12.03.2015 Views

Effects of prosodic and segmental context on /g ... - Speech Prosody

Effects of prosodic and segmental context on /g ... - Speech Prosody

Effects of prosodic and segmental context on /g ... - Speech Prosody

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Cole, J., J.I. Hualde, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> K. Iskarous. 1999. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Prosodic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Segmental C<strong>on</strong>text <strong>on</strong> /g/-Leniti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

Spanish. In O. Fujimura, B.D. Joseph, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> B. Palek (eds.), Proceedings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Fourth Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Linguistics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ph<strong>on</strong>etics C<strong>on</strong>ference, pp. 575-589.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>segmental</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>on</strong> /g/-leniti<strong>on</strong> in Spanish<br />

Jennifer Cole, José I. Hualde, Khalil Iskarous<br />

University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Illinois<br />

jscole@uiuc.edu | jihualde@uiuc.edu | iskarous@uiuc.edu<br />

This paper reports <strong>on</strong> an acoustic experiment to investigate <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>segmental</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

factors governing the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> intervocalic /g/ in Castilian Spanish.<br />

Significant variati<strong>on</strong> in the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /g/-spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, measured here in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> relative<br />

acoustic energy, is found to be c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed by stress <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> the quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the vowels<br />

adjacent to /g/. Spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is typically characterized as a process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> weakening or<br />

articulatory reducti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> the experimental findings are c<strong>on</strong>sidered in light <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the view<br />

that reducti<strong>on</strong> results from speech ec<strong>on</strong>omy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>/or gesture overlap. The observed effect<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stress <strong>on</strong> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with existing research that correlates gesture<br />

strength with the strength <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g> positi<strong>on</strong> in which the gesture occurs. The<br />

experimental results also indicate significant effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vowel quality <strong>on</strong> the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, which c<strong>on</strong>flict in part with the analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> as a general<br />

phenomen<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> intervocalic speech reducti<strong>on</strong>. The findings suggest that a complete<br />

account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the variati<strong>on</strong> in degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> must include c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

dynamics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the movement between specific c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vowel gestures.<br />

1. Introducti<strong>on</strong><br />

A widely-known feature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish is the leniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the voiced obstruents /b,d,g/ in intervocalic<br />

positi<strong>on</strong>, a process also referred to as spirantizati<strong>on</strong>. Ph<strong>on</strong>ological <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>etic discussi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong> in Spanish <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> other languages typically characterize it as a process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> weakening or<br />

articulatory reducti<strong>on</strong> (Browman & Goldstein 1991; Harris &Lindsey 1995:69; Kenstowicz<br />

1994:35; Kirchner 1998). Spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is sometimes also viewed as an assimilati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stricture (or<br />

s<strong>on</strong>ority) from adjacent vowels, especially in those cases where the spirant is flanked by vowels <strong>on</strong><br />

both sides (Ladefoged 1997:604; Lass:1984:181-3; Weismer 1997:210). Ph<strong>on</strong>ological analyses<br />

within the generative traditi<strong>on</strong> (eg., Goldsmith 1981; Harris 1969; Mascaró 1984, 1991) also treat<br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong> as a process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> assimilati<strong>on</strong>, in which the spirant acquires the [+c<strong>on</strong>tinuant] feature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the flanking vowels. Our goal in this paper is to gain a better underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

Spanish by investigating the factors that govern variati<strong>on</strong> in the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> as they<br />

relate to the mechanisms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech producti<strong>on</strong> that give rise to articulatory reducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

assimilati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

1


A number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> works <strong>on</strong> Spanish ph<strong>on</strong>etics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>ology have noted that spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

subject to a certain amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> variability within a given dialect or idiolect (Amastae 1986, 1989,<br />

1995; Zamora & Guitart 1982:102; Barrutia & Terrell 1982:61). C<strong>on</strong>sequently, spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is best<br />

characterized in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g>s that favor the c<strong>on</strong>tinuant or n<strong>on</strong>-c<strong>on</strong>tinuant realizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /b,d,g/,<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not as an obligatory, categorical ph<strong>on</strong>ological rule (Mascaro 1991). But there is another type <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

variability in spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that is variati<strong>on</strong> in the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong>. The c<strong>on</strong>tinuant<br />

alloph<strong>on</strong>es <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /b,d,g/ have been described by Quilis (1988: 221-224) as ranging from narrow<br />

fricative segments to approximants with vowel-like qualities, a descripti<strong>on</strong> which is based <strong>on</strong> visual<br />

inspecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spectograms <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> auditory impressi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The data in (1) present typical examples <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g>s where spirantizati<strong>on</strong> occurs. The<br />

voiced obstruents /b,d,g/ are realized as c<strong>on</strong>tinuants [B, D, V] respectively, in intervocalic positi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

both word-internally (1a) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> across word boundaries (1b).<br />

(1) a. sabe [sáBe] ‘s/he knows’<br />

lado [láDo] ‘side’<br />

lago [láVo] ‘lake’<br />

b. la bala [laBála] ‘the bullet’<br />

para dos [pa\aDós] ‘for two’<br />

mala gota [malaVóta] ‘bad drop’<br />

Acoustic analysis reveals that the c<strong>on</strong>tinuant alloph<strong>on</strong>es [B, D, V] have very little highfrequency<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-periodic energy, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> are thus better defined as approximants than as fricatives or<br />

spirants (Romero 1995). 1 Both in the present study an in our earlier pilot study <strong>on</strong> Spanish<br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, we find that there is c<strong>on</strong>siderable variati<strong>on</strong> in the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> overall energy c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

in the c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant regi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> intervocalic /b,d,g/, with tokens ranging from a complete stop to a vocalic<br />

glide. C<strong>on</strong>sider the spectogram in figure 1 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the phrase pide todo ‘ask for everything’. This phrase<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tains two instances <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /d/ in intervocalic positi<strong>on</strong>, each following a word-level stressed vowel.<br />

The /d/ in pide has the small amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> acoustic energy characteristic <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a voiced stop, while the /d/<br />

1 Although Quilis describes c<strong>on</strong>tinuant alloph<strong>on</strong>es that are narrow fricatives, we find very few tokens that<br />

fit that descripti<strong>on</strong> in our data.<br />

2


in todo has much more energy, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> is in fact difficult to distinguish visually from the regi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

flanking vowels. 2<br />

Figure 1: Variati<strong>on</strong> in the realizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> intervocalic /d/ in a single utterance.<br />

The qualitative aspect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> variati<strong>on</strong> in spirantizati<strong>on</strong> goes unreported in nearly all published<br />

work <strong>on</strong> Spanish ph<strong>on</strong>ology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>etics. The majority <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> investigators rely instead <strong>on</strong> a simple<br />

binary distincti<strong>on</strong> between c<strong>on</strong>tinuant <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>-c<strong>on</strong>tinuant alloph<strong>on</strong>es. 3<br />

2. Sources <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> variati<strong>on</strong><br />

Under the view that spirantizati<strong>on</strong> results from the mechanisms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech producti<strong>on</strong> that govern<br />

reducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> assimilati<strong>on</strong>, we are led to c<strong>on</strong>sider two possible sources for variati<strong>on</strong> in the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

2 This example, culled from our earlier pilot study, is shown here because it illustrates variati<strong>on</strong> in the<br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /d/ in the speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a single speaker, within a single utterance. Similar variati<strong>on</strong> can be<br />

seen for each speaker in the experiment reported here, by comparing /g/ tokens across stimuli; there is no<br />

single stimulus item that c<strong>on</strong>tains more than <strong>on</strong>e token <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /g/. In Figure 1, the segmentati<strong>on</strong> indicated by the<br />

spectrogram labeling is aided by auditory impressi<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in this case c<strong>on</strong>stitutes <strong>on</strong>ly a rough placement <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the transiti<strong>on</strong>s between /d/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> the adjacent vowels.<br />

3 An excepti<strong>on</strong> is Romero (1995), who argues for a finer distincti<strong>on</strong> than the traditi<strong>on</strong>al binary <strong>on</strong>e, since he<br />

distinguishes approximant <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fricative alloph<strong>on</strong>es. The latter are identified in realizati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequences <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the type /sb/ in certain Andalusian dialects.<br />

3


spirantizati<strong>on</strong>: <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>segmental</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Prosodic structure has been found in recent work to<br />

play a role in c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing the magnitude <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an articulatory gesture. <strong>Speech</strong> gestures have been<br />

shown to be affected by their positi<strong>on</strong> in the word <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phrase, relative to accent <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> initial <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> final<br />

edges. For example, Byrd (1996) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Browman & Goldstein (1992), am<strong>on</strong>g others, have shown<br />

that speech gestures in word-initial positi<strong>on</strong> differ in magnitude from gestures in medial <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> final<br />

positi<strong>on</strong>s. Similar findings obtain for gestures in phrase <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sentence initial positi<strong>on</strong> (Fouger<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Keating 1996, 1997). With respect to accent, Beckman & Edwards (1994), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Beckman, Edwards<br />

& Fletcher (1992) show that jaw <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lip movements are <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten larger <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> faster in syllables that bear<br />

word stress or nuclear accent than in unaccented or reduced syllables. Pierrehumbert & Talkin<br />

(1992) dem<strong>on</strong>strate similar results for laryngeal gestures, which are found to be <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> greater<br />

magnitude in positi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g> strength at the word <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phrase levels. In another case, deJ<strong>on</strong>g<br />

(1996) argues that flapping in English results from reducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant closing gesture in<br />

unstressed positi<strong>on</strong>s following the nuclear accent. These findings collectively indicate that<br />

articulatory gestures are greatest in domain-initial positi<strong>on</strong>s relative to n<strong>on</strong>-initial positi<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in<br />

positi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word-level stress <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phrasal accent relative to the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding unstressed or<br />

unaccented positi<strong>on</strong>s. If spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is correctly viewed as a reducti<strong>on</strong> in the magnitude <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong>, i.e., a type <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> articulatory reducti<strong>on</strong>, we then expect to find the greatest<br />

degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> in intervocalic positi<strong>on</strong>s that are <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally weak at the word or phrase<br />

level, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> less spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, ie., more stop-like realizati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /b,d,g/, in intervocalic positi<strong>on</strong>s that<br />

are <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally str<strong>on</strong>g.<br />

Segmental <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g> may also influence the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, under two hypotheses<br />

about speech producti<strong>on</strong>. First, the hypothesis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> articulatory ec<strong>on</strong>omy (Kohler 1990, 1991;<br />

Lindblom 1983, 1990) indicates that a c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant closing gesture will involve the minimal<br />

displacement possible from the articulatory c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> adjacent vowels, so l<strong>on</strong>g as the gesture<br />

still results in a perceptually salient c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the hypothesis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> gestural overlap<br />

(Browman & Goldstein 1989, 1990; Fowler & Saltzman, 1993) states that gestures for adjacent<br />

segments may overlap, especially in cases where the segments make c<strong>on</strong>flicting dem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <strong>on</strong> shared<br />

articulators. 4 Overlap gives rise to gesture blending, which can result in a perceived assimilati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

some cases, or gesture reducti<strong>on</strong> in others. One hypothesis regarding spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is that it results<br />

4 This discussi<strong>on</strong> summarizes Farnetani’s (1997) overview <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> other models <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> coarticulati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

4


from the opening or “s<strong>on</strong>orizati<strong>on</strong>” <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant closing gesture due to overlap with the more<br />

open gesture <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the vowel. 5<br />

The present study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish spirantizati<strong>on</strong> explores variati<strong>on</strong> in the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong><br />

through an acoustic analysis in which spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is quantified in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> relative acoustic energy<br />

in the c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant regi<strong>on</strong>. In this study we focus exclusively <strong>on</strong> intervocalic /g/, giving detailed<br />

c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> to the kinds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>segmental</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g> factors that can influence this particular<br />

c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant closing gesture. We relate our findings to an earlier study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ours <strong>on</strong> Spanish<br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong> that included /b/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /d/ in additi<strong>on</strong> to /g/, which did not however examine <str<strong>on</strong>g>segmental</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g> in detail. Given the limited scope <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the present study, our findings speak <strong>on</strong>ly to the<br />

mechanisms involved in velar spirantizati<strong>on</strong>; however, in future research we plan to extend the<br />

study to examine c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing factors for the dental <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> bilabial c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ants as well. The<br />

experiment described below looks for c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> three factors <strong>on</strong> the amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> energy<br />

in intervocalic /g/:<br />

• The positi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stress relative to /g/: ˈVgV vs. VˈgV<br />

• The quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the preceding vowel: igV, egV vs. ugV, ogV vs. agV<br />

• The quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the following vowel: Vgi, Vge vs. Vgu, Vgo vs. Vga<br />

3. Experimental Methods<br />

3.1 Materials<br />

The acoustic analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> intervocalic /b,d,g/ is based <strong>on</strong> speech data collected from three subjects, 1<br />

female <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2 males, all native speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Castilian Spanish. Each subject read a list <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 59 words<br />

c<strong>on</strong>taining intervocalic /g/, produced in a carrier phrase, in a normal c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>al style <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech<br />

rate. Instructi<strong>on</strong>s were given in Spanish by <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the experimenters, who also dem<strong>on</strong>strated the<br />

desired c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>al speaking style. The word list was balanced for the locati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stress: half <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the words had stress <strong>on</strong> the vowel immediately preceding /g/ (ˈVgV), while the other half had stress<br />

<strong>on</strong> the vowel immediately following /g/ (VˈgV). The words also varied in the quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the vowels<br />

preceding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> following /g/. Each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the vowels five /i,e,o,u,a/ occurs in positi<strong>on</strong>s both preceding<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> following /g/. The fr<strong>on</strong>t vowels /i,e/ were classed together in c<strong>on</strong>structing the stimuli set, as<br />

5 deJ<strong>on</strong>g (1995) discusses this hypothesis as an account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English flapping, but rejects it based <strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>flicting evidence from articulatory data.<br />

5


were the n<strong>on</strong>-low back vowels /u,o/. There were roughly equal numbers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words with the vowel<br />

preceding /g/ from each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the three vowel classes: /i,e/ (21), /u,o/ (21) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /a/ (17). Similarly, there<br />

were roughly equal numbers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words with the vowel following /g/ from the same classes: /i,e/ (18),<br />

/u,o/ (20), /a/ (21). Each word was repeated three times, for a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 177 tokens per speaker.<br />

3.2 Procedures<br />

Speakers were recorded directly <strong>on</strong>to a Sun Sparc10, using the Entropic ESPS/Waves+ recording<br />

facility. Recording sessi<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>ducted under quiet c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. <strong>Speech</strong> was sampled at 8kHz<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> segmentati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the target c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> flanking vowels was d<strong>on</strong>e using ESPS/Waves+,<br />

based <strong>on</strong> waveform, spectrogram <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> listening.<br />

The digitized speech segments were analyzed to produce an energy measurement for each<br />

target c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant. The energy measurement is a ratio <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the time-averaged energy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant<br />

(an RMS measure) to the time-averaged energy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the whole word. The ratio measure was d<strong>on</strong>e to<br />

normalize for differences in overall volume <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> individual utterances. 6<br />

4. Results<br />

The energy measurements obtained from the entire data set reveal significant variati<strong>on</strong> for each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the three speakers, illustrated in Figure 2 with a chart <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> energy values, collapsed over<br />

all c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, for each speaker. The findings from this experiment are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with our earlier<br />

pilot study <strong>on</strong> Spanish spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, where we also found significant variati<strong>on</strong> in the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong> for all three voiced obstruents, /b,d,g/, in the speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 5 speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Castilian<br />

Spanish.<br />

Three results are obtained from our analysis relevant to the factors that c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> variati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

the degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong>. First, comparing /g/ in words with stress <strong>on</strong> the preceding vowel against<br />

words with stress <strong>on</strong> the following vowel shows that for each speaker, the tokens <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /g/ in post-stress<br />

positi<strong>on</strong> have significantly more energy than /g/ in pre-stress positi<strong>on</strong> (Student’s t-test, two-tailed, p<br />

< .05), as seen in Figure 3. This finding shows that the producti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> intervocalic /g/ is influenced<br />

by <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Specifically, /g/ preceding a stressed vowel is str<strong>on</strong>ger, in the sense <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> having a<br />

6 Other normalizati<strong>on</strong> strategies were also evaluated, in which the c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant energy was normalized against<br />

the energy in the preceding vowel, the following vowel or the stressed vowel. The results were relatively<br />

stable across these various measures.<br />

6


more complete closure, than /g/ preceding an unstressed vowel. In other words, the presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<br />

following word-level stress inhibits spirantizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /g/. 7<br />

1<br />

/g/ Energy Variati<strong>on</strong><br />

0.9<br />

0.8<br />

0.7<br />

Relative Energy<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.3<br />

0.2<br />

0.1<br />

JH RM MP<br />

Figure 2: Range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> variati<strong>on</strong> in relative /g/ energy values by subject. Energy values calculated as a<br />

ratio <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> RMS energy in the /g/ interval over the time-averaged energy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the whole word. In this <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

subsequent boxplots, the box encloses 50% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the data around the median (from the lower quartile<br />

to the upper quartile), while the top <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> bottom brackets extending from the box show the maximum<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> minimum values, respectively. Outliers are shown as tick marks bey<strong>on</strong>d the value <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

maximum or minimum. The median is indicated by the line through the box.<br />

7 The word-level stress in these data was also the nuclear phrase-level stress in most <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the utterances.<br />

7


1<br />

JH<br />

1<br />

RM<br />

1<br />

MP<br />

0.9<br />

0.9<br />

0.9<br />

0.8<br />

0.8<br />

0.8<br />

0.7<br />

0.7<br />

0.7<br />

Relative Energy<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.3<br />

0.3<br />

0.3<br />

0.2<br />

0.2<br />

0.2<br />

0.1<br />

0.1<br />

0.1<br />

0<br />

0<br />

0<br />

V‘g gV‘<br />

V‘g gV‘<br />

V‘g gV‘<br />

p


amagar<br />

sugus<br />

5000<br />

0<br />

↓g<br />

5000<br />

0<br />

↓g<br />

−5000<br />

0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000<br />

−5000<br />

0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000<br />

5000<br />

0<br />

↓g<br />

5000<br />

0<br />

↓g<br />

−5000<br />

0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000<br />

−5000<br />

0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000<br />

5000<br />

5000 ↓g<br />

0<br />

↓g<br />

0<br />

−5000<br />

0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000<br />

−5000<br />

0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000<br />

Figure 4: Waveform comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /g/ in the <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /a/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /u/ vowels by speaker RM. The left<br />

three panels show [aVa] from three different tokens <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the word amagar. The right three panels<br />

show [uVu] from three different tokens <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the word sugus.<br />

The third speaker, MP, showed no significant difference in the energy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /g/ in low vowel<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g>s compared to other vowel <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g>s. Figure 5 shows the comparis<strong>on</strong> between /g/ in the<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /a/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /u,o/ vowels. Comparis<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /a/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g>s with /i,e/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g>s reveals no c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

patterns across speakers. The effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /a/, the maximally open vowel, in minimizing spirantizati<strong>on</strong><br />

is unexpected in light <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the view <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> as a process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> s<strong>on</strong>orizati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The third result obtained was that for all three speakers /g/ has significantly greater energy in<br />

the <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the n<strong>on</strong>-low back vowels /u,o/ than in the <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the fr<strong>on</strong>t vowels /i,e/ (Student’s<br />

t-test, two-tailed, p < .05), as shown in Figure 6. This effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /u,o/ cannot be attributed to their<br />

9


lesser height relative to /i,e/, because the lowest vowel <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> all, /a/, c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s the least energy in<br />

adjacent /g/.<br />

1<br />

JH<br />

1<br />

RM<br />

1<br />

MP<br />

0.9<br />

0.9<br />

0.9<br />

0.8<br />

0.8<br />

0.8<br />

0.7<br />

0.7<br />

0.7<br />

Relative Energy<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.3<br />

0.3<br />

0.3<br />

0.2<br />

0.2<br />

0.2<br />

0.1<br />

0.1<br />

0.1<br />

0<br />

0<br />

0<br />

/a/ /u,o/<br />

/a/ /u,o/<br />

/a/ /u,o/<br />

p


1<br />

JH<br />

1<br />

RM<br />

1<br />

MP<br />

0.9<br />

0.9<br />

0.9<br />

0.8<br />

0.8<br />

0.8<br />

0.7<br />

0.7<br />

0.7<br />

Relative Energy<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.3<br />

0.3<br />

0.3<br />

0.2<br />

0.2<br />

0.2<br />

0.1<br />

0.1<br />

0.1<br />

0<br />

/i,e/ /u,o/<br />

p


1<br />

JH<br />

1<br />

RM<br />

1<br />

MP<br />

0.9<br />

0.9<br />

0.9<br />

0.8<br />

0.8<br />

0.8<br />

0.7<br />

0.7<br />

0.7<br />

Relative Energy<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.6<br />

0.5<br />

0.4<br />

0.3<br />

0.3<br />

0.3<br />

0.2<br />

0.2<br />

0.2<br />

0.1<br />

0.1<br />

0.1<br />

0<br />

0<br />

0<br />

/i,e/ /u,o/<br />

/i,e/ /u,o/<br />

/i,e/ /u,o/<br />

p


two languages: in Spanish the <strong>on</strong>set c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant ([V] in our data) undergoes a much greater reducti<strong>on</strong><br />

than the following unstressed vowel, which is not subject to nearly the same degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reducti<strong>on</strong> as<br />

an unstressed vowel in English. The inhibitory effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> following stress <strong>on</strong> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is also<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Beckman & Edwards (1994) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Beckman, Edwards & Fletcher<br />

(1992) for jaw <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lip movements, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> with the results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Pierrehumbert & Talkin (1992) for<br />

laryngeal gestures. In all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> these cases, gestures are found to have greater magnitude in <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally<br />

str<strong>on</strong>g positi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., the <strong>on</strong>set to an accented/stressed syllable), with reduced gestures occurring in<br />

the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g>ally weak positi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., the <strong>on</strong>set to an unaccented/unstressed syllable).<br />

The c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stress is <strong>on</strong>e indicati<strong>on</strong> that spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is sensitive to <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

structure. Further evidence was found in our earlier pilot study, where we observed that<br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong> is largely inhibited in utterance-initial positi<strong>on</strong>. The stop-like realizati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /b,d,g/<br />

that we observed in utterance-initial positi<strong>on</strong> are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with traditi<strong>on</strong>al ph<strong>on</strong>ological<br />

descripti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> showed much less variati<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong> degree compared to<br />

intervocalic tokens. Our observati<strong>on</strong>s are also c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the findings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Fouger<strong>on</strong> & Keating<br />

(1996,1997) that the initial positi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a sentence or phrase c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s a greater magnitude <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

articulatory gestures. With respect to word-initial positi<strong>on</strong>, our earlier study did not provide<br />

evidence for the finding reported in Byrd (1996) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Browman & Goldstein (1992) that articulatory<br />

gestures are str<strong>on</strong>ger in this positi<strong>on</strong>, also. Rather, we found that spirantizati<strong>on</strong> has the same effect<br />

<strong>on</strong> intervocalic /b,d,g/ regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whether the VCV sequence is word internal or occurs across a<br />

word boundary, as in V#CV.<br />

Our data reveal two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vowels flanking /g/. Spirantizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /g/ is inhibited when it is flanked <strong>on</strong> both sides by the low vowel /a/, when it is flanked <strong>on</strong> both<br />

sides by a fr<strong>on</strong>t vowel /i,e/, or when it is followed by /i,e/. These inhibitory effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vowel quality<br />

<strong>on</strong> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> were an unexpected finding based <strong>on</strong> a view <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> as resulting either<br />

from ec<strong>on</strong>omy-driven articulatory reducti<strong>on</strong>, or from gestural overlap. The ec<strong>on</strong>omy hypothesis<br />

requires that the c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant closing gesture be executed with the minimal displacement from the<br />

adjacent vowels. Since the c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong> locati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /a/ is farthest am<strong>on</strong>g all vowels from the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong> locati<strong>on</strong> for /g/, we would expect that the closing gesture for /g/ will be effected less<br />

completely in the <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> flanking /a/ vowels. Our data reveal the opposite pattern. Similarly,<br />

since the movement between /i/ or /e/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a palatalized /g/ is greater than the movement between<br />

(n<strong>on</strong>-palatalized) /g/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /u/ or /o/, we expect <strong>on</strong> the basis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ec<strong>on</strong>omy that /g/ c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong> will be<br />

13


more complete in the <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /u,o/. Again, our results c<strong>on</strong>flict with this expectati<strong>on</strong>, showing<br />

more complete c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong> in the <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the fr<strong>on</strong>t vowels /i,e/ than the back vowels /u,o/. Our<br />

findings are in c<strong>on</strong>flict with the findings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lindblom, Pauli <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sundberg (1975), whose bite-block<br />

experiment shows that apical c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ants in VCV utterances are produced with a t<strong>on</strong>gue body<br />

c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong> requiring the least movement from the adjacent vowels. The vowel quality effects<br />

found in our data are similarly unexpected under the analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> gesture<br />

overlap. In particular, the overlapping <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /g/ with an adjacent /a/ is expected to result in the<br />

maximal reducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the /g/ closing gesture, c<strong>on</strong>trary to our findings. The open jaw positi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /a/<br />

is expected to exert the greatest opening effect <strong>on</strong> the /g/ closing gesture, compared to vowels<br />

produced with less open jaw positi<strong>on</strong>s. The gesture overlap analysis is perhaps more c<strong>on</strong>sistent with<br />

the inhibitory effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> flanking /i,e/ vowels <strong>on</strong> spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, relative to flanking /u,o/. There is<br />

complete sharing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the articulators involved in /g/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /u,o/ producti<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> less sharing with /g/<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /i,e/. Since gesture overlap increases as gestures are increasingly shared between adjacent<br />

targets, the expectati<strong>on</strong> is for greatest overlap, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> hence greatest spirantizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /g/ in the <str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>text</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> adjacent vowels with a dorso-velar c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The asymmetry between /u,o/ vowels <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, which c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> greater degrees <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /i,e/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /a/ vowels <strong>on</strong> the other h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, which c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> lesser degrees <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

spirantizati<strong>on</strong>, has a parallel in the articulatory dynamics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> VgV sequences. Movement between /g/<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /i,e/ or /a/ vowels involves a change in the overall positi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the t<strong>on</strong>gue body, while there is<br />

virtually no change in t<strong>on</strong>gue body positi<strong>on</strong> for movements between /g/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /u/ or /o/. We suggest<br />

that the effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> adjacent vowel quality <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong> degree for /g/ is a c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

dynamics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> t<strong>on</strong>gue body movement. Movement <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the t<strong>on</strong>gue body is necessary to produce a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant closing gesture, whether the movement is in the horiz<strong>on</strong>tal dimensi<strong>on</strong>, the vertical<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>, or both. When the t<strong>on</strong>gue body positi<strong>on</strong> is changed in the movement between /g/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an<br />

adjacent vowel, the closing gesture for /g/ is most fully realized. But when no overall change in the<br />

t<strong>on</strong>gue body positi<strong>on</strong> is required, as in the movement between /g/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adjacent /u/ or /o/, then the<br />

closing gesture for /g/ is not effectively realized. In support <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this view, we note that in a number<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> tokens from our data, it appears that the /g/ is produced as an unrounded velar glide when flanked<br />

by /u,o/ vowels, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> is barely or not at all distinguishable from the vowels based <strong>on</strong> auditory<br />

impressi<strong>on</strong>. This account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vowel quality as a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing factor for spirantizati<strong>on</strong> suggests that<br />

14


different vowel quality effects will be found for the dental <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> bilabial spirants due to differences in<br />

the dynamics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the VCV gestures involved. We plan to pursue this hypothesis in future research.<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

The authors gratefully acknowledge the support <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Illinois<br />

Research Board. Cole’s research is supported in part through NSF grant SBR-9319368.<br />

An earlier versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this paper benefited from the helpful guidance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Mary Beckman,<br />

John Coleman, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Janet Pierrehumbert, who bear no resp<strong>on</strong>sibility for the use to which<br />

we put their advise in the present work.<br />

References<br />

Amastae, J<strong>on</strong>. (1986). A syllable-based analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish spirantizati<strong>on</strong>. In Carmen<br />

Silva-Corvalán <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Osvaldo Jaeggli (eds.), Studies in Romance Linguistics (pp. 2-<br />

23). Dordrecht: Foris.<br />

Amastae, J<strong>on</strong>. (1989). The intersecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> s-aspirati<strong>on</strong>/deleti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> spirantizati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

H<strong>on</strong>duran Spanish. Language Variati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Change 1:169-183.<br />

Amastae, J<strong>on</strong>. (1995). Variable spirantizati<strong>on</strong>: c<strong>on</strong>straint weighting in three dialects.<br />

Hispanic Linguistics 6/7: 267-285.<br />

Barrutia, Richard <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tracy Terrell. (1982). F<strong>on</strong>ética y f<strong>on</strong>ología españolas. New York:<br />

Wiley.<br />

Beckman, Mary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Jan Edwards. (1994). Articulatory evidence for differentiating stress<br />

categories. In Patricia A. Keating (ed.), Ph<strong>on</strong>ological Structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ph<strong>on</strong>etic<br />

Form: Papers in Laboratory Ph<strong>on</strong>ology III (pp. 7-33). Cambridge: Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Beckman, Mary, Jan Edwards <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Janet Fletcher. (1992). Prosodic structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> tempo in<br />

a s<strong>on</strong>ority model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> articulatory dynamics. In Gerard J. Docherty <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> J. Robert<br />

Ladd (eds.), Papers in Laboratory Ph<strong>on</strong>ology II: Gesture, Segment, <strong>Prosody</strong> (pp.<br />

68-86). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Browman, Catherine <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Louis Goldstein. (1991). Gestural structures: distinctiveness,<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ological processes, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> historical change. In Ignatius G. Mattingly <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Michael Studdert-Kennedy (eds.), Modularity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Motor Theory <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Speech</strong><br />

Percepti<strong>on</strong>: Proceedings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a C<strong>on</strong>ference to H<strong>on</strong>or Alvin M. Liberman (pp. 313-<br />

338). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.<br />

15


Browman, Catherine <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Louis Goldstein. (1992). Articulatory ph<strong>on</strong>ology: an overview.<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>etica 49:155-80.<br />

Byrd, Dani. (1996). Influences <strong>on</strong> articulatory timing in c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant sequences. J. Ph<strong>on</strong>. 24:<br />

209-244.<br />

Cole, Jennifer, José I. Hualde <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Khalil Iskarous. (1997). Is Spanish spirantizati<strong>on</strong> a<br />

unitary process? Paper presented at LSRL 27, Irvine, CA, February 1997.<br />

DeJ<strong>on</strong>g, Kenneth. (1996). American English flapping revisited. Paper presented at the 5 th<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ference in Laboratory Ph<strong>on</strong>ology, Evanst<strong>on</strong>, Ill.<br />

Farnetani, Edda. (1997). Coarticulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>nected speech processes. In William J.<br />

Hardcastle <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> John Laver (eds.), The H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ph<strong>on</strong>etic Sciences (pp. 371-<br />

404). Oxford <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell.<br />

Fouger<strong>on</strong>, Cecile <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Patricia Keating. (1996). Variati<strong>on</strong>s in velic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lingual articulati<strong>on</strong><br />

depending <strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g> positi<strong>on</strong>s: Results for two French speakers. UCLA<br />

Working Papers in Ph<strong>on</strong>etics 92: 88-96.<br />

Fouger<strong>on</strong>, Cecile <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Patricia Keating. (1997). Articulatory strengthening at edges <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>prosodic</str<strong>on</strong>g> domains. JASA 101: 3728-40.<br />

Fowler, Carol <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Eliott Saltzman. (1993). Coordinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> coarticulati<strong>on</strong> in speech<br />

producti<strong>on</strong>. Language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Speech</strong>, 36: 171-95.<br />

Goldsmith, John. (1981). Sub<str<strong>on</strong>g>segmental</str<strong>on</strong>g>s in Spanish ph<strong>on</strong>ology. In William Cressey <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

D<strong>on</strong>na Jo Napoli (eds.), Linguistic Symposium <strong>on</strong> Romance Languages 9 (pp. 1-<br />

16). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Georgetown University Press.<br />

Harris, James. (1969). Spanish ph<strong>on</strong>ology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<br />

Harris, John <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ge<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f Lindsey. (1995). The elements <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>ological representati<strong>on</strong>. In<br />

Jacques Dur<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Francis Katamba (eds.), Fr<strong>on</strong>tiers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ph<strong>on</strong>ology: Atoms,<br />

Structures, Derivati<strong>on</strong>s (pp. 34-79). L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> New York: L<strong>on</strong>gman<br />

Hualde, José I. (1989). Procesos c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ánticos y estructuras geométricas en espan)ol.<br />

Lingu_ística (ALFAL) 1:7-44.<br />

Kenstowicz, Michael. (1994). Ph<strong>on</strong>ology in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, MA <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Oxford: Blackwell.<br />

Kirchner, Robert. (1998). An Effort-Based Approach to C<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant Leniti<strong>on</strong>. Doctoral<br />

dissertati<strong>on</strong>, UCLA.<br />

16


Kohler, K.J. (1990). Segmental reducti<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>nected speech in German: ph<strong>on</strong>ological<br />

facts <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>etic explanati<strong>on</strong>s. In William J. Hardcastle <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> A. Marchal (eds.),<br />

<strong>Speech</strong> Producti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Speech</strong> Modelling (pp. 69-92). Dordrecht: Kluwer.<br />

Kohler, K.J. (1991). The ph<strong>on</strong>etics/ph<strong>on</strong>ology issue in the study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> articulatory reducti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>etica 48: 180-192.<br />

Ladefoged, Peter. (1997). Linguistic ph<strong>on</strong>etic descripti<strong>on</strong>s. In William J. Hardcastle <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

John Laver (eds.), The H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ph<strong>on</strong>etic Sciences (pp. 589-618). Cambridge,<br />

MA <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Oxford: Blackwell.<br />

Lass, Roger. (1984). Ph<strong>on</strong>ology: An Introducti<strong>on</strong> to Basic C<strong>on</strong>cepts. Cambridge:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Lindblom, Bjorn. (1983). Ec<strong>on</strong>omy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech gestures. In Peter F. MacNeilage (ed.), The<br />

Producti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Speech</strong> (pp. 217-45). New York: Springer Verlag.<br />

Lindblom, Bjorn. (1990).Explaining ph<strong>on</strong>etic variati<strong>on</strong>: a sketch <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the H&H theory. In<br />

William J. Hardcastle <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Alan. Marchal (eds.), <strong>Speech</strong> Producti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Speech</strong><br />

Modeling (pp. 403-40). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.<br />

Lindblom, Bjorn, Pauli, S. <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> J. Sundberg. (1975). Modeling coarticuati<strong>on</strong> in apical<br />

stops. In G. Fant (ed.), <strong>Speech</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong>, Vol. 2, pp. 87-94. Uppsala:<br />

Almqvist <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Wiksell.<br />

Martínez Celdrán, Eugenio. (1991). Sobre la naturaleza f<strong>on</strong>étical de los alóf<strong>on</strong>os de<br />

/b,d,g/ en espan)ol y sus distintas denominaci<strong>on</strong>es. Verba 18:235-53.<br />

Mascaró, Joan. (1984). C<strong>on</strong>tinuant spreading in Basque, Catalan, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish. In Mark<br />

Ar<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> RichardT. Oehrle (eds.), Language Sound Structure (pp. 287-298).<br />

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<br />

Mascaró, Joan. (1991). Iberian spirantizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tinuant spreading. In Albert<br />

Branchadell et al (eds.), Catalan Working Papers in Linguistics 1991 (pp. 167-<br />

179). Barcel<strong>on</strong>a: Univ. Autònoma de Barcel<strong>on</strong>a.<br />

Pierrehumbert, Janet <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> David Talkin. (1992). Leniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> /h/ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> glottal stop. In Gerard<br />

J. Docherty <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> J. Robert Ladd (eds.), Papers in Laboratory Ph<strong>on</strong>ology II:<br />

Gesture, Segment, <strong>Prosody</strong> (pp. 90-117). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Quilis, Ant<strong>on</strong>io. (1988). F<strong>on</strong>ética acústica de la lengua española. Madrid: Gredos.<br />

17


Romero, Joaqun. (1995). Gestural Organizati<strong>on</strong> in Spanish: An Experimental Study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Spirantizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Aspirati<strong>on</strong>. Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> C<strong>on</strong>necticut.<br />

Weismer, Gary. (1997). Motor <strong>Speech</strong> Disorders. In William J. Hardcastle <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> John<br />

Laver (eds.), The H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ph<strong>on</strong>etic Sciences (pp. 191-219). Cambridge, MA<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Oxford: Blackwell.<br />

Zamora, Munné, Juan M. Guitart, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Jorge M. Guitart. (1982). Dialectología<br />

hispanoamericana. Almar: Salamanca.<br />

18

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!