11.07.2015 Views

Priming in Recognition Memory for Categorized Lists - American ...

Priming in Recognition Memory for Categorized Lists - American ...

Priming in Recognition Memory for Categorized Lists - American ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

RECOGNITION PRIMING 567Table 2Average Reaction Times (<strong>in</strong> Milliseconds) and Accuracies <strong>for</strong>Critical Probes <strong>in</strong> Experiment las a Function of CategoryStatus and of List Membership of the PrimesCategory of primeSameMeanRTConditional accuracyUnconditional accuracyDifferentMeanRTConditional accuracyUnconditional accuracyExtra*740.77.90786.79.90List status of critical probeTargetIntra"696.79.90742.78.90Extra"825.77.87866.75.86LureIntra'817.75.86837.74.86Note. Only correct responses to both prime and critical probe are <strong>in</strong>cluded<strong>in</strong> the RT measure, and <strong>in</strong>dividual observations underly<strong>in</strong>g eachsubject's mean are log-trans<strong>for</strong>med, with values exceed<strong>in</strong>g 2,000 ms notenter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the analysis. Means across subjects are based on antilogsof each <strong>in</strong>dividual's mean. Trimmed observations count as conditionalerrors, but are not part of the unconditional error measure.1 List status of prime.merit 1 that Neely et al. mentioned as a possible cause <strong>for</strong> theepisodic prim<strong>in</strong>g effect; namely, that lures were responded tomore slowly than targets. Episodically primed test items werethus always preceded by a faster response than test items thatwere not episodically primed. If that RT difference resultedfrom a difference <strong>in</strong> difficulty, this would be cause <strong>for</strong> concernbecause difficult items often exert an effect on subsequent easyitems <strong>in</strong> a test sequence (Kiger & Glass, 1981). However, giventhat response latencies to targets and lures can often be identical(e.g., Sternberg, 1966), it seems more reasonable to use accuracyas an <strong>in</strong>dex of difficulty of respond<strong>in</strong>g. Accuracy did notdiffer between lures and targets serv<strong>in</strong>g as primes (.863 and.870, respectively). Given the absence of any differences there,one should conclude that the episodic prim<strong>in</strong>g effect did notresult from a difference <strong>in</strong> difficulty <strong>in</strong> respond<strong>in</strong>g to theprimes.Overall, then, Experiment I showed that a facilitative semanticprim<strong>in</strong>g effect can, under some conditions at least, obta<strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong> both targets and lures. The task now was to del<strong>in</strong>eate theconditions under which <strong>in</strong>hibition is obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>for</strong> lure process<strong>in</strong>g.In particular, the next step was to produce an <strong>in</strong>hibition oflure process<strong>in</strong>g while controll<strong>in</strong>g retention <strong>in</strong>terval.Experiment 2This experiment was designed to test the type of prime process<strong>in</strong>ghypothesis. If the type of process<strong>in</strong>g were the criticaldeterm<strong>in</strong>ant of the effects of semantic prim<strong>in</strong>g, Experiment 2should show an <strong>in</strong>hibition <strong>for</strong> rejection of lures, because theprimes used here were similar to the ones used by Macht andO'Brien (1980). Whereas they used a sentence-verification taskDIFFERENT CATEGORY PRIMESAME CATEGORY PRIMEEXTRA INTRA EXTRALIST STATUS OF PRIMEFigure 1. Obta<strong>in</strong>ed reaction times <strong>for</strong> all conditions <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1.INTRA


RECOGNITION PRIMING 569Table 4Average Reaction Times (<strong>in</strong> Milliseconds) and Accuracies <strong>for</strong>Critical Probes <strong>in</strong> Experiment 2 as a Functionof the Category Status of the PrimeCategory of primeSameMeanRTConditional accuracyUnconditional accuracyDifferentMeanRTConditional accuracyUnconditional accuracyList status of criticalprobeTarget722.81.93771.80.90Lure948.73.86905.77.88Note. Only correct responses to both prime and critical item are <strong>in</strong>cluded<strong>in</strong> the RT measure, and <strong>in</strong>dividual observations underly<strong>in</strong>g eachsubject's mean are log-trans<strong>for</strong>med, with values exceed<strong>in</strong>g 2,000 ms notenter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the analysis. Means across subjects are based on antilogsof each <strong>in</strong>dividual's mean. Trimmed observations count as conditionalerrors, but are not part of the unconditional error measure.with<strong>in</strong>-subjects analysis. Overall conditional accuracy level wasat 80%. As <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1, unconditional error rates were thesame across both prim<strong>in</strong>g conditions <strong>for</strong> targets (.08) and lures(.13). No analyses were per<strong>for</strong>med on this measure becausemeans did not differ with<strong>in</strong> a response class. Table 4 presentsthe means <strong>for</strong> both conditional and unconditional accuracyscores.Response latency. Dur<strong>in</strong>g computation of each subject'smean, the same cutoff (2,000 ms) was used as <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1.This resulted <strong>in</strong> the loss of 2% of the observations. The 2X2ANOVA on RT means revealed that lures were responded tomore slowly (926.7 ms) than targets (746.2 ms), with F{ 1,15) =72.46, MS e = 7,195. Furthermore, the <strong>in</strong>teraction betweenprime category and list status of the critical item was also foundto be significant, F{1, 15)= 1\.26,MS C = 3,010, whereas therewas no ma<strong>in</strong> effect of category of prime, F{\, 15) < 1. Figure 2and Table 4 display the RT means underly<strong>in</strong>g this result. A posthoc exploration of the means us<strong>in</strong>g a Duncan's multiple rangetest <strong>in</strong>dicated that, as would be predicted by the type of primeprocess<strong>in</strong>g hypothesis, a same-category prime facilitated thesubsequent verification of a positive probe, whereas it <strong>in</strong>hibited(<strong>in</strong> comparison to a different-category prime) the rejection of asubsequent lure.DiscussionTo summarize, Experiments 1 and 2 both used the same listlength and almost identical retention <strong>in</strong>tervals. Yet the resultswere quite different, depend<strong>in</strong>g on what type of prime was used.Consequently, retention <strong>in</strong>terval could not have contributed tothe difference <strong>in</strong> outcomes. At the same time, the data re<strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>cethe presence of two discrepant outcomes <strong>in</strong> the recognitionprim<strong>in</strong>g area. These patterns have now been shown to be replicableunder a variety of conditions. In particular, Experiment 2presents the first demonstration of a semantic prim<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>hibitionat longer retention <strong>in</strong>tervals.Experiment 2 also provided the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of an explanation<strong>for</strong> these discrepant outcomes, based on the type of process<strong>in</strong>grequired <strong>for</strong> the prime. The task used to achieve prim<strong>in</strong>g hasbeen clearly established as a critical determ<strong>in</strong>ant of what happensto process<strong>in</strong>g of lures: A semantic and associative task resulted<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>hibition (Experiment 2), whereas an episodic task(Experiment 1) resulted <strong>in</strong> facilitation. It is not clear yet, however,whether the prim<strong>in</strong>g task must be associative <strong>in</strong> nature, <strong>in</strong>addition to be<strong>in</strong>g semantic, to produce an <strong>in</strong>hibition of lureprocess<strong>in</strong>g. Experiments 3 and 4 were designed to <strong>in</strong>vestigatethe effects of an item-predicated yet semantic prim<strong>in</strong>g task.Experiments 3 and 4Experiments 3 and 4 observed prim<strong>in</strong>g as a function of anitem-predicated semantic task, by requir<strong>in</strong>g a lexical-decisiontask to be per<strong>for</strong>med on the prime. The lexical-decision taskcorresponds most closely to a s<strong>in</strong>gle item episodic recognitiontask as it was used <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1. The only difference betweenExperiments 3 and 4, <strong>in</strong> turn, was the length of the study list.Experiment 3: MethodSubjects and arrangement of materials. Twenty-two subjects participated<strong>in</strong> Experiment 3. Each category <strong>in</strong> the <strong>Categorized</strong> Word Poolwas randomly split <strong>in</strong>to two sets (A and B) of 16 items each. Identity ofSets A and B was alternated <strong>for</strong> every second subject. Eight items wererandomly sampled from the respective Set A to <strong>for</strong>m one category onthe study list, and eight items were selected from Set B to provide lures<strong>for</strong> that category on the test list. Categories were randomly allocated totrials.For the test list, four nonstudied items from Set A were taken to functionas word (experimental) primes <strong>for</strong> that given category. Pseudowordprimes were obta<strong>in</strong>ed by chang<strong>in</strong>g the spell<strong>in</strong>g of four members of SetB of that category such that they became pseudowords. A subject wouldnever see items from Set B and their pseudoword derivations together<strong>in</strong> the experiment. These pseudowords obeyed English spell<strong>in</strong>g rulesand were pronounceable. They also ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed a resemblance to items1000 TARGETDIFFERENTCATEGORY PRIMESAMECATEGORY PRIMECRITICAL ITEMLUREFigure 2. Obta<strong>in</strong>ed reaction times <strong>for</strong> all conditions <strong>in</strong> Experiment 2.


570 STEPHAN LEWANDOWSKYTable 5Examples of a Study List and a Test List Used<strong>in</strong> Experiments 3 and 4TOOLSJIGSAWBOLTSTest list pairword? HAMMERBOLTSword? WRENCHPSYCHOLOGYword? GEOGRAPHYASTRONOMYword ? AGRONOMYSCREWSStudy listTest listCategoryof primesamedifferentsamedifferentSCIENCESPSYCHOLOGYTOPOGRAPHYList status ofcritical probetargettargetlurelureNote. One example is given <strong>for</strong> each experimental condition. Fillerprobes and nonexperimental observations (i.e., pseudoword primes) areomitted. Right-hand columns specify levels of experimental variables<strong>for</strong> prime-critical probe pair on the left.from Set B of the category, which pilot work had shown to be necessary<strong>in</strong> order to obta<strong>in</strong> a robust prim<strong>in</strong>g effect. For example, pseudowords <strong>for</strong>the category PROFESSIONS were items such as MURCHENT, ARCHITOCT,JANETUR, and the like. Highly similar pseudowords have been used be<strong>for</strong>e<strong>in</strong> prim<strong>in</strong>g research (cf. Antos, 1979;Lupker, 1984), and they presentan extension of other f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (Shulman & Davison, 1977; Smith etal., 1983) that prim<strong>in</strong>g effects are magnified if pseudowords are usedthat are more similar to real words. For the subsequent discussion, referto Table 5 <strong>for</strong> an example of the k<strong>in</strong>d of stimulus arrangement used <strong>in</strong>Experiments 3 and 4.<strong>Recognition</strong> test items were distributed as follows: Half of all studyitems of a given category and half of the correspond<strong>in</strong>g set of lures wererandomly designated to serve as fillerprobes. A fillerwas the firstmemberof each of the test triplets, similar to Experiment 2. Assign men I offiller items to position on the test list was random. Half of the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>grecognition test items (two lures, two targets from each category) wereassigned to follow word (experimental) primes such that each experimentalcondition was represented once <strong>for</strong> each category. Critical responseswere observed only <strong>for</strong> those probes that followed word primes.As <strong>in</strong> Experiments 1 and 2, critical probes Owes or targets) were takeneither from the same category as the prime, or from the one differentcategory randomly yoked to it. Consequently, a given category item presentedas a prime was equally likely to be followed by a probe from thesame or from the different category. Similarly, because half of the criticalitems from a given category were targets and the others were lures,probability of a yes versus a no response follow<strong>in</strong>g a word prime was alsoheld equal. F<strong>in</strong>ally, nonexperimental primes (i.e., pseudowords) werefollowed by recognition test items whose properties were the same asthose follow<strong>in</strong>g experimental primes. <strong>Recognition</strong> responses to probesfollow<strong>in</strong>g pseudowords were of no <strong>in</strong>terest. Primes and the probes follow<strong>in</strong>gthem were distributed throughout the test list <strong>in</strong> a random order.In summary, each category provided four critical probes (one <strong>in</strong> eachcondition) and also four word primes, yield<strong>in</strong>g four observations percondition per trial per subject, or a total of 32 per subject <strong>for</strong> each condition.Procedure. Instructions emphasized both accuracy and latency.The study list was presented <strong>in</strong> the same fashion as <strong>in</strong> the preced<strong>in</strong>gexperiments. Each triplet <strong>in</strong> the test sequence was shown as follows: Thefiller (a recognition test item) appeared on the screen and was, follow<strong>in</strong>grespond<strong>in</strong>g, replaced 400 ms later by the prime (a lexical-decision item).The prime was pr<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>verse video (i.e., dark green on a light greenbackground) with the prompt "word?" to the left of it. Upon per<strong>for</strong>m<strong>in</strong>gthe yes/no lexical decision by us<strong>in</strong>g the usual response keys, theprime disappeared and was replaced 400 ms later by the critical probe,which was pr<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong> normal video (i.e., bright green on a dark background).Aga<strong>in</strong>, follow<strong>in</strong>g respond<strong>in</strong>g, the next triplet was presented400 ms later.Prior to onset of the experimental procedure, subjects received apractice trial consist<strong>in</strong>g of 10 study items (the same across all subjects,taken from the Toronto Word Pool; Friendly, Frankl<strong>in</strong>, Hoffman, & Rub<strong>in</strong>,1982), followed by a test sequence of 20 triplets similar <strong>in</strong> setup tothe experimental trials.Experiment 3: ResultsAccuracy. The 2X2 with<strong>in</strong>-subjects analysis of conditionalaccuracy showed that there was a significant ma<strong>in</strong> effect of liststatus of the critical probe (targets: .80 vs. lures: .67), with F(l,21) = 40.7, MS e = .0094. In addition, the analysis uncovered aslight trend toward more accurate respond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the same categorycondition than <strong>in</strong> the different category condition, F{ 1,21) = 3.13,p


RECOGNITION PRIMING 571Table 6Average Reaction Times (<strong>in</strong> Milliseconds) and Accuracies <strong>for</strong>Critical Probes <strong>in</strong> Experiment 3 as a Functionof the Category Status of the PrimeCategory of primeSameMeanRTConditional accuracyUnconditional accuracyDifferentMeanRTConditional accuracyUnconditional accuracyList status of criticalprobeTarget884.78.93925.82.94Lure1256.65.781223.68.81Note. Only correct responses to both prime and critical probe are <strong>in</strong>cluded<strong>in</strong> the RT measure, and <strong>in</strong>dividual observations underly<strong>in</strong>g eachsubject's mean are log-trans<strong>for</strong>med, with values exceed<strong>in</strong>g 3,500 ms notenter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the analysis. Means across subjects are based on antilogsof each <strong>in</strong>dividual's mean. Trimmed observations count as conditionalerrors, but are not part of the unconditional error measure.rately than targets, F(\, 19) = 16.11, MS C = .0161. Contrary toExperiment 3, however, there was no <strong>in</strong>dication of an effect ofthe semantic prim<strong>in</strong>g manipulation. The grand mean of conditionalaccuracy was 77%, with a value of 0.83 <strong>for</strong> targets, and0.71 <strong>for</strong> lures, respectively. A similar difference between levelsof the list status variable was obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>for</strong> unconditional errors.Targets (.06 errors) and lures (.18) were responded to with thesame degree of accuracy regardless of prim<strong>in</strong>g condition. Table7 depicts the accuracy measures and also the RT means.Response latency. The trimm<strong>in</strong>g scheme was the same asthat used <strong>in</strong> Experiment 3 (3,500-ms cutoff) result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> theloss of 1% of the observations. As <strong>in</strong> Experiment 3, critical lureswere responded to more slowly (1067.6 ms) than targets (839.3ms), with F{U 19) = 85.22, MS e = 12,233. The category of theprime had no effect with F{ 1, 19) = 2.39, MS* = 1,618. Instead,the <strong>in</strong>teraction was aga<strong>in</strong> significant as shown by an F( 1, 19) =4.41, MS C = 3,932.Contrary to Experiment 3, the <strong>in</strong>hibition of lure process<strong>in</strong>gwas the stronger effect this time, and it was significant us<strong>in</strong>g aDuncan's test. The facilitation of target verification, on theother hand, was reduced to a nonsignificant difference.DiscussionBoth Experiment 3 and Experiment 4 produced an <strong>in</strong>teractionbetween the list status of the critical probe and the prim<strong>in</strong>gcondition. Target verification was facilitated by a prior lexicaldecision on a word from the same category, whereas process<strong>in</strong>gof lures was <strong>in</strong>hibited. Although neither experiment on its ownwas characterized by a great statistical robustness, the patternof the <strong>in</strong>teraction was replicated across both studies and bothretention <strong>in</strong>tervals. The experiments showed that prim<strong>in</strong>gthrough a semantic task, even if that task is not associative, <strong>in</strong>hibitslure process<strong>in</strong>g.This effect could not have been the result of the mere presenceof pseudowords <strong>in</strong> Experiments 3 and 4, because Experiment2 did not conta<strong>in</strong> any pseudowords and yet showed thesame pattern of results. Similarly, an explanation <strong>for</strong> the resultsof Experiments 3 and 4 based on work by Balota and Chumbley(1984) can be ruled out. Balota and Chumbley suggested thatlexical decision may also <strong>in</strong>volve an assessment of the familiarityof an item, similar <strong>in</strong> spirit to the Atk<strong>in</strong>son and Juola (1973,1974) recognition model. Thus, <strong>in</strong> Experiments 3 and 4, subjectscould have generalized such a familiarity-based strategyfrom the lexical-decision primes to the recognition probes.Such a strategy would of course predict <strong>in</strong>hibition of lure process<strong>in</strong>g.But this explanation can be ruled out, given that Experiment2 <strong>in</strong>volved a category membership judgment that, aga<strong>in</strong>accord<strong>in</strong>g to Balota and Chumbley, does not <strong>in</strong>volve a familiaritycheck.General DiscussionBe<strong>for</strong>e one can discuss the implications of the results of thefour present experiments, several potential causes <strong>for</strong> concernand alternative explanations must be dealt with. First, it mustbe ensured that per<strong>for</strong>mance on the primes themselves did notcovary with per<strong>for</strong>mance on the critical items. Ideally, givenproper randomization, reaction time and accuracy of responsesto primes should not differ at all between conditions with<strong>in</strong> anexperiment. Indeed, the observed variations between meanswere slight: For Experiments 1-4, the largest difference <strong>in</strong> reactiontime (computed with the appropriate trimm<strong>in</strong>g scheme)between primes <strong>for</strong> the different conditions was 20, 23, 16, and20 ms, respectively. The correspond<strong>in</strong>g maximum differencesper experiment <strong>in</strong> accuracy were 1%, 5%, 3%, and 3%, respectively.Of the 32 F tests <strong>in</strong> ANOVAS that were per<strong>for</strong>med on theprime data <strong>for</strong> all experiments, only one was found to be unexpectedlysignificant. (In addition, lure primes <strong>in</strong> Experiment Iwere responded to more slowly than target primes, but this isTable 7Average Reaction Times (<strong>in</strong> Milliseconds) and Accuracies <strong>for</strong>Critical Probes <strong>in</strong> Experiment 4 as a Functionof the Category Status of the PrimeCategory of primeSameMean RTConditional accuracyUnconditional accuracyDifferentMeanRTConditional accuracyUnconditional accuracyList status of criticalprobeTarget831.82.94847.83.96Lure1089.72.821046.70.83Note. Only correct responses to both prime and critical probe are <strong>in</strong>cluded<strong>in</strong> the RT measure, and <strong>in</strong>dividual observations underly<strong>in</strong>g eachsubject's mean are log-trans<strong>for</strong>med, with values exceed<strong>in</strong>g 3,500 ms notenter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the analysis. Means across subjects are based on antilogsof the <strong>in</strong>dividual's mean. Trimmed observations count as conditionalerrors, but are not part of the unconditional error measure.


572 STEPHAN LEW\NDOWSKYwhat is often found <strong>in</strong> a recognition experiment, and is there<strong>for</strong>enot unexpected). Consequently, differences <strong>in</strong> per<strong>for</strong>manceon the primes could not have contributed to the results.Second, the overall per<strong>for</strong>mance level on the primes acrossexperiments must also be close to <strong>in</strong>variant <strong>in</strong> order to ensurethat primes were of roughly equal difficulty. Indeed, the grandmeans of accuracy did not vary much across studies, with valuesof 86%, 80%, 85%, and 86% <strong>for</strong> Experiments 1-4, respectively.Note how primes <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1 were responded towith precisely the same accuracy as those used <strong>in</strong> Experiment4. Yet, those two experiments uncovered widely different patternsof results.Third, <strong>in</strong> all experiments that showed an <strong>in</strong>hibition <strong>for</strong> therejection of lures, subjects had to switch from one task (categoryjudgment or lexical decision) to another (recognition)when respond<strong>in</strong>g to prime and critical item, respectively. Contraryto that, <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1, which obta<strong>in</strong>ed facilitation, subjectsper<strong>for</strong>med the same task on the prime and on the criticalitem. It is thus conceivable that this difference between experimentswas responsible <strong>for</strong> the different outcomes. However, thisappears unlikely, because respond<strong>in</strong>g to targets was facilitatedthrough semantic prim<strong>in</strong>g regardless of whether the prim<strong>in</strong>gtask was the same as the recognition task. It is implausible toassume that task switch<strong>in</strong>g was responsible <strong>for</strong> drastically alter<strong>in</strong>gresponses to critical lures, given that it had no effect on targetverification. Furthermore, I have conducted several otherunpublished experiments that required task switch<strong>in</strong>g betweenprime and critical probe, but that nevertheless demonstrated afacilitation <strong>for</strong> lures. Taken together, these two po<strong>in</strong>ts shouldallay any fears that the present results were an artifact of thetask switch<strong>in</strong>g required <strong>in</strong> Experiments 2-4 but not <strong>in</strong> Experiment1.The fourth and last issue concerns another proceduraldifference between Experiment 1 on one hand and Experiments2-4 on the other hand. None of the primes used <strong>in</strong> the last threeexperiments had been presented to subjects at study. Contraryto that, half the primes used <strong>in</strong> Experiment 1 had been studied.The reason <strong>for</strong> this procedural difference between experimentslies <strong>in</strong> the essence of the task dimension that was manipulated.An episodic task can only be per<strong>for</strong>med after a prior studyphase, whereas a semantic task can be (and usually is) per<strong>for</strong>medon novel items that subjects have not seen previously <strong>in</strong>the same experiment. However, it is, <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, possible toper<strong>for</strong>m a semantic judgment on an item that has been studiedpreviously (e.g., Neely & Durgunoglu, 1985). The present setof experiments cannot serve to elucidate that special case ofepisodically studied primes that require semantic responses.Thus, <strong>for</strong> the summary below, it must be kept <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d thatprimes requir<strong>in</strong>g episodic and semantic responses always <strong>in</strong>volvedstudied and novel items, respectively.Summary of ResultsThe results from all four experiments can then be summarizedas follows: If a purely semantic decision is required <strong>for</strong> theprime, subsequent process<strong>in</strong>g of negative recognition probes is<strong>in</strong>hibited. This is the case regardless of whether the semanticjudgment is one that is predom<strong>in</strong>antly item predicated or onethat requires associative <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation. If, on the other hand, aprime is used that requires an episodic judgment, process<strong>in</strong>g oflures is facilitated. This facilitation is <strong>in</strong>creased if the prime hadbeen present on the study list.The effect of primes on target probes, on the other hand, doesnot appear to vary as a function of the nature of the prime process<strong>in</strong>g.That is, verification of targets is facilitated after an episodicas well as after a semantic prim<strong>in</strong>g task. Aga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>for</strong> a primerequir<strong>in</strong>g an episodic judgment, that facilitation is greater whenthe prime had been studied.This pattern is clearly <strong>in</strong>consistent with the retention <strong>in</strong>tervalhypothesis of Neely et al. (1983). Inhibition of lure process<strong>in</strong>ghas now been obta<strong>in</strong>ed at retention <strong>in</strong>tervals rang<strong>in</strong>g from 15 s(Macht & O'Brien, 1980) through approximately 60 s (Experiment4) to 120 s (Experiments 2 and 3). Conversely, facilitationhas been obta<strong>in</strong>ed at retention <strong>in</strong>tervals rang<strong>in</strong>g from 80 s (Experiment1) through 150 s (Neely et al., 1983) and several m<strong>in</strong>utes(Johns, 1985) up to 24 hr (Taylor & Juola, 1974). We nowknow that the type of prime process<strong>in</strong>g, not retention <strong>in</strong>tervalor list length, is the critical predictor of outcomes <strong>in</strong> prim<strong>in</strong>g ofrecognition memory. The retention <strong>in</strong>terval hypothesis of Neelyet al. should thus be discarded unless future research can proveits usefulness. Beyond rejection of that hypothesis, however,how can the different effects of different prim<strong>in</strong>g tasks be expla<strong>in</strong>ed?From a purely heuristic po<strong>in</strong>t of view, the episodic/semanticdimension is the most useful one to capture the pattern of results.That is, we can now predict an outcome on the basis ofknowledge of the type of process<strong>in</strong>g that is required <strong>for</strong> theprime. A semantic task yields <strong>in</strong>hibition, whereas an episodictask results <strong>in</strong> facilitation. Beyond its predictive capabilities, theepisodic/semantic dichotomy can only provide a rather vagueframework <strong>for</strong> an explanation <strong>for</strong> the present data. Accord<strong>in</strong>gto that vague framework, the data may be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as show<strong>in</strong>gthat prim<strong>in</strong>g may sometimes be restricted to episodic memoryonly, <strong>in</strong> which case the consequences of prim<strong>in</strong>g are differentfrom the situation when all category items (studied or nonstudied)are primed. If episodic memory (i.e., the list) is primedonly (through a recognition probe function<strong>in</strong>g as prime), subsequentepisodic process<strong>in</strong>g is facilitated. On the other hand, ifthe entire category is primed (through a prime requir<strong>in</strong>g a semanticjudgment), subsequent rejection of a lure is <strong>in</strong>hibitedbecause the familiarity of all items <strong>in</strong> a category has beenraised.If this reason<strong>in</strong>g is put <strong>in</strong>to the more detailed terms of anAtk<strong>in</strong>son and Juola type model, from which all research <strong>in</strong> recognitionprim<strong>in</strong>g appears to depart, an account <strong>for</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> the present paper can be proposed: (a) A prime that requiresa semantic judgment raises the level of activation of all items <strong>in</strong>a category, studied or nonstudied. Familiarity-based responsesto targets are thus facilitated, whereas these responses are <strong>in</strong>hibited<strong>for</strong> lures. This mechanism expla<strong>in</strong>s all <strong>in</strong>hibitory effects,(b) A prime that requires an episodic judgment, on the otherhand, activates the studied list only. Consequently, targets arefacilitated because the familiarity of old items from that categoryhas been raised. To expla<strong>in</strong> the effect on lures, one mustassume that prim<strong>in</strong>g of the list also speeds up the search process(as suggested by Neely et al., 1983) <strong>for</strong> subsequent probes andthat this has the same quantitative effect on lures that the rise<strong>in</strong> familiarity has on targets, (c) When the prime is not from the


same category, there is nevertheless some residual prim<strong>in</strong>g ofthe list, provided the prime requires an episodic judgment andis itself a target. This expla<strong>in</strong>s purely episodic prim<strong>in</strong>g <strong>for</strong> targets<strong>in</strong> Experiment 1. To account <strong>for</strong> the effect on lures, it mustaga<strong>in</strong> be assumed that residual list prim<strong>in</strong>g also speeds up subsequentsearch.This account is highly speculative and clearly post hoc, butcomponents of it have been suggested previously. Macht andO'Brien (1980) derived their predictions by us<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t a fromabove. Neely et al. (1983) assumed a speed<strong>in</strong>g of search to underliefacilitation <strong>for</strong> lures (see po<strong>in</strong>ts b and c). What the previouspo<strong>in</strong>ts do, then, is to <strong>in</strong>tegrate elements from the previousliterature and to specify the conditions under which each ofthese processes is relevant. Because those conditions (semanticvs. episodic tasks required on the primes) are denned <strong>in</strong>dependentlyand can be known a priori, the account can serve to derivepredictions <strong>for</strong> other prim<strong>in</strong>g studies-ConclusionThe present experiments have demonstrated that prim<strong>in</strong>gcan facilitate or <strong>in</strong>hibit process<strong>in</strong>g of lures even when list lengthand retention <strong>in</strong>terval are controlled. The critical variable thatdeterm<strong>in</strong>es whether facilitation or <strong>in</strong>hibition arises is the typeofjudgment that must be per<strong>for</strong>med on the prime. If a semanticjudgment is required (regardless of what type of <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation is<strong>in</strong>volved) lure process<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>in</strong>hibited. If the prime requires anepisodic judgment, on the other hand, subsequent rejection oflures is facilitated.These data can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed if it is assumed that the list canbe primed selectively. The idea of selective list prim<strong>in</strong>g is consistentwith the view that episodic and semantic memory may constitutetwo different memory systems (Tulv<strong>in</strong>g, 1983). The ideais also consistent with the work of Wickens and his associates(Wickens, Moody, & Dow, 1981; Wickens, Moody, & Vidulich,1985) who also demonstrated that lists of vary<strong>in</strong>g lengths arestored <strong>in</strong> memory as a well-def<strong>in</strong>ed and closely <strong>in</strong>terl<strong>in</strong>ked entity.ReferencesAnderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory, and thought. Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.Anderson, J. R. (1983a). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA:Harvard University Press.Anderson, J. R. (1983b). A spread<strong>in</strong>g activation theory of memory.Journal of Verbal Learn<strong>in</strong>g and Verbal Behavior, 22, 26-295.Antos, S. J. (1979). Process<strong>in</strong>g facilitation <strong>in</strong> a lexical decision task.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Per<strong>for</strong>mance,5, 527-545.Atk<strong>in</strong>son, R. C, Herrmann, D. J., & Wescourt, K. T. (1974). Searchprocesses <strong>in</strong> recognition memory. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Theories <strong>in</strong>cognitive psychology; The Loyola symposium (pp. 101-146). Potomac,MD: Erlbaum.Atk<strong>in</strong>son, R. C, & Juola, J. F. (1973). Factors <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g speed andaccuracy of word recognition. In S. Kornblum (Ed.), Attention andper<strong>for</strong>mance IV(pp. 583-612). New York: Academic Press.Atk<strong>in</strong>son, R. C, & Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes <strong>in</strong>recognition memory. In D. H. Krantz, R. C. Atk<strong>in</strong>son, R. D. Luce,& P. Suppes (Eds.), Contemporary developments <strong>in</strong> mathematicalpsychology (pp. 243-293). San Francisco: Freeman.RECOGNITION PRIMING 573Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a goodmeasure of lexical access? The role of word frequency <strong>in</strong> the neglecteddecision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perceptionand Per<strong>for</strong>mance, 10, 340-357.Bennet, R. W. (1975). Proactive <strong>in</strong>terference <strong>in</strong> short-term memory:Fundamental <strong>for</strong>gett<strong>in</strong>g processes. Journal of Verbal Learn<strong>in</strong>g andVerbal Behavior, 14, 123-144.Blaxton, T. A., & Neely, J. H. (1983). Inhibition from semantically relatedprimes: Evidence of a category-specific <strong>in</strong>hibition. <strong>Memory</strong> &Cognition, 11,500-510.Coll<strong>in</strong>s, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spread<strong>in</strong>g-activation theory ofsemantic process<strong>in</strong>g. Psychological Review, 82,407-428.deGroot, A. M. B., Thomassen, A. J. W. M, & Hudson, P. T. W. (1982).Associative facilitation of word recognition as measured from a neutralprime. <strong>Memory</strong> & Cognition, 10, 358-370.Friendly, M., Frankl<strong>in</strong>, P. E., Hoffmann, D., & Rub<strong>in</strong>, D. C. (1982). TheToronto Word Pool: Norms <strong>for</strong> imagery, concreteness, orthographicvariables, and grammatical usage <strong>for</strong> 1080 words. Behavior ResearchMethods & Instrumentation, 14, 375-399.Gorfe<strong>in</strong>, D. S., & Jacobson, D. E. (1973). <strong>Memory</strong> search <strong>in</strong> a Brown-Peterson short-term memory paradigm. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology, 99, 82-87.Henik, A., Friedrich, F. J., & Kellogg, W. A. (1983). The dependenceof semantic relatedness effects upon prime process<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>Memory</strong> &Cognition, 11, 366-373.Herrmann, D. J., & Harwood, J. R. (1980). More evidence <strong>for</strong> the existenceof separate semantic and episodic stores <strong>in</strong> long-term memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Memory</strong>,6, 467-478.Irw<strong>in</strong>, D. I., & Lupker, S. J. (1983). Semantic prim<strong>in</strong>g of pictures andwords: A levels of process<strong>in</strong>g approach. Journal of Verbal Learn<strong>in</strong>gand Verbal Behavior, 22, 45-60.Johns, E. E. (1985). Effects of list organization on item recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>Memory</strong>, and Cognition,11, 605-620,Kiger, J. I., & Glass, A. L. (1981). Context effects <strong>in</strong> sentence verification.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception andPer<strong>for</strong>mance, 7, 688-700.Lewandowsky, S. (1982). An exam<strong>in</strong>ation of various control conditions<strong>for</strong> prim<strong>in</strong>g research <strong>in</strong> recognition memory. Unpublished manuscript,University of Toronto.Lorch, R. F. (1982). <strong>Prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and search processes <strong>in</strong> semantic memory:A test of three models of spread<strong>in</strong>g activation. Journal of VerbalLearn<strong>in</strong>g and Verbal Behavior, 21,468-492.Lupker, S. J. (1984). Semantic prim<strong>in</strong>g without association: A secondlook. Journal of Verbal Learn<strong>in</strong>g and Verbal Behavior, 23, 709-733.Macht, M. L., & O'Brien, E. J. (1980). Familiarity-based respond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>item recognition: Evidence <strong>for</strong> the role of spread<strong>in</strong>g activation. Journalof Experimental Psychology: Human Learn<strong>in</strong>g and <strong>Memory</strong>, 6,301-318.Macht, M. L., & Spear, N. E. (1977). <strong>Prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> effects <strong>in</strong> episodic memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology; Human Learn<strong>in</strong>g and<strong>Memory</strong>, i, 733-741.McKoon, G., Ratcliff, R., & Dell, G. (1985). The role of semantic <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation<strong>in</strong> episodic retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>Memory</strong>, and Cognition, 11, 742-751.Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation <strong>in</strong> recogniz<strong>in</strong>gpairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227-234.Monsell, S. (1978). Recency, immediate recognition memory, and reactiontime. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 465-501.Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1974). Human memory: Theory and data. Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum.Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1976). Item and order <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation <strong>in</strong> short-term


574 STEPHAN LEWANDOWSKYserial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105,191-216.Neely, J. H., & Durgunoglu, A. (1985). Dissociative episodic and semanticprim<strong>in</strong>g effects <strong>in</strong> episodic recognition and lexical decisiontasks. Journal of <strong>Memory</strong> and Language, 24,466-489.Neely, J. H., Schmidt, S. R., & Roediger, H. L., III. (1983). Inhibitionfrom related primes <strong>in</strong> recognition memory. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>Memory</strong>, and Cognition, 9,196-211.Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control.In R. L. Solso (Ed.), In<strong>for</strong>mation process<strong>in</strong>g and cognition: TheLoyola symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1981a). Automatic and strategic prim<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> recognition. Journal of Verbal Learn<strong>in</strong>g and Verbal Behavior, 20,204-215.Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1981 b). Does activation really spread? PsychologicalReview. 88, 454-462.Shulman, H. G., & Davison, T. C. B. (1977). Control properties of semanticcod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a lexical decision task. Journal of Verbal Learn<strong>in</strong>gand Verbal Behavior, 16.91-98.Smith, M. C. (1979). Contextual facilitation <strong>in</strong> a letter search task dependson how the prime is processed. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Per<strong>for</strong>mance, 5, 239-251.Smith, M. C, Theodor, L., & Frankl<strong>in</strong>, P. E. (1983). The relationshipbetween contextual facilitation and depth of process<strong>in</strong>g. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>Memory</strong>, and Cognition, 9, 697-712.Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, S. (1966). High-speed scann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> human memory. Science,153, 652-654.Taylor, G. A., & Juola, J. F. (1974). <strong>Prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> effects on recognition per<strong>for</strong>mance.Bullet<strong>in</strong> ofthePsychonomic Society, 3, 277-279.Tulv<strong>in</strong>g, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulv<strong>in</strong>g & W.Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381-403). NewYork: Academic Press.Tulv<strong>in</strong>g, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New \brk: Ox<strong>for</strong>dUniversity Press.Wickens, D. D., Moody, M. J., & Dow, R. (1981). The nature and tim<strong>in</strong>gof the retrieval processes and <strong>in</strong>terference effects. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: General, 110, 1-20.Wickens, D. D., Moody, M. J., & Vidulich, M. (1985). Retrieval timeas a function of memory set size, type of probes, and <strong>in</strong>terference <strong>in</strong>recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>Memory</strong>, and Cognition, 11, 154-164.Received June 24,1985Revision received September 30,1985 •Low Publication Prices <strong>for</strong> APA Members and AffiliatesKeep<strong>in</strong>g You Up-to-DateAll APA members (Fellows, Members, and Associates) receive — as part of their annual dues — subscriptions tothe <strong>American</strong> Psychologist, the APA Monitor, and Psychology Today.High School Teacher and Student Affiliates receive subscriptions to the APA Monitor and Psychology Today, andthey can subscribe to the <strong>American</strong> Psychologist at a significantly reduced rate.In addition, all members and affiliates are eligible <strong>for</strong> sav<strong>in</strong>gs of up to 50% on other APA journals, as well assignificant discounts on subscriptions from cooperat<strong>in</strong>g societies and publishers (e.g.. the British PsychologicalSociety, the <strong>American</strong> Sociological Association, and Human Sciences Press).Essential ResourcesAPA members and affiliates receive special rates <strong>for</strong> purchases of APA books, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the PublicationManual of the APA, the Master Lectures, and APA's Guide to Research Support.Other Benefits of MembershipMembership <strong>in</strong> APA also provides eligibility <strong>for</strong> low-cost <strong>in</strong>surance plans cover<strong>in</strong>g life: medical and <strong>in</strong>comeprotection; hospital <strong>in</strong>demnity; accident and travel; Keogh retirement; office overhead; and student/school.professional, and liability.For more <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation, write to <strong>American</strong> Psychological Association,Membership Records. 1200 Seventeenth Street NW. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton. DC 20036

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!