) (0.516)<br />

Highest quintile - - - - - 2.283*** - 2.377***<br />

(0.532) (0.535)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers – residual part (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile - - - - - - -1.053** -1.118**<br />

(0.498) (0.503)<br />

3 rd quintile - - - - - - -0.542 -0.693<br />

(0.498) (0.505)<br />

4 th quintile - - - - - - -0.757 -0.972*<br />

(0.497) (0.503)<br />

Highest quintile - - - - - - -0.595 -0.757<br />

(0.496) (0.497)<br />

Adj R2 0.2476 0.2518 0.2543 0.2542 0.2547 0.2569 0.2527 0.2572<br />

All regressions include basic demographic controls.<br />

95


Table 7.4: The effects of neighbourhood on Behaviour<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Total index of multiple - -0.012 -0.051 - - - - -<br />

deprivation (0.008) (0.075)<br />

Education domain score - - 0.185 0.004 - - - -<br />

(0.336) (0.204)<br />

Access domain score - - -0.168 -0.278 - - - -<br />

(0.268) (0.214)<br />

Income domain score - - 0.037 -0.000 - - - -<br />

(0.084) (0.064)<br />

Employment domain - - -0.045 -0.111 - - - -<br />

score (0.128) (0.085)<br />

Health domain score - - -0.091 -0.073 - - - -<br />

(0.316) (0.315)<br />

Housing domain score - - -0.081 -0.156 - - - -<br />

(0.251) (0.225)<br />

Child poverty index - - 0.024 0.031 - - - -<br />

score (0.037) (0.035)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> behaviour score of peers (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile 0.195 0.210 0.182 0.187 0.195 - - -<br />

(0.458) (0.459) (0.459) (0.459) (0.458)<br />

3 rd quintile 0.011 0.033 -0.036 -0.025 0.011 - - -<br />

(0.459) (0.459) (0.463) (0.462) (0.459)<br />

4 th quintile 0.672 0.706 0.646 0.654 0.672 - - -<br />

(0.460) (0.461) (0.463) (0.463) (0.460)<br />

Highest quintile 0.770* 0.828* 0.746 0.756 0.770* - - -<br />

(0.463) (0.464) (0.469) (0.469) (0.463)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> behaviour score of peers – predicted value (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile - - - - - -0.806* -0.832*<br />

(0.460) (0.460)<br />

3 rd quintile - - - - - -1.088** -0.995**<br />

(0.464) (0.465)<br />

4 th quintile - - - - - -0.171 0.005<br />

(0.469) (0.470)<br />

Highest quintile - - - - - -0.308 -0.249<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> behaviour score of peers – residual part (base = lowest quintile)<br />

(0.471) (0.475)<br />

2 nd quintile - - - - - - -0.919** -0.965**<br />

(0.458) (0.459)<br />

3 rd quintile - - - - - - 1.119** 1.164**<br />

(0.458) (0.462)<br />

4 th quintile - - - - - - -0.410 -0.350<br />

(0.458) (0.463)<br />

Highest quintile - - - - - - 0.716 0.665<br />

(0.458) (0.460)<br />

Adj R2 0.0641 0.0642 0.0639 0.0640 0.0641 0.0644 0.0662 0.0666<br />

All regressions include basic demographic controls.<br />

Table 7.4 repeats this analysis for behaviour. The measures of local ward level deprivation are<br />

all individually <strong>and</strong> collectively insignificant, suggesting no neighbourhood influences on early<br />

96


<strong>child</strong> behaviour. But the peer gro<strong>up</strong> behaviour score is very weakly associated with outcomes<br />

such that a very high behaviour score is correlated weakly with that for the <strong>child</strong>. Decomposing<br />

this in<strong>to</strong> the predicted <strong>and</strong> residual components leads <strong>to</strong> no clear patterns. Hence there is little<br />

evidence here that neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong> influences the behaviour of young <strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

97


8. Combined Analysis<br />

The approach in this last section is <strong>to</strong> examine how the introduction of the alternative gro<strong>up</strong>s of<br />

proximal variables studied in the previous sections acts as transmission routes for the more<br />

distal indica<strong>to</strong>rs - income, parental education, lone parent status etc - on the attainment <strong>and</strong><br />

behaviour outcomes. The intention is <strong>to</strong> see whether these behaviours are the route by which<br />

<strong>background</strong> family circumstances affect outcomes. Causality cannot be proven, but if, for<br />

example, we observe that well educated parents appear <strong>to</strong> be raising <strong>child</strong> educational<br />

attainment through engaging in more reading <strong>and</strong> teaching activity then this appears <strong>to</strong> be a<br />

plausible causal chain. Likewise if the income effect is coming from reduced books or <strong>to</strong>ys in<br />

the home or less <strong>child</strong>care, outings <strong>and</strong> the like, then it seems reasonable <strong>to</strong> view these as the<br />

impact of reduced financial circumstances. However, there will be a lot of grey areas where<br />

causality is at best tentative. We then are assessing which of the proximal influences measured<br />

in ALSPAC are associated with educational attainment <strong>and</strong> behaviour outcomes directly <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong><br />

what extent they appear <strong>to</strong> be the routes through which dimensions of family <strong>background</strong> are<br />

operating.<br />

8.1 Summary of Influences<br />

Table 8.1 lists the variable gro<strong>up</strong>ings that we are going <strong>to</strong> explore as potential routes by which<br />

distal fac<strong>to</strong>rs are working. These gro<strong>up</strong>ings cover pre-school <strong>child</strong>care, parenting behaviour,<br />

aspects of the home environment <strong>and</strong> out-of-home activities, mother’s family <strong>background</strong>,<br />

mother’s health <strong>and</strong> social networks <strong>and</strong> neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>. Income has a dual role<br />

here in that we explore how the proximal variables might be routes by which income works but<br />

also we assess whether income is a route through which lone parenthood or parental education<br />

operate.<br />

98


Table 8.1: Gro<strong>up</strong>s of variables used as controls in Entry Assessment regressions<br />

Basic<br />

demographics<br />

Mother’s highest<br />

qualification<br />

Partner’s highest<br />

qualification<br />

Lone parent<br />

status<br />

Income Childcare Parenting<br />

Log aver<strong>age</strong><br />

weekly net<br />

income<br />

Financial<br />

difficulties<br />

pre-birth<br />

Care by partner<br />

pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Care by<br />

friend/relative<br />

pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Care by paid<br />

person pre-<strong>age</strong><br />

2<br />

Maternal<br />

reading score<br />

Teaching score<br />

18 month TV<br />

score<br />

Outings Scores &<br />

Home<br />

Environment<br />

Department s<strong>to</strong>re<br />

outings score<br />

Library outings<br />

score<br />

Book score<br />

Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

<strong>and</strong> family<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Tenure of mother’s<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

Mother’s father<br />

absent from<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

Mother’s mother’s<br />

highest qualification<br />

Maternal health,<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong><br />

social networks<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port<br />

score<br />

Locus of control<br />

score<br />

Neighbourhood<br />

Education<br />

domain<br />

deprivation score<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA<br />

score of peers –<br />

predicted<br />

component<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA<br />

score of peers –<br />

residual<br />

component<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at<br />

birth<br />

Centre-based<br />

care pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Breastfeeding<br />

Toy score<br />

Mother has a his<strong>to</strong>ry<br />

of truancy<br />

Ethnicity<br />

Nursery class<br />

<strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Mother feels school<br />

was a valuable<br />

experience<br />

Gender<br />

LEA nursery<br />

school <strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Birth weight<br />

Private day<br />

nursery <strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Special care unit<br />

at birth<br />

Nursery (type<br />

undefined) <strong>age</strong><br />

3/ 4<br />

Younger siblings<br />

by 47 months<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> <strong>age</strong><br />

3/ 4<br />

Older siblings<br />

Other centrebased<br />

care <strong>age</strong><br />

3/ 4<br />

Cohort year<br />

Partner’s<br />

employment at<br />

47 months<br />

8.2 How do Parental Education, Lone Parenthood, mother’s <strong>age</strong>, ethnicity <strong>and</strong> siblings<br />

impact on <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong>?<br />

This section begins the substantive discussion of how these proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs suggest family<br />

<strong>background</strong> influences <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong>. So, for example, one question of particular interest is<br />

whether more educated parents tend <strong>to</strong> provide more cognitive stimulation for their <strong>child</strong>ren. It<br />

also assesses how the proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs we study here are working independently of others or<br />

how they are correlated.<br />

Table 8.2 shows how these gro<strong>up</strong>s of proximal variables reduce the observed correlation of<br />

basic demographics (family <strong>background</strong> characteristics) with attainment. Each gro<strong>up</strong> is entered<br />

separately <strong>and</strong> then they are all combined. This gives a sense of the impact of each variable<br />

99


gro<strong>up</strong>ing. Column 1 thus shows a basic regression without these proximal controls, column 2<br />

introduces family income <strong>and</strong> columns 3-8 then introduce the gro<strong>up</strong>s of controls considered<br />

above, whilst column 9 includes all the measures <strong>to</strong>gether. Our focus is <strong>to</strong> examine whether<br />

family characteristics become less important as other proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs are added <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> get a<br />

sense of the routes by which family <strong>background</strong> operates <strong>to</strong> influence attainment. So Column 1<br />

of Table 8.2 has the set of <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs described in section 2 whilst Column 9 has<br />

introduced the full set of routes of influence we explore. A comparison between these two<br />

columns suggests the extent <strong>to</strong> which each <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>r is related <strong>to</strong> the EA score through<br />

these mechanisms. At the end of this section we show more formal decompositions of the<br />

contributions the observed mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs play in acting as routes by which parental<br />

education (<strong>and</strong> income) influence outcomes.<br />

8.2.1 Parental Education<br />

This comparison for parental education shows the coefficients are reduced substantially. The<br />

raw gap between attainment of <strong>child</strong>ren with the least <strong>and</strong> most educated mothers was 5.5 points<br />

or ½ a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. Conditioning on our mediating influences reduces this <strong>to</strong> 2.4 points<br />

(44% of the original value). So our gro<strong>up</strong>s of mediating influences can explain a little over half<br />

of the effect of parental education on attainment. We have already assessed whether better<br />

education parents do these mechanisms more effectively <strong>and</strong> the evidence suggests not. So the<br />

observed decline in the strength of the relationship between parental education <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

attainment as we add in proximal controls suggests the routes by which parental education<br />

operate.<br />

Looking at the impact of each gro<strong>up</strong> in turn suggests that parenting behaviour <strong>and</strong> the home<br />

environment measures are the strongest mechanisms through which parental education operates.<br />

However, family income is also a substantive route through which parental education<br />

(especially partner’s education) impacts on attainment, as are the neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer<br />

gro<strong>up</strong>. The simple correlations in Table 3.1 (also see Appendix A3) showed that better educated<br />

parents eng<strong>age</strong>d in reading <strong>to</strong> their <strong>child</strong>ren more <strong>and</strong> their <strong>child</strong>ren watched less TV especially<br />

at <strong>age</strong> 3. They also provided a home with higher book <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>y scores <strong>and</strong> eng<strong>age</strong>d in more trips<br />

<strong>to</strong> the library. These are aspects of behaviour <strong>and</strong> the home environment shown <strong>to</strong> influence<br />

<strong>child</strong> attainment. Pre-school <strong>child</strong>care is not a major route by which well-educated parents<br />

increase their <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment. Mother’s own <strong>child</strong>hood events <strong>and</strong> her locus of control<br />

<strong>and</strong> social networks are more important in explaining the impact of low education relative <strong>to</strong><br />

middle education.<br />

100


8.2.2 Lone Parenthood<br />

Lone parenthood goes from being a marginally negative influence on <strong>child</strong> outcomes <strong>to</strong> a<br />

marginally positive one when these routes of influence are included. However, lone parenthood<br />

is not a statistically significant influence on early attainment in any specification. Low income<br />

<strong>and</strong> low book <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>y scores are the main fac<strong>to</strong>rs which turn around the point estimates. One<br />

other interesting feature of these results is that the lone parent coefficients are left broadly<br />

unchanged once parenting behaviour controls are introduced. This would suggest that <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

of lone parents are not doing less well because of parenting behaviours, where time constraints<br />

might be expected <strong>to</strong> bite. The main conclusion is that <strong>child</strong>ren of lone mothers are not<br />

performing worse than their peers, once other fac<strong>to</strong>rs are conditioned on (especially their <strong>age</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> income). But as noted in Section 2 our sample of lone parents is not representative <strong>and</strong> so<br />

care needs <strong>to</strong> be exercised here.<br />

8.2.3 Mother’s Age<br />

Teen motherhood has an especially large negative association with the <strong>child</strong>’s cognitive<br />

attainment. The proximal routes explored can explain around 40% of this attainment deficit,<br />

with family income being the most substantive route. But all fac<strong>to</strong>rs make some contribution.<br />

The positive benefits of older motherhood are much better explained by the observed mediating<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs, with parenting behaviour <strong>and</strong> income being the major two influences.<br />

8.2.4 Ethnic Minorities<br />

Children from non-white families do slightly less well on entry <strong>to</strong> school than white <strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

There are <strong>to</strong>o few observations among these communities <strong>to</strong> allow for more detailed breakdown<br />

by ethnicity. About a third of this can be explained by observed differences in the gro<strong>up</strong>s of<br />

more proximal influences used here. The biggest influence is in the home environment (book<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>y scores) <strong>and</strong> outings measures. Income also plays a role. Parenting behaviour, maternal<br />

employment <strong>and</strong> mental health <strong>and</strong> social s<strong>up</strong>port are unimportant as routes explaining the gap<br />

between non-whites <strong>and</strong> whites.<br />

101


Table 8.2: Estimated effects of basic demographics on Entry Assessment, various<br />

specifications (N = 5010)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)<br />

Mother’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none -2.150*** -1.984*** -2.014*** -1.744*** -1.683*** -1.710*** -1.864*** -1.924*** -0.991**<br />

(0.398) (0.398) (0.397) (0.396) (0.397) (0.406) (0.403) (0.395) (0.404)<br />

A-level 1.278*** 1.057*** 1.177*** 0.771** 0.893*** 0.915*** 1.111*** 1.012*** 0.110<br />

(0.320) (0.321) (0.322) (0.320) (0.320) (0.328) (0.322) (0.318) (0.329)<br />

Degree 3.314*** 2.928*** 2.953*** 2.752*** 2.686*** 2.740*** 3.085*** 2.847*** 1.442***<br />

(0.484) (0.487) (0.492) (0.486) (0.485) (0.507) (0.487) (0.481) (0.514)<br />

Partner’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none -1.657*** -1.460*** -1.618***<br />

(0.373) (0.374) (0.372)<br />

A-level 0.560* 0.459 0.461<br />

(0.331) (0.331) (0.329)<br />

Degree 2.546*** 2.176*** 2.197***<br />

(0.449) (0.452) (0.451)<br />

Lone parent status (base = never lone parent)<br />

Intermittently, with<br />

partner at 47 months<br />

-0.021<br />

(0.829)<br />

Intermittently, no -1.003<br />

partner at 47 months (0.637)<br />

Since birth -1.507<br />

(1.273)<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth (base = 25-29)<br />

1 -2.153*** -2.045***<br />

(0.759) (0.756)<br />

Older siblings (base = none)<br />

1 -1.477***<br />

(0.328)<br />

2<br />

-3.063***<br />

(0.438)<br />

3+ -4.827***<br />

(0.648)<br />

-1.396***<br />

(0.328)<br />

-2.974***<br />

(0.437)<br />

-4.660***<br />

(0.648)<br />

0.089<br />

(0.830)<br />

-0.873<br />

(0.638)<br />

-1.078<br />

(1.282)<br />

-3.617***<br />

(0.975)<br />

-0.861**<br />

(0.387)<br />

0.540*<br />

(0.303)<br />

1.003**<br />

(0.441)<br />

-1.829***<br />

(0.686)<br />

3.519***<br />

(0.248)<br />

-2.597***<br />

(0.665)<br />

-0.831***<br />

(0.259)<br />

-0.434<br />

(0.550)<br />

-0.153<br />

(0.313)<br />

-1.888**<br />

(0.757)<br />

-1.351***<br />

(0.334)<br />

-2.898***<br />

(0.444)<br />

-4.351***<br />

(0.654)<br />

Partner’s employment status at 47 months (base = in work)<br />

Not in work -1.132** -0.385 -1.013*<br />

(0.533) (0.546) (0.532)<br />

-1.526***<br />

(0.368)<br />

0.447<br />

(0.326)<br />

2.235***<br />

(0.445)<br />

0.119<br />

(0.818)<br />

-0.885<br />

(0.630)<br />

-1.165<br />

(1.255)<br />

-3.756***<br />

(0.968)<br />

-0.892**<br />

(0.384)<br />

0.461<br />

(0.301)<br />

0.663<br />

(0.438)<br />

-2.090***<br />

(0.676)<br />

3.359***<br />

(0.247)<br />

-2.761***<br />

(0.660)<br />

-0.810***<br />

(0.257)<br />

-0.239<br />

(0.545)<br />

-0.263<br />

(0.309)<br />

-1.912**<br />

(0.749)<br />

-0.716**<br />

(0.332)<br />

-2.246***<br />

(0.440)<br />

-3.868***<br />

(0.646)<br />

-0.947*<br />

(0.526)<br />

-1.436***<br />

(0.370)<br />

0.407<br />

(0.328)<br />

2.301***<br />

(0.446)<br />

0.193<br />

(0.822)<br />

-0.761<br />

(0.632)<br />

-0.874<br />

(1.263)<br />

-3.856***<br />

(0.971)<br />

-0.908**<br />

(0.385)<br />

0.569*<br />

(0.302)<br />

0.910**<br />

(0.437)<br />

-1.633**<br />

(0.680)<br />

3.461***<br />

(0.247)<br />

-2.725***<br />

(0.663)<br />

-0.802***<br />

(0.259)<br />

-0.310<br />

(0.547)<br />

-0.178<br />

(0.311)<br />

-2.164***<br />

(0.753)<br />

-1.377***<br />

(0.343)<br />

-2.871***<br />

(0.450)<br />

-4.379***<br />

(0.657)<br />

-0.820<br />

(0.530)<br />

-1.485***<br />

(0.375)<br />

0.487<br />

(0.330)<br />

2.394***<br />

(0.450)<br />

0.202<br />

(0.828)<br />

-0.834<br />

(0.640)<br />

-1.114<br />

(1.272)<br />

-3.664***<br />

(0.981)<br />

-0.834**<br />

(0.389)<br />

0.693***<br />

(0.305)<br />

1.155***<br />

(0.444)<br />

-2.075***<br />

(0.687)<br />

3.544***<br />

(0.249)<br />

-2.795***<br />

(0.667)<br />

-0.761***<br />

(0.261)<br />

-0.326<br />

(0.551)<br />

-0.179<br />

(0.313)<br />

-2.077***<br />

(0.759)<br />

-1.398***<br />

(0.328)<br />

-2.966***<br />

(0.438)<br />

-4.555***<br />

(0.649)<br />

-1.034*<br />

(0.533)<br />

-1.573***<br />

(0.373)<br />

0.535<br />

(0.330)<br />

2.371***<br />

(0.449)<br />

0.238<br />

(0.830)<br />

-0.714<br />

(0.639)<br />

-0.836<br />

(1.287)<br />

-3.857***<br />

(0.977)<br />

-0.871**<br />

(0.388)<br />

0.626**<br />

(0.304)<br />

1.107**<br />

(0.440)<br />

-2.060***<br />

(0.683)<br />

3.535***<br />

(0.249)<br />

-2.680***<br />

(0.667)<br />

-0.840***<br />

(0.260)<br />

-0.375<br />

(0.551)<br />

-0.222<br />

(0.313)<br />

-2.177***<br />

(0.757)<br />

-1.410***<br />

(0.328)<br />

-2.953***<br />

(0.438)<br />

-4.648***<br />

(0.647)<br />

-0.934*<br />

(0.533)<br />

-1.481***<br />

(0.370)<br />

0.426<br />

(0.328)<br />

2.022***<br />

(0.448)<br />

0.115<br />

(0.822)<br />

-0.844<br />

(0.632)<br />

-1.064<br />

(1.263)<br />

-3.601***<br />

(0.971)<br />

-0.781**<br />

(0.385)<br />

0.383<br />

(0.303)<br />

0.785*<br />

(0.438)<br />

-1.683**<br />

(0.679)<br />

3.532***<br />

(0.247)<br />

-2.681***<br />

(0.663)<br />

-0.821***<br />

(0.258)<br />

-0.212<br />

(0.547)<br />

-0.226<br />

(0.310)<br />

-1.829**<br />

(0.753)<br />

-1.353***<br />

(0.326)<br />

-2.893***<br />

(0.434)<br />

-4.200***<br />

(0.644)<br />

-0.884*<br />

(0.529)<br />

-1.080***<br />

(0.368)<br />

0.095<br />

(0.323)<br />

1.183***<br />

(0.449)<br />

1.047<br />

(0.830)<br />

0.408<br />

(0.679)<br />

1.026<br />

(1.299)<br />

-2.412**<br />

(0.967)<br />

-0.430<br />

(0.385)<br />

0.221<br />

(0.300)<br />

0.411<br />

(0.440)<br />

-1.416**<br />

(0.679)<br />

3.343***<br />

(0.244)<br />

-2.510***<br />

(0.652)<br />

-0.736***<br />

(0.254)<br />

-0.169<br />

(0.538)<br />

-0.227<br />

(0.307)<br />

-1.640**<br />

(0.744)<br />

-0.675*<br />

(0.349)<br />

-2.049***<br />

(0.455)<br />

-3.125***<br />

(0.661)<br />

-0.071<br />

(0.538)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.2381 0.2429 0.2486 0.2609 0.2538 0.2433 0.2424 0.2550 0.2889<br />

Notes<br />

1. Controls for each specification are specified overleaf.<br />

102


Table 8.2: (continued) Controls included in Estimated effects of basic demographics on<br />

Entry Assessment, various specifications<br />

Specification (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)<br />

Controls included<br />

X<br />

Income X X<br />

Childcare X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Outings & home<br />

environment<br />

Maternal <strong>background</strong> X X<br />

Maternal health etc. X X<br />

Neighbourhood X X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

8.2.5 Gender, Birth Weight <strong>and</strong> Siblings<br />

Coefficients on low birth weight, gender of the <strong>child</strong> <strong>and</strong> the presence of younger siblings are<br />

not greatly affected by the introduction of the gro<strong>up</strong>s of proximal variables. The coefficients on<br />

older siblings are substantially reduced by inclusion of parenting behaviours, suggesting that the<br />

influence of siblings is <strong>to</strong> slow <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> by reducing parental inputs <strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong>,<br />

especially teaching <strong>and</strong> reading.<br />

8.2.6 Partner’s Employment Status<br />

The small negative effect from a non-working partner at 47 months is fully explained by the<br />

mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs. The dominant route by which this is operating is family income. So the<br />

modest negative effects of an absent (lone parenthood) <strong>and</strong> a workless father seem <strong>to</strong> operate<br />

almost entirely through income.<br />

8.3 Which Influences are Important?<br />

We explore next which particular dimensions of the set of proximal influence variables remain<br />

important once we condition on the full set of influences described in Table 8.1. We follow the<br />

same logic as in section 8.2 where we add each set of influences in turn <strong>and</strong> assess which<br />

influences remain important. For example, parenting <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>care may vary systematically<br />

<strong>to</strong>gether. Once parenting behaviour is conditioned <strong>up</strong>on, <strong>child</strong>care may become less relatively<br />

important than when its impact is measured in isolation.<br />

8.3.1 Childcare<br />

Table 8.3 shows that the broad pattern of the results for care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 discussed above are not<br />

greatly affected by other influences considered here. However, controlling family income does<br />

reduce the positive effects of nursery centres. The most important change in estimated impact of<br />

pre-school entry provider comes after controlling for the neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong><br />

103


(column 7). The peer gro<strong>up</strong> is defined on the basis of the pre-school attended. The results<br />

suggest that much of the positive effects of private nurseries <strong>and</strong> all the adverse effects of LEA<br />

nurseries are driven by peer gro<strong>up</strong> composition. This is also partly true for the other (probably<br />

specialist) care. Private care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong> private care prior <strong>to</strong> school entry <strong>and</strong> LEA nursery<br />

care become insignificant when all fac<strong>to</strong>rs are conditioned <strong>up</strong>on (column 8). So the initially<br />

apparent beneficial association of attending a private nursery prior <strong>to</strong> school entry comes from<br />

the association with the <strong>child</strong> coming from a more affluent family with more home teaching,<br />

books, <strong>to</strong>ys, outings etc <strong>and</strong> from attending a pre-school with similar high attaining <strong>child</strong>ren. So<br />

once we condition on the peer gro<strong>up</strong> there is no evidence that private nurseries are<br />

outperforming Nursery classes. In addition, LEA nurseries are performing poorly solely because<br />

of the concentration of <strong>child</strong>ren from deprived <strong>background</strong>s in this pre-school setting.<br />

The results suggest that conditional on all other mediating influences <strong>and</strong> <strong>background</strong><br />

characteristics:<br />

• Limited hours of care by partner <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 is modestly beneficial <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong><br />

(this could well be outside the partners working hours)<br />

• Extended care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 by friends, gr<strong>and</strong>parents <strong>and</strong> other relatives is modestly<br />

harmful.<br />

• Nursery care (in school class or private setting) when <strong>age</strong>d 3-4 is modestly beneficial <strong>to</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment on entry <strong>to</strong> school.<br />

• Following earlier analysis in Section 5, there is no evidence that full-time Nursery<br />

exposure at 3 or 4 is more beneficial than part-time.<br />

• There is no evidence that attending a Nursery is more beneficial for those with less<br />

educated mothers. (Note that the lack of assessment of quality of <strong>child</strong>care means these<br />

results are less reliable than other studies of the influence of quality <strong>child</strong>care on<br />

<strong>development</strong> such as the EPPE project, see Sylva et al. 2004.)<br />

104


• Table 8.3: Estimated effects of <strong>child</strong>care arrangements on Entry Assessment,<br />

various specifications (N = 5010)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Childcare pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Used partner 5-19 hours per 0.629* 0.624* 0.681** 0.628* 0.637* 0.565 0.623* 0.616*<br />

week (0.346) (0.345) (0.341) (0.343) (0.345) (0.345) (0.344) (0.340)<br />

Used partner 20+ hours per -0.008 0.005 0.030 -0.027 0.028 -0.063 0.092 0.024<br />

week (0.345) (0.344) (0.341) (0.342) (0.345) (0.344) (0.343) (0.340)<br />

Used friend/relative 5-19 -0.235 -0.255 -0.211 -0.200 -0.242 -0.253 -0.197 -0.135<br />

hours per week (0.316) (0.316) (0.312) (0.313) (0.316) (0.315) (0.314) (0.311)<br />

Used friend/relative 20+<br />

-<br />

hours per week<br />

-0.818** -0.897*** -0.879*** -0.760** -0.868** 0.896*** -0.862** -0.769**<br />

(0.341) (0.340) (0.337) (0.338) (0.341) (0.340) (0.339) (0.336)<br />

Used paid person 5-19 hours -0.117 -0.297 -0.395 -0.270 -0.334 -0.241 -0.327 -0.389<br />

per week (0.473) (0.474) (0.468) (0.470) (0.474) (0.473) (0.471) (0.466)<br />

Used paid person 20+ hours 0.599 0.258 0.348 0.339 0.220 0.291 0.178 0.290<br />

per week (0.399) (0.404) (0.400) (0.401) (0.404) (0.404) (0.402) (0.399)<br />

Used centre-based care 5-19 0.234 0.100 0.079 0.206 0.085 0.146 0.073 0.084<br />

hours per week (0.608) (0.608) (0.600) (0.603) (0.607) (0.606) (0.605) (0.597)<br />

Used centre-based care 20+ 1.604* 1.320 1.216 1.405* 1.195 1.232 1.384* 1.135<br />

hours per week (0.836) (0.836) (0.826) (0.829) (0.836) (0.835) (0.835) (0.825)<br />

Childcare <strong>age</strong> 3 <strong>to</strong> 4<br />

Nursery class 0.693* 0.775* 0.812** 0.746* 0.784** 0.762* 1.044** 0.982**<br />

(0.399) (0.399) (0.394) (0.396) (0.399) (0.399) (0.408) (0.403)<br />

LEA nursery -1.233** -1.106** -1.127** -1.159** -1.031** -1.066** -0.198 -0.271<br />

(0.486) (0.485) (0.479) (0.481) (0.486) (0.485) (0.514) (0.508)<br />

Private day nursery 1.544*** 1.400*** 1.281*** 1.288*** 1.287*** 1.393*** 0.817* 0.654<br />

(0.429) (0.430) (0.424) (0.427) (0.430) (0.429) (0.445) (0.439)<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> 0.421 0.377 0.374 0.258 0.307 0.326 0.135 0.044<br />

(0.340) (0.340) (0.335) (0.337) (0.340) (0.339) (0.341) (0.337)<br />

Nursery (type undefined) 1.150*** 1.130*** 1.186*** 1.148*** 1.103*** 1.131*** 1.089*** 1.122***<br />

Other<br />

(0.358) (0.358) (0.353) (0.355) (0.358) (0.358) (0.364) (0.359)<br />

-<br />

-3.674*** -3.529*** -2.996*** -3.636*** -3.501*** 3.569*** -2.610** -2.381**<br />

(1.111) (1.109) (1.096) (1.099) (1.108) (1.108) (1.115) (1.101)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.2486 0.2519 0.2735 0.2664 0.2552 0.2550 0.2611 0.2889<br />

Controls included<br />

Basic demographics X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Outings & home<br />

environment<br />

Maternal <strong>background</strong> X X<br />

Maternal health etc. X X<br />

Neighbourhood X X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

8.3.2 Parenting Behaviour<br />

Table 8.4 shows that the broad pattern of parenting behaviour results remain unchanged when<br />

other influences are conditioned on. The impact of TV watching <strong>and</strong> breastfeeding are reduced<br />

substantially in magnitude. Conditioning on family income is important in reducing the<br />

importance of these behaviours <strong>and</strong> the home environment (books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys) <strong>and</strong> outings are<br />

105


important in reducing the impact of TV watching, suggesting these are partial substitutes. TV is<br />

used more by low-income families perhaps because it is cheap entertainment. Teaching<br />

behaviour is almost completely unaffected by this conditioning <strong>and</strong> remains the most important<br />

influence of measured attainment at 5.<br />

Table 8.4: Estimated effects of parenting behaviours on Entry Assessment, various<br />

specifications (N = 5010)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Maternal reading score (base = high)<br />

Very low - - - - - -<br />

2.427*** 2.367*** 2.353*** 1.691*** 2.291*** 2.237*** -2.281*** -1.584***<br />

(0.541) (0.540) (0.538) (0.556) (0.540) (0.541) (0.535) (0.553)<br />

Low - - -<br />

- -<br />

1.418*** 1.464*** 1.472*** -1.048** 1.445*** 1.389*** -1.393*** -0.999**<br />

(0.486) (0.484) (0.483) (0.491) (0.486) (0.485) (0.480) (0.488)<br />

Medium -0.627* -0.642** -0.630* -0.436 -0.651** -0.603* -0.576* -0.409<br />

Teaching score (base =<br />

high)<br />

Very low -<br />

3.846***<br />

(0.326) (0.325) (0.324) (0.329) (0.326) (0.325) (0.323) (0.327)<br />

-<br />

3.793***<br />

-<br />

3.808***<br />

-<br />

3.550***<br />

-<br />

3.861***<br />

-<br />

3.749*** -3.812*** -3.654***<br />

(0.576) (0.574) (0.573) (0.579) (0.574) (0.574) (0.570) (0.576)<br />

Low - - - - - -<br />

2.036*** 2.059*** 2.138*** 1.982*** 2.049*** 2.005*** -2.077*** -2.022***<br />

(0.453) (0.452) (0.451) (0.457) (0.452) (0.452) (0.449) (0.453)<br />

Medium - - - - - -<br />

0.979*** 0.999*** 1.084*** 1.031*** 1.011*** 0.998*** -1.020*** -1.108***<br />

(0.296) (0.295) (0.295) (0.296) (0.295) (0.296) (0.293) (0.295)<br />

18 month TV score (base = 0-2 hours per week)<br />

3 –5 - -<br />

0.896*** 0.816*** -0.678** -0.690** -0.745** -0.775** -0.684** -0.376<br />

(0.316) (0.316) (0.315) (0.316) (0.316) (0.315) (0.314) (0.316)<br />

6 - - - - - -<br />

1.877*** 1.761*** 1.562*** 1.530*** 1.587*** 1.675*** -1.574*** -1.057**<br />

(0.458) (0.458) (0.458) (0.460) (0.459) (0.458) (0.455) (0.460)<br />

Duration of breastfeeding (base = never)<br />

Breastfed 6 1.099*** 1.106*** 1.115*** 0.891** 1.002*** 1.034*** 0.994*** 0.680*<br />

months (0.357) (0.356) (0.357) (0.357) (0.357) (0.357) (0.354) (0.358)<br />

Adjusted R-<br />

squared 0.2609 0.2650 0.2735 0.2709 0.2680 0.2664 0.2780 0.2889<br />

Controls<br />

included<br />

Basic<br />

demographics<br />

X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Childcare X X<br />

Outings &<br />

home<br />

environment<br />

Maternal<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Maternal<br />

health etc.<br />

Neighbourhood X X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

So the key parenting influences are:<br />

106


• Maternal teaching behaviour in the pre-school period has a large effect on school entry<br />

attainment levels among <strong>child</strong>ren. The magnitude is such that <strong>child</strong>ren of mothers who are in<br />

the lowest 8% in terms of <strong>age</strong> of starting <strong>and</strong> range of teaching are 3.5 points behind other<br />

similar <strong>child</strong>ren (or 14 percentile points lower in the distribution). Those <strong>child</strong>ren in the next<br />

11% of the distribution are 2 points (or 8 percentile points) behind in the distribution. These<br />

are big effects. Note in passing they are larger than the conditional effects of mothers<br />

education <strong>and</strong> there is no evidence that teaching by the better educated mothers are more<br />

effective.<br />

• Maternal reading is less important but less intensive reading does result in substantial<br />

deficits.<br />

• Other parenting behaviours such as 6+ hours of TV a week at 18 months <strong>and</strong> breast-feeding<br />

beyond 6 months are of moderate importance.<br />

8.3.3 Outings <strong>and</strong> the Home Environment<br />

The outings <strong>and</strong> home environment measures are strongly correlated with parenting behaviours<br />

around TV watching <strong>and</strong> reading. Low income also reduces the predicted effects of the poor<br />

home environment measures. It is obvious that reading requires books but the results suggest<br />

that the quality of the home environment - in terms of the variety of books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys available - is<br />

important in <strong>and</strong> of itself <strong>and</strong> that this is associated with low income.<br />

Table 8.5 shows the key Outings <strong>and</strong> Home Environment influences are:<br />

• A very low book score (which in turn is associated with low reading <strong>and</strong> low income) -<br />

which affects the 6% of <strong>child</strong>ren (those in homes reporting no more than 9 <strong>child</strong>ren’s<br />

books in the home on 5 occasions before school entry) - remains a substantive influence<br />

on <strong>child</strong> attainment conditional on confounding influences.<br />

• In the same vein, a very low <strong>to</strong>y score (measured at 24 months <strong>and</strong> covering 7% of<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren with lowest cumulative score for a range of <strong>to</strong>ys) is also found <strong>to</strong> be detrimental<br />

<strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong> cognitive <strong>development</strong>.<br />

• There is no evidence that better educated parents are providing higher quality books or<br />

<strong>to</strong>ys, in terms of their impact on <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> (see section 3).<br />

• Trips <strong>to</strong> Libraries <strong>and</strong> not attending department s<strong>to</strong>res remain weak influences on<br />

<strong>development</strong>.<br />

107


Table 8.5: Estimated effects of outings scores <strong>and</strong> home environment on Entry<br />

Assessment, various specifications (N = 5010)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Outings scores<br />

Department s<strong>to</strong>res -0.496* -0.559** -0.504* -0.655** -0.537** -0.558** -0.463* -0.511*<br />

(0.271) (0.270) (0.270) (0.269) (0.270) (0.270) (0.268) (0.267)<br />

Library 1.171*** 1.234*** 1.239*** 0.907*** 1.147*** 1.224*** 1.189*** 0.864***<br />

(0.282) (0.281) (0.281) (0.284) (0.282) (0.281) (0.279) (0.283)<br />

Book score (base = high)<br />

Very low -3.525*** -3.325*** -3.321*** -2.067*** -3.131*** -3.129*** -3.112*** -1.741***<br />

(0.613) (0.612) (0.611) (0.628) (0.614) (0.614) (0.609) (0.626)<br />

Low -1.530*** -1.434*** -1.415*** -0.673* -1.371*** -1.341*** -1.309*** -0.490<br />

(0.362) (0.361) (0.360) (0.372) (0.362) (0.362) (0.359) (0.370)<br />

Medium -0.902** -0.869** -0.844** -0.510 -0.846** -0.792** -0.852** -0.421<br />

(0.366) (0.365) (0.364) (0.365) (0.365) (0.366) (0.363) (0.363)<br />

Toy score (base = high)<br />

Very low -1.854*** -1.842*** -1.813*** -1.176* -1.951*** -1.726*** -1.775*** -1.199**<br />

(0.606) (0.605) (0.602) (0.604) (0.606) (0.606) (0.600) (0.603)<br />

Low -1.019** -0.893** -0.917** -0.447 -0.889** -0.845* -0.891** -0.460<br />

(0.437) (0.436) (0.435) (0.436) (0.436) (0.436) (0.433) (0.434)<br />

Medium 0.026 0.090 0.062 0.367 0.104 0.091 0.093 0.341<br />

(0.351) (0.351) (0.349) (0.350) (0.351) (0.351) (0.348) (0.348)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.2538 0.2579 0.2664 0.2709 0.2611 0.2596 0.2709 0.2889<br />

Controls included<br />

Basic demographics X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Childcare X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Maternal <strong>background</strong> X X<br />

Maternal health etc. X X<br />

Neighbourhood X X<br />

8.3.4 Mother’s Childhood <strong>and</strong> <strong>Family</strong> Background<br />

Table 8.6 shows the mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong> influences are sharply reduced<br />

in importance once other influences are included. Mother’s mother’s education becomes<br />

insignificant <strong>and</strong> housing tenure of <strong>child</strong>hood home is very marginal. Current low income, a<br />

poor home environment <strong>and</strong> parenting behaviours are the influences driving these terms <strong>to</strong><br />

insignificance. The mother having grown <strong>up</strong> in an intact family throughout her <strong>child</strong>hood has a<br />

modest positive influence as does not having a his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy <strong>and</strong> having a positive attitude<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards her own education experience. These fac<strong>to</strong>rs describing the mother’s own <strong>child</strong>hood are<br />

generally not a major source of influence on the <strong>child</strong> once current circumstances are<br />

conditioned on. This suggests that there is a degree of intergenerational persistence in low<br />

income or deprivation that the contemporaneous elements are picking <strong>up</strong>, rather than these<br />

108


eing alternative routes of influence. However, there is evidence that the mother’s education<br />

experience has a modest lasting effect on her <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>.<br />

Table 8.6: Estimated effects of mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong> controls on<br />

Entry Assessment, various specifications (N = 5010)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Tenure of mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = mortg<strong>age</strong>/owned<br />

Council rented -1.000*** -0.893*** -0.807** -0.779** -0.824** -0.830** -0.707** -0.569*<br />

(0.344) (0.343) (0.343) (0.340) (0.342) (0.343) (0.341) (0.338)<br />

Other rented -0.374 -0.299 -0.410 -0.416 -0.285 -0.227 -0.282 -0.494<br />

(0.779) (0.777) (0.775) (0.767) (0.771) (0.777) (0.771) (0.760)<br />

Other 0.720 0.796 0.779 0.873 0.743 0.794 0.823 0.789<br />

(0.691) (0.690) (0.687) (0.681) (0.684) (0.690) (0.684) (0.674)<br />

Mother’s father present in <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = not at all)<br />

Throughout 1.699** 1.731** 1.495** 1.725** 1.581** 1.594** 1.455** 1.226*<br />

(0.699) (0.697) (0.696) (0.688) (0.691) (0.698) (0.692) (0.684)<br />

Partly 1.427* 1.542** 1.297* 1.530** 1.437* 1.477* 1.296* 1.101<br />

(0.760) (0.758) (0.757) (0.748) (0.752) (0.759) (0.752) (0.743)<br />

Mother’s mother’s highest qualification (base = CSE/none)<br />

Vocational/Olevel<br />

0.738** 0.717** 0.548 0.626* 0.546 0.650* 0.543 0.247<br />

(0.346) (0.345) (0.345) (0.341) (0.343) (0.345) (0.343) (0.340)<br />

A-level 1.328*** 1.299*** 1.193*** 0.962** 1.067** 1.232*** 1.117** 0.643<br />

(0.454) (0.453) (0.452) (0.449) (0.450) (0.453) (0.450) (0.445)<br />

Degree 0.083 0.180 0.231 0.279 0.084 0.061 0.111 0.141<br />

(0.782) (0.780) (0.780) (0.771) (0.774) (0.780) (0.775) (0.767)<br />

Mother has a his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy (base = yes)<br />

No 1.387** 1.315** 1.221** 1.265** 1.442*** 1.124** 1.264** 1.172**<br />

(0.544) (0.543) (0.541) (0.536) (0.540) (0.545) (0.539) (0.534)<br />

Mother feels school was a valuable experience (base = not sure/generally not/no)<br />

Yes very 0.487 0.367 0.332 -0.089 0.048 0.090 0.437 -0.287<br />

(0.399) (0.398) (0.397) (0.396) (0.397) (0.403) (0.395) (0.397)<br />

Yes generally -0.321 -0.397 -0.433 -0.670** -0.646* -0.557 -0.379 -0.854**<br />

(0.338) (0.337) (0.336) (0.334) (0.336) (0.339) (0.335) (0.334)<br />

Adjusted R-<br />

squared 0.2433 0.2473 0.2552 0.2680 0.2611 0.2499 0.2604 0.2889<br />

Controls<br />

included<br />

Basic<br />

demographics<br />

X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Childcare X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Outings & home<br />

environment<br />

Maternal health<br />

etc.<br />

Neighbourhood X X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

109


8.3.5 Mother’s Health, social networks <strong>and</strong> <strong>Family</strong> Conflict<br />

This gro<strong>up</strong> of potential influences were not found <strong>to</strong> have a large impact of <strong>child</strong>ren’s<br />

educational <strong>development</strong>. The influences remaining significant in this gro<strong>up</strong> of variables were<br />

only the mother’s locus of control measure <strong>and</strong> the indica<strong>to</strong>rs of social networks <strong>and</strong> s<strong>up</strong>port.<br />

This, in itself, suggested that mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health <strong>and</strong> within family conflict had<br />

little influence on a <strong>child</strong>’s educational <strong>development</strong> pre-school. Table 8.7 indicates that, after<br />

conditioning on other influences, the social networks score becomes insignificant <strong>and</strong> the locus<br />

of control term is reduced in importance, largely through its correlation with poorer parenting<br />

scores <strong>and</strong> a worse home environment. Mothers who feel their actions have little impact eng<strong>age</strong><br />

in less teaching <strong>and</strong> reading <strong>to</strong> their <strong>child</strong>ren. However, there remains a modest independent<br />

effect of this fac<strong>to</strong>r over <strong>and</strong> above this.<br />

Table 8.7: Estimated effects of maternal health, attitudes <strong>and</strong> social networks on Entry<br />

Assessment, various specifications (N = 5010)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score (base = low)<br />

Medium 0.580* 0.500 0.503 0.226 0.244 0.433 0.487 0.077<br />

(0.325) (0.325) (0.324) (0.322) (0.324) (0.326) (0.323) (0.321)<br />

High 1.203*** 1.130*** 1.070*** 0.725** 0.793** 1.064*** 1.068*** 0.495<br />

(0.338) (0.338) (0.338) (0.336) (0.338) (0.340) (0.336) (0.336)<br />

Locus of control score (base = Internal)<br />

Medium -0.906*** -0.808*** -0.795*** -0.654** -0.712** -0.717** -0.732** -0.543*<br />

(0.303) (0.303) (0.301) (0.299) (0.300) (0.304) (0.300) (0.298)<br />

External -1.698*** -1.434*** -1.471*** -1.136** -1.183** -1.234** -1.319*** -0.916*<br />

(0.471) (0.473) (0.471) (0.469) (0.470) (0.479) (0.470) (0.470)<br />

Adjusted R-<br />

squared 0.2424 0.2462 0.2550 0.2664 0.2596 0.2498 0.2601 0.2889<br />

Controls<br />

included<br />

Basic<br />

demographics<br />

X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Childcare X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Outings score X X<br />

Maternal<br />

<strong>background</strong>.<br />

Neighbourhood X X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

110


8.3.6 Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> Peer Gro<strong>up</strong><br />

Table 8.8 completes this exercise by looking at how the relationships between neighbourhood<br />

<strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> attainment are related <strong>to</strong> other mediating influences. We have already<br />

seen in section 7 that the peer gro<strong>up</strong> within the pre-school provider plays a substantial role in<br />

how the neighbourhood impacts on <strong>child</strong>ren. However, apart from that impact, there is little<br />

further overlap with the other mediating influences: the point estimates on the educational<br />

domain score barely move as other fac<strong>to</strong>rs are added in. This leaves what drives the<br />

neighbourhood influences unclear.<br />

Table 8.8: Estimated peer <strong>and</strong> neighbourhood effects on Entry Assessment, various<br />

specifications (N = 5010)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Education domain<br />

score<br />

-0.919*** -0.853*** -0.924*** -0.841*** -0.740*** -0.812*** -0.836*** -0.787***<br />

(0.146) (0.147) (0.157) (0.144) (0.146) (0.147) (0.146) (0.156)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers – predicted component (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile 0.374 0.416 0.263 0.321 0.436 0.455 0.426 0.265<br />

(0.475) (0.474) (0.480) (0.469) (0.470) (0.474) (0.474) (0.473)<br />

3 rd quintile 1.436*** 1.403*** 0.965** 1.368*** 1.497*** 1.376*** 1.379*** 0.943**<br />

(0.469) (0.469) (0.477) (0.463) (0.465) (0.468) (0.468) (0.469)<br />

4 th quintile 2.481*** 2.406*** 1.919*** 2.444*** 2.455*** 2.335*** 2.338*** 1.857***<br />

(0.479) (0.478) (0.488) (0.472) (0.474) (0.478) (0.478) (0.481)<br />

Highest quintile 2.642*** 2.462*** 1.775*** 2.514*** 2.621*** 2.421*** 2.443*** 1.741***<br />

(0.484) (0.485) (0.509) (0.478) (0.482) (0.485) (0.485) (0.503)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers – residual component (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile -1.014** -1.046** -0.858* -1.032** -1.115** -1.037** -1.039** -0.894*<br />

(0.466) (0.465) (0.469) (0.460) (0.461) (0.465) (0.464) (0.463)<br />

3 rd quintile -0.647 -0.625 -0.475 -0.697 -0.731 -0.593 -0.583 -0.537<br />

(0.472) (0.471) (0.476) (0.466) (0.467) (0.471) (0.471) (0.470)<br />

4 th quintile -0.914* -0.906* -0.652 -0.942** -0.974** -0.885* -0.892* -0.677<br />

(0.472) (0.472) (0.478) (0.466) (0.468) (0.472) (0.471) (0.471)<br />

Highest quintile -0.702 -0.684 -0.555 -0.653 -0.775* -0.727 -0.643 -0.635<br />

(0.457) (0.457) (0.458) (0.452) (0.453) (0.457) (0.456) (0.452)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.2550 0.2573 0.2611 0.2758 0.2709 0.2604 0.2601 0.2889<br />

Controls included<br />

Basic demographics X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Childcare X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Outings & home<br />

environment<br />

Maternal<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Maternal health etc. X X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

The peer gro<strong>up</strong> influences are moderately affected by the inclusion of other mediating<br />

influences. The already weak relationship of the <strong>child</strong> cognitive outcome with the residual<br />

111


component of the peer gro<strong>up</strong> (that part not explained by parental <strong>background</strong>) is further reduced<br />

<strong>and</strong> its statistical significance becomes very borderline. The positive relationship of <strong>child</strong><br />

attainment with higher attaining peer gro<strong>up</strong>s is modestly reduced by the inclusion of the<br />

<strong>child</strong>care measures that include the type of provider that the peer gro<strong>up</strong> <strong>and</strong> the <strong>child</strong> attends.<br />

This is probably because attending a provider which has <strong>child</strong>ren drawn from families with<br />

characteristics that encour<strong>age</strong> attainment (high incomes, parental teaching, reading etc) has a<br />

strong influence on a <strong>child</strong>’s attainment.<br />

8.4 How do Parental Education <strong>and</strong> Income Influence a Child’s Early Development?<br />

Here we explore fully how income <strong>and</strong> parental education work through the proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

that we have studied here <strong>to</strong> produce variations in <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong>. We start by drawing<br />

<strong>to</strong>gether what all the fac<strong>to</strong>rs addressed here can say about early <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong>. Table 8.9<br />

decomposes the variation in Entry Assessment tests at <strong>age</strong> 4 <strong>to</strong> 5 in<strong>to</strong> that which all the data<br />

used here can explain <strong>and</strong> the remaining unexplained variation. This unexplained element will<br />

include the (no doubt) substantial measurement error (suggested by the only moderate<br />

correlation of EA with measured attainment two years later). Then we further decompose the<br />

explained variation in<strong>to</strong> the fac<strong>to</strong>rs underlying it. Throughout we focus on parental education<br />

<strong>and</strong> family income.<br />

The first block of results (columns 1 <strong>to</strong> 6) looks at the <strong>to</strong>tal variation in <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

starts by showing that the attainment gap between the aver<strong>age</strong> scores in <strong>to</strong>p <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quintiles<br />

is some 25 points (or 2.5 st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations). The <strong>child</strong>’s <strong>age</strong> when tested explains around 2.6<br />

points (or 10%) of this variation. The first three columns show that if only family income <strong>and</strong><br />

parental education are considered (only conditioning on <strong>age</strong> of <strong>child</strong> at assessment) then only<br />

11% of the gap between the best <strong>and</strong> worst performers is explained by parental education <strong>and</strong><br />

family income (2.6 points out of the 22.8 points gap after the <strong>age</strong> variation of <strong>child</strong>ren is<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>red out). The inclusion of all other demographic <strong>and</strong> proximal influences observed in the<br />

ALSPAC data raises the percent<strong>age</strong> variation explained (other than by <strong>age</strong> at assessment) <strong>to</strong><br />

23% (the sum of the values in column 5 excluding that for one of <strong>child</strong> at assessment). So just<br />

over three quarters of the <strong>to</strong>tal gap is unexplained. This unexplained variation will include<br />

measurement errors, genetic fac<strong>to</strong>rs, unmeasured fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> specific variation.<br />

Nevertheless, explaining a quarter of the <strong>to</strong>tal difference between the best <strong>and</strong> poorest<br />

performing <strong>child</strong>ren is substantial.<br />

112


The largest contribution <strong>to</strong> this explained variation is made by the basic demographic fac<strong>to</strong>rs, in<br />

particular gender, <strong>age</strong> of mother, numbers of siblings <strong>and</strong> low birth weight. These fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

explain 5% of the <strong>to</strong>tal attainment gap (6% once <strong>age</strong> variation is netted out) <strong>and</strong> around 25% of<br />

the <strong>to</strong>tal contribution made by all observed characteristics within the data (other than <strong>age</strong> of<br />

<strong>child</strong>). These fac<strong>to</strong>rs are largely unalterable, except perhaps teen<strong>age</strong> births or large families.<br />

After this, three gro<strong>up</strong>s of influences are of broadly similar magnitude, these being parental<br />

education, parenting behaviours <strong>and</strong> the neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong> (neighbourhood <strong>and</strong><br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong> in turn contribute roughly equally <strong>to</strong> this effect). The home environment, <strong>child</strong>care<br />

<strong>and</strong> residual family income experienced make modest contributions <strong>to</strong> the attainment gap with<br />

mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health proving the least important. Note that in this context the<br />

discussion of income <strong>and</strong> parental education are the residual contributions uncorrelated with the<br />

other proximal influences. Our set of proximal influences (neighbourhood, <strong>child</strong>care, parenting<br />

behaviours, the home environment, mothers <strong>up</strong>bringing <strong>and</strong> her mental <strong>and</strong> physical health) can<br />

explain around 13% of the attainment gap between the <strong>to</strong>p <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m fifths of <strong>child</strong> by<br />

attainment (after netting out <strong>age</strong> at time test taken).<br />

So parental teaching <strong>and</strong> reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren are the strongest routes of influence observable of<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren doing well, in terms of educational attainment, on school entry. The peer gro<strong>up</strong> which<br />

the <strong>child</strong> attends pre-school with <strong>and</strong> neighbourhood of residence are also very strong<br />

influences. The home environment (books, <strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong> trips <strong>to</strong> the library) is the next most<br />

important gro<strong>up</strong> of influences (note these are strongly related <strong>to</strong> family income). The <strong>child</strong>care<br />

experienced (other than through its association with peer gro<strong>up</strong>) prior <strong>to</strong> school entry is of<br />

modest importance. The mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> her mental <strong>and</strong> physical health are minor<br />

influences. <strong>Family</strong> income <strong>and</strong> parental education continue <strong>to</strong> have influences on attainment<br />

over <strong>and</strong> above through the mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs but the role of income is modest. The residual<br />

effect of parental education could reflect that that better educated parents undertake the same<br />

mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs more effectively or there are other influences not captured in our model (an<br />

obvious c<strong>and</strong>idate is genetic influences). We explored whether there was clear evidence of<br />

whether better educated parents undertake some activities more effectively through interactions<br />

<strong>and</strong> this hypothesis was generally rejected. So whilst it could be the case that better educated<br />

parents are doing everything better <strong>to</strong> same degree, the lack of variations across different routes<br />

makes it more probable that the remaining influence of parental education reflects unmeasured<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs.<br />

113


Table 8.9: Decomposition of range of mean scores for various gro<strong>up</strong>s (Entry Assessment)<br />

Difference in mean<br />

scores<br />

Explained variation –<br />

contribution of:<br />

Age of <strong>child</strong> at<br />

assessment<br />

Income<br />

Points<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of attainment<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Points<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of<br />

household income<br />

Points<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Children of mothers with<br />

CSE/no qualifications vs. a<br />

degree<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

Points<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

25.4 100 100 25.4 100 100 6.6 100 100 9.5 100 100<br />

2.6 10.2 50.4 2.6 10.1 32.2 -0.1 -1.9 -2.0 -0.1 -0.7 -0.7<br />

0.7 2.7 13.6 0.3 1.3 4.1 1.6 24.7 25.2 0.7 7.4 7.4<br />

Parental Education 1.9 7.3 36.0 0.9 3.4 10.8 1.5 22.2 22.7 3.8 40.4 40.4<br />

Other Demographic<br />

Variables<br />

- - - 1.3 5.0 15.8 0.7 10.0 10.2 0.6 6.7 6.7<br />

Childcare - - - 0.3 1.3 4.3 0.3 4.6 4.7 0.4 4.2 4.2<br />

Parenting - - - 0.8 3.2 10.1 0.4 5.9 6.0 0.9 9.1 9.1<br />

Outings & Home<br />

Environment<br />

Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

<strong>and</strong> family<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Maternal health,<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> social<br />

networks<br />

Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong><br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong><br />

Unexplained<br />

variation<br />

- - - 0.6 2.3 7.4 0.5 8.1 8.2 1.1 11.1 11.1<br />

- - - 0.3 1.1 3.4 0.3 4.9 5.0 0.6 6.3 6.3<br />

- - - 0.2 0.7 2.2 0.3 4.5 4.6 0.4 4.3 4.3<br />

- - - 0.8 3.0 9.6 1.0 14.9 15.2 1.1 11.3 11.3<br />

20.3 79.7 - 17.5 68.7 - 0.1 2.1 - 0.0 0.0 -<br />

Focusing on parental education <strong>and</strong> family income, we can explore how the observed mediating<br />

routes transmit education <strong>and</strong> income in<strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong>’s attainment. The second block of Table 8.9<br />

does this for family income. It shows that the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest quartiles of the income<br />

distribution have aver<strong>age</strong> attainment gap of 6.6 points. So those from deprived <strong>and</strong> affluent<br />

families are about two thirds of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation apart on school entry. Parental education<br />

<strong>and</strong> demographic characteristics of the family, especially numbers of siblings <strong>and</strong> mother’s <strong>age</strong>,<br />

are important determinants of this gap. These reflect influences on attainment, which although<br />

correlated with income, are probably causal in themselves rather than being a reflection of how<br />

income impacts on <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>. The number of siblings could be an exception <strong>to</strong><br />

this general rule as more <strong>child</strong>ren mean available income is being spread more thinly. The<br />

mother’s <strong>up</strong>bringing, which reflects the impact of her pre-birth deprivation on the attainment of<br />

the <strong>child</strong>, is also unlikely <strong>to</strong> be influenced by current income changes. Rather it reflects<br />

intergenerational effects of deprivation (though note it only makes a small residual<br />

114


contribution). Of the proximal influences where income may well make a difference, the<br />

strongest are the neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>, the home environment (books, <strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong><br />

outings) <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>care. These are plausible routes by which income impacts on <strong>child</strong><br />

<strong>development</strong> irrespective of other family circumstances. Parenting, <strong>and</strong> mother’s mental <strong>and</strong><br />

physical health play s<strong>up</strong>porting roles. So whilst it is possible that parenting styles <strong>and</strong> the mental<br />

health of the mother improve with higher incomes, it is less obvious that these fac<strong>to</strong>rs link<br />

income <strong>and</strong> attainment. Perhaps the most important result here is that although parenting<br />

behaviour is very important in driving a <strong>child</strong>’s pre-school attainment, it is not strongly related<br />

<strong>to</strong> family income. Nor, as noted above, is there any evidence that better educated families teach<br />

pre-school <strong>child</strong>ren more effectively than those with lesser educational qualifications. The<br />

identified family income effects are operating more substantially through the neighbourhood<br />

<strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>, the home environment <strong>and</strong> pre-school <strong>child</strong>care. What is striking about the<br />

neighbourhood effects is that once we have conditioned on family income <strong>and</strong> the basic<br />

demographics all the other proximal influences considered have little or no conditional<br />

correlation with neighbourhood. This suggests that measures of family functioning <strong>and</strong> home<br />

circumstances are not correlated with the neighbourhood effects observed.<br />

Of the <strong>to</strong>tal 6.6 point attainment gap between rich <strong>and</strong> poor some 1.6 points, or a quarter, is<br />

unrelated <strong>to</strong> the other influences considered. This residual component can represent three<br />

possible fac<strong>to</strong>rs. First, there are missing elements or mechanisms driven by income; second,<br />

high income families do the mechanisms reported here more effectively in an additive way or<br />

third, there are unmeasured <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs correlated with income which mean that the<br />

residual income effects are not causal. Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>, the home environment<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>care are mediating influences with strong monetary components. These fac<strong>to</strong>rs account<br />

for 26% of the raw correlation between income <strong>and</strong> attainment. Including mother’s mental<br />

health <strong>and</strong> parenting behaviours would raise this <strong>to</strong> around 35% but perhaps the lower value is<br />

closer <strong>to</strong> the truth. Note that another 25% of the original variation in attainment correlated with<br />

income remains attributed <strong>to</strong> family income. This residual correlation then reflects neither<br />

parental education, parenting styles nor other influences studied here. This <strong>to</strong>o could be a causal<br />

influence of income but its status is uncertain. So, at most, around half of the raw difference in<br />

attainment between the richest <strong>and</strong> poorest pre-school <strong>child</strong>ren can be thought of as genuinely<br />

driven by income, with a lower bound of around a quarter 10 .<br />

10 US welfare experimental studies suggest that raising income as a result of in-work welfare s<strong>up</strong>plements raising<br />

<strong>child</strong> attainment measures among pre-school <strong>child</strong>ren (see Clark-Kauffman et al. (2003). This research suggests<br />

that around a 20% increase in income results in improved attainment of the order of 5 <strong>to</strong> 7 per cent of a st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

deviation. The results here suggest that increased income from the <strong>to</strong>p <strong>to</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quintiles, approximately a tripling<br />

115


Moving on <strong>to</strong> parental education, the gap in attainment between <strong>child</strong>ren of mothers with CSE<br />

level qualifications or lower (i.e. below GCSE level A-C) <strong>and</strong> degree holding mothers is much<br />

larger than the gap between the poorest <strong>and</strong> richest families at 9.5 points. Around 40% of this<br />

raw unconditional gap is not explained by other fac<strong>to</strong>rs observed in the data. So the proximal<br />

influences considered do more work in explaining how parental education is transmitted <strong>to</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>ren than they do for income. If we include residual income as another mechanism this rises<br />

<strong>to</strong> nearly half. Of the proximal influences the most important contribution comes from the home<br />

environment, parenting behaviours (especially teaching <strong>and</strong> reading), <strong>and</strong> the<br />

neighbourhood/peer gro<strong>up</strong>. The residual correlation with parental education could reflect<br />

omitted fac<strong>to</strong>rs such as genetic inheritance, other transmission mechanisms not covered here, or<br />

that the better educated impart learning in general way unrelated <strong>to</strong> the mechanisms considered<br />

here. Nevertheless, 60% of the early learning deficit associated with less educated parents is<br />

explained <strong>and</strong> the results suggest that the home environment (which is strongly related <strong>to</strong><br />

income), parenting behaviours, family income, neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> the peer gro<strong>up</strong> the <strong>child</strong> is<br />

attending pre-school with are the main drivers. Early differences in pre-school <strong>child</strong>care<br />

arrangements are only a minor fac<strong>to</strong>r.<br />

8.5 Behaviour<br />

We repeat this analysis for the behaviour scores. The range of fac<strong>to</strong>rs found <strong>to</strong> have an influence<br />

on behaviour (conditional only on basic demographics) is substantially different than for<br />

educational attainment <strong>and</strong> these are shown in Table 8.10. In particular, mother’s physical <strong>and</strong><br />

mental health <strong>and</strong> within family conflict issues are far more important for behaviour than for<br />

attainment, as are mother’s attitude <strong>to</strong>, or bonding with, the <strong>child</strong> <strong>and</strong> discipline strategies.<br />

Basic Demographics <strong>and</strong> Behaviour Outcomes<br />

Table 8.11 reports the coefficients on the basic demographic variables as each gro<strong>up</strong> of<br />

proximal variables is introduced in<strong>to</strong> the model separately <strong>and</strong> then combined in the final<br />

of income, would raise attainment by 34% of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation (the unexplained component plus the correlations<br />

with <strong>child</strong>care <strong>and</strong> home environment which are plausibly driven by income). Whilst not strictly comparable, the<br />

order of magnitude is not way out of line. Interestingly this literature also highlights the roles of <strong>child</strong>care <strong>and</strong><br />

outings as routes by which income changes influence attainment. The Moving To Opportunity experiments in the<br />

US where some families are moved out of ghet<strong>to</strong> areas in<strong>to</strong> more affluent ones on a r<strong>and</strong>om basis has also been<br />

shown <strong>to</strong> improve education outcomes but results are for primary school <strong>age</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren (see Goering <strong>and</strong> Feins, 2003,<br />

for a discussion of MTO results).<br />

116


column. The aim is <strong>to</strong> show how the influence of <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs is mediated by the<br />

proximal variables.<br />

8.5.1 Parental Education<br />

Parental education was not strongly related <strong>to</strong> behavioural outcomes of the <strong>child</strong>ren in our<br />

earlier analyses. The role of parental education is further diminished when the gro<strong>up</strong>s of<br />

proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs are introduced. Perhaps surprisingly, parenting behaviours - which include<br />

discipline mechanisms <strong>and</strong> mothers bonding with the <strong>child</strong> - are not important in this process.<br />

Rather it is the home environment (<strong>to</strong>ys, books etc) <strong>and</strong> mothers mental health <strong>and</strong> selfconfidence<br />

that are important for behaviour. So the influence of parental education on behaviour<br />

appears <strong>to</strong> work mainly through the home environment, mother’s locus of control, self-esteem<br />

<strong>and</strong> reduced depression/anxiety. When all other fac<strong>to</strong>rs are included parental education has a<br />

very modest residual effect.<br />

Table 8.10: Gro<strong>up</strong>s of variables used as controls in Behaviour regressions<br />

Basic<br />

demographics<br />

Mother’s<br />

highest<br />

qualification<br />

Partner’s<br />

highest<br />

qualification<br />

Lone parent<br />

status<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong><br />

at birth<br />

Ethnicity<br />

Gender<br />

Birth weight<br />

Special care<br />

unit at birth<br />

Younger<br />

siblings by 47<br />

months<br />

Older siblings<br />

Income Childcare Parenting<br />

Log aver<strong>age</strong><br />

weekly net<br />

income<br />

Financial<br />

difficulties prebirth<br />

Care by partner<br />

pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Care by<br />

friend/relative pre<strong>age</strong><br />

2<br />

Care by paid<br />

person pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Centre-based care<br />

pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Nursery class <strong>age</strong><br />

3/ 4<br />

LEA nursery<br />

school <strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Private day<br />

nursery <strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Nursery (type<br />

undefined) <strong>age</strong> 3/<br />

4<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> <strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Other centre-based<br />

care <strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Maternal<br />

reading score<br />

Paternal<br />

reading score<br />

Teaching score<br />

Talking whilst<br />

occ<strong>up</strong>ied score<br />

18 month TV<br />

score<br />

38 month TV<br />

score<br />

Smoked in<br />

pregnancy<br />

Maternal<br />

bonding score<br />

Types of<br />

discipline<br />

117<br />

Outings Scores<br />

& Home<br />

Environment<br />

Park/playground<br />

outings score<br />

Visits <strong>to</strong><br />

friends/relatives<br />

outings score<br />

Places of interest<br />

outings score<br />

Places of<br />

entertainment<br />

outings score<br />

Book score<br />

Toy score<br />

Damp/<br />

condensation/<br />

mould<br />

Mother’s<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong><br />

family<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Mother’s<br />

relationship with<br />

mother score<br />

Mother has a<br />

his<strong>to</strong>ry of<br />

truancy<br />

Mother feels<br />

school was a<br />

valuable<br />

experience<br />

Maternal<br />

health,<br />

attitudes<br />

<strong>and</strong> social<br />

networks<br />

Mother’s<br />

physical<br />

health<br />

Social<br />

networks<br />

score<br />

Social<br />

s<strong>up</strong>port score<br />

Total CCEI<br />

score<br />

Self-esteem<br />

score<br />

Locus of<br />

control score<br />

Mother’s<br />

satisfaction<br />

with<br />

relationship<br />

Partner’s<br />

employment at<br />

47 months<br />

8.5.2 Lone Parenthood<br />

Children in lone parent families exhibit substantially worse behaviour scores but these appear <strong>to</strong><br />

mainly <strong>to</strong> be working through parenting differences <strong>and</strong> family income. Lone parents with a<br />

Neighbourhood<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> behaviour<br />

score of peers –<br />

predicted component<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> behaviour<br />

score of peers –<br />

residual component


new partner at 47 months are still observed as having moderately worse behaviour scores, which<br />

may reflect tensions brought on by re-partnering. Mother’s mental health <strong>and</strong> self-esteem/locus<br />

of control scores also appears <strong>to</strong> be an important mediating influence for the impact of lone<br />

parent status on <strong>child</strong> behaviour.<br />

8.5.3 Mother’s Age<br />

Teen mothers <strong>and</strong>, <strong>to</strong> a slightly lesser degree, mothers in their early 20s have <strong>child</strong>ren with<br />

substantially worse behaviour scores. This is transmitted through differences in parenting<br />

behaviour, especially discipline <strong>and</strong> maternal bonding <strong>and</strong> in mother’s mental health <strong>and</strong> selfesteem.<br />

So, conditional on these proximal routes, teen motherhood has only a modest<br />

independent effect on behaviour <strong>and</strong> motherhood in the early 20s have no adverse effects. Note<br />

these mothers have very low scores on maternal bonding self-esteem <strong>and</strong> locus of control.<br />

Hence the bulk of the relationship between teen motherhood <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s behaviour is<br />

explained by our mediating influences.<br />

8.5.4 Ethnicity, Gender <strong>and</strong> Siblings <strong>and</strong> a Non-Working partner<br />

Ethnicity is not a predic<strong>to</strong>r of behaviour problems at <strong>age</strong> 4 <strong>and</strong> the impact of low birth weight<br />

<strong>and</strong> gender are again largely unaffected by these proximal controls. Numbers of siblings are not<br />

strongly associated with behaviour variation, but the extent <strong>to</strong> which they are associated reflects<br />

poor mother mental health <strong>and</strong> family conflict scores. A non-working partner is also associated<br />

with poorer behaviour scores. Again this is substantially operating through mother’s mental<br />

health <strong>and</strong> family conflict. Income also plays a role here.<br />

118


Table 8.11: Estimated effects of basic demographics on Behaviour, various specifications<br />

(N = 9416)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)<br />

Mother’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none 1.071*** 0.908*** 1.034***<br />

(0.317) (0.316) (0.318)<br />

A-level -1.253*** -1.069*** -1.262***<br />

(0.268) (0.269) (0.271)<br />

Degree -1.841*** -1.570*** -1.763***<br />

(0.378) (0.380) (0.388)<br />

Partner’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none 0.249 0.041 0.234<br />

(0.311) (0.310) (0.311)<br />

A-level -0.608** -0.439 -0.607**<br />

(0.279) (0.278) (0.279)<br />

Degree -0.751** -0.329 -0.667*<br />

(0.356) (0.360) (0.360)<br />

Lone parent status (base = never lone parent)<br />

Intermittently, with<br />

partner at 47 months<br />

2.209***<br />

(0.585)<br />

Intermittently, no 1.544***<br />

partner at 47 months (0.455)<br />

Since birth 2.511***<br />

(0.955)<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth (base = 25-29)<br />

1 0.798 0.635<br />

(0.563) (0.561)<br />

Older siblings (base = none)<br />

1 1.275***<br />

(0.253)<br />

2 0.795**<br />

(0.333)<br />

3+ 1.065**<br />

(0.490)<br />

1.111***<br />

(0.252)<br />

0.618*<br />

(0.332)<br />

0.763<br />

(0.488)<br />

1.954***<br />

(0.590)<br />

1.358***<br />

(0.461)<br />

2.224**<br />

(0.969)<br />

2.982***<br />

(0.653)<br />

1.263***<br />

(0.312)<br />

-0.049<br />

(0.251)<br />

0.070<br />

(0.362)<br />

0.523<br />

(0.544)<br />

-2.501***<br />

(0.202)<br />

0.848<br />

(0.571)<br />

0.536**<br />

(0.212)<br />

0.560<br />

(0.473)<br />

0.692***<br />

(0.240)<br />

0.860<br />

(0.564)<br />

1.373***<br />

(0.258)<br />

0.930***<br />

(0.339)<br />

1.157**<br />

(0.496)<br />

Partner’s employment status at 47 months (base = in work)<br />

Not in work 1.904*** 1.151*** 1.848***<br />

(0.377) (0.392) (0.379)<br />

1.074***<br />

(0.296)<br />

-0.950***<br />

(0.251)<br />

-1.695***<br />

(0.356)<br />

0.260<br />

(0.289)<br />

-0.334<br />

(0.259)<br />

-0.637*<br />

(0.333)<br />

1.370**<br />

(0.599)<br />

0.660<br />

(0.513)<br />

1.212<br />

(0.916)<br />

2.092***<br />

(0.606)<br />

0.807***<br />

(0.291)<br />

0.126<br />

(0.233)<br />

0.680**<br />

(0.335)<br />

0.205<br />

(0.503)<br />

-1.785***<br />

(0.189)<br />

0.407<br />

(0.532)<br />

0.418**<br />

(0.198)<br />

0.593<br />

(0.440)<br />

0.607***<br />

(0.224)<br />

0.781<br />

(0.524)<br />

0.620**<br />

(0.243)<br />

0.622*<br />

(0.321)<br />

0.977**<br />

(0.465)<br />

1.630***<br />

(0.351)<br />

0.687**<br />

(0.314)<br />

-0.805***<br />

(0.267)<br />

-1.389***<br />

(0.377)<br />

0.033<br />

(0.306)<br />

-0.391<br />

(0.275)<br />

-0.311<br />

(0.353)<br />

2.334***<br />

(0.577)<br />

1.487***<br />

(0.455)<br />

1.875**<br />

(0.942)<br />

2.830***<br />

(0.641)<br />

1.211***<br />

(0.307)<br />

-0.048<br />

(0.248)<br />

0.022<br />

(0.357)<br />

0.216<br />

(0.536)<br />

-2.367***<br />

(0.200)<br />

0.714<br />

(0.562)<br />

0.458**<br />

(0.209)<br />

0.533<br />

(0.466)<br />

0.661***<br />

(0.236)<br />

0.715<br />

(0.555)<br />

1.021***<br />

(0.261)<br />

0.353<br />

(0.342)<br />

0.245<br />

(0.495)<br />

1.495***<br />

(0.373)<br />

0.535*<br />

(0.316)<br />

-0.936***<br />

(0.266)<br />

-1.336***<br />

(0.379)<br />

0.248<br />

(0.306)<br />

-0.664**<br />

(0.274)<br />

-0.756**<br />

(0.351)<br />

1.954***<br />

(0.577)<br />

1.308***<br />

(0.456)<br />

1.926**<br />

(0.941)<br />

2.529***<br />

(0.642)<br />

1.020***<br />

(0.307)<br />

-0.230<br />

(0.246)<br />

-0.291<br />

(0.354)<br />

0.383<br />

(0.534)<br />

-2.506***<br />

(0.199)<br />

0.862<br />

(0.562)<br />

0.556***<br />

(0.209)<br />

0.423<br />

(0.466)<br />

0.680***<br />

(0.236)<br />

0.602<br />

(0.555)<br />

1.141***<br />

(0.249)<br />

0.462<br />

(0.329)<br />

0.468<br />

(0.484)<br />

1.658***<br />

(0.372)<br />

0.518*<br />

(0.303)<br />

-0.893***<br />

(0.256)<br />

-1.441***<br />

(0.362)<br />

0.151<br />

(0.293)<br />

-0.522**<br />

(0.263)<br />

-0.454<br />

(0.337)<br />

1.833***<br />

(0.597)<br />

0.266<br />

(0.480)<br />

1.428<br />

(0.989)<br />

2.347***<br />

(0.613)<br />

0.763***<br />

(0.294)<br />

-0.111<br />

(0.236)<br />

-0.336<br />

(0.339)<br />

0.130<br />

(0.513)<br />

-2.474***<br />

(0.191)<br />

0.761<br />

(0.538)<br />

0.548***<br />

(0.200)<br />

0.264<br />

(0.446)<br />

0.883***<br />

(0.226)<br />

1.005*<br />

(0.531)<br />

0.886***<br />

(0.239)<br />

0.292<br />

(0.315)<br />

0.272<br />

(0.462)<br />

1.167***<br />

(0.357)<br />

1.011***<br />

(0.317)<br />

-1.226***<br />

(0.269)<br />

-1.815***<br />

(0.379)<br />

0.228<br />

(0.311)<br />

-0.621**<br />

(0.279)<br />

-0.798**<br />

(0.358)<br />

2.125***<br />

(0.585)<br />

1.450***<br />

(0.456)<br />

2.391**<br />

(0.955)<br />

2.992***<br />

(0.650)<br />

1.242***<br />

(0.311)<br />

-0.063<br />

(0.251)<br />

-0.056<br />

(0.360)<br />

0.666<br />

(0.542)<br />

-2.512***<br />

(0.202)<br />

0.834<br />

(0.571)<br />

0.528**<br />

(0.212)<br />

0.513<br />

(0.473)<br />

0.692***<br />

(0.239)<br />

0.748<br />

(0.563)<br />

1.280***<br />

(0.252)<br />

0.781**<br />

(0.333)<br />

0.998**<br />

(0.490)<br />

1.831***<br />

(0.378)<br />

0.410<br />

(0.296)<br />

-0.465*<br />

(0.251)<br />

-0.891**<br />

(0.365)<br />

0.129<br />

(0.282)<br />

-0.264<br />

(0.252)<br />

-0.209<br />

(0.331)<br />

1.110*<br />

(0.638)<br />

-0.206<br />

(0.563)<br />

0.340<br />

(1.012)<br />

1.450**<br />

(0.600)<br />

0.421<br />

(0.286)<br />

0.142<br />

(0.228)<br />

0.409<br />

(0.332)<br />

-0.273<br />

(0.494)<br />

-1.858***<br />

(0.184)<br />

0.330<br />

(0.516)<br />

0.448**<br />

(0.192)<br />

0.391<br />

(0.427)<br />

0.606***<br />

(0.218)<br />

0.654<br />

(0.510)<br />

0.541**<br />

(0.250)<br />

0.270<br />

(0.327)<br />

0.223<br />

(0.466)<br />

0.985***<br />

(0.357)<br />

Adjusted R2 0.0525 0.0618 0.0546 0.1861 0.0838 0.0828 0.1612 0.0554 0.2416<br />

Controls included<br />

Income<br />

Childcare<br />

Parenting<br />

Outings & home env<br />

Maternal <strong>background</strong><br />

Maternal health etc.<br />

Neighbourhood<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

119


8.5.5 Childcare <strong>and</strong> Behaviour<br />

The use of friends <strong>and</strong> relative carers <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong> also centre based care are weakly<br />

associated with adverse behavioural outcomes before other proximal influences are controlled<br />

for. There is little evidence of behaviour variations associated with care immediately prior <strong>to</strong><br />

school entry (outside specialist or social service run care). Table 8.12 introduces the other<br />

proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> the weak negative results <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 are largely unaffected suggesting no<br />

correlation with other proximal influences <strong>and</strong> that it is early <strong>child</strong>care of these forms which is<br />

slightly damaging. The results for pre-school <strong>child</strong>care disappear once parenting is controlled<br />

for. Poor mother’s mental health has the same effect. This suggests that LEA nursery <strong>and</strong> other<br />

care types are not the driver of these adverse correlations but parenting behaviours <strong>and</strong> mothers<br />

poor mental health, locus of control etc., which is consistent with those using these types of care<br />

having special needs identified by social services.<br />

8.5.6 Parenting <strong>and</strong> Behaviour<br />

Table 8.13 repeats this process focusing on the parenting measures. These are very strong<br />

drivers of behaviour scores, especially maternal bonding at 8 months. The introduction of other<br />

proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs tends <strong>to</strong> reduce this a little. Mother’s mental health, self-esteem, locus of<br />

control <strong>and</strong> family conflict are the key correlated influences. The results suggest that poor<br />

physical health, maternal depression <strong>and</strong> low self-esteem intertwine with weaker parenting<br />

behaviours <strong>to</strong> produce adverse behavioural scores.<br />

120


Table 8.12: Estimated effects of <strong>child</strong>care arrangements on Behaviour, various<br />

specifications (N = 9416)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Childcare pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Used partner 5-19 hours<br />

per week<br />

Used partner 20+ hours per<br />

week<br />

Used friend/relative 5-19<br />

hours per week<br />

Used friend/relative 20+<br />

hours per week<br />

Used paid person 5-19<br />

hours per week<br />

Used paid person 20+<br />

hours per week<br />

Used centre-based care 5-<br />

19 hours per week<br />

Used centre-based care 20+<br />

hours per week<br />

Childcare <strong>age</strong> 3 <strong>to</strong> 4<br />

-0.212 -0.236 -0.148 -0.185 -0.221 -0.189 -0.239 -0.151<br />

(0.279) (0.277) (0.260) (0.274) (0.274) (0.263) (0.277) (0.254)<br />

-0.473* -0.504* -0.205 -0.458* -0.562** -0.371 -0.501* -0.267<br />

(0.277) (0.275) (0.261) (0.272) (0.272) (0.261) (0.275) (0.254)<br />

0.550** 0.573** 0.392 0.754*** 0.639** 0.727*** 0.579** 0.651***<br />

(0.258) (0.257) (0.240) (0.255) (0.254) (0.244) (0.257) (0.235)<br />

0.785*** 0.881*** 0.776*** 0.922*** 0.970*** 0.919*** 0.876*** 0.893***<br />

(0.278) (0.277) (0.259) (0.275) (0.274) (0.263) (0.277) (0.253)<br />

0.167 0.331 -0.145 0.293 0.136 0.293 0.297 -0.111<br />

(0.386) (0.385) (0.359) (0.380) (0.380) (0.365) (0.385) (0.349)<br />

-0.058 0.276 -0.093 0.203 0.151 0.180 0.236 -0.081<br />

(0.324) (0.326) (0.305) (0.323) (0.322) (0.310) (0.326) (0.299)<br />

1.500*** 1.638*** 1.065** 1.735*** 1.463*** 1.305*** 1.651*** 1.050**<br />

(0.476) (0.474) (0.442) (0.468) (0.468) (0.449) (0.474) (0.430)<br />

-0.157 0.114 -0.139 -0.055 0.066 -0.153 0.119 -0.289<br />

(0.607) (0.606) (0.565) (0.599) (0.597) (0.574) (0.605) (0.551)<br />

Nursery class 0.046 -0.020 -0.157 0.127 -0.046 -0.093 0.029 -0.069<br />

(0.391) (0.389) (0.362) (0.384) (0.384) (0.368) (0.399) (0.361)<br />

LEA nursery school 1.239*** 1.062** 0.491 1.087** 0.887* 0.496 1.219** 0.419<br />

(0.468) (0.466) (0.435) (0.461) (0.460) (0.442) (0.494) (0.450)<br />

Private day nursery 0.106 0.171 0.039 0.516 0.199 0.053 0.250 0.111<br />

(0.398) (0.397) (0.369) (0.392) (0.391) (0.376) (0.406) (0.369)<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> 0.244 0.295 -0.049 0.516* 0.428 0.589** 0.380 0.373<br />

(0.309) (0.308) (0.287) (0.304) (0.304) (0.292) (0.310) (0.281)<br />

Nursery (type undefined) 0.311 0.335 0.026 0.433 0.319 0.147 0.221 -0.049<br />

(0.331) (0.330) (0.307) (0.325) (0.325) (0.312) (0.331) (0.300)<br />

Other 2.196** 2.044*** 0.993 1.965** 1.624* 1.193 2.387** 1.116<br />

(0.992) (0.988) (0.920) (0.974) (0.974) (0.936) (0.998) (0.905)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.0546 0.0641 0.1916 0.0919 0.0909 0.1635 0.0670 0.2416<br />

Controls included<br />

Basic demographics X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Outings & home<br />

environment<br />

Maternal <strong>background</strong> X X<br />

Maternal health etc. X X<br />

Neighbourhood X X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

121


Table 8.13: Estimated effects of parenting behaviours on Behaviour, various specifications<br />

(N = 9416)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Maternal reading score (base = high)<br />

Very low<br />

2.118*** 2.083*** 2.052*** 1.565*** 1.852*** 1.766*** 2.038*** 1.357***<br />

(0.415) (0.414) (0.416) (0.425) (0.412) (0.405) (0.414) (0.417)<br />

Low<br />

1.639*** 1.656*** 1.620*** 1.286*** 1.446*** 1.390*** 1.632*** 1.029***<br />

(0.377) (0.376) (0.377) (0.378) (0.375) (0.368) (0.376) (0.370)<br />

Medium<br />

0.369 0.397 0.383 0.110 0.325 0.349 0.358 0.090<br />

(0.255) (0.255) (0.255) (0.257) (0.253) (0.248) (0.254) (0.252)<br />

Paternal reading score (base = 2)<br />

0.985*** 0.947*** 0.918*** 0.818** 0.936*** 0.625* 0.954*** 0.530<br />

0 (0.345) (0.344) (0.346) (0.343) (0.342) (0.337) (0.344) (0.338)<br />

0.287 0.340 0.346 0.295 0.366 0.155 0.355 0.171<br />

1 (0.334) (0.333) (0.334) (0.332) (0.332) (0.326) (0.333) (0.325)<br />

-0.575* -0.521* -0.505* -0.411 -0.414 -0.265 -0.523* -0.145<br />

3 (0.302) (0.301) (0.301) (0.300) (0.299) (0.295) (0.301) (0.294)<br />

-0.942** -0.915** -0.927** -0.629 -0.839** -0.504 -0.927** -0.281<br />

4 (0.403) (0.401) (0.403) (0.401) (0.399) (0.393) (0.401) (0.395)<br />

Teaching score (base = high)<br />

Very low<br />

1.567*** 1.536*** 1.515*** 1.027** 1.422*** 1.556*** 1.498*** 1.119***<br />

(0.418) (0.417) (0.418) (0.420) (0.415) (0.408) (0.417) (0.411)<br />

Low<br />

1.135*** 1.155*** 1.206*** 0.799** 1.068*** 1.072*** 1.174*** 0.843**<br />

(0.353) (0.352) (0.352) (0.354) (0.350) (0.344) (0.351) (0.346)<br />

Medium<br />

0.478** 0.503** 0.522** 0.318 0.431* 0.546** 0.497** 0.369*<br />

(0.229) (0.228) (0.228) (0.228) (0.227) (0.223) (0.228) (0.223)<br />

Talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied score (base = high)<br />

Low<br />

2.579*** 2.526*** 2.561*** 2.264*** 2.261*** 2.193*** 2.523*** 1.936***<br />

(0.365) (0.364) (0.365) (0.364) (0.363) (0.356) (0.364) (0.357)<br />

Medium<br />

1.057*** 1.042*** 1.051*** 0.911*** 0.954*** 0.898*** 1.038*** 0.801***<br />

(0.223) (0.223) (0.223) (0.222) (0.221) (0.218) (0.222) (0.218)<br />

38 month TV score (base =


Table 8.14: Estimated effects of Home Environment <strong>and</strong> Outings scores on Behaviour,<br />

various specifications (N = 9416)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Outings scores<br />

Park/playground -0.978*** -0.964*** -0.982*** -0.548** -0.924*** -0.736*** -0.938*** -0.453**<br />

(0.230) (0.229) (0.230) (0.219) (0.227) (0.219) (0.230) (0.214)<br />

Visits <strong>to</strong><br />

-0.692*** -0.680*** -0.744*** -0.523** -0.479** -0.214 -0.672*** -0.304<br />

friends/relatives (0.221) (0.220) (0.223) (0.207) (0.218) (0.212) (0.220) (0.207)<br />

Places of interest -1.141*** -1.082*** -1.096*** -0.796*** -1.039*** -0.803*** -1.076*** -0.624***<br />

(0.258) (0.258) (0.258) (0.243) (0.255) (0.246) (0.258) (0.239)<br />

Places of entertainment -0.389 -0.357 -0.436* -0.489** -0.406* -0.452* -0.357 -0.572**<br />

(0.249) (0.249) (0.249) (0.234) (0.246) (0.237) (0.249) (0.229)<br />

Book score (base = high)<br />

Very low 2.112*** 2.000*** 1.970*** 0.436 1.763*** 1.235*** 1.951*** 0.064<br />

(0.468) (0.467) (0.467) (0.456) (0.462) (0.447) (0.467) (0.447)<br />

Low 1.736*** 1.689*** 1.652*** 0.839*** 1.511*** 1.254*** 1.655*** 0.610**<br />

(0.291) (0.291) (0.291) (0.281) (0.288) (0.278) (0.291) (0.276)<br />

Medium 0.711** 0.689** 0.697** 0.293 0.606** 0.279 0.641** 0.029<br />

(0.297) (0.296) (0.296) (0.280) (0.293) (0.283) (0.296) (0.274)<br />

Toy score (base = high)<br />

Very low 3.147*** 3.088*** 3.028*** 1.694*** 2.727*** 2.291*** 3.096*** 1.309***<br />

(0.480) (0.479) (0.479) (0.455) (0.474) (0.459) (0.479) (0.446)<br />

Low 2.096*** 1.997*** 1.982*** 1.006*** 1.765*** 1.537*** 1.989*** 0.846**<br />

(0.355) (0.354) (0.355) (0.336) (0.351) (0.339) (0.354) (0.330)<br />

Medium 1.179*** 1.141*** 1.152*** 0.600** 1.005*** 0.901*** 1.129*** 0.515*<br />

(0.290) (0.289) (0.289) (0.272) (0.286) (0.276) (0.289) (0.267)<br />

Damp/condensation/mould (base = none)<br />

Minor problem 1.226*** 1.132*** 1.143*** 0.647*** 0.994*** 0.614*** 1.143*** 0.423**<br />

(0.220) (0.220) (0.220) (0.207) (0.218) (0.211) (0.220) (0.203)<br />

Serious problem 2.559*** 2.118*** 2.112*** 1.270*** 1.751*** 0.823* 2.119*** 0.547<br />

(0.440) (0.444) (0.444) (0.417) (0.439) (0.425) (0.443) (0.410)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.0838 0.0893 0.0919 0.1992 0.1108 0.1740 0.0916 0.2416<br />

Controls included<br />

Basic demographics X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Childcare X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Maternal <strong>background</strong> X X<br />

Maternal health etc. X X<br />

Neighbourhood X X<br />

8.5.7 Home Environment <strong>and</strong> Behaviour<br />

The reported evidence of outings <strong>and</strong> the home based book <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>y score are correlated with<br />

behavioural scores before other proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs are considered (table 8.14). There is a strong<br />

overlap between these fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> parenting behaviours which causes large declines in the their<br />

importance when parenting is also included (column 4) or in the final column with all fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

considered. Yet apart from the overlap with parenting behaviours these relationships are not<br />

123


greatly affected by the other proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs. Damp housing was also strongly correlated with<br />

adverse behaviour. Here mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health reflects part of this correlation.<br />

Outings <strong>to</strong> the park, places of interest such as the Zoo, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> places of entertainment, <strong>and</strong> high<br />

<strong>to</strong>y scores are associated with minor improvements in behaviour scores. Low book scores <strong>and</strong><br />

damp housing conditions are very minor influences on behaviour. All these fac<strong>to</strong>rs, in turn, are<br />

strongly related <strong>to</strong> family income.<br />

8.5.8 Mother’s Childhood <strong>and</strong> Behaviour<br />

The reported evidence of the mother’s relationship with her mother <strong>and</strong> how positive the mother<br />

feels about her own schooling are strongly correlated with behavioural outcomes of the <strong>child</strong><br />

when other influences are not considered (table 8.15). These attitudes are strongly correlated in<br />

turn with reports of physical <strong>and</strong> mental health (depression <strong>and</strong> self-esteem) <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> a lesser<br />

degree with parenting behaviour. So once these proximal influences are conditioned on the<br />

mother’s own <strong>child</strong>hood experiences of school <strong>and</strong> relationship with her mother have only a<br />

modest effect<br />

Table 8.15: Estimated effects of mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong> controls on<br />

Behaviour, various specifications (N = 9416)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Mother’s relationship with own mother score (base = lowest quartile)<br />

2 nd quartile -0.798**<br />

(0.317)<br />

3 rd quartile -2.250***<br />

(0.305)<br />

Highest quartile -3.522***<br />

(0.304)<br />

-0.649**<br />

(0.317)<br />

-2.085***<br />

(0.305)<br />

-3.342***<br />

(0.304)<br />

-0.690**<br />

(0.317)<br />

-2.115***<br />

(0.305)<br />

-3.359***<br />

(0.304)<br />

-0.453<br />

(0.297)<br />

-1.394***<br />

(0.287)<br />

-1.988***<br />

(0.289)<br />

-0.567*<br />

(0.313)<br />

-1.860***<br />

(0.302)<br />

-2.966***<br />

(0.302)<br />

0.241<br />

(0.306)<br />

-0.690**<br />

(0.298)<br />

-1.148***<br />

(0.303)<br />

-0.643**<br />

(0.316)<br />

-2.076***<br />

(0.304)<br />

-3.355***<br />

(0.303)<br />

0.045<br />

(0.293)<br />

-0.627**<br />

(0.285)<br />

-0.841***<br />

(0.292)<br />

Mother has his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy (base = yes)<br />

No 1.610***<br />

(0.403)<br />

1.434***<br />

(0.402)<br />

1.388***<br />

(0.402)<br />

0.861**<br />

(0.381)<br />

1.516***<br />

(0.398)<br />

0.900**<br />

(0.387)<br />

1.377***<br />

(0.402)<br />

0.593<br />

(0.374)<br />

Mother feels school was a valuable experience (base = not sure/generally not/no)<br />

Yes very<br />

Yes generally<br />

-2.695***<br />

(0.317)<br />

-1.530***<br />

(0.267)<br />

-2.591***<br />

(0.316)<br />

-1.472***<br />

(0.266)<br />

-2.559***<br />

(0.316)<br />

-1.463***<br />

(0.267)<br />

-1.837***<br />

(0.298)<br />

-1.018***<br />

(0.251)<br />

-2.302***<br />

(0.314)<br />

-1.226***<br />

(0.264)<br />

-0.795**<br />

(0.312)<br />

-0.512**<br />

(0.259)<br />

-2.537***<br />

(0.316)<br />

-1.456***<br />

(0.266)<br />

-0.614**<br />

(0.300)<br />

-0.321<br />

(0.248)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.0828 0.0890 0.0909 0.2010 0.1108 0.1653 0.0916 0.2416<br />

Controls included<br />

Basic demographics<br />

Income<br />

Childcare<br />

Parenting<br />

Outings & home env.<br />

Maternal health etc.<br />

Neighbourhood<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

124


Table 8.16: Estimated effects of maternal health, attitudes <strong>and</strong> social networks controls on<br />

Behaviour, various specifications (N = 9416) (continued)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Mother's physical health score (base = v high)<br />

Very low<br />

2.779*** 2.754*** 2.776*** 1.961*** 2.676*** 2.680*** 2.785*** 1.912***<br />

(0.373) (0.373) (0.374) (0.360) (0.371) (0.373) (0.373) (0.359)<br />

Low<br />

1.286*** 1.276*** 1.293*** 0.801** 1.221*** 1.171*** 1.317*** 0.749**<br />

(0.340) (0.340) (0.341) (0.327) (0.338) (0.340) (0.340) (0.327)<br />

High<br />

0.711** 0.711** 0.730** 0.374 0.699** 0.670** 0.747** 0.369<br />

(0.329) (0.329) (0.329) (0.316) (0.327) (0.329) (0.329) (0.315)<br />

Social networks score (base = low)<br />

Medium<br />

-0.803*** -0.783*** -0.806*** -0.757*** -0.520** -0.734*** -0.779*** -0.603**<br />

(0.246) (0.246) (0.246) (0.236) (0.246) (0.246) (0.246) (0.237)<br />

-1.337***<br />

High<br />

(0.262)<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score (base = low)<br />

-0.536**<br />

Medium<br />

(0.259)<br />

-1.324***<br />

High<br />

(0.292)<br />

Total CCEI score (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2nd quintile<br />

3rd quintile<br />

4th quintile<br />

1.353***<br />

(0.301)<br />

2.083***<br />

(0.320)<br />

2.230***<br />

(0.336)<br />

Highest quintile<br />

3.826***<br />

(0.381)<br />

Self-esteem score (base = highest quintile)<br />

Lowest quintile<br />

3.705***<br />

(0.364)<br />

2 nd quintile<br />

2.208***<br />

(0.353)<br />

3 rd quintile<br />

1.691***<br />

(0.323)<br />

4 th quintile<br />

1.063***<br />

(0.321)<br />

Locus of control score (base = Internal)<br />

Medium<br />

0.637***<br />

(0.236)<br />

External<br />

1.743***<br />

(0.359)<br />

-1.297***<br />

(0.263)<br />

-0.511**<br />

(0.259)<br />

-1.305***<br />

(0.292)<br />

1.343***<br />

(0.301)<br />

2.069***<br />

(0.320)<br />

2.170***<br />

(0.337)<br />

3.723***<br />

(0.383)<br />

3.680***<br />

(0.365)<br />

2.185***<br />

(0.354)<br />

1.682***<br />

(0.323)<br />

1.055***<br />

(0.321)<br />

-1.367***<br />

(0.264)<br />

-0.514**<br />

(0.259)<br />

-1.276***<br />

(0.292)<br />

1.338***<br />

(0.301)<br />

2.065***<br />

(0.320)<br />

2.156***<br />

(0.338)<br />

3.677***<br />

(0.383)<br />

3.724***<br />

(0.365)<br />

2.216***<br />

(0.354)<br />

1.687***<br />

(0.323)<br />

1.066***<br />

(0.321)<br />

0.620***<br />

(0.237)<br />

0.623***<br />

(0.237)<br />

1.667*** 1.627***<br />

(0.361) (0.361)<br />

Mother’s satisfaction with relationship (base = highest quintile)<br />

Lowest quintile<br />

2.350*** 2.311*** 2.300***<br />

(0.353) (0.353) (0.353)<br />

2 nd quintile<br />

1.428*** 1.412*** 1.384***<br />

(0.337) (0.337) (0.337)<br />

3 rd quintile<br />

1.663*** 1.658*** 1.650***<br />

(0.329) (0.329) (0.330)<br />

4 th quintile<br />

0.791** 0.789** 0.795**<br />

(0.331) (0.331) (0.331)<br />

-1.088***<br />

(0.253)<br />

-0.305<br />

(0.249)<br />

-0.790***<br />

(0.282)<br />

0.998***<br />

(0.290)<br />

1.493***<br />

(0.310)<br />

1.309***<br />

(0.328)<br />

2.252***<br />

(0.378)<br />

2.743***<br />

(0.353)<br />

1.348***<br />

(0.341)<br />

0.928***<br />

(0.312)<br />

0.529*<br />

(0.309)<br />

0.765***<br />

(0.227)<br />

1.559***<br />

(0.348)<br />

1.685***<br />

(0.342)<br />

0.873***<br />

(0.325)<br />

1.045***<br />

(0.318)<br />

0.491<br />

(0.319)<br />

-0.810***<br />

(0.266)<br />

-0.458*<br />

(0.258)<br />

-1.151***<br />

(0.291)<br />

1.306***<br />

(0.299)<br />

2.073***<br />

(0.318)<br />

2.160***<br />

(0.335)<br />

3.744***<br />

(0.381)<br />

3.403***<br />

(0.363)<br />

1.996***<br />

(0.352)<br />

1.597***<br />

(0.321)<br />

0.979***<br />

(0.319)<br />

0.559**<br />

(0.235)<br />

1.520***<br />

(0.359)<br />

2.194***<br />

(0.351)<br />

1.366***<br />

(0.335)<br />

1.581***<br />

(0.328)<br />

0.743**<br />

(0.330)<br />

-1.168***<br />

(0.263)<br />

-0.397<br />

(0.260)<br />

-1.116***<br />

(0.293)<br />

1.301***<br />

(0.301)<br />

1.994***<br />

(0.320)<br />

2.054***<br />

(0.337)<br />

3.585***<br />

(0.384)<br />

3.426***<br />

(0.369)<br />

2.015***<br />

(0.355)<br />

1.566***<br />

(0.324)<br />

0.983***<br />

(0.321)<br />

0.514**<br />

(0.237)<br />

1.346***<br />

(0.365)<br />

2.166***<br />

(0.353)<br />

1.292***<br />

(0.337)<br />

1.553***<br />

(0.330)<br />

0.756**<br />

(0.331)<br />

-1.301***<br />

(0.263)<br />

-0.504*<br />

(0.259)<br />

-1.289***<br />

(0.291)<br />

1.347***<br />

(0.301)<br />

2.057***<br />

(0.320)<br />

2.179***<br />

(0.337)<br />

3.699***<br />

(0.383)<br />

3.669***<br />

(0.364)<br />

2.176***<br />

(0.353)<br />

1.686***<br />

(0.323)<br />

1.073***<br />

(0.320)<br />

0.617***<br />

(0.236)<br />

1.644***<br />

(0.361)<br />

2.336***<br />

(0.353)<br />

1.422***<br />

(0.336)<br />

1.670***<br />

(0.329)<br />

0.784**<br />

(0.331)<br />

-0.798***<br />

(0.258)<br />

-0.246<br />

(0.249)<br />

-0.622**<br />

(0.283)<br />

0.937***<br />

(0.290)<br />

1.451***<br />

(0.309)<br />

1.240***<br />

(0.328)<br />

2.170***<br />

(0.380)<br />

2.458***<br />

(0.357)<br />

1.203***<br />

(0.343)<br />

0.831***<br />

(0.312)<br />

0.476<br />

(0.308)<br />

0.643***<br />

(0.228)<br />

1.261***<br />

(0.351)<br />

1.598***<br />

(0.343)<br />

0.808**<br />

(0.325)<br />

1.006***<br />

(0.318)<br />

0.473<br />

(0.318)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1612 0.1617 0.1635 0.2318 0.1740 0.1653 0.1641 0.2416<br />

Controls included<br />

Basic demographics X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Childcare X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Outings & home<br />

environment<br />

X<br />

X<br />

Maternal <strong>background</strong> X X<br />

Neighbourhood X X<br />

125


8.5.9 Mother’s Mental <strong>and</strong> Physical Health, <strong>Family</strong> Conflict <strong>and</strong> Behaviour<br />

Mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health <strong>and</strong> family conflict are strongly related <strong>to</strong> behavioural<br />

outcomes of <strong>child</strong>ren (table 8.16). There could be some reverse causality, in that the <strong>child</strong>ren’s<br />

behaviour is affecting the mother, although this seems unlikely for physical health <strong>and</strong> measures<br />

of conflict. The main cross-correlation between these indica<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> other proximal influences is<br />

through parenting behaviours (which include maternal bonding <strong>and</strong> discipline behaviours).<br />

There is a strong interrelationship between maternal bonding, mental health (self-esteem, locus<br />

of control <strong>and</strong> depression). All the measures retain a substantive independent effect on<br />

behaviour outcomes. The social s<strong>up</strong>port scores become modest in their effects once other fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

are conditioned on.<br />

Table 8.17: Estimated neighbourhood effects on Behaviour, various specifications (N =<br />

9416)<br />

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> behaviour score of peers – predicted component (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile -0.756 -0.837* -0.845* -0.627 -0.891** -0.833* -0.580 -0.585<br />

(0.462) (0.460) (0.464) (0.429) (0.454) (0.454) (0.436) (0.420)<br />

3 rd quintile -0.790* -0.937** -0.888* -0.948** -1.113** -0.911** -0.657 -0.902**<br />

(0.467) (0.465) (0.466) (0.432) (0.458) (0.458) (0.440) (0.423)<br />

4 th quintile 0.264 0.028 -0.063 -0.344 -0.303 -0.148 -0.096 -0.625<br />

(0.472) (0.470) (0.484) (0.438) (0.465) (0.464) (0.445) (0.439)<br />

Highest quintile -0.004 -0.261 -0.571 -0.403 -0.515 -0.385 -0.457 -0.780*<br />

(0.475) (0.475) (0.502) (0.442) (0.469) (0.468) (0.449) (0.455)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> behaviour score of peers – residual component (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile -0.955** -0.971** -1.030** -1.308*** -1.058** -0.937** -0.975** -1.293***<br />

(0.462) (0.459) (0.460) (0.427) (0.453) (0.453) (0.435) (0.417)<br />

3 rd quintile 1.121** 1.165** 1.123** 1.015** 0.998** 1.132** 1.154*** 0.960**<br />

(0.464) (0.462) (0.463) (0.429) (0.456) (0.455) (0.437) (0.419)<br />

4 th quintile -0.380 -0.371 -0.501 -0.251 -0.319 -0.307 -0.000 -0.118<br />

(0.466) (0.464) (0.466) (0.431) (0.457) (0.457) (0.439) (0.422)<br />

Highest quintile 0.617 0.658 0.675 0.520 0.495 0.713 0.703 0.484<br />

(0.462) (0.460) (0.460) (0.428) (0.454) (0.454) (0.436) (0.417)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.0554 0.0647 0.0670 0.1937 0.0916 0.0916 0.1641 0.2416<br />

Controls included<br />

Basic demographics X X X X X X X X<br />

Income X X X X X X X<br />

Childcare X X<br />

Parenting X X<br />

Outings & home<br />

environment<br />

Maternal<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Maternal health etc. X X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

X<br />

126


8.5.10 Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> Peer Gro<strong>up</strong><br />

No neighbourhood influences were apparent for behaviour in our preliminary investigation but<br />

there were very modest peer gro<strong>up</strong> effects. These, however, were only significant for the third<br />

quintile <strong>and</strong> that made the pattern look rather inconclusive. Conditioning on other mediating<br />

influences tends <strong>to</strong> produce a more consistent picture (table 8.17). Those <strong>child</strong>ren attending preschools<br />

with other <strong>child</strong>ren with poorly reported behaviour have slightly better scores. This<br />

probably reflects that the mother (who gives the report) is witnessing the behaviour of other<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren at the pre-school <strong>and</strong> this is influencing their assessment of their own <strong>child</strong>. These<br />

minor effects are thus not probably genuine but reflect reporting bias.<br />

8.6 How do Parental Education <strong>and</strong> Income Influence a Child’s Early Behaviour?<br />

Here we explore fully how income <strong>and</strong> parental education work through the proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong><br />

influence <strong>child</strong>ren’s behaviour. First we show the unconditional relationships between parental<br />

education, family income <strong>and</strong> the <strong>child</strong>’s behaviour (table 8.18). The behavioural measure is<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ardised such that the mean gap between the highest quartile of scores <strong>and</strong> the lowest is 25<br />

points (2.5 st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations). <strong>Family</strong> income <strong>and</strong> parental education can explain less than 5%<br />

of this even when no other fac<strong>to</strong>rs are considered. Once other fac<strong>to</strong>rs are introduced family<br />

income <strong>and</strong> parental education are almost trivial in their conditional correlation with behaviour<br />

scores. However, parenting behaviours including maternal bonding, teaching, reading etc. <strong>and</strong><br />

mother mental <strong>and</strong> physical health, especially self-esteem CCEI <strong>and</strong> locus of control scores, are<br />

important predic<strong>to</strong>rs of the gap. All <strong>to</strong>ld the model can explain around 25% of this gap <strong>and</strong> of<br />

the 6.5 points explained, 4.9 are attributable <strong>to</strong> these two gro<strong>up</strong>s of influences. Of the remaining<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs the home environment offers a modest contribution. Childcare, like parental education,<br />

income, neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> maternal employment, has no substantive part <strong>to</strong> play in our data.<br />

The differences in raw behaviour scores between the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest education <strong>and</strong> income<br />

gro<strong>up</strong>s are thus modest in size <strong>and</strong> explained mainly by the cross-correlation with parenting <strong>and</strong><br />

mother mental <strong>and</strong> physical health. It should be noted again that our behavioural measure is<br />

solely mother reported <strong>and</strong> does not correlate strongly with teacher based assessments for the<br />

sub-sample where this is available <strong>and</strong> so these results need <strong>to</strong> be treated with some caution.<br />

127


Table 8.18: Decomposition of range of mean scores for various gro<strong>up</strong>s (Behaviour)<br />

Difference in mean<br />

scores<br />

Explained variation –<br />

contribution of:<br />

Income<br />

Points<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of attainment<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Points<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of<br />

household income<br />

Points<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Children of mothers with<br />

CSE/no qualifications vs. a<br />

degree<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

Points<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

25.2 100 100 25.2 100 100 4.6 100 100 4.6 100 100<br />

0.7 2.8 62.3 0.1 0.6 2.1 0.6 13.0 13.1 0.3 6.8 6.8<br />

Parental Education 0.4 1.7 37.7 0.2 0.8 3.2 0.6 14.2 14.2 1.5 33.4 33.4<br />

Other Demographic<br />

Variables<br />

- - - 0.4 1.8 6.8 0.3 7.5 7.5 0.1 3.2 3.2<br />

Childcare - - - 0.1 0.3 1.0 0.0 0.9 0.9 0.1 2.2 2.2<br />

Parenting - - - 2.9 11.5 44.1 0.9 20.6 20.7 0.6 13.5 13.5<br />

Outings & Home<br />

Environment<br />

Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

<strong>and</strong> family<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Maternal health,<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> social<br />

networks<br />

Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong><br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong><br />

Unexplained<br />

variation<br />

- - - 0.4 1.8 6.5 0.4 8.4 8.5 0.4 9.4 9.4<br />

- - - 0.3 1.1 4.0 0.2 4.5 4.5 0.3 6.3 6.3<br />

- - - 2.0 7.9 30.4 1.4 30.2 30.4 1.1 24.0 24.0<br />

- - - 0.1 0.5 1.8 0.1 1.2 1.2 0.1 1.2 1.2<br />

24.1 95.5 - 18.6 73.9 - -0.0 -0.4 - 0.0 0.0 -<br />

128


129


9. Conclusions<br />

This research has used the highly detailed Avon Longitudinal Survey of Parents <strong>and</strong> Children<br />

(ALSPAC), a birth cohort of 12,000 <strong>child</strong>ren born in the Avon area in 1991-2, <strong>to</strong> explore the<br />

origins of variation in pre-school behaviour <strong>and</strong> educational attainment scores at school entry.<br />

The intention has been <strong>to</strong> explore what fac<strong>to</strong>rs are correlated with early learning deficits <strong>and</strong><br />

behavioural problems <strong>and</strong>, in particular, <strong>to</strong> assess how raw correlations between parental<br />

education <strong>and</strong> family income are transmitted in<strong>to</strong> the observed outcomes through mediating<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs.<br />

Under this approach we explore seven gro<strong>up</strong>ings of fac<strong>to</strong>rs which are very detailed in the<br />

ALSPAC study <strong>and</strong> which may act as mediating influences between family <strong>background</strong> (income<br />

<strong>and</strong> education) <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s outcomes. These are parenting behaviour, the neighbourhood <strong>and</strong><br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong>, maternal employment, maternal mental <strong>and</strong> physical health, mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

experiences, the home environment <strong>and</strong> pre-school <strong>child</strong>care. We refer <strong>to</strong> these as proximal<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs.<br />

Educational Attainment<br />

Our measure of educational attainment on school entry is based on teacher delivered tests<br />

undertaken in the Avon area at this time. This test score is fairly strongly correlated with later<br />

test scores at <strong>age</strong> 7 measured by the ALSPAC team <strong>and</strong> with the national KS1 tests. Our<br />

available information can explain around a quarter of the gap between the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest<br />

achieving quarters of the distribution (once <strong>age</strong> related variation, which explains 10% of the<br />

overall variation, is excluded).<br />

We find the following results for proximal or mediating influences in explaining the variation<br />

between the highest <strong>and</strong> least achieving <strong>child</strong>ren:<br />

• Maternal employment patterns have no significant influence.<br />

o Parenting behaviours, especially teaching <strong>and</strong> reading activities by parents are very<br />

important.<br />

• The key parenting influences is maternal teaching behaviour in the pre-school period.<br />

The magnitude is such that <strong>child</strong>ren of mothers who are in the lowest 8% in terms of <strong>age</strong><br />

of starting <strong>and</strong> range of maternal teaching are 3.5 points behind other similar <strong>child</strong>ren (or<br />

14 percentile points lower in the distribution). Those <strong>child</strong>ren in the next 11% of the<br />

distribution are 2 points (or 8 percentile points lower in the distribution) behind in the<br />

130


distribution. These are large effects.<br />

o Maternal reading is less important but less intensive reading does result in substantial<br />

deficits.<br />

o Other parenting behaviours (6+ hours of TV a week at 18 months <strong>and</strong> breast-feeding<br />

beyond 6 months) are of moderate importance.<br />

• Neighbourhoods <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>s are almost as important as parenting behaviours.<br />

o For neighbourhood, the key indica<strong>to</strong>r is not financially based but is a measure of the<br />

educational attainment of the wider population including adults <strong>and</strong> older <strong>child</strong>ren in<br />

the neighbourhood (note there is a strong correlation between educational attainment<br />

<strong>and</strong> income/employment at this area level)<br />

o In addition the attainment of peer <strong>child</strong>ren attending the same pre-school is a<br />

substantial influence on a <strong>child</strong>’s attainment. The analysis suggests that if a <strong>child</strong> is<br />

attending a pre-school where the other <strong>child</strong>ren have well educated parents who are<br />

heavily eng<strong>age</strong>d in teaching <strong>and</strong> providing a high quality home learning environment<br />

this benefits the focus <strong>child</strong> over <strong>and</strong> above their own family learning environment.<br />

• Outings <strong>and</strong> Home Environment are the next most substantive in their impact on<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment.<br />

o A very low book score (which in turn is associated with low reading <strong>and</strong> low<br />

income) which affects the 6% of <strong>child</strong>ren has a substantive influence on <strong>child</strong><br />

attainment conditional on confounding influences.<br />

o A very low <strong>to</strong>y score is also detrimental <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong> cognitive <strong>development</strong>.<br />

o There is no evidence that better educated parents are providing higher quality books<br />

or <strong>to</strong>ys, in terms of their impact on <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong>.<br />

o Trips <strong>to</strong> Libraries <strong>and</strong> not attending department s<strong>to</strong>res have weak (positive)<br />

influences on <strong>development</strong>.<br />

• Childcare is a much more modest influence on <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment (other than through<br />

the peer gro<strong>up</strong> noted above) <strong>and</strong> the results suggest that:<br />

o Limited hours of care by partner <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 is modestly beneficial <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

<strong>development</strong><br />

o Extended care by friends, gr<strong>and</strong>parents <strong>and</strong> other relatives <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 is modestly<br />

harmful.<br />

o Nursery care (in school class or private setting) prior <strong>to</strong> entry in<strong>to</strong> school is modestly<br />

131


eneficial <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment on entry.<br />

o There is no evidence that full-time Nursery exposure at 3 or 4 is more beneficial than<br />

part-time. Nor is there evidence that attending a Nursery is more beneficial for those<br />

with less educated mothers.<br />

• Mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health, family conflict <strong>and</strong> mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood are not<br />

major influences on <strong>child</strong>ren’s educational attainment at <strong>age</strong> 4.<br />

We then focus on the routes by which family income <strong>and</strong> parental education transmit <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

educational outcomes.<br />

• There is a <strong>to</strong>tal 6.6 point attainment gap between rich <strong>and</strong> poor <strong>child</strong>ren. Around a<br />

quarter of this raw correlation between income <strong>and</strong> attainment can be explained by<br />

observable proximal influences that have a plausible causal link. The main influences are<br />

the home environment, the neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>care. Another<br />

quarter of the original variation in attainment correlated with income remains attributed<br />

<strong>to</strong> family income. This residual correlation reflects neither parental education, parenting<br />

styles nor other influences studied here. This <strong>to</strong>o could be a causal influence of income<br />

but its status is uncertain. So at most around half of the raw difference in attainments<br />

between the richest <strong>and</strong> poorest pre-school <strong>child</strong>ren can be thought of as genuinely<br />

driven by income <strong>and</strong> probably more like a third.<br />

• The gap in attainment between <strong>child</strong>ren of mothers with CSE level qualifications or<br />

lower <strong>and</strong> degree holding mothers is much larger than between the poorest <strong>and</strong> richest<br />

families. The proximal influences considered here do more work in explaining how<br />

parental education is transmitted through <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren than they do for income. Together<br />

the major proximal influences account for some 40% of raw gap in attainment between<br />

the <strong>child</strong>ren of well <strong>and</strong> less educated mothers. If we include residual income as another<br />

mechanism this rises <strong>to</strong> nearly a half. Of the proximal influences the most important<br />

contribution comes from the home environment, the neighbourhood/peer gro<strong>up</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> a<br />

lesser degree parenting behaviours (through teaching <strong>and</strong> reading). Whilst parenting<br />

behaviours are the strongest predic<strong>to</strong>rs of overall <strong>child</strong> attainment they differ only<br />

moderately between well <strong>and</strong> poorly educated mothers <strong>and</strong> hence they do not dominate<br />

the relationship between parental education <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> outcomes. Around 40% of this raw<br />

unconditional gap remains unexplained by other fac<strong>to</strong>rs observed in the data. This<br />

residual correlation with parental education could also reflect omitted fac<strong>to</strong>rs such as the<br />

132


genetic inheritance or other transmission mechanisms not measured in ALSPAC.<br />

Children’s Behaviour<br />

The results for behaviour show less effect of income <strong>and</strong> education. Once other fac<strong>to</strong>rs are<br />

introduced family income <strong>and</strong> parental education have almost trivial conditional correlation with<br />

measured behaviour.<br />

• Parenting behaviours (including maternal bonding, teaching, reading etc.) are extremely<br />

important predic<strong>to</strong>rs of behaviour. The most important parental behaviours are parental<br />

bonding at 8 months, discipline strategies <strong>and</strong> talking whilst engaging in other activities<br />

such as housework. Low teaching <strong>and</strong> reading scores are weakly associated with adverse<br />

behaviour (though causality could run the other way).<br />

• There is a strong inter-relationship between maternal bonding <strong>and</strong> mental health<br />

(measured as self-esteem, locus of control <strong>and</strong> depression).<br />

• The results suggest that poor physical health, maternal depression/anxiety <strong>and</strong> low selfesteem<br />

are intertwined with weaker parenting behaviours <strong>to</strong> produce adverse behavioural<br />

scores. There could be some reverse causality here, in that the <strong>child</strong>’s behaviour is<br />

affecting the mother, although we use measures of these fac<strong>to</strong>rs taken at least 2 years<br />

before the <strong>child</strong>’s behavioural outcomes are observed <strong>to</strong> minimise this.<br />

• Of the remaining fac<strong>to</strong>rs the home environment has a modest association.<br />

• Childcare, like parental education, income <strong>and</strong> maternal employment, has no substantive<br />

part <strong>to</strong> play.<br />

• The differences in raw behaviour scores between the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest education <strong>and</strong><br />

income gro<strong>up</strong>s are modest in size <strong>and</strong> explained mainly by the correlation of parenting<br />

<strong>and</strong> mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health.<br />

In Summary, the key findings are:<br />

• Parents teaching (e.g. teaching <strong>child</strong>ren a number of items such as shapes, colours <strong>and</strong><br />

numbers) <strong>and</strong> reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren during the pre-school period is extremely important for<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s early learning. However, differences in teaching <strong>and</strong> reading do not explain<br />

very much of the early learning deficits of those from poorer or less educated families.<br />

There is no evidence that better educated parents teach <strong>and</strong> read <strong>to</strong> young <strong>child</strong>ren more<br />

effectively; they just do it somewhat more often.<br />

• Exposure <strong>to</strong> pre-school <strong>child</strong>care is modestly beneficial <strong>to</strong> early learning but it is a minor<br />

133


fac<strong>to</strong>r in explaining early learning deficits observed. Full-time <strong>child</strong>care prior <strong>to</strong> school<br />

entry is no more beneficial than part-time but .attending a pre-school containing high<br />

achieving <strong>child</strong>ren is beneficial..<br />

• Centre based care at or before <strong>age</strong> 2 is as beneficial <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s learning as other paid<br />

care (e.g. <strong>child</strong> minders) but is associated with worse behaviours. Long hours of care (20<br />

or more hours a week) <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> the <strong>age</strong> of 2 by unpaid carers, such as friends <strong>and</strong> relatives,<br />

is associated with both lower attainment <strong>and</strong> worse behaviour, whilst fathers<br />

involvement before <strong>age</strong> 2 is modestly beneficial.<br />

• The home environment in terms of books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong> the attainment of the peer gro<strong>up</strong><br />

attending the same pre-school are substantial fac<strong>to</strong>rs behind the early learning deficits of<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren from poorer <strong>and</strong> less educated families. These proximal are strongly related <strong>to</strong><br />

family income levels.<br />

• Mother’s mental <strong>and</strong> physical health, especially stress, anxiety <strong>and</strong> self-esteem, <strong>and</strong> low<br />

quality relationships between parents are strongly related <strong>to</strong> poor behavioural outcomes.<br />

134


135


Up To 7<br />

Part 2<br />

The Persistence of Early Learning Variations<br />

Through the First Two Years of Schooling<br />

10. Introduction<br />

Part 1 covered <strong>development</strong> in the pre-school period, <strong>and</strong> looked at what insights the Children<br />

of the 90s database could provide about how dimensions of family <strong>background</strong> transmit in<strong>to</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s educational <strong>and</strong> behavioural <strong>development</strong> on entry in<strong>to</strong> primary school. Part 2 looks<br />

at the persistence of the picture <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 7 through the first two years of schooling. It aims <strong>to</strong><br />

assess how the first two years of education in school changes the picture of the key influential<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs on <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> prior <strong>to</strong> school entry. In particular, the aim is <strong>to</strong> assess how aspects<br />

of early learning deficits based on family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> the role of pre-school <strong>child</strong>care<br />

experiences persist or decay on school entry. In addition, we explore the importance of the<br />

particular school attended for the degree of persistence of early educational (dis-)advant<strong>age</strong>s.<br />

Both studies use the same rich ALSPAC Children of the 90s database developed at the<br />

University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l.<br />

At <strong>age</strong> 7, attainment is measured by Key St<strong>age</strong> 1 tests (see section on Data <strong>and</strong> Measures used<br />

for details) <strong>and</strong> an independent assessment of literacy <strong>and</strong> langu<strong>age</strong> <strong>development</strong>. The two <strong>age</strong> 7<br />

measures record very similar subject matter <strong>and</strong> are very highly correlated. They are in effect<br />

two replications of measuring <strong>child</strong> attainment. So we combine these two <strong>age</strong> 7 measures so as<br />

<strong>to</strong> improve the accuracy of the measure used.<br />

Drawn from the first part of this study, we look at a range of family <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs often<br />

associated with variance in educational achievement, such as teen motherhood, family size,<br />

parental education <strong>and</strong> income. We look at how <strong>child</strong> outcomes relate <strong>to</strong> family functioning <strong>and</strong><br />

early education experiences. We focus on six main gro<strong>up</strong>s of fac<strong>to</strong>rs, which were found in the<br />

previous pre-school study <strong>to</strong> mediate between dimensions of family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> attainment.<br />

These are<br />

• Parenting patterns - covering reading, activities (e.g. singing, bathing, playing),<br />

136


teaching, etc.<br />

• The home learning environment <strong>and</strong> outings<br />

• Child care us<strong>age</strong> covering types <strong>and</strong> extent of care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 1/2 <strong>and</strong> more detailed<br />

study of <strong>child</strong> care exposure <strong>and</strong> limited measures of quality prior <strong>to</strong> school entry at <strong>age</strong><br />

4<br />

• Maternal physical <strong>and</strong> mental health, especially depression<br />

• Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood experiences<br />

• The neighbourhood of residence <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>s in pre-school providers<br />

The details of how the components which make <strong>up</strong> each gro<strong>up</strong>ing were identified are shown in<br />

Part 1.<br />

The approach of the report<br />

In our examination of these interrelationships in Part 1, we focused particularly on the impact of<br />

parental education <strong>and</strong> income on outcomes, examining the pathways (the fac<strong>to</strong>rs listed above)<br />

through which income <strong>and</strong> parental education operate <strong>to</strong> affect <strong>child</strong> outcomes at <strong>age</strong> 5.<br />

Causality in this area is obviously difficult <strong>to</strong> infer with complete confidence owing <strong>to</strong> the<br />

complexities of such interrelationships. Nevertheless we explored plausible pathways (or<br />

proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs) through which the distal fac<strong>to</strong>rs (income <strong>and</strong> parental education) might affect<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>. As well as parental education <strong>and</strong> income, we also explored other<br />

family characteristics <strong>and</strong> their relationship with our mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs. These are <strong>age</strong> of mother<br />

at birth of <strong>child</strong>, numbers of siblings <strong>and</strong> lone parenthood. Here we examine whether these early<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood influences persist on entry <strong>to</strong> school. The expectation is that the entry in<strong>to</strong> formal<br />

schooling may mitigate some dimensions of disadvant<strong>age</strong> from the pre-school period.<br />

The modelling approach adopted for the data observed at <strong>age</strong> 7 replicates the results reported in<br />

the first part of the analysis for <strong>age</strong> 5. Then we formally test a value-added model for<br />

differences in <strong>child</strong> trajec<strong>to</strong>ries <strong>to</strong> see if value-added is associated with these fac<strong>to</strong>rs. Since<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren are at school <strong>and</strong> subject <strong>to</strong> school influences, we test the value-added models for<br />

robustness <strong>to</strong> inclusion of school effects (dummies for each school in the Avon district). Where<br />

we observe substantive changes in the relationship between a fac<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong> educational attainment<br />

at <strong>age</strong>s 5 <strong>and</strong> 7, an assessment needs <strong>to</strong> be made as <strong>to</strong> whether this is a genuine change or<br />

reflects systematic reporting errors. This is discussed further below when we discuss the data<br />

<strong>and</strong> measures used.<br />

137


11. Data <strong>and</strong> Measures of Child Outcomes<br />

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents <strong>and</strong> Children (ALSPAC) is a cohort study of around<br />

14,000 <strong>child</strong>ren born in the Avon area of the UK in 1991 <strong>and</strong> 1992. Hence the pre-school period<br />

we consider was after the bulk of the major expansion of early return <strong>to</strong> work by mothers that<br />

occurred in the mid-late 1980s, (see Gregg et al. 2003) but before the guaranteed half day places<br />

at pre-school for four <strong>and</strong> most recently 3 year olds. The early school period (often described as<br />

Infants in the school system) is for the three school years starting 1995-1997 <strong>and</strong> is completed<br />

by 1999. This period pre-dates the bulk of the moves <strong>to</strong> reductions in class sizes in primary<br />

schools, but does partially cover the introduction of the literacy hour in September 1998. Hence<br />

the last two of our three-school-year cohort experienced some literacy exposure, but it should be<br />

noted that on its introduction the focus of the literacy hour was mainly on slightly older<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

Mothers in the ALSPAC Survey complete <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> three surveys a year, one relating <strong>to</strong> the<br />

characteristics of herself <strong>and</strong> the household in general <strong>and</strong> two relating <strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong> 11 . In<br />

addition, mothers answered four questionnaires during their pregnancies. The ALSPAC survey<br />

also contains data from sources other than self-completion questionnaires. The ALSPAC team<br />

have run a number of clinics for <strong>child</strong>ren from the <strong>age</strong> of seven onwards in which they are able<br />

<strong>to</strong> directly assess various aspects of the <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>development</strong>. Records from schools, referred<br />

<strong>to</strong> below, can also be matched <strong>to</strong> the individual <strong>child</strong>ren so data is available on school-based<br />

assessments at <strong>age</strong>s 4 <strong>to</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong> again for <strong>age</strong>s 6 <strong>to</strong> 7.<br />

11.1 Measures of Cognitive attainment<br />

The two school-based measures of cognitive <strong>development</strong> available in ALSPAC are the Entry<br />

Assessment (EA) test taken at <strong>age</strong> 4 or 5 <strong>and</strong> the Key St<strong>age</strong> 1 assessment, which is administered<br />

in Year 2 at <strong>age</strong> 6 or 7. Each test is composed of four sub-scores that capture ability in reading,<br />

writing, mathematics <strong>and</strong> langu<strong>age</strong> skills (EA only) or spelling (KS1 only). Our third<br />

assessment of cognitive ability was administered, by the ALSPAC team, <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren at the <strong>age</strong><br />

of 7. This ALSPAC test was labelled Focus @ 7 <strong>and</strong> is a literacy score. This score is composed<br />

of a number of sub-scores, capturing skills in reading, spelling <strong>and</strong> the manipulation of words.<br />

To combine the sub-scores for each measure in<strong>to</strong> one overall score we used the technique of<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis. This method allows the data <strong>to</strong> dictate the relative weights attached <strong>to</strong> each<br />

11 The mother’s partner also received annual questionnaires but the response is patchy.<br />

138


component <strong>and</strong> so <strong>to</strong> distil the maximum possible information in<strong>to</strong> a single measure. Each of the<br />

three resulting scores were then normalised <strong>to</strong> have a mean of 100 <strong>and</strong> a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of<br />

10.<br />

As is inevitable in a survey of the scale of ALSPAC, attrition results in smaller sample sizes for<br />

the later assessments of <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong>. So by <strong>age</strong> 7 there were 9,500 <strong>child</strong>ren in the full<br />

study. In addition <strong>to</strong> this problem, parents were required <strong>to</strong> give written permission for the<br />

release of the school-based test results. The sample sizes for the Entry Assessment <strong>and</strong> Key<br />

St<strong>age</strong> 1 scores are therefore substantially smaller than the full sample. The EA test had an<br />

additional sample loss because this was not a national test at that time <strong>and</strong> hence <strong>child</strong>ren who<br />

had left the Avon area or entered in<strong>to</strong> some independent schools were not tested. Likewise the<br />

Focus @ 7 test required parents <strong>to</strong> bring the <strong>child</strong> <strong>to</strong> an ALSPAC clinic <strong>and</strong> this <strong>to</strong>o results in a<br />

reduced sample. Part 1 of this analysis used the attainment measures at 4 or 5. At <strong>age</strong> 7 we have<br />

two alternative measures of cognitive <strong>development</strong>, one based on a school based participation in<br />

the national Key St<strong>age</strong> 1 test for which we only have <strong>child</strong>ren whose parents provided<br />

permissions for this matching, <strong>and</strong> an ALSPAC assessment centre based test which is available<br />

for those who attended the clinics held by the ALSPAC team. As most permissions came at<br />

these clinics the two samples strongly overlap. The ALSPAC Focus @ 7 test has 7357<br />

observations <strong>and</strong> the KS1 tests 6030 <strong>and</strong> 5852 provide information on both scales. The EA test<br />

has just under 5,000 cases all drawn from the KS1 sample.<br />

The reduced sample sizes raise the question of whether there is major variation in the<br />

characteristics of the population reporting a test score <strong>and</strong> those that don’t as a result of attrition<br />

<strong>and</strong> giving of permission <strong>to</strong> match <strong>to</strong> the ALSPAC data/attending a clinic. We investigated this<br />

issue for a range of observable characteristics such as household income, mother’s <strong>age</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

educational attainment, etc <strong>and</strong> concluded that there is little variation in the composition of each<br />

sample. The only clear sample composition difference is an under-representation of lone parents<br />

in the ALSPAC sample. This is substantive among never partnered lone parents <strong>and</strong> those<br />

becoming lone parents within the first year or so of the <strong>child</strong>’s life. Hence there appears <strong>to</strong> be no<br />

substantive attrition bias in the sample composition as measured on observable characteristics<br />

other than <strong>to</strong>o few lone parents but there may well be unobservable differences. We have chosen<br />

not <strong>to</strong> re-weight the data <strong>to</strong> raise the size of lone parent population, as those remaining may be<br />

unrepresentative of those not reporting. However, the analysis needs <strong>to</strong> have a specific caveat<br />

about the representativeness of the lone parent sample.<br />

139


As a guide <strong>to</strong> interpreting the size of our estimates, assuming a normal distribution an increment<br />

of 1 point (i.e. a tenth of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation) results in a shift from the median <strong>to</strong> the 54 th<br />

percentile, while an increment of 5 points (or half a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation) results in a shift from<br />

the median <strong>to</strong> the 69 th percentile. Table 11.1 shows the pairwise correlations between the EA<br />

score, <strong>and</strong> two other measures of cognitive attainment – the Key St<strong>age</strong> 1 score, <strong>and</strong> an ALSPAC<br />

administered literacy score taken at 7 years.<br />

Table 11.1: Pairwise correlations between entry assessment, KS1 <strong>and</strong> Focus at 7, all <strong>age</strong><br />

adjusted.<br />

Entry<br />

Assessment<br />

KS1 Focus @ 7<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

KS1 – F @<br />

7 score<br />

Entry<br />

Assessment<br />

1.0000<br />

KS1 0.5523 1.0000<br />

F @ 7 0.4389 0.8366 1.0000<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> KS1- F<br />

@ 7 score<br />

0.5105 0.9482 0.9430 1.0000<br />

Max. sample<br />

size<br />

4, 996 6,030 7, 329 7, 537<br />

Here KS1 <strong>and</strong> Focus @ 7 scores were adjusted <strong>to</strong> allow for variation in <strong>age</strong>. KS1 is undertaken<br />

at a point in time when <strong>child</strong>ren’s <strong>age</strong>s will vary within the year. The ALSPAC clinics<br />

approached <strong>child</strong>ren when they reached a specific <strong>age</strong> (so as <strong>to</strong> smooth the flow of <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

attending through out a year) <strong>and</strong> so there is little <strong>age</strong> variation among the <strong>child</strong>ren in the Focus<br />

@ 7 data. To adjust for these differences, KS1 scores were regressed against month of birth <strong>and</strong><br />

year of taking the exam, <strong>and</strong> then st<strong>and</strong>ardised <strong>to</strong> again give a mean of 100 <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

deviation of 10. Focus @ 7 scores were regressed against the <strong>age</strong> of the <strong>child</strong> when taking the<br />

test <strong>and</strong> also st<strong>and</strong>ardised. The school based Key St<strong>age</strong> 1 <strong>and</strong> the ALSPAC literacy test have a<br />

very high correlation of 0.84, which suggests these can be considered as two alternative<br />

estimates of cognitive attainment with some measurement error.<br />

As the two measures are coincident in timing <strong>and</strong> very similar in content <strong>and</strong> extremely strongly<br />

correlated, taking an aver<strong>age</strong> of the two will reduce measurement errors in the data for those<br />

where we have both observations. So with both the scores now cleaned for differential <strong>age</strong> when<br />

the test was sat, an aver<strong>age</strong> score was calculated by simply summing the two scores <strong>and</strong><br />

dividing them by two. In order <strong>to</strong> maximise the sample where only one of the test score<br />

observations is available we use data where just one of the two measures is observed, but<br />

140


included dummies for which source is used for this single observation. This is more of an issue<br />

for the maximum <strong>age</strong> 7 sample than for the case where we restrict ourselves <strong>to</strong> the presence of a<br />

valid Entry Assessment score at <strong>age</strong> 4/5 as well. This is because the EA sample is the most<br />

restrictive <strong>and</strong> relies on permissions, which overlap almost perfectly with KS1 <strong>and</strong> strongly with<br />

attending a clinic. EA scores were also adjusted for <strong>age</strong>, so that when determining the value<br />

added both scores would be clean. The EA test is moderately strongly correlated with test scores<br />

at <strong>age</strong> 7. For this test there is a higher correlation with the KS1 score, which is also a schoolbased<br />

assessment. The correlation with the aver<strong>age</strong>d <strong>age</strong> 7 measure is 0.5. Hence the EA test is<br />

a reasonably strong predic<strong>to</strong>r of later attainment but there is clearly substantial error, as the <strong>age</strong><br />

difference between EA <strong>and</strong> the Focus @ 7 <strong>and</strong> Key St<strong>age</strong> 1 tests is around two years. As the<br />

aver<strong>age</strong>d measure at 7 comes from two independent assessments of the <strong>child</strong> at the same date,<br />

the bulk of any measurement error will be in the EA test. As this test was undertaken by<br />

teachers <strong>and</strong> has room for more individual discretion than the national KS1 test, there is a<br />

danger of teacher specific reporting effects in the EA scores. In comparing the results for the EA<br />

test scores <strong>and</strong> later measures we need <strong>to</strong> be conscious of the potential teacher specific bias <strong>and</strong><br />

of the potential for teachers <strong>to</strong> have subjective judgements more generally.<br />

11.2 Predic<strong>to</strong>rs of attainment by Age 5 <strong>and</strong> the Role of Mediating Influences<br />

In Part 1 of the study, the rich ALSPAC data was explored <strong>to</strong> identify the major predic<strong>to</strong>rs of<br />

cognitive attainment on entry in<strong>to</strong> school. The basic demographic characteristics of families are<br />

strongly correlated with <strong>child</strong> attainment on school entry <strong>and</strong> Part 1 attempted <strong>to</strong> suggest what<br />

aspects of family functioning <strong>and</strong> decision making act as the routes by which these <strong>background</strong><br />

characteristics impact on <strong>child</strong> outcomes.<br />

Table 11.2 provides a listing of the variables included in the final analysis, which formed a<br />

substantive contribution <strong>to</strong> attainment. They are gro<strong>up</strong>ed under headings that will form the basis<br />

of our discussions here about which fac<strong>to</strong>rs decay in their impact <strong>and</strong> which persist in the early<br />

school years. The main gro<strong>up</strong>ings that predicted attainment at <strong>age</strong> 5 were demographic fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

including parental education, <strong>age</strong> of mother, gender <strong>and</strong> numbers of siblings. In addition, key<br />

proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs covered parenting behaviours, which included teaching <strong>and</strong> reading <strong>to</strong> the<br />

<strong>child</strong>, neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>s who attended the same pre-school <strong>and</strong> the home leaning<br />

environment which covered books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys. Mothers’ mental <strong>and</strong> physical health <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

experiences were not major influences on educational outcomes.<br />

The raw gap between attainment of <strong>child</strong>ren with the least <strong>and</strong> most educated mothers was 9.5<br />

141


points at <strong>age</strong> 5 or a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. Other <strong>background</strong> characteristics <strong>and</strong> our mediating<br />

influences can explain around 60% of the effect of parental education on attainment. The results<br />

suggested that parenting behaviour <strong>and</strong> the home environment measures are the strongest<br />

mechanisms through which parental education operates. However, family income is also a<br />

substantive route through which parental education (especially partner’s education) impacts on<br />

attainment, as are the neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>. Pre-school <strong>child</strong>care is not a major route<br />

by which well-educated parents increase their <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment, nor do we find that<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren from disadvant<strong>age</strong>d families or with less well educated parents benefit especially from<br />

attending pre-school nurseries or similar provision. The study also assessed whether bettereducated<br />

parents undertake the mediating activities (e.g. reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> teaching the <strong>child</strong>) more<br />

effectively; the evidence suggests not.<br />

Table 11.2: Gro<strong>up</strong>s of variables used as controls in Entry Assessment regressions<br />

Basic<br />

demographics<br />

Mother’s highest<br />

qualification<br />

Partner’s highest<br />

qualification<br />

Lone parent<br />

status<br />

Income Childcare Parenting<br />

Log aver<strong>age</strong><br />

weekly net<br />

income<br />

Care by partner<br />

pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Care by<br />

friend/relative<br />

pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Care by paid<br />

person pre-<strong>age</strong><br />

2<br />

Maternal<br />

reading score<br />

Teaching score<br />

18 month TV<br />

score<br />

Outings Scores &<br />

Home<br />

Environment<br />

Department s<strong>to</strong>re<br />

outings score<br />

Library outings<br />

score<br />

Book score<br />

Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

<strong>and</strong> family<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Tenure of mother’s<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

Mother’s father<br />

absent from<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

Mother’s mothers<br />

highest qualification<br />

Maternal health,<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong><br />

social networks<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port<br />

score<br />

Locus of control<br />

score<br />

Neighbourhood<br />

Education domain<br />

deprivation score<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of<br />

peers – predicted<br />

component<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of<br />

peers – residual<br />

component<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at<br />

birth<br />

Centre-based<br />

care pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Breastfeeding<br />

Toy score<br />

Mother has a his<strong>to</strong>ry<br />

of truancy<br />

Ethnicity<br />

Nursery class<br />

<strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Mother feels school<br />

was a valuable<br />

experience<br />

Gender<br />

LEA nursery<br />

school <strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Birth weight<br />

Private day<br />

nursery <strong>age</strong> 3/ 4<br />

Special care unit<br />

at birth<br />

Nursery (type<br />

undefined) <strong>age</strong><br />

3/ 4<br />

Younger siblings<br />

by 47 months<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> <strong>age</strong><br />

3/ 4<br />

Older siblings<br />

Other centrebased<br />

care <strong>age</strong><br />

3/ 4<br />

Cohort year<br />

Partner’s<br />

employment at<br />

47 months<br />

142


Lone parenthood was not a statistically significant influence on early attainment in any<br />

specification. The main conclusion was that <strong>child</strong>ren of lone mothers are not performing worse<br />

than their peers, once other fac<strong>to</strong>rs are conditioned on (especially mothers <strong>age</strong> <strong>and</strong> income). But,<br />

as already noted, our sample of lone parents is not representative <strong>and</strong> so care needs <strong>to</strong> be<br />

exercised here.<br />

Teen motherhood has an especially large negative association with the <strong>child</strong>’s cognitive<br />

attainment at <strong>age</strong> 5. The proximal routes explored can explain around 40% of this attainment<br />

deficit, with family income being the most substantive route. But all fac<strong>to</strong>rs make some<br />

contribution. The positive benefits of older motherhood are much better explained by the<br />

observed mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs, with parenting behaviour <strong>and</strong> income being the major two<br />

influences.<br />

Children from non-white families appear <strong>to</strong> do slightly less well on entry <strong>to</strong> school than white<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren. About a third of this can be explained by observed differences in the gro<strong>up</strong>s of more<br />

proximal influences used here. The biggest influence is in the home environment (book <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>y<br />

scores) <strong>and</strong> outings measures. Income also plays a role. Parenting behaviour, maternal<br />

employment <strong>and</strong> mental health <strong>and</strong> social s<strong>up</strong>port are unimportant as routes explaining the gap<br />

between non-whites <strong>and</strong> whites.<br />

Coefficients on low birth weight, gender of the <strong>child</strong> <strong>and</strong> the presence of younger siblings are<br />

not greatly affected by the introduction of the gro<strong>up</strong>s of proximal variables. The coefficients on<br />

older siblings are substantially reduced by inclusion of parenting behaviours, suggesting that the<br />

influence of siblings is <strong>to</strong> slow <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> by reducing parental inputs <strong>to</strong> the <strong>child</strong>,<br />

especially teaching <strong>and</strong> reading.<br />

The raw difference in attainment at <strong>age</strong> 5 between <strong>child</strong>ren from the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest quintiles<br />

of family income was 6.6 points or two thirds of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. Around three quarters of<br />

this correlation is explained by other <strong>background</strong> demographics (mainly parental education) <strong>and</strong><br />

our mediating influences. The main routes through which income impacts on <strong>child</strong>ren by <strong>age</strong> 5<br />

are through the home learning environment (books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys) <strong>and</strong> the neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> preschool<br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong>.<br />

143


11.3 Strategy for Comparing Findings at Ages 5 <strong>and</strong> 7<br />

In what follows we present results in four columns, all of which contain the same set of<br />

variables from Part 1 of the study, representing the key drivers of early learning in <strong>child</strong>ren prior<br />

<strong>to</strong> entry in<strong>to</strong> school. We report the results of a single large regression in sections. We examine<br />

how the situations at <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong> 7 compare on a common sample (4996 observations), whether<br />

the <strong>age</strong> 7 results are robust <strong>to</strong> including a larger maximum possible sample (7537 observations),<br />

<strong>and</strong> whether there are significant changes in the effect of behaviors <strong>and</strong> parental attributes on a<br />

value added measure between <strong>age</strong>s 5 <strong>and</strong> 7. In the first column we show results for the<br />

maximum sample size we can observe at <strong>age</strong> 7, this is followed by the common sample that has<br />

both <strong>age</strong> 7 <strong>and</strong> <strong>age</strong> 8 EA test results <strong>and</strong> this is followed in turn by the EA results for this same<br />

sample. The final column is a simple value added (or change) measure that is just the <strong>age</strong> 7<br />

score minus the <strong>age</strong> 5 score.<br />

144


145


12. Background Characteristics<br />

We start by looking at the impact on attainment at <strong>age</strong> 7 of the basic demographic<br />

characteristics that are strong predic<strong>to</strong>rs of <strong>age</strong> 5 assessment. We first present results without the<br />

mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs included so that broader relationships between family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

attainment can be observed (Table 12.1). The results comparing the larger <strong>and</strong> restrictive<br />

samples (columns 1 <strong>and</strong> 2) for the aver<strong>age</strong> attainment score at <strong>age</strong> 7 are very similar. In no case<br />

are the estimated coefficients significantly different from each other. In addition, the<br />

comparison between the <strong>age</strong> 7 score results <strong>and</strong> the EA test results show very similar patterns in<br />

most cases.<br />

There are a number of results where the general pattern of stability does not hold. Firstly, we<br />

see that <strong>child</strong>ren of lone parents are performing less well in the <strong>age</strong> 7 data than the <strong>age</strong> 5 data,<br />

especially in the exp<strong>and</strong>ed sample. However, for sampling reasons already discussed, the lone<br />

parent results need <strong>to</strong> be treated with caution. Next we come <strong>to</strong> mother’s <strong>age</strong>, where although<br />

the signs are similar the magnitudes of the adverse effects are substantially lower. This shift<br />

may reflect a reduction in the power of the mediating influences associated with teen<br />

motherhood or a general catch <strong>up</strong> by these <strong>child</strong>ren. Alternatively it could reflect bias in the EA<br />

results if teachers are systematically under-scoring <strong>child</strong>ren with young mothers. The same s<strong>to</strong>ry<br />

applies <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren from ethnic minorities. In the EA tests these p<strong>up</strong>ils were scored as under<br />

performing compared <strong>to</strong> those from the white population. However, in the aver<strong>age</strong> test results at<br />

<strong>age</strong> 7 this has completely turned around. Again this could reflect rapidly improving <strong>child</strong><br />

<strong>development</strong> or that teachers were systematically under-scoring these <strong>child</strong>ren on school entry.<br />

The gender of the <strong>child</strong> makes less of a difference at <strong>age</strong> 7 than at <strong>age</strong> 5, although girls are still<br />

significantly ahead of boys in educational <strong>development</strong>. This may well be a genuine<br />

<strong>development</strong>al catch <strong>up</strong>. Likewise very low birth weight is associated with significantly less of a<br />

deficit in the <strong>age</strong> 7 data.<br />

Table 12.2 reports results with the mediating influences included. These differ from the results<br />

reported in table 12.1 <strong>to</strong> the extent that the mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs transmit family <strong>background</strong><br />

through <strong>to</strong> attainment. If some dimensions of these pre-school entry mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs are<br />

becoming less important as the <strong>child</strong> grows we would see that they would explain less of the<br />

family <strong>background</strong> effects. This will be explored in more detail in the final section of the report.<br />

Broadly the patterns of results are very similar after this conditioning. This is easiest <strong>to</strong> see in<br />

the value added measures in Tables 12.1 <strong>and</strong> 12.2. The introduction of the mediating variables<br />

146


leaves the value added regressions almost identical. It follows that the influence of the<br />

mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs in transmitting the relationship between family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> educational<br />

attainment has remained very much the same as the <strong>child</strong>ren have <strong>age</strong>d. So any differences<br />

between the two <strong>age</strong>s in the effect of family <strong>background</strong> impact on <strong>child</strong> outcomes is driven by<br />

the unexplained component of each family <strong>background</strong> measure. With regard <strong>to</strong> the <strong>age</strong> of<br />

motherhood, it is now the case that almost all the attainment gap, at <strong>age</strong> 7, can be explained by<br />

the mediating influences. The attainment gap between <strong>child</strong>ren of teen mothers <strong>and</strong> for those<br />

<strong>age</strong>d over 35 of 1.5 points at <strong>age</strong> 5 (Table 12.1) has fallen <strong>to</strong> just 0.33 points after conditioning<br />

on the mediating routes through which such a distal fac<strong>to</strong>r impacts on <strong>child</strong>ren. For numbers of<br />

siblings, parental education <strong>and</strong> family income the mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs make substantial<br />

contributions <strong>to</strong> explaining how family <strong>background</strong> impacts on <strong>child</strong> attainment at 7. However,<br />

for gender, low birth weight <strong>and</strong> ethnicity explain virtually nothing. So at this <strong>age</strong> the fact that<br />

girls are ahead of boys does not reflect increased parental teaching or reading <strong>to</strong> girls or<br />

exposure <strong>to</strong> different peer gro<strong>up</strong>s.<br />

147


Table 12.1 Basic Demographics without mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs included – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7<br />

<strong>and</strong> Entry Assessment at 5<br />

Full Sample Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

148<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Log of household income<br />

Early Measure<br />

1.452***<br />

(0.280)<br />

Mother’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

-2.146***<br />

(0.315)<br />

A-level<br />

0.953***<br />

(0.243)<br />

Degree<br />

1.948***<br />

(0.340)<br />

Partner’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

-1.476***<br />

(0.296)<br />

A-level<br />

0.408<br />

(0.256)<br />

Degree<br />

1.994***<br />

(0.325)<br />

Lone parent status (base = never lone parent)<br />

Intermittently, with partner at 47<br />

months<br />

Intermittently, no partner at 47<br />

months<br />

Since birth<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth (base = 25-29)<br />

1<br />

(0.573)<br />

Older siblings (base = none)<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3+<br />

-1.406***<br />

(0.247)<br />

-2.520***<br />

(0.327)<br />

-3.732***<br />

(0.494)<br />

Partner’s employment status at 47 months (base = in work)<br />

-0.776*<br />

Not in work<br />

(0.418)<br />

1.810***<br />

(0.367)<br />

-1.935***<br />

(0.393)<br />

1.064***<br />

(0.318)<br />

2.184***<br />

(0.483)<br />

-1.579***<br />

(0.369)<br />

0.446<br />

(0.327)<br />

2.369***<br />

(0.449)<br />

-0.947<br />

(0.827)<br />

-0.174<br />

(0.673)<br />

0.041<br />

(1.281)<br />

-1.142<br />

(0.971)<br />

-0.606<br />

(0.384)<br />

0.297<br />

(0.300)<br />

0.477<br />

(0.434)<br />

1.326**<br />

(0.676)<br />

2.840***<br />

(0.246)<br />

-1.033<br />

(0.660)<br />

-0.605**<br />

(0.257)<br />

-1.177**<br />

(0.545)<br />

-0.138<br />

(0.309)<br />

-1.690**<br />

(0.746)<br />

-1.603***<br />

(0.324)<br />

-2.928***<br />

(0.432)<br />

-3.854***<br />

(0.639)<br />

-0.752<br />

(0.540)<br />

2.189***<br />

(0.387)<br />

-2.205***<br />

(0.414)<br />

1.031***<br />

(0.335)<br />

3.040***<br />

(0.510)<br />

-1.540***<br />

(0.389)<br />

0.417<br />

(0.345)<br />

2.280***<br />

(0.473)<br />

0.706<br />

(0.872)<br />

0.264<br />

(0.710)<br />

0.067<br />

(1.352)<br />

-3.329***<br />

(1.025)<br />

-0.755*<br />

(0.405)<br />

0.578*<br />

(0.317)<br />

0.982**<br />

(0.458)<br />

-2.080***<br />

(0.713)<br />

3.642***<br />

(0.260)<br />

-2.760***<br />

(0.696)<br />

-0.787***<br />

(0.271)<br />

-0.270<br />

(0.575)<br />

-0.217<br />

(0.326)<br />

-2.351***<br />

(0.787)<br />

-1.371***<br />

(0.342)<br />

-3.167***<br />

(0.456)<br />

-4.890***<br />

(0.675)<br />

-0.448<br />

(0.570)<br />

Simple Value Added<br />

-0.379<br />

(0.404)<br />

0.270<br />

(0.432)<br />

0.033<br />

(0.350)<br />

-0.856<br />

(0.532)<br />

-0.040<br />

(0.406)<br />

0.029<br />

(0.360)<br />

0.089<br />

(0.494)<br />

-1.653*<br />

(0.910)<br />

-0.438<br />

(0.741)<br />

-0.026<br />

(1.410)<br />

2.188**<br />

(1.069)<br />

0.149<br />

(0.422)<br />

-0.281<br />

(0.331)<br />

-0.505<br />

(0.478)<br />

3.406***<br />

(0.744)<br />

-0.801***<br />

(0.271)<br />

1.727**<br />

(0.726)<br />

0.182<br />

(0.283)<br />

-0.907<br />

(0.600)<br />

0.079<br />

(0.340)<br />

0.661<br />

(0.822)<br />

-0.232<br />

(0.357)<br />

0.239<br />

(0.475)<br />

1.036<br />

(0.704)<br />

-0.304<br />

(0.595)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1361 0.1378 0.1678 0.0116


Table 12.2 Basic Demographics – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7 <strong>and</strong> Entry Assessment at 5<br />

Full Sample Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

149<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Log of household income<br />

Early Measure<br />

0.986***<br />

(0.292)<br />

Mother’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

-1.294***<br />

(0.324)<br />

A-level<br />

0.415*<br />

(0.252)<br />

Degree<br />

1.146***<br />

(0.365)<br />

Partner’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

-1.137***<br />

(0.296)<br />

A-level<br />

0.316<br />

(0.253)<br />

Degree<br />

1.579***<br />

(0.328)<br />

Lone parent status (base = never lone parent)<br />

Intermittently, with partner at 47<br />

months<br />

Intermittently, no partner at 47<br />

months<br />

Since birth<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth (base = 25-29)<br />

1<br />

(0.571)<br />

Older siblings (base = none)<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3+<br />

(0.429) (0.541) (0.562) (0.601)<br />

-0.905***<br />

(0.267)<br />

-1.897***<br />

(0.346)<br />

-2.760***<br />

(0.511)<br />

-0.318<br />

(0.308)<br />

-1.472**<br />

(0.747)<br />

-1.136***<br />

(0.351)<br />

-2.394***<br />

(0.456)<br />

-2.946***<br />

(0.663)<br />

-0.314<br />

(0.320)<br />

-1.934**<br />

(0.776)<br />

-0.673*<br />

(0.364)<br />

-2.276***<br />

(0.474)<br />

-3.335***<br />

(0.688)<br />

-0.004<br />

(0.342)<br />

0.461<br />

(0.829)<br />

-0.463<br />

(0.389)<br />

-0.117<br />

(0.506)<br />

0.389<br />

(0.735)<br />

Partner’s employment status at 47 months (base = in work)<br />

-0.455<br />

-0.224<br />

-0.086<br />

-0.139<br />

Not in work<br />

(0.417)<br />

(0.541)<br />

(0.562)<br />

(0.600)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1613 0.1639 0.2174 0.0257


13. Parenting Behaviours <strong>and</strong> Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7<br />

This section <strong>and</strong> the following ones explore whether the mediating influences appear <strong>to</strong> have<br />

different effects at <strong>age</strong>s 5 <strong>and</strong> 7. The mediating influences we consider all pre-date <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong> so<br />

we might expect that some aspects of what effects early <strong>child</strong> learning will dissipate as <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

<strong>age</strong>. We start with parental interactions with their <strong>child</strong>ren between <strong>age</strong>s 8 months <strong>and</strong> 4 years.<br />

Our first report showed that the parenting behaviours in the pre-school period were a strong<br />

influence on EA scores. The largest impact came from maternal teaching behaviour in the preschool<br />

period. The effect of maternal teaching was large, <strong>and</strong> greater than effects of mother’s<br />

education. There was no evidence that the teaching of better educated mothers was more<br />

effective than that of the less educated. Maternal reading behaviour was quantitatively less<br />

important but less intensive reading resulted in substantial deficits. Other parenting behaviours<br />

such as the <strong>child</strong> watching 6+ hours of TV a week at 18 months <strong>and</strong> breast-feeding beyond 6<br />

months were of moderate importance.<br />

Table 13.1 Parenting – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7 <strong>and</strong> Entry Assessment at 5<br />

Full Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Simple Value<br />

Added<br />

Maternal reading score (base = high)<br />

Very low -1.292*** -1.521*** -1.657*** 0.137<br />

(0.428) (0.554) (0.576) (0.615)<br />

Low -0.738** -0.966** -1.015** 0.049<br />

(0.375) (0.490) (0.509) (0.544)<br />

Medium -0.381 -0.346 -0.328 -0.018<br />

(0.253) (0.329) (0.342) (0.365)<br />

Teaching score (base = high)<br />

Very low -2.386*** -3.087*** -3.661*** 0.574<br />

(0.443) (0.578) (0.601) (0.642)<br />

Low -1.721*** -1.865*** -2.055*** 0.190<br />

(0.354) (0.455) (0.473) (0.505)<br />

Medium -0.694*** -0.905*** -1.110*** 0.205<br />

(0.229) (0.296) (0.308) (0.329)<br />

18 month TV score (base = 0-2 hours per week)<br />

3 –5 0.118 0.129 -0.339 0.468<br />

150


Table 13.1 Parenting – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7 <strong>and</strong> Entry Assessment at 5 (continued)<br />

(0.236) (0.317) (0.330) (0.352)<br />

6 0.110 -0.108 -1.085** 0.977*<br />

(0.357) (0.460) (0.478) (0.511)<br />

Duration of breastfeeding (base = never)<br />

Breastfed 6 months<br />

0.784*** 0.968*** 0.763** 0.205<br />

(0.282) (0.359) (0.373) (0.399)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1613 0.1639 0.2174 0.0257<br />

Our analysis of <strong>age</strong> 7 attainment scores (Table 13.1) shows that maternal teaching <strong>and</strong> maternal<br />

reading during pre-school remain significant determinants of cognitive outcomes. The relative<br />

impact of the two behaviours is the same: teaching has a larger impact than reading. Again, the<br />

impact of maternal teaching is greater than the effect of maternal education (shown in Table<br />

13.1). Breastfeeding is also a significant determinant of <strong>age</strong> 7 attainment outcomes, but long<br />

hours of TV watching when young appears no longer <strong>to</strong> affect outcomes.<br />

A comparison of the impact of parental behaviours at <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong> <strong>age</strong> 7 (comparison of columns 2<br />

<strong>and</strong> 3) indicates that, with the sole exception of TV watching, the impact of the parental<br />

behaviours on <strong>child</strong> outcomes is very similar at the two <strong>age</strong>s. There is some indication that the<br />

impact of early teaching behaviour is slightly weaker for <strong>age</strong> 7 than <strong>age</strong> 5 outcomes, but these<br />

changes are not statistically significant in the value added equation.<br />

Given these similarities, it is not surprising that when we examine the effect of these behaviours<br />

on value added – the change in test scores – they have no impact. Put another way, once we<br />

condition on the effect these behaviours have on <strong>age</strong> 5 test scores, there is no diminution after<br />

that <strong>age</strong>. There is one exception: high levels of TV watching when young appears <strong>to</strong> be<br />

associated with high value added between <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong> 7, so the early adverse effect diminishes.<br />

So early TV watching has no lasting effect as <strong>child</strong>ren from households in which the TV is on a<br />

lot when they are young catch <strong>up</strong> relative <strong>to</strong> their peers once at school.<br />

151


14. Outings <strong>and</strong> the Home Learning Environment <strong>and</strong><br />

Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7<br />

The outings <strong>and</strong> home environment measures are strongly correlated with parenting behaviours<br />

around TV watching <strong>and</strong> reading <strong>and</strong> with having a low income. Entry assessment scores at <strong>age</strong><br />

5 were significantly associated with several aspects of the home environment. A very low book<br />

score (which in turn is associated with low reading <strong>and</strong> low income), which affects the 6% of<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren, was a substantive negative influence on <strong>child</strong> attainment. Similarly, a very low <strong>to</strong>y<br />

score (measured at 24 months <strong>and</strong> covering 7% of <strong>child</strong>ren with lowest cumulative score for a<br />

range of <strong>to</strong>ys) was also found <strong>to</strong> be detrimental <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong> cognitive <strong>development</strong>. The pattern of<br />

outings also affected attainment, where trips <strong>to</strong> libraries had a positive impact <strong>and</strong> attending<br />

department s<strong>to</strong>res a negative impact on <strong>development</strong> at <strong>age</strong> 5.<br />

The results for attainment at <strong>age</strong> 7 indicate that these behaviours have a weaker link with<br />

attainment. The effect of being in a home with a low book score is negatively <strong>and</strong> significantly<br />

associated with the <strong>age</strong> 7 attainment score. The effect is somewhat smaller than at <strong>age</strong> 5<br />

although the change is not statistically significant. The number of <strong>to</strong>ys is no longer significantly<br />

associated with attainment. The library as the destination of outings retains a significant impact<br />

but one that is around half the size of its impact on <strong>age</strong> 5 outcomes. Trips <strong>to</strong> department s<strong>to</strong>res<br />

when young have no effect on attainment at <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

Conditioning on the link between these behaviours <strong>and</strong> outcomes at <strong>age</strong> 5, none of these<br />

behaviours have a significant effect by <strong>age</strong> 7. In other words, once we have taken in<strong>to</strong> account<br />

their effect on entry assessment scores, they do not explain any of the variance in <strong>age</strong> 7 scores.<br />

However, there is a pattern across all these variables that their impact has declined somewhat.<br />

So these results suggest that the effects of the early home learning environment, which are<br />

strongly linked <strong>to</strong> income, have modestly weakened by <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

152


Table 14.1 Outings <strong>and</strong> Home Environment – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7 <strong>and</strong> Entry Assessment at<br />

5<br />

Full Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

Simple Value<br />

Added<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Outings scores<br />

Department s<strong>to</strong>res -0.075 -0.249 -0.536* 0.287<br />

(0.207) (0.268) (0.279) (0.298)<br />

Library 0.402* 0.412 0.909*** -0.496<br />

(0.217) (0.284) (0.296) (0.316)<br />

Book score (base = high)<br />

Very low -1.391*** -1.059* -1.833*** 0.773<br />

(0.488) (0.628) (0.652) (0.696)<br />

Low -0.303 -0.274 -0.505 0.231<br />

(0.285) (0.371) (0.385) (0.411)<br />

Medium -0.036 -0.043 -0.473 0.429<br />

(0.277) (0.364) (0.378) (0.404)<br />

Toy score (base = high)<br />

Very low 0.101 -0.715 -1.364** 0.649<br />

(0.474) (0.604) (0.628) (0.671)<br />

Low 0.108 -0.196 -0.478 0.282<br />

(0.333) (0.435) (0.452) (0.483)<br />

Medium 0.237 0.038 0.389 -0.351<br />

(0.265) (0.350) (0.364) (0.388)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1613 0.1639 0.2174 0.0257<br />

153


15. Pre-School Childcare <strong>and</strong> Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7<br />

The study of exposure <strong>to</strong> early <strong>child</strong>care is discussed in three sections here, as well as in Part 1<br />

of the report. Firstly, exposure <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> the <strong>age</strong> of 24 months is considered, measured<br />

by the types of care used <strong>and</strong> whether this was for 5-19 or over 20 hours per week. Then the<br />

type of <strong>child</strong>care or early education setting immediately prior <strong>to</strong> entry in<strong>to</strong> school, normally at<br />

<strong>age</strong> 4 is considered. Finally we measure who the <strong>child</strong> went <strong>to</strong> pre-school with – the peer gro<strong>up</strong>.<br />

The last measure is considered later in the study whilst here we deal with the type of provision.<br />

Table 15.1 Childcare Utilisation – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7 <strong>and</strong> Entry Assessment at 5<br />

Full Sample Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Childcare pre-<strong>age</strong> 3<br />

Used partner 5-19 hours per 0.001 0.109 0.584 -0.475<br />

week (0.259) (0.342) (0.355) (0.380)<br />

Used partner 20+ hours per 0.086 0.137 0.031 0.106<br />

week (0.259) (0.342) (0.355) (0.379)<br />

Used friend/relative 5-19 -0.494** -0.555* -0.234 -0.321<br />

hours per week (0.240) (0.312) (0.324) (0.346)<br />

Used friend/relative 20+ hours -0.567** -0.683** -0.907*** 0.225<br />

per week (0.260) (0.338) (0.351) (0.375)<br />

Used paid person 5-19 hours -0.415 -0.323 -0.501 0.178<br />

per week (0.343) (0.467) (0.486) (0.519)<br />

Used paid person 20+ hours -0.374 -0.042 0.117 -0.159<br />

per week (0.293) (0.401) (0.416) (0.445)<br />

Used centre-based care 5-19 -0.303 -0.645 -0.085 -0.560<br />

hours per week (0.430) (0.599) (0.622) (0.664)<br />

Used centre-based care 20+ 0.841 0.396 0.903 -0.507<br />

hours per week (0.562) (0.827) (0.859) (0.918)<br />

Childcare <strong>age</strong> 3 <strong>to</strong> 4<br />

Nursery class -0.568* -0.344 1.047** -1.391***<br />

(0.314) (0.403) (0.418) (0.447)<br />

LEA nursery school 0.281 0.267 -0.192 0.459<br />

(0.391) (0.513) (0.533) (0.569)<br />

Private day nursery -0.115 -0.126 0.707 -0.832*<br />

(0.330) (0.439) (0.456) (0.487)<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> 0.401 0.060 -0.081 0.141<br />

(0.247) (0.331) (0.344) (0.367)<br />

Nursery (type undefined) -0.022 -0.067 1.157*** -1.224***<br />

(0.263) (0.357) (0.371) (0.396)<br />

Other -0.745 -0.787 -1.859 1.072<br />

(0.767) (1.119) (1.162) (1.242)<br />

Simple Value Added<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1613 0.1639 0.2174 0.0257<br />

The key findings in our assessment of the relationship between pre-school <strong>child</strong>care <strong>and</strong><br />

attainment at school entry were:<br />

• Limited hours of care by a partner (normally the father of the <strong>child</strong>) <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 is<br />

154


modestly beneficial <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> (this could well be outside the other partner’s,<br />

usually the mother’s working hours)<br />

• Extended care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 by friends, gr<strong>and</strong>parents <strong>and</strong> other relatives is modestly<br />

harmful.<br />

• Nursery care (in school class or private setting) when <strong>age</strong>d 3-4 is modestly beneficial <strong>to</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment on entry <strong>to</strong> school.<br />

• There is no evidence that full-time Nursery exposure at 3 or 4 is more beneficial than<br />

part-time.<br />

• There is no evidence that attending a Nursery is more beneficial for those with less<br />

educated mothers.<br />

Table 15.1 shows these patterns have substantially changed by the time <strong>child</strong>ren have<br />

experienced the first two years of full-time schooling. The impact of nursery attendance in the<br />

immediate pre-school period has vanished for all three gro<strong>up</strong>s of nursery care, school based,<br />

private <strong>and</strong> unknown. The robustness of this result across all three types of nursery care makes it<br />

clear this is a general phenomenon. Hence exposure <strong>to</strong> pre-school learning in a nursery setting<br />

has no greater effect on attainment at 7 than just attending a parent run playgro<strong>up</strong> or indeed<br />

nothing at all (although few <strong>child</strong>ren had no pre-school exposure). Note that the lack of<br />

assessment of quality of <strong>child</strong>care means these results are less reliable than other studies of the<br />

influence of quality <strong>child</strong>care on <strong>development</strong> such as the EPPE project (Sylva et al. 2004).<br />

The one persistent finding is that <strong>child</strong>care by friends or relatives is associated with modestly<br />

adverse educational outcomes at <strong>age</strong> 7 <strong>and</strong> that this is now true for 5-19 hours as well as over 20<br />

hours a week.<br />

155


16. Mother’s Childhood <strong>and</strong> <strong>Family</strong> Background <strong>and</strong><br />

Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7<br />

The influence of the mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong> on <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment at <strong>age</strong><br />

5 was modest. The mother having grown <strong>up</strong> in an intact family throughout her <strong>child</strong>hood had a<br />

modest positive influence as did not having a his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy <strong>and</strong> having a positive attitude<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards her own education experience. So these fac<strong>to</strong>rs describing the mother’s own <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

are generally not a major source of influence on the <strong>child</strong> once current circumstances are<br />

conditioned on. By <strong>age</strong> 7 even these modest effects had further diminished <strong>and</strong> the only<br />

sustained influence is the mother not having a his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy in her own <strong>child</strong>hood.<br />

Table 16.1 Mother’s Childhood <strong>and</strong> <strong>Family</strong> Background – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7 <strong>and</strong> Entry<br />

Assessment at 5<br />

Full Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Simple Value<br />

Added<br />

Tenure of mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = mortg<strong>age</strong>/owned<br />

Council rented -0.605** -0.248 -0.499 0.252<br />

(0.269) (0.340) (0.353) (0.377)<br />

Other rented 0.695 1.042 -0.571 1.613*<br />

(0.564) (0.762) (0.791) (0.845)<br />

Other 0.106 -0.001 0.800 -0.802<br />

(0.513) (0.676) (0.702) (0.750)<br />

Mother’s father present in <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = not at all)<br />

Throughout 0.680 0.593 1.267* -0.674<br />

(0.527) (0.691) (0.718) (0.766)<br />

Partly 0.451 0.305 1.137 -0.831<br />

(0.571) (0.749) (0.778) (0.831)<br />

Mother’s mother’s highest qualification (base = CSE/none)<br />

0.225 0.269 0.354 -0.085<br />

Vocational/O-level<br />

(0.260) (0.341) (0.354) (0.378)<br />

A-level 0.014 -0.035 0.730 -0.764<br />

156


Table 16.1 Mother’s Childhood <strong>and</strong> <strong>Family</strong> Background – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7 <strong>and</strong> Entry<br />

Assessment at 5 (continued)<br />

Full Sample Restricted Sample<br />

Simple Value<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Added<br />

(0.326) (0.447) (0.464) (0.496)<br />

Degree 0.308 0.295 0.141 0.154<br />

(0.528) (0.769) (0.799) (0.853)<br />

Mother has a his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy (base = yes)<br />

No 1.246*** 1.436*** 1.281** 0.155<br />

(0.423) (0.535) (0.556) (0.594)<br />

Mother feels school was a valuable experience (base = not sure/generally not/no)<br />

Yes very 0.322 0.309 -0.321 0.631<br />

(0.302) (0.399) (0.414) (0.442)<br />

Yes generally -0.068 -0.285 -0.952*** 0.666*<br />

(0.258) (0.335) (0.348) (0.372)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1613 0.1639 0.2174 0.0257<br />

157


17. Mother’s Health, social networks, <strong>Family</strong> Conflict <strong>and</strong><br />

Attainment Up <strong>to</strong> 7<br />

This gro<strong>up</strong> of potential influences were not found <strong>to</strong> have a large impact on <strong>child</strong>ren’s<br />

educational <strong>development</strong>. The variable from this gro<strong>up</strong> retaining a significant effect was only<br />

the mother’s locus of control measure. This, in itself, suggested that mother’s mental <strong>and</strong><br />

physical health <strong>and</strong> within family conflict had little influence on a <strong>child</strong>’s educational<br />

<strong>development</strong> pre-school. As Table 17.1 indicates, the role of the mother’s locus of control<br />

persists <strong>and</strong> may have increased marginally in strength. Mothers who feel their actions have<br />

little impact also eng<strong>age</strong> in less teaching <strong>and</strong> reading of their <strong>child</strong>ren than do other parents. So<br />

this influence is quite substantial in that it may causally reduce parental teaching as well as<br />

having a modest independent effect over <strong>and</strong> above this.<br />

Table 17.1 Maternal Health, Attitudes <strong>and</strong> Social Networks – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7 <strong>and</strong> Entry<br />

Assessment at 5<br />

No. of<br />

observations<br />

Full Sample Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

Score<br />

7537<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

4996<br />

Entry<br />

Assessment<br />

4996 4996<br />

Simple Value<br />

Added<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score (base = low)<br />

Medium 0.317 0.102 0.038 0.065<br />

(0.244) (0.322) (0.335) (0.357)<br />

High 0.177 0.074 0.463 -0.389<br />

(0.259) (0.337) (0.351) (0.375)<br />

Locus of control score (base = Internal)<br />

Medium -0.370 -0.386 -0.591* 0.205<br />

(0.228) (0.299) (0.311) (0.332)<br />

External -1.423*** -1.646*** -1.046** -0.600<br />

(0.372) (0.472) (0.490) (0.524)<br />

Adjusted R-<br />

squared 0.1613 0.1639 0.2174 0.0257<br />

158


18. Neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> Peer Gro<strong>up</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Attainment Up <strong>to</strong><br />

7<br />

This section looks at how the relationships between neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

attainment are related <strong>to</strong> other mediating influences. As part of the Focus @ 7 face-<strong>to</strong>-face<br />

interviews with mother <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>, the mother was asked <strong>to</strong> document <strong>child</strong>care providers used<br />

from the <strong>age</strong> of 3. Importantly this included the name of the provider as well as the broad type<br />

of care <strong>and</strong> number of sessions attended. In many cases therefore we can observe many <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

attending the same pre-school provider immediately prior <strong>to</strong> school entry. From this then we can<br />

get a measure of attainment among the <strong>child</strong>’s pre-school peer gro<strong>up</strong>.<br />

We were concerned that many <strong>child</strong>ren from one provider would be assessed by the same<br />

teacher for their EA score so that any idiosyncratic teacher effects in scoring would muddy this<br />

measure of the pre-school peer gro<strong>up</strong>. Such teacher effects should be just r<strong>and</strong>om measurement<br />

error over the whole population. But for <strong>child</strong>ren within the same school this practice would<br />

give the impression that the peer gro<strong>up</strong> was very influential whereas in reality it was just teacher<br />

bias (a teacher scoring the <strong>child</strong>ren higher or lower than aver<strong>age</strong>). So we construct our measure<br />

of the peer gro<strong>up</strong> from those who attended the same pre-school but did not attend the same<br />

school (<strong>and</strong> so were scored for EA by a different teacher). These observations are thus<br />

independent of the <strong>child</strong>. Where a <strong>child</strong> attends more than one provider we aver<strong>age</strong> across the<br />

different providers, weighting by the number of sessions attended. This peer gro<strong>up</strong> measure will<br />

capture a range of influences from the natural attainment of the other <strong>child</strong>ren, their family<br />

<strong>background</strong> etc. <strong>and</strong> any common influence of the pre-school. It thus includes any effects of the<br />

quality of the pre-school provider.<br />

We can go further in separating influences. To get at <strong>child</strong>care quality we do the following. We<br />

predict the other <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment using the general model containing all family <strong>background</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> mediating influences provided in the data, but excluding the pre-school provider. This is<br />

then a measure of the extent <strong>to</strong> which the <strong>child</strong>ren forming the peer gro<strong>up</strong> are from more<br />

affluent, better educated families etc. This measure therefore does not contain any element of<br />

the quality of pre-school provider as it is based only on the family characteristics of the <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

attending it. The unexplained residual from these estimates (the difference between the actual<br />

EA score <strong>and</strong> the predicted score of the peer gro<strong>up</strong>) then reflects the variation in attainment<br />

across <strong>child</strong>ren in the peer gro<strong>up</strong> that is not explained by the <strong>child</strong>’s <strong>background</strong>. This will<br />

contain the idiosyncratic component for each individual <strong>child</strong> aver<strong>age</strong>d across the peer gro<strong>up</strong>,<br />

159


which will be influenced by the quality of the common attendance of the same pre-school<br />

provider. It will also reflect any measurement error in the data. Large amounts of measurement<br />

error will make it hard <strong>to</strong> observe <strong>child</strong>care quality for the small samples which attend each<br />

provider.<br />

Table 18.1 shows, for the EA test, having attended a pre-school provider whose intake is from<br />

families who have characteristics associated with high attaining <strong>child</strong>ren results in gains for a<br />

<strong>child</strong>. This is probably because attending a provider that has <strong>child</strong>ren drawn from families with<br />

high incomes, positive parenting behaviours, etc, has a strong peer gro<strong>up</strong> influence on a <strong>child</strong>.<br />

The residual component of attainment among peer gro<strong>up</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren, the part not predicted by each<br />

<strong>child</strong>’s <strong>background</strong> characteristics, is negatively associated with attainment. If this reflected<br />

attainment amongst a common population attending the same pre-school then this relationship<br />

should be positive. So we can find no evidence of a pre-school quality effect, this is probably<br />

because this residual is being swamped by measurement error in the data. Finally, coming from<br />

a ward where education levels in the population are low is associated with less <strong>child</strong> attainment.<br />

By <strong>age</strong> 7 the main peer gro<strong>up</strong> effect disappears. Those who attended a pre-school with <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

whose family <strong>background</strong>s are associated with high achieving event (<strong>and</strong> who went on <strong>to</strong><br />

different primary schools) are no longer out performing other <strong>child</strong>ren. So early peer gro<strong>up</strong><br />

influences appear very short lived. Put <strong>to</strong>gether with the evidence on the lack of effects from the<br />

types of pre-school providers used, this suggests that the form of pre-school education has no<br />

lasting impact on <strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

160


Table 18.1 Neighbourhood – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score at 7 <strong>and</strong> Entry Assessment at 5<br />

Full Sample Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Simple Value<br />

Added<br />

Ward education -0.501*** -0.789*** -0.827*** 0.039<br />

domain score (0.116) (0.157) (0.163) (0.174)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers – predicted component (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile<br />

3 rd quintile<br />

4 th quintile<br />

Highest quintile<br />

0.094 -0.053 0.164 -0.217<br />

(0.472) (0.507) (0.527) (0.563)<br />

0.454 0.168 0.939* -0.771<br />

(0.473) (0.513) (0.533) (0.569)<br />

0.730 0.384 1.864*** -1.480**<br />

(0.479) (0.523) (0.543) (0.580)<br />

0.106 -0.590 1.688*** -2.278***<br />

(0.496) (0.558) (0.579) (0.619)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers – residual component (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile -1.040** -1.058** -0.989* -0.070<br />

3 rd quintile<br />

4 th quintile<br />

Highest quintile<br />

Full Sample Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

Simple Value<br />

Added<br />

(0.467) (0.499) (0.518) (0.554)<br />

-1.356*** -1.356*** -0.683 -0.673<br />

(0.468) (0.502) (0.521) (0.557)<br />

-0.995** -1.034** -0.966* -0.068<br />

(0.468) (0.501) (0.520) (0.555)<br />

-1.719*** -1.635*** -0.802 -0.833<br />

(0.460) (0.491) (0.511) (0.545)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1613 0.1639 0.2174 0.0257<br />

The measured impact of the neighbourhood, captured by the extent of low educational<br />

attainment in different parts of the population remains as strong at <strong>age</strong> 7 as at <strong>age</strong> 5. So whilst<br />

the neighbourhood effect remains persistent, the influence of the peer gro<strong>up</strong> at a pre-school<br />

provider is temporary.<br />

161


19. Overview of <strong>Family</strong> Background <strong>and</strong> Child<br />

Development on Entry <strong>to</strong> School<br />

The final substantive section brings <strong>to</strong>gether the major <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> mediating influences<br />

<strong>and</strong> provides an overview of the key drivers in <strong>child</strong> attainment by <strong>age</strong> 7. We start <strong>to</strong> do this by<br />

partitioning out the amount of the <strong>to</strong>tal gap in attainment between the <strong>to</strong>p <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quarters of<br />

the attainment distribution at <strong>age</strong> 7 that can be explained by each gro<strong>up</strong>ing of influences.<br />

Table 19.1 decomposes the variation in Aver<strong>age</strong> Attainment Score from tests at <strong>age</strong> 7 in<strong>to</strong> that<br />

which all the data used here can explain <strong>and</strong> the remaining unexplained variation. This<br />

unexplained element will include any measurement error <strong>and</strong> unmeasured influences. Then we<br />

further decompose the explained variation in<strong>to</strong> the fac<strong>to</strong>rs underlying it. As well as the <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

attainment picture we also focus on attainment gaps across parental education <strong>and</strong> family<br />

income.<br />

The gap in aver<strong>age</strong> attainment between those in the <strong>to</strong>p <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quarters of the population of<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren is just under 24 points or two st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations. In Columns 1-3 we only decompose<br />

the amount of this gap that is associated with family income at <strong>age</strong> 3-4 <strong>and</strong> parental education<br />

(unconditional of other fac<strong>to</strong>rs). Together these explain around 10% of the overall attainment<br />

gap at <strong>age</strong> 7 with parental education being around three times as powerful as income.<br />

Introducing all other <strong>background</strong> controls <strong>and</strong> all the mediating fac<strong>to</strong>rs raises the amount of the<br />

attainment gap explained <strong>to</strong> nearly 20%. The strong influences other than income <strong>and</strong> parental<br />

education are other <strong>background</strong> measures (gender, ethnicity, numbers of siblings etc.), parenting<br />

activity, the home learning environment, neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> mothers’ <strong>child</strong>hood experiences.<br />

Childcare <strong>and</strong> mothers’ mental <strong>and</strong> physical health are of negligible associated with the<br />

attainment gap.<br />

162


Table 19.1: Decomposition of range of mean scores for various gro<strong>up</strong>s (Aver<strong>age</strong> score of<br />

Key St<strong>age</strong> One <strong>and</strong> Focus at 7) – Restricted sample<br />

Difference in mean<br />

scores<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of attainment<br />

Points<br />

Explained variation – contribution of:<br />

%<br />

%<strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation Points<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation Points<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of<br />

household income<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation Points<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Children of mothers with<br />

CSE/no qualifications vs. a<br />

degree<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

23.8 100 100 23.8 100 100 5.9 100 100 8.4 100 100<br />

Income<br />

0.6 2.7 27.6 0.3 1.3 6.9 1.7 28.3 29.2 0.7 8.0 8.0<br />

Parental Education 1.7 7.3 72.7 1.0 4.2 22.7 1.7 28.6 29.5 4.0 47.4 47.4<br />

Other Demographic<br />

Variables<br />

- - - 1.1 4.6 25.4 0.4 6.0 6.2 0.6 6.8 6.8<br />

Childcare - - - 0.1 0.6 3.1 0.1 1.8 1.8 0.2 2.7 2.7<br />

Parenting - - - 0.5 2.3 12.6 0.2 3.4 3.5 0.6 6.5 6.5<br />

Outings & Home<br />

Environment<br />

Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

<strong>and</strong> family<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Maternal health,<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> social<br />

networks<br />

- - - 0.3 1.5 8.0 0.3 5.9 6.0 0.6 7.1 7.1<br />

- - - 0.3 1.2 6.6 0.4 7.0 7.2 0.7 7.9 7.9<br />

- - - 0.2 1.0 5.3 0.4 7.3 7.6 0.6 6.8 6.8<br />

Neighbourhood - - - 0.4 1.7 9.3 0.5 8.7 9.0 0.6 6.7 6.7<br />

Unexplained<br />

variation<br />

21.5 90.3 - 19.4 81.7 - 0.2 2.9 - 0.0 0.0 -<br />

The attainment gap between the richest <strong>and</strong> poorest quarters of the <strong>child</strong>ren is 5.9 points (or just<br />

under 0.6 of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation). This is a substantial attainment gap. Once we condition on all<br />

other measures of family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> the mediating influences around 28% of this gap<br />

remains unexplained (<strong>and</strong> is thus just correlated with income). Parental education <strong>and</strong> other<br />

<strong>background</strong> influences are associated with around 35% of the attainment gap between rich <strong>and</strong><br />

poor. The remaining influences are our proximal fac<strong>to</strong>rs, the routes through which income could<br />

be having a direct influence. Childcare <strong>and</strong> parenting are relatively minor routes through which<br />

income influences the attainment differences between rich <strong>and</strong> poor <strong>child</strong>ren. The more<br />

substantial influences are neighbourhood (including the pre-school peer gro<strong>up</strong>), maternal mental<br />

<strong>and</strong> physical health, mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> the home learning environment.<br />

The attainment gap between those with degree educated mothers <strong>and</strong> those with mothers with<br />

163


very low or no qualifications, is 8.4 points (or 0.8 of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation). Around half of this<br />

gap is unexplained by the model <strong>and</strong> could reflect genetic <strong>and</strong> other influences. <strong>Family</strong> income<br />

<strong>and</strong> other family <strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs are associated with around 15% of this gap but all the other<br />

proximal influences are powerful transmitters of parental education with the exception of<br />

<strong>child</strong>care choices. So our mediating or proximal influences explain more of how parental<br />

education is transmitted <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren than of how family income is transmitted, although our<br />

measure of family income is somewhat dated by <strong>age</strong> 7, being an aver<strong>age</strong> of income at <strong>age</strong>s 3<br />

<strong>and</strong> 4.<br />

When compared with the results for Entry Assessment at <strong>age</strong> 5, (Table 19.2) the raw<br />

relationships between attainment <strong>and</strong> family income <strong>and</strong> parental education at <strong>age</strong> 7 are very<br />

similar. However, once we introduce all <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> mediating influences it is clear that the<br />

model explains less of the overall variation in attainment at <strong>age</strong> 7 than at <strong>age</strong> 5. There is a<br />

general decline in the predictive power of pre-school experiences on <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> as the<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren <strong>age</strong> (from 25% of the attainment gap between the highest <strong>and</strong> least able quarters of the<br />

population, <strong>to</strong> around 20%). This could reflect a simple <strong>age</strong>ing process or other influences<br />

coming <strong>to</strong> bear, the most obvious being schooling. Looking at the influences in detail, by<br />

comparing Tables 19.1 <strong>and</strong> 19.2, suggests widespread small declines in the influences of<br />

parenting behaviours, neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>s, the home learning environment <strong>and</strong> other<br />

<strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs. The influence of parental education <strong>and</strong> income is more stable.<br />

When we focus on the attainment gap between the richest <strong>and</strong> poorest quarters of <strong>child</strong>ren we<br />

see a small convergence in attainment as the gap reduces from 6.9 points at <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>to</strong> 5.9 at <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

This convergence is especially apparent in the decline in the impact of neighbourhood <strong>and</strong> peer<br />

gro<strong>up</strong> in explaining the attainment gap between rich <strong>and</strong> poor. There are also declines in many<br />

other influences.<br />

164


Table 19.2: Decomposition of range of mean scores for various gro<strong>up</strong>s (Entry Assessment)<br />

– Restricted sample<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of attainment<br />

Points<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation Points<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation Points<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of<br />

household income<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation Points<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Children of mothers with<br />

CSE/no qualifications vs. a<br />

degree<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Difference in mean<br />

scores<br />

25.3 100 100 25.3 100 100 6.9 100 100 9.9 100 100<br />

Explained variation –<br />

contribution of:<br />

Income<br />

0.7 2.9 26.2 0.4 1.5 6.3 1.8 25.6 26.2 0.8 7.7 7.7<br />

Parental Education 2.1 8.3 73.8 1.0 3.9 16.8 1.5 22.3 22.9 4.1 41.6 41.6<br />

Other Demographic<br />

Variables<br />

- - - 1.4 5.5 23.6 0.5 7.3 7.5 0.5 4.6 4.6<br />

Childcare - - - 0.3 1.1 4.9 0.2 3.4 3.4 0.4 3.7 3.7<br />

Parenting - - - 0.9 3.4 14.5 0.4 5.7 5.8 0.9 8.8 8.8<br />

Outings & Home<br />

Environment<br />

Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

<strong>and</strong> family<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Maternal health,<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> social<br />

networks<br />

- - - 0.7 2.7 11.8 0.6 8.5 8.7 1.1 11.6 11.6<br />

- - - 0.3 1.2 5.0 0.3 4.7 4.8 0.6 6.0 6.0<br />

- - - 0.2 0.9 3.7 0.4 5.1 5.2 0.5 4.8 4.8<br />

Neighbourhood - - - 0.8 3.1 13.2 1.0 15.1 15.5 1.1 11.2 11.2<br />

Unexplained<br />

variation<br />

22.4 88.8 - 19.4 76.8 - 0.2 2.4 - 0.0 0.0 -<br />

As noted above, one of the reasons the model is explaining less of the patterns of attainment at<br />

<strong>age</strong> 7 may be that <strong>child</strong>ren are now attending school. To explore this in part, we repeat Table<br />

19.1 in Table 19.3, but now also include dummy variables for each school in the sample where<br />

more than four <strong>child</strong>ren attend (the aver<strong>age</strong> is 20). These school fixed effects will capture<br />

variation in a school’s contribution <strong>to</strong> its <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment from the typical (mean) school.<br />

This school effect may vary systematically with aspects of <strong>child</strong>ren’s pre-school <strong>background</strong>,<br />

perhaps because more affluent or educated parents seek out better schools (see Gibbons <strong>and</strong><br />

Machin, 2003) or perhaps because schools are more effective in raising attainment for certain<br />

gro<strong>up</strong>s of <strong>child</strong>ren than for others <strong>and</strong> these kinds of <strong>child</strong>ren are concentrated in some schools.<br />

The detailed regression information is contained in Appendix A4.<br />

165


Table 19.3: Decomposition of range of mean scores for various gro<strong>up</strong>s (Aver<strong>age</strong> score of<br />

Key St<strong>age</strong> One <strong>and</strong> Focus at 7) – Restricted sample<br />

Including School Dummies<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of attainment<br />

Points<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation Points<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation Points<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Top <strong>and</strong> bot<strong>to</strong>m quartiles of<br />

household income<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation Points<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Children of mothers with<br />

CSE/no qualifications vs. a<br />

degree<br />

%<br />

% <strong>to</strong>tal<br />

explained<br />

variation<br />

variation<br />

accounted<br />

accounted<br />

for by:<br />

for by:<br />

Difference in mean<br />

scores<br />

23.8 100 100 23.8 100 100 5.9 100 100 8.4 100 100<br />

Explained variation –<br />

contribution of:<br />

Income<br />

0.6 2.7 27.3 0.4 1.7 6.8 2.0 33.9 34.2 0.9 10.6 10.6<br />

Parental Education 1.7 7.1 72.7 1.0 4.0 16.2 1.6 27.9 28.2 3.9 46.5 46.5<br />

Other Demographic<br />

Variables<br />

- - - 1.1 4.5 18.0 0.2 4.0 4.0 0.5 6.3 6.3<br />

Childcare - - - 0.2 0.7 2.7 0.2 2.9 2.9 0.3 3.4 3.4<br />

Parenting - - - 0.6 2.6 10.6 0.3 5.3 5.3 0.7 8.8 8.8<br />

Outings & Home<br />

Environment<br />

Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood<br />

<strong>and</strong> family<br />

<strong>background</strong><br />

Maternal health,<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> social<br />

networks<br />

- - - 0.3 1.4 5.8 0.4 6.0 6.0 0.6 7.3 7.3<br />

- - - 0.3 1.2 4.7 0.4 7.0 7.1 0.7 8.2 8.2<br />

- - - 0.2 0.7 2.9 0.3 5.6 5.7 0.4 5.1 5.1<br />

Neighbourhood - - - 0.4 1.7 7.0 0.6 9.9 10.0 0.6 7.2 7.2<br />

School Effects - - - 1.5 6.3 25.4 -0.2 -3.3 -3.4 -0.3 -3.3 -3.3<br />

Unexplained<br />

variation<br />

21.5 90.3 - 17.8 75.1 - 0.1 1.0 - 0.0 0.0 -<br />

When we introduce school fixed effects the explana<strong>to</strong>ry power of the model at <strong>age</strong> 7 rises <strong>to</strong><br />

25% (similar <strong>to</strong> that explained at <strong>age</strong> 5). The variation in school performance is now the most<br />

powerful gro<strong>up</strong> of predic<strong>to</strong>rs within the model, explaining around 6% of the overall variation.<br />

Including school fixed effects has little impact on the other fac<strong>to</strong>rs included, but the<br />

contributions of income, parenting behaviours <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>care rise slightly. So the variation in the<br />

impact of a specific school on <strong>child</strong>ren is weakly negatively correlated with income, involved<br />

parents <strong>and</strong> attending nurseries prior <strong>to</strong> school entry. This is clearer (for income) when we<br />

explore the attainment gap between the richest <strong>and</strong> poorest quarters of <strong>child</strong>ren (income<br />

measured at <strong>age</strong>s 3-4). At <strong>age</strong> 5 this gap was predicted <strong>to</strong> be 6.9 points but at 7 it has fallen<br />

slightly <strong>to</strong> 5.9 points, which may reflect that the income measure is more dated but also the<br />

166


school fixed effects (bot<strong>to</strong>m row), which are weakly negatively correlated with this gap. In their<br />

absence the gap between rich <strong>and</strong> poor would be wider at 6.1 points. So variations in school<br />

effectiveness have slightly narrowed the attainment gap between rich <strong>and</strong> poor <strong>child</strong>ren. We see<br />

similar results when considering parental education. The importance of parental education in<br />

predicting attainment has narrowed a little from <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 7 (a 9.9 point gap in attainment<br />

between those with most <strong>and</strong> least educated mothers at <strong>age</strong> 5 has fallen <strong>to</strong> 8.4 by <strong>age</strong> 7).<br />

However, in the absence of variations in school effectiveness this would be 0.3 points higher at<br />

8.7 points. So school effectiveness is helping very slightly <strong>to</strong> mitigate early (dis-)advant<strong>age</strong>s.<br />

The unexplained income effect <strong>and</strong> the mediating effects associated with parenting, the home<br />

learning environment <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>care all rise in importance (comparing columns 7-9 in Tables<br />

19.1 <strong>and</strong> 19.3).<br />

Table 19.4 reports the retrieved school fixed effects. These measure the mean deviation for all<br />

p<strong>up</strong>ils in a school from the others in the model conditional on all other systematic influences.<br />

The range of these school effects between the 10 th <strong>and</strong> 90 th percentiles is from approximately –4<br />

<strong>to</strong> +4 points. So predicted school effectiveness is a very large influence on <strong>child</strong> attainment.<br />

There is normally thought <strong>to</strong> be residual unobserved heterogeneity in cross-sectional data such<br />

as this, whereby p<strong>up</strong>ils with low predicted attainment may have other adverse unobserved<br />

characteristics. The model would then be expected <strong>to</strong> find that schools serving poor <strong>and</strong> low<br />

achieving <strong>child</strong>ren would look worse than other schools because of the unmeasured<br />

characteristics of the p<strong>up</strong>ils, rather than reflecting true school effectiveness. We actually find<br />

that the schools serving poorer <strong>child</strong>ren are predicted <strong>to</strong> have very marginally higher<br />

effectiveness. So the school effect very slightly reduces attainment gaps between the affluent<br />

<strong>and</strong> the poor for the common EA (<strong>age</strong> 5) <strong>and</strong> aver<strong>age</strong> <strong>age</strong> 7 samples. The correlation between<br />

log of household income <strong>and</strong> the school fixed effects is -0.0127 but is not significant. The<br />

correlation between mother's qualification <strong>and</strong> school is also negative, -0.0607, <strong>and</strong> is<br />

significant at 1%. However, on the larger sample at <strong>age</strong> 7 these effects diminish <strong>to</strong> be close <strong>to</strong><br />

zero. So variations in school effectiveness are not making a substantive contribution <strong>to</strong> the<br />

narrowing the attainment gap. But it is still possible that the modest general improvement might<br />

reflect schooling in general rather than specific school effectiveness. Alternatively the<br />

narrowing of attainment gaps between the deprived <strong>and</strong> affluent may merely reflect the <strong>age</strong>ing<br />

process whereby measures of family <strong>background</strong> before 5 have a diminished impact.<br />

167


Table 19.4 Retrieved School Fixed Effects Coefficients- Restricted sample, aver<strong>age</strong> scores<br />

10% 25% Median 75% 90%<br />

-3.93 -1.62 0.35 2.87 3.59<br />

168


169


20. Conclusions<br />

This report draws <strong>to</strong>gether the research undertaken at the Centre for Market <strong>and</strong> Public<br />

Organisation at the University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l for the Department for Education <strong>and</strong> Skills on<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive <strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong> behaviour <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 7. Part 1 covered <strong>development</strong> in the<br />

pre-school period, <strong>and</strong> looks at what insights the Children of the 90s database can provide about<br />

how dimensions of family <strong>background</strong> transmit in<strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s educational <strong>and</strong> behavioural<br />

<strong>development</strong> on entry in<strong>to</strong> primary school. The aim here is <strong>to</strong> look at the persistence of the<br />

picture <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 5 through the first two years of schooling. In particular the aim is <strong>to</strong> assess<br />

how aspects of early learning (dis-)advant<strong>age</strong>s based on family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> the role of preschool<br />

<strong>child</strong>care experiences persist or decay on school entry. Part 2 of the report is not seeking<br />

<strong>to</strong> identify what are key aspects of the family’s circumstances between <strong>age</strong>s 5 <strong>and</strong> 7 that<br />

influence <strong>development</strong> through that period but <strong>to</strong> see whether family circumstances prior <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong><br />

5 persist. One exception <strong>to</strong> this is we do explore the importance of the particular school attended<br />

<strong>to</strong> the degree of persistence of early educational (dis-)advant<strong>age</strong>s.<br />

Part 2 examines cognitive attainment at <strong>age</strong> 7. Drawn from Part 1, we look at a range of family<br />

<strong>background</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs often associated with variance in educational achievement, such as teen<br />

motherhood, family size, parental education <strong>and</strong> income, <strong>and</strong> investigate by what routes effects<br />

are transmitted <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren by measures family functioning <strong>and</strong> early education experiences. We<br />

focus on six main gro<strong>up</strong>s of fac<strong>to</strong>rs, which were found in the previous pre-school study <strong>to</strong><br />

mediate between dimensions of family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> attainment.<br />

The main findings are:<br />

• <strong>Family</strong> Background: In general most dimensions of family <strong>background</strong> have slightly<br />

weaker influence at <strong>age</strong> 7, after two years of schooling, than at <strong>age</strong> 5. This is most<br />

marked for <strong>age</strong> of mother, for low birth weight <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> for boys (as the gender gap<br />

diminishes but far from disappears). The fall in influence is less, however, for income,<br />

parental education <strong>and</strong> sibling numbers. This pattern of effects is not influenced by the<br />

inclusion or exclusion of our mediating influences.<br />

• Parenting patterns: Most effects, covering reading, activities, teaching <strong>and</strong> breast<br />

feeding, remain highly persistent, that is they are still important at <strong>age</strong> 7. There is a<br />

weakening of the impact early TV watching on <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive <strong>development</strong>.<br />

• The home learning environment <strong>and</strong> outings: Again there is evidence of a minor<br />

reduction in the impact of the home learning environment on <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment at 7<br />

170


compared <strong>to</strong> 5. The <strong>to</strong>y score, having few <strong>to</strong>ys in the home, becomes indistinguishable<br />

from having no effect at <strong>age</strong> 7. Whereas there was a significant relationship at <strong>age</strong> 5.<br />

• Child care: We look at us<strong>age</strong>, covering types <strong>and</strong> extent of care <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 2 1/2 <strong>and</strong><br />

more detailed study of <strong>child</strong> care exposure <strong>and</strong> limited measures of quality prior <strong>to</strong><br />

school entry at <strong>age</strong> 4. The reliance on friends <strong>and</strong> relatives for early care has persistently<br />

adverse effects on attainment, but all other early care arrangements appear broadly<br />

indistinguishable from each other <strong>and</strong> have no significant impact on outcomes at <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

Variations in the impact of exposure <strong>to</strong> different early learning environments<br />

immediately prior <strong>to</strong> school entry at <strong>age</strong> 4, all disappear after two years of schooling. So<br />

that while <strong>child</strong>ren who have just attended parent organised play gro<strong>up</strong>s were behind<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren who had attended nursery schools at <strong>age</strong> 5, there is no substantive difference in<br />

attainment by <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

• Maternal physical <strong>and</strong> mental health <strong>and</strong> social s<strong>up</strong>port networks: These fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

had little impact on <strong>child</strong>ren’s cognitive <strong>development</strong> but the importance of mothers’<br />

lack of locus of control score, the extent <strong>to</strong> which the mother feels her choices <strong>and</strong><br />

actions can affect the family’s circumstances, remains persistent through the early school<br />

years.<br />

• Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood experiences: The only persistent effect here is the association of<br />

poorer cognitive outcomes where the mother has a his<strong>to</strong>ry of truanting from school.<br />

• The neighbourhood of residence <strong>and</strong> peer gro<strong>up</strong>s in pre-school providers: By <strong>age</strong> 7<br />

the peer gro<strong>up</strong> effect at pre-school disappears. Those who attended a pre-school with<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren from family <strong>background</strong>s associated with high achievement (<strong>and</strong> who went on<br />

<strong>to</strong> different primary schools) are no longer out performing other <strong>child</strong>ren. So early peer<br />

gro<strong>up</strong> influences appear very short lived. Put <strong>to</strong>gether with the evidence on the lack of<br />

effects from the types of pre-school providers used, this suggests that the form of preschool<br />

education has no lasting impact on <strong>child</strong>ren. Ward characteristics still have an<br />

effect on outcomes at 7 as at 5, but impact is smaller at <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

• Overall Patterns of Attainment <strong>and</strong> Role of <strong>Family</strong> Background: The overall gap<br />

between the lowest achieving <strong>and</strong> highest achieving <strong>child</strong>ren become less predicted by<br />

measures of family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> the mediating influences studied in this research.<br />

The amount of the gap explained falls from 25% at <strong>age</strong> 5 <strong>to</strong> 20% at <strong>age</strong> 7. The variation<br />

in school effectiveness measured by individual school dummies captures almost exactly<br />

5% of this variation. So family <strong>background</strong> remains very powerful in predicting<br />

attainment but variation in schools attended appears <strong>to</strong> have a substantive effect by <strong>age</strong><br />

171


7. The elements of the model which are showing a lesser impact at <strong>age</strong> 7 than <strong>age</strong> 5 are<br />

aspects of family <strong>background</strong> other than family income <strong>and</strong> parental education, these<br />

being parenting <strong>and</strong> home learning environment, peer gro<strong>up</strong> influences <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> care<br />

type. So the modest weakening of the relationship of parental education <strong>and</strong> family<br />

income on attainment stems almost entirely from the weakening of the mediating<br />

influences, especially peer gro<strong>up</strong>, the home learning environment <strong>and</strong> parenting.<br />

The general picture is of a large degree of persistence in family <strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> proximal<br />

influences on <strong>child</strong>ren between the <strong>age</strong>s of 5 <strong>and</strong> 7, but there is a slight weakening of the<br />

importance of family circumstances. In particular, the <strong>child</strong> care provider attended <strong>and</strong> the<br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong> attending this provider have declining influence. This possibly suggests that the<br />

school peer gro<strong>up</strong> replaces the pre-school one for outcomes at <strong>age</strong> 7.<br />

Implications for future research<br />

This research makes a substantive contribution <strong>to</strong> the underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the origins of early<br />

learning differences in <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> their persistence on entry in<strong>to</strong> school. As these <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>age</strong><br />

ALSPAC will continue <strong>to</strong> offer a unique method of tracking how early learning <strong>and</strong> behaviour<br />

are transmitted through <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> in<strong>to</strong> adulthood <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> assess how other influences come<br />

forward <strong>to</strong> mitigate or accentuate early differences. The data will also be able <strong>to</strong> track whether<br />

recent government initiatives can alter this process, for instance, the Literacy Hour programme<br />

which these <strong>child</strong>ren will have experienced after <strong>age</strong> 7 or programmes such as the Community<br />

Trust Fund of anti-social behaviour.<br />

<strong>Family</strong> <strong>background</strong> is observed <strong>to</strong> be associated with major differences in attainment <strong>and</strong><br />

behaviour on school entry. These family <strong>background</strong> influences cross the dimensions of parental<br />

education, family income, <strong>age</strong> of mother at birth <strong>and</strong> numbers of siblings within the family. The<br />

early learning deficits of those from poorer <strong>and</strong> less educated families remain largely intact<br />

through the first two years of schooling but they are not continuing <strong>to</strong> worsen. These early<br />

learning deficits substantially stem from the association between the measures of family<br />

<strong>background</strong> <strong>and</strong> a set of proximal influences drawn from the data. The key proximal influences<br />

differ between behaviour <strong>and</strong> early learning outcomes. For both income <strong>and</strong> parental education,<br />

the mediating influence which is most important for early behaviour outcomes is the mother’s<br />

mental <strong>and</strong> physical health. This includes anxiety, stress <strong>and</strong> a weak locus of control.<br />

Disciplining behaviour is also important. For early learning, the physical home leaning<br />

172


environment (books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys), parental teaching <strong>and</strong> reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> the make <strong>up</strong> of<br />

peer gro<strong>up</strong>s in pre-school settings are key proximal influences.<br />

Through the first two years of schooling, the pattern of early learning across <strong>child</strong>ren remains<br />

broadly intact, in that most of the drivers of early learning differences before entry <strong>to</strong> school<br />

remain undiminished after two years of school. The deficits associated with having a teen<strong>age</strong><br />

mother weaken <strong>and</strong> those from ethnic minorities are doing better than Whites on aver<strong>age</strong> by <strong>age</strong><br />

7 (we do not have large enough samples <strong>to</strong> examine specific ethnic gro<strong>up</strong>s). The evidence that<br />

pre-school setting influences <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong> on school entry, including evidence that the<br />

pre-school peer gro<strong>up</strong> matters (which other <strong>child</strong>ren attend the same pre-school) show clear<br />

signs of diminishing through the first two years of schooling. A caveat is that this study does<br />

not have the evidence of quality in the pre-school setting available in other studies.<br />

Policy Implications<br />

Emerging evidence for the UK suggests that the extent of social (or intergeneration income)<br />

mobility lessens between cohorts of <strong>child</strong>ren born in 1958 (NCDS) <strong>and</strong> in 1970 (BCS) – see<br />

Bl<strong>and</strong>en Gregg <strong>and</strong> Machin, 2005. Additionally, Feinstein (2003) has identified that much, but<br />

far from all, of the educational deficit of more deprived <strong>child</strong>ren emerges prior <strong>to</strong> <strong>age</strong> 7. This<br />

research <strong>and</strong> other related pieces has stimulated a governmental interest in social mobility <strong>and</strong><br />

the role of early years experiences in shaping <strong>child</strong>ren’s opportunities. This piece of research<br />

looks at the evidence from a recent birth cohort (from 1991 <strong>to</strong> 1992) <strong>to</strong> assess the extent of early<br />

learning deficits associated with a number of dimensions of family <strong>background</strong>. The research<br />

goes on <strong>to</strong> assess the role of a range of measurable proximal influences (e.g. parental<br />

behaviours, choices <strong>and</strong> circumstances), <strong>and</strong> assess how important these are in the emerging<br />

evidence of early <strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong> in particular in transmitting dimensions of family<br />

<strong>background</strong> in<strong>to</strong> <strong>development</strong> outcomes. When discussing the policy implications there are<br />

instantly issues around causality. At its heart the question is whether the activity observed as<br />

being associated with different outcomes, for example parents reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren, is in it self<br />

the fac<strong>to</strong>r that makes the difference or does it reflect broader underlying differences between<br />

parents. So, if a policy was launched that successfully got parents who currently do not read <strong>to</strong><br />

their <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>to</strong> do so, but changed no other behaviours or attitudes, would we observe effects<br />

on <strong>child</strong>ren’s attainment on the scale suggested by this study? Alternatively, is reading <strong>to</strong> your<br />

<strong>child</strong> symp<strong>to</strong>matic of underlying differences between parents that do not change. The simple<br />

answer is that a study such as this can not answer these questions <strong>and</strong> what is required is<br />

173


experimental or pathfinder trials of initiatives (some of which may well already exist) <strong>to</strong> assess<br />

the potential for policy <strong>to</strong> make a difference.<br />

There are four broad areas of potential intervention, in the light of this study, that are worthy of<br />

policy discussion or experimentation.<br />

Parental Teaching <strong>and</strong> Reading<br />

First, is parental teaching, reading <strong>and</strong> activity with <strong>child</strong>ren. This is the single most powerful<br />

gro<strong>up</strong>ing of proximal influences on <strong>child</strong>ren’s early cognitive <strong>development</strong>. The extent <strong>to</strong> which<br />

different teaching behaviours lie behind deficits associated with low parental education or<br />

family income is worth reviewing. Such activity by parents is widespread but far from universal.<br />

The extent <strong>to</strong> which poorer or less well educated parents do less of these activities is quite<br />

modest. Nor is there any evidence that better educated parents do these activities more<br />

effectively. However, as these activities are so important the deficits observed among poorer<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren or those with less well educated parents are still important components of early<br />

disadvant<strong>age</strong>. To address this issue there are two, potentially complimentary approaches, the<br />

first is <strong>to</strong> promote teaching <strong>and</strong> reading among parents <strong>and</strong> the second is <strong>to</strong> exp<strong>and</strong> early years<br />

education in order <strong>to</strong> provide effective stimulation away from the home. Both these strategies<br />

are being tried, under Sure Start in the first instance <strong>and</strong> exp<strong>and</strong>ed pre-school education (free<br />

half day places from <strong>age</strong> 3) for the later (discussed below). The effectiveness of these<br />

programmes is not yet clear. There have been a range of parenting s<strong>up</strong>port programmes tried<br />

around the world with very mixed results. This literature needs study <strong>and</strong> best practice<br />

strategies tried under the auspices of Sure Start. The evidence suggests this is a hard thing <strong>to</strong> get<br />

right, certainly first time round, <strong>and</strong> so a range of trials <strong>and</strong> promulgating best practice across<br />

the country is probably required.<br />

Physical Home Learning Environment<br />

The second key area for potential intervention is one that is closely related <strong>to</strong> parental teaching<br />

activities, <strong>and</strong> that is the physical home leaning environment. For langu<strong>age</strong>, literacy <strong>and</strong> maths<br />

<strong>development</strong> this is mainly facilitated within the home through books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys (although outings<br />

are of minor importance). For behavioural <strong>development</strong> damp <strong>and</strong> overcrowding also play a<br />

role. These aspects of home environment are strongly influenced by income levels. Again there<br />

are two possible routes for influencing this. First, an indirect route is <strong>to</strong> give poorer <strong>to</strong> middle<br />

income families more financial resources <strong>and</strong> hope that they will increase spending on such<br />

items. Evidence from Gregg, Waldfogel <strong>and</strong> Washbrook (2004) suggests that the recent<br />

174


increases in tax credits etc. have resulted in increased spending on these items <strong>and</strong> a closing of<br />

the gap for poorer families with the more affluent. However, parents are balancing a whole set<br />

of needs for their <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> only a small share of the <strong>to</strong>tal increases in income goes on such<br />

items. Indeed, spending on cars <strong>and</strong> travel, clothing (including <strong>child</strong>ren’s clothing) <strong>and</strong> food are<br />

clearly high priorities for poor families, although spending on cigarettes <strong>and</strong> alcohol actually<br />

falls. A direct route could be <strong>to</strong> provide free books <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>y clubs, perhaps through Sure Start.<br />

These are targeted but fail <strong>to</strong> allow parents <strong>to</strong> prioritise spending. Such book schemes are being<br />

tried in the UK e.g., Bookstart operated by the Book Trust, but it is crucial that they are<br />

evaluated effectively <strong>to</strong> assess whether they can make a difference.<br />

Pre-school Education <strong>and</strong> Childcare<br />

The third major element, which the government is also investing heavily in, is pre-school<br />

education. Free half-day places for all <strong>and</strong> 3 <strong>and</strong> 4 year olds are now in place. The government<br />

also subsidises wrap around care heavily for low <strong>to</strong> middle income parents <strong>and</strong> is piloting<br />

extending care <strong>to</strong> younger <strong>child</strong>ren in poorer areas <strong>and</strong> also seeking <strong>to</strong> place an emphasis on<br />

high quality. The evidence presented here is that the majority of 4 year olds already receive<br />

some form of pre-school care, as under 40% of these <strong>child</strong>ren were not in a Nursery setting prior<br />

<strong>to</strong> school entry. The evidence presented in this report indicates that exposure <strong>to</strong> aver<strong>age</strong> quality<br />

nursery care (we cannot measure quality in each pre-school so we can only gro<strong>up</strong> all providers<br />

of a particular type, which will represent the aver<strong>age</strong> quality of that type of care) produces<br />

modest improvements in learning outcomes, most of which disappear after two years of<br />

schooling. The peer gro<strong>up</strong> mix at the pre-school provider matters a lot for early learning <strong>and</strong><br />

behaviour outcomes, suggesting that services provided exclusively for the poor or troubled<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren is not desirable. Again though the bulk of these benefits are lost after two years in<br />

school. This suggests that exposure <strong>to</strong> typical pre-school <strong>child</strong>care is not a major reason for<br />

variations in early learning attainment. However, the importance of high quality care <strong>and</strong> its<br />

potential benefits <strong>to</strong> deprived <strong>child</strong>ren cannot be studied here <strong>and</strong> better evidence should be used<br />

(such as the EPPE study).<br />

Mothers Mental Health<br />

For measured behaviour the key influence is the mothers mental <strong>and</strong> physical health, especially<br />

depression, stress, low self-esteem <strong>and</strong> an external locus of control. These measures also<br />

correlate strongly with discipline strategies employed by parents. As with other intervention<br />

there is potential for general <strong>and</strong> targeted action. Mental health treatment, especially in nonacute<br />

forms such as depression is often described as the Cinderella service. However, new<br />

175


treatments such as Cognitive Behaviour Therapy <strong>and</strong> new drugs mean that the potential <strong>to</strong><br />

address depression nationally appears <strong>to</strong> more possible than ever before. The benefits of<br />

treatment for both parents <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren, in terms of behavioural outcomes, could be substantial.<br />

Targeted initiatives for parents around disciplining responses may be deliverable alongside<br />

parenting s<strong>up</strong>port for teaching <strong>and</strong> reading. An external sense of locus of control naturally fits<br />

within this, <strong>to</strong> convince parents that their actions can make a difference.<br />

176


177


References<br />

Bates, E., Dale, P.S., <strong>and</strong> Thal, D. (1995) Individual differences <strong>and</strong> their implications for<br />

theories of langu<strong>age</strong> <strong>development</strong>. In P.Fletcher <strong>and</strong> B. MacWhinney (Eds.) The H<strong>and</strong>book of<br />

Child Langu<strong>age</strong>. Oxford: Blackwell<br />

Bayder, N <strong>and</strong> Brooks-Gunn, J (1991) ‘Effects of maternal employment <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>-care<br />

arrangements on preschoolers’ cognitive <strong>and</strong> behavioural outcomes: Evidence from the <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth’ Developmental Psychology, 27, pp. 932-945<br />

Belsky, J <strong>and</strong> Eggebeen, D (1991) ‘Early <strong>and</strong> extensive maternal employment <strong>and</strong> young<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren’s socioemotional <strong>development</strong>: Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of<br />

Youth’ Journal of Marri<strong>age</strong> <strong>and</strong> the <strong>Family</strong>, 53, pp. 1083-1110<br />

Blake, J. (1989) <strong>Family</strong> Size <strong>and</strong> Achievement, Berkeley, Calif.: University of California Press.<br />

Bl<strong>and</strong>en, . Gregg, P. <strong>and</strong> Machin, S. (2005) Intergenerational Mobility in Europe <strong>and</strong> North<br />

America, A Report S<strong>up</strong>ported by the Sut<strong>to</strong>n Trust, Centre for Economic Performance, LSE<br />

Bloom, L. (1998) Langu<strong>age</strong> acquisition in its <strong>development</strong>al context. Volume e 2, H<strong>and</strong>book<br />

of Child Psychology (Fifth edition) New York: Wiley<br />

Bornstein, M., <strong>and</strong> Bradley, R.H. (Eds.) (2003) Socioeconomic status, parenting <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

<strong>development</strong>. Mahwah, NJ.: Erlbaum<br />

Bradley, R.H., Caldwell, B.M., Rock, S.L. <strong>and</strong> Ramey, C.L. et al. (1989). Home environment<br />

<strong>and</strong> cognitive <strong>development</strong> in the first 3 years of life. A collaborative study involving six sites<br />

<strong>and</strong> three ethnic gro<strong>up</strong>s in North America. Developmental Psychology, 25, 217-235.<br />

Bradley, R.H., <strong>and</strong> Corwyn, R.F. (2002) Socioeconomic status <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> <strong>development</strong>. Annual<br />

Review of Psychology 53 371-399<br />

Broman, S., Nichols, P. L., Shaughnessy, P. <strong>and</strong> Kennedy, W. (1987) Retardation in Young<br />

Children: a Developmental Study of Cognitive Deficit, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979) The ecology of human <strong>development</strong>. Cambridge: Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986) ‘Ecology of the family as a context for human <strong>development</strong>’,<br />

Developmental Psychology 22: 723–42.<br />

Bronfenbrenner, U., McClell<strong>and</strong>, P., Wething<strong>to</strong>n, E., Moen, P., <strong>and</strong> Ceci, S. (1996) The state of<br />

Americans: this generation <strong>and</strong> the next. New York: Free Press<br />

Bronfenbrenner, U., <strong>and</strong> Morris, P. (1998) The ecology of <strong>development</strong>al processes. In<br />

W.Damon <strong>and</strong> R.M.Lerner (eds) The |H<strong>and</strong>book of Child Psychology Vol 1 Theoretical Models<br />

of Human Development. New York: Wiley<br />

Bronfenbrenner, U., <strong>and</strong> Evans, G.W. (2000) Developmental science in the 21 st century:<br />

emerging theoretical models, research designs, <strong>and</strong> empirical findings. Social Development 9<br />

115-125<br />

178


Brooks-Gunn, J, Han, W <strong>and</strong> Waldfogel, J (2002) ‘Maternal Employment <strong>and</strong> Child Outcomes<br />

in the First Three Years of Life: The NICHD Study of Early Child Care’ Child Development,<br />

73(4), pp. 1052-1072<br />

Burgess, S, Gregg P, Propper, C, Washbrook, E (2002) ‘Maternity rights <strong>and</strong> mothers’ return <strong>to</strong><br />

work’, CMPO Working Paper no. 02/055, University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l<br />

Campbell, S.B. (1995) Behaviour problems in preschool <strong>child</strong>ren: a review of recent research.<br />

JCPP 36 113-150<br />

Clark-Kauffman, E., G. Duncan <strong>and</strong> P. Morris (2003) ‘How Welfare Policies Affect Child <strong>and</strong><br />

Adolescent Achievement’, American Economic Review, 93, 299-303.<br />

Crain-Thoreson, C. <strong>and</strong> Dale, P. S. (1992) ‘Do early talkers become early readers? Linguistic<br />

precocity, preschool langu<strong>age</strong>, <strong>and</strong> emergent literacy’, Developmental Psychology 28: 421–9.<br />

David, T., Goouch, K., Powell,S., <strong>and</strong> Abbott, L. (2003) Birth <strong>to</strong> three matters: a review of the<br />

literature complied <strong>to</strong> inform The Framework <strong>to</strong> S<strong>up</strong>port Children in their Earliest Years.<br />

London: DfES Research report 444. HMSO<br />

Desai, S, Chase-Lansdale, PL <strong>and</strong> Michael, RT (1989) ‘Mother or market? Effects of maternal<br />

employment on the intellectual ability of 4-year-old <strong>child</strong>ren’ Demography, 26, pp. 545-561<br />

Duncan, G.J., Brooks-Gunn, J., <strong>and</strong> Klebanov, P. (1994) Economic deprivation <strong>and</strong> early<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood <strong>development</strong>. Child Development 65 296-318<br />

Dunn, J. (1993) Young <strong>child</strong>ren's close relationships. Newbury Park, CA.: S<strong>age</strong>. Gallaway, C.,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Richards, B. (Eds.) (1994) Input <strong>and</strong> interaction in langu<strong>age</strong> acquisition. Cambridge:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Duyme, M., Dumaret, A., <strong>and</strong> Tomkiewicz, S. (1999) How much can we boost IQs of dull<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren? : a late adoption study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 96 8790-<br />

8794<br />

Eisenberg, N. <strong>and</strong> Mussen, P. H. (1989) The Roots of Prosocial Behaviour in Children,<br />

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Ermisch, J <strong>and</strong> Francesconi, M (2000) ‘The effect of parents’ employment on <strong>child</strong>ren’s<br />

educational attainment’ Working Paper 2000-31, Institute for Social <strong>and</strong> Economic Research,<br />

University of Essex<br />

Evans, G.W., Maxwell, L.E., <strong>and</strong> Hart, B (1999) Parental langu<strong>age</strong> <strong>and</strong> verbal responsiveness <strong>to</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>ren in crowded homes. Developmental Psychology 35 (4): 1020-1023<br />

Evans, G.W. (2004) The environment of <strong>child</strong>hood poverty. American Psychologist 59 (20 77-<br />

92<br />

Eysenck, H. J. (1971) Race, Intelligence <strong>and</strong> Education, London: Temple Smith.<br />

Feinstein, L. (2003) ‘Inequality in the Cognitive Development of British Data in the 1970<br />

Cohort’, Economica, 70, 73-97<br />

179


Gibbons, S. <strong>and</strong> S. Machin 2003, ‘Valuing English Primary Schools’, Journal of Urban<br />

Economics, 53 (2), 197-219.<br />

Goering, J. <strong>and</strong> J. Feins Eds. (2003) Choosing a Better Life? Evaluating the Moving <strong>to</strong><br />

Opportunity Social Experiment, Urban Institute Press, Washing<strong>to</strong>n DC.<br />

Greenstein , TN (1995) ‘Are the “most advant<strong>age</strong>d” <strong>child</strong>ren truly disadvant<strong>age</strong>d by early<br />

maternal employment?’ Journal of <strong>Family</strong> Issues, 16, pp. 149-169<br />

Gregg, P. Gutierrez-Domenech, M. <strong>and</strong> Waldfogel, J . (2003) The Employment of Married<br />

Mothers in Great Britain: 1974-2000 CMPO Working Paper no. 03/078<br />

Gregg, P. Washbrook, E. Propper, C. <strong>and</strong> Burgess, S. (2005) The Effects of a Mothers Return <strong>to</strong><br />

Work Decision on Child Development in the UK, Economic Journal, vol. 115 February F48-80<br />

Gregg, P. Waldfogel, J <strong>and</strong> Washbrook, E. (2004) That’s The Way The Money Goes:<br />

Expenditure Patterns As Real Incomes Rise For The Poorest Families With Children in John<br />

Hills <strong>and</strong> Kitty Stewart (eds) An Equal Society? New Labour, Poverty, Inequality <strong>and</strong> Exclusion<br />

Policy Press<br />

Gregg, Washbrook <strong>and</strong> Meadows 2004 preschool effects.<br />

Haney, M., <strong>and</strong> Hill, J. (2004) Relationships between parent-teaching activities <strong>and</strong> emergent<br />

literacy in preschool <strong>child</strong>ren. Early Child Development <strong>and</strong> Care 174 (3) 215-228<br />

Hart, B., <strong>and</strong> Risley, T.R. (1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young<br />

American <strong>child</strong>ren. Baltimore: Brookes<br />

Harvey, E (1999) ‘Short-Term <strong>and</strong> Long-Term Effects of Early Parental Employment on<br />

Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth’ Developmental Psychology, 35(2), pp.<br />

445-459<br />

Heath, S. B. (1983) Ways with Words, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Hern<strong>and</strong>ez, D.J. (1997). Child <strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong> the social demography of <strong>child</strong>hood. Child<br />

Development, 68, 149-169. Hills, J., Le Gr<strong>and</strong>, J., <strong>and</strong> Piachaud, D. (2002) Underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

social exclusion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.<br />

Hill, J, Waldfogel, J, Brooks-Gunn, J <strong>and</strong> Han, W (2001) ‘Towards a better estimate of causal<br />

links in <strong>child</strong> policy: The case of maternal employment <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> outcomes’ mimeo, Columbia<br />

University School of Social Work<br />

Hills, J., Le Gr<strong>and</strong>, J., <strong>and</strong> Piachaud, D. (Eds.) (2002) Underst<strong>and</strong>ing social exclusion. Oxford:<br />

OUP<br />

Hoff, E., <strong>and</strong> Naigles, L. (2002) How <strong>child</strong>ren use input <strong>to</strong> acquire a lexicon. Child<br />

Development 73 (2) 418-433<br />

Hoff, E. (2003) The specificity of environmental influence: socio-economic status affects early<br />

vocabulary <strong>development</strong> via maternal speech. Child Development 74 1368-1378<br />

Howe, N., <strong>and</strong> Rinaldi, C.M. (2004) ‘You be the big sister’: Maternal-preschooler discourse,<br />

perspective-taking, <strong>and</strong> sibling caretaking. Infant <strong>and</strong> Child Development 13 217-234<br />

180


Hubbs-Tait, L., Culp, A.M., Culp, R.E., <strong>and</strong> Miller, C.E. (2002) Relation of maternal cognitive<br />

stimulation, emotional s<strong>up</strong>port, <strong>and</strong> intrusive behavior during Head Start <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren’s<br />

kindergarten cognitive abilities. Child Development 73 (2) 110-131<br />

Huttenlocher, J. (1998). Langu<strong>age</strong> input <strong>and</strong> langu<strong>age</strong> growth. Preventive Medicine, 27, 195-<br />

199.<br />

Jencks, C. et al (1972) Inequality: a reassessment of the effect of family <strong>and</strong> schooling in<br />

America. Basic Books<br />

Johnson-Glenberg, M., <strong>and</strong> Chapman, R. (2004) Predic<strong>to</strong>rs of parent-<strong>child</strong> langu<strong>age</strong> during<br />

novel task play: a comparison between typically developing <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> individuals with Down<br />

syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 48 (3) 225-238<br />

Joshi, H <strong>and</strong> Verropoulu, G (2000) Maternal employment <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong> outcomes, Smith Institute<br />

Report<br />

Katz L.F., J.R. Kling <strong>and</strong> J. B. Liebman (2001) ‘Moving <strong>to</strong> Opportunity in Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Early<br />

Results of a R<strong>and</strong>omised Experiment’ Quarterly Journal of Economics 116, 607-654.<br />

Kaye, K. (1984) The Mental <strong>and</strong> Social Life of Babies, London: Methuen.<br />

Kim-Cohen, J., Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., <strong>and</strong> Taylor, A. (2004) Genetic <strong>and</strong> environmental<br />

processes in young <strong>child</strong>ren’s resilience <strong>and</strong> vulnerability <strong>to</strong> socio-economic deprivation. Child<br />

Development 75 (3) 651-668<br />

Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. <strong>and</strong> Dresner Barnes, H. (1992) ‘Individual differences in early<br />

vocabulary <strong>development</strong>: redefining the referential–expressive distinction’, Journal of Child<br />

Langu<strong>age</strong> 19: 287–301.<br />

Ludwig, J., G. Duncan <strong>and</strong> P. Hirschfield (2001) ‘Urban Poverty <strong>and</strong> Juvenile Crime: Evidence<br />

from a R<strong>and</strong>omised Housing Mobility Experiment’ Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116, 655-<br />

680.<br />

Ludwig, J., H. Lad <strong>and</strong> G. Duncan (2001) ‘Urban Poverty <strong>and</strong> Educational Outcomes’<br />

Brookings-Whar<strong>to</strong>n Papers on Urban Affairs, 147-2001.<br />

Luthar, S.S. (1999) Poverty <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren's adjustment. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks: S<strong>age</strong> Meadows, S.<br />

(1993) The <strong>child</strong> as thinker. London: Routledge.<br />

Meadows, S. (1996) Parent-<strong>child</strong> interaction <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren's cognitive <strong>development</strong>.<br />

Psychology Press.<br />

Hove:<br />

Meadows, S. (2005) The Child as Thinker. Second edition. London: Routledge<br />

Meins, E. (1997) Security of attachment <strong>and</strong> maternal tu<strong>to</strong>ring strategies: Interaction within the<br />

zone of proximal <strong>development</strong> British Journal of Developmental Psychology 15: 129-144<br />

Meins, E. (1998) The effects of security of attachment <strong>and</strong> maternal attribution of meaning on<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren's linguistic acquisitional style. Infant Behaviour <strong>and</strong> Development 21 (2): 237-252<br />

Meins, E., Fernyhough, C, Fradley, E, et al. (2001) Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers'<br />

comments on infants' mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. Journal of<br />

181


Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry 42 (5): 637-648<br />

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2002) Structure process outcome: direct <strong>and</strong><br />

indirect effects of caregiving quality on young <strong>child</strong>ren's <strong>development</strong>. Psychological Science<br />

13 (3) 199-206.<br />

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2000). The relation of <strong>child</strong> care <strong>to</strong> cognitive <strong>and</strong><br />

langu<strong>age</strong> <strong>development</strong>: results from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Development,<br />

71, 958-978.<br />

Ochs, E., <strong>and</strong> Schieffelin, B. (1995) The impact of langu<strong>age</strong> socialisation on grammatical<br />

<strong>development</strong>. In P.Fletcher <strong>and</strong> B. MacWhinney (Eds.) The H<strong>and</strong>book of Child Langu<strong>age</strong>.<br />

Oxford: Blackwell<br />

Parcel, TL <strong>and</strong> Menaghan, EG (1994) ‘Early parental work, family social capital, <strong>and</strong> early<br />

<strong>child</strong>hood outcomes’ American Journal of Sociology, 99, pp. 972-1009<br />

Petrill, S.A., <strong>and</strong> Deater-Deckard, K. (2004) Task orientation, parental warmth <strong>and</strong> SES account<br />

for a significant proportion of the shared environmental variance in general cognitive ability in<br />

early <strong>child</strong>hood: evidence from a twin study. Developmental Science 7 (1) 25-32<br />

Ramey, C.T., <strong>and</strong> Ramey, S.L. (1998) Early intervention <strong>and</strong> early experience. American<br />

Psychologist 53 109-120<br />

Rice, M. L. (1989) ‘Children’s langu<strong>age</strong> acquisition’, American Psychologist 44: 149– 56.<br />

Roberts, E, Bornstein, M. H., Slater, A. M.,<strong>and</strong> Barrett, J. (1999) Early Cognitive Development<br />

<strong>and</strong> Parental Education Infant <strong>and</strong> Child Development 8: 49-62<br />

Rogoff, B. (1998) Cognition as a collaborative process. Volume 2, H<strong>and</strong>book of Child<br />

Psychology (Fifth edition) New York: Wiley<br />

Rogoff, B., Paradise, R., Mej, F, Da Arauz, R., Correa-Chavez, M. <strong>and</strong> Angelillo, C. (2003)<br />

Firsth<strong>and</strong> learning through intent participation. Annual Review of Psychology 54:175-203<br />

Ruhm, C (2004) ‘Parental Employment <strong>and</strong> Child Cognitive Development’ Journal of Human<br />

Resources, 39(1), pp. 155-192<br />

Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: <strong>development</strong> <strong>and</strong> individual differences.<br />

Child Development, 63, 1-19.<br />

Senechal, M., <strong>and</strong> LeFevre, J. (2002) Parental involvement in the <strong>development</strong> of <strong>child</strong>ren’s<br />

reading skills: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development 73 (2) 445-460<br />

Slater, A. (1995) Individual differences in infancy <strong>and</strong> later IQ. Journal of Child Psychology<br />

<strong>and</strong> Psychiatry 36 69-112<br />

Snow, C.E. (1995) Issues in the study of input: finetuning,universality, individual <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>development</strong>al differences, <strong>and</strong> necessary causes. In P.Fletcher <strong>and</strong> B. MacWhinney (Eds.) The<br />

H<strong>and</strong>book of Child Langu<strong>age</strong>. Oxford: Blackwell<br />

Sylva, K., Melhuish, E. C., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I. <strong>and</strong> Taggart, B.(2004),The<br />

182


Effective Provision of Pre-School Education (EPPE) Project: Technical Paper 12 - The Final<br />

Report: Effective Pre-School Education. London: DfES / Institute of Education, University of<br />

London.<br />

Thompson, T. (2004) Failure-avoidance: parenting, the achievement environment of the home<br />

<strong>and</strong> strategies for reduction. Learning <strong>and</strong> Instruction 14 3-26<br />

Tizard, B. <strong>and</strong> Hughes, M. (1984) Young Children Learning, London: Fontana.<br />

V<strong>and</strong>ell, D <strong>and</strong> Ramaman, J (1992) ‘Effects of early <strong>and</strong> recent maternal employment on<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren from low-income families’ Child Development, 63, pp. 938-935<br />

Waldfogel, J, Han, W <strong>and</strong> Brooks-Gunn, J (2002) ‘The Effects of Early Maternal Employment<br />

on Child Cognitive Development’ Demography, 39(2), pp. 369-392<br />

Wells, C.G. (1985) Langu<strong>age</strong> <strong>development</strong> in the preschool years. Cambridge: Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Williams, M., <strong>and</strong> Rask, H. (2003) Literacy through play: how families with able <strong>child</strong>ren<br />

s<strong>up</strong>port their literacy <strong>development</strong>. Early Child Development <strong>and</strong> Care 173 (5) 527-533<br />

183


Appendix 1<br />

BASIC DEMOGRAPHICS<br />

A1 1.1 Mother’s Highest Qualification<br />

Mother’s were asked during the questionnaire at 32 weeks gestation about their highest<br />

educational qualification. There were 940 missing observation, but as it was felt that many<br />

mother’s with no educational qualification merely left the whole question blank, these mother’s<br />

were gro<strong>up</strong>ed in<strong>to</strong> the ‘CSE/none’ category <strong>and</strong> only those mother’s who ticked ‘not known’<br />

remained coded as ‘missing’.<br />

Mother’s highest qualification Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

CSE/none 2133 17.60<br />

Vocational/O-level 5174 42.70<br />

A-level 2670 22.03<br />

Degree 1553 12.82<br />

Missing 588 4.85<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 1.2 Partner’s Highest Qualification<br />

The mother also answered this question at 32 weeks gestation. When the question was left<br />

blank, it was assumed the partner had no educational qualification. Only when the mother ticked<br />

‘not known’ or ‘no partner’ was the qualification coded as ‘missing’ or ‘no partner’<br />

respectively.<br />

Partner’s highest qualification Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

CSE/none 2678 22.10<br />

Vocational/O-level 3309 27.31<br />

A-level 2940 24.26<br />

Degree 2094 17.28<br />

No partner 125 1.03<br />

Missing 972 8.02<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

184


A1 1.3 Lone Parent Status<br />

Mother’s were asked about single parent status at 8, 21, 33 <strong>and</strong> 47 months. The lone parent<br />

status variable was derived from these responses.<br />

Lone parent status Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

Always had a partner 7230 59.66<br />

No partner at some point but had by 47 months 322 2.66<br />

No partner at some point <strong>and</strong> none by 47 months 546 4.51<br />

No partner at some point, don’t know at 47<br />

months<br />

269 2.22<br />

Never had a partner 125 1.03<br />

Info missing but had partner at 47 months 1301 10.74<br />

Info missing <strong>and</strong> don’t know if had partner at 47<br />

months<br />

2325 19.19<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 1.4 Mother’s Age At Birth<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

< 20 434 3.58<br />

20 – 24 2112 17.43<br />

25 – 29 4771 39.37<br />

30 – 34 3548 29.28<br />

35 + 1253 10.34<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 1.5 Ethnicity<br />

The mother was asked at 32 weeks gestation in which ethnic gro<strong>up</strong> she would place herself <strong>and</strong><br />

her partner. The options were white, black/Caribbean, black/African, black/other, Indian,<br />

Pakistani, Bangladeshi, Chinese or other. If any answer other than white was recorded for either<br />

the mother or the partner, the <strong>child</strong> was classified as non-white.<br />

Child’s ethnicity Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

White 10747 88.69<br />

Non-white 512 4.23<br />

Missing 859 7.09<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 1.6 Gender<br />

Child’s sex Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

Male 6258 51.64<br />

Female 5860 48.36<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 1.7 Birth weight<br />

This data came from a mixture of sources: obstetric data, ALSPAC measurers <strong>and</strong> birth<br />

185


notification. If there was more than one weight recorded for a particular <strong>child</strong>, the lower figure<br />

was taken. If the figures disagreed by more that 100g the data was coded as being missing. The<br />

lowest recorded birth weight was 200g <strong>and</strong> the highest was 5640g. The median birth weight was<br />

3410g. Here, low is taken as being less than 2500g.<br />

Child’s birth weight Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

Low 608 5.02<br />

Below Median 4949 40.84<br />

Above Median 6412 52.91<br />

Missing 149 1.23<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 1.8 Special Care Unit At Birth<br />

When the <strong>child</strong> was 4 weeks old the mother was asked if the baby had been admitted <strong>to</strong> a special<br />

care unit or a neonatal intensive care unit or other hospital ward. The mother could either<br />

respond with ‘yes’, ‘no’ or ‘unsure’. Unsure was coded as missing.<br />

SCBU Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

No 10679 88.13<br />

Yes 791 6.53<br />

Missing 648 5.35<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 1.9 Younger Siblings By 47 Months<br />

At 47 months the mother was asked how many younger siblings the study <strong>child</strong> had living with<br />

them or visiting at least once a week. They were <strong>to</strong>ld <strong>to</strong> include any half-brothers or sisters,<br />

step-brothers or sisters <strong>and</strong> fostered or adopted <strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

Younger Siblings Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

0 5693 46.98<br />

1 3593 29.65<br />

More than 1 335 2.76<br />

Missing 2497 20.61<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

186


A1 1.10 Older Siblings<br />

At 47 months the mother was asked how many older siblings the study <strong>child</strong> had living with<br />

them or visiting at least once a week. They were <strong>to</strong>ld <strong>to</strong> include any half-brothers or sisters,<br />

step-brothers or sisters <strong>and</strong> fostered or adopted <strong>child</strong>ren.<br />

Older Siblings Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

0 4281 35.33<br />

1 3408 28.12<br />

2 1389 11.46<br />

3 or more 543 4.48<br />

Missing 2497 20.61<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 1.11 Cohort Year<br />

This is simply a variable recording in which year the Entry Assessment examination <strong>to</strong>ok place.<br />

Cohort Year Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

1995 1088 8.98<br />

1996 2970 24.51<br />

1997 955 7.88<br />

Missing 7105 58.63<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 1.12 Partner’s Employment Status At 47 Months<br />

At 47 months the mother was asked if she had a partner <strong>and</strong> also if this partner was currently<br />

employed.<br />

Partner in Employment Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

Yes 7839 64.69<br />

No 800 6.60<br />

No Partner 671 5.54<br />

Missing 2808 23.17<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

CHILDCARE<br />

When the <strong>child</strong> was 8 weeks old the mother was asked<br />

“Who regularly looks after your baby when you’re not there?”<br />

partner<br />

baby’s gr<strong>and</strong>parent<br />

other relative<br />

friend/neighbour<br />

187


paid person outside your home (e.g. <strong>child</strong>minder)<br />

paid person in your home (e.g. nanny, babysitter)<br />

day nursery (crèche)<br />

other (please describe)<br />

If the mother’s answered yes <strong>to</strong> any of these options they were then asked <strong>to</strong> give the number of<br />

hours per week the baby was cared for in this way <strong>and</strong> the <strong>age</strong> of the baby when this began. A<br />

similar question was again asked at 8 months, 15 months <strong>and</strong> 24 months, with the same options<br />

for answers <strong>and</strong> the same request for information on the number of hours each method of care<br />

was used for per week.<br />

If a mother used any type of <strong>child</strong>care for over 5 hours when the <strong>child</strong> was 8 weeks, 8 months,<br />

15 months OR 24 months, they were added <strong>to</strong> that category. The same applies if the type of<br />

<strong>child</strong>care was used for over 20 hours.<br />

A1 2.1 Care by partner pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Overall use of partner by 24 Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

months<br />

< 5 hrs pwk before 24 months 2545 21.00<br />

5-19 hrs pwk before 24<br />

months<br />

3947 32.57<br />

20+ hrs pwk before 24<br />

months<br />

4588 37.86<br />

Missing 1038 8.57<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 2.2 Care by friend/relative pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Overall use of friend/relative Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

by 24 months<br />

< 5 hrs pwk before 24 months 5312 43.84<br />

5-19 hrs pwk before 24<br />

months<br />

2892 23.87<br />

20+ hrs pwk before 24 months 2506 20.68<br />

Missing 1408 11.62<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

188


A1 2.3 Care by paid person pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Overall use of paid person by Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

24 months<br />

< 5 hrs pwk before 24 months 7871 64.95<br />

5-19 hrs pwk before 24<br />

months<br />

939 7.75<br />

20+ hrs pwk before 24<br />

months<br />

1511 12.47<br />

Missing 1797 14.83<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 2.4 Centre-based care pre-<strong>age</strong> 2<br />

Overall use of centre-based Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

care by 24 months<br />

< 5 hrs pwk before 24 months 9191 75.85<br />

5-19 hrs pwk before 24<br />

months<br />

560 4.62<br />

20+ hrs pwk before 24<br />

months<br />

334 2.76<br />

Missing 2033 16.78<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

During the Focus at 7 clinics, mothers <strong>to</strong>ok part in an interview about day-care. Therefore,<br />

information used <strong>to</strong> produce the variables for types of <strong>child</strong>care used post <strong>age</strong> 3 is motherreported<br />

retrospective data.<br />

A1 2.5 Nursery Class <strong>age</strong>3/4<br />

Used nursery class 36 months Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

+<br />

No 10981 90.62<br />

Yes 1137 9.38<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 2.6 LEA Nursery Class <strong>age</strong> 3/4<br />

Used LEA nursery school 36 Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

months +<br />

No 11436 94.37<br />

Yes 682 5.63<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

189


A1 2.7 Private Day Nursery <strong>age</strong> 3/4<br />

Used private day nursery 36 Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

months +<br />

No 11124 91.80<br />

Yes 994 8.20<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 2.8 Nursery (type undefined) <strong>age</strong> 3/4<br />

Used nursery (type undefined) Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

36 months +<br />

No 10279 84.82<br />

Yes 1839 15.18<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 2.9 Playgro<strong>up</strong> <strong>age</strong> 3/4<br />

Used Playgro<strong>up</strong> 36 months + Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

No 7462 61.58<br />

Yes 4656 38.42<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 2.10 Other centre-based care <strong>age</strong> 3/4<br />

Used other centre-based care Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

36 months +<br />

No 11995 98.98<br />

Yes 123 1.02<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

PARENTING<br />

A1 3.1a Maternal Reading Score<br />

Question: How often do you (the mother) read your <strong>child</strong> s<strong>to</strong>ries or show him/her pictures in books? (N=8177)<br />

Questionnaire Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points response points response points<br />

6 months often 3.5 - - occasionally 1.5 - - hardly ever 0.5<br />

18 months nearly every 3-5 times 1-2 times a less than<br />

never<br />

day 4 a week 3 week 2 once a week 1<br />

0<br />

38 months often 3.5 - - sometimes 2 rarely 1 never 0<br />

42 months<br />

nearly every 3-5 times 1-2 times a less than<br />

never<br />

day 4 a week 3 week 2 once a week 1<br />

0<br />

Max<br />

possible 15 Min possible 0.5<br />

190


A1 3.1b Categories of maternal reading score<br />

Disaggregated Final<br />

categories categories<br />

Points Freq % % %<br />

2.5 1 0.0<br />

3.5 13 0.2<br />

4.5 23 0.3<br />

5.5 48 0.6<br />

6 3 0.0<br />

6.5 92 1.1 8.7 - 8.7<br />

7 21 0.3 (very<br />

7.5 135 1.7 low)<br />

8 41 0.5<br />

8.5 199 2.4<br />

9 133 1.6<br />

9.5 222 2.7<br />

10 335 4.1 9.5 - 9.5<br />

10.5 220 2.7 (low)<br />

11 687 8.4<br />

10.2<br />

11.5 148 1.8<br />

25.8<br />

12 1202 14.<br />

7 15.6<br />

(medium)<br />

12.5 75 0.9<br />

13 1772 21.<br />

7<br />

13.5 60 0.7 29.1<br />

14 550 6.7 56.0<br />

15 2197 26. -<br />

9<br />

26.9 (high)<br />

Total 8177<br />

A1 3.2a Paternal Reading Score<br />

Question: How often does your partner read your <strong>child</strong> s<strong>to</strong>ries or show him/her pictures in books? (N=7443)<br />

Questionnaire Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points response points response points<br />

6 months often 1 - - occasionally 0 - - hardly ever 0<br />

18 months<br />

nearly<br />

3-5 times a 1-2 times a less than<br />

never<br />

every day 1 week 1 week 0 once a week 0<br />

0<br />

38 months often 1 - - sometimes 0 rarely 0 never 0<br />

42 months<br />

nearly<br />

3-5 times a 1-2 times a less than<br />

never<br />

every day 1 week 1 week 0 once a week 0<br />

0<br />

Max<br />

possible 4<br />

Min<br />

possible 0<br />

191


A1 3.2b Categories of paternal reading score (intact families only)<br />

Points Freq %<br />

0 1402 18.8<br />

1 1403 18.8<br />

2 1673 22.5<br />

3 2143 28.8<br />

4 822 11.0<br />

Total 7443<br />

A1 3.3a Maternal Teaching Score<br />

Question: Earliest <strong>age</strong> at which mother tries <strong>to</strong> teach <strong>child</strong>… (N=8775)<br />

Item Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

Age of <strong>child</strong> points Age of <strong>child</strong> points Age of <strong>child</strong> points Age of <strong>child</strong> points<br />

clapping games<br />

(ie pat-a-cake)<br />

18 months 3 - - - - not at all 0<br />

parts of the body 18 months 3 - - - - not at all 0<br />

<strong>to</strong> wave bye-bye 18 months 3 - - - - not at all 0<br />

Colours 18 months 3 30 months 2 42 months 1 not at all 0<br />

Alphabet 18 months 3 30 months 2 42 months 1 not at all 0<br />

Numbers 18 months 3 30 months 2 42 months 1 not at all 0<br />

nursery rhymes 18 months 3 30 months 2 42 months 1 not at all 0<br />

Songs 18 months 3 30 months 2 42 months 1 not at all 0<br />

shapes <strong>and</strong> sizes 18 months 3 30 months 2 42 months 1 not at all 0<br />

politeness (ie<br />

please <strong>and</strong> thank 18 months 3 30 months 2 42 months 1 not at all 0<br />

you)<br />

Max possible 30 Min possible 0<br />

192


A1 3.3b Categories of maternal teaching score<br />

Disaggregated<br />

categories Final categories<br />

Points Freq % % %<br />

10 1 0.0<br />

11 2 0.0<br />

12 2 0.0<br />

13 5 0.1<br />

14 15 0.2<br />

15 5 0.1<br />

16 6 0.1 7.5 - 7.5<br />

17 10 0.1 (very<br />

18 24 0.3 low)<br />

19 35 0.4<br />

20 74 0.8<br />

21 105 1.2<br />

22 142 1.6<br />

23 234 2.7<br />

24 387 4.4 - 4.4 11.0<br />

25 579 6.6 - 6.6 (low)<br />

26 925 10.5 - 10.5<br />

27 1212 13.8 - 13.8 42.1<br />

28 1561 17.8 - 17.8 (medium)<br />

29 1498 17.1 - 17.1 39.3<br />

30 1953 22.3 - 22.3 (high)<br />

Total 8775<br />

A1 3.4a Talking Whilst Occ<strong>up</strong>ied Score<br />

Question: How often do you talk <strong>to</strong> your <strong>child</strong> while you do housework or are occ<strong>up</strong>ied in some other way ?<br />

(N=8088)<br />

Questionnaire Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points response points response points<br />

6 months always 4 Often 3 sometimes 2 rarely 1 never 0<br />

18 months always 4 Often 3 sometimes 2 rarely 1 never 0<br />

30 months always 4 Often 3 sometimes 2 rarely 1 never 0<br />

42 months always 4 Often 3 sometimes 2 rarely 1 never 0<br />

Max<br />

possible 16<br />

Min<br />

possible 0<br />

193


A1 3.4b Categories of talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied score<br />

Disaggregated Final<br />

categories categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

1 1 0.0 %<br />

4 3 0.0<br />

5 4 0.0<br />

6 10 0.1<br />

7 17 0.2 10.8<br />

8 65 0.8 - 10.8<br />

9 135 1.7 (low)<br />

10 207 2.6<br />

11 430 5.3<br />

12 827 10.2 - 10.2<br />

13 1088 13.5 - 13.5<br />

14 1401 17.3 - 17.3 41.0<br />

15 1708 21.1 - 21.1 (medium)<br />

16 2192 27.1 - 27.1 48.2<br />

Total 8088 100.0<br />

(high)<br />

A1 3.5a 18 Month TV Score<br />

Question: (At 18 months) Do you usually have the television on in the …? (N = 9026)<br />

Item Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points<br />

Mornings Yes, every day 2 Yes, some days 1 No, hardly ever 0<br />

Afternoons Yes, every day 2 Yes, some days 1 No, hardly ever 0<br />

Evenings Yes, every day 2 Yes, some days 1 No, hardly ever 0<br />

Max possible 6 Min possible 0<br />

A1 3.5b Categories of 18 month TV score<br />

Disaggregated<br />

categories<br />

Final<br />

categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

0 185 2.0<br />

1 636 7.0 25.4<br />

2 1469 16.3 (low)<br />

3 2288 25.3<br />

4 2183 24.2 60.8<br />

5 1018 11.3 (medium)<br />

6 1247 13.8 -<br />

13.8<br />

(high)<br />

Total 9026 100.0<br />

194


A1 3.6a 38 Month TV Score<br />

Question: (At 38 months) How much time does your <strong>child</strong> spend watching television…?<br />

(N = 9014)<br />

Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response pointsresponse points response points<br />

Most weekday more than 2 1 <strong>to</strong> 2 hours less than<br />

days<br />

hours a day 10 a day 5 1 hour 2.5 not at all 0<br />

Most weekend<br />

days<br />

more than 2<br />

hours a day 4<br />

1 <strong>to</strong> 2 hours<br />

a day 2<br />

less than<br />

1 hour 1 not at all 0<br />

Max possible 14 Min possible 0<br />

A1 3.6b Categories of 38 month TV score<br />

Disaggregated<br />

categories<br />

Final<br />

categories<br />

Points Freq % % %<br />

0 151 1.7<br />

1 33 0.4 2.1<br />

2 4 0.0<br />

2.5 126 1.4<br />

3.5 1631 18.1 19.5<br />

4 2 0.0<br />

4.5 640 7.1 75.4<br />

5 17 0.2 (low)<br />

5.5 608 6.7 14.3<br />

6 25 0.3<br />

6.5 2867 31.8 - 31.8<br />

9 686 7.6<br />

10 5 0.1 7.7<br />

11 44 0.5<br />

12 437 4.8 5.3 24.6<br />

14 1738 19.3 - 19.3 (high)<br />

Total 9014 100.0<br />

A1 3.7 Breast-feeding<br />

Duration of breastfeeding Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

Never 3142 25.93<br />

0 – 6 months 4710 38.87<br />

6 + months 3847 31.75<br />

Missing 419 3.46<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

195


A1 3.8 Smoking during pregnancy<br />

Smoked in pregnancy Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

No 8977 74.08<br />

Yes 3066 25.30<br />

Missing 75 0.62<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 3.9a Maternal Bonding Score<br />

Question: (At 8months) Please indicate what your feelings are…?<br />

(N = 9014)<br />

Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points response points<br />

I really enjoy my baby Feel exactly 3 Feel often 2<br />

Feel<br />

sometimes 1 Feel never 0<br />

I would have preferred that we had not<br />

had this baby when we did Feel never 3<br />

Feel<br />

sometimes 2 Feel often 1 Feel exactly 0<br />

I feel confident with my baby Feel exactly 3 Feel often 2<br />

Feel<br />

sometimes 1 Feel never 0<br />

I dislike the mess that surrounds a baby Feel never 3<br />

Feel<br />

sometimes 2 Feel often 1 Feel exactly 0<br />

It is a great pleasure <strong>to</strong> watch my baby<br />

develop Feel exactly 3 Feel often 2<br />

Feel<br />

sometimes 1 Feel never 0<br />

I really can not bear it when the baby<br />

cries Feel never 3<br />

Feel<br />

sometimes 2 Feel often 1 Feel exactly 0<br />

I feel constantly unsure if I’m doing the<br />

right thing for my baby Feel never 3<br />

Feel<br />

sometimes 2 Feel often 1 Feel exactly 0<br />

I feel I should be enjoying my baby but<br />

I’m not Feel never 3<br />

Feel<br />

sometimes 2 Feel often 1 Feel exactly 0<br />

Babies are fun Feel exactly 3 Feel often 2<br />

Feel<br />

sometimes 1 Feel never 0<br />

Max possible 27<br />

Min<br />

possible 0<br />

196


A1 3.9b Categories of mother’s maternal bonding score<br />

Categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

4 1 0.01<br />

5 1 0.01<br />

6 1 0.01<br />

7 1 0.01<br />

8 4 0.04<br />

9 4 0.04<br />

10 7 0.06<br />

11 5 0.05<br />

12 24 0.22<br />

13 21 0.19 21.82<br />

14 31 0.28<br />

15 48 0.44<br />

16 54 0.49<br />

17 108 0.98<br />

18 167 1.52<br />

19 202 1.84<br />

20 379 3.44<br />

21 548 4.98<br />

22 795 7.22<br />

23 1100 10.00 23.83<br />

24 1523 13.84<br />

25 1850 16.81 - 16.81<br />

26 2171 19.73 - 19.73<br />

27 1960 17.81 - 17.81<br />

Total 11005 100.0 100.0<br />

A1 3.10a Types of discipline<br />

Question: About how often do you do the following when your study <strong>child</strong> is naughty …?<br />

Never Rarely Once a month Once a week Daily Total<br />

Freq % Freq % Freq % Freq % Freq % Freq %<br />

Ignore him 2082 22.16 3573 38.03 726 7.73 2201 23.43 813 8.65 9395 100.00<br />

Smack him 1037 11.04 3789 40.33 1289 13.72 2815 29.96 465 4.95 9395 100.00<br />

Shout at him 68 0.72 980 10.43 647 6.89 3902 41.53 3798 40.43 9395 100.00<br />

Send him <strong>to</strong> is<br />

bedroom/ naughty 1744 18.56 2085 22.19 1613 17.17 3408 36.27 545 5.80 9395 100.00<br />

chair/ step etc<br />

Take away treats 1272 13.54 2940 31.29 1487 15.83 3229 34.37 467 4.97 9395 100.00<br />

Tell him off 12 0.13 276 2.94 272 2.90 2927 31.15 5908 62.88 9395 100.00<br />

Bribe him 2850 30.34 3191 33.96 845 8.99 2130 22.67 379 4.03 9395 100.00<br />

197


These results were condensed <strong>to</strong> create variables for the type of discipline being administered<br />

either more or less than once a week.<br />

Rarely Frequently Missing Total<br />

Freq % Freq % Freq % Freq %<br />

Ignore him 6381 52.66 3014 24.87 2723 22.47 12118 100.00<br />

Smack him 6115 50.46 3280 27.07 2723 22.47 12118 100.00<br />

Shout at him 1695 13.99 7700 63.54 2723 22.47 12118 100.00<br />

Send him <strong>to</strong> is<br />

bedroom/ naughty 5442 44.91 3953 32.62 2723 22.47 12118 100.00<br />

chair/ step etc<br />

Take away treats 5699 47.03 3696 30.50 2723 22.47 12118 100.00<br />

Tell him off 560 4.62 8835 72.91 2723 22.47 12118 100.00<br />

Bribe him 6886 56.82 2509 20.70 2723 22.47 12118 100.00<br />

OUTINGS SCORES AND HOME ENVIRONMENT<br />

A1 4.1a Outings scores<br />

Question: About how often do you take your <strong>child</strong> <strong>to</strong>…?<br />

a) local shops e) park or playground<br />

b) department s<strong>to</strong>re f) library<br />

c) s<strong>up</strong>ermarket g) places on interest (eg zoo)<br />

d) visits <strong>to</strong> friends or family h) places of entertainment (eg funfair)<br />

Questionnaire Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

18 months nearly every day 4<br />

once a<br />

week 3<br />

once a<br />

month 2<br />

a few times<br />

per year 1 never 0<br />

30 months nearly every day 4<br />

once a<br />

week 3<br />

once a<br />

month 2<br />

a few times<br />

per year 1 never 0<br />

once a once a a few times<br />

week 3 month 2 per year 1 never 0<br />

42 months nearly every day 4<br />

Max possible<br />

Each of 8<br />

individual scores 12<br />

Min possible<br />

Each of 8<br />

individual scores 0<br />

198


A1 4.1b Categories of place-specific outings scores<br />

Local shops Department s<strong>to</strong>res S<strong>up</strong>ermarket Park or playground Visits <strong>to</strong> friends or family Library Places of interest Places of entertainment<br />

categories categories categories categories categories categories categories categories<br />

Score Freq % % Freq % % Freq % % Freq % % Freq % % Freq % % Freq % % Freq % %<br />

0 1 0.0 77 0.9 0 0.0 7 0.1 1 0.0 2090 25.3 38 0.5 344 4.2<br />

1 1 0.0 134 1.6 4 0.0 16 0.2 3 0.0 736 8.9 56.6 78 0.9 600 7.3 60.1<br />

2 1 0.0 269 3.3 47.5 8 0.1 33 0.4 5 0.1 973 11.8 (low) 338 4.1 47.8 1197 14.5 (low)<br />

3 3 0.0 1105 13.4 (low) 20 0.2 131 1.6 12 0.1 880 10.6 2143 25.8 (low) 2816 34.2<br />

4 8 0.1 1098 13.4 40 0.5 23.0 225 2.7 42.3 26 0.3 863 10.4 1381 16.6 1351 16.4<br />

5 13 0.2 35.3 1218 14.8 102 1.2 (low) 385 4.7 (low) 40 0.5 50.4 740 9.0 1219 14.7 906 11.0<br />

6 44 0.5 (low) 1745 21.2 270 3.2 651 7.9 117 1.4 (low) 1058 12.8 1240 14.9 566 6.9<br />

7 84 1.0 1001 12.2 446 5.3 877 10.6 155 1.9 476 5.8 798 9.6 275 3.3<br />

8 240 2.9 710 8.6 1037 12.4 1170 14.2 384 4.6 257 3.1 43.4 552 6.6 52.2 120 1.5 39.9<br />

9 1272 15.2 759 9.2 52.5 4832 57.7 2540 30.8 1883 22.6 186 2.3 (high) 472 5.7 (high) 53 0.6 (high)<br />

10 1284 15.4 77 0.9 (high) 889 10.6 77.0 1114 13.5 57.7 1578 18.9 4 0.0 47 0.6 11 0.1<br />

11 1611 19.3 64.7 14 0.2 433 5.2 (high) 648 7.8 (high) 1741 20.9 49.6 2 0.0 9 0.1 3 0.0<br />

12 3788 45.4 (high) 13 0.2 295 3.5 462 5.6 2393 28.7 (high) 0 0.0 2 0.0 0 0.0<br />

Total 8350 100 8220 100 8376 8259 100 8338 100 8265 100 8317 100 8242 100<br />

199


A1 4.1c Categories of <strong>to</strong>tal outings score<br />

Disaggregated<br />

categories<br />

Final<br />

categories<br />

Disaggregated<br />

categories<br />

Final<br />

categories<br />

Points Freq % % % Points Freq % % %<br />

18 1 0.0 56 396 5.1<br />

24 1 0.0 57 365 4.7<br />

9.8<br />

25 1 0.0 58 377 4.8<br />

9.3<br />

26 2 0.0 59 349 4.5<br />

37.4<br />

28 1 0.0 60 361 4.6 (high)<br />

8.3<br />

29 4 0.1 61 287 3.7<br />

30 1 0.0 62 321 4.1<br />

31 4 0.1 63 243 3.1 10.0<br />

32 6 0.1 64 214 2.7<br />

33 10 0.1 65 210 2.7<br />

34 8 0.1 9.7 66 165 2.1<br />

9.7 -<br />

35 14 0.2<br />

(very 67 136 1.7<br />

36 20 0.3 low) 68 99 1.3<br />

37 21 0.3 69 83 1.1<br />

38 26 0.3 70 64 0.8<br />

39 36 0.5 71 52 0.7<br />

40 51 0.7 72 43 0.6<br />

41 73 0.9 73 28 0.4<br />

42 87 1.1 74 22 0.3 12.1 - 12.1<br />

43 100 1.3 75 11 0.1 (very<br />

44 124 1.6 76 7 0.1 high)<br />

45 162 2.1 77 7 0.1<br />

46 174 2.2 78 6 0.1<br />

47 205 2.6 79 2 0.0<br />

11.4<br />

48 228 2.9<br />

80 3 0.0<br />

49 283 3.6 19.9 81 1 0.0<br />

50 314 4.0 (low) 82 1 0.0<br />

8.5<br />

51 348 4.5<br />

86 1 0.0<br />

52 350 4.5<br />

9.8<br />

Total 7793 100.0<br />

53 415 5.3<br />

21.0<br />

54 438 5.6 (medium)<br />

11.2<br />

55 431 5.5<br />

200


A1 4.2a Book score<br />

Question: About how many books does the <strong>child</strong> have of his/her own? (N=7924)<br />

Questionnaire Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points response points<br />

6 months 10 or more 3 3 <strong>to</strong> 9 2 1 <strong>to</strong> 2 1 none 0<br />

18 months 10 or more 3 3 <strong>to</strong> 9 2 1 <strong>to</strong> 2 1 none 0<br />

24 months 4 or more 2.5 2 <strong>to</strong> 3 1.5 1 0.5 none 0<br />

30 months 10 or more 3 3 <strong>to</strong> 9 2 1 <strong>to</strong> 2 1 none 0<br />

42 months 10 or more 3 3 <strong>to</strong> 9 2 1 <strong>to</strong> 2 1 none 0<br />

Max<br />

possible 14.5<br />

A1 4.2b Categories of book score<br />

Disaggregated<br />

categories Final categories<br />

Points Freq % % %<br />

0 1 0.0<br />

3 2 0.0<br />

3.5 2 0.0<br />

4.5 11 0.1<br />

5.5 19 0.2<br />

6 1 0.0<br />

6.5 48 0.6 7.7 - 7.7<br />

7 1 0.0 (very<br />

7.5 78 1.0 low)<br />

8.5 152 1.9<br />

9 1 0.0<br />

9.5 291 3.7<br />

10 1 0.0<br />

10.5 690 8.7 - 8.7 23.6<br />

11 2 0.0 (low)<br />

14.9<br />

11.5 1177 14.9<br />

12.5 1525 19.2 - 19.2 -<br />

19.2<br />

(medium)<br />

13.5 2391 30.2 - 30.2 49.5<br />

14.5 1531 19.3 - 19.3 (high)<br />

Total 7924 100.0<br />

Min<br />

possible 0<br />

201


A1 4.3a Toy score<br />

Question: How many of the following does you <strong>child</strong> have at home that he can look at or play with? (N=8911)<br />

Component Toy Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points response points<br />

1 Cuddly animals 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

2 Dolls 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

3 A swing 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

4 <strong>to</strong>y cars or lorries 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

5 Jigsaw puzzles 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

6 Mobiles 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

7 Building bricks or blocks 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

8 Balls 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

9 A walker (that he puts things in <strong>and</strong> pushes) 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

10 A walker (that he sits in <strong>and</strong> walks) 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

11 <strong>to</strong>ys where he has <strong>to</strong> fit things <strong>to</strong>gether 4 or more 3 2 or 3 2 one 1 none 0<br />

Max possible 33 Min possible 0<br />

202


A1 4.3b Categories of <strong>to</strong>y score<br />

Disaggregated<br />

categories<br />

Final<br />

categories<br />

Points Freq % % %<br />

1 2 0.0<br />

3 1 0.0<br />

5 1 0.0<br />

6 1 0.0<br />

7 2 0.0<br />

8 4 0.0<br />

9 4 0.0 7.6 - 7.6<br />

10 18 0.2 (very<br />

11 23 0.3 low)<br />

12 61 0.7<br />

13 110 1.2<br />

14 175 2.0<br />

15 275 3.1<br />

16 436 4.9<br />

17 568 6.4 11.3 20.3<br />

18 801 9.0 - 9.0 (low)<br />

19 926 10.4 - 10.4<br />

20 1041 11.7 - 11.7<br />

21 1068 12.0 - 12.0 54.2<br />

22 999 11.2 - 11.2 (medium)<br />

23 795 8.9 - 8.9<br />

24 649 7.3 - 7.3<br />

25 450 5.0<br />

26 247 2.8<br />

27 134 1.5<br />

28 64 0.7<br />

29 29 0.3 10.7 18.0<br />

30 19 0.2 (high)<br />

31 3 0.0<br />

32 1 0.0<br />

33 5 0.1<br />

Total 8911 100.0<br />

A1 4.4 Damp/ condensation/ mould score<br />

Damp/ condensation/ mould at Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

33 months<br />

None 4474 39.92<br />

Yes, but not serious problem 4233 34.93<br />

Fairly/very serious 650 5.36<br />

Missing 2761 22.78<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

203


MOTHER’S CHILDHOOD AND FAMILY BACKGROUND<br />

A1 5.1 Tenure of mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

Tenure of mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

home<br />

Council housing 2141 17.67<br />

Being bought/ owned 6698 55.27<br />

Other rented 295 2.43<br />

Other 367 3.03<br />

Missing 2617 21.60<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 5.2 Mother’s father present in <strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

Mother’s father present in Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

<strong>child</strong>hood home<br />

Throughout 4474 39.92<br />

Partly 4233 34.93<br />

Not at all 650 5.36<br />

Missing 2761 22.78<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

A1 5.3 Mother’s mothers educational attainment<br />

Mother’s highest qualification Frequency Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

CSE/none 4046 33.39<br />

Vocational/O-level 2767 22.83<br />

A-level 1369 11.30<br />

Degree 403 3.33<br />

Missing 3533 29.15<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

204


A1 5.4a Mother’s relationship with her mother<br />

At 12 weeks gestation the mother was asked a series of questions relating <strong>to</strong> her relationship<br />

with her own mother. A ‘mothers relationship with her mother’ variable was derived from these<br />

answers by simply summing all the responses <strong>and</strong> then subtracting 12.<br />

Question: Please tick the box <strong>to</strong> indicate how you mostly remember your mother in your first 16 years.<br />

My mother …<br />

Lowest score Intermediate scores Highest score<br />

response points response points response points<br />

Spoke <strong>to</strong> me with a warm <strong>and</strong> friendly voice Never 1 Sometimes 2 Usually 3<br />

Helped me as much as I needed Never 1 Sometimes 2 Usually 3<br />

Seemed emotionally cold <strong>to</strong> me Usually 1 Sometimes 2 Never 3<br />

Appeared <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> my problems <strong>and</strong><br />

worries Never 1 Sometimes 2 Usually 3<br />

Was affectionate <strong>to</strong> me Never 1 Sometimes 2 Usually 3<br />

Made me feel I wasn’t wanted Usually 1 Sometimes 2 Never 3<br />

Talked things over with me Never 1 Sometimes 2 Usually 3<br />

Praised me Never 1 Sometimes 2 Usually 3<br />

Enjoyed talking things over with me No 1 - - Yes 3<br />

Frequently smiled at me No 1 - - Yes 3<br />

Seemed <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> what I needed or<br />

wanted No 1 - - Yes 3<br />

Could make me feel better when I was <strong>up</strong>set No 1 - - Yes 3<br />

Min<br />

possible 0 Max possible 24<br />

205


A1 5.4b Categories of mother’s relationship with her mother score<br />

Categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

0 52 0.49<br />

1 44 0.41<br />

2 60 0.56<br />

3 62 0.58<br />

4 97 0.91<br />

5 76 0.71<br />

6 109 1.02<br />

7 101 0.95<br />

8 142 1.33 22.17<br />

9 127 1.19<br />

10 127 1.19<br />

11 136 1.28<br />

12 138 1.30<br />

13 153 1.44<br />

14 193 1.81<br />

15 220 2.07<br />

16 238 2.24<br />

17 284 2.67<br />

18 370 3.48<br />

19 436 4.10 22.43<br />

20 657 6.17<br />

21 924 8.68<br />

22 1324 12.44<br />

23 1549 14.56 27.00<br />

24 3022 28.40 - 28.40<br />

Total 10641 100.0<br />

A1 5.5 Mother has a his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy<br />

Mother has a his<strong>to</strong>ry of Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

truancy<br />

No 8746 72.17<br />

Yes 864 7.13<br />

Missing 2508 20.70<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

206


A1 5.6 Mother feels school was a valuable experience<br />

Mother feels school was a Frequency<br />

Percent<strong>age</strong><br />

valuable experience<br />

Very 2412 19.90<br />

Generally 4826 39.83<br />

Not sure 1646 13.58<br />

Generally not/ no 833 6.87<br />

Missing 2401 19.81<br />

Total 12118 100.00<br />

MATERNAL HEALTH, ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL NETWORKS<br />

A1 6.1a Mother’s physical health<br />

At various times the mother was asked <strong>to</strong> describe her physical health. These results were<br />

summed <strong>to</strong> produce an aggregate score. If one observation was missing, the mean of the other<br />

observations was added <strong>to</strong> make <strong>up</strong> the fifth observation. If two observations were missing then<br />

the mean of the other observations was added twice. This is why some results are not integers.<br />

Question: How would you describe your health now? (N=7924)<br />

Questionnaire Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points response points<br />

Always fit Mostly fit <strong>and</strong><br />

Hardly ever<br />

8 weeks <strong>and</strong> well 3 well 2 Often unwell 1 well 0<br />

8 months<br />

Always fit Mostly fit <strong>and</strong><br />

Hardly ever<br />

<strong>and</strong> well 3 well 2 Often unwell 1 well 0<br />

21 months<br />

Always fit Mostly fit <strong>and</strong><br />

Hardly ever<br />

<strong>and</strong> well 3 well 2 Often unwell 1 well 0<br />

Always fit Mostly fit <strong>and</strong><br />

Hardly ever<br />

33 months<br />

47 months<br />

<strong>and</strong> well 3<br />

Always fit<br />

<strong>and</strong> well 3<br />

well 2 Often unwell 1<br />

Mostly fit <strong>and</strong><br />

well 2 Often unwell 1<br />

well 0<br />

Hardly ever<br />

well 0<br />

Max<br />

possible 15 Min possible 0<br />

207


A1 6.1b Categories of mother’s health score<br />

Categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

0 4 0.04<br />

1 1 0.01<br />

1.667 1 0.01<br />

2 2 0.02<br />

3 1 0.01<br />

3.333 1 0.01<br />

3.75 2 0.02<br />

4 5 0.05<br />

5 33 0.31 9.25<br />

6 35 0.33<br />

6.25 17 0.16<br />

6.667 29 0.27<br />

7 88 0.82<br />

7.5 47 0.44<br />

8 173 1.61<br />

8.333 63 0.59<br />

8.75 118 1.10<br />

9 371 3.45<br />

10 1869 17.39 - 17.39<br />

11 1167 10.86<br />

11.25 411 3.82 14.68<br />

11.667 302 2.81<br />

12 1194 11.11 17.36<br />

12.5 370 3.44<br />

13 1323 12.31<br />

13.333 271 2.52 17.80<br />

13.75 319 2.97<br />

14 1182 11.00 - 11.00<br />

15 1348 12.54 - 12.54<br />

Total 10747 100.0 100.0<br />

208


A1 6.2a Social networks score<br />

At 12 weeks gestation, the mother had <strong>to</strong> answer questions about family <strong>and</strong> friends. A ‘social networks’ variable was derived from these<br />

answers by simply summing all the responses.<br />

Lowest score Intermediate scores Highest score<br />

response points response points response points response points<br />

How many of your relatives <strong>and</strong> your<br />

partner’s relatives do you see at least twice a<br />

year? None 0 1 1 2 - 4 2 > 4 3<br />

About how many friends do you have? None 0 1 1 2 - 4 2 > 4 3<br />

Overall, would you say that you belong <strong>to</strong> a<br />

close circle of friends? No 1 - - - - Yes 2<br />

How many people are there that you can talk<br />

<strong>to</strong> about personal problems? None 0 1 1 2 - 4 2 > 4 3<br />

How many people talk <strong>to</strong> you about their<br />

personal problems or their private feelings? None 0 1 1 2 - 4 2 > 4 3<br />

If you have <strong>to</strong> make an important decision,<br />

how many people are there with whom you<br />

can discuss it? None 0 1 1 2 - 4 2 > 4 3<br />

How many people are there among your<br />

family <strong>and</strong> friends from whom you could<br />

borrow £100 if you needed <strong>to</strong>? None 0 1 1 2 - 4 2 > 4 3<br />

How many of your family <strong>and</strong> friends would<br />

help you in times of trouble? None 0 1 1 2 - 4 2 > 4 3<br />

During the last month, how many times did<br />

you get <strong>to</strong>gether with one or more friends? None 0 1 1 2 - 4 2 > 4 3<br />

During the last month, how many times did<br />

you get <strong>to</strong>gether with one of your relatives or<br />

your partner’s relatives? None 0 1 1 2 - 4 2 > 4 3<br />

Min<br />

possible 1<br />

Max<br />

possible 29<br />

209


A1 6.2b Categories of mother’s social networks score<br />

Categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

3 2 0.02<br />

4 1 0.01<br />

5 5 0.04<br />

6 3 0.03<br />

7 7 0.06<br />

8 14 0.12<br />

9 16 0.14<br />

10 28 0.25<br />

11 29 0.26<br />

12 43 0.38 36.14<br />

13 74 0.66<br />

14 86 0.77<br />

15 118 1.05<br />

16 174 1.55<br />

17 232 2.07<br />

18 318 2.83<br />

19 489 4.36<br />

20 594 5.29<br />

21 764 6.81<br />

22 1059 9.44<br />

23 1183 10.54<br />

24 1258 11.21 32.55<br />

25 1212 10.80<br />

26 1023 9.12<br />

27 922 8.22 31.30<br />

28 837 7.46<br />

29 729 6.50<br />

Total 11220 100.0 100.0<br />

210


A1 6.3a Social s<strong>up</strong>port score<br />

At 12 weeks gestation, mothers were asked <strong>to</strong> answer questions concerning how much s<strong>up</strong>port they felt they had. Their responses <strong>to</strong> these<br />

questions were summed <strong>to</strong> produce a ‘social s<strong>up</strong>port’ score.<br />

Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points response points<br />

Sometimes<br />

I have no one <strong>to</strong> share my feelings with Never feel 3 feel 2 Often feel 1 Exactly feel 0<br />

My partner provides the emotional s<strong>up</strong>port I Exactly<br />

Sometimes<br />

need<br />

feel 3 Often feel 2 feel 1 Never feel 0<br />

There are other pregnant women with whom Exactly<br />

Sometimes<br />

I can share my experiences<br />

feel 3 Often feel 2 feel 1 Never feel 0<br />

I believe in moments of difficulty my<br />

neighbours would help me<br />

Exactly<br />

feel 3 Often feel 2<br />

Sometimes<br />

feel 1 Never feel 0<br />

Sometimes<br />

I’m worried that my partner might leave me Never feel 3 feel 2 Often feel 1 Exactly feel 0<br />

There is always someone with whom I can<br />

share my happiness <strong>and</strong> excitement about<br />

my pregnancy<br />

Exactly<br />

feel 3 Often feel 2<br />

Sometimes<br />

feel 1 Never feel 0<br />

If I feel tired I can rely on my partner <strong>to</strong><br />

take over<br />

Exactly<br />

feel 3 Often feel 2<br />

Sometimes<br />

feel 1 Never feel 0<br />

If I was in financial difficulty I know my<br />

family would help if they could<br />

Exactly<br />

feel 3 Often feel 2<br />

Sometimes<br />

feel 1 Never feel 0<br />

If I was in financial I know my friends<br />

would help if they could<br />

Exactly<br />

feel 3 Often feel 2<br />

Sometimes<br />

feel 1 Never feel 0<br />

If all fails I know the state will s<strong>up</strong>port <strong>and</strong><br />

assist me<br />

Exactly<br />

feel 3 Often feel 2<br />

Sometimes<br />

feel 1 Never feel 0<br />

Max<br />

possible 30<br />

Min<br />

possible 0<br />

211


A1 6.3b Categories of mother’s social s<strong>up</strong>ports score<br />

Categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

0 1 0.01<br />

1 2 0.02<br />

2 3 0.03<br />

3 8 0.07<br />

4 8 0.07<br />

5 21 0.20<br />

6 59 0.55<br />

7 46 0.43<br />

8 80 0.75 31.22<br />

9 124 1.16<br />

10 156 1.46<br />

11 206 1.93<br />

12 259 2.43<br />

13 313 2.93<br />

14 395 3.70<br />

15 451 4.23<br />

16 583 5.46<br />

17 618 5.79<br />

18 721 6.76<br />

19 765 7.17<br />

20 805 7.54 37.27<br />

21 861 8.07<br />

22 825 7.73<br />

23 758 7.10<br />

24 789 7.39<br />

25 606 5.68<br />

26 409 3.83 31.49<br />

27 394 3.69<br />

28 178 1.67<br />

29 96 0.90<br />

30 131 1.23<br />

Total 10671 100.0 100.0<br />

A1 6.4a Total CCEI score<br />

The CCEI score is actually the sum of three separate scores, all of which are derived by<br />

summing the responses <strong>to</strong> various different questions. The same questions were asked at 8<br />

weeks gestation, 32 weeks gestation, 8 weeks, 8 months, 21 months <strong>and</strong> 33 months. The<br />

variable used in the analysis is a mean score for the entire preschool period.<br />

212


Question: Please indicate the way you feel at this st<strong>age</strong>…<br />

Lowest score Intermediate scores Highest score<br />

response points response points response points response points<br />

ANXIETY SUBSCORE<br />

Do you feel <strong>up</strong>set for no obvious reason? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 0 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Have you felt as though you might faint? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Not very<br />

Do you feel uneasy <strong>and</strong> restless? Never 0 often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you sometimes feel panicky? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 2 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you worry a lot? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you feel strung-<strong>up</strong> inside? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 0 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you ever have the feeling you are<br />

going <strong>to</strong> pieces? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 0 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you have bad dreams which <strong>up</strong>set you<br />

when you wake <strong>up</strong>? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

DEPRESSION SUBSCORE<br />

Do you feel that life is <strong>to</strong>o much effort? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you regret much of your past<br />

behaviour? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 0 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you wake unusually early in the<br />

morning? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 0 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you experience long periods of<br />

sadness? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you find yourself needing <strong>to</strong> cry? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you lose the ability <strong>to</strong> feel sympathy<br />

for others? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 0 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Not very<br />

Can you think quickly? Very often 0 Often 0 often 2 Never 2<br />

Do you have <strong>to</strong> make a special effort <strong>to</strong><br />

Not very<br />

face <strong>up</strong> <strong>to</strong> a crisis or difficulty? Never 0 often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

SOMATIC SUBSCORE<br />

Do you get troubled by dizziness or<br />

shortness of breath? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you feel sick or have indigestion? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 0 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you feel tingling or prickling<br />

sensations in your body, arms or legs? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you find that you have little or no<br />

appetite? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 0 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Do you feel tired or exhausted? Never 0<br />

Not very<br />

often 1 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Not very<br />

Can you get off <strong>to</strong> sleep alright? Very often 0 Often 0 often 2 Never 2<br />

Do you often have excessive sweating or<br />

Not very<br />

fluttering of the heart? Never 0 often 0 Often 2 Very often 2<br />

Min<br />

Max<br />

possible 0<br />

possible 46<br />

213


A1 6.4b Categories of mother’s CCEI score<br />

Categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

0 – 1.833 104 0.86<br />

2 – 3.833 637 5.28 20.08<br />

4 – 5.833 1401 11.60<br />

6 – 6.25 283 2.34<br />

6.333 – 7.833 1483 12.28 21.00<br />

8 – 9 1053 8.72<br />

9.166 – 9.833 684 5.66<br />

10 – 11.833 1490 12.34 19.22<br />

12 148 1.23<br />

12.166 – 13.833 1116 9.24<br />

14 – 15.833 956 7.91 19.91<br />

16 – 16.666 333 2.76<br />

16.75 – 17.833 414 3.43<br />

18 – 19.833 570 4.72<br />

20 – 21.833 423 3.50<br />

22 – 23.833 292 2.42<br />

24 – 25.833 236 1.95<br />

26 – 27.833 172 1.42<br />

28 – 29.833 121 1.00<br />

30 – 31.833 68 0.56 19.79<br />

32 – 33.833 45 0.37<br />

34 – 35.833 22 0.18<br />

36 – 37.833 13 0.11<br />

38 – 39.833 10 0.08<br />

40 – 41.833 4 0.03<br />

42 – 43.833 0 0<br />

44 – 45.833 1 0.01<br />

Total 12079 100.0 100.0<br />

214


A1 6.5a Self-esteem score<br />

At 33 months, mothers were asked <strong>to</strong> answer questions <strong>to</strong> try <strong>to</strong> measure how much self esteem<br />

the mother has. Their responses <strong>to</strong> these questions were summed <strong>to</strong> produce a ‘self esteem’<br />

score.<br />

Question: Below are some statements. Please say how true they are <strong>to</strong> you.<br />

Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response point response point response point response point response point<br />

I feel that I am a person of Almost<br />

Sometimes Seldom<br />

Often true<br />

worth, at least equal <strong>to</strong> others always true 4<br />

3 true 2 true 1<br />

Never true 0<br />

I feel I have a number of good Almost<br />

Sometimes Seldom<br />

Often true<br />

qualities<br />

always true 4<br />

3 true 2 true 1<br />

Never true 0<br />

I am able <strong>to</strong> do things as well as Almost<br />

Sometimes Seldom<br />

Often true<br />

most other people<br />

always true 4<br />

3 true 2 true 1<br />

Never true 0<br />

I feel I do not have much <strong>to</strong> be<br />

Sometimes<br />

Almost<br />

Never true Seldom true Often true<br />

proud of<br />

4 3 true 2<br />

1 always true 0<br />

I take a positive attitude Almost<br />

Sometimes Seldom<br />

Often true<br />

<strong>to</strong>wards myself<br />

always true 4<br />

3 true 2 true 1<br />

Never true 0<br />

Sometimes I think I am no good<br />

Sometimes<br />

Almost<br />

Never true Seldom true Often true<br />

at all<br />

4 3 true 2<br />

1 always true 0<br />

I am a useful person <strong>to</strong> have Almost<br />

Sometimes Seldom<br />

Often true<br />

around<br />

always true 4<br />

3 true 2 true 1<br />

Never true 0<br />

I feel I cannot do anything right Never true 4<br />

Seldom true 3<br />

Sometimes<br />

Almost<br />

Often true<br />

true 2<br />

1 always true 0<br />

Almost<br />

Sometimes Seldom<br />

Often true<br />

always true 4<br />

3 true 2 true 1<br />

Never true 0<br />

When I do a job I do it well<br />

I feel that my life is not very<br />

useful<br />

I am unlucky<br />

Sometimes<br />

Never true Seldom true 4 3 true 2<br />

Sometimes<br />

Never true Seldom true 4 3 true 2<br />

Max<br />

possible 44<br />

Often true<br />

Often true<br />

1<br />

1<br />

Almost<br />

always true 0<br />

Almost<br />

always true 0<br />

Min<br />

possible 0<br />

215


A1 6.5b Categories of mother’s self esteem score<br />

Categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

0,1 1 4.53<br />

2,3 0 6.66<br />

4,5 1 8.46<br />

6,7 5 10.34<br />

8,9 6 10.05<br />

10,11 10 10.94<br />

12,13 21 10.83 20.91<br />

14,15 36 9.27<br />

16,17 77 8.01<br />

18,19 131 6.64<br />

20,21 195 5.46<br />

22,23 349 3.70<br />

24,25 515 2.06<br />

26,27 627 1.39<br />

28,29 756 0.82 17.27<br />

30,31 875 0.38<br />

32,33 1023 0.22 21.78<br />

34,35 1033 0.10<br />

36,37 949 0.06 20.39<br />

38,39 976 0.05<br />

40,41 799 0.01<br />

42,43 629 0.00 19.66<br />

44 428 0.01<br />

Total 9442 100.0 100.0<br />

216


A1 6.6a Locus of control score<br />

At 12 weeks gestation mothers were asked <strong>to</strong> complete some questions regarding their outlook<br />

on life. The results were summed <strong>to</strong> produce a ‘locus of control’ score. Low scores therefore<br />

represent somebody who does feel they have control <strong>and</strong> hence we have labelled those with a<br />

low score as having a high locus of control ranking.<br />

Highest score Lowest score<br />

response points response points<br />

Did getting good marks at school mean a<br />

great deal <strong>to</strong> you? No 1 Yes 0<br />

Are you often blamed for things that just<br />

aren’t your fault? Yes 1 No 0<br />

Do you feel that most of the time it doesn’t<br />

pay <strong>to</strong> try hard because things never turn out<br />

right anyway? Yes 1 No 0<br />

Do you feel that if things start out well in<br />

the morning that it’s going <strong>to</strong> be a good day<br />

no matter what you do? Yes 1 No 0<br />

Do you believe that whether or not people<br />

like you depends on how you act? No 1 Yes 0<br />

Do you believe that when bad things are<br />

going <strong>to</strong> happen they are just going <strong>to</strong><br />

happen no matter what you try <strong>to</strong> do <strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p<br />

them? Yes 1 No 0<br />

Do you feel when good things happen they<br />

happen because of hard work? No 1 Yes 0<br />

Do you feel that when someone doesn’t like<br />

you there’s little you can do about it? Yes 1 No 0<br />

Did you usually feel that it was almost<br />

useless <strong>to</strong> try in school because most other<br />

<strong>child</strong>ren were cleverer than you? Yes 1 No 0<br />

Are you the kind of person who believes<br />

that planning ahead makes things turn out<br />

better? No 1 Yes 0<br />

Most of the time, do you feel that you have<br />

little <strong>to</strong> say about what you family decides<br />

<strong>to</strong> do? Yes 1 No 0<br />

Do you think it’s better <strong>to</strong> be clever than <strong>to</strong><br />

be lucky? No 1 Yes 0<br />

Max<br />

possible 12<br />

Min possible 0<br />

217


A1 6.6b Categories of mother’s locus of control score<br />

Categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

0 215 2.19<br />

1 665 6.78 37.75<br />

2 1250 12.74 (internal)<br />

3 1574 16.04<br />

4 1724 17.57<br />

5 1626 16.57 46.75<br />

6 1237 12.61 (medium)<br />

7 770 7.85<br />

8 430 4.38<br />

9 194 1.98 15.50<br />

10 96 0.98 (external)<br />

11 28 0.29<br />

12 3 0.03<br />

Total 9812 100.0 100.0<br />

A1 6.7a Mother’s satisfaction with relationship<br />

At 33 months the mother was asked <strong>to</strong> complete some questions concerning her satisfaction<br />

with her relationship with her partner. These results were summed <strong>to</strong> produce a composite score.<br />

Question: People vary greatly in the amount they are satisfied or dissatisfied with their relationship. How do you feel<br />

about the following aspects of your life <strong>to</strong>gether?<br />

Highest score Intermediate scores Lowest score<br />

response points response points response points response point<br />

Very<br />

Moderately Somewhat Very<br />

H<strong>and</strong>ling family finances<br />

satisfied 3 satisfied 2 dissatisfied 1 dissatisfied 0<br />

Demonstrations of affection<br />

Very<br />

satisfied 3<br />

Moderately<br />

satisfied 2<br />

Somewhat<br />

dissatisfied 1<br />

Very<br />

dissatisfied 0<br />

Sex<br />

Very<br />

satisfied 3<br />

Moderately<br />

satisfied 2<br />

Somewhat<br />

dissatisfied 1<br />

Very<br />

dissatisfied 0<br />

Amount of time spent <strong>to</strong>gether<br />

Very<br />

satisfied 3<br />

Moderately<br />

satisfied 2<br />

Somewhat<br />

dissatisfied 1<br />

Very<br />

dissatisfied 0<br />

Making major decisions<br />

Very<br />

satisfied 3<br />

Moderately<br />

satisfied 2<br />

Somewhat<br />

dissatisfied 1<br />

Very<br />

dissatisfied 0<br />

Household tasks<br />

Very<br />

satisfied 3<br />

Moderately<br />

satisfied 2<br />

Somewhat<br />

dissatisfied 1<br />

Very<br />

dissatisfied 0<br />

Leisure time interests <strong>and</strong> activities<br />

Very<br />

satisfied 3<br />

Max<br />

possible 21<br />

Moderately<br />

satisfied 2<br />

Somewhat<br />

dissatisfied 1<br />

Very<br />

dissatisfied 0<br />

Min<br />

possible 0<br />

218


A1 6.7b Categories of mother’s satisfaction with partner score<br />

Categories<br />

Points Freq % %<br />

0 17 0.19<br />

1 9 0.10<br />

2 18 0.20<br />

3 18 0.20<br />

4 14 0.16<br />

5 46 0.52<br />

6 62 0.71 19.69<br />

7 94 1.07<br />

8 121 1.38<br />

9 170 1.94<br />

10 244 2.78<br />

11 404 4.60<br />

12 512 5.83<br />

13 684 7.79 20.10<br />

14 1081 12.31<br />

15 888 10.11 20.49<br />

16 911 10.37<br />

17 863 9.83<br />

18 785 8.94 18.77<br />

19 689 7.85<br />

20 510 5.81 20.96<br />

21 642 7.31<br />

Total 8782 100.0 100.0<br />

INCOME<br />

A1 7.1 Education, Skills <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

E<br />

This domain measures education deprivation in as direct a way as possible. This is<br />

predominantly measured by lack of qualifications amongst adults <strong>and</strong> <strong>child</strong>ren of different <strong>age</strong>s<br />

in a local area. Indica<strong>to</strong>rs of <strong>child</strong>ren <strong>age</strong>d 16 <strong>and</strong> over who are not in full time education <strong>and</strong><br />

the proportion of 17 year olds who have not successfully applied for higher education have also<br />

been included. Both of these participation measures are important aspects of area deprivation.<br />

n, Skills <strong>and</strong> Training Deprivation: Summary of Indica<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

• Working <strong>age</strong> adults with no qualifications (3 years aggregated LFS data at district level,<br />

modelled<br />

<strong>to</strong> ward level) for 1995-1998<br />

• Children <strong>age</strong>d 16 <strong>and</strong> over who are not in full-time education (Child Benefit data – DSS) for<br />

1999<br />

• Proportions of 17-19 year old population who have not successfully applied for HE (UCAS<br />

219


data)<br />

for 1997 <strong>and</strong> 1998<br />

• KS2 primary school performance data (DfEE, converted <strong>to</strong> ward level estimates) for 1998<br />

• Primary school <strong>child</strong>ren with English as an additional langu<strong>age</strong> (DfEE) for 1998<br />

• Absenteeism at primary level (all absences, not just unauthorised) (DfEE) for 1998<br />

A1 7.2 Geographical Access <strong>to</strong> Services<br />

Access <strong>to</strong> essential services is an important aspect of people’s everyday lives. While this is true<br />

for all people, we have focused solely on people with low incomes (on benefits) for the first<br />

three indica<strong>to</strong>rs as they are more likely <strong>to</strong> be experiencing the disadvant<strong>age</strong> of lack of access <strong>to</strong><br />

services more acutely than those on higher incomes, who are in principle more able <strong>to</strong> afford<br />

public or private transport. Access <strong>to</strong> primary schools was measured for all 5-8 year olds.<br />

Geographical Access <strong>to</strong> Services: Summary of Indica<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

• Access <strong>to</strong> a post office (General Post Office Counters) for April 1998<br />

• Access <strong>to</strong> food shops (Data Consultancy) 1998<br />

• Access <strong>to</strong> a GP (NHS, BMA, Scottish Health Service) for Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 1997<br />

• Access <strong>to</strong> a primary school (DfEE) for 1999<br />

220


Appendix A2<br />

Figure A2.1<br />

Distribution of maternal reading score (with final categories)<br />

%<br />

of<br />

sa<br />

m<br />

pl<br />

e<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15<br />

Maternal reading score<br />

Figure A2.2<br />

Distribution of paternal reading score (intact families only)<br />

30<br />

25<br />

% of sample<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4<br />

# times partner recorded as frequent reader<br />

Figure A2.3<br />

Distribution of talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied score (with final<br />

categories)<br />

30<br />

25<br />

% of sample<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

Talking score<br />

221


Figure A2.4<br />

Distribution of 18 month TV score (with final categories)<br />

30<br />

25<br />

% of sample<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

0<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

18 month TV score<br />

5<br />

6<br />

Figure A2.5a<br />

Distribution of maternal teaching score (with final<br />

categories)<br />

% of sample<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30<br />

Maternal teaching score<br />

Figure A2.5b<br />

Distribution of 38 month TV score (with final categories)<br />

35<br />

30<br />

% of sample<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

0<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

38 month TV score<br />

222


Figure A2.6a<br />

Figure A2.6b<br />

50<br />

50<br />

% of sample<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

% of sample<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12<br />

Local shops<br />

Department s<strong>to</strong>res<br />

Figure A2.6c<br />

Figure A2.6d<br />

% of sample<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

% of sample<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12<br />

S<strong>up</strong>ermarket<br />

Park or playground<br />

Figure A2.6e<br />

Figure A2.6f<br />

40<br />

40<br />

% of sample<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

% of sample<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12<br />

Visits <strong>to</strong> friends or family<br />

Library<br />

Figure A2.6g<br />

Figure A2.6h<br />

40<br />

40<br />

% of sample<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

% of sample<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12<br />

Places of interest<br />

Places of entertainment<br />

223


Figure A2.7<br />

Distribution of <strong>to</strong>tal outings score (with final categories)<br />

6<br />

5<br />

% of sample<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

18<br />

20<br />

22<br />

24<br />

26<br />

28<br />

30<br />

32<br />

34<br />

36<br />

38<br />

40<br />

42<br />

44<br />

46<br />

48<br />

50<br />

52<br />

54<br />

56<br />

58<br />

60<br />

62<br />

64<br />

66<br />

68<br />

70<br />

72<br />

74<br />

76<br />

78<br />

80<br />

82<br />

84<br />

86<br />

Total outings score<br />

Figure A2.8<br />

Distribution of book score (with final categories)<br />

40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

0<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

% of sample<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

Book score<br />

Figure A2.9<br />

Distribution of 24 month <strong>to</strong>y score (with final categories)<br />

% of sample<br />

14<br />

12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

10<br />

11<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

16<br />

17<br />

18<br />

19<br />

20<br />

21<br />

22<br />

23<br />

24<br />

25<br />

26<br />

27<br />

28<br />

29<br />

30<br />

31<br />

32<br />

33<br />

Toy score<br />

224


Appendix A3<br />

Appendix Tables A3.1 <strong>to</strong> A3.12 show the interaction of each of the parenting measures<br />

with a measure of parental education. (All regressions include the basic set of controls in<br />

addition.) Each cell contains the coefficient (<strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard error) corresponding <strong>to</strong> that<br />

combination of education <strong>and</strong> parenting score. All coefficients are relative <strong>to</strong> a base combining<br />

the most highly educated <strong>and</strong> the highest parenting score. Hence, we would expect all the<br />

estimates <strong>to</strong> be negative for the EA scores <strong>and</strong> positive for the Behaviour scores. Moving<br />

<strong>up</strong>wards <strong>and</strong> leftwards from the base in the bot<strong>to</strong>m right corner of each table, we might expect<br />

the estimates <strong>to</strong> become more negative (positive for Behaviour) in both directions. Moving<br />

<strong>up</strong>wards, the effects will be more negative if, for a given amount of parenting, less educated<br />

parents are less effective or productive than more educated parents. The figures given in the<br />

bot<strong>to</strong>m row of each table are F-tests of the null hypothesis that there is no difference in the<br />

effect of being in a particular parenting category according <strong>to</strong> education. So high values here,<br />

say greater than 0.1000, imply that there are no ‘efficiency’ effects for that parenting gro<strong>up</strong> –<br />

less educated parents are no better or worse than more educated parents for a given level of<br />

involvement. The figure on the far right of the bot<strong>to</strong>m row, in the bot<strong>to</strong>m left triangle, is a test of<br />

the hypothesis that there are jointly no efficiency effects for any level of the parenting variable.<br />

This is a key summary statistic – high values tell us that we can drop the interaction terms <strong>and</strong><br />

retain our original specification.<br />

Moving from right <strong>to</strong> left, the estimates will become more negative if, for a given educational<br />

gro<strong>up</strong>, lower parenting scores result in worse outcomes. It may be, for example, that certain<br />

types of parenting behaviour only matter in certain households, i.e. those in which the parents<br />

are highly educated. The figures in the last column of the tables are F-tests of the null<br />

hypothesis that for the given education gro<strong>up</strong>, the parenting behaviour has no effect. So values<br />

close <strong>to</strong> zero here imply that the parenting measure has an impact on <strong>child</strong>ren in that particular<br />

parental education gro<strong>up</strong>. The figure in the bot<strong>to</strong>m right corner, in the <strong>to</strong>p left triangle, is a test<br />

that outcomes do not vary with parenting jointly for every educational gro<strong>up</strong>.<br />

225


Guide <strong>to</strong> interpretation:<br />

Bot<strong>to</strong>m left triangle Top right triangle<br />

High Low The parenting variable matters for outcomes<br />

but the effectiveness of the parenting<br />

doesn’t vary with education<br />

High High The parenting variable doesn’t matter for<br />

outcomes<br />

Low High The parenting variable doesn’t matter for<br />

outcomes but education does (shouldn’t<br />

observe this because all regressions control<br />

for education level separately)<br />

Low Low The parenting variable matters for outcomes<br />

<strong>and</strong> its effectiveness varies with education<br />

Table A3.1: Interactions between mothers’ reading scores <strong>and</strong> maternal education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2516) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0869)<br />

Mother's education Mother’s reading score Mother’s reading score<br />

Very low Low Medium High Prob equal Very low Low Medium High<br />

Prob equal<br />

CSE/none -3.253 -1.020 -1.584 0.745 0.0021 3.807*** 3.795*** 0.514 -0.086 0.0000<br />

(1.709) (1.821) (1.561) (1.520) (1.356) (1.438) (1.255) (1.222)<br />

vocational/O-level -4.898*** -3.216** -1.772 -1.329 0.0000 5.242*** 3.612*** 2.018 1.072 0.0000<br />

(1.465) (1.451) (1.372) (1.348) (1.206) (1.193) (1.130) (1.107)<br />

A-level -3.496 -2.913 -1.346 -0.091 0.0034 5.840*** 4.152*** 2.637** 0.966 0.0000<br />

(2.073) (1.703) (1.520) (1.451) (1.575) (1.411) (1.268) (1.212)<br />

Degree/higher -3.853 -3.267** -2.560*** 0.000 0.0046 4.751*** 4.682*** 1.612** 0.000 0.0000<br />

(2.098) (1.463) (0.933) (base) (1.456) (1.162) (0.685) (base)<br />

0.0000 0.0000<br />

Prob equal 0.6480 0.5405 0.9053 0.1843 0.5625 0.4916 0.9050 0.2170 0.4494 0.6341<br />

Table A3.2: Interactions between partners’ reading scores <strong>and</strong> paternal education (intact<br />

families only)<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2527) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0870)<br />

Partner's education<br />

# times partner observed as highly involved in<br />

reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

# times partner observed as highly involved in<br />

reading <strong>to</strong> <strong>child</strong><br />

0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4<br />

Prob<br />

equal<br />

CSE/none -3.026** -3.044** -2.709* -1.646 -2.227 0.6241 1.167 -0.561 -0.256 -0.307 -2.999** 0.0047<br />

(1.447) (1.499) (1.478) (1.489) (1.794) (1.113) (1.151) (1.150) (1.152) (1.430)<br />

vocational/O-level -2.054 -0.905 -0.882 -1.015 -1.197 0.5560 0.730 0.518 -0.159 -0.890 -1.606 0.0117<br />

(1.414) (1.392) (1.384) (1.366) (1.519) (1.113) (1.105) (1.087) (1.081) (1.199)<br />

A-level -3.490** -2.171 -1.066 -0.951 -1.044 0.0174 1.842 0.908 0.549 -0.426 -1.879 0.0000<br />

(1.492) (1.471) (1.437) (1.406) (1.502) (1.166) (1.144) (1.127) (1.100) (1.193)<br />

Degree/higher -0.146 -1.769 -0.516 -0.576 0.000 0.6635 1.992 2.512*** 0.883 -0.012 0.000 0.0043<br />

(1.408) (1.299) (1.083) (0.985) (base) (1.025) (0.898) (0.814) (0.734) (base)<br />

0.2701 0.0000<br />

Prob equal 0.1831 0.2935 0.3118 0.8766 0.6665 0.6554 0.6001 0.0627 0.6353 0.8188 0.1846 0.5276<br />

226


Table A3.3: Interactions between mothers’ teaching scores <strong>and</strong> maternal education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2567) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0729)<br />

Mother's education Mother’s teaching score Mother’s teaching score<br />

Very low Low Medium High Prob equal Very low Low Medium High<br />

Prob equal<br />

CSE/none -4.919** -3.203 -2.147 -1.427 0.0407 1.888 -0.656 -0.354 -2.161 0.0002<br />

(2.139) (2.033) (1.888) (1.894) (1.587) (1.545) (1.435) (1.443)<br />

vocational/O-level -6.149*** -3.918** -2.483 -1.482 0.0000 1.575 1.693 -0.156 -1.708 0.0000<br />

(1.858) (1.786) (1.720) (1.725) (1.433) (1.393) (1.331) (1.334)<br />

A-level -4.497** -3.243 -1.932 0.166 0.0000 2.534 0.517 -1.085 -2.496 0.0000<br />

(2.139) (2.030) (1.894) (1.888) (1.670) (1.587) (1.479) (1.477)<br />

Degree/higher -5.527*** -2.299 -1.138 0.000 0.0053 2.489** 1.386 0.409 0.000 0.1194<br />

(1.665) (1.240) (0.804) (base) (1.145) (0.995) (0.599) (base)<br />

0.0000 0.0000<br />

Prob equal 0.7649 0.8486 0.8676 0.4752 0.9124 0.8899 0.2353 0.7339 0.3729 0.2824<br />

Table A3.4: Interactions between talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied scores <strong>and</strong> maternal education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2443) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0858)<br />

Mother's education Talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied score Talking whilst occ<strong>up</strong>ied score<br />

Low Medium High Prob equal<br />

CSE/none -4.561** -0.875 -0.902 0.0071 6.270*** 3.286*** 0.495 0.0000<br />

(1.811) (1.566) (1.542) (1.431) (1.203) (1.183)<br />

vocational/O-level -2.098 -1.685 -1.000 0.1252 6.816*** 3.670*** 1.382 0.0000<br />

(1.484) (1.391) (1.384) (1.168) (1.083) (1.079)<br />

A-level -1.103 -0.546 -0.302 0.6934 6.351*** 3.655*** 1.083 0.0000<br />

(1.705) (1.507) (1.494) (1.333) (1.191) (1.182)<br />

degree/higher -2.155* -1.256 0.000 0.1122 4.971*** 2.265*** 0.000 0.0000<br />

(1.127) (0.822) (base) (0.875) (0.591) (base)<br />

0.0146 0.0000<br />

Prob equal 0.2300 0.6663 0.8191 0.5337 0.5848 0.6028 0.4970 0.9422<br />

Table A3.5a: Interactions between 18 month TV scores <strong>and</strong> combined parental education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2465) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0633<br />

Parents' education 18 month TV score 18 month TV score<br />

6 3-5 0-2 P equal 6 3-5 0-2 P equal<br />

Neither A-level + -3.143*** -1.864 -0.285 0.0004 1.626** 0.708 -0.110 0.0108<br />

(1.065) (0.960) (1.050) (0.828) (0.750) (0.828)<br />

one A-level + -2.086 -1.415 -0.412 0.1518 2.955*** 0.598 -0.078 0.0022<br />

(1.191) (0.842) (0.907) (0.958) (0.640) (0.694<br />

both A-level + -3.668 -0.780 0.000 0.1790 0.851 1.962** 0.000 0.0027<br />

(2.128) (0.789) (base) (1.465) (0.570) (base)<br />

0.0008 0.0000<br />

P equal 0.5916 0.5212 0.9009 0.8567 0.2598 0.1105 0.9906 0.2158<br />

227


Table A3.5b: Interactions between 38 month TV scores <strong>and</strong> combined parental education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2438) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0659)<br />

Parents' education 38 month TV score 38 month TV score<br />

10 + hrs pwk < 10 hrs pwk P equal 10 + hrs pwk < 10 hrs pwk P equal<br />

Neither A-level + -2.202** -1.252 0.0335 0.514 -1.198 0.0000<br />

(0.963) (0.923) (0.767) (0.734)<br />

one A-level + -1.948** -0.668 0.0626 0.731 -0.999 0.0023<br />

(0.914) (0.748) (0.727) (0.576)<br />

both A-level + 0.111 0.000 0.9252 1.412 0.000 0.1141<br />

(1.183) (base) (0.894) (base)<br />

0.0473 0.0000<br />

P equal 0.2414 0.3829 0.4961 0.7178 0.2002 0.5203<br />

Table A3.6: Interactions between breast feeding duration <strong>and</strong> maternal education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2468) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0634)<br />

Mother's education Duration of Breastfeeding Duration of Breastfeeding<br />

Never<br />

0 – 3<br />

Months<br />

3 – 6<br />

Months<br />

6 – 12<br />

Months<br />

12 +<br />

Months<br />

Prob<br />

equal Never<br />

0 – 3<br />

Months<br />

3 – 6<br />

Months<br />

6 – 12<br />

Months<br />

12 +<br />

Months Prob equal<br />

CSE/none -3.824 -4.253 -0.931 -4.620 -1.170 0.0073 -0.967 -0.557 -1.068 -1.801 -4.545 0.0141<br />

(4.868) (4.882) (4.922) (4.933) (5.050) (3.353) (3.370) (3.408) (3.415) (0.397)<br />

vocational/O-level -3.780 -3.422 -3.992 -2.784 -2.395 0.1149 0.472 0.209 -0.840 -0.253 -0.644 0.0457<br />

(4.701) (4.700) (4.710) (4.705) (4.737) (3.282) (3.282) (3.292) (3.288) (3.320)<br />

A-level -2.579 -0.657 0.055 0.489 -0.880 0.0024 4.376 4.703 3.878 4.705 3.555 0.3312<br />

(5.040) (5.028) (5.030) (5.021) (5.049) (3.839) (3.823) (3.825) (3.815) (3.841)<br />

degree/higher -1.603 -1.592 -1.578 -0.442 0.000 0.5087 -1.183 1.346 -0.269 -0.009 0.000 0.3882<br />

(1.786) (1.239) (1.113) (0.939) (base) (1.401) (0.957) (0.872) (0.705) (base)<br />

0.0004 0.0101<br />

Prob equal 0.9476 0.6827 0.4776 0.4282 0.9254 0.0295 0.2902 0.3068 0.3370 0.1643 0.0581 0.2533<br />

Table A3.7: Interactions between maternal bonding score <strong>and</strong> maternal education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2419) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.1144)<br />

Mother's education Maternal Bonding Score Maternal Bonding Score<br />

Lowest<br />

Quintile<br />

2 nd<br />

Quintile<br />

3 rd<br />

Quintile<br />

4 th<br />

Quintile<br />

Highest<br />

Quintile<br />

Prob<br />

equal<br />

Lowest<br />

Quintile<br />

2 nd<br />

Quintile<br />

3 rd<br />

Quintile<br />

4 th<br />

Quintile<br />

Highest<br />

Quintile<br />

Prob equal<br />

CSE/none 1.990 2.477 2.984 2.181 1.212 0.6001 7.221*** 4.665** 3.340 2.538 -0.472 0.0000<br />

(2.630) (2.614) (2.682) (2.630) (2.604) (1.884) (1.870) (1.900) (1.879) (1.852)<br />

vocational/O-level 0.021 -0.069 0.519 -0.585 -0.110 0.4877 8.390*** 5.710*** 4.405** 3.446** 0.745 0.0000<br />

(2.446) (2.441) (2.449) (2.445) (2.450) (1.753) (1.749) (1.758) (1.753) (1.756)<br />

A-level 2.492 1.941 1.877 1.720 1.810 0.8784 10.243*** 7.513*** 6.011*** 4.852** 2.995 0.0000<br />

(2.607) (2.598) (2.618) (2.610) (2.641) (1.897) (1.891) (1.909) (1.903) (1.928)<br />

degree/higher 1.031 1.852 0.361 1.116 0.000 0.6543 6.264*** 4.049*** 3.778*** 2.040 0.000 0.0000<br />

(1.561) (1.586) (1.668) (1.660) (base) (1.083) (1.090) (1.155) (1.146) (base)<br />

0.8302 0.0000<br />

Prob equal 0.3620 0.3086 0.4534 0.2483 0.6206 0.8238 0.0638 0.1096 0.2804 0.2830 0.0854 0.6620<br />

228


Table A3.8: Interactions types of discipline <strong>and</strong> maternal education<br />

Entry Assessment (N = 5010) Behaviour (N = 9416)<br />

Mother’s education Ignores <strong>child</strong> Ignores <strong>child</strong><br />

Rarely Frequently P equal Rarely Frequently P equal<br />

CSE/ none -0.245 -0.603 0.7973 2.603*** 2.292** 0.7795<br />

(0.805) (1.303) (0.582) (1.052)<br />

vocational/ O-level -0.052 1.052 0.3449 1.905*** 2.070** 0.8657<br />

(0.421) (1.139) (0.338) (0.950)<br />

A-level -0.708 -0.161 0.6776 1.941*** 2.070 0.9069<br />

(0.581) (1.261) (0.451) (1.070)<br />

degree/higher 0.079 0.000 0.9176 1.976*** 0.000 0.0004<br />

(0.765) (base) (0.559) (base)<br />

0.7435 0.0112<br />

P equal 0.7909 0.3623 0.6707 0.7592 0.1129 0.3192<br />

Smacks <strong>child</strong><br />

Smacks <strong>child</strong><br />

Rarely Frequently P equal Rarely Frequently P equal<br />

CSE/ none -0.737 -0.747 0.9939 2.128*** 2.252** 0.9099<br />

(0.776) (1.306) (0.570) (1.054)<br />

Vocational/ O-level -0.739* 0.838 0.1711 1.747*** 2.116** 0.7015<br />

(0.409) (1.137) (0.328) (0.950)<br />

A-level -1.130** -0.299 0.5241 1.388*** 1.980 0.5914<br />

(0.576) (1.258) (0.452) (1.069)<br />

degree/higher 0.351 0.000 0.6893 1.251** 0.000 0.0497<br />

(0.879) (base) (0.637) (base)<br />

0.5142 0.3513<br />

P equal 0.5617 0.4311 0.5441 0.6656 0.1149 0.3194<br />

Shouts at <strong>child</strong><br />

Shouts at <strong>child</strong><br />

Rarely Frequently P equal Rarely Frequently P equal<br />

CSE/ none 1.179 0.528 0.6255 3.213*** 3.727*** 0.6318<br />

(1.066) (1.552) (0.714) (1.205)<br />

Vocational/ O-level -0.877 0.401 0.2805 2.561*** 3.210*** 0.5072<br />

(0.607) (1.281) (0.469) (1.046)<br />

A-level -0.017 0.121 0.9155 2.127*** 2.836** 0.5173<br />

(0.722) (1.417) (0.557) (1.176)<br />

degree/higher -0.107 0.000 0.9013 1.361** 0.000 0.0255<br />

(0.859) (base) (0.609) (base)<br />

0.3851 0.1349<br />

P equal 0.3178 0.9822 0.3705 0.2064 0.0065 0.0398<br />

Sends <strong>child</strong> <strong>to</strong> room<br />

Sends <strong>child</strong> <strong>to</strong> room<br />

Rarely Frequently P equal Rarely Frequently P equal<br />

CSE/ none -0.532 -0.644 0.9352 1.023* 1.949* 0.3988<br />

(0.778) (1.312) (0.569) (1.059)<br />

Vocational/ O-level 0.527 1.371 0.4673 1.637*** 2.191** 0.5668<br />

(0.409) (1.144) (0.327) (0.955)<br />

A-level -0.194 0.033 0.8604 1.362*** 2.060* 0.5223<br />

(0.544) (1.273) (0.429) (1.078)<br />

degree/higher -0.209 0.000 0.7833 1.516*** 0.000 0.0054<br />

(0.760) (base) (0.545) (base)<br />

0.8995 0.0537<br />

P equal 0.5013 0.2156 0.4928 0.8000 0.1253 0.3209<br />

229


Table A3.8: Interactions types of discipline <strong>and</strong> maternal education (Continued)<br />

Entry Assessment (N = 5010) Behaviour (N = 9416)<br />

Mother’s education Takes away treats Takes away treats<br />

Rarely Frequently P equal Rarely Frequently P equal<br />

CSE/ none -0.739 -1.035 0.8275 0.451 1.928* 0.1763<br />

(0.770) (1.311) (0.564) (1.058)<br />

Vocational/ O-level 0.390 1.018 0.5864 0.612* 1.994** 0.1520<br />

(0.409) (1.141) (0.328) (0.953)<br />

A-level -1.048* -0.557 0.7044 0.499 1.934* 0.1904<br />

(0.557) (1.264) (0.435) (1.073)<br />

degree/higher 1.284 0.000 0.1060 0.364 0.000 0.5253<br />

(0.794) (base) (0.573) (base)<br />

0.4499 0.5130<br />

P equal 0.0396 0.1896 0.0782 0.9808 0.1724 0.5322<br />

Tells off <strong>child</strong><br />

Tells off <strong>child</strong><br />

Rarely Frequently P equal Rarely Frequently P equal<br />

CSE/ none 2.054 1.294 0.6085 0.837 2.044 0.3071<br />

(1.447) (1.981) (0.969) (1.462)<br />

Vocational/ O-level -0.198 0.796 0.4635 2.556*** 3.641*** 0.3240<br />

(0.930) (1.600) (0.729) (1.283)<br />

A-level -0.560 -0.567 0.9963 0.784 1.947 0.3329<br />

(1.231) (1.854) (0.963) (1.500)<br />

degree/higher 0.192 0.000 0.8925 1.083 0.000 0.3052<br />

(1.418) (base) (1.056) (base)<br />

0.4859 0.2857<br />

P equal 0.5120 0.7827 0.4978 0.3168 0.0445 0.0870<br />

Bribes <strong>child</strong><br />

Bribes <strong>child</strong><br />

Rarely Frequently P equal Rarely Frequently P equal<br />

CSE/ none 0.023 -0.434 0.7453 2.749*** 2.416** 0.7697<br />

(0.825) (1.296) (0.621) (1.047)<br />

Vocational/ O-level -0.557 0.992 0.1881 2.006*** 2.226** 0.8228<br />

(0.446) (1.133) (0.354) (0.948)<br />

A-level -0.960 -0.119 0.5285 2.637*** 2.372** 0.8136<br />

(0.624) (1.255) (0.480) (1.067)<br />

degree/higher -0.389 0.000 0.6348 1.033* 0.000 0.0760<br />

(0.819) (base) (0.582) (base)<br />

0.4539 0.4613<br />

P equal 0.8096 0.4690 0.6848 0.1205 0.0702 0.0731<br />

230


Table A3.9: Interactions between place-specific outings scores <strong>and</strong> maternal education<br />

Entry Assessment (N = 5010) Behaviour (N = 9416)<br />

Department s<strong>to</strong>re<br />

Department s<strong>to</strong>re<br />

Low High P equal Low High P equal<br />

CSE/ none -2.835 -3.594 0.3295 0.175 -1.300 0.0157<br />

(4.225) (4.215) (2.553) (2.545)<br />

Vocational/ O-level 0.790 0.152 0.1141 1.246 0.785 0.1698<br />

(3.984) (3.986) (2.467) (2.466)<br />

A-level 1.339 0.699 0.2423 2.006 1.688 0.4717<br />

(4.234) (4.234) (2.748) (2.749)<br />

degree/higher -0.054 0.000 0.9435 -0.172 0.000 0.7604<br />

(0.768) (base) (0.565) (base)<br />

0.3082 0.0813<br />

P equal 0.4619 0.4221 0.7365 0.7999 0.5439 0.4622<br />

Park<br />

Park<br />

Low High P equal Low High P equal<br />

CSE/ none -1.082 -1.535 0.5726 2.800 2.128 0.2714<br />

(4.370) (4.372) (3.070) (3.065)<br />

Vocational/ O-level -1.391 -0.842 0.1893 0.447 -1.019 0.0000<br />

(3.796) (3.795) (2.756) (2.755)<br />

A-level -3.911 -4.476 0.3251 0.898 0.096 0.0860<br />

(3.913) (3.904) (2.888) (2.880)<br />

degree/higher 0.528 0.000 0.5226 -0.263 0.000 0.6688<br />

(0.825) (base) (0.614) (base)<br />

0.4851 0.0002<br />

P equal 0.6147 0.4556 0.4592 0.7233 0.6117 0.2441<br />

Entry Assessment (N = 5010) Behaviour (N = 9416)<br />

Mother’s education Friends/relatives Friends/relatives<br />

Low High P equal Low High P equal<br />

CSE/ none 0.717 0.954 0.7531 -1.827 -2.394 0.3237<br />

(3.169) (3.183) (2.140) (2.153)<br />

Vocational/ O-level -4.556** -4.721** 0.6825 2.441 1.551 0.0074<br />

(2.118) (2.120) (1.867) (1.865)<br />

A-level -4.249* -3.239 0.0601 4.355* 3.472 0.0440<br />

(2.490) (2.497) (2.295) (2.304)<br />

degree/higher 0.725 0.000 0.3459 1.434** 0.000 0.0116<br />

(0.769) (base) (0.568) (base)<br />

0.3162 0.0011<br />

P equal 0.0380 0.0805 0.0745 0.2369 0.2402 0.4605<br />

Library<br />

Library<br />

Low High P equal Low High P equal<br />

CSE/ none 10.087** 11.222*** 0.1876 2.245 1.702 0.4112<br />

(4.194) (4.233) (2.826) (2.862)<br />

Vocational/ O-level 13.419*** 14.937*** 0.0002 1.456 0.593 0.0119<br />

(3.992) (3.993) (2.672) (2.675)<br />

A-level 8.929* 10.150** 0.0224 -4.845 -6.338** 0.0006<br />

(4.574) (4.574) (3.180) (3.180)<br />

degree/higher -0.958 0.000 0.2351 -0.334 0.000 0.5730<br />

(0.807) (base) (0.593) (base)<br />

0.0002 0.0008<br />

P equal 0.0045 0.0025 0.0249 0.0586 0.0275 0.0228<br />

231


Table A3.9: Interactions between place-specific outings scores <strong>and</strong> maternal education<br />

(continued)<br />

Places of interest<br />

Places of interest<br />

Low High P equal Low High P equal<br />

CSE/ none -3.670 0.111 0.0000 -1.100 -1.398 0.6654<br />

(5.816) (5.826) (4.234) (4.244)<br />

Vocational/ O-level -1.960 -1.392 0.2202 -4.362 -5.635 0.0011<br />

(5.596) (5.595) (4.068) (4.062)<br />

A-level 2.884 3.507 0.3211 -0.414 -2.107 0.0009<br />

(5.842) (5.825) (4.357) (4.341)<br />

degree/higher -1.654* 0.000 0.0864 0.980 0.000 0.1691<br />

(0.964) (base) (0.713) (base)<br />

0.0002 0.0001<br />

P equal 0.4543 0.6596 0.0458 0.4258 0.3541 0.4553<br />

Places of entertainment Places of entertainment<br />

Low High P equal Low High P equal<br />

CSE/ none -3.504 -4.790 0.1691 1.120 -0.572 0.0153<br />

(4.629) (4.653) (2.993) (3.013)<br />

Vocational/ O-level -7.073 -6.991 0.8578 3.085 2.261 0.0329<br />

(4.361) (4.363) (2.802) (2.804)<br />

A-level -5.071 -5.121 0.9356 1.941 2.490 0.2661<br />

(4.511) (4.518) (2.998) (2.999)<br />

degree/higher 1.529* 0.000 0.0681 0.546 0.000 0.3705<br />

(0.838) (base) (0.610) (base)<br />

0.2619 0.0141<br />

P equal 0.2184 0.4391 0.3594 0.7681 0.5423 0.1456<br />

Table A3.10: Interactions between book scores <strong>and</strong> combined parental education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2514) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0704)<br />

Parents' education Book score Book score<br />

Very low Low Medium High P equal Very low Low Medium High P equal<br />

neither A-level + -4.013*** -2.232** -1.652* -0.875 0.0004 1.596* 1.187 -0.197 -1.008 0.0000<br />

(1.135) (0.975) (0.994) (0.942) (0.864) (0.751) (0.774) (0.733)<br />

One A-level + -5.415*** -3.277*** -1.603* -0.045 0.0000 3.586** 1.769** 0.117 -1.178* 0.0000<br />

(1.895) (0.953) (0.972) (0.790) (1.453) (0.745) (0.757) (0.605)<br />

both A-level + -5.384* -0.787 0.115 0.000 0.2458 2.275 1.985** 0.485 0.000 0.1029<br />

(2.786) (1.183) (1.006) (base) (2.045) (0.868) (0.734) (base)<br />

0.0000 0.0000<br />

P equal 0.7168 0.1481 0.3258 0.3947 0.3579 0.4034 0.6173 0.7659 0.1500 0.6631<br />

232


Table A3.11: Interactions between <strong>to</strong>y scores <strong>and</strong> combined parental education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2483) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0717)<br />

Parents' education Toy score Toy score<br />

Very low Low Medium High P equal Very low Low Medium High P equal<br />

neither A-level + -2.804** -2.393** -1.531 -0.699 0.0379 3.910*** 2.469*** 1.192 -0.536 0.0000<br />

(1.347) (1.195) (1.140) (1.223) (1.015) (0.909) (0.867) (0.937)<br />

one A-level + -4.587*** -2.031* -1.185 -0.671 0.0501 2.385* 2.386*** 1.114 -0.297 0.0019<br />

(1.700) (1.147) (1.011) (1.115) (1.339) (0.873) (0.752) (0.856)<br />

Both A-level + -0.246 -1.361 -0.508 0.000 0.7733 4.916*** 2.052** 1.566** 0.000 0.0036<br />

(2.142) (1.298) (0.904) (base) (1.526) (0.916) (0.652) (base)<br />

0.0490 0.0000<br />

P equal 0.2113 0.7494 0.5734 0.8172 0.8504 0.3461 0.9201 0.7695 0.8482 0.9095<br />

Table A3.12: Interactions between damp/mould/condensation scores <strong>and</strong> combined parental<br />

education<br />

Entry Assessment (adj R2 = 0.2434) Behaviour (adj R2 = 0.0664)<br />

Parents' education Damp score Damp score<br />

None<br />

Minor<br />

Problem<br />

Serious<br />

Problem<br />

P equal<br />

None<br />

Minor<br />

Problem<br />

Serious<br />

Problem<br />

P equal<br />

Neither A-level + 0.120 0.463 -0.916 0.3004 -2.441* -1.148 -0.361 0.0001<br />

(1.853) (1.855) (2.024) (1.400) (1.402) (1.535)<br />

one A-level + 0.581 0.882 3.703* 0.0468 -2.671** -1.298 0.054 0.0015<br />

(1.785) (1.783) (2.115) (1.338) (1.333) (1.614)<br />

both A-level + 1.591 1.796 0.000 0.5805 -1.686 -0.183 0.000 0.0219<br />

(1.761) (1.726) (base) (1.291) (1.275) (base)<br />

0.1399 0.0000<br />

P equal 0.3622 0.3977 0.6509 0.0802 0.3467 0.2318 0.8140 0.6707<br />

233


Appendix A4<br />

Tables of Regression including School Dummies<br />

Table A.4.1 Basic Demographics – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score with school dummies<br />

Full Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Restricted Sample Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

Score<br />

234<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Log of household income<br />

Early Measure<br />

1.394***<br />

(0.322)<br />

Mother’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

-1.155***<br />

(0.326)<br />

A-level<br />

0.483*<br />

(0.254)<br />

Degree<br />

1.253***<br />

(0.367)<br />

Partner’s highest qualification (base = vocational/O-level)<br />

CSE/none<br />

A-level<br />

Degree<br />

-1.056***<br />

(0.298)<br />

0.233<br />

(0.255)<br />

1.475***<br />

(0.331)<br />

Lone parent status (base = never lone parent)<br />

Intermittently, with partner at 47 months<br />

-0.530<br />

(0.650)<br />

Intermittently, no partner at 47 months<br />

-0.120<br />

(0.531)<br />

Since birth<br />

-0.068<br />

(0.979)<br />

Mother’s <strong>age</strong> at birth (base = 25-29)<br />

1<br />

(0.575)<br />

Older siblings (base = none)<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3+<br />

-0.846***<br />

(0.268)<br />

-1.845***<br />

(0.349)<br />

-2.818***<br />

(0.515)<br />

Partner’s employment status at 47 months (base = in work)<br />

-0.431<br />

Not in work<br />

(0.417)<br />

1.809***<br />

(0.425)<br />

-0.753*<br />

(0.410)<br />

0.566*<br />

(0.336)<br />

1.476***<br />

(0.529)<br />

-1.217***<br />

(0.374)<br />

0.162<br />

(0.328)<br />

1.735***<br />

(0.462)<br />

-0.775<br />

(0.842)<br />

0.042<br />

(0.682)<br />

1.518<br />

(1.332)<br />

-0.288<br />

(1.004)<br />

-0.012<br />

(0.392)<br />

0.011<br />

(0.307)<br />

-0.138<br />

(0.447)<br />

1.736**<br />

(0.712)<br />

2.538***<br />

(0.247)<br />

-0.893<br />

(0.662)<br />

-0.667***<br />

(0.258)<br />

-0.952*<br />

(0.544)<br />

-0.335<br />

(0.313)<br />

-1.237<br />

(0.760)<br />

-1.073***<br />

(0.356)<br />

-2.371***<br />

(0.466)<br />

-3.050***<br />

(0.674)<br />

-0.227<br />

(0.547)<br />

1.639***<br />

(0.408)<br />

-0.745*<br />

(0.394)<br />

-0.048<br />

(0.323)<br />

1.402***<br />

(0.508)<br />

-1.127***<br />

(0.359)<br />

0.124<br />

(0.315)<br />

1.178***<br />

(0.444)<br />

0.676<br />

(0.809)<br />

0.008<br />

(0.656)<br />

0.726<br />

(1.280)<br />

-2.516***<br />

(0.965)<br />

-0.531<br />

(0.376)<br />

0.201<br />

(0.295)<br />

0.359<br />

(0.430)<br />

-0.431<br />

(0.684)<br />

3.511***<br />

(0.237)<br />

-2.596***<br />

(0.636)<br />

-0.798***<br />

(0.248)<br />

-0.371<br />

(0.523)<br />

-0.145<br />

(0.300)<br />

-1.240*<br />

(0.730)<br />

-0.683**<br />

(0.343)<br />

-2.195***<br />

(0.448)<br />

-3.108***<br />

(0.647)<br />

-0.187<br />

(0.525)<br />

Simple Value Added<br />

0.170<br />

(0.439)<br />

-0.007<br />

(0.423)<br />

0.614*<br />

(0.347)<br />

0.074<br />

(0.546)<br />

-0.089<br />

(0.386)<br />

0.038<br />

(0.339)<br />

0.557<br />

(0.478)<br />

-1.451*<br />

(0.870)<br />

0.034<br />

(0.705)<br />

0.792<br />

(1.377)<br />

2.227**<br />

(1.038)<br />

0.519<br />

(0.405)<br />

-0.191<br />

(0.317)<br />

-0.497<br />

(0.462)<br />

2.168***<br />

(0.735)<br />

-0.973***<br />

(0.255)<br />

1.703**<br />

(0.684)<br />

0.131<br />

(0.267)<br />

-0.581<br />

(0.563)<br />

-0.190<br />

(0.323)<br />

0.004<br />

(0.785)<br />

-0.390<br />

(0.368)<br />

-0.177<br />

(0.482)<br />

0.058<br />

(0.696)<br />

-0.039<br />

(0.565)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1845 0.1902 0.3513 0.1813


Table A4.2 Childcare – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score with school dummies<br />

Full Sample Restricted Sample Entry Simple Value<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score Aver<strong>age</strong> Score Assessment Added<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Childcare pre-<strong>age</strong> 3<br />

Used partner 5-19 hours per -0.036 0.064 0.489 -0.425<br />

week (0.260) (0.345) (0.332) (0.357)<br />

Used partner 20+ hours per 0.000 -0.000 0.020 -0.021<br />

week (0.260) (0.346) (0.332) (0.357)<br />

Used friend/relative 5-19 -0.535** -0.643** -0.287 -0.356<br />

hours per week (0.241) (0.315) (0.303) (0.326)<br />

Used friend/relative 20+ -0.546** -0.667* -0.822** 0.155<br />

hours per week (0.261) (0.343) (0.330) (0.354)<br />

Used paid person 5-19 hours -0.402 -0.299 -0.544 0.244<br />

per week (0.344) (0.473) (0.455) (0.489)<br />

Used paid person 20+ hours -0.420 -0.059 0.145 -0.204<br />

per week (0.293) (0.404) (0.388) (0.417)<br />

Used centre-based care 5-19 -0.310 -0.678 -0.100 -0.578<br />

hours per week (0.432) (0.608) (0.585) (0.629)<br />

Used centre-based care 20+ 1.075* 0.945 1.752** -0.808<br />

hours per week (0.564) (0.843) (0.810) (0.871)<br />

Childcare <strong>age</strong> 3 <strong>to</strong> 4<br />

Nursery class -0.512 -0.222 1.053** -1.276**<br />

(0.369) (0.517) (0.497) (0.534)<br />

LEA nursery school -0.076 -0.334 0.402 -0.737<br />

(0.431) (0.603) (0.580) (0.623)<br />

Private day nursery -0.032 0.028 0.600 -0.571<br />

(0.337) (0.457) (0.439) (0.472)<br />

Playgro<strong>up</strong> 0.398 -0.001 -0.191 0.190<br />

(0.254) (0.348) (0.334) (0.359)<br />

Nursery (type undefined) 0.081 0.070 0.919** -0.849**<br />

(0.278) (0.394) (0.379) (0.408)<br />

Other -0.768 -0.909 -0.969 0.060<br />

(0.791) (1.203) (1.156) (1.243)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1845 0.1902 0.3513 0.1813<br />

235


Table A4.3 Parenting – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score with school dummies<br />

Full Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Entry<br />

Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Simple Value<br />

Added<br />

Maternal reading score (base = high)<br />

Very low -1.358*** -1.664*** -1.598*** -0.066<br />

(0.431) (0.562) (0.540) (0.581)<br />

Low -0.752** -1.014** -0.745 -0.268<br />

(0.377) (0.497) (0.477) (0.513)<br />

Medium -0.452* -0.442 -0.298 -0.145<br />

(0.254) (0.332) (0.319) (0.344)<br />

Teaching score (base = high)<br />

Very low -2.476*** -3.257*** -4.063*** 0.807<br />

(0.445) (0.586) (0.563) (0.606)<br />

Low -1.734*** -1.862*** -2.290*** 0.428<br />

(0.357) (0.464) (0.446) (0.479)<br />

Medium -0.661*** -0.863*** -1.312*** 0.449<br />

(0.231) (0.301) (0.289) (0.311)<br />

18 month TV score (base = 0-2 hours per week)<br />

3 –5 0.009 -0.050 -0.191 0.141<br />

(0.237) (0.323) (0.310) (0.334)<br />

6 0.056 -0.283 -0.747* 0.464<br />

(0.360) (0.469) (0.450) (0.484)<br />

Duration of breastfeeding (base = never)<br />

Breastfed 6 months<br />

0.902*** 1.202*** 0.914*** 0.288<br />

(0.284) (0.365) (0.351) (0.377)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1845 0.1902 0.3513 0.1813<br />

236


Table A4.4 Outings <strong>and</strong> Home Environment – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score with school dummies<br />

Full Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Entry<br />

Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Simple Value<br />

Added<br />

Outings scores<br />

Department s<strong>to</strong>res -0.076 -0.289 -0.351 0.062<br />

(0.211) (0.278) (0.267) (0.287)<br />

Library 0.436** 0.449 0.975*** -0.527*<br />

(0.219) (0.290) (0.278) (0.299)<br />

Book score (base = high)<br />

Very low -1.322*** -0.921 -2.208*** 1.287**<br />

(0.490) (0.634) (0.610) (0.656)<br />

Low -0.307 -0.283 -0.689* 0.406<br />

(0.286) (0.375) (0.361) (0.388)<br />

Medium -0.007 0.021 -0.314 0.334<br />

(0.278) (0.368) (0.354) (0.381)<br />

Toy score (base = high)<br />

Very low 0.041 -0.904 -1.296** 0.392<br />

(0.478) (0.614) (0.590) (0.634)<br />

Low 0.058 -0.265 -0.352 0.087<br />

(0.334) (0.440) (0.423) (0.455)<br />

Medium 0.196 -0.001 0.482 -0.482<br />

(0.266) (0.354) (0.340) (0.366)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1845 0.1902 0.3513 0.1813<br />

237


Table A4.5 Mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood <strong>and</strong> family <strong>background</strong> – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score with school<br />

dummies<br />

Full Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Restricted Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Entry<br />

Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Simple Value<br />

Added<br />

Tenure of mother’s <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = mortg<strong>age</strong>/owned<br />

Council rented -0.491* -0.107 -0.416 0.308<br />

(0.271) (0.346) (0.332) (0.357)<br />

Other rented 0.757 1.115 0.580 0.534<br />

(0.564) (0.768) (0.738) (0.793)<br />

Other 0.380 0.418 1.179* -0.761<br />

(0.514) (0.683) (0.656) (0.706)<br />

Mother’s father present in <strong>child</strong>hood home (base = not at all)<br />

Throughout 0.696 0.555 0.624 -0.069<br />

(0.528) (0.696) (0.669) (0.719)<br />

Partly 0.439 0.211 0.337 -0.126<br />

(0.573) (0.756) (0.727) (0.782)<br />

Mother’s mother’s highest qualification (base = CSE/none)<br />

Vocational/O-level<br />

0.316 0.416 0.190 0.226<br />

(0.261) (0.344) (0.331) (0.356)<br />

A-level 0.192 0.325 0.661 -0.337<br />

(0.327) (0.454) (0.436) (0.469)<br />

Degree 0.436 0.538 0.402 0.136<br />

(0.530) (0.785) (0.755) (0.812)<br />

Mother has a his<strong>to</strong>ry of truancy (base = yes)<br />

No 1.140*** 1.266** 1.231** 0.035<br />

(0.426) (0.544) (0.522) (0.562)<br />

Mother feels school was a valuable experience (base = not sure/generally not/no)<br />

Yes very 0.371 0.368 -0.016 0.383<br />

(0.304) (0.406) (0.390) (0.419)<br />

Yes generally 0.017 -0.181 -0.471 0.290<br />

(0.260) (0.340) (0.326) (0.351)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1845 0.1902 0.3513 0.1813<br />

238


Table A4.6 Maternal health, attitudes <strong>and</strong> social networks – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score with<br />

school dummies<br />

Full Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Restricted Sample Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

Score<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Simple Value Added<br />

Social s<strong>up</strong>port score (base = low)<br />

Medium 0.388 0.175 -0.109 0.284<br />

(0.246) (0.327) (0.314) (0.338)<br />

High 0.198 0.085 0.020 0.065<br />

(0.261) (0.342) (0.329) (0.354)<br />

Locus of control score (base = low)<br />

Medium -0.261 -0.206 -0.651** 0.445<br />

(0.228) (0.302) (0.290) (0.312)<br />

High -1.041*** -1.137** -0.680 -0.457<br />

(0.376) (0.480) (0.461) (0.496)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1845 0.1902 0.3513 0.1813<br />

Table A4.7 Neighbourhood – Aver<strong>age</strong> Score with school dummies<br />

Full Sample<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> Score<br />

Restricted Sample Aver<strong>age</strong><br />

Score<br />

Entry Assessment<br />

No. of observations 7537 4996 4996 4996<br />

Simple Value Added<br />

Education domain score<br />

-0.220 -0.818*** -0.486* -0.331<br />

(0.145) (0.260) (0.250) (0.269)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers – predicted component (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile -0.315 -0.343 0.887 -1.230**<br />

(0.541) (0.579) (0.556) (0.598)<br />

3 rd quintile 0.587 0.428 1.302** -0.874<br />

(0.554) (0.595) (0.572) (0.615)<br />

4 th quintile 0.933* 0.759 1.525*** -0.766<br />

(0.565) (0.609) (0.585) (0.629)<br />

Highest quintile -0.027 -0.427 1.513** -1.939***<br />

(0.583) (0.636) (0.612) (0.658)<br />

Aver<strong>age</strong> EA score of peers – residual component (base = lowest quintile)<br />

2 nd quintile -0.675 -0.701 -0.357 -0.344<br />

(0.531) (0.567) (0.545) (0.586)<br />

3 rd quintile -0.828 -0.842 0.132 -0.975*<br />

(0.533) (0.571) (0.548) (0.590)<br />

4 th quintile -0.543 -0.486 0.295 -0.781<br />

(0.548) (0.586) (0.563) (0.606)<br />

Highest quintile -0.961* -0.880 0.154 -1.034*<br />

(0.542) (0.579) (0.557) (0.599)<br />

Adjusted R-squared 0.1845 0.1902 0.3513 0.1813<br />

239


Copies of this publication can be obtained from:<br />

DfES Publications<br />

P.O. Box 5050<br />

Sherwood Park<br />

Annesley<br />

Nottingham<br />

NG15 0DJ<br />

Tel: 0845 60 222 60<br />

Fax: 0845 60 333 60<br />

Minicom: 0845 60 555 60<br />

Online: www.dfespublications.gov.uk<br />

© University of Bris<strong>to</strong>l 2006<br />

Produced by the Department for Education <strong>and</strong> Skills<br />

ISBN 978 1 84478 840 8<br />

Ref No: RR808A<br />

www.dfes.go.uk/research

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!