11.07.2015 Views

Puzzle of Inheritance: Genetics and the Methods - SDSC Education

Puzzle of Inheritance: Genetics and the Methods - SDSC Education

Puzzle of Inheritance: Genetics and the Methods - SDSC Education

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Copyright 1997 by BSCSAll rights reserved. B SCS grants permission to reproduce materials from this module for noncommercial, educationaluse. This permission, however, does not cover reproduction <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se items for any o<strong>the</strong>r use. For permissions <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rrights under this copyright, please contact <strong>the</strong> Permissions Department, BSCS, 5415 Mark Dabling Blvd., ColoradoSprings, Colorado 80918-3842, U.S.A. FAX 719 531-9104.This material is based on work supported by <strong>the</strong> United States Department <strong>of</strong> Energy under grant numbers DE-FG03-93ER615884 <strong>and</strong> DE-FG03-95ER61989. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed in <strong>the</strong> publicationare those <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> authors <strong>and</strong> do not necessarily reflect <strong>the</strong> views <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> United States Department <strong>of</strong> Energy.


ForewordAs biology teachers, you have two large tasks before you: to help your students gain a conceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> living systems, <strong>and</strong> to help your students underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science. This secondtask, conveying to your students a fundamental grasp <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ways <strong>of</strong> science, is vitally important regardless <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> future career choices your students will make. If <strong>the</strong>y are to become informed citizens, able to appreciate<strong>and</strong> use <strong>the</strong> knowledge put before <strong>the</strong>m, your students must underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> scientific explanations<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> power <strong>and</strong> rigor <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> requirements <strong>of</strong> science.Science assumes that <strong>the</strong> universe <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> natural phenomena it encompasses are ordered <strong>and</strong> predictable.Scientific explanations reflect that assumption; <strong>the</strong>y are driven by data that are collected carefully <strong>and</strong> analyzedrigorously. The data must be reliable; that is, <strong>the</strong> same data must result when o<strong>the</strong>r investigators repeat <strong>the</strong>original observations or experiments. Because science is a human endeavor, it is impossible (<strong>and</strong> perhaps noteven wise) to eliminate all subjective judgments from <strong>the</strong> processes <strong>of</strong> science. The methods <strong>of</strong> science <strong>and</strong>scientific habits <strong>of</strong> mind, however, help to minimize <strong>the</strong> negative influence <strong>of</strong> subjectivity by holding all scientists<strong>and</strong> all scientific explanations to <strong>the</strong> same st<strong>and</strong>ards. If <strong>the</strong> corpus <strong>of</strong> scientific knowledge is to remain reliable,<strong>the</strong>se st<strong>and</strong>ards must apply everywhere that science is done. From a school laboratory to <strong>the</strong> Pasteur Institute,from beginning students in North America to Nobel laureates from India, <strong>the</strong> requirements for validscientific explanations do not change. In this sense, science may be as close as <strong>the</strong> world comes to a universalset <strong>of</strong> values.We have designed this module to help convey some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se principles <strong>of</strong> science. We tested this moduleextensively <strong>and</strong> found it to be effective with high school <strong>and</strong> introductory college biology students. We hopeyou <strong>and</strong> your students find <strong>the</strong>se materials interesting <strong>and</strong> helpful. We welcome your feedback <strong>and</strong> have provideda response form for that purpose. We look forward to hearing from you.Joseph D. McInerney, Principal InvestigatorB. Ellen Friedman, PhD, Project DirectorBiological Sciences Curriculum Study (BSCS)Summary <strong>of</strong> ContentsThe module contains two major components. The first is <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers, which introduces <strong>the</strong>module, describes <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project, <strong>and</strong> addresses issues in <strong>the</strong> philosophy <strong>of</strong> science <strong>and</strong> some<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ethical, legal, <strong>and</strong> social implications (ELSI) <strong>of</strong> research in genetics. This Overview also provides a survey<strong>of</strong> fundamental genetics concepts <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> new, nontraditional concepts <strong>of</strong> inheritance. The second component,Classroom Activities, provides six instructional activities appropriate for high school or introductory collegestudents. The first section <strong>of</strong> this component also is addressed to teachers; it contains specific suggestionsfor teaching each activity, including an annotated set <strong>of</strong> student materials. The o<strong>the</strong>r two sections <strong>of</strong> ClassroomActivities are to be photocopied <strong>and</strong> distributed to students. Use <strong>of</strong> instructional Activities 1-5 requires thatstudents have completed a basic study <strong>of</strong> genetics such as is found in an introductory biology course.iv


Evaluation Form forThe <strong>Puzzle</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Inheritance</strong>: <strong>Genetics</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> ScienceYour feedback is important. After you have used <strong>the</strong> module, please take a few minutes to complete <strong>and</strong>return this form to us. (BSCS, Attn: HGN3, 5415 Mark Dabling Blvd., Colorado Springs, Colorado 80918-3842)1. Please evaluate <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers by marking this form <strong>and</strong> providing written comments or suggestionson a separate sheet.Sections used not helpful very helpfulI. Introduction 1 2 3 4 5II. The Human Genome Project 1 2 3 4 5III. The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science 1 2 3 4 5IV. ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social 1 2 3 4 5Implications <strong>of</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>V. <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> 1 2 3 4 5Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>Glossary 1 2 3 4 52. Please evaluate <strong>the</strong> Classroom Activities by marking this form <strong>and</strong> providing written comments or suggestionson a separate sheet. Rate activities for <strong>the</strong>ir effectiveness at teaching NMS (nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science)concepts or genetics concepts.Activities used NMS Concepts <strong>Genetics</strong> Conceptsnot helpful very helpful not helpful very helpfulEngage Activity 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5Activity 1 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5Activity 2 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5Activity 3 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5Activity 4 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5Activity 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 53. What are <strong>the</strong> major strengths <strong>of</strong> this module?v


4. What are <strong>the</strong> major weaknesses <strong>of</strong> this module?5. Please rate <strong>the</strong> overall effectiveness <strong>of</strong> this module: not effective very effective1 2 3 4 56. Please provide a description <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> classes in which you used this module: (circle response)College: 2 year 4 year High school: grade 9 10 11 12Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior Level <strong>of</strong> class: basic honors 2nd yearHow many students used <strong>the</strong> module? ________How many students per class? ________Ethnicity: approximate % <strong>of</strong> minorities: ________________________________________________________Description <strong>of</strong> school: ______________________________________________________________________College: liberal arts science High school: urban suburban rural7. Have you used BSCS materials before? ❒ yes ❒ noHave you used <strong>the</strong> first BSCS genome module, Mapping <strong>and</strong> Sequencing <strong>the</strong> Human Genome: Science,Ethics, <strong>and</strong> Public Policy? ❒ yes ❒ no. If yes, please provide on a separate sheet feedback that youthink will help us if we revise that module.Have you used <strong>the</strong> second BSCS genome module, The Human Genome Project: Biology, Computers, <strong>and</strong>Privacy? ❒ yes ❒ no8. Please provide your name <strong>and</strong> contact information below:Name _____________________________________________________________________________________School ____________________________________________________________________________________Mailing address _________________________________________________________ ❒ home or ❒ work______________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________Phone__________________________________________________________________ ❒ home or ❒ workFAX_____________________________________________________________________ ❒ home or❒ workE-mail address______________________________________________________________________________Was <strong>the</strong> address on your mailing label for receipt <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module correct? ❒ yes❒ novi


ContentsForeword <strong>and</strong> Summary <strong>of</strong> Contents . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ivEvaluation Form. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vOVERVIEW FOR TEACHERS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1Section I: Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3Section II: The Human Genome Project . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9Section III: The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13Section IV: ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications <strong>of</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>. . . . . . . . . . 25Section V: <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33Glossary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55References <strong>and</strong> Related Literature . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61CLASSROOM ACTIVITIES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65Teacher Pages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67Engage Activity: Scientific Investigation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69Activity 1: St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73Activity 2: Puzzling Pedigrees . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89Activity 3: Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99Activity 4: Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111Activity 5: What Do We Know? How Do We Know It?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119Special Copymasters . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127Activity 1: St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129Activity 2: Puzzling Pedigrees . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141Activity 3: Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 149Activity 4: Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 163Student Pages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 165Engage Activity: Scientific Investigation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167Activity 1: St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169Activity 2: Puzzling Pedigrees . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 173Activity 3: Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 177Activity 4: Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181Activity 5: What Do We Know? How Do We Know It?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 183vii


Overviewfor TeachersThis component introduces <strong>the</strong> module <strong>and</strong> provides adiscussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project, <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong>science, <strong>and</strong> related ethical issues. It also reviews basicconcepts in genetics, including some nontraditionalexplanations <strong>of</strong> inheritance.


Section I:Introduction“Discovery is to see what everyone elsehas seen, but think what no one elsehas thought.”Albert Szent-GyörgyiDevelopment <strong>of</strong> this curriculum project for highschool <strong>and</strong> college biology classes grew out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Human Genome Project (HGP), described in SectionII <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h e r s. The HGP, a 15-year project, is an important step in our pursuit <strong>of</strong>knowledge about inheritance <strong>and</strong> about <strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong>genes in a variety <strong>of</strong> biological processes rangingfrom evolution to human health <strong>and</strong> diseases.Although <strong>the</strong> HGP is not conceptually novel, it providesa new level <strong>of</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ing through itsscope: a well-organized collection <strong>of</strong> genetic informationnow brings within reach many questionsthat previously were unaddressable. The motivationbehind <strong>the</strong> HGP, <strong>the</strong>n, is tw<strong>of</strong>old. It is drivenby <strong>the</strong> need for basic knowledge (a database containingmaps <strong>and</strong> sequence data for entiregenomes), <strong>and</strong> it is driven by applications <strong>of</strong> thisknowledge, such as clinical studies. Not only does<strong>the</strong> HGP contribute to discoveries about genetics,it also is a significant l<strong>and</strong>mark in <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> science.It involves a large-scale collaboration <strong>of</strong> manyindependent groups <strong>of</strong> scientists, <strong>and</strong> each groupis interdisciplinary. Large scientific collaborationsare not new—<strong>the</strong> physics installation near Geneva,Switzerl<strong>and</strong> (CERN, <strong>the</strong> European Center forNuclear Research) is one such example—but verylarge collaborations are unusual.The primary job <strong>of</strong> HGP scientists is to locate <strong>and</strong>identify all <strong>the</strong> information stored in <strong>the</strong> humangenetic material. (A photomicrograph <strong>of</strong> all 46human chromosomes is presented in Figure 1.) Inaddition, <strong>the</strong> HGP will study <strong>the</strong> genomes <strong>of</strong> a variety<strong>of</strong> nonhuman organisms.The HGP <strong>of</strong>fers a fine opportunity to witness <strong>the</strong>dynamics <strong>of</strong> science. Those responsible for <strong>the</strong> stru c-ture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HGP recognized that <strong>the</strong> impact <strong>of</strong> thislarge project will extend far beyond <strong>the</strong> genetics dataalone, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y established <strong>the</strong> Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong>Social Implications (ELSI) component <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> projectto address <strong>the</strong>se concerns. ELSI provides a mechanismto inform <strong>the</strong> public about <strong>the</strong> discoveries <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> HGP <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>fers a way for <strong>the</strong> public to voice itsinterests <strong>and</strong> concerns about this work during <strong>the</strong>many years <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> project. As part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ELSI effort ,<strong>the</strong> U.S. Department <strong>of</strong> Energy (DOE) has funded <strong>the</strong>development <strong>of</strong> this educational module, The Pu z z l e<strong>of</strong> <strong>Inheritance</strong>: <strong>Genetics</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science,by <strong>the</strong> Biological Sciences Curriculum Study (BSCS).3


Section I ■ Introductiondevelopment for teachers who want to exp<strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong>ir underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science,<strong>of</strong> ELSI topics <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir application to <strong>the</strong> classroom,<strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> a variety <strong>of</strong> nontraditional explanations<strong>of</strong> inheritance. The O v e rview for Te a c h-e r s provides a discussion <strong>of</strong> this inform a t i o n ,<strong>and</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Classroom Activities c o m p o n e n t<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module provides experience in communicatingsome <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se topics. For some teachers,<strong>the</strong> activities-based approach to teachingalso will be new.Figure 1 A human karyotype: This photomicrographshows <strong>the</strong> full complement <strong>of</strong> 46 human chromosomesarranged by size, b<strong>and</strong>ing pattern, <strong>and</strong> chromosomenumber. Notice that <strong>the</strong>re are two copies <strong>of</strong> each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>22 autosomes plus one copy <strong>of</strong> each type <strong>of</strong> sex chromosome(X <strong>and</strong> Y); this individual is a male.GOALS FOR THE MODULEThis module is designed to meet <strong>the</strong> following goals:■ The Nature <strong>and</strong> <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science (NMS)The first goal is to involve students in classroomactivities that require explicit opportunities toapply scientific methods in a thought-provokingcontext, ra<strong>the</strong>r than as a cookbook-style list <strong>of</strong>steps. Students will glimpse <strong>the</strong> overarchingnature <strong>of</strong> science in <strong>the</strong> process.■ Concepts in <strong>Genetics</strong> The second goal is toupdate <strong>the</strong> genetics curriculum to include some<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nontraditional concepts <strong>of</strong> inheritancethat address processes not adequately explainedby classical Mendelian genetics. It is, <strong>of</strong> course,our view <strong>of</strong> inheritance that is “nontraditional”ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> genetic mechanisms <strong>the</strong>mselves.Indeed, some nontraditional topics, such asmitochondrial inheritance <strong>and</strong> genomic imprinting,are widespread phenomena.■ Pr<strong>of</strong>essional Development A third major goalis to provide an opportunity for pr<strong>of</strong>essionalOur rationale for <strong>the</strong>se goals lies in <strong>the</strong> import a n c e<strong>of</strong> a scientifically literate public <strong>and</strong> in <strong>the</strong> need forc u rriculum materials that support an inquiry- b a s e dapproach to underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods<strong>of</strong> science. In addition, we recognize thatalthough teachers have a perpetual need to ke e pabreast <strong>of</strong> new scientific knowledge, <strong>the</strong>y have limitedtime to seek it out. We combined <strong>the</strong>se goalsbecause recent discoveries <strong>of</strong> nontraditional modes<strong>of</strong> inheritance exemplify <strong>the</strong> processes scientistsuse to test <strong>and</strong> exp<strong>and</strong> scientific explanations.When coupled with selected episodes from <strong>the</strong> histo ry <strong>of</strong> genetics, <strong>the</strong>se newly discovered mechanisms<strong>of</strong> inheritance can provide compellinglessons in <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> science.“We can never require that science beproductive; we can only require that itbe sound.”J. Michael BishopAlthough this module touches on <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong>genetics, particularly in Activities 1 <strong>and</strong> 3, we feltthat we could not adequately present <strong>the</strong> history,<strong>the</strong> methods, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> overarching nature <strong>of</strong> sciencein one module. For this reason, we selected <strong>the</strong>methods <strong>of</strong> science as <strong>the</strong> main focus <strong>of</strong> our firstgoal, <strong>and</strong> we chose to present, to a lesser extent, <strong>the</strong>broader view <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> science as an endeavorwith cultural ties. If you have time, we encourageyou to extend your study <strong>of</strong> biology to include <strong>the</strong>h i s t o ry <strong>of</strong> genetics. If you choose to exp<strong>and</strong> yourtreatment <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> history <strong>and</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> genetics oro<strong>the</strong>r aspects <strong>of</strong> biology, you may find <strong>the</strong> publicationtitled Teaching About <strong>the</strong> History <strong>and</strong> Nature4©1997 by BSCS.


Introduction ■ Section I<strong>of</strong> Science <strong>and</strong> Te c h n o l o g y (Social Science <strong>Education</strong>Consortium <strong>and</strong> BSCS, 1996 1 ) useful to yourplans. Students need to see that modern science isnot “better than”—or indeed qualitatively differentfrom—science <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> previous several decades. Ourlimited presentation <strong>of</strong> some aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> history<strong>of</strong> genetics emphasizes that history extends beyondnames <strong>and</strong> dates. Students can appreciate that currentscientific knowledge is <strong>the</strong> product <strong>of</strong> an intellectualexploration, <strong>and</strong> one that continues tochange. This module lets students see that scientificknowledge changes according to rigorous criteria,not by <strong>the</strong> whim <strong>of</strong> an influential individual or bypopular consensus.These materials were designed <strong>and</strong> written by curriculumdevelopers at BSCS working with an extern a lwriting team <strong>and</strong> with <strong>the</strong> guidance <strong>of</strong> an advisorycommittee. In addition to external reviews by a group<strong>of</strong> specialists in teaching, bioethics, philosophy <strong>of</strong> science,<strong>and</strong> genetics, all materials in this module haveundergone extensive, formal field testing to ensure<strong>the</strong>ir efficacy in <strong>the</strong> classroom (see Figure 2). Themodule was tested with more than 1,000 students at14 high school <strong>and</strong> 2 college sites in <strong>the</strong> United States<strong>and</strong> Canada. O<strong>the</strong>r high school <strong>and</strong> college teachersi n f o rmally tested specific components <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module.We used <strong>the</strong> large body <strong>of</strong> evaluation data—combinedwith direct observations from site visits, externalreview data, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> recommendations <strong>of</strong> oura d v i s o ry committee—to guide <strong>the</strong> revision (<strong>and</strong> insome cases rewriting) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> field-test materials inpreparing this final version <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module. The modulewill be distributed free <strong>of</strong> charge to tens <strong>of</strong> thous<strong>and</strong>s<strong>of</strong> high school <strong>and</strong> college biology teachersthrough <strong>the</strong> support <strong>of</strong> DOE.HOW TO USE THE MODULEThis module provides a detailed discussion for teachers(<strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h e r s) <strong>and</strong> six activities forstudents, complete with background material <strong>and</strong> suggestionsfor teaching (<strong>the</strong> Classroom Activities). Thematerial contained in <strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h e r sis foryour own use. It may extend your experience <strong>and</strong> thusFigure 2 Field-test orientation: Teacher <strong>and</strong> writerJohn Zola conducts Activity 5 in an orientation session forfield-test teachers at BSCS.be helpful for teaching <strong>the</strong> activities, but it is not essentialto your use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instructional activities. Figure 3shows <strong>the</strong> layout <strong>of</strong> materials in <strong>the</strong>se two components.Notice that within <strong>the</strong> Classroom Activities w eprovide <strong>the</strong>se sections: <strong>the</strong> teacher pages (includingannotated student material), special copymasters foreach activity, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> regular student pages.A summary <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> six activities is provided in Figure 4.Notice that <strong>the</strong>se include a brief activity to introduce<strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science (Engage Activity) <strong>and</strong> fivemore classroom activities that combine experiencewith scientific methods <strong>and</strong> genetics topics. Time maynot permit you to teach all six activities. We recommendthat you introduce <strong>the</strong> module with <strong>the</strong> EngageA c t i v i t y. You can use it early in <strong>the</strong> term, immediatelyprior to studying genetics, or immediately prior to1Available from Social Science <strong>Education</strong> Consortium, Inc., P. O. Box 21270, Boulder Colorado 80308-4270; ISBN 0-8 9 9 9 4 - 3 8 6 - 1 .5©1997 by BSCS.


Section I ■IntroductionOverview for TeachersI IntroductionII The Human Genome ProjectIII The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> ScienceI V ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications<strong>of</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>V <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong>Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>IIIClassroom ActivitiesTeacher PagesEngage, Activities 1-5Special CopymastersActivities 1-4III Student PagesEngage, Activities 1-5Figure 3 The module at a glance: The module consists<strong>of</strong> two large components, <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Classroom Activities.using <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r activities in <strong>the</strong> module. For <strong>the</strong> majorp a rt <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module, choose any or all <strong>of</strong> Activities 1-5.We present <strong>the</strong>m in <strong>the</strong> recommended sequence, buteach activity is autonomous, <strong>and</strong> you can use <strong>the</strong>activities effectively in a different order. If you teachActivity 4 without Activity 3, you may need to augmenti n f o rmation about trinucleotide repeats; if you teachboth activities, keep <strong>the</strong>m in sequence. The final activit y, Activity 5, provides <strong>the</strong> best closure for <strong>the</strong> module<strong>and</strong> can serve as a perf o rmance assessment for evaluatingstudent progress for <strong>the</strong> module as a whole. Werecommend that you close with Activity 5 regardless<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r activities you have selected. The timerequired to teach all <strong>the</strong> activities is approximately 7class periods <strong>of</strong> 50 minutes each. Please note thatActivities 1-5 require previous knowledge <strong>of</strong> fundamentalprinciples <strong>of</strong> genetics.Along with each classroom activity, <strong>the</strong> module providesbackground material <strong>and</strong> annotations that <strong>of</strong>ferstrategies for <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> each activity. After you determine<strong>the</strong> activities you will use, you will need to copy<strong>the</strong> student pages, including worksheets <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rsupporting material provided as copymasters. Noticethat <strong>the</strong> section for copymasters (h<strong>and</strong>-outs used atvarious points during an activity) is separated from<strong>the</strong> section for regular student pages (for which youwill need one copy per student).These materials take a constructivist approach toteaching. (For an introductory discussion <strong>of</strong> constructivism,see Trowbridge <strong>and</strong> Bybee, 1990.) Thisapproach is based in part on forming a bridge fromwhat students already know to what <strong>the</strong>y will learn.The approach also helps students to be active partnersin constructing <strong>the</strong>ir new knowledge ra<strong>the</strong>rthan being told what <strong>the</strong>y are to learn. The teacherguides students ra<strong>the</strong>r than simply telling <strong>the</strong>m. Theconstructivist approach, which encourages inquiry,is apparent in this module in <strong>the</strong> way students areallowed to arrive at <strong>the</strong>ir conclusions in a step-wisefashion. For example, in Activity 2, <strong>the</strong> students usevarious data to build a concept <strong>of</strong> inheritance (mitochondrialinheritance) that is new to <strong>the</strong>m. Thisapproach to teaching takes more time than traditionallectures, but it generally leads to longer-lastingresults. In addition, <strong>the</strong> active participation <strong>of</strong> studentsduring <strong>the</strong> learning process increases <strong>the</strong> likelihoodthat students will apply what <strong>the</strong>y learn tonovel situations.In addition to <strong>the</strong> Classroom Activities, <strong>the</strong> moduleprovides a useful resource for teachers in <strong>the</strong>O v e rview for Te a c h e r s . You can use <strong>the</strong> O v e rv i e wfor Te a c h e r s as a st<strong>and</strong>-alone piece or as a supplementto <strong>the</strong> annotated activities to extend <strong>the</strong> backgroundmaterial provided for each activity. TheO v e rview discusses some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> interests <strong>of</strong>philosophers <strong>of</strong> science <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> bioethicists. TheO v e rview also describes new discoveries in geneticsthat require nontraditional explanations <strong>of</strong> inheritance.The various sections <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> O v e rview forTe a c h e r s go beyond <strong>the</strong> presentation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se topicsin student pages. As <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project<strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r areas <strong>of</strong> genetics progress, new inform a-tion likely will emerge about <strong>the</strong> nontraditionalexamples <strong>of</strong> inheritance presented in this module,<strong>and</strong>, in time, <strong>the</strong>se topics may become part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>st<strong>and</strong>ard biology curriculum. For now, <strong>the</strong>se examplesnot only entice students (<strong>and</strong> teachers) with<strong>the</strong> prospect <strong>of</strong> new insights into genetics, <strong>the</strong>y alsodemonstrate how science works.6©1997 by BSCS.


Introduction ■ Section IFigure 4 Summary <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> activitiesEngage ActivityStudents are stimulated to think about <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science through a simple exercise <strong>of</strong>observation <strong>and</strong> measurement. This activity does not require knowledge <strong>of</strong> genetics.Activity 1St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong>Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsStudents build a logical sequence <strong>of</strong> milestone explanations <strong>of</strong> inheritance based on conceptualconnections ra<strong>the</strong>r than just chronology. Students also evaluate evidence for credibility<strong>and</strong> correlate evidence to milestone explanations. The activity provides a review <strong>of</strong> fundamentalgenetics concepts <strong>and</strong> stimulates discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science.Activity 2Puzzling PedigreesStudents study a collection <strong>of</strong> pedigrees to identify <strong>the</strong> inheritance pattern in each. Two pedigreesprovide a special challenge: <strong>the</strong>y show a pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance that is not readilyexplained by traditional Mendelian concepts. Students formulate hypo<strong>the</strong>ses <strong>and</strong> collect statisticaldata (coin flips) to test <strong>the</strong>ir hypo<strong>the</strong>ses. They also read two brief articles that provideinformation about mitochondrial inheritance to help <strong>the</strong>m build a new explanation. Finally,students record <strong>the</strong>ir ideas about <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science.Activity 3Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveriesin ScienceThis activity is designed as a mystery. The task is to make discoveries about genetic anticipation.Students sort <strong>and</strong> analyze clues to build two pedigrees, one that shows a family history<strong>of</strong> Huntington disease <strong>and</strong> one for a family affected by myotonic dystrophy. Studentslook for patterns <strong>of</strong> disease onset <strong>and</strong> correlate those to molecular data related to trinucleotiderepeats, thus building a nontraditional explanation <strong>of</strong> inheritance.Activity 4Should Teenagers Be Testedfor <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene?The class reads a letter about <strong>the</strong> policy <strong>of</strong> withholding tests for Huntington disease fromasymptomatic minors <strong>and</strong> conducts an ethical analysis in <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> a debate about <strong>the</strong>value <strong>of</strong> this policy.Activity 5What Do We Know?How Do We Know It?The module concludes with a discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> differences between pseudoscience <strong>and</strong> science,using popular science <strong>and</strong> health claims. Students focus on observations about <strong>the</strong>methods <strong>of</strong> science made throughout <strong>the</strong> module. This activity provides a performanceassessment for <strong>the</strong> entire module.7©1997 by BSCS.


Section II:The HumanGenome Project“These are exciting <strong>and</strong> challengingtimes for biological researchers. Thewealth <strong>of</strong> information <strong>and</strong> capabilitiesnow being generated by <strong>the</strong> variousgenome projects <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r biologicalendeavors will lead over <strong>the</strong> next twodecades to more insights into livingsystems than have been amassed in <strong>the</strong>past two millennia. Biology is tru l yu n d e rgoing a revolution.”David A. Smith(retiring director, Health Effects <strong>and</strong> Life SciencesResearch Division, Office <strong>of</strong> Health <strong>and</strong> EnvironmentalResearch, DOE, 19 December 1996)At <strong>the</strong> time this module was being developed, in <strong>the</strong>winter <strong>of</strong> 1996, <strong>the</strong> U.S. Department <strong>of</strong> Energy(DOE) held <strong>the</strong> fifth annual Contractor- G r a n t e eWorkshop. The workshop provided an opportunityfor participants in <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project(HGP) who are supported by DOE to report on <strong>and</strong>discuss <strong>the</strong>ir progress. Through a series <strong>of</strong> brief oralpresentations <strong>and</strong> poster displays, participants builta picture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> current state <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> project <strong>and</strong> discussednew collaborations that will fur<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> effort.One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> most obvious characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> eventwas <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> cooperation among participants.The group was diverse, comprising geneticists <strong>and</strong>molecular biologists, biochemists <strong>and</strong> physicalchemists, computer scientists, engineers, judges,lawyers, educators, <strong>and</strong> genetic counselors. Thenature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> project requires <strong>the</strong> expertise <strong>of</strong> individualsfrom many disciplines, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> HGP hashelped to spawn new areas <strong>of</strong> study, such as informatics,<strong>and</strong> new technologies, including capillarybasedequipment for multiplex DNA sequencing.Here, we <strong>of</strong>fer a brief overview <strong>of</strong> some key features<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> work. 2WHAT IS THE HUMAN GENOME PROJECT?The Human Genome Project is a large, intern a t i o n a l l ycoordinated effort in biological research directed atcreating a series <strong>of</strong> maps <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> DNA <strong>of</strong> humans <strong>and</strong>several o<strong>the</strong>r species, with each map providing greaterdetail (resolution). Figure 5 shows <strong>the</strong> rate <strong>of</strong> progress2A more detailed description <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fundamental techniques <strong>and</strong> background for <strong>the</strong> HGP is available in <strong>the</strong> first twogenome curriculum modules that BSCS developed through DOE funding. The first module is a print curr i c u l u mtitled Mapping <strong>and</strong> Sequencing The Human Genome: Science, Ethics, <strong>and</strong> Public Po l i c y. The second module, T h eHuman Genome Project: Biology, Computers, <strong>and</strong> Privacy, combines print <strong>and</strong> s<strong>of</strong>t w a r e .9


The Human Genome Project ■ Section IIFigure 6 Human Genome Program Centers (as <strong>of</strong> June 1996)Baylor College <strong>of</strong> MedicineCenterResearch FocusPhysical mapping; emphasis on chromosomes 6, 15, 17, XChildren’s Hospital <strong>of</strong> Philadelphia Genetic <strong>and</strong> physical maps <strong>of</strong> chromosome 22Lawrence Berkeley LaboratoryPhysical map <strong>of</strong> chromosome 21; improvements in sequencingautomation <strong>and</strong> informaticsLawrence Livermore National LaboratoryGenetic <strong>and</strong> physical maps <strong>of</strong> chromosome 19 (completed)Los Alamos National LaboratoryMaps <strong>of</strong> chromosome 16; techniques for high-speed mapping <strong>and</strong>sequencing (completed)Stanford University Maps <strong>of</strong> chromosome 4University <strong>of</strong> California, Berkeley,Drosophila Genome CenterPhysical map <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Drosophila genomeUniversity <strong>of</strong> Iowa CooperativeHuman Linkage CenterExpansion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetic linkage map for <strong>the</strong> entire human genomeUniversity <strong>of</strong> Texas Health Science Maps <strong>of</strong> chromosome 3Center at San AntonioUniversity <strong>of</strong> UtahDevelopment <strong>of</strong> genomic technologiesWashington University School <strong>of</strong> MedicineMaps <strong>of</strong> chromosomes 7 <strong>and</strong> X; sequencing <strong>of</strong> C. elegans genomeWhitehead Institute for Biomedical Research<strong>and</strong> Massachusetts Institute <strong>of</strong> TechnologyMaps <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mouse genome; STS map <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> entire human genomeE. coli Genome Project, University <strong>of</strong> Wisconsin Sequencing <strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>of</strong> Escherichia coliHuman Genome Program Centers help focus <strong>and</strong>coordinate <strong>the</strong> work. Involvement <strong>of</strong> different centerschanges as <strong>the</strong> project proceeds. Figure 6 listssome <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genome centers <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> focus <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>irwork. For an update, consult <strong>the</strong> DOE web site athttp://www.ornl.gov/hgmis or <strong>the</strong> NIH web site athttp://www.nchgr.nih.gov.In addition to direct efforts to map <strong>and</strong> sequencep a rticular human chromosomes or those <strong>of</strong> modelorganisms, <strong>the</strong>se centers improve existing technologies<strong>and</strong> develop new technologies to increase <strong>the</strong>speed, accuracy, <strong>and</strong> cost-effectiveness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HGP.These centers also act to stimulate <strong>and</strong> coordinatecollaboration among investigators not formally associatedwith <strong>the</strong> HGP. The work is not limited to <strong>the</strong>United States. The Human Genome Organization(HUGO), formed in 1988, coordinates intern a t i o n a lscientific collaboration between Canada, <strong>the</strong> UnitedStates, Italy, <strong>the</strong> United Kingdom, France, <strong>the</strong> Commonwealth<strong>of</strong> Independent States, <strong>the</strong> EuropeanC o m m u n i t y, <strong>and</strong> Japan. The United Nations <strong>Education</strong>al,Scientific, <strong>and</strong> Cultural Organization11©1997 by BSCS.


Section II ■ The Human Gemone Project( U N E SCO) promotes <strong>the</strong> continued involvement <strong>of</strong>developing nations in genome activities <strong>and</strong> alsos u p p o rts international conferences <strong>and</strong> exchanges.HOW IS THE HUMAN GENOME PROJECTRELATED TO THE NATURE OF SCIENCE?As David Smith’s quote (p. 9) indicates, <strong>the</strong> HumanGenome Project is exp<strong>and</strong>ing our collection <strong>of</strong> geneticdata enorm o u s l y. Simply mapping <strong>and</strong> sequencing<strong>the</strong> entire human genome will not, however, provideus with answers to all <strong>of</strong> our questions about genetics.Knowing <strong>the</strong> sequence <strong>of</strong> a stretch <strong>of</strong> DNA or itslocation on a given chromosome does n o t reveal <strong>the</strong>significance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> region. If <strong>the</strong> region encodes protein,<strong>the</strong> DNA sequence will predict accurately <strong>the</strong>c o rresponding amino acid sequence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> protein,but nei<strong>the</strong>r sequence will tell us <strong>the</strong> biological functi o n <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> protein in <strong>the</strong> absence <strong>of</strong> additional data.Fo rt u n a t e l y, much is known about <strong>the</strong> biologicalfunction <strong>of</strong> various proteins, <strong>and</strong> data are accumulatingrapidly. With <strong>the</strong> foundation <strong>of</strong> a detailed <strong>and</strong>organized bank <strong>of</strong> genome data, biologists can const ruct a more complete picture <strong>of</strong> living systems.Although <strong>the</strong> HGP will not answer all questions aboutgenetics, it will provide an invaluable databank. Inaddition, as <strong>the</strong>y collect data, researchers have made<strong>and</strong> will continue to make discoveries about specificgenes, <strong>the</strong> organization <strong>of</strong> genetic information, evolu t i o n a ry relationships at <strong>the</strong> molecular level, <strong>and</strong>new technologies in laboratory <strong>and</strong> computer applications.The genome map <strong>and</strong> sequence data wills e rve as powerful tools for future research in genetics,evolution, <strong>and</strong> medicine, in this way fueling <strong>the</strong>work <strong>of</strong> scientists in years to come.The HGP also provides a classic demonstration <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science. For example, <strong>the</strong>HGP shows one aspect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> science—intermarriage <strong>of</strong> technology <strong>and</strong> science—in severalways. The decision to begin <strong>the</strong> HGP was based inpart on <strong>the</strong> availability <strong>of</strong> new technologies forcloning <strong>and</strong> sequencing DNA <strong>and</strong> for storing <strong>and</strong>analyzing huge amounts <strong>of</strong> map <strong>and</strong> sequence data.The HGP continues to pursue <strong>and</strong> produce impressivenew technologies for <strong>the</strong>se purposes.The HGP illustrates <strong>the</strong> power <strong>of</strong> cooperationamong groups <strong>of</strong> researchers <strong>and</strong> among pr<strong>of</strong>essionalswho have different types <strong>of</strong> expertise. This largeproject shows <strong>the</strong> way in which new work is built onold work. The vast pre-existing body <strong>of</strong> knowledgeabout <strong>the</strong> behavior <strong>and</strong> location <strong>of</strong> many genes in<strong>the</strong> organisms being studied is a critical resource for<strong>the</strong> mapping <strong>and</strong> sequencing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genome <strong>of</strong> each<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se species.The existence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ELSI program is testimony to<strong>the</strong> social context <strong>of</strong> scientific research. Society’sconcerns about genetic disorders such as cysticfibrosis <strong>and</strong> Huntington disease help to guide decisionsabout those areas <strong>of</strong> research that <strong>the</strong> HGP willsupport <strong>and</strong> pursue. In addition, <strong>the</strong> ELSI component<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HGP shows <strong>the</strong> concern <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> project’sdirectors <strong>and</strong> participants to acknowledge <strong>and</strong>explore <strong>the</strong> impact <strong>of</strong> this work on society.(Reports on progress in specific scientific <strong>and</strong> ELSIareas <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HGP show <strong>the</strong> rapid change in knowledgeas a result <strong>of</strong> this work. A special Genome Issue<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> publication Science [Vol. 274, 25 October1996] provides <strong>the</strong> latest available update as thismodule goes to press.)12©1997 by BSCS.


Section III:The <strong>Methods</strong><strong>of</strong> Science“The power <strong>of</strong> accurate observation is<strong>of</strong>ten called cynicism, by those who donot possess it.”George Bernard Shaw“The goal <strong>of</strong> science is not to open <strong>the</strong>door to everlasting wisdom, but to seta limit on everlasting error.”Galileo,in <strong>the</strong> Bertolt Brecht play, GalileoAs thinking organisms, humans constantly observe<strong>and</strong> consciously interpret <strong>the</strong> world around <strong>the</strong>m.Science <strong>of</strong>fers a rigorous method <strong>of</strong> investigating <strong>the</strong>natural world, a method that is based on carefulo b s e rvation <strong>and</strong> well-reasoned argument thatincludes <strong>the</strong> formation <strong>and</strong> testing <strong>of</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>ses.Science has demonstrated remarkable power todescribe <strong>the</strong> natural world accurately <strong>and</strong> to identify<strong>the</strong> underlying causes <strong>of</strong> natural phenomena.TREATMENT OF THE METHODS OFSCIENCE IN THIS MODULEScientific knowledge endures because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>strength <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> methods used to obtain it. Thei n s t ructional activities provided by this moduledemonstrate <strong>the</strong> durability <strong>of</strong> scientific concepts <strong>and</strong>scientific methods. Some activities deal with geneticsconcepts never mentioned or observed by GregorMendel in his nineteenth-century work, yet <strong>the</strong>senew discoveries also involve <strong>the</strong> fundamental principles<strong>of</strong> genetics that he did report. New discoveriessometimes refute pre-existing scientific explanations<strong>of</strong> natural processes, but much more <strong>of</strong>ten <strong>the</strong>yrefine or exp<strong>and</strong> prior knowledge. New work generallybuilds on old work ra<strong>the</strong>r than razing <strong>the</strong> site.Too <strong>of</strong>ten <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science are presented as aset <strong>of</strong> steps to be memorized. Students recite <strong>the</strong>mby rote, with little underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> less application.This module takes a very different approach,providing activities that help students experience<strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science <strong>and</strong> connect <strong>the</strong>se methodswith events in <strong>the</strong>ir own lives. Presentation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>methods <strong>of</strong> science is interwoven into <strong>the</strong> learningexperience that reveals new concepts in genetics.Activity 5 is designed specifically to help studentsconnect <strong>the</strong>ir science process skills with familiarevents outside <strong>the</strong> classroom.THE GOALS OF SCIENCE ANDMETHODS USED TO MEET THEMIn this section <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module we present a discussion<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> goals <strong>of</strong> science in order to provide a contextfor <strong>the</strong> discussion <strong>of</strong> scientific methods. In addition,13


Section III ■ The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Sciencewe discuss <strong>the</strong> rationale for, <strong>and</strong> benefit <strong>of</strong>, <strong>the</strong> rigorouslydisciplined methods <strong>of</strong> science. The initialdiscussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science(NMS) is framed around six questions, summarizedin Figure 7. These questions reappear in variousways throughout <strong>the</strong> student activities <strong>and</strong> are usedfor student assessment in Activity 5.This discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science is notexhaustive; ra<strong>the</strong>r, it builds a view <strong>of</strong> science in <strong>the</strong>language <strong>of</strong> questions <strong>and</strong> concerns that arise in <strong>the</strong>laboratory <strong>and</strong> in <strong>the</strong> classroom. Please keep in mindthat this view <strong>of</strong> science is not new; early geneticistsused <strong>the</strong>se methods, for example. The fact thatMendel used rigorous scientific methods <strong>and</strong> criteriato guide his work is <strong>the</strong> reason that MendelianFigure 7 A snapshot <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> goals <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong>science1. What does science try to do?■ Science tries to provide causal explanations fornatural phenomena.2. How do we know that causal explanations are on<strong>the</strong> right track?Causal explanations are on <strong>the</strong> right track when■ <strong>the</strong>y demonstrate predictive power;■ o<strong>the</strong>r observations (evidence) rule out competingcausal explanations.3. What counts as evidence in science?Evidence in science includes■ empirical data;■ existing explanations about related phenomena thatare supported by independent data.4. What makes science objective?■ Science is conducted by a rigorous set <strong>of</strong> methods.■ Authority <strong>and</strong> fame by <strong>the</strong>mselves are not sufficientto establish <strong>the</strong> scientific validity <strong>of</strong> an explanation.5. How <strong>and</strong> why does scientific knowledge change?■ Scientific explanations always are open to change.■ New evidence may show that an existing explanationis inadequate <strong>and</strong> that it needs to change.6. What is <strong>the</strong> difference between pseudoscience<strong>and</strong> science?■ Pseudoscience fails to meet <strong>the</strong> intellectually rigorousrequirements <strong>of</strong> science.■ Internal consistency sets scientific knowledge apartfrom pseudoscience.genetics continues to provide reliable explanationsfor many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fundamental processes <strong>of</strong> inheritance.These same methods that have recentlybrought to light some intriguing “nontraditional”explanations <strong>of</strong> genetic mechanisms will guidefuture scientists to new discoveries that will continueto change our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> inheritance.1. What does science try to do?■ Science tries to provide causal explanations fornatural phenomena.Science is an organized pursuit <strong>of</strong> knowledge pertainingto natural processes. Often, science seeks toprovide causal explanations <strong>of</strong> those processes.(Ano<strong>the</strong>r term for a scientific explanation is “hypo<strong>the</strong>sis.”A hypo<strong>the</strong>sis is a scientific explanation that isbased on evidence, that is intended to account forsome aspect <strong>of</strong> a natural process, <strong>and</strong> that can betested.) A causal explanation proposes an account <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> processes <strong>and</strong> mechanisms that produce a givenbiological phenomenon. One must establish a logicalor meaningful connection between a phenomenon<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> event purported as its cause, <strong>and</strong> this connectionis made through observation, through testing(which <strong>of</strong>ten includes experimentation), <strong>and</strong>,<strong>of</strong>ten, through statistical analysis. These rigoroussteps are necessary to distinguish <strong>the</strong> actual causesfrom r<strong>and</strong>omly or coincidentally associated events.It is not easy to establish causality. People who donot employ scientific reasoning, however, sometimesmistakenly assume that one event is <strong>the</strong> cause<strong>of</strong> a second event solely because it precedes <strong>the</strong> secondevent. Science requires a much stronger associationto establish a causal process. A classic examplefrom folklore states that eating strawberries whilepregnant can cause birthmarks on one’s newbornchild. The timing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> putative cause (eating strawberries)precedes <strong>the</strong> observed result (birthmark),but <strong>the</strong>re is no credible <strong>and</strong> relevant evidence tosupport <strong>the</strong> first event as a cause <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> second one.Constant <strong>and</strong> regular temporal association <strong>of</strong> eventss u p p o rts <strong>the</strong> proposal <strong>of</strong> a causal relationship, but sciencerequires fur<strong>the</strong>r analysis. Geneticists use statisticalmethods to test large sets <strong>of</strong> evidence to determinewhe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>ir regular <strong>and</strong> constant associationoccurs more frequently than chance would predict.Scientists also look for a step-wise biological process14©1997 by BSCS.


The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science ■ Section Ithat starts with <strong>the</strong> cause <strong>and</strong> ends with <strong>the</strong> observ e deffect. An example is a mutation in <strong>the</strong> gene phenylalaninehydroxylase. If an individual is homozygousfor this mutation, <strong>the</strong> enzyme will be deficient, blocking<strong>the</strong> metabolism <strong>of</strong> phenylalanine to tyrosine.Phenylalanine is converted through an alternate pathwayto phenylpyruvic acid <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r metabolites thatcan cause mental retardation, one symptom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>disorder phenylketonuria (PKU). The associationsbetween <strong>the</strong>se steps are well established; <strong>the</strong>y constitutevalid evidence for <strong>the</strong> genetic causation <strong>of</strong> PKU .Evidence for a causal relationship also is streng<strong>the</strong>nedwhen it is observed by more than one person <strong>and</strong>when o<strong>the</strong>r lines <strong>of</strong> evidence point to <strong>the</strong> same conclusions.A good explanation <strong>of</strong> causation also displays<strong>the</strong> power to predict <strong>the</strong> outcome <strong>of</strong> related eventsa c c u r a t e l y.The causes <strong>of</strong> biological phenomena are complex<strong>and</strong> diverse, <strong>and</strong> we build our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>m in stages. Explanations <strong>of</strong>ten begin withsimple, accurate observations that do not immediatelyattempt to address causation. This point isbrought out for students in <strong>the</strong> Engage Activity <strong>of</strong>this module. Generally, however, scientists are lookingfor clues to <strong>the</strong> mechanisms that underlie specificbiological processes, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y begin to build anunderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se processes by explainingwhatever mechanisms are accessible to <strong>the</strong>ir observationat a given time. This approach is analogous toteasing out <strong>the</strong> threads in a knot; it is easier <strong>and</strong>more efficient to attack <strong>the</strong> knot bit by bit ra<strong>the</strong>rthan trying to untie it all in one motion.The more fully we identify various aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>causes <strong>of</strong> a given phenomenon, <strong>the</strong> more we reinforce<strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> our explanation. As we unravelnew layers in a scientific puzzle, through new observationsor new reasoning, we test new explanationsagainst existing knowledge. If a new explanation isconsistent with previous knowledge, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> newwork reinforces <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> earlier work. If,however, <strong>the</strong> new explanation is not consistent wi<strong>the</strong>xtant knowledge, we must ei<strong>the</strong>r discard <strong>the</strong> newexplanation, or revise <strong>the</strong> prior knowledge.One key point to consider for students is <strong>the</strong>ir perception<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word “<strong>the</strong>ory. ” Students <strong>of</strong>ten mistakenlythink that this word means about <strong>the</strong> same as anephemeral guess or a hunch; <strong>the</strong>y see a <strong>the</strong>ory as anunsubstantiated idea. A reference called Usage <strong>and</strong>Abusage, A Guide to Good English ( Pa rtridge, 1994)states that “<strong>the</strong>ory is occasionally used loosely. . .where e x p e c t a t i o n or o p i n i o n would be preferable.”For instance, a student might say, “In <strong>the</strong>ory, I shoulddo well on tomorr o w’s test.” The actual meaning <strong>of</strong> at h e o ry in a scientific context is very different, <strong>and</strong>you need to emphasize this point explicitly for students.A <strong>the</strong>ory, as scientists use <strong>the</strong> term, refers to alarge-scale explanation, or series <strong>of</strong> explanations, thatdescribes <strong>the</strong> causes <strong>of</strong> many natural processes(Rosenberg, 1985). An accepted scientific <strong>the</strong>ory isv e ry well substantiated by evidence, it has been builtlogically upon valid assumptions, <strong>and</strong> it has beenextensively tested. If a <strong>the</strong>ory were not substantiated,it would not be taken seriously by <strong>the</strong> scientific commu n i t y, no matter who proposed it. A scientific <strong>the</strong>oryis not established nor refuted on <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> personalopinion that fails to follow <strong>the</strong> discipline <strong>of</strong>scientific methods; ra<strong>the</strong>r, a <strong>the</strong>ory is an explanation<strong>of</strong> far-reaching significance so well tested <strong>and</strong> suppo rted by such an abundance <strong>of</strong> credible evidencethat it becomes a broadly accepted <strong>and</strong> fundamentalscientific concept. There are a number <strong>of</strong> powerf u l<strong>the</strong>ories in biology: for example, cell <strong>the</strong>ory, chromosome<strong>the</strong>ory, germ <strong>the</strong>ory, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> evolution.Each is supported by overwhelming amounts <strong>of</strong>evidence. The following discussion describes <strong>the</strong> role<strong>of</strong> evidence <strong>and</strong> testing in <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> building scientificexplanations.2. How do we know that causal explanationsare on <strong>the</strong> right track?Causal explanations are on <strong>the</strong> right trackwhen■<strong>the</strong>y demonstrate predictive power;■ o<strong>the</strong>r observations (evidence) rule out competingcausal explanations.The short answer is that explanations are on <strong>the</strong>right track when <strong>the</strong>y enable us to predict what wecan later observe. This simple statement describes apowerful property <strong>of</strong> scientific explanation. By “predict,”scientists mean something very specific: <strong>the</strong>ability to describe accurately <strong>and</strong> in precise detail anoutcome <strong>of</strong> a particular natural event based on <strong>the</strong>explanation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cause or causes <strong>of</strong> that event.Observation confirms <strong>the</strong> accuracy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> prediction.15©1997 by BSCS.


Section III ■ The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> ScienceFor example, if we hypo<strong>the</strong>size that a particular DNAsequence functions as an enhancer <strong>of</strong> transcription,we might predict that inserting that sequence nextto a gene will increase <strong>the</strong> amount <strong>of</strong> messenger RNAproduced from that gene. This outcome is testable,observable, <strong>and</strong>, indeed, quantifiable. If <strong>the</strong> amount<strong>of</strong> RNA does increase, this observation reinforces <strong>the</strong>hypo<strong>the</strong>sized causal role <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> enhancer sequence.Prediction in <strong>the</strong> scientific sense need not be solelyabout future events. Instead, we can ask whe<strong>the</strong>r aproposed causal explanation would have predictedphenomena that have already happened <strong>and</strong> can beobserved. By “predict” we mean that <strong>the</strong> explanationis consistent with <strong>the</strong> observed outcome. For example,<strong>the</strong> explanation <strong>of</strong> genetic code lets a scientistpredict how a particular point-mutation in a DNAsequence will have altered <strong>the</strong> amino acid order in<strong>the</strong> encoded protein. By checking <strong>the</strong> actual aminoacid sequence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mutant gene product, <strong>the</strong>results can confirm or refute <strong>the</strong> predictive power <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> explanation.Although accurate prediction helps to validate thatan explanation is on <strong>the</strong> right track, an accurate predictionis not by itself sufficient for this purpose.Two o<strong>the</strong>r criteria are important: a) we must ru l eout competing explanations <strong>of</strong> what we observ ebefore we can be confident that we have <strong>the</strong> corr e c tcause, <strong>and</strong> b) we must ensure that <strong>the</strong> proposedcausal explanations are consistent with what weknow about related biological processes or o<strong>the</strong>rareas <strong>of</strong> knowledge, such as chemistry or physics. Insome cases, only a partial answer is possiblebecause <strong>the</strong> available data provide insight into a limitednumber <strong>of</strong> possible explanations. Perhaps <strong>the</strong>investigator can eliminate all but two or three explanations.Such scientific work has considerable valuein that it simplifies <strong>the</strong> questions to be addressed infuture work <strong>and</strong> allows scientists to focus <strong>the</strong>irenergies <strong>and</strong> resources on <strong>the</strong> most promising lines<strong>of</strong> inquiry.Ideally, competing explanations will be inconsistentwith <strong>the</strong> observed data, but still o<strong>the</strong>r criteria mustbe met before we can accept an explanation as valid.A proposed causal hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that predicts <strong>the</strong> databut that is highly implausible on o<strong>the</strong>r grounds is notwell confirmed. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, a valid explanationmust be based on <strong>the</strong> combination <strong>of</strong> credible evidencethat is relevant to <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis in question,on sound reasoning, <strong>and</strong> on reliable assumptions.Here, “relevant” means that <strong>the</strong> evidence has a rigorousconnection with <strong>the</strong> idea being tested, ei<strong>the</strong>rto support or refute it. A conclusion or inference thatdoes not follow from <strong>the</strong> premise is a non sequitur,<strong>and</strong> it is not relevant evidence. An example <strong>of</strong> correctbut irrelevant evidence is found in Activity 1 withregard to sex determination; <strong>the</strong> observation thathuman females produce fewer gametes than dohuman males is correct, but it does not address <strong>the</strong>issue at h<strong>and</strong>.In summary, we know a scientific explanation is on<strong>the</strong> right track when we can answer “yes” to <strong>the</strong> followingquestions:■Does <strong>the</strong> proposed causal mechanism predictwhat we observe?■ Does no o<strong>the</strong>r causal mechanism predict or fitwith that data?■ Is our hypo<strong>the</strong>sis reasonable given scientificallyreliable knowledge about o<strong>the</strong>r related areas <strong>of</strong>biology (or any o<strong>the</strong>r science)?“The great tragedy <strong>of</strong> science: abeautiful hypo<strong>the</strong>sis destroyed byan ugly fact.”T.H. HuxleyOne <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> strengths <strong>of</strong> a scientific explanation is thatit is testable. Indeed, explanations that cannot betested do not count as scientifically valid. The ideasdiscussed in <strong>the</strong> preceding paragraphs explain whycontrolled experiments are important, particularly toestablish a causal relationship. If we propose that Acauses B, in <strong>the</strong> ideal case we would like to holdeverything fixed or unchanged except A. Then wecan change or manipulate A <strong>and</strong> observe <strong>the</strong> consequences.If B changes only when A does, we havegood evidence that A causes B.Controlled experiments in <strong>the</strong> laboratory attempt toget as close to <strong>the</strong> above ideal as possible, althoughno laboratory experiment literally controls or holdsfixed every possible confounding factor. For example,if you were studying <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> light on plantgrowth, you might design an experiment in whichyou keep water <strong>and</strong> temperature constant <strong>and</strong> vary16©1997 by BSCS.


The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science ■ Section IIIlight intensity. You have not, <strong>of</strong> course, controlledpotential fluctuations in seismic activity, air pollution,or solar flare activity. Only <strong>the</strong> major <strong>and</strong> obvious factorsare addressed. Moreover, laboratory experimentsare not <strong>the</strong> only way to answer scientific questions.Observation <strong>of</strong> naturally occurring events,without manipulation by <strong>the</strong> investigator, can helpd e t e rmine <strong>the</strong> extent to which an explanation is on<strong>the</strong> right track. In short, nature provides us with “naturalexperiments.” These kinds <strong>of</strong> tests are widespreadin science—cosmology <strong>and</strong> astronomy rely on<strong>the</strong>m heavily, as does, <strong>of</strong> course, biology. Often, studentsthink <strong>of</strong> evidence as being exclusively <strong>the</strong> property <strong>of</strong> manipulated experiments. It is important tohelp students appreciate <strong>the</strong> power <strong>of</strong> careful observation<strong>of</strong> natural events. Much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> initial work <strong>of</strong>mapping <strong>and</strong> sequencing in <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Projectis essentially observational.3. What counts as evidence in science?Evidence in science includes■empirical data, <strong>and</strong>■ existing explanations about related phenomenathat are supported by independent data.Evidence in science is based on rigorous observationsthat must meet <strong>the</strong> criterion <strong>of</strong> being publiclyobservable. The fact that o<strong>the</strong>rs can confirm orrefute what you claim to observe provides a crucialtest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> scientific validity <strong>of</strong> your explanation.Rumor, speculation, mystical experiences, <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>runsubstantiated information are not accepted ascredible scientific data. Students usually can recitethat scientific experiments must be “repeatable,” butit is important to challenge students to describewhat that statement actually means. Experimentalresults that are observable by o<strong>the</strong>r investigatorshave <strong>the</strong> highest scientific merit. If an experiment orobservation is repeated by <strong>the</strong> same or ano<strong>the</strong>r person,<strong>and</strong> if <strong>the</strong> resultant data are <strong>the</strong> same, <strong>the</strong> credibility<strong>of</strong> that evidence improves.Evidence is more compelling in its ability to supportan explanation when <strong>the</strong> following are true:a. An explanation is streng<strong>the</strong>ned when we havemultiple lines <strong>of</strong> evidence—that is, when we haveevidence from different sources. Different lines <strong>of</strong>evidence for <strong>the</strong> same hypo<strong>the</strong>sis reduce <strong>the</strong> likelihoodthat we ignored confounding factors orthat we spuriously concluded that our hypo<strong>the</strong>sispredicts <strong>the</strong> observable data.b. The more precise our data are <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> more preciselyour causal hypo<strong>the</strong>ses predict what weo b s e rve, <strong>the</strong> stronger is our evidence. To meet<strong>the</strong>se conditions, a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis must be stated carefu l l y, particularly avoiding a focus that is too broad.If <strong>the</strong> scope <strong>of</strong> a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis is excessive, too manyfactors come into play, <strong>and</strong> it is difficult to designmeaningful tests that rule out confounding causes.That is why one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> most important skills in scienceis <strong>the</strong> ability to ask a good question, that is, aquestion that is precise <strong>and</strong> that one can investigateby focusing on one or a few variables. Fo rexample, although biologists investigate <strong>the</strong> nature<strong>of</strong> life, <strong>the</strong>y do not conduct experiments that ask<strong>the</strong> broad question, “What is life?” They ask, ra<strong>the</strong>r,more concise questions that address components<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> larger question. For example, <strong>the</strong>se questionsare more addressable:■ What are <strong>the</strong> major macromolecules found in livingsystems?■ What happens to a cell if we remove its geneticmaterial?■What genes must be present for a cell to functionproperly?Students <strong>of</strong>ten have trouble delineating <strong>the</strong>ir questionsfor term papers or science fairs. You shouldhelp <strong>the</strong>m underst<strong>and</strong> that good science begins witha good question.In addition, if our data are imprecise, <strong>the</strong>n we shouldhave less confidence in our conclusions. An impreciseobservation, such as “The bacterial populationincreased quickly,” is much less useful than is a quantitativeobservation, such as “The population sizedoubled in 33 minutes.” A hypo<strong>the</strong>sis may fail to predict<strong>the</strong> observed data because those data are inaccurate.If a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis fits with data we know to beinaccurate, we must lose confidence in <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis in question.c. Evidence also comes from <strong>the</strong> application <strong>of</strong>knowledge gained through previous work.Hypo<strong>the</strong>ses that already have been tested successfullycan <strong>and</strong> should be used as background knowledgefor testing <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> new causal explanations.A successful test provides reinforcement <strong>of</strong>17©1997 by BSCS.


Section III ■ The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Scienceprevious conclusions at <strong>the</strong> same time that it helpsto establish <strong>the</strong> new idea. A test that fails shouldlead us to re-examine <strong>the</strong> old, as well as <strong>the</strong> new,idea. This situation should cause us to considerm o d i fying existing concepts if sufficient data warrant<strong>the</strong> change. Scientific hypo<strong>the</strong>ses usuallym a ke reference to o<strong>the</strong>r scientific explanations,<strong>and</strong> consistency between explanations is required.Indeed, internal consistency <strong>of</strong> scientific explanationsis an important criterion for validity.4. What makes science objective?■ Science is conducted by a rigorous set <strong>of</strong> methods.■ Authority <strong>and</strong> fame by <strong>the</strong>mselves are not sufficientto establish <strong>the</strong> scientific validity <strong>of</strong> anexplanation.The world exists, regardless <strong>of</strong> our knowledge <strong>of</strong> it; sciencestrives to achieve a description <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> naturalworld that accurately reflects reality. A scientist maywant to believe that his or her preferred hypo<strong>the</strong>sis isc o rrect, but nature does not have to cooperate. Scientificmethods are designed to make <strong>the</strong> corr e l a t i o nbetween reality <strong>and</strong> our descriptions <strong>of</strong> reality as closeas is possible, considering that <strong>the</strong> descriptions arebuilt by humans with limited senses <strong>and</strong> within <strong>the</strong> limitedtime available to explore <strong>the</strong> universe. Scientificunderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> a natural process usually is built instages. For example, investigators recognized <strong>the</strong> existence<strong>of</strong> blood as a liquid <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> cellsfloating in that liquid before <strong>the</strong>y identified hemoglobinas <strong>the</strong> molecular carrier <strong>of</strong> oxygen in red bloodcells. Each stage <strong>of</strong> investigation narrowed <strong>the</strong> gapbetween <strong>the</strong> reality <strong>of</strong> how oxygen was being transpo rted <strong>and</strong> our knowledge <strong>of</strong> that process. The earliestviews were incomplete, but <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong>y werederived using rigorous scientific criteria resulted in<strong>the</strong>ir durability: liquid blood in our vessels does,indeed, carry oxygen throughout <strong>the</strong> body, <strong>and</strong> continuedresearch has revealed many details <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> process.Even when scientists attempt to be completelyobjective, <strong>the</strong>y may find it difficult to do so. Oursenses are limited, although technologies greatlyexp<strong>and</strong> what we can detect. We also are influencedby our emotions, by societal views, by economics,<strong>and</strong> by job pressures.Scientific <strong>the</strong>ories are built according to a rigorous, disciplinedprocess, but <strong>the</strong>ories may not be perf e c treflections <strong>of</strong> nature. The possibility for this discrepancyunderlines <strong>the</strong> need for rigorous criteria in const ruction <strong>of</strong> scientific explanations. The methods <strong>of</strong>science include <strong>the</strong> requirement for publicly observable<strong>and</strong> repeatable evidence, <strong>the</strong> testing <strong>of</strong> an explanationby prediction <strong>and</strong> observation, <strong>the</strong> connectionbetween existing <strong>and</strong> new explanations, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>requirement to rule out competing explanations.These methods provide a st<strong>and</strong>ard that helps preventindividual scientists from finding only what <strong>the</strong>y w a n tto see, whe<strong>the</strong>r consciously through ambition <strong>and</strong>pride, or subconsciously. The system is not perf e c t .The particular path that discovery takes is influencedby human values, particularly because cultural viewso ften determine to which arenas <strong>of</strong> work researchfunds will be directed. The methods <strong>of</strong> science, howev e r, are powerful—though not foolpro<strong>of</strong>—safeguardsthat help to limit <strong>the</strong> impact <strong>of</strong> bias <strong>and</strong> subjectivityin <strong>the</strong> acquisition <strong>and</strong> application <strong>of</strong> scientificknowledge. A system planted on a firm foundation <strong>of</strong>valid premise, sound reasoning, <strong>and</strong> credible evidencehas <strong>the</strong> ability to endure.The collective nature <strong>of</strong> science also encourages objectivityin <strong>the</strong> field. The results <strong>of</strong> individual scientists ares c rutinized by <strong>the</strong> scientific community. Scientistsmust report <strong>the</strong>ir findings, <strong>and</strong> communication mustuse st<strong>and</strong>ardized descriptions so that results are meaningfulto any informed person to whom <strong>the</strong>y are communicated.For example, in <strong>the</strong> Engage Activity <strong>of</strong> thismodule, students discover that imprecise or nonst<strong>and</strong>ardunits <strong>of</strong> measure applied to a description <strong>of</strong> apeanut greatly diminish <strong>the</strong> ability <strong>of</strong> that descriptionto help ano<strong>the</strong>r person to identify <strong>the</strong> same peanut.Scientific communications are subjected to peerreview in journals <strong>and</strong> meetings, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> award <strong>of</strong>research grants also requires assessment by o<strong>the</strong>r scientists.In addition, <strong>the</strong> division <strong>of</strong> labor in scientificresearch provides fur<strong>the</strong>r opportunities for critique <strong>of</strong>research <strong>and</strong> for <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> multiple lines <strong>of</strong>evidence <strong>and</strong> helps to build a more complete explanation.These procedural requirements contribute to <strong>the</strong>intellectual discipline <strong>and</strong> rigor <strong>of</strong> science.“To punish me for my contempt forauthority, Fate made me an authoritymyself.”Albert Einstein18©1997 by BSCS.


The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science ■ Section IIIScience is not monolithic; it attempts to be authoritative,but it is not authoritarian. Cert a i n l y, recognition<strong>of</strong> an “authority” in a particular area <strong>of</strong> studyencourages o<strong>the</strong>r scientists to pay attention to whatis reported by that individual or research group,but <strong>the</strong> requirements for supporting evidence <strong>and</strong>all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r criteria <strong>of</strong> valid science still apply. Afamous name in itself does not establish disciplinedmethods or produce <strong>the</strong> credible evidencerequired to substantiate an idea presented to <strong>the</strong>scientific community. The rules <strong>of</strong> science are <strong>the</strong>same for everyone, <strong>and</strong> anyone who proposes anew explanation for a natural phenomenon ultimatelymust answer <strong>the</strong> same two questions fromany o<strong>the</strong>r scientist: “Where are your data?” <strong>and</strong>“How do you know <strong>the</strong>y are sound?” The establishment<strong>of</strong> valid scientific explanations depends uponreview <strong>and</strong> critique by <strong>the</strong> scientific community atlarge, but <strong>the</strong> critique begins with <strong>the</strong> individual scientist.Scientists are trained to be critics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>irown work. Indeed, <strong>the</strong> British biologist Pe t e rMedawar noted that “most <strong>of</strong> a scientist’s woundsare self-inflicted” (Pyke, 1996). In addition, at anygiven time, a minority view on some important scientificconcept usually exists. With time, if sufficientevidence is brought to light, even an unpopularview can be validated.5. How <strong>and</strong> why does scientific knowledgechange?■ Scientific explanations always are open to change.■ New evidence may show that an existing explanationis inadequate <strong>and</strong> that it needs to change.Most scientific explanations are reinforced by newo b s e rvations. For example, Mendelian geneticsremains durable because it explains an enorm o u snumber <strong>of</strong> observed phenomena. Not all aspects <strong>of</strong>inheritance are explained adequately, however. Scientistsobserved that, despite <strong>the</strong> power <strong>of</strong>Mendelian genetics, <strong>the</strong>se explanations alone cannotexplain phenomena such as variability in onset<strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> some genetic diseases (geneticanticipation). An explanation for anticipationbecame possible with <strong>the</strong> discovery <strong>of</strong> unstabletrinucleotide repeats in mutant genes associatedwith disorders that exhibit anticipation, such asHuntington disease. (See Section V <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>O v e rview for Te a c h e r s, page 43, for a detailed discussion.)In this process, geneticists did not rejectMendelian explanations; <strong>the</strong>re was no scientific reasonto do so. Mendelian explanations endured, butgeneticists recognized that <strong>the</strong>se explanations wereincomplete, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y remedied that problem withnew explanations.Because scientific knowledge is dynamic, as geneticsamply illustrates, scientists expect that explanationswill be extended <strong>and</strong> refined. “Scientists rarely concludethat a question is finally answered or that a naturalphenomenon is completely explained” (AAAS,1989). The corpus <strong>of</strong> accepted scientific knowledge,however, changes only when rigorous criteria aremet; it is not changed based on whim or to pleaseimportant people.Sometimes, new explanations must await new technology.For example, genetics could explain <strong>the</strong> generalinheritance <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease for manyyears before it could explain <strong>the</strong> variability in <strong>the</strong> age<strong>of</strong> onset <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> symptoms <strong>of</strong> this genetic disorder.With <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> molecular techniques forcloning <strong>and</strong> sequencing DNA, scientists discoveredunstable trinucleotide repeats, thus providing <strong>the</strong>foundation <strong>of</strong> an explanation for genetic anticipation.Future work will pursue a more thoroughexplanation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mechanisms through which largenumbers <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats in particular genesbring about genetic disorders; at present our knowledgeis limited to <strong>the</strong> simple correlation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> presence<strong>of</strong> repeats <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> disorders.“The domain <strong>of</strong> ignorance includes all<strong>the</strong> things we know we don’t know, all<strong>the</strong> things we don’t know we don’tknow, <strong>and</strong> all <strong>the</strong> things we think weknow but don’t.”Ann Kerwin6. What is <strong>the</strong> difference between pseudoscience<strong>and</strong> science?■ Pseudoscience fails to meet <strong>the</strong> intellectually rigorousrequirements <strong>of</strong> science.■ Internal consistency sets scientific knowledgeapart from pseudoscience.Pseudoscience can be defined as <strong>the</strong> promotion <strong>of</strong>unsubstantiated, allegedly scientific opinions. Some19©1997 by BSCS.


Section III ■ The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se opinions may be very appealing <strong>and</strong> thus<strong>the</strong>y gain popularity, but <strong>the</strong>y lack supporting, credibleevidence. Pseudoscientific ideas have not beentested reliably. Often, pseudoscientific ideas are builton inaccurate premises, or <strong>the</strong>y do not follow logicallyfrom what is observable. Indeed, pseudosciencecould describe concepts that might be accurate,but <strong>the</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> scientific method in <strong>the</strong>pseudoscientific assertions prevents us from beingable to determine <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ideas beingadduced. Pseudoscience <strong>of</strong>ten involves claims forwhich it is almost impossible to provide scientificevidence. Just as a poorly formulated hypo<strong>the</strong>siscannot easily be tested, pseudoscientific claims usuallyare so vague, ill-formed, <strong>and</strong> undetailed that <strong>the</strong>ymake no specific predictions <strong>and</strong> cannot be testedthrough credible experiments.Pseudoscientists are those who refuse to adhere to scientificst<strong>and</strong>ards—<strong>the</strong>y do not try to rule out competingexplanations, <strong>the</strong>y do not subject <strong>the</strong>ir results to<strong>the</strong> scrutiny <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> scientific community, <strong>the</strong>y do notrely on publicly observable data, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y do not test<strong>the</strong>ir hypo<strong>the</strong>ses against scientific results elsewhere.O ften, ideas based in pseudoscience are inconsistentwith o<strong>the</strong>r well-tested concepts. Indeed, pseudoscientificclaims may not be internally consistent. Wi t h o u trigorous criteria for evidence <strong>and</strong> reasoning, proponents<strong>of</strong> pseudoscience are not likely to recognize<strong>the</strong>se inconsistencies. We may find ourselves drawntoward <strong>the</strong> ideas put forth by pseudoscientistsbecause <strong>the</strong>y have an emotional appeal, but, when wedo so, <strong>of</strong>ten it is because we are being intellectuallyl a z y. Science is not easy, but it does provide soundresults. New scientific findings sometimes challengec o m f o rting, long-st<strong>and</strong>ing assumptions about <strong>the</strong> naturalworld, but scientists must go where <strong>the</strong> data take<strong>the</strong>m even if <strong>the</strong> destination is a bit unsettling. Many <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> characteristics that establish <strong>the</strong> overarchingnature <strong>of</strong> science are summarized in Figure 8.THE STUDENT’S VIEW OF SCIENTIFIC METHODSToo <strong>of</strong>ten students see science as ei<strong>the</strong>r a collection<strong>of</strong> facts to be memorized or as a set <strong>of</strong> experiments,by which students mean manipulations <strong>of</strong> chemicals<strong>and</strong> glassware in a laboratory. Students may not recognize<strong>the</strong> rigorous thinking skills that go into <strong>the</strong>careful observation, <strong>the</strong> reasoned explanation, or <strong>the</strong>design <strong>and</strong> interpretation <strong>of</strong> experiments as essentialelements in <strong>the</strong> practice <strong>of</strong> science. It is possible tohave students experience <strong>the</strong>se <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r elements<strong>of</strong> scientific investigation by making <strong>the</strong> thoughtprocesses as evident <strong>and</strong> important as <strong>the</strong> processesFigure 8 Some characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> scienceScientific explanations arerooted in causal processes.Scientific knowledge isdurable but dynamic.A historical view <strong>of</strong> science showssimilarities between biology <strong>and</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r fields <strong>of</strong> study.Science provides explanations for naturalphenomena.Causal processes are <strong>of</strong>ten complex<strong>and</strong> thus cannot be summarized insimple, universal laws.Scientific explanations are based onevidence.Multiple lines <strong>of</strong> evidence streng<strong>the</strong>nan explanation.A scientific explanation is a continuum<strong>of</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ing that exp<strong>and</strong>s as newevidence for causal processes comesto light.Different levels <strong>of</strong> evidence exist <strong>and</strong>thus provide different levels <strong>of</strong> confidencein an explanation.As new evidence appears, scientificexplanations are modified; explanatorypower generally increases through thisprocess.New work builds on old work.Science (at its best) is nei<strong>the</strong>r monolithicnor authoritarian.Science is open to new ideas, includingideas initially expressed by aminority <strong>of</strong> scientists.Scientists expect that explanations willbe extended <strong>and</strong> refined.Because science is a human endeavor,it reflects human behaviors <strong>and</strong> limitationsin human abilities.The views expressed in <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rcolumns <strong>of</strong> this figure are true for scienceas a whole, but <strong>the</strong>y may notapply consistently to each individualscientist.Not all good science must be experimental(“bench”) science; observationalstudies <strong>of</strong>ten provide powerful lines <strong>of</strong>evidence <strong>and</strong> alter scientific explanations.This especially is <strong>the</strong> case for historicalsciences such as paleontology.©1997 by BSCS.20


The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science ■ Section III<strong>of</strong> physical manipulation. The following discussionelaborates four simple components <strong>of</strong> scientificinvestigation (see Figure 9). Students may benefitfrom your calling attention to <strong>the</strong>se steps. As you doso, however, you should point out that <strong>the</strong>se componentsdo not always—perhaps not even most <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> time—occur in <strong>the</strong> sequence indicated. Theprocess is much more fluid than a simple listing <strong>of</strong>steps conveys, <strong>and</strong> a scientist may be engaged in anyor all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se steps at <strong>the</strong> same time. In fact, mostscientists probably do not proceed consciouslythrough <strong>the</strong>se steps. It is, ra<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>ir training <strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong> requirements <strong>of</strong> scientific validity that lead <strong>the</strong>mto incorporate all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se steps at some point as<strong>the</strong>y pursue answers to a particular question. Thei m p o rtant point for students, <strong>the</strong>n, is not thatresearch follows some prescribed, lock-step process,but ra<strong>the</strong>r that all research embraces <strong>the</strong> same habits<strong>of</strong> mind.Careful <strong>and</strong> precise observation, coupled with accuratereporting, is a powerful scientific tool. Observ a-tions involve descriptions <strong>of</strong> events as <strong>the</strong>y occur in<strong>the</strong> natural world or as <strong>the</strong> outcome <strong>of</strong> experiments.O b s e rvations in science must be as accurate <strong>and</strong> reliableas possible. Precision in instrumentation is crucial;technology that malfunctions (for example, afaulty DNA sequencing machine or a bug in computers<strong>of</strong>tware) can lead to inaccurate observations. Reliableobservations are those that are reported bymore than one observer <strong>and</strong> that are, in principle,replicable by any future observ e r. Reports that cannotbe replicated at present have limited reliability, evenFigure 9 Simplified steps in <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> discoveryDefine a question based on previous knowledge <strong>and</strong>rigorous observation.Propose a potential explanation (a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis). Thisexplanation must take into account what is alreadyknown, <strong>and</strong> it must be testable.Test <strong>the</strong> explanation. If evidence supports it, <strong>the</strong> explanationgains validity. If evidence does not support <strong>the</strong>explanation, <strong>the</strong> explanation must be ab<strong>and</strong>oned <strong>and</strong> anew one sought.Test new evidence against existing explanations. Ifcredible evidence shows that existing explanations areinadequate, <strong>the</strong>y must be modified. The new explanationsmust meet scientific criteria, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y will requirerepeated testing to establish <strong>the</strong>ir predictive power.though <strong>the</strong>y could be replicated at some point in <strong>the</strong>future. Until that occurs, such reports should playonly a very limited role in scientific investigation.Observations must be done carefully to minimize <strong>the</strong>effect <strong>of</strong> extraneous factors. One must report observationswith precision. An example from Activity 3illustrates <strong>the</strong>se points. If a student reports that aparticular person has a copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene associatedwith Huntington disease containing trinucleotiderepeats, or even that <strong>the</strong> gene has many trinucleotiderepeats, <strong>the</strong> information is insufficient toindicate whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> symptoms <strong>of</strong> Huntington diseaselikely will result. It is more precise <strong>and</strong> moremeaningful to note that <strong>the</strong> gene has “more than 40trinucleotide repeats,” because about 40 copies <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> CAG repeat represent a threshold level thatleaves <strong>the</strong> region very unstable <strong>and</strong> generally resultsin Huntington disease. It is even more precise toreport <strong>the</strong> exact number <strong>of</strong> CAG repeats, such as 95,thus supplying <strong>the</strong> data one needs to predict moreaccurately <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> symptoms for <strong>the</strong> personin question.Careful observations can be <strong>the</strong> source by which ascientist’s attention is called to a particular problemor unexplained phenomenon. After recognizing aproblem or puzzle, a scientist must formulate atestable question (hypo<strong>the</strong>sis). Often <strong>the</strong> question isa smaller part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> overall puzzle. Testing <strong>the</strong>hypo<strong>the</strong>sis comes next, <strong>and</strong> testing requires additionalcareful observations <strong>and</strong> reporting. The proposedexplanation generally describes a causalprocess. As scientists pursue explanations for naturalphenomena, <strong>the</strong>y also must take into accountexisting or durable knowledge. Students <strong>of</strong>ten aresurprised to learn that <strong>the</strong> first place to find an explanationor <strong>the</strong> answer to a question is <strong>the</strong> library, not<strong>the</strong> laboratory. Because reinventing <strong>the</strong> wheel is nota good use <strong>of</strong> research time <strong>and</strong> funds, scientistslook for adequate explanations in <strong>the</strong> existing,durable knowledge that already has met <strong>the</strong> rigorouscriteria <strong>of</strong> scientific validity.The study <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease <strong>of</strong>fers an example<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> scientific literature as part <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> discovery process. Scientists investigating Huntingtondisease read reports that a molecular mechanismhad been found to explain genetic anticipation21©1997 by BSCS.


Section III ■ The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science(variability in age <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>and</strong> in severity <strong>of</strong> symptoms)for ano<strong>the</strong>r autosomal dominant disorder,myotonic dystrophy. Sequencing <strong>the</strong> mutantmyotonic dystrophy gene revealed an expansion in<strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats from one generation<strong>of</strong> affected individuals to <strong>the</strong>ir more severelyaffected <strong>of</strong>fspring. This observation led investigatorsto look for unstable trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong>mutant Huntington disease gene. They tested <strong>the</strong>chromosomes <strong>of</strong> individuals known to have Huntingtondisease <strong>and</strong> confirmed <strong>the</strong> phenomenon <strong>of</strong>exp<strong>and</strong>ed repeats. Note that <strong>the</strong> establishment <strong>of</strong>causality for genetic anticipation occurred in stages.Scientists first established that anticipation occurs<strong>and</strong> subsequently identified unstable trinucleotiderepeats as <strong>the</strong> generalized cause <strong>of</strong> anticipation. Studentsmodel this sequential process <strong>of</strong> discovery in<strong>the</strong> procedural steps <strong>of</strong> Activity 3. The next step forscientists is to determine how this phenomenon <strong>of</strong>unstable repeats brings about <strong>the</strong> related genetic disorder.In Activity 3, you can help students see thatpieces <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> puzzle still are missing.Activity 2 addresses extranuclear inheritance in <strong>the</strong>f o rm <strong>of</strong> mitochondrial inheritance. In this activity,students initially may select a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis from amongtraditional inheritance patterns to explain <strong>the</strong> patternin two pedigrees that show mitochondrial inheritance.Students must test <strong>the</strong>ir hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, using statistics;<strong>the</strong>y flip a coin to model <strong>the</strong> likelihood that agiven trait will occur according to <strong>the</strong> inheritance patte rn <strong>the</strong>y have chosen. The failure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir hypo<strong>the</strong>sisto predict <strong>the</strong> observed outcome with any significantdegree <strong>of</strong> accuracy will suggest that <strong>the</strong>hypo<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>of</strong> a traditional inheritance pattern is notadequate. Students <strong>the</strong>n must seek a new explanationoutside <strong>the</strong> durable <strong>and</strong> traditional patterns <strong>of</strong>inheritance. This process <strong>of</strong> discovery leads <strong>the</strong> studentsto build a nontraditional explanation <strong>of</strong> inheritance—mitochondrialtransmission—to account for<strong>the</strong> inheritance pattern <strong>the</strong>y observe in <strong>the</strong> puzzlingp e d i g r e e s .The development <strong>of</strong> a new explanation usually doesnot negate existing knowledge. More <strong>of</strong>ten, new explanationsextend existing scientific knowledge. The disco v e ry <strong>of</strong> DNA <strong>and</strong> RNA as genetic material that houses<strong>the</strong> genetic code did not refute <strong>the</strong> observation thatchromosomes are <strong>the</strong> location <strong>of</strong> genes; ra<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>molecular data brought a more detailed <strong>and</strong> refinedview <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene. Development <strong>of</strong> a new explanationrequires much more than imagination. New ideasneed to connect conceptually to existing knowledge,<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y must be supported by a sufficiently largebody <strong>of</strong> evidence to make <strong>the</strong>m convincing. Sometimesit is readily apparent that existing knowledgecannot explain <strong>the</strong> phenomenon in question. Fo rexample, Mendelian genetics assumes that genes arestable, <strong>and</strong> it explains Huntington disease on <strong>the</strong> basis<strong>of</strong> autosomal dominant transmission. Substantial evidencefrom family pedigrees <strong>and</strong>, more recently, fromdirect analysis <strong>of</strong> DNA supports this explanation.Mendelian genetics, however, has no explanation forvariation in age <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> Huntingtondisease. As a consequence, geneticists defined a newproblem: What characteristic in <strong>the</strong> mutant Huntingtondisease gene causes this variability?SUMMARYDisciplined scientific investigation is designed toproduce ever better explanations for how thingswork in <strong>the</strong> natural world. There are, however, limitationsto this process that are worth emphasizingfor students. This process does not, for example,lead to absolute truth, where “absolute” means final<strong>and</strong> complete: scientific explanations always areopen to change. The foregoing review <strong>of</strong> geneticanticipation showed that Mendelian genetics is not<strong>the</strong> final explanation for transmission <strong>of</strong> genetic disorders.Although scientists have added a new chapterabout trinucleotide repeats, this chapter remainsincomplete because we do not know how <strong>the</strong> number<strong>of</strong> repeats increases nor do we know how <strong>the</strong>repeats influence basic cellular processes. We are<strong>the</strong>refore helpless to alter <strong>the</strong> outcome o<strong>the</strong>r thanto choose not to reproduce. Thus, genetic explanations,like all scientific explanations, are openended.This does not mean that open-ended explanationsare false or poorly supported or wort h l e s s .Open-ended explanations that result from scientificinvestigation in all cases have greater intellectualweight <strong>and</strong> authority than proposed explanationsthat have not been subjected to <strong>the</strong> requirements <strong>of</strong>scientific investigation. Scientific explanations maybe incomplete, but it is not true that anything goes.This is an important point for your students.22©1997 by BSCS.


The <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> Science ■ Section IIIBecause science, at its best, produces powerful explanationsthat always are open to critical analysis <strong>and</strong>revision—even if only in <strong>the</strong> direction <strong>of</strong> greater precision—scientists<strong>the</strong>mselves must be open to <strong>the</strong>work <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs. It is for this reason that science is apublic activity; it is not <strong>the</strong> private province <strong>of</strong> any onescientist. As a result <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> collective nature <strong>of</strong> science,scientists expect o<strong>the</strong>r scientists will review <strong>and</strong> test<strong>the</strong>ir work. As a fur<strong>the</strong>r result, science proceeds necessarilyalong multiple, <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten complementary,lines <strong>of</strong> investigation, as we saw with <strong>the</strong> cross-fert i l-ization <strong>of</strong> research on Huntington disease <strong>and</strong>research on myotonic dystrophy. This collectivenature <strong>of</strong> science helps avoid bias <strong>and</strong> reduces subje c t i v i t y, <strong>and</strong> it imposes on all scientists <strong>the</strong> intellectualdiscipline that comes with being accountable too<strong>the</strong>r scientists.<strong>Genetics</strong> also <strong>of</strong>fers a look at <strong>the</strong> cultural <strong>and</strong> historicalcontext <strong>of</strong> science. The rediscovery <strong>of</strong> Mendel’swork 35 years after its publication demonstrates <strong>the</strong>importance <strong>of</strong> precise communication in science<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong> a cultural context that is ready toaccept new knowledge. The examples <strong>of</strong> nontraditionalinheritance included in this module are not<strong>the</strong> first examples <strong>of</strong> extensions <strong>of</strong> Mendel’s explanations;even genetic linkage was a new twist onMendel’s ideas made possible by <strong>the</strong> observation <strong>of</strong>chromosome behavior.23©1997 by BSCS.


Section IVELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong>Social Implications <strong>of</strong>Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>Genetic knowledge <strong>and</strong> technology raise numerousethical, legal, <strong>and</strong> social issues, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> U.S. Congresshas m<strong>and</strong>ated approximately three percent <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> annual budget for <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project(HGP) to support research, discussion, <strong>and</strong> publiceducation about <strong>the</strong> social implications <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>human genome research. The goals <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ethical,Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications (ELSI) program <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>HGP are as follows:■ to anticipate <strong>and</strong> address <strong>the</strong> ethical, legal, <strong>and</strong>social implications for individuals <strong>and</strong> society <strong>of</strong>mapping <strong>and</strong> sequencing <strong>the</strong> human genome;■ to stimulate public discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> issues; <strong>and</strong>■ to develop policy options to ensure that <strong>the</strong>information is used for <strong>the</strong> benefit <strong>of</strong> individuals<strong>and</strong> society.The activities in this module address all three goals,<strong>and</strong> Activity 4 particularly emphasizes <strong>the</strong>se ideasthrough ethical <strong>and</strong> policy questions raised by genetictesting in conjunction with new genetics concepts.Research, including that supported by <strong>the</strong> HumanGenome Project, continues to uncover new mechanisms<strong>of</strong> inheritance, <strong>and</strong> with <strong>the</strong>se discoveries willcome additional ELSI concerns.A central concept in this module is that new geneticknowledge has led to a rethinking <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> gene. In classical Mendelian genetics, <strong>the</strong> gene isseen as a physically stable entity. The traditional conceptassumes that <strong>the</strong> gene does not change duringtransmission, nor does it move from a fixed place within<strong>the</strong> genome. Nontraditional inheritance extends <strong>the</strong>concept <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene to a much more flexible unit <strong>of</strong>i n f o rmation. Genes are sometimes unstable, in term s<strong>of</strong> location <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> sequence. One example <strong>of</strong> instabilityis <strong>the</strong> expansion or contraction <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong>nucleotides in a given gene. Ano<strong>the</strong>r example involvesgenetic elements, called transposons, that can movewithin <strong>the</strong> genome. Scientists did not predict <strong>the</strong>sechanges as <strong>the</strong>y constructed <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene,a process that began with Mendel <strong>and</strong> resumed ine a rnest in <strong>the</strong> early decades <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> twentieth centuryfollowing <strong>the</strong> rediscovery <strong>of</strong> his work. The central ELSIquestion in this module is: How does our changingconcept <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene <strong>and</strong> inheritance raise ethical,legal, <strong>and</strong> social implications for individuals,communities, <strong>and</strong> society?This module focuses on ELSI topics that have notreceived a great deal <strong>of</strong> attention in <strong>the</strong> literature.These topics merit consideration in this module particularlybecause <strong>the</strong>y concern <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods<strong>of</strong> science. For instance, questions <strong>of</strong> interest herei n c l u d e :■ What is <strong>the</strong> nature (<strong>and</strong> value) <strong>of</strong> genetic knowledgeproduced by <strong>the</strong> HGP <strong>and</strong> related geneticsresearch?25


Section IV ■ ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications <strong>of</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>■ How does society perceive <strong>and</strong> use that knowledge?■ How does society influence research?O<strong>the</strong>r topics <strong>of</strong> interest focus on ethical issues associatedwith <strong>the</strong> HGP, particularly <strong>the</strong> knowledge thatno one is free from <strong>the</strong> risk <strong>of</strong> genetically relatedhealth problems. One topic <strong>of</strong> particular concern forstudents is <strong>the</strong> question <strong>of</strong> how to evaluate scientificexplanations, including scientific reports in <strong>the</strong> laypress. Finally, ethical issues involve prudential judgmentsabout <strong>the</strong> clinical application <strong>of</strong> genetic information<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> decision-making capacity <strong>of</strong>teenagers with respect to such information.DEVELOPING NEW CONCEPTS OF THE GENE,GENETIC HEALTH, AND GENETIC DISEASEA basic concept in genetics is that phenotype is afunction <strong>of</strong> genotype <strong>and</strong> environmental influences.For example, your potential height at adulthood hasa genetically determined upper limit, but a variety <strong>of</strong>environmental factors such as nutrition <strong>and</strong> generalhealth help to determine to what extent you reachthat potential. To different degrees, genotype <strong>and</strong>environment account for <strong>the</strong> observed variations inhuman traits, including <strong>the</strong> human conditions thatwe call “health,” “disease,” “deform i t y,” “disorder, ”“sickness,” “normal,” or “abnormal.” Health generallyrefers to conditions that permit useful ranges <strong>of</strong> functionor activity, while normal is a quality based on statisticalconcepts <strong>and</strong> refers to a condition relative to<strong>the</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r humans. In addition, <strong>the</strong> termn o rmal can reflect societal values.We humans value certain species-typical functionsthat we regard as states <strong>of</strong> “health.” Having genesthat contribute to valued ranges <strong>of</strong> function can becharacterized as genetic health, with <strong>the</strong> proviso that<strong>the</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> such genes depends on environmentalfactors. Conditions labeled “disease” fall outsidevalued ranges <strong>of</strong> function; in cases where <strong>the</strong>reis a genetic basis for <strong>the</strong>se conditions, <strong>the</strong>y are usuallytermed “genetic disorders.” We disvalue <strong>the</strong>results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se statistically abnormal functions if<strong>the</strong>y hinder or prevent activity, comfort, or survival.Please note that students in general biology courseso ften encounter a misleading view <strong>of</strong> geneticsbecause <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> widespread use <strong>of</strong> examples involvingsingle gene disorders. Students may think <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>action <strong>of</strong> genes solely in terms <strong>of</strong> mutations <strong>and</strong> disorders,overlooking <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> vast majority <strong>of</strong>genes function well; if <strong>the</strong>y did not, we would not behere. For this reason, we de-emphasize <strong>the</strong> term“affected” in <strong>the</strong> review <strong>of</strong> traditional inheritance patternsat <strong>the</strong> start <strong>of</strong> Activity 2 <strong>and</strong> refer instead to <strong>the</strong>presence or absence <strong>of</strong> a trait, which may or may notbe a genetic disorder.Humans may consider anatomic structure or physiologicalfunctions to be <strong>of</strong> diminished value on a number<strong>of</strong> grounds, including: aes<strong>the</strong>tic (for example,polydactyly); instrumental, that is, conditions that donot allow us to achieve our goals (for example,dementia); <strong>and</strong> survival (for example, cystic fibrosis).Having genes that contribute to disvalued ranges <strong>of</strong>function can be characterized as genetic disease.Sometimes, as in <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease, environmentdoes not play a significant role in <strong>the</strong> development<strong>of</strong> a disorder. For that reason, single-gene disorderssuch as Huntington disease, PKU, Ta y- S a c h sdisease, or cystic fibrosis will be expressed regardless<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> natural environment. The same observ a t i o napplies to some polygenic disorders, such as congenitalheart disease or cleft lip <strong>and</strong> palate. In some diseasesin which genetics plays a role, such as cancer<strong>and</strong> predisposition to heart disease, environmentalfactors significantly influence phenotype.It is worth reminding students that some s t a t i s t i c a l l ya b n o rmal traits are not labeled as such because we donot disvalue <strong>the</strong>m. Valued <strong>and</strong> unusual traits includehaving superior intelligence, being very tall, or beingable to run very fast. Thus, perception <strong>of</strong> genetichealth <strong>and</strong> disease depends on values <strong>and</strong> statisticalconcepts, not just <strong>the</strong> latter. In <strong>the</strong> classroom, a discussion<strong>of</strong> values in this context must proceed withtact, particularly considering <strong>the</strong> likelihood that a studentor family member has a disvalued genetic condition.Students need to see that it is <strong>the</strong> t r a i t, <strong>and</strong> not<strong>the</strong> i n d i v i d u a l, that has diminished value.In Mendelian genetics, genetic health <strong>and</strong> diseaseare defined in terms <strong>of</strong> whe<strong>the</strong>r an individual has aninherited malfunction that is disvalued. Some scholarshave noted an oversimplified application <strong>of</strong>Mendelian genetics with respect to human health<strong>and</strong> disease. When oversimplified, genetic health26©1997 by BSCS.


ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications <strong>of</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section IV<strong>and</strong> disease are “ei<strong>the</strong>r/or” phenomena: an individualei<strong>the</strong>r has <strong>the</strong> phenotype in question (forinstance, Ta y-Sachs disease) or does not have it.Even classic, single-gene disorders, however, showvariation in expression, so this dichotomous view <strong>of</strong>genetics is not very accurate or helpful. Nontraditionalinheritance provides an opportunity toexp<strong>and</strong> our explanations <strong>of</strong> complex <strong>and</strong> nuancedconcepts <strong>of</strong> genetic health <strong>and</strong> disease that will continueto evolve. For example, an underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>trinucleotide repeat expansion provides inform a-tion about <strong>the</strong> variable age <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong>symptoms <strong>of</strong> genetic disorders that exhibit anticipation.Recognition <strong>of</strong> variability <strong>of</strong>ten is excludedfrom <strong>the</strong> more simple concept <strong>of</strong> genetic health <strong>and</strong>disease drawn from Mendelian genetics. Va r i a b i l i t ycan be a function <strong>of</strong> penetrance, <strong>the</strong> proportion <strong>of</strong>individuals with a given genotype who exhibit a n y<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phenotypic features <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> related trait. Va r i-ability also can be a function <strong>of</strong> variable expressivity,<strong>the</strong> range <strong>of</strong> phenotypic effects in individuals with agiven genotype (see O v e rview for Te a c h e r s, page37, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> introduction to Activity 3 for more informationon penetrance <strong>and</strong> variable expressivity).Keep in mind, however, that human perceptions <strong>of</strong>genetic health <strong>and</strong> disease are not equivalent to <strong>the</strong>e v o l u t i o n a ry advantage or disadvantage <strong>of</strong> specifictraits. For instance, <strong>the</strong> selective advantage (part i a lresistance to <strong>the</strong> malarial parasite) <strong>of</strong> heterozygosityfor <strong>the</strong> sickle-cell trait is present regardless <strong>of</strong>whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> individual or society recognizes it.New concepts <strong>of</strong> genes, health, <strong>and</strong> disease alsorequire recognition that some individuals carry genesthat may result in <strong>the</strong>ir developing a genetic disorder,even if <strong>the</strong>y do not yet manifest any symptoms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>d i s o r d e r. Breast cancer is a case in point. A youngwoman who has <strong>the</strong> B RC A 1 gene has an 80 percentlife-time risk <strong>of</strong> breast cancer, even though she maybe symptom-free at present. Should we view thisasymptomatic woman as diseased because we know<strong>the</strong> B RC A 1 gene likely will make her sick at somepoint? The classification <strong>of</strong> asymptomatic individualschallenges us to revise our traditional underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> health <strong>and</strong> disease from a concept <strong>of</strong> polar oppositesto a view <strong>of</strong> endpoints on a complex continuum.An important implication <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Human GenomeProject is that we likely will discover that each <strong>of</strong> ushas genetic risk factors for one or ano<strong>the</strong>r disorder.In o<strong>the</strong>r words, we need to confront <strong>the</strong> fact that noone is free <strong>of</strong> genetically based risk for disease <strong>and</strong>d i s a b i l i t y. Genetic health is, <strong>the</strong>refore, a relative term .A perception <strong>of</strong> health does not support genetic perfection,<strong>and</strong> we should discard a concept <strong>of</strong> genetichealth that aims to eliminate a l l risk factors. Pu tano<strong>the</strong>r way, <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> genetic health will haveto include <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> risk factors for at leastsome genetic diseases. The more serious <strong>the</strong> diseaseor <strong>the</strong> higher <strong>the</strong> risk, <strong>the</strong> lower <strong>the</strong> genetic health.Students may find <strong>the</strong> idea <strong>of</strong> risk factors to be a challengingconcept. The difficulty results because, at thispoint in <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> genetics, <strong>the</strong>re is no consensuson <strong>the</strong> concepts <strong>of</strong> genetic health <strong>and</strong> disease thataddresses <strong>the</strong>se challenges. You may want to take <strong>the</strong>occasion, if time permits, to discuss with students <strong>the</strong>challenge <strong>of</strong> formulating scientifically sound concepts<strong>of</strong> health <strong>and</strong> disease. For example, you mightask, “Given an individual who has a large number <strong>of</strong>trinucleotide repeats <strong>and</strong> who will not develop symptomsuntil later in life, at what point do we considerthat individual to be diseased?” The answers willdepend on <strong>the</strong> point at which anatomy <strong>and</strong> physiologyare statistically abnormal, on which values studentsthink are relevant to assessing <strong>the</strong> individual’scondition, <strong>and</strong> on whe<strong>the</strong>r students can defend <strong>the</strong>relevance <strong>and</strong> application <strong>of</strong> those values.THE SOCIAL SETTING FOR SCIENCEScience has a social aspect, seen in <strong>the</strong> need for communication<strong>and</strong> collaboration among scientists <strong>and</strong> in<strong>the</strong> reciprocal influences between science <strong>and</strong> societ y. There is, however, a distinction between <strong>the</strong> ideathat science is culturally “conditioned” <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> claimthat science is culturally “constru c t e d .” 3 This distinctionis very important. The former view, which is consistentwith <strong>the</strong> material presented in this module,holds that scientific explanations describe fundamentalprocesses <strong>and</strong> causal relationships that are3The claim that science is culturally constructed is made by some individuals who are known as postmodernists ord e c o n s t ru c t i o n i s t s .27©1997 by BSCS.


Section IV ■ ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications <strong>of</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>grounded in natural phenomena. As such, scientificexplanations have predictive power. Engineers,physicians, <strong>and</strong> technicians can manipulate naturalphenomena using <strong>the</strong> knowledge gained from science.Because scientific knowledge is constru c t e daccording to rigorous criteria, it should, for <strong>the</strong> mostp a rt, withst<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> fads <strong>and</strong> popular beliefs <strong>of</strong> anyp a rticular society at any point in history. Yet <strong>the</strong> pathby which scientific discovery proceeds is cert a i n l yconditioned by history <strong>and</strong> culture. Social <strong>and</strong> politicalvalues do help to shape science, particularly <strong>the</strong>agenda <strong>of</strong> research at any given time, because modern scientific research <strong>of</strong>ten requires large amounts<strong>of</strong> money. Those values also shape <strong>the</strong> ways in whichwe apply scientific knowledge. Examples includedecisions about genetic testing, which reflect our valuesabout autonomy <strong>and</strong> privacy, <strong>and</strong> decisions about<strong>the</strong> preservation <strong>of</strong> global biodiversity, which reflectour values about obligations to future generations.To assert that <strong>the</strong> practice <strong>and</strong> application <strong>of</strong> sciencereflect social <strong>and</strong> cultural values is different, however,from asserting that those values are so influential that<strong>the</strong>y render <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science ineffectual. Thissituation would make it nearly impossible to know,with cert a i n t y, anything about <strong>the</strong> natural world. Thisidea is an extreme representation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> view that scienceis culturally constructed, but it could suggestthat any explanation for a natural phenomenon isvalid, even if it has no scientifically reliable, evidenti a ry basis. The material presented in this modulecontradicts this extreme view, proposing instead that<strong>the</strong> rigorous discipline <strong>of</strong> scientific methods limits<strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> cultural influences on <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> scientificexplanations. Physicist A. Sokal states, “Thelaws <strong>of</strong> nature...are not social constructions; <strong>the</strong> universeexisted long before we did. Our <strong>the</strong>ories about<strong>the</strong> laws <strong>of</strong> nature are social constructions. The goal<strong>of</strong> science is for <strong>the</strong> latter to approximate as closely aspossible <strong>the</strong> form e r” (unpublished letter to <strong>the</strong> N e wYork Ti m e s, 22 May 1996).There also is a danger in allowing students <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>general public to think that debates about scientificknowledge are conducted in a manner similar to thatapplied to political issues, by popular vote. Science isbuilt through intellectually disciplined consensus, butnot through <strong>the</strong> consensus <strong>of</strong> voting. Scientific consensusoccurs when a sufficient segment <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> scientificcommunity, having followed rigorous scientificmethods, is convinced by evidence <strong>and</strong> well-reasonedargument to accept an idea. To “vote” in sciencewould ignore <strong>the</strong> intellectual discipline <strong>of</strong> scientificmethods, for example, <strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong> evidence in scientificinvestigation. (The patient awaiting an appendectomymay hope that <strong>the</strong> surgeon’s knowledge <strong>of</strong>anatomy is well founded in evidence <strong>and</strong> is closelyaligned with reality <strong>and</strong> not <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> whim or popularviews alone.) We encourage you to help studentssee <strong>the</strong> difference in <strong>the</strong>se views; Activity 5 providessome classroom applications.Scientific knowledge is established as a result <strong>of</strong> scientiststesting <strong>the</strong>ir methods <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir results against<strong>the</strong> criticisms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir colleagues, a process thatshows <strong>the</strong> collective nature <strong>of</strong> most scientificresearch. Students probably will experience difficultyin learning how to step back from <strong>the</strong>ir ideas sothat <strong>the</strong>y will not take personally conscientious critiquesby o<strong>the</strong>r students. Your task as teacher is toinsist that such critiques are never personal—what iscalled <strong>the</strong> ad hominem fallacy in logic—<strong>and</strong> that <strong>the</strong>critique always is directed to <strong>the</strong> idea expressed inwhat was said, <strong>and</strong> not to <strong>the</strong> student who said it.Students whose ideas are criticized should be asked,“How would you respond to <strong>the</strong> criticism <strong>of</strong> youridea?” In this way, students are taught that critiquesshould focus on ideas, <strong>and</strong> that ideas, onceexpressed, become public property for carefulassessment. Participating in such disciplined discourseprepares students to be responsible citizens.Although critiques <strong>of</strong> new ideas can establish <strong>the</strong>irscientific validity, society’s response to new scientificknowledge will depend on society’s values.ETHICAL AND POLICY IMPLICATIONSThe ELSI program has led to <strong>the</strong> identification <strong>and</strong>discussion <strong>of</strong> many important ethical, legal, <strong>and</strong>social implications <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project.These include privacy <strong>and</strong> confidentiality <strong>of</strong> genetici n f o rmation, uses <strong>and</strong> misuses <strong>of</strong> genetics in <strong>the</strong> past<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir relevance to current situations, prevention<strong>of</strong> discrimination in employment <strong>and</strong> insurance, <strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong> intrusion <strong>of</strong> genetic information into reproductivedecision-making. The first two modules developedby BSCS —Mapping <strong>and</strong> Sequencing <strong>the</strong>Human Genome: Science, Ethics, <strong>and</strong> Public Po l i c y28©1997 by BSCS.


ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications <strong>of</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section IV<strong>and</strong> The Human Genome Project: Biology, Computers,<strong>and</strong> Po l i c y—address a number <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se issues.The student activities in this module raise ethical <strong>and</strong>policy issues about access by teenagers to genetictesting <strong>and</strong> information about <strong>the</strong>mselves. You canorganize student discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se issues effectivelyaround two major concepts: prudential decisionmaking<strong>and</strong> developmental autonomy <strong>of</strong> teenagers.In Activity 4, Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong>Mutant HD Gene?, students will encounter <strong>the</strong> presentpolicy <strong>of</strong> not testing asymptomatic minors whoare at risk for Huntington disease. The justification forthis policy is as follows. First, it may be harmful to giveparents (or <strong>the</strong>ir child) <strong>the</strong> news <strong>of</strong> a genetic diagnosisfor which <strong>the</strong>re is no medical treatment at present. Toprovide such information could adversely affect howparents raise <strong>the</strong>ir child, <strong>and</strong> it may be cruel to informa child that he or she has an untreatable disorder. Second,in all likelihood, even with a high number <strong>of</strong> CAGrepeats, <strong>the</strong> onset <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease will occura fter <strong>the</strong> child has reached legal majority, that is, <strong>the</strong>age <strong>of</strong> eighteen. Testing will be available at that time,<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> individual, who <strong>the</strong>n will be a legal adult, c<strong>and</strong>ecide whe<strong>the</strong>r to be tested. Third, before <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong>m a j o r i t y, children, including teenagers, do not possess<strong>the</strong> autonomy <strong>of</strong> adults <strong>and</strong> so should not be regardedas having a right to <strong>the</strong> information or <strong>the</strong> ability toh<strong>and</strong>le that information in a competent, adult fashion.The first two justifications involve prudential judgments,a way to make reliable, well-reasoned decisionsunder conditions <strong>of</strong> uncert a i n t y. The third justificationinvolves developmental autonomy,p a rticularly <strong>the</strong> decision-making capacity <strong>of</strong> teenagersin <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> access to genetic inform a t i o n .The first two justifications concern avoiding unnecessa ry risk when it is uncertain that <strong>the</strong> event actually willo c c u r. For example, <strong>the</strong> first justification assumes thatmost children <strong>and</strong> most parents will react very badly to<strong>the</strong> news <strong>of</strong> a diagnosis <strong>of</strong> a disorder for which <strong>the</strong>re isno treatment. The second justification buttresses thisconsideration by pointing out that it is unnecessary togive parents <strong>of</strong> at-risk children genetic inform a t i o nthat will have no significance until <strong>the</strong> child reachesadulthood. Prudential judgments are <strong>the</strong> tools that weuse to manage uncert a i n t y, always a feature <strong>of</strong> genetici n f o rmation because our knowledge <strong>of</strong> genetics isincomplete. In addition, we use prudential judgmentsto make decisions about an uncertain future, whenour or someone else’s future interests are at risk inways that are to some degree unknown (in Huntingtondisease, as to likelihood <strong>of</strong> occurrence) but whosenegative value is known (for example, causing someoneto despair or become depressed). Prudential judgmentsare not linear, but nuanced <strong>and</strong> complex. Prudentialjudgments balance <strong>the</strong> perceived likelihood <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> occurrence <strong>of</strong> an event against its known disvalue.In simple terms, how bad is <strong>the</strong> outcome <strong>and</strong> how likelyis it to occur? When <strong>the</strong> issues are weighed, differentoutcomes are possible, depending on one’s estimate<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> incidence <strong>of</strong> occurrence <strong>and</strong> how much one disvalues<strong>the</strong> risk. Such estimates should be based on scientificinformation <strong>and</strong> challenged in reasoned publicdiscourse. Students should appreciate this variability ifa s ked to justify <strong>the</strong>se two aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir judgment.Thus, prudential judgments enable one to deal with<strong>the</strong> ethical issues raised by <strong>the</strong> activity.The third justification involves developmental autonomy.Students likely will resist <strong>the</strong> proposition that,just because <strong>the</strong>y are not yet eighteen, <strong>the</strong>y cannotmake decisions for <strong>the</strong>mselves. Remind studentsthat <strong>the</strong> legal age <strong>of</strong> majority is based on a roughsense <strong>of</strong> when most people should be treated likeadults <strong>and</strong> on <strong>the</strong> need for a very simple legal criterion.Age meets <strong>the</strong> latter test very readily; one hasonly to look at someone’s driver’s license to determine<strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> years since that individual wasborn. The problem, <strong>of</strong> course, is that some peoplehave adult decision-making capacity before legalmajority, some acquire that capacity long after reachinglegal majority, <strong>and</strong> some never attain it. Thisproblem raises <strong>the</strong> need for a definition <strong>of</strong> decisionmakingcapacity. Recent work in bioethics has developedan account <strong>of</strong> this capacity, shown in Figure 10.Notice that <strong>the</strong>re is no age requirement in thisdescription <strong>of</strong> autonomous decision-making. In <strong>the</strong>medical care <strong>of</strong> adolescents having various kinds <strong>of</strong>c a n c e r, most physicians are willing to regard ateenaged patient as an adult in <strong>the</strong> management <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> cancer if <strong>the</strong> teenager can display <strong>the</strong> six skillslisted above. Many teenagers can <strong>and</strong> do display<strong>the</strong>se skills. In some cases, <strong>the</strong> young person hasgrown up with <strong>the</strong> disease <strong>and</strong> can take adultresponsibility for its management.29©1997 by BSCS.


Section IV ■ ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications <strong>of</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>Figure 10 Skills in capacity for decision-making1. An individual can learn <strong>and</strong> recall information about<strong>the</strong> topic at h<strong>and</strong>.2. An individual can reason from present events to <strong>the</strong>irlikely future effects; that is, an individual displays <strong>the</strong>ability to reason causally.3. An individual has <strong>and</strong> can use values to assess <strong>the</strong>worth <strong>of</strong> those likely future effects.4. An individual can express a preference based on <strong>the</strong>first three skills <strong>and</strong>, if asked, can give an acceptableaccount <strong>of</strong> how he or she arrived at <strong>the</strong> preference.5. An individual can carry out his or her preference.6. An individual is prepared to live with <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> his orher preference in his or her own life <strong>and</strong> in o<strong>the</strong>rs’ l i v e s ;that is, one is prepared to be accountable to o<strong>the</strong>rs,even in situations in which one injures o<strong>the</strong>rs’ interests.It is a fact that teenagers sometimes display difficultywith skill 2 because <strong>the</strong>y have not gained sufficientm a s t e ry <strong>of</strong> causal reasoning. With respect to skill 3,<strong>the</strong>ir values may be unstable because <strong>the</strong>y still aredeveloping <strong>the</strong>ir identities <strong>and</strong> plans for <strong>the</strong> future.With respect to skill 5, some may be unwilling toaccept genetic testing. With respect to skill 6, <strong>the</strong>ymay be unwilling to assume responsibility for <strong>the</strong>consequences <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir decisions for <strong>the</strong>mselves <strong>and</strong>o<strong>the</strong>rs. Learning that one has <strong>the</strong> gene for a disorderat any age involves implications for o<strong>the</strong>rs, for example,siblings or cousins who may be at risk, friendshipsthat may be altered or even broken, <strong>and</strong> parentswho frequently take on <strong>the</strong> burden <strong>of</strong> caregivingfor <strong>the</strong>ir children when <strong>the</strong>y become chronically ill in<strong>the</strong>ir late 20s or 30s. The latter complication is illustratedin <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r chronic illness, AIDS.Many young adults with AIDS-related diseases haver e t u rned to <strong>the</strong>ir parents’ homes for care <strong>and</strong> suppo rt. Students should be encouraged to take seriously<strong>the</strong>se limitations on <strong>the</strong> developmental autonomy<strong>of</strong> teenagers <strong>and</strong> to show convincingly why <strong>the</strong>se limitationsdo not apply in <strong>the</strong>ir own case. Unlike adults,who may face few burdens <strong>of</strong> pro<strong>of</strong> about <strong>the</strong>ir decision-makingcapacity, teenagers must demonstrate<strong>the</strong>ir capacity, especially when <strong>the</strong> stakes are high.The situation described in Activity 4 is such an example,in which a teenager may learn that she or he hasa fatal, nontreatable genetic disorder.Skill 6 raises a challenge beyond <strong>the</strong> problem <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>decision-making capacity <strong>of</strong> teenagers. If one learnsas a result <strong>of</strong> genetic testing that one has a mutantHD gene with more than 50 trinucleotide repeats,<strong>the</strong>n one’s <strong>of</strong>fspring will be at greater risk for developingHuntington disease than if this gene has alower number <strong>of</strong> repeats. In <strong>the</strong> former case, <strong>the</strong>gene is likely to exp<strong>and</strong> to larger sizes in future generations<strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring who inherit <strong>the</strong> gene. In ethics,concern for <strong>the</strong> well-being <strong>of</strong> one’s <strong>of</strong>fspring is called“obligations to future generations.” This term also isused in <strong>the</strong> literature <strong>of</strong> environmental ethics torefer to our obligations to those not yet born,regardless <strong>of</strong> whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y are one’s own <strong>of</strong>fspring.There are a number <strong>of</strong> conceptual issues concerningobligations to future generations. First, to whom areyou obligated in <strong>the</strong> future? If you contribute to anindividual’s genes, <strong>the</strong>n you are one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> causes <strong>of</strong>that outcome. Normally, in ethics, each <strong>of</strong> us isresponsible for <strong>the</strong> events that we cause as a result <strong>of</strong>present decisions <strong>and</strong> actions. Knowing that one hasa gene for an autosomal dominant genetic disorder,<strong>and</strong> procreating in light <strong>of</strong> that knowledge, makesone accountable if one’s children get <strong>the</strong> gene <strong>and</strong>develop <strong>the</strong> disorder.Second, how far into <strong>the</strong> future are you obligated?This matter becomes difficult because <strong>the</strong> transmission<strong>of</strong> an autosomal dominant genetic disorder toone’s gr<strong>and</strong>children <strong>and</strong> beyond is not entirely afunction <strong>of</strong> one’s own decisions; <strong>the</strong>se results followfrom <strong>the</strong> decisions <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs (for example, affectedmembers <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> family). The issue, <strong>the</strong>refore,becomes <strong>the</strong> extent to which one has an ethicalobligation to attempt to influence <strong>the</strong> choices <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs.There is little consensus on this complex ethicalissue because <strong>the</strong> scholarly literature on this topic isjust now developing. Students should be encouragedto think through this issue toge<strong>the</strong>r, attemptingto identify lines <strong>of</strong> argument that are well supportedby scientific information <strong>and</strong> by criteria forethical argument.Third, suppose that an individual has been tested<strong>and</strong> found to have a mutant HD gene that contains40 CAG repeats, a number at <strong>the</strong> threshold for instability.On transmission to children, this number <strong>of</strong>repeats is likely to exp<strong>and</strong>, particularly if <strong>the</strong> parentin question is male. What is <strong>the</strong> individual’s obligationto children who, if <strong>the</strong>y are affected, likely will30©1997 by BSCS.


ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications <strong>of</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section IVhave an earlier <strong>and</strong> more severe form <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> disorder?This question is an issue <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ethical significance<strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation in determining ourobligations to future generations. The dilemmainvolves <strong>the</strong> degree to which we should disvalue diseasesin <strong>the</strong>ir more severe forms. It seems obviousthat we should. Students can consider how <strong>the</strong> qualitativedimensions <strong>of</strong> genetic disorders should beincluded in our concepts <strong>of</strong> genetic health <strong>and</strong> disease<strong>and</strong> evaluated in our determination <strong>of</strong> ourobligations to future generations.The fourth issue also is controversial. Is <strong>the</strong>re anobligation to avoid having children who will have anautosomal dominant genetic disorder? A relativelynew biomedical technology, pre-implantation diagnosis,allows couples at risk to use in vitro fertilizationto produce embryos <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n test <strong>the</strong>seembryos genetically when <strong>the</strong>y reach <strong>the</strong> morulastage in vitro. Embryos found to have <strong>the</strong> gene forHuntington disease can be discarded, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>embryo transferred to <strong>the</strong> woman’s uterus will befree <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mutant gene.SUMMARYChanging concepts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene, a central <strong>the</strong>me <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> student activities, requires us to rethink concepts<strong>of</strong> genetic health <strong>and</strong> disease. In addition to“ei<strong>the</strong>r/or” issues <strong>of</strong> whe<strong>the</strong>r an individual has <strong>the</strong>gene for a disease, <strong>the</strong>re are now issues <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> variable<strong>of</strong> age <strong>of</strong> onset <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> disease to betaken into account, as well as phenomena such asimprinting <strong>and</strong> uniparental disomy (see Overviewfor Teachers, Section V). As <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project<strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r lines <strong>of</strong> research continue to exp<strong>and</strong>our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> inheritance, we must confront<strong>the</strong> fact that no one is free <strong>of</strong> genetically based risk.Genetic health <strong>and</strong> disease thus come to be viewedas <strong>the</strong> endpoints <strong>of</strong> a complex continuum.The social setting for science c o n c e rns <strong>the</strong> cultural<strong>and</strong> historical conditioning <strong>of</strong> genetic knowledge.Students need to develop some comfort with <strong>the</strong>idea that at any one moment scientific knowledge isa function <strong>of</strong> its history to date. As a consequence,scientific knowledge is durable but not necessarilyadequate to explain all our future observations. Scientificknowledge is incomplete in principle; it isnever certain <strong>and</strong> final. Science requires constantcritique <strong>of</strong> new <strong>and</strong> existing information; thisrequirement means that science is a self- c o rr e c t i n ge n t e r p r i s e .Ethical issues in <strong>the</strong> student activities concern prudentialjudgments <strong>and</strong> developmental autonomy.P rudential judgments require students to beginwith <strong>the</strong> best information that science <strong>of</strong>fers <strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong>n weigh <strong>the</strong> value <strong>and</strong> disvalue <strong>of</strong> future, butu n c e rtain, consequences. Students must becomec o m f o rtable with <strong>the</strong> idea that uncert a i n t y — w h i c heven <strong>the</strong> most rigorous science cannot eliminatecompletely—can be managed in an intellectuallyserious manner.We have defined developmental autonomy by sixdiscrete but related skills that one can observe inindividuals. Based on this definition, a person whocan demonstrate those skills should be accorded <strong>the</strong>right (moral if not legal) to make personal decisionsabout access to genetic testing. Unlike adults, forwhom <strong>the</strong> law <strong>and</strong> ethics assume such capacity, studentsmust become comfortable with <strong>the</strong> idea that,as teenagers, <strong>the</strong>y face <strong>the</strong> burden <strong>of</strong> pro<strong>of</strong> for beingaccorded such a right.31©1997 by BSCS.


Section V:<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts<strong>and</strong> Nontraditional<strong>Inheritance</strong>“To be humane, we must ever be readyto pronounce that wise, ingenious <strong>and</strong>modest statement, ‘I do not know.’”GalileoThis teaching module uses examples <strong>of</strong> nontraditionalinheritance to illustrate <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods<strong>of</strong> science. By “nontraditional” we mean modes<strong>of</strong> inheritance that are not fully explained byMendel’s laws <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> classic underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>genetics that scientists have constructed in <strong>the</strong> yearssince Mendel. It is not, however, <strong>the</strong> actual methods<strong>of</strong> inheritance that are nontraditional; ra<strong>the</strong>r, it isthat our initial underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> inheritance did notencompass <strong>the</strong>se processes. This section <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Overview for Teachers reviews classic concepts ingenetics that are a prerequisite for using this module.These concepts typically are taught in an introductorybiology course. This section also describesmore unusual types <strong>of</strong> inheritance. Two <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> latter,mitochondrial inheritance (extranuclear inheritance)<strong>and</strong> expansion <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats (relatedto genetic anticipation), serve as examples in <strong>the</strong>Classroom Activities.As reflected in <strong>the</strong> opening genetics activity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>module, Activity 1, St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong>G i a n t s, modern genetics is being built on a foundation<strong>of</strong> discovery that spans more than a hundredyears. Figure 11, Milestones in <strong>the</strong> Underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> <strong>Inheritance</strong>, provides a snapshot <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong>genetics through a chronological account <strong>of</strong> some<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> major concepts <strong>of</strong> inheritance that have beendeveloped during <strong>the</strong> past 150 years. The dates aremuch less important than <strong>the</strong> intellectual history,that is, <strong>the</strong> connections between ideas <strong>and</strong> discoveries.These milestones demonstrate <strong>the</strong> durability<strong>of</strong> most genetics concepts, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y suggest <strong>the</strong>dependence <strong>of</strong> discoveries on pre-existing knowledge<strong>and</strong> on technology available at <strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>d i s c o v e ry. The conclusions reported here were suppo rted by evidence derived from observation <strong>and</strong>/orexperimentation. For <strong>the</strong> most recent years, wehave selected a few <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> vast number <strong>of</strong> discoveriesnow being published. One criterion was tochoose work that marks <strong>the</strong> first <strong>of</strong> a series <strong>of</strong> discoveries.Many human genes related to complex diseasessuch as cancer are being identified as part <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> HGP, <strong>and</strong> biologists are beginning to underst<strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong> genetic basis <strong>of</strong> complex traits, including cert a i nbehaviors. Activity 1 uses a small set <strong>of</strong> milestoneexplanations to challenge students to think abouthow our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> genetics has been const ructed. Students examine <strong>the</strong> relationshipbetween ideas without, we hope, undo concernabout dates or exact chronology.33


Section V ■ <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>Figure 11 Milestones in <strong>the</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> inheritance1865 Mendel proposed laws <strong>of</strong> inheritance based on his experiments with garden pea plants <strong>and</strong> was largelyignored.1 8 8 3 Roux proposed that filaments (later named “chromosomes”) in <strong>the</strong> nucleus are <strong>the</strong> bearers <strong>of</strong> hereditary factors.1883 van Beneden studied meiosis in a round worm <strong>and</strong> showed that gametes contain half as many chromosomesas somatic cells <strong>and</strong> that <strong>the</strong> somatic number <strong>of</strong> chromosomes is reestablished at fertilization.1890 Altmann discovered what later were called mitochondria.1900 Mendel’s work was independently rediscovered by Correns, de Vries, <strong>and</strong> von Tschermak, who also did experi -ments in plant breeding.1901 de Vries adopted <strong>the</strong> term “mutation” to describe alterations in <strong>the</strong> hereditary material by studying primroses.1902 McClung argued from observations in insects that sex is determined at <strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong> fertilization <strong>and</strong> is dependentupon <strong>the</strong> sex determinant acquired.1902-1909 Bateson introduced <strong>the</strong> terms “genetics,” “homozygote,” “heterozygote,” “F1,” <strong>and</strong> “F2.”1903 Sutton <strong>and</strong> Boveri independently correlated Mendel’s <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> independent assortment with <strong>the</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong>chromosomes during meiosis <strong>and</strong> proposed what came to be known as <strong>the</strong> “chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> heredity.”1906 Bateson, Saunders, <strong>and</strong> Punnett reported <strong>the</strong> first case <strong>of</strong> linkage (in <strong>the</strong> sweet pea).1909 Janssens suggested that exchanges between chromosomes occur during crossing over in meiosis.1909 Johannsen realized <strong>the</strong> distinction between <strong>the</strong> appearance <strong>of</strong> an organism <strong>and</strong> its genetic constitution, <strong>and</strong>invented <strong>the</strong> terms “phenotype” <strong>and</strong> “genotype” to describe <strong>the</strong>se. He also coined <strong>the</strong> word “gene.”1911 Morgan proposed that three fruit-fly genes are linked on <strong>the</strong> X chromosome, strongly supporting <strong>the</strong> chromosome<strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> heredity.1911 Greenfield observed “anticipation” in myotonic dystrophy pedigrees by noticing cataracts in <strong>the</strong> earlier generations<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se families. Many argued that anticipation was an artifact because <strong>of</strong> bias in finding parent-child pairs.1914 Bridges discovered a rare meiotic error in fruit flies <strong>and</strong> thus associated a specific gene with a specific chromosome.This discovery established <strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> heredity.1915 J.B.S. Haldane, Sprunt, <strong>and</strong> N.M. Haldane described <strong>the</strong> first example <strong>of</strong> linkage in vertebrates (mice).1927 Muller induced mutations by exposing fruit flies to X rays.1931 Stern <strong>and</strong>, independently, Creighton <strong>and</strong> McClintock provided cytological pro<strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong> crossing over during meiosis.1933 Morgan received a Nobel Prize for his development <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene based on his work on fruit flies.1944 Avery, MacLeod, <strong>and</strong> McCarty transformed <strong>the</strong> phenotype <strong>of</strong> pneumococcus <strong>and</strong> proposed that DNA (not protein)is <strong>the</strong> hereditary chemical.1950 McClintock discovered transposable genetic elements in maize <strong>and</strong> initially was ignored.1952 Hershey <strong>and</strong> Chase showed that phage DNA enters <strong>the</strong> bacterial cell on infection, but that most phage proteinremains outside.1953 Watson <strong>and</strong> Crick proposed a chemical structure for DNA.1956 Tjio <strong>and</strong> Levan demonstrated that humans have 46 chromosomes.1959 Chävremont, Chävremont-Comhaire, <strong>and</strong> Baeckel<strong>and</strong> demonstrated that mitochondria have DNA.1961 Crick, Barnett, Brenner, <strong>and</strong> Watts-Tobin showed that <strong>the</strong> genetic language is made up <strong>of</strong> three-letter words.Nirenberg, Matthaei, Ochoa, <strong>and</strong> Khorana worked out <strong>the</strong> DNA code for each amino acid.©1997 by BSCS.34


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section V1962 Watson, Crick, <strong>and</strong> Wilkins received Nobel Prizes for <strong>the</strong>ir studies on <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> DNA.1968 Holley, Khorana, <strong>and</strong> Nirenberg received Nobel Prizes for discoveries concerning interpretation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> geneticcode <strong>and</strong> its function in protein syn<strong>the</strong>sis.1974 Hutchison, Newbold, Potter, <strong>and</strong> Edgell demonstrated <strong>the</strong> maternal inheritance <strong>of</strong> mitochondrial DNAin horsedonkeyhybrids.1980 Engel proposed <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> “uniparental disomy,” where both chromosomes <strong>of</strong> a pair are inherited from <strong>the</strong>same parent.1981 A group <strong>of</strong> scientists worked out <strong>the</strong> complete nucleotide sequence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> human mitochondrial genome.1983 McClintock received a Nobel Prize for her discovery <strong>of</strong> transposable genetic elements.1983 Gusella, et al. found a DNAmarker on chromosome 4 that lay close to <strong>the</strong> Huntington disease gene. Genetictesting based on linkage analysis became possible.1983 Rastan <strong>and</strong> Cattanarch used <strong>the</strong> term “imprinting” with reference to X chromosome behavior.1 9 8 4 Pohlman, Feder<strong>of</strong>f, <strong>and</strong> Messing determined <strong>the</strong> nucleotide sequence <strong>of</strong> a maize transposable geneticelement.1986 Surani used <strong>the</strong> term “genomic imprinting” to describe <strong>the</strong> differential expression <strong>of</strong> maternal or paternal geneticmaterial located on autosomes.1988 Spence, et al. documented a case <strong>of</strong> cystic fibrosis caused by uniparental disomy.1990 Blum, Bakalara, <strong>and</strong> Simpson described RNAediting in trypanosomes.1991 Exp<strong>and</strong>ing trinucleotide repeats were found to play a role in fragile X syndrome <strong>and</strong> Kennedy disease.1992 An unstable trinucleotide repeat region was found in <strong>the</strong> myotonic dystrophy gene, <strong>and</strong> a correlation between<strong>the</strong> size <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> repeat region <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> disease was demonstrated, thus explaining <strong>the</strong> molecularbasis <strong>of</strong> anticipation.1993 The Huntington Disease Collaborative Group discovered <strong>the</strong> Huntington disease gene <strong>and</strong> found that it containsan unstable trinucleotide repeat that is <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mutation.1994 Miki <strong>and</strong> co-workers announced <strong>the</strong> identification <strong>of</strong> a human gene, BRCA1, that influences susceptibility tobreast <strong>and</strong> ovarian cancer.1995 Fleischmann <strong>and</strong> co-workers published <strong>the</strong> first complete DNA sequence <strong>of</strong> a free-living organism, <strong>the</strong> bacteriumHaemophilus influenzae, as part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HGP.1996 Brown <strong>and</strong> co-workers identified a gene in mice, <strong>the</strong> fosB gene, that is required for an essential behavior, nurturing<strong>the</strong>ir young.1996 More than 600 scientists worked toge<strong>the</strong>r to complete <strong>the</strong> sequencing <strong>of</strong> a eukaryotic species, yeast(Saccharomyces cerevisiae).TRADITIONAL CONCEPTS IN GENETICSOur modern underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> inheritance is const ructed around an ever-growing collection <strong>of</strong> fundamentalconcepts. Figure 12 lists <strong>the</strong> traditionalconcepts <strong>of</strong> inheritance on which this modulebuilds. A student <strong>of</strong> genetics generally encountersat one time <strong>the</strong> entire array <strong>of</strong> basic ideas thatexplain what we know <strong>of</strong> inheritance. In contrast,researchers in genetics have had to build this pictureone concept at a time, without <strong>the</strong> advantage<strong>of</strong> seeing beyond <strong>the</strong> currently available evidence.These concepts are discussed here with emphasison <strong>the</strong>ir historical development.Alleles segregate. Although Gregor Mendel, workingin <strong>the</strong> 1850s <strong>and</strong> 1860s, did not know about genes orchromosomes, he deduced many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir fundamentalcharacteristics <strong>and</strong> functions from his studies <strong>of</strong>traits in peas. (See Figure 1-1 in Activity 1 for a port r a i t35©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■ <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>Figure 12 Traditional concepts <strong>of</strong> inheritance■ Alleles segregate (Mendel’s law <strong>of</strong> segregation).■ Non-alleles assort independently (Mendel’s law <strong>of</strong> independentassortment) except when loci are linked.■ Traits can show a dominant or recessive pattern <strong>of</strong>inheritance.■ Genotype gives rise to phenotype.■ Chromosomes carry hereditary information.■ DNAis <strong>the</strong> informational component <strong>of</strong> chromosomes.Rare events (mutations) can change this information.■ <strong>Inheritance</strong> is nuclear <strong>and</strong> vertical (from parent to<strong>of</strong>fspring).■ The genetic contribution from each parent is equal (insexually reproducing species).■ Genes are <strong>the</strong> units <strong>of</strong> inheritance <strong>and</strong> are <strong>the</strong> source<strong>of</strong> heritable biological variation.■ Genes occur at fixed locations (loci) distributed alongchromosomes.■ During meiosis, chromosomes can exchange geneticmaterial through a process called crossing over.■ Genes on <strong>the</strong> same chromosome are “linked.” Thecloser <strong>the</strong>y are, <strong>the</strong> lower is <strong>the</strong> chance that <strong>the</strong>y willseparate during crossing over.■ The laws <strong>of</strong> probability help to explain patterns <strong>of</strong>inheritance.<strong>of</strong> Mendel.) Mendel’s first law (<strong>the</strong> law <strong>of</strong> segregation)states that <strong>the</strong> two alleles <strong>of</strong> a gene separate from eacho<strong>the</strong>r during <strong>the</strong> formation <strong>of</strong> gametes so that agamete carries one allele or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r, but not both.Progeny are <strong>the</strong>n produced by <strong>the</strong> r<strong>and</strong>om combination<strong>of</strong> gametes from <strong>the</strong> two parents. The sophistication<strong>of</strong> Mendel’s reasoning is remarkable given <strong>the</strong>sparse information he had available. E. van Beneden’slater studies <strong>of</strong> meiosis (see <strong>the</strong> 1883 entry for vanBeneden in Figure 11) showed that gametes containhalf as many chromosomes (<strong>the</strong> haploid number) asdo somatic cells, <strong>and</strong> that <strong>the</strong> diploid number <strong>of</strong> chromosomesis reestablished at fertilization. This workprovided fur<strong>the</strong>r evidence for Mendel’s hypo<strong>the</strong>ses.In 1904, Lucien Cuénot studied coat color in mice <strong>and</strong>found that a gene can have more than two alleles. Anygiven individual, however, has only two <strong>of</strong> those alleles,one on each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> homologous chromosomeswhere <strong>the</strong> gene in question resides.Non-alleles assort independently, except when lociare linked. Mendel’s second law, <strong>the</strong> law <strong>of</strong> independentassortment, concerns <strong>the</strong> inheritance <strong>of</strong> two ormore traits. The law states that <strong>the</strong> genes for differenttraits behave independently in <strong>the</strong> production <strong>of</strong>gametes so long as those genes are on different chromosomes.Mendel found, for example, that <strong>the</strong> determination<strong>of</strong> color in his pea plants—yellow orgreen—was independent <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> determination <strong>of</strong>shape—smooth or wrinkled. Independent studies byWalter S. Sutton <strong>and</strong> Theodor Boveri in 1903 corr e l a t-ed <strong>the</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong> chromosomes during meiosis withboth <strong>of</strong> Mendel’s laws <strong>of</strong> inheritance. They recognizeda resemblance between <strong>the</strong> separation <strong>of</strong> homologouschromosomes at meiosis <strong>and</strong> Mendel’s proposedseparation <strong>of</strong> trait determinants at gamete formation(<strong>the</strong> law <strong>of</strong> segregation). They also suggestedthat chromosomes act independently in <strong>the</strong>ir movementduring meiosis, which would explain Mendel’slaw <strong>of</strong> independent assortment. A gamete receivesp a t e rnally <strong>and</strong> maternally derived chromosomes;thus one trait can be inherited from <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r whileano<strong>the</strong>r comes from <strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>r. Independent assortmentincreases genetic variation by creating new combinations<strong>of</strong> alleles in gametes.Traits can show a dominant or recessive pattern <strong>of</strong>i n h e r i t a n c e . Mendel deduced from his pea studiesthat <strong>the</strong> trait determined by one allele masked <strong>the</strong>expression <strong>of</strong> its partner because he did not observ ephenotypes that were a mixture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> traits determinedby <strong>the</strong> two different alleles <strong>of</strong> a gene. Manyinherited human traits, including disorders such asHuntington disease (dominant) <strong>and</strong> cystic fibrosis(recessive), display dominant or recessive inheritance.In your teaching, take care to distinguishbetween <strong>the</strong> inheritance <strong>of</strong> a g e n e <strong>and</strong> a t r a i t. It is notgenes <strong>the</strong>mselves that are dominant or recessive, nor<strong>the</strong> way genes are inherited. Instead, it is <strong>the</strong> inheritancepattern <strong>of</strong> a t r a i t that is ei<strong>the</strong>r dominant orrecessive. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> genes on phenotypefollows a dominant or recessive pattern. Fu r-t h e rmore, different mutations in <strong>the</strong> same gene canhave different patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance. For example,<strong>the</strong> disorder osteogenesis imperfecta exhibits dominantinheritance as a result <strong>of</strong> certain mutations <strong>and</strong>displays recessive inheritance as a result <strong>of</strong> differentmutations in <strong>the</strong> same gene (Scriver, et al., 1995, T h eMetabolic <strong>and</strong> Molecular Bases <strong>of</strong> Inherited Disease,Chapters 1 <strong>and</strong> 134).36©1997 by BSCS.


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section VAs Lucien Cuénot showed in 1904, a gene can havemore than two alleles, even though a diploid organismn o rmally can possess only two <strong>of</strong> those alleles. Not allgenes have alleles that show a clearly dominant orrecessive pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance. In multiple allelic systems,<strong>the</strong> pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance can vary according to<strong>the</strong> particular combination <strong>of</strong> alleles in <strong>the</strong> organism.For instance, a particular allele might result in dominantinheritance in combination with one partner butrecessive inheritance if paired with ano<strong>the</strong>r allele.Some alleles result in codominant inheritance, meaningthat heterozygotes display a mixture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phenotypesthat would be present in <strong>the</strong> two differenthomozygous conditions. A good example is <strong>the</strong>human ABO blood antigen system, where A <strong>and</strong> B allelescan be expressed simultaneously. An individualwho has two copies (doses) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same alleles for agiven trait is said to be homozygous for that trait. Anindividual with different alleles for a given trait is saidto be heterozygous; William Bateson introduced thoselabels in <strong>the</strong> first decade <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> twentieth century.Genotype gives rise to phenotype. In many ways, thisconcept is <strong>the</strong> most basic view <strong>of</strong> inheritance. The“genotype” is <strong>the</strong> genetic composition <strong>of</strong> an organism,which, at its finest level <strong>of</strong> detail, means <strong>the</strong>sequence <strong>of</strong> nucleotides in <strong>the</strong> DNA. The “phenotype”comprises <strong>the</strong> physical characteristics <strong>of</strong> anorganism, including internal characteristics thatrequire special detection methods to be observed.The terms genotype <strong>and</strong> phenotype were inventedby Wilhelm L. Johannsen in 1909 when he realized<strong>the</strong> distinction between <strong>the</strong> appearance <strong>of</strong> an organism<strong>and</strong> its genetic constitution. Phenotype resultsfrom a combination <strong>of</strong> genotypic <strong>and</strong> environmentalinfluences. The contribution <strong>of</strong> environmental influencesvaries for different traits.There are several examples <strong>of</strong> single genes whoseexpression is modified by o<strong>the</strong>r factors, such that <strong>the</strong>phenotypic expression <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene is variable. Theterms “penetrance” <strong>and</strong> “variable expressivity” areused to describe this phenomenon. Penetrance is<strong>the</strong> proportion <strong>of</strong> individuals with a given genotypewho express any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phenotypic features <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>trait. Variable expressivity is <strong>the</strong> range <strong>of</strong> phenotypiceffects in individuals with a given genotype. Anexample <strong>of</strong> a genetic disorder that displays incompletepenetrance is <strong>the</strong> dominant trait ectrodactyly;some people carrying <strong>the</strong> mutant allele do not have<strong>the</strong> deformity called “lobster claw” h<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> yet<strong>the</strong>y can pass it on to <strong>the</strong>ir children. Myotonic dystrophyis an example <strong>of</strong> a genetic disorder that displaysvariable expressivity, with symptoms that rangefrom cataracts to extreme muscle disorders <strong>and</strong>mental retardation. In this example, variable expressivityresults from an unusual genetic mechanism:differences in <strong>the</strong> degree <strong>of</strong> expansion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> unstabletrinucleotide repeat within <strong>the</strong> mutant gene formyotonic dystrophy. Penetrance <strong>and</strong> variable expressivitydepend on a variety <strong>of</strong> factors including environmentalinfluences, <strong>the</strong> action <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r genes, orchance. These phenomena continually cloud ourability to make exact predictions about <strong>the</strong> likely outcomes<strong>of</strong> particular genotypes.Counterbalancing <strong>the</strong>se effects is <strong>the</strong> knowledgethat, for certain disorders, a particular genotype goesa long way towards predicting an ultimate outcome.For example, someone who inherits both alleles forTay-Sachs disease will die <strong>of</strong> that disorder sometimein childhood. In Huntington disease, a greater number<strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats makes it more likely that<strong>the</strong> affected person will show signs <strong>and</strong> symptoms <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> disorder earlier in life. Unfortunately, at present,it is not possible to determine exactly when symptomswill first appear, or <strong>the</strong> rate at which <strong>the</strong> disorderwill progress.Chromosomes carry hereditary inform a t i o n . A searly as 1883, biologists proposed that chromosomescarry genetic information. Wilhelm Roux proposedthis hypo<strong>the</strong>sis after observing filaments within<strong>the</strong> nucleus that stained with basic dyes. In 1888,W. Waldeyer gave <strong>the</strong>se filaments <strong>the</strong> name “chromosomes,”for “colored bodies,” because <strong>the</strong> chromosomesstained very darkly. The first experimentalevidence that <strong>the</strong>re is a constant <strong>and</strong> regular associationbetween chromosomes <strong>and</strong> observable characteristics<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> organism came from C.E. McClung’swork in grasshoppers. He suggested in 1902 that <strong>the</strong>presence <strong>of</strong> a distinctive chromosome (<strong>the</strong> X chromosome)is related to sex determination becausefemales have two copies <strong>of</strong> this chromosome <strong>and</strong>males have only one.DNA is <strong>the</strong> informational component <strong>of</strong> chromoso m e s . Chromosomes in eukaryotes are composed <strong>of</strong>37©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■ <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>DNA <strong>and</strong> associated proteins. Prokaryotic chromosomesgenerally consist <strong>of</strong> a single molecule <strong>of</strong> DNA.In 1939, E. Knapp <strong>and</strong> H. Schreiber obtained prelimin a ry evidence that <strong>the</strong> DNA component <strong>of</strong> chromosomesis responsible for <strong>the</strong> transmission <strong>of</strong> genetici n f o rmation when <strong>the</strong>y correlated mutations to ultravioletlight. They noticed that <strong>the</strong> wavelength <strong>of</strong> ultravioletlight that most effectively generated mutationsin <strong>the</strong> sperm <strong>of</strong> a type <strong>of</strong> liverw o rt corresponded to<strong>the</strong> absorption spectrum <strong>of</strong> DNA. In 1944, Oswald T.Av e ry, Colin M. MacLeod, <strong>and</strong> Maclyn McCarty providedmore conclusive evidence by transforming <strong>the</strong>phenotype <strong>of</strong> pneumococcus bacteria with a purifiedsubstance that <strong>the</strong>y showed to be DNA. In 1952,Alfred D. Hershey <strong>and</strong> Martha Chase showed that <strong>the</strong>DNA component <strong>of</strong> bacteriophage viruses entered<strong>the</strong> host bacterial cell, whereas <strong>the</strong> protein componentremained outside <strong>the</strong> cell <strong>and</strong> was not involvedin viral infection <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> bacterium. These discoveriessuggested that DNA might be <strong>the</strong> carrier <strong>of</strong> heredita ry information in higher organisms as well.In 1953, James D. Watson <strong>and</strong> Francis H.C. Crick proposeda structure for <strong>the</strong> DNA molecule, which hassince been confirmed by electron microscopy. Wa t-son <strong>and</strong> Crick’s proposed double helical stru c t u r ewith complementary str<strong>and</strong>s was based on severallines <strong>of</strong> evidence that had accumulated during <strong>the</strong>few years prior to <strong>the</strong>ir proposal. (The icon used todenote <strong>the</strong> titles in <strong>the</strong> Overview <strong>and</strong> classroom activitiesdepicts this structure.) Erwin Chargraff noticedin 1950 that <strong>the</strong> amounts <strong>of</strong> adenine <strong>and</strong> thymine inDNA are equal, as are <strong>the</strong> amounts <strong>of</strong> cytosine <strong>and</strong>guanine. The proportions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two pairs variedwidely between species, however. This observ a t i o n ,toge<strong>the</strong>r with <strong>the</strong> X-ray crystallographic studies by<strong>the</strong> research group <strong>of</strong> Maurice H. F. Wilkins <strong>and</strong> byRosalind E. Franklin <strong>and</strong> R. G. Gosling, led Wa t s o n<strong>and</strong> Crick to propose <strong>the</strong>ir structural model. The significance<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> DNA is that it provides amechanism adequate to explain <strong>the</strong> storage <strong>and</strong>transmission <strong>of</strong> genetic inform a t i o n .Because DNA is <strong>the</strong> molecule responsible for <strong>the</strong>transmission <strong>of</strong> genetic information, <strong>the</strong>n it followsthat changes in DNA structure (mutations) can resultin altered characteristics <strong>of</strong> an organism. Mutation isan essential feature <strong>of</strong> heredity because it increasesgenetic variation, <strong>the</strong> raw material <strong>of</strong> evolution.<strong>Inheritance</strong> is nuclear <strong>and</strong> vertical (from parent to<strong>of</strong>fspring). Mendel’s laws <strong>of</strong> inheritance provided alarge piece <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> answer to <strong>the</strong> age-old question,“Why do <strong>of</strong>fspring resemble <strong>the</strong>ir parents?” His lawsare based on <strong>the</strong> principle that heritable factors (<strong>the</strong>genotype) that influence characteristics (<strong>the</strong> phenotype)are passed from parent to progeny in a r<strong>and</strong>ombut ma<strong>the</strong>matically explainable manner. Genetici n f o rmation is encoded in <strong>the</strong> sequence <strong>of</strong>nucleotides in DNA. Prokaryotic cells (bacteria) haveone large DNA molecule <strong>and</strong>, in many cases, severalsmall circles <strong>of</strong> DNA known as plasmids. In eukaryoticcells, DNA is <strong>the</strong> major informational component<strong>of</strong> thread-like structures called chromosomes,which are found in <strong>the</strong> nucleus. Because chromosomesare observable by light microscopy, <strong>the</strong>ir connectionto inheritance was recognized earlier thanwas that <strong>of</strong> DNA. These observations, coupled with<strong>the</strong> phenomena discussed in <strong>the</strong> next two paragraphs,support <strong>and</strong> help to explain Mendel’s observations,<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y account for <strong>the</strong> predominant patterns<strong>of</strong> inheritance.The genetic contribution from each parent is equal(in sexually reproducing species). In 1883, E. vanBeneden showed that meiosis produces gametesthat have half <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> chromosomes (haploidnumber, N) <strong>of</strong> that found in somatic cells (diploidnumber, 2N) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same organism. This reductionin chromosome number, from 2N to N, is essentialfor gametes to function in sexual reproduction.Without <strong>the</strong> reduction, each generation woulddouble <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> chromosomes <strong>and</strong> hencedouble <strong>the</strong> genetic information.T. H. Montgomery studied spermatogenesis in variousspecies <strong>of</strong> H e m i p t e r a ( t rue bugs) <strong>and</strong> concludedin 1901 that maternal chromosomes pair only withp a t e rnal chromosomes during meiosis. In studyingsea urchin embryos in 1903, Theodor Boveri foundthat this organism needs a full set <strong>of</strong> chromosomes todevelop normally <strong>and</strong> deduced that individual chromosomescarry different essential elements. Thiso b s e rvation led him to conclude not only that meiosisreduces <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> chromosomes by half, butthat meiosis specifically halves each pair <strong>of</strong> chromosomes.Therefore, it follows that <strong>the</strong> combination <strong>of</strong>two gametes in fertilization produces a zygote thathas an equal genetic contribution from each parent.38©1997 by BSCS.


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section VGenes are <strong>the</strong> units <strong>of</strong> inheritance <strong>and</strong> are <strong>the</strong>source <strong>of</strong> heritable biological variation. The view <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> gene is constantly evolving, as described in PetterPortin’s review (Portin, 1993). First conceived asan abstract unit <strong>of</strong> inheritance, <strong>the</strong> gene now is seenas a complex molecular entity whose properties arebecoming known. A gene is a portion <strong>of</strong> DNA thatencodes information needed for <strong>the</strong> production <strong>of</strong> afunctional product. This product is ei<strong>the</strong>r a proteinor a functional RNA, such as a ribosomal component.Protein is produced by <strong>the</strong> translation <strong>of</strong> an intermediatemolecule, mRNA. Features <strong>of</strong> a typical eukaryoticgene, illustrated in Figure 13, include <strong>the</strong> codingregion (<strong>the</strong> part that determines <strong>the</strong> final proteinsequence) <strong>and</strong> various control regions that determinewhen, where, <strong>and</strong> how much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene productis made. The coding region may contain noncodinginserts called introns, which are excisedduring RNA processing. The portions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> codingregion that correspond to <strong>the</strong> final version <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>RNA message (mRNA) are called exons. The initialRNA product contains copies <strong>of</strong> exons <strong>and</strong> introns,but <strong>the</strong> mRNA has been processed to remove intronsprior to translation.There are about 80,000 genes in <strong>the</strong> completehuman genetic endowment (<strong>the</strong> genome), but <strong>the</strong>seaccount for less than five percent <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> total DNAcontent (see For Your Information: How many genesdo humans have?). Scientists do not clearly know<strong>the</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> remainder <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> DNA at thistime. There may exist slightly different forms <strong>of</strong> aparticular gene—a function <strong>of</strong> slight variations inbase sequences—<strong>and</strong> more than one version can bepresent in a diploid organism. These different forms(alleles) lead to variation in <strong>the</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> traitsamong individuals within a species.There are many differences in <strong>the</strong> DNA sequencebetween individuals, most <strong>of</strong> which make no differenceto <strong>the</strong> phenotype. Some variations withingenes, however, can have biological consequences.Some genetic variations lead to harmless phenotypicdifferences such as variations in hair <strong>and</strong> eye color,whereas o<strong>the</strong>rs can have more serious consequences,ei<strong>the</strong>r advantageous or disadvantageous. ADNA variation that causes a gene product to bealtered or deficient in some way can manifest itself asa genetic disorder such as cystic fibrosis. These heri-Figure 13 Features <strong>of</strong> a gene: Notice that <strong>the</strong> mRNA,when fully processed, may be shorter than <strong>the</strong> gene fromwhich it was copied. In addition to removal <strong>of</strong> introns, processinginvolves two chemical modifications, a “cap” stru c-ture <strong>and</strong> a polyA tail, that are required for stability <strong>and</strong>t r a n s l a t i o n .table changes in <strong>the</strong> DNA sequence result from mutationsin germline cells. (Some mutations occur insomatic cells <strong>and</strong> are n o t heritable, as August We i s s-man demonstrated in <strong>the</strong> late 1800s. Germ cells areseparated from somatic cells. Weissman’s workhelped to disprove Lamarckian’s notions about <strong>the</strong>inheritance <strong>of</strong> acquired characteristics.)Heritable genetic variation is an essential prerequisiteto biological evolution in that it provides <strong>the</strong>39©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■ <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>For Your InformationHow many genes do humans have?Estimates <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> genes in <strong>the</strong> human genome have varied over <strong>the</strong> years. Scientists havetried several approaches to determine <strong>the</strong> precise number, including estimating <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> proteinsexpressed in a particular cell type, estimating <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> different RNA molecules produced,<strong>and</strong> estimating <strong>the</strong> density <strong>of</strong> genes on chromosomes by looking at <strong>the</strong> DNA sequence. All <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>se approaches have severe limitations. For example, not all genes are expressed in any one celltype; some gene products are not proteins; variable RNA splicing allows <strong>the</strong> same gene to producedifferent mRNAs; <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> density <strong>of</strong> genes on chromosomes varies. The sequence-based approach is<strong>the</strong> most accurate method for determining where a given gene begins <strong>and</strong> ends, <strong>and</strong>, once <strong>the</strong>Human Genome Project has achieved its goal <strong>of</strong> sequencing <strong>the</strong> entire genome, we will have a betterestimate <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> human genes. Note that it may not be possible to tell from <strong>the</strong> DNAsequence whe<strong>the</strong>r a gene actually is functional. As more <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genome is sequenced, <strong>the</strong> predictednumber <strong>of</strong> genes is decreasing, perhaps because <strong>the</strong> regions first examined happened to be particularlygene-rich. One recent estimate is that <strong>the</strong> human genome contains approximately 80,000 genes.array <strong>of</strong> r<strong>and</strong>om but transmissable changes on whichnatural selective pressure acts. Changes in genesgive rise to differences in phenotype, <strong>and</strong> some <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>se may be selected for or against in <strong>the</strong> struggleto leave viable <strong>of</strong>fspring that constitutes Darwinianfitness. The great evolutionary biologist Ernst Mayrreminds us that natural selection is a two-stepprocess. The first step is <strong>the</strong> production <strong>of</strong> newgenetic <strong>and</strong> phenotypic variations in a population <strong>of</strong>organisms. The second step is selection itself, whichtests those variations against <strong>the</strong> environment. Gradually,<strong>the</strong> collection <strong>of</strong> genes in a species shifts, anexample <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> evolution at work. Consider<strong>the</strong> situation at <strong>the</strong> level <strong>of</strong> proteins. For someproteins, such as histones, sequence <strong>and</strong>, consequently,structure are highly conserved. We infer that<strong>the</strong> function <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se proteins remains essentiallyunchanged, <strong>and</strong> that almost any change (mutation)is a disadvantage for <strong>the</strong> organism. O<strong>the</strong>r proteins,however, are much more tolerant <strong>of</strong> variation, <strong>and</strong>more differences accumulate in <strong>the</strong> genes thatencode <strong>the</strong>m.Genes occur at fixed locations (loci) distributedalong chromosomes. Implicit in <strong>the</strong> classic view <strong>of</strong>inheritance is <strong>the</strong> assumption that a gene remains ina fixed location on its chromosome, with <strong>the</strong> notedexception <strong>of</strong> recombination during meiosis. Even inthat case, <strong>the</strong> relative position <strong>of</strong> a gene compared to<strong>the</strong> location <strong>of</strong> its neighbors is generally fixed (seenext discussion). Mendel’s observations <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r principles <strong>of</strong> genetics described here are basedlargely on this assumption.During meiosis, chromosomes can exchange geneticmaterial through a process called crossing over.Crossing over <strong>of</strong> chromosomes during meiosis wasfirst observed in 1909, when F.A. Janssens suggestedthat exchanges between nonsister (paternal <strong>and</strong>maternal) chromatids produced chiasmata (crossshapedjunction points). In 1931, Curt Sternobtained cytological evidence <strong>of</strong> crossing over, ano b s e rvation confirmed independently by H.B.Creighton <strong>and</strong> Barbara McClintock in <strong>the</strong> same year.J.H. Taylor provided <strong>the</strong> most convincing evidencein 1965—by radioisotope labeling <strong>of</strong> grasshopperchromosomes—that <strong>the</strong>re is an exchange <strong>of</strong> DNA atchiasmata. Crossing over results in genetic recombination<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> production <strong>of</strong> novel combinations <strong>of</strong>alleles. This recombination is one source <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> intraspecificgenetic variation that makes evolution possible.The unique combination <strong>of</strong> genetic informationderived from two parents also provides a variety <strong>of</strong>characteristics on which natural selection acts.Genes on <strong>the</strong> same chromosome are “linked.” I n1906, William Bateson, E.R. Saunders, <strong>and</strong> ReginalC. Punnett—while studying inheritance in <strong>the</strong>40©1997 by BSCS.


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section Vsweet pea—discovered <strong>the</strong> first exception toMendel’s law <strong>of</strong> independent assortment. Theyfound that purple flowers <strong>and</strong> long pollen—bothdominant traits—were inherited toge<strong>the</strong>r moreo ften than predicted by Mendel’s second law. (Thelaws <strong>of</strong> probability were essential for <strong>the</strong>se scientiststo notice that something unusual was happening;see <strong>the</strong> discussion under <strong>the</strong> next heading.)The reason for this discrepancy is that <strong>the</strong>se investigatorswere looking at two genes on <strong>the</strong> samechromosome, whereas five <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> seven traits thatMendel studied are located on different chromosomes(<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r two are far apart on <strong>the</strong> samechromosome <strong>and</strong> effectively behave independently).The two sweet-pea traits were not always inheritedtoge<strong>the</strong>r because <strong>the</strong>y sometimes were separatedby crossing over during meiosis, but <strong>the</strong>ywere inherited toge<strong>the</strong>r more <strong>of</strong>ten than Mendelpredicted because <strong>the</strong>y are close toge<strong>the</strong>r on <strong>the</strong>same chromosome. Genes that lie close toge<strong>the</strong>ron a chromosome are said to be more closelyl i n ked than those that are far<strong>the</strong>r apart. The fart h e ra p a rt loci are on <strong>the</strong> same chromosome, <strong>the</strong> more<strong>the</strong>ir behavior is independent in meiosis because <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> crossing over that can occur between <strong>the</strong>m.Thomas Hunt Morgan was instrumental in developing<strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> heredity by formulating<strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> gene linkage. In 1911, he proposedthat <strong>the</strong> fruit-fly genes for white eyes, yellowbody, <strong>and</strong> miniature wings are linked on <strong>the</strong> X chromosome.He <strong>and</strong> his colleagues subsequently foundthat <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> linkage groups is equal to <strong>the</strong>haploid number <strong>of</strong> chromosomes, <strong>and</strong> that <strong>the</strong> lineararrangement <strong>of</strong> genes within <strong>the</strong> linkage groupcorresponds to <strong>the</strong> linear cytological structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>chromosome. Morgan, who did so much to develop<strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> gene linkage, is immortalized in <strong>the</strong>term “centimorgan.” This term is a unit <strong>of</strong> distanceused in linkage maps. Two loci that are one centimorganapart are separated by recombination anaverage <strong>of</strong> one in one hundred times (or one percent<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> time, that is, in one gamete out <strong>of</strong> onehundred).Substantiation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> hereditycame in 1914 from <strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong> one <strong>of</strong> Morgan’sstudents, Calvin Bridges, who found a rare exceptionto <strong>the</strong> linkage <strong>of</strong> genes on <strong>the</strong> fruit-fly X chromosome.He noticed that <strong>the</strong> exceptional inheritance <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> sex-linked white eye color was accompanied bysimilar exceptional behavior <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sex chromosomes;he proposed meiotic nondisjunction as amechanism for this unusual inheritance. (Meioticnondisjunction refers to <strong>the</strong> improper separation <strong>of</strong>homologues during meiotic cell division. It isresponsible for some cases <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> common chromosomaldisorder known as Down syndrome.)The laws <strong>of</strong> probability help to explain patterns <strong>of</strong>inheritance. In analyzing his pea-breeding experiments,Mendel was a pioneer in <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> rigorousma<strong>the</strong>matical analysis <strong>of</strong> his biological data. Thenumbers <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> various phenotypes <strong>of</strong> his plants ledhim to propose that each “particulate factor” (gene)has two “alternative forms” (alleles) <strong>and</strong> that <strong>the</strong> distribution<strong>of</strong> alleles among gametes is r<strong>and</strong>om. In1889, Francis Galton (a cousin <strong>of</strong> Charles Darwin)<strong>and</strong> his associate Karl Pearson demonstrated that<strong>the</strong>re is a statistical association between traits shownby parents <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>of</strong>fspring, even when <strong>the</strong> traitshows a continuous variation such as that for heightor weight. In 1909, nine years after <strong>the</strong> rediscovery <strong>of</strong>Mendel’s work, Herman Nilsson-Ehle studied seedcoatcolor in wheat. He proposed that continuoustraits are determined by multiple genes, each <strong>of</strong>which segregates according to Mendelian principles,<strong>and</strong> each <strong>of</strong> which has a small but additive effect.Your students presumably have encountered <strong>the</strong> laws<strong>of</strong> probability in <strong>the</strong>ir study <strong>of</strong> Mendel’s experiments.In revisiting <strong>the</strong>se principles, stress that <strong>the</strong> laws <strong>of</strong>probability are applicable only when <strong>the</strong> sample numberis large enough. For example, a small sample sizemay not display <strong>the</strong> predicted Mendelian distributionsfor a given gene or trait. Independent probability,ano<strong>the</strong>r important concept, states that <strong>the</strong> outcome<strong>of</strong> one independent event does not influence <strong>the</strong> outcome<strong>of</strong> subsequent independent events. This concept<strong>of</strong>ten is summarized by <strong>the</strong> phrase, “probabilityhas no memory.” For instance, when considering <strong>the</strong>inheritance <strong>of</strong> an autosomal recessive disorder suchas cystic fibrosis, <strong>the</strong> chance that each child <strong>of</strong> heterozygousparents will be affected is one in four; it isnot correct to say that <strong>the</strong> birth <strong>of</strong> one affected childmeans that <strong>the</strong> next three will be unaffected. You canemphasize this point by examining <strong>the</strong> traditionalpedigrees (A-H) in Activity 2, Puzzling Pe d i g r e e s.41©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■ <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>Ma<strong>the</strong>matical principles also were central to <strong>the</strong>derivation <strong>and</strong> application <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Hardy-Weinbergequilibrium in population genetics. W.E. Castle, in1903, recognized a relationship between gene <strong>and</strong>genotype frequencies. Then, in 1908, Godfrey H.Hardy <strong>and</strong> Wilhelm Weinberg independently discovereda ma<strong>the</strong>matical relationship that explains <strong>the</strong>general stability <strong>of</strong> gene <strong>and</strong> genotype frequencies ina population as genes are passed from generation togeneration. The Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium shows,contrary to prior assertions, that a dominant trait willnot drive its recessive counterpart from a population<strong>of</strong> organisms. Equilibrium—stability in gene <strong>and</strong>genotype frequencies—is <strong>the</strong> rule, assuming <strong>the</strong>absence <strong>of</strong> evolutionary forces such as selection orgenetic drift, <strong>and</strong> if <strong>the</strong>re is minimal mutation.GENETICS AND EVOLUTIONNo overview <strong>of</strong> classical genetics, even so brief atreatment as presented here, is complete without adiscussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> relationship between genetics <strong>and</strong>evolution, <strong>the</strong> conceptual thread that binds all <strong>of</strong> biolog y. Charles Darwin realized that variation among <strong>the</strong>members <strong>of</strong> any population <strong>of</strong> organisms is fuel for<strong>the</strong> fires <strong>of</strong> natural selection. In fact, Ernst Mayra s s e rts that one <strong>of</strong> Darwin’s most important contributionswas <strong>the</strong> firm establishment among biologiststhat a species is not composed <strong>of</strong> one fixed, identicaltype, but ra<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> unique individuals that differ fromone ano<strong>the</strong>r in a variety <strong>of</strong> ways. Fu rt h e rmore, Darwinrealized that <strong>the</strong> only variations that are importantto natural selection <strong>and</strong> speciation are those thatcan be passed from generation to generation.D a rwin had a vexing problem, however: he could notdefine a valid mechanism by which variations couldbe transmitted unchanged from one generation to<strong>the</strong> next. The prevailing wisdom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> time—<strong>the</strong>late-nineteenth century—was blending inheritance,<strong>the</strong> view that <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> both parentsblend toge<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring, producing progenythat are intermediate in character between <strong>the</strong> twoparents. Fleeming Jenkin, an engineer, argued in <strong>the</strong>1860s that blending inheritance would make differentialselection impossible because all members <strong>of</strong> apopulation would be <strong>the</strong> same. Fu rt h e rmore, withina few generations, blending inheritance would vitiate<strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> any new variations that arose.Darwin, who published The Origin <strong>of</strong> Species in1859, could have found substantial help in <strong>the</strong> work<strong>of</strong> his contemporary, Gregor Mendel, who publishedhis paper “Experiments in Plant-Hybridization” in1865. Although Mendel almost certainly was aware<strong>of</strong> Darwin’s work, most historians <strong>of</strong> science agreethat Darwin never read Mendel’s paper, for, had hedone so, he likely would have recognized thatMendel’s experiments provided solutions to his naggingproblem. Mendel had demonstrated <strong>the</strong> particulatenature <strong>of</strong> inheritance, showing that hereditaryinformation is carried by some discrete elements in<strong>the</strong> germ cells. These discrete elements permit <strong>the</strong>transmission <strong>of</strong> traits in unchanged form. We nowcall <strong>the</strong>se elements genes.Ironically, <strong>the</strong> rediscovery <strong>of</strong> Mendel’s work in 1900(about two decades after Darwin’s death) causedserious problems for Darwin’s <strong>the</strong>ory. As evolutionarybiologist Douglas Futuyma (1986) explains, biologists<strong>of</strong> that era “dismissed continuous variationamong individuals as inconsequential <strong>and</strong> largelynongenetic, <strong>and</strong> emphasized <strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong> discontinuousvariants that displayed Mendelian ratios <strong>and</strong>clearly particulate inheritance.” In 1918, however,Ronald A. Fisher found supporting evidence forNilssan-Ehle’s proposition about multiple genes.Fisher showed that continuous variation (height, forexample) results from multiple genes that are inheritedin Mendelian fashion <strong>and</strong> that have small, additiveeffects.The growth <strong>of</strong> population genetics <strong>and</strong> its accompanyingma<strong>the</strong>matical models led to <strong>the</strong> Modern Syn<strong>the</strong>sis<strong>of</strong> Evolution during <strong>the</strong> 1930s <strong>and</strong> 1940s. Thisreconciliation <strong>of</strong> Mendel <strong>and</strong> Darwin involved some<strong>of</strong> this century’s greatest biologists, among <strong>the</strong>mSewall Wright, J.B.S. Haldane, Ronald Fisher, ErnstM a y r, George Gaylord Simpson, TheodosiusDobzhansky, <strong>and</strong> G. Ledyard Stebbins.The 1944 discovery by Av e ry, McCart y, <strong>and</strong> MacLeodthat DNA is <strong>the</strong> genetic material <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> 1953 delineation<strong>of</strong> DNA’s structure by Watson <strong>and</strong> Crick providedadditional opportunities to investigate geneticaspects <strong>of</strong> organic evolution, including <strong>the</strong> causes,rates, <strong>and</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> mutation. One obvious <strong>and</strong> currentexample <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> relationship between genetics<strong>and</strong> evolutionary biology is <strong>the</strong> ability to compare42©1997 by BSCS.


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section VDNA base sequences among species to help establishphylogenetic relationships.In summary, <strong>the</strong> growth <strong>of</strong> genetics <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> growth<strong>of</strong> evolution <strong>the</strong>ory are intimately related. Becausegenetics is <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> root source <strong>of</strong> biologicalvariation, which is central to evolutionary mechanisms,an underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> basic principles in geneticsis central to an underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> evolution itself.NONTRADITIONAL CONCEPTS IN GENETICSThe following sections describe some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> concepts<strong>of</strong> inheritance that have been discovered since<strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> classical underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>inheritance in <strong>the</strong> late nineteenth <strong>and</strong> early twentiethcenturies. Keep in mind that it is not <strong>the</strong> inheritancethat is nontraditional, but ra<strong>the</strong>r our underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> inheritance that is new. Figure 14lists <strong>the</strong> nontraditional concepts <strong>of</strong> inheritancedescribed in this Overview.Some heritable traits are extranuclear. Mendelianprinciples <strong>of</strong> inheritance apply to chromosomes thatare found in <strong>the</strong> nucleus, but not to DNA that islocated elsewhere in <strong>the</strong> cell. For instance, mitochondrialDNA is packaged into a circular chromosome,<strong>of</strong> which <strong>the</strong>re are thous<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> copies perFigure 14 New (nontraditional) concepts <strong>of</strong>inheritance■ Some heritable traits are extranuclear (mitochondrialinheritance, cytoplasmic inheritance).■ Genetic anticipation (increased severity <strong>of</strong> a geneticdisorder in later generations) correlates with expansion<strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats.■ Genomic imprinting can alter <strong>the</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> geneticinformation <strong>and</strong> distinguish its parental origin.■ Both chromosomes <strong>of</strong> a pair may, in rare cases, comefrom one parent (uniparental disomy).■ Some genes are mobile <strong>and</strong> can insert <strong>the</strong>mselves innew chromosomal locations.■ Genes may, in rare cases, undergo horizontal transferbetween individuals or species.■ Many traits result from expression <strong>of</strong> more than onegene combined with environmental factors (multifactorialinheritance).■ Genetic information specified by genomic sequencemay be altered during RNAediting.cell. All <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mitochondria in a fertilized zygote arecontributed by <strong>the</strong> ovum; none come from <strong>the</strong>sperm. Activity 2 introduces students to <strong>the</strong> concept<strong>of</strong> maternal inheritance <strong>of</strong> mitochondrial genes.(Copymaster 2-5 in Activity 2 includes an illustration<strong>of</strong> mitochondrial inheritance.) A similar phenomenon<strong>of</strong> extranuclear inheritance occurs in <strong>the</strong> inheritance<strong>of</strong> chloroplast DNA.Genetic anticipation correlates with expansion <strong>of</strong>trinucleotide repeats. Traditional principles <strong>of</strong> geneticspredict that <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> a gene remains stableas it passes from parent to child. Even in situationswhere a mutation arises in a gene, <strong>the</strong> alteredstructure generally remains fixed <strong>and</strong> is inherited inthat form. The recent discovery <strong>of</strong> unstable trinucleotiderepeats, however, has shown that parts <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> gene are not so stable.The term “trinucleotide repeat” refers to a specificDNA sequence <strong>of</strong> three nucleotides that is repeatedover <strong>and</strong> over again, one after <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r. The precisenumber <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se repeated trinucleotides at part i c u l a rlocations in <strong>the</strong> genome varies from person to person.These so-called “polymorphic” (multiple form s )repeat regions are largely stable; that is, when a particularchromosome is passed from parent to child,<strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> repeated trinucleotides remains <strong>the</strong>same. The size <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> repeated unit can vary from twoto several hundred nucleotides. The variability <strong>and</strong>stability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se polymorphic repeat regions provide<strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> DNA fingerprinting, which is used inforensic <strong>and</strong> medical application (see For Your Information:DNA fingerprinting, p. 44).Most regions <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats stay <strong>the</strong> samelength. Some trinucleotide repeat regions are unusual,however, in that <strong>the</strong>y are unstable once <strong>the</strong>yexceed a particular size. Phenotypic effects <strong>of</strong> thisphenomenon are evident in cases where <strong>the</strong> unstabletrinucleotide repeat region lies within <strong>the</strong> portion<strong>of</strong> a gene that is transcribed to RNA (althoughnot necessarily in <strong>the</strong> coding region, see Figure 13).Molecular biologists uncovered <strong>the</strong> first example <strong>of</strong>this phenomenon in 1991 with <strong>the</strong> discovery <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>gene causing fragile X syndrome, an X- l i n ked disorderthat is <strong>the</strong> most common cause <strong>of</strong> inheritedmental retardation (see For Your Inform a t i o n :Fragile X syndrome, p. 45). Here, research43©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■ <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>For Your InformationDNA fingerprintingSome DNA differences between two individuals can be detected only by comparing nucleotidesequences. There are, however, two basic kinds <strong>of</strong> DNA variation, or polymorphism, that are detectedmore easily, as shown in <strong>the</strong> illustration. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se types <strong>of</strong> polymorphism is <strong>the</strong> restrictionfragment length polymorphism (RFLP)—<strong>the</strong> variable length <strong>of</strong> DNA fragments produced by <strong>the</strong> presenceor absence <strong>of</strong> a recognition site for a specific restriction endonuclease.A second type <strong>of</strong> DNA polymorphism is an array <strong>of</strong> repeated DNA sequences that varies in <strong>the</strong> number<strong>of</strong> repeated units. Not all <strong>the</strong> arrays <strong>of</strong> repeated sequences are polymorphic, but those that are serve asexcellent markers for tracing <strong>the</strong> inheritance <strong>of</strong> particular regions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genome. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> reasonsthat repeat polymorphisms are more useful than RFLPs is that <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> alleles at a particularlocus can be highly variable. In contrast, an RFLP usually has only two alleles (<strong>the</strong> presence or absence<strong>of</strong> restriction-endonuclease recognition site). Repeat polymorphisms are <strong>the</strong>refore very informative.If one investigates a repeat polymorphism by Sou<strong>the</strong>rn analysis, <strong>the</strong> location <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> probe determines<strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> result. If <strong>the</strong> probe corresponds to <strong>the</strong> repeated sequence itself, <strong>the</strong>n b<strong>and</strong>s representinga l l genomic locations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> polymorphism will be evident. This approach is <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> DNAfingerprinting method developed byAlec Jeffreys in 1985, where each DNAsample from an individual looks like abar code. This technique can be usedfor forensic purposes by matchingDNA from specimens at a crime scenewith DNA isolated from suspects.A l t e r n a t i v e l y, DNA fingerprinting canbe used to rule out or provide strongevidence for parentage, because eachb<strong>and</strong> in someone’s “bar code” hasbeen inherited from one or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rparent. For instance, if <strong>the</strong>re areb<strong>and</strong>s in a child’s DNA fingerprintthat do not appear in <strong>the</strong> DNA patternei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> supposed fa<strong>the</strong>r or <strong>the</strong>known mo<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> paternitymust be questioned.Ano<strong>the</strong>r way to follow repeat polymorphismsis to use a probe just outside<strong>the</strong> repeat region, where <strong>the</strong>DNA sequence is unique, or to use<strong>the</strong> polymerase chain reaction (PCR)for specific isolation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> repeatregion. This method can be useful infinding DNA markers linked to diseasegenes.DNA polymorphisms. (a) RFLP: In allele 2, a missing recognitionsite for a restriction enzyme results in digestion <strong>of</strong> fewer sites <strong>and</strong> in<strong>the</strong> generation <strong>of</strong> a different pattern <strong>of</strong> fragments. (b) Repeat Po l y-morphism: The number <strong>of</strong> sequence repeats may very between alleles.When <strong>the</strong> repeat region in each allele is cut out using a specificrestriction enzyme or amplified by PCR, <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> repeats can bec o m p a r e d .44©1997 by BSCS.


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section VFor Your InformationFragile X syndromeclinical featuresThe principal features <strong>of</strong> fragile X syndrome in affected males are mental retardation, long face, <strong>and</strong>large testes after puberty. Carrier females do not have a distinctive appearance, but about one-thirdare mildly mentally retarded.incidenceFor human males, this disorder is <strong>the</strong> most common cause <strong>of</strong> inherited mental retardation, occurringin 1:1000-2000 male births. Incidence in females is lower, around 1:8000.inheritanceFragile X syndrome, as <strong>the</strong> name implies, is an X-linked trait; it shows variable penetrance <strong>and</strong>expressivity. About one-fifth <strong>of</strong> males who transmit <strong>the</strong> mutant gene have no symptoms <strong>the</strong>mselves.About one-third <strong>of</strong> heterozygous females show some clinical features. The likelihood that symptomswill appear in family members increases in later generations. This observation originally was called<strong>the</strong> “Sherman paradox.” Because <strong>of</strong> new molecular data, <strong>the</strong> Sherman paradox now is attributed toexpansion <strong>of</strong> a trinucleotide repeat region.gene <strong>and</strong> proteinThe gene responsible for fragile X syndrome lies on <strong>the</strong> X chromosome <strong>and</strong> was isolated in 1991. The geneproduct is a protein that has RNA-binding properties, but its precise function in <strong>the</strong> cell is still being investigated.The gene contains a polymorphic <strong>and</strong> unstable CGG trinucleotide repeat in <strong>the</strong> portion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genethat is transcribed, although <strong>the</strong> repeat region lies outside <strong>the</strong> coding region. The normal range <strong>of</strong> variationin <strong>the</strong> trinucleotide repeat number is 5-50; beyond that, <strong>the</strong> repeat becomes unstable <strong>and</strong> can exp<strong>and</strong> duringtransmission from parent to child. Expansion is more pronounced when an unstable allele is inheritedfrom <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r. (The number <strong>of</strong> repeats correlates to <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> symptoms see Figure 15).disease mechanismExpansion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trinucleotide repeat in fragile X syndrome disrupts production <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> protein product,which under normal circumstances is found at highest levels in <strong>the</strong> brain <strong>and</strong> testes. Lack <strong>of</strong> this proteinpresumably prevents normal development <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se tissues in particular.diagnosisDiagnosis based on clinical manifestations generally occurs in infancy or early childhood. Large ears <strong>and</strong>slow development are indications. Diagnosis has been possible by cytogenetic analysis; this test uses lymphocytes<strong>and</strong> can be done at any age, including prenatally. Cytogenetic analysis relies on detection <strong>of</strong>fragility <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> X chromosome characteristic <strong>of</strong> this mutation. A preferable diagnosis uses molecular geneticanalysis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> associated repeats in <strong>the</strong> gene associated with fragile X mental retardation (F M R 1).treatmentNo treatment is available for fragile X syndrome.revealed a polymorphic CGG trinucleotide repeatthat lies in <strong>the</strong> region <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene transcribed intomRNA but outside <strong>the</strong> coding region. Studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>fragile X gene revealed a correlation between <strong>the</strong>number <strong>of</strong> CGG repeats <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> phenotype. Allmale patients with fragile X syndrome had morethan 200 CGG repeats, whereas unaffected peoplefrom outside <strong>the</strong>se families had between 5 <strong>and</strong> 50CGG repeats. A third group <strong>of</strong> people withbetween 50 <strong>and</strong> 200 CGG repeats were those familymembers who were unaffected carriers <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>d i s o r d e r. Alleles with greater than 50 CGG repeats45©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>Figure 15 The number <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeatscorrelates to phenotype for certain genetic disorders:Notice <strong>the</strong> overlap <strong>of</strong> symbols, suggesting that <strong>the</strong>number <strong>of</strong> repeats is not <strong>the</strong> only factor to influencemanifestation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> disorder.usually increased in size during passage from parentto child, especially when <strong>the</strong> unstable allelecame from <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r. Generally, <strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong>number <strong>of</strong> repeats, <strong>the</strong> higher <strong>the</strong> likelihood thatexpansion will occur during transmission to <strong>the</strong>next generation. We do not know <strong>the</strong> precisemechanism <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> instability (which is usually, butnot always, expansion) <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> reason for <strong>the</strong> differencesin parental inheritance.Soon after <strong>the</strong> discovery <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fragile X gene <strong>and</strong>its unstable trinucleotide repeats, biologists discovered<strong>the</strong> same type <strong>of</strong> mutation in several o<strong>the</strong>rdisorders (Figure 15). Two <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se, myotonic dystrophy<strong>and</strong> Huntington disease, serve as examplesin Activity 3, Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ( s e eFor Your Information on each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se topics:Myotonic dystrophy, Huntington disease <strong>and</strong> P r e-dictive testing for HD). Myotonic dystrophy <strong>and</strong>Figure 16 Correlation between <strong>the</strong> trinucleotiderepeat number <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> clinical features <strong>of</strong>myotonic dystrophy: The numbers represent howmany copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trinucleotide repeats are present ineach allele.Huntington disease exhibit “anticipation,” which is<strong>the</strong> increasing severity <strong>and</strong>/or earlier onset <strong>of</strong>symptoms in generations <strong>of</strong> an affected family. Thisphenomenon is particularly apparent in myotonicd y s t r o p h y, where <strong>the</strong>re is a good corr e l a t i o nbetween <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> unstable trinucleotiderepeat <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> symptoms (see Fi g-ure 16). As with fragile X syndrome, <strong>the</strong> relationshipbetween <strong>the</strong> larger number <strong>of</strong> repeats <strong>and</strong>more severe symptoms in myotonic dystrophy isu n c l e a r. Fu rt h e r, as with fragile X syndrome,myotonic dystrophy <strong>and</strong> Huntington disease showincreased anticipation when <strong>the</strong> passage is througha parent <strong>of</strong> one particular sex. Anticipation isgreater when <strong>the</strong> passage is through mo<strong>the</strong>rs whohave myotonic dystrophy; in <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> Hunting-46©1997 by BSCS.


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section VFor Your InformationMyotonic dystrophyclinical featuresThe symptoms <strong>of</strong> myotonic dystrophy (DM) are extremely variable. In late onset <strong>of</strong> DM, symptomsinclude a hollow facial appearance accompanied by sagging facial muscles. O<strong>the</strong>r symptoms includecataracts, frontal balding, heart arrhythmia, myotonia (inability to release a grip), muscle weakness,defective speech, bronchitis, <strong>and</strong> mental retardation. A severe congenital form has major muscle weakness,moderate mental retardation, <strong>and</strong> exhibits myotonia in early childhood.incidenceDM is <strong>the</strong> most common adult muscular dystrophy, occurring at roughly 1:1,800-8,000 in European<strong>and</strong> American populations, 1:18,000 in Japan, <strong>and</strong> very rarely in Africa. Reports <strong>of</strong> incidence varydepending on <strong>the</strong> criteria used to establish that an individual is affected.inheritanceDM is inherited as an autosomal dominant disorder that shows anticipation. Expressivity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mutantgene varies, <strong>and</strong> penetrance can be incomplete. Congenital DM is inherited maternally.gene <strong>and</strong> proteinThe mutation has been mapped to a location on chromosome 19 (19q 13.2-13.3). The affectedgene has a region <strong>of</strong> unstable trinucleotide repeats (CTG). Ranges from 50 to several thous<strong>and</strong>copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> repeat are associated with affected individuals. The locus is referred to as D M P K(myotonic dystrophy protein kinase). The gene product is a protein kinase, sometimes calledmyotonin kinase.disease mechanismThe exact mechanism is not understood. Symptoms develop as a result <strong>of</strong> weakness, atrophy, <strong>and</strong>myotonia <strong>of</strong> affected muscles, especially in <strong>the</strong> face <strong>and</strong> neck muscles <strong>and</strong> in <strong>the</strong> extremities. For congenitalDM, <strong>the</strong> major cause <strong>of</strong> death likely is from <strong>the</strong> involvement <strong>of</strong> respiratory muscles.diagnosisDiagnosis <strong>of</strong> adult-onset DM is based on <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> clinical features described above, coupledwith family history, molecular genetic data, <strong>and</strong>, possibly, evidence <strong>of</strong> myotonia based on a test by electromyography.Congenital DM is diagnosed by <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> this disorder in <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r, seen toge<strong>the</strong>rwith reduced fetal movements, muscle weakness, respiratory difficulties, <strong>and</strong> mental retardation.DM can be diagnosed by prenatal genetic tests combined with ultrasound.treatmentPhysical <strong>the</strong>rapy <strong>and</strong> surgery for cataracts are useful for mild adult symptoms. External pacemakersmay help patients who have atrioventricular blocks.ton disease, anticipation is greater through affectedfa<strong>the</strong>rs. We do not know <strong>the</strong> mechanisms for<strong>the</strong>se sex differences, but <strong>the</strong>y could involve differentialinstability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trinucleotide repeats duringmeiosis; ano<strong>the</strong>r possible mechanism is imprinting(see <strong>the</strong> following discussion).Several o<strong>the</strong>r genetic disorders are associated withunstable trinucleotide repeats. A review in <strong>the</strong>1995 Annual Review <strong>of</strong> <strong>Genetics</strong> “ Tr i n u c l e o t i d eRepeat Expansion <strong>and</strong> Human Disease,” by C.T.A s h l e y, Jr., <strong>and</strong> S.T. Wa rren, describes nine diseasesin which expansion <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats is47©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■ <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>For Your InformationHuntington diseaseclinical featuresHuntington disease (HD) produces chorea (twitching or twisting involuntary movements that occurwhen <strong>the</strong> affected person attempts voluntary movement) <strong>and</strong> dementia. Speech <strong>of</strong>ten is affected,<strong>and</strong> memory lapses may occur. HD is a late-onset disease that usually manifests itself in middle ageor later, although <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> onset varies from one generation to ano<strong>the</strong>r.incidenceApproximately 1:10,000-20,000 for predominately Caucasian populations; 1:333,000 in Asian populations(particularly in Japan); incidence is lowest among African Black populations.inheritanceHD is inherited as an autosomal dominant disorder that exhibits genetic anticipation.gene <strong>and</strong> proteinThe mutant gene for HD was isolated in 1993, although detection by linkage analysis was availableabout 10 years earlier. The gene is called IT-15, <strong>and</strong> it is located on chromosome 4, in <strong>the</strong> 4p16.3region. The trinucleotide CAG is repeated a variable number <strong>of</strong> times, in t<strong>and</strong>em, in this gene.Repeat numbers in <strong>the</strong> range 40-100 are unstable <strong>and</strong> result in HD. There is evidence <strong>of</strong> incompletepenetrance for people with alleles carrying repeats in <strong>the</strong> range 36-39. The protein encoded by thisgene is named huntingtin.disease mechanismHD is a neurological disorder, but we do not yet know <strong>the</strong> exact biochemical mechanism.diagnosisDiagnosis is made by <strong>the</strong> combined presence <strong>of</strong> chorea <strong>and</strong> dementia, positive family history, <strong>and</strong>/orgenetic test results showing more than 40 trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> associated gene.treatmentThere is no treatment to alter <strong>the</strong> progress <strong>of</strong> this disorder, although we can manage <strong>the</strong> symptomsto some degree with drugs.known to play a role. Since that time, research hasshown that ano<strong>the</strong>r disorder, Friedreich ataxia, isassociated with an unstable GAA repeat. This disorder<strong>of</strong>ten begins in childhood or adolescencewith clumsiness involving <strong>the</strong> extremities. Generally, <strong>the</strong>re is no mental deficiency, but <strong>the</strong> neurologicalproblems progress steadily. Death generallyresults in <strong>the</strong> mid to late thirties. It is usually inheritedas an autosomal recessive disorder, but about10 percent <strong>of</strong> cases indicate autosomal dominanti n h e r i t a n c e .Genomic imprinting can alter <strong>the</strong> expression <strong>of</strong>genetic information <strong>and</strong> distinguish its parental origi n . Mendelian principles predict that each parentcontributes one copy <strong>of</strong> a particular autosomal geneto <strong>of</strong>fspring. Implicit in this prediction is <strong>the</strong> assumptionthat <strong>the</strong> two copies <strong>of</strong> a given autosomal genebehave in <strong>the</strong> same way, such that <strong>the</strong> parental origin<strong>of</strong> a particular allele does not affect its expression.Although this assumption appears to be correct formost genes, <strong>the</strong>re are exceptions. In some genes, allelesare differentially expressed, depending on <strong>the</strong>ir48©1997 by BSCS.


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section VFor Your InformationPredictive testing for Huntington diseaseMolecular biologists have known since 1983 that <strong>the</strong> gene responsible for Huntington disease (HD) liesnear one end <strong>of</strong> human chromosome 4. At that time, <strong>the</strong> gene itself had not been found, but <strong>the</strong>re was apolymorphic DNA marker whose pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance closely matched that <strong>of</strong> HD in affected families,<strong>and</strong> which <strong>the</strong>refore was closely linked to <strong>the</strong> gene. Research soon identified additional polymorphicmarkers close to <strong>the</strong> HD gene, <strong>and</strong> predictive testing by linkage analysis became an option for families in1986. Linkage analysis uses <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> identifiable markers located quite near a gene <strong>of</strong> interest toidentify <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> that gene as being highly probable. This illustration shows predictive testing <strong>of</strong>Huntington disease by linkage analysis. The mutant gene is traveling with <strong>the</strong> “aw” haplotype.There were a number <strong>of</strong> limitations to predictive testing by linkage analysis. Although <strong>the</strong> markersaround <strong>the</strong> HD gene were linked closely, <strong>the</strong>re was a 4 percent probability that <strong>the</strong>y would be separatedby recombination.That situation meantthat if a person at riskfor HD inherited <strong>the</strong>same alleles for <strong>the</strong>markers that ano<strong>the</strong>raffected member <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong>ir family had, thisperson could be toldwith only 96 percentcertainty that he orshe also would develop<strong>the</strong> disease. Ano<strong>the</strong>rproblem with linkageanalysis was that<strong>the</strong> polymorphic markerswere not “informative”in some families.For instance, if anaffected parent hadidentical polymorphicmarkers on both chromosomes,it was not possible to tell which <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chromosomes had been passed on to his or her children—<strong>the</strong>chromosome with <strong>the</strong> HD gene, or <strong>the</strong> one without <strong>the</strong> HD gene. Therefore, it was impossibleto predict who in <strong>the</strong> family would develop HD. In addition, linkage analysis usually was notpossible in <strong>the</strong> absence <strong>of</strong> DNA from an affected individual or o<strong>the</strong>r key family members.The discovery <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene made direct predictive testing possible. By using Sou<strong>the</strong>rnanalysis or <strong>the</strong> polymerase chain reaction (PCR), scientists can measure <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trinucleotiderepeat region in this gene. A person who has more than 40 trinucleotide repeats eventuallywill develop HD, unless death occurs by some o<strong>the</strong>r cause first. See Figure 15 for an indication <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> range <strong>of</strong> repeat numbers seen in <strong>the</strong> HD gene.49©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■ <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>parental source. This phenomenon is called “genomicimprinting” (Figure 17). For each imprinted gene,ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> paternal or maternal allele, or both, arem a r ked in some way that influences gene expression.It is usually <strong>the</strong> marked allele that is suppressed. Wedo not underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> imprinting process completely, but it gives an allele a distinctive behavior thatreflects <strong>the</strong> parental source. 4A primary imprint likely is acquired at <strong>the</strong> particulargenetic location during meiosis, <strong>and</strong> this imprint ismaintained on <strong>the</strong> particular chromosome in <strong>the</strong>haploid gamete. After fertilization, <strong>the</strong> imprint ismaintained on <strong>the</strong> paternal or maternal chromosomethroughout DNA replication in <strong>the</strong> diploidzygote. That imprint <strong>the</strong>n leads to functional differencesbetween <strong>the</strong> paternal <strong>and</strong> maternal alleles insomatic cells. Imprinting is reversible in that <strong>the</strong>imprint is erased during <strong>the</strong> production <strong>of</strong> germcells, from which gametes are derived. Imprintingmay involve DNA methylation, although evidence is,at present, insufficient to establish <strong>the</strong> molecularmechanism with certainty.P r a d e r-Willi syndrome <strong>and</strong> Angelman syndromeillustrate genomic imprinting in humans. Prader-Willi syndrome is characterized by obesity, excessiveappetite, small h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> feet, short stature,small sexual organs, <strong>and</strong> mental retardation. In contrast,Angelman syndrome is characterized bysevere mental retardation, inappropriate laughter,seizures, <strong>and</strong> uncoordinated movements. Thegenes responsible for <strong>the</strong>se disorders are in <strong>the</strong>same region <strong>of</strong> chromosome 15 (15q11-13). A tinydeletion in this region can result ei<strong>the</strong>r in Prader-Figure 17 Genomic imprinting: (a.) The pedigreeshows <strong>the</strong> pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance <strong>of</strong> an imprinted genethat behaves differently in individual 3 than it did in individual2. ( b . ) A gene in <strong>the</strong> gametes <strong>of</strong> individual 1 has afemale imprint that affects its behavior when it is inheritedmaternally by individual 2. In his germ cells, <strong>the</strong>imprint will be erased, to be replaced by his own imprint.4The term imprinting has an interesting history. It was used earlier in a genetic context to refer to <strong>the</strong> selective elimination<strong>of</strong> a paternal chromosome or to <strong>the</strong> selective inactivation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> X chromosome derived from <strong>the</strong> male parent.©1997 by BSCS.50


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section VWilli syndrome or Angelman syndrome; <strong>the</strong> disorderthat results depends, in part, on <strong>the</strong> parentalorigin <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> chromosome carrying <strong>the</strong> deletion(see Figure 18). If <strong>the</strong> chromosome 15 deletion is <strong>of</strong>m a t e rnal origin, Angelman syndrome results. Prader-Willi syndrome results if <strong>the</strong> chromosome 15deletion is paternal in origin. This difference suggeststhat <strong>the</strong> region containing <strong>the</strong> genes associatedwith <strong>the</strong>se disorders normally carries an imprintthat distinguishes <strong>the</strong> maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal alleles<strong>and</strong> that influences gene expression.Additional evidence for imprinting comes from extensivestudies in mice. Embryos that have a full set <strong>of</strong>chromosomes derived from only one parent invariablyfail to develop properly, even though <strong>the</strong>y have a completeset <strong>of</strong> genes. Without a maternal set <strong>of</strong> chromosomes,<strong>the</strong> embryo is abnormal, <strong>and</strong> without a paternalset <strong>of</strong> chromosomes, <strong>the</strong> placenta fails to develop.That phenomenon also occurs in humans. Theseo b s e rvations suggest that <strong>the</strong> parental source <strong>of</strong> achromosome can influence its effect; normal cellsrequire a complement <strong>of</strong> maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal genes.It also is not possible to generate a viable embryo from<strong>the</strong> fusion <strong>of</strong> two female ova, or from <strong>the</strong> fusion <strong>of</strong>oocyte chromosomes with chromosomes from <strong>the</strong>first polar body (a by-product <strong>of</strong> meiosis).Both chromosomes <strong>of</strong> a pair may, in rare cases, comefrom one parent. Mendelian principles predict that, ina pair <strong>of</strong> chromosomes, one homolog has been contributedfrom each parent. In rare situations, however,this pattern is not <strong>the</strong> case, <strong>and</strong> both chromosomes <strong>of</strong>a pair are inherited from <strong>the</strong> same parent. This unusualform <strong>of</strong> inheritance is called “uniparental disomy. ”This situation may arise from a starting condition <strong>of</strong> trisomy—<strong>the</strong>extra chromosome <strong>the</strong>n is lost. If it was <strong>the</strong>only homolog from one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> parents, uniparentaldisomy results. If <strong>the</strong> remaining pair <strong>of</strong> chromosomescame from one parent <strong>and</strong> are i d e n t i c a l, <strong>the</strong> conditionis called uniparental “i s od i s o m y.” If <strong>the</strong> remaining pair<strong>of</strong> chromosomes came from one parent <strong>and</strong> are d i f-f e r e n t homologs, <strong>the</strong> result is uniparental “h e t e r od i s-o m y.” Errors in chromosome replication can result in<strong>the</strong>se unusual patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance.Figure 18 Genomic imprinting results in differentphenotypic expression in <strong>the</strong>se two human geneticd i s o r d e r s : Deletion in <strong>the</strong> same region <strong>of</strong> human chromosome15 behaves differently depending on <strong>the</strong> parent<strong>of</strong> origin. Maternal inheritance results in Angelman syndrome;paternal inheritance causes Prader-Willi syndrome.Uniparental disomy can produce a similar effect.The phenotypic consequences <strong>of</strong> uniparental disomyinclude <strong>the</strong> unusual inheritance <strong>of</strong> autosomal recessivetraits <strong>and</strong> aberrations related to imprinting. Uniparentaldisomy in humans was first recognized in <strong>the</strong>late 1980s, when a girl with cystic fibrosis <strong>and</strong> shortstature was found to have two identical copies <strong>of</strong> muchor all <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> maternal chromosome 7. Her mo<strong>the</strong>r wasa heterozygous carrier <strong>of</strong> a cystic fibrosis allele, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>daughter maternally inherited a duplicate copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>mutant allele, resulting in uniparental isodisomy forchromosome 7. The short stature probably resultedfrom an imprinting effect. This cystic fibrosis exampleshows one pattern that can signal uniparental disomy:a child expresses a recessive trait but, surprisingly, it isnot true that b o t h parents carry <strong>the</strong> recessive allele, aswould be expected based on traditional concepts <strong>of</strong>recessive inheritance.If <strong>the</strong> chromosome pair for which <strong>the</strong>re is uniparentaldisomy includes an imprinted gene, <strong>and</strong> if<strong>the</strong> donor parent is <strong>the</strong> one whose alleles are suppressed,<strong>the</strong>n that gene will not be expressed in <strong>the</strong><strong>of</strong>fspring. That situation is one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mechanisms bywhich Prader-Willi syndrome can occur, since <strong>the</strong>inheritance <strong>of</strong> two maternal chromosomes 15 has<strong>the</strong> same effect as a localized deletion in <strong>the</strong> appropriatepart <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paternal chromosome 15. Prader-Willi syndrome can occur as a result <strong>of</strong> (maternal)uniparental isodisomy or heterodisomy.51©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>Some genes are mobile <strong>and</strong> can insert <strong>the</strong>mselves innew chromosomal locations. Classic laws <strong>of</strong> inheritanceassume that a gene occurs at a fixed location in<strong>the</strong> genome. There are, however, “transposablegenetic elements” that are mobile. Barbara McClintock,while performing breeding experiments inmaize (Zea mays) in <strong>the</strong> 1940s, was one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> firstscientists to recognize that genes could movearound in <strong>the</strong> genome. She saw odd patterns in <strong>the</strong>inheritance <strong>of</strong> pigments in maize kernels that onecould not explain by conventional genetic <strong>the</strong>ories.She eventually concluded that a few genes do nothave fixed locations in <strong>the</strong> genome but instead movefrom one location to ano<strong>the</strong>r during <strong>the</strong> passagefrom parent to progeny.Barbara McClintock’s discovery <strong>of</strong> transposablegenetic elements is a good example <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature<strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science. She made observations in<strong>the</strong> 1940s that did not fit <strong>the</strong> classic underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>inheritance, <strong>and</strong> she proposed new explanations forthose observations. Although her hypo<strong>the</strong>ses werenot widely accepted at first, she continued her work<strong>and</strong>, gradually, with <strong>the</strong> accumulation <strong>of</strong> more evidence,her views gained strength. In <strong>the</strong> 1970s,experiments in molecular biology provided a mechanismfor <strong>the</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong> transposons. Moleculardata demonstrated that small pieces <strong>of</strong> DNA sometimesmove around <strong>the</strong> genome, triggering changesin gene expression. McClintock’s hypo<strong>the</strong>sis is nowwidely acknowledged, <strong>and</strong> she won <strong>the</strong> Nobel Prizefor Physiology or Medicine in 1983. McClintock isone among many scientists, male <strong>and</strong> female, whostruggle to gain acceptance <strong>of</strong> revolutionary ideasthat contradict <strong>the</strong> prevailing wisdom. Science isinherently self-correcting, so one expects that accurateexplanations ultimately will prevail.Since <strong>the</strong> original discovery <strong>of</strong> transposable geneticelements in maize, research has uncovered similarfactors in o<strong>the</strong>r species, including bacteria, fruit flies,<strong>and</strong> humans. Transposable genetic elements are likeviruses in that <strong>the</strong>y can incorporate <strong>the</strong>mselves into<strong>the</strong> host genome; <strong>the</strong>y do not, however, have a proteincoat <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>refore are restricted to movementwithin a single cell. In moving, a transposable geneticelement breaks away from its site in <strong>the</strong> genome<strong>and</strong> inserts itself somewhere else, ei<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> sameor a different chromosome. Sometimes a copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>element is left in <strong>the</strong> original location; o<strong>the</strong>r times itis excised completely. Not surprisingly, <strong>the</strong> movement<strong>of</strong> transposable genetic elements can be quitedisruptive. Insertion <strong>of</strong> a transposable genetic elementinto a gene can turn <strong>of</strong>f its expression (whichis what was happening in McClintock’s genes formaize pigment). Inaccurate repair <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> DNA at <strong>the</strong>original site can cause a gene mutation.Genes may, in rare cases, undergo horizontaltransfer between individuals or species. Mendelianpatterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance apply to <strong>the</strong> vertical transmission<strong>of</strong> genes from parent to child. Genes can betransmitted horizontally, however, as in <strong>the</strong> transfer<strong>of</strong> genes between individuals or species. The classicexample <strong>of</strong> this is <strong>the</strong> demonstration that one canchange <strong>the</strong> phenotype <strong>of</strong> bacteria by transferringDNA from one strain to ano<strong>the</strong>r (see <strong>the</strong> entry for1944 in Figure 11). Whereas gene transfer betweenprokaryotes is not uncommon, <strong>the</strong>re is little evidenceto show that it has happened in eukaryotes.Never<strong>the</strong>less, it is possible that a transposable geneticelement called a P element was passed from onespecies <strong>of</strong> fruit fly to ano<strong>the</strong>r sometime in <strong>the</strong> last 50years. It is possible that this DNA transfer occurredthrough <strong>the</strong> mouthpiece <strong>of</strong> a parasitic mite. Ano<strong>the</strong>rpossibility for cross-species gene transfer is throughviral vectors.Many traits result from expression <strong>of</strong> more thanone gene combined with environmental factors(multifactorial inheritance). Classic inheritancepatterns are more easily recognized in traits thatresult from single genes than in those traits thatresult from more than one gene (polygenic) <strong>and</strong> thatare influenced significantly by environmental factors.This combination <strong>of</strong> environmental influence <strong>and</strong>action <strong>of</strong> multiple genes is called multifactorial inheritance.These types <strong>of</strong> traits might appear to run infamilies, but without any recognizable pattern. Figure19 illustrates <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> a multifactorial trait.(Many single gene traits are subject to environmentalinfluences. Although multiple factors are at workin such circumstances, <strong>the</strong> term multifactorial generallyis reserved for polygenic traits that are influencedby environment.)When Mendel’s laws were rediscovered at <strong>the</strong> turn <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> twentieth century, <strong>the</strong> factors <strong>of</strong> inheritance52©1997 by BSCS.


<strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong> ■ Section VFigure 19 Interaction <strong>of</strong> genotype, phenotype, <strong>and</strong> environmental influences: Many traits result from <strong>the</strong> combinedinfluence <strong>of</strong> several genes plus environmental factors.were viewed as particulate <strong>and</strong> fixed so that outcomesfrom any given genotype would be similar, ifnot identical. Since that time, we have learned that avariety <strong>of</strong> factors can affect <strong>the</strong> phenotype in additionto what is prescribed by <strong>the</strong> genotype. Environment,for example, <strong>of</strong>ten influences genotypicexpression. Plants or people that inherit genotypesfor tall adult stature will show that phenotype only ifplaced in an environment where nutritional factorsallow <strong>the</strong>m to reach <strong>the</strong> potential <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir genotype.If placed in a nutritionally restricted setting, no genotypewill allow <strong>the</strong>m to reach great stature. Similarly,we know that genes can act on each o<strong>the</strong>r or contributedirectly to phenotype. For example, someinherited anemias may be less severe in individualswho have counterbalancing genes for <strong>the</strong> increasedproduction <strong>of</strong> red blood cells. Finally, <strong>the</strong>re is astrong element <strong>of</strong> chance in <strong>the</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> manygenes, an element that is not yet well understood.Chance may play a role in <strong>the</strong> time at which a certaingene is turned on or <strong>of</strong>f, which can affect <strong>the</strong> timing<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> appearance or relative degree <strong>of</strong> expression <strong>of</strong>a trait.Multifactorial traits (as distinct from single-gene traitsthat show variable expression) can be divided intothree classes characterized by continuous variation,threshold traits, <strong>and</strong> complex adult disorders. In traitsthat show continuous variation, an “abnormal” phenotypesimply represents an extreme variation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>n o rmal range. Examples include many kinds <strong>of</strong> nonspecificmental retardation <strong>and</strong> unusual height (very53©1997 by BSCS.


Section V ■ <strong>Genetics</strong>: Basic Concepts <strong>and</strong> Nontraditional <strong>Inheritance</strong>s h o rt or very tall). In multifactorial threshold traits,<strong>the</strong>re is a clear distinction between normal <strong>and</strong>a b n o rmal phenotypes. In <strong>the</strong>se cases, <strong>the</strong>re may bean underlying continuous variation until a part i c u l a rthreshold is reached, at which point <strong>the</strong> abnorm a lphenotype appears. This model has been proposedto explain several common congenital abnorm a l i t i e ssuch as cleft lip <strong>and</strong> palate <strong>and</strong> neural tube defects(such as spina bifida). In complex adult disorderssuch as coronary art e ry disease <strong>and</strong> diabetes mellitus,<strong>the</strong>re is a variety <strong>of</strong> genetic <strong>and</strong> environmental influences.Here, a person is genetically predisposed to ap a rticular illness, but can impose a strong influenceon <strong>the</strong> outcome by lifestyle choices (such as diet).Genetic information specified by genomic sequencemay be altered during RNA editing. Classic geneticprinciples predict that an mRNA molecule, whichdirects <strong>the</strong> syn<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>of</strong> a protein from amino acidcomponents, is faithfully transcribed from its parentgene. Biologists have known for about 20 years that<strong>the</strong> coding region <strong>of</strong> a eukaryotic gene is usuallyi n t e rrupted by introns that are excised during mRNAprocessing, but <strong>the</strong>y assumed that <strong>the</strong> coding port i o n<strong>of</strong> mRNA remained unaltered during <strong>the</strong> conversionfrom precursor RNA to mature mRNA.Research in recent years has uncovered a phenomenoncalled RNA editing, where crucial coding informationis added to <strong>the</strong> mRNA after its transcriptionfrom DNA. The best understood example <strong>of</strong> RNAediting is that seen in trypanosome parasites, whichcause sleeping sickness <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r illnesses. In 1986,molecular biologists made <strong>the</strong> baffling observationthat mRNAs from some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mitochondrial genesare longer than <strong>the</strong> genes from which <strong>the</strong>y arederived. Additional (uridine) bases had been addedat specific points throughout <strong>the</strong> mRNAs, makingsense <strong>of</strong> what, at <strong>the</strong> DNA level, was nonsense.Research done in 1990 proposed an explanation forthis observation: smaller circles <strong>of</strong> DNA in <strong>the</strong> trypanosomemitochondria, whose function was previouslyunknown, produce small “guide RNAs” thatfind <strong>and</strong> correct omissions in <strong>the</strong> mRNA. The evolutionarysignificance <strong>of</strong> such a mechanism is still amystery. Perhaps RNA editing is related to <strong>the</strong> wellknownability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trypanosome to change its antigenicstructure <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>reby confound <strong>the</strong> immuneresponse <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> host organism.SUMMARYThis section <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h e r s e m p h a-sizes that our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> inheritance since <strong>the</strong>time <strong>of</strong> Mendel has endured <strong>and</strong> changed—<strong>and</strong>,indeed, is still changing. Traditional Mendeliangenetics continues to describe accurately <strong>the</strong> majority<strong>of</strong> what we observe in inheritance, <strong>and</strong> new discoveriescontinue to refine <strong>and</strong> extend our level <strong>of</strong>underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> genetics.For additional information on some human geneticdisorders, contact <strong>the</strong> following organizations.Alliance <strong>of</strong> Genetic Support Groups35 Wisconsin Circle, Suite 440Chevy Chase, MD 20815-70151-301-652-5553FAX: 1-301-654-0171E-mail: alliance@capaccess.orghttp://medhlp.netusa.net/www/agsg.htmHuntington’s Disease Society <strong>of</strong> America140 West 22nd Street, Sixth FloorNew York, NY 10011-24201-800-345-HDSA (4372)1-212-242-1968h t t p :// n e u r o - w w w 2 . m g h vard.edu/hdsa/hdsamain.nclk. h a rThe National Fragile X Foundation1441 York Street, Suite 215Denver, CO 802061-800-688-87651-303-333-6155Muscular Dystrophy Association3300 East Sunrise DriveTucson, AZ 85718-32081-602-529-2000http://www.mdausa.orgFor CompuServe users, you can use <strong>the</strong> CommunicationService Forum <strong>and</strong> ask <strong>the</strong> experts online.Get into <strong>the</strong> system <strong>and</strong> type GO MDA.54©1997 by BSCS.


GlossaryWe have provided this glossary for your convenience.The memorization <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se terms is not anobjective <strong>of</strong> this module, <strong>and</strong> we encourage you notto turn this list, or any portion <strong>of</strong> it, into a test.affected: <strong>the</strong> condition <strong>of</strong> having a particular trait,usually used in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> a disadvantageoustrait, as for a disease symptom.allele: one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> alternative forms <strong>of</strong> a gene at agiven locus.anticipation: earlier onset or increased severity <strong>of</strong>an inherited disorder in subsequent generations.(See trinucleotide repeats.)autosome: a chromosome o<strong>the</strong>r than a sex chromosome.Humans have 22 pairs <strong>of</strong> autosomes.c h i a s m a : (plural is chiasmata) <strong>the</strong> point <strong>of</strong> crossingover during prophase I <strong>of</strong> meiosis in which <strong>the</strong>re is anactual exchange <strong>of</strong> genetic material between <strong>the</strong>paired maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal copies <strong>of</strong> chromosomes.coding region: a stretch <strong>of</strong> DNA sequence (in agene) that encodes protein.codominant: <strong>the</strong> condition in which a pair <strong>of</strong> allelesfor a given locus equally contributes to <strong>the</strong> phenotype<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> heterozygote who bears <strong>the</strong>m.congenital: existing from birth (note that this termdoes not distinguish whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> condition is inherited,or environmental, or both).c r e d i b l e : (in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> scientific methods)<strong>the</strong> condition <strong>of</strong> being reliable <strong>and</strong> based onacceptable methods, as in reference to evidencethat meets scientific criteria for accuracy <strong>and</strong>r e p r o d u c i b i l i t y.cytogenetics: a subdiscipline <strong>of</strong> genetics that combines<strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cell with <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> genetics,<strong>of</strong>ten focusing on <strong>the</strong> structure, function, <strong>and</strong> behavior<strong>of</strong> chromosomes.deletion: (in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> molecular genetics) <strong>the</strong>absence <strong>of</strong> one or more nucleotides normally foundin a gene, resulting in a mutation.developmental autonomy: (in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong>ethics) having sufficient mental <strong>and</strong> emotional maturityto be capable <strong>of</strong> informed consent.diploid: having two copies <strong>of</strong> each chromosome; adiploid cell has a chromosome number <strong>of</strong> 2N. Inhumans, <strong>the</strong> diploid number is 46.D M : abbreviation for <strong>the</strong> human genetic disordermyotonic dystrophy. This disorder shows variablee x p r e s s i v i t , ywith symptoms ranging from cataracts <strong>and</strong>55


Glossarymild muscle weakness to extreme muscle wasting <strong>and</strong>contraction, impairment <strong>of</strong> some organ function, <strong>and</strong>mental retardation. DM displays genetic anticipation asa result <strong>of</strong> unstable trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> mutantgene associated with <strong>the</strong> disorder.DOE: <strong>the</strong> United States Department <strong>of</strong> Energy.dominant: <strong>the</strong> pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance in which anallele expresses its phenotypic effect even in heterozygotes<strong>and</strong> masks that <strong>of</strong> some o<strong>the</strong>r allele at <strong>the</strong>same locus. A trait expressed by dominant inheritanceis called a dominant trait.ELSI: Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications, a division<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project.e u ka ry o t e : an organism in which cells have amembrane-bound nucleus. Eukaryotes have o<strong>the</strong>rsubcellular organelles, such as mitochondria or, in<strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> plant cells, chloroplasts. Humans aree u k a ry o t e s .expression: in <strong>the</strong> molecular context, <strong>the</strong> transcription<strong>of</strong> a gene into RNA products, some <strong>of</strong> which arealso translated into proteins. In a larger genetic context,expression refers to <strong>the</strong> phenotypic manifestation<strong>of</strong> an allele.extranuclear inheritance: inheritance from DNAsources external to <strong>the</strong> chromosomes found in <strong>the</strong>nucleus. Extranuclear inheritance can derive frommitochondrial DNA or chloroplast DNA. O<strong>the</strong>r termsused for this phenomenon are maternal inheritance(for mitochondrial inheritance), cytoplasmic inheritance,or extrachromosomal inheritance.gamete: a sexual reproductive cell. Gametes arehaploid, having a single (N) complement <strong>of</strong> geneticmaterial. In humans, <strong>the</strong> gametes are ova <strong>and</strong> sperm.g e n e : <strong>the</strong> basic unit <strong>of</strong> heredity, in terms <strong>of</strong> function<strong>and</strong> in <strong>the</strong> physical sense. A gene is a region <strong>of</strong> DNAthat is transcribed (into RNA) plus <strong>the</strong> regulatory DNAsequences necessary for transcription. (Not all genesencode protein. For example, in <strong>the</strong> genes for ribosomalRNA, <strong>the</strong> transcript is <strong>the</strong> functional product.)genome: <strong>the</strong> entire complement <strong>of</strong> genetic material.The Human Genome Project defines <strong>the</strong> humangenome as a single haploid set <strong>of</strong> nuclear chromosomes,plus <strong>the</strong> mitochondrial genome.g e n o t y p e :<strong>the</strong> genetic make-up <strong>of</strong> a cell or organism.Genotype can be contrasted with <strong>the</strong> phenotype (<strong>the</strong>detectable attributes). Genotype also can refer to <strong>the</strong>p a rticular allelic make-up for a given gene.haploid: a cell (or organism) having only one chromosomeset (N). (See gamete <strong>and</strong> diploid.)HD: abbreviation for <strong>the</strong> human genetic disorderHuntington disease. HD is a disorder <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nervoussystem, usually with adult onset <strong>of</strong> symptoms. Thedisorder displays genetic anticipation as a result <strong>of</strong>unstable trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> mutant gene.The disorder is fatal.heritable: capable <strong>of</strong> being transmitted to <strong>of</strong>fspring.heterodisomy: a particular example <strong>of</strong> uniparentaldisomy (q.v.) in which <strong>the</strong> two chromosomes arenonidentical homologs. (See also isodisomy.)heterozygous: <strong>the</strong> condition <strong>of</strong> having two differentalleles at a particular locus on a pair <strong>of</strong> chromosomes(homologs).HGP: <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project; a large, collaborative,scientific research effort funded in <strong>the</strong> UnitedStates by <strong>the</strong> U.S. Department <strong>of</strong> Energy (DOE) <strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong> National Institutes <strong>of</strong> Health (NIH).homolog: one <strong>of</strong> a pair <strong>of</strong> chromosomes that containequivalent genetic information. In germ l i n ecells, homologs pair with one ano<strong>the</strong>r during meiosis.One homolog is derived from <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r <strong>and</strong>one from <strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>r.homozygous: <strong>the</strong> condition <strong>of</strong> having identical allelesfor a particular gene at a given locus in a chromosomepair.hypo<strong>the</strong>sis: a testable idea or explanation proposedin response to previous knowledge <strong>and</strong> specificobservations.imprinting: (in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> genetics) a processthrough which a gene becomes marked chemicallyin a manner that reflects <strong>the</strong> sex <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> parent transmitting<strong>the</strong> gene. Imprinting alters <strong>the</strong> function <strong>of</strong> aparticular allele such that an allele inherited from <strong>the</strong>male parent behaves differently from an allele that isinherited from <strong>the</strong> female parent. Not all genes areimprinted. The exact mechanism for imprinting isnot known.56©1997 by BSCS.


Glossaryinformatics: <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> information processing.In <strong>the</strong> field <strong>of</strong> biology, informatics generally refers to<strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> genetic sequence data.isodisomy: a particular example <strong>of</strong> uniparental disomy(q.v.) in which <strong>the</strong> two chromosomes are identical.(See also heterodisomy.)karyotype: <strong>the</strong> entire set <strong>of</strong> chromosomes <strong>of</strong> anindividual cell made visible by staining <strong>and</strong>microscopy <strong>and</strong> arranged by size <strong>and</strong> chromosomenumber.l i n kage analysis: a laboratory technique that determines<strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> a gene <strong>of</strong> interest by <strong>the</strong> presence<strong>of</strong> identifiable markers located close to <strong>the</strong> gene.linked genes: genes located on <strong>the</strong> same chromosome.Genes that are close toge<strong>the</strong>r—tightlylinked—are less likely to be separated during recombination.locus: <strong>the</strong> location <strong>of</strong> a gene on a chromosome.marker: (in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> genetics) an identifiableallele that expresses a known phenotype or a moleculartag (such as a DNA or RNA fragment) to signal<strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> an allele or chromosomal location <strong>of</strong>interest. DNA or RNA fragments also are used to bindto, <strong>and</strong> thus identify, a particular genetic sequence ina nucleic acid fragment.meiosis: a specialized process <strong>of</strong> cell division thatreduces <strong>the</strong> chromosome number to <strong>the</strong> single(haploid) complement (N). Meiosis takes place ingermline cells. Meiosis produces 4 haploid daughtercells from one diploid cell, involving one round <strong>of</strong>DNA replication.Mendelian genetics: <strong>the</strong> fundamental concepts <strong>of</strong>genetic transmission put forward by Mendel <strong>and</strong>exp<strong>and</strong>ed to include knowledge <strong>of</strong> genetic linkage<strong>and</strong> sex chromosomes.methylation: a chemical process that adds a methylgroup to a molecule; this process can be carried outin living systems by enzymes called methylases. DNAcan be methylated at specific sites, a process thatmay regulate gene expression. Methylation has beensuggested as a possible mechanism <strong>of</strong> genomicimprinting. Methylation is involved in X-chromosomeinactivation.mitochondrial DNA: <strong>the</strong> DNA found in mitochondria,<strong>the</strong> organelles <strong>of</strong> eukaryotic cells that are importantin energy-related reactions. Mitochondrial DNAcan replicate independently <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genomic DNA foundin <strong>the</strong> nucleus, <strong>and</strong> cells have many mitochondria. Inhumans, mitochondria are transmitted matern a l l y.mRNA: messenger RNA, <strong>the</strong> fully processed form <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> transcript copied from <strong>the</strong> DNA sequence <strong>of</strong> agene <strong>and</strong> used to direct protein syn<strong>the</strong>sis.multifactorial inheritance: inheritance in which<strong>the</strong> phenotype results from combined action <strong>of</strong>more than one gene (polygenes) with additionalenvironmental influences.mutation: a physical change in genetic material,such as a base-pair substitution or deletion in DNA.Chromosome breaks or rearrangements involvelarge-scale mutations. If <strong>the</strong> mutation occurs in body(somatic) cells, <strong>the</strong> results affect only <strong>the</strong> individualbearing those cells; if <strong>the</strong> mutation is in germlinecells, <strong>the</strong> change can be transmitted to <strong>of</strong>fspring.n o n d i s j u n c t i o n :improper separation <strong>of</strong> homologsor sister chromatids during meiosis or mitosis. Duringmeiosis, this process can result in a diploid conditionfor a particular chromosome in one gamete <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>absence <strong>of</strong> that chromosome in ano<strong>the</strong>r gamete.nontraditional inheritance: an informal term thatrefers to new concepts <strong>of</strong> inheritance that describeprocesses that were not traditionally understood ortaught in Mendelian genetics. For example, imprintingis a process not explained by traditional Mendelianc o n c e p t s .ovum: a female gamete, commonly called an “egg.”PCR: polymerase chain reaction. A laboratory techniquethat exploits DNA polymerases (enzymes tha<strong>the</strong>lp replicate DNA) derived from bacteria that live athigh altitudes. The technique permits <strong>the</strong> in vitroproduction <strong>of</strong> large amounts <strong>of</strong> DNA copied from avery small amount <strong>of</strong> sample DNA.penetrance: <strong>the</strong> proportion <strong>of</strong> individuals with agiven genotype who express any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phenotypicfeatures <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trait. Incomplete penetrance refers to<strong>the</strong> situation in which less than 100 percent <strong>of</strong> individualswith a given genotype express <strong>the</strong> associatedphenotype.57©1997 by BSCS.


Glossaryphenotype: <strong>the</strong> externally or internally detectablecharacteristics <strong>of</strong> an organism that represent <strong>the</strong>influences <strong>of</strong> environment <strong>and</strong> genetic information(<strong>the</strong> genotype).polygenic: a condition that results from <strong>the</strong> interaction<strong>of</strong> several genes, each <strong>of</strong> which contributes asmall effect to <strong>the</strong> trait in question.polymerase chain reaction: see PCR.polymorphic: literally, having more than one form,such as different lengths for restriction fragments <strong>of</strong>DNA, or <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> two or more genetically distincttypes in a population.probe: (in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> molecular genetics) a substancelabeled with radioactivity or a fluorescingcompound that is used to identify a gene, transcript,or protein through a binding reaction.prokaryote: a colonial or single-celled organismwhose cells lack a membrane-bound nucleus. Aprokaryote has a relatively simple cell structure withoutorganelles such as mitochondria or chloroplasts.Bacteria are prokaryotes.p rudential decision-making: <strong>the</strong> ethical term thatrefers to making a choice through a disciplined analysisinvolving well-reasoned discourse, a consideration<strong>of</strong> various aspects <strong>of</strong> an issue, <strong>and</strong> a weighing <strong>of</strong> risk.recessive: a pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance in which <strong>the</strong> phenotypiceffects <strong>of</strong> an allele are masked in heterozygoteswhen one <strong>of</strong> certain o<strong>the</strong>r alleles is present. Atrait expressed by recessive inheritance is termed arecessive trait. A recessive trait is expressed only inhomozygotes.relevant: (in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> science) <strong>the</strong> condition<strong>of</strong> relating to or addressing a particular idea. Relevantevidence is evidence that is useful to support orcontradict an explanation.restriction endonuclease recognition site: a specificDNA sequence that is recognized <strong>and</strong> cut(digested) by specific members <strong>of</strong> a class <strong>of</strong> bacterialenzymes known as restriction enzymes. This processsupplies a naturally occurring immune function forbacteria <strong>and</strong> is exploited in <strong>the</strong> laboratory to makepossible cloning <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r molecular techniquesinvolving specifically sized DNA fragments.restriction fragment length polymorphisms(RFLPs): <strong>the</strong> small differences in <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> DNAfragments produced through cutting with restrictionenzymes (enzymes that cut DNA at specificsequences). Genetic variation between individuals isreflected in small differences in <strong>the</strong> length <strong>of</strong> DNAbetween <strong>the</strong> sites recognized by restriction enzymes,thus producing RFLPs. These differences can beexploited to map <strong>the</strong> location <strong>of</strong> genes.ribosome: structure within cells on which proteinsyn<strong>the</strong>sis occurs. Ribosomes are composed <strong>of</strong> ribosomalRNA (rRNA) <strong>and</strong> proteins.somatic cell: a cell that does not produce gametes.In humans, somatic cells are all cells except <strong>the</strong>germline cells in ovaries <strong>and</strong> testes that will undergomeiosis. A mutation in a somatic cell affects <strong>the</strong> function<strong>of</strong> that cell <strong>and</strong> all body cells derived from it, butit is not passed on to <strong>of</strong>fspring.spermatogenesis: <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> sperm, aprocess that involves meiosis.t h e o ry : (in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> science) an explanation <strong>of</strong>a fundamental principle that has been so thoroughlytested <strong>and</strong> supported by multiple lines <strong>of</strong> evidencethat it is accepted by <strong>the</strong> scientific community.transcription: <strong>the</strong> process through which an RNAmolecule is syn<strong>the</strong>sized by complementary basepairing using DNA as a template. For example, messengerRNA (mRNA) is a product <strong>of</strong> transcription.translation: <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> syn<strong>the</strong>sizing a proteinmolecule. An RNA message (mRNA) directs <strong>the</strong>order <strong>of</strong> amino acids being bonded toge<strong>the</strong>r to forma protein. This process takes place on structuresknown as ribosomes.transposable genetic element: a transposon; thiselement is a small sequence <strong>of</strong> DNA that does notnecessarily remain at a fixed locus within <strong>the</strong> chromosome.A transposon can jump to a new location,exerting its influence on genes near <strong>the</strong> new locus.trinucleotide repeat: a specific sequence <strong>of</strong> threenucleotides (subunits <strong>of</strong> DNA) that is repeated, <strong>of</strong>tenin large numbers, in a continuous stretch <strong>of</strong> DNA inparticular genes. The mutant gene for Huntingtondisease, for instance, contains <strong>the</strong> trinucleotide CAGrepeated many times. When <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> repeats58©1997 by BSCS.


Glossarypasses a threshold number (around 40 in Huntingtondisease) <strong>the</strong> stretch <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeatsbecomes unstable <strong>and</strong> may increase (or, rarely,decrease) between one generation <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> next.Large numbers <strong>of</strong> repeats result in disease. Trinucleotiderepeats are part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> biological explanationfor genetic anticipation (q.v.).uniparental disomy: a relatively rare geneticprocess through which both copies <strong>of</strong> a particularchromosome (both homologs) are derived from <strong>the</strong>same parent.validity: (in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> science) <strong>the</strong> condition <strong>of</strong>having met scientific criteria, such as being supportedby credible evidence, being built on correctpremises, <strong>and</strong> showing sound reasoning.variable expressivity: <strong>the</strong> range <strong>of</strong> phenotypiceffects in individuals with a given genotype. (Notethat small differences in <strong>the</strong> genotype may be presentbut not obvious.)X-linked trait: a pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance in which <strong>the</strong>allele for <strong>the</strong> trait in question is present on <strong>the</strong> Xchromosome. Males have only one X chromosome,inherited from <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r. For this reason, X-linkedtraits cannot be passed from fa<strong>the</strong>r to son.zygote: <strong>the</strong> cell that results from <strong>the</strong> fusion <strong>of</strong> amale <strong>and</strong> a female gamete. A fertilized ovum (eggcell) is a zygote.59©1997 by BSCS.


References <strong>and</strong>Related LiteratureAmerican Association for <strong>the</strong> Advancement <strong>of</strong>Science (AAAS). (1989). Science for All Americans.Washington, D.C.: American Association for <strong>the</strong>Advancement <strong>of</strong> Science.American Association for <strong>the</strong> Advancement <strong>of</strong>Science (AAAS). (1993). Benchmarks for ScienceLiteracy. New York: Oxford University Press.American Society <strong>of</strong> Human <strong>Genetics</strong> (ASHG) <strong>and</strong>American College <strong>of</strong> Medical <strong>Genetics</strong> (ACMG).(1995). Points to consider: Ethical, legal, <strong>and</strong> psychologicalimplications <strong>of</strong> genetic testing in children<strong>and</strong> adolescents. American Journal <strong>of</strong>Human <strong>Genetics</strong> 57:1233-1241.Ashley, C.T. <strong>and</strong> S.T. Warren. (1995). Trinucleotiderepeat expansion <strong>and</strong> human disease. AnnualReview <strong>of</strong> <strong>Genetics</strong> 29:703-728.Barlow, D.P. (1993). Methylation <strong>and</strong> imprinting:From host defense to gene regulation? Science260(5106):309-310.Barlow, D.P. (1994). Imprinting: A gamete’s point <strong>of</strong>view. Trends in <strong>Genetics</strong> 10(6):194-199.B a rtolomei, M.S. (1994). The search for imprintedgenes. News <strong>and</strong> Views, Nature <strong>Genetics</strong> 6 ( 3 ) : 2 2 0 - 2 2 1 .Bell, J. (1947). Dystrophia myotonia <strong>and</strong> allied disease,in The Tr e a s u ry <strong>of</strong> Human <strong>Inheritance</strong> IV. Nerv o u sDiseases <strong>and</strong> Muscular Dystrophies. Vol. 5, p. 343.Brown, J.R., et al. (1996). A defect in nurturing inmice lacking <strong>the</strong> immediate early gene fosB. Cell86(2):297-309.Bult, C.J., et al. (1996). Complete genome sequence<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> methanogenic archaeon, Methanococcusjannaschii. Science 273(5278):1058-1073.Buxton, J., et al. (1992). Detection <strong>of</strong> an unstablefragment <strong>of</strong> DNA specific to individuals withmyotonic dystrophy. Nature 355(6360):547-548.Cann, R.L., M. Stoneking, <strong>and</strong> A.C. Wilson. (1987).Mitochondrial DNA <strong>and</strong> human evolution. Nature325(6099):31-36.Chakraborty, R., et al. (1996). Segregation distortion<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> CTG repeats at <strong>the</strong> myotonic dystrophylocus. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Human <strong>Genetics</strong>59(1):109-118.Collins, F.S. (1995). Ahead <strong>of</strong> schedule <strong>and</strong> underbudget: The Genome Project passes its fifth birthday.Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 92:10821-10823.Cooper, N.G. (ed.) (1994). The Human GenomeProject: Deciphering <strong>the</strong> Blueprint <strong>of</strong> Heredity. MillValley, CA: University Science Books.61


References <strong>and</strong> Related LiteratureDriscoll, D.J. (1993). Genomic imprinting <strong>and</strong>human disease. International Pediatrics 8(1):6-13.Edelson, E. (1991). Tracing human linages. Mosaic2(3):56-63.Futuyma, D.J. (1986). Evolutionary Biology.Sunderl<strong>and</strong>, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc.G<strong>of</strong>feau, A., et al. (1996). Life with 6000 genes.Science 274(5287):546-567.Gossman, L.I. (1990). Invited editorial: MitochondrialDNA in sickness <strong>and</strong> in health. American Journal <strong>of</strong>Human <strong>Genetics</strong> 4 6 ( 3 ) : 4 1 5 - 4 1 7 .Griffiths, A.J.F., J.H. Miller, D.T. Suzuki, R.C.Lewontin, <strong>and</strong> W.M. Gelbart. (1993). AnIntroduction to Genetic Analysis, 5th edition. NewYork: W.H. Freeman <strong>and</strong> Company.Hall, J.G. (1990). Genomic imprinting: Review <strong>and</strong>relevance to human disease. American Journal <strong>of</strong>Human <strong>Genetics</strong> 46(5):857-873.Hall, J.G. (1992). Genomic imprinting <strong>and</strong> its clinicalimplications. New Engl<strong>and</strong> Journal <strong>of</strong> Medicine326(12):827-829.Hamshere, M.G. <strong>and</strong> J.D. Brook. (1996). Myotonicdystrophy, knockouts, warts <strong>and</strong> all. Trends in<strong>Genetics</strong> 12(9):332-334.Harley, H.G., et al. (1992). Expansion <strong>of</strong> an unstableDNA region <strong>and</strong> phenotypic variation in myotonicdystrophy. Nature 355(6360):345-346.Harpending, H. (1994). Gene frequencies, DNAsequences, <strong>and</strong> human origins. Perspectives inBiology <strong>and</strong> Medicine 37(3):384-394.Harris, S., C. Moncrieff, <strong>and</strong> K. Johnson. (1996).Myotonic dystrophy: will <strong>the</strong> real gene please stepforward! Human Molecular <strong>Genetics</strong> 5:1417-1423.Holton, G. (1993). Science <strong>and</strong> Anti-science.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Jorde, L.B., J.C. Carey, <strong>and</strong> R.L. White. (1995). M e d i c a lG e n e t i c s. St. Louis, MO: Mosby-Year Book, Inc.King, R.C. <strong>and</strong> W.D. Stansfield. (1990). A Dictionary<strong>of</strong> <strong>Genetics</strong>, 4th Edition. New York: OxfordUniversity Press, Inc.Lyon, M.F. (1993). Epigenetic inheritance in mammals.Trends in <strong>Genetics</strong> 9(4):123-128.MacDonald, M.E., et al. (1993). Capturing a CAGeykiller. Genome Analysis Volume 7: GenomeRearrangement <strong>and</strong> Stability, 25-41. Cold SpringHarbor, NY: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press.Mange, E.J. <strong>and</strong> A.P. Mange. (1994). Basic Human<strong>Genetics</strong>. Sunderl<strong>and</strong>, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc.Martin, J.B. (1993). Molecular genetics <strong>of</strong> neurologicaldiseases. Science 262(5134):674-676.Mayr, E. (1991). One Long Argument: CharlesDarwin <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Genesis <strong>of</strong> Modern EvolutionaryThought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.McBride, G. (1991). Nontraditional inheritance—II:The clinical implications. Mosaic 22(3):12-25.McInerney, J.D. <strong>and</strong> R. Moore. (1993). Voting in science:Raise your h<strong>and</strong> if you want humans to have48 chromosomes (editorial). The American BiologyTeacher 55(3):132-133.McInnis, M.G. (1996). Invited editorial. Anticipation:An old idea in new genes. American Journal <strong>of</strong>Human <strong>Genetics</strong> 59:973-979.McKusick, V.A. (1992). Mendelian <strong>Inheritance</strong> inMan: Catalogs <strong>of</strong> Autosomal Dominant, AutosomalRecessive, <strong>and</strong> X-linked Phenotypes, 10th Edition.Vol. 1 & 2. Baltimore <strong>and</strong> London: The JohnsHopkins University Press.Miki, Y., et al. (1994). A strong c<strong>and</strong>idate for <strong>the</strong>breast <strong>and</strong> ovarian cancer susceptibility geneBRCA1. Science 266(5182):66-71.Monckton, D.G. <strong>and</strong> C.T. Caskey. (1995).Anticipation <strong>and</strong> repeat expansion in myotonic dystrophy.Circulation 91:513-520.Moore, J.A. (1994). Science as a Way <strong>of</strong> Knowing:The Foundations <strong>of</strong> Modern Biology. Cambridge,MA: Harvard University Press.Morell, V. (1993). The puzzle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> triple repeats.Science 260(5113):1422-1423.Mott, F.W. (1911). A lecture on heredity <strong>and</strong> insanity.Lancet 1:1251-1259.Nance, M.A. (1996). Invited editorial: Huntington62©1997 by BSCS.


References <strong>and</strong> Related Literaturedisease—Ano<strong>the</strong>r chapter rewritten. AmericanJournal <strong>of</strong> Human <strong>Genetics</strong> 59(1):1-6.National Research Council (NRC). (1994). NationalScience <strong>Education</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ards Draft Document(Life Science St<strong>and</strong>ards K-12). Washington, D.C.:National Research Council.National Research Council (NRC). (1996). NationalScience <strong>Education</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ards. Washington, D.C.:National Academy Press.Nelson, D. L. (1993). Six human genetic disordersinvolving mutant trinucleotide repeats. GenomeAnalysis Volume 7: Genome Rearrangement <strong>and</strong>Stability (ed. Kay E. Davies <strong>and</strong> Stephen T. Warren).Cold Spring Harbor, NY: Cold Spring HarborLaboratory Press.Partridge, E. (1994). Usage <strong>and</strong> Abusage: A GuideTo Good English, J. Whitcut (Revision ed.). London,Engl<strong>and</strong>: Penguin Books Ltd.Penrose, L. (1947). The problem <strong>of</strong> anticipation inpedigrees <strong>of</strong> dystrophia myotonica. AnnalsEugenics 14:125-132.Peterson, K. <strong>and</strong> C. Sapienza. (1993). Imprinting <strong>the</strong>genome: Imprinted genes, imprinting genes, <strong>and</strong> ahypo<strong>the</strong>sis for <strong>the</strong>ir interaction. Annual Review <strong>of</strong>G e n e t i c s, 27:7-31. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews Inc.Portin, P. (1993). The concept <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene: Shorthistory <strong>and</strong> present status. The Quarterly Review <strong>of</strong>Biology 68(2):173-223.Pyke, D. (1996). Peter Medawar <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> English language.Perspectives in Biology <strong>and</strong> Medicine39(4):555-568.Ranen, et al. (1995). Anticipation <strong>and</strong> instability <strong>of</strong>IT-15 (CAG)N repeats in parent-<strong>of</strong>fspring pairs withHuntington disease. The American Journal <strong>of</strong>Human <strong>Genetics</strong> 57(3):593-602.Rastan, S. <strong>and</strong> B.M. Cattanach. (1983). Interactionbetween <strong>the</strong> XCE locus <strong>and</strong> imprinting <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paternalX chromosome in mouse yolk-sac endoderm.Nature 303(5918):635-637.Rennie, J. (1993). DNA’s new twists. S c i e n t i f i cA m e r i c a n2 6 9 ( 3 ) : 1 2 2 - 1 3 2 .Rosenberg, A. (1985). The Structure <strong>of</strong> BiologicalS c i e n c e. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Rossiter, B.J.F. <strong>and</strong> C.T. Caskey. (1995). Humangenetic predisposition to disease. In: MolecularBiology <strong>and</strong> Biotechnology. R.A. Meyers (ed.). NewYork, NY: VCH Publishers, Inc.Rubinsztein, D.C., et al. (1996). Phenotypic characterization<strong>of</strong> individuals with 30-40 CAG repeats in<strong>the</strong> Huntington disease (HD) gene reveals HDcases with 36 repeats <strong>and</strong> apparently normal elderlyindividuals with 36-39 repeats. American Journal <strong>of</strong>Human <strong>Genetics</strong> 59(1):16-22.Scriver, C.R., et al. (1995). The Metabolic <strong>and</strong>Molecular Bases <strong>of</strong> Inherited Disease Vol. 1. NewYork: McGraw-Hill, Inc.Shreeve, J. (1995). Chapter 3, The Ne<strong>and</strong>ertalEnigma. New York: William Morrow <strong>and</strong> Company.Singer, M. <strong>and</strong> P. Berg. (1991). Genes & Genomes: AChanging Perspective. Mill Valley, CA: UniversityScience Books.Smeets, D.E., et al. (1992). Prader-Willi syndrome <strong>and</strong>Angelman syndrome in cousins from a family with atranslocation between chromosomes 6 <strong>and</strong> 15. N e wEngl<strong>and</strong> Journal <strong>of</strong> Medicine 3 2 6 ( 1 2 ) : 8 0 7 - 8 1 1 .Social Science <strong>Education</strong> Consortium (SSEC) <strong>and</strong>Biological Sciences Curriculum Study (BSCS).(1996). Teaching about <strong>the</strong> History <strong>and</strong> Nature <strong>of</strong>Science <strong>and</strong> Technology. Boulder, CO: SocialScience <strong>Education</strong> Consortium.Tamarin, R.H. (1993). Principles <strong>of</strong> <strong>Genetics</strong>, 4<strong>the</strong>dition. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown Publishers.Tarleton J. (1993/94). New inheritance patterns:New counseling dilemmas. Perspectives in GeneticCounseling 15(4):1, 8.Thorne, A.G. <strong>and</strong> M.H. Wolp<strong>of</strong>f. (1992). The multiregionalevolution <strong>of</strong> humans. Scientific American266(4):76-83.Trowbridge, L.W. <strong>and</strong> R.W. Bybee. (1990). BecomingA Secondary Science Teacher. Columbus, OH:Merrill Publishing Company.U.S. Department <strong>of</strong> Health <strong>and</strong> Human Services <strong>and</strong>U.S. Department <strong>of</strong> Energy. (1990). U n d e r s t a n d i n gOur Genetic <strong>Inheritance</strong>. The U.S. Human Genome63©1997 by BSCS.


ClassroomActivitiesThis component <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module providessix classroom activities. Material is divided into■ teacher pages, containing suggestions to teach eachactivity <strong>and</strong> including annotated student pages;■ special copymasters, containing material needed for h<strong>and</strong>outsor to make overhead transparencies;■ student pages, containing procedural material to copyfor <strong>the</strong> students’ use.Please see Section I, pages 5-7 <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h e r s,for a description <strong>of</strong> how to use <strong>the</strong> activities.


TEACHER PAGES FORCLASSROOM ACTIVITIESPlease note that we provide a summary<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science(NMS) concepts <strong>and</strong> genetics conceptsused in each activity.


Engage ActivityScientific InvestigationNMS CONCEPTS■ Science depends on data derived from carefulobservation <strong>and</strong> experimentation.■Scientifically useful observations <strong>and</strong> experimentsare those that o<strong>the</strong>rs can repeat.■ The measurements <strong>and</strong> descriptions that scientistsuse must be universal so that one scientistcan underst<strong>and</strong> or repeat <strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs.■ These requirements help scientists to communicateeffectively, to verify work done by o<strong>the</strong>rs,<strong>and</strong> to detect errors in <strong>the</strong>ir own work <strong>and</strong> in <strong>the</strong>work <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs.GENETICS CONCEPTSThis activity does not address genetics concepts.FOCUSThis brief <strong>and</strong> easy activity helps students to thinkabout one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hallmarks <strong>of</strong> scientific investigation:careful observation, carefully recorded. Theactivity provides a mechanism to introduce <strong>the</strong> generaltopic <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science.U n l i ke <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r activities <strong>of</strong> this module, <strong>the</strong>Engage Activity does n o t require any knowledge <strong>of</strong>genetics. For this reason, <strong>the</strong> activity can be used at<strong>the</strong> start <strong>of</strong> a biology course, prior to a study <strong>of</strong>genetics, to introduce how science works. Altern a-t i v e l y, <strong>the</strong> Engage Activity can introduce <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>ractivities <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module.OBJECTIVESAs students complete this activity, <strong>the</strong>y should1. underst<strong>and</strong> that careful observation, measurement,<strong>and</strong> recording <strong>of</strong> data are essential to science;2. make <strong>the</strong>ir own observations, measurements, <strong>and</strong>recordings;3. use <strong>the</strong>ir own observations, measurements, <strong>and</strong>recordings, <strong>and</strong> those <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r students, to compare<strong>the</strong> utility <strong>of</strong> different types <strong>of</strong> data;4. experience <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized measurement;<strong>and</strong>5. discuss <strong>the</strong> difference between observation <strong>and</strong>inference.ESTIMATED TIME30 minutesPREPARATION AND MATERIALSYou will need to provide <strong>the</strong> following for each team<strong>of</strong> 3 students:■ metric ruler■ 20-cm piece <strong>of</strong> string69


Engage Activity ■Scientific Investigation■unshelled peanuts to fill one small bowl (removeall discolored, cracked, or broken peanuts)■ balance■ magnifying glassINTRODUCTIONAlthough students <strong>of</strong>ten read about <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong>science at <strong>the</strong> start <strong>of</strong> a science course, few studentsinternalize <strong>and</strong> use that knowledge without directexperience. The Engage Activity does not addresscausal explanations <strong>of</strong> natural processes, but it doesshow a number <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fundamental methods <strong>of</strong> science,including <strong>the</strong> need for careful observation <strong>and</strong>precise note-taking if scientific work is to be communicatedto o<strong>the</strong>r scientists <strong>and</strong> repeated (see SectionIII <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers). St<strong>and</strong>ardizedmeasurements <strong>and</strong> terminology also help with thisprocess. This activity shows some characteristics <strong>of</strong>careful observation <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> power <strong>of</strong> accuratelyrecorded evidence.STRATEGIES FOR TEACHING THE ACTIVITYThe Engage Activity should be simple <strong>and</strong> fun,although its message is very powerful. Keep a quickpace. You may want to compare <strong>the</strong> issues addressedin Questions 5 <strong>and</strong> 6 <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Analysis with <strong>the</strong> sixquestions posed in Section III <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Overview forTeachers.Annotated Student MaterialSeparate student pages contain <strong>the</strong> material shown in bold typeface.Here, we provide an annotated version <strong>of</strong> student materials to help you conduct <strong>the</strong> activity.PROCEDUREDivide <strong>the</strong> class into teams <strong>of</strong> 3 <strong>and</strong> provide eachteam with a set <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> materials. Students within ateam share materials, but students work individually. Each student observes <strong>the</strong> peanut he or shec h o o s e s .1. Share materials with your team, but workindividually. Select a peanut <strong>and</strong> carefullyobserve it to determine <strong>the</strong> distinguishingcharacteristics that identify this particularpeanut. Record your observations on apiece <strong>of</strong> paper. Do not mark or crack <strong>the</strong>peanut. You may use <strong>the</strong> equipment providedto help you with your observations.Allow no more than 5 minutes for this step.2. Return each team member’s peanut to <strong>the</strong>bowl. Mix up <strong>the</strong> peanuts.3. Use your notes to find your peanut again.Allow no more than 2 minutes for this step.4. Raise your h<strong>and</strong> if you are absolutely certainyou have found your own peanut.Have one student calculate <strong>the</strong> percentage <strong>of</strong> studentswho raised <strong>the</strong>ir h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> write this numberon <strong>the</strong> chalkboard.5. What evidence did you use to locate yourpeanut <strong>and</strong> distinguish it from <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs in<strong>the</strong> bowl?Sample student responses:■ I measured it.■ I described its shape.■ I drew it.■ I traced it.■ I described how it smelled or looked.■ I found its mass.6. E xchange your team’s bowl <strong>of</strong> peanuts <strong>and</strong>o b s e rvation notes with those from ano<strong>the</strong>rteam. Now work individually with one set <strong>of</strong>o b s e rvations from ano<strong>the</strong>r student <strong>and</strong> try t<strong>of</strong>ind <strong>the</strong> particular peanut it describes.Allow no more than 5 minutes for students to identify<strong>the</strong> new peanuts.7. Raise your h<strong>and</strong> if you are absolutely cert a i nthat you have found <strong>the</strong> correct peanut.Again, have a student calculate <strong>the</strong> percentage <strong>and</strong>record this number on <strong>the</strong> chalkboard.70©1997 by BSCS.


Scientific Investigation ■ Engage ActivityANALYSIS1. What observations were most valuable infinding a specific peanut?Students should recognize that <strong>the</strong> most helpful informationwas that which o<strong>the</strong>rs could easily underst<strong>and</strong><strong>and</strong> apply. This information is objective <strong>and</strong> has universal,transferable value—for example, length, circumference,width measured in centimeters, or mass measuredin grams. Quantitative information is helpful ifaccurate <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized. The least helpful inform a-tion likely was qualitative, that is, information whoseinterpretation is more subjective—for example, “It’spretty big; it smelled good; it has an interesting shape.”2. What role did your notes or <strong>the</strong> notes <strong>of</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>rstudent play in helping you to locate a particularpeanut? (If your memory was a betterguide, what does that say about your notes?)If students’ memories were more helpful than <strong>the</strong>irnotes, <strong>the</strong>ir observations likely did not produce quantitativedata. Notes preserve <strong>the</strong> accuracy <strong>of</strong> observ a-tions <strong>and</strong> make <strong>the</strong>m transferable. This issue providesan opportunity to emphasize <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong>careful note taking <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> communication in science.3. People <strong>of</strong>ten confuse o b s e r v a t i o n s w i t hi n f e r e n c e s. O b s e r v a t i o n s are collectedusing your senses, ei<strong>the</strong>r directly or exp<strong>and</strong>edby technology using devices such asmicroscopes, X-ray machines, or microwavesensors on satellites. Inferences are ideas orconclusions based on what you observe oralready know. Using this distinction, which<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> following statements are observations<strong>and</strong> which are inferences?■ If this peanut is roasted, <strong>the</strong> seeds will notgerminate.-inference■ The shell has a rough surface.-observation■ The shell is uniformly colored.-observation■ The peanuts came from a plant.-inference■ The shell has two lobes <strong>and</strong> is smaller indiameter between <strong>the</strong>m.-observation■ This peanut will taste good.-inference■ Squirrels would eat <strong>the</strong>se peanuts.-inference■ The surface markings on <strong>the</strong> shell are inrows, running lengthwise.-observation4. Now, look at your notes <strong>and</strong> label any inferencesthat you included.Emphasize that both observation <strong>and</strong> inference areimportant in science, but that <strong>the</strong>se two processesshould not be confused. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong>an inference generally increases with <strong>the</strong> amount <strong>of</strong>supporting data. For example, <strong>the</strong> inference that agiven trait is genetic becomes more valid if <strong>the</strong> traitalso occurs in many <strong>of</strong>fspring.Sometimes, however, even many repeated observ a-tions do not confirm an inference beyond question,<strong>and</strong> we must, <strong>the</strong>refore, always be cautious aboutour inferences. For example, we infer that dogs wag<strong>the</strong>ir tails because <strong>the</strong>y are happy, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>re aremany thous<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> observations that support thisinference. Without direct (<strong>and</strong> unlikely) confirm a-tion from dogs, however, our inference remains ass u c h .5. What counts as evidence in science?At this early stage, you may want to use this questionprimarily to stimulate thinking about what isrequired <strong>of</strong> scientific evidence. Students already maysee that careful observation <strong>and</strong> measurement, universalunits <strong>of</strong> measure, reliance on data, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ability to repeat an investigation with <strong>the</strong> sameresults are a few <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> things that characterize a scientificapproach to asking questions <strong>and</strong> seekinganswers about <strong>the</strong> natural world.6. What makes science objective? Why is objectivityimportant?Use <strong>the</strong> question to poll student opinion, ra<strong>the</strong>rthan looking for specific answers. Objectivity in sciencedepends on rigorous methods, such as <strong>the</strong>need for credible evidence. Objectivity helps build<strong>the</strong> durability <strong>of</strong> scientific knowledge by connectingit closely to reality ra<strong>the</strong>r than to unsubstantiatedo p i n i o n .71©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong>Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsNMS CONCEPTSNew work builds on old work; <strong>the</strong> scientific communityrequires supporting, credible evidence to accept anexplanation; credibility depends on <strong>the</strong> rigor <strong>of</strong> investigation(precision <strong>and</strong> replicability); science reflects<strong>the</strong> culture in which it takes place (minor concept).GENETICS CONCEPTSThe activity reviews fundamental (Mendelian) conceptstraditionally taught in genetics <strong>and</strong> in basicmolecular genetics. (See Section V <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Overviewfor Teachers.) These concepts are listed below.■ parental source for inheritance■ chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> inheritance■ segregation■ independent assortment■ DNA as <strong>the</strong> genetic material■ sex determination■ genetic code■ linkage■ mutationFOCUSStudents combine a brief review <strong>of</strong> basic geneticswith an initial exploration <strong>of</strong> how scientists use evidenceto support explanations <strong>of</strong> genetic phenomena.The activity directly addresses <strong>the</strong> first four <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>six questions raised in <strong>the</strong> discussion <strong>of</strong> methods <strong>of</strong>science in <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers (Section III).Students also indirectly consider <strong>the</strong> cultural context<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> scientific discovery.Students arrange milestone explanations in a conceptualsequence to see that ideas in genetics are interconnected.They <strong>the</strong>n compare <strong>the</strong>ir sequences to <strong>the</strong>actual history <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se genetics milestones. Studentsalso evaluate <strong>the</strong> relative worth <strong>of</strong> different types <strong>of</strong>evidence on two levels: credibility <strong>and</strong> usefulness.OBJECTIVESAs students complete this activity, <strong>the</strong>y should1. review some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> basic concepts <strong>of</strong> genetics;2. recognize <strong>and</strong> use criteria for determining <strong>the</strong>credibility <strong>of</strong> scientific evidence, including precision,replicability, <strong>and</strong> controlled observation;3. recognize <strong>the</strong> association (relevance) betweencredible evidence <strong>and</strong> a particular scientific explanation(milestone concept);4. recognize that new work builds on old work; <strong>and</strong>5. build an underst<strong>and</strong>ing that genetics has a historybased on accumulated knowledge <strong>and</strong> a strongrecord <strong>of</strong> evidence.ESTIMATED TIMEOne 50-minute class period73


Activity 1 ■St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsPREPARATION AND MATERIALSStudents will work in 8 teams. To save paper, youmay want to laminate <strong>the</strong> Milestone Cards <strong>and</strong> EvidenceCards so that you can reuse <strong>the</strong>m. You willneed to provide <strong>the</strong> following:■ 8 sets <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 1-1, Milestones in Underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>Genetics</strong>, cut apart (1 set per team)■ 1 overhead transparency <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 1-2, HistoricSequence <strong>of</strong> Milestones, or 1 copy for eachstudent■ 1 copy <strong>of</strong> Evidence Cards corresponding to aparticular milestone for each team; <strong>the</strong>re will bea different set <strong>of</strong> cards for each team (see Copymaster1-3)■ chalkboard, flip chart, large sheet <strong>of</strong> paper, oroverhead projector to display <strong>the</strong> milestoneconcepts in Part I■ flip chart or butcher paper for an NMS poster(optional)COPYMASTERS USED IN THIS ACTIVITYCopymaster 1-1Milestones in Underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>Genetics</strong>Copymaster 1-2Historic Sequence <strong>of</strong> MilestonesCopymaster 1-3Evidence CardsINTRODUCTIONThis activity challenges students to explore <strong>the</strong>methods <strong>of</strong> science during a review <strong>of</strong> fundamentalconcepts in genetics. A brief discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> methods<strong>of</strong> science is included here for your convenience.For a more detailed discussion, see SectionIII in <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers.How do we know what we know? This activity dealswith two aspects <strong>of</strong> science that help to answer thatquestion: (1) scientific explanations require credibleevidence, <strong>and</strong> (2) new work builds on old work.The relationship between evidence <strong>and</strong> explanation isbuilt on several assumptions. The evidence must becredible according to scientific criteria such as accuracy<strong>and</strong> repeatability. Authority or fame alone does notestablish validity for a particular hypo<strong>the</strong>sis or foropinions <strong>of</strong>fered as evidence. Recognition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>authority or fame <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> proponent <strong>of</strong> a scientific ideamay attract attention, but this notoriety in itself is notsufficient for <strong>the</strong> scientific community to accept <strong>the</strong>idea unless sufficient credible evidence supports <strong>the</strong>claim. Evidence also must relate to <strong>the</strong> issue in question;even credible evidence is not useful when it doesnot address <strong>the</strong> question under consideration.Ano<strong>the</strong>r step to determine what we know scientificallyis to examine an explanation for <strong>the</strong> soundness<strong>of</strong> its reasoning. A good explanation starts with accurateassumptions, is guided by credible evidence,<strong>and</strong> is built through logical connections. If thisprocess is done well, <strong>the</strong> explanation will have predictivepower. Because many scientific explanationsaim to discover causal relationships, explanationscan be tested according to <strong>the</strong>ir ability to predictspecific outcomes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> proposed causal event.Causality is a related, but not central, concept inActivity 1, <strong>and</strong> you may want to exp<strong>and</strong> your discussionsto include it. Question 1 <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Analysis is agood place to help students underst<strong>and</strong> what isimportant about causality <strong>and</strong> how to establish it. Adescription <strong>of</strong> causality is included in Section III <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers.Finally, science is embedded in cultural history. Scienceproceeds as a chronological sequence <strong>of</strong> discoveries,but <strong>the</strong> paths it follows largely reflect <strong>the</strong>culture that supports it. Topics <strong>of</strong> concern to scientistsat a particular time reflect many aspects <strong>of</strong> societalvalues. For example, if society views cancer,heart disease, <strong>and</strong> AIDS as important problems,agencies in <strong>the</strong> public <strong>and</strong> private sector will makefunds available for research in those areas. Researchthat attracts attention also depends on <strong>the</strong> currentlevel <strong>of</strong> technology for acquiring, storing, <strong>and</strong> analyzingdata. Sometimes, a creative idea or questioncannot be formulated as a testable hypo<strong>the</strong>sisbecause <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> limitations in technology, resources,or existing information. In <strong>the</strong>se cases, <strong>the</strong> great ideamust wait until history creates <strong>the</strong> right setting for it.STRATEGIES FOR TEACHING THE ACTIVITYThe foregoing aspects <strong>of</strong> science are incorporatedinto <strong>the</strong> tasks <strong>of</strong> this activity. Although <strong>the</strong> NMS conceptsmay not be entirely new to students, <strong>the</strong>se74©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1concepts <strong>of</strong>ten are not presented in a way thatmakes <strong>the</strong>m useful. A student may have a concretedefinition <strong>of</strong> how science works, but may not be ableto apply <strong>the</strong> concept ei<strong>the</strong>r in thinking or action.This activity, like much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module, is designed totake <strong>the</strong> students’ underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong>science that extra step into experience.In Pa rt I <strong>of</strong> this activity, students see how new workbuilds on old work <strong>and</strong> how milestone explanationsattempt to establish a causal relationship. Studentsa rrange <strong>the</strong> milestones into a sequence that make sconnections between ideas, focusing on <strong>the</strong> conceptsra<strong>the</strong>r than on <strong>the</strong> dates <strong>of</strong> discovery. (Knowing<strong>the</strong> dates <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> discoveries is n o t <strong>the</strong> objective.)Ask students to describe <strong>the</strong> basis for <strong>the</strong>sequence <strong>of</strong> milestones <strong>the</strong>y construct—for example,simple to complex or general to specific—<strong>and</strong>to speculate why this sequence might differ from<strong>the</strong> historical sequence. Emphasize that <strong>the</strong> historicalsequence is not necessarily “<strong>the</strong> right answer, ”but do not hesitate to challenge what appear to beconceptual flaws in a student’s sequence. For example,molecular details about mutation <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>genetic code would not precede knowledge thatDNA is <strong>the</strong> genetic material.In Part II, students focus on <strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong> credible evidenceto support a valid scientific explanation. Ineach team, students study four pieces <strong>of</strong> evidence tojudge <strong>the</strong>ir credibility <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir usefulness relative toa particular milestone explanation in genetics. Eachteam has a different milestone. You may want to have<strong>the</strong> class define “credible” in terms <strong>of</strong> evidence. In<strong>the</strong> Annotated Student Material, we suggest questionsat <strong>the</strong> start <strong>of</strong> Part II to stimulate thinking aboutthis topic before <strong>the</strong> teams begin <strong>the</strong>ir work.The questions in <strong>the</strong> Analysis help to make <strong>the</strong>aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> science e x p l i c i t for your students,but do not assume that students will recognize<strong>the</strong>se characteristics without reinforcement. At thistime, students may begin to brainstorm ideas for aposter about <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science. Ifyou use this technique, students will add ideas to thisNMS poster throughout <strong>the</strong> module. You will need away to display <strong>the</strong> collection <strong>of</strong> student ideas aboutscience, perhaps using a large sheet <strong>of</strong> butcher paper.The task <strong>of</strong> adding ideas to this poster should beopen-ended, <strong>and</strong> student responses will vary widely.Note from <strong>the</strong> field test: Teachers found that <strong>the</strong>task <strong>of</strong> putting milestones into a meaningfulsequence drew comments from some students whonormally were nonparticipatory.The following summary <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> this activityshould help you underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> intent <strong>of</strong> each part .ACTIVITY 1 AT A GLANCEPart I:Part II:Analysis:Milestones in Underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>Genetics</strong>In a jigsaw fashion, teams build a meaningful sequence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> milestone explanations.Students compare <strong>the</strong> actual history <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> milestone explanations with <strong>the</strong>ir own sequence<strong>and</strong> think about <strong>the</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> any differences.How Good Is <strong>the</strong> Evidence?Students review <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> credible scientific evidence. Each team evaluates four evidencecards for credibility <strong>and</strong> judges whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> credible evidence supports <strong>the</strong> team’sone milestone explanation in genetics, thus exploring <strong>the</strong> requirements for an explanationto be accepted as scientifically valid.Students reflect on many aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> science. They may begin to summarize<strong>the</strong>se ideas on a poster to be used throughout <strong>the</strong> module.75©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsAnnotated Student MaterialSeparate student pages contain <strong>the</strong> material shown in bold typeface.Here, we provide an annotated version <strong>of</strong> student materials to help you conduct <strong>the</strong> activity.If you make a scientific discovery, will peoplestill rely on it one hundred years later? Scientistscontinue to use <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ories <strong>of</strong> inheritancedescribed by Gregor Mendel (Figure 1-1)—<strong>the</strong>y are remarkably durable after morethan a century. Since <strong>the</strong> rediscovery <strong>of</strong>Mendel’s work in about 1900, biologists havemade great strides in determining <strong>the</strong> mechanisms<strong>of</strong> heredity. Knowledge about geneticshas exp<strong>and</strong>ed in <strong>the</strong> last two decades withtechnical advances in molecular biology <strong>and</strong>,most recently, with <strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HumanGenome Project (HGP). This huge project willi d e n t i fy genetic relationships (maps) <strong>and</strong>chromosomal locations <strong>of</strong> all human genes<strong>and</strong> will attempt to determine <strong>the</strong> DNAsequence for <strong>the</strong> entire genome <strong>of</strong> H o m os a p i e n s. Mapping <strong>and</strong> sequencing will bedone for o<strong>the</strong>r species, too, including selectedbacteria, yeast, a plant, <strong>and</strong> several animals p e c i e s .D i s c o v e ry in <strong>the</strong> HGP or any field <strong>of</strong> scienceoccurs in stages. Similarly, <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong>genetics is much more than a simple record<strong>of</strong> dates, names, <strong>and</strong> discoveries; it is anaccount <strong>of</strong> how our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> inheritance<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene has grown <strong>and</strong> changed.M o d e rn geneticists (Figure 1-2) are “st<strong>and</strong>ingon <strong>the</strong> shoulders <strong>of</strong> giants” who came beforet h e m .PROCEDUREOptional IntroductionYou may want to introduce this activity with a briefpreliminary discussion about how scientific knowledgeis built. An interesting way to start <strong>the</strong> discussionis to ask, “Can you recall a scientific explanationthat was once held to be valid <strong>and</strong> later found to beinaccurate? Describe that explanation <strong>and</strong> how itchanged.”Figure 1-1 Gregor Mendel (1822-1884), a pioneerin <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> inheritance: His explanations werebased on observation <strong>of</strong> traits, use <strong>of</strong> careful records,<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> ma<strong>the</strong>matical analysis <strong>of</strong> his data.If students do not respond, suggest an explanationsuch as <strong>the</strong> geocentric conception <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cosmos.Point out that it appeared sensible to assume that<strong>the</strong> sun, moon, stars, <strong>and</strong> planets revolved around<strong>the</strong> Earth because <strong>the</strong>y appeared to move across <strong>the</strong>sky while <strong>the</strong> Earth felt stationary to <strong>the</strong> observer.The astronomer Copernicus ga<strong>the</strong>red evidencethrough careful observation <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>the</strong>matical calculationsto provide a different explanation: <strong>the</strong> Earth<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r planets orbit <strong>the</strong> sun (a heliocentricview). Although <strong>the</strong> center was perceived differently,<strong>the</strong> idea <strong>of</strong> orbits remained. Ano<strong>the</strong>r example <strong>of</strong> anidea that changed is that <strong>of</strong> a flat Earth. Studentsshould be able to cite evidence (such as Magellan’s76©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1voyage around <strong>the</strong> world 1 <strong>and</strong> photographs fromspace) that refutes this idea.Ano<strong>the</strong>r explanation that changed in light <strong>of</strong> new evidenceis that for <strong>the</strong> cause <strong>of</strong> disease. For years,people thought factors such as bad air, getting caughtin <strong>the</strong> rain, or having evil spirits caused disease.These views gradually changed <strong>and</strong>, in <strong>the</strong> mid-nineteenthcentury, scientists provided convincing pro<strong>of</strong><strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> germ <strong>the</strong>ory as <strong>the</strong> basis for communicabledisease. The central assumption <strong>of</strong> germ <strong>the</strong>ory isthat microorganisms can invade o<strong>the</strong>r organisms <strong>and</strong>cause illness. The French scientist Louis Pasteur tookone large step toward acceptance <strong>of</strong> that explanationwhen he dispelled <strong>the</strong> notion <strong>of</strong> spontaneous generation.Pa s t e u r, in a series <strong>of</strong> experiments using thinne c ked flasks <strong>and</strong> sterile techniques, showed thatorganisms did not grow spontaneously from nonlivingmatter. He also found direct evidence to support<strong>the</strong> germ <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> disease when he identified threetypes <strong>of</strong> microorganisms that were pathogenic fors i l k w o rms, showing <strong>the</strong> causal connection between apathogen <strong>and</strong> a disease. A German scientist, RobertKoch, also supplied evidence for this <strong>the</strong>ory with hisd i s c o v e ry <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> bacterial pathogen that causesanthrax in cattle. 2Earlier notions about causes <strong>of</strong> disease, such as gettingcold <strong>and</strong> tired, are not without some merit.These conditions are not primary agents <strong>of</strong> infectiousdisease, but our improved underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>immunology shows us that <strong>the</strong>se factors do contributeto disease by impairing immune function.Most changes in scientific knowledge reflect <strong>the</strong>expansion <strong>of</strong> inadequate explanations ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong>expulsion <strong>of</strong> entirely inaccurate ones.If your students have a fairly good underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>molecular genetics, <strong>the</strong>y may appreciate <strong>the</strong> additionalexample <strong>of</strong> discovering introns in eukaryotic genes.Because early molecular biology focused mainly onFigure 1-2 Modern studies in genetics: Althoughmodern geneticists use molecular techniques such ascloning <strong>and</strong> sequencing, geneticists continue to usemicroscopic techniques, particularly in cytogenetics.bacteria, most <strong>of</strong> which lack introns, <strong>the</strong> discovery <strong>of</strong>introns came as a surprise <strong>and</strong> required a modificationin our description <strong>of</strong> genes. (Figure 13 in <strong>the</strong> O v e rv i e wfor Te a c h e r sillustrates introns in gene stru c t u r e . )Indicate that, as <strong>the</strong>y work through this module, studentswill come to underst<strong>and</strong> more about how scientificknowledge changes <strong>and</strong> develops, <strong>and</strong> abouthow scientists go about <strong>the</strong>ir work. Students also willlearn about some new findings in genetics.Part I: Milestones inUnderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>Genetics</strong>Much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> information about genetics inyour biology textbook would have amazed1. Magellan’s voyage also resulted in a reconceptualization <strong>of</strong> time. Although Magellan was killed in <strong>the</strong> Philippines before<strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> voyage, his crew kept careful records <strong>of</strong> dates during <strong>the</strong> 3-year journey. When <strong>the</strong>y returned to <strong>the</strong>irhome port in Spain, <strong>the</strong>y found that <strong>the</strong>y had lost a day. This led to underst<strong>and</strong>ing that <strong>the</strong> Earth’s rotation results indifferent time zones—<strong>and</strong> even a different day—on some parts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> globe. We now acknowledge <strong>and</strong> correct for thisphenomenon with <strong>the</strong> International Date Line.2. The article by P.A. Small <strong>and</strong> N.S. Small, Mankind’s Magnificent Milestone: Smallpox Eradication, The American BiologyTeacher, 58(5):264-271, provides a nice overview <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> application <strong>of</strong> scientific process to <strong>the</strong> eradication <strong>of</strong> an infectiousdisease.77©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giantsbiologists a hundred years ago. Those scientists,driven by curiosity to answer complexquestions <strong>of</strong> heredity, slowly pieced toge<strong>the</strong>rlayer after layer <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> milestone explanationsthat we now accept as valid. The most significantexplanations st<strong>and</strong> as milestone events,each <strong>of</strong> which marks a great shift in our underst<strong>and</strong>ing.Think about what scientists neededto know before <strong>the</strong>y could add each new milestoneto <strong>the</strong> body <strong>of</strong> genetics knowledge. Youare going to build a sequence <strong>of</strong> milestoneexplanations. When you do, your sequencemay reflect <strong>the</strong> actual progress <strong>of</strong> genetics during<strong>the</strong> last hundred or so years, or it mayreflect o<strong>the</strong>r ways that history could haveplayed itself out during <strong>the</strong>se early years <strong>of</strong>discovery.Assign students to teams <strong>and</strong> distribute a set <strong>of</strong> milestoneexplanations to each team.1. Your team will receive a set <strong>of</strong> eight milestoneexplanations <strong>of</strong> inheritance. Decide how <strong>the</strong>semilestone explanations could form a meaningfulsequence, <strong>and</strong> be prepared to reportyour sequence <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> reasons you chose it.After <strong>the</strong> teams have had a chance to build asequence, poll <strong>the</strong> class for examples <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> choiceseach team has made. You may want to have eachteam turn in a written sequence, or you may want tocall on two or three teams to report. Ask <strong>the</strong> studentsto explain <strong>the</strong> criteria <strong>the</strong>y used to sequence<strong>the</strong> milestones. A h<strong>and</strong>y way to display results <strong>of</strong> differentteams is to prepare a poster for each milestone<strong>and</strong> have a student place <strong>the</strong> posters insequence, or prepare a set <strong>of</strong> milestones strips onoverhead transparency film for display. To start <strong>the</strong>discussion, you could display a milestone from <strong>the</strong>middle <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> actual sequence, choose ano<strong>the</strong>r milestone,<strong>and</strong> ask whe<strong>the</strong>r it should come before orafter <strong>the</strong> first one.Important: Keep in mind (<strong>and</strong> emphasize for <strong>the</strong>students) that <strong>the</strong>ir job is not to guess <strong>the</strong> dates <strong>and</strong>chronology <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se events. Instead, <strong>the</strong>ir task is toreason how <strong>the</strong> milestone explanations might buildin a logical way. Keep <strong>the</strong> discussion brief, <strong>and</strong> do notpress for consensus.2. Your teacher will show you <strong>the</strong> actualsequence <strong>of</strong> milestone events that occurredin <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> genetics. Compare it to <strong>the</strong>sequence you helped build with <strong>the</strong> class.Might <strong>the</strong> events have occurred just as easilyin <strong>the</strong> order you built?Display an overhead made from Copymaster 1-2 thatshows <strong>the</strong> historical sequence <strong>of</strong> milestone concepts<strong>and</strong>/or distribute a copy to each student. Emphasizeto <strong>the</strong> class that <strong>the</strong> connections between discoveriesare far more important than <strong>the</strong> dates on which<strong>the</strong> discoveries occurred.The historical sequence for <strong>the</strong>se events is given inCopymaster 1-2, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> sequence presented is somewhatarbitrary in that some ideas gained strength frommultiple experiments that occurred over a number <strong>of</strong>years. For instance, an early experiment that indicatedthat DNA is <strong>the</strong> genetic material took place in 1944,but additional evidence provided in 1952 convinced<strong>the</strong> scientific community. The work <strong>of</strong> Mendel, whichmight have influenced research in genetics in <strong>the</strong> latterpart <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nineteenth century, failed to do sobecause it was not recognized widely <strong>and</strong> understooduntil around 1900. The actual sequence <strong>of</strong> events inscientific history does not necessarily represent <strong>the</strong>o n l y, or even <strong>the</strong> best, sequence in terms <strong>of</strong> logicalconnections because scientists are at times limited bytechnology or make imperfect choices about <strong>the</strong> nextquestion to be answered.3. What technologies or cultural issues mighthave influenced <strong>the</strong> timing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> milestones<strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r discoveries in genetics?Sample student responses include:■ <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> a microscope,■ staining techniques,■ computers, <strong>and</strong>■ <strong>the</strong> laboratory techniques <strong>of</strong> molecular biology.Less obvious answers include:■ better communication among scientists,■ money available to support research,■ political interests at <strong>the</strong> time,■ rediscovery <strong>and</strong> openness to Mendel’s paper,■ <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> more sophisticated statistics,<strong>and</strong>■ <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> computers to store <strong>and</strong> compare data.78©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1Part II: How Good Is <strong>the</strong> Explanation?The milestone explanations you have beenusing have lasted for many years. Why? Usethis part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> activity to explore how weknow whe<strong>the</strong>r a scientific explanation is on<strong>the</strong> right track <strong>and</strong>, thus, whe<strong>the</strong>r it will survive<strong>the</strong> test <strong>of</strong> time.If your students have a basic grasp <strong>of</strong> what is meantby credible evidence, continue to Steps 4 <strong>and</strong> 5. Ifnot, conduct a brief discussion to establish this idea.You may want to use Question 3 from <strong>the</strong> Analysishere as an introduction. O<strong>the</strong>rwise, this questionwill serve as a review <strong>of</strong> this part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> activity.The student version <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Evidence Cards <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ircorresponding Milestone Explanation is provided inCopymaster 1-3. An annotated version appears in<strong>the</strong>se teacher pages.4. Your teacher will give your team a set <strong>of</strong> EvidenceCards <strong>and</strong> one Milestone Card. Yourfirst task is to evaluate <strong>the</strong> Evidence Cards<strong>and</strong> keep only those that are credible. Todetermine whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> information on anygiven Evidence Card is credible, discusswith your teammates <strong>the</strong> criteria you canuse to evaluate <strong>the</strong> evidence. Write your reasonsfor accepting or rejecting <strong>the</strong> statedevidence.Allow a few minutes for teams to make <strong>the</strong>ir choices.This step is brief because <strong>the</strong> “bogus” evidence cardsare fairly obvious. The value <strong>of</strong> this step is that studentsmust articulate <strong>the</strong>ir criteria. Ask a few teamsto give examples <strong>of</strong> why <strong>the</strong>y accepted or rejectedevidence.5. Now decide whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> evidence youretained is helpful in supporting or refuting<strong>the</strong> milestone explanation. Explain yourdecision. (Hint: Some evidence will be helpful;o<strong>the</strong>r evidence may not be related to<strong>the</strong> milestone explanation.)Allow enough time for students to compare ideasabout <strong>the</strong> relationship between evidence <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>genetics concept on <strong>the</strong> Milestone Card, but keep<strong>the</strong>ir discussion brief. Ask several teams to justify <strong>the</strong>helpfulness <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> evidence relative to <strong>the</strong> milestoneexplanations. (To keep <strong>the</strong> activity moving, you maynot want to have all <strong>the</strong> teams do this.)You may need to remind students that, for evidenceto support a scientific explanation, <strong>the</strong> evidencemust be related to <strong>the</strong> main ideas in that explanation.Related evidence should help to distinguishbetween one explanation <strong>and</strong> competing views. If<strong>the</strong> explanation discusses causality, supporting evidenceneeds to show a constant <strong>and</strong> regular associationbetween <strong>the</strong> stated cause <strong>and</strong> effect. For example,in 1927, after H.J. Muller exposed fruit flies toheavy doses <strong>of</strong> X rays, <strong>of</strong>fspring <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> exposed fliesexhibited mutations. This experiment can be repeatedwith <strong>the</strong> same results, supporting <strong>the</strong> notion thatradiation is a causal agent <strong>of</strong> mutations. Later experimentsprovided a more detailed explanation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>cause by demonstrating <strong>the</strong> molecular damage inDNA that results from exposure to X rays.ANALYSISNot all scientific discoveries are great milestonesthat change our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>natural world. Scientists put an enormousamount <strong>of</strong> work into even relatively simplediscoveries, as you would expect when youconsider <strong>the</strong> rigors <strong>of</strong> investigation. Thesesmall pieces provide a valuable part <strong>of</strong> a largerpuzzle. Gradually, we build our underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> inheritance. The rarity <strong>of</strong> great leaps inunderst<strong>and</strong>ing can be a frustrating aspect <strong>of</strong>scientific work.Science is carried out by people who mustearn a living <strong>and</strong> who want to fulfill personalgoals, factors that might influence <strong>the</strong>ir work.Think about <strong>the</strong> social <strong>and</strong> cultural setting inwhich research takes place as you respond to<strong>the</strong>se questions.1. What does science try to do?Sample student responses include:■ It makes discoveries.■ It finds facts.■ It explains things.For additional discussion, see Section III <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Overview for Teachers, page 14. You may want tointroduce <strong>the</strong> idea <strong>of</strong> causal explanations.79©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants2. How do we know an explanation is on <strong>the</strong>right track?Sample student responses include:■ The explanation predicts events accurately.■ An explanation is supported by good evidence.■ More than one line <strong>of</strong> evidence supports <strong>the</strong>explanation.■ The explanation is consistent with o<strong>the</strong>r scientificknowledge.For a full discussion, see Section III <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Overviewfor Teachers, page 15.3. What counts as credible evidence in science?Sample student responses include:■ The evidence was <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> laboratory work(an experiment).■ There is statistical evidence (numbers) or <strong>the</strong>experiment yields <strong>the</strong> same result whenr e p e a t e d.■ Experiments include adequate controls.■ The evidence survives challenges by <strong>the</strong> scientificcommunity.■ The evidence is accurate.You might want to record some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> responses on<strong>the</strong> chalkboard or on a large sheet <strong>of</strong> paper under<strong>the</strong> heading “Criteria for Credible Evidence.” Keep<strong>the</strong> list visible in <strong>the</strong> classroom as a prompt for Steps4 <strong>and</strong> 5.4. Mendel developed a simple, yet elegant, systemto explain inheritance. What has happenedto his system?Mendel’s work has endured because it still explains agreat many <strong>of</strong> our observations about inheritance.Scientists, however, have proposed many new explanationsabout inheritance since Mendel’s time. Thosethat satisfied <strong>the</strong> requirements <strong>of</strong> scientific investigationhave become incorporated into an ever- e x p a n d-ing body <strong>of</strong> knowledge about inheritance.5. Begin to make a poster that records yourideas about <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> science, ifyour teacher instructs you to do so.Have students brainstorm ideas <strong>and</strong> record <strong>the</strong>m onposter paper under <strong>the</strong> heading “Nature <strong>and</strong> <strong>Methods</strong><strong>of</strong> Science.” Display this NMS poster in <strong>the</strong> classroom.Encourage students to add ideas throughout<strong>the</strong> module.Sample student responses about <strong>the</strong> characteristics<strong>of</strong> science include:■ More than one source <strong>of</strong> evidence makes anexplanation stronger.■ Evidence needs to be repeatable.■ Explanations show <strong>the</strong> connection between acause <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> resulting event.■ Mendel’s explanations are valid today.■ A new discovery must fit with an existing explanationor <strong>the</strong> explanation would have to change.■ New work builds on old work.■ When people are concerned about inheriteddisease, <strong>the</strong>re will be more money for geneticsresearch.■ A famous name is not enough to get your explanationaccepted.■ You have to do experiments.■ You have to share your data.ANNOTATIONS TO THE COPYMASTERS:COPYMASTERS 1-1 AND 1-3Here, we number each milestone in sequence, followedby its corresponding evidence. Actual copymastersomit numbers to avoid influencing students.MILESTONES IN UNDERSTANDINGGENETICS - NO. 1Question:Why do <strong>of</strong>fspring resemble <strong>the</strong>ir parents?Milestone Explanation:Parents contribute genetic material to<strong>the</strong>ir <strong>of</strong>fspring.Evidence 1A:A scientist looked through a microscopeat dividing cells in <strong>the</strong> tail fins <strong>of</strong>a salam<strong>and</strong>er. As mitosis proceeded,she saw that chromosomes movedapart in equal numbers into <strong>the</strong> newlyforming daughter cells. O<strong>the</strong>r scientistsobserved this phenomenon incells undergoing mitosis.80©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1This evidence is■ credible; <strong>the</strong> statement is based on carefulobservation <strong>and</strong> is repeatable.■ helpful, but only partly supportive because,although it shows a possible role for chromosomesin passing information to daughter cells,mitosis <strong>and</strong> cell division in tail fins have nothingto do with inheritance from parents to <strong>of</strong>fspring.(This is one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> observations that led to an explanation<strong>of</strong> DNA replication <strong>and</strong> cell division [W. Flemming,1879].)Evidence 1B:A scientist crossed pea plants <strong>and</strong> carefullyrecorded <strong>the</strong> appearance <strong>of</strong> cert a i ntraits in <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring. When he crosseda strain that has only purple flowerswith one that has only white flowers,<strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring always had purple flowers.When he crossed <strong>the</strong>se <strong>of</strong>fspring to produce<strong>the</strong> next generation, however, hesaw both colors <strong>of</strong> flower in <strong>the</strong> new<strong>of</strong>fspring in regular proport i o n s .This work was repeated later by o<strong>the</strong>rscientists who saw <strong>the</strong> same results.This evidence is■ credible; this experiment is repeatable with <strong>the</strong>same results.■ helpful; this information provides evidence thatflower color is a trait inherited from parents, <strong>and</strong>that both parents may contribute to <strong>the</strong> phenotype<strong>of</strong> future <strong>of</strong>fspring. The white color trait isnot lost, but hidden; <strong>the</strong>re must be at least tw<strong>of</strong>actors involved, one from each parent.(This is one <strong>of</strong> Mendel’s classic experiments, reportedin 1865 <strong>and</strong> repeated by o<strong>the</strong>rs in 1900. Theimplication is that each parent contributes one <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> alleles for flower color, but that purple color is<strong>the</strong> dominant trait.)Evidence 1C:Jorge noticed that a classmate, Susan,has curly hair. When he met her mo<strong>the</strong>r,he noticed that she also has curly hair.This evidence is scientifically bogus; although it suggeststhat genetic information passes from parentsto <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring (Susan), <strong>the</strong> evidence is so limited inits scope that it is not <strong>of</strong> much value. The curly hairmay result from o<strong>the</strong>r than genetic influences; forexample, maybe Susan <strong>and</strong> her mo<strong>the</strong>r have usedperms to curl <strong>the</strong>ir hair.Evidence 1D:Mendel said that characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspringlikely come from something <strong>the</strong><strong>of</strong>fspring get from <strong>the</strong>ir parents.This evidence is scientifically bogus; stated this way,<strong>the</strong> information is unclear <strong>and</strong> unsubstantiated. If itsaid “Tom” instead <strong>of</strong> “Mendel,” people would beeven less likely to credit <strong>the</strong> statement. Authoritywithout a basis in scientific evidence is not meaningfulin supporting a scientific explanation.MILESTONES IN UNDERSTANDINGGENETICS - NO. 2Question:How are traits distributed in <strong>of</strong>fspring?Milestone Explanation:Alleles <strong>of</strong> one gene segregate in <strong>the</strong>formation <strong>of</strong> gametes.(Reproductive cells [gametes] formduring meiosis. Each gamete containsone allele from <strong>the</strong> pair <strong>of</strong> alleles presentin <strong>the</strong> parent.)Evidence 2A:People say that sons express <strong>the</strong> traits<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>r, while daughters have all<strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’s characteristics.This evidence is scientifically bogus; it is based onhearsay <strong>and</strong> lacks a scientific basis.Evidence 2B:Offspring in each generation arei d e n t i c a l .81©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsThis evidence is scientifically bogus; <strong>the</strong> statement isnot correct, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>re is no information about how<strong>the</strong> observation was made. Even if it were credible,<strong>the</strong> evidence would not be helpful because <strong>the</strong> evidencedoes not address <strong>the</strong> distribution <strong>of</strong> traits.Evidence 2C:Mendel speculated that traits are inheritedbased on discrete units <strong>of</strong> inheritance.He tested <strong>the</strong> law <strong>of</strong> segregationby observing height in several generations<strong>of</strong> pea plants. He saw a distribution<strong>of</strong> tall to dwarf in <strong>the</strong> F2 generation<strong>of</strong> 3:1. Then <strong>the</strong> F2 plants werefertilized with <strong>the</strong>ir own pollen (selfed).Mendel found that <strong>the</strong> dwarf F2plants produced dwarf F3 plants, buttwo-thirds <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tall F2 plants producedmixed <strong>of</strong>fspring, dwarf <strong>and</strong> tall.This F3 test <strong>of</strong> segregation has beenrepeated many times with <strong>the</strong> sameresults.This evidence is■ credible; <strong>the</strong> evidence is drawn from carefulobservation <strong>and</strong> collections <strong>of</strong> quantitative data;it is repeatable.■ helpful; <strong>the</strong> ratios in F2 <strong>and</strong> F3 generations fit<strong>the</strong> predictions based on <strong>the</strong> segregation <strong>of</strong> allelesin <strong>the</strong> production <strong>of</strong> gametes.Evidence 2D:A scientist named W.S. Sutton observedchromosomes in cells undergoingmeiosis. He noticed that <strong>the</strong> chromosomesbehaved in a way that is consistentwith Mendel’s observations aboutinheritance patterns. Many o<strong>the</strong>robservations <strong>of</strong> meiosis by o<strong>the</strong>r scientistsconfirmed this behavior <strong>of</strong> chromosomesin <strong>the</strong> nucleus.This evidence is■ credible; o<strong>the</strong>r scientists have replicated Sutton’sfindings.■ helpful; this is evidence that chromosomesbehave in a way that is consistent with Mendel’slaws <strong>of</strong> inheritance <strong>and</strong> will fit with <strong>the</strong>ir ma<strong>the</strong>maticalpredictions.(This observation by Walter S. Sutton in 1903 helpedestablish <strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> inheritance.This same evidence was used to support ano<strong>the</strong>rmilestone explanation; this overlap is a commonoccurrence in science.)MILESTONES IN UNDERSTANDINGGENETICS - NO. 3Question:Where are genes located?Milestone Explanation:Genes are located on chromosomes.(This idea is <strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong>inheritance. In euka ryotic cells, geneticmaterial is located in <strong>the</strong> nucleus ins t ructures called chromosomes, for <strong>the</strong>irdark-staining characteristic. The namecomes from <strong>the</strong> Greek words c h r o m a[color] <strong>and</strong> s o m a [body].)Evidence 3A:Using staining techniques <strong>and</strong> a microscope,C. Nageli discovered a set <strong>of</strong>structures in <strong>the</strong> nuclei <strong>of</strong> cells. O<strong>the</strong>rscientists observed that <strong>the</strong>se structureschange <strong>and</strong> become visible with amicroscope at certain times in <strong>the</strong> cellcycle. Years after Nageli’s observation,a developmental biologist, W. Roux,observed <strong>the</strong>se structures in <strong>the</strong> cellnucleus, <strong>and</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r scientist, W.Waldeyer, saw <strong>the</strong> structures <strong>and</strong>named <strong>the</strong>m chromosomes.This evidence is■ credible; o<strong>the</strong>r scientists have replicatedNageli’s findings.■ somewhat helpful in that it establishes that chromosomesexist. It is not conclusive, however,because <strong>the</strong>re is no evidence about <strong>the</strong>ir geneticrole.(C. Nageli discovered chromosomes in 1842; Wi l h e l mRoux saw <strong>the</strong>m in 1883, but did not have evidence <strong>of</strong>82©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1<strong>the</strong>ir role in inheritance. W. Waldeyer gave chromosomes<strong>the</strong>ir name in 1888.)Evidence 3B:A scientist named W.S. Sutton observedchromosomes in cells undergoingmeiosis. He noticed that <strong>the</strong> chromosomesbehaved in a way that is consistentwith Mendel’s observations aboutinheritance patterns. Many o<strong>the</strong>robservations <strong>of</strong> meiosis by o<strong>the</strong>r scientistsconfirmed this behavior <strong>of</strong> chromosomesin <strong>the</strong> nucleus.This evidence is■ credible; o<strong>the</strong>r scientists have replicated Sutton’sfindings.■ helpful; this is evidence that chromosomesbehave in a way that is consistent with Mendel’slaws <strong>of</strong> inheritance <strong>and</strong> will fit with <strong>the</strong>ir ma<strong>the</strong>maticalpredictions.(This observation by Walter S. Sutton in 1903 helpedestablish <strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> inheritance.)This evidence is scientifically bogus because it isimprecise. The pr<strong>of</strong>essor has provided no details,<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> evidence is based on <strong>the</strong> fame <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> universityalone. There is no substantiating scientific evidence.Perhaps <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>essor is a specialist in somethingunrelated to biology <strong>and</strong> knows little aboutgenetics.MILESTONES IN UNDERSTANDINGGENETICS - NO. 4Question:What determines <strong>the</strong> sex <strong>of</strong> ano r g a n i s m ?Milestone Explanation:In most sexually reproducing organisms,special chromosomes determ i n esex.(Humans have sex chromosomes, designatedX <strong>and</strong> Y. The combination <strong>of</strong>sex chromosomes in an organismdetermines its sex.)Evidence 3C:A scientist, T. Boveri, showed that seaurchin embryos develop normally onlywhen <strong>the</strong>y have a full set <strong>of</strong> chromosomes.Embryos with more or fewerchromosomes than <strong>the</strong> normallyobserved number did not developproperly. Many o<strong>the</strong>r scientists havemade <strong>the</strong> same observations in o<strong>the</strong>rorganisms.This evidence is■ credible; it is precise, testable, <strong>and</strong> repeatable.■ helpful; it connects cause <strong>and</strong> effect to genotype<strong>and</strong> phenotype.(This observation by T. Boveri in 1903 was one line<strong>of</strong> evidence that led to <strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong>inheritance.)Evidence 3D:A pr<strong>of</strong>essor at Harvard thinks thatchromosomes contain genes.Evidence 4A:Many studies have shown that humanfemales have two X chromosomes, <strong>and</strong>human males have one X chromosome<strong>and</strong> one Y chromosome. PatriciaJacobs <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r scientists alsoshowed that, although <strong>the</strong> normalhuman sex chromosome complementis XX or XY, o<strong>the</strong>r patterns do occurrarely. For instance, in humans, XXYindividuals are male, <strong>and</strong> XO individualsare female.This evidence is■ credible; it is precise, <strong>and</strong> several lines <strong>of</strong> investigationsupport it.■ helpful, especially when added to our knowledgethat chromosomes are <strong>the</strong> mediators <strong>of</strong>inheritance. This is evidence that sex determinationresults from <strong>the</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Y chromosomein females, or <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Y chromosomein human males.83©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsEvidence 4B:During <strong>the</strong>ir lifetime, human malesproduce many sperm, but females produceonly a few eggs. Many years <strong>of</strong>studies showed that females are bornwith all <strong>the</strong> egg cells <strong>the</strong>y will everhave (although <strong>the</strong> eggs must matureindividually during successive menstrualcycles). Males, however, producemillions <strong>of</strong> new sperm cells every fewdays until a very advanced age.This evidence is■ credible; <strong>the</strong> conclusions are based on “manyyears <strong>of</strong> studies.”■ not helpful; <strong>the</strong> information is correct, but it hasnothing to do with sex determination.Evidence 4C:A scientist named C.E. McClung foundthat grasshoppers produce equal quantities<strong>of</strong> two different types <strong>of</strong> sperm ,one <strong>of</strong> which contains an extra chromosome.Three years later, two o<strong>the</strong>r scientists,N. Stevens <strong>and</strong> E.B. Wi l s o n ,d e t e rmined that female grasshoppershave two copies <strong>of</strong> one particular chromosome,whereas males have only one.This evidence is■ credible; it is precise, <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r scientists canreplicate <strong>the</strong> data.■ helpful; this is evidence that <strong>the</strong> extra chromosomein grasshopper females determines sex.(These are <strong>the</strong> studies <strong>of</strong> C.E. McClung in 1902, <strong>and</strong>N. Stevens <strong>and</strong> E.B. Wilson in 1905.)Evidence 4D:There is a saying that a pregnantwoman can determine <strong>the</strong> sex <strong>of</strong> herchild by eating spicy foods to producea male or cool foods to produce afemale.This evidence is scientifically bogus; this evidence isbased on hearsay <strong>and</strong> folklore. In addition, <strong>the</strong> question<strong>and</strong> milestone explanation address <strong>the</strong> geneticdetermination <strong>of</strong> sex in <strong>of</strong>fspring; by <strong>the</strong> time awoman knows she is pregnant, <strong>the</strong> genotype <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>potential <strong>of</strong>fspring already is determined. What sheeats or drinks can affect <strong>the</strong> developing phenotype<strong>of</strong> her fetus, sometimes adversely (alcohol, for example,can cause a variety <strong>of</strong> problems in <strong>the</strong> developingfetus, including low birth weight <strong>and</strong> retardation),but it cannot affect <strong>the</strong> sex <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fetus.MILESTONES IN UNDERSTANDINGGENETICS - NO. 5Question:Why do some traits occur toge<strong>the</strong>r in<strong>of</strong>fspring?Milestone Explanation:Some genes are located on <strong>the</strong> samechromosome (linkage).Evidence 5A:The host <strong>of</strong> a popular talk-show aboutsports says that <strong>the</strong> best baseball pitchershave brown eyes.This evidence is scientifically bogus; this comment isbased on hearsay ra<strong>the</strong>r than scientific evidence, so it isnot credible. At first glance, <strong>the</strong> comment may appearrelevant to students because it suggests that two traits,pitching well <strong>and</strong> eye color, are related. Even if <strong>the</strong>rewere data to support <strong>the</strong> statement, many o<strong>the</strong>r factorsmight be involved, for example, light sensitivity in blueeyedplayers. This situation does not indicate a geneticconnection between two heritable traits.Evidence 5B:Many microscopic studies show thatchromosome pairs can exchange materialduring meiosis, resulting in a newcombination <strong>of</strong> alleles in that pair <strong>of</strong>chromosomes. This phenomenon isgenetic recombination, which resultsfrom crossing over.This evidence is■ credible; it is based on “many microscopic studies.”■ somewhat helpful, but not strongly supportive.This observation suggests that some genes on84©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1one chromosome may not behave as though<strong>the</strong>y are linked when <strong>the</strong>y are separated by asubstantial distance, such that recombinationcan occur.Evidence 5C:Three scientists demonstrated thatpurple flowers <strong>and</strong> long pollen wereinherited toge<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> sweet peamore <strong>of</strong>ten (more than 75% <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>time) than predicted by Mendel’s law<strong>of</strong> independent assortment (50%).This evidence is■ credible; <strong>the</strong> scientists conducted experiments<strong>and</strong> kept track <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir data.■ helpful; this is evidence that <strong>the</strong>se genes areassociated in some way.(The experiment is <strong>the</strong> 1906 observation <strong>of</strong> W. Bateson,E.R. Saunders, <strong>and</strong> R.C. Punnett; it demonstrateslinkage—a noted exception to Mendel’s law<strong>of</strong> independent assortment. The two genes are on<strong>the</strong> same chromosome <strong>and</strong> are not always separatedby crossing over during meiosis.)Evidence 5D:A study <strong>of</strong> human pedigrees shows thatcertain traits such as hemophilia <strong>and</strong>color blindness occur at a much higherfrequency in males than in females.These traits appear to depend on <strong>the</strong>inheritance <strong>of</strong> mutations located on<strong>the</strong> X chromosome. When a man hasboth <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se traits, studies show that<strong>the</strong>re is a greater than 50% chance thatany bro<strong>the</strong>rs will have both disorders,or nei<strong>the</strong>r one.This evidence is■ credible; it relies on <strong>the</strong> observation <strong>of</strong> multiplecases <strong>and</strong> on careful ma<strong>the</strong>matical analysis.■ helpful; in this case, <strong>the</strong> two traits are on <strong>the</strong>same chromosome (X) <strong>and</strong> are not inheritedindependently, supporting <strong>the</strong> idea that <strong>the</strong>y arelinked. The distance between genes on onechromosome determines how tightly <strong>the</strong>y arelinked. If <strong>the</strong> genes are very tightly linked, <strong>the</strong>probability is low that <strong>the</strong>y will be separated duringcrossing over.(This work was done by Julia Bell <strong>and</strong> J.B.S. Haldanein <strong>the</strong> 1930s <strong>and</strong> was <strong>the</strong> first to show linkage inhumans. It provided early groundwork for <strong>the</strong> impetus<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project to map all humangenes <strong>and</strong> identify those that are disease-related.)MILESTONES IN UNDERSTANDINGGENETICS - NO. 6Question:What molecular component in chromosomescarries genetic information?Milestone Explanation:DNA carries genetic information.Evidence 6A:Three scientists purified DNA frombacteria that grew in smooth colonies.They put this DNA into bacteria thatnormally grew in rough colonies. Thebacteria that had been given <strong>the</strong> DNAproduced many generations <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspringthat formed smooth colonies.This experiment produced <strong>the</strong> sameresults when repeated.O<strong>the</strong>r scientists (many years later)transferred a specific fragment <strong>of</strong> DNAfrom bacteria to a plant, <strong>and</strong> a bacterialtrait appeared in <strong>the</strong> plant.This evidence is■ credible; it is based on experimentation, <strong>and</strong>o<strong>the</strong>rs have replicated <strong>the</strong> results.■ helpful; this is evidence that DNA containsi n f o rmation that determines phenotype, suchas traits that affect <strong>the</strong> appearance <strong>of</strong> a bacterialcolony or particular genes in a plant. The a c c u-m u l a t i o n <strong>of</strong> evidence that supports an explanationmany years after it was first proposed iscommon <strong>and</strong> shows <strong>the</strong> durability <strong>of</strong> scientificknowledge. You may want to point out this phenomenonafter <strong>the</strong> students have assembled<strong>the</strong>ir conceptual sequence <strong>of</strong> milestone explanations.85©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants(The first observation is <strong>the</strong> classic experiment byO.T. Avery, C.M. MacLeod, <strong>and</strong> M. McCarty, performedin 1944. They transformed pneumococcusby transferring DNA from one strain to ano<strong>the</strong>r.Modern genetic engineering provides many examples<strong>of</strong> transformation by DNA, even across speciesboundaries. For instance, <strong>the</strong> gene for a toxin fromBacillus thurengensis that is effective against insectshas been inserted into several species <strong>of</strong> plantsincluding tobacco, tomato, <strong>and</strong> cotton.)Evidence 6B:Two scientists studied viruses to determinehow <strong>the</strong>y infect bacteria. Theylabeled <strong>the</strong> DNA <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> protein components<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> viruses using radioactivechemicals. This allowed <strong>the</strong>m totrace <strong>the</strong> movement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> DNA <strong>and</strong>protein. Only labeled DNA entered <strong>the</strong>bacterial cells during <strong>the</strong> infectionprocess. O<strong>the</strong>r investigations repeated<strong>the</strong>se studies.This evidence is■ credible; <strong>the</strong> evidence results from experimentsthat o<strong>the</strong>rs have replicated.■ helpful; this is evidence that DNA, not protein,is responsible for infection by bacteriophagev i ruses.(This is <strong>the</strong> 1952 experiment <strong>of</strong> A.D. Hershey <strong>and</strong> M.Chase.)Evidence 6C:Scientists have extracted DNA frommany different cell types in one organism<strong>and</strong> from <strong>the</strong> cells <strong>of</strong> many differentspecies.This evidence is■ credible; it is based on work with numeroussamples.■ helpful, but certainly not conclusive. The factthat all cells appear to have DNA supports <strong>the</strong>hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that DNA is <strong>the</strong> genetic material. Allcells, however, also have proteins, carbohydrates,<strong>and</strong> lipids, so this evidence is in no wayconclusive.Evidence 6D:Scientist Francis Crick <strong>and</strong> a studentnamed James Watson agreed that DNAmight be <strong>the</strong> genetic material.This evidence is scientifically bogus. Without scientificevidence, <strong>the</strong> opinion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se men provesnothing. Students might be naive about this bogusevidence because <strong>the</strong>se scientists did go on to discover<strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> DNA (although even that evidencealone does not prove its role as geneticm a t e r i a l ) .MILESTONES IN UNDERSTANDINGGENETICS - NO. 7Question:How is <strong>the</strong> genetic information used tomake proteins encoded?Milestone Explanation:In DNA, a triplet <strong>of</strong> nucleotide basesencodes each amino acid in <strong>the</strong> resultantprotein.Evidence 7A:To determine how <strong>the</strong> genetic codemight work, scientists noted thatDNA has four different bases that canbe arranged in various sequences.The code must be able to specify <strong>the</strong>20 different amino acids found inproteins. Ma<strong>the</strong>matical principlespredict <strong>the</strong> following about <strong>the</strong> geneticcode:one-base codetwo-base codes p e c i f i e s 4 amino acids at mosts p e c i f i e s 16 amino acids at mostthree-base code s p e c i f i e s 64 amino acids at mostf o u r-base codeThis evidence iss p e c i f i e s 256 amino acids at most■ credible; it is based on ma<strong>the</strong>matical principlesthat are clearly demonstrable <strong>and</strong> testable.■ helpful; <strong>the</strong> three-base code has <strong>the</strong> minimalcomplexity required to encode twenty aminoacids. This evidence does not prove <strong>the</strong> tripletnature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetic code, but it is supportive<strong>and</strong> it suggests a testable hypo<strong>the</strong>sis.86©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1Evidence 7B:Investigators found that removal <strong>of</strong>three nucleotides from a gene causes<strong>the</strong> resulting protein to lose one aminoacid. However, removal <strong>of</strong> one or twonucleotides from a gene causes muchmore disruption in <strong>the</strong> resulting proteinstructure. O<strong>the</strong>r scientists quicklyrepeated <strong>the</strong>se experiments <strong>and</strong> got<strong>the</strong> same results.This evidence is■ credible; it is based on experiments that o<strong>the</strong>rshave replicated.■ helpful; this is evidence that <strong>the</strong> protein-codingi n f o rmation occurs in groups <strong>of</strong> threenucleotides.(This is <strong>the</strong> 1961 experiment where Francis Crick, L.Barnett, S. Brenner, <strong>and</strong> S.J. Watts-Tobin, used amutagenic chemical [pr<strong>of</strong>lavin] that adds or removesnucleotides from DNA. A one- or two-nucleotidechange results in a frameshift <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> protein-codinginformation; removal <strong>of</strong> three nucleotides simplyremoves one amino acid from <strong>the</strong> protein <strong>and</strong> usuallyis less disruptive to protein function. You maywant to take time to discuss this idea <strong>of</strong> a frameshiftmutation <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> a triplet code.)Evidence 7C:Many types <strong>of</strong> chemical analysis haveshown that DNA contains about equalamounts <strong>of</strong> four different components(<strong>the</strong> nucleotides, which contain basesabbreviated A, G, C, <strong>and</strong> T). In contrast,proteins are made <strong>of</strong> 20 different components(amino acids), <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y vary inamount in different proteins.This evidence is■ credible; it is based on “many types <strong>of</strong> chemicalanalysis.”■ not directly supportive <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> milestone explanation.In fact, this early observation led manyscientists to conclude that protein is <strong>the</strong> geneticmaterial because its structure appeared to becapable <strong>of</strong> more variation. DNA appeared to betoo regular. The difficulty arose because scientistsdid not know <strong>the</strong> actual structure <strong>of</strong> DNAthat permits <strong>the</strong> sequence <strong>of</strong> four bases to producean enormous number <strong>of</strong> variations (hence,different genes). The variation evident in protein(phenotype) is actually <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> geneticinformation ra<strong>the</strong>r than its source.Evidence 7D:A shampoo is advertised as containingDNA <strong>and</strong> able to enrich hair.This evidence is scientifically bogus (but typical);<strong>the</strong> claims included in advertisements are notalways substantiated, although <strong>the</strong>y are supposedto be. Sometimes, advertising combines two unrelatedphenomena in <strong>the</strong> hope that <strong>the</strong> consumerwill infer a connection. For example, this advert i s e-ment combines “contains DNA” (which could bet rue) <strong>and</strong> “enriches hair” (which implies but doesnot state that DNA does this; it is a questionableclaim in any event). In addition, <strong>the</strong>re is no scientificevidence to support this association. Fu rt h e r-more, even if DNA did enrich hair, that fact doesnot address DNA’s role as genetic material (hair isprotein, not a living cell).MILESTONES IN UNDERSTANDINGGENETICS - NO. 8Question:How does a new, heritable trait appearin a population?Milestone Explanation:Mutations change <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> DNAin reproductive cells (gametes).Evidence 8A:People who build large musclesthrough exercise will have childrenwho also have large muscles.This evidence is scientifically bogus; it is not substantiatedby any data from careful observation <strong>and</strong>measurement. In addition, even if it were credible,this evidence would not be helpful as an explanation<strong>of</strong> heritable change. Strong parents may pass along agenetic makeup for heavy build to <strong>the</strong>ir children, but87©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giantsthis transfer does not depend on <strong>the</strong> acquired trait <strong>of</strong>muscle building from exercise by <strong>the</strong> parent.Evidence 8B:A scientist named Hermann J. Mullerexposed fruit flies to increasing doses<strong>of</strong> radiation in <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> X rays. Hekept careful records <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong>mutant traits that appeared in <strong>the</strong>ir<strong>of</strong>fspring. Muller found that <strong>the</strong>re wasa direct correlation between <strong>the</strong> number<strong>of</strong> mutations <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> amount <strong>of</strong>radiation: more X rays produced moremutant <strong>of</strong>fspring. (Later researchshowed that X rays damage chromosomes.)O<strong>the</strong>r scientists have repeatedthis experiment, <strong>and</strong> similar experimentshave been repeated many times.This evidence is■ credible; it is based on careful observation <strong>and</strong> aprecise record <strong>of</strong> results. O<strong>the</strong>rs have replicatedMuller’s results.■ somewhat helpful; it suggests a causal relationship,although it does not directly address DNAstructure as <strong>the</strong> basis for physical change.(Muller received <strong>the</strong> 1946 Noble Prize for this [<strong>and</strong>related] work, which he reported in 1927.)Evidence 8C:Investigators found that removal <strong>of</strong>three nucleotides from a gene causes<strong>the</strong> resulting protein to lose one aminoacid. However, removal <strong>of</strong> one or twonucleotides from a gene causes muchmore disruption in <strong>the</strong> resulting proteinstructure. O<strong>the</strong>r scientists quicklyrepeated <strong>the</strong>se experiments <strong>and</strong> got<strong>the</strong> same results.This evidence is■ credible; it is based on evidence that o<strong>the</strong>rs havereplicated.■ helpful; this is evidence that <strong>the</strong> protein-codinginformation occurs in groups <strong>of</strong> threen u c l e o t i d e s .(This is <strong>the</strong> 1961 experiment where Francis Crick, L.Barnett, S. Brenner, <strong>and</strong> S.J. Watts-Tobin, used amutagenic chemical [pr<strong>of</strong>lavin] that adds or removesnucleotides from DNA. A one- or two-nucleotidechange results in a frameshift <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> protein-codinginformation; removal <strong>of</strong> three nucleotides simplyremoves one amino acid from <strong>the</strong> protein <strong>and</strong> usuallyis less disruptive to protein function. This sameevidence was used to support ano<strong>the</strong>r milestoneexplanation; this overlap is a common occurrence inscience.)Evidence 8D:Investigators at <strong>the</strong> Toronto Hospitalfor Sick Children studied <strong>the</strong> DNA <strong>of</strong>children who have cystic fibrosis (CF).The investigators also studied <strong>the</strong> parents<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se children. They found that<strong>the</strong>se children <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir parents havechanges in <strong>the</strong>ir DNA that are not presentin unaffected children or in personswho do not carry <strong>the</strong> CF gene.Fur<strong>the</strong>r investigation has revealedmore than 600 different DNA mutationsin <strong>the</strong> CF gene.This evidence is■ credible. The investigators collected data on CFpatients <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir parents <strong>and</strong> compared thosedata to o<strong>the</strong>r data from unaffected persons <strong>and</strong>noncarriers. O<strong>the</strong>r investigators found additionalmutations in <strong>the</strong> CF gene.■ helpful; <strong>the</strong> evidence shows a relationshipbetween changes in genotype <strong>and</strong> changes inphenotype. In addition, <strong>the</strong> work builds on ourbasic underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> Mendelian genetics, inthis case, autosomal recessive inheritance.(This is <strong>the</strong> 1989 work <strong>of</strong> Lap Chee Tsui <strong>and</strong> his colleaguesin Toronto. This group isolated <strong>the</strong> CFgene.)88©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 2Puzzling PedigreesNMS CONCEPTSExisting explanations are tested with new evidence;new evidence is used to build hypo<strong>the</strong>ses.GENETICS CONCEPTSThis activity reviews traditional (Mendelian) inheritancepatterns, <strong>and</strong> introduces <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong>extranuclear (mitochondrial) inheritance.FOCUSActivity 2 permits students to review pedigrees <strong>and</strong>major patterns <strong>of</strong> traditional (Mendelian) inheritance.Students also discover a pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritancethat does not “fit” those with which <strong>the</strong>y aref a m i l i a r. The students use pedigrees that show thisnew pattern <strong>and</strong> related data to discover that <strong>the</strong>reare modes <strong>of</strong> genetic transmission, such as mitochondrialinheritance, that do not follow Mendelianp a t t e rns. The activity lets students experience <strong>the</strong>cycle <strong>of</strong> evidence <strong>and</strong> explanation through whichscientific knowledge is built, tested, used, <strong>and</strong> modified.When reliable data do not fit with existingexplanations, <strong>the</strong> data are n o t discarded; instead,existing explanations change or new explanationsare built.OBJECTIVESAs students complete this activity, <strong>the</strong>y should1. review familiar patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance: autosomaldominant <strong>and</strong> recessive, X-linked dominant <strong>and</strong>recessive;2. discover that some patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance do notfollow Mendelian patterns;3. use some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science: observe, ga<strong>the</strong>revidence, propose explanations for patterns <strong>of</strong>transmission in <strong>the</strong> puzzling pedigrees, <strong>and</strong> look foradditional evidence to support or refute <strong>the</strong>ir newexplanation; <strong>and</strong>4. add to <strong>the</strong> NMS poster (optional; see Activity 1).ESTIMATED TIMEOne 50-minute class periodPREPARATION AND MATERIALSYou will need to provide <strong>the</strong> following:■ overhead projector■overhead transparency <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 2-1, PedigreesA-H (optional)■overhead transparency <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 2-2, PedigreesI <strong>and</strong> J (optional)89


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling Pedigrees■overhead transparency <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 2-3, PedigreesI´ <strong>and</strong> J´ (optional)■coins for simple statistical model (at least 1 coinper team)■ Copymaster 2-4, Building an Explanation (1 copyper student)■ Copymaster 2-5, Science Art i c l e s (1 copy pers t u d e n t )COPYMASTERS USED IN THIS ACTIVITYCopymaster 2-1Pedigrees A-H (optional)Copymaster 2-2Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J (optional)Copymaster 2-3Pedigrees I´ <strong>and</strong> J´ (optional)Copymaster 2-4Building an Explanation (worksheet)Copymaster 2-5Science ArticlesINTRODUCTIONThis activity forms a bridge between what studentsalready know about fundamental patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance<strong>and</strong> modes <strong>of</strong> transmission that likely are newto <strong>the</strong>m. The activity challenges students to underst<strong>and</strong>that scientific knowledge in genetics is bothlasting <strong>and</strong> flexible, with emphasis on <strong>the</strong> development<strong>of</strong> new explanations when explanations basedon our current underst<strong>and</strong>ing are not adequate toaccount for new observations. Students review traditionalpatterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance, discover a new <strong>and</strong>puzzling pattern (mitochondrial inheritance) that<strong>the</strong>y attempt to explain, <strong>and</strong> articulate <strong>the</strong> way <strong>the</strong>yhave used scientific processes in this activity. (SeeSection V in <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers for informationabout extranuclear inheritance.)An interesting consequence <strong>of</strong> our awareness <strong>of</strong> mitochondrialinheritance is <strong>the</strong> ability to trace matern a llineages for <strong>the</strong> human species back to ancient times.The topic is popularly called “<strong>the</strong> mitochondrial Eves t o ry,” <strong>and</strong> it has received much media attention. Yo umight direct students to <strong>the</strong> following articles aboutthis interesting <strong>and</strong> somewhat controversial topic:■ Thorne, A.G <strong>and</strong> M.H. Wolp<strong>of</strong>f. The multiregionalevolution <strong>of</strong> humans, Scientific American266(4):76-83, April 1992;■Wilson, A.C. <strong>and</strong> R.L. Cann. The recent Africangenesis <strong>of</strong> humans, Scientific American,266(4):68-73, April 1992; <strong>and</strong>■ Chapter 3 <strong>of</strong> The Ne<strong>and</strong>ertal Enigma, by JamesShreeve, 1995, William Morrow <strong>and</strong> Company.Primary literature sources for your own use include:■ Cann, R.L., M. Stoneking, <strong>and</strong> A.C. Wilson. MitochondrialDNA <strong>and</strong> human evolution, Nature325:31-36, 1987;■ Vigilant, L., et al. African populations <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>evolution <strong>of</strong> human mitochondrial DNA, Science253:1503-1507, 1991.STRATEGIES FOR TEACHING THE ACTIVITYA chart summarizing <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> this activity isincluded at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Strategies discussion. Itshould help you underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> intent <strong>of</strong> each part<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> activity.Prior to using this module, students must have completeda basic study <strong>of</strong> genetics. Please see Figure 12in <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers for a list <strong>of</strong> fundamentalgenetics concepts; we assume that students arefamiliar with <strong>the</strong>se ideas. Part I <strong>of</strong> Activity 2 reviewsbasic Mendelian inheritance to provide a contrastwith mitochondrial inheritance. If your students arenot familiar with <strong>the</strong> information presented in Figure2-2, or if <strong>the</strong>y are not experienced in using pedigrees,you should teach that basic material beforeusing this activity. For simplicity <strong>of</strong> comparison, wehave maintained <strong>the</strong> same family structure in <strong>the</strong>pedigrees. Ask students to justify <strong>the</strong>ir choices as<strong>the</strong>y assign known patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance to PedigreesA-H.In Activity 2, <strong>the</strong> constructivist approach to teaching isobvious in <strong>the</strong> way students search for a new concept<strong>of</strong> inheritance (mitochondrial inheritance) to explainsome puzzling data. (See Section I <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> O v e rv i e wfor Te a c h e r s for a brief discussion <strong>of</strong> constru c t i v i s tteaching.) Students’ t h i n k i n g is as important as <strong>the</strong>irfinding <strong>the</strong> correct answer. They exercise <strong>the</strong>ir powers<strong>of</strong> observation <strong>and</strong> reasoning as <strong>the</strong>y propose <strong>and</strong> testvarious hypo<strong>the</strong>ses to explain a surprising pattern <strong>of</strong>inheritance. In so doing <strong>the</strong>y experience <strong>the</strong> methods90©1997 by BSCS.


Puzzling Pedigrees ■ Activity 2<strong>of</strong> science. Help make this aspect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> activity overtso that students recognize <strong>the</strong> ways in which <strong>the</strong>y are“doing science.”In Parts II <strong>and</strong> III, allow students to explore possibleexplanations, but insist on carefully stated hypo<strong>the</strong>ses,testing, <strong>and</strong> sound reasoning. Discourage guessing.You can help students distinguish hypo<strong>the</strong>sisformation from guessing by probing for students’justifications <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir responses. “It’s autosomaldominant” could be a scientific hypo<strong>the</strong>sis or a wildguess. If students <strong>of</strong>fer this explanation, ask <strong>the</strong>m todescribe <strong>the</strong> evidence. (This approach also is usefulin <strong>the</strong> review in Part I.)A scientific hypo<strong>the</strong>sis is based on existing observ a-tions, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n it is tested through additional observ a-tion <strong>and</strong> experimentation. Emphasize that sometimeswe make an observation that is not adequatelyexplained by existing concepts; we <strong>the</strong>n must seek anew explanation. In Pa rt III, remind students that it isokay not to have a final answer, but <strong>the</strong>y must document<strong>the</strong>ir observations <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir reasoning carefullyon <strong>the</strong> worksheet in Copymaster 2-4. Students neednot reach <strong>the</strong> explanation <strong>of</strong> mitochondrial inheritanceprior to <strong>the</strong> Analysis. If all students reach <strong>the</strong> correctconclusion prior to completing all <strong>the</strong> steps, skipahead to <strong>the</strong> Analysis, but do insist that students justify<strong>the</strong>ir conclusions. The articles titled “Extra” DNA<strong>and</strong> Fe rtilization: Sperm Meets Ovum (Copymaster 2-5) describe extranuclear DNA as a concept larger than<strong>the</strong> example <strong>of</strong> mitochondrial inheritance alone. Thisconceptual approach will help students contrast what<strong>the</strong>y learn about extranuclear inheritance with <strong>the</strong>more traditional view <strong>of</strong> nuclear inheritance.This activity affords many opportunities to make studentsconscious <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science <strong>the</strong>y areputting to use. For example, <strong>the</strong> suggestions <strong>of</strong>fered inFigure T2-2 (page 96 <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Annotated Student Material)show one way to call students’ attention to <strong>the</strong> methods<strong>of</strong> science at work in <strong>the</strong>ir own h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> mind.Please note that in <strong>the</strong> pedigrees <strong>and</strong> throughout <strong>the</strong>activity we have consciously avoided <strong>the</strong> usual term i-nology <strong>of</strong> “an affected individual” to refer to peoplewho show <strong>the</strong> trait in question. Our reason is that“affected” generally refers to a disorder, <strong>and</strong> all tooo ften students come away from <strong>the</strong>ir study <strong>of</strong> inheritancethinking that genetics is only about genes thatdo not work properly. We want to help students realizethat m o s t genes we inherit are functional, asshown by <strong>the</strong> fact that we are functional organisms. Toaccomplish this awareness, we indicate that a “trait ispresent” instead <strong>of</strong> saying an “individual is affected.”The final emphasis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Analysis is to show how scienceworks. Students reflect on what <strong>the</strong>y have donein this activity <strong>and</strong> add to <strong>the</strong> NMS wall poster begunin Activity 1 (optional; see Activity 1, page 80 for anexplanation).ACTIVITY 2 AT A GLANCEPart I:Part II:Part III:Analysis:Review <strong>of</strong> <strong>Inheritance</strong> PatternsAs a review, students examine Pedigrees A-H <strong>and</strong> identify <strong>the</strong>m according to traditional patterns<strong>of</strong> inheritance with which <strong>the</strong>y are familiar.Puzzling PedigreesStudents examine Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J, which do not fit traditional patterns, <strong>and</strong> recognize <strong>the</strong>need for a new explanation.Testing an ExplanationStudents develop hypo<strong>the</strong>ses to explain <strong>the</strong> puzzling pedigrees. They use a coin toss to test <strong>the</strong>irinitial hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y use new information (science articles) to refine <strong>the</strong>ir explanation.Students discuss how <strong>the</strong>y have used <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science in this activity, thinking abouthow scientific explanations arise.Hints to shorten <strong>the</strong> activity: Assign <strong>the</strong> review <strong>of</strong> Pedigrees A-H as homework, <strong>and</strong> follow this assignment with a brief classdiscussion. Alternatively, have students analyze only four pedigrees for <strong>the</strong> review, including one example <strong>of</strong> each traditionalpattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance.91©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling PedigreesAnnotated Student MaterialSeparate student pages contain <strong>the</strong> material shown in bold typeface.Here, we provide an annotated version <strong>of</strong> student materials to help you conduct <strong>the</strong> activity.How do scientists decide <strong>the</strong> direction for <strong>the</strong>irresearch? For example, scientists want to knowhow genes are involved in cancer. This problemis too large <strong>and</strong> complex to tackle all at once.Scientists, <strong>the</strong>refore, break large problemsinto smaller, focused parts. They propose atestable explanation, called a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, totry to answer one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> smaller questions.These hypo<strong>the</strong>ses show where <strong>and</strong> how tolook for answers.As scientists make observations <strong>and</strong> recorddata, <strong>the</strong>y usually find that <strong>the</strong> new evidencereinforces what <strong>the</strong>y already know. Sometimes,however, <strong>the</strong> new data do not fit what isknown. If <strong>the</strong> evidence is reliable, scientistscannot just ignore it. Instead, <strong>the</strong>y must lookfor new explanations to extend or modify whatis known. In this activity, you will put yourskills <strong>of</strong> observation <strong>and</strong> your previous knowledge<strong>of</strong> heredity to work to study some puzzlingpatterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance.PROCEDUREPart I: Review <strong>of</strong> <strong>Inheritance</strong> Patterns1. D e t e rmine <strong>the</strong> most likely p a t t e rn <strong>of</strong> inheritancefor each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pedigrees shown in Figure2-1. To help you, Figure 2-2 lists <strong>the</strong> characteristics<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> four patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritancethat you studied in <strong>the</strong> genetics unit <strong>of</strong> yourbiology class. Your teacher will direct you towork on this task alone or with a part n e r. Yo uwill have 10 minutes to complete this task.Figure 2-1 Pedigrees showing <strong>the</strong> inheritance <strong>of</strong> eight human traits©1997 by BSCS.92


Puzzling Pedigrees ■ Activity 2Figure 2-2 Four patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritanceAutosomal dominantMales <strong>and</strong> females are equally likely to have <strong>the</strong> trait.Traits do not skip generations (generally).The trait is present whenever <strong>the</strong> corresponding gene ispresent (generally).There is male-to-male transmission.X-linked dominantAll daughters <strong>of</strong> a male who has <strong>the</strong> trait will alsohave <strong>the</strong> trait.There is no male-to-male transmission.A female who has <strong>the</strong> trait may or may not pass <strong>the</strong> genefor that trait to her son or daughter.Autosomal recessiveMales <strong>and</strong> females are equally likely to have <strong>the</strong> trait.Traits <strong>of</strong>ten skip generations.Often, both parents <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring who have <strong>the</strong> trait are heterozygotes(<strong>the</strong>y carry at least one copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> allele).Only homozygous individuals have <strong>the</strong> trait.Traits may appear in siblings without appearing in <strong>the</strong>ir parents.If a parent has <strong>the</strong> trait, those <strong>of</strong>fspring who do not have itare heterozygous carriers <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trait.X-linked recessiveThe trait is far more common in males than in females.All daughters <strong>of</strong> a male who has <strong>the</strong> trait are heterozygouscarriers.The son <strong>of</strong> a female carrier has a 50 percent chance <strong>of</strong> having<strong>the</strong> trait.There is no male-to-male transmission.Mo<strong>the</strong>rs <strong>of</strong> males who have <strong>the</strong> trait are ei<strong>the</strong>r heterozygouscarriers or homozygous <strong>and</strong> express <strong>the</strong> trait.Daughters <strong>of</strong> female carriers have a 50 percent chance <strong>of</strong>being carriers.Note from <strong>the</strong> field test: Students found <strong>the</strong> summary<strong>of</strong> traditional inheritance patterns presented inFigure 2-2 to be very useful. You may want to incorporatethis chart into your earlier study <strong>of</strong> Mendelianinheritance <strong>and</strong> have students refer to it when <strong>the</strong>ydo Activity 2.Decide whe<strong>the</strong>r individual or team work will bemore productive <strong>and</strong>, if appropriate, form teams.Limit <strong>the</strong> work to 10 minutes <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n conduct abrief class discussion so each student can determine<strong>the</strong> accuracy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir work. Projecting an overheadtransparency <strong>of</strong> pedigrees A-H (Copymaster 2-1) mayaid discussion. Insist that students provide evidence<strong>and</strong> reasoning to support <strong>the</strong>ir choices <strong>and</strong> do notjust list <strong>the</strong> answer. Remind students that <strong>the</strong>yshould propose <strong>the</strong> most likely pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance.Some pedigrees may demonstrate more thanone pattern.The most likely patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance for <strong>the</strong> PedigreesA-H are given in Figure T2-1. (Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> Jwill be presented to students in Part II).Part II: Puzzling Pedigrees2. Study two new pedigrees (Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J)shown in Figure 2-3. Both pedigrees illustrate<strong>the</strong> same trait. Try to identify <strong>the</strong> inheritancep a t t e rn illustrated by <strong>the</strong>se two pedigrees.Explain your responses, stating specific examplesto support your explanation.Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J demonstrate mitochondrial inheritance(Figure T2-1). To help with <strong>the</strong> discussion,you might want to project an overhead transparency<strong>of</strong> Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J (Copymaster 2-2). Make certainthat students underst<strong>and</strong> that both pedigreesshow <strong>the</strong> same trait. Solicit a variety <strong>of</strong> responses.Figure T2-1 Pa t t e rns <strong>of</strong> inheritance for <strong>the</strong> pedigreesA & GD & FB & EC & HI & Jautosomal dominantautosomal recessiveX-linked dominantX-linked recessivemitochondrial (presented later)93©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling PedigreesFigure 2-3 Two puzzling pedigreesPedigree J should be a real puzzle to <strong>the</strong> studentsbecause <strong>the</strong>re is no obvious connection to <strong>the</strong> patte rns in Figure 2-2. Students might identify Pe d i g r e eI as autosomal dominant. If not, ask a few questionsso that students propose this pattern. For example:“Does <strong>the</strong> trait skip generations?” (Students maysay it does in pedigree J; actually, it vanishes.) “Do<strong>the</strong> parents <strong>of</strong> individuals with <strong>the</strong> trait have <strong>the</strong> trait<strong>the</strong>mselves?” (Female parents have <strong>the</strong> trait.) Alternativelyy o u could propose autosomal dominantinheritance as a challenge to students.NOTE: These data do not rule out a dominant,maternal-effect allele, but students are unlikely tosuggest this explanation.Part III: Testing an ExplanationHow good is <strong>the</strong> explanation <strong>of</strong> inheritanceyou suggested in Part II? When you suggestedan inheritance pattern, you were making ahypo<strong>the</strong>sis. A scientific hypo<strong>the</strong>sis is a testableexplanation. To determine how good yourhypo<strong>the</strong>sis is, use evidence to test it.3. Analyze <strong>the</strong> data (evidence) you haveobserved from <strong>the</strong> pedigrees to see whe<strong>the</strong>r<strong>the</strong>y support or conflict with your hypo<strong>the</strong>sis.(Your teacher will provide a worksheetfor your convenience.) Record your explanationfor <strong>the</strong> inheritance patterns in PedigreesI <strong>and</strong> J as “Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 1.” (Rememberthat both pedigrees demonstrate <strong>the</strong> sametrait.) Next, list evidence that supports yourexplanation <strong>and</strong> evidence that conflicts withit. Refer specifically to Pedigree I or J as youreport this evidence.Distribute a copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> worksheet (Copymaster 2-4)to each student to help <strong>the</strong>m organize <strong>the</strong>ir explanations.If students choose autosomal dominant, conflictingevidence includes <strong>the</strong> fact that all six children<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> second generation in Pedigree I have <strong>the</strong> trait.This situation could suggest that <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r ishomozygous for <strong>the</strong> trait. In that case, however,Pedigree J does not fit <strong>the</strong> same pattern.4. You can reason, calculate, or do experimentsto test a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. For example, assumethat <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> first generation <strong>of</strong>Pedigree I is heterozygous. For a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis<strong>of</strong> “autosomal dominant”:a. Consider each child singly. What is <strong>the</strong>94©1997 by BSCS.


Puzzling Pedigrees ■ Activity 2probability that <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r will transmit<strong>the</strong> gene in question? (You can calculate <strong>the</strong>probability based on what you know aboutautosomal dominant inheritance.)The probability is 1/2 (50%) for each conception.b. You actually can model <strong>the</strong> events in <strong>the</strong>pedigree <strong>and</strong> use <strong>the</strong> data to test thishypo<strong>the</strong>sis. Use a coin to represent <strong>the</strong>two alleles involved in this trait. Heads willrepresent <strong>the</strong> allele responsible for <strong>the</strong>trait. Tails will represent <strong>the</strong> allele thatdoes not produce <strong>the</strong> trait. Model <strong>the</strong>inheritance shown in <strong>the</strong> second generationby tossing <strong>the</strong> coin six times, once foreach child. Record <strong>the</strong> results, using ac h a rt like <strong>the</strong> one illustrated in Figure 2-4.Occasionally, a student will get six heads in <strong>the</strong> cointoss. If so, <strong>the</strong> following analysis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> third generationshould clarify this point. Lead a class discussionthat ties <strong>the</strong> model <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> coin toss to <strong>the</strong> proposedgenotypes based on <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>and</strong> to <strong>the</strong> actualphenotypes shown in <strong>the</strong> second generation <strong>of</strong>Pedigree I. The data likely will show that <strong>the</strong>re is onlya small chance—(1/2) 6 or 1/64—that all six children in<strong>the</strong> second generation <strong>of</strong> Pedigree I will inherit <strong>the</strong>allele responsible for <strong>the</strong> trait, as would be true forautosomal dominant inheritance.Record results from each toss in <strong>the</strong> appropriate column.heads(allele for trait is present)tails(allele for trait is absent)Reminder: In autosomal dominant inheritance, if <strong>the</strong>allele is present, <strong>the</strong> trait will be present.Figure 2-4 A coin-toss modeling experimentIf you want to emphasize <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> samplesize for accurate data, or in case a student gets sixheads, you may choose to analyze <strong>the</strong> next generationfor additional data. Do this with Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong>J or use Pedigrees I´ <strong>and</strong> J´ (Copymaster 2-3), whichshow an extra generation.This analysis can best be done family by family sothat students can see <strong>the</strong> pattern in terms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sex<strong>of</strong> each parent for that generation. Again, data from<strong>the</strong> coin toss should refute <strong>the</strong> likelihood that <strong>the</strong>observed phenotypes result from autosomal dominantinheritance. This conclusion forces <strong>the</strong> studentsto return to <strong>the</strong> original problem because ourpresent knowledge <strong>of</strong> inheritance patterns does notprovide an explanation for inheritance shown in<strong>the</strong>se pedigrees.c. Your coin toss gave you empirical(observable) data. You also can calculate<strong>the</strong> likelihood that all six children inherited<strong>the</strong> trait from <strong>the</strong> female parent.(Hint: Recall your answer to Question4a, <strong>and</strong> remember that <strong>the</strong> probabilityfor each child is based on a separateevent. Use <strong>the</strong> product rule, based on <strong>the</strong>second law <strong>of</strong> probability, to find <strong>the</strong>probability for all six children.)If students are familiar with <strong>the</strong> second law <strong>of</strong> probability,<strong>the</strong>y should be able to calculate <strong>the</strong> probabilityas (1/2) 6 or 1/64. If <strong>the</strong>y are not familiar with thislaw, explain it to <strong>the</strong>m <strong>and</strong> let <strong>the</strong>m do <strong>the</strong> calculations.(The second law <strong>of</strong> probability states that <strong>the</strong>probability that independent events will occurtoge<strong>the</strong>r is <strong>the</strong> product <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir individual probabilities.)After discussing <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> coin tosses <strong>and</strong> what<strong>the</strong>y may mean, you may find it helpful to review what<strong>the</strong> students have done to this point. Emphasize that<strong>the</strong>y have used several important methods <strong>of</strong> sciencein <strong>the</strong>ir work, as shown in Figure T2-2, on page 96.5. Suppose that <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r exhibiting <strong>the</strong> traitin <strong>the</strong> first generation <strong>of</strong> Pedigree I ishomozygous for a dominant trait. Wouldthat explanation be consistent with <strong>the</strong>results you observed in her <strong>of</strong>fspring (<strong>the</strong>second generation)? Use <strong>the</strong> data from <strong>the</strong>coin test to support your conclusion. Whatabout <strong>the</strong> results in <strong>the</strong> third generation <strong>of</strong>Pedigree I?95©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling PedigreesFigure T2-2 <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> science used in Activity 2■ Students may have noticed that Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J donot fit any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> four classic patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance,<strong>and</strong> thus <strong>the</strong>y identified a problem or question.■ The examination <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pedigrees is an example <strong>of</strong>observation.■ The proposal that Pedigree I could illustrate autosomaldominant inheritance is a testable hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. (Studentsmay have o<strong>the</strong>r hypo<strong>the</strong>ses as well.)■ Coin tossing is designed to test this hypo<strong>the</strong>sis by col -lecting data.■ Coin tossing is an example <strong>of</strong> a model system.The observation that all six <strong>of</strong>fspring in <strong>the</strong> secondgeneration show <strong>the</strong> trait is consistent with autosomaldominant inheritance if <strong>the</strong> first-generationmo<strong>the</strong>r is homozygous. However, <strong>the</strong> results in <strong>the</strong>third generation are inconsistent with this explanationbecause <strong>of</strong>fspring <strong>of</strong> males who have <strong>the</strong> trait donot inherit <strong>and</strong> express it. To be certain, however,more examples (a larger sample size) would be helpful.Once again, you may want to use Pedigree I´.6. Examine Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J again carefully<strong>and</strong> summarize your observations on yourworksheet.Students may use <strong>the</strong> additional data in Pedigrees I´<strong>and</strong> J´ if you have introduced this material.a. Indicate whe<strong>the</strong>r your tests confirm orrefute <strong>the</strong> pattern you hypo<strong>the</strong>sized.Record your response as your “conclusions.”(Hint: Your conclusion could be in<strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> a question.)b. If your earlier choice was refuted, canyou now propose a new explanation? Ifso, record it under “Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 2” onyour worksheet. If not, do not worry.Steps 7-9 may help you.In Pedigree J, only <strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> first generationhas <strong>the</strong> trait. None <strong>of</strong> his children or gr<strong>and</strong>childrenhas it. In Pedigree I, <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> first generationhas <strong>the</strong> trait <strong>and</strong> all <strong>of</strong> her children have it. In <strong>the</strong>third generation <strong>of</strong> Pedigree I, all children <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> second-generationfemales have <strong>the</strong> trait. None <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>children <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> second-generation males has <strong>the</strong>trait.7. Use Questions 7a <strong>and</strong> 7b to help you find anew explanation:a. Examine each instance where <strong>the</strong> trait ispassed to <strong>of</strong>fspring. What do you observeabout <strong>the</strong> source <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> inherited trait?The source is <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r.b. Mendelian genetics assumes that in allcases each parent contributes equally to<strong>the</strong> genotype <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring. Do <strong>the</strong> datashown in Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J demonstratethis idea?No. Based on <strong>the</strong>se pedigrees it appears that <strong>the</strong> traitshows up more <strong>of</strong>ten if it is inherited from <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r;<strong>of</strong> course, <strong>the</strong>se data are limited.Modify your conclusions or propose a newhypo<strong>the</strong>sis if your ideas have changed.At this point, do not expect students to have a completeexplanation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mechanism <strong>the</strong>y are observing.A good response might be:O b s e rv a t i o n : The trait in each pedigree shows upwith surprising frequency in later generations. It is to<strong>of</strong>requent in I <strong>and</strong> too rare in J to be explained easily.Conclusion: There may be some unknown inheritancepattern or process at work here that makesthis allele transfer from <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r only. (Or, statedas a question, “What mechanisms <strong>of</strong> inheritancewould result in a pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritance exclusivelyfrom <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r?”)As a final attempt to build a new explanation,use Questions 8 <strong>and</strong> 9 to guide your thinking.When you have recorded your best explanation,continue to <strong>the</strong> Analysis. (Your explanationmay be incomplete. What is important isthat you justify your reasoning with evidence.)8. Could <strong>the</strong> inheritance <strong>of</strong> an allele carried on<strong>the</strong> X or Y chromosome explain <strong>the</strong> pattern<strong>of</strong> inheritance in Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J? Explainyour response.Females have <strong>the</strong> trait, so <strong>the</strong> allele is not carried on<strong>the</strong> Y chromosome. Pedigree J shows no transmission<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene in question; if <strong>the</strong> allele were on96©1997 by BSCS.


Puzzling Pedigrees ■ Activity 2<strong>the</strong> X chromosome, you would predict presence <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> trait in <strong>of</strong>fspring unless <strong>the</strong> inheritance wererecessive. In <strong>the</strong> latter case, <strong>the</strong> pattern shown inPedigree I is not likely: too many <strong>of</strong>fspring have <strong>the</strong>t r a i t .9. Consider <strong>the</strong> four patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance inFigure 2-2. What do <strong>the</strong>y assume about <strong>the</strong>location <strong>of</strong> genetic material in <strong>the</strong> cell?These patterns result from <strong>the</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong> geneticmaterial in <strong>the</strong> nucleus.a. In humans, sperm cells <strong>and</strong> egg cellstransmit genetic information to <strong>of</strong>fspring.How do <strong>the</strong>se cells differ?Discussion should focus eventually on one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>major differences between <strong>the</strong> egg <strong>and</strong> sperm: size.An egg cell is about 1,000 times larger than a spermcell. An egg consists <strong>of</strong> a nucleus <strong>and</strong> a considerableamount <strong>of</strong> cytoplasm. The sperm essentially is anucleus with a tail. The illustration included in <strong>the</strong>news article, Fe rtilization: Sperm Meets Ovum(Copymaster 2-5), will help establish this point whenyou distribute copies later in <strong>the</strong> activity.b. In addition to a complete set <strong>of</strong> chromosomes(46), what does <strong>the</strong> developingzygote need to grow <strong>and</strong> develop? (Hint:What structures are in <strong>the</strong> cytoplasm?)A constant source <strong>of</strong> energy is necessary for growth<strong>and</strong> differentiation. Do not be too concerned if studentsdo not mention all <strong>the</strong> organelles <strong>and</strong> structures,but <strong>the</strong>y should mention mitochondria.ANALYSISDistribute Copymaster 2-5, which includes two sciencearticles to be used in <strong>the</strong> Analysis. Have studentsread <strong>the</strong> introductory paragraph <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Analysis. If you are approaching <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> a classperiod, assign <strong>the</strong> questions as homework. Ask <strong>the</strong>students to respond to <strong>the</strong> questions in <strong>the</strong> Analysis.Follow this assignment with a brief discussionto address any problems students have underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>the</strong> mechanism involved in mitochondriali n h e r i t a n c e .1. Read <strong>the</strong> two articles your teacher distributes.What bearing does <strong>the</strong> information in<strong>the</strong> articles have on your explanation <strong>of</strong>Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J?If students have not already identified mitochondrialinheritance as <strong>the</strong> explanation for Pedigrees I<strong>and</strong> J, <strong>the</strong>y should do so now. The first art i c l e ,“Extra” DNA, informs students that organellessuch as mitochondria <strong>and</strong> chloroplasts contain<strong>the</strong>ir own DNA. The second article, Fe rt i l i z a t i o n :S p e rm Meets Ovum, reminds students that only<strong>the</strong> maternal mitochondria survive in <strong>the</strong> zygote.Combined, <strong>the</strong>se ideas should lead students to <strong>the</strong>explanation <strong>of</strong> mitochondrial inheritance for Pe d i-grees I <strong>and</strong> J.If <strong>the</strong>y have made this explanation earlier, <strong>the</strong>se articlesprovide additional support.2. What lasting scientific knowledge aboutinheritance did you use in this activity?Sample student responses include:■ Parents contribute genetic material to <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>of</strong>fspring.■ Alleles <strong>of</strong> one gene segregate in <strong>the</strong> formation <strong>of</strong>gametes.■ A special pair <strong>of</strong> chromosomes determines sexin humans.■ DNA carries genetic information. This is a lastingexplanation that is part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>mitochondrial inheritance.■ Mutations change <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> DNA.■ Some traits show dominant inheritance whilesome show recessive inheritance.3. What new explanations for inheritance didyou use in this activity?Sample student responses include:■ Mitochondria have <strong>the</strong>ir own DNA (mtDNA).■ Some human traits result from <strong>the</strong> action <strong>of</strong>genes in mtDNA.■ There is genetic material (DNA) in addition tothat in <strong>the</strong> nucleus (extranuclear inheritance).■ Some traits are inherited from <strong>the</strong> maternalsource exclusively.■ Mitochondria are inherited maternally.Help students view mitochondrial inheritance as anexample <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> broader concept that genetic materialis not limited to <strong>the</strong> nucleus.97©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling Pedigrees4. What methods <strong>of</strong> science did you use in thisactivity?Sample student responses include:■ I used observation, as in <strong>the</strong> work with <strong>the</strong> pedigrees<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> coin toss.■ I acquired evidence (pedigree, coin toss, articleon mitochondrial DNA).■ I tested new evidence against existing explanations.■ I formulated a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis.■ I modeled an experiment about like l i h o o d(coin toss).98©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveriesin ScienceNMS CONCEPTSExplanations that do not account for new evidenceare inadequate; careful observation <strong>and</strong> data collectionprovide credible evidence; new work builds onold work; cultural influences can affect scientificresearch.GENETICS CONCEPTSStudents use traditional Mendelian patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance<strong>and</strong> learn that gene alteration can occur duringinheritance. They learn that unstable trinucleotiderepeats help to explain genetic anticipation.FOCUSIn this activity, students encounter genetic anticipationin <strong>the</strong> human genetic disorders Huntington disease(HD) <strong>and</strong> myotonic dystrophy (DM) <strong>and</strong> discoverthat traditional genetics concepts do notreadily explain this phenomenon. Students <strong>the</strong>n useevidence at several levels to demonstrate anticipation<strong>and</strong> to provide an explanation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> geneticmechanisms that underlie <strong>the</strong> process. The activity isdesigned as a mystery, where students use clues tosolve <strong>the</strong> mystery <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation. Studentsreflect on (1) <strong>the</strong> connection between evidence <strong>and</strong>explanation in describing causal processes; (2) <strong>the</strong>lasting yet new nature <strong>of</strong> our genetics knowledge;<strong>and</strong> (3) <strong>the</strong> historical context <strong>of</strong> scientific underst<strong>and</strong>ing.OBJECTIVESAs students complete this activity, <strong>the</strong>y should1. develop a brief historical perspective <strong>of</strong> ourunderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease <strong>and</strong>myotonic dystrophy <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir impact on families;2. acknowledge <strong>the</strong> lasting ability <strong>of</strong> Mendelian <strong>and</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r traditional genetics concepts to explainmost <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetic processes involved in <strong>the</strong>sedisorders;3. use case-history evidence, pedigree construction,<strong>and</strong> new molecular data to demonstrate geneticanticipation <strong>and</strong> to construct an explanation for itsunderlying mechanism (instability <strong>of</strong> trinucleotiderepeat sequences in genes associated with Huntingtondisease <strong>and</strong> myotonic dystrophy);4. recognize <strong>and</strong> experience <strong>the</strong> cooperative nature<strong>of</strong> scientific research by reviewing <strong>the</strong> historical,step-wise construction <strong>of</strong> genetics explanations<strong>and</strong> by cooperating with o<strong>the</strong>r students to solve<strong>the</strong> mystery; <strong>and</strong>5. think about how our evolving underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> gene influences our perceptions <strong>of</strong> health <strong>and</strong>disease.99


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceESTIMATED TIME75-100 minutesPREPARATION AND MATERIALSStudents will work in 8 teams. You will need to provide<strong>the</strong> following:■ 1 copy <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 3-1, HD Clues PresentingFamilial Relationships <strong>and</strong> Health Data, cutapart to distribute to each team as indicated■ 8 copies <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 3-2, HD Clues PresentingMolecular Data (1 copy per team)■ 1 overhead transparency <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 3-3,Template for <strong>the</strong> HD Pedigree (optional)■ 8 copies <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 3-3 (1 copy per team)■ 1 copy per student <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 3-4, ScienceArticle about a <strong>Genetics</strong> Discovery■ 8 copies <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 3-5, DM Clues Presenting(A) Familial Relationships <strong>and</strong> Health; <strong>and</strong>(B) Molecular Data (1 copy <strong>of</strong> parts A <strong>and</strong> B perteam)■ 8 copies <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 3-6, Template for <strong>the</strong> DMPedigree (1 copy per team)■ 8 copies <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 3-7, Completed Pedigrees(optional; 1 copy per team)■ 8 copies <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 3-8, H i s t o ry <strong>of</strong> HuntingtonDisease - Vi g n e t t e s(optional; 1 copy per team)Students will work in eight teams <strong>of</strong> approximatelyfour each. Students will assemble <strong>the</strong> Huntingtondisease pedigree in a jigsaw fashion as a class activity,using clues contributed from <strong>the</strong> eight teams. Youwill need one complete set <strong>of</strong> familial <strong>and</strong> healthclue slips for HD (Copymaster 3-1) distributed asindicated among <strong>the</strong> teams, <strong>and</strong> one copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>molecular clues for HD (Copymaster 3-2) for eachteam. The molecular clues can be cut into separatestrips for each student in a team or used as a singlesheet that <strong>the</strong> team consults. You may want to laminate<strong>the</strong> clues so that <strong>the</strong>y can be reused easily.For <strong>the</strong> myotonic dystrophy pedigree, we suggestthat you have each team use a complete set <strong>of</strong> familialhealth clues <strong>and</strong> molecular data (Copymaster 3-5)<strong>and</strong> quickly put toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> pedigree as a team. Youcan cut clues apart so that each student in <strong>the</strong> teamworks with several clues, or you can use <strong>the</strong> clues asa single sheet that <strong>the</strong> team consults. If you like, providea template for each pedigree (Copymasters 3-3<strong>and</strong> 3-6) to help students organize <strong>the</strong> data. You alsomay find it useful to make a transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> templatesso that you can coordinate <strong>the</strong> class discussion.We have provided <strong>the</strong> completed HD <strong>and</strong> DMpedigrees in Copymaster 3-7 for your convenience<strong>and</strong> in <strong>the</strong> event you wish to shorten this activity bystarting with completed pedigrees.COPYMASTERS USED IN THIS ACTIVITYCopymaster 3-1HD Clues Presenting Familial Relationships<strong>and</strong> Health DataCopymaster 3-2HD Clues Presenting Molecular DataCopymaster 3-3Template for <strong>the</strong> HD Pedigree (optional)Copymaster 3-4Science Article about a <strong>Genetics</strong> DiscoveryCopymaster 3-5DM Clues Presenting ( A ) Familial Re l a t i o n s h i p s<strong>and</strong> Health; <strong>and</strong> (B) Molecular DataCopymaster 3-6Template for <strong>the</strong> DM Pedigree (optional)Copymaster 3-7Completed Pedigrees (optional)Copymaster 3-8History <strong>of</strong> Huntington Disease - Vignettes(optional)INTRODUCTIONGenetic anticipation is a term introduced in 1911 byF. W. Mott to describe earlier onset <strong>of</strong> symptoms orgreater severity <strong>of</strong> symptoms in subsequent generationswith certain genetic disorders. This phenomenonprovides a powerful example <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sequential nature<strong>of</strong> scientific explanation <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> relationship betweenexisting explanations <strong>and</strong> new evidence. New evidenceo ften challenges existing explanations. For example,scientists observed an unusual feature <strong>of</strong> severalhuman genetic disorders, including Huntington disease<strong>and</strong> myotonic dystrophy, that is, <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> onsetor severity <strong>of</strong> symptoms <strong>of</strong>ten shifted in subsequentgenerations <strong>of</strong> families with <strong>the</strong>se disorders. Many100©1997 by BSCS.


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3years passed, however, before sufficient evidence accumulatedto establish <strong>the</strong> phenomenon clearly. Oneproblem was that traditional Mendelian genetics didnot explain genetic anticipation. A better explanationhad to await <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> new technologies thatwould contribute additional lines <strong>of</strong> evidence, a situationcommon in scientific investigations.In <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation, an initial debatecentered on whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> evidence from <strong>the</strong> casehistories represented a legitimate biological phenomenonor resulted from bias in data collection.Given <strong>the</strong> limitations on collecting data from <strong>and</strong>about humans, one must be especially careful toavoid bias in <strong>the</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong> case histories. Earlyideas about anticipation appear in work by <strong>the</strong> nineteenthcentury “psychiatrist” Benedict Au g u s t eMorel (<strong>the</strong> term psychiatrist was not <strong>the</strong>n in use).He spoke <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> degeneration, in whichprogressive severity <strong>and</strong> earlier onset <strong>of</strong> an illnessfrom parent to <strong>of</strong>fspring was seen. (See McInnis,1996, for an excellent review <strong>of</strong> early ideas aboutanticipation.) Even when <strong>the</strong>re was enough evidenceto confirm <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation,existing genetics concepts did not explain howthis anticipation could occur. With <strong>the</strong> development<strong>of</strong> sophisticated molecular techniques, however, scientistsidentified <strong>the</strong> fundamental molecular mechanismsassociated with this phenomenon. The genesin several disorders that display anticipation havebeen shown to contain unstable stretches <strong>of</strong> a particulartrinucleotide repeated many times. (See page43 in Section V <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h e r s for amore detailed discussion.)Transmission <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genes associated with HD or DMmay result in a change in <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> repeats.There is a threshold level at which instabilitybecomes marked; when <strong>the</strong> trinucleotide expansionreaches a critical level, severe phenotypic effectsresult. For example, individuals with more than 39copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trinucleotide repeat CAG found in <strong>the</strong>mutant HD gene are scored as positive on genetictests; most likely, <strong>the</strong>y will develop <strong>the</strong> disorder,unless <strong>the</strong>y die from some o<strong>the</strong>r cause first. When<strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene has many more than 50 repeats,<strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> onset is likely to be earlier than in caseswith a smaller number <strong>of</strong> repeats. In addition, <strong>the</strong>reis evidence that <strong>the</strong> parent <strong>of</strong> origin makes some differencein <strong>the</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene. Theage <strong>of</strong> onset is somewhat earlier if <strong>the</strong> gene is inheritedpaternally (Ranen, et al. Anticipation <strong>and</strong> instability<strong>of</strong> IT-15 (CAG)N repeats in parent-<strong>of</strong>fspringpairs with Huntington Disease. The American Journal<strong>of</strong> Human <strong>Genetics</strong> 57(3):593-602, 1995). Apparently,<strong>the</strong> expansion in repeats is more likely to begreater with transmission from <strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>r than from<strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r.Genetic anticipation in DM results in a change in <strong>the</strong>age <strong>of</strong> onset, <strong>and</strong> in a dramatic difference in symptomsfrom one generation to ano<strong>the</strong>r. The unstablerepeats in <strong>the</strong> mutant DM gene contain <strong>the</strong> trinucleotideCTG. As <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> CTG repeats reaches50-80, mild symptoms, including cataracts, appear fairlylate in life. The repeat number can reach <strong>the</strong> thous<strong>and</strong>s,<strong>and</strong> severe symptoms can appear from birt h ,including muscle disfunction <strong>and</strong> mental retardation.Modification <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> traditional view <strong>of</strong> dominantinheritance to include anticipation may seem quiteremarkable, but scientists have continued to add to<strong>and</strong> modify <strong>the</strong>ir underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> genetics since<strong>the</strong> early twentieth century. When Mendel’s lawswere rediscovered in 1900, <strong>the</strong> factors <strong>of</strong> inheritancewere viewed as particulate <strong>and</strong> fixed so thatoutcomes from any given genotype would be verys i m i l a r, if not identical. Over <strong>the</strong> years, we havel e a rned that a variety <strong>of</strong> nongenetic factors canaffect <strong>the</strong> phenotype. Environment, for example,o ften influences gene expression. Organisms reach<strong>the</strong>ir full potential height, based on genotype, onlyif placed in an environment where nutritional <strong>and</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r environmental factors are optimal. Ano<strong>the</strong>rinfluence on gene expression results from genesthat play a role in <strong>the</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r genes. Fo rexample, some inherited anemias may be lesssevere in individuals with counterbalancing geneswhose expression increases <strong>the</strong> production <strong>of</strong> redblood cells. Fi n a l l y, <strong>the</strong>re is a strong element <strong>of</strong>chance in <strong>the</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> many genes in <strong>the</strong> form<strong>of</strong> environmental factors or o<strong>the</strong>r elements we donot yet underst<strong>and</strong> thoroughly. The genotype establishes<strong>the</strong> limits <strong>of</strong> a given phenotype, but <strong>the</strong> ultimatephenotype reflects a variety <strong>of</strong> influences.Some differences in <strong>the</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> traits appearas phenomena known as penetrance <strong>and</strong> variable101©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Scienceexpressivity. Penetrance refers to <strong>the</strong> proportion <strong>of</strong>individuals with a given genotype who express <strong>the</strong>associated phenotype. Penetrance is an all-or-nonephenomenon; one ei<strong>the</strong>r expresses <strong>the</strong> trait or doesnot. If every individual with a particular genotype ina given population expresses <strong>the</strong> trait in question,<strong>the</strong> trait is said to be completely penetrant. If lessthan 100 percent <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> people with that genotypeexpress <strong>the</strong> trait, <strong>the</strong> trait is incompletely penetrant.Variable expressivity refers to <strong>the</strong> range <strong>of</strong> physicaleffects <strong>and</strong> to <strong>the</strong> timing <strong>of</strong> expression for <strong>the</strong> characteristics<strong>of</strong> a genetic disorder. Environmental variables,o<strong>the</strong>r genes, <strong>and</strong> chance may influenceexpressivity <strong>and</strong> penetrance; <strong>the</strong>se factors can cloudour ability to make exact predictions about <strong>the</strong> likelihood<strong>of</strong> particular outcomes <strong>of</strong> genotypes. Observationsrelated to penetrance <strong>and</strong> variable expressivityrequired modifications <strong>of</strong> early Mendelianmodels. Although geneticists now take penetrance<strong>and</strong> variable expressivity for granted, <strong>the</strong>y were asremarkable at <strong>the</strong> time <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir discovery as arerecent discoveries such as unstable trinucleotiderepeats or genomic imprinting. Section V <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Overview for Teachers provides an extensive discussion<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> changing concepts <strong>of</strong> inheritance.For a more detailed explanation <strong>of</strong> HD <strong>and</strong> DM, <strong>the</strong>irclinical aspects, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> discovery <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> molecularmechanisms, also see <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers,Section V, pages 43-49. In addition, <strong>the</strong> first genomemodule from BSCS, Mapping <strong>and</strong> Sequencing <strong>the</strong>Human Genome: Science, Ethics, <strong>and</strong> Public Policy,has an extensive description <strong>of</strong> HD.One final consideration is an examination <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>methods for establishing causation in a scientificexplanation. Consider a simple analogy. You go toyour car one morning <strong>and</strong> notice <strong>the</strong> tire is flat—that is an observation. You examine <strong>the</strong> tire <strong>and</strong> finda large nail embedded in it. The evidence suggests acause for <strong>the</strong> flat, because you can show <strong>the</strong> presence<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nail <strong>and</strong> because sound reasoning (<strong>and</strong>perhaps <strong>the</strong> experience from previous flats) shows amechanism by which a nail could cause <strong>the</strong> flat—itcreates a hole through which air escapes. Unfort u-n a t e l y, while <strong>the</strong> principle is <strong>the</strong> same, showing acausal relationship in science <strong>of</strong>ten is not so easy. Inthis activity, students consider <strong>the</strong> relationshipbetween a large number <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> manifestation <strong>of</strong> a genetic disorder. Thea s s o c i a t i o n — l i ke <strong>the</strong> nail in <strong>the</strong> tire—begins toestablish cause, but <strong>the</strong> reasoning to show how <strong>the</strong>presence <strong>of</strong> repeats causes disease is not yet established.A more extensive discussion <strong>of</strong> causality isincluded in Section III <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h e r s,page 14.STRATEGIES FOR TEACHING THE ACTIVITYIn this activity, <strong>the</strong> reasoning process is primary. Studentsuse evidence to justify <strong>and</strong> to modify scientificexplanations. Students must propose answers <strong>and</strong>ideas, but <strong>the</strong>y also must j u s t i fy how <strong>the</strong>y arrived at<strong>the</strong>ir conclusions. Students should see that,although <strong>the</strong> explanation for a new genetic mechanismmust be added to <strong>the</strong> existing body <strong>of</strong> geneticsknowledge to account for genetic anticipation, ourfundamental underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> dominant inheritanceis lasting <strong>and</strong> explains most o<strong>the</strong>r features <strong>of</strong>Huntington disease <strong>and</strong> myotonic dystrophy. In thisa c t i v i t y, students build <strong>the</strong>ir explanation <strong>of</strong> geneticanticipation in stages. First, <strong>the</strong>y use pedigree evidenceto support <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> anticipation.Next, <strong>the</strong>y use molecular evidence to suggest <strong>the</strong>underlying mechanism.This procedure <strong>of</strong>fers opportunities for you to makeexplicit to students several key aspects <strong>of</strong> scientificinvestigations. Emphasize that observational data,such as <strong>the</strong> information from case histories or pedigrees,are important scientific evidence. Many studentsmistakenly think <strong>of</strong> experiments as <strong>the</strong> onlysource <strong>of</strong> useful data, but in studies <strong>of</strong> human genetics,scientists are limited in <strong>the</strong> types <strong>of</strong> experiments<strong>the</strong>y can do using human subjects. People marry <strong>and</strong>have children with whomever <strong>the</strong>y choose, so usefulobservation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> resultant patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritanceis particularly important.The activity begins as an inquiry patterned after amystery <strong>and</strong> is structured to demonstrate <strong>the</strong> needto use information in a precise manner. The mysteryapproach <strong>and</strong> use <strong>of</strong> clue slips serve mainly to make<strong>the</strong> activity fun <strong>and</strong> to arouse interest, although thisstructure does provide some aspects that modelhow science is done. For example, <strong>the</strong> jigsawapproach used for building <strong>the</strong> HD pedigree reflects<strong>the</strong> need for communication among scientists <strong>and</strong>102©1997 by BSCS.


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3gives students some practice with quantitative dataas <strong>the</strong>y work out dates <strong>of</strong> birth, death, <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rcharacteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> people in <strong>the</strong> pedigrees. However,if you prefer not to devote class time to this initialgame-like process, skip Steps 2-4 in Part II, <strong>and</strong>move ahead by distributing <strong>the</strong> completed HD <strong>and</strong>DM pedigrees (Copymaster 3-7).The mystery approach may seem a bit unstru c t u r e d<strong>and</strong> even noisy at first, <strong>and</strong> it is important to keep studentsfocused on <strong>the</strong> task. We recommend that youkeep an open, somewhat unstructured style so that<strong>the</strong> opening steps (Pa rt II, Steps 2-3) have <strong>the</strong> feel <strong>of</strong>a game. In part i c u l a r, in Step 2, give individual teamsonly 1-2 minutes to see how <strong>the</strong>ir clues could fittoge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>and</strong>, perhaps, to suggest a pedigree as a usefulway to organize data. You may want to wait untilstudents make <strong>the</strong> suggestion to build a pedigree(possibly prompted by a question from you abouthow to organize clues) before you h<strong>and</strong> out <strong>the</strong> templates.Keep assembly <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pedigree an active <strong>and</strong>exciting aspect <strong>of</strong> this part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> procedure. You canhelp keep a quick <strong>and</strong> fun pace by asking questionsabout specific relationships in <strong>the</strong> pedigree. For example,ask “Who are <strong>the</strong>se twins?” or “Who is Andrew’ sfa<strong>the</strong>r?” Suggestions for subtle guidance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> inquiryappear at several steps in <strong>the</strong> Procedure. You also canuse this part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> procedure as an opportunity tochallenge students to read, reason, <strong>and</strong> calculate carefullyas <strong>the</strong>y determine <strong>the</strong> details <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pedigree.Students build an explanation <strong>of</strong> anticipation in <strong>the</strong>following sequence:■The opening dialogue establishes <strong>the</strong> mystery <strong>of</strong>whe<strong>the</strong>r anticipation is real or a function <strong>of</strong> biasin observation.■ The health data in <strong>the</strong> HD pedigree support <strong>the</strong>existence <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation in this disorder.■ The molecular data in <strong>the</strong> pedigree for HD<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> science article about trinucleotiderepeat expansion suggest a mechanism fora n t i c i p a t i o n .■ The data for <strong>the</strong> DM pedigree supply an additionalline <strong>of</strong> evidence.■ The vignettes provide a historical context fordiscovery about genetic anticipation (optional).“Activity 3 at a Glance” follows <strong>and</strong> it summarizes <strong>the</strong>structure <strong>and</strong> goals <strong>of</strong> each part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> activity. Noticethat <strong>the</strong> information available at each major step isseparated by 20 years <strong>of</strong> research in genetics.We have separated <strong>the</strong> examination <strong>of</strong> clues into twophases to correspond to two discoveries: identifying<strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation, <strong>and</strong> identifyingevidence for <strong>the</strong> underlying molecular mechanism.In this way, <strong>the</strong> activity has <strong>the</strong> essence <strong>of</strong> an openinquiry while <strong>of</strong>fering some guidance to help studentsmake <strong>the</strong>se discoveries. For <strong>the</strong> activity to beeffective, it is essential that students distinguish<strong>the</strong>se two levels <strong>of</strong> discovery.ACTIVITY 3 AT A GLANCEPart I:Part II:Identifying <strong>the</strong> MysteryStep 1: Students read a dialogue set in 1957.Outcome: Students recognize <strong>the</strong> mystery <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation.Ga<strong>the</strong>ring Clues to <strong>the</strong> MysterySteps 2-4: Students in each team receive clues about familial relationships <strong>and</strong> generalhealth in <strong>the</strong> HD family. Students build a pedigree to analyze <strong>the</strong> clues.Outcome: Students recognize evidence for genetic anticipation. (They lack evidence for<strong>the</strong> underlying mechanism.)Steps 5-7: Students in each team receive additional clues that provide molecular data aboutmembers <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HD family.Outcome: Students identify evidence for a genetic mechanism that causes genetic anticipation(unstable trinucleotide repeats). This evidence also streng<strong>the</strong>ns <strong>the</strong> case for genetic anticipation.103©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceACTIVITY 3 AT A GLANCEStep 8: Students repeat <strong>the</strong> process for a DM family.Outcome: Additional evidence streng<strong>the</strong>ns <strong>the</strong> students’ explanations.Part III:Analysis:Discovering <strong>the</strong> History <strong>of</strong> Genetic Anticipation (optional)Students study vignettes that help <strong>the</strong>m see <strong>the</strong> conceptual history <strong>of</strong> our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong>genetic anticipation.Students summarize what <strong>the</strong>y have learned <strong>and</strong> make connections to NMS.Annotated Student MaterialSeparate student pages contain <strong>the</strong> material shown in bold typeface.Here, we provide an annotated version <strong>of</strong> student materials to help you conduct <strong>the</strong> activity.How do you know that a new scientific idea isc o rrect? Lots <strong>of</strong> people have good ideas abouthow to explain what <strong>the</strong>y observe in living systems,but a good idea alone is not enough. Scientistsconstantly have to decide whe<strong>the</strong>r a newidea is valid, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y do so by using <strong>the</strong> requirements<strong>of</strong> science. To meet <strong>the</strong>se criteria, a goodidea must be based on logical reasoning, <strong>and</strong> itmust be tested, preferably many times <strong>and</strong> inmany different ways. A scientist draws an idea(hypo<strong>the</strong>sis) from early observations <strong>of</strong> a problem<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n collects evidence to determ i n ewhe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> idea is scientifically valid. If <strong>the</strong>hypo<strong>the</strong>sis seems to be valid, <strong>the</strong> scientist willshare it with o<strong>the</strong>r scientists. They, in turn, mayrepeat <strong>the</strong> tests to see whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> evidence isreproducible, or <strong>the</strong>y may add new lines <strong>of</strong> evidencefrom different tests. If enough evidenceaccumulates, <strong>the</strong> scientific community accepts<strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis as valid, <strong>and</strong> it becomes part <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> established body <strong>of</strong> scientific knowledge. Insimple terms, people <strong>the</strong>n refer to <strong>the</strong> new ideaas part <strong>of</strong> “what <strong>the</strong>y know about genetics.”This approach to building an explanation for anatural process sounds fairly simple, but it canbe complicated. For example, how do you tell<strong>the</strong> difference between discovery <strong>of</strong> a newprocess <strong>and</strong> misinterpretation <strong>of</strong> data? Such adebate arose in genetics among people whostudied two inherited disorders, Huntington disease(HD) <strong>and</strong> myotonic dystrophy (DM). In thisa c t i v i t y, you will step into <strong>the</strong> shoes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> scientistsinvolved in this debate <strong>and</strong> use <strong>the</strong>requirements <strong>of</strong> science to follow <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong>this intriguing investigation. Read <strong>the</strong> descriptions<strong>of</strong> HD <strong>and</strong> DM in Figures 3-1 <strong>and</strong> 3-2 if youare not familiar with <strong>the</strong>se disorders.Note: Your students likely have studied Huntingtondisease. If so, have <strong>the</strong>m summarize what <strong>the</strong>y knowabout this genetic disorder before using Figure 3.1.PROCEDUREPart I: Identifying <strong>the</strong> MysteryThe following fictitious dialogue <strong>and</strong> series <strong>of</strong> questionsintroduce students to <strong>the</strong> idea <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation.Call <strong>the</strong> students’ attention to <strong>the</strong> date for<strong>the</strong> opening dialogue, which is 1957.1. The dialogue below takes you back in historyto <strong>the</strong> year 1957. Read <strong>the</strong> dialogue <strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong>n answer Questions 1a-1e.Note that in this dialogue we use <strong>the</strong> term“chorea”; this name for HD was <strong>the</strong> commonterm used in <strong>the</strong> 1950s.The year is 1957. This dialogue isp a rt <strong>of</strong> a discussion that mighthave taken place between aphysician <strong>and</strong> a geneticist; wewill call <strong>the</strong>m Dr. A. <strong>and</strong> Dr. B.As you read <strong>the</strong>ir discussion, thinkabout w h y <strong>the</strong>y have different views.104©1997 by BSCS.


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3Dr. A: “I’ve been seeing a patient named Allenwho may have Huntington’s chorea. Ifso, I think he is a good example <strong>of</strong>genetic anticipation.”Dr. B: “You mean that this disease runs in hisfamily, but is showing up at an earlierage in Allen than it did in his parents orgr<strong>and</strong>parents?”D r. A: “ E x a c t l y. Allen is only 40, <strong>and</strong> already hehas short periods <strong>of</strong> memory loss <strong>and</strong>sudden problems with awkward movement<strong>of</strong> his legs. When I interviewed him,I learned that his mo<strong>the</strong>r died when shewas 65, but, starting at age 55, peoplewho knew her said she was a bit ‘odd’ inher thinking. She spent her last five yearsin a wheelchair, suffering from severemuscle <strong>and</strong> movement problems. Sheappeared to have Huntington’s chorea.”D r. B: “But that doesn’t prove that geneticanticipation is a real phenomenon. Whatdo you know about Allen’s matern a lgr<strong>and</strong>parents <strong>and</strong> great-gr<strong>and</strong>parents?”Dr. A: “Not much. His mo<strong>the</strong>r’s mo<strong>the</strong>r diedwhen she was very young, during childbirth,so all we know is that she did notshow signs <strong>of</strong> Huntington’s chorea up to<strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 24. Allen’s gr<strong>and</strong>fa<strong>the</strong>r livedto be about 70, but Allen knew nothingabout his general health. But you knowgeneticists have reported cases <strong>of</strong> anticipationfor years. What about Dr. Bell’sdiscussion <strong>of</strong> it in her 1947 publicationin The Treasury <strong>of</strong> Human <strong>Inheritance</strong>?She makes a good argument for geneticanticipation in ano<strong>the</strong>r genetic disease,myotonic dystrophy.”Dr. B: “You mean her mention that patients inan earlier generation <strong>of</strong> a known pedigreegenerally had only mild symptoms,such as cataracts, when <strong>the</strong>y weremiddle-aged, while —”D r. A: “— while <strong>the</strong>ir children <strong>and</strong> gr<strong>and</strong>childrenhad muscle wasting <strong>and</strong> mental illness,more severe in each subsequent generation—anexample <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation.”Figure 3-1 Huntington Disease (HD): Summary <strong>of</strong> SymptomsHuntington disease came to public attention in <strong>the</strong> 1940s when <strong>the</strong> well-known folk singer Woody Guthrie began to showsymptoms <strong>of</strong> this fatal genetic disorder. Huntington disease received notoriety again in <strong>the</strong> 1980s <strong>and</strong> early 1990s whenresearchers, including Dr. Nancy Wexler, hunted for <strong>and</strong> located <strong>the</strong> mutant gene that causes HD. Dr. Wexler caught <strong>the</strong> public’sinterest in part because she had a direct <strong>and</strong> personal concern with this genetic killer: her mo<strong>the</strong>r died <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease,so Dr. Wexler knew she was at risk.HD is inherited as an autosomal dominant disorder. It is rare, occurring at a frequency <strong>of</strong> about 1/20,000 in Western countries<strong>and</strong> less <strong>of</strong>ten in Asia <strong>and</strong> elsewhere. The onset <strong>of</strong> symptoms generally occurs in adulthood, but <strong>the</strong> age varies among individuals<strong>and</strong> between generations <strong>of</strong> affected people. The disease involves neurological function, <strong>and</strong> early symptoms includetwitchy muscles, awkwardness <strong>of</strong> movement, <strong>and</strong> memory dysfunction. Eventually symptoms become severe, <strong>and</strong> deathresults. The gene is located on chromosome 4.Figure 3-2 Myotonic Dystrophy (DM): Summary <strong>of</strong> SymptomsMyotonic dystrophy is a fairly rare inherited human disorder that occurs in 1/8000 Caucasians <strong>and</strong> even less frequently ino<strong>the</strong>r groups. DM shows a large degree <strong>of</strong> variability in <strong>the</strong> type <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> symptoms. The age <strong>of</strong> onset may vary fromone family member to ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> those affected. In its mildest forms, DM leaves <strong>the</strong> individual asymptomatic until late in adulthood.Then it may show up only as cataracts on <strong>the</strong> eyes, which affect vision. Because cataracts are not uncommon in elderlypeople, many individuals with extremely mild DM symptoms may not realize <strong>the</strong>y have inherited this disorder. (Conversely, justbecause a relative <strong>of</strong> yours has had cataracts, do not assume that DM occurs in your family.)More serious symptoms include muscle abnormalities such as a breakdown <strong>of</strong> muscle tissue <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> inability to relax a muscleafter it has been contracted. O<strong>the</strong>r symptoms involve damage to <strong>the</strong> heart or <strong>the</strong> sexual organs (gonads). Some individualswho have inherited DM show mental retardation as children. A less severe form results in unusual drowsiness <strong>and</strong> inattentivenessin adults.Myotonic dystrophy is inherited as an autosomal dominant disorder. The gene is located on chromosome 19.105©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceD r. B: “ ( Laughing) Perhaps. But you haven’tmentioned a different publication fromthat year, <strong>the</strong> one by that geneticist Pe n-rose. He mentions Bell’s work <strong>and</strong>r e p o rts from o<strong>the</strong>r geneticists, but hepoints out that simple Mendelian inheritance<strong>of</strong> a dominant trait won’t result in<strong>the</strong> increasing problems <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> diseasein children <strong>and</strong> gr<strong>and</strong>children <strong>of</strong> someonewho has <strong>the</strong> disease gene.”Dr. A: “Then how do you explain <strong>the</strong> observedanticipation?”Dr. B: “I don’t, at least, not yet. It may be anerror in observation. Besides, I wouldneed more data.”References:J. Bell (1947), Dystrophia myotonica <strong>and</strong> allied disease,in The Tr e a s u ry <strong>of</strong> Human <strong>Inheritance</strong> IV.Nervous diseases <strong>and</strong> muscular dystrophies, vol 5,p. 343; <strong>and</strong> L.S. Penrose (1947), The problem <strong>of</strong>anticipation in pedigrees <strong>of</strong> dystrophia myotonica,Annals <strong>of</strong> Eugenics 14:125-132.Think about <strong>the</strong> dialogue as you respond to<strong>the</strong>se questions.a. What is genetic anticipation?The term refers to a pattern <strong>of</strong> earlier onset <strong>and</strong>more severe symptoms <strong>of</strong> a genetic disorder in latergenerations.b. Why does Dr. B. want more data?The sampling error decreases in a study when <strong>the</strong>reare more data. (Remind students <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir coin-tossexperiment in Activity 2.) In addition, multiple lines<strong>of</strong> evidence ei<strong>the</strong>r streng<strong>the</strong>n an explanation orshow that it is inadequate or inaccurate. Ei<strong>the</strong>r way,more data generally help. The anecdotal case discussedin <strong>the</strong> fictitious dialogue would have had lesssignificance than <strong>the</strong> research publications to whichDr. A. <strong>and</strong> Dr. B. refer, because <strong>the</strong>se publications arebased on more than one case.c. Dr. B. refers to “an error in observation.”How might such an error arise? Whatcould be done about it?If a physician thinks genetic anticipation occurs, heor she might look for it in at-risk <strong>of</strong>fspring, thus seeingdisease symptoms earlier than might o<strong>the</strong>rwisebe <strong>the</strong> case. This pattern could result in a falseimpression <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> earlier onset <strong>of</strong> symptoms in children<strong>and</strong> gr<strong>and</strong>children <strong>of</strong> an affected individual. Acontrolled case study would provide for <strong>of</strong>fspring tobe examined at <strong>the</strong> same ages in a large number <strong>of</strong>families. In addition, <strong>the</strong> study would need to bedone by more than one physician, perhaps with a“blind” study, where <strong>the</strong> physicians examine patientswithout being told what disease is suspected.d. What have you learned about dominantinheritance? Why is genetic anticipationnot explained by this genetic concept?Dominant inheritance predicts that one-half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><strong>of</strong>fspring <strong>of</strong> affected parents will be affected <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>presence <strong>of</strong> one allele will result in phenotypicexpression. The concept <strong>of</strong> dominant inheritancedoes not explain genetic anticipation because thistraditional concept does not include any mechanismthat would result in affected individuals in differentgenerations manifesting <strong>the</strong> disorder at an earlierage, or with more severe symptoms, than did <strong>the</strong>irancestors.e. What else could be done to solve thismystery?Collect additional data; analyze patterns in morefamilies to see whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>re is consistent evidencefor genetic anticipation. Urge students to articulatewhat patterns <strong>the</strong>y might look for in <strong>the</strong> data here<strong>and</strong> with data in <strong>the</strong> next part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> activity. Thesepatterns include <strong>the</strong> age at which symptoms appear,<strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> death, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> degree <strong>of</strong> severity for HD<strong>and</strong> DM.Part II: Ga<strong>the</strong>ring Clues to <strong>the</strong> MysteryDistribute <strong>the</strong> first set <strong>of</strong> HD clues (from Copymaster3-1) among <strong>the</strong> teams.The year is 1977. You have collected interv i e wdata from many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> members<strong>of</strong> Allen’s family. Use what youknow from <strong>the</strong> dialogue <strong>and</strong>from this new case-historyi n f o rmation to answer <strong>the</strong> ChallengeQuestions in Figure 3-3.106©1997 by BSCS.


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3Figure 3-3 Challenge QuestionsScientists had to think about genetic anticipation in severalsteps. They needed to break <strong>the</strong> problem intoaddressable questions.■ What evidence do you have to support or refute <strong>the</strong>existence <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation?■ Do you have evidence that supports a possible explanationfor <strong>the</strong> genetic mechanism that causes anticipation?If yes, explain.2. Each team has different clues obtained fromcase histories. You only have a few minutes toconsider your team’s clues before you poolyour information with that from o<strong>the</strong>r teams.Prompt your students to suggest building a pedigreebefore you give each team <strong>the</strong> template for <strong>the</strong> HDpedigree (Copymaster 3-3). Give students onlyabout two minutes to see how <strong>the</strong>ir clues fit toge<strong>the</strong>rbefore you begin <strong>the</strong> team jigsaw to build <strong>the</strong>whole pedigree.3. Follow your teacher’s instructions abouthow to report your preliminary findings.Devise a way to display <strong>the</strong> clues from all teams,assembled in a jigsaw fashion. You may write on <strong>the</strong>board or on an overhead transparency <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pedigreetemplate (Copymaster 3-3). Remember that<strong>the</strong> clues for each team represent one section <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>pedigree. Notice that <strong>the</strong> individual from <strong>the</strong> dialogue,Allen, is marked on <strong>the</strong> template. Ask studentsto volunteer information about <strong>the</strong> identities<strong>and</strong> health data for each family member, perhapss t a rting with someone directly related to Allen. Fo rexample, you could point to Allen’s twin daughters<strong>and</strong> ask, “Does any team have information about<strong>the</strong>se family members?” Make this step very quick<strong>and</strong> fun. You may find it most effective to haveteams volunteer information in an inform a l ,u n s t ructured way.If you prefer a more orderly approach (<strong>and</strong> lessnoise), try calling on students by teams to ask forspecific information. When <strong>the</strong> pedigree is complete,move to <strong>the</strong> next step.4. What does this evidence tell you about <strong>the</strong>Challenge Questions in Figure 3-3 (if anything)?Students should be able to cite evidence for <strong>the</strong> e x i s-tence <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation; <strong>the</strong>y do not yet have evidencefor <strong>the</strong> causal mechanism. If students have difficultydoing this, use some intermediate questions tohelp <strong>the</strong>m structure <strong>the</strong>ir thinking. For example, ask<strong>the</strong>m to compare <strong>the</strong> health <strong>and</strong> lifespan <strong>of</strong> Mart h a ,her son Allen, <strong>and</strong> her gr<strong>and</strong>son Andrew, paying particularattention to <strong>the</strong>ir ages when symptoms appear.Distribute <strong>the</strong> article from Copymaster 3-4 <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>HD molecular clues from Copymaster 3-2. Each teamwill have one complete set <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se clues.Now <strong>the</strong> year is 1997. M o l e c u-lar biologists have devised atest to identify <strong>the</strong> mutantgene for HD <strong>and</strong> to determ i n esome <strong>of</strong> its special properties.In addition, many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>family members have had <strong>the</strong> test; in somecases blood samples stored from older,deceased members were tested. Your teacherwill supply you with an article that describes<strong>the</strong> new molecular test for HD <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> results<strong>of</strong> this test when perf o rmed on <strong>the</strong> members<strong>of</strong> Allen’s family.5. Read <strong>the</strong> article <strong>and</strong> record <strong>the</strong> inform a t i o nfrom <strong>the</strong> HD molecular clues on your HDpedigree. Report this information to <strong>the</strong> classwhen your teacher instructs you to do so.Give students a few minutes to read <strong>the</strong> article <strong>and</strong>record <strong>the</strong> information on <strong>the</strong>ir pedigrees, <strong>the</strong>nquickly have <strong>the</strong> class volunteer <strong>the</strong> molecular datato add to <strong>the</strong> class pedigree.6. Why does each person tested have t w o r e p e a tn u m b e r s ?There are two copies <strong>of</strong> each chromosome <strong>and</strong> consequentlytwo copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene being tested.7. Using this new evidence, once again address<strong>the</strong> Challenge Questions in Figure 3-3.Now students have a new line <strong>of</strong> evidence to support<strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation, plus evidence107©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Scienceto suggest a possible mechanism (expansion <strong>of</strong> trinucleotiderepeats). You could help stimulate <strong>the</strong> students’thinking by asking a question such as, “Whymight <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> his parents’ test cause Evan todecide against being tested?” (The parents have onlya few trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> gene associatedwith HD.)Challenge Questions (from Figure 3-3)■ What evidence do you have to support or refute<strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation?There is evidence to support <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> geneticanticipation. The pattern <strong>of</strong> disease corresponds to<strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> CAG repeats, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong>repeats increases in some individuals in later generations.For example, in <strong>the</strong> HD pedigree, Allen has 46repeats in <strong>the</strong> mutant copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene,<strong>and</strong> he dies <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease. Linda, his wife,has only 22 copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> repeats at most, <strong>and</strong> shedoes not develop <strong>the</strong> disease. Their oldest child,A n d r e w, dies <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease at an even earlierage than did his fa<strong>the</strong>r, while a second child, Debbie,does not have <strong>the</strong> disease well past <strong>the</strong> age at whichAllen became ill with HD. Andrew has 69 repeats ina copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene; Debbie has, at most,13 CAG repeats associated with <strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene.Students may provide many o<strong>the</strong>r examples.■ Do you have evidence that supports a possibleexplanation for <strong>the</strong> genetic mechanism thatcauses anticipation? If yes, explain.Yes. The instability <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats from onegeneration to <strong>the</strong> next suggests <strong>the</strong> molecular basisfor <strong>the</strong> pattern <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation.8. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> strengths <strong>of</strong> a good scientificexplanation is that it is supported by multiplelines <strong>of</strong> evidence. Use a set <strong>of</strong> clues for adifferent family to look for additional evidencethat supports your answers to <strong>the</strong>preceding questions.Distribute a complete set <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> DM familial/healthclues (part A from Copymaster 3-5) <strong>and</strong> a copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>template for <strong>the</strong> DM pedigree (Copymaster 3-6) toeach team. You may want to have teams race to seewhich one can complete <strong>the</strong> pedigree first, or assign<strong>the</strong> pedigree as homework, giving each student acopy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> clues <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> template. After studentsset up <strong>the</strong> pedigree, distribute <strong>the</strong> molecular cluesfor myotonic dystrophy (part B from Copymaster 3-5) to each team. Give each team time to organize itsclues before asking for <strong>the</strong> students’ response to <strong>the</strong>challenge questions.The pedigree for myotonic dystrophy provides additionalevidence to support <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> geneticanticipation, plus additional evidence to support<strong>the</strong> explanation that unstable trinucleotide repeatsin <strong>the</strong> mutant gene are part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mechanismunderlying genetic anticipation. Emphasize thisi m p o rtant aspect <strong>of</strong> scientific methods by askingstudents how <strong>the</strong> DM data affect <strong>the</strong>ir explanation<strong>of</strong> anticipation.Part IV: Discovering <strong>the</strong> History<strong>of</strong> Genetic AnticipationThis section is optional, but it provides a rich contextfor <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation.As such, <strong>the</strong> examples show many features <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> science, including how new work buildson old work, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> need for sufficient evidence tos u p p o rt a new explanation. (Sufficient evidence ismore than limited anecdotal evidence or unsubstantiatedideas <strong>of</strong>fered by an individual scientist who hasconsiderable status in <strong>the</strong> scientific community. )The sequence <strong>of</strong> discovery demonstrated by<strong>the</strong> clues you used reflects <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> discoveryabout genetic anticipation. Use <strong>the</strong>brief scenes (vignettes) that your teacher willgive you to build a more complete picture <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> discovery about HD <strong>and</strong> geneticanticipation. As you read, try to determine atwhat point in <strong>the</strong> sequence <strong>the</strong> opening dialoguebetween <strong>the</strong> fictitious Drs. A. <strong>and</strong> B.might have taken place. What additionalvignettes might follow <strong>the</strong>se in <strong>the</strong> future?Distribute one copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> vignettes to each team(Copymaster 3-8).9. Read <strong>the</strong> vignettes <strong>and</strong> discuss with yourteammates what <strong>the</strong>y tell you about <strong>the</strong>history <strong>of</strong> our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> HD.10. As a team, draw up a brief outline <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>history <strong>of</strong> discovery about HD.108©1997 by BSCS.


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 311. Individually, write a new vignette that representswhat you predict may be <strong>the</strong> nextlevel <strong>of</strong> discovery about HD.12. After you complete <strong>the</strong> vignette, explain<strong>the</strong> aspects <strong>of</strong> science on which you basedyour predictions.ANALYSIS1. What is <strong>the</strong> relationship between <strong>the</strong> number<strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> mutantHD or DM gene <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> resulting phenotype?There is a threshold number in <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> HD. Individualswho have about 39 or more repeats almostcertainly develop <strong>the</strong> disease unless <strong>the</strong>y die fromsome o<strong>the</strong>r cause first. Usually <strong>the</strong>re are not morethan 120 repeats in <strong>the</strong> HD mutation. In <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong>DM, <strong>the</strong> threshold number is about 50-80 for mildsymptoms. Those who have severe symptoms <strong>of</strong> DMhave inherited hundreds to more than 2,000 trinucleotiderepeats in <strong>the</strong>ir mutant DM gene. The morerepeats above <strong>the</strong> threshold number, <strong>the</strong> more likely<strong>the</strong> disease phenotype will develop, <strong>the</strong> more likely<strong>the</strong> onset will be early, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> more likely <strong>the</strong>affected person will display more severe symptoms.2. How does what you know about geneticanticipation contrast with <strong>the</strong> traditional,Mendelian view <strong>of</strong> autosomal dominantinheritance?The traditional underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> autosomal dominantinheritance includes no assumptions about <strong>the</strong>ability <strong>of</strong> an allele to change its size <strong>and</strong> does not,<strong>the</strong>refore, identify a mechanism that explains geneticanticipation.3. How have you used <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> sciencein this activity?Sample student responses include:■ We collected <strong>and</strong> analyzed data.■ We reviewed earlier work done on this problem.■ We made our data <strong>and</strong> conclusions available foranalysis by o<strong>the</strong>rs.■ We assumed that <strong>the</strong>se observations are explainablein terms <strong>of</strong> natural causes.You may want to have students add to or consult <strong>the</strong>NMS poster.4. Write a vignette (brief scene or part <strong>of</strong> a story )about <strong>the</strong> future health <strong>of</strong> Pe t e r, Cathy, <strong>and</strong>Sean. Use your case-history data <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> art i-cle you read to support your description.Student responses may be more creative, but <strong>the</strong>basic information should show <strong>the</strong> following: IfPeter has inherited HD from his fa<strong>the</strong>r, his symptomsmay occur at an early age, because his fa<strong>the</strong>rhad a large number <strong>of</strong> repeats. The trinucleotidenumber could, however, decrease. DNA analysiswould determine Pe t e r’s number <strong>of</strong> repeats <strong>and</strong>provide a better prediction. Cathy is not likely tohave HD because her mo<strong>the</strong>r, Paula, has at mostonly 13 repeats, <strong>and</strong> at that number <strong>the</strong> gene is generallystable. In <strong>the</strong> DM family, severe symptomsshould already be apparent for Sean because he hasa large number <strong>of</strong> repeats.109©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 4Should TeenagersBe Tested for <strong>the</strong>Mutant HD Gene?NMS CONCEPTSNew discoveries in science <strong>and</strong> new technologiessometimes raise difficult ethical questions.GENETICS CONCEPTSGenetic anticipation in Huntington disease corr e l a t e sto <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> unstable trinucleotide repeats;genetic testing for Huntington disease can predictwhat is likely to happen to a person with <strong>the</strong> disease.FOCUSThis activity challenges students to explore a policycommonly followed by genetic-testing centers towithhold genetic tests for Huntington disease (HD)from asymptomatic patients under <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 18. Studentswill use <strong>the</strong>ir prior knowledge about HD <strong>and</strong>reflect on <strong>the</strong>ir own decision-making capabilities as<strong>the</strong>y analyze a policy that raises significant ethicalissues.OBJECTIVESAs <strong>the</strong> students complete this activity, <strong>the</strong>y should1. revisit <strong>the</strong> scientific <strong>and</strong> clinical implications <strong>of</strong>HD;2. ga<strong>the</strong>r <strong>and</strong> evaluate information <strong>and</strong> make <strong>and</strong>analyze arguments as tools for ethical inquiry;3. take <strong>and</strong> defend a position on whe<strong>the</strong>r to recommendthat genetic-testing centers <strong>of</strong>fer genetictesting for HD to asymptomatic teenagers; <strong>and</strong>4. state alternative viewpoints on an accepted policyamong genetic-testing centers.ESTIMATED TIME30-45 minutes plus homeworkThis activity can be taught as a fairly brief, focuseddiscussion <strong>of</strong> ethical issues; <strong>the</strong> estimated classroomtime <strong>of</strong> 30-45 minutes reflects this approach. Theactivity has <strong>the</strong> potential, however, to generate muchdeeper discussion. You may want to explore in moredetail some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> concerns that students raise.PREPARATION AND MATERIALSYou will need to provide <strong>the</strong> following:■ 1 copy per student <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 4-1, Worksheetfor a Discussion about PolicyIf you have not taught Activity 3 prior to beginningthis activity, you may want to review some characteristics<strong>of</strong> HD, including <strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong> trinucleotiderepeat expansion. For details, see Activity 3 <strong>and</strong> page43 in Section V <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers. If youplan to teach Activities 3 <strong>and</strong> 4, we strongly recommendpresenting <strong>the</strong>m in numerical order.111


Activity 4 ■ Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene?COPYMASTERS USED IN THIS ACTIVITYCopymaster 4-1Worksheet for a Discussion about PolicyINTRODUCTIONAn important responsibility <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Human GenomeProject (HGP) is to examine <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> projecton individuals, communities, <strong>and</strong> society. Bothmajor funding sources for <strong>the</strong> HGP (<strong>the</strong> UnitedStates Department <strong>of</strong> Energy [DOE] <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>National Institutes <strong>of</strong> Health [NIH]) provideresources to support <strong>the</strong> investigation <strong>of</strong> relatedissues. This component <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HGP is called ELSI(Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Social Implications; see SectionIV in <strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h e r s for more inform a-tion about ELSI). Activity 4 addresses ELSI issuesconnected with genetic testing <strong>and</strong> related specificallyto analysis <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats in Huntingtondisease.Issues Associated with Genetic Testing: Thedecision for a health-care provider to conduct agenetic test is based on a variety <strong>of</strong> factors. Healthcarepr<strong>of</strong>essionals are trained to reduce risks to <strong>the</strong>irpatients, including psychosocial risks. Such risks arereal in testing for <strong>the</strong> HD mutation. Anxiety <strong>and</strong>depression may arise in response to a positive testresult. A similar issue received attention in <strong>the</strong> mid-1980s, when health-care pr<strong>of</strong>essionals had to decidehow to h<strong>and</strong>le testing for exposure to <strong>the</strong> AIDSvirus, HIV. At that point, <strong>the</strong> connection between apositive test for exposure to HIV <strong>and</strong> development<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> fatal disease AIDS was not yet clear (although<strong>the</strong> correlation has since been established to <strong>the</strong> satisfaction<strong>of</strong> virtually all scientists). Keep in mind that<strong>the</strong>re is a distinction between having <strong>the</strong> HD gene<strong>and</strong> having <strong>the</strong> HD mutation. (Everyone has <strong>the</strong>gene; it is <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> a mutation in that genethat results in <strong>the</strong> genetic disorder known as Huntingtondisease.)O<strong>the</strong>r factors that a health-care provider considerswhen making a decision about genetic testinginclude <strong>the</strong> following questions:■ Can <strong>the</strong> disorder, once diagnosed, be treated?■ Does <strong>the</strong> patient exhibit symptoms, or is <strong>the</strong>order for a test based on family history alone?■ What is <strong>the</strong> balance <strong>of</strong> benefit versus harmbrought about by knowledge <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> test results?The issue becomes even more complex when <strong>the</strong>patient to be tested is a minor, that is, under 18 years<strong>of</strong> age.The request for a genetic test may come from <strong>the</strong>parents or from <strong>the</strong> minor. When <strong>the</strong> minor is anadolescent, <strong>the</strong> issue becomes particularly complicatedbecause <strong>the</strong> patient may exhibit a considerabledegree <strong>of</strong> autonomy regarding his or her health-caredecisions. A report from <strong>the</strong> American Society <strong>of</strong>Human <strong>Genetics</strong> (ASHG) <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> American College<strong>of</strong> Medical <strong>Genetics</strong> (ACMG), titled Points to Consider:Ethical, Legal, <strong>and</strong> Psychological Implications <strong>of</strong>Genetic Testing in Children <strong>and</strong> Adolescents, is auseful resource for teaching this activity (Am. J.Hum. Genet. 57:1233-1241, 1995). The report pointsout that in <strong>the</strong>se cases “...<strong>the</strong> primary goal <strong>of</strong> genetictesting should be to promote <strong>the</strong> well-being <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>child.” The report also addresses <strong>the</strong> impact on familymembers: “Presymptomatic diagnosis in childrenalso has <strong>the</strong> potential to alter <strong>the</strong> relationships thatexist between parents <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>of</strong>fspring <strong>and</strong> amongsiblings.” The child who tests positive may beoverindulged or may be treated as a scapegoat. Both<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se problems can occur, however, even in <strong>the</strong>absence <strong>of</strong> testing. The testing <strong>of</strong> a child (or indeedany o<strong>the</strong>r family member) also has implications forall members <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> family. In some cases, this forewarningwill be welcomed; in o<strong>the</strong>rs, it may beunwanted. Genetic testing <strong>of</strong> a child will ease someaspects <strong>of</strong> uncertainty, but people differ greatly in<strong>the</strong>ir response to such news.In <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> genetic testing for <strong>the</strong> Huntingtonmutation, most health-care providers <strong>and</strong> genetictesting centers adhere to a policy that denies tests tominors who do not exhibit symptoms. This denialextends to requests from <strong>the</strong> parents, who are <strong>the</strong>legal guardians <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> child’s health. The rationale forthis policy is that HD generally occurs in <strong>the</strong> adultyears (particularly during middle-age), <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>re isno treatment. For <strong>the</strong>se reasons, <strong>the</strong>re is no immediate,physical health benefit from a specific diagnosisbased on genetic testing for a minor. The psychologicaleffects can be mixed. While some individuals prefer<strong>the</strong> release from uncert a i n t y, o<strong>the</strong>rs could view a112©1997 by BSCS.


Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene? ■ Activity 4positive result as a death sentence <strong>and</strong> react in waysthat are destructive to <strong>the</strong>mselves or <strong>the</strong>ir families.Genetic testing requires informed consent, <strong>and</strong> somegeneticists argue that this requirement automaticallyrules out children, <strong>and</strong> even teenagers, who generallyare judged incapable <strong>of</strong> providing such consent. As<strong>the</strong> ASHG/ACMG report makes clear, however, thisview <strong>of</strong> minors is far too broad <strong>and</strong> may not be realistic.Some specialists are beginning to recognize thatsome adolescents <strong>and</strong> young children have sufficientautonomy in consent <strong>and</strong> decision-making, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>authors suggest that <strong>the</strong> desires <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se youthsshould be taken into account. In any event, one mustweigh <strong>the</strong> b a l a n c e <strong>of</strong> potential harm <strong>and</strong> benefit.The current policy is to delay <strong>the</strong> decision to test for<strong>the</strong> HD mutation until <strong>the</strong> individual is an adult <strong>and</strong>can make <strong>the</strong> decision, ra<strong>the</strong>r than letting parentsremove this option by making <strong>the</strong> choice <strong>the</strong>mselves.Note that a change in policy most likelywould result in parents being permitted to make <strong>the</strong>decision, ra<strong>the</strong>r than leaving <strong>the</strong> decision to <strong>the</strong>minor in question. This situation is reflected in Step2 <strong>of</strong> this activity. Ei<strong>the</strong>r way, issues <strong>of</strong> ethical decisionmaking(discussed in <strong>the</strong> following paragraphs) willarise. To keep Activity 4 focused, we have structuredit around our assumption that this change is <strong>the</strong>most likely alternative to <strong>the</strong> current policy. O<strong>the</strong>roptions for a new policy are worth considering, however.For example, <strong>the</strong> new policy could allow competentminors to make <strong>the</strong> choices, ei<strong>the</strong>r as a generalrule or in special cases. (See comments underStrategies for Teaching <strong>the</strong> Activity.)Ethical Decision-making in <strong>the</strong> Classroom: Ethicaldecision-making is a complex process thatincludes many variables, but one can examine <strong>the</strong>process in concrete terms that teenagers can underst<strong>and</strong>.Activity 4 involves students in a discussion <strong>of</strong>ethical issues surrounding a debate about a genetictestingpolicy. Although this discussion is not a formalethical analysis, it is grounded, in prominentways, in <strong>the</strong> ethics <strong>of</strong> problem-solving. This activityraises two central issues: “prudential judgment” <strong>and</strong>“developmental autonomy.” The former refers to <strong>the</strong>factors necessary for judging an issue wisely; <strong>the</strong> latterinvolves criteria for judging that someone is sufficientlycapable <strong>of</strong> making his or her own decisions,independent <strong>of</strong> parents or guardians.We use prudential judgments when our future interestsare at risk in uncertain or unknown ways. Prudentialjudgments concern <strong>the</strong> probability that <strong>the</strong>events for which we are at risk will occur. Such judgmentsalso weigh <strong>the</strong> benefits against <strong>the</strong> harmsinherent in particular outcomes or consequences.Hopefully, <strong>the</strong> act <strong>of</strong> judgment is followed by choosingto pursue <strong>the</strong> option that, at least, minimizesharm <strong>and</strong>, at best, provides benefit. Prudential judgmentsdo not necessarily involve situations with a“right” or “wrong” answer. They will exhibit variabilityby <strong>the</strong>ir very nature because <strong>the</strong>y involve humanvalues, probability, <strong>and</strong> relative risk. Prudential judgmentscan be made sloppily or rigorously. Rigorouslymade prudential judgments follow a process <strong>of</strong>intellectual discipline in which one weighs risks <strong>and</strong>benefits carefully. Sloppily made judgments are morelikely to result in surprises, because <strong>the</strong> ramifications<strong>of</strong> a decision are not examined carefully beforeh<strong>and</strong>.The ethical criteria one employs in this intellectualprocess will determine <strong>the</strong> quality <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> judgmentsone makes.Developmental autonomy, in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> thisactivity, concerns <strong>the</strong> decision-making capacity <strong>of</strong>teenagers with regard to access to genetic information<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> larger issue <strong>of</strong> whe<strong>the</strong>r a minor is ableto make informed decisions about personal health<strong>and</strong> medical services. Elements <strong>of</strong> this ability includeprocessing information, reasoning causally, assessing<strong>the</strong> worth <strong>of</strong> likely future events using value judgmentsthat are based on facts <strong>and</strong> that have beenwell considered, expressing a preference in light <strong>of</strong>those judgments, carrying out preferences, <strong>and</strong>accepting responsibility for one’s choice or action. Aformal discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se issues is provided in <strong>the</strong>Overview for Teachers (Section IV; see especiallypages 29-31).STRATEGIES FOR TEACHING THE ACTIVITYAs indicated in <strong>the</strong> Introduction, this activity is simplifiedbecause it chooses <strong>the</strong> most likely alternativeto current policy (this alternative being that parentsnormally will decide for <strong>the</strong>ir children) <strong>and</strong> lets studentscompare that alternative to <strong>the</strong> current policy.This comparison begins in Step 2 <strong>of</strong> Part I <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>activity. There are, however, many o<strong>the</strong>r interestingoptions, such as a change in policy that would put113©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 4 ■ Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene?<strong>the</strong> burden <strong>of</strong> decision on <strong>the</strong> minor. To provide amore open-ended approach, you could ask studentsat Step 2 to decide whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> policy shouldchange <strong>and</strong>, if so, to what alternative. Do not get intoan involved discussion at this point. Just identify analternative suggested by <strong>the</strong> class <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n call for avote based on <strong>the</strong> two options: to maintain <strong>the</strong> currentpolicy or to change <strong>the</strong> policy to one suggestedby <strong>the</strong> class. With this open-ended approach, <strong>the</strong>sample responses provided in Question 4a <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Annotated Student Material may change.Please note that we refer to choices that involve childrenor <strong>the</strong>ir parents, but many <strong>of</strong> your students maylive in households that do not have a traditional set<strong>of</strong> parents. A student, for example, may live with asingle parent or with gr<strong>and</strong>parents. Please be sensitiveto <strong>the</strong>se differences where possible.The following aspects <strong>of</strong> prudential decision-makingare important in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> this activity. Consider<strong>the</strong> policy to deny genetic testing to asymptomaticteenagers who are at risk for HD. Is this policy unjustifiablebecause it prevents risks that prudent peoplecould take for <strong>the</strong>mselves? Is <strong>the</strong> policy justifiablebecause it protects <strong>the</strong> right <strong>of</strong> minors to make <strong>the</strong>sedecisions later in adulthood, ra<strong>the</strong>r than beingsuperseded by parental authority? As you explore<strong>the</strong>se issues, help your students to recognize that<strong>the</strong>re is a wide range <strong>of</strong> prudential judgments, fromthose that are highly risk-averse to those that aremuch less so. You can do so by contrasting examplesfrom each end <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> spectrum.The distinction between genetic testing for minorsor adults also rests in part on developmental autonomywith regard to decision-making. Help studentstest <strong>the</strong>ir own developmental autonomy by using <strong>the</strong>debate in this activity. Students can record <strong>the</strong>irresponses on Copymaster 4-1. We provide somesample responses on <strong>the</strong> annotated version <strong>of</strong> Copymaster4-1, page 117. Questions 1 <strong>and</strong> 2 <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Analysisalso allude to developmental autonomy.The following summary <strong>of</strong> this activity will give you asnapshot <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> procedure.ACTIVITY 4 AT A GLANCEPart I:Part II:Analysis:Identifying <strong>the</strong> IssueStudents learn about an actual health-care policy that withholds genetic testing for asymptomaticteenagers who are at risk for HD. The students express <strong>the</strong>ir opinions about <strong>the</strong>policy by voting to retain it or amend it.Using Ethical Decision-makingStudents apply skills in ethical decision-making <strong>and</strong> analyze <strong>the</strong> topic in depth before <strong>the</strong>yvote again.Students examine <strong>the</strong> usefulness <strong>of</strong> scientific evidence <strong>and</strong> look at o<strong>the</strong>r ethical issuesrelated to new discoveries in genetics; <strong>the</strong>y personalize <strong>the</strong>ir discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> policy ongenetic testing.114©1997 by BSCS.


Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene? ■ Activity 4Annotated Student MaterialSeparate student pages contain <strong>the</strong> material shown in bold typeface.Here, we provide an annotated version <strong>of</strong> student materials to help you conduct <strong>the</strong> activity.How should you go about making good choicesabout important issues in life? Should youbase your decisions on a whim, a coin toss, ora call to a psychic hot line? Or should you relyon well-informed, well-reasoned analysis? Scientificreasoning is a disciplined way to underst<strong>and</strong>events in <strong>the</strong> natural world. Similarly, adiscussion <strong>of</strong> ethical issues brings reason <strong>and</strong>discipline to decisions that involve preferencesbased on values. We draw our valuesfrom many sources, including history, law,religion, <strong>and</strong> family. Part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> task <strong>of</strong> ethicsis to identify <strong>the</strong>se values clearly <strong>and</strong> to showwhy o<strong>the</strong>rs should regard <strong>the</strong>m as important.A discussion <strong>of</strong> ethical issues may apply towhat we do as individuals or to how publicpolicy is made. In this activity, you will use <strong>the</strong>principles <strong>of</strong> sound decision-making <strong>and</strong>ethics to decide whe<strong>the</strong>r a policy that excludesteenagers from genetic testing is a good publicpolicy.PROCEDUREPart I: Identifying <strong>the</strong> Issue1. People <strong>of</strong>ten express <strong>the</strong>ir opinions or concernsthrough a letter to <strong>the</strong> editor <strong>of</strong> anewspaper. Read <strong>the</strong> letter shown in Figure4-1.If students have completed Activity 3, <strong>the</strong>y will guessthat <strong>the</strong> letter is from Jean Wu, <strong>and</strong> that it refers toher husb<strong>and</strong>, Nathaniel, <strong>and</strong> son, Peter. Because <strong>the</strong>letter is personal, she likely would omit <strong>the</strong>ir names<strong>and</strong> her surname.2. Assume that a change in policy would giveparents <strong>the</strong> right to request an HD test for aperson who is under 18 years <strong>of</strong> age. Decidewhe<strong>the</strong>r you think <strong>the</strong> policy <strong>of</strong> not testingminors should st<strong>and</strong> or be changed. Registeryour vote when your teacher polls <strong>the</strong>class.Give students just enough time to read <strong>the</strong> letter,<strong>the</strong>n restate <strong>the</strong> issue <strong>and</strong> call for a vote. Record <strong>the</strong>class poll “for” <strong>and</strong> “against” changing <strong>the</strong> policy.Part II: Using Ethical Decision-making3. Form teams as directed by your teacher toconduct a discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ethical issues in<strong>the</strong> letter. Regardless <strong>of</strong> your personalopinion on this issue, work with your teamto draw up two lists <strong>of</strong> opposing ideas aboutthis policy, using <strong>the</strong> worksheet provided byyour teacher. In <strong>the</strong> left column, list reasons<strong>the</strong> current policy is good. In <strong>the</strong> right column,list reasons <strong>the</strong> policy should changeor why <strong>the</strong> new policy is better. Record all<strong>of</strong> your team’s ideas.Distribute Copymaster 4-1 to each student. Ask studentsto form teams <strong>of</strong> three or four. Have studentsdiscuss <strong>and</strong> record <strong>the</strong>ir reasons. Allow about 10minutes to complete this exercise.4. Present your team’s ideas to <strong>the</strong> class. Addnotes to your own worksheet <strong>of</strong> new ideas thatarise during <strong>the</strong> discussion with o<strong>the</strong>r teams.Record <strong>the</strong> students’ responses on <strong>the</strong> chalkboard,on a transparency, or on a large sheet <strong>of</strong> paper. Youmay want to remind students that HD is a late-onsetcondition; teenagers, <strong>the</strong>refore, may not show symptomseven if <strong>the</strong>y will develop <strong>the</strong> disease later on.Thus, <strong>the</strong>y could make a decision about testing after<strong>the</strong>y are adults. In addition, <strong>the</strong>re is currently notreatment for HD even though <strong>the</strong> disorder can bediagnosed; <strong>the</strong>re is considerable emotional stressassociated with testing. Future research, however,may find <strong>the</strong>rapies that help.The following questions may help you guide this discussion.Be certain to leave enough time for a secondvote.a. Reflect on <strong>the</strong> reasons <strong>the</strong> class <strong>of</strong>fered for makinga decision about testing for HD. What major115©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 4 ■ Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene?Figure 4-1 Letter to <strong>the</strong> editor <strong>of</strong> a city newspaperethical issues or common areas <strong>of</strong> concern did<strong>the</strong> class identify ?You may find that this case raises at least two areas <strong>of</strong>concern, <strong>the</strong> first involving prudential decision-making<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> second involving developmental autonomy(for example, right to know, competence, <strong>and</strong>maturity). The Introduction to this activity reviews<strong>the</strong>se topics. Sample responses are provided in FigureT4-1, <strong>the</strong> annotated version <strong>of</strong> Copymaster 4-1.Students may express <strong>the</strong>se same ideas in much simplerlanguage. Responses will vary depending onwhat option you <strong>and</strong> your class chose for <strong>the</strong> policychange (parents decide, or minors decide, or someo<strong>the</strong>r plan).b. How would retaining <strong>the</strong> current policy help orinjure <strong>the</strong> teenagers in question, or <strong>the</strong>ir familymembers? How would changing <strong>the</strong> policy helpor injure <strong>the</strong> teenagers or <strong>the</strong>ir family members?How should we evaluate those outcomes?Ask students to defend <strong>the</strong>ir selection.Note from <strong>the</strong> field test: Most field-test teachersfound that a few students change <strong>the</strong>ir votes, whichshows <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> discussion.116©1997 by BSCS.


Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene? ■ Activity 4Figure T4-1 Annotated Worksheet for a Discussion about PolicyCurrent policy: To withhold genetic testing for Huntington disease for asymptomatic minors.Reasons to maintain policyReasons to change policy(Prudential Judgments)Bad events could result (such as depression, divorce, orloss <strong>of</strong> employment).Bad events should be avoided even if <strong>the</strong> risks are low.Testing won’t change <strong>the</strong> outcome.There is no treatment at present.Parents might not want to pay for college.Testing later may be done in a context <strong>of</strong> (future)treatment.Testing could adversely affect one’s status as an insurancecarrier.Bad events do not necessarily follow.We have an obligation to know about our genes for <strong>the</strong>sake <strong>of</strong> future generations.Parents <strong>and</strong> teenagers have a right to know.Teenagers may find out that <strong>the</strong>y tested negative for HD.If tested, teenagers might choose to live differently, knowing<strong>the</strong>y will become ill later.Knowledge <strong>of</strong> one’s genetic status can help with planning,such as whe<strong>the</strong>r to develop relationships.(Developmental Autonomy)Many teenagers cannot make informed decisions.The majority <strong>of</strong> teenagers are nei<strong>the</strong>r cognitively noremotionally mature.Genetic causality is too complex for teenagers to underst<strong>and</strong>.Some teenagers are self-centered <strong>and</strong> ignoreconsequences to o<strong>the</strong>rs.Many teenagers have values that are too unstable.Teenagers found to have <strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene might notchoose to go to college or to seek employment.Knowing could adversely affect o<strong>the</strong>r children in <strong>the</strong> familywho are too young to deal with <strong>the</strong> problem.Many teenagers can make informed decisions.Some teenagers can underst<strong>and</strong> genetic causal relations,if <strong>the</strong>y choose to learn.Early onset could mean that symptoms will appear only afew years after <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> consent. Knowing sooner couldgive <strong>the</strong> teenager more time to make important decisions.There are some teenagers who are cognitively <strong>and</strong>emotionally mature.Knowing could affect o<strong>the</strong>r children in <strong>the</strong> family or closerelatives, perhaps reducing worry.5. Consider <strong>the</strong> discussion <strong>of</strong> ethical issuesperformed by <strong>the</strong> class <strong>and</strong> vote again: Is<strong>the</strong> current policy good <strong>and</strong> worth maintaining,or should it be changed so that parents<strong>of</strong> minors at risk for HD can have <strong>the</strong>irchildren tested?Point out to students <strong>the</strong> important differencebetween deciding for or against a policy <strong>and</strong> decidingwhe<strong>the</strong>r one actually would agree to be tested.An individual can vote to allow testing <strong>and</strong> still electnot to be tested personally. Take a vote to establishwhat most students think should be done.Even if you have taught <strong>the</strong> activity as written, youmay want to challenge students with a question suchas, “In what o<strong>the</strong>r ways could <strong>the</strong> policy bechanged?”ANALYSIS1. Given that <strong>the</strong>re is no treatment at presentfor HD, how does acquiring scientific evidenceabout <strong>the</strong> presence or absence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>HD mutation (from a genetic test) change<strong>the</strong> discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> issue?Fears or concerns about developing HD will be basedon family history. If this case-history information isthorough <strong>and</strong> coupled with an informed scientific117©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 4 ■ Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene?underst<strong>and</strong>ing, speculation is limited to a consideration<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> odds. For example, a child has a 50:50chance <strong>of</strong> inheriting <strong>the</strong> mutation if one parent isknown to be affected; <strong>the</strong> issue is less clear if a gr<strong>and</strong>parentis known to have HD. With <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>genetic test, direct evidence is available, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>issue is reduced to one <strong>of</strong> two possibilities: (a) <strong>the</strong>individual has not inherited <strong>the</strong> mutation <strong>and</strong> will notdevelop HD; or (b) <strong>the</strong> individual has <strong>the</strong> mutation<strong>and</strong>, short <strong>of</strong> death by ano<strong>the</strong>r means or <strong>the</strong> advent<strong>of</strong> a medical treatment, <strong>the</strong> individual almost cert a i n-ly will develop HD. The number <strong>of</strong> trinucleotiderepeats present in <strong>the</strong> mutant gene also helps to predict<strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> onset. (See Activity 3 <strong>and</strong> Section V <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h e r s for more detail about thisf o rm <strong>of</strong> genetic evidence.)2. What additional social <strong>and</strong> ethical issuesmight new knowledge in genetics raise?Responses will vary, but may include <strong>the</strong> following:■ How much information should we be allowed tohave access to <strong>and</strong> when? For example, shouldwe regulate access to genetic information byschools, insurance companies, or by agencies <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> government?■ What constitutes genetic discrimination?■ How should we respond to pressure imposed fortesting by families, employers, or institutions?■ Who pays for predictive testing?■ How much funding should we devote to developinggenetic tests <strong>and</strong> treatments?■ If you are found to carry a disease-related genebut are asymptomatic, are you sick? What wouldo<strong>the</strong>rs (your family, your teacher, your employer,your health-insurance company) think aboutyour state <strong>of</strong> health?■ What issues concerning reproductive decisionmakingdoes new knowledge in genetics raise?3. As a homework assignment, write a responseto Jean W.’s letter. You might let her knowwhe<strong>the</strong>r y o u would choose to be tested, <strong>and</strong>w h y. In your letter, describe what scientificevidence from a genetic test will tell you <strong>and</strong>how that information affects your choice. Inaddition, show how <strong>the</strong> discussion <strong>of</strong> ethicalissues that your class conducted has influencedyour ideas.Student responses should reflect an underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> scientific significance <strong>of</strong> testing, an underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> behavior <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HD allele, <strong>and</strong> a well-reasoned,clearly supported, personal opinion about testing.118©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 5What Do We Know?How Do We Know It?NMS CONCEPTSThis activity reviews all concepts addressed in <strong>the</strong>module, with particular emphasis on <strong>the</strong> following:■ A scientifically valid explanation contains supportingevidence, is well reasoned, is based on asound premise, is testable, <strong>and</strong> demonstrates acausal relationship.■ An explanation can be tested for scientific validity.■ The scientific enterprise reflects <strong>the</strong> culture inwhich it is embedded <strong>and</strong> may raise issues <strong>of</strong>ethical concern.■ Much <strong>of</strong> scientific knowledge is long-lasting,but new explanations arise in response to newe v i d e n c e .■ Scientific knowledge <strong>and</strong> skill are relevant tononscientists.GENETICS CONCEPTSThis activity reviews traditional genetics <strong>and</strong> somenontraditional explanations <strong>of</strong> inheritance introducedin <strong>the</strong> module, including extranuclear (mitochondrial)inheritance, genetic anticipation, <strong>and</strong>instability <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats.FOCUSActivity 5 serves three purposes: (1) it is a syn<strong>the</strong>sisthat helps students evaluate what <strong>the</strong>y havel e a rned in <strong>the</strong> module about <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science<strong>and</strong> about genetics; (2) it provides a bridgebetween <strong>the</strong>se studies in <strong>the</strong> classroom <strong>and</strong> issuesin <strong>the</strong> world outside <strong>of</strong> school; <strong>and</strong> (3) it providesan opportunity to assess student progress. Studentsexamine popular claims that may be scientificor pseudoscientific <strong>and</strong> critique <strong>the</strong>se claimsfor scientific validity. The last part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> activityassesses students’ underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> how scienceworks, <strong>of</strong> genetics concepts, <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> culturalaspects <strong>of</strong> science. It also assesses <strong>the</strong> ability <strong>of</strong>individual students to evaluate popular claims forscientific validity.OBJECTIVESAs students complete this activity, <strong>the</strong>y should1. review <strong>the</strong>ir underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> genes <strong>and</strong> inheritance;2. consider how <strong>and</strong> why genetics explanationschange;3. evaluate popular media topics for scientific validity;<strong>and</strong>4. draw toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>ir ideas about <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong>methods <strong>of</strong> science from throughout <strong>the</strong> module.ESTIMATED TIMEOne 50-minute class period, plus homework119


Activity 5 ■ What Do We Know? How Do We Know It?PREPARATION AND MATERIALSAdvance preparation: Several days before conducting<strong>the</strong> activity, ask students to find articles thatm a ke claims about popular science or pseudosciencetopics that pique <strong>the</strong>ir curiosity. These articlesshould come from a variety <strong>of</strong> media sources,some reliable, some less so. Alternatively, you maywant to supply a sample <strong>of</strong> publications (for example,local newspapers, National Enquirer, or Discover)or specific articles to ensure a good selection.Have students bring to class <strong>the</strong> articles or notesabout <strong>the</strong> articles (if <strong>the</strong>y were reported in nonprintsources). Encourage selection <strong>of</strong> diverse topics, suchas reports <strong>of</strong> UFOs, crystal power, diet claims, orreports <strong>of</strong> medical treatments.On <strong>the</strong> day <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> activity, students will work in 4teams. You will need to provide <strong>the</strong> following:■ 4 large sheets <strong>of</strong> paper (for charts, plus maskingtape to post <strong>the</strong>m)■ markers <strong>of</strong> 4 different colors (one color for eachteam)■ articles about popular claims that <strong>the</strong> studentsor you supply (<strong>the</strong> source <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> articles must beindicated)Before class, post <strong>the</strong> sheets <strong>of</strong> paper where studentscan see <strong>the</strong>m <strong>and</strong> can write on <strong>the</strong>m. Allowsufficient room around each chart so that a team canga<strong>the</strong>r when <strong>the</strong> students are listing <strong>the</strong>ir ideas.INTRODUCTION“Science is a way to call <strong>the</strong> bluff <strong>of</strong>those who only pretend to knowledge.It is a bulwark against mysticism,against superstition, against religionmisapplied to where it has no businessbeing. If we’re true to its values, it cantell us when we’re being lied to.”“Finding <strong>the</strong> occasional straw <strong>of</strong> truthawash in a great ocean <strong>of</strong> confusion<strong>and</strong> bamboozle requires vigilance,dedication, <strong>and</strong> courage. But if wedon’t practice <strong>the</strong>se tough habits <strong>of</strong>thought, we cannot hope to solve <strong>the</strong>truly serious problems that face us—<strong>and</strong> we risk becoming a nation <strong>of</strong>suckers, a world <strong>of</strong> suckers, up forgrabs by <strong>the</strong> next charlatan who sauntersalong.”Carl Sagan,The Demon-haunted World: Science as a C<strong>and</strong>lein <strong>the</strong> Dark, New York: R<strong>and</strong>om House, 1995.Activities 1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 3 in this module emphasize that<strong>the</strong> principles <strong>of</strong> inheritance established by Mendel<strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r geneticists have proven remarkablydurable for more than a century. Continued researchin genetics builds on <strong>the</strong> robust foundation const ructed by <strong>the</strong>se early workers. In recent years,geneticists <strong>and</strong> molecular biologists have addede n o rmous amounts <strong>of</strong> new knowledge to that foundation.For example, advances in molecular biology—whichallow scientists to isolate, sequence, modify, <strong>and</strong> transfer DNA or RNA—have formed ap o w e rful conduit between traditional genetics <strong>and</strong>b i o c h e m i s t ry. Now, we can explain at a molecularlevel concepts such as mutation <strong>and</strong> linkage. Byextending our knowledge to cover <strong>the</strong> completegenomes <strong>of</strong> several species, <strong>the</strong> Human Genome Projectwill enhance our underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> basic geneticsas it generates new technologies, provides insightsinto disease processes, sheds light on gene regulation,<strong>and</strong> increases our knowledge <strong>of</strong> evolution.In light <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se, <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r, modern technologies <strong>and</strong>discoveries, <strong>the</strong> durability <strong>of</strong> fundamental principlesestablished by Mendel <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r early geneticists iseven more remarkable. Their ideas have enduredbecause <strong>the</strong>se scientists held <strong>the</strong>mselves accountableto <strong>the</strong> basic tenets <strong>of</strong> scientific exploration. Mostn o t a b l y, early geneticists insisted—as do all scientists—thatnew explanations be supported by evidencethat meets <strong>the</strong> requirements <strong>of</strong> science. Thisclosing activity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> module is intended to remindyour students <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se important aspects <strong>of</strong> science.Although some <strong>of</strong> your students may somedaybecome physicians, genetic counselors, or researchgeneticists, not all will have a direct need for sophisticatedknowledge <strong>of</strong> genetics. They a l l, however, willneed to underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> apply <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> scienceas <strong>the</strong>y interpret news reports, listen to advice fromphysicians, or make decisions about public policy.120©1997 by BSCS.


What Do We Know? How Do We Know It? ■ Activity 5This activity asks students to demonstrate <strong>the</strong>irunderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science by challenging<strong>the</strong>m to critique media reports <strong>of</strong> populartopics that ei<strong>the</strong>r mimic science or are scientific.Note in particular <strong>the</strong> issue raised by <strong>the</strong> discussionquestion at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> Part I <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Analysis. If scientificexplanations were voted upon, <strong>the</strong>y would nolonger count as science, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> corpus <strong>of</strong> scientificknowledge would be subject to <strong>the</strong> whims <strong>of</strong> politics<strong>and</strong> popularity. We recommend an editorial publishedin The American Biology Teacher titled “Votingin Science: Raise Your H<strong>and</strong> If You Want Humansto Have 48 Chromosomes” (J.D. McInerney <strong>and</strong> R.Moore, ABT 55(3), March 1993, pp. 132-133). Yourstudents may enjoy reading it as a follow-up to thislast activity in <strong>the</strong> module. Even reading <strong>the</strong> title tostudents could challenge <strong>the</strong>m to think about <strong>the</strong>distinction between voting on an issue in a club or apolitical setting versus voting on results to establishscientific outcomes. (The funding <strong>of</strong> scientificresearch is somewhat subject to <strong>the</strong> whims <strong>of</strong> politics.That process is different, however, from subjectingscientific knowledge itself to acceptance orrejection on <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> politics or popularity.)History records two particularly significant episodeswhere voting, in a sense, influenced <strong>the</strong> corpus <strong>of</strong>scientific knowledge, nei<strong>the</strong>r to beneficial effect. In<strong>the</strong> seventeenth century, <strong>the</strong> Catholic Church haddecreed that <strong>the</strong> biblical account <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>heavens, an account that presented Earth as <strong>the</strong> center<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> universe, was <strong>the</strong> inerrant word <strong>of</strong> God<strong>and</strong>, as such, not open to question. The greatastronomer Galileo had data to <strong>the</strong> contrary, but <strong>the</strong>Church had <strong>the</strong> power, including <strong>the</strong> power <strong>of</strong> life<strong>and</strong> death, <strong>and</strong> thus its “vote” prevailed.From <strong>the</strong> late 1940s through <strong>the</strong> mid-1960s, <strong>the</strong>unfounded, Lamarckian views <strong>of</strong> Tr<strong>of</strong>im Lysenkoheld sway over Soviet genetics, indeed, all <strong>of</strong> Sovietbiology. Lysenko’s unsupported belief in <strong>the</strong> inheritance<strong>of</strong> acquired characteristics appealed to Stalin’sviews <strong>of</strong> a society that could be molded into a Communistcollective. Ly s e n koist views were forcedupon all Soviet biologists <strong>and</strong> those biologists incountries that were under Soviet domination. Asgenetics <strong>and</strong> molecular biology were exploding in<strong>the</strong> West, <strong>the</strong> Soviet government turned its back onbiology <strong>and</strong> embraced ideology. Western journalswere forbidden or severely censored, <strong>and</strong> Sovietgeneticists who refused to adhere to Lysenko’s viewswere stripped <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir positions. Some died in laborcamps. The impact <strong>of</strong> this perversion <strong>of</strong> scienceextended beyond <strong>the</strong> scientific community. Because<strong>the</strong> Soviet five-year agricultural plans were derivedfrom Lysenko’s misguided views <strong>of</strong> heredity, <strong>the</strong>ywere doomed to fail. Some political historiansbelieve that <strong>the</strong> collapse <strong>of</strong> Soviet agriculture ultimatelyled to <strong>the</strong> downfall <strong>of</strong> Nikita Khrushchev,Soviet premier from 1958 to 1964.This activity provides students with a chance to place<strong>the</strong>ir underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong>science into a cultural setting through <strong>the</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong>science in <strong>the</strong> popular media. In doing so, studentscement <strong>the</strong>ir own underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> requirements<strong>of</strong> science <strong>and</strong> exercise <strong>the</strong>ir ability to thinkcritically <strong>and</strong> make informed decisions.STRATEGIES FOR TEACHING THE ACTIVITYBegin by telling students that this activity brings <strong>the</strong>module to a close by asking <strong>the</strong>m to evaluate what<strong>the</strong>y have learned <strong>and</strong> to use that new knowledge toanalyze current issues <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir choice. Students likelywill find <strong>the</strong> popular articles engaging. Note thatthis activity works best when <strong>the</strong> rotation from chartto chart is crisp <strong>and</strong> quick. The goal is to generatelots <strong>of</strong> information in a short time, leaving plenty <strong>of</strong>time to analyze it. Students also have a sense <strong>of</strong> ownershipbecause <strong>the</strong>y select <strong>the</strong> popular media topicsto be critiqued.It is critical to keep this discussion focused on criteriafor a good s c i e n t i f i c explanation. The discussionshould not be an emotional exchange <strong>of</strong>u n s u p p o rted opinions, because <strong>the</strong>n it only willmodel shared ignorance. The important issue ishow to judge <strong>the</strong> scientific validity <strong>of</strong> a claim basedon <strong>the</strong> information available. Some popular claims,such as <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> garlic on fighting various infections,may be valid but reported in an unreliablew a y. For example, if a report makes extravagantclaims for <strong>the</strong> healing power <strong>of</strong> garlic without providingany scientific evidence, <strong>the</strong> source is not reliable<strong>and</strong> students should not conclude that <strong>the</strong>claim has been shown to be valid. You could askstudents what is missing in <strong>the</strong>se situations (such as121©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 5 ■ What Do We Know? How Do We Know It?scientific tests, extensive observations, or reasoningabout how a process takes place). In many cases,students could pursue this missing information inmore reliable sources.Use <strong>the</strong> Assessment to evaluate different aspects <strong>of</strong>student learning. Question 2b in <strong>the</strong> Assessmentshould help students see that scientific knowledgecontinues to grow with new work building on old.Question 3 is very open-ended; it provides a chancefor students to see <strong>the</strong> influence <strong>of</strong> society in science<strong>and</strong> vice versa. Finally, <strong>the</strong> exercise in Question 4provides an opportunity to test each student’s abilityto use <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science to critique reportsencountered in <strong>the</strong> popular media. This performanceassessment mimics real-life needs for scientificunderst<strong>and</strong>ing.The following summary provides a snapshot <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>activity.ACTIVITY 5 AT A GLANCEPart I:Part II:Part III:Critique <strong>of</strong> popular media topicsTeams work in carousel fashion to list examples <strong>and</strong> evaluate <strong>the</strong>m for scientific merit.AnalysisThe class discusses <strong>the</strong> merits <strong>of</strong> popular media topics <strong>and</strong> considers how science appliesto <strong>the</strong>ir own lives.AssessmentStudents review what <strong>the</strong>y have learned about how science works <strong>and</strong> about lasting <strong>and</strong>new explanations in genetics. They apply <strong>the</strong>ir skills individually to evaluate popularmedia topics.122©1997 by BSCS.


What Do We Know? How Do We Know It? ■ Activity 5Annotated Student MaterialSeparate student pages contain <strong>the</strong> material shown in bold typeface.Here, we provide an annotated version <strong>of</strong> student materials to help you conduct <strong>the</strong> activity.“They say getting too much suncan give you cancer.”Who are “<strong>the</strong>y”? Should you believe what“<strong>the</strong>y” say, <strong>and</strong>, if so, should you change yourbehavior in any way?All <strong>of</strong> us must assume responsibility for makingdecisions that affect us <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs, butthis responsibility does not guarantee that wewill make good decisions. That skill requireslots <strong>of</strong> practice <strong>and</strong> careful reasoning, <strong>and</strong> itrequires intelligent analysis <strong>of</strong> inform a t i o n .For health-related comments, such as <strong>the</strong> precedingquote, you should consider <strong>the</strong> scientificvalidity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> statement to determ i n ewhe<strong>the</strong>r you think <strong>the</strong> statement is reliable.Even if you decide that it is reliable, <strong>the</strong>choice <strong>of</strong> what to do about it still remainsyours. (Is having a tan or relaxing in <strong>the</strong> sunlightworth <strong>the</strong> risk <strong>of</strong> getting skin cancer?How can you reduce <strong>the</strong> risk?) Whatever youdecide, <strong>the</strong> ability to determine <strong>the</strong> scientificvalidity <strong>of</strong> a popular claim is a valuable skillfor any person.As you work through this last activity, you willevaluate what you have learned in <strong>the</strong> previousactivities.PROCEDUREPart I: Critique <strong>of</strong> Popular Media TopicsYou are going to critique claims from <strong>the</strong> popularpress for <strong>the</strong>ir scientific validity.1. Recall from <strong>the</strong> previous activities <strong>the</strong>requirements <strong>of</strong> a good scientific explanation<strong>and</strong> list <strong>the</strong>m.Spend one or two minutes polling students to makea collective class list. See Section III <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Overviewfor Teachers for a detailed discussion.2. Join your team <strong>and</strong> look at <strong>the</strong> articles <strong>the</strong>team members have collected. Choose oneto present to <strong>the</strong> class. Discuss <strong>the</strong> reasons<strong>the</strong> article gives (if any) to support <strong>the</strong> claimbeing reported. (Be prepared to list <strong>the</strong> ideafrom <strong>the</strong> article <strong>and</strong> to give supporting evidence,if any, on a chart in <strong>the</strong> classroom.You have 5-10 minutes to prepare.)You may let students work from articles <strong>the</strong>y bring toclass, or you may want to provide a collection <strong>of</strong> art i-cles yourself. To avoid redundancy on <strong>the</strong> charts, youcan circulate during <strong>the</strong> preparation time <strong>and</strong>encourage students not to pick similar topics.Note from <strong>the</strong> field test: Some students had difficultylooking for supporting evidence <strong>and</strong> acknowledgingsources, such as a reference to a report in arefereed technical journal. Offer helpful suggestions,but keep <strong>the</strong> activity moving quickly. Omissions orweaknesses in underst<strong>and</strong>ing can be addressed comfortablyin Part II, <strong>the</strong> Analysis.3. When your teacher gives <strong>the</strong> signal, movewith your team to one chart <strong>and</strong> quickly list<strong>the</strong> claim from your team’s article <strong>and</strong> indicate<strong>the</strong> source. Next show w h y this claim issupposed to be correct, according to <strong>the</strong> art i-cle. Spend only 2 minutes at this task.123©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 5 ■ What Do We Know? How Do We Know It?Time <strong>the</strong> steps <strong>and</strong> stop each team after two minutes.Next, have teams quickly rotate to <strong>the</strong> nextchart.4. Move with your team to <strong>the</strong> next chart. Read<strong>the</strong> ideas listed by <strong>the</strong> previous team <strong>and</strong>critique this claim for its scientific validity.You have 3 minutes.Stop <strong>the</strong> task at three minutes. Note that a team’scomments can be identified by <strong>the</strong> color <strong>of</strong> its markingpen.Part II: AnalysisRespond to <strong>the</strong> following questions as <strong>the</strong>class discusses <strong>the</strong> charts.This discussion should n o t be an emotionalexchange <strong>of</strong> opinions <strong>and</strong> beliefs. Instead, it shouldmodel a scientific analysis. The task is not to determinewhich students value each claim, but ra<strong>the</strong>r toevaluate <strong>the</strong> potential scientific validity <strong>of</strong> each claim.First, help students decide whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> claim isaddressable by science or remains in <strong>the</strong> realm <strong>of</strong>untestable belief, religion, or o<strong>the</strong>r nonscientificways <strong>of</strong> knowing. If a claim is testable scientifically,students should determine <strong>the</strong> weight <strong>of</strong> evidence, ifany is available. The following questions may guide<strong>the</strong> discussion.1. What is <strong>the</strong> difference between a belief <strong>and</strong>an explanation that meets <strong>the</strong> requirements<strong>of</strong> science?This distinction is important for your students.Encourage <strong>the</strong>m to see that some ideas, such as creationism,<strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> Tarot cards, or o<strong>the</strong>r means <strong>of</strong>fortune-telling, may appeal to people emotionally<strong>and</strong> thus may encourage belief. Such beliefs, however,are based on faith, or belief in supernaturalevents, or on revealed knowledge; <strong>the</strong>y are notbased on reliable, scientific evidence. Such beliefsabsolutely do not meet <strong>the</strong> requirements <strong>of</strong> scientificvalidity <strong>and</strong> thus are not taught as a part <strong>of</strong> scienceor recognized as legitimate subjects for scientificresearch. The question <strong>of</strong> an individual’s belief insuch topics, particularly religious subjects, is a matter<strong>of</strong> personal choice, completely external to a study<strong>of</strong>, <strong>and</strong> respect for, scientific knowledge. If any studentshave listed creationism as a science-basedclaim, this discussion may provide an opportunityfor you to help <strong>the</strong> class underst<strong>and</strong> that evolution isa scientifically valid explanation <strong>of</strong> biological changethat is fully accepted by <strong>the</strong> scientific community,while creationism is not. Ongoing scientific debatesabout some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subtleties <strong>of</strong> evolutionary processdo not constitute any fundamental disagreementabout <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> evolution itself. The issue in abiology class is not whe<strong>the</strong>r a student should believein creationism or whe<strong>the</strong>r it is “right or wrong”; <strong>the</strong>issue is that it is not supported by scientific evidence<strong>and</strong> thus is not a scientific explanation. Evolution iswell-supported scientifically <strong>and</strong> is accepted as a scientificexplanation; <strong>the</strong>refore it is suitable for studyin a biology class.You also may encounter concern about <strong>the</strong> term “<strong>the</strong>ory.” Students frequently misunderst<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> meaning<strong>of</strong> this word in a scientific setting. Students may thinka <strong>the</strong>ory is a “guess.” In informal use, one may hear anexchange such as this: “In <strong>the</strong>ory, I was going to win<strong>the</strong> lottery.” This ligh<strong>the</strong>arted use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> word “<strong>the</strong>ory”may leave students with <strong>the</strong> incorrect idea that at h e o ry is an unfounded speculation. In science, a <strong>the</strong>ory is a well-supported conceptual framework thatexplains a large number <strong>of</strong> observations about <strong>the</strong> naturalworld. Scientific <strong>the</strong>ories also have predictivep o w e r, that is, <strong>the</strong>y can help to predict <strong>the</strong> occurr e n c e<strong>of</strong> natural phenomena. Chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory, forexample, predicts <strong>the</strong> occurrence <strong>of</strong> Mendelian ratios.Students generally accept atomic <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>and</strong> chromosome<strong>the</strong>ory without discomfort. The <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> evolutionis no different in its scientific validity, althougho ften it is singled out as a topic <strong>of</strong> concern because itconflicts with <strong>the</strong> religious beliefs <strong>of</strong> some people.Evolution <strong>the</strong>ory is a well-reasoned scientific explanationthat is supported by an overwhelming <strong>and</strong> evergrowingbody <strong>of</strong> evidence as diverse as <strong>the</strong> fossilrecord or molecular experiments that demonstrateselection. (See Section III <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> O v e rview for Te a c h-e r s for more discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> scientific evidenceto address scientific explanations.)Many popular beliefs persist because it is easy to beintellectually lazy. If it is not easy to dem<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong>locate supporting or refuting evidence, people maytend to follow <strong>the</strong> easier path <strong>and</strong> respond based ona whim ra<strong>the</strong>r than evidence. In addition, somepeople embrace unsubstantiated explanations124©1997 by BSCS.


What Do We Know? How Do We Know It? ■ Activity 5simply because <strong>the</strong>y feel better doing so—<strong>the</strong>y findcomfort in <strong>the</strong>ir beliefs. It is extremely importantthat students learn to distinguish well-reasoned, scientificallyjustified explanations from those that arerooted in unsupported assertions.2. Based on <strong>the</strong> information presented on <strong>the</strong>charts, are any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> claims scientificallyvalid?Students will <strong>of</strong>fer a variety <strong>of</strong> opinions about whichtopics are scientifically sound <strong>and</strong> which are not.3. How could your actions be influenced byknowing whe<strong>the</strong>r a claim is scientificallyvalid?A student who underst<strong>and</strong>s <strong>the</strong> criteria for a scientificallyvalid claim may be less likely to be swayed byfalse claims that appear in <strong>the</strong> media or that havegeneral public currency. The choice to buy certainover-<strong>the</strong>-counter medications, for example, might beinfluenced by assessing <strong>the</strong> efficacy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se products.Some topics, such as religious beliefs, fall outside<strong>the</strong> reach <strong>of</strong> science <strong>and</strong> should not be treatedas scientific explanations.4. Many health or science claims have significancein society because <strong>the</strong>y are popular;that is, <strong>the</strong>y have emotional appeal for manypeople. How would <strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong> scientists bedifferent if scientific explanations were v o t e du p o n for popularity ra<strong>the</strong>r than held up to<strong>the</strong> scientific st<strong>and</strong>ards discussed in thism o d u l e ?This question may provoke considerable discussion; ifso, judge whe<strong>the</strong>r it is worth taking <strong>the</strong> time to pursueit. At this point, you could introduce <strong>the</strong> editorial citedon p. 121 (“Voting in Science: Raise Your H<strong>and</strong> If Yo uWant Humans to Have 48 Chromosomes”).You might want to close <strong>the</strong> module with a final lookat <strong>the</strong> NMS poster.Part III: AssessmentRespond to <strong>the</strong> following questions or assignmentsaccording to your teacher’s instru c t i o n s .Students can be assessed individually or as a team,using written or oral responses.1. Recall your experiences from <strong>the</strong> module asyou answer <strong>the</strong> following questions:a. What does science try to do?b. How do we know that our scientificexplanations are on <strong>the</strong> right track?c. What counts as evidence in science?d. What makes science objective?e. How <strong>and</strong> why does scientific knowledgechange?f. What is <strong>the</strong> difference between pseudoscience<strong>and</strong> science?This material is based on Section III <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Overviewfor Teachers. That section may help you evaluate studentresponses.2. The previous activities used genetics examplesto show how science works.a. What genetics concepts did you discussin this module?Traditional genetics concepts are presented as milestoneexplanations in Activity 1. Expect students tolist many <strong>of</strong> those or similar concepts. Figure 12 onpage 36 <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Overview for Teachers also summarizestraditional concepts. Students also should listsome new genetics concepts if <strong>the</strong>y have used Activity2 or Activity 3. Samples <strong>of</strong> how students mightstate <strong>the</strong>se concepts follow:■ Some genetic information is inherited from <strong>the</strong>mo<strong>the</strong>r without equivalent information from <strong>the</strong>fa<strong>the</strong>r.■ Offspring inherit some genes that are not part <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> nuclear chromosomes.■ Some organelles have <strong>the</strong>ir own genes.■Some genetic disorders get worse or showdecreased age <strong>of</strong> onset in subsequent generations.■ Some genes change as <strong>the</strong>y pass to <strong>the</strong> next generation.■ Mutant genes that cause Huntington disease <strong>and</strong>myotonic dystrophy have unstable trinucleotiderepeats.■ The severity <strong>of</strong> HD <strong>and</strong> DM is related to <strong>the</strong>number <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> mutantgene associated with each disorder.■ The number <strong>of</strong> repeats in mutant HD or DM125©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 5 ■ What Do We Know? How Do We Know It?genes helps predict whe<strong>the</strong>r an individual willdevelop <strong>the</strong> disease, <strong>the</strong> likely age <strong>of</strong> onset, <strong>and</strong>how severe <strong>the</strong> symptoms may be.b. Were any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetics concepts you listednew to you? Identify <strong>the</strong>m <strong>and</strong> indicate<strong>the</strong>ir relationship to earlier knowledge.Concepts related to extranuclear (mitochondrial)inheritance or to genetic anticipation likely are newto students. These concepts connect to <strong>and</strong> extendexisting knowledge. For example, explanations <strong>of</strong>anticipation also take into account what we knowabout dominant inheritance. Ideas such as <strong>the</strong>se maybe st<strong>and</strong>ard textbook material in <strong>the</strong> future because<strong>the</strong>y have become so widely accepted that <strong>the</strong>y areno longer novel. This process parallels <strong>the</strong> growth <strong>of</strong>scientific underst<strong>and</strong>ing.3. How are science <strong>and</strong> society related? Give atleast two examples.Responses will vary. For example, scientific discoveriesin genetics <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r fields (such as deciphering<strong>the</strong> genetic code, identifying <strong>the</strong> function <strong>of</strong> specificgenes, or developing <strong>the</strong> computer chip) actuallychange <strong>the</strong> way a society functions. These discoverieshave altered diagnosis <strong>and</strong> treatment <strong>of</strong> disease,thus changing attitudes toward disease, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>yhave changed <strong>the</strong> way we store <strong>and</strong> use informationin our business <strong>and</strong> leisure activities. Similarly, <strong>the</strong>concerns <strong>of</strong> a society help determine <strong>the</strong> path thatresearch takes. Issues considered important by societyare more likely to receive funding or to bebrought to <strong>the</strong> attention <strong>of</strong> scientists. In some cases,society’s interest takes <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> guidelines orrestrictions in science, such as restrictions on <strong>the</strong>use <strong>of</strong> human subjects or safety precautions in laboratoriesthat work with recombinant DNA.4. Your job is to critique two claims that you seer e p o rted in popular media. One should be ascience or health report that you consider tobe scientifically reliable, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r shouldbe a claim that you think is not scientificallysubstantiated. (Remember, a claim could bebased on a correct idea but reported poorly,making <strong>the</strong> r e p o rt scientifically unsound.)Assessment <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> student responses should include<strong>the</strong>se criteria:■ Is <strong>the</strong> critique clearly presented?■ Does <strong>the</strong> student recognize <strong>the</strong> role <strong>and</strong> significance<strong>of</strong> scientific evidence (or its lack)?■Does <strong>the</strong> student recognize <strong>the</strong> importance <strong>of</strong>reliable reasoning <strong>and</strong> relevance <strong>of</strong> evidence?■ Does <strong>the</strong> student see <strong>the</strong> predictive power <strong>of</strong> scientificexplanations?■ Does <strong>the</strong> student appreciate <strong>the</strong> power <strong>of</strong> multiplelines <strong>of</strong> evidence?■Does <strong>the</strong> student distinguish between crediblescientific evidence <strong>and</strong> unreliable information?■ Does <strong>the</strong> student distinguish between an explanationthat is well-supported by scientific evidence<strong>and</strong> an opinion?■Does <strong>the</strong> critique clearly cite <strong>the</strong> source <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>reports?126©1997 by BSCS.


SPECIAL COPYMASTERS FORCLASSROOM ACTIVITIESThese copymasters are used as h<strong>and</strong>-outsor to make overhead transparencies.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1Copymaster 1-1: Milestones in Underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>Genetics</strong>Here, we omit numbers for milestones to avoid directing students to a particular sequence.MILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:Why do <strong>of</strong>fspring resemble <strong>the</strong>ir parents?Milestone Explanation:Parents contribute genetic material to <strong>the</strong>ir<strong>of</strong>fspring.MILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:How are traits distributed in <strong>of</strong>fspring?Milestone Explanation:Alleles <strong>of</strong> one gene segregate in <strong>the</strong> formation<strong>of</strong> gametes.(Reproductive cells [gametes] form duringmeiosis. Each gamete contains one allelefrom <strong>the</strong> pair <strong>of</strong> alleles present in <strong>the</strong>p a r e n t . )MILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:Where are genes located?Milestone Explanation:Genes are located on chromosomes.(This idea is <strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong>inheritance. In eukaryotic cells, geneticmaterial is located in <strong>the</strong> nucleus in structurescalled chromosomes, for <strong>the</strong>ir darkstainingcharacteristic. The name comesfrom <strong>the</strong> Greek words chroma [color] <strong>and</strong>soma [body].)MILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:What determines <strong>the</strong> sex <strong>of</strong> an organism?Milestone Explanation:In most sexually reproducing organisms, specialchromosomes determine sex.(Humans have sex chromosomes, designatedX <strong>and</strong> Y. The combination <strong>of</strong> sex chromosomesin an organism determines its sex.)129©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsCopymaster 1-1: Milestones in Underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>Genetics</strong>Here, we omit numbers for milestones to avoid directing students to a particular sequence.MILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:Why do some traits occur toge<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>of</strong>fspring?Milestone Explanation:Some genes are located on <strong>the</strong> same chromosome(linkage).MILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:What molecular component in chromosomescarries genetic information?Milestone Explanation:DNA carries genetic information.MILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:How is <strong>the</strong> genetic information used tomake proteins encoded?Milestone Explanation:In DNA, a triplet <strong>of</strong> nucleotide basesencodes each amino acid in <strong>the</strong> resultantprotein.MILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:How does a new, heritable trait appear in apopulation?Milestone Explanation:Mutations change <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> DNA inreproductive cells (gametes).130©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1Copymaster 1-2: Historic Sequence <strong>of</strong> Milestones1. Parents contribute genetic material to <strong>the</strong>ir<strong>of</strong>fspring.2. Alleles <strong>of</strong> one gene segregate in <strong>the</strong> formation<strong>of</strong> gametes.3. Genes are located on chromosomes.4. In most sexually reproducing organisms, specialchromosomes determine sex.5. Some genes are located on <strong>the</strong> samechromosome (linkage).6. DNA carries genetic information.7. In DNA, a triplet <strong>of</strong> nucleotide bases encodeseach amino acid in <strong>the</strong> resultant protein.8. Mutations change <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> DNA in reproductivecells (gametes).131©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsCopymaster 1-3: Evidence CardsMILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:Why do <strong>of</strong>fspring resemble <strong>the</strong>ir parents?Milestone Explanation:Parents contribute genetic material to <strong>the</strong>ir<strong>of</strong>fspring.parents <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspringEvidence A:A scientist looked through a microscope atdividing cells in <strong>the</strong> tail fins <strong>of</strong> a salam<strong>and</strong>er.As mitosis proceeded, she saw that chromosomesmoved apart in equal numbers into<strong>the</strong> newly forming daughter cells. O<strong>the</strong>r scientistsobserved this phenomenon in cellsundergoing mitosis.parents <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspringEvidence B:A scientist crossed pea plants <strong>and</strong> carefullyrecorded <strong>the</strong> appearance <strong>of</strong> certain traits in<strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring. When he crossed a strain thathas only purple flowers with one that hasonly white flowers, <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring always hadpurple flowers. When he crossed <strong>the</strong>se <strong>of</strong>fspringto produce <strong>the</strong> next generation, however,he saw both colors <strong>of</strong> flower in <strong>the</strong> new<strong>of</strong>fspring in regular proportions.This work was repeated later by o<strong>the</strong>r scientistswho saw <strong>the</strong> same results.parents <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspringEvidence C:Jorge noticed that a classmate, Susan, hascurly hair. When he met her mo<strong>the</strong>r, henoticed that she also has curly hair.parents <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspringEvidence D:Mendel said that characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspringlikely come from something <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspringget from <strong>the</strong>ir parents.132©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1Copymaster 1-3: Evidence CardsMILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:How are traits distributed in <strong>of</strong>fspring?Milestone Explanation:Alleles <strong>of</strong> one gene segregate in <strong>the</strong> formation<strong>of</strong> gametes.(Reproductive cells [gametes] form duringmeiosis. Each gamete contains one allelefrom <strong>the</strong> pair <strong>of</strong> alleles present in <strong>the</strong> parent.)segregationEvidence A:People say that sons express <strong>the</strong> traits <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>fa<strong>the</strong>r, while daughters have all <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r’scharacteristics.segregationEvidence B:Offspring in each generation arei d e n t i c a l .segregationEvidence C:Mendel speculated that traits are inheritedbased on discrete units <strong>of</strong> inheritance. Hetested <strong>the</strong> law <strong>of</strong> segregation by observingheight in several generations <strong>of</strong> pea plants.He saw a distribution <strong>of</strong> tall to dwarf in <strong>the</strong>F2 generation <strong>of</strong> 3:1. Then <strong>the</strong> F2 plantswere fertilized with <strong>the</strong>ir own pollen (selfed).Mendel found that <strong>the</strong> dwarf F2 plantsproduced dwarf F3 plants, but two-thirds <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> tall F2 plants produced mixed <strong>of</strong>fspring,dwarf <strong>and</strong> tall. This F3 test <strong>of</strong> segregationhas been repeated many times with <strong>the</strong>same results.segregationEvidence D:A scientist named W.S. Sutton observed chromosomesin cells undergoing meiosis. Henoticed that <strong>the</strong> chromosomes behaved in away that is consistent with Mendel’s observationsabout inheritance patterns. Many o<strong>the</strong>robservations <strong>of</strong> meiosis by o<strong>the</strong>r scientistsconfirmed this behavior <strong>of</strong> chromosomes in<strong>the</strong> nucleus.133©1997 by BSCS.


Activity I ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsCopymaster 1-3: Evidence CardsMILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:Where are genes located?Milestone Explanation:Genes are located on chromosomes.(This idea is <strong>the</strong> chromosome <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong>inheritance. In eukaryotic cells, geneticmaterial is located in <strong>the</strong> nucleus in structurescalled chromosomes, for <strong>the</strong>ir darkstainingcharacteristic. The name comesfrom <strong>the</strong> Greek words chroma [color] <strong>and</strong>soma [body].)genes on chromosomesEvidence A:Using staining techniques <strong>and</strong> a microscope, C.Nageli discovered a set <strong>of</strong> structures in <strong>the</strong>nuclei <strong>of</strong> cells. O<strong>the</strong>r scientists observed that<strong>the</strong>se structures change <strong>and</strong> become visiblewith a microscope at certain times in <strong>the</strong> cellcycle. Years after Nageli’s observation, a developmentalbiologist, W. Roux, observed <strong>the</strong>ses t ructures in <strong>the</strong> cell nucleus, <strong>and</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r scientist,W. Wa l d e y e r, saw <strong>the</strong> structures <strong>and</strong>named <strong>the</strong>m chromosomes.genes on chromosomesEvidence B:A scientist named W.S. Sutton observed chromosomesin cells undergoing meiosis. Henoticed that <strong>the</strong> chromosomes behaved in away that is consistent with Mendel’s observationsabout inheritance patterns. Many o<strong>the</strong>robservations <strong>of</strong> meiosis by o<strong>the</strong>r scientistsconfirmed this behavior <strong>of</strong> chromosomes in<strong>the</strong> nucleus.genes on chromosomesEvidence C:A scientist, T. Boveri, showed that sea urchinembryos develop normally only when <strong>the</strong>yhave a full set <strong>of</strong> chromosomes. Embryoswith more or fewer chromosomes than <strong>the</strong>normally observed number did not developproperly. Many o<strong>the</strong>r scientists have made<strong>the</strong> same observations in o<strong>the</strong>r organisms.genes on chromosomesEvidence D:A pr<strong>of</strong>essor at Harvard thinks that chromosomescontain genes.134©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1Copymaster 1-3: Evidence CardsMILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:What determines <strong>the</strong> sex <strong>of</strong> an organism?Milestone Explanation:In most sexually reproducing organisms, specialchromosomes determine sex.(Humans have sex chromosomes, designatedX <strong>and</strong> Y. The combination <strong>of</strong> sex chromosomesin an organism determines its sex.)sex determinationEvidence A:Many studies have shown that humanfemales have two X chromosomes, <strong>and</strong>human males have one X chromosome <strong>and</strong>one Y chromosome. Patricia Jacobs <strong>and</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r scientists also showed that, although<strong>the</strong> normal human sex chromosome complementis XX or XY, o<strong>the</strong>r patterns do occurrarely. For instance, in humans, XXY individualsare male, <strong>and</strong> XO individuals are female.sex determinationEvidence B:During <strong>the</strong>ir lifetime, human males producemany sperm, but females produce only a feweggs. Many years <strong>of</strong> studies showed thatfemales are born with all <strong>the</strong> egg cells <strong>the</strong>ywill ever have (although <strong>the</strong> eggs mustmature individually during successive menstrualcycles). Males, however, produce millions<strong>of</strong> new sperm cells every few days untila very advanced age.sex determinationEvidence C:A scientist named C.E. McClung found thatgrasshoppers produce equal quantities <strong>of</strong> twodifferent types <strong>of</strong> sperm, one <strong>of</strong> which containsan extra chromosome. Three years later,two o<strong>the</strong>r scientists, N. Stevens <strong>and</strong> E.B. Wi l-son, determined that female grasshoppershave two copies <strong>of</strong> one particular chromosome,whereas males have only one.sex determinationEvidence D:There is a saying that a pregnant woman c<strong>and</strong>etermine <strong>the</strong> sex <strong>of</strong> her child by eatingspicy foods to produce a male or cool foodsto produce a female.135©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsCopymaster 1-3: Evidence CardsMILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:Why do some traits occur toge<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>of</strong>fspring?Milestone Explanation:Some genes are located on <strong>the</strong> same chromosome(linkage).linkageEvidence A:The host <strong>of</strong> a popular talk-show about sportssays that <strong>the</strong> best baseball pitchers havebrown eyes.linkageEvidence B:Many microscopic studies show that chromosomepairs can exchange material duringmeiosis, resulting in a new combination <strong>of</strong>alleles in that pair <strong>of</strong> chromosomes. Thisphenomenon is genetic recombination,which results from crossing over.linkageEvidence C:Three scientists demonstrated that purpleflowers <strong>and</strong> long pollen were inheritedtoge<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> sweet pea more <strong>of</strong>ten (morethan 75% <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> time) than predicted byMendel’s law <strong>of</strong> independent assortment(50%).linkageEvidence D:A study <strong>of</strong> human pedigrees shows that certaintraits such as hemophilia <strong>and</strong> color blindnessoccur at a much higher frequency inmales than in females. These traits appear todepend on <strong>the</strong> inheritance <strong>of</strong> mutations locatedon <strong>the</strong> X chromosome. When a man hasboth <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se traits, studies show that <strong>the</strong>re isa greater than 50% chance that any bro<strong>the</strong>rswill have both disorders, or nei<strong>the</strong>r one.136©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1Copymaster 1-3: Evidence CardsMILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:What molecular component in chromosomescarries genetic information?Milestone Explanation:DNA carries genetic information.DNA as genetic materialEvidence A:Three scientists purified DNA from bacteriathat grew in smooth colonies. They put thisDNA into bacteria that normally grew inrough colonies. The bacteria that had beengiven <strong>the</strong> DNA produced many generations<strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring that formed smooth colonies.This experiment produced <strong>the</strong> same resultswhen repeated.O<strong>the</strong>r scientists (many years later) transferreda specific fragment <strong>of</strong> DNA from bacteriato a plant, <strong>and</strong> a bacterial trait appearedin <strong>the</strong> plant.DNA as genetic materialEvidence B:Two scientists studied viruses to determinehow <strong>the</strong>y infect bacteria. They labeled <strong>the</strong>DNA <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> protein components <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>viruses using radioactive chemicals. Thisallowed <strong>the</strong>m to trace <strong>the</strong> movement <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>DNA <strong>and</strong> protein. Only labeled DNA entered<strong>the</strong> bacterial cells during <strong>the</strong> infectionprocess. O<strong>the</strong>r investigations repeated <strong>the</strong>sestudies.DNA as genetic materialEvidence C:Scientists have extracted DNA from manydifferent cell types in one organism <strong>and</strong>from <strong>the</strong> cells <strong>of</strong> many different species.DNA as genetic materialEvidence D:Scientist Francis Crick <strong>and</strong> a student namedJames Watson agreed that DNA might be <strong>the</strong>genetic material.137©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 1 ■ St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsCopymaster 1-3: Evidence CardsMILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:How is <strong>the</strong> genetic information used tomake proteins encoded?Milestone Explanation:In DNA, a triplet <strong>of</strong> nucleotide basesencodes each amino acid in <strong>the</strong> resultantprotein.genetic codeEvidence A:To determine how <strong>the</strong> genetic code mightwork, scientists noted that DNA has four differentbases that can be arranged in varioussequences. The code must be able to specify<strong>the</strong> 20 different amino acids found in proteins.Ma<strong>the</strong>matical principles predict <strong>the</strong>following about <strong>the</strong> genetic code:one-base code specifies 4 amino acids at mosttwo-base code specifies 16 amino acids at mostthree-base code s p e c i f i e s 64 amino acids at mostf o u r-base code s p e c i f i e s 256 amino acids at mostgenetic codeEvidence B:Investigators found that removal <strong>of</strong> threenucleotides from a gene causes <strong>the</strong> resultingprotein to lose one amino acid. However,removal <strong>of</strong> one or two nucleotides from agene causes much more disruption in <strong>the</strong>resulting protein structure. O<strong>the</strong>r scientistsquickly repeated <strong>the</strong>se experiments <strong>and</strong> got<strong>the</strong> same results.genetic codeEvidence C:Many types <strong>of</strong> chemical analysis have shownthat DNA contains about equal amounts <strong>of</strong>four different components (<strong>the</strong> nucleotides,which contain bases abbreviated A, G, C, <strong>and</strong>T). In contrast, proteins are made <strong>of</strong> 20 differentcomponents (amino acids), <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>yvary in amount in different proteins.genetic codeEvidence D:A shampoo is advertised as containing DNA<strong>and</strong> able to enrich hair.138©1997 by BSCS.


St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> Shoulders <strong>of</strong> Giants ■ Activity 1Copymaster 1-3: Evidence CardsMILESTONES INUNDERSTANDING GENETICSQuestion:How does a new, heritable trait appear in apopulation?Milestone Explanation:Mutations change <strong>the</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> DNA inreproductive cells (gametes).mutationsEvidence A:People who build large muscles throughexercise will have children who also havelarge muscles.mutationsEvidence B:A scientist named Hermann J. Muller exposedf ruit flies to increasing doses <strong>of</strong> radiation in<strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> X rays. He kept careful records <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> mutant traits that appeared in<strong>the</strong>ir <strong>of</strong>fspring. Muller found that <strong>the</strong>re was adirect correlation between <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong>mutations <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> amount <strong>of</strong> radiation: moreX rays produced more mutant <strong>of</strong>fspring.(Later research showed that X rays damagechromosomes.) O<strong>the</strong>r scientists have repeatedthis experiment, <strong>and</strong> similar experimentshave been repeated many times.mutationsEvidence C:Investigators found that removal <strong>of</strong> threenucleotides from a gene causes <strong>the</strong> resultingprotein to lose one amino acid. However,removal <strong>of</strong> one or two nucleotides from agene causes much more disruption in <strong>the</strong>resulting protein structure. O<strong>the</strong>r scientistsquickly repeated <strong>the</strong>se experiments <strong>and</strong> got<strong>the</strong> same results.mutationsEvidence D:Investigators at <strong>the</strong> Toronto Hospital for SickChildren studied <strong>the</strong> DNA <strong>of</strong> children whohave cystic fibrosis (CF). The investigators alsostudied <strong>the</strong> parents <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se children. Theyfound that <strong>the</strong>se children <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir parentshave changes in <strong>the</strong>ir DNA that are not presentin unaffected children or in persons whodo not carry <strong>the</strong> CF gene. Fu r<strong>the</strong>r investigationhas revealed more than 600 differentDNA mutations in <strong>the</strong> CF gene.139©1997 by BSCS.


Puzzling Pedigrees ■ Activity 2Copymaster 2-1: Pedigrees A-H141©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling PedigreesCopymaster 2-1: Pedigrees A-H142©1997 by BSCS.


Puzzling Pedigrees ■ Activity 2Copymaster 2-2: Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J143©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling PedigreesCopymaster 2-3: Pedigrees I´ <strong>and</strong> J´144©1997 by BSCS.


Puzzling Pedigrees ■ Activity 2Copymaster 2-4: Building an ExplanationHypo<strong>the</strong>sis 1:Supporting Evidence:Conflicting Evidence:Conclusions:Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 2:Supporting Evidence:Conflicting Evidence:Conclusions:145©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling PedigreesCopymaster 2-5 Science ArticlesHow much DNA is enough? Humans have 46 DNAcontainingchromosomes in each diploid-cell nucleus;<strong>the</strong> peas that Mendel studied have only 14 chromosomes.In contrast, a bacterium, which is a prokaryote,contains one large molecule <strong>of</strong> DNA that serves as itschromosome. (To compare eukaryotic <strong>and</strong> prokaryoticcells, see <strong>the</strong> illustration.) Each species has a specificamount <strong>of</strong> DNA in its chromosome set, but this DNA isnot <strong>the</strong> whole story. Many cells contain “extra” DNA inaddition to <strong>the</strong> major genome. Of course, this DNA is“extra” only in <strong>the</strong> way people may view it. For instance,students <strong>of</strong> genetics usually begin by studying <strong>the</strong>genes found on chromosomes in <strong>the</strong> nucleus <strong>of</strong> aeukaryotic cell. In this article, we describe <strong>the</strong> existence<strong>of</strong> DNA in addition to <strong>the</strong> chromosome set.In some bacterial cells, DNA outside <strong>the</strong> genomeoccurs as tiny circular molecules known as plasmids.The plasmid DNA replicates on its own, <strong>and</strong> it has beenused by scientists as a tool for cloning.In eukaryotic cells, some DNA is found outside <strong>the</strong>nucleus. For example, green plant cells that carry outphotosyn<strong>the</strong>sis have small organelles called chloroplasts.Chloroplasts contain <strong>the</strong> green pigment neededfor photosyn<strong>the</strong>sis. In addition, chloroplasts have <strong>the</strong>irown DNA, separate from <strong>the</strong> chromosomes found in<strong>the</strong> cell nucleus.All species <strong>of</strong> eukaryotes have o<strong>the</strong>r organellesknown as mitochondria, which also have <strong>the</strong>ir own DNA(mtDNA). Mitochondria help generate energy for <strong>the</strong>cell. A human muscle cell, for instance, may contain severalhundred mitochondria in its cytoplasm. Mitochondriahelp provide energy for <strong>the</strong> muscle to contract. ThemtDNA in each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se mitochondria can replicate onits own, separate from <strong>the</strong> DNA <strong>of</strong> chromosomes in <strong>the</strong>n u c l e u s .Human mitochondrial DNA has been fairly wellstudied. Each mitochondrion has multiple copies <strong>of</strong>its mtDNA, <strong>and</strong> each copy is made up <strong>of</strong> 16,569 basepairs arranged as a double-str<strong>and</strong>ed, circular molecule.The entire mtDNA sequence was identified in<strong>the</strong> 1980s. It includes genes that code for transferRNAs, ribosomal RNAs (different from those in <strong>the</strong>rest <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> cell), <strong>and</strong> a few enzymes needed for <strong>the</strong>e n e r g y-related functions <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mitochondria. MtDNA“Extra” DNA?has a higher rate <strong>of</strong> mutation than does nuclear DNA,<strong>and</strong> molecular biologists have identified some disordersin humans that result from mutant mitochondrialgenes. As you might expect, given <strong>the</strong> role <strong>of</strong> mitochondria,<strong>the</strong>se disorders <strong>of</strong>ten involve defects inenergy production. O<strong>the</strong>rs involve problems relatedto muscles <strong>and</strong> nerves.ReferencesEdelson, E. (1991). Tracing human linages. Mosaic 2(3):56-63.Gossman, L.I. (1990). Invited editorial: Mitochondrial DNA insickness <strong>and</strong> in health. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 46(3):415-417.Harpending, H. (1994). Gene frequencies, DNA sequences,<strong>and</strong> human origins. Persp. Biol. Med. 37(3):384-394.Lyon, M.F. (1993). Epigenetic inheritance in mammals. TrendsGenet. 9(4):123-128.Martin, J.B. (1993). Molecular genetics <strong>of</strong> neurological diseases.Science 262:674-676.McBride, G. (1991). Nontraditional inheritance—<strong>the</strong> clinicalimplications. Mosaic 22(3):12-25.Rennie, J. (1993). DNA’s new twists. Sci. Am. 269(3):122-132.Tarleton, J. (1993/94). New inheritance patterns: New counselingdilemmas. Persp. Genet. Coun. 15(4):1, 8.©1997 by BSCS.146


Puzzling Pedigrees ■ Activity 2Copymaster 2-5 Science ArticlesSperm Meets OvumWhat happens when sperm(male gametes) contact <strong>the</strong> ovum(female gamete)? Although manysperm reach <strong>the</strong> ovum, only a singles p e rm enters <strong>and</strong> fertilizes <strong>the</strong>female gamete. What enables <strong>the</strong>s p e rm to reach this destination?Sperm are propelled by a whiplikemotion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sperm tail. Once in<strong>the</strong> vicinity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ovum, spermmove specifically toward it.To reach <strong>the</strong> ovum, sperm haveto pass through <strong>the</strong> uterus <strong>and</strong> into<strong>the</strong> fallopian tubes, where fertilizationoccurs. This path is an enormousdistance compared with <strong>the</strong>tiny size <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sperm, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>sperm require much energy duringthis journey. Sperm rely on energyrelease in <strong>the</strong> mitochondria topower <strong>the</strong>ir movement. Mitochondriaare found in <strong>the</strong> upper tail section<strong>of</strong> sperm.Sperm are tiny compared to <strong>the</strong>ovum, which is a very large cell. Theovum’s cytoplasm contains chromosomesthat have partially completedmeiosis, structures that helpprocess new proteins, <strong>and</strong> manymitochondria.Once a sperm successfullyenters <strong>and</strong> fertilizes <strong>the</strong> ovum, it nolonger needs its long tail. The tailsection disintegrates after <strong>the</strong> spermpenetrates <strong>the</strong> ovum, <strong>and</strong> only <strong>the</strong>sperm nucleus, which carries <strong>the</strong>p a t e rnal chromosomes, surv i v e sinside <strong>the</strong> ovum. O<strong>the</strong>r dramaticchanges take place in <strong>the</strong> membrane<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ovum that prevent <strong>the</strong> entry<strong>of</strong> any additional sperm. Changesinduced by <strong>the</strong> entry <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> singlesperm include <strong>the</strong> formation <strong>of</strong> aprotective layer around <strong>the</strong> newlyfertilized ovum. In addition, entry <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> sperm stimulates completion <strong>of</strong>meiosis in <strong>the</strong> ovum in preparationfor <strong>the</strong> final combination <strong>of</strong> paternal<strong>and</strong> maternal chromosomes in <strong>the</strong>nucleus <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> new zygote.147©1997 by BSCS.


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3Copymaster 3-1: HD Clues Presenting Familial Relationships <strong>and</strong> Health DataHD CLUES FOR TEAM 11Horace had only one child, a son named Allen, who was born in 1916.1Martha married Horace in 1910, when she was 24 years old. Horace was 29 at <strong>the</strong> time.1Martha’s gr<strong>and</strong>daughter, Kristen, was <strong>the</strong> twin who never married,but her identical twin sister, Ann, did marry.1Ann’s fa<strong>the</strong>r, Allen, died <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease at <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 60, 10 years after symptoms appeared.HD CLUES FOR TEAM 22Ann’s paternal gr<strong>and</strong>mo<strong>the</strong>r, Martha, began to show signs <strong>of</strong> confused thought at <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 55.2Horace’s only son was married to Linda.2The twins’ older bro<strong>the</strong>r, Andrew, looked a lot like his mo<strong>the</strong>r, Linda.2Kristen was not as close emotionally to her identical twin sister, Ann, afterAnn married Greg in 1961. Ann was 17 <strong>and</strong> Greg was 19. Kristen never married.149©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceCopymaster 3-1: HD Clues Presenting Familial Relationships <strong>and</strong> Health DataHD CLUES FOR TEAM 33Martha died in 1951 at <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 65, <strong>of</strong> what might have been Huntington disease.She died before her youngest gr<strong>and</strong>child, Debbie, was born.3When Bill married Andrew’s youngest sister, Debbie, in 1974, she was 19 <strong>and</strong> he was 22. Bill had no idea hewas marrying into a family with a history <strong>of</strong> HD. He knew <strong>of</strong> no one in his family who had <strong>the</strong> disease.3Debbie <strong>and</strong> her husb<strong>and</strong>, Bill, bought a savings bond for <strong>the</strong>ir gr<strong>and</strong>daughter,Cathy. Cathy’s mo<strong>the</strong>r, Paula, thanked her parents for this gift.3Andrew was one year older than his twin sisters, but he was 12 years older than his sister Debbie.HD CLUES FOR TEAM 44Horace was born in 1881 <strong>and</strong> died at <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 72.He was very active <strong>and</strong> had a clear mind throughout his life.4Ann <strong>and</strong> Greg realized when <strong>the</strong>ir son, Nathaniel, was born in 1962 that Ann’s fa<strong>the</strong>r had Huntingtondisease. Ann may have inherited it too. Ann <strong>and</strong> Greg were so poor <strong>and</strong> so worried about Ann’s healththat <strong>the</strong>y placed <strong>the</strong>ir son, Nathaniel, for adoption by <strong>the</strong> Wu family.4Jean’s husb<strong>and</strong>, Nathaniel, wanted her to name <strong>the</strong>ir son after him, but she insisted on <strong>the</strong> name “Peter”instead. The year was 1992. Jean was 32 years old.4Debbie was only 7 years old when her older sister, Ann, <strong>and</strong> her husb<strong>and</strong>,Greg, placed <strong>the</strong>ir son for adoption.©1997 by BSCS.150


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3Copymaster 3-1: HD Clues Presenting Familial Relationships <strong>and</strong> Health DataHD CLUES FOR TEAM 55When Christina was a little girl, her fa<strong>the</strong>r, Andrew, began to show symptoms <strong>of</strong> HD.His movements became awkward <strong>and</strong> his muscles twitched. Andrew was 37 years old.5Martha’s gr<strong>and</strong>son, Andrew, died <strong>of</strong> HD when he was 50 years old,13 years after his symptoms first appeared. The year was 1993.5Allen’s gr<strong>and</strong>son, Joseph, was born in 1970. Joseph was three years younger than his sister,Christina, <strong>and</strong> five years older than his cousin, Paula.5Joseph feared Huntington disease. His big sister, Christina, began to show symptoms when she was only 26years old. Joseph passed his 27th birthday, however, without showing symptoms <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> disease.HD CLUES FOR TEAM 66Evan’s Aunt Kristen got sick soon after his Aunt Ann. At <strong>the</strong> time she showed symptoms <strong>of</strong> HD, Ann was 39years old. Kristen was 40 when her HD symptoms appeared. Evan’s mo<strong>the</strong>r, Debbie, was healthy.6When she was a teenager, Paula liked to “give orders” to her younger bro<strong>the</strong>r,Evan, who was born in 1982. She was seven years older than Evan.6When Paula gave birth to Cathy in 1997, Paula’s mo<strong>the</strong>r, Debbie, became a gr<strong>and</strong>mo<strong>the</strong>r.She did not feel like a gr<strong>and</strong>mo<strong>the</strong>r; she was only 42 years old, <strong>and</strong> kept active physically <strong>and</strong> mentally,attending dance class <strong>and</strong> running her own business.6Evan is younger than his cousins, Joseph, Christina, <strong>and</strong> Nathaniel. Evan is 8 years younger than his bro<strong>the</strong>rin-law,Bob, <strong>and</strong> only 10 years older than Nathaniel’s son, Peter.151©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceCopymaster 3-1: HD Clues Presenting Familial Relationships <strong>and</strong> Health DataHD CLUES FOR TEAM 77Jama gave birth to a daughter, Christina, in 1967. Jama was 22 years old.7Paula <strong>and</strong> her bro<strong>the</strong>r, Evan, are healthy. Their cousin, Christina, is not as lucky; she has suffered from twitchymuscles <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r symptoms <strong>of</strong> HD since 1993, when she was 26 years old.7Jama married Andrew in 1965, when she was 20, long before she knew that Andrewhad inherited <strong>the</strong> fatal disorder known as Huntington disease.7Bob <strong>and</strong> his fa<strong>the</strong>r-in-law, Bill, took photos <strong>the</strong> day Bob’s daughter,Cathy, took her first steps. Bob’s wife, Paula, was away, visiting her bro<strong>the</strong>r, Evan.HD CLUES FOR TEAM 88Paula’s mo<strong>the</strong>r, Debbie, has not shown any symptoms <strong>of</strong> HD as <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 42, although Debbie’s twin sistersbegan to show symptoms before this age.8When Nathaniel was 33 years old, in 1995, he began to suffer from lapses <strong>of</strong> memory. His mo<strong>the</strong>r, Ann, <strong>and</strong>her twin sister still appeared healthy when <strong>the</strong>y were 33. Later, <strong>the</strong>y developed HD.8Linda was alert <strong>and</strong> active in sports throughout her fifties. By <strong>the</strong> time she reached 70,in 1992, she no longer played tennis, but she was still healthy.8Nathaniel’s mo<strong>the</strong>r, her twin sister, <strong>and</strong> Nathaniel’s Uncle Andrew all showed symptoms <strong>of</strong>HD when <strong>the</strong>y were fairly young. Nathaniel’s mo<strong>the</strong>r was 39, his Aunt Kristen was 40,<strong>and</strong> his Uncle Andrew was 37 when <strong>the</strong>y showed HD symptoms.©1997 by BSCS.152


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3Copymaster 3-2: HD Clues Presenting Molecular Data (one copy per team)In 1993, researchers developed a test for <strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene. A particular trinucleotide may berepeated many times in <strong>the</strong> mutant gene. The following clues provide molecular data about <strong>the</strong> familyyou have been studying.HD PEDIGREE - MOLECULAR DATABy <strong>the</strong> time Bob married Paula, he knew that <strong>the</strong> genetic data for her trinucleotide repeat countwere (13, 12). He wasn’t tested because his family had no history <strong>of</strong> HD.When Evan heard <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetic tests done on his parents <strong>and</strong> sister, he chose not to be tested.Christina was tested for HD <strong>the</strong> year her fa<strong>the</strong>r died <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> disease. Her trinucleotide repeats counts were (93, 7).Joseph had a genetic test for HD before he would ask Becky to marry him. When he found out his testresults were (7, 6), he asked her right away. The year was 1993; Becky was 24 years old.Although Becky’s family had no history <strong>of</strong> HD, she <strong>of</strong>fered to be tested because she knew how concernedher husb<strong>and</strong>, Joseph, was about this disease. Her counts were (20, 8).Andrew’s wife, Jama, had <strong>the</strong>se counts for trinucleotides in <strong>the</strong> gene associated with HD: (7, 18).In 1992, at age 30, Nathaniel Wu applied for a job as a laboratory technician. As part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> applicationprocess, he was tested for several genetic disorders <strong>and</strong> was shocked to find out that he had inheritedan HD mutation. His trinucleotide repeat counts are (72, 19).Dr. Engle had been so curious about <strong>the</strong> possibility <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation in Allen’s family that hepreserved a tissue sample from Allen <strong>and</strong> his wife, Linda, in 1957. Forty years later, an HD researchertested <strong>the</strong> samples <strong>and</strong> found <strong>the</strong>se results: Allen (46, 13); Linda (6, 22).In 1993, <strong>the</strong> year Andrew died <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease, <strong>the</strong> diagnosis was confirmed by a genetic test thatshowed he had one mutant HD gene with 69 trinucleotide repeats. The homologous copy had only 6repeats, apparently inherited from his mo<strong>the</strong>r.The twins, Kristen <strong>and</strong> Ann, were tested for HD. Here are <strong>the</strong> results: Kristen (64, 22), Ann (64, 22).Greg’s counts were (11, 19).Nathaniel knew his son, Peter, had a 50 percent chance <strong>of</strong> inheriting HD. The chance could havebeen higher, except that his wife’s counts for trinucleotide repeats in this gene were only (8, 19).Debbie was terrified that she had inherited HD, as had her three elder siblings.When she was tested, she found out that her trinucleotide counts were (13, 6).Bill’s counts were (8, 12).We have no record <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trinucleotide repeat counts for Horace or Martha; <strong>the</strong>y lived too long ago to be tested.153©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceCopymaster 3-3: Template for <strong>the</strong> HD Pedigree©1997 by BSCS.154


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3Copymaster 3-4: Science Article About a <strong>Genetics</strong> DiscoveryNew <strong>Genetics</strong> Discovery:Trinucleotide Repeat ExpansionAlthough most DNA sequences are transmitted fromparent to child as exact copies, <strong>the</strong>re are occasionalexceptions. There can be an increase or decrease, forexample, in <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> copies <strong>of</strong> a repeated trinucleotidesequence found in certain genes. The changeoccurs when <strong>the</strong> gene is passed from parent to <strong>of</strong>fspring.Instability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trinucleotide repeats increases if <strong>the</strong>gene has a critical number <strong>of</strong> copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trinucleotiderepeats in a row. This phenomenon is called trinucleotiderepeat expansion, <strong>and</strong> it plays a role in <strong>the</strong>inheritance <strong>of</strong> several genetic disorders in humans.Two <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se disorders, myotonic dystrophy (DM)<strong>and</strong> Huntington disease (HD), are inherited as autosomaldominant traits, but <strong>the</strong> features <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> diseases canvary widely from one person to ano<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>and</strong> from onegeneration to ano<strong>the</strong>r. The mutant genes for both DM<strong>and</strong> HD have been identified. The mutant HD gene islocated on chromosome 4, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> mutation that causesDM is on chromosome 19.Both mutant genes contain a region <strong>of</strong> DNA wherethree specific nucleotides <strong>of</strong> DNA are repeated over <strong>and</strong>over—many times more than <strong>the</strong> number found in normalcopies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se genes. The number <strong>of</strong> copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>trinucleotide repeat influences symptoms in <strong>the</strong> associateddisorder.Genetic tests can determine <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> repeats ineach copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> DM- or HD-associated gene. (An individualinherits one copy <strong>of</strong> a gene from <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r <strong>and</strong> onefrom <strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>r.) The higher <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> repeats, <strong>the</strong>earlier <strong>the</strong> symptoms appear, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> symptoms may bemore severe, particularly with DM. For example, DMpatients with about 50-80 copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trinucleotideCTG have mild symptoms such as cataracts late in adulthood.O<strong>the</strong>r DM patients have more severe symptoms,such as muscle wasting <strong>and</strong> retardation as young adults.These patients generally have 80 to a few hundredcopies <strong>of</strong> CTG in <strong>the</strong> mutant gene. The most severelyaffected patients show characteristic muscle problems<strong>and</strong> retardation when <strong>the</strong>y are children. Individuals withsevere DM have hundreds to more than 2,000 copies <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> CTG trinucleotide repeat.People who have HD can have anywhere from about39 copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sequence CAG to more than 120 copies.Two-thirds <strong>of</strong> HD patients have 40-50 copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> CAGtrinucleotide in <strong>the</strong> HD mutant gene. A copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>nucleotides in <strong>the</strong> gene associated with HD is passedfrom parent to child without change, except for thosenucleotides represented by <strong>the</strong> letters in bold in <strong>the</strong>illustration. The number <strong>of</strong> times CAG is repeated canchange during transmission, ei<strong>the</strong>r exp<strong>and</strong>ing or contracting.As a result <strong>of</strong> this change, <strong>the</strong> child who inherits<strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene could exhibit a different phenotypefrom that <strong>of</strong> his or her parent, perhaps with adifferent age <strong>of</strong> onset. The chance that <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong>repeats will exp<strong>and</strong> is greater if <strong>the</strong> mutant HD gene isinherited from <strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>r than it is from <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r.ReferencesChakraborty, R., et al. (1996). Segregation distortion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>CTG repeats at <strong>the</strong> myotonic dystrophy locus. Am. J. Hum.Genet. 59:109-118.MacDonald, M.E. et al. (1993). Capturing a CAGey killer.Genome Analysis Volume 7: Genome Rearrangement <strong>and</strong>Stability. Cold Spring Harbor, NY: Cold Spring Harbor LaboratoryPress.Nance, M.A. (1996). Invited Editorial: Huntington disease—Ano<strong>the</strong>r chapter rewritten. Am. J. Hum. Genet.59:1-6.DNA sequence from a mutant gene for HD, with <strong>the</strong> CAG repeat in <strong>the</strong> middle:AG C TAG C AGAC T GAT C GAT GTAC GTAC GTTAG C TAGT G C AT GAG C GAT G C TAG C TTAG C TAGTC TAT G C ATTAG C ATC AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG CC AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AGC AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AGCAG C AG C AG C AG C AG C AGT G G C AT C GAT G C AT GAT C TAG C ATAG GAC T C TAGAGAC C C C ATGCATTACGATTACGATTATCGACCCCATAGGGATCGTACGATGCATCGATGCAGCATG155©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceCopymaster 3-5 Part A:DM Clues Presenting Familial Relationships <strong>and</strong> Health DataSean gets a lot <strong>of</strong> attention from his gr<strong>and</strong>parents, Kevin <strong>and</strong> Elizabeth, because he is <strong>the</strong>ir only gr<strong>and</strong>child.Elizabeth was 55 when Sean was born. Kevin was 54 <strong>and</strong> ill.Sean spent weeks at birth in 1991 in intensive care because his severe weakness made breathing difficult.Doctors suspected he had a severe form <strong>of</strong> myotonic dystrophy. His parents,Maureen <strong>and</strong> Jonathan, were very worried.Amelda <strong>and</strong> Bob had three children. Their oldest son, Kevin, <strong>and</strong> youngest son, David, were 6 years apart.A middle child, Miriam, was born in 1938, 1 year after Kevin. She had cataracts when shewas 36 <strong>and</strong> suffered from muscle weakness.When Kevin was 36, in 1973, he began to go bald <strong>and</strong> his eyesight weakened. The doctor discovered he hadcataracts, which are unusual in an adult that young. Kevin also had moderate muscle weakness. His onlydaughter, Maureen, was 9 years old at that time.Kevin’s mo<strong>the</strong>r, Amelda, had cataracts by <strong>the</strong> time she reached age 45 in 1967. His fa<strong>the</strong>r,Bob, was healthy all his life <strong>and</strong> died at age 80, in 1994, <strong>the</strong> same year Kevin died.Amelda suffered from mild muscle weakness, but she lived to be 71 years old, dying in 1993,when her oldest son, Kevin, was 56 years old.Amelda’s mo<strong>the</strong>r, Bertha, developed cataracts in her late 50s, but o<strong>the</strong>rwise she had no serious healthproblems until she had a heart attack at age 72. She died in 1965, at age 73.When Bob married Amelda in 1940, he was 26 years old, <strong>and</strong> she was 18. They had 3 children <strong>and</strong>were happy toge<strong>the</strong>r for 53 years, until Amelda died. Bob died a year later.Wilbur was strong <strong>and</strong> active, but he died in a train crash in 1942 at age 52. His widow,Bertha, was devastated, as was his only child, Amelda.Kevin’s daughter, Maureen, married Jonathan in 1985, when she was 21 years old.Six years later <strong>the</strong>ir son, Sean, was born.In 1997, at <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 47, Jonathan is very athletic, with strong muscles, good coordination, <strong>and</strong> sharp eyesight.His wife, Maureen, was less fortunate. She had several miscarriages before her son, Sean, was born.A few years after her marriage at age 25, Maureen got cataracts. A year later doctors discovered she had a heart -rhythm problem <strong>and</strong> muscle weakness. She died young, at age 33, when her son Sean was only 6 years old.David married Julie in 1965. She was 20 years old, 2 years younger than David. As <strong>of</strong> 1997,both are healthy <strong>and</strong> happy.©1997 by BSCS.156


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3Copymaster 3-5 Part B:DM Clues Presenting Molecular DataDavid <strong>and</strong> Julie had two daughters: Cindy (born in 1965) <strong>and</strong> Jean (born in 1968).Both daughters are healthy <strong>and</strong> active in sports.A genetic test can show <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> gene whose mutant form causes DM. Theresults for Sean’s parents were Jonathan (10, 15); Maureen (621, 12).The year Amelda died, she was tested to determine <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats in her mutant DMgene. The results were (211, 6). Her husb<strong>and</strong>, Bob, was also tested. His results were (6, 10).The year Kevin’s mo<strong>the</strong>r died, Kevin <strong>and</strong> his wife, Elizabeth, had genetic testing done.The results showed <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> gene associated with DM.For Elizabeth, <strong>the</strong> numbers were (12, 14); for Kevin, <strong>the</strong>y were (400, 6).Although Sean was a baby, his severe symptoms <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> health problems <strong>of</strong> his mo<strong>the</strong>r alerted doctors totest him for DM. He had a remarkably high number <strong>of</strong> trinucleotide repeats in one copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene.His test results were (1232, 15).David was tested for DM. His trinucleotide repeat numbers were (6, 10). His wife, Julie, was tested, too.Her results were (18, 19).Cindy’s test results were (6, 19) for trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> gene associated with DM.Her sister’s results were (10, 18).157©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceCopymaster 3-6: Template for <strong>the</strong> DM Pedigree©1997 by BSCS.158


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3Copymaster 3-7: Completed HD Pedigree159©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceCopymaster 3-7: Completed DM Pedigree©1997 by BSCS.160


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3Copymaster 3-8: History <strong>of</strong> Huntington Disease - VignettesMiriam was tested for <strong>the</strong> mutant DM gene. Her trinucleotide repeats were (430, 10).VIGNETTE ONE: THE YEAR IS 1914Jeffrey, an enthusiastic young medical student, sits down at <strong>the</strong> cafe table already occupied by his two friends,Joe <strong>and</strong> Morris. Jeffrey excitedly shows <strong>the</strong>m a translation <strong>of</strong> a medical report originally published by aNorwegian doctor, Johan Lund, in 1860. He tells Morris <strong>and</strong> Joe that one <strong>of</strong> his pr<strong>of</strong>essors brought <strong>the</strong> articleto class. Joe reads it quickly <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>s it to Morris.“This is a report <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same genetic disorder described by Dr. Huntington in 1872. But Dr. Lund called <strong>the</strong> condition‘twitches’ instead <strong>of</strong> ‘hereditary chorea,’” Jeffrey points out.“No, Lund just says that ‘twitches’ is <strong>the</strong> common name,” Morris replies. “He calls it ‘chorea <strong>of</strong> St. Vitus.’”“The name isn’t <strong>the</strong> issue,” Joe comments. “Both reports are based on multiple case histories <strong>and</strong> several generations.Huntington mentions a slightly earlier age <strong>of</strong> onset than does Lund. They both mention <strong>the</strong> combination<strong>of</strong> neurological symptoms, including uncontrolled muscle movements, memory loss, <strong>and</strong> even dementia.And both doctors report <strong>the</strong> hereditary nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> disease.”“But nei<strong>the</strong>r doctor <strong>of</strong>fers any explanation for <strong>the</strong> variation in progress <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> disease—rapid decline in mostpatients, but later <strong>and</strong> slower decline in o<strong>the</strong>rs. And nei<strong>the</strong>r doctor can explain <strong>the</strong> mechanism that causes <strong>the</strong>awful symptoms,” Jeffrey notes.“And no one has any idea <strong>of</strong> a cure...” says Morris. The three young men finish <strong>the</strong>ir c<strong>of</strong>fee in silence.VIGNETTE TWO: THE YEAR IS 1957Allen <strong>and</strong> Linda enter <strong>the</strong> house in silence. Both are thinking about what <strong>the</strong> neurologist, Dr. Engle, had told<strong>the</strong>m after he examined Allen. The doctor suspected that Allen had inherited a frightening illness he calledHuntington’s chorea. They had just heard <strong>the</strong> name <strong>of</strong> this disorder on <strong>the</strong> news. The famous folk singer,Woody Guthrie, had this illness. And now, perhaps, Allen had it, too.Dr. Engle’s tentative diagnosis was based in part on <strong>the</strong> memory losses Allen had been experiencing. At his relativelyyoung age <strong>of</strong> 40, Allen should not have <strong>the</strong>se lapses. In addition, Linda had mentioned to <strong>the</strong> doctor thatAllen sometimes moved oddly, in an awkward “twitchy” way. When Dr. Engle asked about Allen’s parents, helearned that Allen’s mo<strong>the</strong>r had shown similar symptoms, but <strong>the</strong>y had not even started until she was 55, some15 years older than Allen is now. She had lived ano<strong>the</strong>r ten years after <strong>the</strong> symptoms appeared. Dr. Englethought that Allen had inherited Huntington’s chorea from his mo<strong>the</strong>r. That would fit with its pattern <strong>of</strong> autosomaldominant inheritance. And it would fit with <strong>the</strong> pattern <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation that Dr. Engle had readabout in cases <strong>of</strong> Huntington’s chorea <strong>and</strong> myotonic dystrophy. However, he knew that his colleague, Dr. R<strong>and</strong>all,was skeptical that genetic anticipation really occurred. It could just be a misperception because <strong>of</strong> a lack<strong>of</strong> scientific rigor in <strong>the</strong> way data were collected as case histories.However, this scientific debate was far from <strong>the</strong> reality <strong>of</strong> life for Allen <strong>and</strong> Linda. They thought only aboutAllen’s symptoms <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> danger that might be ahead for Allen <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir four young children. Had <strong>the</strong>y unwit-161©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceCopymaster 3-8: History <strong>of</strong> Huntington Disease - Vignettestingly passed on this terrible legacy to <strong>the</strong>m?VIGNETTE THREE: THE YEAR IS 1986Ann <strong>and</strong> Greg visit a genetic counselor. Ann has begun to show symptoms <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease, a disorderthat runs in her family. Her bro<strong>the</strong>r Andrew is very ill with it, <strong>and</strong> her fa<strong>the</strong>r died <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> disease. Ann wants toknow if she should try to have an adoption agency contact her biological son, Nathaniel, who was placed foradoption when he was a newborn. Should she warn him that he is at risk? Now that Ann knows that she has<strong>the</strong> disorder, <strong>the</strong> known risk for Nathaniel is 50 percent. He is 24 years old, an adult who might want to be tested.The counselor explains to Ann that, although <strong>the</strong> gene for HD had just been mapped to chromosome 4,<strong>the</strong>re is still no widely available test, although one might be available soon. She suggests that Ann should waituntil that time to tell Nathaniel <strong>of</strong> his biological family <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> risk <strong>of</strong> HD.VIGNETTE FOUR: THE YEAR IS 1997Jean Wu makes an appointment with a genetic counselor. She had been to this <strong>of</strong>fice several times because herhusb<strong>and</strong>, Nathaniel, has Huntington disease in its early stages. This time, however, she has come to talk to Dr.Feynmann about her son, Peter. She wants to have her son tested for <strong>the</strong> disease. She worries constantly aboutnot knowing her son’s fate with regard to HD. Dr. Feynmann explains that, although Jean <strong>and</strong> her husb<strong>and</strong> havebeen tested, <strong>the</strong> current policy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> clinic (<strong>and</strong> most clinics) is to withhold testing for children who are minors<strong>and</strong> who do not show any symptoms. That way <strong>the</strong> minors can make <strong>the</strong> choice to be tested for <strong>the</strong>mselveswhen <strong>the</strong>y are adults, ra<strong>the</strong>r than having a parent decide for <strong>the</strong>m.©1997 by BSCS.162


Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene? ■ Activity 4Copymaster 4-1 Worksheet for a Discussion About PolicyCurrent policy: To withhold genetic testing for Huntington disease for asymptomatic minorsReasons to maintain policyReasons to change policy163©1997 by BSCS.


STUDENT PAGES FORCLASSROOM ACTIVITIESPhotocopy <strong>the</strong>se pages for student use.


Engage ActivityScientific InvestigationPROCEDURE1. Share materials with your team, but work individually.Select a peanut <strong>and</strong> carefully observe it todetermine <strong>the</strong> distinguishing characteristics thatidentify this particular peanut. Record your observationson a piece <strong>of</strong> paper. Do not mark or crack<strong>the</strong> peanut. You may use <strong>the</strong> equipment providedto help you with your observations.2. Return each team member’s peanut to <strong>the</strong> bowl.Mix up <strong>the</strong> peanuts.3. Use your notes to find your peanut again.4. Raise your h<strong>and</strong> if you are absolutely certain youhave found your own peanut.5. What evidence did you use to locate your peanut<strong>and</strong> distinguish it from <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs in <strong>the</strong> bowl?6. Exchange your team’s bowl <strong>of</strong> peanuts <strong>and</strong> observationnotes with those from ano<strong>the</strong>r team. Nowwork individually with one set <strong>of</strong> observationsfrom ano<strong>the</strong>r student <strong>and</strong> try to find <strong>the</strong> particularpeanut it describes.7. Raise your h<strong>and</strong> if you are absolutely certain thatyou have found <strong>the</strong> correct peanut.ANALYSIS1. What observations were most valuable in finding aspecific peanut?2. What role did your notes or <strong>the</strong> notes <strong>of</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>rstudent play in helping you to locate a particularpeanut? (If your memory was a better guide, whatdoes that say about your notes?)3. People <strong>of</strong>ten confuse observations with inferences.Observations are collected using your senses,ei<strong>the</strong>r directly or exp<strong>and</strong>ed by technologyusing devices such as microscopes, X- r a ymachines, or microwave sensors on satellites.Inferences are ideas or conclusions based on whatyou observe or already know. Using this distinction,which <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> following statements are observations<strong>and</strong> which are inferences?■ If this peanut is roasted, <strong>the</strong> seeds will notgerminate.■ The shell has a rough surface.■ The shell is uniformly colored.■ The peanuts came from a plant.■ The shell has two lobes <strong>and</strong> is smaller indiameter between <strong>the</strong>m.■ This peanut will taste good.■ Squirrels would eat <strong>the</strong>se peanuts.■ The surface markings on <strong>the</strong> shell are inrows, running lengthwise.4. Now, look at your notes <strong>and</strong> label any inferencesthat you included.5. What counts as evidence in science?6. What makes science objective? Why is objectivityimportant?167


Activity 1St<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong>Shoulders <strong>of</strong> GiantsIf you make a scientific discovery, will people still relyon it one hundred years later? Scientists continue touse <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>ories <strong>of</strong> inheritance described by GregorMendel (Figure 1-1)—<strong>the</strong>y are remarkably durableafter more than a century. Since <strong>the</strong> rediscovery <strong>of</strong>Mendel’s work in about 1900, biologists have madegreat strides in determining <strong>the</strong> mechanisms <strong>of</strong>heredity. Knowledge about genetics has exp<strong>and</strong>ed in<strong>the</strong> last two decades with technical advances in molecularbiology <strong>and</strong>, most recently, with <strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> Human Genome Project (HGP). This huge projectwill identify genetic relationships (maps) <strong>and</strong>chromosomal locations <strong>of</strong> all human genes <strong>and</strong> willattempt to determine <strong>the</strong> DNA sequence for <strong>the</strong>entire genome <strong>of</strong> Homo sapiens. Mapping <strong>and</strong>sequencing will be done for o<strong>the</strong>r species, too,including selected bacteria, yeast, a plant, <strong>and</strong> severalanimal species.D i s c o v e ry in <strong>the</strong> HGP or any field <strong>of</strong> science occursin stages. Similarly, <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> genetics is muchmore than a simple record <strong>of</strong> dates, names, <strong>and</strong> discoveries;it is an account <strong>of</strong> how our underst<strong>and</strong>ing<strong>of</strong> inheritance <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> gene has grown <strong>and</strong>changed. Modern geneticists (Figure 1-2) are“st<strong>and</strong>ing on <strong>the</strong> shoulders <strong>of</strong> giants” who camebefore <strong>the</strong>m.PROCEDUREPart I: Milestones inUnderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>Genetics</strong>Much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> information about genetics in yourbiology textbook would have amazed biologists ahundred years ago. Those scientists, driven bycuriosity to answer complex questions <strong>of</strong> heredity,slowly pieced toge<strong>the</strong>r layer after layer <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> milestoneexplanations that we now accept as valid. Themost significant explanations st<strong>and</strong> as milestoneevents, each <strong>of</strong> which marks a great shift in ourunderst<strong>and</strong>ing. Think about what scientists neededto know b e f o r e <strong>the</strong>y could add each new milestoneto <strong>the</strong> body <strong>of</strong> genetics knowledge. You are goingto build a sequence <strong>of</strong> milestone explanations.When you do, your sequence may reflect <strong>the</strong> actualprogress <strong>of</strong> genetics during <strong>the</strong> last hundred or soyears, or it may reflect o<strong>the</strong>r ways that history couldhave played itself out during <strong>the</strong>se early years <strong>of</strong>d i s c o v e ry.1. Your team will receive a set <strong>of</strong> eight milestoneexplanations <strong>of</strong> inheritance. Decide how <strong>the</strong>semilestone explanations could form a meaningfulsequence, <strong>and</strong> be prepared to report yoursequence <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> reasons you chose it.169


Activity 2Puzzling PedigreesHow do scientists decide <strong>the</strong> direction for <strong>the</strong>irresearch? For example, scientists want to know howgenes are involved in cancer. This problem is toolarge <strong>and</strong> complex to tackle all at once.Scientists, <strong>the</strong>refore, break large problems intos m a l l e r, focused parts. They propose a testableexplanation, called a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, to try to answer one<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> smaller questions. These hypo<strong>the</strong>ses showwhere <strong>and</strong> how to look for answers.As scientists make observations <strong>and</strong> record data,<strong>the</strong>y usually find that <strong>the</strong> new evidence reinforceswhat <strong>the</strong>y already know. Sometimes, however, <strong>the</strong>new data do not fit what is known. If <strong>the</strong> evidence isreliable, scientists cannot just ignore it. Instead, <strong>the</strong>ymust look for new explanations to extend or modifywhat is known. In this activity, you will put your skills<strong>of</strong> observation <strong>and</strong> your previous knowledge <strong>of</strong>heredity to work to study some puzzling patterns <strong>of</strong>inheritance.PROCEDUREPart I: Review <strong>of</strong> <strong>Inheritance</strong> Patterns1. Determine <strong>the</strong> most likely pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritancefor each <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pedigrees shown in Figure 2-1 onpage 174. To help you, Figure 2-2 lists <strong>the</strong> characteristics<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> four patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance thatyou studied in <strong>the</strong> genetics unit <strong>of</strong> your biologyclass. Your teacher will direct you to work on thistask alone or with a partner. You will have 10 minutesto complete this task.Part II: Puzzling Pedigrees2. Study two new pedigrees (Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J) shownin Figure 2-3. B o t hpedigrees illustrate <strong>the</strong> same trait.Try to identify <strong>the</strong> inheritance pattern illustrated by<strong>the</strong>se two pedigrees. Explain your responses, statingspecific examples to support your explanation.Part III: Testing an ExplanationHow good is <strong>the</strong> explanation <strong>of</strong> inheritance you suggestedin Part II? When you suggested an inheritancepattern, you were making a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. A scientifichypo<strong>the</strong>sis is a testable explanation. To determinehow good your hypo<strong>the</strong>sis is, use evidence to test it.3. Analyze <strong>the</strong> data (evidence) you have observedfrom <strong>the</strong> pedigrees to see whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y supportor conflict with your hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. (Your teacher willprovide a worksheet for your convenience.)Record your explanation for <strong>the</strong> inheritance patternsin Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J as “Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 1.”(Remember that both pedigrees demonstrate <strong>the</strong>same trait.) Next, list evidence that supports your173


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling PedigreesFigure 2-1 Pedigrees showing <strong>the</strong> inheritance <strong>of</strong> eight human traitsFigure 2-2 Four patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritanceAutosomal dominantMales <strong>and</strong> females are equally likely to have <strong>the</strong> trait.Traits do not skip generations (generally).The trait is present whenever <strong>the</strong> corresponding gene ispresent (generally).There is male-to-male transmission.X-linked dominantAll daughters <strong>of</strong> a male who has <strong>the</strong> trait will alsohave <strong>the</strong> trait.There is no male-to-male transmission.A female who has <strong>the</strong> trait may or may not pass <strong>the</strong> genefor that trait to her son or daughter.Autosomal recessiveMales <strong>and</strong> females are equally likely to have <strong>the</strong> trait.Traits <strong>of</strong>ten skip generations.Often, both parents <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring who have <strong>the</strong> trait are heterozygotes(<strong>the</strong>y carry at least one copy <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> allele).Only homozygous individuals have <strong>the</strong> trait.Traits may appear in siblings without appearing in <strong>the</strong>ir parents.If a parent has <strong>the</strong> trait, those <strong>of</strong>fspring who do not have itare heterozygous carriers <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> trait.X-linked recessiveThe trait is far more common in males than in females.All daughters <strong>of</strong> a male who has <strong>the</strong> trait are heterozygouscarriers.The son <strong>of</strong> a female carrier has a 50 percent chance <strong>of</strong> having<strong>the</strong> trait.There is no male-to-male transmission.Mo<strong>the</strong>rs <strong>of</strong> males who have <strong>the</strong> trait are ei<strong>the</strong>r heterozygouscarriers or homozygous <strong>and</strong> express <strong>the</strong> trait.Daughters <strong>of</strong> female carriers have a 50 percent chance <strong>of</strong>being carriers.174©1997 by BSCS.


Puzzling Pedigrees ■ Activity 2Figure 2-3 Two puzzling pedigreesexplanation <strong>and</strong> evidence that conflicts with it.Refer specifically to Pedigree I or J as you reportthis evidence.4. You can reason, calculate, or do experiments to testa hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. For example, assume that <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>rin <strong>the</strong> first generation <strong>of</strong> Pedigree I is heterozygous.For a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis <strong>of</strong> “autosomal dominant”:a. Consider each child singly. What is <strong>the</strong> probabilitythat <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r will transmit <strong>the</strong> gene inquestion? (You can calculate <strong>the</strong> probabilitybased on what you know about autosomal dominantinheritance.)b. You actually can model <strong>the</strong> events in <strong>the</strong> pedigree<strong>and</strong> use <strong>the</strong> data to test this hypo<strong>the</strong>sis.Use a coin to represent <strong>the</strong> two alleles involvedin this trait. Heads will represent <strong>the</strong> alleleresponsible for <strong>the</strong> trait. Tails will represent <strong>the</strong>allele that does not produce <strong>the</strong> trait. Model <strong>the</strong>inheritance shown in <strong>the</strong> second generation bytossing <strong>the</strong> coin six times, once for each child.Record <strong>the</strong> results, using a chart like <strong>the</strong> oneillustrated in Figure 2-4.c. Your coin toss gave you empirical (observ a b l e )data. You also can calculate <strong>the</strong> likelihood that allsix children inherited <strong>the</strong> trait from <strong>the</strong> femaleparent. (Hint: Recall your answer to Question4a, <strong>and</strong> remember that <strong>the</strong> probability for eachchild is based on a separate event. Use <strong>the</strong> productrule, based on <strong>the</strong> second law <strong>of</strong> probability,to find <strong>the</strong> probability for all six children.)5. Suppose that <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r exhibiting <strong>the</strong> trait in <strong>the</strong>first generation <strong>of</strong> Pedigree I is homozygous for adominant trait. Would that explanation be consistentwith <strong>the</strong> results you observed in her <strong>of</strong>fspringRecord results from each toss in <strong>the</strong> appropriate column.heads(allele for trait is present)tails(allele for trait is absent)Reminder: In autosomal dominant inheritance, if <strong>the</strong>allele is present, <strong>the</strong> trait will be present.Figure 2-4 A coin-toss modeling experiment175©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 2 ■ Puzzling Pedigrees(<strong>the</strong> second generation)? Use <strong>the</strong> data from <strong>the</strong>coin test to support your conclusion. What about<strong>the</strong> results in <strong>the</strong> third generation <strong>of</strong> Pedigree I?6. Examine Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J again carefully <strong>and</strong> summarizeyour observations on your worksheet.a. Indicate whe<strong>the</strong>r your tests confirm or refute<strong>the</strong> pattern you hypo<strong>the</strong>sized. Record yourresponse as your “conclusions.” (Hint: Yourconclusion could be in <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> a question.)b. If your earlier choice was refuted, can you nowpropose a new explanation? If so, record itunder “Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis 2” on your worksheet. Ifnot, do not worry. Steps 7-9 may help you.7. Use Questions 7a <strong>and</strong> 7b to help you find a newexplanation:a. Examine each instance where <strong>the</strong> trait is passedto <strong>of</strong>fspring. What do you observe about <strong>the</strong>source <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> inherited trait?b. Mendelian genetics assumes that in all caseseach parent contributes equally to <strong>the</strong> genotype<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>of</strong>fspring. Do <strong>the</strong> data shown inPedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J demonstrate this idea?Modify your conclusions or propose a new hypo<strong>the</strong>sisif your ideas have changed.As a final attempt to build a new explanation, useQuestions 8 <strong>and</strong> 9 to guide your thinking. When youhave recorded your best explanation, continue to<strong>the</strong> Analysis. (Your explanation may be incomplete.What is important is that you justify your reasoningwith evidence.)8. Could <strong>the</strong> inheritance <strong>of</strong> an allele carried on <strong>the</strong> Xor Y chromosome explain <strong>the</strong> pattern <strong>of</strong> inheritancein Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J? Explain your response.9. Consider <strong>the</strong> four patterns <strong>of</strong> inheritance in Figure2-2. What do <strong>the</strong>y assume about <strong>the</strong> location <strong>of</strong>genetic material in <strong>the</strong> cell?a. In humans, sperm cells <strong>and</strong> egg cells transmitgenetic information to <strong>of</strong>fspring. How do <strong>the</strong>secells differ?b. In addition to a complete set <strong>of</strong> chromosomes(46), what does <strong>the</strong> developing zygote need togrow <strong>and</strong> develop? (Hint: What structures arein <strong>the</strong> cytoplasm?)ANALYSIS1. Read <strong>the</strong> two articles your teacher distributes.What bearing does <strong>the</strong> information in <strong>the</strong> articleshave on your explanation <strong>of</strong> Pedigrees I <strong>and</strong> J?2. What lasting scientific knowledge about inheritancedid you use in this activity?3. What new explanations for inheritance did youuse in this activity?4. What methods <strong>of</strong> science did you use in thisa c t i v i t y ?176©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveriesin ScienceHow do you know that a new scientific idea is correct?Lots <strong>of</strong> people have good ideas about how toexplain what <strong>the</strong>y observe in living systems, but agood idea alone is not enough. Scientists constantlyhave to decide whe<strong>the</strong>r a new idea is valid, <strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong>y do so by using <strong>the</strong> requirements <strong>of</strong> science. Tomeet <strong>the</strong>se criteria, a good idea must be based onlogical reasoning, <strong>and</strong> it must be tested, preferablymany times <strong>and</strong> in many different ways. A scientistdraws an idea (hypo<strong>the</strong>sis) from early observ a t i o n s<strong>of</strong> a problem <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n collects evidence to determinewhe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> idea is scientifically valid. If <strong>the</strong>hypo<strong>the</strong>sis seems to be valid, <strong>the</strong> scientist willshare it with o<strong>the</strong>r scientists. They, in turn, mayrepeat <strong>the</strong> tests to see whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> evidence isreproducible, or <strong>the</strong>y may add new lines <strong>of</strong> evidencefrom different tests. If enough evidenceaccumulates, <strong>the</strong> scientific community accepts <strong>the</strong>hypo<strong>the</strong>sis as valid <strong>and</strong> it becomes part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>established body <strong>of</strong> scientific knowledge. In simplet e rms, people <strong>the</strong>n refer to <strong>the</strong> new idea as part <strong>of</strong>“what <strong>the</strong>y know about genetics.”This approach to building an explanation for a naturalprocess sounds fairly simple, but it can be complicated.For example, how do you tell <strong>the</strong> differencebetween discovery <strong>of</strong> a new process <strong>and</strong>misinterpretation <strong>of</strong> data? Such a debate arose ingenetics among people who studied two inheriteddisorders, Huntington disease (HD) <strong>and</strong> myotonicdystrophy (DM). In this activity, you will step into<strong>the</strong> shoes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> scientists involved in this debate<strong>and</strong> use <strong>the</strong> requirements <strong>of</strong> science to follow <strong>the</strong>h i s t o ry <strong>of</strong> this intriguing investigation. Read <strong>the</strong>descriptions <strong>of</strong> HD <strong>and</strong> DM in Figures 3-1 <strong>and</strong> 3-2 ifyou are not familiar with <strong>the</strong>se disorders.PROCEDUREPart I: Identifying <strong>the</strong> Mystery1. The dialogue below takes you back in history to1957. Read <strong>the</strong> dialogue <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n answer Questions1a-1e.Note that in this dialogue we use <strong>the</strong> term “chorea”;this name for HD was <strong>the</strong> common term used in <strong>the</strong>1950s.The year is 1957. This dialogue ispart <strong>of</strong> a discussion that mighthave taken place between a physician<strong>and</strong> a geneticist; we will call<strong>the</strong>m Dr. A. <strong>and</strong> Dr. B. As you read<strong>the</strong>ir discussion, think about why<strong>the</strong>y have different views.Dr. A: “I’ve been seeing a patient named Allen whomay have Huntington’s chorea. If so, I thinkhe is a good example <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation.”177


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in ScienceDr. B: “You mean that this disease runs in his family,but is showing up at an earlier age in Allenthan it did in his parents or gr<strong>and</strong>parents?”Dr. A: “ E x a c t l y. Allen is only 40, <strong>and</strong> already he hass h o rt periods <strong>of</strong> memory loss <strong>and</strong> suddenproblems with awkward movement <strong>of</strong> hislegs. When I interviewed him, I learned thathis mo<strong>the</strong>r died when she was 65, but, startingat age 55, people who knew her said shewas a bit ‘odd’ in her thinking. She spen<strong>the</strong>r last five years in a wheelchair, sufferingfrom severe muscle <strong>and</strong> movement problems.She appeared to have Huntington’schorea.”Dr. B: “But that doesn’t prove that genetic anticipationis a real phenomenon. What do youknow about Allen’s maternal gr<strong>and</strong>parents<strong>and</strong> great-gr<strong>and</strong>parents?”Dr. A: “Not much. His mo<strong>the</strong>r’s mo<strong>the</strong>r died whenshe was very young, during childbirth, so allwe know is that she did not show signs <strong>of</strong>Huntington’s chorea up to <strong>the</strong> age <strong>of</strong> 24.Allen’s gr<strong>and</strong>fa<strong>the</strong>r lived to be about 70, butAllen knew nothing about his general health.But you k n o w geneticists have report e dcases <strong>of</strong> anticipation for years. What about Dr.Bell’s discussion <strong>of</strong> it in her 1947 publicationin The Treasury <strong>of</strong> Human <strong>Inheritance</strong>? Shemakes a good argument for genetic anticipationin ano<strong>the</strong>r genetic disease, myotonicdystrophy.”D r. B: “You mean her mention that patients in an earliergeneration <strong>of</strong> a known pedigree generallyhad only mild symptoms, such as cataracts,when <strong>the</strong>y were middle-aged, while — ”Dr. A: “— while <strong>the</strong>ir children <strong>and</strong> gr<strong>and</strong>childrenhad muscle wasting <strong>and</strong> mental illness, moresevere in each subsequent generation — anexample <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation.”D r. B: “(Laughing) Perhaps. But you haven’t mentioneda different publication from that year,<strong>the</strong> one by that geneticist Penrose. He mentionsBell’s work <strong>and</strong> reports from o<strong>the</strong>r geneticists,but he points out that simple MendelianFigure 3-1 Huntington Disease (HD): Summary <strong>of</strong> SymptomsHuntington disease came to public attention in <strong>the</strong> 1940s when <strong>the</strong> well-known folk singer Woody Guthrie began to showsymptoms <strong>of</strong> this fatal genetic disorder. Huntington disease received notoriety again in <strong>the</strong> 1980s <strong>and</strong> early 1990s whenresearchers, including Dr. Nancy Wexler, hunted for <strong>and</strong> located <strong>the</strong> mutant gene that causes HD. Dr. Wexler caught <strong>the</strong> public’sinterest in part because she had a direct <strong>and</strong> personal concern with this genetic killer: her mo<strong>the</strong>r died <strong>of</strong> Huntington disease,so Dr. Wexler knew she was at risk.HD is inherited as an autosomal dominant disorder. It is rare, occurring at a frequency <strong>of</strong> about 1/20,000 in Western countries<strong>and</strong> less <strong>of</strong>ten in Asia <strong>and</strong> elsewhere. The onset <strong>of</strong> symptoms generally occurs in adulthood, but <strong>the</strong> age varies among individuals<strong>and</strong> between generations <strong>of</strong> affected people. The disease involves neurological function, <strong>and</strong> early symptoms includetwitchy muscles, awkwardness <strong>of</strong> movement, <strong>and</strong> memory dysfunction. Eventually symptoms become severe, <strong>and</strong> deathresults. The gene is located on chromosome 4.Figure 3-2 Myotonic Dystrophy (DM): Summary <strong>of</strong> SymptomsMyotonic dystrophy is a fairly rare inherited human disorder that occurs in 1/8000 Caucasians <strong>and</strong> even less frequently ino<strong>the</strong>r groups. DM shows a large degree <strong>of</strong> variability in <strong>the</strong> type <strong>and</strong> severity <strong>of</strong> symptoms. The age <strong>of</strong> onset may vary fromone family member to ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> those affected. In its mildest forms, DM leaves <strong>the</strong> individual asymptomatic until late in adulthood.Then it may show up only as cataracts on <strong>the</strong> eyes, which affect vision. Because cataracts are not uncommon in elderlypeople, many individuals with extremely mild DM symptoms may not realize <strong>the</strong>y have inherited this disorder. (Conversely, justbecause a relative <strong>of</strong> yours has had cataracts, do not assume that DM occurs in your family.)More serious symptoms include muscle abnormalities such as a breakdown <strong>of</strong> muscle tissue <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> inability to relax a muscleafter it has been contracted. O<strong>the</strong>r symptoms involve damage to <strong>the</strong> heart or <strong>the</strong> sexual organs (gonads). Some individualswho have inherited DM show mental retardation as children. A less severe form results in unusual drowsiness <strong>and</strong> inattentivenessin adults.Myotonic dystrophy is inherited as an autosomal dominant disorder. The gene is located on chromosome 19.©1997 by BSCS.178


Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Science ■ Activity 3inheritance <strong>of</strong> a dominant characteristic won’tresult in <strong>the</strong> increasing problems <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> diseasein children <strong>and</strong> gr<strong>and</strong>children <strong>of</strong> someone whohas <strong>the</strong> disease gene.”Dr. A: “Then how do you explain <strong>the</strong> observedanticipation?”D r. B: “I don’t, at least, not yet. It may be an error ino b s e rvation. Besides, I would need moredata.”References:J. Bell (1947), Dystrophia myotonica <strong>and</strong> allied disease,in The Tr e a s u ry <strong>of</strong> Human <strong>Inheritance</strong> IV. N e rvous diseases<strong>and</strong> muscular dystrophies, vol 5, p. 343; <strong>and</strong> L.S.Penrose (1947), The problem <strong>of</strong> anticipation in pedigrees<strong>of</strong> dystrophia myotonica, Annals <strong>of</strong> Eugenics14:125-132.Think about <strong>the</strong> dialogue as you respond to <strong>the</strong>sequestions.a. What is genetic anticipation?b. Why does Dr. B. want more data?c. Dr. B. refers to “an error in observation.” Howmight such an error arise? What could be doneabout it?d. What have you learned about dominant inheritance?Why is genetic anticipation n o texplained by this genetic concept?e. What else could be done to solve this mystery?Part II: Ga<strong>the</strong>ring Clues to <strong>the</strong> MysteryThe year is 1977. You have collectedinterview data from many <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> members <strong>of</strong> Allen’s family. Usewhat you know from <strong>the</strong> dialogue<strong>and</strong> from this new case-historyinformation to answer <strong>the</strong> ChallengeQuestions in Figure 3-3.2. Each team has different clues obtained from casehistories. You only have a few minutes to consideryour team’s clues before you pool your informationwith that from o<strong>the</strong>r teams.3. Follow your teacher’s instructions about how toreport your preliminary findings.Figure 3-3 Challenge QuestionsScientists had to think about genetic anticipation in severalsteps. They needed to break <strong>the</strong> problem intoaddressable questions.■ What evidence do you have to support or refute <strong>the</strong>existence <strong>of</strong> genetic anticipation?■ Do you have evidence that supports a possible explanationfor <strong>the</strong> genetic mechanism that causes anticipation?If yes, explain.4. What does this evidence tell you about <strong>the</strong> ChallengeQuestions in Figure 3-3 (if anything)?Now <strong>the</strong> year is 1997. Molecularbiologists have devised a test toidentify <strong>the</strong> mutant gene for HD<strong>and</strong> to determine some <strong>of</strong> its specialproperties. In addition, many<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> family members have had <strong>the</strong>test; in some cases blood samples stored from older,deceased members were tested. Your teacher willsupply you with an article that describes <strong>the</strong> newmolecular test for HD <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> this testwhen performed on <strong>the</strong> members <strong>of</strong> Allen’s family.5. Read <strong>the</strong> article <strong>and</strong> record <strong>the</strong> information from<strong>the</strong> HD molecular clues on your HD pedigree.Report this information to <strong>the</strong> class when yourteacher instructs you to do so.6. Why does each person tested have two repeatnumbers?7. Using this new evidence, once again address <strong>the</strong>Challenge Questions in Figure 3-3.8. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> strengths <strong>of</strong> a good scientific explanationis that it is supported by multiple lines <strong>of</strong> evidence.Use a set <strong>of</strong> clues for a different family tolook for additional evidence that supports youranswers to <strong>the</strong> preceding questions.Part IV: Discovering <strong>the</strong> History<strong>of</strong> Genetic AnticipationThe sequence <strong>of</strong> discovery demonstrated by <strong>the</strong>clues you used reflects <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> discovery about179©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 3 ■ Clues <strong>and</strong> Discoveries in Sciencegenetic anticipation. Use <strong>the</strong> brief scenes (vignettes)that your teacher will give you to build a more completepicture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> discovery about HD<strong>and</strong> genetic anticipation. As you read, try to determineat what point in <strong>the</strong> sequence <strong>the</strong> opening dialoguebetween <strong>the</strong> fictitious Drs. A. <strong>and</strong> B. mighthave taken place. What additional vignettes mightfollow <strong>the</strong>se in <strong>the</strong> future?9. Read <strong>the</strong> vignettes <strong>and</strong> discuss with your teammateswhat <strong>the</strong>y tell you about <strong>the</strong> history <strong>of</strong> ourunderst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> HD.10. As a team, draw up a brief outline <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> history<strong>of</strong> discovery about HD.11. Individually, write a new vignette that representswhat you predict may be <strong>the</strong> next level <strong>of</strong> discoveryabout HD.1 2 .After you complete <strong>the</strong> vignette, explain <strong>the</strong> aspects<strong>of</strong> science on which you based your predictions.ANALYSIS1. What is <strong>the</strong> relationship between <strong>the</strong> number <strong>of</strong>trinucleotide repeats in <strong>the</strong> mutant HD or DMgene <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> resulting phenotype?2. How does what you know about genetic anticipationcontrast with <strong>the</strong> traditional, Mendelian view<strong>of</strong> autosomal dominant inheritance?3. How have you used <strong>the</strong> methods <strong>of</strong> science in thisactivity?4. Write a vignette (brief scene or part <strong>of</strong> a story)about <strong>the</strong> future health <strong>of</strong> Peter, Cathy, <strong>and</strong> Sean.Use your case-history data <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> article you readto support your description.180©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 4Should TeenagersBe Tested for <strong>the</strong>Mutant HD Gene?How should you go about making good choicesabout important issues in life? Should you base yourdecisions on a whim, a coin toss, or a call to a psychichot line? Or should you rely on well-informed, wellreasonedanalysis? Scientific reasoning is a disciplinedway to underst<strong>and</strong> events in <strong>the</strong> naturalworld. Similarly, a discussion <strong>of</strong> ethical issues bringsreason <strong>and</strong> discipline to decisions that involve preferencesbased on values. We draw our values frommany sources, including history, law, religion, <strong>and</strong>family. Part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> task <strong>of</strong> ethics is to identify <strong>the</strong>sevalues clearly <strong>and</strong> to show why o<strong>the</strong>rs should regard<strong>the</strong>m as important. A discussion <strong>of</strong> ethical issues mayapply to what we do as individuals or to how publicpolicy is made. In this activity, you will use <strong>the</strong> principles<strong>of</strong> sound decision-making <strong>and</strong> ethics to decidewhe<strong>the</strong>r a policy that excludes teenagers from genetictesting is a good public policy.PROCEDUREPart I: Identifying <strong>the</strong> Issue1. People <strong>of</strong>ten express <strong>the</strong>ir opinions or concernsthrough a letter to <strong>the</strong> editor <strong>of</strong> a newspaper. Read<strong>the</strong> letter shown in Figure 4-1.2. Assume that a change in policy would give parents<strong>the</strong> right to request an HD test for a person who isunder 18 years <strong>of</strong> age. Decide whe<strong>the</strong>r you think<strong>the</strong> policy <strong>of</strong> not testing minors should st<strong>and</strong> or bechanged. Register your vote when your teacherpolls <strong>the</strong> class.Part II: Using Ethical Decision-making3. Form teams as directed by your teacher to conducta discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ethical issues in <strong>the</strong> letter.Regardless <strong>of</strong> your personal opinion on this issue,work with your team to draw up two lists <strong>of</strong>opposing ideas about this policy, using <strong>the</strong> worksheetprovided by your teacher. In <strong>the</strong> left column,list reasons <strong>the</strong> current policy is good. In <strong>the</strong>right column, list reasons <strong>the</strong> policy shouldchange or why <strong>the</strong> new policy is better. Record all<strong>of</strong> your team’s ideas.4. Present your team’s ideas to <strong>the</strong> class. Add notesto your own worksheet <strong>of</strong> new ideas that ariseduring <strong>the</strong> discussion with o<strong>the</strong>r teams.5. Consider <strong>the</strong> discussion <strong>of</strong> ethical issues performedby <strong>the</strong> class <strong>and</strong> vote again: Is <strong>the</strong> currentpolicy good <strong>and</strong> worth maintaining, or should itbe changed so that parents <strong>of</strong> minors at risk forHD can have <strong>the</strong>ir children tested?181


Activity 4 ■ Should Teenagers Be Tested for <strong>the</strong> Mutant HD Gene?Figure 4-1 Letter to <strong>the</strong> editor <strong>of</strong> a city newspaperANALYSIS1. Given that <strong>the</strong>re is no treatment at present for HD,how does acquiring scientific evidence about <strong>the</strong>presence or absence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> HD mutation (from agenetic test) change <strong>the</strong> discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> issue?2. What additional social <strong>and</strong> ethical issues mightnew knowledge in genetics raise?3. As a homework assignment, write a response toJean W.’s letter. You might let her know whe<strong>the</strong>ryou would choose to be tested, <strong>and</strong> why. In yourletter, describe what scientific evidence from agenetic test will tell you <strong>and</strong> how that informationaffects your choice. In addition, show how <strong>the</strong> discussion<strong>of</strong> ethical issues that your class conductedhas influenced your ideas.182©1997 by BSCS.


Activity 5What Do We Know?How Do We Know It?“They say getting too much sun cangive you cancer.”Who are “<strong>the</strong>y”? Should you believe what “<strong>the</strong>y” say,<strong>and</strong>, if so, should you change your behavior in anyway?All <strong>of</strong> us must assume responsibility for making decisionsthat affect us <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs, but this responsibilitydoes not guarantee that we will make good decisions.That skill requires lots <strong>of</strong> practice <strong>and</strong> carefulreasoning, <strong>and</strong> it requires intelligent analysis <strong>of</strong> information.For health-related comments, such as <strong>the</strong>preceding quote, you should consider <strong>the</strong> scientificvalidity <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> statement to determine whe<strong>the</strong>r youthink <strong>the</strong> statement is reliable. Even if you decidethat it is reliable, <strong>the</strong> choice <strong>of</strong> what to do about itstill remains yours. (Is having a tan or relaxing in <strong>the</strong>sunlight worth <strong>the</strong> risk <strong>of</strong> getting skin cancer? Howcan you reduce <strong>the</strong> risk?) Whatever you decide, <strong>the</strong>ability to determine <strong>the</strong> scientific validity <strong>of</strong> a popularclaim is a valuable skill for any person.As you work through this last activity, you will evaluatewhat you have learned in <strong>the</strong> previous activities.PROCEDUREPart I: Critique <strong>of</strong> Popular Media TopicsYou are going to critique claims from <strong>the</strong> popularpress for <strong>the</strong>ir scientific validity.1. Recall from <strong>the</strong> previous activities <strong>the</strong> requirements<strong>of</strong> a good scientific explanation <strong>and</strong> list<strong>the</strong>m.2. Join your team <strong>and</strong> look at <strong>the</strong> articles <strong>the</strong> teammembers have collected. Choose one to presentto <strong>the</strong> class. Discuss <strong>the</strong> reasons <strong>the</strong> article gives(if any) to support <strong>the</strong> claim being reported. (Beprepared to list <strong>the</strong> idea from <strong>the</strong> article <strong>and</strong> togive supporting evidence, if any, on a chart in <strong>the</strong>classroom. You have 5-10 minutes to prepare.)3. When your teacher gives <strong>the</strong> signal, move withyour team to one chart <strong>and</strong> quickly list <strong>the</strong> claim183


Activity 5 ■ What Do We Know? How Do We Know It?from your team’s article <strong>and</strong> indicate <strong>the</strong> source.Next show why this claim is supposed to be correct,according to <strong>the</strong> article. Spend only 2 minutesat this task.4. Move with your team to <strong>the</strong> next chart. Read <strong>the</strong>ideas listed by <strong>the</strong> previous team <strong>and</strong> critiquethis claim for its scientific validity. You have 3m i n u t e s .Part II: AnalysisRespond to <strong>the</strong> following questions as <strong>the</strong> class discusses<strong>the</strong> charts.1. What is <strong>the</strong> difference between a belief <strong>and</strong> an explanationthat meets <strong>the</strong> requirements <strong>of</strong> science?2. Based on <strong>the</strong> information presented on <strong>the</strong>charts, are any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> claims scientifically valid?3. How could your actions be influenced by knowingwhe<strong>the</strong>r a claim is scientifically valid?4. Many health or science claims have significance insociety because <strong>the</strong>y are popular; that is, <strong>the</strong>yhave emotional appeal for many people. Howwould <strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong> scientists be different if scientificexplanations were voted upon for popularityra<strong>the</strong>r than held up to <strong>the</strong> scientific st<strong>and</strong>ards discussedin this module?Part III: AssessmentRespond to <strong>the</strong> following questions or assignmentsaccording to your teacher’s instructions.1. Recall your experiences from <strong>the</strong> module as youanswer <strong>the</strong> following questions:a. What does science try to do?b. How do we know that our scientific explanationsare on <strong>the</strong> right track?c. What counts as evidence in science?d. What makes science objective?e. How <strong>and</strong> why does scientific knowledge change?f. What is <strong>the</strong> difference between pseudoscience<strong>and</strong> science?2. The previous activities used genetics examples toshow how science works.a. What genetics concepts did you discuss in thismodule?b. Were any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetics concepts you listednew to you? Identify <strong>the</strong>m <strong>and</strong> indicate <strong>the</strong>irrelationship to earlier knowledge.3. How are science <strong>and</strong> society related? Give at leasttwo examples.4. Your job is to critique two claims that you seereported in popular media. One should be a scienceor health report that you consider to be scientificallyreliable, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r should be a claimthat you think is not scientifically substantiated.(Remember, a claim could be based on a correctidea but reported poorly, making <strong>the</strong> report scientificallyunsound.)184©1997 by BSCS.


NONPROFIT ORG.U.S. POSTAGEPA I DCOLO. SPGS., CO.PERMIT NO. 461INNOVATIVE SCIENCE EDUCATION SINCE 19585415 Mark Dabling Blvd.Colorado Springs, CO 80918-3842ADDRESS CORRECTION REQUESTEDRETURN POSTAGE GUARANTEEDFREE - A monograph for <strong>the</strong> biology classroom(<strong>the</strong> third genome module from BSCS)The <strong>Puzzle</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Inheritance</strong>:<strong>Genetics</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Methods</strong> <strong>of</strong> ScienceContains ■ an extensive Overview for Teachers, including discussion <strong>of</strong>many new discoveries in genetics■ Classroom Activities , with teacher background material,copymasters, <strong>and</strong> student pages for six activitiesDesigned to ■ provide materials to teach about <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>and</strong> methods <strong>of</strong>science; update <strong>the</strong> content <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genetics curriculum; <strong>and</strong>provide pr<strong>of</strong>essional development for teachersDeveloped by ■ BSCS (Biological Sciences Curriculum Study)Supported by ■ The United States Department <strong>of</strong> Energy, as part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>Human Genome Project

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!