10.07.2015 Views

Language and life history: A new perspective on the development ...

Language and life history: A new perspective on the development ...

Language and life history: A new perspective on the development ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>primates, infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lactati<strong>on</strong> end with erupti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong>first permanent molars (Smith & Tompkins 1995). Inhumans, by c<strong>on</strong>trast, <strong>the</strong>re is an interval of about threeyears between weaning – breast-feeding usually is disc<strong>on</strong>tinuedat 30 to 36 m<strong>on</strong>ths, according to ethnographicobservati<strong>on</strong>s in traditi<strong>on</strong>al societies <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> historical accountsfrom Europe <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> America – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> erupti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> first permanentmolars, an event that usually takes place at 5 to 6years of age.An important factor in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human infancywas bipedalism, which realigned <strong>the</strong> spine <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> narrowed<strong>the</strong> pelvis (Leutenegger 1974; 1980; McHenry 1975).This change created an unfavorable ratio between <strong>the</strong>smaller maternal birth canal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> large fetal head(Leutenegger 1974) – <strong>the</strong> brain of modern human ne<strong>on</strong>atesis larger than <strong>the</strong> brains of o<strong>the</strong>r primates, even though itachieves a smaller percentage of its total growth at birth(Harvey & Clutt<strong>on</strong>-Brock 1985; Lindburg 1982) – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>this produced what Washburn (1960) called an “obstetricaldilemma.” 2 This dilemma was eased when some amount ofskull <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> brain growth – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> motor <strong>development</strong> – wereadaptively deferred into <strong>the</strong> postnatal period, increasinginfant dependency <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> need of postnatal care. In anumber of important respects, <strong>the</strong>se c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s – indanger of being seen as design flaws – offered more <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>better opportunities for social, vocal, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexical learning(Bjorklund 1997; Locke 1993; 1999).This tendency to view helplessness as a socially <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitivelybeneficial trait is supported by anthropologicalaccounts, which indicate that most hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rermo<strong>the</strong>rs rarely put <strong>the</strong>ir babies down, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>n do so forno more than a few sec<strong>on</strong>ds, usually remaining within ameter (Draper 1976; Hill & Hurtado 1996; K<strong>on</strong>ner1976; Lee 1979a; LeVine 1980). Separati<strong>on</strong> cries usuallyevoke pick-up <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> breast-feeding (LeVine & LeVine1966). When infants cannot be carried, <strong>the</strong>y are oftenleft in <strong>the</strong> care of o<strong>the</strong>rs (Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es 1993; Estioko-Griffin 1986; Hawkes et al. 1997; Hewlett 1991). In mosto<strong>the</strong>r primate species, infants are equipped to cling to<strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’s body hair, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> are less likely to be caredfor by o<strong>the</strong>r members of <strong>the</strong> social group. Thus, although<strong>the</strong> durati<strong>on</strong> of infancy is not l<strong>on</strong>ger in humans thanin some of <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r primates, its quality is higherbecause <strong>the</strong> greater h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ling required by <strong>the</strong> humaninfant produces more intense social stimulati<strong>on</strong> during aperiod in which <strong>the</strong> brain grows at a compensatorilyrapid rate.The primacy of brain growth over o<strong>the</strong>r body systems isillustrated in Figure 2. In <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>born, 87% of restingmetabolic rate (RMR) is devoted to brain growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>functi<strong>on</strong>. This has <strong>the</strong> effect of co-opting, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thusprol<strong>on</strong>ging, <strong>the</strong> growth of o<strong>the</strong>r systems. By <strong>the</strong> age offive years, <strong>the</strong> RMR devoted to <strong>the</strong> brain is still high at44%, whereas in <strong>the</strong> adult <strong>the</strong> RMR figure is 20 to 25%.At comparable stages of <strong>development</strong>, <strong>the</strong> RMR devotedto <strong>the</strong> relatively large chimpanzee brain is about 45, 20,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 9% for a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>born, a five-year-old, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an adult(Le<strong>on</strong>ard & Roberts<strong>on</strong> 1992).Stages in <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of language corresp<strong>on</strong>d to<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages, to a degree, but language researchhas not been carried out within a <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> framework.Thus, our characterizati<strong>on</strong> of language in each of<strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>togenetic stages below is based <strong>on</strong> pre-existingevidence.Figure 2. Growth curves for different body tissues (from Bogin1999b). The Brain curve is for total weight of <strong>the</strong> brain (Cabanaet al. 1993). The Dentiti<strong>on</strong> curve is <strong>the</strong> median maturity score forgirls based <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> seven left m<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ibular teeth (I1, I2, C, PM1,PM2, M1, M2) using <strong>the</strong> reference data of Demirjian (1986).The Body curve represents growth in stature or total bodyweight, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Reproductive curve represents <strong>the</strong> weight of<strong>the</strong> g<strong>on</strong>ads <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> primary reproductive organs (after Scamm<strong>on</strong>1930).2.2. Infant languageThe basic structure of language is laid down in infancy,which is paradoxical since <strong>the</strong> word derives from <strong>the</strong>Latin infans, literally “not speaking.” In <strong>the</strong> first phase of<strong>development</strong>, vocal learning, infants learn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> may reproduceaspects of <strong>the</strong> prosodic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> segmental characteristicsof <strong>the</strong> ambient language. This phase commences beforebirth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tinues in infancy (Locke 1997; Locke &Snow 1997). By six m<strong>on</strong>ths, most infants have heardenough speech to recognize a few words <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> stereotypedphrases (cf. Locke 1993), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to experience some perceptualreorganizati<strong>on</strong> (Kuhl et al. 1992), a process thatc<strong>on</strong>tinues in succeeding m<strong>on</strong>ths (cf. Werker & Curtin2005). At seven m<strong>on</strong>ths, infants typically begin to makealternating jaw movements while ph<strong>on</strong>ating (MacNeilage1998), an act that produces well-formed syllables(Koopmans-van Beinum & van der Stelt 1986; Oller2000). An apparently universal ph<strong>on</strong>etic repertoirebegins to emerge at this time (Locke 1983; 1990; 2000a).Four m<strong>on</strong>ths later, infants utter sounds in ways thatreflect prior exposure to speech (Vihman 1996).Around <strong>the</strong> end of <strong>the</strong> first year, most infants attempt tospeak, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> may even – <strong>the</strong>n or in succeeding m<strong>on</strong>ths –improvise sound-meaning relati<strong>on</strong>ships that may betreated by <strong>the</strong> family as invented words (Leopold 1949).A few m<strong>on</strong>ths later, infants develop a suite of cognitivetraits that will enable language to be used at a basal levelof creativity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> efficiency. These center <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> ability toinfer, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> dispositi<strong>on</strong> to share, <strong>the</strong> intenti<strong>on</strong>s ofo<strong>the</strong>rs (Tomasello et al. 2005). Around this time, severaldifferent communicative acts begin to emerge, withmany in frequent use by age three or four years (Snowet al. 1996; We<strong>the</strong>rby et al. 1988; also see <strong>the</strong> review inAdams 2002).Infants usually begin to combine lexical items at age 18to 24 m<strong>on</strong>ths. In <strong>the</strong> following year, having learnedsomething about grammatical structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rules,infants display <strong>the</strong> ability to underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> computenovel forms <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequences of words. In <strong>the</strong> typicalEnglish-learning infant, <strong>the</strong>refore, all areas of linguisticstructure – ph<strong>on</strong>ological, lexical, morphological, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>syntactic – exhibit some degree of <strong>development</strong> by ageBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 261


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>36 m<strong>on</strong>ths (Locke 1993; 1997). Thus, by <strong>the</strong> time infancydraws to a close, <strong>the</strong> rudiments of a structural linguisticsystem, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> basic comp<strong>on</strong>ents of a functi<strong>on</strong>al communicativesystem, are operative.2.3. Stage 2: ChildhoodThe childhood stage is peculiar to humans, having beenevoluti<strong>on</strong>arily inserted between <strong>the</strong> infant <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juvenilestages that characterize social mammals (Bogin 1990).Childhood is defined by several <strong>development</strong>al characteristics,for example, a slowing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> stabilizati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> rate ofgrowth; immature dentiti<strong>on</strong>; feeding characteristics, suchas dependence <strong>on</strong> older people for food; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavioralcharacteristics, including immature motor c<strong>on</strong>trol. Theevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary value of childhood lies in <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’sfreedom to disc<strong>on</strong>tinue nursing her three-year-old, whichenabled her to initiate a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> pregnancy. Doing so enhancedreproductive output without increasing <strong>the</strong> risk of mortalityfor <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r, or her infant or older children, for in cooperativelybreeding societies o<strong>the</strong>rs were available to helpcare for <strong>the</strong> young.As shown in Figure 1, <strong>the</strong> end of <strong>the</strong> rapid growth decelerati<strong>on</strong>of infancy marks <strong>the</strong> beginning of childhood, when<strong>the</strong> growth rate levels off at about 5 cm per year. Stabilizati<strong>on</strong>of growth rate is rare in mammals; typically, growthc<strong>on</strong>tinues to decelerate between infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adulthood(Brody 1945; Tanner 1962). In childhood, brain growthtapers off, at least by weight (Fig. 2), with its masspeaking at about age seven years (Cabana et al. 1993),with a small increase occurring later at puberty (Durst<strong>on</strong>et al. 2001; Sowell et al. 2001). Myelinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> somenerve pro<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>rati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinue into adulthood (Bjorklund &Pellegrinni 2002; Taupin & Gage 2002).In virtually all o<strong>the</strong>r mammalian species, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in all n<strong>on</strong>humanprimates, maternal lactati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinues until <strong>the</strong>erupti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> first permanent molar (Smith & Tompkins1995). With adult molars <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r teeth in place, <strong>the</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r primates move <strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> juvenile stage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mustforage for food <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>rwise care for <strong>the</strong>mselves withlittle or no assistance. In <strong>the</strong> human infant, of course,o<strong>the</strong>r foods are supplied prior to weaning. But <strong>the</strong> cessati<strong>on</strong>of lactati<strong>on</strong> places a great nutriti<strong>on</strong>al burden <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>weaned infant, with <strong>the</strong> high metabolic dem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s of abrain that is still growing rapidly. The typical three-yearoldis too motorically immature to forage or prepare itsown food, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> too limited by deciduous dentiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> asmall gastrointestinal tract to eat <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> digest <strong>the</strong> adult diet.The soluti<strong>on</strong> to this “feeding crisis” is for older membersof <strong>the</strong> social group to provide specially prepared foods thatare high in energy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> nutrients until self-care becomespossible, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in various hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rer societies this iswhat is d<strong>on</strong>e (Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es 1993; Estioko-Griffin 1986;Hewlett 1991). 3 Summarizing <strong>the</strong> data from manyhuman societies, Lancaster <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lancaster (1983) calledthis type of childcare <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> feeding “<strong>the</strong> hominid adaptati<strong>on</strong>,”for no o<strong>the</strong>r primate or mammal is so activelyinvolved in <strong>the</strong>se ways. The l<strong>on</strong>g period of food provisi<strong>on</strong>ing,extending from ages three to seven years, largely defines<strong>the</strong> childhood stage of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>. By age seven,usually, <strong>the</strong> four permanent first molars have erupted<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> permanent incisors have begun to replace “milk”incisors. With parallel changes in <strong>the</strong> size <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> strengthof <strong>the</strong> jaws, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in <strong>the</strong> muscles of masticati<strong>on</strong>,children – <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> threshold of juvenility – become ableto eat <strong>the</strong> same foods as adults.During this period, mo<strong>the</strong>rs often participate in childhoodfeeding if <strong>the</strong>y are nei<strong>the</strong>r pregnant nor nursing a<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> infant, but <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> reproductive efforts are likely toc<strong>on</strong>sume most of <strong>the</strong>ir free energy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> time. This is <strong>the</strong>case for all o<strong>the</strong>r mammals, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> it is <strong>the</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> why <strong>the</strong>birth interval between successful pregnancies is equal to<strong>the</strong> age of first molar erupti<strong>on</strong> in species without <strong>the</strong>human style of social care after weaning. For humans,early weaning (by age three years) relative to molar erupti<strong>on</strong>(age six years) reduces <strong>the</strong> birth interval <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> allows<strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r to successfully produce two offspring in <strong>the</strong>time it takes chimpanzees <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> orangutans to successfullyrear <strong>on</strong>e. Thus, we see childhood as an adaptati<strong>on</strong> thatenhances <strong>the</strong> quantity of maternal reproductive outputwithout sacrificing <strong>the</strong> quality of any offspring. Wediscuss <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r aspects of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary basis forchildhood in more detail later.On <strong>the</strong> social fr<strong>on</strong>t, childhood typically produces <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>extrafamilial friendships, frequently co-timed withgrowing disenchantment with siblings (Dunn 1996). Insome modern societies, social hierarchies have alreadydeveloped by <strong>the</strong> age of five. In <strong>the</strong>se hierarchies, lowstatuschildren direct positive behaviors to <strong>the</strong>ir higherstatus peers, evidently without reciprocati<strong>on</strong> (Strayer &Trudel 1984). Children with good communicati<strong>on</strong> skillsare likely to be particularly popular (Asher & Renshaw1981; Gottman et al. 1975; Putallaz & Gottman 1981),whereas children with speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language disorderstend to be unpopular <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> l<strong>on</strong>ely, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> may even be victimized(Blood & Blood 2004; C<strong>on</strong>ti-Ramsden & Botting2004; Davis et al. 2002). Children with pragmatic orhigher-level language processing disorders are at risk ofserious peer interacti<strong>on</strong> problems (Botting & C<strong>on</strong>ti-Ramsden 2000), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> similar problems may arise in laterstages of <strong>development</strong> (Place & Becker 1991). Significantly,patterns of friendship <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rejecti<strong>on</strong> in childhoodset up modes of relating that frequently persist throughsucceeding stages into adulthood (Kohlberg et al. 1972).In what has come to be known as <strong>the</strong> “five to seven yearshift” (Sameroff & Haith 1996), <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavioralcapabilities emerge, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se enable greatersocial independence (Bogin 1999a). Walking becomesadult-like by age seven years (Bogin 1999b). Cognitive<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> emoti<strong>on</strong>al <strong>development</strong>s permit <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> levels of selfsufficiency(Tomasello & Call 1997). Seven-year-olds canperform many basic tasks, including food preparati<strong>on</strong>,infant care, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r domestic tasks, with little or nosupervisi<strong>on</strong> (Rogoff 1996; Weisner 1996). The n<strong>on</strong>linguistictraits associated with human childhood <strong>the</strong>reforeinclude:1. slow <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> steady rate of body growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> relativelysmall body size2. large, fast-growing brain3. higher resting metabolic rate than any o<strong>the</strong>r mammalianspecies4. immature dentiti<strong>on</strong>5. dependence <strong>on</strong> older people for care <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> feeding6. motor <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive advances.No o<strong>the</strong>r mammalian species has this entire suite offeatures, or a <strong>development</strong>al period that is comparableto childhood.262 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>2.4. Child languageSome structural <strong>development</strong>s that begin in infancy c<strong>on</strong>tinueinto childhood, with c<strong>on</strong>sequences that affectspeech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language. One qualitative change involves<strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship between pharynx height <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> oral cavitylength, which changes from birth until six to eight years,when it reaches, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> stabilizes at, <strong>the</strong> (1:1) ratio thatpermits adult vowel producti<strong>on</strong> (Fitch & Giedd 1999;Lieberman et al. 2001). A sec<strong>on</strong>d, quantitative change isa decrease in <strong>the</strong> rate of brain growth by weight. Duringchildhood, <strong>the</strong>re are c<strong>on</strong>tinuing improvements in ph<strong>on</strong>ology,vocabulary, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sentence length (cf. Berko Gleas<strong>on</strong>2001), as well as syntax (Clark et al. 1986; Karmiloff-Smith 1985).As childhood draws to an end, a disc<strong>on</strong>tinuity in nativelanguage learning occurs. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>s acquired after <strong>the</strong>age of six are often produced with an accent that reflectsinterference from previously learned languages (Asher &Garcia 1969; Flege & Fletcher 1992). In large epidemiologicalsamples of Midwestern American children, sixyears of age was also treated as <strong>the</strong> approximate age ofnative language mastery, based <strong>on</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardized tests thatare oriented to school performance (Shriberg et al. 1999;Tomblin et al. 1996; 1997). 4 Thus, in <strong>on</strong>e modern societymuch of <strong>the</strong> childhood period is deemed necessary tomaster <strong>the</strong> basic structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> elementary vocabulary ofa knowledge-based linguistic system. We will see laterthat if language is delayed at this age, <strong>the</strong>re are likely tobe c<strong>on</strong>tinuing problems in juvenility, adolescence, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>adulthood, though <strong>the</strong>se may have less to do with linguisticknowledge than later emerging capabilities in <strong>the</strong> area ofvocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal expressi<strong>on</strong>.But <strong>the</strong>re is more to be accomplished in childhood. Oneadvance involves verbal fluency, which c<strong>on</strong>tinues toimprove throughout this stage (Starkwea<strong>the</strong>r 1987).Ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>development</strong> relates to automaticity. If somethinggoes wr<strong>on</strong>g with <strong>the</strong> sensory system that guides ambientlearning during childhood, <strong>the</strong>re is likely to be significantdeteriorati<strong>on</strong> of speech. Clinical studies indicate that all ofchildhood is needed to achieve a speaking ability that cantolerate <strong>the</strong> disc<strong>on</strong>tinued stimulati<strong>on</strong> entailed by acquireddeafness (Binnie et al. 1982; Cowie & Douglas-Cowie1983; Plant 1984; Waldstein 1990). Deafness in later <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>stages poses less of a threat to speech.Some of <strong>the</strong> communicative skills arising in childhooddo so in t<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>em with certain cognitive advances. Onesuch <strong>development</strong> is <strong>the</strong> “<strong>the</strong>ory of o<strong>the</strong>r minds,” whichtypically emerges between ages two <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> four years(Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen et al. 1993), enabling children to take <strong>the</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> of o<strong>the</strong>rs. Ano<strong>the</strong>r is an improvement in autobiographicalmemory, which usually occurs between agesthree <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> eight years (Nels<strong>on</strong> 1996), allowing childrento describe sequential events <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to share memories of<strong>the</strong>ir own experience. O<strong>the</strong>r skills that improve duringchildhood include discourse <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> narrati<strong>on</strong> (Girolamettoet al. 2001).Some of <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>s that occur in childhoodrelate to verbal competiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance. Theseinclude joking (McGhee 1979; Shultz & Horibe 1974)<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> use of preassembled verbal routines (Gleas<strong>on</strong> &Weintraub 1976). In many cultures, children play verbalgames with peers using material learned from familymembers earlier in infancy, or from unrelated children<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juveniles later <strong>on</strong>. These games mark <strong>the</strong> beginningof various sorts of verbal competiti<strong>on</strong> (Gossen 1976a;1976b; Sanches & Kirshenblatt-Gimblett 1976). Theyalso point to a dispositi<strong>on</strong> toward verbal creativity that,we will suggest, may have played a key role in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>of language (see sect. 3.4).During childhood, <strong>the</strong>re is a trend for males to speakassertively to get <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> maintain attenti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to makeevident <strong>the</strong>ir desires, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> for girls to speak softly inorder to promote interpers<strong>on</strong>al closeness <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> harm<strong>on</strong>y(Austin et al. 1987; Cook et al. 1985; Miller et al. 1986;Sachs 1987). The linguistic acquisiti<strong>on</strong>s of infancy arethus joined by factors in childhood, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> succeedingstages, that influence mode of expressi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitate<strong>development</strong> of vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal skills (Dundes et al.1970; Gossen 1976b; Sherzer 2002).Since it begins with weaning, childhood would also haveliberated <strong>the</strong> young from c<strong>on</strong>tinuous maternal restraint.The freedom of irresp<strong>on</strong>sible progeny to range unsupervisedover greater distances elevated <strong>the</strong> need forparents to warn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> instruct, giving <strong>the</strong>m <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r kinselfish reas<strong>on</strong>s to send h<strong>on</strong>est signals (see sect. 3.5). Butchildhood also put <strong>the</strong> young in a positi<strong>on</strong>, for <strong>the</strong> firsttime, to know about, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thus to c<strong>on</strong>vey informati<strong>on</strong>about, events occurring in <strong>the</strong> absence of o<strong>the</strong>rs. Therefore,we also suggest that childhood h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ed children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>ir extended families a key ingredient of human language– displacement – in <strong>the</strong> form of <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> opportunities,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> needs, to talk about things not immediately present(Hockett 1977).O<strong>the</strong>r advances in maturati<strong>on</strong>, learning, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>solidati<strong>on</strong>are necessary for <strong>the</strong> young to speak informatively,attractively, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> persuasively, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> hence, competitively.If in evoluti<strong>on</strong>, as now, <strong>the</strong>se <strong>development</strong>s occurredduring a stage that follows childhood, communicativeability in preceding stages would have been relevant. Wepropose that individuals with l<strong>on</strong>ger intervals betweeninfancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence would have had more opportunityto improvise <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learn vocal material, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> any rules ofdeployment. Selecti<strong>on</strong> for vocal ability <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, ultimately,for language, would thus have worked reciprocally toextend childhood.2.5. Stage 3: JuvenilityJuvenility is <strong>the</strong> next stage of human <strong>development</strong>. Inmammals generally <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> primates particularly, juvenilesare sexually immature but independent of o<strong>the</strong>rs for survival(Pereira 1993; Pereira & Altmann 1985). It is notunusual in traditi<strong>on</strong>al human societies for juveniles tofind much of <strong>the</strong>ir own food, avoid predators, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>compete with adults for food <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> space (Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es1993; Weisner 1987). The beginning of human juvenilityis marked by adrenarche <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, in some juveniles, a progressiveincrease in <strong>the</strong> secreti<strong>on</strong> of adrenal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>rogens.These horm<strong>on</strong>es may launch <strong>the</strong> growth of axillary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>pubic hair. The physical changes induced by adrenarcheare accompanied by cognitive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social advances, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>re are sexually dimorphic vocal changes during thisstage as well (Wuyts et al. 2003).In juvenility, rate of growth declines <strong>on</strong>ce again, givingthis stage <strong>the</strong> slowest growth rate since infancy (Fig. 1).Frequently, <strong>the</strong> decline follows a mid-growth spurt, inBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 263


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>those children who experience it, but <strong>the</strong> rate of growthdeclines even in children without a detectable spurt. Thecause of this decrease in growth rate is unknown, butJans<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> van Schaik (1993) have proposed severalbenefits. The first is that it provides additi<strong>on</strong>al time for<strong>the</strong> brain growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning required for reproductivesuccess in various species of social mammals. Social carnivores,elephants, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> primates must all learn how to livewithin <strong>the</strong> social hierarchy of <strong>the</strong> group. Juvenility alsooffers opportunities to learn complex feeding skills suchas how to hunt, how to open fruits or seeds with protectivecoverings, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> where <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> when to find food. Reproductiveskills must also be learned, including ways to compete formates <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> care for offspring. Joffe (1997) has reportedpositive correlati<strong>on</strong>s between <strong>the</strong> length of juvenility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>both group size <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> volume of social areas of <strong>the</strong> brain.Jans<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> van Schaik (1993) also proposed that slowergrowth reduces feeding competiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> maintains asmaller body, which is less threatening to adults. Growingup in a social group that is composed of individuals of differentages <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social status can be difficult. This is especiallytrue for juveniles who must compete with older, moreexperienced, individuals for food, space, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>rresources. Fortunately, <strong>the</strong>ir relatively small <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> slowlygrowing bodies require less food than adult bodies, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>may also afford <strong>the</strong>m some protecti<strong>on</strong> from adult competiti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> attack as <strong>the</strong>y present a n<strong>on</strong>-threateningjuvenile morphology.In humans, <strong>the</strong> decline of growth rate bottoms out atabout age 10 years in girls, two years before it does so inmales, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> this marks <strong>the</strong> end of <strong>the</strong> juvenile stage. Asfor <strong>the</strong> functi<strong>on</strong> of juvenility, it seems reas<strong>on</strong>able tosuppose that in humans, like <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r primates, thisstage offers opportunities to prepare for <strong>the</strong> social complexitiesof adolescence, as well as adulthood. Wepropose that juvenility also provides <strong>the</strong> young withchances to learn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> perfect skills associated with <strong>the</strong>social, pragmatic, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performative uses of language.2.6. Juvenile languageCoates (1986) has pointed out that, for all <strong>the</strong> advancesoccurring in childhood, “a knowledge of grammar, ph<strong>on</strong>ology<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> [<strong>the</strong>] lexic<strong>on</strong> is not enough – it does not make <strong>the</strong>child competent; children need to master not <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong>formal rules of language, but also rules for <strong>the</strong> appropriateuse of language” (p. 121). To be sure, juvenility accommodatesadditi<strong>on</strong>al syntactic advances (cf. Nippold 1998),with some English forms c<strong>on</strong>tinuing to emerge in earlyadulthood (Marchman et al. 1991), but many of <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>development</strong>s affect performance. These include anincrease in speech-breathing capacity (Engstrom et al.1956; Hoit et al. 1990) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fur<strong>the</strong>r increases in fluency(Starkwea<strong>the</strong>r 1987). Prior to <strong>the</strong> adolescent “voicebreak,” <strong>the</strong>re are anatomical changes that affect <strong>the</strong> ratioof <strong>the</strong> fundamental frequency to <strong>the</strong> ratio of higherformant frequencies (Sachs et al. 1973; also see Austin &Leeper 1975; Vuorenkoski et al. 1978).There are changes, too, that occur bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> sentencelevel, in <strong>the</strong> quality of extended discourse <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> narratives(Bamberg 1987; Burles<strong>on</strong> 1982; Karmiloff-Smith 1985).These pragmatic advances facilitate a variety of sociallyrelevant activities, from gossip to storytelling, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tributeto successful competiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> courtship in <strong>the</strong> runup to sexual maturity. In Turkey, boys engage in verbalduels – ritualistic insults <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> replies that require “skill inremembering <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> selecting appropriate retorts to provocativeinsults” (Dundes et al. 1970, p. 135). These duelsoccur primarily from ages 8 to 14 years, effectively bridgingjuvenility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence.In <strong>on</strong>e sense, juvenility is analogous to infancy. Whereas<strong>the</strong> linguistic knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> structure gained in infancywill help to satisfy <strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong>al needs of childhood,juvenility provides opportunities to achieve <strong>the</strong> persuasive<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> attractive use of speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> ability to fluentlymanipulate elaborate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> socially appropriate utterances,that will be valued in adolescence.2.7. Stage 4: AdolescenceIn humans, uniquely, <strong>the</strong>re is a distinct skeletal growthspurt in both sexes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in almost all skeletal elements of<strong>the</strong> body after several years of gently decreasing juvenilegrowth. The <strong>on</strong>set of this spurt, al<strong>on</strong>g with puberty org<strong>on</strong>adarche (an event of <strong>the</strong> neuroendocrine system)marks <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>set of adolescence (Bogin 1999b). Neuroendocrinologicalchanges differentially affect <strong>the</strong> vocaltract <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fundamental frequency of <strong>the</strong> two sexes, femalesrevealing negligible changes, males displaying a significantincrease in tract length <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> decrease in frequency, withfur<strong>the</strong>r drops in <strong>the</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong> from adolescence to adulthood(Fitch & Giedd 1999; Lieberman et al. 2001; Pedersenet al. 1986; 1990; Vuorenkoski et al. 1978). The critical variableis testoster<strong>on</strong>e, which increases <strong>the</strong> length <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> massof <strong>the</strong> vocal folds, lowers <strong>the</strong> fundamental frequency, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>alters <strong>the</strong> vibratory characteristics of <strong>the</strong> vocal folds(Abitol et al. 1999; Beckford et al. 1985; Titze 1989).Development of o<strong>the</strong>r sec<strong>on</strong>dary sexual characteristics<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a growth spurt in height <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> weight also occur inadolescence. These physical changes are accompanied byintensificati<strong>on</strong> of preexisting friendships <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>of <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> relati<strong>on</strong>ships. The <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> affiliati<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> membershipin peer groups, facilitate intimacy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mutualsupport (Whitmire 2000). Adolescence ends with <strong>the</strong>attainment of adult stature <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> biosocial skillsneeded for successful reproducti<strong>on</strong>. This occurs, <strong>on</strong>average, at about 19 years of age in women <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 21 to 25years in men (Bogin 1999b; 2001).It is significant that <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>dary sexual <strong>development</strong> ofboys <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> girls progresses differently through adolescence.Boys become fertile about two years after puberty <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>set of <strong>the</strong> adolescent growth spurt. At this time <strong>the</strong>ystill look like boys, as <strong>the</strong>ir body hair, stature, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> muscularityare still juvenile in appearance. The peak of <strong>the</strong>ir growthspurt (called “peak height velocity,” or PHV) is still, <strong>on</strong>average, <strong>on</strong>e year away, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> patterns of body hair do notbecome adult-like for three years, with a muscle spurt<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adult stature still four years away (about age 18 <strong>on</strong>average). Girls, in c<strong>on</strong>trast, will have completed abouthalf <strong>the</strong>ir breast <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pubic hair <strong>development</strong> by <strong>the</strong> timeof <strong>the</strong>ir PHV. Menarche occurs about a year after PHV.At this point in <strong>development</strong>, girls have <strong>the</strong> outwardappearance of women, but <strong>the</strong>y are not fertile. Developmentof an adult frequency of ovulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adult size of<strong>the</strong> birth canal do not occur until approximately age 18(Bogin 1999b).Bogin (1999a; 1999b) has hypo<strong>the</strong>sized that <strong>the</strong>sesex-specific patterns of adolescent <strong>development</strong> c<strong>on</strong>fer264 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>significant reproductive advantages <strong>on</strong> our species, partlyby allowing adolescents to learn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice adult ec<strong>on</strong>omic,social, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexual behaviors before reproducing.He noted that girls best learn <strong>the</strong>ir adult social roleswhile <strong>the</strong>y are infertile but perceived by adults asmature, whereas boys best learn <strong>the</strong>ir adult social roleswhile <strong>the</strong>y are sexually mature but not yet perceived assuch by adults. The percepti<strong>on</strong> of fertility in girls allows<strong>the</strong>m to enter <strong>the</strong> social-ec<strong>on</strong>omic-sexual world of adultwomen, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to practice many skills without <strong>the</strong> risk ofpregnancy. The horm<strong>on</strong>al status <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fertility of adolescentboys primes <strong>the</strong>m to attend to <strong>the</strong> social-ec<strong>on</strong>omic-sexualworld of adult men. As <strong>the</strong>se boys are still juvenile inoutward <strong>development</strong>, <strong>the</strong>y can interact <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learn fromolder adolescents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adults without seeming tocompete for important resources, including women. Theyounger adolescent boy can “apprentice” to older adolescents<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> men, make mistakes that would be very costlyto adults, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>rwise learn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice skills thatmay be essential to adult survival <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reproducti<strong>on</strong>.Without <strong>the</strong> adolescent growth spurt, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> sexspecifictiming of maturati<strong>on</strong> events that occur <strong>the</strong>n, thisunique style of social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cultural learning could notoccur, but <strong>the</strong>re is c<strong>on</strong>siderable variati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> waysadolescence is treated by individual cultures. Humanadolescence is <strong>the</strong>refore best understood in terms of itsbiological underpinning <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its cultural manifestati<strong>on</strong>s.2.8. Adolescent languageIn adolescence, <strong>the</strong>re are fur<strong>the</strong>r changes in linguistic communicati<strong>on</strong>in three broad areas. The first is linguisticc<strong>on</strong>tent, including vocabulary, <strong>the</strong> more nuanced grammaticaloperati<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> idiomatic phrases (Nippold & Taylor2002), including slang. The sec<strong>on</strong>d area of adolescent<strong>development</strong> involves delivery, including improvement inspeaking rate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fluency. There is a huge increase in respiratorycapacity at this stage, which may influence someaspects of speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> voice (Cook & Hamann 1961; Hoitet al. 1990). The third area involves a host of functi<strong>on</strong>sthat begin to surface in childhood but flower in adolescence.One such functi<strong>on</strong> is social talking, a predominantlyrelati<strong>on</strong>al use of speech in which <strong>the</strong> topic is frequentlyo<strong>the</strong>r people (Gottman & Mettetal 1986). This dispositi<strong>on</strong>to gossip steadily increases during adolescence, especiallyin females (Raffaelli & Duckett 1989; also see sect. 6),whereas <strong>the</strong> tendency to tease peers is more evidentin males (see review in Eckert 2003). O<strong>the</strong>r functi<strong>on</strong>s –several, like gossip, falling outside <strong>the</strong> area of pragmaticsas usually defined – include joking, deceiving, mollifying,negotiating, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> persuading, with increases in <strong>the</strong> use ofsarcasm (McTear & C<strong>on</strong>ti-Ramsden 1992; Nippold 1998;Paul 1995). These skills are not highly appraised by classroomteachers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language clinicians, who place a highervalue <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> logical expressi<strong>on</strong> of thought (Reed et al.1998; Reed & Spicer 2003), but <strong>the</strong>y facilitate achievementof two things that matter a great deal toadolescents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adults: status <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> relati<strong>on</strong>ships. 5This discussi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> emergence of grammatical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>pragmatic functi<strong>on</strong>s should not distract us from a basicfact about <strong>the</strong> role of language in this stage of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Adolescentsdo not merely learn additi<strong>on</strong>al linguistic features <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>practice <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> rules of usage. They also modify materiallearned in earlier stages, <strong>the</strong>reby changing aspects of<strong>the</strong>ir native language (Eckert 1999; Labov 2001).Because adolescents are more parentally independentthan ever, but still geographically limited, a str<strong>on</strong>g localaffiliati<strong>on</strong> tends to influence <strong>the</strong>ir pers<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social identity.An effective way to signal this affiliati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> increasingaut<strong>on</strong>omy is through linguistic markers, particularlyph<strong>on</strong>etic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocal <strong>on</strong>es. “The relatively high degree ofph<strong>on</strong>ological innovati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> adolescent age group,”wrote Eckert (1988), “is an indicati<strong>on</strong> that <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>of adolescent social structure provides a majorimpetus for ph<strong>on</strong>ological change” (p. 197).3. How did vocal-verbal behavior evolve?Reports suggest that apes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> m<strong>on</strong>keys do not vocalize asoften as humans. Wild chimpanzees are silent much of <strong>the</strong>time (Gardner et al. 1989), evidently preferring <strong>the</strong> manualto <strong>the</strong> vocal modality (Tomasello et al. 1985; 1989). Trivers(1974) <strong>on</strong>ce commented that gorillas appeared to be“selected for silence.” By c<strong>on</strong>trast, our own species isgiven to near-c<strong>on</strong>stant chatter in familiar social circumstanceseven when it appears that very little is beingsaid (Locke 1998c). How – under what envir<strong>on</strong>mentalpressures – did human sound-making capacities evolve?3.1. Natural selecti<strong>on</strong>To evolve, biological traits must offer a selective advantage,even if it is small <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> inc<strong>on</strong>sistent, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> this advantagewill usually be linked to food, defense, reproducti<strong>on</strong>, orsome combinati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong>se. Building <strong>on</strong> key c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>sin evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary biology (Dawkins 1986; Maynard Smith1969), Pinker <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Bloom (1990) argued that natural selecti<strong>on</strong>is <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly way to explain <strong>the</strong> origin of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>o<strong>the</strong>r complex abilities. In doing so, <strong>the</strong>y said little aboutany role that selecti<strong>on</strong> might have played in <strong>development</strong>.But as Hogan (1988) has pointed out, natural selecti<strong>on</strong>“should operate at all stages of <strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> adult outcome, since any <strong>development</strong>alprocess that reduces <strong>the</strong> probability of reaching adulthoodwill be very str<strong>on</strong>gly selected against” (p. 97). Hence, webegin this secti<strong>on</strong> with several <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> proposals relating tovocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal selecti<strong>on</strong> as it may have operated ininfancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> childhood.3.2. Parental selecti<strong>on</strong>With evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary changes that narrowed <strong>the</strong> female pelvis<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> deferred fetal brain <strong>development</strong> (sect. 2.1), infants’need of care would have increased. As care requirementsgrew, c<strong>on</strong>flict <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> competiti<strong>on</strong> between <strong>the</strong> infant <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>its parents would also have escalated. Resp<strong>on</strong>sibility for<strong>on</strong>e soluti<strong>on</strong>, according to Trivers (1974), would havebeen borne largely, but not exclusively, by <strong>the</strong> infant – abehavioral change that included more effective use ofcare-elicitati<strong>on</strong> signals, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> possibly an improvement in<strong>the</strong> signals <strong>the</strong>mselves.The parental selecti<strong>on</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis proposes that someof <strong>the</strong> vocal ability presupposed by spoken languagesemerged from infancy, having been asserted initially byhominin infants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> supported by interacti<strong>on</strong>s with <strong>the</strong>irparents (Locke 2006). According to <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis,infants who issued more effective care-elicitati<strong>on</strong> signalsBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 265


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>(e.g., measured or strategic levels of cry) were betterpositi<strong>on</strong>ed to receive care than infants who issued stressvocalizati<strong>on</strong>s noxiously or inc<strong>on</strong>solably – behaviors thatinvite neglect <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> abuse in primates generally, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>forecast language-learning problems in humans. Thehypo<strong>the</strong>sis also envisi<strong>on</strong>s that infants who cooed <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>babbled at appropriate intervals were more likely toengage with adults, to be liked by <strong>the</strong>m, to receive moresophisticated forms of care as infancy progressed, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> togenerate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learn complex ph<strong>on</strong>etic patterns. Infantswho were able to m<strong>on</strong>itor adult reacti<strong>on</strong>s to <strong>the</strong>ir behaviors(Chisolm 2003) would have been able to discoverwhich vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s had <strong>the</strong> most beneficial effects, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>thus could use structured vocalizati<strong>on</strong> to maximum effect.It is also possible that syllabic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> articulatory activityplayed a “decoupling” role (Oller 2004), making availablefor recombinati<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> discrete movements, hence <strong>the</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>eticsegments, that make ph<strong>on</strong>ological systems possible(Studdert-Kennedy 1998; 2005; Studdert-Kennedy &Goldstein 2003). Fur<strong>the</strong>r elaborati<strong>on</strong> of vocal repertoires,we propose, would have occurred later in <strong>development</strong>under different pressures, potentially enhancing fitness in<strong>on</strong>e or more of <strong>the</strong>se stages, particularly adolescence.3.3. Trickle-up ph<strong>on</strong>eticsWe have suggested that heightened dependency, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>impending withdrawal of parental support, encouragedmore complex <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> clever ways of vocalizing in infancy. Itis plausible that some of <strong>the</strong>se more sophisticated patternsof sound making trickled upwards.While <strong>the</strong>re is a dispositi<strong>on</strong> to think about infants asimitators of adult behavior, <strong>the</strong>re is, in fact, a str<strong>on</strong>gertendency for parents to copy <strong>the</strong>ir infants (Uzgiris et al.1989). One reas<strong>on</strong> that <strong>the</strong>y do this may be to c<strong>on</strong>trol<strong>the</strong>ir infant’s attenti<strong>on</strong>. Meltzoff (1990) found that14-m<strong>on</strong>th-olds attended to an adult if he precisely replicated<strong>the</strong> infants’ own acti<strong>on</strong>s, doing so far morefrequently than when <strong>the</strong> adult performed unmatchingacti<strong>on</strong>s. The infants’ motivati<strong>on</strong> to attend may relate to<strong>the</strong> fact that c<strong>on</strong>tingent resp<strong>on</strong>ding causes infants tosmile, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> may be innately pleasurable (Wats<strong>on</strong> 1972;2001). This arrangement – movements by infants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>replicati<strong>on</strong>s by adults – c<strong>on</strong>stitutes a dyadic system that<strong>the</strong> infants can c<strong>on</strong>trol (Chisholm 2003).Such effects are also dem<strong>on</strong>strable in <strong>the</strong> case of speech.Pawlby (1977) reported that at age 17 to 43 weeks, morethan 90% of <strong>the</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>etic matching in her study wasattributable to mo<strong>the</strong>rs imitating <strong>the</strong>ir children; Pawlbycommented that infants “pay special attenti<strong>on</strong> (in that<strong>the</strong>y laugh <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> smile <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> appear to be pleased) when<strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs <strong>the</strong>mselves imitate an acti<strong>on</strong> which <strong>the</strong> childhas just performed” (p. 220).These interacti<strong>on</strong>s can produce durable changes ofadult speech, changes that we have termed “trickle upph<strong>on</strong>etics” (Locke 2004b). For example, in a number ofdisparate cultures, parents use specialized “baby words”(Fergus<strong>on</strong> 1964). The c<strong>on</strong>stituent c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>antal soundsare primarily stops, nasals, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> glides, frequently recurring,reduplicatively, with low vowels (Locke 1983).These items resemble <strong>the</strong> familiar forms of babbling, abehavior that is favorably regarded by parents (Bloomet al. 1993). Such st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard lexical items as “mama” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>“papa” fur<strong>the</strong>r illustrate <strong>the</strong> strength of <strong>the</strong> tendency toincorporate infant vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s into <strong>the</strong> adult lexic<strong>on</strong>(Jakobs<strong>on</strong> 1960; Locke 1985).Since Mivart (1871) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, later, Garstang (1922), it hasbeen recognized that for a species to evolve, changesmust occur in <strong>development</strong> (de Beer 1951/1958; Gottlieb1992; Gould 1977; Northcutt 1990). This does not meanthat <strong>the</strong> principal agent of change, or <strong>on</strong>e of <strong>the</strong> principalagents, must itself be a developing infant or child, but thatis exactly what we propose in <strong>the</strong> case of parental selecti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> trickle-up ph<strong>on</strong>etics (see sect. 13).3.4. Instinct for inventivenessThese cases, in which <strong>development</strong>al changes appearto influence evoluti<strong>on</strong>, suggest that human infants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>children may possess “instincts for inventiveness”(Marler 1991, p. 63). Marler was thinking about s<strong>on</strong>gbirdswhen he used that phrase, but it appears that when linguisticallydeprived children are exposed <strong>on</strong>ly to fragmentarysymbolic behavior, whe<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> form of vocal pidginsor manual gestures, <strong>the</strong>y also improvise lexical material<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammatical structure (Bickert<strong>on</strong> 1984; Goldin-Meadow & Feldman 1977; Goldin-Meadow & Myl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>er1998; Newport 1981; Senghas & Coppola 2001; Senghaset al. 2004). In effect, <strong>the</strong>y invent language.It could be argued, of course, that since modern childrenhave all <strong>the</strong> genes required for language, <strong>the</strong>ir vocal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal creativity is a product of linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong>.But <strong>the</strong>re is evidence that o<strong>the</strong>r primate infants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juvenilesalso come up with creative soluti<strong>on</strong>s to envir<strong>on</strong>mentalproblems. These behaviors have included food washing inJapanese macaques (Kawai 1965; Kawamura 1959), foodacquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social behavior in chimpanzees (Wranghamet al. 1996), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> tool use in orangutans (van Schaik2004). In <strong>the</strong> macaques, adult animals were later observedcarrying out <strong>the</strong> same behaviors as <strong>the</strong> infants. Am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>chimpanzees, behaviors originating with infants or juvenilestended to diffuse laterally, remaining at <strong>the</strong> subadultlevel. But if questi<strong>on</strong>s remain about transmissi<strong>on</strong> (Galef1990; Hauser 1988), few seem to doubt <strong>the</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>that <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> behaviors frequently originate in <strong>the</strong> young.One investigator has even raised <strong>the</strong> idea of a “criticalperiod” for <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> skill (nut cracking)in chimpanzees (Matsuzawa 1996).3.5. Mo<strong>the</strong>r t<strong>on</strong>guesAt some point, of course, <strong>the</strong>re had to be pressures notmerely to improvise <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more complex signals butalso to use <strong>the</strong>m meaningfully, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> this is <strong>the</strong> focus of arecent kin selecti<strong>on</strong> proposal by Fitch (2004; also seeBrockway 2003, p. 120). Where it had been assumedthat breeding was <strong>the</strong> sole means of gene transmissi<strong>on</strong>,kin selecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory proposes that individuals also passal<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir genes by helping genetically related individuals<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir way to reproductive success (Hamilt<strong>on</strong> 1964), atleast if <strong>the</strong> cost of doing so is not too great (Trivers 1974).Fitch (2004) extended kin selecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory by proposingthat parents benefit by preferentially communicating accurateinformati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong>ir offspring. He reas<strong>on</strong>ed that inhominin families <strong>the</strong>re would have been a role for “cheaph<strong>on</strong>esty” – <strong>the</strong> ability of parents to exchange accurate informati<strong>on</strong>with <strong>the</strong>ir offspring <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> with o<strong>the</strong>r kin who shared<strong>the</strong>ir genes. They could do this without significant cost or266 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>effort, Fitch suggested, because speech is metabolicallyinexpensive. Systems of communicati<strong>on</strong> that evolved in<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of kin selecti<strong>on</strong> were labeled “mo<strong>the</strong>r t<strong>on</strong>gues.”The better-informed young, according to Fitch’s (2004)proposal, would be more likely to survive into adulthood,passing <strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong>ir own offspring genes associated withthis improved system of communicati<strong>on</strong>. Although hetook no positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> how language evolved, his proposaldoes address pressures within kin groups to achievesome means of exchanging informati<strong>on</strong>. These arrangementswould have broadened later, according to Fitch,to include genetically unrelated individuals, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> elaboratedto meet <strong>the</strong> requirements, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pressures, associatedwith informati<strong>on</strong> transmissi<strong>on</strong>.We suggest, <strong>the</strong>n, that <strong>the</strong> kin group provided a c<strong>on</strong>textin which it was advantageous to exchange informati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>that infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> childhood furnished raw vocal materialthat would have favored any system of spoken communicati<strong>on</strong>.But selecti<strong>on</strong> would have needed to operate ino<strong>the</strong>r stages if that material was ever to elaboratebey<strong>on</strong>d some restricted core. The performative <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> creativenature of in-group verbal behavior in juvenility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>adolescence suggests that <strong>the</strong>se stages may have playedan important role in <strong>the</strong> process.3.6. Peer effects in adolescenceIn secti<strong>on</strong> 2.8 we discussed <strong>the</strong> fact that adolescents not<strong>on</strong>ly manipulate language in <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> ways, but also revise it.In evoluti<strong>on</strong>, similar things may have occurred, whe<strong>the</strong>rin s<strong>on</strong>g or a more speech-like format, in resp<strong>on</strong>se to <strong>the</strong> horm<strong>on</strong>al<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s that characterize <strong>the</strong> approachto sexual maturity. At <strong>the</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ological level of language,changes, which frequently involve an increase in <strong>the</strong> complexityof articulati<strong>on</strong>, serve to identify members of socialgroups (Labov 2001). Of significance to <strong>on</strong>e of our majorclaims (see sect. 4.3), n<strong>on</strong>st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard forms tend to originatepreferentially with males, doing so at some point betweenlate juvenility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> early adolescence (cf. Eckert 2003).4. The social c<strong>on</strong>text of vocal-verbal evoluti<strong>on</strong>We assume that at sexual maturity, vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal performancesincreased fitness by facilitating attainment ofsocial rank <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mating relati<strong>on</strong>ships, <strong>the</strong>reby improvingaccess to associated benefits. Links between vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>social factors may be found in arrangements bearingsome resemblance to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>texts in which language evolved– oral societies. We turn our attenti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong>se now,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>n look at natural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> as <strong>the</strong>y mayhave applied at adolescence.4.1. Oral societiesOral societies are of interest because <strong>the</strong>y appear toresemble ancient <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary societies more closelythan do o<strong>the</strong>r extant groups (see Lee 1979b, p. 32), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>may provide a clearer view of <strong>the</strong> initial benefits of vocal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal behavior in our species. Life in modernsocieties makes it difficult to think of <strong>the</strong> human capacityfor verbal communicati<strong>on</strong> as anything but <strong>the</strong> ability tolearn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> use a linguistic code. This is largely because<strong>the</strong> prevailing definiti<strong>on</strong> of language, according to Linell(1982), “is deeply influenced by a l<strong>on</strong>g traditi<strong>on</strong> of analyzing<strong>on</strong>ly written language” (p. 1), a practice encouraged by“objectivist linguistics” (Lakoff & Johns<strong>on</strong> 1980). Towitness what may be <strong>the</strong> vestiges of earlier effects, wewill evaluate evidence from tribal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r oral cultures.Fortunately, oral societies have been a popular target ofanthropologists, who have noted a belief in <strong>the</strong> “magical”power of words (cf. Tambiah 1983). This power, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rhetoricalforce more generally, have been available to thosewho used an excepti<strong>on</strong>ally broad range of words, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>phrases that are “ornamented” by material from outside<strong>the</strong> more limited repertoires of <strong>the</strong>ir listeners (Abrahams1970c; 1989; Garrett 1993; Sherzer 1990; Stra<strong>the</strong>rn 1971).C<strong>on</strong>tent has included archaic or esoteric language, metaphors,met<strong>on</strong>ymy, formulas, riddles, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> special prosodicpatterns as well as over-learned phrases (e.g., Bauman1975; Comaroff 1975; Gossen 1976a; Malinowski 1935).These devices have been witnessed in places as dispersedas Melanesia, Amaz<strong>on</strong>ia, Africa, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> North America.Examples come from:The Trobri<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Isl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s of New Guinea, where Malinowski(1922) noted that lexical “power” was achieved through<strong>the</strong> use of “archaisms, mythical names <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> strange compounds,formed according to unusual linguistic rules”(p. 432).Central Brazil, where <strong>the</strong> “plaza speech” of Suya Indians“has a special rhythm, sets of formulas, place of delivery[<strong>the</strong> center of <strong>the</strong> village plaza], <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> style of delivery”(Seeger 1981, p. 85). “The ph<strong>on</strong>etic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rhythmic featuresof speech are altered, generally by exaggeratingrhythms <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> stressing unstressed syllables. All plazaspeech is also highly repetitive” (p. 186).Amaz<strong>on</strong>ia (South America), where <strong>the</strong> Pa’ikwené peopleare c<strong>on</strong>sidered “good speakers” based <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir “grammatical,rhetorical, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performative competence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>vocal quality,” as well as “<strong>the</strong> ethical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> emoti<strong>on</strong>alweight of <strong>the</strong>ir words” (Passes 2004, p. 8).Nor<strong>the</strong>rn Transvaal, where <strong>the</strong> Venda people’s knowledgeof words gives <strong>the</strong>m a “magical power” that “is often sufficientto impress people, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> it need have no practicalapplicati<strong>on</strong>” (Blacking 1961, p. 4).Certain neighborhoods of Philadelphia, where <strong>the</strong> verbalc<strong>on</strong>tests of African-Americans use language that is“different from <strong>the</strong> everyday language of <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>testants”insofar as it includes “changes in pitch, stress,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sometimes syntax ... formulaic patterns ... rhymewithin <strong>the</strong>se patterns ... <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a change of speechrhythms ...” (Abrahams 1970a, p. 50).The primary effect of this sort of material, according toBauman (1975), is that it “fixes <strong>the</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> audiencemore str<strong>on</strong>gly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> performer, binds <strong>the</strong> audienceto <strong>the</strong> performer in a relati<strong>on</strong>ship of dependence thatkeeps <strong>the</strong>m caught up in his display” (p. 295). 6 Reliance<strong>on</strong> speech formulas will prove significant later, for n<strong>on</strong>literalmaterial – which also exists in modern languages in<strong>the</strong> form of idioms <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> metaphors – does not approachadult proficiency until late juvenility or early adolescence.A sec<strong>on</strong>d interesting property of oral societies is <strong>the</strong>“extraordinarily ag<strong>on</strong>istic” nature of verbal performance.“Orality,” according to Ong (1982), “situates knowledgewithin a c<strong>on</strong>text of struggle. Proverbs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> riddles are notBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 267


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>used simply to store knowledge, [...] but to engage o<strong>the</strong>rsin verbal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intellectual combat: utterance of <strong>on</strong>e proverbor riddle challenges hearers to top it with a more appositeor a c<strong>on</strong>tradictory <strong>on</strong>e” (p. 44). Later we will revisit this“ag<strong>on</strong>istic” property.4.2. Speech, attenti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> powerIf <strong>the</strong>re are links between speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong>, as <strong>the</strong>anthropological literature suggests, <strong>the</strong>n individuals mayuse speech as “an advertising device,” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “will competewith each o<strong>the</strong>r in eloquence to draw attenti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong>mselves<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to obtain status from <strong>the</strong> audience” (Dessalles1998, p. 142). In many oral societies, speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> status,or power, go h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>. These societies are locatedin places as dispersed as:New Zeal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, where, am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Maori, “oratory is <strong>the</strong>prime qualificati<strong>on</strong> for entry into <strong>the</strong> power game”(Salm<strong>on</strong>d 1975, p. 50).Central Peru, where <strong>the</strong> Amuesha people “describe a trueleader as ... ‘<strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e who is powerful due to his or herwords’” (Santos-Granero 1991, p. 301).Ethiopia, where, am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Mursi, “<strong>the</strong> most frequentlymenti<strong>on</strong>ed attribute of an influential man is his abilityto speak well in public” (Turt<strong>on</strong> 1975, p. 176).Central Brazil, where “plaza speech, described as “a politicallyimportant act” (Seeger 1981, p. 185).Nor<strong>the</strong>rn Transvaal, where, am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Venda people, “Thegreatest h<strong>on</strong>our seems to be accorded to those who canmanipulate words <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sentences” (Blacking 1961, p. 4).The Trobri<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Isl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, where <strong>the</strong> tribal leaders, or “Bigmen,” have also been called “rhetoric thumpers” (Reay1959).South Africa, where <strong>the</strong> Tshidi people c<strong>on</strong>sider oratoricalability as “a significant comp<strong>on</strong>ent of political success<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> means by which politicians dem<strong>on</strong>strate <strong>the</strong>iracumen” (Comaroff 1975, p. 143).Inner city neighborhoods of Philadelphia, where <strong>the</strong>African-American “man of words, <strong>the</strong> good talker, hasan important place in <strong>the</strong> social structure of <strong>the</strong> group,not <strong>on</strong>ly in adolescence but throughout most of his<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>” (Abrahams 1970a, p. 44).South America, where “Speaking is more than a privilege,it is a duty of <strong>the</strong> chief. It is to him that <strong>the</strong> mastery ofwords falls.” Throughout <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinent, “It can be saidnot that <strong>the</strong> chief is a man who speaks, but that hewho speaks is a chief” (Clastres 1987, p. 41).This relati<strong>on</strong>ship between speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> power may belinked to a c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> between speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>ein which vocal loudness plays a role. “A great Maoriorator,” wrote Salm<strong>on</strong>d (1975), “jumps to his feet with aloud call <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> immediately dominates <strong>the</strong> speakingground”(p. 56). Am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Pa’ikwené, “good speaking”involves speech that is “str<strong>on</strong>g in t<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> volume ...speaking loud-<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>-str<strong>on</strong>g embodies a pers<strong>on</strong>’s health,strength, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, bey<strong>on</strong>d that, humanness” (Passes 2004, p. 8).The effect of vocal-verbal behavior <strong>on</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>n status is evident as early as childhood. In a classroomstudy of three- to six-year-old German kindergartners,Hold-Cavell <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Borsutzky (1986) found that glanceswere frequently preceded by speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that childrenwho attracted attenti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong>mselves by verbalizingearly in <strong>the</strong> school year were far more likely than o<strong>the</strong>rsto rank highly at <strong>the</strong> end of <strong>the</strong> year. In adulthood, individualswho speak in l<strong>on</strong>g sentences or hold <strong>the</strong> floor for l<strong>on</strong>gperiods of time tend to receive more gazes than o<strong>the</strong>rs,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to be perceived as unusually powerful (Abramovitch1976; Bales et al. 1951; Dabbs & Ruback 1984; Exlineet al. 1975; Kalma 1991; Kend<strong>on</strong> & Cook 1969; Mulac1989).4.3. Verbal performanceVerbal performances <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> competiti<strong>on</strong>s have been documentedin oral cultures located, diversely, in Africa,Asia, South America, Mexico, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> several differentplaces in North America (Ayoub & Barnett 1965;Dundes et al. 1970; Faris 1966; Gossen 1976b). These performancestake a number of forms – in African-Americangroups, for example, rapping, shucking <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> jiving, coppinga plea, gripping, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sounding (<strong>on</strong>ce known as “playing <strong>the</strong>dozens”). Each of <strong>the</strong>se forms “must be judged as performanceart,” according to Abrahams (1970b), <strong>the</strong> evaluati<strong>on</strong>based less <strong>on</strong> “<strong>the</strong> elements of <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong>, suchas complexity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> originality,” than <strong>on</strong> its “sp<strong>on</strong>taneity,virtuosity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> comm<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>” (p. 147). 7 One also finds inanthropological literatures various references to <strong>the</strong> useof <strong>the</strong> voice without speech, whe<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> s<strong>on</strong>gs of <strong>the</strong>Suya Indians of Brazil (Seeger 1981), <strong>the</strong> s<strong>on</strong>g challengesof <strong>the</strong> Fiji Indians (Brenneis & Padarath 1975), or <strong>the</strong> s<strong>on</strong>gduels or drum s<strong>on</strong>gs of Eskimos (Hoebel 1964; Mirsky1937).The social value of performative speech has beenattested by African-American teenagers in Los Angeles.Folb (1980) interviewed a black teenage boy who said,“yo’ rap is your thing. I’s like your pers<strong>on</strong>ality. Like youkin style <strong>on</strong> some dude by rappin’ better’n he do.Show’im up. Outdo him c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>-wise.” Folb commented,“Who you are (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> very often how well yousurvive) depends ... heavily <strong>on</strong> how well you talk.”Words, she said, “are tools for power <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> gain ... a goodrap can save your <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>” (p. 90). That rapping is more of aperformance than a verbal exchange is suggested by <strong>the</strong>fact that African-American men in Chicago ghettos rapto ra<strong>the</strong>r than with <strong>the</strong>ir peers (Kochman 1969).Kochman found that rapping <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r verbal registersenable <strong>the</strong> speaker “to manipulate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trolpeople <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> situati<strong>on</strong>s to give himself a winning edge”(p. 34).It is of more than passing interest that many of <strong>the</strong>actors in traditi<strong>on</strong>al verbal performances have been male.For example:Am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Limba people of Sierra Le<strong>on</strong>e, “it is a specificallymasculine quality to be able to ‘speak’ well”(Finnegan 1967, p. 70).In an Ethiopian village studied by Turt<strong>on</strong> (1975), publicspeaking was “a prerogative of adult males” (p. 170).The orators in South Bali, according to Hobart (1975),were “usually adult men aged from thirty to sixty withreputati<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong>ir knowledge of law <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> persuasivenessas speakers” (p. 77).268 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Kuna Indians of San Blas, Panama, Sherzer(1990) reported, “it is most often men who are <strong>the</strong>public performers of verbal art” (p. 3).Am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Suya Indians of Brazil, plaza speech “is spoken<strong>on</strong>ly by fully adult men, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>e of its forms is spokenexclusively by chiefs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ritual specialists” (Seeger1981, p. 85).Am<strong>on</strong>g African-Americans in various parts of <strong>the</strong> UnitedStates, verbal competiti<strong>on</strong>s such as rapping <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> soundinghave been carried out predominantly by males(Abrahams 1973; Ferris 1972; Wils<strong>on</strong> 1969). “Sounding,”according to Abrahams (1970a), “occurs <strong>on</strong>ly incrowds of boys” (p. 47).The male dominati<strong>on</strong> of verbal performance in traditi<strong>on</strong>alsocieties has received little recogniti<strong>on</strong>. “It is an interestingfeature of <strong>the</strong> scholarship in <strong>the</strong> field,” wrote Kuiper (1996),“that this fact is seldom commented <strong>on</strong>” (p. 87).Verbal performances typically have an “audience,”which makes it possible for status to change following averbal victory or defeat (Abrahams 1962; 1989; Mitchell-Kernan 1973). These audiences include women as wellas men. Finnegan (1967) found that am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Limbapeople, “a woman is expected to sit <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> listen, clap toshow her respect <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> appreciati<strong>on</strong>, or join in <strong>the</strong> chorusof <strong>the</strong> s<strong>on</strong>gs” (p. 70). In Cat Harbour, a small fishing communityin Newfoundl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, Faris (1966) found that storieswere typically told in local shops by men. The women ofCat Harbour “listen intently <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> inc<strong>on</strong>spicuously to <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong> men,” wrote Faris (p. 239), usingany <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> informati<strong>on</strong> later to engage o<strong>the</strong>r women insocial interacti<strong>on</strong>s. Similar sexual divisi<strong>on</strong>s have beenreported by o<strong>the</strong>r anthropologists (e.g., Salm<strong>on</strong>d 1975).Verbal c<strong>on</strong>tests tend to create distance between <strong>the</strong>participants. This frequently appeals to men, who typicallyplace a high value <strong>on</strong> individualism <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> aut<strong>on</strong>omy(Kashima et al. 1995). But women are more inclinedtoward collectivism <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intimacy, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>y tend toembrace more private forms of speech, <strong>on</strong>es that reduce<strong>the</strong> distance between interlocutors. We will see shortly(sect. 6) that <strong>the</strong>re is a female advantage in privatelyoriented forms of social communicati<strong>on</strong>.5. Natural selecti<strong>on</strong> in adolescenceWe begin our treatment of natural selecti<strong>on</strong> in adolescenceby discussing vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal behaviors that mayhave facilitated pers<strong>on</strong>al rank <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social relati<strong>on</strong>ships– two critical features of primate societies (Dunbar1996) – possibly by c<strong>on</strong>trolling attenti<strong>on</strong> (Locke 2000b). 8Most primate species live in rigidly structured groups,with a dominance hierarchy. Males typically earn <strong>the</strong>irrank. Noting that socially dominant primates are usually<strong>the</strong> focus of subordinates’ attenti<strong>on</strong>, Chance claimed thatit is <strong>the</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong>-holding ability of an animal that placesit near <strong>the</strong> top of <strong>the</strong> hierarchy (Chance 1967; Chance &Jolly 1970). One anecdote suggests that sound-makingattracts attenti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>reby increases rank. Goodall(1998) described a subordinate chimpanzee that elevatedhis rank by frightening high-ranking animals, which hedid by loudly banging kerosene cans toge<strong>the</strong>r.Primates may also elevate or announce <strong>the</strong>ir rank byvocalizing. In young vervet m<strong>on</strong>keys, high-rankingindividuals have been found to vocalize more frequentlythan those holding middle <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lower ranks (Locke &Hauser 1999). In mature babo<strong>on</strong>s, acoustic properties of<strong>the</strong> male’s voice (in “loud calls”) have been found topredict age, competitive ability, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> stamina, as well asrank (Fischer et al. 2004; Kitchen et al. 2003). Thesedata suggest that primates who listen to adult male vocalizati<strong>on</strong>sare likely to pick up important cues to fitness – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>y do listen; Cheney <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Seyfarth (2005b) refer toprimates as “skilled voyeurs.” Primates also derive informati<strong>on</strong>about rank from calls made to males by o<strong>the</strong>rmales. In studies of chimpanzees, alpha, high-ranking,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mid-ranking males were discriminable based <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>frequency of occurrence of pant-grunts that were directedto <strong>the</strong>m by lower-ranking males (Hayaki et al. 1989;Muller & Wrangham 2004).If, in humans, status may be elevated by unusually elaborate<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fluent uses of <strong>the</strong> voice, <strong>on</strong>e would expectmembers of traditi<strong>on</strong>al societies to be keenly aware of individualdifferences in verbal proficiency. Gossen (1976b)found that am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Chamula people of Mexico, “<strong>the</strong>ability to wage a good verbal duel serves as <strong>on</strong>e of <strong>the</strong>earliest signs of social maturity, intelligence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguisticeloquence” (p. 141). Firth (1975) commented that in <strong>on</strong>etraditi<strong>on</strong>al society, <strong>the</strong> Southwestern Pacific isl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ofTikopia, <strong>the</strong> people “are very c<strong>on</strong>scious of great individualdifferences in speaking quality” (p. 41). Turt<strong>on</strong> (1975) indicatedthat am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Mursi, “it seems to be popularlyassumed that men ei<strong>the</strong>r have or do not have <strong>the</strong> abilityto speak well in public” (p. 177). O<strong>the</strong>r work in Africa <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>elsewhere indicates that unusually good speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> storytellersare not <strong>on</strong>ly distinguished from o<strong>the</strong>rs, but h<strong>on</strong>oredin various ways, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that poor speakers are subjected toridicule (Abrahams 1989; Finnegan 1967; Shostak 1981).In <strong>the</strong> United States, Labov (1972; 1973) found thatAfrican-Americans’ use of <strong>the</strong> black vernacular, includingn<strong>on</strong>-st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard or within-group forms, reaches a peak atages 9 to 18 years – that is, in <strong>the</strong> juvenile <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescentperiods. Those with poor c<strong>on</strong>trol of <strong>the</strong> vernacular areknown as “lames.” Lames lack <strong>the</strong> ability to participatein toasts, jokes, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal competiti<strong>on</strong>s. According toLabov, <strong>the</strong>y are forced to operate from outside <strong>the</strong> group. 95.1. Sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>Although Pinker <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Bloom (1990) offered a spirited argumentfor <strong>the</strong> natural selecti<strong>on</strong> of language, <strong>the</strong>re is noreas<strong>on</strong> to think that <strong>the</strong>y meant to exclude sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>– a distinct but related process (Anderss<strong>on</strong> 1994;Harvey & Arnold 1982; Mayr 1972), which was originallyproposed for language by Darwin (1879/2004, pp. 109–110). Sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> takes two forms: an epigamicvariant that involves traits appealing to <strong>the</strong> opposite sex,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an intrasexual form that involves traits advantageousin competiti<strong>on</strong>s am<strong>on</strong>g members of <strong>the</strong> same sex(M. E. Hamilt<strong>on</strong> 1985). It is likely that both forms ofsexual selecti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tributed to <strong>the</strong> elaborati<strong>on</strong> of vocal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal behavior in <strong>the</strong> hominin line.In early stages of linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong>, it seems reas<strong>on</strong>ableto suppose that sexually mature individuals benefitedfrom some level of c<strong>on</strong>trol over, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> appreciati<strong>on</strong> of,vocal behavior – later from more heavily articulated vocalizati<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbalizati<strong>on</strong> – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that displays in thismodality may have been performed more comm<strong>on</strong>ly byBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 269


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>males, since it is generally <strong>the</strong> case, across <strong>the</strong> animalkingdom, that females choose <strong>the</strong>ir mates <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> basis ofperceptible traits (Anderss<strong>on</strong> 1994; Mealey 2000), presumablybecause <strong>the</strong>y have more to lose if <strong>the</strong>y make abad choice (Trivers 1972). Accordingly, several scholarshave sought support for a sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis,but not without disappointment (e.g., Fitch 2004; Miller2000), since <strong>the</strong> literature, read uncritically, has suggestedthat males perform worse than females <strong>on</strong> a variety ofverbal tests, doing so across <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>span, beginning in<strong>the</strong> early stages of language <strong>development</strong> (Fens<strong>on</strong> et al.1994; Huttenlocher et al. 1991; Lutchmaya et al. 2002;Morisset et al. 1995). A more comprehensive review of<strong>development</strong>al studies, however, indicates that boys so<strong>on</strong>catch up with girls (Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen et al. 2004); <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> acloser inspecti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> tests that are administered toolder children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adults reveals carefully timed, verballyinstructed measures of anagram ability, vocabulary, verbalmemory, word associati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>(Hines 1990; Kimura 2002; Maccoby & Jacklin 1974).The requisite skills are not displayed sp<strong>on</strong>taneously <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>vibrantly in public before an audience of resp<strong>on</strong>sivepeers, but quietly <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> obediently in <strong>the</strong> seclusi<strong>on</strong> of testrooms, typically with a paper <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pencil.Even <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>se tests, <strong>the</strong> female advantage has beennegligible; in <strong>the</strong>ir meta-analysis of 165 studies, Hyde<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Linn (1988) call <strong>the</strong> gender difference “so small thatit can effectively be c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be zero” (p. 64). Butwhere, <strong>on</strong>e might ask, is <strong>the</strong> male advantage that biologistshave come to expect? If it exists, male superiority would befound in vocal or verbal skills that are indicative of fitness.One would expect <strong>the</strong>se to be observable in <strong>the</strong> public useof language by adolescents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> young adults, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> this iswhere <strong>the</strong>y are witnessed. C<strong>on</strong>sistent with a sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, anthropological research, as we haveseen, reveals that performative applicati<strong>on</strong>s of languagein <strong>the</strong> form of speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> voice c<strong>on</strong>sistently favor males.Did performative ability increase fitness? Miller (2000)has argued that sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> shaped human languagedirectly (epigamically), through mate choice, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>indirectly, through its effects <strong>on</strong> social status.Verbal courtship can be viewed narrowly as face-to-face flirtati<strong>on</strong>,or broadly as anything we say in public that mightincrease our social status or pers<strong>on</strong>al attractiveness in <strong>the</strong>eyes of potential mates. Sexual flirtati<strong>on</strong> during early courtshipaccounts for <strong>on</strong>ly a small percentage of language use, but it is<strong>the</strong> percentage with <strong>the</strong> most important evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary effects.This is <strong>the</strong> time when <strong>the</strong> most important reproductivedecisi<strong>on</strong>s are made, when individuals are accepted or rejectedas sexual partners <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> basis of what <strong>the</strong>y say. (Miller 2000,pp. 356–57)Spoken language may also have played a role in sexualselecti<strong>on</strong> outside of courtship (intrasexually) by publicizingvarious male qualities. It was through public speaking <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>debate, according to Miller, that individuals were able toadvertise <strong>the</strong>ir knowledge, clear thinking, social tact, goodjudgment, wit, experience, morality, imaginati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> selfc<strong>on</strong>fidence.Under Pleistocene c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>the</strong> sexual incentivesfor advertising such qualities would have persisted throughoutadult <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, in almost every social situati<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> put minds<strong>on</strong> public display, where sexual choice could see <strong>the</strong>m clearlyfor <strong>the</strong> first time in evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>. (Miller 2000, p. 357)We agree that “language” helped to exhibit <strong>the</strong>sequalities, but it could not have d<strong>on</strong>e so without <strong>the</strong>assistance of a powerful accomplice, <strong>on</strong>e whose acti<strong>on</strong>has escaped <strong>the</strong> notice of most <strong>the</strong>oreticians. It wasspeech. Although speech is often thought of as little morethan a system for transmitting linguistically encoded informati<strong>on</strong>,it has qualities of its own, qualities that attractattenti<strong>on</strong>. Without attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>re will be few listeners,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> little exposure to <strong>the</strong> individual’s language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mind.In <strong>the</strong> case of speech, unlike language, <strong>the</strong>re are clear<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>sistent differences between males <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> females.6. Sex effects in speechWe have witnessed a str<strong>on</strong>g trend for verbal performancesto be carried out by adolescents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adults who are male.At <strong>the</strong>se same stages, however, females are more likely toengage in intimate talk, especially talk that includes selfdisclosure(Derlega et al. 1993; Mort<strong>on</strong> 1978), whe<strong>the</strong>rin traditi<strong>on</strong>al or modern societies.As we saw earlier, <strong>the</strong> dispositi<strong>on</strong> to gossip – that is, todisclose informati<strong>on</strong> about o<strong>the</strong>rs – also increases duringadolescence, especially in females. Studies of womenacross two educati<strong>on</strong>al levels, several different cultures,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> eight decades in <strong>the</strong> twentieth century agree thatadult women are also more likely to gossip than men(Bischoping 1993; also see Locke 2005). The reas<strong>on</strong> mayhave to do with <strong>the</strong> fact that gossip serves an affiliativefuncti<strong>on</strong> (Emler 2001), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that females tend to cometoge<strong>the</strong>r in times of stress (Taylor et al. 2000). InEngl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, gossip networks have existed for at least fivehundred years (Capp 2003). Membership in such networksis required for mutual aid, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> cost of membershipis willingness to gossip. The rules are simple,according to a report <strong>on</strong> modern English networks: “nogossip, no compani<strong>on</strong>ship” (Bott 1971). 10Although sex differences in speaking are frequentlyattributed to culture (Mey 1985), <strong>the</strong> patterns identifiedhere cut across cultures. The adolescent male attracti<strong>on</strong>to verbal dueling may also be supported by physiologicalfactors. C<strong>on</strong>sider <strong>the</strong> widely attested associati<strong>on</strong> betweentestoster<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> physical aggressi<strong>on</strong> (Dabbs 2000). Sinceadolescent males, who have relatively high levels of testoster<strong>on</strong>e,need ways to avoid hurting <strong>the</strong>mselves, it may beadaptive for <strong>the</strong>m to use verbal sport <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> humor in placeof combat (Marsh 1978). C<strong>on</strong>sistent with this possibilityis <strong>the</strong> fact that verbal duelists always “lose,” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tests are always stopped, if <strong>the</strong> participants becomeangry (Kochman 1969; 1983). Therefore, we speculatethat testoster<strong>on</strong>e promotes verbal dueling (see <strong>the</strong>reference to trial lawyers in sect. 8). 11When adolescent females wish to aggress againstano<strong>the</strong>r member of <strong>the</strong>ir sex, <strong>the</strong>y frequently use gossipto enlist <strong>the</strong> support of peers, greatly surpassing males inthis practice. Like males’ involvement in public performances,this difference between <strong>the</strong> sexes surfaces inlate juvenility, spans a number of different cultures, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>c<strong>on</strong>tinues through adolescence into adulthood (Björkqvistet al. 1992a; 1992b; 1994; Burbank 1994; also see Crick &Bigbee 1998; Galen & Underwood 1997).Private disclosures bear little resemblance to publictaunts, but <strong>the</strong>ir functi<strong>on</strong>s are similar. In gossip as wellas rap, verbal skill is used to raise <strong>on</strong>e’s own status, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>to lower that of competitors, while also broadcasting <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>enforcing community st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ards (Abrahams 1973). These270 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>sex differences betray dissociati<strong>on</strong>s between language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>speech, for, approaching maturity, males <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> females haveroughly <strong>the</strong> same knowledge of <strong>the</strong> structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “c<strong>on</strong>tents”of language but deploy that knowledge, throughspeech, in different ways. 12 In males, moreover, speechappears to play a role in courtship.7. CourtshipIf women deliver <strong>the</strong>ir first infant at age 19 but usuallybecome fertile <strong>on</strong>e to two years earlier, as cross-culturalevidence indicates (Bogin 2001), it is necessary to askwhat <strong>the</strong> vocal-verbal behavior of courting males is likeduring <strong>the</strong> intervening or preceding period. For it is atthis time that females are likely to make a choice.That <strong>the</strong> voice may play a role in courtship is suggestedby some research in n<strong>on</strong>human primates. Am<strong>on</strong>g babo<strong>on</strong>s,for example, <strong>the</strong>re is evidence that males who issue quietgrunts when approaching females are more likely toachieve affiliati<strong>on</strong> than those who approach without vocalizing(Palombit et al. 1999). In humans, Anolli <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ciceri(2002) found that successful (Italian) seducers beganaddressing <strong>the</strong>ir female targets with unusual levels ofloudness, “orotundity” having been found in o<strong>the</strong>r work<strong>on</strong> male vocalizati<strong>on</strong> to evoke percepti<strong>on</strong>s termed “energetic,”“healthy,” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “interesting,” al<strong>on</strong>g with severalo<strong>the</strong>r attractive qualities (Addingt<strong>on</strong> 1968). Then, as <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> proceeded, <strong>the</strong> men lowered <strong>the</strong>ir vocalintensity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> frequency, later returning to initial levels ofloudness, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a faster rate of articulati<strong>on</strong> – a variableassociated with perceived competence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> influence(Ray 1986; Scherer 1979; Smith et al. 1975) – by<strong>the</strong> end of <strong>the</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong>. 13There is also some anthropological evidence <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> roleof vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal behavior in courtship, much of it from<strong>the</strong> African-American community. The male teenagerwho extolled rap’s benefits (see sect. 4.3) went <strong>on</strong> to say,“you can rap to a young lady, you tryin’ to impress her,catch her acti<strong>on</strong> – you know – get wid her sex-wise”(Folb 1980, p. 91). Hannerz (1969), who studied rap inWashingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C., wrote that “A good line can attract<strong>the</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> of a woman who passes by in <strong>the</strong> street <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>open <strong>the</strong> way to a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>quest, while at <strong>the</strong> same timeit may impress o<strong>the</strong>r men with <strong>on</strong>e’s way with women”(p. 84; also see Kochman 1969). In Philadelphia, accordingto Abrahams (1970a), words “functi<strong>on</strong> powerfully in <strong>the</strong>sexual battle which is typical of adolescent <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>” (p. 44).For speech to figure into courtship, of course, femalesmust also have a corresp<strong>on</strong>ding perceptual preferencefor <strong>the</strong> speech of courting males (Endler & Basolo 1998;Fisher 1930; Harvey & Arnold 1982). A teenage girlfrom <strong>the</strong> same neighborhood as <strong>the</strong> rapper quoted abovesaid, “I likes to hear a bro<strong>the</strong>r who knows how to talk.D<strong>on</strong>’ hafta blow heavy, can sweet talk you too. D<strong>on</strong>’hafta make whole buncha sense, l<strong>on</strong>g sounds pretty”(Folb 1980, p. 101).If females are to evaluate <strong>the</strong> verbal or vocal qualities ofcourting males, as Miller (2000) suggested, <strong>the</strong>y need tohear <strong>the</strong>m speak. Males seem <strong>on</strong>ly too happy to provideopportunities. When men <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> women speak, men interruptmore often <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> hold <strong>the</strong> floor l<strong>on</strong>ger than women,even though females typically speak more often thanmen when engaged in same-sex c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s (Frances1979; James & Drakich 1993; Simkins-Bullock &Wildman 1991).But do women just wait for vocal or verbal cues tofitness, or do <strong>the</strong>y do something more actively? Ifwomen choose mates based, in part, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>ytalk, it would not be surprising if <strong>the</strong>y attempted to elicitspeech from male suitors (cf. Fisher 1930, p. 152). Ethologicalresearch suggests that several behaviors, includingfemale head nodding, smiling, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reclining, may functi<strong>on</strong>as displays whose effect is to encourage males to speak(Grammer et al. 2000; Kennedy & Camden 1983), thusto reveal informati<strong>on</strong> about <strong>the</strong>ir vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguisticskills, al<strong>on</strong>g with social, cognitive, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r abilities.Clearly, a great deal more needs to be learned about <strong>the</strong>role of verbal behavior in courtship. In <strong>the</strong> meantime, cluesto <strong>the</strong> role of verbal behavior in courtship emerge from <strong>the</strong>case of vocal or verbal deficits. Studies have c<strong>on</strong>sistentlyobserved negative social reacti<strong>on</strong>s to speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> voicedisorders, regardless of whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> listeners were peersor adults, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> errors were real or faked, or very mild(Crowe Hall 1991; DeThorne & Watkins 2001; Freeby &Madis<strong>on</strong> 1989; Lass et al. 1991a; 1991b; Mowrer et al.1978; Ruscello et al. 1988; Silverman 1976; Silverman &Paulus 1989). If this finding is taken with <strong>the</strong> fact thatmany children with language disorders present, in adolescence,with articulatory as well as pragmatic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performativelimitati<strong>on</strong>s (see sects. 10 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 11), <strong>the</strong>re would seem tobe plenty of raw material for a courtship problem. For,adolescents who cannot produce verbally complex materialin a timely fashi<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> with appropriate prosody, may beunable to impress, amuse, or dominate in <strong>the</strong> quest forstatus or reproductive advantage.Such would seem to be <strong>the</strong> case with stutterers, whousually have normal knowledge of language but speakdisfluently. Interviews indicate that stutterers, who arepredominantly male, fear <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> avoid courtship situati<strong>on</strong>s(Samps<strong>on</strong> et al. 2003) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> have difficulty forming <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>maintaining intimate relati<strong>on</strong>ships (Linn & Caruso1998). Women, for <strong>the</strong>ir part, discover that if <strong>the</strong>y marry astutterer <strong>the</strong>ir social <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> will be greatly limited (Boberg &Boberg 1990). In a survey of attitudes toward individualswith h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>icaps, <strong>on</strong>ly 7% of resp<strong>on</strong>dents said <strong>the</strong>y wouldbe willing to marry a severe stutterer (Shears & Jensema1969).8. Reproductive advantageIf speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language problems can compromise courtship,do <strong>the</strong>y reduce sexual opportunities? Recently, al<strong>on</strong>gitudinal investigati<strong>on</strong> was reported in which individualswith language disorders in childhood were seenagain in <strong>the</strong>ir mid-twenties. It was found that well overhalf of <strong>the</strong> subjects had problems in establishing sp<strong>on</strong>taneous,reciprocal relati<strong>on</strong>ships. More than <strong>on</strong>e-third of<strong>the</strong> subjects had no particular friends <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> two-thirds hadnever had sexual intercourse – a marked deficiency comparedto statistics <strong>on</strong> comparably aged members of <strong>the</strong>populati<strong>on</strong> at large. Two-thirds also c<strong>on</strong>tinued to live athome with <strong>the</strong>ir parents (Howlin et al. 2000).Were evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary ancestors with c<strong>on</strong>spicuous vocal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal talent reproductively advantaged? It may beworth revisiting two relati<strong>on</strong>ships of <strong>the</strong> sort that undergirdcompetitive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performative applicati<strong>on</strong>s of speech.BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 271


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>In <strong>the</strong> first, men who speak in a way that secures attenti<strong>on</strong>are also likely to be high in dominance, as we saw insecti<strong>on</strong> 4.2. The sec<strong>on</strong>d relati<strong>on</strong>ship is between <strong>the</strong>attractive or forceful use of speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testoster<strong>on</strong>e.Testoster<strong>on</strong>e is correlated with, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> may increase, <strong>the</strong>boldness with which individuals enter a room, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>tendency to work in public arenas, such as <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>atricalstage or courtroom. Actors generally display high levelsof testoster<strong>on</strong>e (Dabbs et al. 1990; 2001), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> triallawyers have 30% more testoster<strong>on</strong>e than o<strong>the</strong>r types oflawyers (Dabbs et al. 1998).The more specific link to speech is that trial lawyers, asPhilbrick (1949) wrote, “exercise <strong>the</strong>ir power in court bymanipulating <strong>the</strong> thoughts <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> opini<strong>on</strong>s of o<strong>the</strong>rs” (p. v).They are, according to Dabbs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> his colleagues, “goodat presenting c<strong>on</strong>crete details in a straight-talking <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>compelling way that dramatically captures <strong>the</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong>of a jury in trial court” (Dabbs et al. 1998, p. 91). Thereare obvious similarities between trial lawyers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>“Big men” of traditi<strong>on</strong>al societies. In 1984, <strong>the</strong>re were3,827 members of <strong>the</strong> American Associati<strong>on</strong> of TrialLawyers. Men comprised 99.9% of <strong>the</strong> membership(Walter 1988), which is nearly 20% more males thanworked as primary care physicians in <strong>the</strong> following year(Franks & Bertakis 2003). 14 This research suggests thatwomen who are attracted to men with a comm<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ingway of speaking may get a mate for <strong>the</strong>mselves,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fa<strong>the</strong>r for <strong>the</strong>ir children, who has high levels oftestoster<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> status, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> unusual access to valuedresources.There are o<strong>the</strong>r, more specific c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s betweenaspects of <strong>the</strong> male voice <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> several indices of fitness.These include links between depth of vocal frequency<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> both testoster<strong>on</strong>e (Dabbs & Mallinger 1999; Pedersenet al. 1986) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testis volume (Harries et al. 1997), wheretestoster<strong>on</strong>e levels independently predict coital frequency(Halpern et al. 1993; 1998; Udry 1988), social expressivity(Dabbs & Ruback 1988), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social dominance (Schaalet al. 1996). Reduced formant dispersi<strong>on</strong> is also correlatedwith skull <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> body size in primates, <strong>the</strong> magnitudebeing greater in macaques (Fitch 1997) than in humans(G<strong>on</strong>zález 2004). It is thus unsurprising that our adaptiveancestors availed <strong>the</strong>mselves of this particular medium,which could broadcast fitness informati<strong>on</strong> while simultaneouslytransmitting <strong>the</strong> units of language, to saynothing of <strong>the</strong> social, indexical, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> emoti<strong>on</strong>al informati<strong>on</strong>that would have been embedded <strong>the</strong>rein (Cheney &Seyfarth 2005b). Recently, it was reported that in bothmen <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> women, opposite-sex ratings of vocal attractivenesspredicted age of first sexual intercourse, numberof sexual partners, number of competing sexual relati<strong>on</strong>ships,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> number of partners that were involved ino<strong>the</strong>r sexual relati<strong>on</strong>ships (Hughes et al. 2004). Theauthors c<strong>on</strong>cluded that “voice may have been an importantparameter of mate choice” in human evoluti<strong>on</strong> (Hugheset al. 2004, p. 303).In <strong>the</strong> previous secti<strong>on</strong>s, we identified several differentforms of selecti<strong>on</strong> that may have applied across <strong>the</strong> wholeof <strong>development</strong>, from infancy to sexual maturity, including<strong>the</strong> uniquely human stages of childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence.Our goal in doing so was to offer an evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary accountof our species’ capacity to improvise, learn, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trolcomplex vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s. It is appropriate now that weexamine <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages <strong>the</strong>mselves.9. Evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> stagesWe have proposed that when childhood exp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ed, <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>opportunities arose, especially within families, for <strong>the</strong>negotiati<strong>on</strong> of more structured <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complex forms of vocalizati<strong>on</strong>;<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that with a premium in this c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>on</strong> h<strong>on</strong>estsignaling, benefits would have accrued to families thatwere able to deploy <strong>the</strong>se more complex forms meaningfully,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thus to warn, advise, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> inform each o<strong>the</strong>r.We have fur<strong>the</strong>r proposed that this material, arisingfrom infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> exp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing in childhood, persisted intojuvenility when it c<strong>on</strong>tributed to teasing, joking, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>gossip in both sexes, behaviors that would have facilitatedachievement of group-oriented goals; as well as into adolescence,when performative skills c<strong>on</strong>tributed to intrasexualcompetiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> epigamic selecti<strong>on</strong>, reinforcingall previous <strong>development</strong>s <strong>on</strong> which those skills depend.We have proposed relati<strong>on</strong>ships between language, atrait unique to humans, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> two <strong>on</strong>togenetic stages thatare also unique to humans. To underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>of language, <strong>the</strong>refore, we believe it may be necessaryto ask how <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> when <strong>the</strong>se stages came to exist, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thiswe attempt to do here. In succeeding secti<strong>on</strong>s, we look atevidence for <strong>development</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tinuity between <strong>the</strong> levelsof language, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interc<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> rules <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> skills that influence its use, offering speculati<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong> how <strong>the</strong>se c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong>s may have come about.It is obviously difficult to rec<strong>on</strong>struct <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages from fossil remains al<strong>on</strong>e. Fortunately,it is possible to draw relevant inferences from syn<strong>the</strong>sesof comparative anatomical, physiological, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ethologicaldata, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> from archaeological findings. This work is facilitatedby parametric data <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> brain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> body growth ofn<strong>on</strong>human primates, as well as of humans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir ancestors(cf. Harvey & Clutt<strong>on</strong>-Brock 1985; Harvey et al. 1987;Martin 1983).9.1. Evoluti<strong>on</strong> of childhoodWith an <strong>on</strong>togenetic approach to linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong>,structures o<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> skull become important. Forexample, teeth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> jaws are useful because of <strong>the</strong> str<strong>on</strong>gcorrelati<strong>on</strong> between tooth formati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> erupti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>o<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> events (Smith & Tompkins 1995).Figure 3 is an attempt to represent <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> ofhuman <strong>development</strong>, though at present <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly reliabledata are associated with Pan <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> H. sapiens. Known orestimated adult brain sizes are given at <strong>the</strong> top of eachbar. Mean age at erupti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> first permanent molar(M1) is graphed across <strong>the</strong> histograms, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> identifiednumerically at <strong>the</strong> base (Smith & Tompkins 1995).Appearing about 3.9 milli<strong>on</strong> years ago, Australopi<strong>the</strong>cusafarensis shares many anatomical features with n<strong>on</strong>homininp<strong>on</strong>gid (ape) species, including an adult brainsize of about 400 cc <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a pattern of dental <strong>development</strong>indistinguishable from extant chimpanzees (C<strong>on</strong>roy &Vannier 1991; Dean et al. 2001; Sim<strong>on</strong>s 1989; Smith1991). Therefore, <strong>the</strong> chimpanzee <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> A. afarensis aredepicted as sharing <strong>the</strong> typical tripartite stages of postnatalgrowth of social mammals: infancy, juvenility, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adulthood(Pereira & Fairbanks 1993). Following <strong>the</strong> definiti<strong>on</strong>soffered earlier, infancy represents <strong>the</strong> period offeeding by lactati<strong>on</strong>, juvenility represents a period offeeding independence prior to sexual maturati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>272 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Figure 3. The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of hominin <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> during <strong>the</strong> first 20 years of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> (original figure from Bogin 1999b; see <strong>the</strong> text foradditi<strong>on</strong>al sources of data). P/A ¼ Pan <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Australopi<strong>the</strong>cus afarensis; Aa ¼ Australopi<strong>the</strong>cus africanus; Hh ¼ Homo habilis;He1 ¼ early Homo erectus; He2 ¼ late Homo erectus; Hs¼ Homo sapiens. Mean brain sizes are given at <strong>the</strong> top of each histogram(calculated from several sources; e.g., Relethford 2006). Mean age at erupti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> first permanent molar (M1) is graphed across<strong>the</strong> histograms, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> identified numerically at <strong>the</strong> base (Smith & Tompkins 1995).adulthood begins following puberty <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexual maturati<strong>on</strong>.The durati<strong>on</strong> of each stage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> age at which eachstage ends are based <strong>on</strong> empirical data for chimpanzees.A probable descendent of A. afarensis is <strong>the</strong> fossilspecies A. africanus, dating from about three milli<strong>on</strong>years ago. Achievement of <strong>the</strong> larger adult brain size ofA. africanus (average of 442 cc) may have required anadditi<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> length of <strong>the</strong> fetal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>/or infancyperiods. Figure 3 depicts a <strong>on</strong>e-year extensi<strong>on</strong> of infancy.As <strong>the</strong> figure indicates, <strong>the</strong> first permanent molar (M1)of <strong>the</strong> chimpanzee erupts at age 3.1 years, even thoughinfancy c<strong>on</strong>tinues for nearly two more years. Before age5 years, young chimpanzees are dependent <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>mo<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> will not survive if she dies or becomesunable to provide care <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> food (Goodall 1983; Nishidaet al. 1990). After <strong>the</strong> erupti<strong>on</strong> of M1, <strong>the</strong>y may be ableto manage an adult diet but still must learn how to find<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> process foods, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> it takes time to learn how toopen shelled fruits <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> extract insects from nests. Thismay be why chimpanzees extend infancy bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong>erupti<strong>on</strong> of M1. It is likely that early hominins, such asA. afarensis <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> A. africanus, followed a pattern ofgrowth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> very similar to chimpanzees<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> also extended infancy for at least <strong>on</strong>e year bey<strong>on</strong>d<strong>the</strong> age of M1 erupti<strong>on</strong>. Analyses of postcranial anatomy<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> archaeological records suggest a similarity between<strong>the</strong> behavioral capacities of australopi<strong>the</strong>cine-grade hominins<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> extant chimpanzees (Plummer 2004; Potts 1988).Tardieu (1998) has shown that <strong>the</strong> pattern of femurgrowth in H. habilis departed from that of <strong>the</strong> australopi<strong>the</strong>cinesbut resembled <strong>the</strong> pattern seen in later hominins.The distinctive femur shape of <strong>the</strong> more recenthominins, she suggests, is due to a prol<strong>on</strong>gati<strong>on</strong> of childhoodstage of growth. Thus, we speculate that <strong>the</strong>process of linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong> took a step forward abouttwo milli<strong>on</strong> years ago with <strong>the</strong> el<strong>on</strong>gati<strong>on</strong> of childhood.O<strong>the</strong>rs have made a similar suggesti<strong>on</strong>, although <strong>the</strong>ywere acting <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> assumpti<strong>on</strong> that <strong>the</strong> critical variablewas enlargement of <strong>the</strong> adult brain (e.g., Mi<strong>the</strong>n 1996).A childhood stage for <strong>the</strong> earliest members of <strong>the</strong> genusHomo is also supported by a comparis<strong>on</strong> of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> apereproductive strategies. There are limits to <strong>the</strong> delaybetween birth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexual maturity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> between successfulbirths, that any species can tolerate. Am<strong>on</strong>g wild chimpanzees,for example, females reach menarche at 11 to 12years of age <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> have <strong>the</strong>ir first births at an average age of14. The average period between successful births in <strong>the</strong>wild is 5.6 years, as infant chimpanzees are dependent<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>rs for about five years. Actuarial data collected<strong>on</strong> wild-living animals indicate that 35–38% of alllive-born chimpanzees survive to <strong>the</strong>ir mid-20s. The chimpanzeethus operates at reproductive threshold. 15The great apes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fossil hominins, such as Australopi<strong>the</strong>cus,may have created this demographic dilemmaby forestalling weaning. We suggest that early Homo overcamethis reproductive limit by reducing <strong>the</strong> length ofinfancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> inserting childhood between infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>juvenility. Free from <strong>the</strong> dem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s of nursing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>physiological brake that frequent nursing places up<strong>on</strong> ovulati<strong>on</strong>,mo<strong>the</strong>rs could reproduce so<strong>on</strong> after <strong>the</strong>ir progenyreached childhood, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> indeed this is what happens inmodern humans. Am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Ju/’hoasi, a traditi<strong>on</strong>alhunting <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ga<strong>the</strong>ring society of sou<strong>the</strong>rn Africa, awoman’s age at first birth averages 19 years. Birthsc<strong>on</strong>tinue, <strong>on</strong> average, every 3.6 years, resulting in a fertilityrate of 4.7 children per woman (Howell 1979; Short 1976).Am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Hadza, <strong>the</strong> inter-birth interval is even shorter,as women stop nursing about <strong>on</strong>e year earlier, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> average6.15 births per woman (Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es et al. 1992).A brief childhood stage for H. habilis is <strong>the</strong>refore indicatedin Figure 3. This stage begins after <strong>the</strong> erupti<strong>on</strong> ofM1 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lasts for about <strong>on</strong>e year. Even <strong>on</strong>e year of childhoodwould have provided reproductive advantages to<strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r. A behavioral c<strong>on</strong>sequence of reproductiveselecti<strong>on</strong> for childhood may have accrued to <strong>the</strong> child,that is, learning how to find <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> process adult types offoods while still protected by older individuals. Duringthis learning phase, H. habilis children would need to besupplied with special weaning foods. There is archaeologicalevidence for just such a scenario. H. habilis seems tohave intensified its dependence <strong>on</strong> st<strong>on</strong>e tools. For,<strong>the</strong>re are more st<strong>on</strong>e tools, more carefully manufacturedBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 273


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>tools, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a greater diversity of st<strong>on</strong>e tool types associatedwith H. habilis than with any earlier hominins (Klein1989). There is c<strong>on</strong>siderable evidence that some of <strong>the</strong>setools were used to scavenge animal carcasses, especiallyto break open l<strong>on</strong>g b<strong>on</strong>es <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> extract b<strong>on</strong>e marrow(Plummer 2004; Potts 1988). This behavior may be interpretedas a strategy to feed children. Such scavenging mayhave been needed to provide <strong>the</strong> essential amino acids,some of <strong>the</strong> minerals, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, especially, <strong>the</strong> fat (densesource of energy) that children require for growth of <strong>the</strong>brain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> body (Le<strong>on</strong>ard & Roberts<strong>on</strong> 1992; Le<strong>on</strong>ardet al. 2003).A fur<strong>the</strong>r increase in brain size occurred during <strong>the</strong> timeof H. erectus, which began about 1.9 milli<strong>on</strong> years ago. Theearliest adult specimens have mean brain sizes of 826 cc,but many individual adults had brain sizes between 840<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 1059 cc (Coqueugnoit et al. 2004). Inserti<strong>on</strong> or expansi<strong>on</strong>of childhood would have provided <strong>the</strong> time needed fora rapid, human-like, pattern of postnatal brain growth. 16It should be noted in Figure 3 that <strong>the</strong> model of humanevoluti<strong>on</strong> proposed here predicts that from Australopi<strong>the</strong>custo H. erectus infancy shrinks as childhood exp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s. In<strong>the</strong> time of early H. erectus <strong>the</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong> from infancy tochildhood took place before M1 erupti<strong>on</strong>. If <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> childhoodwas stolen from infancy, or reduced its length, <strong>the</strong>nH. erectus would have enjoyed a greater reproductiveadvantage than any previous hominin. This seems tohave been <strong>the</strong> case, since H. erectus populati<strong>on</strong>s increasedin size <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> began to spread throughout Africa <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>rregi<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong> Old World (Antón 2003).Figure 3 also shows later H. erectus, with an averageadult brain size of 983 cc <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a fur<strong>the</strong>r expansi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong>childhood stage. In additi<strong>on</strong> to larger brains (somemeasuring 1100 cc), <strong>the</strong> archaeological record for laterH. erectus shows increased complexity of technology(tools, fire, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> shelter) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social organizati<strong>on</strong> (Antón2003; Klein 1989). These techno-social advances, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> aparallel increase in learning, may well reflect extendedchildhood (Bogin & Smith 1996). The evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary transiti<strong>on</strong>to archaic, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> finally, modern H. sapiens exp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s<strong>the</strong> childhood stage to its current length.As for how childhood abetted language, we speculatethat <strong>the</strong>re was a process of coevoluti<strong>on</strong>, according towhich an initially short childhood period facilitated <strong>the</strong>emergence of a small amount of vocal-verbal behavior.Even if <strong>the</strong> original cause of childhood was n<strong>on</strong>linguistic,as we propose, any <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> time between infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juvenilitywould have provided <strong>the</strong> young with additi<strong>on</strong>al opportunitiesfor vocal-verbal interacti<strong>on</strong> during a time ofincreased independence, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> would have given adults<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> reas<strong>on</strong>s to h<strong>on</strong>estly inform any dependents in <strong>the</strong>ircare (Fitch 2004). The dispositi<strong>on</strong> to do so is suggestedby <strong>the</strong> fact that primate mo<strong>the</strong>rs appear to prevent <strong>the</strong>iryoung from eating alien substances, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> warn off younganimals that approach toxic fruits (Caro & Hauser 1992).Young hominins also would have needed to know aboutplants as well as game, tools, shelter, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> predators.Even a small amount of vocal-verbal behavior wouldhave facilitated warnings <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructi<strong>on</strong>. Childhoodleng<strong>the</strong>ned, enabling more language which, in turn,extended childhood, until <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>togenetic stagereached its present length. Childhood would thus haveoffered up <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> behaviors, <strong>on</strong>es that would also provebeneficial in <strong>the</strong> run up to sexual maturity.9.2. Evoluti<strong>on</strong> of adolescenceThis brings us to <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r uniquelyhuman stage of <strong>development</strong>. N<strong>on</strong>human primates lackchildhood, but <strong>the</strong>y also lack a post-juvenile period of dramaticgrowth of <strong>the</strong> sort that defines human adolescence.Unfortunately, very little is known about <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> ofadolescence. It is possible, however, to suggest somebenefits of this late stage of <strong>development</strong> in terms of reproductivebiology, social ecology, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reproductive success.Our claim, detailed elsewhere (Bogin 1999a; 1999b;2003), is that adolescence became part of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> because it c<strong>on</strong>ferred significant reproductiveadvantages <strong>on</strong> our evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary ancestors, in part by allowingadolescents to learn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice adult ec<strong>on</strong>omic,social, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexual behaviors before reproducing. As indicatedin secti<strong>on</strong> 2.7, <strong>the</strong> basic argument for <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> benefits of human adolescence is that girls bestlearn <strong>the</strong>ir adult social roles while <strong>the</strong>y are infertile butperceived by adults to be mature, whereas boys bestlearn <strong>the</strong>ir adult social roles while <strong>the</strong>y are sexuallymature but not yet perceived to be such by adults. Theproposed patterns corresp<strong>on</strong>d exactly to <strong>the</strong> course ofgrowth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> taken by boys <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> girls. Onaverage, healthy girls begin to develop adult-like fatpatterns (e.g., breast, hips) at about age 10 to 11 years,which is two years before <strong>the</strong> peak of <strong>the</strong> adolescentspurt. Menarche occurs a year after <strong>the</strong> spurt <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> isfollowed by about three years of adolescent sterility,ovulati<strong>on</strong> being rare or absent. The female pelvis alsogrows slowly during adolescence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> does not reach fulladult size, a necessity for successful birth, until aboutage 18 years. Externally, however, <strong>the</strong>se infertile girlslook like women <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> this percepti<strong>on</strong> prompts adults toinclude <strong>the</strong> girls in a suite of adult social, ec<strong>on</strong>omic,sexual, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> political activities. The girls learn from involvementin <strong>the</strong>se activities <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> arrive at young adulthood, atabout age 19 years, with c<strong>on</strong>siderable experience thattranslates into reproductive success. This may be whyhumans have <strong>the</strong> highest rate of birth survival of anyspecies: 50 to 60% in traditi<strong>on</strong>al foraging societies comparedwith 35% in chimpanzees (Lancaster & Lancaster1983). 17In <strong>the</strong> reverse pattern that characterizes male adolescence,fertile sperm are produced at about 13.5 years ofage, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> yet external features of male adults (body size,muscularity, body hair, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r sexual features) do notdevelop until about age 18 years. The horm<strong>on</strong>es thatcause spermatogenesis may prime boys to be emoti<strong>on</strong>ally<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitively attentive to <strong>the</strong> behavior of adult men<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> women. In most human societies, boys pass throughseveral rites of passage that help <strong>the</strong>m learn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> practiceimportant adult male ec<strong>on</strong>omic, social, political, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>reproductive activities. By age 20 or so, <strong>the</strong>se boys graduateto manhood, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> are expected to compete successfullywith o<strong>the</strong>r men <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to assume <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>sibilitiesassociated with marriage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> paternity.As for when adolescence evolved, it is possible that thisstage first appeared in H. erectus, a hominin that originallyevolved in Africa more than <strong>on</strong>e milli<strong>on</strong> years ago (Antón&Leigh 2003). The evidence for this is patterns of tooth formati<strong>on</strong>that are not directly linked to <strong>the</strong> presence of anadolescent growth spurt, but are n<strong>on</strong>e<strong>the</strong>less suggestive.O<strong>the</strong>r research indicates that H. erectus, later hominins274 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>such as H. antesesor (800,000 BP) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> H. heidelbergensis(400 – 500,000 BP), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> even <strong>the</strong> Ne<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ertals of40,000 BP grew up too quickly to have adolescence(Ramirez-Rossi & Bermudez de Castro 2004). Wheneveradolescence evolved, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> it will take a good deal moreevidence to be sure, an <strong>on</strong>togenetic approach would ascribemodern human language to some more recent period.10. Pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performanceWe have sampled evidence indicating that knowledge of<strong>the</strong> basic structure of language is usually present by <strong>the</strong>end of childhood. We have also seen that <strong>the</strong> skillneeded to use language adaptively in social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexualsituati<strong>on</strong>s is not c<strong>on</strong>spicuous until adolescence. Thus, webelieve it is possible that important aspects of languagenot <strong>on</strong>ly do not develop until sexual maturity, but cannotdo so because biological functi<strong>on</strong>s associated with thatstage played an evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary role in <strong>the</strong>ir c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>,much as <strong>the</strong>y now play a role in <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>development</strong>.Many of <strong>the</strong> changes occurring in adolescence (as wesaw in sect. 2.8) fall into <strong>the</strong> categories of pragmatics<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance. Pragmatics refers to <strong>the</strong> inference ofspeakers’ intenti<strong>on</strong>s from <strong>the</strong> literal meaning of <strong>the</strong>ir utterances(Austin 1975; Searle 1972). Inasmuch as <strong>the</strong> abilityto infer intenti<strong>on</strong>s presupposes real-world knowledge,pragmatics is <strong>the</strong> area of linguistic communicati<strong>on</strong> thatmust be, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> is, <strong>the</strong> last to develop.It is also <strong>the</strong> area of human language that has leastappealed to Linguistics, which has been more c<strong>on</strong>cernedwith grammar (Haberl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> & Mey 1977). With grammaras <strong>the</strong> central focus, <strong>the</strong>re may have been less reas<strong>on</strong> toanalyze <strong>the</strong> things that people actually say, for, asNewmeyer (2004) wrote, utterance material “is all butuseless for providing insights into <strong>the</strong> grammar of anyindividual speaker” (p. 698).Clinicians have reinforced this bias by restricting <strong>the</strong>mselvesto st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardized tests, which typically exclude pragmatics.Ten years ago, several clinical investigators wrotethat pragmatics “defies assessment under st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardizedc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s” (Tomblin et al. 1996, p. 1286). This combinati<strong>on</strong>of biases <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>straints facilitated <strong>the</strong> transformati<strong>on</strong>of “language” from a vibrant form of social acti<strong>on</strong> intoa stable mental code that fit <strong>the</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al bias of literatecultures, with additi<strong>on</strong>al advantages to scholars, whowished to study its organizati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> clinicians, wh<strong>on</strong>eeded to measure it.Still, infants who are slow to develop <strong>the</strong> linguistic codefrequently experience pragmatic difficulties in childhood(Ninio et al. 1994; Prutting & Kirchner 1987). In somecases, pragmatic problems are correlated with lingeringlinguistic deficiencies (Le<strong>on</strong>ard 1986; Tomblin et al.1996); in o<strong>the</strong>r cases, pragmatic deficits seem to emergeafter earlier language deficiencies have resolved(Girolametto et al. 2001), suggesting some degree of independencebetween grammar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pragmatics (Culatta et al.1983; Schelletter & Lein<strong>on</strong>en 2003). These differences arepartially explained by <strong>the</strong> fact that pragmatics encompassesa number of diverse behaviors, some more closely dependent<strong>on</strong> linguistic knowledge than o<strong>the</strong>rs (McTear &C<strong>on</strong>ti-Ramsden 1992).In juvenility or adolescence, individuals with a <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<strong>development</strong>al language delay may appear to normalize,based <strong>on</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ardized tests, but many are ineffectivewhen it comes to <strong>the</strong> verbally performative behaviorsthat blossom during those stages. Bergman (1987) hasitemized <strong>the</strong>se deficits in adolescence:When communicati<strong>on</strong> deficits persist into adolescence, disorderedlanguage may produce increasingly noxious social c<strong>on</strong>sequences.Inability to keep abreast of verbal exchange usuallysets <strong>the</strong> language disordered adolescent apart socially, withresultant feelings of l<strong>on</strong>eliness <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of being misunderstood.These youths are unable to acquire in-group slang expressi<strong>on</strong>s;are likely to misunderst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> metaphors, jokes, puns, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sarcasticremarks; may be unable to follow verbal dialogues; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> areusually poor in rapid humorous verbal exchanges. Speech maybe rambling, imprecise, fragmented, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>/or tangential. Typicallyaware of having communicati<strong>on</strong> problems, languagedisordered adolescents may be reluctant to c<strong>on</strong>tribute toc<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s, be socially reticent, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> avoid resp<strong>on</strong>dingthrough silence, off-h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ed gestures, or saying “d<strong>on</strong>’t know.”(p. 162)Several of <strong>the</strong> performative deficits claimed by Bergmanhave since been documented by researchers (Bishop 1989;Fujiki & Brint<strong>on</strong> 1991; Ninio et al. 1994; Nippold 1998;Paul 1995). Still, <strong>the</strong>y may escape <strong>the</strong> notice of teachers,clinicians, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r resp<strong>on</strong>sible adults because, as wesaw earlier, verbal performances are frequently heldwhen <strong>the</strong>se people are not around.Individuals who are able to carry out linguistic operati<strong>on</strong>sthat occur early in infancy, such as lexical storage<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> object naming, do not necessarily have what it takesto satisfy <strong>the</strong> performative criteria associated with subsequentstages – at least not <strong>on</strong> formal grounds. But inevoluti<strong>on</strong>, individuals who achieved <strong>the</strong> ability toperform attractively <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> competitively by adolescencewould surely have benefited from doing so, indirectlyreinforcing previous accomplishment in earlier stages.As Hogan (1988) pointed out, selecti<strong>on</strong> for behaviors insexual maturity automatically credits relevant <strong>development</strong>sin earlier stages as well.11. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speechInsofar as selecti<strong>on</strong> for speech reinforced previous linguistic<strong>development</strong>s, it may also have streng<strong>the</strong>ned c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>sbetween articulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammar. For <strong>on</strong>e thing,<strong>the</strong>re is evidence suggesting that “language” problems ininfancy may persist as speech, or even purely articulatory,problems in adolescence (Beitchman et al. 1994). In al<strong>on</strong>gitudinal investigati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ducted in Engl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, adolescentswho had been linguistically disordered in childhoodscored significantly lower than c<strong>on</strong>trols <strong>on</strong> a variety oflanguage measures, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> were poor at repeating n<strong>on</strong>words,sentences, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> articulatorily complex material (Stothardet al. 1998). In a similar study carried out in Sweden,language-disordered six-year-olds were still behind at <strong>the</strong>age of 18, especially when it came to repeating l<strong>on</strong>g <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>complicated words, which <strong>the</strong>y did slowly <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> err<strong>on</strong>eously(Nauclér & Magnuss<strong>on</strong> 2002). In <strong>the</strong> United States, agroup of elementary schoolchildren with carefully evaluatedarticulatory disorders was seen 28 years later, at whichtime <strong>the</strong>re was evidence of c<strong>on</strong>tinuing problems witharticulati<strong>on</strong> (distorti<strong>on</strong>s of /s/, /z/, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> /r/ were plentiful),as well as reduced performance <strong>on</strong> tests of languagecomprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intelligence. Significantly, <strong>the</strong>seadults also evinced abnormalities in <strong>the</strong> area of prosody,BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 275


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>ir speech striking listeners as slow <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> m<strong>on</strong>ot<strong>on</strong>ous(Felsenfeld et al. 1992).Findings in behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> molecular genetics alsosupport links between language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech, whilehinting at previously unexamined c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s ofspeech to o<strong>the</strong>r linguistic domains. In studies of m<strong>on</strong>ozygotic<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dizygotic twins, for example, <strong>the</strong> highest ratesof c<strong>on</strong>cordance have come from twins in whom <strong>the</strong> disorderwas primarily expressive; some had disorders that werepurely of <strong>the</strong> articulatory type (Bishop et al. 1995; Lewis1990; Lewis & Thomps<strong>on</strong> 1992). In typically developing7- to 13-year-old children, Bishop (2001) also found thatMZ twins were more c<strong>on</strong>cordant than DZ twins <strong>on</strong> rateof speeded articulati<strong>on</strong> of polysyllabic words. These findingsseem to fit with <strong>the</strong> possibility that selecti<strong>on</strong> acted<strong>on</strong> performance, thus <strong>on</strong> factors relating to precisi<strong>on</strong>,speed, complexity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fluency of articulati<strong>on</strong>. In doingso, it may have reinforced those behaviors, indirectlyenhancing related acquisiti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>s thathad occurred earlier in <strong>development</strong>.The role played by producti<strong>on</strong> factors has also beenhighlighted by studies of a particular family in Engl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>.In <strong>the</strong> early 1990s, researchers discovered <strong>the</strong> KE family,which had serious communicati<strong>on</strong> disorders at each of itsthree living generati<strong>on</strong>s. Although <strong>the</strong>se problemsincluded a severe oral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal dyspraxia, accompaniedby deficits at o<strong>the</strong>r levels of language (Fletcher 1990;Hurst et al. 1990; Vargha-Khadem 1990), a grammaticallyfocused investigati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> family found precisely what itwas looking for: grammatical problems (Gopnik 1990;Gopnik & Crago 1991). Nearly a decade later, geneticistsfound a defective gene in <strong>the</strong> family, FOXP2, which is situated<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g arm of chromosome 7 (Lai et al. 2000;2001). But, in parallel with <strong>the</strong> genetics work, o<strong>the</strong>rteams of clinical investigators c<strong>on</strong>firmed <strong>the</strong> dyspraxicdisorder, which involved both speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>speechmovements of <strong>the</strong> articulators (Vargha-Khadem et al.1995; 1998). In functi<strong>on</strong>al imaging studies, affectedfamily members revealed significant underactivati<strong>on</strong> ofBroca’s area in both hemispheres, c<strong>on</strong>sistent with adeficit of speech producti<strong>on</strong> (Liégeous et al. 2003).What is relevant here is that <strong>the</strong> primary problem of <strong>the</strong>KE family was rec<strong>on</strong>ceptualized as an oral-motor difficultythat was accompanied by grammatical difficulties. Several<strong>the</strong>orists toyed with <strong>the</strong> idea that <strong>the</strong> KE family’s oralmotordifficulties caused <strong>the</strong>ir grammatical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>difficulties. “One possibility,” wrote Watkinset al. (2002), “is that <strong>the</strong> deviant articulati<strong>on</strong> results inpoor ph<strong>on</strong>ology, rendering morphological producti<strong>on</strong>difficult” (p. 461). “It might be <strong>the</strong> case,” Marcus <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Fisher (2003) speculated, “that a deficit restricted to <strong>the</strong>motor system is fully resp<strong>on</strong>sible for <strong>the</strong> wide-rangingprofile of impairment” (p. 261). There could have been a“flow-<strong>on</strong> effect from articulati<strong>on</strong> to syntax to comprehensi<strong>on</strong>,”wrote Corballis (2004, p. 548).How did our ancestors get from articulated ph<strong>on</strong>ati<strong>on</strong> tosyntax? According to Bickert<strong>on</strong> (2000):The ability to make <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> distinguish a greater range of speechsounds would make possible a wider variety of sound combinati<strong>on</strong>s,which, given a larger <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more efficient memory forwords, would give rise to a steadily increasing vocabulary.Undoubtedly, <strong>the</strong>se factors <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> processes would have combinedto yield a much richer means of communicati<strong>on</strong>am<strong>on</strong>g hominins. (p. 157)This would not automatically produce syntax, asBickert<strong>on</strong> noted, but it may have led to o<strong>the</strong>r changesthat favored ph<strong>on</strong>ology, beginning with <strong>the</strong> ritualizati<strong>on</strong>of vocal patterns (Locke 2004a; in press b; Oller 2004;Richman 2000) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ultimately <strong>the</strong>ir segmentati<strong>on</strong> intodiscrete linguistic units (Studdert-Kennedy 1998; 2005;Studdert-Kennedy & Goldstein 2003).12. What bel<strong>on</strong>gs in <strong>the</strong> language faculty?Evidence examined earlier revealed c<strong>on</strong>tinuity between<strong>the</strong> lexical delays of infancy – even where <strong>the</strong>y appearedto resolve – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> pragmatic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performative problemsof adolescence. If we attempt to account for this c<strong>on</strong>tinuity,two possibilities emerge immediately. Accordingto <strong>the</strong> first, pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance are social applicati<strong>on</strong>sof linguistic knowledge, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thus depend <strong>on</strong> thatknowledge. This could explain <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>al associati<strong>on</strong>between scores <strong>on</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard tests of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>experimental measures of pragmatics.A more satisfying possibility is that delayed lexical <strong>development</strong>points to a weakened faculty of language, sinceeven <strong>the</strong> earliest use of words requires <strong>the</strong> acti<strong>on</strong> of mechanismsthat bel<strong>on</strong>g to this faculty. This is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with aparadox about language <strong>development</strong>. Infants rarely vocalizeat normal levels of frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complexity during<strong>the</strong> babbling stage; <strong>the</strong>y imitate aspects of <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>r’sspeech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> produce isolated words – behaviors thatostensibly require no grammatical ability at all – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>n stumble as <strong>the</strong>y enter <strong>the</strong> domains of morphology<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> syntax (Locke 1998a). Whatever problems arise at<strong>the</strong> grammatical level of language are typically forecastby deficiencies in early lexical <strong>development</strong> (Bates &Goodman 1997), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> possibly even <strong>the</strong> precursors toword learning (Oller et al. 1999). These facts suggestthat <strong>the</strong> earlier <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> later behaviors, as different as <strong>the</strong>yare, bel<strong>on</strong>g to a system of linked neural resources, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>that weakness in <strong>the</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong> of an early comp<strong>on</strong>entindexes weakness in o<strong>the</strong>rs, including <strong>the</strong> performativeareas that happen to develop late. According to this explanati<strong>on</strong>,early lexical delay predicts problems of usage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>interpretati<strong>on</strong> in adolescence even if <strong>the</strong> delay, by testsof structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tent, seems to resolve l<strong>on</strong>g beforethat stage. The sec<strong>on</strong>d account is <strong>the</strong>refore able toh<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>le problems of interpretati<strong>on</strong> as well as usage.At first glance, it may seem adventurous to assign lexical<strong>development</strong> to <strong>the</strong> same faculty as o<strong>the</strong>r comp<strong>on</strong>ents oflanguage. For lexical <strong>development</strong> rests <strong>on</strong> a number ofperceptual, attenti<strong>on</strong>al, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r cognitive systems thatare not, in <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>the</strong>mselves, linguistic. But a wider <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> faculty of language has recently becomeavailable. In an attempted rec<strong>on</strong>ciliati<strong>on</strong> of evoluti<strong>on</strong>arybiology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistics, Hauser, Chomsky, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Fitchhave proposed a broad linguistic faculty that extendsbey<strong>on</strong>d a narrow computati<strong>on</strong>al core to include <strong>the</strong> “biologicalcapacity of humans that allows us (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not, forexample, chimpanzees) to readily master any humanlanguage without explicit instructi<strong>on</strong>” (Hauser et al.2002, p. 1571; also see Fitch et al. 2005).The broad model offered by Hauser <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> colleaguesoffers hope to those who might like to see linguistic knowledge<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> structure accommodated under <strong>the</strong> same<strong>the</strong>oretical roof as pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal performance.276 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>If <strong>development</strong>al evidence suggests that some pragmaticabilities st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in closer relati<strong>on</strong>ship to lexical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammaticalknowledge than o<strong>the</strong>rs, as it does (McTear & C<strong>on</strong>ti-Ramsden 1992), <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> faculty of language shouldinclude <strong>the</strong>se related, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> possibly interdependent,specializati<strong>on</strong>s. Certainly <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary accountoffered here suggests that it should. For, if pragmatic<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performative abilities were selected, it is hard to seehow <strong>the</strong>y could have escaped some degree of integrati<strong>on</strong>with earlier linguistic <strong>development</strong>s <strong>on</strong> which <strong>the</strong>y wouldhave depended.What bel<strong>on</strong>gs in <strong>the</strong> language faculty is a matter thatshould ultimately be decided empirically, based <strong>on</strong> evidenceof various sorts, including lesi<strong>on</strong>, imaging, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> processingstudies, even if <strong>the</strong> interpretati<strong>on</strong> of some findingsproves problematic (Thomas & Karmiloff-Smith 2002).Here, we rely <strong>on</strong> normal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> atypical patterns of <strong>development</strong>,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an additi<strong>on</strong>al line of argument <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evidence –evoluti<strong>on</strong> itself. For <strong>the</strong> traits that are present in modernhumans are traits that evolved. If language evolved, <strong>the</strong>n<strong>the</strong> comp<strong>on</strong>ents of language were selected. If selecti<strong>on</strong>applied to behaviors affecting care in infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> childhood,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> competiti<strong>on</strong> in juvenility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence,<strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> behaviors that were selected during <strong>the</strong>sestages would now be included in <strong>the</strong> trait. The net resultwould be a series of functi<strong>on</strong>al interrelati<strong>on</strong>ships, since<strong>the</strong> behaviors that appeared early enabled later <strong>development</strong>s,which, in turn, reinforced <strong>the</strong>ir own precursors.Thus, <strong>the</strong> human faculty of language would extendbey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> ability to learn a linguistic code <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to use itsimply to inform; it would extend to <strong>the</strong> ability to speakskillfully so as to compete <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cooperate in social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>sexual maturity.13. Vocal <strong>on</strong>togeny, linguistic phylogenyWe have discussed selecti<strong>on</strong> as it may have applied inadolescence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adulthood, but have additi<strong>on</strong>allyproposed a role for infants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> children – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>irparents – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a role for juveniles. In doing so, we haveintroduced proto-linguistic c<strong>on</strong>tent to two pre-existingc<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s of evoluti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>e relating to <strong>the</strong> role of <strong>development</strong>,<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r to <strong>the</strong> role of behavior. The suggesti<strong>on</strong>that <strong>development</strong> plays a role in evoluti<strong>on</strong> was raised byMivart (1871) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> developed by Garstang (1922), whoargued that <strong>on</strong>togeny was no “animated cinema show ofancestral portraits” (p. 100), as Haeckel had claimed inhis late nineteenth century recapitulatory doctrine, butactually plays a reverse role by “creating” evoluti<strong>on</strong>. 18Gould (1977) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gottlieb (1992) have discussed <strong>development</strong>alc<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s to evoluti<strong>on</strong> in some detail, <strong>the</strong> latteralso claiming, after Bates<strong>on</strong> (1988), that individuals’behaviors play a central role in evoluti<strong>on</strong>, especiallywhen <strong>the</strong>y move about <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thus alter <strong>the</strong>ir envir<strong>on</strong>ment. 19Gottlieb (1992) offered a three-stage model of <strong>the</strong>process by which <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavioral <strong>development</strong>s produceevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary change. In <strong>the</strong> first stage, a modificati<strong>on</strong> of<strong>development</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s forces a previously unexpressedbehavior to <strong>the</strong> surface, followed in <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d stage bymorphological <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> physiological alterati<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, in <strong>the</strong>final stage, by a change in <strong>the</strong> genetic compositi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong>populati<strong>on</strong>. If we apply Gottlieb’s model here, bipedalismis seen as a key event in Stage I, for this was a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> behaviorthat, in time, produced pelvic narrowing. Remodeling of<strong>the</strong> birth canal caused a shift of skull <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> brain <strong>development</strong>into <strong>the</strong> postnatal period, increasing <strong>the</strong> degree<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> durati<strong>on</strong> of infant helplessness. When weaningwas brought forward <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sibling competiti<strong>on</strong> increased,infants – at an age when <strong>the</strong>ir brain was developingrapidly – experienced additi<strong>on</strong>al pressures to do thingsthat would preserve <strong>the</strong> flow of care. In Stage II,hominin infants exploited pre-existing but quiescentmeans of signaling <strong>the</strong>ir needs to, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> appraising <strong>the</strong> reacti<strong>on</strong>sof, heavily burdened caregivers, some doing so moreeffectively than o<strong>the</strong>rs. In <strong>the</strong> final stage of evoluti<strong>on</strong>arychange, Stage III, <strong>the</strong> capacity to produce more complexvocalizati<strong>on</strong>s made its way through <strong>the</strong> populati<strong>on</strong>, acquiringa genetic foundati<strong>on</strong>.In <strong>the</strong> example above, evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary change applied to<strong>the</strong> earliest stage of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, but infants who achievedeffective care <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> engagement signals would havecarried some form of <strong>the</strong> relevant c<strong>on</strong>trol behaviors intochildhood, juvenility, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence, where <strong>the</strong>yenabled additi<strong>on</strong>al capabilities – skills that facilitated,am<strong>on</strong>g o<strong>the</strong>r things, <strong>the</strong> quest for status, sex, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>additi<strong>on</strong>al resources. The <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> skills were reinforced byreproductive success, automatically streng<strong>the</strong>ning, in asec<strong>on</strong>d hit, <strong>the</strong> precursors that had persisted in someform from earlier stages. In doing so, comp<strong>on</strong>ents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>specific applicati<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong> human voice became cues tofitness <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a genetic specializati<strong>on</strong> emerged, <strong>on</strong>e thatwould direct <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>, learning, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> strategicuse of complex vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s in future generati<strong>on</strong>s.The <strong>development</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s that favored evoluti<strong>on</strong> ofvocal-motor c<strong>on</strong>trol in infancy – helplessness <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dependency– would also have promoted perceptual sensitivity<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> nurturance in caregivers. For, with more competingoffspring to care for, mo<strong>the</strong>rs were forced to discriminateh<strong>on</strong>est cries from bogus distress signals (now more plentiful<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> clever than ever) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to infer signs of physical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>social maturati<strong>on</strong> from o<strong>the</strong>r vocal cues. They were alsoforced to provide more intensive care to individualinfants. There would have been a range of variati<strong>on</strong> in<strong>the</strong> dispositi<strong>on</strong> or ability to carry out <strong>the</strong>se functi<strong>on</strong>s,too, with selecti<strong>on</strong> acting <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetic correlates ofpercepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> care.14. C<strong>on</strong>cluding remarksThe <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> approach taken here has produced severalobservati<strong>on</strong>s, speculati<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s. We began byreiterating an earlier speculati<strong>on</strong> that a brief childhoodwas inserted into <strong>the</strong> hominin line around <strong>the</strong> time ofH. habilis, offering reproductive benefits to mo<strong>the</strong>rs. We<strong>the</strong>n speculated that childhood would have facilitated<strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language by extending parental investmentto <strong>the</strong>ir offspring during a period of intense symboliccreativity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> increasing aut<strong>on</strong>omy. Noting that manyevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>the</strong>ories are built <strong>on</strong> skeletal remains, wesuggested that a <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> approach to language – abehavior that relies <strong>on</strong> soft tissues – makes additi<strong>on</strong>altypes of evidence available, including findings <strong>on</strong> bipedalism<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pelvic narrowing, c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of rapid braingrowth after birth, growth of l<strong>on</strong>g b<strong>on</strong>es, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dentiti<strong>on</strong>.Selecti<strong>on</strong> of linguistically relevant behaviors that occurin infancy, childhood, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juvenility (stages that areBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 277


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>rarely invoked in <strong>the</strong>ories of evoluti<strong>on</strong>) may assume botha direct <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an indirect form. In <strong>the</strong> direct case, behaviorsof <strong>the</strong> young (e.g., infants’ strategic use of creative <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>complex vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s) forestall withdrawal of care,increasing <strong>the</strong> probability of survival to reproductive age.In <strong>the</strong> indirect form, it is <strong>the</strong> behavior of sexually matureindividuals that is selected, typically in <strong>the</strong> quest forstatus <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mating opportunities, but that behavior is <strong>on</strong>lyavailable to be selected because it, or a precursive form,has a prior <strong>development</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>. These cases of selecti<strong>on</strong>in adolescence or early adulthood automatically reinforce<strong>the</strong> earlier, enabling behaviors.In o<strong>the</strong>r primates, it has been proposed that <strong>the</strong> juvenileperiod is beneficial because it permits developing animalsto prepare for <strong>the</strong> complexities of adult behavior. Similarly,we reas<strong>on</strong>ed that human juvenility may have provideda period in which capabilities emerging inprevious stages could be “practiced,” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>n, in adolescence,more fully exercised for social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexual purposes.Although we suggested that selecti<strong>on</strong> operated throughout<strong>on</strong>togeny, we also speculated that in adolescence, performativebehaviors (including socially attractive aspects ofarticulate vocalizati<strong>on</strong>) increased fitness, established apreferred mode of expressi<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> laid <strong>the</strong> groundworkfor speech as <strong>the</strong> universal modality of language.We based our speculati<strong>on</strong>, in part, <strong>on</strong> anthropologicalreports indicating that in oral societies – which lack <strong>the</strong>written language bias of modern cultures <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> modernLinguistics – fluent, rhythmic, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> elegant speech attractsattenti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitates <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of status <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>power. This trend is most evident in <strong>the</strong> case of men, inc<strong>on</strong>trast with <strong>the</strong> “female advantage” that has beenreported in <strong>the</strong> psychometric literature, discouraging <strong>the</strong>oristsin search of a selecti<strong>on</strong>-based model in which malesdisplay, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> are chosen for, <strong>the</strong>ir verbal talent. Witnessinga male bias for performative speech, we speculated thatselecti<strong>on</strong> produced sex differences that remain evident inverbal behavior today.If childhood entered <strong>the</strong> human genome first, <strong>the</strong>amount of vocal-motor c<strong>on</strong>trol available to our premodernancestors may have remained limited, even if enhanced injuvenility, until <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of adolescence. Then, <strong>the</strong>performative applicati<strong>on</strong>s of such behaviors, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> behaviors<strong>the</strong>mselves, would almost certainly have diversified.Hence, we think <strong>the</strong>re may have been a l<strong>on</strong>g period inevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> in which vocal behaviors were displayedwithin a familial c<strong>on</strong>text, before adolescenceteased <strong>the</strong>m into <strong>the</strong> open in more elaborate form.We proposed that important aspects of language cannotdevelop until sexual maturity because functi<strong>on</strong>s thatemerge at that stage played, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> still play, a key role inits c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>. When selecti<strong>on</strong> for vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbalperformance in adolescence reinforced previous <strong>development</strong>s,it swept specializati<strong>on</strong>s associated with speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>voice, linguistic c<strong>on</strong>tent <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> structure, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rules of usageinto <strong>on</strong>e large specializati<strong>on</strong>. It is difficult to escape <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that <strong>the</strong> faculty of language includes severaldisparate capabilities, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that <strong>the</strong>se capabilities werestitched toge<strong>the</strong>r in evoluti<strong>on</strong>, as <strong>the</strong>y are in moderntimes, by <strong>the</strong> whole of human <strong>on</strong>togeny.ACKNOWLEDGMENTSThe authors wish to acknowledge helpful discussi<strong>on</strong>s with, orcomments by, Tom Dickins, Tecumseh Fitch, Cathy Flanagan,Gilbert Gottlieb, Peter LaFreniere, Kim Oller, Ca<strong>the</strong>rineSnow, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> especially, Michael Studdert-Kennedy.NOTES1. Arbib (2005a; 2005b) has suggested that at some point inevoluti<strong>on</strong>, hominins may have used manual gestures <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocalsounds to communicate; <strong>the</strong> gestures – in pantomime – carryingsemantic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> possibly sequential informati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> sort nowc<strong>on</strong>veyed by syntax (Goldstein et al., in press).2. In human ne<strong>on</strong>ates, body weight is nearly twice that ofchimpanzees, <strong>the</strong> difference being due, in large measure, to<strong>the</strong> abundance of fat. This extra fat provides a source of energyto support <strong>the</strong> large <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> still rapidly growing brain of <strong>the</strong>human infant (Bogin 1999b), which is three times larger than<strong>the</strong> chimpanzee brain at birth (Martin 1983). In fact, human<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>borns are neurologically more advanced than eight o<strong>the</strong>rmammalian species that have been studied; two m<strong>on</strong>thsbefore birth, <strong>the</strong> human brain is already more developed than<strong>the</strong> brain of <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>born macaque (Clancy et al. 2001).Fur<strong>the</strong>r disparities develop at birth, when brain growth tapersoff in <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r primates, whereas in humans it c<strong>on</strong>tinues for<strong>the</strong> next year in a rapid, fetal-like trajectory (Martin 1983).Because of <strong>the</strong>se differences, <strong>the</strong> brains of human ne<strong>on</strong>atesrequire more nutriti<strong>on</strong> than do <strong>the</strong> brains of o<strong>the</strong>r primate <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>borns(Le<strong>on</strong>ard & Roberts<strong>on</strong> 1992; see below); <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, althoughcomparative data are not readily available, it is clear that socialstimulati<strong>on</strong> also plays a significant role in human neurological<strong>development</strong> (e.g., Rutter & O’C<strong>on</strong>nor 2004).3. The “gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mo<strong>the</strong>r hypo<strong>the</strong>sis” is indirectly relevant to ourdiscussi<strong>on</strong>. Human beings are <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly primate species, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>eof <strong>on</strong>ly a few mammalian species, in which adult females have amenopause <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a cessati<strong>on</strong> of ovulati<strong>on</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g before death.Women are usually infertile by <strong>the</strong> age of 50, but may have twodecades of post-reproductive <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> in traditi<strong>on</strong>al societies(Pavelka & Fedigan 1991). The “gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mo<strong>the</strong>r hypo<strong>the</strong>sis”posits that this post-reproductive stage of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> evolved becauseolder women st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to gain more reproductive advantage byhelping <strong>the</strong>ir daughters <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>children than by investing inmore children of <strong>the</strong>ir own (Bogin & Smith 1996; Hamilt<strong>on</strong>1966). Hill <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hurtado (1991) tested this predicti<strong>on</strong> againstobjective ethnographic data derived from <strong>the</strong>ir work with <strong>the</strong>Ache hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rers of South America. The Ache data showthat offspring with gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mo<strong>the</strong>rs survive at somewhat higherrates than those without gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mo<strong>the</strong>rs, but <strong>the</strong> effect is notnearly enough to account for menopause. In a review of <strong>the</strong>Ache data <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r cases derived from hunting-ga<strong>the</strong>ring <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>agricultural societies, Austad (1994) found no evidence “thathumans can assist <strong>the</strong>ir descendants sufficiently to offset <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>arycost of ceasing reproducti<strong>on</strong>” (p. 255). Still, interest in<strong>the</strong> “gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mo<strong>the</strong>r hypo<strong>the</strong>sis” c<strong>on</strong>tinues; Hawkes et al. (1998)used ma<strong>the</strong>matical modeling techniques to show ways thatmenopause could have evolved under Darwinian selecti<strong>on</strong>.4. When is <strong>the</strong> human capacity for language, or linguisticcommunicati<strong>on</strong>, fully instated in <strong>the</strong> young? When does itbecome fully operati<strong>on</strong>al? In <strong>the</strong> case of printed language, itwas <strong>on</strong>ce possible to specify <strong>the</strong> materials that <strong>on</strong>e needed toread, such as <strong>the</strong> Bible or <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>spaper, or to link competence tolarger individual or cultural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ec<strong>on</strong>omic goals. In <strong>the</strong> case ofspoken language, <strong>the</strong>re seem to be no clear-cut criteria formastery. Therefore, <strong>the</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard that has developed in modernsocieties, perhaps by default, is linked most immediately tosocial reacti<strong>on</strong>s in childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to academic success, includingliteracy. At <strong>the</strong> age of six, more than 96% of MidwesternAmerican children pass normed tests that emphasize ph<strong>on</strong>ology,morphology, syntax, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocabulary. These tests are sensitiveto language skills that are deemed necessary for success ineducati<strong>on</strong>al programs (Tomblin et al. 1996).5. Some of <strong>the</strong>se functi<strong>on</strong>s, including sarcasm, joking, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>idiomatic interpretati<strong>on</strong>, are h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>led primarily by <strong>the</strong> so-calledn<strong>on</strong>linguistic (right) cerebral hemisphere (cf. McD<strong>on</strong>ald 2000;278 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Paradis 1998; Van Lancker 1990). This could be taken to meanthat <strong>the</strong> way people talk is not a linguistic matter, but traditi<strong>on</strong>aldefiniti<strong>on</strong>s of language have emphasized message c<strong>on</strong>tent <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>creativity. If rules of use are subsumed under “language,” <strong>the</strong>n<strong>the</strong> right hemisphere is also “linguistic.”6. That powerful public men tend to make frequent use ofwords that are rare, archaic, or esoteric suggests that acomm<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of this material may be related to some o<strong>the</strong>r attributethat underlies <strong>the</strong> ability to persuade or manipulate people.Studies indicate that in modern societies <strong>the</strong> use of words thatare rare, l<strong>on</strong>g, abstract, diverse, or unusual is correlated withformal measures of intelligence (Carroll 1993; Gustafss<strong>on</strong> &Holmberg 1992; Gustafss<strong>on</strong> & Undheim 1996; Ullstadius et al.2002; Vetterli & Furedy 1997).7. In citing anthropological work that c<strong>on</strong>centrates <strong>on</strong> separate<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sometimes “exotic” oral societies, <strong>the</strong>re is a possibilitythat subgroups within literate societies will be ignored. In aworking-class tavern in sou<strong>the</strong>rn Wisc<strong>on</strong>sin, according to ananthropologist-customer (LeMasters 1975), social success wasdependent <strong>on</strong> “<strong>the</strong> ability to ‘dish it out’ in <strong>the</strong> rapid-fireexchange called ‘joshing’ ... you have to have a quick retort,”he wrote, “<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> preferably <strong>on</strong>e that puts you ‘<strong>on</strong>e up’ <strong>on</strong> youropp<strong>on</strong>ent. People who can’t compete in <strong>the</strong> game lose status”(p. 140).8. After submitting <strong>the</strong> manuscript, we encountered elementsof a similar proposal by Darwin. In The Descent of Man, hesuggested that with “varied t<strong>on</strong>es <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cadences” our evoluti<strong>on</strong>aryancestors “aroused each o<strong>the</strong>r’s ardent passi<strong>on</strong>s, during <strong>the</strong>ircourtship <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rivalry” (1879/2004, p. 639).9. Discriminati<strong>on</strong> against poor speakers does not necessarilybegin in juvenility. When several four-year-olds were encouragedto “play store,” an irrelevant utterance by <strong>on</strong>e – a child withlimited linguistic skills – caused ano<strong>the</strong>r boy to advise a peer,“D<strong>on</strong>’t talk to him; he’s weird” (Rice 1993). “Preschoolersbehave as if <strong>the</strong>y know who talks well <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> who doesn’t,” observedHadley <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Rice (1991), “<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>y prefer to interact with thosewho do” (p. 1315).10. That females tend to operate covertly should not be takento mean that <strong>the</strong>y lack <strong>the</strong> ability to attract attenti<strong>on</strong> or toperform. In early modern Engl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, women raised <strong>the</strong>ir voicesagainst sexual competitors in a strident <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> spectacular way.According to Capp (2003), when threats to <strong>the</strong>ir marriagearose, wives “showed c<strong>on</strong>siderable skill in formulating taunts”(p. 199), calling o<strong>the</strong>r women “drunken fuddling fool” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>“drunken pocky-faced rogue” (p. 257). When women hurled suchinsults, it was not unusual for <strong>the</strong>m to be arrested <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> chargedwith “scolding” (McIntosh 1998).11. Clearly, our focus here is <strong>on</strong> verbal behavior (competiti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance) as enacted by specific activities, suchas insulting <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> joking; particular kinds of material, such as colloquialphrases; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocal attributes, such as rate, fluency, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>rhythm. When it comes to linguistic operati<strong>on</strong>s, testoster<strong>on</strong>e –whe<strong>the</strong>r naturally high or experimentally administered – mayexert <strong>the</strong> opposite effect by suppressing scores <strong>on</strong> languagetests (Christiansen & Knussmann 1987; van Goozen et al.1994).12. However, <strong>on</strong>e might hypo<strong>the</strong>size that men are superior towomen in <strong>the</strong> use of idiomatic material. It has been dem<strong>on</strong>stratedthat <strong>the</strong> speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fluency of aucti<strong>on</strong>eering <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sportsbroadcasting reflect use of over-learned material, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mendominate <strong>the</strong>se professi<strong>on</strong>s (Kuiper 1996; Pawley 1991). But<strong>the</strong>re may also be a male advantage <strong>on</strong> measures of speededarticulati<strong>on</strong>; some studies have revealed a trend toward superiorperformance am<strong>on</strong>g juvenile <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescent males <strong>on</strong> diadochokinetictasks (Fletcher 1972; Robb et al. 1985).13. In young women’s ratings of children’s c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>alspeech, <strong>the</strong> highest degree of variance was accounted for by adynamic factor that included <strong>the</strong> attributes: excitable, loud,unc<strong>on</strong>trolled, bold, active, uninhibited, sp<strong>on</strong>taneous, assertive,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dominant. Overall, ratings were more influenced by qualityof speech than sentence complexity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammaticality(Burroughs & Tomblin 1990). Thus, it is possible that vocalcharacteristics that are favorably regarded in sexual maturityare already present, to some degree, in earlier stages of<strong>development</strong>.14. American trial lawyers are also highly verbal, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> voluble,in <strong>the</strong>ir pers<strong>on</strong>al lives. When Walter (1988) interviewed 34 triallawyers in her dissertati<strong>on</strong> research, she found that many couldnot be shut up. In <strong>on</strong>e of her late afterno<strong>on</strong> interviews, “alawyer talked for two <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a half hours with no sign of c<strong>on</strong>cluding,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> since he had strayed far from <strong>the</strong> topic, <strong>the</strong> interviewerterminated <strong>the</strong> discourse, or tried to, by physically leaving <strong>the</strong>office. This lawyer followed <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tinued speaking up to <strong>the</strong>door of <strong>the</strong> elevator” (Walter 1988, pp. 31–32).15. Goodall (1983) reports that for <strong>the</strong> period 1965 to 1980<strong>the</strong>re were 51 births <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 49 deaths in <strong>on</strong>e community of wildchimpanzees at <strong>the</strong> Gombe Stream Nati<strong>on</strong>al Park, Tanzania.During a ten-year period at <strong>the</strong> Mahale Mountains Nati<strong>on</strong>alPark in Tanzania, researchers counted 74 births, 74 deaths, 14immigrati<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 13 emigrati<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>on</strong>e community (Nishidaet al. 1990). Chimpanzee populati<strong>on</strong> size in each of <strong>the</strong>se twocommunities was, by <strong>the</strong>se data, effectively in equilibrium. Anyadditi<strong>on</strong>al delay in age of females at first birth or <strong>the</strong> timebetween successful births would probably have produced adecline in populati<strong>on</strong>.16. It must be pointed out that <strong>the</strong>re is uncertainty as to <strong>the</strong>existence of childhood in early H. erectus <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its predecessorspecies. One line of evidence is based <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> formati<strong>on</strong> oftooth crown enamel. This method indicates that Australopi<strong>the</strong>cus,H. habilis, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> early H. erectus all matured at a rate fasterthan living humans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> closer to living apes (Dean et al. 2001;Zihlman et al. 2004). The “Mojokerto child” from Java (datedat 1.8 milli<strong>on</strong> years ago) is attributed to H. erectus <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> is <strong>the</strong><strong>on</strong>ly infant specimen preserved well enough to estimate ageat death or cranial capacity. A recent analysis of <strong>the</strong> infant calvaria(i.e., top porti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> skull) indicates that this hominindied between 0.5 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 1.5 years of age <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> had achieved72–84% of adult H. erectus brain size (Coqueugnoit et al.2004). Ano<strong>the</strong>r analysis by Antón (1997) estimates an age atdeath of 4 to 6 years. If <strong>the</strong> younger age at death is correct,<strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> pattern of brain growth is more similar to chimpanzeesthan human beings. In fact, <strong>the</strong> Mojokerto fossil wouldhave grown its brain faster than living chimpanzees(Coqueugnoit et al. 2004). If, however, <strong>the</strong> later age at deathis correct, <strong>the</strong>n Mojokerto followed <strong>the</strong> human trajectory forbrain growth.17. There is indirect evidence for <strong>the</strong> reproductive value ofhuman adolescence in <strong>the</strong> data <strong>on</strong> n<strong>on</strong>human primates. Thefirst-born infants of m<strong>on</strong>keys <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> apes are more likely to diethan are those of humans. Studies of yellow babo<strong>on</strong>s (Altmann1980), toque macaques (Dittus 1977), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> chimpanzees(Teleki et al. 1976) show that 50–60% of firstborn offspringdie in infancy (between birth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> age <strong>on</strong>e year). In hunterga<strong>the</strong>rhuman societies, such as <strong>the</strong> !Kung, about 44% of childrendie in infancy (Howell 1979). The human advantage mayseem small, but over <strong>the</strong> vast course of evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary time evena 6–16% advantage is a powerful selective force. The !Kunglive under very difficult <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> marginal c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s in a desertenvir<strong>on</strong>ment. Their infant mortality rates are at <strong>the</strong> extremehigh end of human populati<strong>on</strong>. For comparis<strong>on</strong>, it may benoted that in most c<strong>on</strong>temporary human societies <strong>the</strong> infantmortality rate is usually below 20% of live births. The nati<strong>on</strong>of Haiti, for example, is <strong>the</strong> poorest country in <strong>the</strong> WesternHemisphere. In 1970 <strong>the</strong> infant mortality rate was 15%(Bogin 2001). In 1960 in <strong>the</strong> United States, about 2.5% ofall live, firstborn children died before <strong>the</strong> age of <strong>on</strong>e year(Vavra & Querec 1973).18. Garstang (1922) also saw phylogeny as “<strong>the</strong> creati<strong>on</strong> ofsuccessive <strong>on</strong>togenies” (p. 84), a characterizati<strong>on</strong> that promptedStuddert-Kennedy (1991), in recent years, to view humanBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 279


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>language as <strong>the</strong> “product of successive <strong>on</strong>togenies” (p. 10; also seeStuddert-Kennedy 2005).19. This process, <strong>on</strong>ce termed “niche picking” by Scarr <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>McCartney (1983), has recently been treated in some detail byOdling-Smee et al. (2003), who rightly regard “niche c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>”as a vastly underplayed process in <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of evoluti<strong>on</strong>arythinking. A brief but interesting discussi<strong>on</strong> of nichec<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> is available in Dawkins (2004), who distinguishesthis kind of engineered <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adaptive alterati<strong>on</strong>, which is encompassedby his extended phenotype <strong>the</strong>ory, from <strong>the</strong> lessDarwinian processes of “niche change.”Open Peer CommentaryInvoking narrative transmissi<strong>on</strong> in oralsocietiesIleana BengaThe Folklore Archive Institute in Cluj-Napoca, Romanian Academy,Cluj-Napoca 400015, Romania.ileana_benga@yahoo.itAbstract: The ethnographic descripti<strong>on</strong> of story-telling <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> narrativetransmissi<strong>on</strong> of cultural facts is an aspect of Locke & Bogin’s (L&B’s)article that should be amplified. Innate shared gene patrim<strong>on</strong>y isbiased by <strong>the</strong> kinship structure of particular societies <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interacts with<strong>the</strong> transmissi<strong>on</strong> of narratives. Trance experiences are ano<strong>the</strong>rinteresting aspect of verbal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ag<strong>on</strong>istic “performances.”<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in both its innate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social aspects mustaffect <strong>the</strong> oral transmissi<strong>on</strong> of culture within traditi<strong>on</strong>al societies.The rules of transmissi<strong>on</strong> are certainly a matter for multidisciplinaryinvestigati<strong>on</strong>. Ethnology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> specific descripti<strong>on</strong> ofstory-telling including performance style <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>text c<strong>on</strong>stitute<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e level of approach to <strong>the</strong> narrative transmissi<strong>on</strong> ofcultural facts (Degh 1995). Ethnology, however, is uniquelyimportant as an avenue to <strong>the</strong> complex syntax that articulateshuman society <strong>on</strong> both synchr<strong>on</strong>ic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> diachr<strong>on</strong>ic scales.Essentially, kin groups across cultures strive to reproduce <strong>the</strong>irknowledge across generati<strong>on</strong>s, particularly favoring <strong>the</strong>ir peers of<strong>the</strong> same generati<strong>on</strong>. Ethnologists c<strong>on</strong>centrate <strong>on</strong> recurrent culturalpractices, with <strong>the</strong> supporting genetic relatedness of kindgroups less salient to <strong>the</strong>m. In any case, selecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> raw vocalability, improving widely variant types of spoken communicati<strong>on</strong>within <strong>the</strong> kin group in <strong>the</strong> very early stages of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> (seesect. 3.5 of <strong>the</strong> target article), seems logically possible, but canhardly be documented.Kinship structures inform <strong>the</strong> mechanisms of cultural transmissi<strong>on</strong>.Natural kinship is accompanied in practically everysociety by cultural kinship (previously called fictive kinship).C<strong>on</strong>ceptually put, natural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cultural kinship may lead to twocomplementary genealogies, with memory-carriers <strong>on</strong>ly partiallyoverlapping in each group. Thus, an important patrim<strong>on</strong>y existsthat is not “individually” genetic, but “communitarily” genetic.To insiders following <strong>the</strong> oral prescripti<strong>on</strong>s of a given society,often <strong>the</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>al aspect of informati<strong>on</strong> transmissi<strong>on</strong> is very sec<strong>on</strong>dary.To <strong>the</strong> ethnologist, <strong>the</strong> interesting questi<strong>on</strong> is not about<strong>the</strong> origin of oral transmissi<strong>on</strong>, or about its evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary trajectory,but about its c<strong>on</strong>tent <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mechanisms, easily searched forin present-day field realities. Ethnographic descripti<strong>on</strong> mustcomplement evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary hypo<strong>the</strong>ses regarding oral culturaltransmissi<strong>on</strong>. Present-day cultures with primarily oral methodsof transmissi<strong>on</strong> have never stopped generating transmissi<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tent <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> are far from <strong>the</strong> static models that <strong>the</strong> targetarticle suggests (sect. 4).Ethnographic evidence is richest in <strong>the</strong> following categories ofcultural transmissi<strong>on</strong>: kinship – <strong>the</strong> transgenerati<strong>on</strong>al divisi<strong>on</strong> ofgoods, both material (e.g., dowry, inheritance) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> spiritual (e.g.,descent <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> widely-accepted instituti<strong>on</strong>s such as god-parenting;Rivers 1907); narratives from belief-tales to fairy-tales; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>cerem<strong>on</strong>ies – including a wide variety of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-cycle or year-cyclecerem<strong>on</strong>ies. All of <strong>the</strong>se kinds of transmissi<strong>on</strong> are observed not<strong>on</strong>ly in <strong>the</strong> case of oral societies, but also in urban <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> literate <strong>on</strong>es.A great deal of attenti<strong>on</strong> is focused in ethnology <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>invoked in secti<strong>on</strong>s 4.1 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 4.2, between verbal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ag<strong>on</strong>isticperformance, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> power. C<strong>on</strong>tests of brilliant performers instory-telling <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> oral narrative transmissi<strong>on</strong> show <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>easpect of cultural transmissi<strong>on</strong>. Ano<strong>the</strong>r aspect is trance, whichis linked to <strong>the</strong> special qualities of precious individuals within<strong>the</strong> community – ano<strong>the</strong>r widespread form of power.Locke & Bogin’s (L&B’s) article c<strong>on</strong>centrates principally <strong>on</strong>individuals c<strong>on</strong>sciously manipulating technical ability forpower. No less powerful, “ordinary” members of oral societiesoften achieve high status using transformed linguistic proficiencyin altered states of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness. I have in mind <strong>the</strong>many techniques of trance, be it ecstatic trance (whose exemplarmodel is <strong>the</strong> classical shaman; Eliade 1951; Humphrey1996), or induced trance (trances without presumed journeyingto o<strong>the</strong>r worlds; De Martino 1961). Both types of trances shareexquisite performances, complete with assistants/interpreters ofoften parallel “languages.” The audience is prepared, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>performance must fit <strong>the</strong> expectancies of <strong>the</strong> community in<strong>the</strong> form of local myths or legends. For <strong>the</strong> individuals performingin a trance state, <strong>the</strong> ability to significantly change <strong>the</strong>irstate of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness accompanies <strong>the</strong>ir linguistic proficiency;it does not originate in such proficiency. The stories of firsth<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>trance experience surely reinvest local narrative patrim<strong>on</strong>y,after necessarily following its trends in shaping <strong>the</strong>trance/ecstasy experience.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> use, not language, is what developsin childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescenceDerek Bickert<strong>on</strong>Department of Linguistics, University of Hawaii, H<strong>on</strong>olulu, HI 96822.derbick@hawaii.rr.com www.derekbickert<strong>on</strong>.comAbstract: That both language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> novel <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages are unique tohumans is an interesting datum. But failure to distinguish betweenlanguage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language use results in an exaggerati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> languageacquisiti<strong>on</strong> period, which in turn vitiates claims that <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>development</strong>al stages were causative factors in language evoluti<strong>on</strong>.Locke & Bogin’s (L&B’s) unusually l<strong>on</strong>g target article has providedan unusually thorough account of how <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> cycle ofhumans differs from those of o<strong>the</strong>r primates. Instead of a soluti<strong>on</strong>to <strong>the</strong> puzzle of how <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> why language evolved, however, we areleft with additi<strong>on</strong>al mysteries: how <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> why childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescenceevolved. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, L&B fail to make a c<strong>on</strong>vincingcase that any causal c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> exists between novel <strong>development</strong>alstages <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language evoluti<strong>on</strong>.What selected for childhood? L&B’s best suggesti<strong>on</strong> is that itenabled mo<strong>the</strong>rs to shorten <strong>the</strong> interval between childbirths,hence to have more offspring. But since this is desirable forany species, why did childhood evolve in <strong>on</strong>e primate al<strong>on</strong>e?L&B have no answer. When <strong>the</strong>y come to adolescence, what<strong>the</strong> authors propose does not merely fail to support <strong>the</strong>irclaims, it works against <strong>the</strong>m. They characterize adolescence asa period for young individuals to rehearse adult ec<strong>on</strong>omic,social, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexual behaviors before being burdened with reproductivechores. Why would such behaviors need rehearsalunless <strong>the</strong>y were noticeably more complex than behaviors of280 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>o<strong>the</strong>r species? What could have made <strong>the</strong>m more complex if not<strong>the</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g pre-existence of a language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a complex culture built<strong>on</strong> it? This suggests that language drove <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-cycle changes, ra<strong>the</strong>rthan <strong>the</strong> reverse.The timing of <strong>the</strong>se changes remains highly problematic.Virtually all <strong>the</strong> evidence comes from teeth. How <strong>the</strong> owners ofthose teeth were organized, <strong>the</strong>ir modes of subsistence, <strong>the</strong>envir<strong>on</strong>ments <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ecologies <strong>the</strong>y shared – all <strong>the</strong>se <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> moreremain blank; as in too many works <strong>on</strong> human evoluti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong>reis very little human evoluti<strong>on</strong>.But <strong>the</strong> major weakness of L&B’s article lies in <strong>the</strong>ir treatmentof language. They seize up<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> distincti<strong>on</strong> by Hauser et al.(2002) between a broad <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a narrow faculty of language, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>misinterpret this as licensing <strong>the</strong> subsuming of structural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>pragmatic elements under a single umbrella. They would haved<strong>on</strong>e better to focus <strong>on</strong> an earlier distincti<strong>on</strong> of Chomsky’sbetween I-language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> E-language (Chomsky 1980).I(nternal)-language is <strong>the</strong> knowledge of language stored in <strong>the</strong>individual’s brain; E(xternal)-language is <strong>the</strong> sum total oflanguage use in a linguistic community. The first may (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>probably must) have a biological foundati<strong>on</strong>; <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d isclearly cultural. If evoluti<strong>on</strong> is a biological process, as generallyassumed, any inquiry into language evoluti<strong>on</strong> should address<strong>the</strong> first ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d. As a minimum, any such studyshould clearly distinguish between <strong>the</strong> language faculty itself<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> uses to which it is put. Nobody would dream of c<strong>on</strong>fusingo<strong>the</strong>r things with <strong>the</strong> uses of those things (e.g., cars with driving,or forks with eating), yet this elementary error occurs repeatedlyin work <strong>on</strong> language evoluti<strong>on</strong>.Jokes, language games, gossip, oratory, extended narrative, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> like are clearly features of language use, whereas ph<strong>on</strong>ology,syntax, morphology, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lexic<strong>on</strong> are comp<strong>on</strong>ents of what is usedin <strong>the</strong> executi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong>se things. Only by lumping <strong>the</strong>se two setstoge<strong>the</strong>r can L&B sustain <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>the</strong>sis that language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>lasts from infancy to adulthood.L&B overestimate <strong>the</strong> time it takes for <strong>the</strong> structural elementsto come <strong>on</strong> line. Stephen Crain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>rs (e.g., Crain 1991;Crain & Thornt<strong>on</strong> 1998) have shown by ingenious experimentsthat most if not all aspects of grammar appear by <strong>the</strong> end ofinfancy (if not before; Crain has pointed out that such experimentsd<strong>on</strong>’t work with children under 36 m<strong>on</strong>ths). Of course,older children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescents use a richer vocabulary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>l<strong>on</strong>ger <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more complex sentences. But this results from interacti<strong>on</strong>sbetween an already-established faculty of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> dem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s placed up<strong>on</strong> it by different facets of normal <strong>development</strong>.Life experience ensures that older children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescentshave more to talk about; intellectual growth enables <strong>the</strong>mto deploy <strong>the</strong>ir full Piagetian deck of reas<strong>on</strong>ing powers; socializati<strong>on</strong>obliges <strong>the</strong>m to use <strong>the</strong>ir linguistic skills in a wide variety ofc<strong>on</strong>texts, each dem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing its own particular, culturally determinedgenres, styles <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> idioms. The “important aspects oflanguage” that L&B in <strong>the</strong>ir Abstract see as requiring “<strong>the</strong>whole of modern <strong>on</strong>togeny” are, without excepti<strong>on</strong>, not aspectsof language at all, but ra<strong>the</strong>r aspects of language use. C<strong>on</strong>sequently<strong>the</strong>ir whole case is seriously weakened.Meanwhile, serious questi<strong>on</strong>s remain. What led <strong>on</strong>e species,but no o<strong>the</strong>r, to break out of <strong>the</strong> mold of animal communicati<strong>on</strong>systems that have proved perfectly adequate for every o<strong>the</strong>rspecies that has ever existed? L&B line up <strong>the</strong> usual suspects –kin selecti<strong>on</strong>, sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>, social selecti<strong>on</strong> – providing noaccount about how <strong>the</strong>se have operated <strong>on</strong> a vast arrayof species without any remotely similar c<strong>on</strong>sequences. How,when, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> why did <strong>the</strong> prerequisites for even a protolanguage –symbolism, predicati<strong>on</strong>, displacement – emerge? Was <strong>the</strong>re aprotolanguage, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> if so what was it like, how did it developinto language? Where did syntax come from, was it adaptedfrom something else? If so, what? If not, where do we go from<strong>the</strong>re? It is such highly specific <strong>development</strong>s in language evoluti<strong>on</strong>that have to be accounted for, not just some amorphoussomething called “language.”Is it an odd <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interesting fact that <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly species withlanguage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly species with childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescenceis <strong>the</strong> same species? Of course. Could <strong>the</strong>re be a c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>somewhere? Possibly. But L&B have not yet showed us <strong>on</strong>e.The role of <strong>development</strong>al immaturity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>plasticity in evoluti<strong>on</strong>David F. Bjorklund <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Jas<strong>on</strong> GrotussDepartment of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Rat<strong>on</strong>, FL 33431.dbjorklu@fau.edu grotuss@msn.comhttp://www.psy.fau.edu/chez/dfb/Abstract: Aspects of cognitive immaturity may serve both to adaptchildren to <strong>the</strong>ir immediate envir<strong>on</strong>ment <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to prepare <strong>the</strong>m forfuture <strong>on</strong>es. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> may have evolved in children’s groups in <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>text of play. Developmental plasticity provides variability up<strong>on</strong>which natural selecti<strong>on</strong> operates, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> such plasticity, that likely playedan important role in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language, characterizes humanchildren today.Locke & Bogin (L&B) should be c<strong>on</strong>gratulated for focusingattenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> role that childhood may have played in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>of language. Their <strong>the</strong>orizing is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with that of scholarsdating back to <strong>the</strong> nineteenth century <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tinuing todaywho postulated a significant role of <strong>on</strong>togeny in phylogeny(e.g., Baldwin 1896; de Beer 1951/1958; Garstang 1922; Gottlieb2002; West-Eberhard 2003). From this <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>, evoluti<strong>on</strong> isbest viewed not as a successi<strong>on</strong> of changes in adult form or functi<strong>on</strong>but as a successi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>on</strong>togenies.Natural selecti<strong>on</strong> has surely had as great an impact (or even agreater impact) early in <strong>on</strong>togeny as it has had in adulthood.Adaptive characteristics in <strong>the</strong> adult phenotype do not emergefully formed, but must develop. Most evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary psychologists<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> anthropologists merely give lip service to selective pressuresduring pre-reproductive periods of <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>span in shaping social<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive abilities that prove adaptive in adulthood. L&B’saccount of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language provides a refreshing c<strong>on</strong>trast<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> should serve as a model for subsequent <strong>the</strong>orizing<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> experimentati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>rabilities that serve an adaptive functi<strong>on</strong> in adulthood.A flexible cognitive system is required for language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> symbolic representati<strong>on</strong> underlying it to evolve. The slowdevelopinghuman brain, with its increased volume relative toour hominid ancestors, afforded <strong>the</strong> plasticity necessary for <strong>the</strong>emergence of <strong>the</strong>se advanced skills. It is children’s brains <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>minds that are <strong>the</strong> most plastic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>sive to envir<strong>on</strong>mentalmodificati<strong>on</strong>s. Moreover, aspects of young children’s immaturecogniti<strong>on</strong>s may be especially adapted to acquiring informati<strong>on</strong>pertinent to <strong>the</strong> niche of childhood (<strong>on</strong>togenetic adaptati<strong>on</strong>s; seeBjorklund 1997), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> may also serve to prepare children for <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>as adults (deferred adaptati<strong>on</strong>s; Hernández Blasi & Bjorklund2003). Examples of such informati<strong>on</strong> or skills fostered by immaturecogniti<strong>on</strong> that have both immediate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> deferred benefitsinclude social relati<strong>on</strong>s developed during play <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language.Although L&B’s account of how language emerges in familyinteracti<strong>on</strong>s during childhood is intriguing, an alternativeaccount is that children invented language in play groups with<strong>the</strong>ir peers (in additi<strong>on</strong> to perfecting it in adolescent groups).Combining words in novel, playful ways may have led not <strong>on</strong>lyto <strong>the</strong> inventi<strong>on</strong> of words, but to early syntax. In this way,language develops not <strong>on</strong>ly within a family, but within a largersocial group. Members of <strong>the</strong>se groups will c<strong>on</strong>tinue to interactthroughout childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> as adults, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> will later use <strong>the</strong>ircomm<strong>on</strong> language to communicate with <strong>the</strong>ir offspring. Thisprovides a better c<strong>on</strong>text for <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cross-generati<strong>on</strong>altransmissi<strong>on</strong> of a language than does <strong>the</strong> family.Children’s ability to invent language is seen when <strong>the</strong>y c<strong>on</strong>vertpidgins into creoles in <strong>the</strong> course of <strong>on</strong>e generati<strong>on</strong> (Bickert<strong>on</strong>BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 281


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>1990). Pidgins are protolanguages used by people from differentlinguistic backgrounds who are brought toge<strong>the</strong>r to live <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>work, whereas creoles are true languages. More c<strong>on</strong>vincing yetof children’s collective ability to invent language comes from agenerati<strong>on</strong> of deaf Nicaraguans who had not been exposed to adeveloped language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> who, prior to attending a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> schoolfor <strong>the</strong> deaf, communicated using idiosyncratic home-signsystems. Shortly after arriving at <strong>the</strong> school, <strong>the</strong>se home signersdeveloped a shared system of signs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammatical devices.This shared system developed into a full-fledged sign languageafter several years <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> several cohorts of typically young, deafindividuals without <strong>the</strong> need for instructi<strong>on</strong>s or adult models(Senghas & Coppola 2001; Senghas et al. 2004).The emergence of <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> skills, such as language or its antecedents,in a group of individuals can place <strong>the</strong>m in novel c<strong>on</strong>texts<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> expose <strong>the</strong>m to <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures. This would surelyhave been <strong>the</strong> case with <strong>the</strong> emergence of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> itsunderlying symbolic abilities. We argue, as have o<strong>the</strong>rs (e.g.,Gottlieb 2002; Lickliter & Schneider, in press; West-Eberhard2003), that <strong>the</strong> neural plasticity of infants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>ir behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive resp<strong>on</strong>ses to novel envir<strong>on</strong>mentsprovide much of <strong>the</strong> stuff up<strong>on</strong> which natural selecti<strong>on</strong> works,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that this may have been especially important in recenthuman cognitive evoluti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., Bjorklund 2006). Such plasticitymay c<strong>on</strong>tinue to afford <strong>the</strong> opportunity for phylogeneticchange in Homo sapiens. For instance, <strong>the</strong> Flynn effect, asteady rise in IQ (particularly fluid intelligence) over <strong>the</strong> pastcentury, may be due to accelerated cognitive <strong>development</strong>(Howard 2001), perhaps in resp<strong>on</strong>se to an increasingly visualenvir<strong>on</strong>ment (see Neisser 1988). We do not believe that <strong>the</strong>human race is <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> verge of a radical evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary change;but <strong>the</strong> neural plasticity evident in c<strong>on</strong>temporary childrenin resp<strong>on</strong>se to changing envir<strong>on</strong>ments likely also characterizedour ancestors <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tributed centrally to <strong>the</strong> emergenceof language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> related sociocognitive abilities in ourforechildren.Rec<strong>on</strong>ciling vague <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> formal models oflanguage evoluti<strong>on</strong>Henry Bright<strong>on</strong>, Rui Mata, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Andreas WilkeCenter for Adaptive Behavior <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, Max Planck Institute for HumanDevelopment, 14195 Berlin, Germany.hbright<strong>on</strong>@mpib-berlin.mpg.de http://www.ling.ed.ac.uk/henryb mata@mpib-berlin.mpg.de http://www-abc.mpibberlin.mpg.de/users/matawilke@mpib-berlin.mpg.dehttp://www-abc.mpib-berlin.mpg.de/users/wilkeAbstract: One way of dealing with <strong>the</strong> pro<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>rati<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>jecturesthat accompany <strong>the</strong> diverse study of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language is todevelop precise <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testable models which reveal o<strong>the</strong>rwise latentimplicati<strong>on</strong>s. We suggest how verbal <strong>the</strong>ories of <strong>the</strong> role of individual<strong>development</strong> in language evoluti<strong>on</strong> can benefit from formal modeling,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vice versa.Research into <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language is growing rapidly <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> itsstudy now cuts across several disciplines. Despite <strong>the</strong> diversesources of insight which make up this field of study, few woulddisagree that underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing how <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> why our species-specificlinguistic communicati<strong>on</strong> system came to be, requires a c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>of <strong>the</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong>s am<strong>on</strong>g three processes: biological evoluti<strong>on</strong>,linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> individual <strong>development</strong> (e.g.,Christiansen & Kirby 2003b). C<strong>on</strong>sequently, we were pleasedto see Locke & Bogin’s (L&B’s) target article focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>eoften-neglected comp<strong>on</strong>ent – individual <strong>development</strong> – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> itsrelati<strong>on</strong> to biological evoluti<strong>on</strong>. However, in order to underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> implicati<strong>on</strong>s of a <strong>the</strong>ory of individual <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> itsrelati<strong>on</strong>ship to <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language, we must go bey<strong>on</strong>dvague models whose implicati<strong>on</strong>s are hard to gauge <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> movetowards more formal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testable models.Dominating <strong>the</strong> study of language evoluti<strong>on</strong> is <strong>the</strong> desire tounderst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> unique form of structural complexity we see inhuman language. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, we seek an explanati<strong>on</strong> ofhow certain forms of complexity arise from an initial statewhere that complexity was lacking. As L&B discuss, language isa communicati<strong>on</strong> system used in many interesting <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> uniqueways. However, it is misleading to assume that by studying <strong>the</strong>communicative uses to which language is put we can gaininsight into why language is so structurally distinct from o<strong>the</strong>rcommunicati<strong>on</strong> systems. L&B emphasize that language is usedto support functi<strong>on</strong>s which c<strong>on</strong>tribute to an individuals’ reproductivesuccess. However, <strong>the</strong> degree to which <strong>the</strong> specific structureof language is required for such functi<strong>on</strong>ing is by no meansclear. First, although most organisms communicate, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thosethat do so effectively are likely to be at an advantage over thosethat do not, <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e species has language. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, <strong>on</strong>e canimagine many c<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>idate communicati<strong>on</strong> systems that fulfillsuch requirements. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, language arguably does afairly bad job as a communicati<strong>on</strong> system (e.g., Chomsky et al.2002). In sum, <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language cannot be explainedby its communicative functi<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e.To fully underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its emergence we have tounderst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interacting adaptive systems that have driven itsevoluti<strong>on</strong>. An important tool in this endeavor is <strong>the</strong> use offormal modeling, which allows us to explore <strong>the</strong> implicati<strong>on</strong>s ofprecise <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testable hypo<strong>the</strong>ses. The growing interest in <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language has been accompanied (some might sayspurred) by an upturn in ma<strong>the</strong>matical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> computati<strong>on</strong>almodels (e.g., Briscoe 2002; Cangelosi & Parisi 2001; Hurford1989; 2005; Kirby 2002; Nowak & Komarova 2001).We would like to highlight how formal approaches to studying<strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language can profit from fur<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of<strong>the</strong> process of individual <strong>development</strong>. First, <strong>development</strong> is acrucial step in determining <strong>the</strong> class of acquirable communicati<strong>on</strong>systems. The <strong>on</strong>togenetic <strong>development</strong> of <strong>the</strong> cognitivemachinery resp<strong>on</strong>sible for processing languages may be tied tostages in <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> course, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> this <strong>development</strong>al path is likelyto be crucial to underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <strong>the</strong> structural characteristics oflanguage. For example, computati<strong>on</strong>al modeling of languageacquisiti<strong>on</strong> has shown <strong>the</strong> importance of c<strong>on</strong>sidering howlanguage structure relates to cognitive systems <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>development</strong>.Elman (1993) used neural network simulati<strong>on</strong>s to showhow networks can learn certain forms of linguistic structure ifmemory is started small <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>n gradually exp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ed. Thismirrors <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of short-term memory capacity inhumans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> suggests that <strong>the</strong> mind may be tuned to develop inparticular ways to facilitate learning. Elman’s work dem<strong>on</strong>strateshow <strong>the</strong> maturati<strong>on</strong>al trajectory over an agent’s <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>time canimpact <strong>on</strong> what is <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> what is not ultimately acquirable. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore,<strong>the</strong> particular form of inductive bias that defines <strong>the</strong>language learner has a knock-<strong>on</strong> effect when we c<strong>on</strong>siderwhich kinds of structure can withst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> repeated culturaltransmissi<strong>on</strong> (Bright<strong>on</strong> et al. 2005b; Smith 2004).Sec<strong>on</strong>d, individual <strong>development</strong> is characterized not <strong>on</strong>ly bychanges in cognitive aspects but also in social aspects, such as<strong>the</strong> structure of social networks. The social networks in which adeveloping individual is situated impacts <strong>on</strong> how language istransmitted between generati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Kerswill & Williams2000; Ragir 2002). If c<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> how language is transmittedfrom <strong>on</strong>e generati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> next impact significantly <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> distributi<strong>on</strong> of linguistic forms (e.g., statistical universals;for a discussi<strong>on</strong> see Bright<strong>on</strong> et al. 2005a), <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> socialnetworks through which language is transmitted are likely toplay a significant role (Smith & Hurford 2003). Hence, <strong>the</strong>implicati<strong>on</strong>s of changing social networks that L&B discusscould be explored by investigating how <strong>the</strong>y impact, over acultural timescale, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> distributi<strong>on</strong> of language’s structuralcharacteristics.282 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Interacti<strong>on</strong> promotes cogniti<strong>on</strong>: The rise ofchildish mindsStephen J. CowleySchool of Psychology, University of Hertfordshire, Hatfield, HertfordshireAL10 9AB, United Kingdom; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Department of Psychology, University ofKwaZulu-Natal, Durban 4041, South Africa.s.j.cowley@herts.ac.ukhttp://www.psy.herts.ac.uk/pub/sjcowley/index.htmlAbstract: Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> shaped language as, cascading in time, socialstrategies became more verbal. Although <strong>the</strong> insight is important, Locke& Bogin (L&B) also advocate a code model of language. Rejecting thisinput-output view, I emphasize <strong>the</strong> interpers<strong>on</strong>al dynamics of dialogue.From this <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>, childish minds as well as language could bederived from <strong>the</strong> selective advantages of a total interacti<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>.For Locke & Bogin (L&B) childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence changed<strong>the</strong> genetic underpinnings of human talk. Rejecting a learned/innate c<strong>on</strong>trast as simplistic, <strong>the</strong>y posit that our vocal-verbalbehavior draws <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> sum total of selecti<strong>on</strong> advantages.Development has created resources that transformed brains<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior. L&B’s achievement is to have shown why <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language must use <strong>the</strong> whole of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>.In L&B’s model, childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence take <strong>on</strong> a major rolein language evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Defending this, <strong>the</strong>y acknowledge thatstructure, pragmatics, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance fall “under <strong>the</strong> same <strong>the</strong>oreticalroof” (sect. 12, para. 4). With Hauser et al. (2002), mastery ofhuman languages depends <strong>on</strong> a “biological capacity” (sect. 12.).Vocal-verbal signaling is purposeful, pragmatic acti<strong>on</strong> usedin managing how people act <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> attend. Going fur<strong>the</strong>r, L&B insecti<strong>on</strong> 4.1, endorse Linell’s (1982) attack <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> “l<strong>on</strong>g traditi<strong>on</strong>of analyzing <strong>on</strong>ly written language.” Turning from exclusive focus<strong>on</strong> sentences, linguistic capacities rec<strong>on</strong>nect with acti<strong>on</strong>. Infacing <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s challenges, selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> favors “foundati<strong>on</strong>albehaviors” (sect. 2) for persuasi<strong>on</strong>, pragmatics, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> management.Not <strong>on</strong>ly does language require <strong>the</strong> whole of modern<strong>on</strong>togeny but, with Hogan (1988), later behavior “automaticallycredits relevant <strong>development</strong>s in earlier stages” (sect. 10, lastpara.). Evoluti<strong>on</strong> thus has cascading effects. Today, an infant’scognitive armory includes behaviors whose selecti<strong>on</strong> derivesfrom later phases of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. We are psychobiological systems whoselived envir<strong>on</strong>ment reflects a <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of, for example, how childrenseek nurture <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescents struggle for status. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> uses<strong>the</strong> ways in which, in evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary time, <strong>development</strong> linksstrategic activity with <strong>the</strong> use of verbal forms. For L&B, languageis based in “soft tissues” (sect. 14). So far so good!L&B n<strong>on</strong>e<strong>the</strong>less think that, as early as age 36 m<strong>on</strong>ths, <strong>the</strong>“basic comp<strong>on</strong>ents of a functi<strong>on</strong>al communicative system areoperative” (sect. 2.2). Since language is code-like, speech ismerely a “preferred modality” (sect. 2). Taking what Sutt<strong>on</strong>(2004) calls an expressivist view, with Fodor, Pinker, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>younger Chomsky, performance depends <strong>on</strong> linguistic knowledge.Given cascade effects, I fail to underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> why L&Bcommit to this input-output view. Instead, <strong>the</strong>y might arguethat, as interacti<strong>on</strong>al dynamics changed, language transformedchild cogniti<strong>on</strong>. Were <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> a source of cognitive effects,<strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>development</strong> would have radical c<strong>on</strong>sequences.First, it could be argued that, as language is based in soft-tissues,talking uses a total <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of social behavior. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, it couldbe claimed that, even today, cognitive dynamics emerge bothinternally <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in public language. With Hauser, Chomsky, Fitch,Linell, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> many o<strong>the</strong>rs, we would reject expressivism. The broadfaculty of language might be seen to arise from infant use ofresources for persuasi<strong>on</strong>, attenti<strong>on</strong>-management, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pragmatics.Anti-expressivists like Carru<strong>the</strong>rs (1996), Clark (1997),Dennett (1991), Linell (1982), Love (2004), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ross (in press)provide diverse reas<strong>on</strong>s for rejecting code-models. Althoughtaking c<strong>on</strong>trasting views <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> “computati<strong>on</strong>al core” (sect. 12)of language, all see thinking as intrinsic to verbal events. Specifically,Linell (in press) rejects code views because language, mind,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> brains are dialogical. Such a <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>, I think, enhances <strong>the</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> view. Intenti<strong>on</strong>al activity, vocalizing, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> managementcome to depend <strong>on</strong> systems of neural c<strong>on</strong>trol. With <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of total <strong>development</strong>, selective advantage goes to individualswho are skilled in sustaining dialogue. Instead of interrogatingsuch views, however, L&B stick with <strong>the</strong> input-output picture. Thisc<strong>on</strong>servatism shows especially in relati<strong>on</strong> to humans under threeyears of age. Not <strong>on</strong>ly are infants seen as largely untouched bychildhood or <strong>development</strong>, but L&B have no interest in how<strong>the</strong>y use attenti<strong>on</strong>, affect, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>verbal expressi<strong>on</strong>. Indeed,L&B’s <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> model ignores <strong>the</strong> use of cultural norms(Cowley et al. 2004; Trevar<strong>the</strong>n 1988), joint activity (Tomasello1999), <strong>the</strong> narrating self (Dennett 1991; Fogel et al. 2002;Nels<strong>on</strong> 1996), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> how brains accommodate silent thinking(Wheeler 2004). Instead of asking how interacti<strong>on</strong> shapes talkingpers<strong>on</strong>s, infants are taken to become human when an innerfaculty starts to operate <strong>on</strong> linguistic forms.Bey<strong>on</strong>d selecti<strong>on</strong> due to nature, kin, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexual choice, L&Bemphasize that social sensitivity may be augmented by selectedparental abilities. Given commitment to code views, however,no role falls to cultural selecti<strong>on</strong>. Even though using parentalbeliefs, human infants differ from o<strong>the</strong>r primates <strong>on</strong>ly because“social stimulati<strong>on</strong>” gives “quality” (sect. 2.1) to <strong>the</strong>ir lives.Indeed, for L&B, cross-specific comparis<strong>on</strong> makes it appropriateto define infancy as lasting to <strong>the</strong> age of three. By this stage, asnoted, <strong>the</strong> structure of language is thought to be “laid down”(sect. 2.2). Although “some degree of integrati<strong>on</strong>” (sect. 12)occurs between pragmatics, performance, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> structure, selecti<strong>on</strong>sensitizes <strong>on</strong>ly parents to expressive biomechanics. This isodd in a <strong>the</strong>ory where code-models are linked with writing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>vocal-verbal signaling with primate abilities. It is even more soin cognitive science where, today, many trace verbal forms tosocial evoluti<strong>on</strong> (see Kirby & Christiansen 2003) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammaris increasingly seen as cultural (Deac<strong>on</strong> 1997; Tomasello 2003).For L&B, however, <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>development</strong> benefitsinfants through mechanisms like trickle-up ph<strong>on</strong>etics, an instinctfor inventiveness, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> increased parental sensitivity. Cascadingfacilitates <strong>the</strong>m nei<strong>the</strong>r in assessing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> managing adults, norin using affect to shape interacti<strong>on</strong>.L&B’s linguistic individualism blemishes <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> model.If <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>development</strong> cascades <strong>on</strong>to modern infants, wewould expect sensitive reacti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se to adult dynamics.Given cultural selecti<strong>on</strong>, infants would gain from linking verbalpatterns with resources used in pragmatics, performance, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>attenti<strong>on</strong> management. With Skinner, Bruner, Tomasello, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Hauser (am<strong>on</strong>g o<strong>the</strong>rs), interacti<strong>on</strong> might be a crucial locus oflearning. Torn from an input-output model, indeed, <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>might be used to interrogate how dynamics alter human cogniti<strong>on</strong>.For example, adults may design interacti<strong>on</strong>al events to promptinfants in using selected natural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cultural resources. Learningto talk may draw <strong>on</strong> how, in real time, adults use verbal patternsto c<strong>on</strong>struct social events. No purely internal language facultycould exploit <strong>the</strong> fluidity of interacti<strong>on</strong>. Accordingly, L&B do notlink <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> with cognitive dynamics. Instead of challenginglinguistic individualism, <strong>the</strong>y focus <strong>on</strong> a modest goal. Even if weadopt a code-model, <strong>the</strong>y show, <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary process will use<strong>the</strong> whole of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>.The phylogeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>togeny of adaptati<strong>on</strong>sThomas E. DickinsSchool of Psychology, University of East L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>, L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> E15 4LZ; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Centre for Philosophy of Natural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Social Science, L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> School ofEc<strong>on</strong>omics, L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> WC2A 2AE, United Kingdom.t.dickins@uel.ac.ukhttp://www.uel.ac.uk/psychology/staff/tom_dickins/index.htmAbstract: Locke & Bogin (L&B) rightly point to <strong>the</strong> absence of <strong>on</strong>togeny in<strong>the</strong>ories of language evoluti<strong>on</strong>. However, <strong>the</strong>y overly rely up<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>togeneticBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 283


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>data to isolate comp<strong>on</strong>ents of <strong>the</strong> language faculty. Only an adaptati<strong>on</strong>istanalysis, of <strong>the</strong> sort seen in evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary psychology, can carve languageat its joints <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lead to testable predicti<strong>on</strong>s about how language works.Locke & Bogin (L&B) begin <strong>the</strong>ir paper by describing <strong>the</strong> recent<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of scholarship in language evoluti<strong>on</strong>. The key point <strong>the</strong>ydraw out is that <strong>on</strong>togeny has been largely ignored, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>iraim is to rectify this omissi<strong>on</strong>. What <strong>the</strong>y do not state is thatmost of <strong>the</strong> work has been <strong>on</strong> phylogeny; research has predominantlyfocused up<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary transiti<strong>on</strong>s that may or maynot have led to human language as it is now.Mapping out phylogenies is not <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly use for evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary<strong>the</strong>ory. Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary psychology (EP) is in <strong>the</strong> business of individuatingtraits through adaptati<strong>on</strong>ist analyses, such that organismsare looked at in terms of <strong>the</strong> ecology in which <strong>the</strong>y live<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> predicti<strong>on</strong>s are made about <strong>the</strong> kinds of psychological adaptati<strong>on</strong>s(mechanisms) required to meet ecologically relevant taskdem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s (Andrews et al. 2003; Dickins 2005). Sometimes this isd<strong>on</strong>e against a backdrop of hypo<strong>the</strong>sised envir<strong>on</strong>ments of evoluti<strong>on</strong>aryadaptedness, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> such hypo<strong>the</strong>ses are generated from,am<strong>on</strong>g o<strong>the</strong>r things, comparative data. This kind of functi<strong>on</strong>alanalysis provides key c<strong>on</strong>straints for subsequent discussi<strong>on</strong> ofproximate mechanisms.If we accept that language has evolved, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>re is littlereas<strong>on</strong> not to, we can <strong>the</strong>n apply EP reas<strong>on</strong>ing to <strong>the</strong> subjectmatter – language itself. By carefully thinking through <strong>the</strong>adapted functi<strong>on</strong>s that language delivers we can begin to individuatecomp<strong>on</strong>ents of this faculty. Then, <strong>on</strong>ce we have an EP<strong>the</strong>ory of language we can, perhaps, begin to think about its phylogeny,for we know what has been selected for. This is a l<strong>on</strong>gproject, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not without methodological problems, not least <strong>the</strong>absence of fossil evidence; but anything else would run <strong>the</strong> riskof generating just-so stories. However, <strong>on</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> EP project hasbeen completed it is hard to imagine what use phylogenetichypo<strong>the</strong>ses could be put to o<strong>the</strong>r than to dem<strong>on</strong>strate that <strong>the</strong>already isolated adaptati<strong>on</strong>s could have evolved.Ontogenetic hypo<strong>the</strong>ses can be used slightly differently fromphylogenetic <strong>on</strong>es, in that <strong>the</strong>y can be tested in <strong>the</strong> laboratory<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, in so doing, aid in <strong>the</strong> individuati<strong>on</strong> of psychological adaptati<strong>on</strong>s.N<strong>on</strong>e<strong>the</strong>less, before <strong>on</strong>e goes into <strong>the</strong> lab <strong>on</strong>e needs topropose a sound evoluti<strong>on</strong>arily based hypo<strong>the</strong>sis about how <strong>on</strong>togenywould pan out. Again, adaptati<strong>on</strong>ist analysis should come first.L&B appear to have operated a somewhat mixed strategy, but<strong>on</strong>e that mostly falls in line with <strong>the</strong> traditi<strong>on</strong> of speculating <strong>on</strong>phylogenies. Their initial observati<strong>on</strong>s about what language isclearly originate from thinking about its adapted functi<strong>on</strong>s. So,as with much c<strong>on</strong>temporary EP, <strong>the</strong>y see language as fulfillinga variety of social signalling tasks. What is more, <strong>the</strong>y moveaway from <strong>the</strong> traditi<strong>on</strong>al Chomskyan focus up<strong>on</strong> grammar<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tent, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> note that language is a many-str<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ed communicati<strong>on</strong>system. They rightly point to <strong>the</strong> qualities of voice,pragmatic inference, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal fluency, am<strong>on</strong>g many o<strong>the</strong>rthings, as sources of signal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> informati<strong>on</strong>. However, <strong>the</strong>y<strong>on</strong>ly use this insight to broadly define <strong>the</strong> aspects of language<strong>the</strong>y are interested in. After this, L&B go <strong>on</strong> to outlinevarious key features of language <strong>development</strong>, which <strong>the</strong>y inturn use to speculate about phylogeny. So, <strong>the</strong>y note that aswe develop from infancy to adulthood social c<strong>on</strong>texts becomemore complex <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> this is matched by increased communicativesophisticati<strong>on</strong>. In particular, <strong>the</strong>y claim that adolescence is aperiod of near-adult social complexity in which <strong>the</strong> rules ofadult <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> can be learnt <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to some extent implementedwithout <strong>the</strong> cost. During this period, language develops suchthat grammar becomes more sophisticated, speech is morefluent, more <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more pragmatic communicati<strong>on</strong> is engagedin, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> native language is modified. Adolescence sees <strong>the</strong><strong>on</strong>set of gossiping about o<strong>the</strong>rs, as well as “joking, deceiving,mollifying, negotiating, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> persuading, with increases in <strong>the</strong>use of sarcasm” (sect 2.8). In brief, <strong>the</strong> social uses of languagebecome more prevalent.L&B discuss <strong>the</strong> possibility that human infancy has been foreshortenedby natural selecti<strong>on</strong> in order to allow maternalresources to be diverted to <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> offspring more rapidly, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>that this in turn led to childhood. Children are semi-independent<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> require less care, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, importantly for L&B, are able toengage in verbal interacti<strong>on</strong>s with adults that will shape <strong>the</strong>irlinguistic <strong>development</strong>. It is during this period that what couldbe referred to as a Chomskyan basis for linguistic communicati<strong>on</strong>is established. L&B fur<strong>the</strong>r hypo<strong>the</strong>sise that <strong>the</strong> social practicefuncti<strong>on</strong>s of adolescence were directly selected for <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thisallowed for <strong>the</strong> emergence of <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r str<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s of linguisticcommunicati<strong>on</strong> discussed above.L&B have essentially married detailed observati<strong>on</strong>s aboutlanguage <strong>development</strong> with a loose <strong>the</strong>sis about <strong>the</strong> phylogenyof <strong>on</strong>togeny, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> without engaging in a detailed adaptati<strong>on</strong>istanalysis. They are undoubtedly right that <strong>on</strong>togeny is a productof natural selecti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> approach, whichlooks at maternal trade-offs, makes evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary sense. But it isunclear what predicti<strong>on</strong>s we can now make about <strong>the</strong> kinds ofproximate mechanism underlying language that we could notmake prior to this argument. One reas<strong>on</strong> for this is <strong>the</strong> slightcircularity of <strong>the</strong> adolescence argument. By observing how adolescentsuse language, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> assuming that this <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-stage is aproduct of natural selecti<strong>on</strong>, L&B suggest that <strong>the</strong> social complexitiesof adolescence drove selecti<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> social signallingfuncti<strong>on</strong>s of language. But it is equally possible that socialcomplexities were able to emerge as a c<strong>on</strong>sequence of socialsignalling abilities. There is nothing in <strong>the</strong> current argumentthat can resolve this, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> no obvious testable predicti<strong>on</strong>s aremade.The target article c<strong>on</strong>cludes by stating that <strong>the</strong> various str<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>sof language “were stitched toge<strong>the</strong>r in evoluti<strong>on</strong>, as <strong>the</strong>y are inmodern times, by <strong>the</strong> whole of human <strong>on</strong>togeny” (sect. 14).This is a different claim from <strong>the</strong> authors’ predominant <strong>on</strong>ethat <strong>development</strong>al stages were selected for – indeed, this is aclaim for a role for <strong>on</strong>togeny in phylogeny, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> is perhaps <strong>the</strong>main point L&B wish to make. But it does not follow from anyof <strong>the</strong>ir observati<strong>on</strong>s. Developmental stages, as L&B have discussed,are <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence of evoluti<strong>on</strong> through natural selecti<strong>on</strong>.Any <strong>development</strong>al “decisi<strong>on</strong>” made by natural selecti<strong>on</strong>will have c<strong>on</strong>sequences that in turn may provide selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lead to phenotypic change, but this is not coded into <strong>the</strong><strong>development</strong>al process. In this way, <strong>on</strong>togeny does not stitchtoge<strong>the</strong>r various capabilities in phylogeny, but ra<strong>the</strong>r specific<strong>on</strong>togenetic pathways are selected for <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> this establishesfur<strong>the</strong>r selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures, <strong>the</strong> outcomes of which are readilyobservable in c<strong>on</strong>temporary <strong>development</strong>.L&B are right to discuss <strong>on</strong>togeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> right to think about <strong>the</strong>multiple str<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s of language, but <strong>the</strong>y should have moved awayfrom historical speculati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> toward EP if <strong>the</strong>y wished tohave made substantive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testable claims about <strong>the</strong> nature of<strong>the</strong> language faculty.The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language: Present behavioralevidence for past genetic reprogramming in<strong>the</strong> human lineageRobert B. EckhardtLaboratory for <strong>the</strong> Comparative Study of Morphology, Mechanics, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Molecules, Department of Kinesiology, Pennsylvania State University,University Park, PA 16802.eyl@psu.eduAbstract: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> can be related functi<strong>on</strong>ally through<strong>the</strong> study of human <strong>on</strong>togeny, thus usefully informing ourunderst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing of several unique aspects of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of species.The operati<strong>on</strong>al principles outlined by Locke & Bogin (L&B)284 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>dem<strong>on</strong>strate that <strong>the</strong> present can provide a useful framework forunderst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <strong>the</strong> past.My own <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>” is c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edby an extended period of familiarity with <strong>the</strong> writingsof <strong>on</strong>e of <strong>the</strong> authors. For more than a decade I have usedtwo editi<strong>on</strong>s of Barry Bogin’s Patterns of Human Growth(1988; 1999) as main texts in an upper-level undergraduatecourse titled Human Growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Development. On firstreading <strong>the</strong> target article against this background, I recalled<strong>the</strong> critical comment <strong>on</strong>ce offered by Samuel Johns<strong>on</strong> withungentle pithiness: “Your work, Sir, is both <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> good, butwhat’s <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> is not good <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> what’s good is not <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>.” However,<strong>the</strong> work by Locke & Bogin (L&B) merits a much more positiveassessment: What is not <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> (elucidati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> stages, shared<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> unique, in human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>) remains as good as it everwas, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> what is <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> (<strong>the</strong> extensi<strong>on</strong> of this <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> tofur<strong>the</strong>ring our knowledge about <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of humanlanguage) is even better.Most readers of Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences are likely to bespecialists in functi<strong>on</strong>al ra<strong>the</strong>r than evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary biology;however, as is <strong>the</strong> case with <strong>the</strong> authors of <strong>the</strong> target article,my scientific work overlaps both domains. As a help toward relating<strong>the</strong>se c<strong>on</strong>trasting <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, I will paraphrase here part of akeynote address by Bruce Latimer that was delivered to <strong>the</strong> 2005joint meeting of <strong>the</strong> American Society of Biomechanics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>Internati<strong>on</strong>al Society of Biomechanics: Scientists who workwith living subjects (from elite athletes endeavoring to set <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>records to stroke victims <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r patients undergoing rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>work) have problems for which soluti<strong>on</strong>s must be found.But those of us who study <strong>the</strong> fossil record of past human evoluti<strong>on</strong>can see <strong>the</strong> adaptive soluti<strong>on</strong>s that nature has evolved; itis our challenge to rec<strong>on</strong>struct <strong>the</strong> problems that required<strong>the</strong>se soluti<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> first place.The work of L&B is particularly fascinating because it beginswith a set of problems, <strong>the</strong> so-called “obstetrical dilemma”triggered by <strong>the</strong> assumpti<strong>on</strong> of upright posture <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> bipedal locomoti<strong>on</strong>,that has been <strong>the</strong> focus of much recent work by my ownresearch group <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> our close colleagues in several o<strong>the</strong>rcountries (e.g., Galik et al. 2004). This research has enabled usto establish <strong>the</strong> origin of bipedalism at about 6 milli<strong>on</strong> yearsbefore <strong>the</strong> present, thus bounding <strong>the</strong> earliest temporal limitsof <strong>the</strong> human lineage that uniquely evolved language. Thesyn<strong>the</strong>sizing work by L&B goes bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> limitati<strong>on</strong>s ofhuman fossil evidence by using <strong>development</strong>al clues stillperceivable in human <strong>on</strong>togeny to attempt to tease out <strong>the</strong>stages that must have existed between <strong>the</strong> rudimentary formsof communicati<strong>on</strong> in living chimpanzees <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> those characteristicof our own species. This sort of approach, escaping <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>fines of so-called hard evidence by reas<strong>on</strong>ing from softtissue features <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behaviors existing in present populati<strong>on</strong>s tocomparable attributes in ancestral groups, holds much promise(Eckhardt 2000). It <strong>the</strong>refore is no criticism to characterizethis target article more as a step in <strong>the</strong> right directi<strong>on</strong> than anultimate formulati<strong>on</strong> of some end point in our underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ingof <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of those aspects of brain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior sciencec<strong>on</strong>cerned with human language – or even to note that a fewof its more unusual ideas have been anticipated (Livingst<strong>on</strong>e1973).I suspect that some paleoanthropological colleagues may feelthat <strong>the</strong> approach taken by L&B departs too much from traditi<strong>on</strong>alreliance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> hard evidence. But recently <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>species Homo floresiensis has been hypo<strong>the</strong>sized <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> basisof a single skull with a chimp-sized brain of about 400 cc, yetwith <strong>the</strong> supposed behavioral capacity to have manufacturedst<strong>on</strong>e microblades as part of complex tools heretofore foundassociated <strong>on</strong>ly with large-brained humans capable of speech.This is a dubious propositi<strong>on</strong> for which <strong>the</strong>re is a better alternativeexplanati<strong>on</strong> in terms of human biology (Henneberg &Thorne 2004). Appropriate use of <strong>development</strong>al clues inferredfrom living populati<strong>on</strong>s promises to provide an approach to behavioralinference in which broadly based <strong>the</strong>ory trumps a narrowevidential base.Enduring excitement in <strong>the</strong> brain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior sciences willbe found in <strong>the</strong> solid advance of knowledge through hypo<strong>the</strong>sisgenerati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testing of <strong>the</strong> sort offered by L&B, ra<strong>the</strong>r thanthrough journalistic sensati<strong>on</strong>alism.Road to language: L<strong>on</strong>ger, more believable,more relevantR. Allen GardnerPsychology Department, University of Nevada – Reno, Reno, NV 89557.gardner@unr.eduAbstract: A realistic <strong>development</strong>al view of language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>recognizes vocabulary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pragmatics as well as grammar with alengthy period of growth in a favorable envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Cross-fostering isa tool of behavioral biology for studying <strong>the</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> betweengenetic endowment <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>al envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Sign languagestudies of cross-fostered chimpanzees measure <strong>development</strong> in a nearlyhuman envir<strong>on</strong>ment.Theoretical linguists of <strong>the</strong> last century seemed to deny hum<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>evelopment.We are presenting an “instantaneous model” of language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>which is surely false in detail, but can very well be accepted as a reas<strong>on</strong>ablefirst approximati<strong>on</strong>. (Chomsky 1967, pp. 441–42)Early followers of Chomsky supported his instantaneousmodel.Children all over <strong>the</strong> world learn to speak <strong>the</strong>ir native language atapproximately <strong>the</strong> same time – 3 to 4 years of age. Within a relativelybrief period, <strong>the</strong> child appears to learn a complicated <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> abstractsystem of rules ... without teaching or training, [<strong>the</strong>y] acquire <strong>the</strong>irnative language at about <strong>the</strong> same time – regardless of just about anyvariable <strong>on</strong>e cares to look at, short of deafness or severe retardati<strong>on</strong>.(Moore 1973, p. 4)Chomsky’s instantaneous model flattened <strong>the</strong> l<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>scape of<strong>development</strong> from toddler to college student. Repeatedly, loyalChomskians found virtually complete grammar in <strong>the</strong> speechof <strong>the</strong> same four-year-olds who cannot yet tie <strong>the</strong>ir own shoelacesor use a knife to spread jam <strong>on</strong> a cracker (Cohen & Gross1979).Locke & Bogin (L&B) recognize vocabulary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pragmatics, aswell as grammar, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> emphasize human <strong>development</strong> frominfancy through adolescence. Their road to language is l<strong>on</strong>ger,more believable, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more relevant. They relate detailedaspects of linguistic skill to human <strong>development</strong>, doing justiceto linguistic <strong>development</strong> as a biological phenomen<strong>on</strong>. Immaturehumans hardly spend <strong>the</strong>ir leng<strong>the</strong>ned childhood vegetating,<strong>the</strong>y spend it interacting with <strong>the</strong>ir parents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir culture.Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary biologist, Lew<strong>on</strong>tin puts it this way:We are not determined by our genes, although surely we are influencedby <strong>the</strong>m. Development depends not <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> materials that have beeninherited from parents – that is, <strong>the</strong> genes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r materials in <strong>the</strong>sperm <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> egg – but also <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> particular temperature, humidity,nutriti<strong>on</strong>, smells, sights, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sounds (including what we call educati<strong>on</strong>)that impinge <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> developing organism. (Lew<strong>on</strong>tin 1991, p. 26)Genetic advances in agriculture produce <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> breeds that aredramatically different from parent stocks. Under c<strong>on</strong>trastingc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, seeds that are virtual cl<strong>on</strong>es mature into dramaticallydifferent plants, often so different that <strong>the</strong>y are hardly recognizableas <strong>the</strong> same species. Animal agriculture reveals equally dramaticinteracti<strong>on</strong>s between genes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Behavioral<strong>development</strong> should be more sensitive to envir<strong>on</strong>ment, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>advanced behavior, such as language, should be still moresensitive.BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 285


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Actual studies of children c<strong>on</strong>tradict Chomskian dicta:[D]escripti<strong>on</strong>s of mo<strong>the</strong>rs’ speech to young children were undertakenin <strong>the</strong> late sixties in order to refute <strong>the</strong> prevailing view that languageacquisiti<strong>on</strong> was largely innate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> occurred almost independently of<strong>the</strong> language envir<strong>on</strong>ment. The results of those mo<strong>the</strong>r’s speechstudies may have c<strong>on</strong>tributed to <strong>the</strong> widespread ab<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>on</strong>ment of thishypo<strong>the</strong>sis about language acquisiti<strong>on</strong> but a general shift from syntacticto semantic-cognitive aspects of language acquisiti<strong>on</strong> would probablyhave caused it to lose its central place as a tenet of research in anycase. (Snow 1977, p. 31)Again according to evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary biologist Lew<strong>on</strong>tin (1991),The trouble with <strong>the</strong> general scheme of explanati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tained in <strong>the</strong>metaphor of [genetic program] is that it is bad biology. If we had <strong>the</strong>complete DNA sequence of an organism <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> unlimited computati<strong>on</strong>alpower, we could not compute <strong>the</strong> organism, because <strong>the</strong> organism doesnot compute itself from its genes. Any computer that did as poor a jobof computati<strong>on</strong> as an organism does from its genetic “program” wouldbe immediately thrown into <strong>the</strong> trash <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its manufacturer would besued by <strong>the</strong> purchaser. (p. 17)L&B emphasize lengthy human <strong>development</strong> from infancy toadolescence. A human child develops by interacting <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> experiencingra<strong>the</strong>r than by incubating <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> unfolding like a flower in apot. In behavioral biology, cross-fostering – parents of <strong>on</strong>egenetic strain rearing infants of ano<strong>the</strong>r genetic strain – is atool for studying <strong>the</strong> critical interacti<strong>on</strong> between genetic endowment<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>al envir<strong>on</strong>ment (Goodenough et al. 1993;Stamps 2003). Cross-fostered gulls adopt species-specificmigratory habits of <strong>the</strong>ir adoptive parents (Harris 1970). Crossfosteredcockatoos adopt species-specific flying <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> feedinghabits of <strong>the</strong>ir adoptive parents (Rowley & Chapman 1986).Cross-fostered voles adopt species-specific maternal habits of<strong>the</strong>ir adoptive parents (McGuire 1988). B. T. Gardner <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Gardner (1989) cross-fostered infant chimpanzees in nearlyhuman households to study <strong>the</strong> effect of a human <strong>development</strong>alenvir<strong>on</strong>ment <strong>on</strong> a closely related species.In Gardner <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gardner (1980) early sign language vocabulariesof chimpanzees Moja, Pili, Tatu, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Dar overlapped wi<strong>the</strong>arly vocabularies of human children as much as child vocabulariesoverlap with each o<strong>the</strong>r. L&B’s lengthy <strong>development</strong>alroad to language implies a gradual, stage-by-stage process. InB. T. Gardner <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gardner (1998) semantic relati<strong>on</strong>s, thatappeared in <strong>the</strong> early phrases of Moja, Tatu, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Dar, appearedin <strong>the</strong> same <strong>development</strong>al sequence reported for human children.Nominative <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> acti<strong>on</strong> phrases appeared first, attributivessec<strong>on</strong>d, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> experience/notice latest in <strong>development</strong>al samples ofchildren <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cross-fostered chimpanzees.L&B emphasize pragmatic <strong>development</strong>. In studies of casualc<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> (Bodamer & Gardner 2002; Jensvold & Gardner2000), cross-fostered chimpanzees used expansi<strong>on</strong>, reiterati<strong>on</strong>,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> incorporati<strong>on</strong> to maintain <strong>the</strong> topic of a c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> ashuman adults <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human children do. C<strong>on</strong>tingencies of chimpanzeerejoinders to probes were comparable to c<strong>on</strong>tingenciesreported for human children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, in adult cross-fosterlings,more comparable to older children than to younger children.In Shaw (2000), adult cross-fosterlings integrated gaze directi<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> turn-taking into c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> way human speakers<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> signers integrate gaze directi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> turn-taking into <strong>the</strong>irc<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> with a pattern of <strong>development</strong> from infant toadult that resembles human <strong>development</strong>. Both directi<strong>on</strong>almodulati<strong>on</strong> to indicate pers<strong>on</strong>, place, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> instrument, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> quantitativemodulati<strong>on</strong> to indicate intensity are essential pragmaticfeatures of human sign language. In Rimpau et al. (1989) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Chalcraft <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gardner (2005), directi<strong>on</strong>al modulati<strong>on</strong> to indicatepers<strong>on</strong>, place, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> instrument appeared in c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>alsamples of Dar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tatu as infant cross-fosterlings. In Chalcraft<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gardner (2005), quantitative modulati<strong>on</strong>s observed inhuman sign language also appeared in Tatu’s infant c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>.This article by L&B moves squarely in <strong>the</strong> path away fromdoctrinaire <strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> points forward to open-ended discoveryas expressed so well by Bruner (1978):bridging of gaps that before were not so much empty as <strong>the</strong>y were filledwith corrosive dogmatism. The gaps between prelinguistic communicati<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language proper as <strong>the</strong> child develops, <strong>the</strong> gap betweengesture <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> word, between holophrases <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sentences, betweenchimps signing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> man talking, between sign languages <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> spoken<strong>on</strong>es, between <strong>the</strong> structure of acti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> structure of language.I think that <strong>the</strong> re<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>al of interest in language as an interactive, communicativesystem has made <strong>the</strong>se “gaps” less like battle grounds where<strong>on</strong>e fights <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dies for <strong>the</strong> uniqueness of man <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more like unknownseas to be mapped. (p. viii)Dynamic systems <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> oflanguageLakshmi J. GogatePsychiatry #1203, State University of New York Health Science Center atBrooklyn, Brooklyn, NY 11203.Lakshmi.Gogate@downstate.eduAbstract: Locke & Bogin (L&B) suggest that <strong>the</strong>oretical principles of<strong>on</strong>togenetic <strong>development</strong> apply to language evoluti<strong>on</strong>. If this is <strong>the</strong>case, <strong>the</strong>n evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>the</strong>ory should utilize epigenetic <strong>the</strong>ories of<strong>development</strong> to <strong>the</strong>orize, model, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> elucidate <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> oflanguage wherever possible. In this commentary, I evoke principles ofdynamic systems <strong>the</strong>ory to evaluate <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary phenomenapresented in <strong>the</strong> target article.Locke & Bogin (L&B) underscore <strong>the</strong> role of <strong>on</strong>togeny in evoluti<strong>on</strong>,to set it apart from several recent language evoluti<strong>on</strong>proposals that have ascribed a less significant role to <strong>on</strong>togeny(e.g., Christiansen & Kirby 2003a; but see Johanss<strong>on</strong> 2005,p. 31; also Oyama et al. 2001 for a general <strong>the</strong>ory). L&B’saccount is comprehensive, with recent, much-needed researchc<strong>on</strong>stituting its foundati<strong>on</strong>. Specifically, <strong>the</strong>y illustrate an importantc<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> between evolving increases in <strong>the</strong> durati<strong>on</strong> spanningchildhood, culminating in a unique <strong>development</strong>al phase inhumans – adolescence – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evolving language abilities. Thegeneral <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> adopted in this article, however, is notentirely <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>. It echoes Oyama’s (2001) general idea that processesof evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> bear striking similarities(Studdert-Kennedy [1991] provides a similar view <strong>on</strong> language;also see Haeckel 1899).Throughout <strong>the</strong> target article, by adopting <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>framework, <strong>the</strong> authors advocate that <strong>the</strong>oretical principles<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phenomena of <strong>on</strong>togenetic <strong>development</strong> are applicable tolanguage evoluti<strong>on</strong>. For example, articulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>ologywere likely precursors to syntax in language evoluti<strong>on</strong> as in <strong>on</strong>togeny.If indeed <strong>on</strong>togeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phylogeny are closely related in thismanner, <strong>the</strong>n evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>the</strong>ory could benefit exp<strong>on</strong>entiallyfrom adopting epigenetic <strong>the</strong>ories of <strong>on</strong>togenetic <strong>development</strong>to <strong>the</strong>orize, model, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> elucidate, wherever possible, aspects of<strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language in humans. In this commentary, <strong>the</strong>refore,I draw up<strong>on</strong> three principles of epigenetic <strong>the</strong>ories of <strong>development</strong>,in particular dynamic systems <strong>the</strong>ory (henceforth DST;Thelen & Smith 1994; 1998; cf. Lickliter, in press; Oyama et. al.2001), to evaluate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> highlight some evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary phenomenaelaborated up<strong>on</strong> by L&B. The primary purpose of this exerciseis to integrate <strong>the</strong> principles underlying, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> draw parallelsbetween, changing systems of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>span <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> species-specific <strong>development</strong>.After all, <strong>the</strong> forces that drive species to evolve can beexplained <strong>on</strong>ly within a framework that makes explicit (a) <strong>the</strong>principles underlying <strong>the</strong> dynamics of complex biologicalsystems, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> (b) <strong>the</strong> multiple causes that drive dynamicalsystems to change over numerous time scales, within <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>span.286 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>According to DST (Thelen & Smith 1998; cf. Lickliter, inpress; Oyama 2001), form is a product of process: Changes indeveloping systems are not predetermined by a gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> design.To <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trary, <strong>the</strong>y are shaped at <strong>the</strong> local level during <strong>the</strong>process of an organism’s interacti<strong>on</strong> with its immediate envir<strong>on</strong>ment.Similarly, <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language did not take place in avacuum (Johanns<strong>on</strong> 2005). Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary changes culminating inlanguage were <strong>the</strong> result of selecti<strong>on</strong>al pressures at <strong>the</strong> local level,always shaped by interacti<strong>on</strong>s between members of <strong>the</strong> species<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir immediate envir<strong>on</strong>ment. L&B provide rich examplesof <strong>the</strong>se local-level selecti<strong>on</strong>al pressures that resulted in keychanges that, in turn, facilitated <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>set of language in evolvinghumans. For example, extensi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> period of brain volumeincrease into postnatal <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, to ease <strong>the</strong> obstetric problem ofcephalo-pelvic disproporti<strong>on</strong> in human females (caused bybipedalism), likely caused increased dependency of infants <strong>on</strong>adults, requiring extensive postnatal care. Greater physical h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>lingby human caregivers, in turn, resulted in greater socialstimulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infant-caregiver interacti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> greater neurological<strong>development</strong>, eventually facilitating language in humans.The preceding example also highlights a sec<strong>on</strong>d tenet of DST:multiple causality in emerging systems. According to this tenet,changes occurring in a system – neurological, physiological, orbehavioral – are <strong>the</strong> complex product of interacti<strong>on</strong> betweenmany factors at multiple levels (Lickliter, in press). Therefore,attributing primary causal status to any <strong>on</strong>e factor or level isinadequate. This tenet, also applicable to language evoluti<strong>on</strong>, iswell represented in Oyama’s idea that change occurs via interactiveco-c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>: “The idea of c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> through interacti<strong>on</strong>of many different factors is applicable to evoluti<strong>on</strong> aswell as <strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> it highlights striking similaritiesbetween <strong>the</strong> two processes” (Oyama 2001, p.6).Third, according to DST (Thelen & Smith 1998), stability orlack of variability is inimical to change. Changes in an emergingsystem are driven by instability or perturbati<strong>on</strong>s (at <strong>the</strong> locallevel) during <strong>on</strong>going organism-envir<strong>on</strong>ment interacti<strong>on</strong>s(Kelso 1997). Therefore, a few collective variables, if accuratelyidentified, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> collective organism’s ability to self-organizein resp<strong>on</strong>se to <strong>the</strong> changes in <strong>the</strong>se variables (or c<strong>on</strong>trolparameters) can explain change at any given point in time. Toidentify <strong>the</strong>se c<strong>on</strong>trol parameters, evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>the</strong>ory mustturn to robust empirical findings from <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-span <strong>development</strong>,as prescribed <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> practiced in <strong>the</strong> target article.Finally, because much <strong>the</strong>orizing about how language evolveddepends <strong>on</strong> post hoc evidence from <strong>on</strong>togeny or <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, as<strong>the</strong> authors suggest, predicting changes al<strong>on</strong>g extended timescalesmight be possible by turning to <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>ly emerging fieldof epigenetic robotics. In this field, computati<strong>on</strong>al modeling ofhuman <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its dynamics over multiple time scalesis feasible. It involves <strong>the</strong> design <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> of artificialsystems with adaptive, <strong>development</strong>al algorithms, using key parametersthat c<strong>on</strong>stitute specific behaviors of developing naturalsystems (Blank et al. 2005; see review by Prince et al. 2005).Future epigenetic models of language evoluti<strong>on</strong> would requirearchitecture that is not fully specified. Fully specified systemsare by nature predetermined by a gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> design (e.g., seeHurford 2002; Kirby 1999b). These systems nei<strong>the</strong>r developnor evolve by adapting to <strong>the</strong>ir local envir<strong>on</strong>ment, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>reforeare not accurate models of natural evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary processes. Analternative approach to modeling evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary changes in biologicalsystems would require designing artificial systems to simulateself-organizing behavior <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>going envir<strong>on</strong>mental interacti<strong>on</strong>sover extended time scales. These systems would adapt to perturbati<strong>on</strong>sat a local level (or levels).In c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>, future models of language evoluti<strong>on</strong> would needto include key parameters that do not fall strictly within <strong>the</strong>language domain. For example, c<strong>on</strong>sistent with <strong>development</strong>al<strong>the</strong>ory (e.g., Gogate et al. 2001; 2006), models of language evoluti<strong>on</strong>should find it useful to include <strong>the</strong> sensory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intersensoryprecursors to language <strong>development</strong>, to accurately model <strong>the</strong>process of language evoluti<strong>on</strong> from basic perceptual processes(Dominey & Boucher [2005] model syntactic acquisiti<strong>on</strong> usingauditory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> visual inputs; Prince & Hollich [2005] modelinfant auditory-visual percepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> word-mapping). Thisdomain-general approach to modeling language evoluti<strong>on</strong>would complement <strong>the</strong> multi-domain <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> embraced byL&B in <strong>the</strong>ir <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> approach to language evoluti<strong>on</strong>.Why d<strong>on</strong>’t chimps talk <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> humans sing likecanaries?Sverker Johanss<strong>on</strong>, a Jordan Zlatev, b <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Peter Gärdenfors ca School of Educati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Communicati<strong>on</strong>, University of Jönköping, Jönköping,SE-551 11, Sweden; b Department of Linguistics, Center for <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Literature, Lund University, Box 201, Lund, SE-221 00, Sweden; c CognitiveScience, Kungshuset, Lundagård, Lund University, Lund, SE-222 22, Sweden.lsj@hlk.hj.se http://home.hj.se/ lsj jordan.zlatev@ling.lu.sehttp://www.ling.lu.se/pers<strong>on</strong>s/JordanZlatev.html?language ¼ Englishpeter.gardenfors@lucs.lu.sehttp://www.fil.lu.se/lucs/staff/pers<strong>on</strong>.asp?id ¼ 42Abstract: We focus <strong>on</strong> two problems with <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary scenarioproposed: (1) It bypasses <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> origins of <strong>the</strong>communicative <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> semiotic features that make language distinct from,say, pleasant but meaningless sounds. (2) It does little to explain <strong>the</strong>absence of language in, for example, chimpanzees: Most of <strong>the</strong>selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures invoked apply just as str<strong>on</strong>gly to chimps. Wesuggest how <strong>the</strong>se problems could possibly be amended.We agree with Locke & Bogin (L&B) <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> importance of c<strong>on</strong>sidering<strong>on</strong>togeny in evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary studies. The unusual hum<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>evelopmental pattern may indeed provide insights c<strong>on</strong>cerninglanguage origins. The emphasis of L&B <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pragmaticaspects of language is likewise commendable. However,<strong>the</strong>se need to be integrated within an account of <strong>the</strong> origins of<strong>the</strong> semantic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammatical aspects that make language aunique semiotic system allowing <strong>the</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong> of detachedrepresentati<strong>on</strong>s (Gärdenfors 2003) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> ofnarrative (D<strong>on</strong>ald 1991). Since this is not <strong>the</strong> case, <strong>the</strong>re are atleast two major problems with <strong>the</strong> specific selective scenariosproposed by L&B.Why not just sing? A major feature of language distinguishingit from all animal vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s is its ability to express an endlessnumber of thoughts in various complex combinati<strong>on</strong>s. A plausiblescenario of language origins needs to explain this enormousexpansi<strong>on</strong> of semiotic functi<strong>on</strong>ality. But Bickert<strong>on</strong>’s argumentagainst Dunbar’s (1993) verbal-grooming hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, aboutwhich he writes “a similar result could have been achievedsimply by using pleasant but meaningless noises” (Bickert<strong>on</strong>2003, p. 79), applies to a c<strong>on</strong>siderable extent against L&B’sscenario as well. Or as an informant of Folb (1980), quoted byL&B, puts it: “D<strong>on</strong>’ hafta make whole buncha sense, l<strong>on</strong>gsounds pretty” (sect. 7 of <strong>the</strong> target article).The first stage in L&B’s two-stage scenario c<strong>on</strong>cerns parentalcareelicitati<strong>on</strong> during infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> childhood. There are certainlystr<strong>on</strong>g selective pressures at work here, but, as noted by L&B, <strong>the</strong>interests of parents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> offspring do not coincide. This wouldmore likely drive <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of manipulative signals ra<strong>the</strong>rthan cooperative interacti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> communicati<strong>on</strong>, as it has, forexample, am<strong>on</strong>g many birds.The sec<strong>on</strong>d stage c<strong>on</strong>cerns sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> young adults. The comm<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>me in L&B’s lengthy discussi<strong>on</strong>of oral societies is <strong>the</strong> importance of form <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> presentati<strong>on</strong>over c<strong>on</strong>tent. One would reas<strong>on</strong>ably expect this to drive <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>of ever more elaborate form – but why would it drive <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>tent? Isn’t it more likely that a scenario drivenby sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> status competiti<strong>on</strong> would result in somethingmore resembling birds<strong>on</strong>g than language? Invoking sex <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 287


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>status as important factors in language origins is in itself notimplausible. But in order to explain <strong>the</strong> distinguishing featuresof language, selecti<strong>on</strong> for c<strong>on</strong>tent must be part of <strong>the</strong> scenario.The proposal of Miller (2000), discussed by L&B at <strong>the</strong> end ofsecti<strong>on</strong> 5, goes in <strong>the</strong> right directi<strong>on</strong>: “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> put minds <strong>on</strong>public display, where sexual choice could see <strong>the</strong>m clearly”(Miller 2000, p. 357, quoted in L&B). Related ideas are proposedby Dessalles (2000), in which status is gained through publicspeech c<strong>on</strong>taining relevant informati<strong>on</strong>, thus placing selectivepressure squarely <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> ability to provide c<strong>on</strong>tent. L&B dorefer to <strong>the</strong> work of Dessalles, but by focusing too much <strong>on</strong>form ra<strong>the</strong>r than c<strong>on</strong>tent, <strong>the</strong>y leave a gaping hole in <strong>the</strong>irargumentati<strong>on</strong>.L&B do attempt to motivate <strong>the</strong> primacy of vocal abilities overgrammar in <strong>the</strong>ir scenario, by arguing that it makes more sensefor speech to evolve before syntax than vice versa. This is notself-evident, for several reas<strong>on</strong>s. First, grammar is not just“syntax” but patterned c<strong>on</strong>tent, which has been emphasized fordecades by functi<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive linguists (e.g., Langacker1987). But L&B d<strong>on</strong>’t even refer to this healthy traditi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mistakenlyseem to equate “linguistics” with Chomskyan linguistics.From <strong>the</strong> cognitive <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>, it is completely possible thatmanual gestures <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> bodily mimesis provided <strong>the</strong> basis for propositi<strong>on</strong>alstructure (Armstr<strong>on</strong>g et al. 1995; Corballis 2002; D<strong>on</strong>ald1991; Zlatev et al. 2005), a possibility that L&B shunt aside in afootnote. And even if speech were <strong>the</strong> original modality, coevoluti<strong>on</strong>between speech capacity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammar should not bediscounted.In brief, by separating vocal abilities from c<strong>on</strong>tent <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>grammar, <strong>the</strong> scenario of L&B is hardly a scenario for languageorigins anymore. The aspects of language that are uniquelyhuman remain unexplained.And why not chimps? In <strong>the</strong> case of specifically humanadaptati<strong>on</strong>s like language, it is not enough to present anevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary scenario showing how useful language would befor our apelike ancestors. The questi<strong>on</strong> that needs to beanswered is not just why our ancestors evolved language, butalso why chimps did not, even though we share a fairly recentcomm<strong>on</strong> ancestor. It must be shown that language was useful<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adaptive specifically for proto-humans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not for protochimpanzees.Many language-origin scenarios fail this test(Bickert<strong>on</strong> 2002; Gärdenfors 2004; Johanss<strong>on</strong> 2005).Using vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s strategically in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of parental-careelicitati<strong>on</strong> is hardly unique for humans – many birds <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mammals, including primates, do so. Given that human childhoodas defined by L&B is <strong>the</strong> result of earlier weaning, thatis, a decrease in parental care allowing diverting resources from<strong>the</strong> current child to its future siblings, it is not clear how thiscan functi<strong>on</strong> as a selecti<strong>on</strong> pressure for human toddlers to bebetter at eliciting care than <strong>the</strong>ir chimp cousins, who stillmanage to get breast-fed at that age.In <strong>the</strong> vocal competiti<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents, impressiveness<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong>-holding are important factors. As noted by L&Bin <strong>the</strong> beginning of secti<strong>on</strong> 5, <strong>the</strong> same factors are important inestablishing status also am<strong>on</strong>g n<strong>on</strong>human primates. Again, it isnot at all obvious why this led to language am<strong>on</strong>g protohumansbut not proto-chimps, despite both being subject tosimilar selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures in similar c<strong>on</strong>texts.Similarly, at <strong>the</strong> end of secti<strong>on</strong> 9.1, L&B claim that for younghominids “even a small amount of vocal-verbal behavior wouldhave facilitated warnings <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructi<strong>on</strong>,” but <strong>the</strong>y do notexplain why <strong>the</strong> same mechanisms would not apply to protochimps.Still, we do believe that buried in L&B’s account <strong>the</strong>re is <strong>the</strong>germ that may save <strong>the</strong>m from <strong>the</strong> “why not apes?” test. It isspelled out in secti<strong>on</strong> 13: “bipedalism is ... a key event ... forthis ... produced, in time, pelvic narrowing. Remodeling of <strong>the</strong>birth canal caused a shift of skull <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> brain <strong>development</strong> into<strong>the</strong> postnatal period, increasing <strong>the</strong> degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> durati<strong>on</strong> ofinfant helplessness” (target article, sect. 13, para. 2). Thistoge<strong>the</strong>r with <strong>the</strong> need of bipedal mo<strong>the</strong>rs to carry <strong>the</strong>iryoung, would have led to <strong>the</strong> need for more cooperati<strong>on</strong> ra<strong>the</strong>rthan competiti<strong>on</strong> between mo<strong>the</strong>rs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infants, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to anincrease in <strong>the</strong> quality of dyadic interacti<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> form ofmutual gaze, mutual imitati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> proto-c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s (Falk2004). These are species-typical characteristics of humanbeings laid down in <strong>the</strong> first nine m<strong>on</strong>ths of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> serving as<strong>the</strong> basis for <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of joint attenti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pointing in<strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d year (Hobs<strong>on</strong> 2002; Tomasello et al. 2005), whichare milest<strong>on</strong>es of infancy bootstrapping into language – ofwhich L&B say nothing.C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>. L&B’s basic proposal that <strong>the</strong>re may bec<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between <strong>the</strong>se two uniquely human features,language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, is interesting <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> worth pursuingfur<strong>the</strong>r. But more care should be taken in designing selecti<strong>on</strong>scenarios, so that <strong>the</strong> proposed selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures actuallywork in <strong>the</strong> right directi<strong>on</strong>, towards <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of <strong>the</strong>“socially shared symbolic system” (Nels<strong>on</strong> & Shaw 2002) – thatis, language – in <strong>the</strong> human lineage <strong>on</strong>ly.The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of childhood as a by-product?Peter KappelerDepartment of Sociobiology/Anthropology, University of Göttingen, 37073Göttingen, Germany.pkappel@gwdg.de http://www.soziobio.uni-goettingen.de/Abstract: The propositi<strong>on</strong> that selective advantages of linguistic skillshave c<strong>on</strong>tributed to shifts in <strong>on</strong>togenetic l<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>marks of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>histories in early Homo sapiens is weakened by neglecting alternativemechanisms of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Moreover, arguments aboutbiological c<strong>on</strong>tinuity through sweeping comparis<strong>on</strong>s with n<strong>on</strong>humanprimates do not support various assumpti<strong>on</strong>s of this scenario.The evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary scenario c<strong>on</strong>cerning <strong>the</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> between <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language evoluti<strong>on</strong> developed by Locke & Bogin(L&B) suffers from two weaknesses. First, while L&B’s focus<strong>on</strong> early stages of <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> cycle is <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> laudable, <strong>the</strong>yfail to recognize <strong>the</strong> possibility that shifts in <strong>the</strong> relative durati<strong>on</strong>of early <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> l<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>marks can also be brought about by selecti<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong> fitness-relevant traits or events later in <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Selecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> adelay in <strong>the</strong> age of first reproducti<strong>on</strong> is <strong>the</strong> best-studied examplein this c<strong>on</strong>text (Promislow & Harvey 1990). It is <strong>the</strong>refore notnecessarily <strong>the</strong> case that “Selecti<strong>on</strong> for vocal ability, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, ultimately,for language would thus have worked reciprocally toextend childhood” (sect. 2.4 of <strong>the</strong> target article). Developmental<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary processes are not clearly separated in <strong>the</strong>ir <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> model. It is <strong>the</strong>refore at least equally plausible that ageof first reproducti<strong>on</strong> has been delayed fur<strong>the</strong>r in Homo sapiens,compared to Pan, Australopi<strong>the</strong>cus, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r species of Homo,for adaptive reas<strong>on</strong>s unrelated to language, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that <strong>the</strong> extratime that became available for juveniles was subsequently usedto develop more refined linguistic skills. Alternatively, linguisticskills acquired early in <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> may have offset some of <strong>the</strong> costs ofdelayed maturity. The hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that <strong>the</strong>se skills were rewardedlater in <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> through mate choice, as originally proposed by Miller(2000), is c<strong>on</strong>vincingly elaborated by <strong>the</strong> authors. However, <strong>the</strong>sebenefits accrue primarily to males, whereas <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>acts primarily <strong>on</strong> females (Stearns 1976) – a discrepancy also notaddressed by <strong>the</strong> authors’ <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> model.Sec<strong>on</strong>d, when discussing various aspects of language <strong>development</strong>,<strong>the</strong> authors use several primate examples in an attempt tosupport <strong>the</strong>ir arguments. However, some of <strong>the</strong>se examplesc<strong>on</strong>sist of sweeping generalizati<strong>on</strong>s across species, sexes, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>age classes that provide weak analogies, at best, to supportclaims about human uniqueness or biological c<strong>on</strong>tinuity. Thefact that a behavior pattern exists “in primates” does not logicallyimply that early humans behaved <strong>the</strong> same way. There is toomuch variability in behavior am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> hundreds of species of288 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>living primates for such generalizing claims, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> we know verylittle about relevant behavioral attributes in extinct primates(Plavcan & van Schaik 1997). The parental selecti<strong>on</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis(sect. 3.2), for example, supposes that infant stress vocalizati<strong>on</strong>sinvite neglect <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> abuse “in primates generally.” Whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>majority of <strong>the</strong> more than 300 living primate species willexhibit <strong>the</strong> same resp<strong>on</strong>se remains to be studied, but in <strong>the</strong> fewprimate species where mo<strong>the</strong>r-infant interacti<strong>on</strong>s have beenstudied in any detail, this is not <strong>the</strong> case (Gouzoules & Gouzoules2002). Generalizati<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> opposite directi<strong>on</strong> are equallyunhelpful. For example, <strong>the</strong> asserti<strong>on</strong> that “apes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> m<strong>on</strong>keysdo not vocalize as often as humans” (sect. 3) can be refutedwith examples of dozens of primate species that permanentlyutter vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s. Humans are <strong>the</strong>refore not unique in thisrespect. Thus, although this article presents some interesting<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> approaches to <strong>the</strong> discussi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> origins of language,I doubt that it will be <strong>the</strong> final word <strong>on</strong> this topic.Apes, humans, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> M. C. Escher: Uniqueness<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tinuity in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of languageBarbara J. KingDepartment of Anthropology, College of William <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Mary, Williamsburg, VA23187.bjking@wm.eduhttp://www.councilhd.ca/announce/announce.htmAbstract: Ontogeny, specifically <strong>the</strong> role of language in <strong>the</strong> human familynow <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in pre<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, is central to Locke & Bogin’s (L&B’s) <strong>the</strong>sis in acompelling way. The unique <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages of childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>adolescence, however, must be interpreted not <strong>on</strong>ly against anexcepti<strong>on</strong>ally “high quality” human infancy but also in light of <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of co-c<strong>on</strong>structed, emoti<strong>on</strong>ally based communicati<strong>on</strong> in ape,hominid, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human infancy.Locke & Bogin (L&B) bring toge<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>vincing data <strong>on</strong> primate<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages with informed speculati<strong>on</strong> about <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>of language. I limit my comments to <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship between <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-stage.Central to that part of L&B’s argument are <strong>the</strong> followingasserti<strong>on</strong>s:1. Few recent proposals about <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language havegiven a primary role to matters of <strong>on</strong>togeny.2. In humans, <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages of childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescenceare added to <strong>the</strong> three stages – infancy, juvenility, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>adulthood – found in o<strong>the</strong>r social mammals.3. Compared to o<strong>the</strong>r primates, <strong>the</strong> quality of human infancyis enhanced by greater social stimulati<strong>on</strong> of offspring by <strong>the</strong>irparents. L&B’s parental selecti<strong>on</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis suggests thatinfants who were better at vocal engagement with <strong>the</strong>ir parentssecured greater care <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, in turn, learned more complex ph<strong>on</strong>eticpatterns. Then, with <strong>the</strong> advent of uniquely human childhood,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of <strong>the</strong> family, <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> opportunities arosethat drove <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language via “negotiati<strong>on</strong> of morestructured <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complex forms of vocalizati<strong>on</strong>” (sect. 9).L&B’s emphasis <strong>on</strong> parent-infant engagement in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of<strong>the</strong> family is a welcome additi<strong>on</strong> to a swelling cache of <strong>the</strong>oreticalformulati<strong>on</strong>s that – c<strong>on</strong>tra <strong>the</strong> first statement above – place<strong>on</strong>togeny fr<strong>on</strong>t <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> center when rec<strong>on</strong>structing <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> oflanguage from n<strong>on</strong>human primate communicati<strong>on</strong>. Humanscommunicate across <strong>the</strong> generati<strong>on</strong>s not as encoders <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> decodersof informati<strong>on</strong> using syntax <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantics but as emoti<strong>on</strong>allyattuned creatures who create meaning toge<strong>the</strong>r as <strong>the</strong>y go about<strong>the</strong>ir daily lives. An emphasis <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of this emoti<strong>on</strong>alengagement (Greenspan & Shanker 2004; Mi<strong>the</strong>n 2005) iscrucial as scholars across <strong>the</strong> disciplines apply <strong>the</strong>mselves withre<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>ed excitement to experimental <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>oretical work <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language (Fitch 2005). Crucially, it arguesagainst a current vogue to “fracti<strong>on</strong>[ate] language into multipleinteracting comp<strong>on</strong>ents” (Fitch 2005, p. 216). “Fracti<strong>on</strong>ating”means looking at speech, syntax, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantics as discretesystems in order to trace <strong>the</strong>ir evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary roots separately.Some insights do emerge from this approach, but real progressrequires that “mechanisms” of language be c<strong>on</strong>sidered in<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>togeny of meaning-making – that is, in ac<strong>on</strong>text that situates <strong>the</strong> origins of language in parent-childcaregiver practices.Some progress has been made already. Startlingly, L&B c<strong>on</strong>struct<strong>the</strong>ir parental selecti<strong>on</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis without reference to<strong>on</strong>togeny-focused scenarios by Borchert <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Zihlman (1990),Falk (2004), Parker (2000), or Savage-Rumbaugh (1994). Collectively<strong>the</strong>se formulati<strong>on</strong>s support a view of Homo infants assupremely vulnerable, owing to <strong>the</strong> coupling of bipedalism <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>big brains. More importantly, <strong>the</strong>y indicate that L&B’s claimsfor human uniqueness (statements two <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> three above) arebest supported by a firm grasp of <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuity in <strong>development</strong>of meaning-making in apes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> humans. This simultaneousembracing of human uniqueness, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of c<strong>on</strong>tinuitywith our closest living relatives, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r, is no paradox.Ra<strong>the</strong>r, it is reminiscent of Escher’s famous drawing in which ahard look transforms fish-in-<strong>the</strong>-water to birds-in-<strong>the</strong>-sky. Look<strong>on</strong>e way at <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> what comes intofocus is human uniqueness; look ano<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> you will seeape-human c<strong>on</strong>tinuity.For wild <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> captive apes, emoti<strong>on</strong>al engagement of infantswith <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>rs, siblings, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r social partners motivates,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> even more so enables, <strong>the</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong> of certain (though notall) communicati<strong>on</strong>al skills. Writing toge<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> separately(Fogel et al. 2006; Greenspan & Shanker 2004; King 2004;King, in press; Shanker & King 2002), Stuart Shanker <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ihave laid out a series of ideas about <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of affectivemeaning-making that are grounded in data from apes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> children.We argue that <strong>the</strong> “vocal-verbal” advances highlighted byL&B (see, e.g., sect. 3.2) evolve from <strong>the</strong> multimodal communicati<strong>on</strong>of primates already highly skilled, from infancy <strong>on</strong>wards, atparticipating in mutually c<strong>on</strong>structive meaning-making. Ourunderst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing of meaning-making involves not <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>duit-liketransfer of informati<strong>on</strong> from sender to receiver in linearfashi<strong>on</strong>, but ra<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> mutual transformati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> acti<strong>on</strong>s,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> perhaps <strong>the</strong> thoughts <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> moods, of two partners in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>textsof ever-changing interacti<strong>on</strong>s (see also Reddy 1993).Posture, gesture, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> facial expressi<strong>on</strong>s are central here, as wellas vocal behavior.To say that after <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d birthday, “infants develop a suiteof cognitive traits that will enable language to be used at a basallevel of creativity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> efficiency” (sect. 2.2) is, <strong>the</strong>n, awkwardshorth<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> at best. It is not cognitive traits al<strong>on</strong>e that enableincreasingly proficient language usage, but also <strong>the</strong> emoti<strong>on</strong>alengagement of infants with <strong>the</strong>ir caretakers; this back-<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>-forthcommunicati<strong>on</strong>, playful <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> loving in some cultures, serious<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> less explicitly affective in o<strong>the</strong>rs, unlocks <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> indeed helpsto create <strong>the</strong> infant’s abilities. What Homo sapiens parents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>infants do toge<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>y have evolved to do toge<strong>the</strong>r; vocalverbalbehavior increases in importance while remaining partof a communicati<strong>on</strong> system that is multimodal.The “greater h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ling required by <strong>the</strong> human infant” as aresult of <strong>the</strong> shift from quadrupedalism to bipedalism doesproduce “more intense social stimulati<strong>on</strong> during a period inwhich <strong>the</strong> brain grows at a compensatorily rapid rate” (sect.2.1). Sec<strong>on</strong>darily, altricial human babies are appreciably differentthan ape babies; brain-growth pattern are distinct in <strong>the</strong> Homolineage. Yet <strong>the</strong> parental social stimulati<strong>on</strong> of infants in hominids<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Homo sapiens evolved from a str<strong>on</strong>g foundati<strong>on</strong> of emoti<strong>on</strong>alengagement present in our ape ancestors. The linguistic accomplishmentsin later <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages in Homo sapiens flow from <strong>the</strong>powerful dynamic foundati<strong>on</strong> set in infancy, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, indeed,should vary with <strong>the</strong> emoti<strong>on</strong>al signature of that parent-infantinteracti<strong>on</strong>. Testable predicti<strong>on</strong>s at both <strong>the</strong> phylogenetic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 289


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>development</strong>al levels follow logically – for example, greaterc<strong>on</strong>tingency <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> emoti<strong>on</strong>al nuance in cross-generati<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong>should lead (whe<strong>the</strong>r phylogenetically or <strong>development</strong>ally)to more advanced communicati<strong>on</strong> (for details, seeGreenspan & Shanker 2004; King 2004). Future primatologicalresearch al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>se lines will surely c<strong>on</strong>tribute in serious waysto evoluti<strong>on</strong>-of-language <strong>the</strong>orizing.Words are not costly displays: Shortcomingsof a testoster<strong>on</strong>e-fuelled model of languageevoluti<strong>on</strong>Chris Knight <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Camilla PowerDepartment of Anthropology, School of Social Sciences, University of EastL<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>, L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> E16 2RD, United Kingdom.chris.knight@uel.ac.uk c.c.power@uel.ac.ukhttp://homepages.uel.ac.uk/C.Knight/Abstract: Only by misc<strong>on</strong>struing <strong>the</strong> term performative are <strong>the</strong> authorsable to argue that males surpass females in “performative applicati<strong>on</strong>s”of language. Linguistic performatives are not costly displays of quality,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> syntax cannot be explained as an outcome of behaviouralcompetiti<strong>on</strong> between pubertal males. However, <strong>the</strong>re is room for amodel in which language co-evolves with <strong>the</strong> unique human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>stage of adolescence.This target article attempts an ambitious syn<strong>the</strong>sis. It is high timethat speculati<strong>on</strong>s about language evoluti<strong>on</strong> were grounded in anadequate underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>.Where <strong>the</strong> article deals with human growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> itappears authoritative; however, <strong>the</strong> specifically linguistic secti<strong>on</strong>sare less c<strong>on</strong>vincing.Locke & Bogin (L&B) claim that “performative applicati<strong>on</strong>s oflanguage ...c<strong>on</strong>sistently favor males” (sect. 5.1, para. 3, emphasisin original). In linguistics, <strong>the</strong> term performative is subject toprecise definiti<strong>on</strong>. Austin (1975: 14) stipulates that “<strong>the</strong>re mustexist an accepted c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al procedure having a certainc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al effect, that procedure to include <strong>the</strong> uttering ofcertain words by certain pers<strong>on</strong>s in certain circumstances.” Asa “c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al effect,” <strong>the</strong> performative force of an utteranceis abstract <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> instituti<strong>on</strong>al – quite unlike <strong>the</strong> material impactwhich an animal signal is designed to produce. Hence, when abride says “I do” during her wedding cerem<strong>on</strong>y, her metamorphosisinto a wife doesn’t depend <strong>on</strong> how she vocalises thosesounds. Provided <strong>the</strong> circumstances are appropriate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> herintenti<strong>on</strong> clear, <strong>the</strong> physical details of her performance – forexample, whe<strong>the</strong>r she whispers or stridently declaims – are irrelevant.Speakers’ communicative intenti<strong>on</strong>s are accomplishedby being socially recognised (Grice 1989); <strong>the</strong>y are not judgedby reference to physical qualities such as amplitude, stamina,or vigour.L&B make <strong>the</strong>ir sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> case by claiming that“important aspects of language cannot appear until sexualmaturity” (target article, Abstract). By this <strong>the</strong>y mean thatyoung children lack sufficient “real world knowledge”(sect.10) – presumably regarding sexual behaviour – to be able tomake pragmatic inferences about speakers’ intenti<strong>on</strong>s. But <strong>the</strong>presence or absence of adult c<strong>on</strong>tent is irrelevant to <strong>the</strong> presenceor absence of key features of language such as performativeforce, which is wholly within <strong>the</strong> capability of four-year-oldsplaying “let’s pretend.” L&B envisage a juvenile phase duringwhich “teasing, joking, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> gossip” serve “group-orientedgoals” (sect. 9). This is unc<strong>on</strong>troversial, but how would such processesbe reinforced through an adolescent phase of intrasexual,epigamic selecti<strong>on</strong>? Can <strong>the</strong> authors clarify <strong>the</strong> circumstances inwhich individualistic male sexual rivalry promotes “grouporientedgoals”?The authors’ evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary model gives pride of place to youthsfighting with rap as chimpanzees pant-hoot or caribou bulls roar.Suggesting that “testoster<strong>on</strong>e promotes verbal dueling” (sect. 6),<strong>the</strong> authors invoke shortages of this horm<strong>on</strong>e to explain whyfemale “performative applicati<strong>on</strong>s” d<strong>on</strong>’t measure up to those ofmales. However, <strong>the</strong>y <strong>the</strong>n let slip an observati<strong>on</strong> that turns thisextraordinary argument <strong>on</strong> its head. Adolescent females, <strong>the</strong>yc<strong>on</strong>cede, gossip against rivals by enlisting “<strong>the</strong> support of peers,greatly surpassing males in this practice” (sect. 6). Only by systematicallyc<strong>on</strong>flating linguistic performatives with bodily performancesdo <strong>the</strong> authors succeed in obfuscating <strong>the</strong> awkward truth:namely, that to enlist <strong>the</strong> support of peers in manipulatingcollective judgements is precisely to deploy “performativeforce.” Here, we encounter a gender bias in “performativeapplicati<strong>on</strong>s” that c<strong>on</strong>tradicts <strong>the</strong>ir entire argument.Gossiping teenage girls, <strong>the</strong>n, compete by enlisting <strong>the</strong> supportof peers in c<strong>on</strong>structing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>testing <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>world. In <strong>the</strong> case of male-<strong>on</strong>-male rap, <strong>the</strong> st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ards are different.As <strong>on</strong>e informant puts it: “D<strong>on</strong>’t hafta make whole bunchsense, l<strong>on</strong>g sounds pretty” (see target article, sect. 7, para. 4).So, while, according to <strong>the</strong> authors, females compete with sociallyrelevant informati<strong>on</strong>, males compete by making pretty sounds.Accepting this c<strong>on</strong>trast for <strong>the</strong> sake of argument, whose strategieswould have driven <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of syntactical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantic complexityin speech? Gossiping is a distinctively linguistic skill(Dunbar 1996). Singing is not. Male-<strong>on</strong>-male vocal competiti<strong>on</strong>may help explain ph<strong>on</strong>ological complexity in <strong>the</strong> s<strong>on</strong>gs of birds,whales, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, arguably, hominin youths; it cannot explain <strong>the</strong>morphosyntactical or semantic complexities of gossip.We readily agree that costly performances are valuable ashard-to-fake indices of individual quality. But how is this relevantto <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language? The issue c<strong>on</strong>cerns more than narrowlyvocal abilities. How <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> why did distinctively human verbalabilities become so decisive in social competiti<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g ourancestors? Am<strong>on</strong>g n<strong>on</strong>human primates, attenti<strong>on</strong> paid to vocalisati<strong>on</strong>smay be symptomatic of dominance, but it is not causative.The reverse is true of humans. Am<strong>on</strong>g hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rers, socialrelati<strong>on</strong>s are best described in terms of “counterdominance”(Erdal & Whiten 1994). In such egalitarian c<strong>on</strong>texts, physicallyunimpressive individuals may gain prestige <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> influencethrough <strong>the</strong>ir verbal fluency. C<strong>on</strong>trary to L&B, <strong>the</strong> pressure <strong>on</strong>speakers is not to show off with spectacular vocal displays.Typically, hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rers avoid signs of pers<strong>on</strong>al ambiti<strong>on</strong> orboastful aggressi<strong>on</strong>. Most valued are c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>alists skilful atmanaging c<strong>on</strong>flicts <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> securing community-wide c<strong>on</strong>sensus.Often, older women have <strong>the</strong> last word. L&B c<strong>on</strong>vey <strong>the</strong> oppositeimpressi<strong>on</strong> by selecting examples of formal oratory typical ofhorticultural “Big Man” societies – as opposed to egalitarianhunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rers who are more likely to be representative ofearly human societies.Unlike animal vocal displays, which are evaluated <strong>on</strong> an analogscale, linguistic messages are digitally encoded. There is nothingintrinsically costly or reliable about a linguistic sign. The distinctivelyhuman language faculty – language in its “narrow” sense –lacks any counterpart in animal social communicati<strong>on</strong> (Hauseret al. 2002), where h<strong>on</strong>esty is underwritten by investment reliablydem<strong>on</strong>strating signal quality (Zahavi & Zahavi 1997). L&B envisagelinguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong> driven by direct behavioural competiti<strong>on</strong>between siblings or adolescent male sexual rivals. But suchdynamics could <strong>on</strong>ly drive <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of signals that areh<strong>on</strong>est because <strong>the</strong>y are costly – exactly what linguistic signsare not.In short, <strong>the</strong> authors show little awareness of <strong>the</strong> scale of challengefacing any <strong>the</strong>ory of language evoluti<strong>on</strong>. To quoteChomsky, language is “based <strong>on</strong> an entirely different principlethan any animal communicati<strong>on</strong> system” (Chomsky 1988,p. 183). As a milest<strong>on</strong>e in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of communicati<strong>on</strong>,“language is off <strong>the</strong> chart” (Chomsky 2002b, p. 146). Above all,what cries out to be explained is <strong>the</strong> abstract computati<strong>on</strong>al principleof digital infinity (Hauser et al. 2002). Instead of attempting290 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>this difficult task, L&B focus <strong>on</strong> features of vocalizati<strong>on</strong> that showc<strong>on</strong>tinuity with analog animal displays.Linguistic topics aside, this article offers an important discussi<strong>on</strong>of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Even here, however, a critical issue is neglected.Modern Darwinism takes account of costs as well asbenefits. What about <strong>the</strong> costs to hominin mo<strong>the</strong>rs in producinglarger-brained, larger-bodied offspring? The combinati<strong>on</strong> ofreduced length of lactati<strong>on</strong> plus extended childhood can bringreproductive advantage <strong>on</strong>ly to mo<strong>the</strong>rs who have reliable allocaresupport. But according to <strong>the</strong>se authors, young males areincreasingly engaged in rap-style sexual display. By c<strong>on</strong>trastwith “show-off” hunting (Hawkes 1991), this offers no materialsupport for mo<strong>the</strong>rs. Would hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rer females preferreliable producers of meat – or clever rappers?The idea of <strong>the</strong> coevoluti<strong>on</strong> of adolescence with language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>symbolic culture is promising; <strong>the</strong> emergence of pubertalinitiati<strong>on</strong> rituals would be central to any such process (Knight2002; Power & Aiello 1997). But <strong>the</strong> selective gender biastowards males needs to be corrected. Am<strong>on</strong>g African hunterga<strong>the</strong>rers,it is not pubertal males but females who undergo <strong>the</strong>most elaborate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> costly initiati<strong>on</strong> rituals.Knowledge of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phrasalvocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>Koenraad KuiperDepartment of Linguistics, University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800,Christchurch, New Zeal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>.k<strong>on</strong>.kuiper@canterbury.ac.nzAbstract: Locke & Bogin’s (L&B’s) main <strong>the</strong>sis can be extended to <strong>the</strong>acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> phrasal vocabulary in that <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of muchphrasal vocabulary combines <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of linguistic knowledgewith pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in that <strong>the</strong> apprenticeship systemfor such learning begins to flower in adolescence.Knowledge of language must involve knowledge of a vocabulary,not just for <strong>the</strong> simple-minded reas<strong>on</strong> that it would not be possibleto say anything without some words to say it with, but also becausesignificant aspects of <strong>the</strong> rest of <strong>the</strong> grammar of a language aredriven from its vocabulary, as is now assumed by many modelsof grammar, for example Minimalism (Chomsky 1996).Therefore, <strong>on</strong>e of <strong>the</strong> central problems for language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>,as for explaining <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language, is how c<strong>on</strong>temporarymembers of <strong>the</strong> species Homo sapiens acquire vocabularyof <strong>the</strong> kind, in <strong>the</strong> quantity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> at <strong>the</strong> rate <strong>the</strong>y do, often <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>basis of apparently little exposure. The acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of a mature,adult vocabulary is a massive task given that in <strong>the</strong> case of aliterate pers<strong>on</strong> a single-word vocabulary is reck<strong>on</strong>ed to bebetween 60,000 (Fromkin 2000) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 150,000 items (Seashore& Eckers<strong>on</strong> 1940), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that <strong>the</strong> phrasal vocabulary is estimatedto be equal to <strong>the</strong> single-word vocabulary (Jackendoff 1995) orto be an order of magnitude larger than this (Mel’čuk 1995,p. 169). Even using a c<strong>on</strong>servative estimate of 120,000 total vocabularyitems, this involves increasing <strong>the</strong> word power by at leastsixteen items per day for every day of a twenty-year-old’s <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>; inMel’čuk’s estimate that would require learning an average ofapproximately ninety items per day.Locke & Bogin’s (L&B’s) hypo<strong>the</strong>ses about human evoluti<strong>on</strong>might be extended as a partial explanati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> fact thathumans are able to acquire vocabulary so rapidly, namely, thatit lies in placing performance <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pragmatics within <strong>the</strong> languagefaculty – that is, intimately c<strong>on</strong>necting <strong>the</strong> use of language with<strong>the</strong> language itself. This is nowhere clearer than in <strong>the</strong> phrasalvocabulary. Kuiper <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tan (1989) have explored in somedetail <strong>the</strong> pragmatic knowledge that must be known for appropriateusage of <strong>the</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong> He’s old enough to be your fa<strong>the</strong>r, forexample.This vocabulary item, which is known by many native speakersof English, is highly c<strong>on</strong>strained in its c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of use. Onelearns it at <strong>the</strong> same time as <strong>on</strong>e learns about <strong>the</strong> nature ofsexual relati<strong>on</strong>ships. But <strong>on</strong>e may <strong>on</strong>ly have heard it <strong>on</strong>ce ortwice. The fact that it is embedded in social practice is <strong>the</strong> linkbetween its acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its utility. It is learned because itmay come in h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>y. Formulae like this abound in a native vocabularywith items ranging from those taught to children as <strong>the</strong>y godoor-to-door <strong>on</strong> Halloween in North America (Berko-Gleas<strong>on</strong>1980) through to <strong>the</strong> arcane expressi<strong>on</strong>s used when crowning aBritish m<strong>on</strong>arch.Such expressi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir use are not c<strong>on</strong>fined to oralsocieties, although <strong>the</strong>se are given, perhaps unnecessarily, preferentialmenti<strong>on</strong> by L&B. Literate societies c<strong>on</strong>tain manysubcultures that are oral societies. Aucti<strong>on</strong>eers (Kuiper &Haggo 1984), supermarket checkout operators, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> priests alllearn a phrasal vocabulary which is as much part of an oral traditi<strong>on</strong>as is that of illiterate bards (Lord 1960). It is learned in<strong>the</strong> same way, as part of being socialised, that is, as part of learning<strong>the</strong> script for a particular role (Goffman 1969). Following L&B, itwould be expected that <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of phrasal vocabulary mustexp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> greatly during adolescence when adult social roles arebeing learned for <strong>the</strong> first time. It is <strong>the</strong>refore not surprisingthat many boys who select roles that involve rich adult oral traditi<strong>on</strong>sdo so during this period <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> apprentice <strong>the</strong>mselves toadult men to acquire <strong>the</strong> necessary vocabulary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> later to practiseits use. Re<strong>on</strong> Murtha, horse-race caller, began to practiserace calling in his adolescence (Kuiper & Austin 1990). Manyaucti<strong>on</strong>eers such as champi<strong>on</strong> tobacco aucti<strong>on</strong>eer “Mac” Burnetteare <strong>the</strong> s<strong>on</strong>s of aucti<strong>on</strong>eers (Kuiper & Tillis 1986). Highschool playgrounds are full of boys imitatively practicing repertoiressuch as those of sports commentators, <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>s readers, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>-up comedians. L&B are right to point out that this aspectof language acquisiti<strong>on</strong> does not rate in <strong>the</strong> normal classroom,or in classroom competency tests, but any high school teacherwho is <strong>on</strong> playground duty can make <strong>the</strong>se observati<strong>on</strong>s.It is also not surprising that phrasal vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> skills in employing it, are begun in apprenticeship mode inadolescence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mature <strong>on</strong>ly later in adulthood. As Lord(1960) points out, mastery of <strong>the</strong> formulaic lexic<strong>on</strong> in terms ofboth its acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its employment takes years to develop.Adult males traditi<strong>on</strong>ally reach <strong>the</strong> peak of <strong>the</strong>ir social influencerelatively late. Again this follows from <strong>the</strong> view that languageacquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance link throughout<strong>the</strong> human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> span <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not just during early <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>.So what <strong>the</strong>n of vocabulary acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its place in evoluti<strong>on</strong>?The phrasal vocabulary normally combines strictly linguisticknowledge in terms of <strong>the</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ology, syntax, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>semantics of a lexicalised phrase, with knowledge of <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sfor its use – that is, its pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performancerequirements. Such vocabulary is socially integrated in that itsuse is a functi<strong>on</strong>, both productively <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> perceptually, of socialevents which recur, such as <strong>the</strong> requirement under certain situati<strong>on</strong>sto apologise, thank some<strong>on</strong>e, launch ships, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> crownkings <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> queens. Those who can do <strong>the</strong>se things in a sociallysancti<strong>on</strong>ed matter have social advantages. Those who cannotare social lepers. Such social advantages clearly provideevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary advantages in <strong>the</strong> way that L&B suggest.If we think of small extended family groups living as hunterga<strong>the</strong>rersin earlier times, many social events would have beenrecurring events (Malinowski 1922/1984). Mostly, <strong>the</strong>refore, afinite but extensive set of vocabulary items would have sufficedto accompany such events <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in some measure to create<strong>the</strong>m. That being so, <strong>the</strong> capacity to signal appropriately wouldhave had evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary advantages for every<strong>on</strong>e, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> superiorability – that is, having at <strong>on</strong>e’s disposal a wide range of vocabulary– would have been of value. Wray (1998) suggests that, <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> basis of such needs-based use, analytic decompositi<strong>on</strong> of anincreasing large communal vocabulary would have allowed for<strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of grammar. On this analysis, vocabulary cameBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 291


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>first <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammar bootstrapped off it. There is evidence that here<strong>on</strong>togeny also may recapitulate phylogeny (Peters 1983; W<strong>on</strong>gFillmore 1976).In terms of mental storage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> retrieval it is also clear that atleast some of <strong>the</strong> phrasal lexic<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that part of <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e-wordlexic<strong>on</strong> which has associated c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of use are right hemispherical,whereas analytic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> syn<strong>the</strong>tic linguistic capacitiesare generally left hemispherical (Van Lancker 1987; Wray 2002).From crying to words: Unique or multilevelselective pressures?Daniela Lenti Boero a <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Luciana Bott<strong>on</strong>i ba Corso di laurea in Scienze Psicologiche e delle Relazi<strong>on</strong>i di Aiuto, Universitéde la Vallée d’Aoste, 11110 Aosta. Italy; b Dipartimento di Scienzedell’Ambiente e del Territorio, Università degli Studi Milano Bicocca, 20126Milan, Italyd.lentiboero@univda.it luciana.bott<strong>on</strong>i@unimib.ithttp://www.disat.unimib.it/bioacousticsAbstract: In <strong>the</strong> first year of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, infants’ utterances change from highintensitycrying to low-intensity acoustic sound strings, acousticallylabelling <strong>the</strong> first word. This transiti<strong>on</strong> implies: (1) decoding ofph<strong>on</strong>etic sounds, (2) encoding of ph<strong>on</strong>etic sounds, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> (3) a uniquelinking of an articulated sound to a specific object. Comparative,<strong>on</strong>togenetic, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phylogenetic aspects are c<strong>on</strong>sidered for multilevelselective pressures.In <strong>the</strong> first year of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, a human infant’s sound emissi<strong>on</strong> changesfrom crying, a high-intensity mammalian signal (Lenti Boero1997), to low-intensity sound strings having a referentialmeaning (i.e., words). Semantics comes earlier in <strong>on</strong>togenythan grammar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> syntax, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>stitutes <strong>the</strong> bricks <strong>on</strong> whichsubsequent language competence is built. Between <strong>the</strong> twoextremes of high-intensity crying <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> low-intensity speech, wefind early sound making: a pleasant, musical, rewarding signalfor both infant <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mo<strong>the</strong>r (de Boyss<strong>on</strong>-Bardie 2001; Papoušek& Papoušek 1981; Ruzza et al. 2003). The transiti<strong>on</strong> from earlycrying to <strong>the</strong> first meaningful word implies <strong>the</strong> existence of <strong>the</strong>following competencies:1. Decoding of ph<strong>on</strong>etic sounds (perceptual)2. Encoding of ph<strong>on</strong>etic sounds (articulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> motor)3. Linking univocally an articulated sound to a specific object(cognitive).It is important to underscore that language includes both perceptualdecoding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> motor encoding comp<strong>on</strong>ents, as earliestclinical studies by Broca <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Wernicke showed (Ellis & Young1988).<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>togeny in <strong>the</strong> first year of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> implies separatedecoding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding competencies. In human communicati<strong>on</strong>artefacts (e.g., human communicati<strong>on</strong> engineered products, liketeleph<strong>on</strong>es, where, in <strong>the</strong> same object, both encoding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> decodingdevices are designed at <strong>the</strong> same time by <strong>the</strong> human maker[who is not blind]) decoding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding features coevolve,but this might not be <strong>the</strong> same for language. (It is importantto c<strong>on</strong>trast human design <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> natural selecti<strong>on</strong>’s design, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>underscore <strong>the</strong> independence of decoding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> encodinglanguage features.) In fact, <strong>the</strong> above-menti<strong>on</strong>ed threecompetencies might have different selective pressures.Referential competence is defined as <strong>the</strong> ability to identifyobjects by means of an ic<strong>on</strong> (acoustic or visual). Earlier studies<strong>on</strong> apes’ language have shown that even <strong>the</strong> nearest apescannot articulate human-like sounds because of <strong>the</strong> lack ofarticulati<strong>on</strong> space <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> nervous motor c<strong>on</strong>trol of <strong>the</strong> larynx(Lieberman et al. 1969; Ploog 1992); however, apes are very competentin referring objects to tokens or human gestures. O<strong>the</strong>rmammals have been shown to utter species-specific soundsrelated to predators (Cheney & Seyfarth 1990; Lenti Boero1992), to underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human words relative to objects (Kaminskiet al. 2004), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to discriminate ph<strong>on</strong>etic sounds (Miller 1977;Morse & Snowd<strong>on</strong> 1975). Thus, from mammalian studies wededuce that competencies 1 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 3 are present (many moreexamples can be given), but competency 2 is lacking.Studies <strong>on</strong> s<strong>on</strong>gbirds have proposed that <strong>the</strong>y should be c<strong>on</strong>sideredinteresting models for human language because of<strong>the</strong>ir ability to articulate strings of sounds <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to crystallizespecies-specific s<strong>on</strong>gs heard in <strong>the</strong> social envir<strong>on</strong>ment, aprocess similar to <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e that human infants undergo in <strong>the</strong>transiti<strong>on</strong> from crying, through early sound making, to correctarticulati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> first word (Hultsch & Todt 2004).S<strong>on</strong>gbirds have competencies 1 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2, but completely lack competency3. Am<strong>on</strong>g birds, parrots (in particular, <strong>the</strong> Gray parrot)are a unique species in this respect, having, like humans, allthree competencies (Pepperberg 1981; 1983). However, competency3 is used <strong>on</strong>ly in animal-human interacti<strong>on</strong>s, analogouslyas am<strong>on</strong>g apes, suggesting that parrots <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> apes do not sp<strong>on</strong>taneouslygenerate socially shared referential systems – apparentlya unique human competency (but see Savage-Rumbaughet al. 1996). If all those competencies appear separately am<strong>on</strong>gvertebrates, <strong>the</strong>y must have different selective pressures.In <strong>the</strong> first m<strong>on</strong>th of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <strong>the</strong> human infant shows competency1 (Mehler et al. 1988), but does not show competency 2. (Ifproper experiments have not yet been d<strong>on</strong>e to explore competency3, <strong>the</strong>se will certainly come later <strong>on</strong>.) Did competency 1undergo selective pressure, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, if so, from which agents?Percepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> discriminati<strong>on</strong> of ph<strong>on</strong>etic sounds cannot beearly selected by parents, being c<strong>on</strong>cealed to <strong>the</strong>m, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> can bedisclosed <strong>on</strong>ly with proper experiments <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> selected for if <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>on</strong>ly if eventually related to fur<strong>the</strong>r sounds <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> babblingemissi<strong>on</strong>.The above-menti<strong>on</strong>ed studies <strong>on</strong> s<strong>on</strong>g learning in s<strong>on</strong>gbirdssuggest <strong>the</strong> existence in a vertebrate species of an inner motorlearning module, expressing itself in future phases. In humans,<strong>the</strong> full expressi<strong>on</strong> of native language sounds might be postp<strong>on</strong>edby <strong>the</strong> immaturity of <strong>the</strong> articulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> nervous motor c<strong>on</strong>trolcomp<strong>on</strong>ent. If percepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> discriminati<strong>on</strong> of ph<strong>on</strong>etic soundsdo not relate to later sound-making <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> babbling, we shouldthink of selective pressures independent from parental selecti<strong>on</strong>.One possibility is that those competencies are a by-product of amore widespread <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> general cognitive module referring toanalysis of acoustic scene performed in <strong>the</strong> natural envir<strong>on</strong>ment.The fact that o<strong>the</strong>r mammalian <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> bird species are able to discriminatehuman ph<strong>on</strong>etic sounds, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that Gray parrots are ableto decode <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> discriminate musical notes from <strong>the</strong> temperatescale, might support this hypo<strong>the</strong>sis (Bott<strong>on</strong>i et al. 2004). Withregard to competency 2, it is important to underscore thathumans are <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly mammalian species endowed with an articulati<strong>on</strong>apparatus allowing <strong>the</strong> emissi<strong>on</strong> of quasi-musical soundsthat, as Locke & Bogin (L&B) correctly state, have been selectedby parental pressures. This apparatus is <strong>the</strong> foundati<strong>on</strong> of vowelemissi<strong>on</strong> (Titze 1994), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, toge<strong>the</strong>r with an improved (in respectto apes) t<strong>on</strong>gue <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> buccal c<strong>on</strong>trol, it lies behind <strong>the</strong> abilityto articulate early music-like sounds <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more refined laterph<strong>on</strong>etic sounds.This articulati<strong>on</strong> process is allowed by <strong>the</strong> descent of larynx in<strong>the</strong> neck, losing c<strong>on</strong>tact with <strong>the</strong> naso-pharynx (we are uniqueam<strong>on</strong>gst mammalian species in this regard). In order to underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>human language as we know it, a key point should be toquesti<strong>on</strong> how <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> why this happened. Camperio Ciani <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Chiarelli (1988), investigating <strong>the</strong> physiology of b<strong>on</strong>e depositi<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reallocati<strong>on</strong> process in <strong>the</strong> cranium during <strong>on</strong>togeny, suggestthat larynx descent is a by-product of <strong>the</strong> systemic morphofuncti<strong>on</strong>aladjustments to include a large prefr<strong>on</strong>tal cortex in <strong>the</strong>braincase. In <strong>the</strong> course of hominid phylogeny, larynx descentshould have happened gradually; we might speculate of anhominid phase during which vocal communicati<strong>on</strong> was nol<strong>on</strong>ger built of fixed mammalian-like sounds but was not yet292 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>ph<strong>on</strong>etically as distinct as it is now. In syn<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>the</strong>re was a “quasimusical” melodic signalling, whose vocal signals were able todenote at least emoti<strong>on</strong>al internal state. This might explain: (1)<strong>the</strong> separati<strong>on</strong> of music <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language competence in c<strong>on</strong>temporaryhuman brain; (2) <strong>the</strong> role of vocal communicati<strong>on</strong> embeddedin <strong>the</strong> verbal <strong>on</strong>e, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> fact that vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal signals areseparately processed in <strong>the</strong> human brain (Zatorre et al. 1992);<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> (3) <strong>the</strong> existence of independent selective pressure forearly musical sounds <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> late ph<strong>on</strong>etic sounds.About juvenility, <strong>the</strong> features of femininespeech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a big leapPierre LiénardDepartment of Psychology, Washingt<strong>on</strong> University in St. Louis, St Louis,MO 63130.p.lienard@qub.ac.ukAbstract: In this commentary, I ask three specific questi<strong>on</strong>s: (1) Whywould a juvenile stage be maintained in humans? (2) What could be asatisfactory evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary scenario explaining <strong>the</strong> features of femininespeech? And (3), what could be <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> of sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> in<strong>the</strong> elicitati<strong>on</strong> of higher informati<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tents in communicative signals?Locke & Bogin (L&B) provide a brilliant account of how <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>ory can help explain <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> humanfaculty of language. Although <strong>the</strong> analysis is quite c<strong>on</strong>vincing<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> arguments are thoroughly developed, I still have somedoubts about specific matters. Though <strong>the</strong>se lingering problemsare not central to L&B’s core hypo<strong>the</strong>ses, providing properanswers could clarify <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lend support to <strong>the</strong> article’s main argumentthat human language must be c<strong>on</strong>ceived as <strong>the</strong> outcome ofnatural selecti<strong>on</strong> operating at all stages of <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> <strong>development</strong>’s end-products.Why maintain a juvenile stage? Compared to o<strong>the</strong>r higherprimates <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> extinct hominids, humans have a slow <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>.Also, in some important aspects, Homo sapiens’ <strong>development</strong>alpattern has been deeply reorganized. The infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juvenilestages are shorter than <strong>the</strong>ir primate equivalents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> two <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>phases appear in its <strong>development</strong>al pattern. According to L&B,infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juvenility could be c<strong>on</strong>ceived as analogous in that,both are preparatory phases. The authors provide a c<strong>on</strong>vincingaccount for infancy’s reframing in human <strong>development</strong>al pattern.An accelerated brain growth that prol<strong>on</strong>gs into early childhoodpalliates <strong>the</strong> shorter human infancy stage. Without touching up<strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> dimensi<strong>on</strong> of language, human infancy can be primarilyc<strong>on</strong>ceived as a phase in which occurs part of <strong>the</strong> brain growthnecessary for achieving some type of aut<strong>on</strong>omy similar to that ofo<strong>the</strong>r primate species’ infants (cf. <strong>the</strong> obstetrical dilemma). So, inhumans, a shorter infancy stage has acquired a species-specificfuncti<strong>on</strong>, somewhat different from its l<strong>on</strong>ger equivalent am<strong>on</strong>go<strong>the</strong>r species. C<strong>on</strong>trary to what <strong>the</strong>y have d<strong>on</strong>e for infancy, L&Bhave not provided much specificati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> species-specificstatus of <strong>the</strong> human juvenile stage.The initiati<strong>on</strong> of juvenility coincides with <strong>the</strong> initiati<strong>on</strong> of asharp decline in growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> tapering-off of brain growth atabout age seven years. Why do we find this interrupti<strong>on</strong>? Whyd<strong>on</strong>’t we find a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous growth from childhood to adolescence?If a juvenile stage has been maintained, it is certainlybecause it has somehow enhanced fitness. Even if it is a survivalfrom a previous evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary stage, human juvenility is <strong>on</strong>lyexplained if a trade-off exists between a five years’ delay of reproducti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a significant enhancement of future reproducti<strong>on</strong>.Also, juvenility, with its definiti<strong>on</strong>al features (no significantbrain growth, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an accelerated decrease in body <strong>development</strong>),should be explained by <strong>the</strong> advantage that childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescenced<strong>on</strong>’t provide. We know that human sociality is a lot morecomplex than <strong>the</strong> kinds of sociality found am<strong>on</strong>g primate species.We also know that durati<strong>on</strong> of juvenility in primates is l<strong>on</strong>ger than<strong>the</strong> durati<strong>on</strong> of its human equivalent. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, a great deal ofhuman middle childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence is spent masteringsocial dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. So what can be <strong>the</strong> specificity of <strong>the</strong> juvenilephase? What is actually being learned, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> what type of <strong>development</strong>alprocess would be dependent up<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> (relative) freezing ofbrain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> body growth? Is <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of social knowledge orskills compensating for some kind of freezing of physical <strong>development</strong>?An answer could be that juvenility is a phase during whichcogniti<strong>on</strong> is subjected to a fast <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> massive energy-c<strong>on</strong>sumptivereorganizati<strong>on</strong>/rec<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong> that would allow subsequentcognitive <strong>development</strong>s to occur. But do we find evidence ofsuch a massive reorganizati<strong>on</strong>/rec<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>?How did <strong>the</strong> features of feminine speech come about? Sexualselecti<strong>on</strong> shaped human language through mate choice. L&B’s<strong>development</strong> of <strong>the</strong> argument seems to be slightly asymmetrical.The account of how <strong>the</strong> features of <strong>the</strong> “masculine speech”might have come into being is impeccable. It is engaging <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>invites us all to fur<strong>the</strong>r investigate <strong>the</strong> matter. However, assometimes seems to be <strong>the</strong> case in some c<strong>on</strong>temporaneoussexual selecti<strong>on</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses, <strong>the</strong> feminine counterpart isinadequately dealt with. And indeed, although L&B stateimportant features of feminine speech, <strong>the</strong>y d<strong>on</strong>’t provide uswith much elaborate justificati<strong>on</strong>s of why it should haveacquired those characteristics. They also make a str<strong>on</strong>g <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>unusual claim opposing <strong>the</strong> well-established idea that womenoften have a linguistic advantage (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> what of foreignlanguages!?). Stating it in a ra<strong>the</strong>r lapidary way, men wouldhave a greater mastery of public use of language while womenwould master speech in o<strong>the</strong>r more intimate spheres. This is avery interesting claim that it shouldn’t be too hard toinvestigate experimentally. Are women generally out-competedwhen placed in a situati<strong>on</strong> of verbal dueling with men? Is <strong>the</strong>topic of <strong>the</strong> dueling of any relevance? One can think of manyo<strong>the</strong>r relevant dimensi<strong>on</strong>s that would be worth investigating.From grunts to symbolic language: What role might sexualselecti<strong>on</strong> play? L&B present various arguments insisting <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> negative social reacti<strong>on</strong>s to vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal deficits. Andsurely, those linguistic disorders are easy-to-pick signals forappraising, for instance, a potential partner’s fitness ...but <strong>the</strong>yare so for people endowed with a full-blown capacity forlanguage who, early <strong>on</strong> in <strong>development</strong>, discover itsimportance. So people with vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal deficits might wellencounter some difficulties in meeting a mate, in stabilizing<strong>the</strong>mselves in a durable relati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in starting a family. Thatdoes not tell us much about <strong>the</strong> previous evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary c<strong>on</strong>textswhen <strong>the</strong> faculty of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech were evolving towards<strong>the</strong>ir present forms. Voice, or its texture, might have indeedbeen an important parameter of mate choice. But what about<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tent? In <strong>the</strong> same line of inquiry, <strong>on</strong>e might w<strong>on</strong>der ifadolescence has played a role in eliciting <strong>the</strong> big leap (ei<strong>the</strong>rincrementally or by successive “jumps”) between <strong>the</strong> uses ofwhat could be termed indexical signs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> signals with highinformati<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tent (actually linguistic, so to speak).How <strong>the</strong> language capacity was naturallyselected: Altriciality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> l<strong>on</strong>g immaturityD. Kimbrough Oller a <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ulrike Griebel ba School of Audiology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Speech-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Pathology, The University ofMemphis, Memphis, TN 38105; b Department of Biology, The University ofMemphis, Memphis, TN 38152.koller@memphis.edu ulrikegriebel@wnm.nethttp://www.ausp.memphis.edu/people/kimoller.htmlAbstract: Critical factors that appear to encourage vocal <strong>development</strong>in humans are altriciality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> l<strong>on</strong>g immaturity. Hominid infants appearBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 293


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>to have evolved a specific tendency to use elaborate vocalizati<strong>on</strong> as ameans of soliciting l<strong>on</strong>g-term investment from caregivers. The<strong>development</strong> of such vocal capacity provides necessary infrastructurefor language <strong>development</strong> across human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>.By highlighting <strong>the</strong> unique human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, Locke & Bogin(L&B) bring into focus <strong>the</strong> need to address <strong>development</strong> in modeling<strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language. Although <strong>the</strong> emphasis in <strong>the</strong>target article is <strong>on</strong> stages spanning birth through adolescence,it also bears emphasizing that even in <strong>the</strong> first six m<strong>on</strong>ths ofhuman <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, vocal <strong>development</strong> is remarkable, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> appears tobring <strong>the</strong> infant to a point of functi<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>textual flexibilityin producti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> use of vocalizati<strong>on</strong> that is never achieved in anyn<strong>on</strong>human primate (Oller & Griebel 2005). The foundati<strong>on</strong>s laidin <strong>the</strong> first six m<strong>on</strong>ths appear to be critical to all subsequent<strong>development</strong> toward vocal language, because it would be impossibleto learn even <strong>the</strong> simplest group-specific words or even toproduce systematic imitati<strong>on</strong> of novel syllable patterns in <strong>the</strong>absence of substantial functi<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>textual flexibility ofvocalizati<strong>on</strong>. Similarly, it is sensible to c<strong>on</strong>clude that ourhominid ancestors, as <strong>the</strong>y broke away from <strong>the</strong> primate background,prior to possessing language must have developedcomm<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of vocal flexibility, because, without it, all fur<strong>the</strong>revoluti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong> of vocal language may well have beenimpossible.This evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary process must have been <strong>the</strong> product ofspecific selecti<strong>on</strong> forces, just as <strong>the</strong>re appear to have beenspecial selecti<strong>on</strong> forces that have produced vocal variability <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>flexibility in o<strong>the</strong>r species with complex communicati<strong>on</strong>systems, especially some cetaceans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> birds (Griebel & Oller,in press). Interestingly, <strong>the</strong>re are more than 200 extant speciesof primates (Martin 1990) but <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> human shows vast vocalflexibility, suggesting that primate <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> circumstances have overwhelminglyfavored vocal systems serving specific, immediatefuncti<strong>on</strong>al needs such as aggressi<strong>on</strong>, distress, or alarm signaling,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to serving those needs with well-defined, unambiguous calls(Griebel & Oller, in press; Hauser 1996). One of <strong>the</strong> most fundamentalquesti<strong>on</strong>s of language evoluti<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>n is: What was uniqueabout hominid <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> circumstances that yielded selecti<strong>on</strong> forceswhere hominids <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>ly hominids came to possess vocalcapacities that vastly exceeded <strong>the</strong> requirements of immediatefuncti<strong>on</strong>al needs?The pattern of modern human <strong>development</strong> in <strong>the</strong> first sixm<strong>on</strong>ths of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> suggests an answer. The human infant is morealtricial <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> faces a l<strong>on</strong>ger immaturity than any o<strong>the</strong>r primate,as is emphasized in <strong>the</strong> target article. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, <strong>the</strong> humaninfant has a need for l<strong>on</strong>g-term caregiver investment that dramaticallyexceeds that of any o<strong>the</strong>r primate. The unique topography<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> richness of human parent-infant face-to-face vocalinteracti<strong>on</strong>s str<strong>on</strong>gly suggests b<strong>on</strong>ding is at stake (see, e.g.,Stern 1974; Trevar<strong>the</strong>n 1979; Tr<strong>on</strong>ick 1982), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that elaborate,c<strong>on</strong>textually flexible infant vocalizati<strong>on</strong> solicits parental investment(Oller 2000). Parents appear to distill fitness informati<strong>on</strong>from infant vocalizati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in many instances to elicit it inface-to-face interacti<strong>on</strong>, presumably because it benefits parentsto make wise decisi<strong>on</strong>s about investment of energies in infantsfacing a l<strong>on</strong>g immaturity (Locke, in press c; Oller 2004).Human caregivers also attend to a variety of infant vocalizati<strong>on</strong>soutside b<strong>on</strong>ding interacti<strong>on</strong>s.Vocalizati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> human infant is <strong>the</strong>n a fitness indicator,providing evidence to caregiving kin about infant well-being.Because <strong>the</strong> premium <strong>on</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g-term investment by caregivers ishigher for <strong>the</strong> human infant than for any o<strong>the</strong>r primate, <strong>the</strong> selecti<strong>on</strong>pressure <strong>on</strong> infant fitness indicators is also higher than forany o<strong>the</strong>r primate. And since altriciality had already increasedin hominids by <strong>the</strong> point of bipedalism, intense selecti<strong>on</strong>pressure <strong>on</strong> infant solicitati<strong>on</strong> of parental investment must havebeen in place very early in hominid evoluti<strong>on</strong>. The pattern ofvocal <strong>development</strong> in <strong>the</strong> human infant suggests, <strong>the</strong>n, that <strong>the</strong>driving engine of evoluti<strong>on</strong> for hominid vocal capacity mayhave been <strong>the</strong> unique c<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong> of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> circumstances(including altriciality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> l<strong>on</strong>g immaturity) that encouragedimmature hominids to seek caregiver investment, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>hominid parent (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r kin) to seek fitness indicati<strong>on</strong>s from<strong>the</strong> infant in vocalizati<strong>on</strong>, in order to make better decisi<strong>on</strong>sregarding investment of caregiving energies across a l<strong>on</strong>g immaturity.This special c<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong> of forces encouraged parentalselecti<strong>on</strong> (Locke, in press c) by vocalizati<strong>on</strong> assessment, apattern that may have increasingly resembled sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>in its effects <strong>on</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, as <strong>the</strong> priority <strong>on</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g-term parentalinvestment became increasingly high. Runaway selecti<strong>on</strong>(rapidly producing salient <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> traits) has l<strong>on</strong>g been wellrecognizedas a product of sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> (Darwin 1871; see areview in Miller 2000), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> it may well be that <strong>the</strong> elaboratehuman vocal capacity was spurred <strong>on</strong> by parental selecti<strong>on</strong> inearly <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> of hominid infants. As noted in <strong>the</strong> target article, thisvocal capacity, fostered in infancy through parental selecti<strong>on</strong>,could have been amplified in adolescence through sexualselecti<strong>on</strong>.Of course, human parents use (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ancient hominid parents nodoubt used) a variety of fitness indicators in making decisi<strong>on</strong>sabout investment in <strong>the</strong>ir young. But vocalizati<strong>on</strong> has uniquestatus as a communicative device am<strong>on</strong>g birds <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mammals.Vocalizati<strong>on</strong> in many species communicates immediate survivalneeds <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> provides a primary fitness indicator in sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>(Hausberger 1997; Hauser 1996; Kroodsma 1999). In humans,vocalizati<strong>on</strong> appears to play a significant role in sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>(Miller 2000), but also a critical role in survival during <strong>the</strong> mostaltricial period of infancy. The importance of this early altricialperiod in fostering <strong>the</strong> creati<strong>on</strong> of vocal capabilities that have afoundati<strong>on</strong>al character for language has been vastly underplayedin most prior portrayals of language evoluti<strong>on</strong>, where <strong>the</strong> focushas so often been <strong>on</strong> how syntax in language came about (see,e.g., Bickert<strong>on</strong> 1981; Pinker & Bloom 1990). Syntax, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> manyprecursors to it, could never have occurred without fundamentalcommunicative flexibility; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> syntax is a feature of language thatappears in <strong>the</strong> human infant <strong>on</strong>ly after many m<strong>on</strong>ths of infrastructural<strong>development</strong> in flexibility of vocal communicati<strong>on</strong>.The target article is <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> mark in highlighting <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>issue. The necessary refocusing <strong>on</strong> <strong>development</strong>al issues inspeculati<strong>on</strong>s about language evoluti<strong>on</strong> will inevitably bring usto <strong>the</strong> beginning of hominid evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thus to <strong>the</strong> beginningof vocal <strong>development</strong> in <strong>the</strong> first m<strong>on</strong>ths of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>.ACKNOWLEDGMENTSThis work was supported by <strong>the</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institutes of Deafness <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>O<strong>the</strong>r Communicati<strong>on</strong> Disorders (R01DC006099-01, D. K. Oller PI<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Eugene Buder Co-PI), by <strong>the</strong> K<strong>on</strong>rad Lorenz Institute forEvoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Cogniti<strong>on</strong> Research, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> by <strong>the</strong> Plough Foundati<strong>on</strong>.Comparative, c<strong>on</strong>tinuity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> computati<strong>on</strong>alevidence in evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>the</strong>ory: Predictiveevidence versus productive evidenceDavid M. W. PowersSchool of Informatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Engineering, Flinders University of South Australia,Adelaide, SA 5001, Australia.David.Powers@flinders.edu.auhttp://www.infoeng.flinders.edu.au/people/pages/powers_davidAbstract: Of three types of evidence available to evoluti<strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong>orists – comparative, c<strong>on</strong>tinuity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> computati<strong>on</strong>al – <strong>the</strong> first islargely productive ra<strong>the</strong>r than predictive. Although comparis<strong>on</strong>between extant species or languages is possible <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> can be suggestiveof evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary processes, leading to <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>development</strong>, comparis<strong>on</strong>with extinct species <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> languages seems necessary for validati<strong>on</strong>.C<strong>on</strong>tinuity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> computati<strong>on</strong>al evidence provide <strong>the</strong> best opportunitiesfor supporting predicti<strong>on</strong>s.294 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Locke & Bogin (L&B) tell two kinds of story, <strong>the</strong> first seeking todem<strong>on</strong>strate that Homo sapiens is unique in having a childhood<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that this is critical to <strong>the</strong> nature of human language, <strong>the</strong>sec<strong>on</strong>d seeking to explain how evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary pressures mayhave given rise to <strong>the</strong>se phenomena. These stories are intimatelyintertwined in <strong>the</strong> target article, with many evoluti<strong>on</strong>aryhypo<strong>the</strong>ses being stated baldly as facts. N<strong>on</strong>e<strong>the</strong>less, L&B arecorrect to emphasize <strong>the</strong> role of <strong>on</strong>togeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> provide auseful review of <strong>development</strong>al evidence. Computati<strong>on</strong>almodels of self-organizati<strong>on</strong> show that significant visual, auditory,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistic features can self-organize with minimalinput, whereas detailed <strong>on</strong>togenesis without self-organizati<strong>on</strong>would seem to exceed <strong>the</strong> capacity of <strong>the</strong> genome, suggestingthat a composite model is required (Willshaw & v<strong>on</strong> derMalsburg 1979).The perinatal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infancy period is certainly critical but L&Bneglect <strong>the</strong> former. Since <strong>the</strong> 1980s we have known that infants,even in utero, can discriminate auditory informati<strong>on</strong>, can distinguish<strong>on</strong>e language from ano<strong>the</strong>r, can distinguish <strong>on</strong>espeaker of a language from ano<strong>the</strong>r, or even distinguish smallph<strong>on</strong>emic changes in a single syllable (Mehler et al. 1988).Indeed, ne<strong>on</strong>ates as much as 10-weeks premature can distinguishlow frequency t<strong>on</strong>es <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> show auditory cortical event relatedpotentials (Kurtzberg et al. 1984; 1986; 1988; Kushnerenko2003).L&B highlight an important phenomen<strong>on</strong> under <strong>the</strong> guise of“trickle-up ph<strong>on</strong>etics.” There has been an implicit assumpti<strong>on</strong>in linguistics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> psycholinguistics that children learn thoseaspects of <strong>the</strong>ir target language that are not innate. Althoughthis seems to be self-evident, it is not true! The child’s idiolectis creatively c<strong>on</strong>structed <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> negotiated in a complex socialenvir<strong>on</strong>ment <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> is not identical to that of ei<strong>the</strong>r parent – parental,sibling, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> peer language is in general modified by <strong>the</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong>with <strong>the</strong> child, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistic, cognitive, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social learningis part of a complex that is not c<strong>on</strong>veniently modular. It is thussimplistic to assume <strong>the</strong>re is a target language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that <strong>the</strong>child’s aim is identificati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> limit (Gold 1967; Powers &Turk 1989). As L&B note, imitati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> infant is morecomm<strong>on</strong> than imitati<strong>on</strong> by <strong>the</strong> infant, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> such parental imitati<strong>on</strong>is associated with increased attenti<strong>on</strong>. The vocabulary of a familyis changed by an infant, as child protowords are adopted, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>grammar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> subject matter <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complexity of child-directedspeech are accommodated to <strong>the</strong> infant. This is most clearlyevident in <strong>the</strong> process of creolizati<strong>on</strong> (Bickert<strong>on</strong> 1984; 1990;1995).An underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing of <strong>the</strong> language acquisiti<strong>on</strong> process thatescapes from <strong>the</strong> naïve idea of a baby replicating his mo<strong>the</strong>r’sgrammar is fundamental to a <strong>the</strong>ory of language evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>predicts that c<strong>on</strong>siderable language evoluti<strong>on</strong> is possiblewithin <strong>the</strong> species. This puts <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>us <strong>on</strong> a <strong>the</strong>ory of macroevoluti<strong>on</strong>to make truly c<strong>on</strong>trastive predicti<strong>on</strong>s with <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> innatemechanisms associated with <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> species. Although L&B do notclearly enunciate a model of language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>/<strong>on</strong>togenesis,<strong>the</strong>y do provide many details <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses that could beuseful in elaborating a testable <strong>the</strong>ory of <strong>on</strong>togenesis; though<strong>the</strong>se details are far from a testable <strong>the</strong>ory in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of innate cognitive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistic capabilities <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>distinctives.An evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>the</strong>ory may be tested in two main ways: (1) bymaking predicti<strong>on</strong>s that may be validated archeologically, genetically,or biochemically; or (2) by building computati<strong>on</strong>al modelsthat dem<strong>on</strong>strate that proposed mechanisms produce <strong>the</strong> predictedeffects. A <strong>development</strong>al <strong>the</strong>ory of language may betested analogously by making predicti<strong>on</strong>s that may be validatedneurologically, behaviourally, or linguistically; or again, byusing computati<strong>on</strong>al modelling.Existing fossil <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> comparative linguistics evidence is “productive”in inspiring <strong>the</strong>ory producti<strong>on</strong> – “productive evidence”has its value in producing <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory but has no value, zerovalue, in establishing its validity. Validati<strong>on</strong> requires making<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>firming <strong>the</strong>ory predicti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> requires <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> evidencethat fits <strong>the</strong> “predictive” framework <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ei<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>firms orrefutes <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory – “predictive evidence” must not havebeen utilized in formulating <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> is <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly kindof evidence that can provide support for <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory, althoughit can never prove it. The riskier <strong>the</strong> predicti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> moreunexpected <strong>the</strong> evidence; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trast with<strong>the</strong> predicti<strong>on</strong>s of competing <strong>the</strong>ories, <strong>the</strong> better <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oryis <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> more support predictive evidence can bring(Popper 1963).L&B make no explicit predicti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> offer no c<strong>on</strong>firmatoryevidence. In particular, <strong>the</strong>ir Figure 3 is speculative <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> proposedevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary sequence has been disc<strong>on</strong>firmed by genetic,biochemical, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r evidence. L&B <strong>the</strong>mselves acknowledgethat “<strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly reliable data are associated with Pan <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> H.sapiens”(sect. 9.1 of <strong>the</strong> target article). These are <strong>the</strong> endpointsin <strong>the</strong> proposed chain – <strong>the</strong> missing links are still missing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>saltati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>the</strong>ory has failed to hypo<strong>the</strong>size mechanisms thatcould explain <strong>the</strong> leaps.We turn <strong>the</strong>refore to <strong>the</strong> third kind of evidence, that of computati<strong>on</strong>almodelling for both evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language (Steels1998) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>/<strong>on</strong>togenesis of language (Powers &Turk 1989). The modelling of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language naturallydivides into several aspects (Hauser et al. 2002; see target article,sect. 12): evoluti<strong>on</strong> of languages within <strong>the</strong> human species;evoluti<strong>on</strong> of communicati<strong>on</strong> capabilities independent ofspecies; c<strong>on</strong>tinuous macroevoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adaptati<strong>on</strong> of organiccognitive, communicative, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistic capacities acrossspecies; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> saltati<strong>on</strong>al mechanisms that might explain <strong>the</strong>missing links <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> missing ancestors.The use of <strong>the</strong> term “evoluti<strong>on</strong>” in relati<strong>on</strong> to language as amedium ra<strong>the</strong>r than a faculty involves a metaphorical step:Darwinian, Lamarckian, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> saltati<strong>on</strong>al evoluti<strong>on</strong> involve hypo<strong>the</strong>sizedmechanisms for genetic mutati<strong>on</strong>, inheritance, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> naturalselecti<strong>on</strong> (ignoring <strong>the</strong> problem of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> genetic<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> proteomic mechanisms <strong>the</strong>mselves); evoluti<strong>on</strong> of humanlanguage within H. sapiens depends <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>togeneticprocesses. L&B espouse a <strong>the</strong>ory of evoluti<strong>on</strong> that requiresmacroevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary steps; however, much of what <strong>the</strong>y describeseems not to be evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary in <strong>the</strong> macro sense but ra<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>cerns<strong>the</strong> emergence of language within <strong>the</strong> species. In ei<strong>the</strong>rcase, computati<strong>on</strong>al modelling is <strong>on</strong>e of <strong>the</strong> most promisingmeans for testing our <strong>the</strong>ories.To deal first with <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> of computati<strong>on</strong>al models ofmacroevoluti<strong>on</strong>, it is necessary to define a starting substrate –species A with specific capabilities – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for recognizingenhanced communicative capability (e.g., sec<strong>on</strong>d signallingof facts not in evidence in <strong>the</strong> here <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> now). Incomputati<strong>on</strong>al terms, <strong>the</strong> distincti<strong>on</strong> between evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary, selforganizing,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning systems arises in relati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> programra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> hardware. Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>development</strong> involvesdifferent individuals having different programs, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> normallyincludes both genetic variants (not true evoluti<strong>on</strong>) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mutati<strong>on</strong>(not macroevoluti<strong>on</strong> of itself) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> falls into <strong>the</strong> domains of artificial<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, genetic programming, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary robotics. It may, forexample, involve <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of different types of neural networks(Luerssen & Powers 2003) or it may be undertaken at a higherlevel of abstracti<strong>on</strong> (Olss<strong>on</strong> & Powers 2003).A <strong>development</strong>al <strong>the</strong>ory of <strong>the</strong> emergence of language in both<strong>the</strong> species <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> individual has more of <strong>the</strong> character of selforganizati<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> unsupervised learning (Powers & Turk 1989).Such processes involve <strong>the</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> of innately determinedstructures <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> capabilities, physical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> envir<strong>on</strong>mental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> boundary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interacti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>straints, to producedetailed neural structure as well as define an individual’s idiolect<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a society’s language. This has been dem<strong>on</strong>strated for variouskinds of visual <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> auditory features as well as for ph<strong>on</strong>ological,morphological, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phrase-level structure (Powers 1992), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>some multi-organism language creati<strong>on</strong> (Steels 1998) also hassomething of this unsupervised character – although in aBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 295


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>composite system with multiple organisms, emergent supervisedlearning/teaching may be part of a larger self-organizati<strong>on</strong>al orevoluti<strong>on</strong>al paradigm.It should be emphasized that evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>al<strong>the</strong>ories of language both extend well bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> usual domain oflinguistics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguists <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> are intrinsically multimodal. For, <strong>the</strong>sensory-motor <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> affective states of <strong>the</strong> individual <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> his peers<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> caregivers are all involved, not just in <strong>the</strong> emergence oflanguage, but in <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of a physical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social worldview – an <strong>on</strong>tology that encompasses our underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing ofspace <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> time, our learning to discriminate between superstitious<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> causal relati<strong>on</strong>ships, as well as our learning to affect<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol our envir<strong>on</strong>ment directly at a physical level <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>less directly through social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistic interacti<strong>on</strong>. Only in<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary robotics are all <strong>the</strong>sediverse aspects of evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>al <strong>the</strong>ory ableto be modelled.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>: Not a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>S<strong>on</strong>ia Ragir a <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Patricia J. Brooks ba Department of Sociology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Anthropology, College of Staten Isl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, CityUniversity of New York, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> American Museum of Natural History, StatenIsl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, NY 10314; b Department of Psychology, College of Staten Isl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Graduate Center, City University of New York, Staten Isl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, NY 10314.ragir@mail.csi.cuny.eduhttp://scholar.library.csi.cuny.edu/ ragir/webpage/htmpbrooks@mail.csi.cuny.eduhttp://www.library.csi.cuny.edu/ psy/brooks.htmAbstract: The uniqueness of human cogniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language has l<strong>on</strong>gbeen linked to systematic changes in <strong>development</strong>al timing. Selecti<strong>on</strong>for postnatal skeletal ossificati<strong>on</strong> resulted in progressive prol<strong>on</strong>gati<strong>on</strong> ofuniversal patterns of primate growth, leng<strong>the</strong>ning infancy, childhood,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> emerged as communicati<strong>on</strong> increased incomplexity within <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> between communities ra<strong>the</strong>r than from selecti<strong>on</strong>for some unique features of childhood or adolescence, or both.From <strong>the</strong> middle of <strong>the</strong> twentieth century <strong>on</strong>ward, a scholarlyliterature has linked evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary changes in <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>alprofile of hominins to terrestrial bipedalism, <strong>the</strong> narrowing of<strong>the</strong> birth canal, encephalizati<strong>on</strong>, prol<strong>on</strong>ged neocortical plasticity,behavioral flexibility, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> profound changes in hominin socialorganizati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., de Beer 1930; 1951/1958; Eisley 1946/1957;Gould 1977; Jeris<strong>on</strong> 1973; M<strong>on</strong>tague 1962). Comparativeresearch has suggested that humans share <strong>the</strong> growth patternstypical of primates, but with a relatively greater prol<strong>on</strong>gati<strong>on</strong> ofeach <strong>development</strong>al phase than any o<strong>the</strong>r mammal of comparablesize (e.g., Finlay et al. 2001; Schultz 1960; Watts 1990). Prol<strong>on</strong>gedcell divisi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>on</strong>e phase led to delayed closure of later phases.For example, a greater number of neur<strong>on</strong>s required more timeto divide, migrate to <strong>the</strong>ir final positi<strong>on</strong>s, establish dendritic c<strong>on</strong>nectivity,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> become myelinated (McKinney 2000). With respectto brain <strong>development</strong>, <strong>the</strong> largest perturbati<strong>on</strong> occurred in <strong>the</strong>latest developing regi<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> neocortex, in particular, <strong>the</strong> prefr<strong>on</strong>talcortex (Deac<strong>on</strong> 1997; Finlay & Darlingt<strong>on</strong> 1995; Finlayet al. 2001). Greater numbers of cortical neur<strong>on</strong>s resulted indata compacting <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> added levels of informati<strong>on</strong> processingthat produced increasingly abstract representati<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong> compactedinput (Calvin 1996; 1998; Kien 1991). Gould (1977)argued that <strong>the</strong>se changes were systematic, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> led to qualitativechanges in <strong>the</strong> complexity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> automaticity of human cogniti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> communicati<strong>on</strong>. Gould <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lew<strong>on</strong>tin (1979) proposed thatlanguage emerged as a by-product of general increases in brainsize, coincident with an increased need for communicati<strong>on</strong>within <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> between tightly organized primate social groups.This well-documented view of language emergence c<strong>on</strong>trastsmarkedly with Locke & Bogin’s (L&B’s) proposal of independentselecti<strong>on</strong> for aspects of language during childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence.What is most surprising about <strong>the</strong>ir proposal is <strong>the</strong> suggesti<strong>on</strong>that childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence are unique to humans, in <strong>the</strong>face of forty years of c<strong>on</strong>trary evidence (e.g., Harvey et al. 1987;Hobs<strong>on</strong> et al. 1981; Schultz 1969). Using skeletal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> horm<strong>on</strong>alevidence from both m<strong>on</strong>keys <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> apes, Watts (1985; 1986;Watts & Gavan 1982) documented a distinct juvenile dependencycomparable to human childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an adolescent growth spurtthat preceded delayed sexual maturati<strong>on</strong> in all higher primates.Extended human childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence appear to be c<strong>on</strong>comitantwith <strong>the</strong> systematic changes in fetal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> postnatal <strong>development</strong>(Gould 1977; Ragir 1985; 2001a; 2001b; Shea 1990). Ifanything, given <strong>the</strong> length of childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> expectancyof humans, sexual maturati<strong>on</strong> occurs relatively early as comparedto o<strong>the</strong>r apes (Shea 1990). The observed n<strong>on</strong>-uniqueness of <strong>the</strong>se<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> phases is indicative of <strong>the</strong> systematic patterns ofprimate growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> described above. The purporteduniqueness of human childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence isseriously undermined by <strong>the</strong> similar patterns of m<strong>on</strong>key, ape,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human growth after infancy, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> weak links between patternsof tooth erupti<strong>on</strong>, facial morphology, weaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> changingsocial-sexual <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive competence (Watts 1990).L&B’s proposal offers an unc<strong>on</strong>vincing combinati<strong>on</strong> of kin<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>sexual-selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures that operate during different <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> phases <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> select for seemingly dissociated competence<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance aspects of language. The kin-selecti<strong>on</strong> argumentfor language formati<strong>on</strong> is seriously flawed; all n<strong>on</strong>humanprimates communicate with <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> successfully raise <strong>the</strong>ir infantswithout language. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> between parent <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> offspringwould not necessarily improve <strong>the</strong> survival of juveniles, norensure “h<strong>on</strong>est” communicati<strong>on</strong>. Studies of emergent Sign<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Creoles indicate greater complexity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> systematicityin <strong>the</strong> negotiated exchange of informati<strong>on</strong> within <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>between groups composed of relative strangers as opposed toclose kin (Kegl et al. 1999; Kend<strong>on</strong> 1980a; 1980b; 1980c;McWhorter 1997; Senghas et al. 2004; Washabaugh 1986).We also str<strong>on</strong>gly disagree with L&B’s emphasis <strong>on</strong> sexual selecti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> competitive aspects of language performance. Primatespractice opportunistic mating in which females fail toshow a general preference for alpha males, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> males mate withany estrous female. Male displays of strength <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cunning temporarilypositi<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>m in a relatively unstable male dominance hierarchythat is likely to c<strong>on</strong>fer up<strong>on</strong> dominant males preferential accessto estrous females for <strong>on</strong>ly a limited part of <strong>the</strong>ir mature reproductive<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. In c<strong>on</strong>trast, dominant females <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir female kin showgreater reproductive success over <strong>the</strong> entire period of reproductivematurity (Grant 2003; Lee & Bowman 1995; van Schaik &Hrdy 1991). On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, adolescence is likely to be importantin <strong>the</strong> formati<strong>on</strong> of languages for o<strong>the</strong>r reas<strong>on</strong>s; humans tend todisperse at adolescence to mate, carrying <strong>the</strong>ir local communicativepractices to nearby groups. This dispersal would promote<strong>the</strong> exchange of skills <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> informati<strong>on</strong>, exp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> regi<strong>on</strong>al politicalaffiliati<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> create mutually intelligible language dialects.Throughout <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>span, speech performance is basic to humanlanguage, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence is not special in this regard. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>use develops in t<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>em with lexic<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammar, with all aspectsof language increasing in complexity in ways that reflect <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>,perhaps, implement changes in cortical organizati<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>acquisiti<strong>on</strong> is possible because infants are socially c<strong>on</strong>nected <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>sensitized to <strong>the</strong> dynamics of interpers<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong> frombirth. Children acquire language by tuning in to <strong>the</strong> communicativeintenti<strong>on</strong>s of o<strong>the</strong>rs, through active social engagement <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>participati<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> (Tomasello 1999). Young children’spragmatic sophisticati<strong>on</strong> manifests itself in <strong>the</strong> form of social routinessuch as peek-a-boo, vocal turn taking, pointing, use ofprosody, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> joint attenti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., Ninio & Snow 1996; Rochatet al. 1999). Children who are not segregated from adult activitiesreadily imitate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> incorporate aspects of mature speech acts into<strong>the</strong>ir repertoires. Their pers<strong>on</strong>al narratives, sense of self, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> autobiographicalmemory are c<strong>on</strong>structed through c<strong>on</strong>versing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>296 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>reminiscing with o<strong>the</strong>rs (Nels<strong>on</strong> & Fivush 2004). Word play <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>language games emerge fully in childhood (Goldman 1998), withmental simulati<strong>on</strong> serving to decouple representati<strong>on</strong> from <strong>the</strong>here <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> now, as children engage in n<strong>on</strong>-literal language in <strong>the</strong>irpretend play. The <strong>development</strong> of language use across <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>spanis a very important topic that will not be well understood if pragmaticsis narrowly c<strong>on</strong>ceived as building <strong>on</strong> grammatical competence(Brooks 2004). Both <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> itsevoluti<strong>on</strong> require an appreciati<strong>on</strong> of how both lexic<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>grammar are rooted in participati<strong>on</strong> in social <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>.Life stages, put in words: Morning, four; no<strong>on</strong>,two; evening, three?Wolfgang M. SchleidtRobert-Hamerling-G.1/22, A-1150, Vienna, Austria.wolfgang.schleidt@univie.ac.at http://www.schleidt.org/wolfgang/Abstract: The social functi<strong>on</strong> of language, as exemplified by “t<strong>on</strong>iccommunicati<strong>on</strong>,” is certainly not restricted to our own species. Anindividual’s cognitive mastering of its envir<strong>on</strong>ment, moreover, is equallyessential for underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <strong>the</strong> nature of any language. In <strong>the</strong> absenceof comparative data, it is premature to claim that language skills at aparticular <strong>development</strong>al stage are uniquely human.Morning, four; no<strong>on</strong>, two; evening, three? This is anamazingly c<strong>on</strong>cise statement of <strong>the</strong> ancient riddle <strong>the</strong> Sphinxposed to Oedipus. This riddle’s soluti<strong>on</strong> – “during infancy wecrawl <strong>on</strong> all four, grown up we walk <strong>on</strong> two legs, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> at old agea cane serves us as a third leg” – gives us a feel for <strong>the</strong>antiquity of human awareness of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s stages, put in words(Schleidt 1992). There are additi<strong>on</strong>al disc<strong>on</strong>tinuities, of course,bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> number of legs. During <strong>the</strong> bipedal stage we noteseveral distinct changes: at school age, when <strong>the</strong> child isallowed to venture into society without c<strong>on</strong>stant maternalsupervisi<strong>on</strong>; at puberty, certainly <strong>the</strong> most striking incisi<strong>on</strong> in<strong>the</strong> individual’s thread of bipedal <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> at menopause, for<strong>the</strong> female <strong>the</strong> beginning of gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mo<strong>the</strong>ring. Currently, <strong>the</strong>“eight stages of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>” proposed by Erik H. Eriks<strong>on</strong> (1950) arewidely accepted, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>social competence are bey<strong>on</strong>d dispute (Schleidt 1992). Theimportance of oral language as an essential tool for <strong>the</strong>individual’s carving its niche within society, in pursuit of itsbiological fitness, is obvious. So, what is <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> in <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>approach taken by Locke & Bogin (L&B)?Childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence: Uniquely human? The <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of humanlanguage, envisi<strong>on</strong>ed by L&B, rests <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir claim that two<strong>development</strong>al stages, childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence, are relatively<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> inserti<strong>on</strong>s into human <strong>development</strong>: a true childhood, asdistinct phases preceding <strong>the</strong> juvenile stage (<strong>on</strong>ly since Homohabilis) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a true adolescence, as distinct phases following <strong>the</strong>juvenile stage (<strong>on</strong>ly since Homo sapiens). From my ownexperience with <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of various species of birds<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mammals, I cannot follow <strong>the</strong> authors’ argumentati<strong>on</strong>,since <strong>the</strong> vast majority of social animals, which rely <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> useof a language to attain reproductive success, have similar substageswithin <strong>the</strong>ir juvenile stage that justify a separate label. Inmammals, for example, we can talk of childhood <strong>on</strong>ce nursinghas stopped but <strong>the</strong> individual is still part of <strong>the</strong> family, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ofadolescence when, prior to sexual maturity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> social b<strong>on</strong>dsdevelop, usually am<strong>on</strong>g juveniles of <strong>the</strong> same gender.The social functi<strong>on</strong> of language. L&B focus <strong>on</strong> differences in<strong>the</strong> oral language in each of <strong>the</strong> four pre-adult <strong>on</strong>togenetic stagesin human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>. This puts an unusually str<strong>on</strong>g emphasis <strong>on</strong><strong>the</strong> social functi<strong>on</strong> of language: talking as a form of communi<strong>on</strong>am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> communicants (phatic communi<strong>on</strong>, Malinowski1923; t<strong>on</strong>ic communicati<strong>on</strong>, Schleidt 1973; gossip, Dunbar1996; etc.). They strike a sympa<strong>the</strong>tic chord with my owninterests in b<strong>on</strong>ding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> communi<strong>on</strong>s, rooted in old c<strong>on</strong>ceptsof psychoanalysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ethology that are rarely recalled <strong>the</strong>sedays. In a nutshell, Eriks<strong>on</strong>’s (1950) eight <strong>development</strong>al stages,based <strong>on</strong> Sigmund Freud’s well-known oral, anal, phallic,latency, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> genital phases, show a striking corresp<strong>on</strong>dence to<strong>the</strong> stages of b<strong>on</strong>ding in birds, as described by K<strong>on</strong>rad Lorenzin his 1935 paper (translated as Lorenz 1970) <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> varioustypes of “compani<strong>on</strong>s” during a bird’s <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> (Schleidt 1992).C<strong>on</strong>sidering <strong>the</strong> recent recogniti<strong>on</strong> of gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mo<strong>the</strong>ring as aspecial form of child care (Hawkes et al. 1998), <strong>the</strong> gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>childcan be added as ano<strong>the</strong>r type of “compani<strong>on</strong>,” a cognitivedomain in its own right. Thus, I hypo<strong>the</strong>size that in each of <strong>the</strong><strong>development</strong>al stages a particular <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> kind of b<strong>on</strong>d mayevolve, a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> type of “compani<strong>on</strong>” can be added to <strong>the</strong> cast ofc<strong>on</strong>specifics, exp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <strong>the</strong> social skills <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitivecompetence (see Table 1).Cogniti<strong>on</strong> without words. Finally I must c<strong>on</strong>fess that, whilestudying L&B’s proposal, I was reminded of two aspects ofcognitive <strong>development</strong> that are intimately interrelated with ouroral language, but remained strikingly absent in <strong>the</strong>irdeliberati<strong>on</strong>: “first words” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “n<strong>on</strong>verbal thinking.” “Firstwords” are <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> tip of an iceberg, of course, since <strong>the</strong>n<strong>on</strong>verbal mapping of patterns in <strong>the</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ment into itsmemory is far ahead of <strong>the</strong> language skills of a growing infant.Thus, <strong>on</strong>e may ask how much sense does it make to discusslanguage <strong>development</strong> without reference to <strong>the</strong> individual’sworld? This caveat applies not <strong>on</strong>ly to infancy, but to all stages.“School age,” for example, is dominated by learning <strong>the</strong> namesof many “things” <strong>the</strong> child is already quite familiar with fromits own experience. The child’s ability to see “things” as cognitiveentities is well illustrated in J. A. Comenius’s Orbis sensualiumTable 1 (Schleidt). Developmental stagesAge (year) Generic term Sigmund Freud Erik H. Eriks<strong>on</strong> K<strong>on</strong>rad Lorenz0–1 Infancy Oral Infancy Parent1–3 Childhood Anal Early childhood Sibling3–6 Childhood Phallic Childhood Friend/foe6–12 Childhood Latency School age Friend/foe12–18 Adolescence Genital Adolescences Friend/foe18–25 Adulthood (courtship phase) Young adulthood Mate25–45 Adulthood (reproductive phase) Adulthood Child45–? Maturity Maturity a Gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>childBased <strong>on</strong> Schleidt (1992), revised. Here I have replaced <strong>the</strong> German terms (“Elternkumpan,” etc.) with English translati<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong> chief compani<strong>on</strong>of each stage (“parent,” etc.) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> added <strong>the</strong> “gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>child” as ano<strong>the</strong>r type of compani<strong>on</strong>.a Erik H. Eriks<strong>on</strong> used <strong>the</strong> term “old age” for <strong>the</strong> age bey<strong>on</strong>d retirement, years 60 to 70.BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 297


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>pictus (Comenius 1659/1968), <strong>the</strong> first “textbook” offering akeyhole look at <strong>the</strong> world, with each of its items labeled <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>named in <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r t<strong>on</strong>gue <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in Latin. And, throughout ouradult <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> we are flooded with impressi<strong>on</strong>s of things we cannotname. We can h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>le such things, manipulate <strong>the</strong>m in a creative,cognitive process without <strong>the</strong> need to name <strong>the</strong>m. When wewatch “higher” animals (e.g., a kitten, raven, chimpanzee) within<strong>the</strong>ir natural envir<strong>on</strong>ment <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> analyze <strong>the</strong>ir behavior it becomesobvious that <strong>the</strong>y can detect, classify, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> recognize a multitudeof “patterns” in <strong>the</strong>ir envir<strong>on</strong>ment, or actually, discrete “things,”furnishings of <strong>the</strong>ir species-specific, individual world, <strong>the</strong>ir veryown “Orbis sensualium.” As we can c<strong>on</strong>dense <strong>the</strong> behaviorpatterns of a species in an “ethogram” (Schleidt & Crawley 1980),we can represent <strong>the</strong> repertoire of patterns in <strong>the</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ment,of things as cognitive entities an individual can recognize, as an“ecogram” (Schleidt 1985). I feel we need to know a lot moreabout <strong>the</strong> cognitive <strong>development</strong> of birds <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mammals beforewe can claim unique features in human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> histories.Is it language that makes humans intelligent?Jo Van Herwegen <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Annette Karmiloff-SmithNeurocognitive Development Unit, Institute of Child Health, L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>,WC1N 1EH, United Kingdom.j.vanherwegen@ich.ucl.ac.uk a.karmiloff-smith@ich.ucl.ac.ukAbstract: The target article by Locke & Bogin (L&B) focuses <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language as a communicative tool. They neglect, however,that from infancy <strong>on</strong>wards humans have <strong>the</strong> ability to go bey<strong>on</strong>dsuccessful behaviour <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to reflect up<strong>on</strong> language (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r domainsof knowledge) as a problem space in its own right. This ability is notfound in o<strong>the</strong>r species <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> may well be what makes humans unique.The target article by Locke & Bogin (L&B) doesn’t merely coverlanguage evoluti<strong>on</strong> but also takes <strong>the</strong> whole of human <strong>on</strong>togenyinto account. However impressive <strong>the</strong> authors’ <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>model is, <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> remains as to whe<strong>the</strong>r it is really languagethat makes humans intelligent. Throughout <strong>the</strong> article <strong>the</strong> authorsfocus <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of language as a communicative toolwhich came into existence through selecti<strong>on</strong>, in order to secure<strong>the</strong> survival of <strong>the</strong> human species. However, <strong>the</strong>re is nodenying that many animals have complex communicati<strong>on</strong>systems, elaborate sensorimotor skills, as well as rich mental representati<strong>on</strong>s(Premack 2004). Something else, <strong>the</strong>n, must makehumans special. We argue that this is <strong>the</strong> ability in humans tore-represent <strong>the</strong>ir representati<strong>on</strong>s into an explicit format, transportablefrom <strong>on</strong>e domain of knowledge to ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong>e.Humans do not <strong>on</strong>ly master <strong>the</strong> basics of <strong>the</strong> language systemfor communicati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong>y also reflect up<strong>on</strong> language as a domainof knowledge. Therefore, language is not merely a communicativetool, it is also a problem space in its own right, making young childrenbehave like little linguists (Karmiloff-Smith 1992).Even infants are sensitive to <strong>the</strong> subtleties of <strong>the</strong> linguisticsound system (Jusczyk & Aslin 1995). By as early as ninem<strong>on</strong>ths, for instance, infants have begun to determine <strong>the</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>otacticsequences of <strong>the</strong>ir native language that helps <strong>the</strong>m tosegment fluent speech into words (Jusczyk 1999). And, evenwhen <strong>the</strong>y become fluent speakers by about 3 to 4 years of age,children do not just learn <strong>the</strong> mapping between words <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>ir corresp<strong>on</strong>ding referents, but also analyse <strong>the</strong> morphologicalsystem. An example comes from Karmiloff-Smith (1992) whenshe quotes a passage between a 4-year-old <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> her mo<strong>the</strong>r:Child: “What’s that?”Mo<strong>the</strong>r: “A typewriter.”Child: “No, you’re <strong>the</strong> typewriter, that’s a typewrite.”(Karmiloff-Smith 1992, p. 31)At <strong>the</strong> age of 4, <strong>the</strong> child is not merely focused <strong>on</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong>but <strong>on</strong> how <strong>the</strong> system works, that is, that <strong>the</strong> suffix “er”is agentive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that it is used after verb stems to refer tohuman agents. This <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> numerous similar examples show howhumans, even young children, c<strong>on</strong>sider language as a form ofknowledge bey<strong>on</strong>d its use as a communicative tool.Such explicit c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of knowledge by children is not <strong>on</strong>lyfound in <strong>the</strong> domain of language. Physics is ano<strong>the</strong>r domain thatbecomes a problem space in its own right for children. Forinstance, 5-year-old children are successful at balancing blockswhere weight is both evenly <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> unevenly distributed. However,7-year-olds fail to succeed in balancing <strong>the</strong> unevenly distributedweight blocks. A finer analysis shows that this failure can beexplained by <strong>the</strong>ir going bey<strong>on</strong>d successful behaviour <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> creating<strong>the</strong> geometric-centre <strong>the</strong>ory, believing that all blocks balance at<strong>the</strong>ir centre. They ignore counterexamples to <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>the</strong>ory, whilec<strong>on</strong>solidating it (Karmiloff-Smith & Inhelder 1975). Only by age9 years do <strong>the</strong>y accomplish <strong>the</strong> task at h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> when <strong>the</strong>y underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> law of torque. So unlike o<strong>the</strong>r species, children do not merelyaim for successful behaviour.If language is not just a communicative tool but also a problemspace in its own right, <strong>the</strong>n we can expect to find populati<strong>on</strong>s inwhich linguistic communicati<strong>on</strong> is not impaired, but language asa domain of knowledge is, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that having fluent language is notsufficient to ensure peer acceptance. The neuro<strong>development</strong>aldisorder, Williams syndrome, is such an example. Despite IQs in<strong>the</strong> 50- to 60-point range, individuals with Williams syndrome(WS) have surprisingly proficient language skills (D<strong>on</strong>nai &Karmiloff-Smith 2000). Yet, <strong>the</strong>ir fluent language does not sufficeto get <strong>the</strong>m accepted by peers. In c<strong>on</strong>trast to o<strong>the</strong>r adolescentswith neuro<strong>development</strong>al disorders, <strong>the</strong>ir peer interacti<strong>on</strong> problemsare not caused by reluctance to join in social c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s.So, fluent language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a desire for interacti<strong>on</strong> do not suffice to geta WS adolescent accepted by peers. Outside <strong>the</strong> field of language,children with WS also do not organize or make <strong>the</strong>ir knowledgemore explicit. For example, adolescents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adults with WS canlearn a vast list of facts; however, <strong>the</strong>y fail to organise <strong>the</strong>se factsinto core <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> peripheral knowledge – a reorganisati<strong>on</strong> processfound in normal 9- to 10-year-olds (Johns<strong>on</strong> & Carey 1998).Ano<strong>the</strong>r populati<strong>on</strong> in which <strong>the</strong> ability to re-represent knowledgeis likely to be impaired is Down syndrome, a <strong>development</strong>aldisorder which, in c<strong>on</strong>trast to WS, involves poor language skills. Al<strong>on</strong>gitudinal case study of block balancing involving a Down syndromechild showed that, in c<strong>on</strong>trast to typical developing children,<strong>the</strong>re was no sign over time (from age 9 to age 11 years)of his developing geometric centre <strong>the</strong>ory. Instead, for 19m<strong>on</strong>ths <strong>the</strong> child remained <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> level of repeated successfulbehaviour of <strong>the</strong> typical 5-year-old. This points to an impairmentin <strong>the</strong> reorganizati<strong>on</strong> of knowledge (Karmiloff-Smith 2006).So even though human language is an impressive communicativetool, humans may be <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly species that can re-represent <strong>the</strong>iracquired knowledge through representati<strong>on</strong>al redescripti<strong>on</strong>. Thisc<strong>on</strong>stitutes an endogenous way of gaining <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> knowledge, leadingto <strong>the</strong> achievement of creativity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> flexible c<strong>on</strong>trol in humans, inc<strong>on</strong>trast to o<strong>the</strong>r species (Karmiloff-Smith 1992). L&B argue thatverbal creativity may have played an important role in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>of language, but <strong>the</strong>y d<strong>on</strong>’t raise <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> as to what makes thisverbal creativity possible. We believe that <strong>the</strong> ability of re-representati<strong>on</strong>,both within <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> across different domains of knowledge, mayturn out to be a possible answer to this questi<strong>on</strong>.Uniqueness of human childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>adolescence?Glenn E. WeisfeldDepartment of Psychology, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI 48202.weisfeld@sun.science.wayne.edusun.science.wayne.edu/psych/people/facultypages/weisfeld.htmAbstract: Locke & Bogin (L&B) propose that humans are unique inpossessing stages of childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence. Arguments to <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>trary include evidence for a similar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adaptive juvenile period in298 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>simians of slow growth, intense play <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> provisi<strong>on</strong>ing withsolid food by adults. Likewise, simians as well as humans undergo acompensatory growth spurt during puberty.I questi<strong>on</strong> two of <strong>the</strong> basic assumpti<strong>on</strong>s of this article: namely, <strong>the</strong>supposed uniqueness of human childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of humanadolescence.In secti<strong>on</strong> 2.3 of <strong>the</strong> target article, Locke & Bogin (L&B)define <strong>the</strong> stage of childhood as comprising a “slow <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> steadyrate of body growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> relatively small body size; large, fastgrowingbrain; higher resting metabolic rate than any o<strong>the</strong>rmammalian species; immature dentiti<strong>on</strong>; dependence <strong>on</strong> olderpeople for care <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> feeding; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> motor <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive advances”(numbered list in original).These traits also seem to characterize young simians afterinfancy. A large, fast-growing brain relative to body weightoccurs in primates generally (J<strong>on</strong>es et al. 1992). As for humanshaving a higher resting metabolic rate than any o<strong>the</strong>r mammal,according to Kleiber’s equati<strong>on</strong>, 1 metabolic rates for mammals<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> birds when plotted against body mass <strong>on</strong> logarithmic coordinatestend to fall al<strong>on</strong>g a straight line. Smaller animals havehigher basal metabolic rates (BMR) per unit body weight th<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>o larger animals. Humans indeed exceed this formula slightly,but <strong>the</strong> peak of exceeding it comes at around 10 kg body weight,or before two years of age – that is, in infancy, not childhood(J<strong>on</strong>es et al. 1992). Moreover, <strong>the</strong> high BMR in infancy seemsmerely to compensate for a low rate at birth. Macaques <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> chimpanzeesfall right <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> Kleiber line. Thus, <strong>the</strong> BMR of humans<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r primates is not remarkable. However, simians mayhave a high overall (not resting) metabolic rate, given <strong>the</strong> energyrequired for climbing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> play. Primates, including but notrestricted to children, use high-energy play to advance motorically<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitively – to learn – during <strong>the</strong> juvenile stage.Humans doubtless exceed all o<strong>the</strong>r simians in <strong>the</strong> extent of provisi<strong>on</strong>ingfor offspring during <strong>the</strong> juvenile stage. The authors notethat <strong>the</strong> first permanent molar erupts significantly later in humansthan in <strong>the</strong> great apes – at age 6 years versus about age 3 years –suggesting that juvenile hominids began to forage independentlyat a relatively old age. However, food sharing with immaturesoccurs in o<strong>the</strong>r primates, for example: golden marmosets(Brown & Mack 1978), male tamarins (Goldizen 1987), maletiti m<strong>on</strong>keys (Starin 1978), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> chimpanzee mo<strong>the</strong>rs (Goodall1968; McGrew 1975; Silk 1978; 1979). Although <strong>the</strong>se may bemainly or exclusively cases of food sharing with infants ra<strong>the</strong>rthan juveniles, <strong>the</strong>y do c<strong>on</strong>stitute food sharing with <strong>the</strong> young.Extensive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> prol<strong>on</strong>ged food sharing occurs in canids such asjackals <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> African hunting dog. In <strong>the</strong> wolf, pups beginreceiving regurgitated meat during <strong>the</strong>ir fourth week, shortlybefore weaning; <strong>the</strong> deciduous teeth begin to be replaced muchlater, at 16 weeks (Mech 1970). So <strong>the</strong> main basis for claiming aunique stage of childhood in humans compared with o<strong>the</strong>r primates(but not canids) would seem to be <strong>the</strong> extent, not <strong>the</strong> existence,of provisi<strong>on</strong>ing. In <strong>the</strong>ir secti<strong>on</strong> 2.3, L&B quote Lancaster<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lancaster (1983) as asserting that no o<strong>the</strong>r primate is soactively involved in childcare <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> feeding, implying that o<strong>the</strong>r primatesalso exhibit <strong>the</strong>se behaviors. Worthman (1993) wentfur<strong>the</strong>r, stating that “Uniquely, humans provisi<strong>on</strong> juveniles” (p.340). Yet, n<strong>on</strong>e of <strong>the</strong>se primatologists has seen fit to c<strong>on</strong>cludethat humans uniquely possess a stage of “childhood.”The asserti<strong>on</strong> that <strong>on</strong>ly humans possess a stage of adolescence ismore puzzling still. The authors claim that, in humans, “uniquely,<strong>the</strong>re is a distinct skeletal growth spurt in both sexes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in almostall skeletal elements of <strong>the</strong> body after several years of gentlydecreasing juvenile growth. The <strong>on</strong>set of this spurt ... marks<strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>set of adolescence” (sect. 2.7). “N<strong>on</strong>human primates lackchildhood, but <strong>the</strong>y also lack a post-juvenile period of dramaticgrowth of <strong>the</strong> sort that defines human adolescence” (sect. 9.2).But an adolescent growth spurt has been documented in primatesgenerally. Tanner (quoted in J<strong>on</strong>es et al. 1992) wrote: “Thecharacteristic form of <strong>the</strong> human growth curves is shared byapes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> m<strong>on</strong>keys. It is apparently distinctive to higher primates;nei<strong>the</strong>r rodents nor cattle have curves that resemble it” (p. 100).Accompanying graphs show spurts of weight gain in adolescentrhesus macaques, chimpanzees, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> humans of both sexes. Thegrowth curves for male chimpanzees <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rhesus macaquesappear to be even more pr<strong>on</strong>ounced than that for boys; <strong>the</strong>curves for females are similar in magnitude for <strong>the</strong> threespecies. Likewise, Deaner et al. (2003) state that for <strong>the</strong> primatespecies with sufficient data <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> point (humans, macaques, mangabeys,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> chimpanzees) a growth spurt follows <strong>the</strong> end of braingrowth. Worthman (1993, p. 342) acknowledges that <strong>the</strong> possibleuniqueness of <strong>the</strong> human adolescent growth spurt has beendebated, partly because many of <strong>the</strong> simian data refer toweight, not length. However, she cites a report showing a distinctskeletal growth spurt in puberty for female rhesus, with asequence of skeletal changes that is <strong>the</strong> same as for humans(Tanner et al. 1990). It would be surprising if a spurt in weightwere not accompanied by an allometric increase in length.The adolescent growth spurt has been explained adaptively ingeneral primate terms – not just in human terms. L&B refer toJans<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> van Schaik’s (1993) explanati<strong>on</strong>: that <strong>the</strong> pubertalgrowth spurt compensates for <strong>the</strong> slow growth of <strong>the</strong> precedingjuvenile period. L&B note that this slow period of <strong>development</strong>allows time for brain growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning in “various species ofsocial mammals” (sect. 2.5). Carnivores, elephants, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> primatesare menti<strong>on</strong>ed specifically. In effect, <strong>the</strong> slower growing, altricialsystems of <strong>the</strong> body defer to <strong>the</strong> precocial nervous system. Slowgrowth also reduces feeding competiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lowers caloricrequirements (e.g., Jans<strong>on</strong> & van Schaik 1993). O<strong>the</strong>r adaptiveadvantages accrue not just to a few primates. Small juvenilesize broadens <strong>the</strong> opportunity for locomoti<strong>on</strong> in trees: a smallerbody can be supported by thinner branches. Small size reducesc<strong>on</strong>servati<strong>on</strong> of bodily heat, but most primates live in warmclimates; fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, juveniles sometimes sleep with <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> arewarmed by <strong>the</strong>ir mo<strong>the</strong>rs (Altmann 1986). Also, young primatesin some species are protected from predators by arboreality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>,especially in terrestrial species, by adult troop members, fur<strong>the</strong>rreducing <strong>the</strong> need for large juvenile size. Last <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> perhaps mostimportant, <strong>the</strong> extensive vigorous arboreal play <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> large brainsof simians are energy intensive, so slow growth frees up energyfor this activity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> learning that it entails.As primates undergo puberty, many of <strong>the</strong>se factors no l<strong>on</strong>geroperate as str<strong>on</strong>gly. Caloric energy is shifted from play to growth.Juvenile primates play more than adolescents undergoing agrowth spurt (Pereira & Altmann 1985). As large size is attained,adolescents can forage fur<strong>the</strong>r afield with less danger, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> canfight for rank or against predators more effectively. Female primatesbenefit from large body size in that <strong>the</strong>ir infants areborn larger. The benefits of slow juvenile growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> learningthat it enhances may be greatest in humans, but this seems to beano<strong>the</strong>r quantitative distincti<strong>on</strong>. Simians as well as humans needan adolescent growth spurt to compensate for slow juvenilegrowth.Primatologists have l<strong>on</strong>g recognized a stage of adolescence inm<strong>on</strong>keys <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> apes. This c<strong>on</strong>sists of <strong>the</strong> period of <strong>the</strong> growthspurt <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r observable pubertal changes – <strong>the</strong> same criteri<strong>on</strong>by which all tribal societies recognize human adolescence(Schlegel & Barry 1991). There seems no reas<strong>on</strong> to ab<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>on</strong> thisc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>. What would we o<strong>the</strong>rwise call <strong>the</strong> period of pubertyin simians?Many unique characteristics of humans have been proposedover <strong>the</strong> years, <strong>on</strong>ly to be discredited in light of subsequentresearch. Given <strong>the</strong> great genetic similarity of humans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>simians, it seems prudent to assume similarity in basic characteristics,such as <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages, in <strong>the</strong> absence of overwhelmingevidence to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trary.NOTE1. Basal metabolic rate ¼ kW B 0.75 , where k is a c<strong>on</strong>stant <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>W B is body weight.BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 299


Commentary/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Melody as a primordial legacy from early rootsof languageKathleen Wermke a <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Werner Mende ba Center for Pre-Speech-Development <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Developmental Disorders,Julius-Maximilians-University, Würzburg, Würzburg, 97070, Germany;b Berlin-Br<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>enburg Academy of Science, Berlin, 10117, Germany.wermke_k@klinik.uni-wuerzburg.de mende@bbaw.deAbstract: The stormy <strong>development</strong> of vocal producti<strong>on</strong> during <strong>the</strong> firstpostnatal weeks is generally underestimated. Our l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal studiesrevealed an amazingly fast unfolding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> combinatorial complexificati<strong>on</strong>of pre-speech melodies. We argue that relying <strong>on</strong> “melody” couldprovide for <strong>the</strong> immature brain a kind of filter to extract <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-relevantinformati<strong>on</strong> from <strong>the</strong> complex speech stream.Locke & Bogin (L&B) emphasize that <strong>the</strong> “<strong>development</strong> oflanguage requires <strong>the</strong> whole of modern human <strong>on</strong>togeny”(target article, Abstract) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> point out that <strong>the</strong> human braingrows at a very fast pace during infancy. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong>ystress a disparity of brain growth after birth in comparis<strong>on</strong> too<strong>the</strong>r primates. We str<strong>on</strong>gly support <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> complement L&B’s<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> based <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> language <strong>development</strong> by <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>data from <strong>the</strong> earliest period of infancy.Our investigati<strong>on</strong>s are focused <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> time functi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> fundamentalfrequency (melody) of infants’ utterances. Melody invocal communicati<strong>on</strong> is a legacy from <strong>the</strong> early roots of orallanguage; it can be traced al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> entire <strong>on</strong>togeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> it isan essential c<strong>on</strong>stituent of prosody. In <strong>on</strong>togeny, its primacy isreflected in <strong>the</strong> amazing perceptive capacities of <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>borns <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>young infants for melody recogniti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., Fernald 1989; Nazziet al. 1998; Ramus et al. 2000; Sansavini et al. 1997) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in <strong>the</strong>ast<strong>on</strong>ishing melody-related performances of <strong>the</strong>ir vocal producti<strong>on</strong>(Wermke 2002).L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal twin studies have revealed a unidirecti<strong>on</strong>al <strong>development</strong>from simple to complex cries during <strong>the</strong> first weeks of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>(Wermke 2002; Wermke & Friederici 2004; Wermke et al. 1996).In a subsequent study we applied a ma<strong>the</strong>matical model in orderto quantitatively compare melody properties of different vocalizati<strong>on</strong>ssuch as soft cries, cooing, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> babbling. A c<strong>on</strong>tinuous unfoldingof melody structures during infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a high structuralsimilarity between melodies of different pre-speech phases wasdem<strong>on</strong>strated (Wermke et al., in preparati<strong>on</strong>). Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, aunidirecti<strong>on</strong>al decrease of melody-arc durati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> FM-amplitudewas observed <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>al path from early crying tospeech-like babbling. These parameters c<strong>on</strong>verge during earlyinfancy toward <strong>the</strong> range of int<strong>on</strong>ati<strong>on</strong> comp<strong>on</strong>ents in laterlanguage. Although melody is a key parameter for characterizinginfants’ utterances during <strong>the</strong> first m<strong>on</strong>ths, o<strong>the</strong>r parameters,describing <strong>the</strong> voluntary c<strong>on</strong>trol of <strong>the</strong> vocal tract, become increasinglyimportant. Beginning at about age three m<strong>on</strong>ths, an increasingarticulatory activity was identified, c<strong>on</strong>nected with couplingphenomena between melody (ph<strong>on</strong>ati<strong>on</strong>) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> res<strong>on</strong>ance properties(articulati<strong>on</strong>; Wermke et al. 2002; Wermke et al. 2005).These processes represent amazingly systematic preparati<strong>on</strong>phases exercising <strong>the</strong> fast transiti<strong>on</strong>s of res<strong>on</strong>ance frequencies (formants)necessary for speech. The establishment of intenti<strong>on</strong>alinteracti<strong>on</strong> between melody <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> formants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a mastering offast formant transiti<strong>on</strong>s seem to require such a training periodbefore being available for intenti<strong>on</strong>al use in vocal producti<strong>on</strong>.So, we could prove a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous <strong>development</strong> of melodies duringearly infancy as well as an engagement of melody in learning toarticulate. This <strong>development</strong> starts immediately after birth, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>all this happens l<strong>on</strong>g before <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e-word stage of language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>(stressed by Jackendoff [2002]) is reached by <strong>the</strong> infant. The<strong>development</strong> of melody skills in <strong>on</strong>togeny precedes by far anysymbolic word use <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rule-based grammar c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>s.Human infants acquire a repertoire of complex melodies withremarkable speed, which corresp<strong>on</strong>ds to <strong>the</strong> fast brain growth atthis early age. There is a general underestimati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> stormypreparati<strong>on</strong> phases c<strong>on</strong>cerning vocal producti<strong>on</strong> during <strong>the</strong> firstweeks, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> target article opens a stage for a general recogniti<strong>on</strong>of <strong>the</strong> much broader range of presuppositi<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong>faculty of language as generally accepted. The huge metabolic<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> growths rates in <strong>the</strong> brain associated with <strong>the</strong>se stormy <strong>development</strong>sprovide a <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> historic substantiati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> significanceof language-related pre-speech <strong>development</strong>, including melody<strong>development</strong> of soft, mitigated crying. 1 L<strong>on</strong>g-term c<strong>on</strong>sequencesof a disturbance in cry melody <strong>development</strong> were dem<strong>on</strong>stratedby comparing infants with a normal language outcome <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thosewith a delayed <strong>on</strong>e (SLI or specific-language-impaired-infants) atage two years (Wermke et al., submitted).In <strong>the</strong> phylogenetic <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>, mo<strong>the</strong>r-infant communicati<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social talking in Locke’s sense (Locke 1995b; 1997)might have ignited behavioural changes to bundle preexistingcompetences into a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> communicati<strong>on</strong> system: symboliclanguage. In its primordial functi<strong>on</strong>, melodies express feelings<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> emoti<strong>on</strong>s, which are tightly c<strong>on</strong>nected with social relati<strong>on</strong>s(Dunbar 1996). An increasing decoupling from affective statesis <strong>on</strong>ly a relatively recent achievement of modern languages.This emancipati<strong>on</strong> was necessary when language became a symbolictool. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong> represents a l<strong>on</strong>g uninterrupted“chain” or better web: The coevoluti<strong>on</strong> of brain, speech interface,language, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> socio-cultural envir<strong>on</strong>ment (well treated inDeac<strong>on</strong> 1997) has a subtle coupling with co-<strong>development</strong>sin <strong>on</strong>togeny, including a critical mo<strong>the</strong>r–infant communicati<strong>on</strong>in infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an adequate social stimulati<strong>on</strong>.From our <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> language evoluti<strong>on</strong>, we offer a complementaryargument to L&B’s argumentati<strong>on</strong> for not being indanger of seeing postnatal helplessness <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> immaturity of <strong>the</strong>brain as a design flaw (cf. target article, sect. 2.1): We argue thatmelody could provide for <strong>the</strong> immature brain of <strong>the</strong> younginfant a kind of filter to extract <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-relevant informati<strong>on</strong> out of<strong>the</strong> complex parental speech stream. Recall that melody is <strong>the</strong>most salient comp<strong>on</strong>ent am<strong>on</strong>g all acoustic comp<strong>on</strong>ents that c<strong>on</strong>tributeto infants’ preferences for “mo<strong>the</strong>rese” speech. The infants’melody skills may be much more than a capability; <strong>the</strong>y are probablya social imperative. Infants from all cultures pass through <strong>the</strong>same stages of melody <strong>development</strong> at <strong>the</strong> same <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> historicalphase. The <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> of L&B implies that spoken language istested <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> shaped by all <strong>the</strong> infants’ brains of a language community,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vice versa. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> by <strong>the</strong> immature brain is<strong>the</strong> critical bottleneck (a language “pelvis”) through which all evolvinglanguage has to pass, generati<strong>on</strong> by generati<strong>on</strong>. This points tol<strong>on</strong>g-term selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures out of small, seemingly inc<strong>on</strong>spicuousphenomena like social sound-making. This <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>imposes seemingly insurmountable c<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> language as anevolving system with respect to its learnability by <strong>the</strong> immaturebrain of <strong>the</strong> infant. But <strong>the</strong>se c<strong>on</strong>straints could eventually enable<strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly possible way to incrementally acquire a rich symboliclanguage. The passage through <strong>the</strong> immature brains of <strong>the</strong>babies has probably minimized <strong>the</strong> abyss between an object <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>its symbolic reference in <strong>the</strong> past at <strong>the</strong> cradle of symboliclanguage, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> does c<strong>on</strong>tinuously repeat this coding process ineach infant acquiring language today. At this critical moment of<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>re is not yet a deep gap between meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>symbol: In young infants, <strong>the</strong> coding of needs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> emoti<strong>on</strong>alstates is not arbitrary, but it is closely coupled to real physiologicalprocesses <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> expressed by vocal producti<strong>on</strong>. However, <strong>the</strong>se earliesttraces of coding seem to precede later complex hierarchicalcoding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> symbolic c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>s. This <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> historic <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>eventually might resolve linguistic coding problems.NOTE1. Only soft crying <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> whining uttered in a c<strong>on</strong>text of lowdistress <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mitigated in its intensity was recorded.ACKNOWLEDGMENTSWe are grateful to <strong>the</strong> German Research Foundati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Max PlanckInstitute for Human Cognitive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences, Leipzig, whichsupported <strong>the</strong> cry studies within <strong>the</strong> framework of <strong>the</strong> German<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Developmental Study (www.glad-study.de).300 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>“<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> impairment gene” does notnecessarily equate to “language gene”Lance WorkmanDepartment of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Bath Spa University,Bath BA2 9BN, United Kingdom.l.workman@bathspa.ac.ukAbstract: The finding of <strong>the</strong> same language deficit in half <strong>the</strong> members of<strong>the</strong> KE family is taken as suggesting that a specific allele (FOXP2) isnormally involved in <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of language. Recent studies,however, questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> exclusivity of FOXP2, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> it is argued that <strong>the</strong>finding of a gene that disrupts language should not be taken as str<strong>on</strong>gevidence for <strong>the</strong> existence of genes that underlie it.Locke & Bogin’s (L&B’s) argument that <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ory providesa fruitful way of re-examining <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship betweenevoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language <strong>development</strong> is, for <strong>the</strong> most part, a compellingif somewhat speculative argument. Both <strong>the</strong> unique <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> uniqueness of language in our speciesrequire explanati<strong>on</strong>. I agree that such patterns should be examinedin <strong>the</strong> light of selective advantages. L&B provide evidenceof selective advantages for a wide range of aspects of language<strong>development</strong>. Where I would urge cauti<strong>on</strong>, however, lies in <strong>the</strong>use of studies of language deficits in <strong>the</strong> “KE” family to implythat <strong>the</strong>se provide evidence of <strong>the</strong> existence of genes directlyrelated to <strong>the</strong> neurological substrate supporting languageproducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> grammar.In <strong>the</strong> early 1990s <strong>the</strong> KE family caused a stir am<strong>on</strong>g linguistswhen it was discovered that more than half of <strong>the</strong> 31 members ofthree generati<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong> family had impaired language abilities(sometimes referred to as specific language impairment, orSLI). It was found that individuals with SLI have a number ofproblems, including a deficit in <strong>the</strong> ability to use inflecti<strong>on</strong>(e.g., producing <strong>the</strong> past tense of a verb) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> problems of articulati<strong>on</strong>,comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> orofacial dyspraxia (i.e., puttingtoge<strong>the</strong>r orofacial movements; Bishop 2002; Gopnik 1990;1997; Pinker & Jackendoff 2005).The finding of <strong>the</strong> same language problem in such a highproporti<strong>on</strong> of family members has been taken by a number ofresearchers as suggestive of a specific underlying geneticproblem. If a specific allele leads to such problems <strong>the</strong>n it hasbeen argued that an alternative allele might normally be involvedin <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of appropriate neural hardware to supportlanguage (Gopnik 1990; Jackendoff 1994; Pinker 1999). L&Bare clearly sympa<strong>the</strong>tic to such an argument.In support of L&B’s stance <strong>the</strong>re is now str<strong>on</strong>g evidence of adefective dominant allele of a single gene that is found <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> l<strong>on</strong>garm of chromosome 7 in <strong>the</strong> impaired members of <strong>the</strong> family –FOXP2 (Lai et al. 2000; 2001). Moreover, as L&B point out, scanningtechniques have dem<strong>on</strong>strated associated neurologicalanomalies of structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> functi<strong>on</strong> in sufferers, such as a bilateraldeficit in activati<strong>on</strong> of Broca’s area (Liégeois et al. 2003) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> abnormalactivati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> caudate nucleus (Vargha-Khadem et al. 1998).Although L&B do not explicitly label FOXP2 as a “grammar gene,”<strong>the</strong>ir use of <strong>the</strong> KE family to back up <strong>the</strong>ir argument that “selecti<strong>on</strong>acted <strong>on</strong> [language] performance” (sect. 11 of <strong>the</strong> target article)implies that this is an allele directly related to language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> hencehas arisen via selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures for that purpose.It is entirely possible that <strong>the</strong> KE family’s SLI does provideevidence of an allele related to <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of language,but currently <strong>the</strong>re are at least two problems with <strong>the</strong> argumentas presented by L&B. First, <strong>the</strong> evidence is presented as if <strong>the</strong>reis general c<strong>on</strong>sensus that an exclusive language-related problemis found in <strong>the</strong> KE family <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that this provides support for <strong>the</strong>existence of a specific defective gene related to language producti<strong>on</strong>.In fact, Gopnik (1997) herself has documented how c<strong>on</strong>troversialthis c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> is, with some experts suggesting that <strong>the</strong>language impairments uncovered might be part of a more generalcognitive deficit, which might include n<strong>on</strong>-linguistic problems(Le<strong>on</strong>ard 1994; Vargha-Khadem et al. 1995). It is certainlypossible that <strong>the</strong> problem is a part of a more general cognitivedeficit since afflicted members of <strong>the</strong> family have IQ scores 18to 19 points lower than those of unimpaired members (includingn<strong>on</strong>verbal aspects of IQ; Vargha-Khadem et al. 1995; 1998).Sec<strong>on</strong>d, even if an allele has been discovered that disruptslanguage producti<strong>on</strong> via <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of neurologicalabnormalities, this is not str<strong>on</strong>g evidence that an alternateallele exists that is directly involved in <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> ofneural circuitry underlying language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech (Karanth2005). As Lai <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> colleagues cauti<strong>on</strong> with regard to <strong>the</strong> FOXP2allele, <strong>the</strong> biological deficit may well be in <strong>the</strong> form of a triggerthat disrupts a key stage of embryogenesis ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> replacementof an allele coding for normal language <strong>development</strong>(Lai et al. 2001; Karanth 2005). Removing an air-filter from aninternal combusti<strong>on</strong> engine will stop a car from running butthat does not mean that <strong>the</strong> air-filter is resp<strong>on</strong>sible for running<strong>the</strong> car (Workman & Reader 2004). Finally, Newbury et al.(2002), through associati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mutati<strong>on</strong> screening analysis,have thrown doubt <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> argument that variants in FOXP2underlie such forms of language impairment.N<strong>on</strong>e of <strong>the</strong>se misgivings c<strong>on</strong>stitutes a deathblow to L&B’sargument; <strong>the</strong>y may well be correct that language <strong>development</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> producti<strong>on</strong> are directly related to specific alleles. But, currently,<strong>the</strong> evidence from families that show language deficitssuch as <strong>the</strong> KE is equivocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> needs to be examined with adegree of cauti<strong>on</strong>. L&B have d<strong>on</strong>e a service to our underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ingof <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship between evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language by weavingtoge<strong>the</strong>r a number of sources of evidence using <strong>the</strong> frameworkof <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ory. They should be careful not to leave anyloose threads.Authors’ Resp<strong>on</strong>seLife <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language: Selecti<strong>on</strong> in<strong>development</strong>John L. Locke a <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Barry Bogin ba Department of Speech–<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>–Hearing Sciences, Lehman College,City University of New York, Br<strong>on</strong>x, NY 10468; b Department of BehavioralSciences, University of Michigan–Dearborn, Dearborn, MI 48128.john.locke@lehman.cuny.edu bbogin@umd.umich.eduhttp://casl.umd.umich.edu/faculty/bbogin/Abstract: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, like o<strong>the</strong>r human traits, could <strong>on</strong>ly haveevolved during <strong>on</strong>e or more stages of <strong>development</strong>. We enlist<strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical framework of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> to account forcertain aspects of linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong>, with special reference toinitial phases in <strong>the</strong> process. It is hypo<strong>the</strong>sized that selecti<strong>on</strong>operated at several <strong>development</strong>al stages, <strong>the</strong> earlier <strong>on</strong>esproducing <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> behaviors that were reinforced by additi<strong>on</strong>al, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>possibly more powerful, forms of selecti<strong>on</strong> during later stages,especially adolescence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> early adulthood. Peer commentarieshave provided opportunities to explain human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> morecomprehensively, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to add details to our account of spokenlanguage. We made no attempt to explain syntax in <strong>the</strong> targetarticle, but we propose here that selecti<strong>on</strong> for “vocal plumage”may have increased our species’ capacity for utterancecomplexity, a <strong>development</strong> that would have benefited all levelsof language.It is a rare pleasure to have comments <strong>on</strong> our target articlefrom so many thoughtful colleagues, representing such awide range of academic disciplines <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cultures. We willbegin our resp<strong>on</strong>se by addressing comments that relateBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 301


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>particularly to our treatment of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>n turnour attenti<strong>on</strong> to replies that pertain more specifically tolanguage.R1. Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Any <strong>the</strong>ory of human biology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> culture needs to explainhow Homo sapiens successfully combines delayed reproducti<strong>on</strong>,helpless <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>borns, a short durati<strong>on</strong> of breastfeeding,a vastly extended period of offspring dependency,an adolescent growth spurt in both sexes, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> femalemenopause a decade or more before death. No o<strong>the</strong>rprimate species shares all of <strong>the</strong>se traits. Theory also isneeded to explain <strong>the</strong> extraordinary size of <strong>the</strong> humanbrain, in relati<strong>on</strong> to overall body size, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> extraordinarycognitive-behavioral abilities of <strong>the</strong> brain, includingsymbolic language, <strong>the</strong>ory of mind, kinship, marriage, religi<strong>on</strong>,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> even nihilistic philosophy. In our target article,we apply <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ory in an attempt to better underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>human nature regarding growth, <strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>language.One commentator, Weisfeld, denies that human beingshave a unique or unusual <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>; in fact he does notuse <strong>the</strong> phrase “<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>” at all in his commentary. IfWeisfeld’s critique is correct, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> whole of our argumentfalls. But, his citati<strong>on</strong>s regarding human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>human primate growth are both selective <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>somewhat old. The most recent citati<strong>on</strong> is from 1993,well before some of <strong>the</strong> most salient work <strong>on</strong> primate<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> was published. We cite that more recentresearch in our target article, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> we provide additi<strong>on</strong>alreferences in this resp<strong>on</strong>se (see below). Moreover,Weisfeld’s interpretati<strong>on</strong>s are idiosyncratic, do not reflect<strong>the</strong> broad c<strong>on</strong>sensus of primate <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> researchers, orare incorrect. The studies he cites (also cited by Ragir &Brooks) alleging that all primates have a skeletal, or evena body weight, growth spurt at <strong>the</strong> time of pubertyoriginated in <strong>the</strong> 1950s (Gavan 1953). Gavan’s originalpaper c<strong>on</strong>cludes that chimpanzees (a sample of 9 males<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 7 females) do not have a pubertal growth spurt.Watts <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gavan (1982) reanalyzed <strong>the</strong> 1953 chimpanzeedata, al<strong>on</strong>g with <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> data for <strong>the</strong> rhesus m<strong>on</strong>key, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>reported that a “very small” positive deviati<strong>on</strong> in b<strong>on</strong>egrowth could be detected at <strong>the</strong> time of puberty. “Verysmall” means less than a 3.0-mm deviati<strong>on</strong>, which occursduring <strong>on</strong>e year, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> detecti<strong>on</strong> scheme requiredpolynomial regressi<strong>on</strong> fitted to <strong>the</strong> b<strong>on</strong>e growth data.We have criticized <strong>the</strong> methods <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> statistics needed toshow <strong>the</strong>se “spurts” (Bogin 1999a; 1999b). The humanadolescent growth spurt is measured in centimeters –with a mean peak velocity of about 7.5 cm per year forgirls <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 9.0 cm per year for boys. The human spurttakes about eight years to complete. Leigh (1996; 2001)shows that, am<strong>on</strong>g 61 primate species, weight growthspurts vary greatly in magnitude <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> even in <strong>the</strong>ir existence– <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e species of New World m<strong>on</strong>key may have aweight spurt; gibb<strong>on</strong>s do not, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>ly males of <strong>the</strong>comm<strong>on</strong> chimpanzee do. Leigh could not c<strong>on</strong>firm thatany n<strong>on</strong>human primate species has a skeletal growthspurt. Hamada <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ud<strong>on</strong>o (2002) published <strong>the</strong> first statisticallysound <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> methodologically rigorous l<strong>on</strong>gitudinalstudy of chimpanzee skeletal growth. They found thatchimpanzees reared within semi-natural social c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sat a zoological park in Japan show no increase in l<strong>on</strong>gb<strong>on</strong>e growth velocity at <strong>the</strong> time of puberty (Fig. R1).In c<strong>on</strong>trast, chimpanzees used in medical research, whowere taken out of <strong>the</strong>ir social groups prior to puberty,stopped or greatly slowed growth during <strong>the</strong> time of experimentati<strong>on</strong>.When returned to <strong>the</strong>ir social group afterexperimentati<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>se chimpanzees experienced rapidgrowth, imitating a growth spurt. But this is not at allsimilar to <strong>the</strong> human adolescent growth spurt. Ra<strong>the</strong>r,Hamada <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ud<strong>on</strong>o (2002) explain that it is a type of“catch-up” growth following <strong>the</strong> stress of social separati<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> medical experimentati<strong>on</strong>. Human beings show thisFigure R1. Model of distance (left) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> velocity (right) curves for chimpanzee growth in body length by years of age. Data for <strong>the</strong>figure come from <strong>the</strong> l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study of captive chimpanzee growth c<strong>on</strong>ducted by Hamada <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ud<strong>on</strong>o (2002). The infancy,juvenile, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mature adult <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages are labeled. In <strong>the</strong> wild, weaning (W) usually takes place between 48–60 m<strong>on</strong>ths of age(Pusey 1983). In captivity, female puberty (P), assessed by age at first maximal perineal swelling, takes place at a mean age of 7.95years (Littlet<strong>on</strong> 2005).302 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>type of “catch-up” growth following chr<strong>on</strong>ic illness, nutriti<strong>on</strong>aldeprivati<strong>on</strong>, or severe emoti<strong>on</strong>al distress (Bogin1999b; Prader et al. 1963).Finally, Weisfeld writes that we should not ab<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong> of using <strong>the</strong> term “adolescence” to apply tosimians. He states, “What would we o<strong>the</strong>rwise call <strong>the</strong>period of puberty in simians?” How about “puberty”?Indeed, primate puberty is a neuroendocrine event of relativeshort durati<strong>on</strong>, taking place in <strong>the</strong> brain (Plant &Barker-Gibb 2004). Human adolescence follows thisevent, lasts for eight years, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> involves ritual behaviorsthat instruct <strong>the</strong> youth in ec<strong>on</strong>omic, sexual, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> politicalbehavior, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> eventually transform <strong>the</strong> youth into asocially acceptable adult ready for marriage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> parenthood.No o<strong>the</strong>r species comes close to this biological,behavioral, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cultural stage of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>.Eckhardt’s comments remind us that <strong>the</strong> homininlineage split off from o<strong>the</strong>r primates at least 6 milli<strong>on</strong>years ago. There is no evidence that chimpanzees, as weknow <strong>the</strong>m today, even existed at that time (McBrearty& Jabl<strong>on</strong>ski 2005). That chimpanzees knuckle-walk <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>humans have childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence merely reflects<strong>the</strong>ir independent evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adaptati<strong>on</strong> to differentecologies. Despite this separati<strong>on</strong> in time, King makes avery good point about c<strong>on</strong>tinuities between primatespecies in many aspects of communicati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social interacti<strong>on</strong>.The disc<strong>on</strong>tinuity, we believe, is <strong>the</strong> humancapacity for emoti<strong>on</strong>al engagement between multiplecaretakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dependent young that is clearly different.A few of <strong>the</strong> commentaries <strong>on</strong> our article seem to have adifferent noti<strong>on</strong> of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ory thanwe do (e.g., Bickert<strong>on</strong>; Dickins; Ragir & Brooks).O<strong>the</strong>rs, including Gogate, Kappeler, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Oller &Griebel, have expressed legitimate differences ofopini<strong>on</strong> with our model of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Let usaddress <strong>the</strong>ir comments with a clear definiti<strong>on</strong> of ourterms. Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> may be defined as <strong>the</strong> strategy anorganism uses to allocate its energy toward growth, maintenance,reproducti<strong>on</strong>, raising offspring to independence,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> avoiding death. For a mammal, it is <strong>the</strong> strategy ofwhen to be born, when to be weaned, how many <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>what type of pre-reproductive stages of <strong>development</strong> topass through, when to reproduce, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> when to die(Bogin & Smith 1996). Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, in this definiti<strong>on</strong>,encompasses <strong>the</strong> major growth, <strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reproductiveevents that occur between <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>death of an organism.Living things <strong>on</strong> earth have greatly different <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>strategies <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages – c<strong>on</strong>sider those insectswith more than a dozen stages from c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> to reproductiveadult. The differences in <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> characteristicscan have profound effects in several biological domains,including growth dynamics, ecology, demography, behavior,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of populati<strong>on</strong>s. Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ory,which is <strong>the</strong> scientific study of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-cycle strategies <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>ir evoluti<strong>on</strong>, provides a way to unify systematic researchin <strong>the</strong> many separate domains, each studied by differentgroups of specialists. One of <strong>the</strong> best introducti<strong>on</strong>s tocurrent <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ory is provided by Stearns (1992).Part of <strong>the</strong> focus of our article links <strong>the</strong> way in whichpeople grow <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> develop with human reproductivesuccess, which is greater than that of any o<strong>the</strong>r mammal.The social mammals, such as wolves, li<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> elephants,rear about 12–18% of <strong>the</strong>ir live-born offspring toadulthood. Our closest living relative, <strong>the</strong> chimpanzee,rears about 36% of its live-born offspring to maturity.But human hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> horticulturists, living intraditi<strong>on</strong>al societies without <strong>the</strong> benefits of modernmedical care, rear about 60% of <strong>the</strong>ir infants to adulthood.Industrial societies of North America <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Western Europesuccessfully rear at least 95% of live-born infants to maturity(Bogin 2001). Some of <strong>the</strong> reas<strong>on</strong>s for this amazinghuman reproductive success are short birth intervals dueto early weaning, <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> childhood stage of<strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human capital-building during <strong>the</strong>juvenile stage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> uniquely human adolescent stageof <strong>development</strong> (Kaplan et al. 2000). Despite <strong>the</strong> delayin first reproducti<strong>on</strong> produced by <strong>the</strong> inserti<strong>on</strong> of adolescence,primiparous human mo<strong>the</strong>rs are physically,socially, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> emoti<strong>on</strong>ally better prepared than o<strong>the</strong>rprimate females. This may be seen in <strong>the</strong> mortality ratesfor infants, which are lower for humans than any o<strong>the</strong>rmammalian species (Bogin 1999b).Perhaps <strong>the</strong> most important attribute leading to reproductivesuccess is human cooperative breeding. By thiswe mean <strong>the</strong> cooperative childcare <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> feeding by n<strong>on</strong>maternalkin <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>-kin that is essential to ensure <strong>the</strong>survival of a weaned child. Cooperative breeding hasevolved independently in several species of mammals,birds, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> insects. In those species, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in many but notall human groups, <strong>the</strong> helpers are close genetic relativesof <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r (Clutt<strong>on</strong>-Brock 2002). By assisting <strong>the</strong>mo<strong>the</strong>r to care for her offspring, <strong>the</strong> helpers increase<strong>the</strong>ir own inclusive fitness, meaning that <strong>the</strong>y increase<strong>the</strong> chances that <strong>the</strong>ir genetic kin will survive to reproductiveage (Hawkes et al. 1998; Paine & Hawkes 2006).Unlike o<strong>the</strong>r species, human societies define kinshiprelati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> basis of social as well as genetic ties. In traditi<strong>on</strong>alhuman societies, kinship is <strong>the</strong> central organizingprinciple for ec<strong>on</strong>omic producti<strong>on</strong>, social organizati<strong>on</strong>,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ideology. Human cooperative breeding, <strong>the</strong>refore,enhances <strong>the</strong> social, ec<strong>on</strong>omic, political, religious, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>cultural “fitness” of <strong>the</strong> group as much or more than itc<strong>on</strong>tributes to genetic fitness.We agree with Gogate’s reference to Dynamic SystemsTheory (DST), according to which causality should not beviewed as a linear sequence since <strong>the</strong> emergence of <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>systems is usually <strong>the</strong> result of interacti<strong>on</strong>s of manyfactors at multiple levels. Gogate makes two o<strong>the</strong>r pointsabout DST, <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language – namely, thatchange is not predetermined, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that change is drivenby instabilities within <strong>the</strong> system. We think that <strong>the</strong>seaspects of DST <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong> help explain disc<strong>on</strong>tinuitiesin <strong>the</strong> biology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior of closely related species suchas chimpanzees <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> people.The c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> stark reality of biological costs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>trade-offs are also part of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ory. Knight &Power state that we do not c<strong>on</strong>sider biological costs inour article, but we do use <strong>the</strong> word “cost” or “costly”four times in both a biological <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a social sense (alsosee sect. R9 of this Resp<strong>on</strong>se). For some reas<strong>on</strong>, wefailed to use <strong>the</strong> term “trade-off” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that is a serious omissi<strong>on</strong>in any article <strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>. A trade-off maybe thought of as <strong>the</strong> competiti<strong>on</strong> between two biological orbehavioral traits. Stearns says that “trade-offs occur whentwo traits compete for materials <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> energy within a singleorganism” or, “when selecti<strong>on</strong> for <strong>on</strong>e trait decreases <strong>the</strong>value of a sec<strong>on</strong>d trait” (1992, p. 223). An example of <strong>the</strong>BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 303


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>first type of trade-off is competiti<strong>on</strong> between organs ortissues of <strong>the</strong> body during growth. For example, shouldenergy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> materials be devoted to growing a large setof muscles or a larger brain? An example of <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>dtype of trade-off is <strong>the</strong> choice of producing <strong>on</strong>e large offspring(k-selecti<strong>on</strong>) or many, smaller offspring (r-selecti<strong>on</strong>;cf. Wils<strong>on</strong> 1980). All living things face <strong>the</strong>se trade-offdecisi<strong>on</strong>s. Some occur <strong>on</strong> a day-to-day basis, o<strong>the</strong>rsoccur over l<strong>on</strong>ger periods of time. Those that have reproductivec<strong>on</strong>sequences <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> occur over generati<strong>on</strong>s aresubject to natural selecti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> effected traits mayevolve over time.Had we discussed trade-offs, we might have forestalled<strong>the</strong> comment of Johanss<strong>on</strong>, Zlatev, & Gärdenfors(Johanss<strong>on</strong> et al.) that our model does not “explain <strong>the</strong>absence of language in, for example, chimpanzees”(emphasis in original; also see sect. R9). Chimpanzeeshave taken <strong>on</strong>e path in <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> trade-offs, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> itincludes a l<strong>on</strong>g period of nursing of infants to ensure survivalversus more rapid reproducti<strong>on</strong>, in <strong>the</strong> hope thatsome offspring survive (also see <strong>the</strong> final paragraph ofsect. 2.7 in <strong>the</strong> target article). Human ancestors evolvedcooperative breeding to have both more offspring <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>more survival, but with a trade-off cost in terms of socialinteracti<strong>on</strong>. That cost entails getting individuals withselfish interests to cooperate. Our model proposes thathuman ancestors intensified <strong>the</strong> use of h<strong>on</strong>est signaling,<strong>the</strong> use of communicative displays such as smiles, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>eventually language as a means to reduce social c<strong>on</strong>flict<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> foster cooperati<strong>on</strong>.Bjorklund & Grotuss pick up <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>me of evoluti<strong>on</strong>arytrade-offs in <strong>the</strong>ir commentary. They refer to <strong>development</strong>alplasticity, ano<strong>the</strong>r important c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mechanismthat we do not menti<strong>on</strong> in our target article but have discussedat some length elsewhere (Bogin 1999b; Locke1997). Bjorklund & Grotuss’s propositi<strong>on</strong> that language isa product of childhood plasticity in “sociocognitive abilities”is entirely c<strong>on</strong>sistent with <strong>the</strong> message of our article. Wethank <strong>the</strong>m for providing ano<strong>the</strong>r important way to lookat <strong>the</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language.The commentaries of Bright<strong>on</strong>, Mata, & Wilke(Bright<strong>on</strong> et al.) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ofPowers point to ano<strong>the</strong>r omissi<strong>on</strong>in our article, namely, <strong>the</strong> importance of ma<strong>the</strong>maticalmodeling. Much of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ory ispredicated <strong>on</strong> ma<strong>the</strong>matical models (e.g., Bogin 1988;Charnov 1993; Gage 1998; Gurven & Walker 2006;Kaplan et al. 2000; Stearns 1992;). Modeling adds clarityof c<strong>on</strong>ceptualizati<strong>on</strong>, delineates <strong>the</strong> variables <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> parametersto be c<strong>on</strong>sidered, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> offers <strong>on</strong>e type of scientifictestability. Modeling is akin to inductive reas<strong>on</strong>ing,whereas <strong>the</strong> approach we take in our article combinesdeductive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> inductive reas<strong>on</strong>ing. We nei<strong>the</strong>rdisagree with our critics nor do we grant <strong>the</strong>ir approachprimacy.Kaplan et al. (2000) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gurven <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Walker (2006) aretwo excellent examples of modeling human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, notmenti<strong>on</strong>ed by any of <strong>the</strong> commentators. Kaplan <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> colleaguesshow that human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> differs from <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>the</strong> chimpanzee in several key dimensi<strong>on</strong>s.Based <strong>on</strong> empirical research with living human foragingsocieties, <strong>the</strong>y show ma<strong>the</strong>matically that adults must provisi<strong>on</strong>human offspring for about 15 years. Until that age<strong>the</strong> young cannot produce enough food to meet <strong>the</strong>irown energetic dem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s. The need for such lengthyprovisi<strong>on</strong>ing requires <strong>the</strong> type of close, cooperative,h<strong>on</strong>est, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, at times, deceptive social c<strong>on</strong>tact that isboth a human defining trait <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitated by symboliclanguage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human cultural behavior (Gurven et al.2000). We also note that <strong>the</strong> provisi<strong>on</strong>ing of young <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>ir language <strong>development</strong> takes nearly all of human<strong>on</strong>togeny prior to reproductive age. This is not likely tobe a coincidence.Gurven <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Walker (2006) build <strong>on</strong> Bogin’s <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>model. They use computer simulati<strong>on</strong>s inductively to showthat slow childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juvenile growth allows for <strong>the</strong> typeof food provisi<strong>on</strong>ing that defines human cooperativebreeding. Moreover, Gurven <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Walker’s modelingshows that slow “growth followed by a rapid adolescentgrowth spurt may have facilitated rising human fertilityrates <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> greater investments in neural capital.”A fundamental distincti<strong>on</strong> in approaches taken in <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> research is clarified by <strong>the</strong> work of ma<strong>the</strong>maticalmodelers such as Gould (1977) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> McKinney (2000),cited in some commentaries, as well as Charnov (1993)<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gage (1998), who were not cited by any of our resp<strong>on</strong>dents.Gould <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> McKinney employ models based <strong>on</strong> heterochr<strong>on</strong>y,which is an evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary process that alters <strong>the</strong>timing of existing <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages. The heterochr<strong>on</strong>ymodel for human evoluti<strong>on</strong> holds that <strong>the</strong> existing <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>stages of human primate ancestors were leng<strong>the</strong>ned toderive <strong>the</strong> human c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (McKinney & McNamara1991). This is, essentially, <strong>the</strong> basis for <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuitymodel for primate biology presented in <strong>the</strong> commentariesalready discussed. Heterochr<strong>on</strong>y does have its place in <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, but we have found it lacking for humanevoluti<strong>on</strong>. We have reviewed <strong>the</strong> literature both for <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>against heterochr<strong>on</strong>y <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> offered <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> empirical evidence– using our inductive approach – against heterochr<strong>on</strong>y(Bogin 1997; 1999a; 1999b). Instead, we found evidencesupporting <strong>the</strong> inserti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> novel <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages ofchildhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence into human <strong>development</strong>.The inserti<strong>on</strong> of childhood shortened <strong>the</strong> infancy stage<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> juvenile stage of o<strong>the</strong>r primate species. Theinserti<strong>on</strong> of adolescence did leng<strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> time to adulthood,but not by leng<strong>the</strong>ning any pre-existing <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>stage. The inductive model <strong>the</strong>orists Charnov <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gagesupport our view. Charnov finds that <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> histories ofmost species of fish, insects, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mammals studied canbe modeled with his invariant <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> symmetries.The glaring excepti<strong>on</strong> is <strong>the</strong> species of Order Primates,especially Homo sapiens. Gage used both ma<strong>the</strong>maticalmodeling <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> empirical data to c<strong>on</strong>trast chimpanzee <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> with regard to demography. He findsthat <strong>the</strong>se two most genetically similar primates are quitedivergent. Chimpanzees have a l<strong>on</strong>ger reproductive <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>span than humans, due to both <strong>the</strong> later <strong>on</strong>set of fertility<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> menopause of human women (chimpanzee do nothave menopause; see Littlet<strong>on</strong> 2005). Chimpanzeessuffer greater infant mortality than humans. Indeed, chimpanzeemortality is greater at all ages prior to sexual maturati<strong>on</strong>.Human demography is unstable, meaning <strong>the</strong>re ismore variability in both mortality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fertility am<strong>on</strong>ghumans than am<strong>on</strong>g chimpanzees. Humans are anunusual primate species for having “baby booms.” Gagesuggests that <strong>the</strong>se fertility booms may have played animportant role in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human culture.Finally, <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> pattern of <strong>the</strong> human speciesdepends <strong>on</strong> low pre-reproductive mortality. This novel304 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>demographic c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> evolved after <strong>the</strong> p<strong>on</strong>gid-homininsplit, perhaps after <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> genus Homo.Gage’s inductive modeling supports <strong>the</strong> essential featuresof our deductive empirical associati<strong>on</strong>s. The evoluti<strong>on</strong> ofhuman childhood greatly increased <strong>the</strong> fertility ofwomen by decreasing both birth spacing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mortality.Cooperative breeding to support weaned but stilldependentchildren was <strong>the</strong> key to lowering mortality.Human culture, including language, evolved as part of<strong>the</strong> “package” that enhanced both cooperative breeding<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> individual reproductive success.Powers advises us to c<strong>on</strong>sider models based <strong>on</strong> geneticprogramming, evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary robotics, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> artificial <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Weare forced to leave such work to o<strong>the</strong>rs as this lies outsideour expertise. Of course, we would be pleased to work wi<strong>the</strong>xperts in those areas. Powers says that our Figure 3 in <strong>the</strong>target article is “disc<strong>on</strong>firmed,” but does not offer anyreference citati<strong>on</strong> to support that ra<strong>the</strong>r str<strong>on</strong>g statement.We do not know of any disc<strong>on</strong>firming evidence forFigure 3, which is based <strong>on</strong> stages or grades of homininevoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not specific species of hominins. Several ofour hominin pale<strong>on</strong>tology colleagues have reprinted thisfigure in <strong>the</strong>ir own work, including textbooks (e.g.,MacD<strong>on</strong>ald & Hershberger 2005; Relethford 2006;Stanford et al. 2006).R2. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>In <strong>the</strong> target article, we noted that <strong>the</strong>orizing in <strong>the</strong> area oflinguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong> is incomplete in two major ways. Thefirst is that most <strong>the</strong>ories presuppose <strong>the</strong> existence of <strong>the</strong>word, relieving <strong>the</strong>m of any perceived need to explainarticulatory or symbolic capabilities. As words lie farbey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> reach of o<strong>the</strong>r primate groups, no <strong>the</strong>ory oflanguage can be complete without an account of speech.We proposed several evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary processes, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fitnessbenefits, that may have increased c<strong>on</strong>trol over a range ofspeech sounds – something that language universallyrequires – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> offered a brief remark about sound–meaning relati<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of parental care.The sec<strong>on</strong>d characteristic that we noted also relates toan omissi<strong>on</strong>. Evoluti<strong>on</strong> is a two-stage process (West-Eberhard 2003). In <strong>the</strong> first stage, a plastic phenotyperesp<strong>on</strong>ds to envir<strong>on</strong>mental variati<strong>on</strong>, producing novelforms that vary genetically. In <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d stage, selecti<strong>on</strong>acts <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> variants. From <strong>the</strong> late nineteenth century to<strong>the</strong> present it has been argued that <strong>the</strong> first stage, <strong>the</strong>origin of novel characters, can <strong>on</strong>ly occur in <strong>development</strong>.It is <strong>the</strong>refore strange, we asserted, that <strong>the</strong>ories oflinguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong> have been largely devoid of any <strong>development</strong>alc<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s. Few <strong>the</strong>orists say anything about<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s of infancy, childhood, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juvenility, oreven, specifically, adolescence. The relevant behaviorsmerely appear in adulthood.One of <strong>the</strong> commentaries points out that we ourselvesomitted something. Bickert<strong>on</strong> w<strong>on</strong>ders why, in ourattempt to explain certain aspects of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> ofspoken language, we devoted so little attenti<strong>on</strong> to “<strong>the</strong>language faculty” that ultimately evolved – <strong>the</strong> capacityfor a lexic<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> especially <strong>the</strong> grammatical codes ofmodern languages – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> so much attenti<strong>on</strong> to “<strong>the</strong> useto which it is put.” No <strong>on</strong>e, he suggests, would c<strong>on</strong>fuseobjects such as cars <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> forks with <strong>the</strong> associated acti<strong>on</strong>sof driving <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> eating. Bickert<strong>on</strong> thus welcomes discussi<strong>on</strong>around <strong>the</strong> possibility that language is an object. Itscurrent use, he also claims, is primarily cognitive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>private. These views of language surely present interestingimplicati<strong>on</strong>s, if not complicati<strong>on</strong>s, for discussi<strong>on</strong>s of itsevoluti<strong>on</strong>.What is wr<strong>on</strong>g with viewing language as an object? Surelyit is not that language has no inventory of forms <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rules,because it does. Indeed, if we are to achieve a comprehensive<strong>the</strong>ory of linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong>, we must explain how ourspecies came to possess <strong>the</strong> capacity to learn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> use linguisticcodes. But in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>the</strong>ory,language-as-code can have serious c<strong>on</strong>sequences if itdistracts us from <strong>the</strong> behavioral processes that led tolanguage. For <strong>the</strong> things that code excludes – <strong>the</strong> emoti<strong>on</strong>s,intenti<strong>on</strong>s, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interacti<strong>on</strong>s of living beings – are, or shouldbe, <strong>the</strong> stuff of reproductive fitness accounts. Cues to pers<strong>on</strong>alidentity, <strong>development</strong>al stage, dominance, aggressiveness,health, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> fecundity may be gleaned from <strong>the</strong>voices of humans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r primates (Cheney & Seyfarth2005a; 2005b; Krauss et al. 2002), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a great deal ofadditi<strong>on</strong>al informati<strong>on</strong>, from h<strong>on</strong>esty <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> character to pers<strong>on</strong>ality<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> temperament, is to be found in speech. Inmany societies, speech also carries informati<strong>on</strong> aboutimportant cultural matters, such as social class, educati<strong>on</strong>,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> regi<strong>on</strong> of birth.If language can be so socially informative, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> so animated,how did it come to be thought of as a staticobject? The culprit, to some extent, may be literacy.Since print is inanimate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> stati<strong>on</strong>ary, <strong>on</strong>e can examine<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> endlessly re-examine language that is expressed inthis form. When words <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sentences are written down,wrote Lakoff <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Johns<strong>on</strong> (1980, p. 204), <strong>the</strong>y “can bereadily looked up<strong>on</strong> as objects.” When literate individualsthink about language, according to Linell (2005, p. 4), what<strong>the</strong>y think about is “an inventory of forms, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rules forgenerating forms.”Is language-as-thought – ei<strong>the</strong>r as a phenomen<strong>on</strong> or aproposal – also fostered by literacy? Goody <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Watt(1968) suggested that “writing, by objectifying words,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> by making <strong>the</strong>m <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir meaning available formuch more prol<strong>on</strong>ged <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intensive scrutiny than is possibleorally, encourages private thought” (p. 62, emphasisadded). Comparative researchers have also expresseddoubt about <strong>the</strong> naturalness or primacy of linguisticthought. Some point out that thought without languageis already present in n<strong>on</strong>human primates (Seyfarth et al.2005). O<strong>the</strong>rs fail to see how <strong>the</strong> linguistic thought hypo<strong>the</strong>sisaccounts for vocal learning or <strong>the</strong> meaning-to-soundmapping that occurs in language, or how <strong>on</strong>e couldappreciate <strong>the</strong> intellectual benefit of a private language“without a rich culturally-c<strong>on</strong>veyed set of words, sociallyaccreted over generati<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> lexic<strong>on</strong>” (Fitch 2005,p. 211). But <strong>the</strong> problem with language-as-thought is notthat it is incorrect; it is that this applicati<strong>on</strong>, if it occurswith significant frequency, is likely to have originatedfairly recently in evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>.Bickert<strong>on</strong> embraces Chomsky (1980) but neglects amore recent work, Chomsky (2002a), that seems toexplain <strong>the</strong> latter’s statements regarding language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>thought. In this later work, Chomsky associates himselfwith a c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> by Hauser (1996) to <strong>the</strong> effect thatlanguage fails to qualify as communicati<strong>on</strong> because it hasdifferent properties than <strong>the</strong> signaling systems of bees,BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 305


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>wolves, birds, squirrels, frogs, apes, m<strong>on</strong>keys, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>rn<strong>on</strong>linguistic species that are addressed in The Evoluti<strong>on</strong>of Communicati<strong>on</strong>. What, <strong>the</strong>n, is language? Chomskyresp<strong>on</strong>ds that it is “a system for expressing thought”(Chomsky 2002a, p. 76). This, he says, makes language“quite different” from <strong>the</strong> signaling systems of animals.Fair enough, but <strong>the</strong> expressi<strong>on</strong> of thought is whatpeople have in mind when <strong>the</strong>y say that language isused to communicate. It is virtually <strong>the</strong> dicti<strong>on</strong>arydefiniti<strong>on</strong>.In <strong>the</strong> target article, we discussed Hauser et al.’s (2002)distincti<strong>on</strong> between broad <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> narrow c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong>faculty of language. Bickert<strong>on</strong> suggests that we misinterpretthis distincti<strong>on</strong> “as licensing <strong>the</strong> subsuming ofstructural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pragmatic elements under a singleumbrella” while evincing no awareness of <strong>the</strong> argumentsthat we actually made for <strong>the</strong> “single umbrella.” In <strong>the</strong>target article, we sampled evidence from <strong>development</strong>,evoluti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> genetics. The messages from <strong>the</strong>se fieldsc<strong>on</strong>verge up<strong>on</strong> a single c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>: <strong>the</strong> capacity forspeech is intimately related to o<strong>the</strong>r levels of language.Developmentally, it is evident that infants who have difficultywith word learning are likely to have problems later<strong>on</strong> with grammar or pragmatics, even if <strong>the</strong>ir lexicaldifficulties abate. Evoluti<strong>on</strong>arily, it is evident, <strong>on</strong> logicalgrounds, that grammar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r areas of language mustbe related. For if some observable behavior increasedfitness at sexual maturity, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>development</strong>al precursorswould have been incidentally reinforced – <strong>development</strong>being c<strong>on</strong>tinuous – by virtue of facilitative relati<strong>on</strong>shipsof <strong>the</strong> sort that have been documented in c<strong>on</strong>temporaryhumans. In behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> molecular genetics, analysesof individuals with problems at multiple levels of languagehave revealed str<strong>on</strong>ger or more c<strong>on</strong>sistent effects in<strong>the</strong> areas of ph<strong>on</strong>etics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>ology, effects that implya vocal influence in <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of all areas oflanguage.The commentary by Bickert<strong>on</strong> was not <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e toraise c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thought. Oneo<strong>the</strong>r, that of Van Herwegen & Karmiloff-Smith,points out that we do not say whe<strong>the</strong>r it is language thatmakes humans intelligent. These commentators claimthat <strong>the</strong> uniqueness of humans depends <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> ability“to re-represent <strong>the</strong>ir representati<strong>on</strong>s into an explicitformat, transportable from <strong>on</strong>e domain of knowledge toano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong>e.” This may be true, but it has nothing to dowith our stated objective, or with any claims that wehave made.Johanss<strong>on</strong> et al. suggest that by failing to deal with “patternedc<strong>on</strong>tent” (something that also c<strong>on</strong>cerned Bright<strong>on</strong>et al., Gogate, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Powers) we do not actually explainlanguage. What we did do was identify <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>changes that would have provided critical opportunitiesfor increased plasticity, flexibility, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> innovati<strong>on</strong> of relevantbehaviors. We also presented arguments for <strong>the</strong>reproductive advantages of vocal, articulate, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> referentialbehaviors during different stages of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, frominfancy to adolescence. We made no attempt to explaingrammar, though a <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> approach such as ourscould well c<strong>on</strong>verge, at some point, with <strong>on</strong>going work <strong>on</strong>ph<strong>on</strong>etic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>ological patterns (Locke 2000a;MacNeilage & Davis 2000; Ohala 2005; Studdert-Kennedy1998; 2005). It could also produce an account of vocalcomplexity that interfaces with syntax (see sect. R5).R3. Developmental c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>of languageIt was gratifying to receive comments <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> role of <strong>development</strong>in evoluti<strong>on</strong> by several scholars who have inspiredour own thoughts in this area. Gardner c<strong>on</strong>gratulates usfor adopting a biological framework, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> for achieving anaccount that is more credible <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> relevant than strictly linguisticformulati<strong>on</strong>s. Bjorklund & Grotuss commend usfor focusing <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> role of childhood in evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Our ownvaluati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> young emphasized <strong>the</strong>ir creativity, in <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>texts of parental care <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> play. These commentatorscall attenti<strong>on</strong> to a possible extrafamilial c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> of<strong>the</strong> young: play groups in which words are combined innovel ways to form <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> words <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rudimentary syntax.They also point to <strong>the</strong> unusual plasticity of <strong>the</strong> developingbrain, suggesting that this might have endowed <strong>the</strong> youngwith a resp<strong>on</strong>siveness to envir<strong>on</strong>mental change surpassingthat of mature individuals. We see merit in both ideas, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>also in <strong>the</strong>ir suggesti<strong>on</strong> that language, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> its antecedents,may have altered <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>texts in which <strong>the</strong> young found<strong>the</strong>mselves, exposing <strong>the</strong>m to novel selecti<strong>on</strong> pressures.One change of c<strong>on</strong>text may have involved increases in<strong>the</strong> size of working groups. In hominins, <strong>the</strong> ability tospeak may have facilitated <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> levels of organizati<strong>on</strong>,enabling increases in group size. This would haveincreased cooperati<strong>on</strong> in some areas, such as predatordefense, while increasing within-group competiti<strong>on</strong> forphysical resources (Locke 2005), though a reverse-ordereffect has been argued in which larger groups encourageda transiti<strong>on</strong> from manual to vocal grooming <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech(Dunbar 1993). Larger groups would also have increasedsociosexual competiti<strong>on</strong>, as we suggested earlier, whichmay have affected males <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> females, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir uses ofvocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal behavior, differently (see sect. R4).When Kuiper refers to “analytic decompositi<strong>on</strong>” of anenlarging “phrasal” vocabulary, he is proposing that suchan act promoted <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of grammatical mechanisms.We agree, having argued in <strong>the</strong> past that in <strong>development</strong>,too, grammatical mechanisms are pressured intoacti<strong>on</strong> by a burge<strong>on</strong>ing store of prosodic forms. In <strong>the</strong>sec<strong>on</strong>d year of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>, about half of infants’ distinct utterancesmay be of this sort (Lieven et al. 1992; Locke 1997). Manypersist in prosodic form, seemingly immune to segmentaldecompositi<strong>on</strong>. We have proposed elsewhere that whenthis sort of material begins to overflow its memorialbanks, typically in <strong>the</strong> third year, it triggers activati<strong>on</strong> ofa system that analyses entries into elements corresp<strong>on</strong>dingto linguistic units (Locke 1993; 1997). This leads to moreefficient storage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> permits grammatical computati<strong>on</strong>.Hence, it may be said that words <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phrasal words not<strong>on</strong>ly precede grammar, but provide its justificati<strong>on</strong>.Johanss<strong>on</strong> et al. say that we fail to show why chimpanzeesdid not evolve language, but that “buried” in ouraccount “is <strong>the</strong> germ that may save” us. Fortunately,<strong>the</strong>y were not forced to dig too deeply to find it. Thegerms – bipedalism, pelvic narrowing, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> helplessness –were key comp<strong>on</strong>ents of our argument. We, like Johanss<strong>on</strong>et al., proposed increases in proximity, attenti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> careas key ingredients in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language as well as its<strong>development</strong> (Locke 1993).King <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Cowley wish we had placed more emphasis<strong>on</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong>. We agree with Cowley that language,which is typically manifested as speech, is more often306 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>produced with o<strong>the</strong>rs than to <strong>the</strong>m, a pattern initially manifestedin infancy. King sees antecedents in <strong>the</strong> acti<strong>on</strong>s ofo<strong>the</strong>r primates, <strong>on</strong>es that result in “mutually c<strong>on</strong>structivemeaning-making.” We would like to know more about<strong>the</strong> acti<strong>on</strong>s King has in mind. King welcomes our emphasis<strong>on</strong> parent-infant engagement, suggesting that messagemakingactivity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> especially <strong>the</strong> emoti<strong>on</strong>al energy thatdrives it, represents a powerful force in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> oflinguistic structure. We agree. Several years ago, wewrote that infants acquire language “merely by dancingwith mature humans,” <strong>the</strong> dance following <strong>on</strong> from “previoussuccess in <strong>the</strong> area of emoti<strong>on</strong>al <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>attachment” (Locke 2002, p. 633).Spoken language owes its existence, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a great deal ofits flexibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> utility, to a “comparably complex, flexibleph<strong>on</strong>ological system” (Fitch 2005, p. 200), something notpresent in <strong>the</strong> Last Comm<strong>on</strong> Ancestor shared with apes.One of <strong>the</strong> more important goals of evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary researchis to achieve an account of how vocal tract activity was submittedto cortical c<strong>on</strong>trol. Oller & Griebel take this <strong>on</strong>,asking how our hominin ancestors achieved <strong>the</strong> capacityto disc<strong>on</strong>nect vocal or, more precisely, articulatory activityfrom its previous envir<strong>on</strong>mental <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> emoti<strong>on</strong>al moorings.In <strong>the</strong>ir own research, taking a recapitulatory stance, <strong>the</strong>yask how this now occurs in developing infants. Oller &Griebel offer an interesting <strong>the</strong>oretical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> analyticframework, <strong>on</strong>e that deserves to be adopted by o<strong>the</strong>rresearchers. They fully underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> possibility thatvocalizati<strong>on</strong> may be associated with requests for care,just as it affects parental decisi<strong>on</strong>s regarding <strong>the</strong> qualityor quantity of care to provide. Nearly two thous<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>years ago, a medical doctor, Soranus of Ephesus(96–138 A.D., translated by Temkin in 1956), claimedthat certain <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>borns were “not worth rearing.” The<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>borns he had in mind, significantly, included thosewho cried atypically <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> failed to resp<strong>on</strong>d appropriatelyto external stimulati<strong>on</strong>.Oller & Griebel stress <strong>development</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> first sixm<strong>on</strong>ths of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Cooing, perhaps <strong>the</strong> first n<strong>on</strong>stress vocalizati<strong>on</strong>,appears at about three m<strong>on</strong>ths. This is an interestingage. It is also when crying declines, al<strong>on</strong>g withinfanticide (Locke, in press c), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> it is generally assumedthat this age is when instrumental crying (<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> smiling)begin. When <strong>the</strong> infant is finally freed from internal discomfortsthat block opti<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> playful forms of vocal behavior,it may begin to “read” o<strong>the</strong>rs to see how <strong>the</strong>y are affected byhis vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s. Doing so will make it easier to use syllablesstrategically to attract <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> engage o<strong>the</strong>rs.It is of course possible for articulatory c<strong>on</strong>trol to emergein associati<strong>on</strong> with endogenous variables, such as <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>of arm <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> activities, as may have occurred inresp<strong>on</strong>se to envir<strong>on</strong>mental changes that freed <strong>the</strong> limbsfrom <strong>the</strong>ir earlier role in ambulati<strong>on</strong> (cf. Locke, in pressa). But <strong>the</strong>re may also have been pressures to signal thatadded to <strong>the</strong> benefits of articulated ph<strong>on</strong>ati<strong>on</strong> by itself.Sharing Oller & Griebel’s c<strong>on</strong>cern with <strong>the</strong> emergenceof vocal-motor c<strong>on</strong>trol, Lenti Boero & Bott<strong>on</strong>i see valuein comparative studies of vocal <strong>development</strong>, especially ins<strong>on</strong>gbirds <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> parrots, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of various communicative practicesin n<strong>on</strong>human primates <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocal behavior in humaninfants. Schleidt also emphasizes comparative studies of<strong>development</strong>. For him, <strong>the</strong> chief c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> of different<strong>on</strong>togenetic stages relates to social factors, primarily <strong>the</strong>formati<strong>on</strong> of close ties to <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> types of compani<strong>on</strong>s.Powers chides us for not saying more about fetal learning.At <strong>on</strong>e time, we could have been accused of saying toomuch about it (Locke 1993; 1994a; 1994b; 1994c; 1997;1999; Locke & Snow 1997), but we repeat here: linguisticallyrelevant stimulati<strong>on</strong> appears to begin at some point in<strong>the</strong> final trimester of gestati<strong>on</strong>, possibly with <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>set ofauditi<strong>on</strong>.R4. Oral societies: Verbal performanceIn <strong>the</strong> target article, we devoted some space to oralsocieties in which orators – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in segments of industrialsocieties, “street orators” – exercise a number of impressivevocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal skills to attract audiences <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> persuade<strong>the</strong>m to <strong>the</strong>ir point of view. No formal training isneeded to do <strong>the</strong>se things, as <strong>the</strong> work <strong>on</strong> “Brazilianbards” c<strong>on</strong>firms (Roazzi et al. 1993; 1994). Kuiper, incommenting that literate societies “c<strong>on</strong>tain many subcultureswhich are oral societies,” stresses that orality isa hardy dispositi<strong>on</strong> that endures in spite of literate pressures.Even in <strong>the</strong> highly literate corridors of academia,oral story-telling reigns supreme (Wickham 1998).“Some people have a way with words,” comedian SteveMartin <strong>on</strong>ce joked, “o<strong>the</strong>r people ... not have way.”Kuiper points out that in many societies, those who “nothave way” cannot h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>le phrasal vocabulary (colloquialisms<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r forms of formulaic material) with somedegree of skill <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> may become “social lepers.” Invarious cultures, people who do not speak well are disrespectedor even censured (Edwards & Sienkewicz 1990;Peek 1981). In <strong>the</strong> target article, we pointed out thatAfrican Americans who lack <strong>the</strong> knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> skillneeded for verbal duels are derided as “lames” (Abrahams1989; Labov 1972; 1973), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> if <strong>the</strong>y fail with women, <strong>the</strong>irraps are ridiculed as “tissue paper” (Anders<strong>on</strong> 1990,p. 115).Clearly, various sorts of verbal gamesmanship are beneficialin adolescence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> early adulthood. This is alsotrue in <strong>the</strong> preceding stages of juvenility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> childhood,when riddling <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> formulaic jokes become popular in avariety of cultures around <strong>the</strong> world. McDowell (1979)encountered four different genres of verbal performancein “chicano” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “anglo” children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> juveniles in Texas:stories, rhymes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> s<strong>on</strong>gs, taunts, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> riddles. Theriddles were used in a competitive or ag<strong>on</strong>istic arena.The taunts expressed aggressi<strong>on</strong> in verbal form, <strong>the</strong>rebypreventing physically injurious activity. These genresalso help increasingly aut<strong>on</strong>omous individuals to makedecisi<strong>on</strong>s about which o<strong>the</strong>rs to include as friends <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>future collaborators, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to decide which groups to join.In adolescence, <strong>the</strong>y may be used as a means of judgingintelligence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> suitability for l<strong>on</strong>g-term relati<strong>on</strong>ships(see sect. R5).Several peers, including Benga, have commented <strong>on</strong>our treatment of verbal performance. Knight & Powerindicate that we use <strong>the</strong> term “performative” differentlyfrom Austin (1975), who cast about for alternativesbefore he settled <strong>on</strong> this term, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> who clearly felt aneed to justify his final choice (Austin 1975, pp. 6–7).However, our use is completely c<strong>on</strong>sistent with <strong>the</strong>relevant literatures. “Performer” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “performance”frequently appear in <strong>the</strong> accounts of oral societies (e.g.,Abrahams 1968; Bauman 1975; Gossen 1974; Ong 1982),BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 307


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>as does “performative” (e.g., Edwards & Sienkewicz 1990;Finnegan 1969; Linell 1982). This is unsurprising since <strong>the</strong>phenomena in questi<strong>on</strong> involve verbal artistry <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an audience.Still, to ensure that readers would not c<strong>on</strong>fuse ourmeaning with Austin’s, we referred to pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performancefive times. In secti<strong>on</strong> 2.8 of <strong>the</strong> target article, wepointed out that a number of verbal functi<strong>on</strong>s that bel<strong>on</strong>gto <strong>the</strong> performative functi<strong>on</strong> (e.g., joking, mollifying) falloutside <strong>the</strong> area of pragmatics as usually defined, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> insecti<strong>on</strong> 10 (title “Pragmatics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance”), wefur<strong>the</strong>r discuss <strong>the</strong>se categories.Knight & Power imply that we erred in claiming that“performative applicati<strong>on</strong>s of language, in <strong>the</strong> form ofspeech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> voice, c<strong>on</strong>sistently favor males.” Evidencefrom <strong>the</strong> (heavily Western) anthropological literature indicatesthat males are, in fact, more likely to engage in publicdisplays of vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal prowess than females. Thereare cultures where both men <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> women speak inpublic, but we have yet to find a culture where <strong>the</strong> maledominance pattern is reversed, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> would expect to findfew in which females outperform males in ag<strong>on</strong>isticdisplays.Knight & Power suggest that any evoluti<strong>on</strong>aryaccounts based <strong>on</strong> male dominance in <strong>the</strong> public displayof vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal behavior are necessarily limited,since <strong>the</strong>se would <strong>on</strong>ly take place in hierarchical or stratifiedsocieties. If this were true, it would be difficult tosquare with anthropological accounts. C<strong>on</strong>sider, forexample, <strong>the</strong> s<strong>on</strong>g duels of <strong>the</strong> Hindi-speaking FijiIndians, an “unstratified” society in Bhatga<strong>on</strong>. In <strong>the</strong>seduels, sung by men in groups of three to six, <strong>the</strong> “singersusually attack, insult, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>er <strong>the</strong> religi<strong>on</strong>, relatives,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pers<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong>ir opp<strong>on</strong>ents,” attempting “to lower<strong>the</strong> prestige of <strong>the</strong>ir opp<strong>on</strong>ents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir opp<strong>on</strong>ents’ religi<strong>on</strong>”(Brenneis & Padarath 1975, p. 283). “Despite anapparent lack of stratificati<strong>on</strong>,” <strong>the</strong> authors c<strong>on</strong>clude,“<strong>the</strong> nature of <strong>on</strong>e’s st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing relative to o<strong>the</strong>r men is arecurring c<strong>on</strong>cern for villagers” (p. 284, emphasis added).In <strong>the</strong> target article, we claimed that “important aspectsof language cannot appear until sexual maturity.” Knight& Power chose to c<strong>on</strong>flate this claim with an equallyunc<strong>on</strong>troversial statement in secti<strong>on</strong> 10, that “<strong>the</strong> abilityto infer intenti<strong>on</strong>s presupposes real-world knowledge,”(sect. 10, para. 2) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>n drew <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that <strong>the</strong>knowledge we had in mind had something to do withsexual behavior. It does not. What we stated is that pragmaticsis <strong>the</strong> last area of language to develop, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mustbe, because it depends <strong>on</strong> knowledge that could not bepresent until <strong>the</strong> individual had witnessed, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> engagedin, behaviors that typically emerge in <strong>the</strong> run up to adulthood.The knowledge we had in mind c<strong>on</strong>cerned <strong>the</strong>social, ec<strong>on</strong>omic, political, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> physical world as it can<strong>on</strong>ly be known to those who interact with it outside of<strong>the</strong> familial c<strong>on</strong>text, with some degree of aut<strong>on</strong>omy.Knight & Power again merge separate statements fromsecti<strong>on</strong> 6. There, we pointed out that <strong>the</strong> verbal dueling ofmales may be supported by testoster<strong>on</strong>e, which is relatedto dominance <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> aggressi<strong>on</strong> (Puts et al. 2005), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that itmay be adaptive for <strong>the</strong>m to use verbal sport <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> humor inplace of physical combat. Knight & Power take this tomean that in females, lower levels of testoster<strong>on</strong>e aresomehow resp<strong>on</strong>sible for <strong>the</strong> documented gossip preferenceassociated with that sex; <strong>the</strong>y <strong>the</strong>n interpret gossip,as a means of disseminating socially relevant informati<strong>on</strong>,to be syntactically more complex than <strong>the</strong> elaborate verbalperformances of males.There are at least two things to say here. The first is thatin males, testoster<strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>tributes to aggressi<strong>on</strong>, which isfrequently exercised physically (Dabbs 2000). To avoidphysically harming each o<strong>the</strong>r, some o<strong>the</strong>r behaviormust be substituted. Marsh (1978) has described <strong>the</strong>“aggro” of young British men, a cerem<strong>on</strong>ial display ofphysical posturing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal taunting that expressesaggravati<strong>on</strong> in a fairly harmless manner. Aggro, hewrites, “is an all-male affair ... females are irrelevant –<strong>the</strong>y form no part of <strong>the</strong> cerem<strong>on</strong>y <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, more importantly,<strong>the</strong>y are not legitimate objects of attack. There is noc<strong>on</strong>flict here except with o<strong>the</strong>r males” (Marsh 1978,p. 28). It is widely recognized that male adolescents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>young men need ways to avoid physically injuring eacho<strong>the</strong>r. Although this can be accomplished with n<strong>on</strong>verbalas well as verbal displays (Kochman 1969; 1983; Marsh1978), in practice, <strong>the</strong> two work toge<strong>the</strong>r. Street gangsthat are physically violent have a high rate of verbalduels (Short & Strodtbeck 1965).Although aggrieved members of both sexes swear at,scold, scorn, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> chastise o<strong>the</strong>rs, <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d thing t<strong>on</strong>ote is <strong>the</strong> str<strong>on</strong>g tendency of females to engage ingossip – a private verbal behavior used primarily or exclusivelywith o<strong>the</strong>r females – to lower <strong>the</strong> status of perceivedopp<strong>on</strong>ents, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to do so during <strong>the</strong> same <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> stages,juvenility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence, in which males engage inverbal duels – public verbal behavior used primarily orexclusively with o<strong>the</strong>r males – in an effort to competefor status <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r resources. However, in our referenceto gossip (sect. 6), we pointed out that <strong>the</strong> str<strong>on</strong>ger femaledispositi<strong>on</strong> to gossip “may have to do with <strong>the</strong> fact thatgossip serves an affiliative functi<strong>on</strong>,” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that females, intimes of stress, “tend to come toge<strong>the</strong>r,” turning to eacho<strong>the</strong>r “for mutual aid” (sect. 6, para. 2, emphasis added).It was in this c<strong>on</strong>text that we discussed sexually dimorphicapplicati<strong>on</strong>s of speech.In searching for <strong>the</strong> biological bases of such differences<strong>the</strong>re are plenty of places to look. C<strong>on</strong>sider <strong>the</strong> birthprocess. In <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r primates, <strong>the</strong>re appears to be a tendencyto give birth al<strong>on</strong>e. Women, <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, seekeach o<strong>the</strong>r out at <strong>the</strong> time of childbirth (Trevathan 1999;Walrath 2003). Even in <strong>the</strong> case of apparent c<strong>on</strong>tradicti<strong>on</strong>sto this pattern, <strong>the</strong> woman typically does not leave <strong>the</strong>group for her first child, or go too far to summ<strong>on</strong> help ifneeded (Biesele 1997).If women gossip more than men, Knight & Power say,this should give <strong>the</strong>m an advantage in <strong>the</strong> area of syntax,but this takes several things for granted. First, it assumesthat gossip is syntactically complex. We accept that itmay be. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, it assumes that <strong>the</strong> more public displaysof verbal prowess by male speakers are syntacticallydeficient. To our knowledge, no such evidence exists.Indeed, <strong>the</strong> reports indicate that <strong>the</strong> more powerful individualsin traditi<strong>on</strong>al societies are unusually expert inmanipulating sentential material.Studies of babo<strong>on</strong>s indicate that here, too, females areless aggressive than <strong>the</strong>ir male counterparts. Seyfarth(1976) has reported that <strong>the</strong> ratio of approach-retreatinteracti<strong>on</strong>s to bouts of overt aggressi<strong>on</strong> is less than 2:1in males, but 20:1 in females. The affiliative functi<strong>on</strong> thatis fulfilled by human gossip may also have antecedents inn<strong>on</strong>human primates. Elsewhere, we have reviewed308 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>research indicating that social grooming is carried out predominatelyby females (cf. Locke 2000b; in press b). Whengrooming, m<strong>on</strong>keys characteristically emit lipsmacks <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>girneys – low amplitude oral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocal activity that, tosome ears, bears faint resemblance to speech (Richman1980; 1987) <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> also is displayed in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of appeasement.In a study of captive Japanese macaques, 90% of all<strong>the</strong> girneys were produced by females (Blount 1985).There is evidence in our own species that girls are lesscompetitive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> domineering than boys (Benens<strong>on</strong> et al.2001; Miller et al. 1986; also see review in Campbell1999). A factor in this pattern may be <strong>the</strong> size of socialgroups. Left to <strong>the</strong>ir own devices, females prefer dyadicinteracti<strong>on</strong>. Males, by c<strong>on</strong>trast, prefer larger groups(Belle 1989). The significance of this is that largergroups foster competiti<strong>on</strong>. In Benens<strong>on</strong> et al. (2001),6- <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 7-year-old boys, but not girls, made a higher proporti<strong>on</strong>of competitive moves in tetrads than in dyads.Liénard asks if <strong>the</strong>re is evidence that women are verballydominated by men, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>re is evidence – at leastwhen it comes to interrupti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> volubility. Investigatorstypically find that females are more inclined to speak thanmales when interacting with a same-sex c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>alpartner of <strong>the</strong>ir own choosing, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in this c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>the</strong>yalso tend to be more talkative in juvenility, childhood,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infancy (see Locke & Hauser 1999 for references).We welcome Wermke & Mende’s emphasis <strong>on</strong>melody. They argue that melody may provide <strong>the</strong> immatureinfant brain with “a kind of filter to extract <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>relevantinformati<strong>on</strong> out of <strong>the</strong> complex parental speechstream” (last para.). They remind us that, to infants, <strong>the</strong>most salient of all acoustic comp<strong>on</strong>ents in speech isprosody, a feature that parents exaggerate when addressing<strong>the</strong>ir infant. It is also a feature that figures into <strong>the</strong>identificati<strong>on</strong> of prepackaged phrasal material, asKuiper underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s.Johanss<strong>on</strong> et al. ask why protohumans need to havespoken when <strong>the</strong>y could have achieved similar benefitsby singing. Our missi<strong>on</strong>, of course, was broader thanvocal c<strong>on</strong>trol; we also took up reference, within <strong>the</strong>c<strong>on</strong>text of interacti<strong>on</strong>s between parents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir young,as did Fitch (2004; 2005). Still, <strong>the</strong>re are at least threereas<strong>on</strong>s to study speech in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with s<strong>on</strong>g. First,<strong>the</strong>re is, as we have pointed out, a logical c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>between <strong>the</strong> use <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol of <strong>the</strong> voice in singing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>speech – an excellent reas<strong>on</strong> for collaborati<strong>on</strong> betweenspeech researchers such as Oller & Griebel <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thosewho study music <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> s<strong>on</strong>g (e.g., Merker 2000; Mi<strong>the</strong>n2005). Sec<strong>on</strong>d, verbal duels <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocal c<strong>on</strong>tests appear toserve similar social functi<strong>on</strong>s in several different societies.Finally, competitive pressures in both modalities may haveincreased our species’ capacity for vocal complexity. Wediscuss <strong>the</strong>se functi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pressures in <strong>the</strong> secti<strong>on</strong> thatfollows.R5. Verbal plumage: Folk IQ?Functi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> s<strong>on</strong>g areimplied by research revealing a male bias <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> an ag<strong>on</strong>istict<strong>on</strong>e in vocal as well as verbal dueling. Hernd<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>McLeod (1980) observed that in Malta, <strong>the</strong> most frequentlyperformed style of folk music, <strong>the</strong> s<strong>on</strong>g duel, orspirtu pr<strong>on</strong>t, “is an improvised poetic duel, sung by twomen, which develops a subject by means of complex metaphoricalinsult.” The spirtu pr<strong>on</strong>t is completely improvised,except for <strong>the</strong> melody. “Due to <strong>the</strong> complexity ofthis sung poetry,” according to Hernd<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> McLeod,“those who attempt it must be extremely intelligent”(p. 147, emphasis added).Several years ago, we suggested that modern menattract <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> impress listeners “with verbal plumage – fluentmastery of a broad range of words <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phrases, many of<strong>the</strong>m outside <strong>the</strong> more limited repertoire of <strong>the</strong>ir audience”(Locke 1998c, p. 92, emphasis in <strong>the</strong> original). In<strong>the</strong> target article (Note 6), we pointed to evidence inindustrialized societies that <strong>the</strong> use of rare, l<strong>on</strong>g, abstract,diverse, or unusual words is correlated with formalmeasures of intelligence. Here we speculate that in evoluti<strong>on</strong>,verbal ornamentati<strong>on</strong> was created so that performers’cognitive abilities could be displayed <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, thus,evaluated by o<strong>the</strong>rs. If so, verbal plumage is something<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> order of a folk IQ test.It is evident that precisely this purpose – intelligenceassessment – is now served by <strong>the</strong> Maltese s<strong>on</strong>g duelsmenti<strong>on</strong>ed above, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that riddles permit similar judgmentsin <strong>the</strong> wide range of cultures in which <strong>the</strong>y areused. In 1783, Chrisfrid Gan<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>er described an ancestralFinnish traditi<strong>on</strong> in which riddles were used to test “<strong>the</strong>acuity, intelligence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> skills” of suitors. When a youngman sought romantic commitment, “three or moreriddles were posed to him, to test his mind with <strong>the</strong>m,”wrote Gan<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>er, “<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> if he could answer <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interpret<strong>the</strong>m, he received <strong>the</strong> girl, o<strong>the</strong>rwise not, but was classifiedas stupid <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> good for nothing” (quoted by Mar<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>a 1976,p. 127, emphasis added; also see Burns 1976).Associati<strong>on</strong>s between verbal ability <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intelligencehave been described in members of African <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> African-American cultures. In a review, Peek (1981) points outthat am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Igbo of sou<strong>the</strong>astern Nigeria, oratoricalability “is directly equated with intelligence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> success,”<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that <strong>the</strong> Barundi people of central Africa associateverbal ability with “successful cleverness” (Peek 1981,p. 22). In Sierra Le<strong>on</strong>e, “it is noticeable how str<strong>on</strong>gly <strong>the</strong>Limba c<strong>on</strong>nect intelligence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speaking” (Finnegan1969, p. 75, emphasis added). In African American communitiestoo, “rap” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “<strong>the</strong> dozens” permit evaluati<strong>on</strong>sof an individual’s ability to compete <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adapt. “Blackculture,” Kochman (1971) has noted, “promotes <strong>the</strong><strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of those skills whichreflect <strong>on</strong> an individual’s underlying intelligence, verbalability, speed, strength, agility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> endurance” (p. 19,emphasis added).But <strong>the</strong> associati<strong>on</strong> between speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intelligence isnot limited to individuals of African descent. Am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>Chamula Indians of sou<strong>the</strong>rn Mexico, <strong>the</strong> ability toperform well in “truly frivolous talk,” a verbal duelfought exclusively by young men, “serves as a fairlydependable index of a man’s social maturity, linguisticcompetence, intelligence, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> political potential” (Gossen1974, p. 106, emphasis added).Since each <strong>development</strong>al stage c<strong>on</strong>tributes to language,we should not be surprised to encounter nascent forms ofverbal performances <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> duels l<strong>on</strong>g before adolescence. Inhis fieldwork with <strong>the</strong> Chamula Indians, Gossen (1976b)heard attempts by 2- or 3-year-old boys to verbally duelwith <strong>the</strong>ir older bro<strong>the</strong>rs, with increases in ph<strong>on</strong>ologicalproficiency at ages 5 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 6 years. In a rare report, verbalBEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 309


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>performances of 3- to 5-year-old African American boyswere captured <strong>on</strong> audiotape <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> transcribed. In <strong>on</strong>e case,a sp<strong>on</strong>taneous rap, a boy expressed his feelings toward<strong>on</strong>e of <strong>the</strong> female research assistants <strong>on</strong> whom he haddeveloped a crush. In <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r case, three boysengaged in “<strong>the</strong> dozens” (Wyatt 1995; 1999). These boyswere playing, but a decade later <strong>the</strong>y would be expectedto enlist <strong>the</strong>ir verbal abilities as adaptive strategies, <strong>on</strong>esthat would help <strong>the</strong>m to attract females, avoid violence,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> compete for status with same-sex peers.To this point, we have exposed few c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>sbetween linguistic functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> form, but if <strong>the</strong>re was areproductive advantage of vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal complexity,runaway selecti<strong>on</strong> – which, as Harvey <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Arnold (1982)point out, would certainly have applied to vocalizati<strong>on</strong> –may have produced a huge increase in <strong>the</strong> complexity ofspeech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <strong>the</strong>refore, <strong>the</strong> possibility of complexity ato<strong>the</strong>r levels of language, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> possibly in o<strong>the</strong>r modalities.If speech were kept simple, observers would have to findo<strong>the</strong>r ways to make <strong>the</strong> necessary inferences.If a performance is rhythmic or euph<strong>on</strong>ious, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>material novel or complex, <strong>the</strong>n it is possible that form<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> functi<strong>on</strong> interact. Are <strong>the</strong>re relati<strong>on</strong>ships between<strong>the</strong> quality of <strong>the</strong> dance <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> ornamentati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong>dancer? There are multiple cues to attractiveness ino<strong>the</strong>r species, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se interact in extremely complexways to produce <strong>the</strong>ir effect (Badyaev & Hill 2003;C<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>olin 2003; van Doorn & Weissing 2004). They evencomplement each o<strong>the</strong>r (Badyaev et al. 2002; Colemanet al. 2004; Johns<strong>on</strong> 1999). In our own species, too, speakersare evaluated al<strong>on</strong>g with <strong>the</strong>ir messages. It is possiblethat in display mode, <strong>the</strong> evaluati<strong>on</strong>s of o<strong>the</strong>rs are basedboth <strong>on</strong> structural complexity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavioral finesse.R6. Genetic support of verbal performanceWe have proposed that vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal skills enable adolescents<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> young adults to engage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> compete witho<strong>the</strong>rs, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to perform in public arenas – <strong>the</strong> relevantabilities increasing <strong>the</strong>ir fitness. Now it could be saidthat <strong>the</strong>se verbal dimorphisms were merely brought to<strong>the</strong> surface <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> made observable by underlying sex differencesin <strong>the</strong> tendency to compete <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> aggress (Fitch 2005,p. 212). But, for behavioral traits, as opposed to physical<strong>on</strong>es that are readily perceptible, such as size, <strong>the</strong> individualmust be motivated to act in <strong>the</strong> first place, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>source of <strong>the</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong> may be an internal change. Infact, if anything was selected directly, it was vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>verbal ability. The indirect effects were <strong>the</strong> supportingphysiological variables. Thus, males’ greater dispositi<strong>on</strong>to compete – sec<strong>on</strong>dary to increases in testoster<strong>on</strong>e –may have increased <strong>the</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>ality of <strong>the</strong>ir vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>verbal ability while also making it public, raising <strong>the</strong> probabilitythat <strong>the</strong>se skills would be selected, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> tightening<strong>the</strong> links between speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testoster<strong>on</strong>e. If so, <strong>on</strong>emight expect a genetic legacy: genes that code specificallyfor verbal expressivity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dominance in modern humans.Recently we came across several reports indicating thatm<strong>on</strong>ozygotic twins, but not dizygotic twins, are highly c<strong>on</strong>cordant<strong>on</strong> measures of social potency <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> expressivec<strong>on</strong>trol, tests that tap <strong>the</strong> ability to act or perform insocial situati<strong>on</strong>s, even where <strong>the</strong> twins have been rearedapart (Gangestad & Simps<strong>on</strong> 1993; Lykken 1982).Significantly, individuals who were unusually social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>expressive reported that <strong>the</strong>y had engaged in sex withmore partners, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> with more partners <strong>on</strong> just <strong>on</strong>eoccasi<strong>on</strong>, than <strong>the</strong>ir less social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> expressive peers(Snyder 1987).With such high c<strong>on</strong>cordance rates, <strong>on</strong>e might expect tosee differences in sociality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> expressiveness appearingearly in <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>. It is interesting, in this c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>,that investigators in o<strong>the</strong>r fields have documented <strong>the</strong>existence of unusually social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> expressive individualsin infancy (cf. Nels<strong>on</strong> 1973; 1981), termed “dramatists”by Wolf <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gardner (1979) since <strong>the</strong>y appear to learnat an early age that different individuals can exchangeroles in identical social situati<strong>on</strong>s. Dramatists also like totell dramatic stories <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> display elaborate forms of imitati<strong>on</strong>.On <strong>the</strong> basis of c<strong>on</strong>tent validity, Gangestad <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Snyder (1986) speculated that such expressive differencesin infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adulthood are manifestati<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong> samelatent factor.This secti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetics of verbal performance is ac<strong>on</strong>venient place to acknowledge <strong>the</strong> kind remarks ofWorkman, who does, however, misinterpret our discussi<strong>on</strong>of FOXP2, thinking that we see in this gene a basisfor linguistic grammar. In fact, we referred to <strong>the</strong> behavioralresearch leading to <strong>the</strong> identificati<strong>on</strong> of this genebecause it related to <strong>the</strong> role in language that is playedby “producti<strong>on</strong> factors.” We pointed out that problemsexperienced by <strong>the</strong> KE family “included a severe oral<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal dyspraxia, accompanied by deficits at o<strong>the</strong>rlevels of language” (emphasis added), <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> quoted fromseveral <strong>the</strong>orists who suggest that speech might have acertain primacy that has been neglected in <strong>the</strong> past. Thissupported our argument that verbal performance wasselected.R7. Performance as adaptati<strong>on</strong>What supported <strong>the</strong> use of vocal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech-like activitybefore <strong>the</strong>re was speech? Knight & Power doubt ouraccount of reproductive advantages because, <strong>the</strong>y say, linguisticsigns represent no “cost” or investment. They fail torecognize several things. First, <strong>the</strong> resting metabolic rate(RMR) of <strong>the</strong> human brain exacts a c<strong>on</strong>tinuously highenergetic cost from infancy, as menti<strong>on</strong>ed in <strong>the</strong> targetarticle <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> in secti<strong>on</strong> R1. Were this not so, manycomplex human activities, including language, would beimpossible. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, articulated ph<strong>on</strong>ati<strong>on</strong> carries a greatdeal of informati<strong>on</strong> about <strong>the</strong> individual, whe<strong>the</strong>r or notit is c<strong>on</strong>sciously transmitted. Apes rely <strong>on</strong> this informati<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> so do we. Third, potentially informative comp<strong>on</strong>entsof <strong>the</strong> voice are supported by physiological activity thatis energetically expensive (e.g., pulm<strong>on</strong>ary pressure; cf.Fitch & Hauser 2002). Fourth, traits will be selected iffemales prefer <strong>the</strong>m, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not everything females preferis costly (Johns<strong>on</strong> 1999). Finally, <strong>the</strong> mere uttering ofcertain words (or o<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>s), especially rare<strong>on</strong>es, may reflect a great deal of prior learning – learningthat presupposes <strong>the</strong> efficient acti<strong>on</strong> of relevant mechanisms.Merely to use a behavior that was learned, orrequires an unusual mode of articulati<strong>on</strong>, can mean that<strong>the</strong> individual has an intact nervous system, has <strong>the</strong>ability to attend, perceive, store, retrieve, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reproducecomplex patterns of behavior, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> can do so meaningfully310 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


Resp<strong>on</strong>se/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>in an appropriate social c<strong>on</strong>text. The energetic requirementsof <strong>the</strong> motoric act, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> veracity of any statements,are not <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly relevant factors.Dickins sees a need to identify proximate mechanismsfor <strong>the</strong> acti<strong>on</strong>s that we propose, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Powers would likeus to have made more explicit predicti<strong>on</strong>s. Perhaps weunderplayed our effort to c<strong>on</strong>struct an adaptive accountof vocalizati<strong>on</strong> in relati<strong>on</strong> to social dominance <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> courtship.In secti<strong>on</strong> 8, we suggested that women who areattracted to men with a comm<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing way of speakingmay get a mate for <strong>the</strong>mselves, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> a fa<strong>the</strong>r for <strong>the</strong>ir children,who has high levels of testoster<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> status, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>unusual access to valued resources. Specifically, we evaluatedresearch that reveals c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between propertiesof male voices, as measured acoustically <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> appraised perceptually,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> both virility <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social dominance. In light ofo<strong>the</strong>r research, we would predict that women prefer <strong>the</strong>voices of men who are high in testoster<strong>on</strong>e, especiallywhen heard during ovulati<strong>on</strong> (Fisher 2004; Puts 2005).In <strong>the</strong> target article we reviewed research indicating that<strong>the</strong> speech of courting men c<strong>on</strong>tains unusually wide frequency<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intensity swings, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that young women,when asked to evaluate utterances extracted from children’sc<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s, expressed a preference for selecti<strong>on</strong>s thatwere classified as excitable, loud, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> unc<strong>on</strong>trolled,am<strong>on</strong>g related variables. Taken toge<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>se behaviorscould suggest <strong>the</strong> operati<strong>on</strong> of a sensory trap (after <strong>the</strong>pattern described in insects <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r animals) in whichsexually mature males mimic <strong>the</strong> vocal qualities thatappear to excite females (Christy 1995; Enquist & Arak1993; Macias Garcia & Ramirez 2005). Surely <strong>the</strong>re is rawmaterial here for testable hypo<strong>the</strong>ses <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> fitness benefitsnot just of speech, but of particular aspects of prosody, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>investigati<strong>on</strong>s could easily be extended into lexical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>o<strong>the</strong>r areas of spoken language. To implement such anagenda in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text of linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong>, it will ofcourse be necessary to investigate <strong>the</strong> perceptual preferencesof sexually mature females <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> use of male vocalizati<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech in social competiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> courtship, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>this is exactly <strong>the</strong> kind of research that we propose. In li<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>ith a st<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ard approach in biology, it would <strong>the</strong>n be possibleto look at which vocal variables were most str<strong>on</strong>glylinked to percepti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> proximate variables that indexfitness. By carrying out a research agenda of this sort, <strong>on</strong>edoes not show how language evolved, of course, but <strong>on</strong>emay be able to argue for <strong>the</strong> plausibility of a particularproposal <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> basis of empirical data.Dickins is not sure that we adequately supported ourclaim that <strong>the</strong> comp<strong>on</strong>ents of language are related, but itcan <strong>on</strong>ly be so if, as we argued in <strong>the</strong> target article <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>repeated earlier in this resp<strong>on</strong>se (sect. R3), <strong>the</strong>y werestitched toge<strong>the</strong>r by sequential patterns of selecti<strong>on</strong>. Behaviorsthat were selected at sexual maturity, such as verbalpersuasiveness, could not have appeared without somelevel of pragmatic <strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> without linguisticskills that rest <strong>on</strong> previous accomplishments in <strong>the</strong> areaof lexical, ph<strong>on</strong>ological, morphological, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> syntactic<strong>development</strong>. We noted in this c<strong>on</strong>text, additi<strong>on</strong>ally, <strong>the</strong>well-attested observati<strong>on</strong> that <strong>development</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>tinuous,behaviors not usually emerging de novo in later <strong>on</strong>togeneticstages without an appearance, in some form, in <strong>on</strong>eor more of <strong>the</strong> preceding <strong>on</strong>es. We also cited evidencesuggesting that individuals having problems in <strong>on</strong>e areaof language tend to have problems in o<strong>the</strong>r areas.Johanss<strong>on</strong> et al. say that we failed to show “thatlanguage was useful <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adaptive specifically for protohumans<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> not for proto-chimpanzees.” Ragir &Brooks seem to think, in parallel, that if language benefitshumans, it should have benefited <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r primatesequally. If it did, <strong>the</strong>y say, <strong>the</strong> behavior would have followed.But <strong>the</strong>se comments miss <strong>the</strong> point. The questi<strong>on</strong>is not who language could have assisted, but ra<strong>the</strong>r, inwhom it could have evolved. In secti<strong>on</strong> R1, we point outthat hominins took a different path, <strong>on</strong>e that led to cooperativebreeding <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> protolanguage, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>reforeevolved a completely different <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>.Ragir & Brooks comment that performance is beneficialat every stage of language <strong>development</strong>, not justin adolescence. That we also believe this is why wedevoted so much attenti<strong>on</strong> to proposals regarding infantbehavior, including parental selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> trickle-up ph<strong>on</strong>etics.In <strong>the</strong> target article, we argued that skillful vocalperformances by adolescent <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adult male homininswould have facilitated <strong>the</strong>ir quest for rank <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reproducti<strong>on</strong>.Ragir & Brooks express doubt about a porti<strong>on</strong> ofthis argument since, <strong>the</strong>y say, female apes fail to show apreference for alpha males. The relevance of this isunclear, since our claim is that <strong>the</strong>re were changes inhominins that occurred following <strong>the</strong>ir divergence fromshared lineage. As indicated in <strong>the</strong> target article, wetrace <strong>the</strong>se changes to pre-adult stages when c<strong>on</strong>trolover <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> more variegated vocal behaviors emergedunder envir<strong>on</strong>mental pressures that remodeled humaninfancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tributed to <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of childhood.R8. PluralismWe noted at <strong>the</strong> beginning of secti<strong>on</strong> R2 that in <strong>the</strong> first stageof evoluti<strong>on</strong>, envir<strong>on</strong>mental variati<strong>on</strong> produces novel formsthat vary genetically, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> that in <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d stage of evoluti<strong>on</strong>,selecti<strong>on</strong> acts <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> variants. According to ouraccount, infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> childhood produced <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> abilities toeffect <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> manipulate vocal forms, with selecti<strong>on</strong> occurringin those stages <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> perhaps more powerfully at sexual maturity.To us, <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language – <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ultimately its nature – are interrelated. Work in <strong>on</strong>e areapotentially affects progress in <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r. Theorists mustintegrate findings in psychology, biology, animal behavior,anthropology, archaeology, genetics, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistics if<strong>the</strong>se benefits are to be appreciated. In our own attemptto explain <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> modificati<strong>on</strong> of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>stages, we needed to integrate findings from archaeologywith findings <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of brains, teeth, endocrinesystems, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r characteristics of modern humans. Wethink that such efforts will eventually produce a small steptoward <strong>the</strong> unificati<strong>on</strong> of a broad base of language-relevantknowledge, emanating from a wide range of disciplines.Theorists also need to explain <strong>development</strong>s across <strong>the</strong>various stages of linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong>, not merely thosepertaining to language as it is, or as it appears, now. Wehave claimed that inserti<strong>on</strong> of <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>togenetic stages intohuman <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> remodeling of o<strong>the</strong>rs, produced<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>al processes that fashi<strong>on</strong>ed novelcommunicative behaviors <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> increased fitness. If so,reciprocal acti<strong>on</strong> by evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>almechanisms – working in an “evo-devo-evo” sequence –may have played a major role in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language.BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 311


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>ReferencesLetters “a” <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “r” appearing before authors’ initials refer to target article<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se, respectively.Abitol, J., Abitol, P. & Abitol, B. (1999) Sex horm<strong>on</strong>es <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> female voice. Journalof Voice 13:424–46. [aJLL]Abrahams, R. D. (1962) “Playing <strong>the</strong> dozens.” Journal of American Folklore73:209–20. [aJLL](1968) Introductory remarks to a rhetorical <strong>the</strong>ory of folklore. Journal ofAmerican Folklore 81:143–58. [rJLL](1970a) Deep down in <strong>the</strong> jungle: Negro narrative folklore from <strong>the</strong> streets ofPhiladelphia. Aldine. [aJLL](1970b) Rapping <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> capping: Black talk as art. In: Black Americans,ed. J. F. Szwed. Voice of America. [aJLL](1970c) Traditi<strong>on</strong>s of eloquence in Afro-American communities. Journal ofInter-American Studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> World Affairs 12:505–27. [aJLL](1973) The training of <strong>the</strong> man of words in talking sweet. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> in Society2:15–29. [aJLL](1989) Black talking <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> streets. In: Explorati<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> ethnography ofspeaking, ed. R. Bauman & J. Sherzer, pp. 240–62. Cambridge UniversityPress. [arJLL]Abramovitch, R. (1976) The relati<strong>on</strong> of attenti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> proximity to rank in preschoolchildren. In: The social structure of attenti<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. R. A. Chance &R. R. Larsen, pp. 153–76. Wiley. [aJLL]Adams, C. (2002) Practiti<strong>on</strong>er review: The assessment of language pragmatics.Journal of Child Psychology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychiatry 43:973–87. [aJLL]Addingt<strong>on</strong>, D. W. (1968) The relati<strong>on</strong>ship of selected vocal characteristics topers<strong>on</strong>ality percepti<strong>on</strong>. Speech M<strong>on</strong>ographs 35:492–503. [aJLL]Altmann, J. (1980) Babo<strong>on</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infants. Harvard University Press. [aJLL](1986) Adolescent pregnancies in n<strong>on</strong>-human primates: An ecological <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>development</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>. In: School-age pregnancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> parenthood:Biosocial dimensi<strong>on</strong>s, ed. J. B. Lancaster & B. A. Hamburg, pp. 247–62.Aldine de Gruyter. [GEW]Anders<strong>on</strong>, E. (1990) Streetwise: Race, class, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> change in an urban community.University of Chicago Press. [rJLL]Anderss<strong>on</strong>, M. (1994) Sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>. Princet<strong>on</strong> University Press. [aJLL]Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W. & Mat<strong>the</strong>ws, D. (2003) Adaptati<strong>on</strong>ism – Howto carry out an exaptati<strong>on</strong>ist program. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences25(4):489–553. [TED]Anolli, L. & Ciceri, R. (2002) Analysis of <strong>the</strong> vocal profiles of male seducti<strong>on</strong>:From exhibiti<strong>on</strong> to self-disclosure. Journal of General Psychology129:149–69. [aJLL]Antón, S. C. (1997) Developmental age <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> tax<strong>on</strong>omic affinity of <strong>the</strong> Mojokertochild, Java, Ind<strong>on</strong>esia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology102(4):497–514. [aJLL](2003) Natural <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Homo erectus. American Journal of PhysicalAnthropology, Supplement 37:126–70. [aJLL]Antón, S. C. & Leigh, S. R. (2003) Growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> in Homo erectus. In:Patterns of growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> in <strong>the</strong> Genus Homo, ed. J. L. Thomps<strong>on</strong>,G. E. Krovitz & A. J. Nels<strong>on</strong>, pp. 219–45. Cambridge UniversityPress. [aJLL]Arbib, M. A. (2005a) From m<strong>on</strong>key-like acti<strong>on</strong> recogniti<strong>on</strong> to human language: Anevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences28(2):105–124. [aJLL](2005b) Interweaving protosign <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> protospeech: Fur<strong>the</strong>r <strong>development</strong>s bey<strong>on</strong>d<strong>the</strong> mirror. Interacti<strong>on</strong> Studies 6:145–71. [aJLL]Armstr<strong>on</strong>g, D., Stokoe, W. & Wilcox, S. (1995) Gesture <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> nature of language.Cambridge University Press. [SJ]Asher, J. & Garcia, R. (1969) The optimal age to learn a foreign language. Modern<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Journal 38:334–41. [aJLL]Asher, S. R. & Renshaw, P. D. (1981) Children without friends: Social knowledge<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social-skill training. In: The <strong>development</strong> of children’s friendships, ed. S. R.Asher & J. M. Gottman. Cambridge University Press. [aJLL]Austad, S. N. (1994) Menopause: An evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>. ExperimentalGer<strong>on</strong>tology 29: 255–63. [aJLL]Austin, A. M. B., Salehi, M. & Leffler, A. (1987) Gender <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>aldifferences in children’s c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s. Sex Roles 16:497–510. [aJLL]Austin, J. L. (1975) How to do things with words: The William JamesLectures delivered at Harvard University in 1955. Clarend<strong>on</strong> Press.[CK, arJLL]Austin, M. D. & Leeper, H. A. (1975) Basal pitch <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> frequency level variati<strong>on</strong>in male <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> female children: A preliminary investigati<strong>on</strong>. Journal ofCommunicati<strong>on</strong> Disorders 8:309–16. [aJLL]Ayoub, M. R. & Barnett, S. A. (1965) Ritualized verbal insult in white high schoolculture. Journal of American Folklore 78:337–44. [aJLL]Badyaev, A. V. & Hill, G. E. (2003) Avian sexual dichromatism in relati<strong>on</strong> tophylogeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ecology. Annual Review of Ecology, Evoluti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Systematics34:27–49. [rJLL]Badyaev, A. V., Hill, G. E. & Weckworth, B. V. (2002) Species divergencein sexually selected traits: Increase in s<strong>on</strong>g elaborati<strong>on</strong> is related todecrease in plumage ornamentati<strong>on</strong> in finches. Evoluti<strong>on</strong>56:412–19. [rJLL]Baldwin, J. M. (1896) A <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> factor in evoluti<strong>on</strong>. American Naturalist 30:441–51,536–53. [DFB]Bales, R. F., Strodtbeck, F. L., Mills, T. M. & Roseborough, M. E. (1951)Channels of communicati<strong>on</strong> in small groups. American Sociological Review16:461–68. [aJLL]Bamberg, M. (1987) The acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of narratives. Mout<strong>on</strong> de Gruyter. [aJLL]Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen, S., Lutchmaya, S. & Knickmeyer, R. (2004) Prenatal testoster<strong>on</strong>ein mind: Amniotic fluid studies. MIT Press. [aJLL]Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Cohen, D. J., eds. (1993) Underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ingo<strong>the</strong>r minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]Bates, E. & Goodman, J. C. (1997) On <strong>the</strong> inseparability of grammar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>lexic<strong>on</strong>: Evidence from acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, aphasia <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> real-time processing.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Cognitive Processes 12:507–84. [aJLL]Bates<strong>on</strong>, P. (1988) The active role of behaviour in evoluti<strong>on</strong>. In: Evoluti<strong>on</strong>aryprocesses <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> metaphors, ed. M.-W. Ho & S. W. Fox. Wiley. [aJLL]Bauman, R. (1975) Verbal art as performance. American Anthropologist77:290–311. [arJLL]Beckford, N. S., Rood, S. R. & Schaid, D. (1985) Androgen stimulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>laryngeal <strong>development</strong>. Annals of Otology, Rhinology, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Laryngology94:634–40. [aJLL]Beitchman, J. H., Brownlie, E. B., Inglis, A., Wild, J., Ma<strong>the</strong>ws, R., Schachter, D.,Kroll, R., Martin, S., Fergus<strong>on</strong>, B. & Lancee, W. (1994) Seven-year follow-upof speech/language-impaired <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol children: Speech/language stability<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> outcome. Journal of <strong>the</strong> American Academy of Child <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> AdolescentPsychiatry 33:1322–30. [aJLL]Belle, D. (1989) Gender differences in children’s social networks <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> supports.In: Children’s social networks <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social supports, ed. D. Belle, pp. 173–88.Wiley. [rJLL]Benens<strong>on</strong>, J. F., Apostoleris, N. H. & Parnass, J. (1997) Age <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sex differencesin dyadic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> group interacti<strong>on</strong>. Developmental Psychology33:538–43. [rJLL]Benens<strong>on</strong>, J. F., Nichols<strong>on</strong>, C., Waite, A., Roy, R. & Simps<strong>on</strong>, A. (2001) Theinfluence of group size <strong>on</strong> children’s competitive behavior. Child Development72:921–28. [rJLL]Bergman, M. M. (1987) Social grace or disgrace: Adolescent social skills <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>learning disability subtypes. Reading, Writing, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Learning Disabilities3:161–66. [aJLL]Berko Gleas<strong>on</strong>, J. (1980) The acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of social speech: Routines <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> politenessformulas. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>: Social psychological <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, ed. H. Giles,W. P. Robins<strong>on</strong> & P. M. Smith, pp. 159–67. Academic Press. [KK]Berko Gleas<strong>on</strong>, J. (2001) The <strong>development</strong> of language. Allyn <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Bac<strong>on</strong>.[aJLL]Bermudez de Castro, J. M., Rosas, A., Carb<strong>on</strong>ell, E., Nicolas, M. E., Rodroguez, J.& Arsuaga, J. L. (1999) A modern human pattern of dental <strong>development</strong> inLower Pleistocene hominins from Atapuerca-TD6 (Spain). Proceedings of <strong>the</strong>Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Science USA 96:4210–13. [aJLL]Bickert<strong>on</strong>, D. (1981) Roots of language. Karoma. [DKO](1984) The language bioprogram hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences7(2):173–88. [aJLL, DMWP](1990) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> species. University of Chicago Press. [DFB, aJLL,DMWP](1995) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human behaviour. UCL Press. [aJLL, DMWP](1997) The role of immaturity in human <strong>development</strong>. Psychological Bulletin122:153–69. [DFB](2000) Can biomusicology learn from language evoluti<strong>on</strong> studies? In: The originsof music, ed. N. L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown. MIT Press. [aJLL](2002) Foraging versus social intelligence in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of protolanguage. In:The transiti<strong>on</strong> to language, ed. A. Wray, pp. 207–25. Oxford UniversityPress. [SJ](2003) Symbol <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> structure: A comprehensive framework. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. H. Christiansen & S. Kirby, pp. 77–93. Oxford UniversityPress. [SJ]Biesele, M. (1997) An ideal of unassisted birth: Hunting, healing, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> transformati<strong>on</strong>am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Kalahari Ju/’hoansi. In: Childbirth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> authoritative knowledge:Cross-cultural <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, ed. R. E. Davis-Floyd & C. F. Sargent,pp. 474–92. University of California Press. [rJLL]Binnie, C., Daniloff, R. & Buckingham, H. (1982) Ph<strong>on</strong>etic disintegrati<strong>on</strong> in afive-year-old following sudden hearing loss. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> HearingDisorders 47:181–89. [aJLL]Bischoping, K. (1993) Gender differences in c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> topics, 1922–1990.Sex Roles 28:1–18. [aJLL]312 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Bishop, D. (1989) Autism, Asperger’s syndrome <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantic-pragmatic disorder:Where are <strong>the</strong> boundaries? British Journal of Disorders of Communicati<strong>on</strong>24:107–21. [aJLL]Bishop, D. V. M. (2001) Motor immaturity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> specific speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> languageimpairment: Evidence for a comm<strong>on</strong> genetic basis. American Journal ofMedical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 114:56–63. [aJLL]Bishop, D. V. M., North, T. & D<strong>on</strong>lan, C. (1995) Genetic basis for specific languageimpairment: Evidence from a twin study. Developmental Medicine <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ChildNeurology 37:56–71. [aJLL]Bishop, D. V. P. (2002) Motor immaturity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> specific speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> languageimpairment: Evidence for a comm<strong>on</strong> genetic basis. American Journal ofMedical Genetics 114:56–63. [LW]Bjorklund, D. F. (1997) The role of immaturity in human <strong>development</strong>.Psychological Bulletin 122:153–69. [DFB, aJLL](2006) Mo<strong>the</strong>r knows best: Epigenetic inheritance, maternal effects, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human intelligence. Developmental Review26(2):213–42. [DFB]Bjorklund, D. F. & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002) The origins of human nature:Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>development</strong>al psychology. American PsychologicalAssociati<strong>on</strong>. [aJLL]Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J. & Kaukiainen, A. (1992a) Do girls manipulate<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> boys fight? Developmental trends regarding direct <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> indirect aggressi<strong>on</strong>.Aggressive Behavior 18:117–27. [aJLL]Björkqvist, K., Österman, K. & Kaukiainen, A. (1992b) The <strong>development</strong> of direct<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> indirect aggressive strategies in males <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> females. In: Of mice <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>women: Aspects of female aggressi<strong>on</strong>, ed. K. Björkqvist & P. Niemelä.Academic Press. [aJLL]Björkqvist, K., Österman, K. & Lagerspetz, K. M. J. (1994) Sex differences in covertaggressi<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g adults. Aggressive Behaviour 20:27–33. [aJLL]Blacking, J. (1961) The social value of Venda riddling. African Studies20:1–32. [aJLL]Blank, D., Kumar, D., Meeden, L. & Marshal, J. (2005) Bringing up robot:Fundamental mechanisms for creating a self-motivated, self-organizingarchitecture. Cybernetics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Systems 36:125–50. [LJG]Blood, G. W. & Blood, I. M. (2004) Bullying in adolescents who stutter:Communicative competence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> self-esteem. C<strong>on</strong>temporary Issues inCommunicative Sciences <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Disorders 31:68–78. [aJLL]Bloom, K., D’Odorico, L. & Beaum<strong>on</strong>t, S. (1993) Adult preferences for syllabicvocalizati<strong>on</strong>s: Generalizati<strong>on</strong>s to parity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> native language. Infant Behavior<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Development 16:109–20. [aJLL]Blount, B. G. (1985) “Girney” vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s am<strong>on</strong>g Japanese Macaque females:C<strong>on</strong>text <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> functi<strong>on</strong>. Primates 26:424–35. [rJLL]Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es, N. (1972) Comparative aspects of mo<strong>the</strong>r-child c<strong>on</strong>tact. In:Ethological studies in child behaviour, ed. N. Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es. CambridgeUniversity Press. [aJLL]Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es, N. G. (1993) The lives of hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>r children: Effects ofparental behavior <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> parental reproducti<strong>on</strong> strategy. In: Juvenile primates,ed. M. E. Pereira & L. A. Fairbanks, pp. 309–26. Oxford UniversityPress. [aJLL]Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es, N. G., Smith, L. C., O’C<strong>on</strong>nel, J. F. & H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ler, J. S. (1992)Demography of <strong>the</strong> Hadza, an increasing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> high density populati<strong>on</strong> ofsavanna foragers. American Journal of Physical Anthropology89:159–81. [aJLL]Boberg, J. M. & Boberg, E. (1990) The o<strong>the</strong>r side of <strong>the</strong> block: The stutterer’sspouse. Journal of Fluency Disorders 15:61–75. [aJLL]Bodamer, M. D. & Gardner, R. A. (2002) How cross-fostered chimpanzeesinitiate <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> maintain c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s. Journal of Comparative Psychology116:12–26. [RAG]Bogin, B. (1988) Patterns of human growth. Cambridge UniversityPress. [arJLL, RBE](1990) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human childhood. BioScience 40:16–25. [aJLL](1997) Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary hypo<strong>the</strong>ses for human childhood. Yearbook of PhysicalAnthropology 40:63–89. [arJLL](1999a) Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> human growth. Annual Review ofAnthropology 28:109–53. [arJLL](1999b) Patterns of human growth, 2nd editi<strong>on</strong>. Cambridge UniversityPress. [arJLL, RBE](2001) The growth of humanity. Wiley-Liss. [arJLL](2003) The human pattern of growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> in pale<strong>on</strong>tological<str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>. In: Patterns of growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> in <strong>the</strong> Genus Homo, ed.J. L. Thomps<strong>on</strong>, G. E. Krovitz & A. J. Nels<strong>on</strong>, pp. 15–44. CambridgeUniversity Press. [aJLL]Bogin, B. & Smith, B. H. (1996) Evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> cycle. AmericanJournal of Human Biology 8:703–16. [arJLL]Borchert, C. M. & Zihlman, A. L. (1990) The <strong>on</strong>togeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phylogeny of symbolizing.In: The <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> of symbols, ed. M. L. Foster & L. J. Botscharow, pp. 15–44.Westview. [BJK]Botha, R. P. (2003) Unravelling <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language. Elsevier. [aJLL]Bott, E. (1971) Family <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social network: Roles, norms, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> external relati<strong>on</strong>shipin ordinary urban families. Tavistock Publicati<strong>on</strong>s. [aJLL]Botting, N. & C<strong>on</strong>ti-Ramsden, G. (2000) Social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavioural difficulties inchildren with language impairment. Child <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Teaching <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Therapy16:105–20. [aJLL]Bott<strong>on</strong>i, L., Massa, R. & Lenti Boero, D. (2003) The Grey parrot (Psittacuserithacus) as musician: An experiment with <strong>the</strong> temperate scale. Ethology,Ecology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 15(2):133–41. [DLB]Bott<strong>on</strong>i, L., Masin, S., Lenti Boero, D. & Pioli, P. (2004) Teaching <strong>the</strong> musicalcode to a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Theoretical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> experientialinsights. In: Abstracts of <strong>the</strong> Internati<strong>on</strong>al Symposium PME04, Padua, Italy.Psychology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Music Educati<strong>on</strong>: In Memoriam of Giuseppe Porzi<strong>on</strong>ato,ed. M. Biasutti, p. 79. CLEUP, The University Press of PadoaUniversity. [DLB]Brenneis, D. & Padarath, R. (1975) “About those scoundrels I’ll let every<strong>on</strong>e know”:Challenge singing in a Fiji Indian community. Journal of American Folklore88:283–91. [arJLL]Bright<strong>on</strong>, H., Kirby, S. & Smith, K. (2005a) Cultural selecti<strong>on</strong> for learnability:Three principles underlying <strong>the</strong> view that language adapts to be learnable.In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> origins: Perspectives <strong>on</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. Tallerman,pp. 291–309. Oxford University Press. [HB]Bright<strong>on</strong>, H., Smith, K. & Kirby, S. (2005b) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> as an evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary system.Physics of Life Reviews 2:177–226. [HB]Briscoe, E. J., ed. (2002) Linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong> through language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>: Formal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> computati<strong>on</strong>al models. Cambridge University Press. [HB]Brockway, R. (2003) Evolving to be mentalists: The “mind-reading mums”hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. In: From mating to mentality: Evaluating evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary psychology,ed. K. Sterelny & J. Fitness. Psychology Press. [aJLL]Brody, S. (1945) Bioenergetics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> growth. Reinhold. [aJLL]Brooks, P. J. (2004) Grammatical competence is not a psychologically validc<strong>on</strong>struct. Journal of Child <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> 31:467–70. [SR]Brown, K. & Mack, D. S. (1978) Food sharing am<strong>on</strong>g captive Le<strong>on</strong>topi<strong>the</strong>cusrosalia. Folia Primatologica 29:268–90. [GEW]Bruner, J. (1978) Foreword. In: Acti<strong>on</strong> gesture <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> symbol: The emergence oflanguage, ed. A. Lock, pp. vii–viii. Academic Press. [RAG]Burbank, V. K. (1994) Fighting women: Anger <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> aggressi<strong>on</strong> in AboriginalAustralia. University of California Press. [aJLL]Burles<strong>on</strong>, B. R. (1982) The <strong>development</strong> of comforting communicati<strong>on</strong> skillsin childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence. Child Development 53:1578–88. [aJLL]Burns, T. A. (1976) Riddling: Occasi<strong>on</strong> to act. Journal of American Folklore89:139–65. [rJLL]Burroughs, E. I. & Tomblin, J. B. (1990) Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language correlates ofadults’ judgments of children. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Disorders55:485–94. [aJLL]Cabana, T., Jolicoeur, P. & Michaud, J. (1993) Prenatal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> postnatal growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>allometry of stature, head circumference, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> brain weight in Québecchildren. American Journal of Human Biology 5:93–99. [aJLL]Calvin, W. H. (1996) How brains think: Evolving intelligence. Basic Books. [SR](1998) The cerebral code: Thinking a thought in <strong>the</strong> mosaics of <strong>the</strong> mind.MIT Press. [SR]Campbell, A. (1999) Staying alive: Evoluti<strong>on</strong>, culture <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> women’s intrasexualaggressi<strong>on</strong>. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 22(2):203–14. [rJLL]Camperio Ciani, A. & Chiarelli, B. (1988) A systematic relati<strong>on</strong>ship between brainsize increase <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> larynx transformati<strong>on</strong> during hominizati<strong>on</strong>. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>origin: A multidisciplinary approach, ed. J. Wind, B. Chiarelli, B. Bichakjian,A. Nocentini & A. J<strong>on</strong>ker, pp. 51–66. (NATO ASI Series D: Behavioural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Social Sciences, 31). Kluwer Academic. [DLB]C<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>olin, U. (2003) The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biological Review78:575–95. [rJLL]Cangelosi, A. & Parisi, D., eds. (2001) Simulating <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language.Springer. [HB]Capp, B. (2003) When gossips meet: Women, family, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> neighbourhood in earlymodern Engl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]Caro, T. M. & Hauser, M. D. (1992) Is <strong>the</strong>re teaching in n<strong>on</strong>human animals?Quarterly Review of Biology 67:151–74. [aJLL]Carroll, J. B. (1993) Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies.Cambridge University Press. [aJLL]Carru<strong>the</strong>rs, P. (1996) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, thought <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>sciousness. Cambridge UniversityPress. [SJC]Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (1999) The origin of complex language. Oxford UniversityPress. [aJLL](2004) Many <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, no c<strong>on</strong>sensus. Science 303:1299–300. [aJLL]Chalcraft, V. J. & Gardner, R. A. (2005) Cross-fostered chimpanzees modulate signsof American Sign <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Gesture 5:107–31. [RAG]Chance, M. R. A. (1967) Attenti<strong>on</strong>-structure as <strong>the</strong> basis of primate rank orders.Man 2:503–18. [aJLL]Chance, M. R. A. & Jolly, C. J. (1970) Social groups of m<strong>on</strong>keys, apes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> men.J<strong>on</strong>athan Cape. [aJLL]BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 313


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Charnov, E. L. (1993) Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> invariants: Some explorati<strong>on</strong>s of symmetry inevoluti<strong>on</strong>ary ecology. Oxford University Press. [rJLL]Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990) How m<strong>on</strong>keys see <strong>the</strong> world. University ofChicago Press. [DLB](2005a) C<strong>on</strong>straints <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> preadaptati<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> earliest stages of languageevoluti<strong>on</strong>. Linguistic Review 22:135–59. [rJLL](2005b) Social complexity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> acquired during eavesdropping byprimates <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r animals. In: Animal communicati<strong>on</strong> networks, ed. P. K.McGregor, pp. 583–603. Cambridge University Press. [arJLL]Chisolm, J. S. (2003) Uncertainty, c<strong>on</strong>tingency, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> attachment: A <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong>ory of <strong>the</strong>ory of mind. In: From mating to mentality: Evaluating evoluti<strong>on</strong>arypsychology, ed. K. Sterelny & J. Fitness. Psychology Press. [aJLL]Chomsky, N. (1967) The formal nature of language. In: Biological foundati<strong>on</strong>s oflanguage, ed. E. H. Lenneberg, pp. 397–442. Wiley. [RAG](1980) Rules <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> representati<strong>on</strong>s. Columbia University Press. [DB, rJLL](1988) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> problems of knowledge. The Managua Lectures.MIT Press. [CK](1996) Minimalism. MIT Press. [KK](2002a) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> brain. In: On nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language, ed. A. Belletti &L. Rizzi, pp. 61–91. Cambridge University Press. [rJLL](2002b) On nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language. Cambridge University Press. [CK]Chomsky, N., Belletti, A. & Rizzi, L. (2002) On nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language. CambridgeUniversity Press. [HB]Christiansen, K. & Knussmann, R. (1987) Sex horm<strong>on</strong>es <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive functi<strong>on</strong>ingin men. Neuropsychobiology 18:27–36. [aJLL]Christiansen, M. & Kirby, S., eds. (2003a) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Oxford UniversityPress. [LJG, aJLL](2003b) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>: C<strong>on</strong>sensus <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>troversies. Trends in CognitiveSciences 7:300–307. [HB, aJLL](2003c) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>: The hardest problem in science? In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. Christiansen & S. Kirby. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]Christy, J. H. (1995) Mimicry, mate choice, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> sensory trap hypo<strong>the</strong>sis.American Naturalist 146:171–81. [rJLL]Clancy, B., Darlingt<strong>on</strong>, R. B. & Finlay, B. L. (2001) Translating <strong>development</strong>al timeacross mammalian species. Neuroscience 105:7–17. [aJLL]Clark, A. (1997) Being <strong>the</strong>re: Putting brain, body <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mind toge<strong>the</strong>r again. MITPress. [SJC]Clark, E. V., Hecht, B. F. & Mulford, R. C. (1986) Coining complex compounds inEnglish: Affixes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> word order in acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. Linguistics 24:7–29. [aJLL]Clastres, P. (1987) Society against <strong>the</strong> state: Essays in political anthropology.Z<strong>on</strong>e Books. [aJLL]Clutt<strong>on</strong>-Brock, T. (2002) Breeding toge<strong>the</strong>r: Kin selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mutualism incooperative vertebrates. Science 296:69–72. [rJLL]Coates, J. (1986) Women, men <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language: A sociolinguistic account of sexdifferences in language. L<strong>on</strong>gman. [aJLL]Cohen, M. A. & Gross, P. J. (1979) The <strong>development</strong>al resource: Behavioralsequences for assessment <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> program planning. Grune & Stratt<strong>on</strong>. [RAG]Coleman, S. W., Patricelli, G. L. & Borgia, G. (2004) Variable female preferencesdrive complex male displays. Nature 428:742–45. [rJLL]Comaroff, J. (1975) Talking politics: Oratory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> authority in a Tswana chiefdom.In: Political language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> oratory in traditi<strong>on</strong>al society, ed. M. Bloch.Academic Press. [aJLL]Comenius, J. A. (1659/1968) Orbis sensualium pictus, trans. C. Hoole & J. Kirt<strong>on</strong>/Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1658, German-Latin editi<strong>on</strong>;first English editi<strong>on</strong>, 1659, J. Kirt<strong>on</strong>; 1968 fascimile reprint of 1659 Englishediti<strong>on</strong>, Oxford University Press). [WMS]C<strong>on</strong>roy, G. C. & Vannier, M. W. (1991) Dental <strong>development</strong> in South Africanaustralopi<strong>the</strong>cines. Part I: Problems of pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> chr<strong>on</strong>ology. AmericanJournal of Physical Anthropology 86:121–36. [aJLL]C<strong>on</strong>ti-Ramsden, G. & Botting, N. (2004) Social difficulties <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> victimizati<strong>on</strong> inchildren with SLI at 11 years of age. Journal of Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> HearingResearch 47:145–61. [aJLL]Cook, A. S., Fritz, J. J., McCornack, B. L. & Visperas, C. (1985) Earlygender differences in <strong>the</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al usage of language. Sex Roles12:909–15. [aJLL]Cook, C. & Hamann, J. (1961) Relati<strong>on</strong> of lung volumes to height in healthypers<strong>on</strong>s between ages of 5 <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 38 years. Journal of Pediatrics59:710–14. [aJLL]Coqueugniot, H., Hublin, J. J., Veill<strong>on</strong>, F., Houet, F. & Jacob, T. (2004) Early braingrowth in Homo erectus <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicati<strong>on</strong>s for cognitive ability. Nature431(7006):299–302. [aJLL]Corballis, M. C. (2002) From h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> to mouth: The origins of language. Princet<strong>on</strong>University Press. [SJ, aJLL](2004) The origins of modernity: Was aut<strong>on</strong>omous speech <strong>the</strong> critical factor?Psychological Review 111:543–52. [aJLL]Cowie, R. & Douglas-Cowie, E. (1983) Speech producti<strong>on</strong> in profound postlingualdeafness. In: Hearing science <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> hearing disorders, ed. M. E. Lutman & M. P.Haggard. Academic Press. [aJLL]Cowley S. J., Moodley, S. & Fiori-Cowley, A. (2004) Grounding signs of culture:Primary intersubjectivity in social semiosis. Mind, Culture <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Activity11(2):109–32. [SJC]Crain, S. (1991) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> absence of experience. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Brain Sciences 14(4):597–611. [DB]Crain, S. & Thornt<strong>on</strong>, R. (1998) Investigati<strong>on</strong>s in universal grammar:A guide to experiments <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of syntax <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantics. MITPress. [DB]Crick, N. R. & Bigbee, M. A. (1998) Relati<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> overt forms of peervictimizati<strong>on</strong>: A multiinformant approach. Journal of C<strong>on</strong>sulting <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Clinical Psychology 66:337–47. [aJLL]Crowe Hall, B. J. (1991) Attitudes of fourth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sixth graders toward peers withmild articulati<strong>on</strong> disorders. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Services in Schools22:334–40. [aJLL]Culatta, B., Page, J. L. & Ellis, J. (1983) Story retelling as a communicativeperformance screening tool. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Services inSchools 14:66–74. [aJLL]Dabbs, J. M. (2000) Heroes, rogues, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> lovers: Testoster<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior.McGraw-Hill. [arJLL]Dabbs, J. M., Alford, E. C. & Fielden, J. A. (1998) Trial lawyers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testoster<strong>on</strong>e:Blue-collar talent in a white-collar world. Journal of Applied Social Psychology28:84–94. [aJLL]Dabbs, J. M, Bernieri, F. J, Str<strong>on</strong>g, R. K, Campo, R. & Milun, R. (2001) Going<strong>on</strong> stage: Testoster<strong>on</strong>e in greetings <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> meetings. Journal of Research inPers<strong>on</strong>ality 35:27–40. [aJLL]Dabbs, J. M., La Rue, D. & Williams, P. M. (1990) Testoster<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> occupati<strong>on</strong>alchoice: Actors, ministers, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r men. Journal of Pers<strong>on</strong>ality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> SocialPsychology 59:1261–65. [aJLL]Dabbs, J. M. & Mallinger, A. (1999) Higher testoster<strong>on</strong>e levels predictlower voice pitch am<strong>on</strong>g men. Pers<strong>on</strong>ality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Individual Differences27:801–804. [aJLL]Dabbs, J. M. & Ruback, R. B. (1984) Vocal patterns in male <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> female groups.Pers<strong>on</strong>ality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Social Psychology Bulletin 10:518–25. [aJLL](1988) Saliva testoster<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pers<strong>on</strong>ality of male college students. Bulletinof <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society 26:244–47. [aJLL]Darwin, C. (1871) The descent of man. Murray. [DKO](1879/2004) The descent of man; <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> selecti<strong>on</strong> in relati<strong>on</strong> to sex.Penguin. [aJLL]Davis, S., Howell, P. & Cooke, F. (2002) Sociodynamic relati<strong>on</strong>ships betweenchildren who stutter <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir n<strong>on</strong>-stuttering classmates. Journal of ChildPsychology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychiatry 43:939–47. [aJLL]Dawkins, R. (1986) The blind watchmaker: Why <strong>the</strong> evidence of evoluti<strong>on</strong> reveals auniverse without design. Nort<strong>on</strong>. [aJLL](2004) Extended phenotype – but not too extended. A reply to Lal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>,Turner <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Jabl<strong>on</strong>ka. Biology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Philosophy 19:377–96. [aJLL]de Beer, G. (1930) Embryology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Clarend<strong>on</strong> Press. [SR]de Beer, G. R. (1951/1958) Embryos <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ancestors. [1958, 3rd editi<strong>on</strong>.]Clarend<strong>on</strong> Press. [DFB, aJLL, SR]de Boyss<strong>on</strong>-Bardies, B. (2001) How language comes to children. From birth to twoyears. MIT Press. [DLB]De Martino, E. (1961) La terra del rimorso. Il Saggiatore. [IB]Deac<strong>on</strong>, T. W. (1997) The symbolic species: The co-evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>human brain. Nort<strong>on</strong>/Allen Lane/University of Chicago Press. [SJC, aJLL,SR, KW]Dean C., Leakey M. G., Reid, D., Schrenk, F., Schwartz, G. T., Stringer, C. &Walker, A. (2001) Growth processes in teeth distinguish modernhumans from Homo erectus <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> earlier hominins. Nature414(6864):628–31. [aJLL]Deaner, R. O., Bart<strong>on</strong>, R. A. & van Schaik, P. (2003) Primate brains <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>histories: Re<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>. In: Primate <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> histories <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> socioecology,ed. P. M. Kappeler & M. E. Pereira, pp. 233–65. University of ChicagoPress. [GEW]Degh, L. (1995) Narratives in society: A performer-centered study of narrati<strong>on</strong>.Academia Scientarum Fennica. [IB]Demirjian, A. (1986) Dentiti<strong>on</strong>. In: Human growth, vol. 2: Postnatal growth, ed.F. Falkner & J. M. Tanner, pp. 269–98. Plenum. [aJLL]Dennett, D. (1991) C<strong>on</strong>sciousness explained. Little, Brown. [SJC]Derlega, V. J., Metts, S., Petr<strong>on</strong>io, S. & Margulis, S. T. (1993) Self-disclosure.Sage. [aJLL]Dessalles, J.-L. (1998) Altruism, status, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> origin of relevance. In: Theevoluti<strong>on</strong> of language, ed. J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy &C. Knight, pp. 130–47. Cambridge University Press. [aJLL](2000) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> hominid politics. In: The evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary emergence oflanguage, ed. C. Knight, M. Studdert-Kennedy & J. Hurford, pp. 62–80.Cambridge University Press. [SJ]DeThorne, L. S. & Watkins, R. V. (2001) Listeners’ percepti<strong>on</strong>s of language usein children. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Services in Schools32:142–48. [aJLL]314 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Dettwyler, K. A. (1995) A time to wean: The hominid blueprint for <strong>the</strong> natural ageof weaning in modern human populati<strong>on</strong>s. In: Breastfeeding: Biocultural<str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, ed. P. Stuart-Macadam, & K. A. Detwyller, pp. 39–74. Aldine deGruyter. [aJLL]Dickins, T. E. (2005) Can <strong>the</strong>re ever be a n<strong>on</strong>-specific adaptati<strong>on</strong>? A resp<strong>on</strong>se toHampt<strong>on</strong>. Journal for <strong>the</strong> Theory of Social Behaviour 35(3):329–40. [TED]Dittus, W. P. J. (1977) The social regulati<strong>on</strong> of populati<strong>on</strong> density <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> age-sexdistributi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> toque m<strong>on</strong>key. Behaviour 63:281–322. [aJLL]Dominey, P. F. & Boucher, J-F. (2005) Developmental stages of percepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>language acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in a perceptually grounded robot. Cognitive SystemsResearch 6:243–59. [LJG]D<strong>on</strong>ald, M. (1991) Origins of <strong>the</strong> modern mind. Three stages in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> ofculture <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cogniti<strong>on</strong>. Harvard University Press. [SJ](1999) Prec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of protolanguages. In: The descent ofmind: Psychological <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <strong>on</strong> hominid evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. C. Corballis &S. E. G. Lea. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]D<strong>on</strong>nai, D. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2000) Williams syndrome: From genotypethrough to <strong>the</strong> cognitive phenotype. American Journal of Medical Genetics:Seminars in Medical Genetics 97(2):164–71. [JVH]Draper, P. (1976) Social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ec<strong>on</strong>omic c<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> child <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> !Kung.In: Kalahari hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rers: Studies of <strong>the</strong> !Kung San <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir neighbors,ed. R. B. Lee & I. DeVore. Harvard University Press. [aJLL]Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993) Coevoluti<strong>on</strong> of neocortical size, group size <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language inhumans. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 16(4):681–94. [SJ, rJLL](1996) Grooming, gossip <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language. Faber <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Faber. [CK,aJLL, WMS, KW]Dundes, A., Leach, J. W. & Özkök, B. (1970) The strategy of Turkish boys’ verbaldueling rhymes. In: Directi<strong>on</strong>s in sociolinguistics: The ethnography ofcommunicati<strong>on</strong>, ed. J. J. Gumperz & D. Hymes. Holt, Rinehart <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Winst<strong>on</strong>. [aJLL]Dunn, J. (1996) Sibling relati<strong>on</strong>ships <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> perceived self-competence: Patterns ofstability between childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> early adolescence. In: The five to seven yearshift: The age of reas<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>sibility, ed. A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith.University of Chicago Press. [aJLL]Durst<strong>on</strong>, S., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., Buitelaar, J. K. &van Engel<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, H. (2001) Anatomical MRI of <strong>the</strong> developing human brain:What have we learned? Journal of <strong>the</strong> American Academy of Child <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Adolescent Psychiatry 40:1012–20. [aJLL]Eckert, P. (1988) Adolescent social structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> spread of linguistic change.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Society 17:183–207. [aJLL](1999) Linguistic variati<strong>on</strong> as social practice. Blackwell. [aJLL](2003) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> gender in adolescence. In: The h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>gender, ed. J. Holmes & M. Meyerhoff. Blackwell. [aJLL]Eckhardt, R. B. (2000) Human paleobiology. Cambridge University Press. [RBE]Edwards, V. & Sienkewicz, T. J. (1990) Oral cultures past <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> present: Rappin’ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Homer. Blackwell. [rJLL]Eisley, L. (1946/1957) The immense journey. Vintage Books. [SR]Eliade, M. (1951) Le chamanisme et les techniques archaiques de l’extase.Payot. [IB]Ellis, A. W. & Young, A. W. (1988) Human cognitive neuropsychology.Erlbaum. [DLB]Elman, J. L. (1993) Learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> in neural networks: The importanceof starting small. Cogniti<strong>on</strong> 48:71–99. [HB]Emler, N. (2001) Gossiping. In: The <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> socialpsychology, ed. W. P. Robins<strong>on</strong> & H. Giles. Wiley. [aJLL]Endler, J. A. & Basolo, A. L. (1998) Sensory ecology, receiver biases <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexualselecti<strong>on</strong>. Trends in Ecology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 13:415–20. [aJLL]Engström, I., Karlberg, P. & Kraepellen, S. (1956) Respiratory studies in children.Acta Paediatrica 46:277–94. [aJLL]Enquist, M. & Arak, A. (1993) Selecti<strong>on</strong> of exaggerated male traits by femaleaes<strong>the</strong>tic senses. Nature 361:446–48. [rJLL]Erdal, D. & Whiten, A. (1994) On human egalitarianism: An evoluti<strong>on</strong>aryproduct of Machiavellian status escalati<strong>on</strong>. Current Anthropology35:175–83. [CK]Eriks<strong>on</strong>, E. H. (1950) Childhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> society, 2nd editi<strong>on</strong>, revised 1963.Nort<strong>on</strong>. [WMS]Estioko-Griffin, A. (1986) Daughters of <strong>the</strong> forest. Natural History95:36–43. [aJLL]Exline, R. V., Ellys<strong>on</strong>, S. L. & L<strong>on</strong>g, B. (1975) Visual behaviour as an aspect ofpower role relati<strong>on</strong>ships. In: N<strong>on</strong>verbal communicati<strong>on</strong> of aggressi<strong>on</strong>, vol. 2,ed. P. Pliner, L. Krames & T. Alloway, pp. 21–52. Plenum. [aJLL]Falk, D. (2004) Prelinguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong> in early hominins: Whence mo<strong>the</strong>rese?Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 27(4):491–503. [SJ, BJK]Faris, J. C. (1966) The dynamics of verbal exchange: A Newfoundl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> example.Anthropologica 8:235–48. [aJLL]Felsenfeld, S., Broen, P. A. & McGue, M. (1992) A 28-year follow-up of adults witha <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of moderate ph<strong>on</strong>ological disorder: Linguistic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pers<strong>on</strong>ality results.Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research 35:1114–25. [aJLL]Fens<strong>on</strong>, L., Dale, P. A., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. & Pethick, S. (1994)Variability in early communicative <strong>development</strong>. M<strong>on</strong>ographs of <strong>the</strong> Society forResearch in Child Development, Serial No. 242, Vol. 59(5). [aJLL]Fergus<strong>on</strong>, C. A. (1964) Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist 66:103–14. [aJLL]Fernald, A. (1989) Int<strong>on</strong>ati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> communicative intent in mo<strong>the</strong>rs’ speech toinfants: Is <strong>the</strong> melody <strong>the</strong> message? Child Development 60:1497–510. [KW]Ferris, W. R. (1972) Black prose narrative in <strong>the</strong> Mississippi Delta: An overview.Journal of American Folklore 85:140–51. [aJLL]Finlay, B. L. & Darlingt<strong>on</strong>, R. B. (1995) Linked regularities in <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of mammalian brains. Science 268:1578–84. [SR]Finlay, B. L., Darlingt<strong>on</strong>, R. B. & Nicastro, N. (2001) Developmental structure inbrain evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 24(2):263–78. [SR]Finnegan, R. (1967) Limba stories <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> story-telling. Oxford UniversityPress. [aJLL](1969) Attitudes to speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Limba of Sierra Le<strong>on</strong>e.Odu 2:61–77. [rJLL]Firth, R. (1975) Speech-making <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> authority in Tikopia. In: Political language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>oratory in traditi<strong>on</strong>al society, ed. M. Bloch. Academic Press. [aJLL]Fischer, J., Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L. (2004) Babo<strong>on</strong> loudcalls advertise male quality: Acoustic features <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir relati<strong>on</strong> to rank, age,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> exhausti<strong>on</strong>. Behavioral Ecology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sociobiology 56:140–48. [aJLL]Fisek, M. H. & Ofshe, R. (1970) The process of status evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Sociometry33:327–46. [aJLL]Fisher, M. L. (2004) Female intrasexual competiti<strong>on</strong> decreases female facialattractiveness. Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> Royal Society of L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> B 271:283–85. [rJLL](2004) Kin selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “mo<strong>the</strong>r t<strong>on</strong>gues”: A neglected comp<strong>on</strong>ent in languageevoluti<strong>on</strong>. In: The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of communicati<strong>on</strong> systems: A comparativeapproach, ed. D. K. Oller & U. Griebel, pp. 275–96. MIT Press. [rJLL]Fisher, R. A. (1930/1958) The genetical <strong>the</strong>ory of natural selecti<strong>on</strong>. Dover. [aJLL]Fitch, W. T. (1997) Vocal tract length <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> formant frequency dispersi<strong>on</strong> correlatewith body size in rhesus macaques. Journal of <strong>the</strong> Acoustical Society of America102:1213–22. [aJLL](2004) Kin selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “mo<strong>the</strong>r t<strong>on</strong>gues”: A neglected comp<strong>on</strong>ent in languageevoluti<strong>on</strong>. In: The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of communicati<strong>on</strong> systems: A comparativeapproach, ed. D. K. Oller & U. Griebel. MIT Press. [aJLL](2005) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language: A comparative review. Biology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Philosophy20:193–230. [BJK, rJLL]Fitch, W. T. & Giedd, J. (1999) Morphology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> of <strong>the</strong> human vocaltract: A study using magnetic res<strong>on</strong>ance imaging. Journal of <strong>the</strong> AcousticalSociety of America 106:1511–22. [aJLL]Fitch, W. T. & Hauser, M. D. (2002) Unpacking “h<strong>on</strong>esty”: Vertebrate vocal producti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of acoustic signals. In: Acoustic communicati<strong>on</strong>, ed.A. Simm<strong>on</strong>s, R. R. Fay & A. N. Popper, pp. 63–137. Springer. [rJLL]Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D. & Chomsky, N. (2005) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> languagefaculty: Clarificati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicati<strong>on</strong>s. Cogniti<strong>on</strong> 97:179–210. [aJLL]Flege, J. E. & Fletcher, K. L. (1992) Talker <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> listener effects <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>of degree of foreign accent. Journal of <strong>the</strong> Acoustical Society of America91:370–89. [aJLL]Fletcher, P. (1990) Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language defects. Nature 346:226. [aJLL]Fletcher, S. G. (1972) Time-by-count measurement of diadochokinetic syllable rate.Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research 15:733–70. [aJLL]Fogel, A., de Koeyer, I., Bellagamba, F. & Bell, H. (2002) The dialogical self in <strong>the</strong>first two years of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>: Embarking <strong>on</strong> a journey of discovery. Theory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Psychology 12:191–205. [SJC]Fogel, A., King, B. J. & Shanker. S. G. (2006) Human <strong>development</strong> in <strong>the</strong> 21stcentury: Visi<strong>on</strong>ary policy ideas from systems scientists. The Council of HumanDevelopment. [BJK]Folb, E. (1980) Runnin’ down some lines: The language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> culture of blackteenagers. Harvard University Press. [SJ, aJLL]Frances, S. J. (1979) Sex differences in n<strong>on</strong>verbal behavior. Sex Roles5:519–35. [aJLL]Franks, P. & Bertakis, K. D. (2003) Physician gender, patient gender, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> primarycare. Journal of Women’s Health 12:73–80. [aJLL]Freeby, N. & Madis<strong>on</strong>, C. L. (1989) Children’s percepti<strong>on</strong>s of peers with articulati<strong>on</strong>disorders. Child Study Journal 19:133–44. [aJLL]Fromkin, V., ed. (2000) Linguistics: An introducti<strong>on</strong> to linguistic <strong>the</strong>ory.Blackwell. [KK]Fujiki, M. & Brint<strong>on</strong>, B. (1991) The verbal n<strong>on</strong>communicator: A case study.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Services in Schools 22:322–33. [aJLL]Gage, T. B. (1998) The comparative demography of primates: With some comments<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> histories. Annual Review of Anthropology27:197–221. [rJLL]Galef, B. G. (1990) Traditi<strong>on</strong> in animals: Field observati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> laboratory analyses.In: Interpretati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> explanati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> study of animal behavior. Vol. 1:Interpretati<strong>on</strong>, intenti<strong>on</strong>ality, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> communicati<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. Bekoff &D. Jamies<strong>on</strong>. Westview. [aJLL]BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 315


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Galen, B. R. & Underwood, M. K. (1997) A <strong>development</strong>al investigati<strong>on</strong> ofsocial aggressi<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g children. Developmental Psychology 33:589–600. [aJLL]Galik, K., Senut, B., Pickford, M., Gommery, D., Treil, J., Kuperavage, A. J. &Eckhardt, R. B. (2004) External <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> internal morphology of <strong>the</strong> BAR 1002’00Orrorin tugenensis femur. Science 305:1450–53. [RBE]Gangestad, S. & Snyder, M. (1986) On <strong>the</strong> nature of self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring: Anexaminati<strong>on</strong> of causal structure. Review of Pers<strong>on</strong>ality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Social Psychology6:65–85. [rJLL]Gärdenfors, P. (2003) How Homo became Sapiens: On <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of thinking.Oxford University Press. [SJ](2004) Cooperati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of symbolic communicati<strong>on</strong>. In: Theevoluti<strong>on</strong> of communicati<strong>on</strong> systems, ed. K. Oller & U. Griebel, pp. 237–56.MIT Press. [SJ]Gardner, B. T. & Gardner, R. A. (1980) Two comparative psychologists look atlanguage acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. In: Child language, vol. 2, ed. K. Nels<strong>on</strong>, pp. 331–69.Gardner Press. [RAG](1989) Prelinguistic <strong>development</strong> of children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> chimpanzees. HumanEvoluti<strong>on</strong> 4:433–60. [RAG](1998) Development of phrases in <strong>the</strong> early utterances of children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>cross-fostered chimpanzees. Human Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 13:161–88. [RAG]Gardner, R. A. & Gardner, B. T. (1989) A cross-fostering laboratory. In: Teachingsign language to chimpanzees, ed.R. A. Gardner, B. T. Gardner & T. E. VanCantfort, pp. 1–28. State University of New York Press. [RAG]Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T. & Drumm, P. (1989) Voiced <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> signed resp<strong>on</strong>ses ofcross-fostered chimpanzees. In: Teaching sign language to chimpanzees, ed.R. A. Gardner, B. T. Gardner & T. E. Van Cantfort. State University ofNew York Press. [aJLL]Garrett, M. M. (1993) Wit, power, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> oppositi<strong>on</strong>al groups: A case study of “puretalk.” Quarterly Journal of Speech 79:303–18. [aJLL]Garstang, W. (1922) The <strong>the</strong>ory of recapitulati<strong>on</strong>: A critical re-statement of <strong>the</strong>biogenetic law. Journal of <strong>the</strong> Linnaean Society of L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> (Zoology)35:81–101. [DFB, aJLL]Gavan, J. A. (1953) Growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> of <strong>the</strong> chimpanzee, a l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> comparative study. Human Biology 25:93–143. [rJLL]Gibs<strong>on</strong>, K. R. & Ingold, T., eds. (1993) Tools, language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cogniti<strong>on</strong> in humanevoluti<strong>on</strong>. Cambridge University Press. [aJLL]Girolametto, L., Wiigs, M., Smyth, R., Weitzman, E. & Pearce, P. S. (2001)Children with a <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of expressive vocabulary delay: Outcomes at 5 years ofage. American Journal of Speech-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Pathology 10:358–69. [aJLL]Gleas<strong>on</strong>, J. B. & Weintraub, S. (1976) The acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of routines in child language.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> in Society 5:129–36. [aJLL]Goffman, I. (1969) The presentati<strong>on</strong> of self in everyday <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Penguin. [KK]Gogate, L. J., Bolzani, L. E. & Betancourt, E. (2006) Attenti<strong>on</strong> to maternalmultimodal naming by 6- to 8-m<strong>on</strong>th-old infants <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning of word-objectrelati<strong>on</strong>s. Infancy 9(3):259–88.[LJG]Gogate, L. J., Walker-Andrews, A. S. & Bahrick, L. E. (2001) The intersensoryorigins of word comprehensi<strong>on</strong>: An ecological-dynamic systems view.Developmental Science 4(1):1–18. [LJG]Gold, E. M. (1967) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> identificati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> limit. Informati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> C<strong>on</strong>trol10:447–74. [DMWP]Goldin-Meadow, S. & Feldman, H. (1977) The <strong>development</strong> of language-likecommunicati<strong>on</strong> without a language model. Science 197:401–403. [aJLL]Goldin-Meadow, S. & Myl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>er, C. (1998) Sp<strong>on</strong>taneous sign systems created bydeaf children in two cultures. Nature 391:279–81. [aJLL]Goldizen, A. W. (1987) Tamarins <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> marmosets: Communal care of offspring.In: Primate societies, ed. B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth,R. W. Wrangham & T. T. Struhsaker, pp. 34–43. University of ChicagoPress. [GEW]Goldman, L. R. (1998) Child’s play: Myth, mimesis <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> make-believe. Berg. [SR]Goldstein, L., Byrd, D. & Saltzman, E. (in press) The role of vocal tract gesturalacti<strong>on</strong> units in underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of ph<strong>on</strong>ology. In: Acti<strong>on</strong> tolanguage via <strong>the</strong> mirror neur<strong>on</strong> system, ed. M. A. Arbib. CambridgeUniversity Press. [aJLL]G<strong>on</strong>zález, J. (2004) Formant frequencies <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> body size of speaker: A weakrelati<strong>on</strong>ship in adult humans. Journal of Ph<strong>on</strong>etics 32:277–87. [aJLL]Goodall, J. (1968) The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in <strong>the</strong> Gombe StreamReserve. Animal Behaviour M<strong>on</strong>ographs 1:165–311. [GEW](1983) Populati<strong>on</strong> dynamics during a 15 year period in <strong>on</strong>e community offree-living chimpanzees in <strong>the</strong> Gombe Nati<strong>on</strong>al Park, Tanzania. Zeitschriftfür Tierpsychologie 61:1–60. [aJLL](1988) In <strong>the</strong> shadow of man. Hought<strong>on</strong> Mifflin. [aJLL]Goodenough, J., McGuire, B. & Wallace, R. A. (1993) Perspectives in animalbehavior. Wiley. [RAG]Goody, J. & Watt, I. (1968) The c<strong>on</strong>sequences of literacy. In: Literacy in traditi<strong>on</strong>alsocieties, ed. J. Goody, pp. 27–68. Cambridge University Press. [rJLL]Gopnik, M. (1990) Feature-blind grammar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dysphasia. Nature344:715. [aJLL, LW](1997) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> deficits <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> genetic factors. Trends in Cognitive Science1:5–9. [LW]Gopnik, M. & Crago, M. B. (1991) Familial aggregati<strong>on</strong> of a <strong>development</strong>allanguage disorder. Cogniti<strong>on</strong> 39:1–50. [aJLL]Gossen, G. H. (1974) Chamulas in <strong>the</strong> world of <strong>the</strong> sun: Time <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> space in aMaya oral traditi<strong>on</strong>. Harvard University Press. [rJLL](1976a) Chamula genres of verbal behavior. In: Toward <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>sin folklore, ed. A. Paredes & R. Bauman. University of Texas Press. [aJLL](1976b) Verbal dueling in Chamula. In: Speech play: Research <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resources for<strong>the</strong> study of linguistic creativity, ed. B. Kirshenblatt-Gimblett, pp. 121–48.University of Pennsylvania Press. [arJLL]Gottlieb, G. (1992) Individual <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>: The genesis of novelbehavior. Erlbaum. [aJLL](2002) Developmental-behavioral initiati<strong>on</strong> of evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary change. PsychologicalReview 109:211–18. [DFB]Gottman, J., G<strong>on</strong>so, J. & Rasmussen, B. (1975) Social interacti<strong>on</strong>, socialcompetence, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> friendship in children. Child Development46:709–18. [aJLL]Gottman, J. M. & Mettetal, G. (1986) Speculati<strong>on</strong>s about social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> affective<strong>development</strong>: Friendship <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> acquaintance through adolescence. In: C<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>sof friends: Speculati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> affective <strong>development</strong>, ed. J. M. Gottman& J. G. Parker. Cambridge University Press. [aJLL]Gould, S. J. (1977) Ontogeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phylogeny. Harvard University Press. [aJLL](1977) Ontogeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Phylogeny. Harvard University Press/BelknapPress. [rJLL, SR]Gould, S. J. & Lew<strong>on</strong>tin, R. C. (1979) The sp<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>rels of San Marco <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>Panglossian paradigm: A critique of <strong>the</strong> adapti<strong>on</strong>ist programme. Proceedingsof <strong>the</strong> Royal Society of L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> B 205:581–98. [SR]Gouzoules, H. & Gouzoules, S. (2002) Primate communicati<strong>on</strong>: By natureh<strong>on</strong>est, or by experience wise? Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of Primatology23:821–48. [PK]Grammer, K. (1989) Human courtship behaviour: Biological basis <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitiveprocessing. In: The sociobiology of sexual <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reproductive strategies, ed. A. E.Rasa, C. Vogel & E. Vol<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Chapman <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hall. [aJLL](1990) Strangers meet: Laughter <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>verbal signs of interest in opposite-sexencounters. Journal of N<strong>on</strong>verbal Behavior 14:209–36. [aJLL]Grammer, K., Kruck, K., Juette, A. & Fink, B. (2000) N<strong>on</strong>verbal behaviour ascourtship signals: The role of c<strong>on</strong>trol <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> choice in selecting partners.Evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Human Behavior 21: 371–90. [aJLL]Grammer, K., Kruck, K. B. & Magnuss<strong>on</strong>, M. S. (1998) The courtship dance:Patterns of n<strong>on</strong>verbal synchr<strong>on</strong>izati<strong>on</strong> in opposite-sex encounters. Journal ofN<strong>on</strong>verbal Behavior 22:3–29. [aJLL]Grant, V. J. (2003) The maternal dominance hypo<strong>the</strong>sis: Questi<strong>on</strong>ing Trivers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Willard. Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary Psychology 1:96–107. [SR]Greenspan, S. I. & Shanker, S. G. (2004) The first idea: How symbols, language, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>intelligence evolved from our primate ancestors. Da Capo. [BJK]Grice, P. (1989) Studies in <strong>the</strong> way of words. Harvard University Press. [CK]Griebel, U. & Oller, D. K. (in press) Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary forces favoring c<strong>on</strong>textual flexibility:Sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>, social cohesi<strong>on</strong>, decepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> protean behavior. In:Evoluti<strong>on</strong> of communicative flexibility: Complexity, creativity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adaptabilityin human <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> animal communicati<strong>on</strong>, ed. D. K. Oller & U. Griebel.MIT Press. [DKO]Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K. & Hurtado, M. (2000) “It’s a w<strong>on</strong>derful <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>”:Signaling generosity am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Ache of Paraguay. Evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> HumanBehavior 21:263–82. [rJLL]Gurven, M. & Walker, R. (2006) Energetic dem<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of multiple dependents <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of slow human growth. Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> Royal Society of L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> B273: 835–41. [rJLL]Gustafss<strong>on</strong>, J.-E. & Holmberg, L. M. (1992) Psychometric properties of vocabularytest items as a functi<strong>on</strong> of word characteristics. Sc<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>inavian Journal ofEducati<strong>on</strong>al Research 36:191–210. [aJLL]Gustafss<strong>on</strong>, J.-E. & Undheim, J. O. (1996) Individual differences in cognitivefuncti<strong>on</strong>s. In: H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book of educati<strong>on</strong>al psychology, ed. D. C. Berliner & R. C.Calfee. Macmillan. [aJLL]Haberl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, H. & Mey, J. L. (1977) Editorial: Linguistics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pragmatics. Journal ofPragmatics 1:1–12. [aJLL]Hadley, P. A. & Rice, M. L. (1991) C<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>al resp<strong>on</strong>siveness of speech- <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>language-impaired preschoolers. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research34:1308–17. [aJLL]Haeckel, E. (1899) Riddle of <strong>the</strong> universe at <strong>the</strong> close of <strong>the</strong> nineteenthcentury. Available at: http://www.ucmp.berkeley.edu/<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>/haeckel.html. [LJG]Halpern, C. T., Udry, J. R., Campbell, B. & Suchindran, C. (1993) Testoster<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>pubertal <strong>development</strong> as predictors of sexual activity: A panel analysis ofadolescent males. Psychosomatic Medicine 55:436–47. [aJLL]Halpern, C. T., Udry, J. R. & Suchindran, C. (1998) M<strong>on</strong>thly measures of salivarytestoster<strong>on</strong>e predict sexual activity in adolescent males. Archives of SexualBehavior 27:445–65. [aJLL]316 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Hamada, Y. & Ud<strong>on</strong>o, T. (2002) L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal analysis of length growth in <strong>the</strong>chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Physical Anthropology118:268–84. [rJLL]Hamilt<strong>on</strong>, M. E. (1985) Sociobiology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of relati<strong>on</strong>sbetween males <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> females. In: Male-female differences: A bio-cultural <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>,ed. R. L. Hall. Praeger. [aJLL]Hamilt<strong>on</strong>, W. (1964) The genetical evoluti<strong>on</strong> of social behavior, I & II. Journal ofTheoretical Biology 7:1–52. [aJLL](1966) The moulding of senescence by natural selecti<strong>on</strong>. Journal of TheoreticalBiology 12:12–45. [aJLL]Hannerz, U. (1969) Soulside. Columbia University Press. [aJLL]Harries, M. L. L., Walker, J. M., Williams, D. M., Hawkins, S. & Hughes, I. A.(1997) Changes in <strong>the</strong> male voice at puberty. Archives of Disease in Childhood77:445–47. [aJLL]Harris, M. P. (1970) Abnormal migrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> hybridizati<strong>on</strong> of Larus argentatus <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>L. fuscus after interspecies fostering experiments. Ibis 112:488–98. [RAG]Harvey, P. H. & Arnold, S. J. (1982) Female mate choice <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> runaway sexualselecti<strong>on</strong>. Nature 297:533–34. [arJLL]Harvey, P. H. & Clutt<strong>on</strong>-Brock, T. H. (1985) Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> variati<strong>on</strong> in primates.Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 39:559–81. [aJLL]Harvey, P. H., Martin, R. D. & Clutt<strong>on</strong>-Brock, T. H. (1987) Life histories incomparative <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>. In: Primate societies, ed. B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney,R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham & T. T. Struhsaker, pp. 181–96.University of Chicago Press. [aJLL, SR]Hausberger, M. (1997) S<strong>on</strong>g acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sharing in <strong>the</strong> starling. In: Socialinfluences <strong>on</strong> vocal <strong>development</strong>, ed. C. T. Snowd<strong>on</strong> & M. Hausberger,pp. 57–84. Cambridge University Press. [DKO]Hauser, M. D. (1988) Inventi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social transmissi<strong>on</strong>: New data from wild vervetm<strong>on</strong>keys. In: Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> ofintellect in m<strong>on</strong>keys, apes, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> humans, ed. R. W. Byrne & A. Whiten. OxfordUniversity Press. [aJLL](1996) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of communicati<strong>on</strong>. MIT Press. [DKO, arJLL]Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. & Fitch, W. T. (2002) The faculty of language:What is it, who has it, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–79. [DB, SJC,CK arJLL, DMWP]Hawkes, K. (1991) Showing off: Tests of an hypo<strong>the</strong>sis about men’s foraging goals.Ethology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sociobiology 12:29–54. [CK]Hawkes, K., O’C<strong>on</strong>nell, J. F. & Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es, N. G. (1997) Hadza women’stime allocati<strong>on</strong>, offspring provisi<strong>on</strong>ing, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of post-menopausal<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>spans. Current Anthropology 38:551–78. [aJLL]Hawkes, K., O’C<strong>on</strong>nell, J. F., Blurt<strong>on</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es, N. G., Alvarez, H. & Chernov, E. L.(1998) Gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mo<strong>the</strong>ring, menopause, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> histories.Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Sciences USA 95:1336–39. [arJLL,WMS]Hayaki, H., Huffman, M. A. & Nishida, T. (1989) Dominance am<strong>on</strong>g malechimpanzees in <strong>the</strong> Mahale Mountains Nati<strong>on</strong>al park, Tanzania: A preliminarystudy. Primates 30:187–97. [aJLL]Henneberg, M. & Thorne, A. (2004) Flores human may be pathologicalHomo sapiens. Before Farming 4, article 1. [RBE]Hernández Blasi, C. & Bjorklund, D. F. (2003) Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>development</strong>alpsychology: A <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> tool for better underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing human <strong>on</strong>togeny. HumanDevelopment 46:259–81. [DFB]Hernd<strong>on</strong>, M. & McLeod, N. (1980) The interrelati<strong>on</strong>ship of style <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>occasi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> Maltese spirtu pr<strong>on</strong>t. In:The ethnography of musicalperformance, ed. N. McLeod & M. Hernd<strong>on</strong>, pp. 147–66. NorwoodEditi<strong>on</strong>s. [rJLL]Hewes, G. W. (1974) Abridged bibliography <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> origin of language. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>origins, ed. R. W. Wescott. Linstok Press. [aJLL]Hewlett, B. S. (1991) Intimate fa<strong>the</strong>rs: The nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>text of Aka Pygmypaternal care. University of Michigan Press. [aJLL]Hill, K. & Hurtado, A. M. (1991) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of premature reproductive senescence<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> menopause in human females: An evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> “Gr<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>mo<strong>the</strong>rHypo<strong>the</strong>sis.” Human Nature 2:313–50. [aJLL](1996) Ache <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>: The ecology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> demography of a foraging people. Aldinede Gruyter. [aJLL]Hines, M. (1990) G<strong>on</strong>adal horm<strong>on</strong>es <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human cognitive <strong>development</strong>. In:Horm<strong>on</strong>es, brain, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behaviour in vertebrates. 1. Sexual differentiati<strong>on</strong>,neuroanatomical aspects, neurotransmitters <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> neuropeptides, ed.J. Balthazart. Karger. (Also in: Comparative Physiology 8:51–63.) [aJLL]Hobart, M. (1975) Orators <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> patr<strong>on</strong>s: Two types of political leader in Balinesevillage society. In: Political language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> oratory in traditi<strong>on</strong>al society, ed.M. Bloch. Academic Press. [aJLL]Hobs<strong>on</strong>, P. (2002) The cradle of thought: Exploring <strong>the</strong> origins of thinking.Macmillan. [SJ]Hobs<strong>on</strong>, W. C., Fuller, G. B., Winter, J. S. D., Faiman, C. & Reyes, P. I. (1981)Reproductive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> endocrine <strong>development</strong> in <strong>the</strong> great apes. In: Reproductivebiology of <strong>the</strong> great apes: Comparative <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> biomedical <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, ed. C. E.Graham, pp. 83–103. Academic Press. [SR]Hockett, C. F. (1977) The view from language: Selected essays, 1948–1974.University of Georgia Press. [aJLL]Hoebel, E. A. (1964) The law of primitive man: A study in comparative legaldynamics. Harvard University Press. [aJLL]Hogan, J. A. (1988) Cause <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> functi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of behavior systems. In:H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book of behavioral neurobiology, vol. 9: Developmental psychobiology<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavioral ecology, ed. E. M. Blass. Plenum. [SJC, aJLL]Hoit, J. D., Hix<strong>on</strong>, T. J., Wats<strong>on</strong>, P. J. & Morgan, W. J. (1990) Speech breathingin children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescents. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research33:51–69. [aJLL]Hold-Cavell, B. C. L. & Borsutzky, D. (1986) Strategies to obtain high regard:L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study of a group of preschool children. Ethology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Sociobiology 7:39–56. [aJLL]Howard, R. W. (2001) Searching <strong>the</strong> real world for signs of risingpopulati<strong>on</strong> intelligence. Pers<strong>on</strong>ality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Individual Differences30(6):1039–59. [DFB]Howell, N. (1979) Demography of <strong>the</strong> Dobe !Kung. Academic Press. [aJLL]Howlin, P., Mawhood, I. & Rutter, M. (2000) Autism <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>al languagedisorder – A comparative follow-up study in early <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>: II. Social, behavioural,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychiatry41:561–78. [aJLL]Hughes, S. M., Dispenza, F. & Gallup, G. G. (2004) Ratings of voice attractivenesspredict sexual behavior <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> body c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>. Evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> HumanBehavior 25:295–304. [aJLL]Hultsch, H. & Todt, D. (2004) Learning to sing. In: Nature’s music: The science ofbirds<strong>on</strong>g, ed. P. R. Marler & H. Slabbekoorn, pp. 80–107. AcademicPress. [DLB]Humphrey, C. (1996) Shamans <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> elders. Clarend<strong>on</strong> Press. [IB]Hurford, J. R. (1989) Biological evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> Saussurean sign as a comp<strong>on</strong>ent of<strong>the</strong> language acquisiti<strong>on</strong> device. Lingua 77:187–222. [HB](2002) Expressi<strong>on</strong>/inducti<strong>on</strong> models of language evoluti<strong>on</strong>: Dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>issues. In: Linguistic evoluti<strong>on</strong> through language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>: Formal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>computati<strong>on</strong>al models, ed. T. Briscoe, pp. 301–44. Cambridge UniversityPress. [LJG](2005) Computer modeling widens <strong>the</strong> focus of language study. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>origins: Perspectives <strong>on</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. Tallerman, pp. 284–89. OxfordUniversity Press. [HB]Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Knight, C., eds. (1998) Approaches to <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language: Social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive bases. Cambridge UniversityPress. [aJLL]Hurst, J. A., Baraitser, M., Auger, E., Graham, F. & Norell, S. (1990) An extendedfamily with a dominantly inherited speech disorder. Developmental Medicine<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Child Neurology 32:352–55. [aJLL]Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Selzer, M. & Ly<strong>on</strong>s, T. (1991) Earlyvocabulary growth: Relati<strong>on</strong> to language input <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> gender. DevelopmentalPsychology 27:236–48. [aJLL]Hyde, J. S. & Linn, M. C. (1988) Gender differences in verbal ability: A metaanalysis.Psychological Bulletin 104:53–69. [aJLL]Jackendoff, R. (1994) Patterns in <strong>the</strong> mind: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human nature. BasicBooks. [LW](2002) Foundati<strong>on</strong>s of language. Oxford University Press. [aJLL, KW]Jackendoff, R. S. (1995) The boundaries of <strong>the</strong> lexic<strong>on</strong>. In: Idioms: Structural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>psychological <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, ed. M. Everaert, E.-J. van der Linden, A. Schenk &R. Schroeder, pp. 133–65. Erlbaum. [KK]Jakobs<strong>on</strong>, R. (1960) Why ‘Mama’ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ‘Papa’? In: Perspectives in psychological<strong>the</strong>ory: Essays in h<strong>on</strong>or of Heinz Werner, ed. B. Kaplan & S. Wapner.Internati<strong>on</strong>al Universities Press. [aJLL]James, D. & Drakich, J. (1993) Underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing gender differences in amount of talk:A critical review of research. In: Gender <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>al speech, ed.D. Tannen. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]Jans<strong>on</strong>, C. H. & van Schaik, C. P. (1993) Ecological risk aversi<strong>on</strong> in juvenileprimates: Slow <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> steady wins <strong>the</strong> race. In: Juvenile primates: Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>,<strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior, ed. M. E. Pereira & L. A. Fairbanks, pp. 57–74.Oxford University Press. [aJLL, GEW]Jensvold, M. L. J. & Gardner, R. A. (2000) Interactive use of sign language bycross-fostered chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology114:335–46. [RAG]Jeris<strong>on</strong>, H. J. (1973) Evoluti<strong>on</strong> of brain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intelligence. Academic Press. [SR]Joffe, T. H. (1997) Social pressures have selected for an extended juvenile period inprimates. Journal of Human Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 32:593–605. [aJLL]Johanns<strong>on</strong>, S. (2005) Origins of language: C<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses.John Benjamins. [LJG, SJ]Johns<strong>on</strong>, K. P. (1999) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of bill colorati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> plumage dimorphismsupports <strong>the</strong> transference hypo<strong>the</strong>sis in dabbling ducks. Behavioral Ecology10:63–67. [rJLL]Johns<strong>on</strong>, S. & Carey, S. (1998) Knowledge enrichment <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual changein Folkbiology: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognitive Psychology37:156–200. [JVH]BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 317


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>J<strong>on</strong>es, S., Martin, R. & Pilbeam, D., eds. (1992) The Cambridge Encyclopedia ofHuman Evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Cambridge University Press. [GEW]Jusczyk, P. W. (1999) How infants begin to extract words from speech. Trends inCognitive Sciences 3(9):323–28. [JVH]Jusczyk, P. W. & Aslin, R. N. (1995) Infants’ detecti<strong>on</strong> of sound patterns in fluentspeech. Cognitive Psychology 29:1–23. [JVH]Kalma, A. (1991) Hierarchisati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dominance assessment at first glance. EuropeanJournal of Social Psychology 21:165–81. [aJLL]Kaminski, J., Call, J. & Fischer, J. (2004) Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidencefor “fast mapping.” Science 304:1682–83. [DLB]Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J. & Hurtado, A. M. (2000) A <strong>the</strong>ory of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>: Diet, intelligence, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> l<strong>on</strong>gevity. Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary Anthropology9:156–85. [rJLL]Karanth, P. (2005) The nature-nurture c<strong>on</strong>troversy in its most recent avatar: Aresp<strong>on</strong>se to Shukla <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Vasanta. Journal of Bioscience 30:139–42. [LW]Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985) Some fundamental aspects of language <strong>development</strong>after age 5. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> acquisiti<strong>on</strong>: Studies in first language <strong>development</strong>,2nd editi<strong>on</strong>, ed. P. Fletcher & M. Garman. Cambridge UniversityPress. [aJLL](1992) Bey<strong>on</strong>d modularity: A <strong>development</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> cognitive science.MIT Press/Bradford Books. [JVH](2006) Modules, genes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>: What have we learned from atypical<strong>development</strong>? In: Processes of change in brain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive <strong>development</strong>:Attenti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance series, vol. 21, ed. Y. Munakata & M. H. Johns<strong>on</strong>.pp. 563–83. Oxford University Press. [JVH]Karmiloff-Smith, A. & Inhelder, B. (1975) If you want to get ahead, get a <strong>the</strong>ory.Cogniti<strong>on</strong> 3(3):195–212. [JVH]Kashima, Y., Yamaguchi, S. K., Choi, S., Gelf<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, M. J. & Yuki, M. (1995) Culture,gender, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> self: A <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> from <strong>the</strong> individualism-collectivism research.Journal of Pers<strong>on</strong>ality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Social Psychology 69:925–37. [aJLL]Kawai, M. (1965) Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of <strong>the</strong> natural troop ofJapanese m<strong>on</strong>keys <strong>on</strong> Koshima islet. Primates 6:1–30. [aJLL]Kawamura, S. (1959) The process of sub-culture propagati<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g Japanesemacaques. Primates 2:43–60. [aJLL]Kegl, J., Senghas, A. & Coppola, M. (1999). Creati<strong>on</strong> through c<strong>on</strong>tact: Signlanguage emergence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sign language change in Nicaragua. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>creati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language change: Creolizati<strong>on</strong>, diachr<strong>on</strong>y, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>, ed.M. DeGraff, pp. 179–238. MIT Press. [SR]Kelso, S. (1997) Dynamic patterns: The self-organizati<strong>on</strong> of brain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior.MIT Press. [LJG]Kend<strong>on</strong>, A. (1980a) A descripti<strong>on</strong> of a deaf-mute sign language from <strong>the</strong>Enga Province of Papua New Guinea with some comparative discussi<strong>on</strong>.Part I: The formati<strong>on</strong>al properties of Enga signs. Semiotica 31(1–2):1–34. [SR](1980b) A descripti<strong>on</strong> of a deaf-mute sign language from <strong>the</strong> Enga Province ofPapua New Guinea with some comparative discussi<strong>on</strong>. Part II: The semioticfuncti<strong>on</strong>ing of Enga signs. Semiotica 31(1–2):81–117. [SR](1980c) A descripti<strong>on</strong> of a deaf-mute sign language from <strong>the</strong> Enga Provinceof Papua New Guinea with some comparative discussi<strong>on</strong>. Part III: Aspectsof utterance c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>. Semiotica 31(3–4):245–313. [SR]Kend<strong>on</strong>, A. & Cook, M. (1969) The c<strong>on</strong>sistency of gaze patterns in socialinteracti<strong>on</strong>. British Journal of Psychology 60:481–94. [aJLL]Kennedy, C. W. & Camden, C. (1983) Interrupti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>verbal genderdifferences. Journal of N<strong>on</strong>verbal Behavior 8:91–108. [aJLL]Kerswill, P. & Williams, A. (2000) Creating a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> town koine: Children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>language change in Milt<strong>on</strong> Keynes. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Society 29:65–115. [HB]Kien, J. (1991) A brain re-organizati<strong>on</strong> which may have paved <strong>the</strong> way for <strong>the</strong>emergence of language. Journal of Human Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 20:157–65. [SR]Kimura, D. (2002) Sex horm<strong>on</strong>es influence human cognitive pattern. NeuroendocrinologyLetters 23:67–77. [aJLL]King, B. J. (2004) The dynamic dance: N<strong>on</strong>vocal communicati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> Africangreat apes. Harvard University Press. [BJK](in press) Editorial. Journal of Developmental Processes 1(1). [BJK]Kirby, S. (1999a) Functi<strong>on</strong>, selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> innateness. Oxford UniversityPress. [aJLL](1999b) Learning, bottlenecks <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infinity: A working model of <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>of syntactic communicati<strong>on</strong>. In: Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> Society for ArtificialIntelligence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Simulati<strong>on</strong> of Behavior, ed. K. Datenhahn & C. Nehanive.pp. 1–9. University of Illinois, Urbana–Champaign Press. [LJG](2002) Natural language from artificial <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Artificial Life 8:185–215. [HB]Kirby, S. & Christiansen, M. (2003) From language learning to language evoluti<strong>on</strong>.In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. Christiansen & S. Kirby, pp. 272–94). OxfordUniversity Press. [SJC]Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M., Fischer, J. & Cheney, D. L. (2003) Loud calls asindicators of dominance in male babo<strong>on</strong>s (Papio cynocephalus ursinus).Behavioral Ecology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sociobiology 53:374–84. [aJLL]Klein, R. G. (1989) The human career: Human biological <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cultural origins.University of Chicago Press. [aJLL]Knight, C. (2002) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> revoluti<strong>on</strong>ary c<strong>on</strong>sciousness. In: The transiti<strong>on</strong> tolanguage, ed. A. Wray, pp. 138–60. Oxford University Press. [CK]Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Hurford, J. R., eds. (2000) The evoluti<strong>on</strong>aryemergence of language: Social functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> origins of linguistic form.Cambridge University Press. [aJLL]Kochman, T. (1969) “Rapping” in <strong>the</strong> black ghetto. Trans-acti<strong>on</strong> 6:26–34. [arJLL](1971) Cross-cultural communicati<strong>on</strong>: C<strong>on</strong>trasting <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, c<strong>on</strong>flicting sensibilities.Florida FL Reporter 9:3–17. [rJLL](1983) The boundary between play <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> play in black verbal dueling. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>in Society 12:329–37. [arJLL]Kohlberg, L., LaCrosse, J. & Ricks, D. (1972) The predictability of adult mentalhealth from childhood behavior. In: Manual of child psychopathology, ed. B. B.Wolman. McGraw-Hill. [aJLL]K<strong>on</strong>ner, M. J. (1976) Maternal care, infant behavior <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>!Kung. In: Kalahari hunter-ga<strong>the</strong>rers: Studies of <strong>the</strong> !Kung San <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>irneighbors, ed. R. B. Lee & I. DeVore. Harvard University Press. [aJLL]Koopmans-van Beinum, F. J. & van der Stelt, J. M. (1986) Early stages in <strong>the</strong><strong>development</strong> of speech movements. In: Precursors of early speech, ed.B. Lindblom & R. Zetterstrom. Stockt<strong>on</strong>. [aJLL]Krauss, R. M., Freyberg, R. & Morsella, E. (2002) Inferring speakers’ physicalattributes from <strong>the</strong>ir voices. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology38:618–25. [rJLL]Krauss, R. M. & Glucksberg, S. (1969) The <strong>development</strong> of communicati<strong>on</strong>:Competence as a functi<strong>on</strong> of age. Child Development 40:255–66. [aJLL]Kroodsma, D. E. (1999) Making ecological sense of s<strong>on</strong>g <strong>development</strong>. In: Thedesign of animal communicati<strong>on</strong>, M. D. Hauser & M. K<strong>on</strong>ishi, pp. 319–42.MIT Press. [DKO]Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N. & Lindblom, B. (1992)Linguistic experience alters ph<strong>on</strong>etic percepti<strong>on</strong> in infants by 6 m<strong>on</strong>ths of age.Science 255:606–608. [aJLL]Kuiper, K. (1996) Smooth talkers: The linguistic performance of aucti<strong>on</strong>eers <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>sportscasters. Erlbaum. [aJLL]Kuiper, K. & Austin, J. P. M. (1990) They’re off <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> racing now: The speech of <strong>the</strong>New Zeal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> race caller. In: New Zeal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ways of speaking English, ed. A. Bell& J. Holmes, pp. 195–220. Multilingual Matters. [KK]Kuiper, K. & Haggo, D. C. (1984) Livestock aucti<strong>on</strong>s, oral poetry <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ordinarylanguage. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Society 13:205–34. [KK]Kuiper, K. & Tan, D. G. L. (1989) Cultural c<strong>on</strong>gruence <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>flict in <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong>of formulae in a sec<strong>on</strong>d language. In: English across cultures: Culturesacross English, ed. O. Garcia & R. O<strong>the</strong>guy, pp. 281–304. Mout<strong>on</strong> deGruyter. [KK]Kuiper, K. & Tillis, F. (1986) The chant of <strong>the</strong> tobacco aucti<strong>on</strong>eer. American Speech60:141–49. [KK]Kurtzberg, D., Hilpert, P. L., Kreuzer, J. A. & Vaughan, H. G., Jr. (1984)Differential maturati<strong>on</strong> of cortical auditory evoked potentials to speechsounds in normal fullterm <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> very low-birthweight infants. DevelopmentalMedicine <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Child Neurology 26:466–75. [DMWP]Kurtzberg, D., Stapells, D. R. & Wallace, I. F. (1988) Event-related potentialassessment of auditory system integrity: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for language<strong>development</strong>. In: Early identificati<strong>on</strong> of infants with <strong>development</strong>aldisabilities, ed. P. Viettze & H. G. Vaughan, Jr., pp. 160–80.Grune & Stratt<strong>on</strong>. [DMWP]Kurtzberg, D., Vaughan, H. G., Jr. & Novak, G. P. (1986) Discriminative brainresp<strong>on</strong>ses to speech sounds in <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>born high risk infant. In: Maturati<strong>on</strong> of<strong>the</strong> CNS <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoked potentials, ed. V. Gallai, pp. 253–59. Elsevier. [DMWP]Kushnerenko, E. V. (2003) Maturati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> cortical auditory event-related brainpotentials in infancy. Doctoral Dissertati<strong>on</strong>, Cognitive Brain Research Unit,Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki. [DMWP]Labov, W. (1972) Sociolinguistic patterns. University of PennsylvaniaPress. [arJLL](1973) The linguistic c<strong>on</strong>sequences of being a lame. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> in Society2:81–115. [arJLL](2001) Principles of linguistic change. Vol. 2: Social factors. Blackwell. [aJLL]Lai, C., Fisher, S., Hurst, J., Levy, E. R., Hodgs<strong>on</strong>, S., Fox, M., Jeremiah, S., Povey,S., Jamis<strong>on</strong>, D. C., Green, E. D., Vargha-Khadem, F. & M<strong>on</strong>aco, A. (2000)The SPCH1 regi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> human 7q31: Genomic characterizati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong>critical interval <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> localizati<strong>on</strong> of translocati<strong>on</strong>s associated with speech<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language disorder. American Journal of Human Genetics67:357–68. [LW]Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Levy, E. R., Hodgs<strong>on</strong>, S., Fox, M.,Jeremiah, S., Povey, S., Jamis<strong>on</strong>, D. C., Green, E. D., Vargha-Khadem, F. &M<strong>on</strong>aco, A. P. (2000) The SPCH1 regi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> human 7q31: Genomiccharacterizati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> critical interval <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> localizati<strong>on</strong> of translocati<strong>on</strong>sassociated with speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language disorder. American Journal of HumanGenetics 67:357–68. [aJLL]Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F. & M<strong>on</strong>aco, A. P. (2001)A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language disorder.Nature 413:519–23. [aJLL, LW]318 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Lakoff, G. & Johns<strong>on</strong>, M. (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of ChicagoPress. [arJLL]Lancaster, J. B. & Lancaster, C. S. (1983) Parental investment: The hominidadaptati<strong>on</strong>. In: How humans adapt, ed. D. J. Ortner, pp. 33–65. Smiths<strong>on</strong>ianInstituti<strong>on</strong> Press. [aJLL, GEW]Langacker, R. (1987) Foundati<strong>on</strong>s of cognitive grammar, vol 1. Stanford UniversityPress. [SJ]Lass, N., Ruscello, D., Bradshaw, K. & Blakenship, B. (1991a) Adolescents’percepti<strong>on</strong>s of normal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> voice-disordered children. Journal ofCommunicati<strong>on</strong> Disorders 24:267–74. [aJLL]Lass, N. J., Ruscello, D. M., Stout, L. L. & Hoffmann, F. M. (1991b) Peerpercepti<strong>on</strong>s of normal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> voice-disordered children. Folia Ph<strong>on</strong>iatrica43:29–35. [aJLL]Lee, P. C. & Bowman, J. E. (1995) Influence of ecology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> energetics <strong>on</strong> primatemo<strong>the</strong>rs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infants. In: Mo<strong>the</strong>rhood in human <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>human primates,ed. C. R. Pryce, R. D. Martin & D. Skuse, pp. 47–58. Karger. [SR]Lee, R. B. (1979a) The !Kung San: Men, women, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> work in a foraging society.Cambridge University Press. [aJLL](1979b) The Dobe !Kung. Holt, Rinehart <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Winst<strong>on</strong>. [aJLL]Leigh, S. R. (1996) Evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human growth spurts. American Journal ofAnthropology 101:455–74. [rJLL](2001) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human growth. Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary Anthropology10:223–36. [rJLL]LeMasters, E. E. (1975) Blue-collar aristocrats: Life-styles at a working-classtavern. University of Wisc<strong>on</strong>sin Press. [aJLL]Lenti Boero, D. (1992) Alarm calls in marmots: Evidence for semanticcommunicati<strong>on</strong>. Ethology, Ecology, Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 4(2):125–38. [DLB](1997) Dal pianto alla parola: Una revisi<strong>on</strong>e della letteratura in una prospettivacomparata (From crying to speech: A review of literature in a comparative<str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>). Ricerche di Psicologia 4:33–79. [DLB]Le<strong>on</strong>ard, L. (1994) Some problems facing accounts of morphological deficits inchildren with specific language impairment. In: Specific language impairmentsin children: Current directi<strong>on</strong>s in research <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong>, pp. 91–105, ed.R. V. Watkins & M. L. Rice. Paul H. Brookes. [LW]Le<strong>on</strong>ard, L. B. (1986) C<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> replies of children with specific languageimpairment. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research 29:114–19. [aJLL]Le<strong>on</strong>ard, W. R. & Roberts<strong>on</strong>, M. L. (1992) Nutriti<strong>on</strong>al requirements <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> humanevoluti<strong>on</strong>: A bioenergetics model. American Journal of Human Biology4:179–95. [aJLL]Le<strong>on</strong>ard, W. R., Roberts<strong>on</strong>, M. L., Snodgrass, J. J. & Kuzawa, C. W. (2003)Metabolic correlates of hominid brain evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Comparative Biochemistry<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Physiology, A. Molecular <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Integrative Physiology136(1):5–15. [aJLL]Leopold, W. F. (1949) Original inventi<strong>on</strong> in infant language. Symposium3:66–75. [aJLL]Leutenegger, W. (1974) Functi<strong>on</strong>al aspects of pelvic morphology in Simianprimates. Journal of Human Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 3:207–22. [aJLL](1980) Encephalizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> obstetrics in primates with particular reference tohuman evoluti<strong>on</strong>. In: Primate brain evoluti<strong>on</strong>: Methods <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>cepts, ed.E. Armstr<strong>on</strong>g & D. Falk. Plenum. [aJLL]LeVine, R. & LeVine, B. (1966) Nyans<strong>on</strong>go: A Gusii community in Kenya.Wiley. [aJLL]LeVine, R. A. (1980) A cross-cultural <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> parenting. In: Parentingin a multicultural society, ed. M. D. Fantini & R. Cárdenas.L<strong>on</strong>gman. [aJLL]Lewis, B. A. (1990) Familial ph<strong>on</strong>ological disorders: Four pedigrees. Journal ofSpeech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Disorders 55:160–70. [aJLL]Lewis, B. A. & Thomps<strong>on</strong>, L. A. (1992) A study of <strong>development</strong>al speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>language disorders in twins. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research35:1086–94. [aJLL]Lew<strong>on</strong>tin, R. C. (1991) Biology as ideology. HarperCollins. [RAG]Lickliter, R. (in press) Dynamical systems <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> psychological science. In: Modern<str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <strong>on</strong> J. R. Kantor <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interbehaviorism, ed. B. D. Midgley & E. K.Morris. Greenwood Press. [LJG]Lickliter, R. & Schneider, S. M. (in press) The role of <strong>development</strong> in evoluti<strong>on</strong>arychange: A view from comparative psychology. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal ofComparative Psychology. [DFB]Lieberman, D. E., McCarthy, R. C., Hiiemae, K. M. & Palmer, J. B. (2001)Ontogeny of postnatal hyoid <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> larynx descent in humans. Archives ofOral Biology 46:117–28. [aJLL]Lieberman, P. (1991) Uniquely human: The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of speech, thought, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>selfless behavior. Harvard University Press. [aJLL]Lieberman, P. H., Klatt, D. H. & Wils<strong>on</strong>, W. H. (1969) Vocal tract limitati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>vowel repertoires of rhesus m<strong>on</strong>key <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r n<strong>on</strong>human primates. Science164:1185–87. [DLB]Liégeois, F., Baldeweg, T., C<strong>on</strong>nelly, A., Gadian, D. G., Mishkin, M. &Vargha-Khadem, F. (2003) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> fMRI abnormalities associated withFOXP2 gene mutati<strong>on</strong>. Nature Neuroscience 6:1230–38. [aJLL, LW]Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. & Dresner Barnes, H. (1992) Individual differencesin early vocabulary <strong>development</strong>: Redefining <strong>the</strong> referential-expressivedistincti<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Child <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> 19:287–310. [rJLL]Lindburg, D. G. (1982) Primate obstetrics: The biology of birth. American Journalof Primatology, Supplement 1:193–99. [aJLL]Linell, P. (1982) The written language bias in linguistics: Its nature, origins <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>transformati<strong>on</strong>s. University of Linköping Press. [SJC, arJLL](2005) The written language bias in linguistics: Its nature, origins <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>transformati<strong>on</strong>s. Routledge. (Reprint, revised editi<strong>on</strong>). [rJLL](in press) Dialogical language, dialogical minds, dialogical brains. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>Sciences. [SJC]Linn, G. W. & Caruso, A. J. (1998) Perspectives <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> effects of stuttering <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>formati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> maintenance of intimate relati<strong>on</strong>ships. Journal of Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>64:12–15. [aJLL]Littlet<strong>on</strong>, J. (2005) Fifty years of chimpanzee demography at Tar<strong>on</strong>ga Park Zoo.American Journal of Primatology 67:281–98. [rJLL]Livingst<strong>on</strong>e, F. B. (1973) Did <strong>the</strong> australopi<strong>the</strong>cines sing? Current Anthropology14:25–29.[RBE]Locke, J. L. (1983) Ph<strong>on</strong>ological acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> change. Academic Press. [aJLL](1985) The role of ph<strong>on</strong>etic factors in parent reference. Journal of Child<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> 12:215–20. [aJLL](1990) Structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimulati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>togeny of spoken language.Developmental Psychobiology 23:621–44. [aJLL](1993) The child’s path to spoken language. Harvard University Press. [arJLL](1994a) Development of <strong>the</strong> capacity for spoken language. In: H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book ofchild language, ed. P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney, pp. 278–302.Blackwell. [rJLL](1994b) Phases in <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of linguistic capacity. In: Evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>neurology of language. Discussi<strong>on</strong>s in Neuroscience, vol. 10, ed. D. C.Gajdusek, G. M. McKhann & C. L. Bolis, pp. 26–33. Elsevier. [rJLL](1994c) The biological building blocks of spoken language. In: Causalmechanisms of behavioural <strong>development</strong>, ed. J. Hogan & J. Bolhuis,pp. 300–24. Cambridge University Press. [rJLL](1995) The child’s path to spoken language, 2nd editi<strong>on</strong>. Harvard UniversityPress. [KW](1997) A <strong>the</strong>ory of neurolinguistic <strong>development</strong>. Brain <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>58:265–326. [arJLL, KW](1998a) Are <strong>development</strong>al language disorders primarily grammatical?Speculati<strong>on</strong>s from an evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary model. In: Exploring <strong>the</strong> speech-languagec<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>, ed. R. Paul. Paul H. Brookes. [aJLL](1998b) Social sound-making as a precursor to spoken language. In: Approachesto <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language: Social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive bases, ed. J. R. Hurford,M. Studdert-Kennedy & C. Knight. Cambridge University Press. [aJLL](1998c) The de-voicing of society. Sim<strong>on</strong> & Schuster. [arJLL](1999) Towards a biological science of language <strong>development</strong>. In: The <strong>development</strong>of language, ed. M. Barrett, pp. 373–95. Psychology Press. [arJLL](2000a) Movement patterns in spoken language. Science 288:449–50. [arJLL](2000b) Rank <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> relati<strong>on</strong>ships in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of spoken language. Journal of<strong>the</strong> Royal Anthropological Institute 7:37–50. [arJLL](2002) Dancing with humans: Interacti<strong>on</strong> as unintended c<strong>on</strong>sequence.Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 25(5):632–33. [Commentary <strong>on</strong> Shanker, S. G.& King, B. J. (2002) The emergence of a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> paradigm in ape languageresearch. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 25(5):605–20.] [rJLL](2004a) How do infants come to c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>the</strong> organs of speech? In: Speech motorc<strong>on</strong>trol in normal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> disordered speech, ed. B. Maassen, R. D. Kent,H. F. M. Peters, P. H. H. M. van Lieshout & W. Hulstijn. OxfordUniversity Press. [aJLL](2004b) Trickle up ph<strong>on</strong>etics: A vocal role for <strong>the</strong> infant. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Brain Sciences 27(4):516. [Commentary <strong>on</strong> Falk, D. (2004) Prelinguisticevoluti<strong>on</strong> in early hominins: Whence mo<strong>the</strong>rese? Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> BrainSciences 27(4):491–503.] [aJLL](2005) Looking for, looking at: Social c<strong>on</strong>trol, h<strong>on</strong>est signals, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> intimateexperience in human evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>. In: Animal communicati<strong>on</strong>networks, ed. P. K. McGregor. Cambridge University Press. [arJLL](2006) Parental selecti<strong>on</strong> of vocal behavior: Crying, cooing, babbling, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language. Human Nature 17:155–68. [DKO, arJLL](in press a) Bimodal signaling in infancy: Motor behavior, reference, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of spoken language. Interacti<strong>on</strong> Studies. [rJLL](in press b) Lipsmacking <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> babbling: Syllables, sociality, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> survival. In:Syllable <strong>development</strong>: The frame/c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> bey<strong>on</strong>d, ed. B. L. Davis &K. Zajdo. Erlbaum. [arJLL]Locke, J. L. & Hauser, M. D. (1999) Sex <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> status effects <strong>on</strong> primate volubility:Clues to <strong>the</strong> origin of vocal languages? Evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Human Behavior20:151–58. [arJLL]Locke, J. L. & Snow, C. E. (1997) Social influences <strong>on</strong> vocal learning in human<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>human primates. In: Social influences <strong>on</strong> vocal <strong>development</strong>, ed.C. Snowd<strong>on</strong> & M. Hausberger, pp. 274–92. Cambridge UniversityPress. [arJLL]BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 319


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Lord, A. B. (1960) The singer of tales. Harvard University Press. [KK]Lorenz, K. (1970) Compani<strong>on</strong>s as factors in <strong>the</strong> bird’s envir<strong>on</strong>ment. In: Studiesin animal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human behaviour, vol. 1, trans. R. Martin, ed. K. Lorenz,pp. 101–258. Harvard University Press. (Original work published1935.) [WMS]Love, N. (2004) Cogniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> language myth. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sciences26/6:525–44. [SJC]Luerssen, M. H. & Powers, D. M. W. (2003) On <strong>the</strong> artificial evoluti<strong>on</strong> of neuralgraph grammars. In: Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> 4th Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong>Cognitive Science, University of New South Wales, July 2003, ed. P. Slezak,pp. 369–77. University of New South Wales Press. [DMWP]Lutchmaya, S., Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen, S. & Raggatt, P. (2002) Foetal testoster<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>vocabulary size in 18- <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> 24-m<strong>on</strong>th-old human infants. Infant Behavior <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Development 24:418–24. [aJLL]Lykken, D. T. (1982) Research with twins: The c<strong>on</strong>cept of emergenesis.Psychophysiology 19:361–73. [rJLL]Maccoby, E. E. & Jacklin, C. M. (1974) The psychology of sex differences. StanfordUniversity Press. [aJLL]MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, K. & Hershberger, S. (2005) Theoretical issues in <strong>the</strong> study of evoluti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>. In: Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <strong>on</strong> human <strong>development</strong>,2nd editi<strong>on</strong>, ed. R. Burgess & K. MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, pp. 21–72. Sage. [rJLL]Macias Garcia, C. & Ramirez, E. (2005) Evidence that sensory traps can evolve intoh<strong>on</strong>est signals. Nature 434:501–505. [rJLL]MacNeilage, P. F. (1998) The frame/c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>the</strong>ory of evoluti<strong>on</strong> of speech producti<strong>on</strong>.Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 21(4):499–511. [aJLL]MacNeilage, P. F. & Davis, B. L. (2000) On <strong>the</strong> origin of internal structure of wordforms. Science 288:527–31. [rJLL]Malinowski, B. (1922/1984) Arg<strong>on</strong>auts of <strong>the</strong> Western Pacific: An account of nativeenterprise <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adventure in <strong>the</strong> archipelagoes of Melanesian New Guinea.Routledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Kegan Paul/Wavel<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Press. [KK, aJLL](1923) The problem of meaning in primitive languages. In: The meaning ofmeaning, ed. C. K. Ogden & I. A. Richards, pp. 451–510. Harcourt. [WMS](1935) Coral gardens <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir magic: A study of <strong>the</strong> methods of tilling <strong>the</strong> soil<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> of agricultural rites in <strong>the</strong> Trobri<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Isl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s. Vol. 2: The language of magic<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> gardening. American Book Company. [aJLL]Mar<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>a, E. K. (1976) Riddles <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> riddling. Journal of American Folklore89:127–37. [rJLL]Marchman, V., Bates, E., Burkhardt, A. & Good, A. (1991) Functi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>straints<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> passive: Toward a model of <strong>the</strong> competence toperform. First <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> 11:65–92. [aJLL]Marcus, G. F. & Fisher, S. E. (2003) FOXP2 in focus: What can genes tell us aboutspeech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:257–62. [aJLL]Marler, P. (1991) The instinct to learn. In: The epigenesis of mind: Essays <strong>on</strong> biology<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cogniti<strong>on</strong>, ed. S. Carey & R. Gelman. Erlbaum. [aJLL]Marsh, P. (1978) Aggro: The illusi<strong>on</strong> of violence. Dent. [arJLL]Martin, R. D. (1983) Human brain evoluti<strong>on</strong> in an ecological c<strong>on</strong>text.The 52nd James Arthur Lecture, American Museum of Natural History,New York. [aJLL](1990) Primate origins <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>: A phylogenetic rec<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>. Princet<strong>on</strong>University Press. [DKO]Matsuzawa, T. (1996) Field experiments <strong>on</strong> use of st<strong>on</strong>e tools in <strong>the</strong> wild. In:Chimpanzee cultures, ed. R. E. Wrangham, W. C. McGrew, F. B. M. de Waal& P. G. Heltne. Harvard University Press. [aJLL]Maynard Smith, J. (1969) The status of neo-Darwinism. In: Towards a <strong>the</strong>oreticalbiology, vol. 2: Sketches, ed. C. H. Waddingt<strong>on</strong>. Aldine. [aJLL]Mayr, E. (1972) Sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> natural selecti<strong>on</strong>. In: Sexual selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>descent of man 1871–1971, ed. B. Campbell. Aldine. [aJLL]McBrearty, S. & Jabl<strong>on</strong>ski, N. G. (2005) First fossil chimpanzee. Nature437:105–108. [rJLL]McD<strong>on</strong>ald, S. (2000) Neuropsychological studies of sarcasm. Metaphor <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Symbol15:85–98. [aJLL]McDowell, J. H. (1979) Children’s riddling. Indiana University Press. [rJLL]McGhee, P. E. (1976) Sex differences in children’s humor. Journal ofCommunicati<strong>on</strong> 26:176–89. [rJLL](1979) Humor: Its origin <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>. W. H. Freeman. [aJLL]McGrew, W. C. (1975) Patterns of plant food sharing by wild chimpanzees. In:C<strong>on</strong>temporary primatology, ed. S. K<strong>on</strong>do, M. Kawai & A. Ehara, pp. 304–309.S. Karger. [GEW]McGuire, B. (1988) The effects of cross-fostering <strong>on</strong> parental behavior of meadowvoles. Journal of Mammalogy 69:332–41. [RAG]McHenry, H. M. (1975) Biomechanical interpretati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> early hominid hip.Journal of Human Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 4:343–55. [aJLL]McIntosh, M. K. (1998) C<strong>on</strong>trolling misbehavior in Engl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 1370–1600.Cambridge University Press. [aJLL]McKinney, M. L. (2000) Evolving behavioral complexity by extending<strong>development</strong>. In: Biology, brains, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior: The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of hum<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>evelopment, ed. S. T. Parker, J. Langer & M. L. McKinney, pp. 25–40. Schoolof American Research Press. [rJLL, SR]McKinney, M. L. & McNamara, K. J. (1991) Heterochr<strong>on</strong>y: The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of<strong>on</strong>togeny. Plenum Press. [rJLL]McTear, M. F. & C<strong>on</strong>ti-Ramsden, G. (1992) Pragmatic disability in children.Singular. [aJLL]McWhorter, J. H. (1997) Toward a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> model of Creole genesis. Peter Lang. [SR]Mealey, L. (2000) Sex differences: Development <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary strategies.Academic Press. [aJLL]Mech, L. D. (1970) The wolf: The ecology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior of an endangered species.Natural History Press. [GEW]Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P. W., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bert<strong>on</strong>cini, J. & Amiel-Tis<strong>on</strong>,C. (1988) A precursor of language acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in young infants. Cogniti<strong>on</strong>29:143–78. [DLB, DMWP]Meltzoff, A. N. (1990) Foundati<strong>on</strong>s for developing a c<strong>on</strong>cept of self: The role ofimitati<strong>on</strong> in relating self to o<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> value of social mirroring, socialmodeling, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> self practice in infancy. In: The self in transiti<strong>on</strong>: Infancy tochildhood, ed. D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly. University of ChicagoPress. [aJLL]Mel’čuk, I. (1995) Phrasemes in language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phraseology in linguistics. In: Idioms:Structural <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> psychological <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, ed. M. Everaert, E.-J. van derLinden, A. Schenk & R. Schroeder, pp. 167–232. Erlbaum. [KK]Merker, B. (2000) Synchr<strong>on</strong>ous chorusing <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human origins. In: The origins ofmusic, ed. N. L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown, pp. 315–27. MITPress. [arJLL]Mey, J. L. (1985) Whose language: A study in linguistic pragmatics. JohnBenjamins. [aJLL]Miller, G. (2000) The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> ofhuman nature. William Heinemann/Anchor Books/Doubleday. [SJ, PK,aJLL, DKO]Miller, J. D. (1977) Percepti<strong>on</strong> of speech sounds in animals: Evidence for speechprocessing by mammalian auditory mechanisms. In: Dahlem workshop <strong>on</strong>recogniti<strong>on</strong> of complex acoustic signals: Life Sciences Report 5, ed. T. Bullock,pp. 49–58. Abak<strong>on</strong>. [DLB]Miller, P. M., Danaher, D. L. & Forbes, D. (1986) Sex-related strategies for copingwith interpers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>flict in children aged five <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> seven. DevelopmentalPsychology 22:543–48. [arJLL]Mirsky, J. (1937) The Eskimo of Greenl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>. In: Cooperati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> competiti<strong>on</strong>am<strong>on</strong>g primitive peoples, ed. M. Mead. Beac<strong>on</strong> Press. [aJLL]Mitchell-Kernan, C. (1973) Signifying. In: Mo<strong>the</strong>r wit from <strong>the</strong> laughing barrel, ed.A. Dundes. Prentice-Hall. [aJLL]Mi<strong>the</strong>n, S. (1996) The pre<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>the</strong> mind: A search for <strong>the</strong> origins of art, religi<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> science. Thames <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Huds<strong>on</strong>. [aJLL](2005) The singing Ne<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>erthals: The origins of music, language, mind <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> body.Weidenfeld & Nicols<strong>on</strong>. [BJK, rJLL]Mivart, St. G. (1871) On <strong>the</strong> genesis of species. Macmillan. [aJLL]M<strong>on</strong>tague, M. F. A. (1962) Time morphology, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> neoteny in <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of man.In: Culture <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of man, ed. M. F. A. M<strong>on</strong>tague, pp. 324–42.Oxford University Press. [SR]Moore, M. M. (1985) N<strong>on</strong>verbal courtship patterns in women: C<strong>on</strong>text <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>sequences.Ethology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sociobiology 6:237–47. [aJLL](1995) Courtship signaling <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescents: “Girls just wanna have fun”? Journalof Sex Research 32:319–28. [aJLL](2002) Courtship communicati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> percepti<strong>on</strong>. Perceptual <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Motor Skills94:97–105. [aJLL]Moore, M. M. & Butler, D. L. (1989) Predictive aspects of n<strong>on</strong>verbal courtshipbehavior in women. Semiotica 76:205–15. [aJLL]Moore, T. E., ed. (1973) Cognitive <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of language.Academic Press. [RAG]Morisset, C. E., Barnard, K. E. & Booth, C. L. (1995) Toddlers’ language<strong>development</strong>: Sex differences within social risk. Developmental Psychology31:851–65. [aJLL]Morse, P. A. & Snowd<strong>on</strong>, C. T. (1975) An investigati<strong>on</strong> of categorical speechdiscriminati<strong>on</strong> by rhesus m<strong>on</strong>keys. Percepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychophysics17:9–16. [DLB]Mort<strong>on</strong>, T. L. (1978) Intimacy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reciprocity of exchange: A comparis<strong>on</strong> of spouses<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> strangers. Journal of Pers<strong>on</strong>ality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Social Psychology 36:72–81. [aJLL]Mowrer, D., Wahl, P. & Doolan, S. (1978) Effects of lisping <strong>on</strong> audience evaluati<strong>on</strong>of male speakers. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Disorders43:140–48. [aJLL]Mulac, A. (1989) Men’s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> women’s talk in same-gender <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mixed-gender dyads:Power or polemic? Journal of <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Social Psychology8:249–70. [aJLL]Muller, M. N. & Wrangham, R. W. (2004) Dominance, aggressi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> testoster<strong>on</strong>ein wild chimpanzees: A test of <strong>the</strong> “challenge hypo<strong>the</strong>sis.” Animal Behaviour67:113–23. [aJLL]Nauclér, K. & Magnuss<strong>on</strong>, E. (2002) How do preschool language problemsaffect language abilities in adolescence? In: Investigati<strong>on</strong>s in clinicalph<strong>on</strong>etics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> linguistics, ed. F. Windsor, M. L. Kelly & N. Hewlett.Erlbaum. [aJLL]320 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Nazzi, T., Bert<strong>on</strong>cini, J. & Mehler, J. (1998) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> discriminati<strong>on</strong> by <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>borns:Toward an underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing of <strong>the</strong> role of rhythm. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Performance 24:756–66. [KW]Neisser, U., ed. (1998) The rising curve: L<strong>on</strong>g-term gains in IQ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> relatedmeasures. American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong>. [DFB]Nels<strong>on</strong>, K. (1996) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> in cogniti<strong>on</strong>: The emergence of <strong>the</strong> mediated mind.Cambridge University Press. [SJC](1973) Structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> strategy in learning to talk. M<strong>on</strong>ographs of <strong>the</strong> Society forResearch in Child Development 38, 1–2 (Serial no. 149). [rJLL](1981) Individual differences in language <strong>development</strong>: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for<strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language. Developmental Psychology 17:170–87. [rJLL](1996) Memory <strong>development</strong> from 4 to 7 years. In: The five to seven year shift:The age of reas<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>sibility, ed. A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith.University of Chicago Press. [aJLL]Nels<strong>on</strong>, K. & Fivush, R. (2004) The emergence of autobiographical memory: Asocial cultural <strong>development</strong>al <strong>the</strong>ory. Psychological Review111(2):486–511. [SR]Nels<strong>on</strong>, K. & L. K. Shaw (2002). Developing a socially shared symbolic system. In:<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, literacy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive <strong>development</strong>, ed.J. Byrnes & E. Amseli, pp.27–57. Erlbaum. [SJ]Newbury, D. F., B<strong>on</strong>ora, E., Lamb, J. A., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S. L., Baird, G.,Jannoun, L., Sl<strong>on</strong>ims, V., Stott, C. M., Merricks, M. J., Bolt<strong>on</strong>, P. F., Bailey,A. J., M<strong>on</strong>aco, A. P. & <strong>the</strong> Internati<strong>on</strong>al Molecular Genetic Study of AutismC<strong>on</strong>sortium. (2002) FOXP2 is not a major susceptibility gene for autism orspecific language impairment. American Journal of Human Genetics70:1318–27. [LW]Newmeyer, F. J. (2004) Grammar is grammar <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> usage is usage. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>80:682–707. [aJLL]Newport, E. (1981) C<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> structure: evidence from American Sign<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language learning. In: Minnesota Symposium <strong>on</strong> Child Psychology,vol. 14: Aspects of <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> of competence, ed. W. A. Collins,pp. 93–124. Erlbaum. [aJLL]Ninio, A. & Snow, C. (1996) Pragmatic <strong>development</strong>. Westview Press. [SR]Ninio, A., Snow, C., Pan, B. A. & Rollins, P. R. (1994) Classifying communicativeacts in children’s interacti<strong>on</strong>s. Journal of Communicati<strong>on</strong> Disorders 27:158–87. [aJLL]Nippold, M. (1998) Later language <strong>development</strong>: The school-age <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescentyears, 2nd editi<strong>on</strong>. Pro-ed. [aJLL]Nippold, M. A. & Taylor, C. L. (2002) Judgments of idiom familiarity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> transparency:A comparis<strong>on</strong> of children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescents. Journal of Speech,<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research 45:384–91. [aJLL]Nishida, T., Takasaki, H. & Takahata, Y. (1990) Demography <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> reproductiveprofiles. In: The chimpanzees of <strong>the</strong> Mahale Mountains: Sexual <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> strategies, ed. T. Nishida, pp. 63–97. University of TokyoPress. [aJLL]Northcutt, R. G. (1990) Ontogeny <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> phylogeny: A re-evaluati<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>ceptualrelati<strong>on</strong>ships <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> some applicati<strong>on</strong>s. Brain, Behavior <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 36(2-3):116–40. [aJLL]Nowak, M. A. & Komarova, N. L. (2001) Towards an evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <strong>the</strong>ory oflanguage. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5:288–95. [HB]Odling-Smee, F. J., Lal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, K. N. & Feldman, M. W. (2003) Nichec<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>: The neglected process in evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Princet<strong>on</strong> UniversityPress. [aJLL]Ohala, J. J. (2005) Ph<strong>on</strong>etic explanati<strong>on</strong>s for sound patterns. Implicati<strong>on</strong>s forgrammars of competence. In: A figure of speech: A festschrift for John Laver,ed. W. J. Hardcastle & J. M. Beck, pp. 23–38. Erlbaum. [rJLL]Oller, D. K. (2000) The emergence of <strong>the</strong> capacity for speech. Erlbaum. [aJLL, DKO](2004) Underpinnings for a <strong>the</strong>ory of communicative evoluti<strong>on</strong>. In: The evoluti<strong>on</strong>of communicati<strong>on</strong> systems: A comparative approach, ed. D. K. Oller &U. Griebel, pp. 49–65. MIT Press. [aJLL, DKO]Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Neal, A. R. & Schwartz, H. K. (1999) Precursors tospeech in infancy: The predicti<strong>on</strong> of speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language disorders. Journal ofCommunicati<strong>on</strong> Disorders 32:223–45. [aJLL]Oller, D. K. & Griebel, U. (2005) C<strong>on</strong>textual freedom in human infant vocalizati<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language. In: Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <strong>on</strong> hum<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>evelopment, ed. R. Burgess & K. MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, pp. 135–66. SagePublicati<strong>on</strong>s. [DKO]Oller, D. K. & Griebel, U., eds. (2004) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of communicati<strong>on</strong> systems: Acomparative approach. MIT Press. [aJLL]Olss<strong>on</strong>, R. & Powers, D. (2003) Machine learning of human language throughautomatic programming In: Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> 4th Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong>Cognitive Science, University of New South Wales, July 2003, ed. P. Slezak,pp. 507–12. University of New South Wales Press. [DMWP]Ong, W. J. (1982) Orality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> literacy: The technologizing of <strong>the</strong> word.Methuen. [arJLL]Oyama, S. (2001) What is <strong>development</strong>al systems <strong>the</strong>ory? In: Cycles of c<strong>on</strong>tingency:Developmental systems <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. S. Oyama, P. E. Griffiths & R. D.Gray, pp. 1–12. MIT Press. [LJG]Oyama, S., Griffiths, P. E. & Gray, R. D. (2001) Cycles of c<strong>on</strong>tingency: Developmentalsystems <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>. MIT Press. [LJG]Paine, R. L. & Hawkes, K., eds. (2006) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Schoolof American Research. [rJLL]Palombit, R. A., Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1999) Male grunts as mediators ofsocial interacti<strong>on</strong> with females in wild chacma babo<strong>on</strong>s (Papio cynocephalusursinus). Behaviour 136:221–42. [aJLL]Papoušek, M. & Papoušek, H. (1981) Musical elements in <strong>the</strong> infant’s vocalizati<strong>on</strong>:Their significance for communicati<strong>on</strong>, cogniti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> creativity. In: Advancesin infancy research, vol. 1, ed. L. P. Lipsitt. Ablex. [DLB]Paradis, M., ed. (1998) Pragmatics in neurogenic communicati<strong>on</strong> disorders.Pergam<strong>on</strong>. [aJLL]Parker, S. T. (2000) Homo erectus infancy <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> childhood: The turning point in <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of behavioral <strong>development</strong> in hominids. In Biology, brains, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>behavior: The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human <strong>development</strong>, ed. S. T. Parker, J. Langer &M. L. McKinney, pp. 279–318. School of American Research Press. [BJK]Passes, A. (2004) The place of politics: Powerful speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> women speakers ineveryday Pa’ikwené (Palikur) <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Journal of <strong>the</strong> Royal AnthropologicalInstitute 10:1–18. [aJLL]Paul, R. (1995) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> disorders from infancy through adolescence:Assessment<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong>. Mosby. [aJLL]Pavelka, M. S. & Fedigan, L. M. (1991) Menopause: A comparative <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Yearbook in Physical Anthropology 34:13–38. [aJLL]Pawlby, S. J. (1977) Imitative interacti<strong>on</strong>. In: Studies in mo<strong>the</strong>r-infant interacti<strong>on</strong>,ed. H. R. Schaffer. Academic Press. [aJLL]Pawley, A. (1991) How to talk cricket: On linguistic competence in a subject matter.In: Currents in Pacific linguistics: Papers <strong>on</strong> Austr<strong>on</strong>esian languages <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ethnolinguisticsin h<strong>on</strong>our of George W. Grace, ed. R. Blust, pp. 339–68. PacificLinguistics, Series C-117. [aJLL]Pedersen, M. F., Møller, S., Krabbe, S. & Bennett, P. (1986) Fundamental voicefrequency measured by electroglottography during c<strong>on</strong>tinuous speech: A <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>exact sec<strong>on</strong>dary sex characteristic in boys in puberty. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal ofPediatric Otorhinolaryngology 11:21–27. [aJLL]Pedersen, M. F., Møller, S., Krabbe, S., Bennett, P. & Svenstrup, B. (1990) Fundamentalvoice frequency in female puberty measured with electroglottographyduring c<strong>on</strong>tinuous speech as a sec<strong>on</strong>dary sex characteristic: A comparis<strong>on</strong>between voice, pubertal stages, oestrogens <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>rogens. Internati<strong>on</strong>alJournal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 20:17–24. [aJLL]Peek, P. (1981) The power of words in African verbal arts. The Journal of AmericanFolklore 94:19–43. [rJLL]Pepperberg, I. M. (1981) Functi<strong>on</strong>al vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s by an African Grey Parrot(Psittacus erithacus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 55:139–60. [DLB](1983) Cogniti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> African Grey parrot: Preliminary evidence for auditory/vocal comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> class c<strong>on</strong>cept. Animal Learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Behavior11:179–85. [DLB]Pereira, M. E. (1993) Evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> juvenile period in mammals. In: Juvenileprimates: Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior, ed. M. E. Pereira & L. A.Fairbanks, pp. 17–27. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]Pereira, M. E. & Altmann, J. (1985) Development of social behavior in free-livingn<strong>on</strong>human primates. In: N<strong>on</strong>human primate models for human growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>development</strong>, ed. E. S. Watts, pp. 217–309. Alan R. Liss. [aJLL, GEW]Pereira, M. E. & Fairbanks, L. A., eds. (1993) Juvenile primates: Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>,<strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]Peters, A. M. (1983) The units of language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. Cambridge UniversityPress. [KK]Philbrick, F. A. (1949) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> law: The semantics of forensic English.Macmillan. [aJLL]Pinker, S. (1999) Words <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rules. Basic Books. [LW]Pinker, S. & Bloom, P. (1990) Natural language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> natural selecti<strong>on</strong>. Behavioral<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 13:707–84. [aJLL, DKO]Pinker, S. & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: What’s special about it?Cogniti<strong>on</strong> 95:201–236. [LW]Place, K. S. & Becker, J. A. (1991) The influence of pragmatic competence <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>likeability of grade-school children. Discourse Processes 14:227–41. [aJLL]Plant, G. (1984) The effects of an acquired profound hearing loss <strong>on</strong> speech producti<strong>on</strong>.British Journal of Audiology 18:39–48. [aJLL]Plant, T. M. & Barker-Gibb, M. L. (2004) Neurobiological mechanisms of pubertyin higher primates. Human Reproducti<strong>on</strong> Update 10:67–77. [rJLL]Plavcan, J. & van Schaik, C. (1997) Interpreting hominid behavior <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> basis ofsexual dimorphism. Journal of Human Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 32:345–74. [PK]Ploog, D. W. (1992) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of vocal communicati<strong>on</strong>. In: N<strong>on</strong>verbal vocalcommunicati<strong>on</strong>, ed. H. Papoušek, U. Jurgens & M. Papoušek, pp. 6–30.Cambridge University Press. [DLB]Plummer, T. (2004) Flaked st<strong>on</strong>es <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> old b<strong>on</strong>es: Biological <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cultural evoluti<strong>on</strong>at <strong>the</strong> dawn of technology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, Suppl.39:118–64. [aJLL]Popper, K. (1963) C<strong>on</strong>jectures <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> refutati<strong>on</strong>s, Routledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> KeganPaul. [DMWP]BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 321


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Potts, R. (1988) Early hominid activities at Olduvai. Aldine de Gruyter. [aJLL]Power, C. & Aiello, L. C. (1997) Female proto-symbolic strategies. In: Women inhuman evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. L. D. Hager, pp. 153–71. Routledge. [CK]Powers, D. M. W. (1992) On <strong>the</strong> significance of closed classes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> boundary c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s:Experiments in lexical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> syntactic learning. In: Background <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>experiments in machine learning of natural language: First SHOE workshop,ITK Proceedings 92/1, Tilburg University, The Ne<strong>the</strong>rl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, ed. W. Daelemans& D. M. W. Powers, pp. 245–66. Available at: http://www.infoeng.flinders.edu.au/papers/19920001.pdf.[DMWP]Powers, D. M. W. & Turk, C. C. R. (1989) Machine learning of natural language.Springer-Verlag. [DMWP]Prader, A. (1984) Biomedical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> endocrinological aspects of normal growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>development</strong>. In: Human growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>, ed. J. Borms, R. Hauspie,A. S<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>, C. Susanne & M. Hebbelinck. Plenum. [aJLL]Prader, A., Tanner, J. M. & V<strong>on</strong> Harnack, G. A. (1963) Catch-up growth followingillness or starvati<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Paediatrics 62:646–59. [rJLL]Premack, D. (2004) Is language <strong>the</strong> key to human intelligence? Science 303:318–20. [JVH]Prince, C. G., Helder, N. & Hollich, G. (2005) Ongoing emergence: A core c<strong>on</strong>ceptin epigenetic robotics. In: Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> Fifth Internati<strong>on</strong>al Workshop <strong>on</strong>Epigenetic Robotics: Modeling cognitive <strong>development</strong> in robotic systems, Nara,Japan. ed. L. Berthouze, F. Kaplan, H. Kozima, H. Yano, J. K<strong>on</strong>czak, G. Metta,J. Nadel, G. S<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ini, G. Stojanov, & C. Balkaneius, pp. 63–70. LundUniversity Cognitive Studies. [LJG]Prince, C. G. & Hollich, G. (2005) Synching models with infants: A perceptual-levelmodel of infant audio-visual synchr<strong>on</strong>y detecti<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Cognitive SystemsResearch 6:205–28. Available at: http://www.cprince.com/PubRes/JCSR04. [LJG]Promislow, D. & Harvey, P. (1990) Living fast <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> dying young: A comparativeanalysis of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> variati<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g mammals. Journal of Zoology, L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>220:417–37. [PK]Prutting, C. A. & Kirchner, D. M. (1987) A clinical appraisal of <strong>the</strong> pragmaticaspects of language. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Disorders52:105–19. [aJLL]Pusey, A. (1983) Mo<strong>the</strong>r-offspring relati<strong>on</strong>ships in chimpanzees after weaning.Animal Behavior 31:363–77. [rJLL]Putallaz, M. & Gottman, J. M. (1981) Social skills <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> group acceptance. In: The<strong>development</strong> of children’s friendships, ed. S. R. Asher & J. M. Gottman.Cambridge University Press. [aJLL]Puts, D. A. (2005) Mating c<strong>on</strong>text <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> menstrual phase affect women’s preferencesfor male voice pitch. Evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Human Behavior 26:388–97. [rJLL]Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C. & Verdolini, K. (2005) Dominance <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> ofsexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Human Behavior27:283–96. [rJLL]Raffaelli, M. & Duckett, E. (1989) “We were just talking ...” – C<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s inearly adolescence. Journal of Youth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Adolescence 18:567–82. [aJLL]Ragir, S. (1985) Retarded <strong>development</strong>: The evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary mechanism underlying<strong>the</strong> emergence of <strong>the</strong> human capacity for language. Journal of Mind <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Behavior 6(4):451–68. [SR](2001a) Changes in perinatal c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s selected for ne<strong>on</strong>atal immaturity.Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 24(2):291–92. [SR](2001b) Toward an underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing of <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ship between bipedal walking,encephalizati<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language origins. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>: Biological,linguistic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> philosophical <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>s, ed. G. Gyori, pp. 73–99. PeterLang. [SR](2002) C<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> communities with indigenous sign languages: Clues to<strong>the</strong> dynamics of language origins. In: The transiti<strong>on</strong> to language, ed. A. Wray,pp. 272–94. Oxford University Press. [HB]Ramirez Rozzi, F. V. & Bermudez De Castro, J. M. (2004) Surprisingly rapidgrowth in Ne<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>erthals. Nature 428:936–99. [aJLL]Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D. & Mehler, J. (2000) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>discriminati<strong>on</strong> by human <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>borns <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> by cott<strong>on</strong>-top tamarin m<strong>on</strong>keys.Science 288:349–51. [KW]Ray, G. B. (1986) Vocally cued pers<strong>on</strong>ality prototypes: An implicit pers<strong>on</strong>ality<strong>the</strong>ory approach. Communicati<strong>on</strong> M<strong>on</strong>ographs 53:266–76. [aJLL]Reay, M. (1959) The Kuma: Freedom <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>formity in <strong>the</strong> New Guinea Highl<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>s.Melbourne University Press. [aJLL]Reddy, M. J. (1993) The c<strong>on</strong>duit metaphor: A case of frame c<strong>on</strong>flict in our languageabout language. In Metaphor <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> thought, ed. A. Ort<strong>on</strong>y, pp. 164–201.Cambridge University Press. [BJK]Reed, V. A., Bradfield, M. & McAllister, L. (1998) The relative importance ofselected communicati<strong>on</strong> skills for successful adolescent peer interacti<strong>on</strong>s:Speech pathologists’ opini<strong>on</strong>s. Clinical Linguistics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ph<strong>on</strong>etics12:205–20. [aJLL]Reed, V. A. & Spicer, L. (2003) The relative importance of selected communicati<strong>on</strong>skills for adolescents’ interacti<strong>on</strong>s with <strong>the</strong>ir teachers: High school teachers’opini<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Services in Schools34:343–57. [aJLL]Relethford, J. (2006) The human species: an introducti<strong>on</strong> to biological anthropology,6 th editi<strong>on</strong>. McGraw-Hill. [arJLL]Rice, M. (1993) “D<strong>on</strong>’t talk to him; he’s weird.” A social c<strong>on</strong>sequences account oflanguage <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social interacti<strong>on</strong>s. In: Enhancing children’s communicati<strong>on</strong>:Research foundati<strong>on</strong>s for interventi<strong>on</strong>, ed. A. P. Kaiser & D. B. Gray. PaulH. Brookes. [aJLL]Richman, B. (1980) Did human speech originate in coordinated vocal music?Semiotica 32:233–44. [rJLL](1987) Rhythm <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> melody in gelada vocal exchanges. Primates28:199–223. [rJLL](2000) How music fixed “n<strong>on</strong>sense” into significant formulas: On rhythm, repetiti<strong>on</strong>,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning. In: The origins of music, ed. N. L. Wallin, B. Merker &S. Brown. MIT Press. [aJLL]Rimpau, J. B., Gardner, R. A. & Gardner, B. T. (1989). Expressi<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>, place,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> instrument in ASL utterances of children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> chimpanzees. In: Teachingsign language to chimpanzees, ed. R. A. Gardner, B. T. Gardner & T. E. VanCantfort. State University of New York Press. [RAG]Rivers, W. H. R. (1907) The marriage of cousins in India. Journal of <strong>the</strong> AsiaticSociety 611–940. [IB]Roazzi, A., Dowker, A. & Bryant, P. (1993) Ph<strong>on</strong>ological abilities of Brazilian streetpoets. Applied Psycholinguistics 14(4):535–51. [rJLL]Roazzi, A., Dowker, A. & Bryant, P. E. (1994) Metalinguistic awareness in Brazilianbards. In: Journeys into cross-cultural psychology, ed. A. Bouvy & F. J. R. V<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>e Vijver, pp. 83–85. Lisse. [rJLL]Robb, M. P., Hughes, M. C. & Frese, D. J. (1985) Oral diadochokinesis inhearing-impaired adolescents. Journal of Communicati<strong>on</strong> Disorders18:79–89. [aJLL]Rochat, P., Querido, J. G. & Striano, T. (1999) Emerging sensitivity to <strong>the</strong> timing<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> structure of protoc<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> in early infancy. Developmental Psychology35:950–57. [SR]Rogoff, B. (1996) Developmental transiti<strong>on</strong>s in children’s participati<strong>on</strong> in socioculturalactivities. In: The five to seven year shift: The age of reas<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>resp<strong>on</strong>sibility, ed. A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith. University of ChicagoPress. [aJLL]Ross, D. (in press) Homo sapiens as ecologically special: What does languagec<strong>on</strong>tribute? <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sciences. [SJC]Rowley, I. & Chapman, G. (1986) Cross-fostering, imprinting <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning in twosympatric species of cockatoos. Behaviour 96:1–16. [RAG]Ruscello, D. M., Lass, N. J. & Podbesek, J. (1988) Listeners’ percepti<strong>on</strong>s of normal<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> voice-disordered children. Folia Ph<strong>on</strong>iatrica 40:290–96. [aJLL]Rutter, M. & O’C<strong>on</strong>nor, T. G. (2004) Are <strong>the</strong>re biological programming effects forpsychological <strong>development</strong>? Findings from a study of Romanian adoptees.Developmental Psychology 40:81–94. [aJLL]Ruzza, B., Rocca, F., Lenti Boero, D. & Lenti C. (2003) Investigating <strong>the</strong> musicalqualities of early infant sounds. In: The Neurosciences <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> music: Annals of <strong>the</strong>New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 999, ed. G. Avanzini, C. Faienza,D. Minciacchi, L. Lopez & L. Majno, pp. 527–30. The New York Academy ofSciences. [DLB]Sachs, J. (1987) Preschool boys’ <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> girls’ language use in pretend play. In:<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, gender, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sex in comparative <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>, ed. S. U. Philips,S. Steele & C. Tanz. Cambridge University Press. [aJLL]Sachs, J., Lieberman, P. & Ericks<strong>on</strong>, D. (1973) Anatomical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cultural determinantsof male <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> female speech. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> attitudes: Current trends <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>prospects, ed. R. W. Shuy & R. W. Fasold. Georgetown UniversityPress. [aJLL]Salm<strong>on</strong>d, A. (1975) Mana makes <strong>the</strong> man: A look at Maori oratory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> politics. In:Political language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> oratory in traditi<strong>on</strong>al society, ed. M. Bloch, pp. 45–63.Academic Press. [aJLL]Sameroff, A. J. & Haith, M. M., eds. (1996) The five to seven year shift: The age ofreas<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>sibility. University of Chicago Press. [aJLL]Samps<strong>on</strong>, B., Gabel, R. & Daniels, D. E. (2003) Relati<strong>on</strong>ship experiences of peoplewho stutter. Poster presented at <strong>the</strong> American Speech-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>-HearingAssociati<strong>on</strong>, Chicago. [aJLL]Sanches, M. & Kirshenblatt-Gimblett, B. (1976) Children’s traditi<strong>on</strong>al speech play<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> child language. In: Speech play: Research <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resources for studyinglinguistic creativity, ed. B. Kirshenblatt-Gimblett. University of PennsylvaniaPress. [aJLL]Sansavini, A, Bert<strong>on</strong>cini, J. & Giovanelli, G. (1997) Newborns discriminate <strong>the</strong>rhythm of multisyllabic stressed words. Developmental Psychology33:3–11. [KW]Santos-Granero, F. (1991) The power of love: The moral use of knowledge am<strong>on</strong>gst<strong>the</strong> Amuesha of Central Peru. Athl<strong>on</strong>e Press. [aJLL]Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. & Lewin, R. (1994) Kanzi: Ape at <strong>the</strong> brink of <strong>the</strong> humanmind. Wiley. [BJK]Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Williams, S. L., Furuichi, T. & Kano, T. (1996) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>perceived: Paniscus branches out. In: Great ape societies, ed. W. C. McGrew,L. F. Marchant & T. Nishida, pp. 173–84. Cambridge UniversityPress. [DLB]322 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Scamm<strong>on</strong>, R. E. (1930) The measurement of <strong>the</strong> body in childhood. In: Themeasurement of man, ed. J. A. Harris, C. M. Jacks<strong>on</strong>, D. G. Paters<strong>on</strong> & R. E.Scamm<strong>on</strong>, pp. 173–215. University of Minnesota Press. [aJLL]Scarr, S. & McCartney, K. (1983) How people make <strong>the</strong>ir own envir<strong>on</strong>ments:A <strong>the</strong>ory of genotype ^ envir<strong>on</strong>ment effects. Child Development54:424–35. [aJLL]Schaal, B., Tremblay, R. E., Soussignan, R. & Susman, E. J. (1996) Male testoster<strong>on</strong>elinked to high social dominance but low physical aggressi<strong>on</strong> in earlyadolescence. Journal of <strong>the</strong> American Academy of Child <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> AdolescentPsychiatry 34:1322–30. [aJLL]Schelletter, C. & Lein<strong>on</strong>en, E. (2003) Normal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language-impaired children’s useof reference: Syntactic versus pragmatic processing. Clinical Linguistics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Ph<strong>on</strong>etics 17:335–43. [aJLL]Scherer, K. R. (1979) Voice <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> speech correlates of perceived social influence insimulated juries. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social psychology, ed. H. Giles &R. St. Clair. Basil Blackwell. [aJLL]Schlegel, A. & Barry, H., III (1991) Adolescence: An anthropological inquiry. FreePress. [GEW]Schleidt, W. M. (1973) T<strong>on</strong>ic communicati<strong>on</strong>: C<strong>on</strong>tinual effects of discrete signs.Journal of Theoretical Biology 42:359–86. [WMS](1985) Learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> descripti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ment. In: Issues in <strong>the</strong> ecology oflearning, ed. T. D. Johnst<strong>on</strong> & A. T. Pietrewicz, pp. 305–25. Erlbaum. [WMS](1992) Biological bases of age specific behaviour – The compani<strong>on</strong>s in man’sworld. Evoluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Cogniti<strong>on</strong> 1:147–59. [WMS]Schleidt, W. M. & Crawley, J. N. (1980) Patterns in <strong>the</strong> behavior of organisms.Journal of Social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Biological Structure 3:1–15. [WMS]Schultz, A. H. (1960) Age changes in primates <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir modificati<strong>on</strong> in man. In:Human growth, ed. J. M. Tanner, pp. 1–20. Pergam<strong>on</strong> Press. [SR](1969) The <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> of primates. Universe Books. [SR]Searle, J. (1972) What is a speech act? In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social c<strong>on</strong>text: Selectedreadings, ed. P. P. Giglioli. Penguin Books. [aJLL]Seashore, R. H. & Eckers<strong>on</strong>, L. D. (1940) The measurement of individual differencesin general English vocabularies. Journal of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology31:14–38. [KK]Seeger, A. (1981) Nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> society in Central Brazil: The Suya Indians of MatoGrosso. Harvard University Press. [aJLL]Senghas, A. & Coppola, M. (2001) Children creating language: How NicaraguanSign <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science12:323–28. [DFB, aJLL]Senghas, A., Kita, S. & Ozyurek, A. (2004) Children creating core properties oflanguage: Evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science305:1779–82. [DFB, aJLL, SR]Seyfarth, R. M. (1976) Social relati<strong>on</strong>ships am<strong>on</strong>g adult female babo<strong>on</strong>s. AnimalBehaviour 24:917–38. [rJLL]Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. & Bergman, T. J. (2005) Primate social cogniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> origins of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9:264–66. [rJLL]Shanker, S. G. & King, B. J. (2002) The emergence of a <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g> paradigm in apelanguage research. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 25(5):605–20. [BJK]Shaw. H. L. (2000) Gaze directi<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>al interacti<strong>on</strong>s of chimpanzees.Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University of Nevada, Reno. [RAG]Shea, B. T. (1990) Dynamic morphology: Growth, <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> ecology inprimate evoluti<strong>on</strong>. In: Primate <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. J. C. DeRousseau,pp. 543–52. Wiley-Liss. [SR]Shears, L. M. & Jensema, C. J. (1969) Social acceptability of anomalous pers<strong>on</strong>s.Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Child 36:91–96. [aJLL]Sherzer, J. (1990) Verbal art in San Blas: Kuna culture through its discourse.Cambridge University Press. [aJLL](2002) Speech play <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal art. University of Texas Press. [aJLL]Short, J. F. & Strodtbeck, F. L. (1965) Group process <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> gang delinquency.University of Chicago Press. [rJLL]Short, R. V. (1976) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of human reproducti<strong>on</strong>. Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> RoyalSociety of L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> B 195:3–24. [aJLL]Shostak, M. (1981) Nisa: The <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> words of a !Kung woman. VintageBooks. [aJLL]Shriberg, L. D., Tomblin, J. B. & McSweeny, J. L. (1999) Prevalence of speechdelay in 6-year-old children <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> comorbidity with language impairment.Journal of Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research 42:1461–81. [aJLL]Shultz, T. R. & Horibe, F. (1974) Development of <strong>the</strong> appreciati<strong>on</strong> of verbal jokes.Developmental Psychology 10:13–20. [aJLL]Silk, J. (1978) Patterns of food sharing am<strong>on</strong>g mo<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infant chimpanzees atGombe Nati<strong>on</strong>al Park, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica 29:129–41. [GEW](1979) Feeding, foraging, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> food sharing behavior in immature chimpanzees.Folia Primatologica 31:12–42. [GEW]Silverman, E. (1976) Listeners’ impressi<strong>on</strong>s of speakers with lateral lisps. Journal ofSpeech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Disorders 41:547–52. [aJLL]Silverman, F. H. & Paulus, P. G. (1989) Peer reacti<strong>on</strong>s to teenagers whosubstitute /w/ for /r/. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Services in Schools20:219–21. [aJLL]Simkins-Bullock, J. A. & Wildman, B. G. (1991) An investigati<strong>on</strong> into <strong>the</strong>relati<strong>on</strong>ships between gender <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language. Sex Roles 24:149–60. [aJLL]Sim<strong>on</strong>s, E. L. (1989) Human origins. Science 245:1343–50. [aJLL]Smith B. H. (1991) Dental <strong>development</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> inhominidae. American Journal of Physical Anthropology86:157–74. [aJLL]Smith, B. H. & Tompkins, R. L. (1995) Toward a <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>the</strong> hominidae.Annual Revue of Anthropology 25:257–79. [aJLL]Smith, B. L., Brown, B. L., Str<strong>on</strong>g, W. J. & Rencher, A. C. (1975) Effects of speechrate <strong>on</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>ality percepti<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Speech 18:145–52. [aJLL]Smith, K. (2004) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of vocabulary. Journal of Theoretical Biology228:127–42. [HB]Smith, K. & Hurford, J. R. (2003) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong> in populati<strong>on</strong>s:Extending <strong>the</strong> iterated learning model. In: Advances in artificial lives:Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> 7th European C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> Artificial Life, ed.W. Banzhaf, T. Christaller, P. Dittrich, J. T. Kim & J. Ziegler, pp. 507–16.Springer. [HB]Snow, C. (1977) Mo<strong>the</strong>r’s speech research: From input to interacti<strong>on</strong>. In: Talking tochildren, ed. C. Snow & C. Fergus<strong>on</strong>, pp. 31–49. Cambridge UniversityPress. [RAG]Snow, C. A., Pan, B. A., Imbens-Bailey, A. & Herman, J. (1996) Learning to saywhat <strong>on</strong>e means: A l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study of children’s speech act use. SocialDevelopment 5:56–84. [aJLL]Snyder, M. (1987) Private realities/public appearances: The psychology ofself-m<strong>on</strong>itoring. Freeman. [rJLL]S<strong>on</strong>nenschein, S. (1988) The <strong>development</strong> of referential communicati<strong>on</strong>: Speakingto different listeners. Child Development 59:694–702. [aJLL]Sowell, E. R., Thomps<strong>on</strong>, P. M., Tessner, K. D. & Toga, A. W. (2001) Mappingc<strong>on</strong>tinued brain growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> gray matter density reducti<strong>on</strong> in dorsal fr<strong>on</strong>talcortex: Inverse relati<strong>on</strong>ships during postadolescent brain maturati<strong>on</strong>. Journalof Neuroscience 21:8819–29. [aJLL]Stamps, J. (2003) Behavioural processes affecting <strong>development</strong>: Tinbergen’s fourthquesti<strong>on</strong> comes of age. Animal Behaviour 66:1–13. [RAG]Stanford, C., Allen, J. S. & Antón, S. C. (2006) Biological anthropology. Pears<strong>on</strong>Educati<strong>on</strong>. [rJLL]Starin, E. D. (1978) Food transfer by wild titi m<strong>on</strong>keys (Callicebus torquatus torquatus).Folia Primatologica 30:145–51. [GEW]Starkwea<strong>the</strong>r, C. W. (1987) Fluency <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> stuttering. Prentice Hall. [aJLL]Stearns, S. (1976) Life-<str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> tactics: A review of <strong>the</strong> ideas. Quarterly Review ofBiology 51:3–47. [PK]Stearns, S. C. (1992) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> histories. Oxford UniversityPress. [rJLL]Steels, L. (1998) Syn<strong>the</strong>sising <strong>the</strong> origins of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning usingco-evoluti<strong>on</strong>, self-organisati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> level formati<strong>on</strong>. In: Approaches to <strong>the</strong>evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language, ed. J. R. Hurford, S.-K. Michael & C. Knight,pp. 384–404. Cambridge University Press. [DMWP]Stern, D. N. (1974) Mo<strong>the</strong>r <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infant at play: The dyadic interacti<strong>on</strong> involvingfacial, vocal, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> gaze behaviors. In: The effect of <strong>the</strong> infant <strong>on</strong> its caregiver,ed.M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum, pp. 187–213. Wiley. [DKO]Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B. & Kaplan, C. A.(1998) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journalof Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research 41:407–18. [aJLL]Stra<strong>the</strong>rn, A. (1971) The rope of moka: Big-men <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cerem<strong>on</strong>ial exchange in MountHagen, New Guinea. Cambridge University Press. [aJLL]Strayer, F. F. & Trudel, M. (1984) Developmental changes in <strong>the</strong> nature<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> functi<strong>on</strong> of social dominance am<strong>on</strong>g young children. Ethology <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Sociobiology 5:279–95. [aJLL]Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1991) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> from an evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary <str<strong>on</strong>g>perspective</str<strong>on</strong>g>.In: Biological <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavioral determinants of language <strong>development</strong>,ed. N. A. Krasnegor, D. M. Rumbaugh, R. L. Schiefelbusch & M. Studdert-Kennedy, pp. 5–28. Erlbaum. [LJG, aJLL](1998) The particulate origins of language generativity: From syllable to gesture.In: Approaches to <strong>the</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of language: Social <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive biases, ed.J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy & C. Knight, pp. 202–21. CambridgeUniversity Press. [arJLL](2005) How did language go discrete? In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> origins: Perspectives<strong>on</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. Tallerman, pp. 48–67. Oxford UniversityPress. [arJLL]Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Goldstein, L. (2003) Launching language: The gesturalorigin of discrete infinity. In: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. Christiansen &S. Kirby. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]Sutt<strong>on</strong>, J. (2004) Cognitive c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s of language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong>of autobiographical memory. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Communicati<strong>on</strong>22:375–90. [SJC]Tallerman, M., ed. (2005) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> origins: Perspectives <strong>on</strong> evoluti<strong>on</strong>. OxfordUniversity Press. [aJLL]Tambiah, S. J. (1983) The magical power of words. Man 3:175–208. [aJLL]Tanner, J. M. (1962) Growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescence, 2nd editi<strong>on</strong>. Blackwell. [aJLL]BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 323


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Tanner, J. M., Wils<strong>on</strong>, M. E. & Reedman, C. G. (1990) Pubertal growth in <strong>the</strong>female rhesus m<strong>on</strong>key: Relati<strong>on</strong> to menarche <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> skeletal maturati<strong>on</strong>.American Journal of Human Biology 2:101–106. [GEW]Tardieu, C. (1998) Short adolescence in early hominids: Infantile <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> adolescentgrowth of <strong>the</strong> human femur. American Journal of Physical Anthropology197:163–78. [aJLL]Taupin, P. & Gage, F. H. (2002) Adult neurogenesis <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> neural stem cells of <strong>the</strong>central nervous system in mammals. Journal of Neuroscience Research69:745–49. [aJLL]Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Grue<str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>ald, T. L., Gurung, R. A. R. &Updegraff, J. A. (2000) Biobehavioral resp<strong>on</strong>ses to stress in females:Tend-<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review107:411–29. [aJLL]Teleki, G. E., Hunt, E. & Pfifferling, J. H. (1976) Demographic observati<strong>on</strong>s(1963-1973) <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> chimpanzees of <strong>the</strong> Gombe Nati<strong>on</strong>al Park, Tanzania.Journal of Human Evoluti<strong>on</strong> 5:559–98. [aJLL]Temkin, O. trans. Soranus of Ephesus (1956) How to recognize <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>new</str<strong>on</strong>g>born that isworth rearing. In: Soranus’ gynecology. Johns Hopkins University Press. [rJLL]Thelen, E. & Smith, L. (1994) A dynamic systems approach to <strong>the</strong> <strong>development</strong> ofcogniti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> acti<strong>on</strong>. MIT Press. [LJG]Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1998) Dynamic systems <strong>the</strong>ories. In: H<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>book of childpsychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human <strong>development</strong>, 5th editi<strong>on</strong>, ed.R. M. Lerner, pp. 563–634. Wiley. [LJG]Thomas, M. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002) Are <strong>development</strong>al disorders like cases ofadult brain damage? Implicati<strong>on</strong>s from c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>ist modelling. Behavioral<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 25(6):727–50. [aJLL]Titze, I. (1989) Physiologic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> acoustic differences between male <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> femalevoices. Journal of <strong>the</strong> Acoustical Society of America 85:1699–707. [aJLL]Titze, I. R. (1994) Principles of voice producti<strong>on</strong>. Prentice Hall. [DLB]Tomasello, M. (1999) The cultural origins of human cogniti<strong>on</strong>. Harvard UniversityPress. [SJC, SR](2003) C<strong>on</strong>structing a language: A usage-based <strong>the</strong>ory of language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>.Harvard University Press. [SJC]Tomasello, M. & Call, J. (1997) Primate cogniti<strong>on</strong>. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005) Underst<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>ing<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sharing intenti<strong>on</strong>s: The origins of cultural cogniti<strong>on</strong>. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> BrainSciences 28:675–91. [SJ, aJLL]Tomasello, M., George, B., Kruger, A., Farrar, J. & Evans, E. (1985) The <strong>development</strong>of gestural communicati<strong>on</strong> in young chimpanzees. Journal of HumanEvoluti<strong>on</strong> 14:175–86. [aJLL]Tomasello, M., Gust, D. & Frost, G. T. (1989) The <strong>development</strong> of gesturalcommunicati<strong>on</strong> in young chimpanzees: A follow up. Primates 30:35–50. [aJLL]Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E. & O’Brien,M. (1997) Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergartenchildren. Journal of Speech, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research40:1245–60. [aJLL]Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L. & Zhang, X. (1996) A system for <strong>the</strong> diagnosis ofspecific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Hearing Research 39:1284–94. [aJLL]Trevar<strong>the</strong>n, C. (1979) Communicati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> cooperati<strong>on</strong> in early infancy. Adescripti<strong>on</strong> of primary intersubjectivity. In: Before speech: The beginnings ofhuman communicati<strong>on</strong>, ed. M. Bullowa, pp. 321–47. Cambridge UniversityPress. [DKO](1988) Universal co-operative motives: How infants begin to know <strong>the</strong> language<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> skills of <strong>the</strong> language of <strong>the</strong>ir parents. In: Acquiring culture, ed. G. Jahoda& I. Lewis, pp. 37–91. Croom Helm. [SJC]Trevathan, W. (1999) Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary obstetrics. In: Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary medicine, ed.W. Trevathan, E. O. Smith & J. J. McKenna, pp. 183–207. Oxford UniversityPress. [rJLL]Trivers, R. L. (1972) Parental investment <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sexual selecti<strong>on</strong>. In: Sexual selecti<strong>on</strong><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> descent of man, 1871–1971, ed. B. Campbell. Aldine. [aJLL](1974) Parent-offspring c<strong>on</strong>flict. American Zoologist 14:249–64. [aJLL]Tr<strong>on</strong>ick, E. Z. (1982) Social interchange in infancy. University Park Press. [DKO]Turt<strong>on</strong>, D. (1975) The relati<strong>on</strong>ship between oratory <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> emergence of influenceam<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Mursi. In: Political language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> oratory in traditi<strong>on</strong>al society, ed.M. Bloch. Academic Press. [aJLL]Udry, J. R. (1988) Biological predispositi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social c<strong>on</strong>trol in adolescent sexualbehavior. American Sociological Review 53:709–22. [aJLL]Ullstadius, E., Gustafss<strong>on</strong>, J-E. & Carlstedt, B. (2002) Influence of general <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>crystallized intelligence <strong>on</strong> vocabulary test performance. European Journalof Psychological Assessment 18:1015–23. [aJLL]Uzgiris, I. C., Bens<strong>on</strong>, J. B., Kruper, J. C. & Vasek, M. E. (1989) C<strong>on</strong>textualinfluences <strong>on</strong> imitative interacti<strong>on</strong>s between mo<strong>the</strong>rs <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> infants. In: Acti<strong>on</strong> ina social c<strong>on</strong>text: Perspectives <strong>on</strong> early <strong>development</strong>, ed. J. Lockman &N. Hazen. Plenum. [aJLL]van Doorn, G. S. & Weissing, F. J. (2004) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of female preferences formultiple indicati<strong>on</strong>s of quality. American Naturalist 164:173–86. [rJLL]van Goozen, S. H. M., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., Gooren, L. J. G., Frijda, N. H. & v<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>e Poll, N. E. (1994) Activating effects of <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>rogens <strong>on</strong> cognitive performance:Causal evidence in a group of female-to-male transexuals. Neuropsychologia32:1153–57. [aJLL]Van Lancker, D. (1987) N<strong>on</strong> propositi<strong>on</strong>al speech: Neurolinguistic studies. In:Progress in <strong>the</strong> psychology of language, vol. 3, ed. A. W. Ellis, pp. 49–118.Erlbaum. [KK](1990) The neurology of proverbs. Behavioural Neurology 3:169–87. [aJLL]van Schaik, C. (2004) Am<strong>on</strong>g orangutans: Red apes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> rise of human culture.Harvard University Press. [aJLL]van Schaik, C. P. & Hrdy, S. B. (1991) Maternal rank <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sex ratios at birth inm<strong>on</strong>keys: Local resource competiti<strong>on</strong> vs. <strong>the</strong> Trivers Willard effect. AmericanNaturalist 138:1555–62. [SR]Vargha-Khadem, F. (1990) Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language defects. Nature 346:226. [aJLL]Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K., Alcock, K., Fletcher, P. & Passingham, R. (1995)Praxic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>verbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a geneticallytransmitted speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language disorder. Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>alAcademy of Science USA 92:930–33. [aJLL, LW]Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K. E., Price, C. J., Ashburner, J., Alcock, K. J.,C<strong>on</strong>nelly, A., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Frist<strong>on</strong>, K. J., Pembrey, M. E., Mishkin, M.,Gadian, D. G. & Passingham, R. (1998) Neural basis of an inherited speech<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language disorder. Proceedings of <strong>the</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Science USA95:12695–700. [aJLL, LW]Vavra, H. M. & Querec, L. J. (1973) A study of infant mortality from linked recordsby age of mo<strong>the</strong>r, total-birth order, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r variables. DHEW Publicati<strong>on</strong>No. (HRA). 74-1851. U. S. Government Printing Office. [aJLL]Vetterli, C. F. & Furedy, J. J. (1997) Correlates of intelligence in computermeasured aspects of prose vocabulary: Word length, diversity, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> rarity.Pers<strong>on</strong>ality <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Individual Differences 22:933–35. [aJLL]Vihman, M. M. (1996) Ph<strong>on</strong>ological <strong>development</strong>: The origins of language in <strong>the</strong>child. Blackwell. [aJLL]Vuorenkoski, V., Lenko, H. L., Tjernlund, P., Vuorenkoski, L. & Perheentupa,J. (1978) Fundamental voice frequency during normal <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> abnormal growth,<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> after <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>rogen treatment. Archives of Disease in Childhood53:201–209. [aJLL]Waldstein, R. S. (1990) Effects of postlingual deafness <strong>on</strong> speech producti<strong>on</strong>:Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong> role of auditory feedback. Journal of <strong>the</strong> Acoustical Societyof America 88:2099–114. [aJLL]Walrath, D. (2003) Rethinking pelvic typologies <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> human birth mechanism.Current Anthropology 44:5–31. [rJLL]Walter, B. (1988) The jury summati<strong>on</strong> as speech genre: An ethnographic study ofwhat it means to those who use it. John Benjamins. [aJLL]Washabaugh, W. (1986) Five fingers for survival. Karoma. [SR]Washburn, S. L. (1960) Tools <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> human evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Scientific American203:63–75. [aJLL]Watkins, K. E., Dr<strong>on</strong>kers, N. F. & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2002) Behavioural analysisof an inherited speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language disorder: Comparis<strong>on</strong> with acquiredaphasia. Brain 125:452–64. [aJLL]Wats<strong>on</strong>, J. S. (1972) Smiling, cooing, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> “<strong>the</strong> game.” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly18:323–39. [aJLL](2001) C<strong>on</strong>tingency percepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> mispercepti<strong>on</strong> in infancy: Some potentialimplicati<strong>on</strong>s for attachment. Bulletin of <strong>the</strong> Menninger Clinic65296–320. [aJLL]Watts, E. S. (1985) Adolescent growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> of m<strong>on</strong>keys, apes <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>humans. In: N<strong>on</strong>human primate models for human growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>,ed. E. S. Watts, pp. 41–65. Alan R. Liss. [SR](1986) The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>the</strong> human growth curve. In: Human growth: A comprehensivetreatise, vol. I, 2 nd editi<strong>on</strong>, ed. F. Falkner & J. M. Tanner, pp.153–66. Plenum. [SR](1990) Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary trends in primate growth <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong>. In: Primate<str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>, ed. J. C. DeRousseau, pp. 89–104.Wiley-Liss. [SR]Watts, E. S. & Gavan J. A. (1982) Postnatal growth of n<strong>on</strong>human primates: Theproblem of adolescent spurt. Human Biology 54(1):53–70. [rJLL, SR]Weisner, T. S. (1987) Socializati<strong>on</strong> for parenthood in sibling caretaking societies. In:Parenting across <strong>the</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> span: Biosocial dimensi<strong>on</strong>s, ed. J. B. Lancaster,J. Altmann, A. S. Rossi & L. R. Sherrod, pp. 237–79. Aldine deGruyter. [aJLL](1996) The 5-7 transiti<strong>on</strong> as an ecocultural project. In: The five to seven yearshift: The age of reas<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>sibility, ed. A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith.University of Chicago Press. [aJLL]Werker, J. F. & Curtin, S. (2005) PRIMIR: A <strong>development</strong>al framework of infantspeech processing. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Learning <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Development 1:197–234. [aJLL]Wermke, K. (2002) Untersuchung der Melodieentwicklung im Säuglingsschrei v<strong>on</strong>m<strong>on</strong>ozygoten Zwillingen in den ersten 5 Lebensm<strong>on</strong>aten. [Investigati<strong>on</strong> ofcry melody <strong>development</strong> of m<strong>on</strong>ozygotic twins within <strong>the</strong> first 5 m<strong>on</strong>ths of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>.]Professorial dissertati<strong>on</strong> (Habilitati<strong>on</strong>): Humboldt-University of Berlin.Available at: http://edoc.hu-berlin.de. [KW]324 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3


References/Locke & Bogin: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>Wermke, K. & Friederici, A. F. (2004) Developmental changes of infantcries – The evoluti<strong>on</strong> of complex vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences27:474–75. [Commentary <strong>on</strong> Soltis, J. (2004) The signal functi<strong>on</strong>s of earlyinfant crying. Behavioral <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brain Sciences 27(4):443–458.] [KW]Wermke, K., Leising, D. & Stellzig-Eisenhauer, A. (submitted) Relati<strong>on</strong> of melodycomplexity in infants’ cries to language outcome in <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d year of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>:A l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study. [KW]Wermke, K., Mende, W., Borschberg, H. & Ruppert, R. (1996) Voice characteristicsof prespeech vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s of twins during <strong>the</strong> first year of <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g>. In:Pathologies of speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> language: C<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s of clinical ph<strong>on</strong>etics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>linguistics, ed. T. W. Powell, pp. 1–8. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Clinical Ph<strong>on</strong>etics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Linguistics Associati<strong>on</strong>. [KW]Wermke, K., Mende, W. & Friederici, A. (in preparati<strong>on</strong>) Melodies at <strong>the</strong> roots oflanguage. [KW]Wermke, K., Mende, W., Kempf, A., Manfredi, C., Bruscagli<strong>on</strong>i, P. & Stellzig-Eisenhauer, A. (2005) Interacti<strong>on</strong> patterns between melodies <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> res<strong>on</strong>ancefrequencies in infants’ pre-speech utterances. In: Models <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> analysis of vocalemissi<strong>on</strong>s for biomedical applicati<strong>on</strong>s: 4th Internati<strong>on</strong>al Workshop, Firenze,October 29–31, 2005, ed. C. Manfredi, pp. 187–90. Firenze UniversityPress. [KW]Wermke, K., Mende, W., Manfredi, C. & Bruscagli<strong>on</strong>i, P. (2002) Developmentalaspects of infant’s cry melody <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> formants. Medical Engineering <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Physics24:501–14. [KW]West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003) Developmental plasticity <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong>. OxfordUniversity Press. [DFB, rJLL]We<strong>the</strong>rby, A. M., Cain, D. H., Y<strong>on</strong>clas, D. G. & Walker, V. G. (1988) Analysis ofintenti<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong> of normal children from <strong>the</strong> pre-linguistic to <strong>the</strong>multi-word stage. Journal of Speech <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hearing Research31:240–52. [aJLL]Wheeler, M. (2004) Is language <strong>the</strong> ultimate artifact? <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sciences26(6):693–715. [SJC]Whitmire, K. A. (2000) Adolescence as a <strong>development</strong>al phase: A tutorial.Topics in <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> Disorders 20:1–14. [aJLL]Wickham, C. (1998) Gossip <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> resistance am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> medieval peasantry. Past <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Present 160:3–24. [rJLL]Willshaw, D. J. & v<strong>on</strong> der Malsburg, C. (1979) A marker inducti<strong>on</strong> mechanism for<strong>the</strong> establishment of ordered neural mappings: Its applicati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> retinotectalproblem. Philosophical Transacti<strong>on</strong>s of <strong>the</strong> Royal Society of L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong> B:Biological Sciences 287:203–43. [DMWP]Wils<strong>on</strong>, E. O. (1980) Sociobiology. Harvard University Press. [rJLL]Wils<strong>on</strong>, P. J. (1969) Reputati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> respectability: A suggesti<strong>on</strong> for Caribbeanethnology. Man 4:70–84. [aJLL]Wolf, D. & Gardner, H. (1979) Style <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> sequence in early symbolic play.In: Symbolic functi<strong>on</strong>ing in childhood, ed. N. Smith & M. Franklin, pp.117–19. Erlbaum. [rJLL]W<strong>on</strong>g Fillmore, L. (1976) The sec<strong>on</strong>d time around: Cognitive <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> social strategiesin sec<strong>on</strong>d language learning. Unpublished Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, StanfordUniversity. [KK]Workman, L. & Reader, W. (2004) Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary psychology. Cambridge UniversityPress. [LW]Worthman, C. M. (1993) Biocultural interacti<strong>on</strong>s in human <strong>development</strong>.In: Juvenile primates: Life <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g>, <strong>development</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior, ed. M. E.Pereira & L. A. Fairbanks, pp. 339–58. Oxford University Press. [GEW]Wrangham, R. E., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M. & Heltne, P. G., eds. (1996)Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press. [aJLL]Wray, A. (1998) Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interacti<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g><str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Communicati<strong>on</strong> 18:47–67. [KK](2002) Formulaic language <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> lexic<strong>on</strong>. Cambridge University Press. [KK]Wray, A., ed. (2002) The transiti<strong>on</strong> to language. Oxford University Press. [aJLL]Wuyts, F. L., Heylen, L., Mertens, F., Du Caju, M., Rooman, R., Van de Heyning,P. H. & De Bodt, M. (2003) Effects of age, sex, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> disorder <strong>on</strong> voice rangeprofile characteristics of 230 children. Annals of Otology, Rhinology, <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>Laryngology 112(6):540–48. [aJLL]Wyatt, T. A. (1995) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Language</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>development</strong> in African American English childspeech. Linguistics <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> Educati<strong>on</strong> 7:7–22. [rJLL](1999) An Afro-centered view of communicative competence.In: C<strong>on</strong>structing incompetence: Disabling evaluati<strong>on</strong>s in clinical <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> socialinteracti<strong>on</strong>, ed. D. Kovarsky, J. F. Duchan & M. Maxwell, pp. 197–221.Erlbaum. [rJLL]Zahavi, A. & A. Zahavi (1997) The h<str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g>icap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’spuzzle. Oxford University Press. [CK]Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E. & Gjedde, A. (1992) Lateralizati<strong>on</strong> ofph<strong>on</strong>etic <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> pitch discriminati<strong>on</strong> in speech precessing. Science256:846–49. [DLB]Zihlman, A., Bolter, D. & Boesch, C. (2004) Wild chimpanzee dentiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>and</str<strong>on</strong>g> itsimplicati<strong>on</strong>s for assessing <str<strong>on</strong>g>life</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>history</str<strong>on</strong>g> in immature hominin fossils. Proceedingsof <strong>the</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Sciences USA 101(29):10541–43. [aJLL]Zlatev, J., Perss<strong>on</strong>, T. & Gärdenfors, P. (2005) Bodily mimesis as “<strong>the</strong> missing link”in human cognitive evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Lund University Cognitive Studies,Vol. 121. [Entire volume.] [SJ]BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2006) 29:3 325

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!