23.04.2015 Views

Factors Influencing Young Children's use of Motives ... - School Sites

Factors Influencing Young Children's use of Motives ... - School Sites

Factors Influencing Young Children's use of Motives ... - School Sites

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Factors</strong> <strong>Influencing</strong> <strong>Young</strong><br />

Children’s <strong>use</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Motives</strong><br />

and Outcomes as Moral<br />

Criteria.<br />

Nelson<br />

(1980)


Morality<br />

• It implies the ability to:<br />

– Distinguish from right and wrong<br />

– Act on distinction<br />

– Experience pride in virtuous act and shame<br />

and guilt over acts that violates one’s moral<br />

standards


Bell Ringer<br />

• When did you start to make moral<br />

judgments ‘good’ & ‘bad’? Age 0-1? 1-4? 5-<br />

7? 8-11?<br />

• Have your views <strong>of</strong> ‘good’ and ‘bad’<br />

changed as you’ve grown older? How?<br />

When? What?<br />

• Where do your views come from?


BACKGROUND<br />

• Jean Piaget<br />

• Morality develops gradually during<br />

childhood<br />

• Under 10 – no consistent evidence for<br />

motive as basis for judgment good/bad,<br />

only outcome<br />

• Above 10 – judgments based on motive<br />

• Stages <strong>of</strong> Development


• Piaget outlined four stages <strong>of</strong> cognitive<br />

development, and gave approximate ages<br />

at which children reached those stages. He<br />

stressed, though, that these ages are only<br />

averages; individual children might go<br />

through the stages at a different speed but<br />

they would always go through the stages in<br />

the same order.


Bell Ringer<br />

• You are each going to receive the same two<br />

moral dilemmas. In your group, you will<br />

have ten minutes on each dilemma to<br />

present your final answer and your groups<br />

reasoning for picking the response you<br />

did. As a class we will do our best to come<br />

to a final conclusion <strong>of</strong> our choice.


Moral Orientation<br />

• heteronomous – ‘subject to another’s<br />

laws/rules’<br />

• autonomous – ‘subject to one’s own<br />

laws/rules’<br />

– Happens at about 7 years old


AIM<br />

• To demonstrate that the children as young<br />

as 3 years old <strong>use</strong> motives and outcomes<br />

as relevant criteria for moral judgments


Hypothesis<br />

• Children as young as 3 years old <strong>use</strong> both<br />

motive and outcome in making moral<br />

judgments if the mode <strong>of</strong> presentation<br />

makes these moral criteria as salient and<br />

keeps them available at the time <strong>of</strong><br />

judgment


Study 1<br />

• Sample:<br />

– 60 pre-school children<br />

– 30 boys and 30 girls<br />

– Age range: 3-4 years old<br />

– Mean age: 3.4 years<br />

– 30 second-grade children<br />

– 15 boys and 15 girls<br />

– Age range: 6-8<br />

– Mean age: 7.4<br />

– From White,<br />

Middle Class,<br />

Urban Area<br />

– Participated with<br />

parental consent


Variables<br />

• INDEPENDENT VARIABLES:<br />

– Motive (Good or Bad)<br />

– Outcome (Good or Bad)<br />

– Type <strong>of</strong> story presentation (Verbal only,<br />

Verbal + implied pictorial, or Verbal + explicit<br />

pictorial)<br />

• DEPENDENT VARIABLES:<br />

– Rating <strong>of</strong> the character


Method<br />

- 4 stories<br />

- In each story, a boy acting with good<br />

or bad motive throws a ball toward a<br />

friend, resulting in a good or bad outcome


Bell Ringer<br />

• Each group will list and explain Piaget’s<br />

stages <strong>of</strong> Moral Development. Do your<br />

best to recall each <strong>of</strong> the stages, the ages<br />

they occur, and a description <strong>of</strong> what is<br />

occurring at each stage. No looking at<br />

notes, so clear your desk! Winning group<br />

gets 110% on the assignment! You have 10<br />

minutes!


Method<br />

1. Children hear story with good<br />

motive and good outcome<br />

2. Children hear story with good motive and<br />

bad outcome<br />

3. Children hear story with bad motive and<br />

bad outcome<br />

4. Children hear story with bad<br />

motive and good outcome


MOTIVE STATEMENTS:<br />

(1) Good Motive:<br />

This boy was playing with a ball; his friend<br />

did not have anything to play with. He<br />

wanted to give the ball to his friend so they<br />

could play catch together with the ball.<br />

(2) Bad Motive:<br />

This boy was playing with a ball; he was<br />

very mad at his friend that day. He wanted<br />

to throw the ball at his friend so he could hit<br />

him on purpose.


OUTCOME STATEMENTS:<br />

(1)Good Outcome:<br />

The boy threw the ball. His friend<br />

caught the ball and was happy to play<br />

with it.<br />

(2) Bad Outcome:<br />

The boy threw the ball. His friend did not<br />

catch the ball; the ball hit his friend on<br />

the head and made him cry.


STORY EXAMPLE:<br />

THIS BOY WAS PLAYING WITH A<br />

BALL; HIS FRIEND DID NOT HAVE<br />

ANYTHING TO PLAY WITH. HE<br />

WANTED TO THROW THE BALL SO HE<br />

AND HIS FRIEND COULD PLAY CATCH<br />

TOGETHER WITH THE BALL. HE<br />

THREW THE BALL. HIS FRIEND DID<br />

NOT CATCH THE BALL; THE BALL HIT<br />

HIS FRIEND ON THE HEAD AND<br />

MADE HIM CRY.


Pictures:<br />

- 2 sets <strong>of</strong> black and white line<br />

drawings<br />

- drawings illustrating the motive<br />

- First set: motives are implied by the<br />

facial expressions. (implied pictorial<br />

presentation)<br />

- Second set: motives are conveyed<br />

explicitly by presenting thought bubbles<br />

(explicit pictorial presentation)


Experimental Design


Procedure<br />

• Children were told to listen very carefully to<br />

the stories beca<strong>use</strong> later they would have to<br />

tell them aloud.<br />

• In both picture-presentation conditions, the<br />

drawings were introduced one by one the<br />

appropriate points <strong>of</strong> the story.<br />

• After each story, children were asked<br />

whether the little boy in the story was a good<br />

boy or a bad boy, or “just okay.”<br />

• Pictures were remained there till the child<br />

made his judgment.


Procedure<br />

• Then they were asked to indicate how good<br />

or how bad the little boy was by pointing to<br />

one <strong>of</strong> the faces on a seven-point rating<br />

scale.<br />

• 1 = very bad, 7 = very good<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7<br />

• After the judgment, drawings were removed<br />

• Children were asked to tell the story aloud.


Results<br />

• By condition<br />

– Motive made little difference<br />

– Outcome had a greater effect on moral<br />

judgements in the ‘picture-explicit motive’<br />

condition<br />

– Outcome information was <strong>use</strong>d more (i.e.<br />

made more difference to judgements) in ‘bad<br />

motive’ stories in the two picture conditions<br />

than verbal only


Results<br />

Nelson's results are complex and difficult to understand at times.<br />

Here are a few key overall findings from across both experiments:<br />

• “…making a moral judgment requires understanding <strong>of</strong> the<br />

evaluative concepts (i.e. ‘good’,<br />

‘bad’) to be applied.”<br />

• “Also required is comprehension <strong>of</strong> the motives or goals involved<br />

(e.g. ‘sharing,’ ‘helping,’<br />

‘hurting.’ ”<br />

• ‘bad’ is more important to younger children (it has a greater effect<br />

on ratings) than 'good'<br />

• young children make more recall errors on motives than on<br />

outcomes<br />

• children develop the concept <strong>of</strong> bad before good (initially ‘good’<br />

= no ‘badness’) so <strong>use</strong> the<br />

first negative cue (whether outcome or motive)


Discussion<br />

• It could be possible that children learn the<br />

concept <strong>of</strong> bad before the concept <strong>of</strong> good<br />

(Piaget, 1932)<br />

• It could be possible that young children<br />

define the concept <strong>of</strong> good as the absence<br />

<strong>of</strong> bad, e.g. “being good is not lying” (Hill<br />

& Hill, 1977).


STUDY 2: BACKGROUND<br />

• In study 1, one negative cue (motive) was<br />

sufficient to produce negative judgment.<br />

• 3-yrs olds’ judgment in bad motive<br />

stories were affected by motive but not<br />

by outcome<br />

• Was that beca<strong>use</strong> motive was presented<br />

first?<br />

• Study 2: reverse the order, i.e. present<br />

outcome first then motive


STUDY 2: METHODOLOGY<br />

• Sample:<br />

– 27 preschool boys and girls<br />

– Mean age: 3.8 years<br />

• Material:<br />

– Same as before<br />

– In all stories, outcome preceded motive<br />

• Procedure:<br />

– Same as before


STUDY 2: RESULTS<br />

• As in study 1:<br />

– When one cue is negative, the other cue has<br />

less effect<br />

– Children made more recall errors when cues<br />

were inconsistent (more likely to give benefit<br />

<strong>of</strong> doubt when motive was good)<br />

• As predicted:<br />

– Judgements in Verbal condition were less<br />

affected by motive than picture conditions


Big Issue - Use <strong>of</strong> Children in<br />

Psychological Research<br />

Advantages<br />

• Studying children gives more<br />

<strong>of</strong> an insight into the<br />

nature/nurture debate, as<br />

young children have had<br />

less environmental<br />

exposure (less nurture).<br />

• Children are likely to be less<br />

affected by some biases (such<br />

as social desirability bias).<br />

Their responses may therefore<br />

be more honest and valid.<br />

Disadvantages<br />

• Children are more likely to<br />

respond to researcher’s cues<br />

beca<strong>use</strong> they are more uncertain<br />

<strong>of</strong> themselves. Risk <strong>of</strong> demand<br />

characteristics.<br />

• Clear ethical issues as the child<br />

themselves has not<br />

given consentto take part, and<br />

may not understand that they have<br />

a right to withdraw.<br />

• <strong>Young</strong> children may not<br />

understand or co-operate with the<br />

experiment (e.g. Langlois<br />

removed infants for 'fussing')


Reflective

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!